William S. Marras, Waldemar Karwowski-Fundamentals and Assessment Tools For Occupational Ergonomics (Occupational Ergonomics Handbook, Second Edition) (2006) PDF
William S. Marras, Waldemar Karwowski-Fundamentals and Assessment Tools For Occupational Ergonomics (Occupational Ergonomics Handbook, Second Edition) (2006) PDF
William S. Marras, Waldemar Karwowski-Fundamentals and Assessment Tools For Occupational Ergonomics (Occupational Ergonomics Handbook, Second Edition) (2006) PDF
Second Edition
fUNDAMENTALS AND
ASSESSMENT TOOLS FOR
OCCUPATIONAL ERGONOMICS
The Occupational Ergonomics Handbook
Second Edition
fUNDAMENTALS AND
ASSESSMENT TOOLS FOR
OCCUPATIONAL ERGONOMICS
Edited by
William S. Marras
The Ohio State University
Columbus, Ohio, U.S.A.
Waldemar Karwowski
University of Louisville
Louisville, Kentucky, U.S.A.
Published in 2006 by
CRC Press
Taylor & Francis Group
6000 Broken Sound Parkway NW, Suite 300
Boca Raton, FL 33487-2742
No part of this book may be reprinted, reproduced, transmitted, or utilized in any form by any electronic, mechanical, or
other means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying, microfilming, and recording, or in any information
storage or retrieval system, without written permission from the publishers.
For permission to photocopy or use material electronically from this work, please access www.copyright.com
(http://www.copyright.com/) or contact the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc. (CCC) 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA
01923, 978-750-8400. CCC is a not-for-profit organization that provides licenses and registration for a variety of users. For
organizations that have been granted a photocopy license by the CCC, a separate system of payment has been arranged.
Trademark Notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or registered trademarks, and are used only for
identification and explanation without intent to infringe.
Fundamentals and assessment tools / [edited by] W.S. Marras and W. Karwowski.-- 2nd ed.
p. cm.
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 0-8493-1937-4
1. Human engineering--Handbooks, manuals, etc. 2. Industrial hygiene--Handbooks, manuals, etc. I.
Marras, William S. (William Steven), date. II. Karwowski, Waldemar, date.
TA166.O258 2005
620.8'2--dc22 2005050633
Development of the 2nd edition of the Occupational Ergonomics handbook was motivated by our desire to
facilitate a wide application of ergonomics knowledge to work systems design, testing, and evaluation in
order to improve the quality of life for millions of workers around the world. Ergonomics (or human
factors) is defined by the International Ergonomics Association (www.iea.cc) as the scientific discipline
concerned with the understanding of interactions among humans and other elements of a system, and
the profession that applies theory, principles, data, and methods to design in order to optimize human
well-being and overall system performance. Ergonomists contribute to the design and evaluation of tasks,
jobs, products, environments, and systems in order to make them compatible with the needs, abilities,
and limitations of people.
The ergonomics discipline promotes a holistic approach to the design of work systems with due
consideration of the physical, cognitive, social, organizational, environmental, and other relevant
factors. The application of ergonomics knowledge should help to improve work system effectiveness
and reliability, increase productivity, reduce employee healthcare costs, and improve the quality of
production processes, services, products, and working life for all employees. In this context, professional
ergonomists, practitioners, and students should have a broad understanding of the full scope and breadth
of knowledge of this demanding and challenging discipline.
Fundamentals and Assessment Tools contains a total of 50 chapters divided into two parts.
Part I introduces the discipline and profession of ergonomics, including the systems approach
and human-centered design, quality management, risk theory in human-machine systems, legal
issues, cost justification for implementing ergonomics interventions, as well as professional certifica-
tion and education issues. The fundamental ergonomics knowledge covered also includes the areas of
epidemiology, engineering anthropometry, biomechanics, motor control, human strength evaluation,
cumulative spine loading, application of basic knowledge for the assessment of loading on the human
back, shoulders, legs, and feet; rehabilitating low back disorders, low-level exertions, pathomechanics
and musculoskeletal injury pathways; understanding of individual factors for musculoskeletal
disorders and adaptation. Other important topics include consideration of cognitive factors, design
of information devices and controls, cognitive processing, multimodal information processing,
tolerances and variation in human performance, the effects of personality, psychosocial work
factors, as well as the aging processes. Finally, the work environment issues, including vision and
visual and tactile performance, noise and auditory effects, vibrometry, and shiftwork are also
discussed.
Part II focuses on ergonomics assessment methods and tools and their validity. These comprise tools
for the assessment of physical and cognitive work demands and efforts. In the physical domain, the
selected topics include methods for evaluating working postures and assessment of the entire body
(REBA, RULA, and LUBA), methods for analysis of upper extremity loading and exposure (such as
v
PLIBEL, HAL, or SHARP method), wrist posture assessment and back assessment (NIOSH Lifting
Equation, 3DSSPM, Industrial Lumbar Motion Monitor, TLVs). Methods that are focused on the
psychophysical assessment techniques, cognitive task analysis, application of subjective scales of effort
and workload, and determination of rest allowances, are also presented.
We hope that the fundamental knowledge presented in this book will help the readers to improve their
understanding of the nature of complex human-artifacts interactions that occur in a variety of working
environments, especially from the perspective of the design, testing, evaluation, and management of
human-compatible systems.
We also hope that this book will be useful to a large number of professionals, practitioners, and
students who strive everyday to optimize the design of systems, products and processes, manage the
workers’ health and safety, and improve the overall quality and productivity of contemporary businesses.
William S. Marras
The Ohio State University
Waldemar Karwowski
University of Louisville
vi
About the Editors
William S. Marras, Ph.D., D.Sc. (Hon), C.P.E., holds the Honda endowed
chair in transportation in the department of industrial, welding, and
systems engineering at The Ohio State University. He is the director of
the biodynamics laboratory and holds joint appointments in the
departments of orthopedic surgery, physical medicine, and biomedical
engineering. He is also the codirector of The Ohio State University
Institute for ergonomics. Dr. Marras received his Ph.D. in bioengineering
and ergonomics from Wayne State University in Detroit, Michigan. He is
also a certified professional ergonomist (CPE).
His research is centered around occupational biomechanics. Specifi-
cally, his research includes workplace biomechanical epidemiologic
studies, laboratory biomechanic studies, mathematical modeling, and
clinical studies of the back and wrist. His findings have been published in over 170 refereed journal
articles, 7 books, and over 25 book chapters. He also holds several patents, including one for the
Lumbar Motion Monitor (LMM). Professor Marras has been selected by the National Academy of
Sciences to serve on several committees investigating causality and musculoskeletal disorders. He also
serves as the chair of the Human Factors Committee for the National Research Council within the
National Academy of Sciences.
His work has attracted national and international recognition. He has been twice winner (1993 and
2002) of the prestigious Swedish Volvo Award for low back pain research as well as Austria’s Vienna
Award for physical medicine. He recently won the Liberty Mutual Prize for injury prevention research.
Recently, he was awarded an honorary doctor of science degree from the University of Waterloo for his
work on the biomechanics of low back disorders.
In his spare moments, Dr. Marras trains in Shotkan karate (a black belt), enjoys playing and listening
to music, sailing, and fishing.
vii
Waldemar Karwowski, Sc.D., Ph.D., P.E., C.P.E., is professor of indus-
trial engineering and director of the center for industrial ergonomics at
the University of Louisville, Louisville, Kentucky. He holds an M.S.
(1978) in production engineering and management from the Technical
University of Wroclaw, Poland, and a Ph.D. (1982) in industrial
engineering from Texas Tech University. He was awarded the Sc.D.
(dr hab.) degree in management science by the Institute for Organiz-
ation and Management in Industry (ORGMASZ), Warsaw, Poland
(June 2004). He is also a board certified professional ergonomist
(BCPE). He also received doctor of science honoris causa from the
South Ukrainian State K.D. Ushynsky Pedagogical University of
Odessa, Ukraine (May 2004). His research, teaching, and consulting
activities focus on human system integration and safety aspects of advanced manufacturing enterprises,
human –computer interaction, prevention of work-related musculoskeletal disorders, workplace and
equipment design, and theoretical aspects of ergonomics science.
Dr. Karwowski is the author or coauthor of more than 300 scientific publications (including more than
100 peer-reviewed archival journal papers) in the areas of work systems design, organization, and
management; macroergonomics; human –system integration and safety of advanced manufacturing;
industrial ergonomics; neuro-fuzzy modeling in human factors; fuzzy systems; and forensics. He has
edited or coedited 35 books, including the International Encyclopedia of Ergonomics and Human
Factors, Taylor & Francis, London (2001).
Dr. Karwowski served as a secretary-general (1997–2000) and president (2000–2003) of the Inter-
national Ergonomics Association (IEA). He was elected as an honorary academician of the International
Academy of Human Problems in Aviation and Astronautics (Moscow, Russia, 2003), and was named the
alumni scholar for research (2004 –2006) by the J. B. Speed School of Engineering of the University
of Louisville. He has received the Jack A. Kraft Innovator Award from the Human Factors and
Ergonomics Society, USA (2004), and serves as a corresponding member of the European Academy of
Arts, Sciences and Humanities.
viii
Contributors
ix
Sven Hinrichsen Kristina Kemmlert Pepe Marlow
Institute of Industrial Psychology, Social Affairs Ergonomics Consultant
Engineering and and Ergonomics Division Concord, NSW, Australia
Ergonomics, RWTH Medical and Social Department
Aachen University National Board of Occupational William S. Marras
Aachen, Germany Safety and Health Department of Industrial, Welding
Solna, Sweden and Systems Engineering
Ninica Howard The Ohio State University
SHARP, Washington State Stephan Konz Columbus, Ohio
Department of Labor Department of Industrial
and Industries Engineering Christopher B. Mayhorn
Olympia, Washington Kansas State University Department of Psychology
Manhattan, Kansas North Carolina State University
Raleigh, North Carolina
Susan J. Isernhagen
DSI Work Solutions, Inc. Melichar Kopas
Lynn McAtamney
Duluth, Minnesota Technical University
National Occupational Health
Kosice, Slovakia and Safety Commission
Julie A. Jacko Nottingham City Hospital
Wallace H. Coulter Department Karl H.E. Kroemer Nottingham, UK
of Biomedical Engineering Industrial Ergonomics Lab
Georgia Institute of Technology Virginia Polytechnic Institute Stuart M. McGill
and Emory University School and State University Department of Kinesiology
of Medicine Blacksburg, Virginia University of Waterloo
Atlanta, Georgia Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
Shrawan Kumar
Department of Physical Gary Mirka
Richard J. Jagacinski
Therapy Department of Industrial
The Ohio State University
University of Alberta Engineering
Columbus, Ohio
Edmonton, Alberta, Canada North Carolina State University
Raleigh, North Carolina
Dieter W. Jahns
Steven A. Lavender
Board of Certification in Tracy L. Mitzner
Industrial, Welding and
Professional Ergonomics School of Psychology
Systems Engineering
Bellingham, Washington Georgia Institute of Technology
The Ohio State University
Atlanta, Georgia
Columbus, Ohio
Bente Rona Jensen
Department of Human Kevin P. Moloney
V. Kathlene Leonard Center for Interactive Systems
Physiology, Institute of Wallace H. Coulter Department
Exercise and Sports Sciences Engineering
of Biomedical Engineering Institute for Health Systems
University of Copenhagen Georgia Institute of Technology
Copenhagen, Denmark Engineering and School of
and Emory University School Industrial and Systems
of Medicine Engineering
Waldemar Karwowski Atlanta, Georgia Georgia Institute of Technology
Center for Industrial Atlanta, Georgia
Ergonomics Soo-Yee Lim
University of Louisville NIOSH Timothy H. Monk
Louisville, Kentucky Atlanta, Georgia Clinical Neuroscience
Research Center
Dohyung Kee Holger Luczak Western Psychiatric Institute
Department of Industrial and Forschungsinstitut für and Clinic
Systems Engineering Rationalisierung (FIR) University of Pittsburgh
Keimyung University an der RWTH Aachen University Medical Center
Taegu, Korea Aachen, Germany Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
x
Max Mulder Paul Salmon Amy L. Spencer
Delft University of Defence Technology Centre for Cognitive Systems
Technology Human Factors Integration Engineering, Inc.
Delft, Netherlands Brunel University Columbus, Ohio
BIT Lab, School of
Susane Mütze-Niewöhner Engineering and Design
Uxbridge, Middlesex, UK Peregrin Spielholz
Institute of Industrial
SHARP, Washington State
Engineering and Ergonomics,
Department of Labor
RWTH Aachen University Nadine Sarter and Industries
Aachen, Germany Department of Industrial and Olympia, Washington
Operations Engineering
Milan Oravec University of Michigan
Technical University Ann Arbor, Michigan Neville Stanton
Kosice, Slovakia Defence Technology Centre
James Sheedy for Human Factors
Maurice Oxenburgh College of Optometry Integration
Ergonomics Consultant The Ohio State University Brunel University
Concord, NSW, Australia Columbus, Ohio BIT Lab, School of Engineering
and Design
Uxbridge, Middlesex, UK
Hana Pačaiová B. Sherehiy
Technical University Department of Industrial
Kosice, Slovakia Engineering R. Brian Stone
University of Louisville Department of Design
Barbara J. Peters Louisville, Kentucky The Ohio State University
Peters and Peters Columbus, Ohio
Santa Monica, California Gwanseob Shin
Department of Industrial
Setenay Tuncel
George A. Peters Engineering
Department of Mechanical,
Peters and Peters North Carolina State
Industrial and Nuclear
Santa Monica, California University
Engineering
Raleigh, North Carolina
University of Cincinnati
Gary S. Robinson Cincinnati, Ohio
Virginia Polytechnic Barbara Silverstein
Institute and State SHARP, Washington State
University Department of Labor Marinus M. Van Paassen
Blacksburg, Virginia and Industries Control and Simulation
Olympia, Washington Engineering Department
Wendy A. Rogers Delft University of Technology
Virginia Polytechnic Juraj Sinay Delft, Netherlands
Institute and State Technical University
University Kosice, Slovakia
Guy Walker
Blacksburg, Virginia
Defence Technology Centre
Gisela Sjøgaard for Human Factors
Kris Rightmire Department of Physiology Integration
Board of Certification in National Institute of Brunel University
Professional Ergonomics Occupational Health BIT Lab, School of
Bellingham, Washington Copenhagen, Denmark Engineering and Design
Uxbridge, Middlesex, UK
Irina Rivilis Philip J. Smith
Department of Public Industrial, Welding and
Health Sciences Systems Engineering Donald E. Wasserman
University of Toronto The Ohio State University D.E. Wasserman, Inc.
Toronto, Canada Columbus, Ohio Cincinnati, Ohio
xi
Thomas R. Waters Beth A. Winkelstein National Research
Division of Applied Research Departments of Bioengineering Council Panel on
and Technology and Neurosurgery Musculoskeletal
National Institute for University of Disorders and the
Occupational Safety and Health Pennsylvania Workplace
Cincinnati, Ohio Philadelphia, Pennsylvania Department of Industrial,
Welding and Systems
David G. Wilder Engineering
Department of Biomedical Charles B. Woolley
Industrial Engineering The Ohio State University
Engineering Columbus, Ohio
University of Iowa University of Michigan
Iowa City, Iowa Ann Arbor, Michigan
xii
Contents
I Fundamentals of Ergonomics
1 A Guide to Certification in Professional Ergonomics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .1-1
Dieter W. Jahns
2 Magnitude of Occupationally-Related Musculoskeletal Disorders . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2-1
National Research Council Panel on Musculoskeletal Disorders and the Workplace
3 Legal Issues in Occupational Ergonomics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3-1
George A. Peters and Barbara J. Peters
4 Cost Justification for Implementing Ergonomics Intervention . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .4-1
Maurice Oxenburgh and Pepe Marlow
5 Humans in Work System Environment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .5-1
Holger Luczak, Sven Hinrichsen, and Susane Mütze-Niewöhner
6 Human Factors and TQM . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .6-1
Colin G. Drury
7 User-Centered Design of Information Technology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .7-1
V. Kathlene Leonard, Kevin P. Moloney, and Julie A. Jacko
8 Application of Risk Theory in Man –Machine–Environment
Systems . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .8-1
Juraj Sinay, Hana Pačaiová, Melichar Kopas, and Milan Oravec
9 Engineering Anthropometry . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .9-1
Karl H.E. Kroemer
10 Human Strength Evaluation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .10-1
Karl H.E. Kroemer
11 Biomechanical Basis for Ergonomics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .11-1
William S. Marras
12 Fundamentals of Manual Control . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .12-1
Max Mulder, Marinus M. Van Paassen, John M. Flach, and Richard J. Jagacinski
13 Cumulative Spine Loading . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .13-1
Jack P. Callaghan
14 Low-Level Static Exertions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .14-1
Gisela Sjøgaard and Bente Rona Jensen
xiii
15 Soft-Tissue Pathomechanics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .15-1
Robert G. Cutlip
16 Mechanisms for Pain and Injury in Musculoskeletal Disorders . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .16-1
Beth A. Winkelstein
17 Ergonomics and Aging . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .17-1
Tracy L. Mitzner, Christopher B. Mayhorn, Arthur D. Fisk, and Wendy A. Rogers
18 Vision and Work . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .18-1
James Sheedy
19 Individual Factors and Musculoskeletal Disorders . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .19-1
Donald C. Cole and Irina Rivilis
20 Rehabilitating Low Back Disorders . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .20-1
Stuart M. McGill
21 Human Adaptation in the Workplace . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .21-1
Ash Genaidy and Setenay Tuncel
22 Rehabilitation Ergonomics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .22-1
Susan J. Isernhagen
23 Visual, Tactile, and Multimodal Information Processing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .23-1
Nadine Sarter
24 Applying Cognitive Psychology to System Development . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .24-1
Philip J. Smith, R. Brian Stone, and Amy L. Spencer
25 The Role Personality in Ergonomics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .25-1
W. Gary Allread
26 Psychosocial Work Factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .26-1
Pascale Carayon and Soo-Yee Lim
27 Biomechanical Modeling of the Shoulder . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .27-1
Krystyna Gielo-Perczak
28 Application of Ergonomics to the Low Back . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .28-1
Kermit G. Davis, III
29 Application of Ergonomics to the Legs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .29-1
Steven A. Lavender
30 Application of Ergonomics of the Foot . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .30-1
Stephan Konz
31 Noise in Industry . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .31-1
John G. Casali and Gary S. Robinson
32 Shiftwork . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .32-1
Timothy H. Monk
33 Vibrometry . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .33-1
Donald E. Wasserman and David G. Wilder
II Assessment Tools
34 Overview of Ergonomic Assessment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .34-1
Chris Hamrick
xiv
35 Low Back Injury Risk Assessment Tools . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .35-1
Gary Mirka and Gwanseob Shin
36 Cognitive Task Analysis — A Review . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .36-1
Paul Salmon, Neville Stanton, Guy Walker, and Damian Green
37 Subjective Scales of Effort and Workload Assessment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .37-1
B. Sherehiy and W. Karwowski
38 Rest Allowances . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .38-1
Stephan Konz
39 Wrist Posture in Office Work . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .39-1
Mircea Fagarasanu and Shrawan Kumar
40 PLIBEL — A Method Assigned for Identification of Ergonomics Hazards . . . . . . .40-1
Kristina Kemmlert
41 The ACGIH TLVw for Hand Activity Level . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .41-1
Thomas J. Armstrong
42 REBA and RULA: Whole Body and Upper Limb Rapid Assessment Tools . . . . . . .42-1
Sue Hignett and Lynn McAtamney
43 An Assessment Technique for Postural Loading on the
Upper Body (LUBA) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .43-1
Dohyung Kee and Waldemar Karwowski
44 The Washington State SHARP Approach to Exposure Assessment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .44-1
Stephen Bao, Barbara Silverstein, Ninica Howard, and Peregrin Spielholz
45 Upper Extremity Analysis of the Wrist . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .45-1
Andris Freivalds
46 Revised NIOSH Lifting Equation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .46-1
Thomas R. Waters
47 Psychophysical Approach to Task Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .47-1
Patrick G. Dempsey
48 Static Biomechanical Modeling in Manual Lifting . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .48-1
Don B. Chaffin and Charles B. Woolley
49 Industrial Lumbar Motion Monitor . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .49-1
William S. Marras and W. Gary Allread
50 The ACGIH TLVw for Low Back Risk . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .50-1
William S. Marras and Chris Hamrick
Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . I-1
xv
I
Fundamentals
of Ergonomics
I-1
I-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
1.1 Introduction
Some form of “quality assurance” effort is natural to most professions. This generally involves develop-
ment of credentialing in educational programs and/or of individuals. Three types of processes are most
common: Accreditation is established for the regulation of instructional programs. It is voluntary and
generally developed and administered by an association of professionals within the field. Certification
involves a voluntary process of evaluation and measurement of individuals, which can then indicate
whether they have achieved a professional level of qualifications as judged by professional peers. It is
developed and administered by a professional association or a group specifically established for profes-
sional development purposes. Licensure, while it does credential individuals, is a mandatory process and
is administered by a political or governing body. When laws are implemented “to protect the public”
from unprofessional practices, it becomes illegal to practice one’s profession without a license. Thus,
these processes are distinguishable by three aspects: (a) the recipient of the credential, (b) the credential-
ing body, and (c) the degree of volunteerism involved in obtaining the credential (Jahns, 1991).
In 1994, Dr. Carol Slappendel reviewed nine ergonomics certification/registration programs in oper-
ation around the world. Her findings are summarized in Table 1.1 and Table 1.2. Since International
Ergonomics Association (IEA) Federated Societies are more oriented towards “information dissemina-
tion,” and not so much towards “control” of the profession as a guild structure, there is an increasing
trend for cooperative, yet independent credentialing agencies. In “open-market” societies there are
also opportunities for sham operators, which makes a supervisory role by IEA Federated Societies desir-
able. Examples of such efforts include the Association of Canadian Ergonomists (ACE), which prior to
developing and launching certification processes and criteria for Canadian ergonomists, recognized the
With updates from Kris Rightmire, Executive Administrator.
1-1
1-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Board of Certification in Professional Ergonomics (BCPE) as a valid and reliable certification organi-
zation. BCPE has also served informally as a consultant for certification efforts underway in Japan
and South Africa.
Similarly in Europe, the center for Registration of European Ergonomists (CREE) works with the ergo-
nomics societies of member countries in the European Union in evaluating and registering applicants for
the “Eur.Erg.” designation. The BCPE and CREE have a “reciprocity” agreement in place. As former
CREE President E. N. Corlett (personal communication, December 11, 1996) wrote: “Our policy at
the moment is to be linked with only one Registering body in each country. Because of our constitution,
this body has to have certain requirements, as laid out in the European Standard 45013 to which we
adhere. We have confirmed that BCPE fulfills these requirements.”
Further, the IEA, in an effort to establish professional practice standards for ergonomists around the
world, as well as harmonize ergonomic credentialing organizations on an international scale, has devel-
oped criteria and procedures for endorsing professional certifying bodies and programs. In 2001, the
BCPE became the first certification organization endorsed by the IEA, in accordance with the following
criteria (Criteria for IEA Endorsement, 2001):
(Table continued)
1-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Fellow of the Ergonomics Society Registered Member for at least 6 yr Not required
plus significant contribution to the
practice of, teaching of, or research
in ergonomics for a period of 10 yr
since becoming an Ordinary
Member plus substantial
contribution to the activities of the
Society
Practitioner on the Professional Must be a Registered Member of the Every 3 yr
Register of the Ergonomics Society Society plus a minimum of 3 yr in
active practice during the preceding
year
Certified professional member of the A suitable qualification plus 3 yr full- Required
Ergonomics Society of Australia time equivalent experience in the
practice of ergonomics
Professional member of the New A tertiary qualification in ergonomics, Not required
Zealand Ergonomics Society or a qualification of which
ergonomics made up a substantial
portion of the course content, plus
experience in the practice of
ergonomics, or teaching or research
of ergonomics relevance
4. The certifying body has a governing body comprised of certified ergonomists, which is impartial
and reflects the range of interests practiced by ergonomists.
5. The certifying body has a governing body responsible for the formulation of policy matters relat-
ing to the operation of the certifying body.
6. The certifying body clearly demonstrates the line of responsibility, the reporting structure and
the relationship between the assessment and certification functions.
7. The certifying body has the financial resources to conduct certification efficiently.
8. The certifying body is operated on a nonprofit basis.
9. The certifying body is explicit about its legal status.
10. The certifying body is staffed by personnel who are knowledgeable about ergonomics and com-
petent in the functions for which they are employed to carry out.
11. The eligibility criteria used by the certifying body are clearly defined and include: specific refer-
ence to qualifications, supervised experience, professional experience in ergonomics and any
forms of evidence required for the certification process; are independent of whether a person
is a member of a relevant ergonomics society; are nondiscriminatory in terms of gender, ethni-
city, religion or physical status; are related to contemporary ergonomics theory and practice; and,
refer to requirements for recency of an individual’s practice.
12. The procedural information provided by the certifying body to applicants includes: literature
clearly outlining the formal procedures to be followed by the applicant in seeking certification;
the deadlines for applying for certification in any year; information on all fees relevant to the
process; the process used by the organization in evaluating the suitability of the applicant for cer-
tification; and, the standards of competency to be applied in all aspects of the review.
A Guide to Certification in Professional Ergonomics 1-5
Information
IEA Action
IFA
Federated BCPE
Societies CREE
Candidate
FIGURE 1.1 Communication and actions among ergonomics societies and certification agencies. (From D. Jahns.
A Guide to Certification in Professional Ergonomics. CRC Press 1998. With permission.)
13. The processes followed by the certifying body are documented properly in accordance with the
minimum IEA criteria for certification. They include: statements and rules relating to the current
process of certification and policies relating to the granting of certification and are reviewed
regularly to ensure their currency in relation to ergonomics practice; and, include a documented
appeal mechanism.
14. For those certifying bodies where an examination forms part of the review, the standards should
be relevant to current practice and should be clearly defined; mechanisms should exist to ensure
confidentiality of the examination and its outcomes; the form of evaluation should be a valid test
of the competencies assessed; and, methods used by the certifying body to test the reliability of
the assessment should be described.
15. When appointing certification personnel, the certifying body must have access to a pool of
qualified and competent certification personnel and to other facilities to carry out a certification
review initially and for recertification purposes; the certifying personnel must be competent in
the areas where they will make evaluations; maintain up to date information on relevant
qualifications, training and experience of certifying personnel; and, provide clear guidelines
relating to duties and responsibilities of certifying personnel.
16. The certifying body should have established processes for giving adequate feedback about
deficiencies to applicants who have not attained certification.
17. The certifying body should keep a record of all policies and regulations relating to its process;
keep a confidential record of details of each certification procedure followed for individual appli-
cants; publish an annual report, including reference to numbers of applicants and outcomes of
1-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
the process; maintain an up to date register of those who have been certified, and make it acces-
sible to public; and, publish its financial statements annually.
18. The certifying body must have already established, or be developing, a recertification process.
That process should define the period of currency for any certification awarded and address cri-
teria relevant to the applicant’s competence in relation to contemporary practice in ergonomics.
The candidates for certification usually follow the pathways shown in Figure 1.1 (solid lines) by
contacting either the certification agency directly or by making inquiry to one of the IEA Federated
Societies, which then coordinates the certification procedures. Both BCPE and CREE have highly coor-
dinated “information exchanges” (dashed lines in Figure 1.1) with the IEA and selected, regionally-active
Federated Societies to harmonize the professional development of ergonomists. Interested readers can
contact the organizations listed in “For Further Information.” A general overview of BCPE certification
criteria and procedures follows for interested individuals.
the life sciences, engineering sciences, and behavioral sciences to comprise a professional level of
ergonomics education.
. Three years of full-time professional practice as an ergonomist with emphasis on design invol-
from a HF/E degree program accredited by an IEA Federated Society, e.g., HFES, ES, is not
required to take Part-I of the exam).
. Payment of all fees levied by the BCPE for processing and maintenance of certification.
2. CPE/CHFP by portfolio review. On September 5, 1996, the BCPE reinstated the portfolio review
process to accommodate senior ergonomics/human factors professionals. Individuals with at
least 15 yr of ergonomic work experience may apply for designation as a CPE or CHFP via port-
folio review if he or she:
. Has a master’s degree in ergonomics or human factors, or an equivalent educational back-
ground in the life sciences, engineering sciences and behavioral sciences to comprise a profes-
sional level of ergonomics education.
. Has at least 15 yr of ergonomic work experience, which emphasizes design involvement (derived
nomic project involvement by means of the BCPE “Application for CPE/CHFP by Portfolio.”
. Pays all fees levied by BCPE for processing and maintenance of certification.
Society, e.g., HFES, ES, is not required to take Part-I of the exam). Parts II and III of the exam may
be taken after fulfilling the other CPE [CHFP] requirements.
4. CEA(Certified Ergonomics Associate). On April 17, 1998, the BCPE established a technical level of
certification to meet the growing need for certified ergonomists who use commonly accepted tools
and techniques for analysis and enhancement of human performance in existing systems, but they
are not required to solve complex and unique problems, develop advanced analytic and measure-
ment technologies, provide a broad systems perspective, or define design criteria and specifica-
tions. The minimum criteria for designation as a CEA are:
. A bachelor’s degree from an accredited university.
. Documentation of education, employment history and work experience by means of the BCPE
. Payment of all fees levied by the BCPE for processing and maintenance of certification.
1. Request application materials from the Online Store of the BCPE web site at www.bcpe.org, by
phone at 888-856-4685, or by sending a check of U.S. $10 to:
BCPE
PO Box 2811
Bellingham WA 98227
Application materials consist of three or four pages of instructions and four or seven pages of
forms to be filled out by the applicant. These include:
Section A — Personal data
Section B — Academic qualifications
Section C — Employment history (CPE/CHFP); ergonomics training hours (CEA)
Section D — Work experience in ergonomic analysis, design, and testing/evaluation
(CPE/CHFP); employment history (CEA)
Section E — Work product description (CPE/CHFP)
Section F — Signature and payment record
2. The candidate completes the application and submits it with (1) the appropriate processing and
examination fee, (2) an official academic degree transcript, and (3) CPE/CHFP only, a work
product (article/technical report/project description/patent/application, etc.).
3. A review panel evaluates all submitted materials and makes a recommendation whether or not the
applicant qualifies to take the written examination.
4. The qualified applicant may take the written examination anytime during his or her 2-yr eligibility
period. Certification is awarded upon receipt of a passing score on the examination.
The nonqualified applicant has up to 2-yr after his or her application is received to correct any
deficiencies or missing elements and to take the written examination. If certification is not awarded
during the 2-yr eligibility period, the applicant is required to reapply for certification.
References
Criteria for IEA Endorsement of Certifying Bodies. 2001. Retrieved May 26, 2004, from http://
www.iea.cc/events/edu_criteria.cfm
Jahns, D. W. 1991. Certification of professional ergonomists: a status report. Proceedings of the 24th
Annual Conference of the HFAC/ACE, Vancouver, BC, Canada.
Slappendel, C. 1994. Harmonising the different approaches to the certification of the ergonomist.
Proceedings of the 12th Triennial Congress of the IEA, Toronto, ON, Canada.
The panel’s effort to evaluate the scientific basis for a relationship between work factors and musculo-
skeletal disorders of the back and upper extremities required comprehensive reviews of the epidemiologic
literature. For each of the two anatomical regions, reviews of the physical and the psychosocial factors
were undertaken. Referring back to Figure 1.2, the review of the epidemiologic evidence addresses
several components. The workplace factors considered include all three main elements and their relation-
ship to the person. The person is considered in terms of the several outcomes reported in these studies,
while adjusting or stratifying for the individual factors that are relevant.
2.1 Methods
Reprint from NRC book.
2-1
2-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
studied are carefully defined so that it is evident how an independent investigator could identify the
same outcome in a different study population. Outcomes are measured either by objective means
or by self-report. For self-reported outcomes, however, there are explicit criteria for how the data
were collected and evidence that the collection method would permit another investigator to repeat
the study in another population.
. The exposure measures are well defined. Self-report of exposure is acceptable so long as the method
of collecting self-reports was well specified and there was evidence that the self-reports were reliable
reflections of exposures. Job titles as surrogates for exposure were acceptable when the exposure of
interest was inherent in the job (e.g., vibration exposure for those operating pneumatic chipping
hammers).
. The article was published in English.
. The article was peer reviewed.
. The study was done within the last 20 yr (preferably).
No specific limitations were placed on study designs acceptable for consideration. The advantages of
prospective studies, however, were recognized. For example, there were sufficient prospective studies of
low back pain to examine these separately among the studies of physical factors and exclusively among
the studies of psychosocial factors.
Because incidence is a rate calculated by following people over time, and many studies are cross-sec-
tional or retrospective (case-control), other measures, such as the prevalence ratio and the odds ratio, have
been developed to summarize the association between exposure and outcomes for these other study
designs. Our analysis focused on associations expressed by such risk estimates as the odds ratio and
the relative risk. These estimates were retrieved from the original article or calculated when sufficient
raw data were presented.
The attributable fraction helps scientists and policy makers recognize that in many cases a variety of
factors contribute to the total incidence of a disease or other outcome, so that removal of an exposure
typically does not reduce the outcome rate to zero. However, in its simplest form, the attributable risk
is a measure that suggests that if the offending exposure were removed (by intervention or regulation),
then the amount of disease outcomes would be estimated to be reduced by the calculated amount. As is
noted in the following para, this simple summary is enmeshed in caveats.
It is important to recognize in this calculation that the result depends on what is included. This is, if
one considers a calculation of one factor as it relates to an outcome and then performs a separate calcu-
lation for another factor for the same outcome, there is overlapping (correlation) between factors that
could make the sum of the two separate factors sum to more than 100%. Attributable fraction, then,
represents a crude but important estimation of the impact of control of risk factors. An estimate of
the attributable fraction for a multifactorial disease such as a musculoskeletal disorder provides only
an estimate of the relative importance of the various factors studied. It is not, and cannot be, considered
a direct estimate of the proportion of the disease in the population that would be eliminated if only this
single factor was removed (Rotham and Greenland, 1998a). Rather it provides guidance to the relative
importance of exposure reduction in those settings in which the exposure under study is prevalent. Con-
sequently, we have not attempted to rank or further interpret the findings for attributable fractions and
have chosen only to report them as a rough guide to the relative importance of the factors in the study
settings in which they have been examined.
In this review, the relative risk in longitudinal studies and the prevalence or odds ratio in cross-
sectional surveys were used to calculate the attributable fraction for the risk factors studied. For
example, if workers exposed to frequent bending and twisting have a prevalence of low back pain that
is three times that of those not exposed, then among the exposed the attributable fraction will be:
By this hypothetical calculation, 67% of low back pain in the exposed group could be prevented by
eliminating work that requires bending and twisting.
2-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
2.1.3.3 Confounding
None of the musculoskeletal disorders examined in this report is uniquely caused by work exposures.
They are what the World Health Organization calls work-related conditions. “Work-related diseases
may be partially caused by adverse working conditions. They may be aggravated, accelerated, or exacer-
bated by workplace exposures, and they may impair working capacity. Personal characteristics and other
environmental and socio-cultural factors usually play a role as risk factors in work-related diseases, which
are often more common than occupational diseases” (World Health Organization, 1985).
In Chapter 3 we note that the epidemiologic study of causes related to health outcomes such as mus-
culoskeletal disorders requires careful attention to the several factors associated with the outcome.
The objective of a study will determine which factor or factors are the focus and which factors
might “confound” the association. In the case of musculoskeletal disorders, a study may have as its
objective the investigation of individual risk factors. Such a study, however, cannot evaluate individual
risk factors effectively if it does not also consider relevant work exposures; the work exposures are
potential confounders of the association with individual risk factors. Conversely, a study that evaluates
work exposures cannot effectively evaluate these factors if it does not also consider relevant individual
risk factors; the individual risk factors are potential confounders of the association with work
exposures.
Therefore, when studying the relationship of musculoskeletal disorders to work, it is necessary to con-
sider the other known factors that cause or modify the likelihood that the disorder will occur, such as
individual factors and nonwork exposures. For example, the frequency of many musculoskeletal
disorders is a function of age, so age has to be taken into account before attributing a musculoskeletal
disorder to a work exposure. Another common concern is whether a recreational exposure accounts
for an outcome that otherwise might be attributed to work.
In every epidemiologic study, confounders need to be measured and, when relevant, included in
the data analysis. The confounders selected for consideration in the analysis of data from a specific
study depend on the types of exposures studied, the types of outcomes measured, and the detail on
potential confounders that can be accurately collected on a sufficient number of the study subjects. As
a consequence, our approach to reviewing epidemiologic studies of work and musculoskeletal dis-
orders documented the attention given to a wide range of potential confounders (see the panel’s
abstract form in Figure 2.1). No study can measure every possible confounder; however, the
papers included by the panel were judged to have given adequate attention to the primary individual
factors that might have confounded the work exposures under study. These include in particular age
and gender, as well as, when necessary and possible, such factors as obesity, cigarette smoking, and
comorbid states.
The role of potential confounders in epidemiologic studies and their proper management is often con-
fusing to the nonepidemiologist. The difficulty stems from the fact that the potential confounder is often
known to be associated with the disease, in this case musculoskeletal disorders. The association of a risk
factor such as age with the disease, however, does not make it a true confounder of the study’s examin-
ation of a separate risk factor such as work exposures. True confounding occurs only when, for example,
both the risk factors being studied (age and work exposures) are associated with the outcome (muscu-
loskeletal disorders) and the two risk factors are also correlated (e.g., those with more work exposure are
also older). Fortunately, as noted in Chapter 3, there are statistical methods available to manage con-
founding that provide a way to “separate,” in this example, the effects of the work exposure from the
effects of age.
The panel recognizes that a number of nonwork factors are associated with or also cause the muscu-
loskeletal disorders under study. These were not separately studied, but they were considered, as necess-
ary, to evaluate the significance of the work factors that were studied. In our judgment, it is evident that
confounding alone is highly unlikely to explain the associations of musculoskeletal disorders with work
that are noted. More detailed consideration of confounding in future studies, however, should further
improve the precision and accuracy of risk estimates.
Magnitude of Occupationally-Related Musculoskeletal Disorders 2-5
FIGURE 2.1 Individual Factors Considered in Analyses Form Used in Describing Studies Included in the Review.
routinized, that is, less predictably structured, valid estimation of both task distributions and task-
specific exposures becomes increasingly challenging.
Typically, both observational and direct measurement techniques generate highly detailed,
accurate exposure analyses for a relatively short period of elapsed time in each job. Most protocols
for these methods assume that the work is cyclical, with little variability over time, so that it is reasonable
to measure exposures for a short period and extrapolate them to the long term. But many jobs do not fit
this model: they are not comprised of work cycles, or the cycles are highly variable in their total duration
or content (the number or sequence of steps that comprise each cycle) and do not account for all of
the work performed by an individual with any given job title. For these jobs, it would be infeasible
to undertake continuous measurements for entire cycles as an exposure assessment strategy,
because either there are no cycles, or a very large number of (long) cycles would have to be recorded
in order to quantify accurately the total and average duration of exposures. With short measuring
times, the data collected are of uncertain representativeness because these time periods do not match
the duration of exposures that are thought to be relevant for the development of musculoskeletal
disorder.
A versatile alternative for estimating physical exposures is the use of data collected directly from
workers. Such reports may address both task-specific exposures within jobs and the distributions of
tasks performed by each worker. In addition to being time-efficient, self-reports permit assessment of
exposures in the past as well as the present and may be structured with task-specific questions or orga-
nized to cover the job as a whole. Some researchers have explicitly recommended a composite approach
to the analysis of nonroutine jobs, in which task-specific exposures are measured directly and the tem-
poral distribution (frequency and duration) of each task is obtained from self-report. Self-reported data
can take various forms, including duration, frequency, and intensity of exposure. In some studies, absol-
ute ratings have agreed well with observations or direct measurements of the corresponding exposures,
while others have diverged significantly, especially with use of continuous estimates or responses that
required choices among a large number of categories (e.g., Burdorf and Laan, 1991; Faucett and
Rempel, 1996; Lindström, et al., 1994; Rossignol and Baetz, 1987; Torgén et al., 1999; Viikari-Juntura,
1996; Wiktorin et al., 1993).
Retrospective recall of occupational exposures has been frequently employed in studies of musculos-
keletal disorders, but there are few data on the reproducibility of such information. Three studies have
examined the potential for differential error (i.e., information bias) in self-reported exposure with respect
to musculoskeletal disorders with mixed results; some risk estimates were biased away from the null
value, some toward it, and others not at all (Torgén et al., 1999; Viikari-Juntura, 1996; Wiktorin et al.,
1993). In the REBUS1 study follow-up population, Toomingas et al. (1997a) found no evidence that indi-
vidual subjects systematically overrated or underrated either exposures or symptoms in the same direc-
tion. Self-reported exposures have promise, but their validity depends on the specific design of the
questions and response categories.
A variety of instrumentation methods exist for direct measurement of such dimensions as muscle force
exertion (electromyography), joint angles and motion frequency (e.g., electrogoniometry), and vibration
(accelerometers). For example, the goniometer has been used in a variety of studies of wrist posture,
including field assessments of ergonomic risk factors (Moore et al., 1991; Wells et al., 1994), comparisons
of keyboard designs (Smutz et al., 1994), and clinical trials (Ojima et al., 1991). Hansson et al. (1996)
evaluated the goniometer for use in epidemiologic studies, and Marras developed a device for measuring
the complex motion of the spine (Marras, 1992). While many consider these methods to represent col-
lectively the standard for specific exposures, each instrument measures only one exposure, and usually
only at one body part. When multiple exposures are present simultaneously and must be assessed at
1
In the original REBUS study conducted in 1969, participants were asked to complete a questionnaire regarding
health status — all selected were given a medical examination. A diagnosis of musculoskeletal disorder required
signs and symptoms. The follow-up study, conducted in 1993, asked the younger participants in the original
REBUS study to participate in a re-examination.
Magnitude of Occupationally-Related Musculoskeletal Disorders 2-7
multiple body parts, the time required to perform instrumented analyses on each subject may limit their
applicability to epidemiologic research (Kilbom, 1994). Another practical concern is the potential
invasiveness that may interfere with job performance, alter work practices, or reduce worker cooperation.
Thus, there is a trade-off between the precision of bioinstrumentation and the time efficiency and flexi-
bility of visual observation and worker self-report. As discussed in Chapter 6, gross categorical exposure
measures (e.g., .10 kg vs. ,10 kg) used in epidemiologic studies may limit the possibility of observing
an exposure–risk relationship; a continuous measure based on bioinstrumentation might make such a
relationship more apparent. Thus, their high accuracy (for the period of measurement) gives these
methods utility for validating other methods on population subsets and added value when they can
be applied in epidemiologic studies.
A large number of observational methods for ergonomic job analysis have been proposed in the last
two decades (see Kilbom, 1994). These include checklists and similar qualitative approaches to identify
peak stressors (e.g., Keyserling et al., 1993; Stetson et al., 1991). The limitation with checklists is that they
provide little information beyond the presence or absence of an exposure, with a possibly curde estimate
of the exposure duration. The qualitative approaches are not likely to provide sufficient detail to effec-
tively assess exposure for epidemiologic studies.
The most common observational techniques used to characterize ergonomic exposures are based on
either time study or work sampling. Both of these techniques require a trained observer to characterize
the ergonomic stressors. Methods based on time study (e.g., Armstrong et al., 1982; Keyserling, 1986) are
usually used to create a continuous or semi-continuous description of posture and, occasionally, force
level. Therefore, changes in the exposure level, as well as the proportion of time a worker is at a given
level, may be estimated. Because methods based on time study tend to be very time intensive, they are
better suited to work with fairly short and easily definable work cycles. A different approach, work
sampling, involves observation of worker(s) at either random or fixed, usually infrequent, time intervals
and is more appropriate for nonrepetitive work (e.g., Karhu et al., 1977; Buchholz et al., 1996). Obser-
vations during work sampling provide estimates of the proportion of time that workers are exposed to
various stressors, although the sequence of events is lost. Though less time intensive than time study,
work sampling still requires too much time for use in an epidemiologic study, especially one that
employs individual measures of exposure.
There are also a few highly detailed, easily used observational analyses for use as an exposure assess-
ment tool in an epidemiologic study. These methods employ subjective ratings made by expert observers.
For example, Rodgers (1988, 1992) has developed methods based on physiological limits of exposure that
rate effort level, duration, and frequency. The method developed by Moore and Garg (1995) employs
ratings similar to those of Rodgers and adds posture and speed of work ratings. Moore and Garg’s
strain index is designed to estimate strain for the distal upper extremity. It is the weighted product of
six factors placed on a common five-point scale (subjective ratings of force, hand/wrist posture, and
speed of work and measurement of duration of exertion, frequency of exertion, and duration of task
per day). The strain index is a single priority score designed to represent risk for upper extremity mus-
culoskeletal disorders and is conceptually similar to the lift index for low back disorders. The lift index
was developed as part of the revised NIOSH lifting equation (Waters et al., 1993) and is the ratio of the
load lifted and the recommended weight limit.
Recently, Latko et al. (1997) developed a method employing visual analog scales for expert rating of
hand activity level (called HAL). The method has also been generalized to assess other physical stressors,
including force, posture, and contact stress (Latko et al., 1997, 1999). The HAL employs five verbal
anchors, so that observers can rate the stressors reliably. In an evaluation, a team of expert observers
comes to a consensus on ratings for individual jobs. These ratings correlated well with two quantitative
measures, recovery time/cycle and exertions/second, and are found to be reliable when compared with
ratings of the same jobs 1.5– 2 yr later (Latko et al., 1997).
In sum, there are many methods for assessment of ergonomic exposures. The challenge for ergo-
nomists and epidemiologists is to determine a method for characterizing the level of exposure that is
efficient enough to permit analysis of intersubject and intrasubject variability across hundreds of subjects
2-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
and that can also produce exposure data at the level of detail needed to examine etiologic relationships
with musculoskeletal disease. The HAL, as developed by Latko, is easy to apply and has proven to be
predictive of the prevalence of upper extremity musculoskeletal disorders in cross-sectional studies.
Phychosocial — back
Work-related factors (longitudinal) 21 6 5 2 2 4 2 3 4 1
Individual factors (longitudinal; 29 9 8 6 2 6 1 1 1
not including studies above)
Psychosocial — upper extremities
All factors (cross-sectional) 25 13 6 1 8
All factors (longitudinal) 3 1 2
Physical — back
Workers only (cross-sectional) 21 21
Community (cross-sectional) 9 7 1 1
Workers (longitudinal) 7 2 4 1
Workers (case-control) 4 1 1 2
Physical — upper extremities
Workers (cross-sectional) 13 2 7 4
Totalc 132 61 19 9 4 10 3 22 4 9 5 2
a
Studies are counted only once regarding clinical evaluation; some studies simply noted that a clinical visit occurred; some further specified that a physical examination was performed; and
some also noted that diagnostic tests were done.
b
Severity usually measured with standardized pain or symptom severity measure.
c
The total number of specific outcomes exceeds the number of studies (i.e., 132), since some studies assessed multiple outcomes.
Source: Reprinted with permission from NACS (Musculoskeletal Disorders and the workplace. Low Back and Upper Extremities # 2001 by the National Academy of Sciences, Courtesy of
the National Academics Press, Washington, D.C.)
2-9
2-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
assessing symptom severity (often with standardized pain and symptom questionnaires), and 9 studies
assessing symptom-related disability. A total of 14 studies assessed the self-reported effect of the muscu-
loskeletal disorder on work status, either as number of sick days (n ¼ 4) or return (or nonreturn) to work
(n ¼ 14). Formal clinical evaluation constituted an outcome in 29 studies, most of which relied on a
physical examination by a physician or other health care professional (e.g., physical therapist). Diagnostic
tests such as x-rays or nerve conduction studies were a standard outcome in only a few studies. Infor-
mation obtained from records constituted an outcome in 16 studies, including claims data, sick days,
or return to work. The predominance of symptoms as an outcome is inherent in the nature of muscu-
loskeletal disorders, which are primarily defined by pain or other symptoms. Indeed, the results of phys-
ical examination and diagnostic tests may be normal in a large proportion of individuals with
musculoskeletal disorders.
There were a greater number of high-quality studies related to back pain than to upper extremity
musculoskeletal disorders. More of the back pain studies were longitudinal rather than cross-sectional,
providing stronger evidence for a potentially causal relationship between particular risk factors and
back disorders. A greater proportion of upper extremity musculoskeletal disorder studies used clinical
evaluation as an outcome.
2.2 Results
TABLE 2.2 Summary of Epidemiologic Studies with Risk Estimates of Null and Positive Associations of Work-
Related Risk Factors and the Occurrence of Back Disorders
Risk Estimate
Null Associationa Positive Association Attributable Fraction (%)
Work-Related Risk Factor n Range n Range n Range
Manual material handling 4 0.90–1.45 24 1.12–3.54 17 11 –66
Frequent bending and twisting 2 1.08–1.30 15 1.29–8.09 8 19 –57
Heavy physical load 0 8 1.54–3.71 5 31 –58
Static work posture 3 0.80–0.97 3 1.30–3.29 3 14 –32
Repetitive movements 2 0.98–1.20 1 1.97 1 41
Whole-body vibration 1 1.10 16 1.26–9.00 11 18 –80
Notes: n ¼ number of associations presented in epidemiologic studies. Details on studies are presented in Appendix
Table 2.1 Appendix Table 2.2, Appendix Table 2.3 and Appendix Table 2.4.
a
Confidence intervals of the risk estimates included the null estimate (1.0). In only 12 of 16 null associations was the
magnitude of the risk estimate presented.
Source: Reprinted with permission from NACS (Musculoskeletal Disorders and the workplace. Low Back and Upper
Extremities # 2001 by the National Academy of Sciences, Courtesy of the National Academics Press, Washington, D.C.)
In general, risk estimates in community-based surveys (Appendix Table 2.2) were smaller than those
in cross-sectional studies in occupational populations (Appendix Table 2.1). A reasonable explanation is
that contrast in exposure is less in community-based studies that survey a large variety of jobs. In various
cross-sectional studies, contrast in exposure has played a role in the selection of subjects.
Multivariate analyses with more than two confounders showed smaller risk estimates (see, e.g., the
longitudinal study by Smedley et al., 1997) than statistical analyses with just one or two confounders
(see, e.g., the longitudinal studies by Gardner et al., 1999; Kraus et al., 1997; Strobe et al., 1988 and
Venning et al., 1987). For lifting as a risk factor, this difference was statistically significant, with
average risks of 1.42 and 2.14. Most studies have adjusted only for a limited number of potential
confounders.
In addition to study design issues, some of the differences in findings appear related to the different
ways exposure was measured. For manual material handling, the seven studies with observations and
direct measurements showed a significantly higher risk estimate than the 21 studies based on question-
naires, with average risk estimates of 2.42 and 1.86, respectively. This finding may be explained by larger
misclassification of exposure in questionnaire studies, or by larger contrast in exposure in studies that
used actual workplace surveys to determine exposure levels. In general, questionnaire studies showed
associations between physical load and back disorders similar to those shown in studies that represented
much more detailed exposure characterization. Therefore, the information from these questionnaire
studies provides useful corroborating evidence.
The magnitude of the risk estimate could not be evaluated in relation to the contrast in exposure, since
exposure parameters were not very comparable. Some studies have used reference groups (low exposure)
that may nonetheless have had measurable exposure to physical load in other studies.
This review concludes that there is a clear relationship between back disorders and physical load
imposed by manual material handling, frequent bending and twisting, physically heavy work, and
whole-body vibration. Although much remains to be learned about exposure-outcome relationships
(see Chapter 3), the epidemiologic evidence presented suggests that preventive measures may reduce
the exposure to these risk factors and decrease the occurrence of back disorders (see Chapter 6).
However, the epidemiologic evidence itself is not specific enough to provide detailed, quantitative guide-
lines for design of the workplace, job, or task. This lack of specificity results from the absence of exposure
measurements on a continuous scale, as opposed to the more commonly used dichotomous (yes/no)
approach. Without continuous measures, it is not Possible to state the “levels” of exposure associated
with increased risk of low back pain.
2-12 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
TABLE 2.3 Summary of Epidemiologic Studies with Risk Estimates of Null and Positive Associations of Specific
Work-Related Physical Exposures and the Occurrence of Upper Extremity Disorders
Risk Estimate
Work-Related Null Associationa Positive Association Attributable Fraction (%)
Risk Factor n Range n Range n Range
Manual material 4 0.90–1.45 24 1.12–3.54 17 11 –66
handling
Repetition 4 2.7–3.3 4 2.3–8.8 3 53 –71
Force 1 1.8 2 5.2–9.0 1 78
Repetition and 0 — 2 15.5–29.1 2 88 –93
force
Repetition and 0 — 1 9.4 1 89
cold
Vibration 6 0.4–2.7 26 2.6–84.5 15 44 –95
Notes: n ¼ number of associations presented in epidemiologic studies. Details on studies are presented in Appendix
Table 2.5.
a
Confidence intervals of the risk estimates included the null estimate (1.0).
Source: Reprinted with permission from NACS (Musculoskeletal Disorders and the workplace. Low Back and Upper Extre-
mities # 2001 by the National Academy of Sciences, Courtesy of the National Academics Press, Washington, D.C.)
Magnitude of Occupationally-Related Musculoskeletal Disorders 2-13
risk. Hand-arm vibration syndrome and other vibration disorders were significantly associated with
vibration exposures in 12 of 13 studies, with risk elevated 2.6– 84.5 times that of nonexposed or low-
exposed comparison workers.
It should be noted that the majority of studies were cross-sectional. Therefore, it is important to con-
sider the temporal direction of the findings. It is likely that the occurrence of upper extremity symptoms
or disorders contributes to increased work-related and nonwork-related stress. If this is the case and a
reciprocal relationship exists, it does not preclude the need to reduce the impact of stress (as either
cause or consequence) on these disorders, given the potential health effects of repeated or prolonged
stress. A second limitation in cross-sectional studies is the healthy-worker effects. This effect refers to
the observation that healthy workers tend to stay in the workforce, and unhealthy workers tend to
leave it. Those who may have left the workforce due to the health condition being studied will be
absent from the study group, resulting in an underestimation of an effect if one is present.
The findings from the studies reviewed indicate that repetition, force, and vibration are particularly
important work-related factors associated with the occurrence of symptoms and disorders in the
upper extremities. Although these findings are limited by the cross-sectional nature of the research
designs, the role of these physical factors is well supported by a number of other studies in which
exposure assessment was less specific (Appendix Table 2.6). Despite indirect objective exposure infor-
mation, the jobs studied appeared to represent conspicuously contrasting ergonomic exposures. These
articles were not used to estimate exposure–response relationships for specific physical hazards (e.g., rep-
etition, force, and posture), but they do provide a foundation for demonstrating a hazard (Appendix
Table 2.6). Only three studies included in the review examined the effects of computer keyboard work
(Bernard et al., 1994; Murata et al., 1996; Sauter et al., 1991). In two, significant associations were
found with pain or discomfort in the upper extremity, and the third found association with slowed
median nerve velocity in subclinical carpal tunnel syndrome.
The attributable fractions related to the physical risk factors that were found to be important provide
additional useful information. They suggest that, when present, each of the physical factors listed in
Table 2.3 is an important contributor to upper extremity disorders. The studies for which attributable
fractions are reported explored associations primarily with hand/wrist disorders such as carpal tunnel
syndrome and hand–arm vibration syndrome. Study of these physical factors in each of the other
upper extremity disorders is indicated to further explore how strong an influence these same factors
might have specifically on the other disorders. Even given the limitations on generalizing from specific
studies, the estimates suggest that substantial benefit could result from reducing the most severe of
these physical risk factors (Table 2.3 and Appendix Table 2.5).
As with other epidemiology study reviews, there are limitations in the available literature. Character-
ization of exposure with sufficient specification to segregate and adequately describe exposure to the
different physical factors for such regions as the neck/shoulder area provides an important example.
Literature reviews by Anderson (1984), Hagberg and Wegman (1987), Sommerich et al. (1993),
Bernard (1997a), and Ariens et al. (2000) provide support for the view that physical work factors are
associated with neck and shoulder musculoskeletal disorders. Had the review of the literature presented
in this chapter been less restrictive regarding study specifications of exposure, it is likely that much stron-
ger conclusions would have been drawn for each of the upper extremity musculoskeletal disorders. Our
review, along with the substantial literature that has used less well-specified exposures, demonstrates the
high priority to be placed on developing better exposure measures for study of the neck/shoulder as well
as the other upper extremity disorders.
An equally important need is for more prospective studies to address individual physical risk factors
and their combination as these relate to each of the upper extremity musculoskeletal disorders. The
cross-sectional findings demonstrating a strong interaction between repetition and force and between
repetition and cold indicate combinations that should be priorities for future study. Given the findings
on work-related psychosocial risk factors and upper extremity disorders (follows later), it will be particu-
larly important to carry out studies that examine the combined effects of physical and psychosocial
factors.
2-14 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
TABLE 2.4 Summary of Work-Related Psychosocial Factors and Back Pain: 21 Prospective Studies
perception that the work could be dangerous for the back. Genéral measures, such as job satisfaction and
stress, showed a very distinct relationship. However, such general measures may reflect other aspects of
the psychological work environment, such as relationships at work or job demands. Therefore, the studies
provide relatively little information about the mechanisms or processes involved. Despite huge differ-
ences in study design and some problems outlined below, the general methodological quality of these
studies is relatively high, and participation rates are good. Few studies employed a theoretical framework,
and a consequence has been difficulty in specifying which predictor variables should be measured.
The relationships examined involve a large number of parameters that may influence the strength of
the association. A given risk factor may, for instance, interact with the outcome variable employed. The
belief that work is dangerous would seem to be relevant for the outcome variable of return to work, but
possibly not for the onset of back pain. Similarly, some risk factors may be relevant only for certain types
of work. As an illustration, for assembly line employment, work pace may be strongly related to future
back pain complaints, but for professionals, such as nurses, it may have a weaker relationship.
The general quality of the studies was high. By selecting prospective investigations, a minimum stan-
dard was set. Nevertheless, there is great diversity in the methodology and this causes several prominent
problems. One concern is that the same concept has been measured in many different ways. Since
reliability and validity are generally not specified, it is possible that two studies claiming to measure
the same entity may in fact be measuring quite different ones. There was also substantial variation
from study to study in the definition and measurement of the outcome variable, and this may have
had considerable consequences on the results obtained. There is, for example, a difference between a
simple report of having had back pain during the past year with dysfunction, with health care visits,
or with sick leave.
TABLE 2.5 Summary of Individual Psychosocial Factors and Back Pain: 38 Prospective Studies
research is therefore to devise studies that include a theoretical perspective. Too often, studies have simply
employed a convenience measure of a “psychological” variable, without considering why or how the vari-
able might work. With a theoretical model, stronger designs could be used that would provide answers to
specific questions.
Few investigations have amply treated the temporal aspects of the problem. The data reviewed suggest
that certain factors are important very early, while others may be important at first consultation or a
recurrence. Moreover, the reciprocal nature of pain and psychological variables was almost always
treated as unidirectional, such as depression causing pain rather than pain affecting depression.
Even though all studies were prospective, methodological shortcomings ranged from selection bias and
inappropriate use of statistical tests to failure to account for the intercorrelation of measures. The use of
self-ratings as both the dependent and independent variables is a particular problem that may inflate risk
estimates. It is difficult to summarize some results, because different terminology and measurement
methods have been used to assess similar concepts (e.g., reluctance to participate in activities such as
“fear-avoidance,” “disability,” or “somatic anxiety”). There is a need to improve the quality of prospective
studies in this area and to foster the use of a more structured terminology.
Some prominent psychological factors do emerge, however. First, a cognitive component represented
by attitudes, beliefs, and thoughts concerning pain, disability, and perceived health seems to be a central
theme. A second theme is an emotional dimension in which distress, anxiety, and depression are central.
Third, a social aspect appears, in which family and work issues seem to be relevant, even if the data are
less convincing. Finally, a behavioral domain emerges, in which coping, pain behaviors, and activity
patterns are consequential elements.
It is tempting to conclude that since the studies included in Appendix Table 2.7 and Appendix Table
2.8 have prospective designs, the observed relationships are causal; however, this may be incorrect.
Although the relationships may be temporal, they need not be causal in nature. Caution in drawing con-
clusions concerning causality does not lessen the value of the reviewed findings, but points to the need for
experimental or other designs to advance understanding.
An important implication is how this knowledge may be incorporated into clinical practice. First,
considerable psychosocial information that could be of the utmost importance in conjunction with
medical examinations may be overlooked if proper assessment of these variables is not conducted.
Second, if psychosocial elements play a central role in back pain, then better interventions could be
designed to deal with these factors to provide better care and prevention.
work might reduce risk by 28 to 48%. Acknowledging the limitations associated with the interpretation
of attributable fractions (as discussed earlier in the chapter) we conclude that these results point to the
potential for structural changes in job supervision, teamwork structures, and the ways in which work may
be organized to reduce risk. The most consistent evidence related to individual psychosocial risk factors
suggests that reduction in depression and anxiety symptoms could reduce the risk for back disorders by
14 to 53%, and reduction in psychological distress could reduce risk by 23 to 63%. This is important
because a number of effective treatments are available for depression, anxiety, and psychological distress.
In a number of studies, the attributable risk associated with a particular psychosocial factor could not be
estimated, because although the factor was significantly associated with back disorders in multivariate
models, the exact data sufficient to calculate relative risk were not provided.
TABLE 2.6 Summary of Epidemiologic Studies: Psychosocial Risk Factors and Work-Related Upper Extremity
Disorders
Risk Estimate
Null Associationa Positive Association Attributable Fraction (%)
Work-Related Risk Factor n Range n Range n Range
Wrist/Forearm
High job demands 4 1.2– 1.4 5 1.6–2.3 4 37– 56
Low decision latitude; low control 8 1.0– 1.7 3 1.6–6.3 3 37– 84
and low stimulus from work
Low social support 4 — 3 1.4–2.1 3 28– 52
Low job satisfaction 4 1.4 0 — — —
High perceived stress 1 1.5 3 — — —
Few rest break opportunities 5 2.7 2 1.5 1 33
Low support nonwork-related 4 — 0 — — —
Worry, tension, psychological 0 — 2 2.3–3.4 2 56– 71
distress, nonwork-related
Shoulder/Upper Arm
High job demands 6 1.1 6 1.5–1.9 3 33– 47
Low decision latitude; low control 8 1.1 6 1.6–1.9 3 37– 47
and low stimulus from work
Low social support 7 1.2 5 — — —
Low job satisfaction 2 — 0 — — —
High perceived job stress 3 1.5 3 — — —
Few rest break opportunities 3 — 1 3.3 1 70
Low support nonwork-related 3 — 0 — — —
Worry, tension, psychological 1 — 1 4.8 — 79
distress, nonwork-related
Elbow/Arm
High job demands 3 1.1 6 2.0–2.4 2 50– 58
Low decision latitude; low control 5 1.0– 3.0 1 2.8 1 64
and low stimulus from work
Low social support 5 1.2– 1.7 0 — — —
Low job satisfaction 2 — 0 — — —
High perceived job stress 1 1.4 2 2.0 1 50
Few rest break opportunities 1 — 1 3.1 1 67
Low support nonwork-related 1 — 0 — — —
Worry, tension, psychological 0 — 1 1.4–1.8 1 28– 44
distress, nonwork-related
All Upper Extremity
High job demands 6 1.1– 1.4 10 1.5–2.4 6 33– 58
Low decision latitude; low control 10 1.1– 1.7 6 1.6–2.8 4 37– 64
and low stimulus from work
Low social support 7 1.2 7 1.4–2.1 3 28– 52
Low job satisfaction 4 1.1– 1.4 0 — — —
High perceived job stress 2 1.4 5 2.0 1 50
Few rest break opportunities 3 1.4– 1.5 3 1.5–3.3 2 33– 70
Low support nonwork-related 3 — 0 — — —
Worry, tension, psychological 1 — 3 1.4–4.8 3 28– 79
distress, nonwork-related
Notes: n ¼ number of associations presented in epidemiologic studies. Details on studies are found in Appendix Table 2.9.
a
Confidence intervals of the risk estimates included the null estimate (1.0). The magnitude of the risk estimate often was
not presented.
Source: Reprinted with permission from NACS (Musculoskeletal Disorders and the workplace. Low Back and Upper
Extremities # 2001 by the National Academy of Sciences, Courtesy of the National Academics Press, Washington, D.C.)
Magnitude of Occupationally-Related Musculoskeletal Disorders 2-19
Reduction in perceived levels of job stress could reduce the risk for upper extremity disorders and symp-
toms by 50%, and reduction in nonwork-related worry, tension, and distress has the potential to reduce
risk by 28 to 79%. These findings highlight the potential impact of modifying both work-related and
nonwork-related sources of stress; however, they must be considered within the limitations presented
earlier in this chapter on the interpretation of attributable fractions. The observation that no study
that considered both psychosocial and physical risk factors met review inclusion criteria is important,
since many models assume a complex interaction among medical, physical/ergonomic, and workplace
and individual psychosocial factors (e.g., Armstrong et al., 1994).
There is a need for more prospective studies. Unlike the area of back pain, there are very few pro-
spective studies of psychosocial risk factors in work-related upper extremity disorders. There is also a
need for more consistent use of measures that assess specific psychosocial exposures. These measures
should have sound psychometric properties (e.g., reliability and validity) that justify their use. The
inclusion of various measures should also be based on well-conceived hypotheses based on
working models of how these factors may affect the occurrence of these symptoms and disorders
(Chapter 7 discusses such models). The case definitions used in studies should be carefully delineated,
and a more consistent use of outcome measures of symptoms, disorders, and functional limitations
should be implemented. The criteria used be select studies for review may have been too restrictive,
given the relative level of sophistication of the psychosocial literature in this area. Nevertheless,
despite this rigor, an association among perceived job stress, high job demands, nonwork-related dis-
tress, and upper extremity disorders was noted. These findings highlight the importance of conduct-
ing additional studied to identify specific factors that contribute to the identified risk factors and to
explain how these interact to influence the development, exacerbation, or maintenance of work-
related upper extremity disorders. It is also important to determine how these psychosocial factors
interact with medical and ergonomic risk factors to modify risk. It is possible that the psychosocial
factors that were not found to be consistently associated with the occurrence of work-related upper
extremity symptoms and disorders may influence the recovery process following onset. It is also poss-
ible that these factors may impact other outcomes, such as functional limitation or the ability to
sustain a full day’s work. The role of psychosocial factors in the exacerbation and maintenance of
these disorders requires further investigation.
This review highlights the potential utility of increased efforts directed at understanding the mechan-
isms by which job stress may impact work-related upper extremity disorders and the biological basis for
such an association. The review also supports the need to investigate approaches that eliminate or reduce
work- and nonwork-related sources of stress in prevention efforts.
2.3 Conclusion
A number of general and specific reviews were identified in which physical and psychosocial factors were
examined in relation to musculoskeletal disorders of the upper extremities and back (see review refer-
ences). These reviews served as a resource to supplement the panel’s efforts to identify relevant epidemio-
logic studies. They also were examined to determine whether conclusions when drawn from the panel’s
review were consistent with previous review efforts. The objectives of the reviews differed; some focused
on specific industries, job, or exposures, but others were more general. As a whole, the findings from
these other reviews are consistent with those arrived at in the panel’s review and provide additional
support for the conclusions.
The approach for considering causal inferences described in Chapter 3 is useful for summarizing our
review of the data from epidemiologic studies. As the tables in this chapter show, a number of studies
were judged to be of sufficient quality for inclusion in this review, and these vary in terms of the
types of designs and measurement approaches. While this variety complicates the generalization of
causal inferences, the summary tables indicate meaningful associations between work-related physical
and psychosocial exposures and musculoskeletal disorders. The tables show not only a preponderance
2-20 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
of evidence for some exposures (e.g., 26 of 32 studies found a significant association between vibration
and upper extremity musculoskeletal disorders), but also a consistency of association for many of the
exposures and outcomes. Although the literature contains mostly cross-sectional surveys, some work
to establish temporality; combined with the available prospective studies, evidence for temporal associ-
ation has been included in this chapter.
Most studies reviewed here also show a meaningful strength of association measured by both estimates
of the relative risk and calculation of attributable risk. The attributable risk provides an estimate of the
proportion of musculoskeletal disorders that might be prevented if effective interventions were
implemented; the calculations are appreciable for most of the exposures summarized here.
While the measure of attributable risk is meaningful for conceptualizing public health impact,
the calculations are presented for one factor at a time and do not account for other factors. As
noted in this chapter, many studies did account for potential confounders that could provide alterna-
tive explanations for the observed findings, but the number of confounders examined in each study
tends to be limited. While this is due to multiple factors (including expense associated with satisfying
sample size requirements), the fact that the associations persist after accounting for the confounders
measured to date supports the fundamental association, but it also justifies more detailed
investigation.
The joint effect of exposures is another element of the risk estimation suggested in Chapter 3 and
illustrated in this chapter. The attributable fraction summarizes the impact of a single exposure.
However, scant attention has been paid to the joint effect, or interaction, of two (or more) exposures,
increasing risk beyond the level of either alone. As noted in Chapter 3, some combinations of exposures
might work jointly, although their individual actions may or may not be significant. The studies by
Silverstein (e.g., Silverstein et al., 1987) showed an interaction between high force and high repetition
for upper extremity disorders among industrial workers. Further investigation for joint effects of
exposures is indicated from the current review. The effect of joint exposures can be investigated
within physical (vibration, force, load, etc.), and psychosocial (job strain, job demand, etc.) domains.
This review indicated the utter lack of studies that were found to be of sufficient quality and that
examine both physical and psychosocial factors together. Because evaluation of each has shown import-
ant effects on the development of musculoskeletal disorders, and some of the current evidence (although
modest) suggests that one does not explain the other, it is unlikely that more detailed investigation will
demonstrate that the association of either with musculoskeletal disorders is due to confounding with the
other. However, additional studies are needed to understand the degree to which each contributes to the
overall incidence of musculoskeletal disorders, and the extent to which both work synergistically in
selected work settings.
While the results presented in this chapter are consistent with one another, it is important to examine
the degree to which they are consistent with the results from the basic science and the biomechanics
studies (Chapter 5 and 6). Some of these studies have been mentioned in this chapter; their results
are generally consistent, providing here some suggestion of biological plausibility for the association
between physical forces and musculoskeletal disorders. The degree of consistency across different
levels of study will be discussed in more detail in the integration chapter.
Most epidemiologic studies have been summarized as having exposure and outcome measures dichot-
omized. The ability to make inferences about dose –response relationship is limited in this context. While
there are step-wise differences in dichotomous measures across studies (e.g., see Boshuizen et al., 1992:
Bovenzi and Zadini, 1992) that make cross-comparisons tantalizing, the differences in comparison
groups and other design features hinder the combining of results for generating inferences on dose –
response relationships. Future studies can help generate strong inferences by paying greater attention to
more refined levels of measurement. While this is a challenge, the strength of the current studies justifies
this effort.
In conclusion, the epidemiologic evidence provides support for associations between
workplace physical and psychosocial exposures and both back and upper extremity musculoskeletal
disorders.
Magnitude of Occupationally-Related Musculoskeletal Disorders 2-21
3.1 Introduction
3-1
3-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
of losing a job if there was a refusal to do unsafe work, possible human error, and the allocation of
human fault. The expert’s deposition took two full days to cover all of the human factors issues
that the opposing counsel thought was relevant to the accident.
Another expert witness testified about a vehicle control or handling problem that may have led to an
accident. The vehicle manufacturer had some written design objectives about driver handling parameters
that were used as a reference. In essence, the vehicle should compensate for driver overreactions or exces-
sive steering wheel inputs, and remain stable under all operational conditions including accident avoid-
ance maneuvers. In addition, the vehicle should signal or give the driver some perceptible signals when
the vehicle handling limits are approached. There should be sufficient stability and predictability that the
driver will not lose control of the vehicle and initiate an accident situation. There were questions as to
how human factors considerations, in a vehicle stability index, could assure controllability and have a
reasonable margin of safety in resisting rollover, tipover, and side slip or slide. The expert was asked
about reasonable design parameters and limits, given various risk-benefit balances, the technical feasi-
bility at the time of manufacture, the cost implications, and the effects on customer satisfaction. Also,
what constitutes an adequate perceptual signal to the driver and what is likely in terms of the driver reac-
tion? Many questions relied upon the subjective judgment of the expert, others upon extrapolation of
known test results, and others on specific published data. Different experts have varying success in
quick, decisive, and subjective answers. The more experience, the greater credibility of the answer. The
more decisive, the more the jury believes and remembers.
There is always the possibility of having a double translation; that is, to first convince the judge, in his
language, of the “reliability” of the proferred testimony and, then, convince a jury in the “everyday
language” that they understand. There is always the question of inequality, that different judges in the
same courthouse may have different standards as to the acceptability of evidence.
However, in most courts, the ultimate question is whether the expert witness testimony will help the
trier of fact (judge or jury) understand the evidence or to determine a fact. Qualifications of the expert
generally go to the weight of the evidence presented, not to its admissibility (Campbell, 2001; Goodstein,
2000).
The issue for the expert witness is to determine whether the judge favors information helpful to the
jury or whether the judge may be excessively strict and politically motivated. The lawyer who retains
the expert should be able to indicate, from past rulings, whether the judge will be zealous in applying
Daubert or its progeny. Daubert applies to all federal courts and is of considerable interest in many
state courts.
Daubert is considered a failure, by many scientists and engineers, because it has erected barriers to
what they believe is valid and relevant information that may not be familiar to the evidentiary gatekeeper.
They may believe that efficient judicial administration may conflict with facilitating the delivery of per-
tinent or illuminating information to the jury. If just one key expert has his testimony curtailed or
excluded, it may torpedo or signal the end of the plaintiff ’s or defense’s case. In fact, it may be quick
judicial resolution of a case based on what seems to be a legal technicality unrelated to the merit of
the claim or defense.
Some of the symptoms indicative of specific brain injury include memory problems (forgetfulness in
personal and occupational activities), problems in vigilance (maintenance of selective attention), poor
divided attention (on concurrent tasks), distractibility (from a perceptual set), speed and accuracy of
information organization and processing, and personality changes such as episodic hyperirritability,
aggressive outbursts, mood swings, emotional blunting, and socially inappropriate behavior. There are
many possible symptoms of deviant behavior (from some norm), good tests to assess various mental
functions, various combinations of localized and general brain insults or damage, generally accepted
diagnostic categories, and many treatment modalities. Similarly, over-the-counter and prescribed medi-
cations affect the brain and may produce undesired behavioral side effects that may or may not contrib-
ute to a human factors problem in a particular situation (Price, 1988).
The critical question may be whether or not the cognitive impairment existed before, during, or after a
particular event. Conversely, did the impairment result from an accident in which there was head impact,
some sudden acceleration or deceleration, or an unusual head rotation? Was there a head concussion (loss
of consciousness), a medical diagnosis of a closed head injury, or an unusual change in work performance?
This suggests that the human factors specialist should be on the alert for unusual behavior, if appro-
priate, consult with other team members of different specialties, and where available review available
documentation including medical records. The opposing counsel may do likewise and ask the expert per-
tinent questions at depositions or at trial.
3.3.8 Complexity
An expert may find that it is fairly easy to discuss complex issues in complex language. The expert may be
familiar with the concepts, symbols, equations, and specialized definitions of terminology used within
the specialty. It may be far more difficult to simplify, be direct, avoid unconditional qualifications that
add ambiguity and uncertainty, and reduce the key concepts to demonstrable analogies and graphic rep-
resentations. The expert should effectively communicate with the jurors, judges, and lawyers at a reason-
able cost in terms of time, money, and effort. Remember that rather complex issues are resolved every day
by judges determining the applicable law and by jurors deciding the factual issues. If the system did not
work, it would have been modified or replaced a long time ago. What this means is that it is up to the
expert to translate complex issues, in his specialty, to a form that can be understood by the average lay-
person (juror) who can rise to the occasion. If they can decide narrow issues in neurosurgery, nuclear
engineering, chemical processing, patent infringement, cost accounting, and pharmacology, it suggests
that they are able to learn, understand, and decide in a relatively short time, under judicial guidance,
if there is effective communication of the complexities of a specialty. Look up, do not look down on
the jurors or others in the legal system. Establish an equality-based rapport with all those in the legal
system who are functioning under time limitations, cost restraints, and often high stress levels. They
are exercising considerable personal responsibility, so should the expert as a member of the litigation
team who still exercises the independent discretion of a professional.
An example of complexity is the human input (control) as it effects vehicle dynamics. The input may be
accomplished by attempting to achieve a desired vehicle direction by steering wheel movements (rate of
movement and excursion angle limits), by depressing the throttle (force, position, and resulting vehicle
acceleration or deceleration), and braking (when, how much, and as effected by weather and road con-
ditions). Is the human input modified by perceptions that the lateral stability of the vehicle is approaching
its limits (requiring a precautionary input) or has it exceeded its limits (requiring corrective action for an
out-of-control vehicle)? Has there been a panic reaction by the driver with excessive, untimely, insufficient,
or inappropriate steering maneuvers? Can the driver be expected to exert a timely and effective human
input, given the handling characteristics of the vehicle? Steer angle changes may be monitored to
prevent overshoot, excessive lateral acceleration, and dampen system oscillations in steady-state cornering
maneuvers. The human input may be modified with active steering (transient steering torque under auto-
matic control) where the steering ratio varies with vehicle speed (Triggs, 1988). The suspension may
modify steering response if there is air suspension or variable torque anti-sway bars. There may be roll
Legal Issues in Occupational Ergonomics 3-9
stabilization, lateral body movement, or other active chassis, suspension, or steering devices. The human
input may be overridden, for a brief period of time, when electronic stabilization systems control quick
impulses to various brakes, effect throttle position or power dynamics, and have active steering to
restore an out-of-control vehicle to a straight ahead position. A yaw velocity and roll velocity sensor
may institute proactive inputs to an electronic control system that completely overrides human input
where the driver’s reaction time is inadequate to meet the needs. There are numerous peer accepted tech-
nical terms that can be used. There are numerous technical devices that can assist in maintaining vehicle
stability or act as automatic driver support systems. They serve to recognize problems inherent in those
human characteristics important to safe vehicle handling. In short, describing the interactions between
driver, the vehicle, and the roadway can be very complex, confusing, and difficult. They may be the subject
of both the human factors specialist and the vehicle dynamics engineer acting in a cooperative fashion.
Complexity is often the result of the use of precise terminology that is appropriate where brevity of
communication between peers is desirable or for those in research where exact replication is important.
However, it may be just bureaucratic clutter or dress up that is unnecessary. The question is always what
purpose does the complexity serve and how can it be truthfully simplified for a lay audience such as a
jury? The university professor is often seen as a person who converts the complex into something that
can be understood and retained by a select group of motivated students. But, the background of those
students is homogenous and elevated compared to jurors.
Lawyers in their pre-trial preparation gather, analyze, and determine the implications of a considerable
body of evidence. As they study the evidence they attempt to narrow issues, condense the key facts, select
among the witnesses, and emphasize certain evidence. It is a process of gradual simplification, not
unknown among other professional disciplines.
A treating physician may be obligated to inform and to explain to the patient something about the
diagnosis of a disease, the treatment options, the prognosis, and the various risks. The physician must
simplify the complexities, tailor the discussion to the needs and inherent level of understanding of the
patients, and secure actual informed consent where necessary. A reasonably direct and honest approach
requires simplification and truth for effective communication to the recipient, if there is to be mutual
trust created or affirmed. The proponent of any discipline must engender personal trust if there is to
be reliance on the analysis and opinion in a complex subject area.
Jury instructions are of particular importance to an expert witness, since they are the operative guide-
lines for the trier-of-fact (usually a jury). These are the landmarks around which all of the testimony is
oriented (i.e., there must be relevancy as to the contested issues of fact in the case before the court). There
have been attempts to simplify the complex legal language of some jury instructions into “plain English”
instructions. The advocacy aspects of a trial, such as final argument, permits the lawyer to use plain
English to explain the meaning of the admitted evidence.
Thus, the simplification of complexities is a continual ongoing process for all those involved in the
litigation process. It may not appear to be simplification with numerous and lengthy depositions,
many motions and declarations, and endless discovery in the form of interrogatories and requests for
production or admission that may or may not be seen by the expert witness, consultant, coordinator,
or remote employee. But, after any search for possible evidence, the simplification process must take
place for all those involved. The court may impose strict limits as to time, both for preparation and
in-court testimony.
before his deposition is taken. A written report may or may not be required that states all opinions and
the basis for those opinions. An early oral report to the retaining attorney may be helpful for him in pre-
paring or responding to interrogatories, the production of documents, and requests for admission. It is
not wise to hold back information on key issues for use at trial or to somehow attempt to supplement or
revise statements made at depositions or in reports. Thus, full and timely disclosure of all opinions
should be made before the deadlines and the start of the trial.
3.4.3 Criticism
Some lawyers like to encourage one expert to criticize another for some perceived mistakes, some viola-
tions of rules, some possible misapplications or misinterpretations, or for omissions or failures to meet
some standard of conduct. For example, the questions might start as follows “what would you have done
under the same circumstances” (a hypothetical question) or “what should be done to achieve a reliable
and fair opinion or conclusion” (a general proposition or standard of care). The lawyer might believe that
his case is strengthened by discrediting the opponent’s expert. The expert may believe that his profession
could be harmed by accusations of sloppy work or deceptive tests. The experts may differ only because
each formulates an opinion on different set of facts, each gives different weight to some key facts, or one
lets advocacy influence his perception. The cardinal rule is for the experts to stay within and conform to
the code of ethics of their profession. Civility should be paramount, despite the actions of others. There
may be ethical rules that encourage a challenge to improper, invalid, or immoral testing, an insufficient or
inappropriate basis for conclusions and opinions, and illegal or unethical practices.
3. All photographs in your possession pertaining to the accident, the accident scene, or the
specified equipment and its component parts, including any associated products involved in
the accident.
4. The entire contents of your file with respect to the subject lawsuit, including all documents
received from counsel or any of their representatives and any documents, which you have com-
piled independent of counsel.
5. True and correct copies of any and all product analyses and derivative charts, diagrams, reports,
computer disks, computer programs, and journal articles in your possession upon which you
relied or will rely in forming your opinions.
6. True and correct copies of all videotapes, audio recordings, computer disks, and photographs of
any testing on the product involved in this case.
7. All correspondences, which were prepared, signed, sent, received, drafted, or delivered by you to
any other person, which pertains to or refers to your involvement in this litigation matter.
8. Any and all reports, memoranda, graphs, drawings, work papers, calculations, images, photo-
graphs, moving pictures, video tapes, computer disks, and correspondences in your possession
concerning the testing done on any similar or identical products, which provide information that
supports any of your opinions.
9. Any and all reports, memoranda, graphs, drawings, work papers, calculations, images, photo-
graphs, moving pictures, videotapes, computer disks, and correspondences in your possession
concerning testing done on any product other than the product in this case that provides infor-
mation regarding any of your opinions.
10. A listing of any other lawsuits in which you have testified as an expert, either at trial or by
deposition, within the preceding 5 yr.
11. All references, articles, publications, presentations, books, book chapters, lecture materials, and
other documents relating to any publication authored, program attended, or any presentation
in which you (the deponent) participated, which in any way relates or refers to the subject
matter and opinions that you (the deponent) may offer, or the area of your expertise as an
expert in this lawsuit.
12. All billings, fee agreements, time records, financial statements, contracts involving fees and costs,
and all correspondence and other documents relating to your retention (the deponent) and
those, which show time and charges incurred by you (the deponent) in connection with your
activities in this lawsuit.
13. All medical records evaluations, neuropsychological tests, consult reports, raw data, x-rays, CT
scans, MRIs, EEGs, SPEC scans, electrodiagnostic findings, and all other files, documents, and
reports relied upon or used by you (the deponent) in connection with your activities in this
lawsuit, arbitration, or mediation.
14. A list of all cases or projects in which you participated as an advisor, consultant, or employee that
related to the design and development of this product, function, service, or system.
15. The names, occupational designations, and addresses of each and every person from whom infor-
mation was obtained that could be utilized by you in this case.
16. A list of all meetings, conferences, or discussions with other experts retained in this matter, and
pertaining to this case, including dates, locations, and the names of those involved.
17. A list of written or oral statements of all witnesses that pertain in any way to this litigation seen or
reviewed by you, including a copy of each statement and any notes or reviews made by you of the
statements.
18. A list of all witnesses interviewed, questioned, heard, or observed that pertain to the accident, the
scene of the accident, or the products, objects, or materials involved in the accident or injury-
causing event.
19. All maps, diagrams, sketches, measurements, and material analyses, related to the injury-causing
event that were made by you or directed by you, which may serve as foundation or demonstrative
evidence supporting your opinions in this case.
3-12 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
3.4.5 Fees
The fees to be charged for professional services should be reasonable under the circumstances. The fees
may vary widely for consultation, analysis, evaluation, travel, deposition time, trial appearances, prep-
aration of demonstrative evidence, the assistance of associates or other professionals, and administrative
support services. There should be a printed and up-to-date fee schedule that is applicable to all kinds of
work. It should not differ for various projects or parties. It is important that the expectations of both the
retaining party and the expert or consultant be clearly known. The expectations include what is to be
achieved within the agreed time and cost estimates. There should be approval of any extraordinary
expenditures such as those for special research, laboratory testing, and special exhibits. Equality of
performance on different projects or cases suggest the need for budgeting sufficient calendar time and
avoiding schedule conflicts, since a crowded or disorganized schedule could disrupt the schedules of
other participants. The knowledge of the project or case schedule is important to avoid last minute
efforts and shortcomings in preparation; for example, “I did not know of that or thought of it” in the
middle of court testimony. In essence, the timely and cost-effective performance should meet or
exceed the comparable accomplishments of peers within the specialty.
The fees should not be excessive. High fees do not suggest that there will be a high level of performance.
Experts are evaluated on past performance and reputation. The past is prologue. Some of the very best
and well-known consultants and experts charge rather moderate fees, divide the costs of travel among
several projects, and share test costs that can be applied to multiple projects. It is the total final cost
that is important, not the hourly rate. Excessive fees may suggest “purchased testimony” and possible
ethical problems.
It is generally assumed that the expert is already familiar and prepared, in a general sense, with the
content of the specialty that is applicable to the project. But, time should be allocated to learn recent
developments in the field, to refine what may be said about possible conflicts on key issues, and to
prepare a list of supporting publications. In other words, an informed estimate of the overall cost
should be made. This is to avoid insufficient preparation that could result in court testimony that is mis-
taken or inadequate, since a trial error can be costly and irremediable. Poor advice given during an urgent
effort to correct a liability problem, within a company, could have serious consequences in terms of mon-
etary costs and human lives. In essence, there may be a fleeting window of opportunity, in a competitive
enterprise, for informed relevant information, rather than “old hat” opinions. Some specialists do choose
the path of least effort with conceptions of consultant-only project aspirations, but most projects related
to litigation are an intellectual challenge requiring “best efforts.” Never underestimate what is known
about your specialty by those in other disciplines, so provide something exceptional for the purchasers
of your services.
Retainers also should be reasonable under the circumstances. Before charging any fees, there should be
full disclosure of any possible conflicts-of-interest and of any potential problems that could render the
professional services impaired, useless, void, or excludable. If problems arise during the job performance,
they should be disclosed early enough to permit repair or replacement of the expert. Do not wait until
after there is a formal disclosure of experts in litigation, since after naming the expert witnesses they may
not be replaceable and a big hole could be left in the trial presentation. It is advantageous to have every-
thing that relates to fees and your professional activities in writing, in anticipation of fee disputes, but
such writings should be reviewed by legal counsel for meaning, effect, legality, and possible interpretation
by other parties. Care is required where money motivates action by others.
to a jury (Davis, 2001). Limitations exist where testimony is based on scientific principles, formulas, dis-
coveries, or procedures developed by others (Rickgauer, 2001).
There is another form of testimony, based on inductive reasoning (from particulars to the whole)
(Holy Cross, 2001). This includes pure opinion based on the expert’s own training, experiences, obser-
vations, and research (Ronnie Jones, 2003). For example, tire expert opinion may not be scientific
testimony (Kumho, 1999).
The logic involved may be a derivative of both hearsay and speculation objections. There is more credi-
bility when opinions are based on direct personal knowledge. There is less credibility when expert
opinions may involve some speculation in just applying someone else’s results, beliefs, procedures,
and conclusions. A juror might not be able to distinguish between the weight that should be given to
direct knowledge as opposed to indirect knowledge, so the judge acts to balance the scales and assure
that only competent testimony reaches the jury.
area of expertise.” An opinion has no evidentiary value without a “reasoned explanation connecting the
factual predicates to the ultimate conclusion.” If the testimony is that it “could have been a cause-in-fact”
that is insufficient, because it is a mere possibility, and then it becomes “the duty of the court to direct a
verdict.” In essence, the court held that the expert opinion must explain why the facts convinced the
expert and, therefore, should convince a jury. A conclusion is not an explanation. If not more likely
(probable) than not, it is speculation that must be excluded.
In the other case, the expert testimony was challenged on the basis of the “possibility of causation”
opinions and that they were “unsupported by peer reviewed scientific and medical literature.” The appel-
late court held that this was a matter of the “credibility and weight of the expert testimony,” and that
“jurors may temper the acceptance of his testimony with a healthy skepticism born of their knowledge
that all human beings are fallible.” In essence, it was not a question of admissibility (by the judge) but of
the weight to be given (by the jury) to the “underlying bases” for the opinions. The opinions were based
on generally accepted methods, tests, interview techniques, and procedures. In other words, the first case
emphasized a strict role for the judge concerning the admissibility of evidence (by excluding expert
opinions). Whereas the second case de-emphasized admissibility and instead relied upon the role of
the jury in determining the weight to be given to expert testimony.
should attempt to discern whether the judge understands the technical concepts, since the jurors may be
in the same position in terms of effective communication.
The trial is often a diverse mixture of law and facts, lawyers and judges, jurors and witnesses, cues and
instructions, and the colorization or expression of different life experiences, cultures, and value systems.
All of these elements interact to make prediction of a trial outcome difficult. It is the uncertainty that acts
as a compromising force in arbitrations, mediations, and settlements before, during, and after trials.
Unfortunately, the expert witness is privy to a rather small portion of the litigation process. The trial con-
sultant may advise only on the selection of the jurors as the trial lawyer attempts to reduce the uncertain-
ties. The uncertainty inherent in the trial process should be understood by the expert witness, since the
expert should be capable of adaptation to variations in format, interpretations of content, and
unexpected or novel issues.
3.5 Bias
Each occupational “discipline” has its own shared beliefs or value systems. They are intended to guide or
influence personal decisions and judgments. This is a form of a known bias. It can easily serve as a factual
filter and may provide an extraneous perceptual flavoring about a fact situation. People can see what they
want to see and perceive what is consistent with their expectations and beliefs. This can result in misun-
derstanding or difficulty when what is appropriate in one discipline (such as ergonomics) is applied,
utilized, or translated for use in another discipline (such as the law). The translator (between disciplines)
needs to know something about both worlds or errors of communication, understanding, and compre-
hension should be created. For example, the research scientist may believe that absolute certainty does
not exist for any conclusion and further research is always necessary, but the law may define scientific
certainty as something that might be only above the 50% level of present belief.
The term peer approval may elicit statements that there is and always should be constructive criticisms
by fellow scientists, appropriate questions as to better methodology, and productive doubts by fellow
scientists. Scientists may believe that attaining full peer approval is just an illusionary concept. This ques-
tioning attitude, by peer group scientists, may be particularly present for research applications (to the
“real world”) or for generalizing research findings (going beyond the original research context).
However, despite rejection of the concepts of full peer approval or the need to attempt to attain such
a approval from other scientists, some proof of peer approval may be a minimum requirement for per-
mitting an opinion to be expressed in a court of law. No peer approval may mean no helpful testimony
for the trier-of-fact. The translator (between disciplines) provides meaning to words and concepts by
using the message recipient’s own language. Obviously, there should be effective, truthful, and meaning-
ful communication by those persons who testify or offer scientific opinions in a societal context. If there
is bias, it should be disclosed.
It is the trial court judge who has “the power” to make decisions regarding whether or not the expert
witness’s testimony will be presented to the jury. If the testimony is admissible, the judge may then decide
on what issues, in what depth or scope, and what limiting jury instructions or judge’s cautionary com-
ments may be given to the jurors. The discretion of the trial judge is considerable. How the judges control
the courtroom and testimony varies widely within a particular jurisdiction or courthouse. They vary
widely in interpreting the law and applying it to the facts of a particular case. This is because of biases
created by strong personal beliefs, basic value systems, their strengths and limits of knowledge in special-
ized areas, the techniques they employ to control the courtroom, the kind of evidence and advocacy being
presented, the possible outcome of the case and its consequences, and possible local reelections or judicial
advancement concerns. Therefore, the expert witness should not be offended, displeased, shocked,
chagrined, or elated at the admissibility of the testimony or the judge’s attitude toward his or her testi-
mony or that of any other expert witness in the case. Perceptions of bias in the courtroom should not
encourage insertion of responsive bias or altered demeanor. The expert witness should be prepared to
comply and to adjust to the trial judge’s rulings. It is the trial judge who controls the legal process by
3-16 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
pretrial, in-trial, and posttrial rulings. The expert’s role is not to respond to perceived bias, but to remain
a calm, independent, neutral person who can assist the trier-of-fact (judge or jury), while maintaining
personal integrity, intellectual rigor, and fairness. Bias may be everywhere, but it should not prevail.
References
Campbell v Metro. Prop. & Gas Ins. Co., 239 F.3d 179, 184 (2d Cir., 2001). Expert qualifications.
Clausen v M/V New Carissa, 339 F.3d 1049 (9th Cir., 2003).
Daubert v Merrell Dow Pharms. Inc., 509 U.S. 579 (1993); Gen. Elec. Co. v Joiner, 522 U.S. 136 (1997); and
Kumho Tire Co., Ltd. v Carmichael, 526 U.S. 137 (1999). The Daubert doctrine is codified in
Federal Rule of Evidence 702. It states: “If scientific, technical, or other specialized knowledge
will assist the trier of fact to understand the evidence or to determine a fact in issue, a witness qua-
lified as an expert by knowledge, skill, experience, training, or education, may testify thereto in the
form of an opinion or otherwise, if (1) the testimony is based upon sufficient facts or data, (2) the
testimony is the product of reliable principles and methods, and (3) the witness has applied the
principles and methods reliably to the facts of the case.”
Davis v Caterpillar, Inc., 787 So. 2d 894 (Fla. 3d DCA, 2001).
Frye v United States, 293 F.1013 (D.C. Cir, 1923).
Goodstein, David, How science works, in Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence, 2nd ed., 2000, 70.
Holy Cross Hops., Inc v Marrone, 816 So. 2d 1113, 1117 (Fla. 4th DCA, 2001).
Jennings v Palomar Pomerado Health Systems, Inc., 114 Cal. App. 4th 1108 (Dec. 2003), 4th Dist., Review
denied.
Kepner, C.H. and Tregoe, B.B., The rational manager: a systematic approach to problem solving and
decision making. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1965.
Kumho Tire Co. v Carmichael, 526 U.S. 137, 141, 142 (1999).
Price, Dennis L., Effects of alcohol and drugs, in Peters, George A. and Peters, Barbara J., Eds., Automotive
Engineering and Litigation, Vol. 2, New York and London: Garland Law Publishing, 1988,
pp. 489–334.
Rickgauer v Sarkar, 804 So. 2d 502, 504 (Fla. 5th DCA, 2001).
Roberti v Andy’s Termite & Pest Control Inc., 113 Cal. App. 4th 893 (Nov. 2003), 2nd Dist., Review denied
2-18-04.
Ronnie Jones and Sylvia Jones v Goodyear Tire & Rubber Co., DCA, 3d Dist., Fla., July 2003 (Case 3
DO1-3583).
Triggs, Thomas J., Speed estimation, in Peters, George A. and Peters, Barbara J., Eds., Automotive Engin-
eering and Litigation, Vol. 2, New York and London: Garland Law Publishing, 1988, pp. 569–598.
Vinal, Robert W., Criminal liability of contractors, engineers, and building owners regarding asbestos
projects, in Peters, George A. and Peters, Barbara J., Eds., Sourcebook on Asbestos Diseases:
Medical, Legal, and Engineering Aspects, Vol. 5, Asbestos Abatement. Salem, NH: Butterworth
Legal Publishers, 1991, pp. 45– 62.
Wang, C. Julius., Product improvement from integrated quantitative techniques, in Peters, George A. and
Peters, Barbara J., Eds., Automotive Engineering and Litigation, Vol. 5, New York: Wiley Law
Publications, 1993, pp. 121 –148.
Peters, George A. and Peters, Barbara J., Warnings, Instructions, and Technical Communication, Tucson,
AZ: Lawyers & Judges Publishing Co., 1999. Note: This book illustrates the factors to be considered
when warnings and instructions become a part of a lawsuit.
Peters, George A. and Peters, Barbara J., Handling Soft Tissue Injury Cases: Legal Aspects, 2nd ed., Vol. 1,
Charlottesville, VA: Lexis Law Publishing, 1999. Note: This book illustrates many of the legal
considerations involved in lawsuits, from the viewpoint of the lawyers. Includes the general pro-
cedures utilized by lawyers in the preparation of their cases.
Kazan, Steven and Moscowitz, Ellyn, The role of the plaintiff ’s attorney in asbestos litigation, in Peters,
George A. and Peters, Barbara J., Eds., Sourcebook on Asbestos Diseases: Medical, Legal, and
Engineering Aspects, Vol. 5, Asbestos Abatement. Salem, NH: Butterworth Legal Publishers,
1991, pp. 1 –24. Note: This chapter and this book illustrate the legal history aspects of lawsuits
or how the information accumulates on a particular problem area.
Watson, Donald, Ed., Architectural Design Data, 7th ed., New York: McGraw-Hill, 1997. Note: This book
contains a range of useful topics, such as lighting, stair design, elevators, door and windows, con-
struction, fire safety, units of measurement, and human figure dimensions. Note: Illustrative of
books that provide background information for human factors analysis.
Peters, George A., Product liability and safety, in Kreith, Frank, Ed., The CRC Handbook of Mechanical
Engineering, Boca Raton, FL and London: CRC Press, 1998, pp. 20.11 –20.15. Note: This book
provides valuable background and reference data with discussions of subjects ranging from engin-
eering design and mathematics to project management and mechanical systems. It may be con-
sidered a supplement to this chapter in this Handbook.
National Research Council and the Institute of Medicine, Musculoskeletal Dirsorders and the Workplace,
Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press, 2001. Note: Illustrates the kind of peer group authored
book given great weight in lawsuits. Deals with a common human factors problem.
4
Cost Justification
for Implementing
Ergonomics
Intervention
4.1 Introduction
4-1
4-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
In this article we will consider the “economics of ergonomics” and show that good working conditions
are compatible with profit. We will discuss:
. The economic reasoning behind cost-benefit analysis in enterprises
. A cost-benefit model that ergonomists and others may use to support their economic arguments
for good working conditions
. Case studies to illustrate that “good ergonomics is good economics”
productivity costs in a generic form, an arrangement that enterprises could use to improve working con-
ditions. Most of her work was published in Swedish but she has summarized some of her ideas in English
(Kupi et al., 1993).
A Special Issue of Applied Ergonomics (Vol. 34, 5, September 2003) was devoted to cost effectiveness.
Unfortunately, none of the articles in this Special Issue proposed a generic method that could be used by
ergonomists and others wishing to implement ergonomics solutions. Each author took their case study
and showed its financial effectiveness, but the methodology was unique to that case and could not easily
be used in other workplaces.
It was a similar situation at the European Conference on cost effectiveness (Mossink and Licher, 1997)
in that very few papers indicated useful working models that could be used by ergonomists.
It seems unfortunate that more models are not developed for occupational health and safety on the
market so that users would be able to choose the model which best fits their needs. Cost-benefit
models that are not suitable are, for example, engineering ones that only derive from the technology
of the design or process and do not express the affect on, and the effect of, the workers and other
persons concerned. It is, at least partly, to fill this gap that the Productivity Assessment Tool (and its pre-
decessor, the Productivity Model) has been developed and will be discussed later in this article.
thus include reduced injury or other costs in a cost-benefit analysis? When we make predictions about the
future we are limited in the accuracy of our predictions. Enterprises must use historical data to make
predictions about the future and hence can only estimate costs and benefits.
We suggest that ergonomists follow the lead of engineers in making use of cost-benefit analysis tools.
Engineers have developed cost-benefit models that suit the questions they want to answer such as “should
we buy that new machine or should we continue to maintain the old one?” Engineers are also comfor-
table with the need to make estimates and how to go about estimation on the basis of their experience.
Think of cost-benefit analysis as just a financial tool to assist in asserting one’s point of view about the
need for, and worth of, one’s ergonomics interventions.
Cost-benefit analysis is actually an economic model and to use it effectively in situations where one’s
proposal is competing for funds against other projects, it is necessary to understand at least some of the
economics behind it.
This follows from an economic philosophy that says increased production costs will push up the price
and so less people will buy the product, which is not always true. For instance, motor vehicle manufac-
turers in Sweden during the last several decades found that improving the skills of their workforce and
giving workers the responsibility for quality has resulted in improved quality of vehicles manufactured.
This has not just saved warranty costs but there is more demand for these higher quality motor vehicles.
Estimating the impact of an ergonomics intervention on future product sales is very complex and
necessarily imprecise. The best estimates of the impact of such projects come from past experience.
Look for case studies of a similar intervention to the one proposed and, if necessary, adjust the estimated
impact up or down, in one’s cost-benefit analysis, to better match one’s enterprise.
recruitment and worker quality and morale but these will not include the social or personal disruptive
effects on the concerned workers. An argument on the basis of social justice may be needed.
The data collected is centered around the people and not the product which, to some extent, is a
point of departure from the organizational concepts originating from the work of, for example,
Deming (1982). The major determination of this cost-benefit analysis is of the workers who
produce the goods or services and not of the goods or services themselves. Although there are
programs that take the starting point as the equipment and processes of the manufacturing or
service systems, this cost-benefit analysis concentrates on the people’s side. In the long run both
approaches need to be integrated but, for the purposes of this article, we will concentrate on the
people’s side.
Certain assumptions need to be made for a cost-benefit analysis model and these include:
. The important and/or critical data relating to employment costs
. The costs for implementing the intervention in the work place
. The financial benefits due to the intervention
These assumptions are little different from the assumptions made when the warehouse manager wants a
new fork lift truck, the call center manager wants new computer software or the hotel manager requires
more cleaning staff. The initial costs may be known but the benefits are only assumptions. Will the fork
lift truck reduce loading time and goods damage? Will the new software bring in more telephone custo-
mers? Will the extra staff increase the standard and the guest fees of the hotel? Determining the future can
only be based on the best guess even if based on experience.
Once the assumptions are agreed upon then data can be collected. Some of the employment data
required are straightforward and usually easy to obtain. These include the direct labor costs of hours
worked, wages, social costs, training, absenteeism, etc. but may also include a portion of the organiz-
ational costs of supervision, management and head office costs.
Other data may be less easy to obtain but, in our experiences, give the greatest return from ergonomics
investment. These include:
. Productivity (gross output and quality)
. Labor turnover
. Error and warranty costs
One can also add equipment and material costs (equipment failures, waste, errors, etc.) particularly if
these are expected to change, for better or worse, due to the intervention. The costs for all these items,
and the eventual savings, are related mainly to the labor costs.
test cases must be comparably effective in terms of injury prevention or other worker benefits and not
solely economic comparisons.
This particular cost-benefit analysis model can be used to answer several economic questions, which
follow.
4.3.3.4 Rehabilitation
As a cost-benefit analysis can be used for individual employees it is suitable for determining the cost of
rehabilitation of an injured worker. The data used assume that the injured employee will come back to
work but with various restrictions on his/her work tasks and the medical and rehabilitation costs can be
added to give a complete financial picture. By these means the financial investment needed to complete a
rehabilitation program can be determined.
The following case studies illustrate the use of cost-benefit analysis in implementing ergonomics
intervention and are restricted to illustrating cost-benefit analysis in productivity measurement (see
Section 4.3.1.1). For more details on these and studies illustrating other uses of cost-benefit analysis,
the reader is referred to Oxenburgh et al. (2004).
4.4 Applications
The following three case studies are “traditional” in that the problems were musculoskeletal
injuries in manual workers with the solutions within the realm of standard ergonomics interventions.
4-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
They illustrate three types of interventions, all of which were costed by the use of cost-benefit
analysis.
The ergonomics interventions (cases) are:
. Section 4.4.1 — almost no-cost changes to the work methods where the management and men
experimented to find the best solution
. Section 4.4.2 — an ergonomics intervention where an ergonomist altered working heights to over-
come poor posture
. Section 4.4.3 — large capital investment where engineers designed new equipment for injury
prevention
We intend that these case studies are illustrative of cost-benefit analysis rather than ergonomics solutions
and are simplified examples of the ones given by Oxenburgh et al. (2004). For a more detailed analysis,
please see this reference.
TABLE 4.1 Cost-Benefit Analysis for Loading and Delivering Bulk Packages
Initial Case Improved Loading
Employment costs for the loading work (units/year)a 50,200 55,700
Truck costs (units/year) 501,400 451,300
Total yearly costs for loading and delivery 551,600 507,000
Intervention costs: management and warehouse staff — 625
time (units)b
Savings (units/year) — 44,600
Pay-back period — 1 week
a
For reasons of confidentiality, “units” are used for costing.
b
A “one-off ” cost.
Cost Justification for Implementing Ergonomics Intervention 4-11
TABLE 4.2 Reduction in Injury Absence, Overtime, and Conveyor Belt Breakdowns
Initial Case Improved Work Case
Total cost of employment for the deshiving area, 56,400 50,400
including overtime (£/year)
Cost of breakdowns (wash line idle) (£/year) 32,000 650
Total cost (£/year) 88,400 51,050
Intervention costs (£)a — 8,830
Savings (£/year) — 37,350
Pay-back Period (months) — 3
a
A “one-off ” cost.
4-12 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
TABLE 4.3 Cost-Benefit Analysis for the Introduction of a Cable Handling Machine Including Direct
Wages and Supervisory Costs
Manual Handling Design and Use of the Cable
of the Cable Handling Machine
Miners’ wage cost to move the cable 36,600 5,200
($ per year)
Direct supervisory costs for total hours on 6,800 980
cable handling ($ per year)
Net labor costs for total hours on cable 43,400 6,180
handling ($ per year)
Intervention costs ($)a 65,300
Savings in labor costs ($ per year) — 37,220
Pay-back period (months) — 21
a
A “one-off ” cost.
References
Ahonen, G., The nation-wide programme for health and safety in SMEs in Finland. Economic evaluation
and incentives for the company management. Protection to Promotion. Occupational Health and
Safety in Small-scale Enterprises. People and Work. Research Reports 25, Finnish Institute of
Occupational Health, 1998, 151–156.
Cost Justification for Implementing Ergonomics Intervention 4-13
Applied Ergonomics, Special Issue: Cost Effectiveness. Vol. 34, number 5, September 2003.
Bohle, P. and Quinlan, M., Managing Occupational Health and Safety, 2nd ed., MacMillan, Melbourne,
Australia, 2000.
Connon, C., Reeb-Whitaker, C., and Curwick, C., Healthy Workplaces Technical Report 67-3-2003, 2003,
Washington State Department of Labor and Industries, Olympia, WA.
Deming, W.E., Out of the Crisis, 1982, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
Kupi, E., Liukkonen, P., and Mattila, M., Staff use of time and company productivity, Nordisk Ergonomi,
4, 9–11, 1993.
Mossink, J. and Licher, F., Proceedings of the European Conference on Costs and Benefits of Occu-
pational Safety and Health, Proceedings of the Conference, Amsterdam, NIA TNO, 1997.
Osborne, D.J., Branton, R., Leal, F., Shipley, P., and Stewart, T., Person-Centred Ergonomics: A Brantonian
View of Human Factors, Taylor & Francis, London, 1993.
Oxenburgh, M.S. and Guldberg, H.H., The economic and health effects on introducing a safe manual
handling code of practice, Int. J. Ind. Ergon., 12, 241–253, 1993.
Oxenburgh, M., Marlow, P., and Oxenburgh, A., Increasing Productivity and Profit through Health &
Safety: The Financial Returns from a Safe Working Environment, 2nd ed., Boca Raton, FL, CRC
Press, 2004.
ProductAbility, 2004. Software for the Productivity Assessment Tool. See www.productAbility.co.uk or
e-mail [email protected]
Spilling, S., Eitrheim, J., and Aarås, A., Cost-benefit analyses of work environment investment at STK’s
plant at Kongsvinger, in The Ergonomics of Working Postures, Corlett, Wilson, and Manencia, Eds.,
Taylor & Francis, London, 1986, pp. 380 –397.
5
Humans in Work
System Environment
5-1
5-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
to the death of bacteria under the influence of light or disinfectants, to the consumptions of an animal by
starvation, and to the decrease of an animal or human population where death rate is higher than birth
rate.” The discovery of this phenomenon of “isomorphy of laws” in the different scientific fields caused
Ludwig von Bertalanffy to aim for a “Unity of Science” with help of the “General System Theory.” It was
apparent to him that this ambitious goal could only be achieved — if ever — in the inscrutable future.
His proceeding is marked mathematically. A standardization of the sciences is finally seen by him as the
“reduction of all science to physics, in the final resolution of all phenomena into physical events.”
The system approach of cybernetics (Wiener, 1961) was developed at about the same time as the
“General System Theory.” This approach is likewise mathematically formed. Wiener and others under-
stand cybernetics (Greek: “kybernetes” ¼ steermanship) as the “entire field of control and communi-
cation theory, whether in the machine or in the animal” (Wiener, 1961). The cybernetics takes up
concepts of feedback, regulation, and control and interprets social systems in a cybernetic way.
Methods, procedures, and realizations of the (automatic) control engineering are generalized and
applied to nontechnical concepts. A process is described with the control loop, which functions auton-
omously on the basis of the exact given premises. A desired value is given to the system. If it comes to a
disturbance, the system implements prior defined corrections autonomously, in order to achieve the pro-
grammed specified condition again. The system compensates any environmental changes with self
change. It experiences information about required changes via feedback of the results of its procedures.
Figure 5.1 shows an example of a control loop with reference to the handling of a boat (Frank, 1964). The
boat can be interpreted as a socio-technical system. The captain formulates the destination. The pilot
determines the particular location (current status) and composes a program to transfer the current
status to the desired status. He has to “save” the desired status, to measure the current status, to
compare the two values and to derive a program. This program has to be conveyed to the steersman
by the pilot in terms of individual decisions (so-called “determined” decisions). The steersman transfers
these orders into navigation positions.
Self-regularization, adaptation, and learning aptitude are, therefore, system specifications, which are
examined by the cybernetics. The cybernetics discusses for the first time the relationship between
system and environment as a problem of constancy and change. In the center of the considerations
the question is formed, how system constancy can be maintained in a changing environment. For the
captain
destination
target situation
pilot
actual data processing and planning
situation
steersman
steering
environment
first time, the stability of a system was not defined as a trait of systems (ontological approach) but as a
problem, which has to be solved perpetually (Luhmann, 1973).
An integration of the “General System Theory” and the cybernetics to a system-theoretical-cybernetic
concept began for the first time with Ashby (1956). He uses the “Black Box theory” to describe the
relations between an experimenter and his environment with special attention to the flow of information.
“The primary data of any investigation of a Black Box consists of a sequence of values of the vector with
two components: input state, output state” (comparable to cybernetics). Referring to the “General
System Theory,” two machines are isomorphic, “if a one– one transformation of the states (input and
output) of the one machine into those of the other can convert the one representation to the other.”
Thus, Ashby explains the fundamentals of cybernetics: the processes within the system and the processes
of communication between system and system. Those are important to study the central theme of
cybernetics — regulation and control. Regulation is essentially related to the flow of variety; without
regulation the variety is large — with regulation it is small.
In the meantime it emerged that various impulses for individual scientific fields proceeded from the
system-theoretical-cybernetic concept. However, their requirement to promote a standardization of
science in different material systems with the help of a discipline-spreading terminology and the
assumed existence of a more formal structure, that is, isomorphic laws, can only be partly fulfilled. In
fact system approaches and theories were developed by the single branches of science. These consider
the specific questions of the individual scientific field and contain the respective specialized terminology.
The system-theoretical-cybernetic approach proceeds from a “shortened” idea of man. Humans are
understood less as socio-emotional nature, which would like to carry out individual motives and
needs. Rather, humans are regarded as a self-regulating system, which compensates for environmental
changes by self changes. Despite this “shortened” idea of man, the view of humans is of importance
as automatic controller for the understanding of human data processing. Related to man–machine
systems a regulation-technical model can appear in such a way that humans function as an automatic
controller and the object, which can be regulated is represented by the machine. The goal value is
given from the “outside.” The task of the automatic control loop consists of adapting the output quantity
(actual value) as precisely as possible to the target value.
Organizations are open, dynamic, and goal-oriented systems, which consist of a social and a technical
subsystem. The social subsystem of an organization contains the employees with their knowledge and
abilities, as well as their individual and group-specific needs. The technical subsystem contains the
entirety of the technical and spatial conditions of work (Figure 5.2).
In order to avoid suboptimal results of system design, technical and social subsystems have to be opti-
mized together. The socio-technical system approach acts on the assumption that technology has a crucial
influence on the organization. However, the organization is not completely determined by the technology.
Also with a given technical system organizational options exist. This clearance of system design, obviously,
increases, if the technical system and the work organization are planned together. When planning, the needs
and requirements of the coworkers in reference to their work are to be considered.
The system “enterprise” is subject to fluctuations. These are caused, on the one hand, by the system
environment (e.g., changes of demand). On the other hand, system fluctuations also have internal
causes (e.g., disturbance at a machine; necessary reworking measures due to errors during the work
execution). A central thesis of the socio-technical system approach proves that enterprises with small
decentralized, self-regulating organizational units are more able to adapt to changes and fluctuations
than central-controlled systems.
With the design of self-regulating organizational units there are three principles to consider (Ulich,
2001): (1) The formation of organizational units, which are relatively independent from each other
should prevent the fluctuations and disturbances propagating uncontrollably. (2) An internal task coherent
of an organizational unit makes it possible for the coworkers to determine their operational procedure on
their own. This design principle stresses the motivational aspects of holistic tasks. (3) The design of the
organization should be product-orientated, if possible. Thus, the formation of independent organizational
units is supported. Furthermore, the work task shows a stronger connection with the product.
The socio-technical system approach raises the claim to provide a theoretical reference framework for
the analysis of practical problems in organizations. Sydow (1985) shows that the socio-technical system
approach only partly comes up to its claim. He justifies his animadversion on the approach among other
things with the fact that central constructs like the one of the technical and that of the social system, their
connection to the task system and to the feeling-orientated system are formulated imprecisely. Beyond
that he shows that, many a time, technology is accepted as fixed although the necessity for a common
optimization of the technical and social system is continually stressed.
The idea of man based on the socio-technical system approach refers particularly to the motive struc-
ture of humans (Sydow, 1985). The focus of attention is the effect of the work task on humans and their
motivation. The socio-technical system approach focuses on intrinsic motives of humans. It shows that
socio-technical system
FIGURE 5.2 Elements of the socio-technical system (in modification of Ulich, E., Arbeitspsychologie, 5th ed.,
Schäeffer-Poeschel, Stuttgart, 2001. With Permission).
5-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
humans are regarded in a social nature. Expectations and needs of humans are, therefore, to be con-
sidered with the design of work organization and technical system. The economic orientation of
humans, which manifests itself in their interest in a maximum payment is played down despite differing
project experiences of the Tavistock researchers (Kelly, 1978; Sydow, 1985).
Despite the aforementioned animadversion on the socio-technical system approach it is very import-
ant from an ergonomic view as the principle of optimizing the social and technical system together cor-
responds with ergonomic aims. Beyond that, it has to be emphasized that small, decentralized
organizational units, according to the socio-technical system approach, in particular correspond with
today’s requirements of high flexibility and short response and lead times in production.
Based on this biological point of view, Malik (1993) transferred the evolution-theoretical approach to
the management of enterprises. The main issue of importance for enterprises is their adaptability to
unforeseeable changes. This ability becomes particularly important in times of difficult economic situ-
ations, in order to increase the probability of survival. On the one hand, the goal is to avoid behavior,
which disturbs or endangers a system. On the other hand, the goal is to take over useful changes. The
evolution-theoretical approach was also transferred to the theory of social systems (Section 5.2.5).
Autopoiesis in the context of social systems means that they strive continuously to maintain their
sense (Krieger, 1996).
Critics of the evolution-theoretical approach provide examples, which this theory does not apply to
(Krieger, 1996). On the level of individuals, political or religious martyrs of all kinds show that for
humans the preservation of an idea (thus, a certain form of sense) can be more important than the pres-
ervation of the own life. The same applies to believers who do not touch animals and plants because of
religious taboo, although they are threatened by starvation.
Humans are to be considered primarily of a biological, rather, than of a socio-emotional nature in the
evolution-theoretical approach. Humans are part of the evolution and their task consists of the best poss-
ible adaptability to their environment. According to this theory, humans act because of self-preservation.
information
control system
raw
material/
semi-
finished
handling-of-
products measuring- tool-handling- tools
workpieces- “work system”
and-control- system
finished system
system
products
auxiliary-
waste disposal
supplies-
materials system auxiliary
system
supplies
power-supply-system
energy
material
energy
information
FIGURE 5.4 Functional structure of the manufacturing system (in modification of Warnecke, H.-J., Dubbel-
Taschenbuch für den Maschinenbau, 18th ed., Springer, Berlin, 1995. With permission.)
In the rationalization research, concepts of the cybernetics, synergetic, and complexity theory were
taken up (Luczak and Fricker, 1996). The situation of the rationalization is formed by increasing the
dynamics of changes, which makes shorter response times necessary and causes an increasing complex-
ity of operational settings of tasks. For the operational rationalization in principle the question is
derived whether the solution is to be searched in the decrease and avoidance of complexity or in
better methods of its handling and control (Warnecke and Hueser, 1995). Approaches, which aim at
excluding a decrease of complexity of operational structures are not regarded as sufficient, however,
for a successful complexity management since usually only short-term efficiency advantages are
achieved for the disadvantage of long-term stability (Bullinger et al., 1993). In terms of the complexity
accomplishment, on the one hand, the connection between operational structures and the system beha-
vior is regarded (Malik, 1992). The question arises how operational structures are to be arranged.
On the other hand, the behavior of the involved people during problem release processes is examined.
For the design of problem solution processes various realizations and experiences are present.
Regarding the modeling and design of order structures, however, few approaches are present. There-
fore, an instrument was developed and evaluated for the modeling and design of complex production
structures (Luczak and Fricker, 1996; Fricker, 1996). As degrees of complexity, variety (Ashby, 1956),
entropy (Shannon, 1976), and the effective complexity (Gell-Mann, 1994) are used. The developed instru-
ment enables the following:
. Representation and quantitative evaluation of interlaced operational coherences and order struc-
tures in the sense of monitoring the structural development
. Evaluation of the operational complexity, the partial equilibrium, and the adaptability to defined
and internal measuring points
. Identification of operational complexity drivers and of starting points for reorganization measures
. Quantification of order and organizational structures.
Humans in Work System Environment 5-9
decay continuously and are reproduced sequentially by the elements of the system itself. An autopoietic
system appears now as entirety contrary to the system-theoretical basic postulate of the necessary can-
didness of complex systems. These entireties are closed in their core region, their internal control
structure.
In the theory of social systems, humans are regarded as a system as well. On the one hand, this personal
system can be part of a superordinate system or part of a system environment. On the other hand, the
system “human” consists of subsystems like the genetic, organic, neuro-physiological, or mental systems.
The idea of man is characterized by a high degree of abstraction. An entire, human-orientated point of
view is not undertaken.
work environment
work piece
acting on
input output
feedback
working equipment /
task
operating resources
acting on
work
material feedback
result
information
energy working person material
information
energy
(changed)
job order
The differentiation between job order and work task provides a first reference point for the fact that the
system purpose is dependent on the point of view on the work system. From the systems environment
point of view, the main purpose of a work system consists of purposeful handling of job orders. Work
systems are regarded by their environment under purpose-rational and economic criteria. From the
point of view of a human who is working in the system, the purpose of the work system is the realization
of personal motives. On the one hand, his work accounts for the assurance of his subsistence via the
payment. On the other hand, the purpose of the system can be seen as an interesting and varied task.
5.3.1.2 Goals of Work System Design and the Idea of Man in Ergonomics
Basic goal of work system design is the optimization of the entire work system. In the sense of the main
objective of ergonomics work systems are to be suitably arranged for both human and economical pur-
poses. Thereby, economy is determined by the best possible proportion of inputs (e.g., raw materials,
energy) to outputs (e.g., finished products, service). Human abilities and technical conditions are to
be adjusted, in order to arrange work systems to be suitable for humans. Ergonomics is based on the
assumption that rationalization and humanization goals work complementary to each other. On the
one hand, humane conditions of work lead to effectiveness (in the sense of result reaching) and efficiency
(in the sense of small resources inputs). On the other hand, effectiveness and efficiency form an import-
ant basis, in order to be able to create humane conditions of work. The consideration of the “resources
coworker,” the “human factors,” has become more important. A one-sided pursuit of one or another goal
clearly leads to suboptimal results.
Ergonomics considers the technology, the organization, and the personnel in the context of human
work. In doing so, ergonomics distinguishes itself from predominantly human being-referred disciplines
by including technical aspects into the working process. On the other hand, a differentiation takes place
5-12 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
towards economic-technical disciplines, which exclusively aims at the optimization of the work result.
Therefore, the requirement of ergonomics must proceed from a human conception, which regards
humans, on the one hand, as a performance generator in work systems and, on the other hand, as a
complex nature whose needs are to be considered during the work system design. Complexity results
from the various, inter-individual performance rates and motives of humans, which can change depend-
ing on the situation and the course of time.
The characteristics of a person who has the ability to perform is influenced by four categories. These are
the constitution-, disposition-, qualification-, and adaptation characteristics (Figure 5.6). Constitution
characteristics are unchangeable ratios in the life cycle of humans. Among these characteristics are
gender, somatotype, and ethnical origin. Disposition characteristics are variable, however, the working
person has no direct influence on this. Disposition characteristics cover the age, body weight, the state
of health, and the intelligence of a working person. Beyond that, the biorhythm belongs to the disposition
characteristics. This means that the human efficiency in the course of a day is subject to fluctuations. The
entirety of all knowledge, skills, and experiences of a working person are understood as qualification
characteristics, which a working person must have for carrying out activities at the workplace. Charac-
teristics, which affect the human ability and are changeable at short notice are called adaptation charac-
teristics. Adaptation characteristics refer to the fatigue and recovery, as well as the strain of a working
person. With the beginning of an activity numerous physical and psychological conversion procedures
start, whereby humans adapt increasingly to the workload caused by the activity. Manual labor, for
example, leads to a rise of the heart rate and the muscle blood circulation, in order to be able to meet
the demanded achievement. The realization became generally accepted that an optimal achievement of
working humans is only ensured in the long term, if the fatigue caused by the work stays within limits.
Most of the determinants mentioned for the ability are mutually dependent on each other, for
example, somatotype and body weight, age and experience, skills and strain. Differences between
people or changes within a person, however, do not have to lead inevitably to differences in the achieve-
ment. Measurable effects on the achievement cannot be determined due to the existence of mutual com-
pensation mechanisms.
Constitution characteristics are solely influenced by the selection of personnel. Disposition character-
istics can also be affected by other measures. Specifications of some characteristics can be changed within
a limited range also through measures of work system design. For example, realizations of the influence
of biorhythm on the human ability effect the shift work models. Qualification characteristics can be
FIGURE 5.6 Individual determinants of human performance (From Luczak, H., 1989, Wesen Menschlicher
Leistung. In: Institut für angewandte. Arbeitswissenschaft (ed.): Arbeitsgestaltung in Produktion und Verwaltung.
Cologne: Bachem, pp. 39 – 60, With Permission).
5-14 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
determined by both, a purposeful selection of personnel and by measures of the work system design and
personnel development. For example, the introduction of a continuous improvement process (CIP) can
lead to the fact that employees exchange knowledge and experiences during operational problem solving,
causing the qualification level within the CIP-team to increase. Adaptation characteristics are not or only
indirectly affected in their developments by a selection of personnel. Measures of the work system design
can exert influence on variables at short notice such as strain and fatigue. For example, organizational
rules of recovery periods can help to avoid an excessive demand and a possible harm to working persons.
The willingness to perform, the work motivation of a person, results from the reciprocal effects
between personal motives of the working person and the motivation potential of the work. Neither
the motives nor the motivation potential of the work alone are sufficient, in order to explain the devel-
opment of work motivation. The motivation potential of work results from the expectations of employees
to be able to achieve their motives. For example, motivation potentials regarding achievement, power, and
integration can be differentiated from each other, since motivation potential always refers to specific
motives (Kleinbeck, 1996). A work situation with a high achievement-related motivation potential is, for
example, characterized by establishing a range of decision and activity for employees in which a complete
and in their view important task with adequate difficulty can be worked on. Motivation potential regarding
power always contains work activities, whenever the work activity is implemented by several employees
simultaneously and the task makes it necessary that a person (e.g., guidance person) affects other people
in the sense of achieving a certain goal. High motivation potential regarding integration exhibit
work activities, which give many opportunities to the working person to achieve social contacts and
maintain them. If motivation potentials are highly developed, these do not lead inevitably to a high
motivation. Motivation potentials are regarded, therefore, as a condition of work motivation. For
example, if a work system is spatially arranged in such a way that it permits interactions between
working persons, this leads to a high motivation for the employees with a strong integration motive.
For people with low integration motive, however, the possibility of exchanging itself with colleagues
does not lead to increased motivation.
Thus, coworkers pursue goals whose reaching or reaching degree has effects on their payment substan-
tially more consistently than goals, which are not coupled to their payment. Thus, the probability of
achieving the objectives rises. Feedbacks, which give information to the employees about the goal reach-
ing degree during the work process positively influence the effects of goal setting on performance. Due to
the knowledge about existing discrepancies between the goal and the current performance level, the
employee can increase action intensity and duration and, thus, eliminate these discrepancies (Locke
and Latham, 1990).
The valid rules of the work process (instructions about operations) are also called work methods. A
work method usually applies to all people who are active in a work system. By work manner the individ-
ual execution of the work method is understood (REFA, 1993). If the work method is given and the dis-
persion of the individual work manner is small, then a high method level is referred to. The method level
is affected, on the one hand, by the level of organization of a work system and, on the other hand, by the
abilities and skills of the employees. These two influencing variables depend again on the repeating fre-
quency of job orders. A high method level — usual in line and mass production — offers only small
interpretation clearance to the employee concerning the redefinition of the job order. Beyond that,
the factors including the comprehensibility and acceptance of the job order, typical expectations and
values of the working person, and existing experiences with considerably similar orders affect the rede-
finition process (Hackman, 1969).
Work tasks can be differentiated into primary and secondary tasks. “Each system or sub-system has,
however, at any given time, one task which may be defined as its primary task — the task which it is
created to perform” (Rice, 1958). Value generating activities are a direct result of a primary task. They
create value for the internal and external customers. The processing of secondary tasks is not directly
valuable, however, it forms a condition for fulfilling the primary task and achieving the goals of a
work system (e.g., annual cost reduction around 3%). Secondary tasks can be separated into tasks of
system regularization, preservation, and optimization (Miller and Rice, 1967; Antoni, 1996). The tasks
of system regularization include, for example, the job order planning and production control. Typical
tasks of system preservation are preventive maintenance and repair related to the technical subsystem
of a work system. With reference to the social subsystem, consisting of the employees, for example, train-
ing measures can be added to the tasks of system preservation. Among tasks of system optimization is, for
example, the implementing of the CIP in a work system. The detailed fixing of the boundaries cannot be
made solely by formal rules set by the enterprise, as the work system, on the one hand, is subject to
changes in the time response (e.g., coworker turnover) and the environment of the work system, on
the other hand, changes its demands (e.g., due to changed customer’s requests) on the system. According
to the theory of social systems (Section 5.2.5), the fixing of the boundaries rather takes place by the work
system itself. Creating boundaries between system and environment is an achievement, which generates
the system partially itself. Even if processes creating the boundaries do not take place consciously in every
case, the creating of the boundaries can be interpreted as another secondary task. The subject of this task
clarifies whether certain activities are to be implemented in the work system or its environment (e.g., a
work system, which is preliminary in the process). Furthermore, the work system and its environment
(e.g., other work systems, supervisor) have to clarify how certain tasks have to be fulfilled. This clarifying
process, thereby, primarily refers to those activities, which were not specified clearly by organizational
rules or the work method.
The type and range of the primary and secondary tasks determine the range for decision and activity
of employees in a work system. Range for decision and activity makes an individual and collective
self-regularization possible and forms a condition for the increase in work motivation of the employees.
takes up, for example, an advisory service. The substantial output of the work system can be, in this
case, immaterial and exist in additional knowledge, which the customer can acquire by the usage of
the advisory service.
FUNCTION
CATEGORY TRANSFORMATION TRANSPORT STORAGE
materials handling
material
and conveying
manufacturing
technique
energy storage
technique
energy
information processing
information
technique
metrology technique technique
control and feedback
control technique
FIGURE 5.7 Classification of technical systems (in modification of Ropohl, G., Eine Systemtheorie der Technik — zur
Grundlegung der allgemeinen Technologie. Hanser, München, 1979. With Permission).
Humans in Work System Environment 5-17
primary function and their substantial output, but apart from their characteristic technique most of the
other techniques are also used in subordinated functions. A manufacturing system, for example, apart
from its main technical function covers handling- and storage-technical subfunctions, as well as
energy- and information-technical subfunctions (Section 5.2.4).
Systematics of technical systems — like Ropohl’s scheme — can serve as a framework for the design of
the technical subsystem of a work system. Further technical systematics can be allocated to the individual
fields, referring to Ropohl’s scheme. For example, in reference to the manufacturing technique (field
“material/transformation” in the scheme) different manufacturing processes (referring to DIN 8580)
can be distinguished (Figure 5.8). These individual manufacturing processes can in turn be differentiated
further. Thus, cutting processes are divided into cutting with geometrically welldefined tool edges and
cutting with nondefined tool edges. The first-mentioned category consists, for example, of turning,
milling, roaching, drilling, and boring. These individual processes can in turn be classified according
to different criteria.
2. metal
3. cutting 4. joining
forming
1. primary
5. coating
shaping
varying influence
determination of
work system purpose technological technical organizational ergonomic alternative concepts
and work object design design design design of work system design
FIGURE 5.9 System ergonomic procedure according to the principle of sequential designing.
In this context, technology should be understood as a describing level of technique. Technology deals
with the systematics and analysis of concrete techniques. Technologies in this sense are, for example,
manufacturing processes (Section 5.2.4 and Section 5.3.2). For instance, the manufacturing process
“turning” (technology) is to be carried out with a corresponding technique (turning machine, control
program, etc.).
manual
human human human
fulfillment
mechanized
fulfillment technique human human
automated
technique technique human
fulfillment
FIGURE 5.10 Levels of functional division between human and technique in a work system.
system. The working person solely has a monitoring function. The individual levels of mechanization
and automation can be distinguished further (Kirchner, 1972).
The concept of supervisory control supplies basic design indications, referring to the degree of
mechanization and automation. “The term supervisory control derives from the close analogy
between a supervisor’s interaction with subordinate people in a human organization and a person’s inter-
action with intelligent automated subsystems” (Sheridan, 2002). A supervisor gives instructions to the
subordinates who have to understand and transform them into detailed actions. The subordinates,
again, present their results to the supervisor. The supervisor has to compare his goals — in analogy
to the cybernetic system approach (Section 5.2.1) — with the results presented by the subordinates.
Then he has to decide on the further action to be taken. The same sort of interaction occurs between
a human supervisor and the automation (Sheridan, 1992). Thus, the automation can be compared to
a subordinate, but with less intelligence. Five roles can be cited for the human supervisor: he has to
plan, to teach, to monitor, to intervene, and to learn.
To find out whether it is better to carry out a task manually or supervisory, Figure 5.11 can be used.
The dashed line in the figure represents the time required for direct manual control. It is assumed that the
more complex a task is, the more time it takes to carry out. The thin curve in the figure shows how much
time someone needs to plan and teach a task. If the task execution time is required, a slice between the
thin curve and the heavy solid curve is needed. The sum of these two values is shown by the heavy solid
curve. The heavy solid curve intersects the dashed line in two places. On the one hand, the left end of the
scale demonstrates that it is better to do simple tasks on your own, because this is much quicker than to
explain it to a computer or another person. On the other hand, the right end represents that very complex
tasks are too hard to figure out how to program and so it is faster to do them manually. If a task is to be
repeated many times and the environmental conditions do not change, it is better to program it. In this
case this automation (and supervisory control) is the fastest way.
manual better-
manual better supervisory better -example: In manufacturing automation is
example:
-example: more economical than doing it by hand (large-batch operation).
It seems impossible
completion It takes less time to teach a computer
time to write a few to takecare of
words by hand children, or write
than to type it in a symphonies.
writing program
on a computer.
direct manual
control
execution
supervisory time sum of
control planning and
teaching time
and execution
time
planning and
teaching time
computer execution
speed compensates the measure of task complexity
time for planning and the task is too complex
teaching overhead to be programmed
manual better supervisory better manual better
FIGURE 5.11 Range in which supervisory control outperforms manual control (in modification of Sheridan, T.B.,
Humans and Automation — System Design and Research Issues, John Wiley & Sons, Santa Monica, 2002. With
Permission).
system approach (Section 5.2.2) are to be considered with the task design. Moreover, regarding the
implementation of group work, important organizational design fundamentals are described in
Section 5.5.6.
organizational system
user computer
user model of
task conceptual design
application
dialogue
dialogue methods syntactic design
manager
display and
execution lexical design input /output
manager
FIGURE 5.12 Model of user – computer interface (in modification of Foley, J.D., and Van Dam, A., Fundamentals of
Interactive Computer of graphics, Addison Wesley, Menlo Park, CA, 1982. With Permission).
4. In the last step (lexical design), it is determined how these input and output tokens are formed
from the available hardware components. “We see, then, that lexical design represents the
binding of hardware capabilities to the hardware — independent tokens of the input and output
languages.”
General strategies of the work system design, apart from this systematic, sequential proceeding, can be
differentiated further and are described in the following.
of the realized work system, however, is still necessary; the correctness of the obtained assumptions
and forecasts must be verified with consideration of the individual characteristics of the working
persons (see also for this stress-strain concepts in Luczak and Rohmert, 1997).
individual human) requirements. Both strategies are faced with limitations in technical and economic
basic conditions, as long as they are represented in a true form.
broadest context
society
S7 and
social and
cooperation
labor politics
between companies V6
work related
political action
medium context
company
S6 and company wide
organizational
structure of the
measurement
factory V5
interaction of the
working actors
narrowest context
work group
S5 and
cooperative
structure of the
processes
work group V4
cooperative
group work
subject system
qualification,
S4 motivation and
work content,
activity system of
V3 types of work
a person
motive related
activity
functional means of a person
S3 tasks and
workplaces
purpose oriented subsystems
(tasks) V2
goal oriented
consciously
upper level of physical means regulated action
willfully steered
S2 organic systems
and tools /
productive subsystems
V1 work means
(sensomotorics)
sensomotoric
automatism
lower level of physical means (operations) autonomous
S1 organic systems
and
reproductive subsystems
work environment
of the body
FIGURE 5.13 Structural and procedural levels of working processes and assigned aspects for the modeling of
humans and work.
The focus of level S3 is on human tasks embedded in motive-related execution procedures in combi-
nation with systems analysis of workplaces. The concept of systems analysis and task analysis leads to a
variety of models — the goals operations methods selection rules (GOMS) model for human–computer
interaction, the work-factor-mento approach to visual inspection and, last but not least, the different
systems of predetermined times for manual assembly. The analytical and constructive “power” of
these models leads to a broad application in industry with millions of workplaces designed and operated
according to an optimization of time consumption in highly repetitive tasks.
5-26 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
The central level, level S4, concerns the human being as an individual working person: the typical
approach on this level is a holistic view of human work as an entity of motivational, qualification-
based, and social-interactive elements, which together result in forms or types of work with characteristic
sets of work content, work demands, and work-induced stressors. The models to anticipate design results
on this level are multifarious according to the specific anthropological perspective of the human: man as
a personality regulating his actions in an environmental context of self-set or accepted tasks in social
interaction with others or man as a person, underlying, withstanding, and influencing his workload
(by a set of stressors) according to his abilities to cope with them in stress-strain concepts. Limitations
of use and abuse of human resources are frequently based upon those concepts, and, thus, standards of
acceptable and endurable working conditions can be designed anticipatorily for a person or a group of
persons after stressor analysis.
Level S5 brings into focus the working groups and group work, which means the cooperation of indi-
vidual working persons with their functions in the network of interactions to other persons that is deter-
mined by division of labor, hierarchy, behavioral traits, participation in decision processes, as well as
questions of communication and information transfer with respect to human relations. Mostly
models of function allocation and simulation models for crew design and crew operations in specific
goal-oriented and task-oriented settings are used to find out the effects of independent variables on
crew performance, crew workload, crew behavior, etc. before implementing a specific design
configuration.
Level S6 describes company organization with special reference to the personnel and to the industrial
relations of employers’ and employees’ representatives. Economic and social aspects in the design of
company structure and functions, related strongly to job design and evaluation, are combined in indus-
trial engineering or operations research models for the preparation of decisions about a management of
human resources. Cost-benefit analysis of design solutions and economical optimization criteria follow-
ing cost-structure models, quantitative production output models or, recent quality-oriented modeling
of production behavior are the tools for the prognostic or anticipatory approach. Whereas level S5
implies clearly a human-oriented approach by the macroergonomic perspective, level S6 derives
design intentions and goals for level S5 or sets limitations for design efforts on this level.
Level S7 is oriented to comprehensive socio-political and societal contexts of work. Typical questions
on this level deal with work regulation and standardization, work in the national economy, structural and
economical components of employment, the labor market, activities of employers, and unions in socio-
politics and cooperations between companies. Econometric models and growth models for national/
international economies try to anticipate the effects of political decisions in complex situations,
obviously with limited success regarding the world-wide problem of unemployment in many national
economies.
The application of the seven-level concept of work sciences to the problem of “anticipation” shows that
the microergonomic models for predictive and prescriptive design reach from level S1 to level S3, whereas
macromodels of work sciences can be assigned to levels S5 to S7. An intermediate position in between
macro and micro, named meso, can be given to level S4 where the working person is seen in a reacting
position to environmental conditions caused by work, as well as in an acting position in coping with the
work environment and, thus, influencing work itself by individual and collective efforts.
effect of certain environmental variables on humans) is normally inalienable in research projects. Thus,
the individual elements of work system approach are regarded as systems in each case and — if necessary
— are to be divided in subsystems. Different aspects of individual elements of a work system are the
center of consideration at every level of ordering model. The system border is displaced in each case
with the change from one level to another (Figure 5.14).
On level S1, the human is divided into physical “subsystems,” for example, the cardiovascular system,
muscles and sinews, etc. The physiological systems of people and their interaction concerning the
broadest context
society
S7 societal and
systems social and
cooperation
labor politics
between companies
medium context
company
S6 company and company wide
systems organizational
structure of the
measurement
factory
narrowest context
work group
S5 group and
systems cooperative
structure of the
processes
work group
subject system
qualification,
S4 job motivation and
systems work content,
activity system of
types of work
a person
FIGURE 5.14 Inter-relations between ordering model and work system approach.
5-28 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
dependence on the work environment and the work task take center stage in these considerations. On level
S2 elementary physical and psychological functions are examined regarding the work person — mostly in
cooperation with working equipment. From these investigations, design recommendations for the tech-
nical system are derived (e.g., references to the design of displays and manual controls). The central
focus of work systems exists, on the one hand, concerning the subsystems of humans (e.g., muscles
and skeletal) and, on the other hand, concerning the technical system (e.g., workplace design). A connec-
tion between human and technical systems is produced by the individual operations of the people.
Therefore, at level S2 a work system is considered through the perspective of an “operator,” so that at
this level it can be spoken from an “operation system.” It is typical for this level of the ordering model
that only subsystems of the work system element “task,” namely operations and activities, are regarded.
At level S3 the work system is not considered from the perspective of operators or individual operations.
The system border shifts in such a way that individual subtasks or activities are no longer regarded in
relation to the work system element “task,” however, one or more total tasks are. Therefore, at this level
“task systems” should be taken into consideration. Questions concerning action regulation in connection
with task systems are examined in relation to humans. With this, the center of attention is the humane
design of the task system. Beyond that, questions of functional and temporal cooperation of humans
and technique are discussed. Primarily, the goal of the utilization of human work is pursued (Section
5.4.1). Jobs are considered at level S4. These develop through the combination of many tasks. The job
description usually takes place independent of the individual working person. From these descriptions
the individual tasks and their cooperation in the “job system” can be derived to side the requirements
of the working person and a quantified job evaluation. Requirement determinations serve, therefore, as
a basis for a choice of personnel, a personnel development and for the design of remuneration. The
center of attention is on the adjustment of humans to the work. On the other hand, it is the design of
“job system” that affects the personality and motivation promoting of the working person. The central
focus of considerations is the adjustment of humans to the “job system.” At level S5 of the model, a
work system is considered in the perspective of “group systems.” Level S5 is not similar to level S4 in
terms of just one working person or only one job is considered rather a majority of people, that is,
jobs. Groups are characterized by the fact that they consist of several coworkers who work together
over a longer period with direct interaction. Members of a “group system” fulfill a whole task together,
but to a certain degree role-differentiated. Apart from the execution of this primary group task, they
also take over secondary tasks (Section 5.3.2). At level S6 such system components of the complex
“company system” are examined, which concern the human work. The system borderline shifts in such
a way so that in reference to the working person all people or a large part of the staff are also included
into the views of the system. Ergonomical questions of the design of the “company system” refer for
example to operational work time regulations, the control of job execution processes, and the organiz-
ation design. If the system is regarded as societal, the system borders of an enterprise and its staff shift all
enterprises of a society or a majority of enterprises.
In the following, models and methods — to each level of the scheme — are presented exemplarily.
These models and methods are able to support a systematical proceeding with the anticipating and
prospective work system design. These approaches and methods are the result of research projects,
which were accomplished by the IAW and FIR at the RWTH Aachen. The exemplarily cited research
results are supposed to show that the use of the work system approach is possible and reasonable on
different levels of ergonomic investigation. Beyond that, the exemplarily represented models and methods
make clear how the system limitations of one level shift ordering models to the next.
According to the work system approach, different influences of work environment affect a working
person (Figure 5.15). These influences hardly ever appear isolated, but mostly in combination
(Section 5.3.2). Concerning heat work, for example, unfavorable climatic conditions take effect in
combination with hard dynamic muscle work (Luczak, 1979). In doing so, the climate conditions are
allocated to environmental influences of a work system, whereas, muscle work is considered as an
action resulting from the work task.
The subjects of investigation concerning heat work are the questions, which “human subsystems” are
especially loaded, how these loads interact, and when “bottlenecks” arise in the human body. For
example, the cardiovascular and the muscle system cooperate, because the amplitude and the frequency
of respiration depend on the muscular load. In the same way, the cardiovascular and the metabolic
system depend on each other due to the perspiration under heat work conditions — that is, an increasing
loss of water and salt — these materials urgently have to be taken in. Therefore, interferences caused by
salt deficiency (e.g., heat convulsions, which effect the cardiovascular system and, therefore, also the
muscle system) can be anticipated.
At the time of publication of the following described study (Luczak, 1979), numerous scientific realiz-
ations were present concerning “pure” muscle work and especially the endurance limit and the recovery
time. However, combinations with environmental influences were examined only sporadically, referring
to a few points of the entire load continuum.
A goal of the investigation was the obtaining of physiologically justified realizations for the determi-
nation of recovery time for heat work. These realizations should cover a wide range of relevant load con-
tinuums. In addition recovery times should be assigned to measurable load values.
The general objective of the investigation is based on the following partial goals:
. Development of a model for the coupling of the thermal regularization system and the cardio-
vascular system
. Examination of the model on the basis of individual results from the literature
. Determination of recovery times and the superposition principles of energetic effects and climatic
loads in the fast-time-simulation based on the model
metabolic system
cardiac circulatory (O2 and nutrition converting)
system
(transport system
and heat balance)
environmental influences
loads resulting from
e.g.
spine
(carrying of loads climate,
and parts of the body)
substances
. Experimental evaluation of the model results concerning the recovery times at selected test
conditions
In the following, physiological bases of the thermal regularization and selected results of the study are
presented.
5.5.3.1 Physiological Bases of the Thermal Regulation
The core body temperature is kept constant within a wide range independent of outside climatic con-
ditions. The desired value of the core body temperature is 378C. High and low outside temperatures,
as well as manual labor lead to measurable deviations in the target value. The temperatures are measured
by central and peripheral thermoreceptive cells.
Smaller climatic loads, warming and cooling stimuli, can be compensated by regulation of the heat
transfer resistance between core areas and body surface. Larger thermal loads are compensated by
increased perspiration, whereby a multiplicity of theoretical and experimental results regarding the
relationship between different, locally distinguishable temperatures and the perspiration level are
indicated.
The core body temperature rises during combined muscular and thermal load. The temperature can
reach a steady degree, as long as heat production and heat dissipation are balanced. The circulatory
system is a limiting factor of the heat dissipation and in supplying the musculature with high-energy
substances. A rise of the body core temperature is usually accompanied with a rise of the heart frequency.
The heat dissipation becomes insufficient, if an increase in blood circulation of the skin is no longer
possible. Other reasons for the insufficient heat dissipation are loss of water and a conversion of thermal
balance (environmental temperature is more than core body temperature) under the conditions of increasing
duration of work load and very high outside temperatures.
5.5.3.2 Model for the Description of the Thermal Regularization System and the
Cardiovascular System
Regarding the determination of the recovery times and because of the interconnection of thermo-regu-
latory and cardio-respiratory indicators it appears necessary to consider these both, several multiple con-
necting physiological systems. The model describes the regulation and rhythm of the momentary heart
frequency, the respiration, the blood pressure, the core temperature and the skin temperature, that is,
those physiological functions, which are predominantly affected by a change in heat work and interfere
in a quantitatively describable way.
In terms of this model different approaches described in the literature were examined. In principle all
the available approaches at the time were similar so that easiest correlation indicated by Behling (1971)
has been transferred into the model. Thereby, a correction factor (Scarperi et al., 1972) was considered.
The factor contains the loss of water in the organism in a long-term test. Behling adopts two blocks, body
core and body surface. A defined heat exchange takes place between these blocks. Perspiration rate,
oxygen intake, and heat development are determined by a weighted sum of core and skin temperatures.
The model was compared and examined with the measured values from physiological experiments
regarding its reactions to thermal and energetic-effective load both for the steady state and for the
courses of the most important model parameters.
5.5.3.3 Test Run to Determine the Recovery Time Diagrams
With the successful examination of the model, the requirements were given to superimpose the load
factors “hard dynamic muscle work” and “thermal environment” for defined work durations and sub-
sequently to pursue the recovery time processes. The climatic conditions of the test runs were varied
effectively within the range of 0– 358C, since the model is appropriate for a temperature range, which
does not include the conversion of thermal balance with the rise in the human body temperature by
external thermal influences.
The energetic load was varied upto 150 W, whereby the fictitious endurance level of the model was
focused at 100 W. A hard dynamic muscle work with high efficiency is presupposed, since the model
Humans in Work System Environment 5-31
equations based on the investigations were accomplished with such work forms. The fictitious endurance
level of 100 W corresponds to the continuous endurance level of a young, healthy, male employee. It is
realistic, because only suitable employees are involved in heat work because of personal selection and
adaptation processes. The values for the work duration were applied in five geometrical stages: 15,
30 min, 1, 2, and 4 h. These values appear meaningful regarding their delimitations, since a continuous
work duration under 15 min represents a special case in operational practice, and a work duration over
4 h may not be accepted due to the laws of working time regulation.
5.5.3.5 Conclusion
Both model results and experimental results were recorded in diagrams. The diagrams indicate that the
recovery time increases exponentially starting from a certain energetic load value and from a certain
effective temperature. The influence of the effective temperature begins with a certain temperature
level. Until this level the thermal regularization system of the human body is able to regulate the body
core temperature independent from the work duration due to a reconciliation of heat production and
heat dissipation.
It is evident in all test results that the four factors “effective temperature, muscular load, work dur-
ation, and individual characteristics of the working person” determine the characteristic curves. The
influence of the effective temperature begins, thereby, from a threshold value. This value is again sup-
posed to be dependent on the energetic-effective load, that is, an endurance level for the superposition
of these two types of loads. Beyond this point, the slope of the course of the curve is determined by the
energetic-effective load. The recovery time increases to the power of the energetic load value and the
effective temperature.
If the model results are compared with the experimental results, then it shows up that the tendency of
the courses of the curves exhibits large agreements (Figure 5.16).
100 experiment
model
50
75 W
50 W
25 W
Vp: Kl
Vp: Gu
tarb= 0,5 h
Wdyn= 50 W Vp: So
20 25 30 35
Teff °C
FIGURE 5.16 Comparison between experimental results and model/test results concerning the recovery time.
difficulty of a movement becomes clear in the observation of the first clumsy trial of playing tennis, ice-
skating, or chipping. This difficulty cannot be handled until the brain is able to store the movement
programs in a way as subprograms. This means that only the order to retrieve the subprogram has
to become conscious and, thus, engage the upper nerve center, after a movement is once available
as a subprogram.
One of this automated work takes place with very little mental load. One possibility to judge the degree
of automation of an activity is to examine the performance proportion, which is possible to carry out
besides the automated activity. This idea is based on the model of a single-channeled information pro-
cessing with defined maximal channel capacity. According to this, information processing degrees orig-
inating from different activities can be summarized additively to the maximal channel capacity.
The absence of the consciousness in automated work makes a wide division of labor with its continu-
ous repetitions bearable, since the “disencumbered” consciousness is able to deal with other things
but work.
Methods originating from information theory and control theory are the appropriate measures, if
one tries to quantify the entirety of information processing with the movement coordination instead
of trying to fractionate only the conscious part. In the majority of cases it is only possible to describe
manual aim movements with the help of information-theoretical and control-theoretical models.
These manual aim movements are interspersed to a certain extent between the individual “stationary”
movement elements, for example, grabbing, assembling, or joining. Regarding the entirety of the
motor activity, the signaling can be analyzed with the help of control technical systems by the use of
actuators handling elements, that is, the motor information transfer in general, which is also called
signal-motor work. Thereby, predications concerning work design can result.
must be taken into account (Luczak and Schlick, 2000, 2001). First, it is required to model human infor-
mation processing in complex task settings. That means the model must be able to represent input–
output transformations, which significantly extend Sternberg’s (1969) classical stimulus-response
approaches. Therefore, the theoretical construct of a “mental model” must be considered, because
tasks of production planning and diagnosis often require an appropriate reasoning space for the oper-
ator. Second, it is required to represent concurrent threads of reasoning and, therefore, cope with
aspects of mental resource allocation. Therefore, a symbiosis of stage- and resource-oriented models
of human information processing is preferred (Kahnemann, 1973). Third, there is a need to model differ-
ent time bases of human information processing, because some tasks, for example, process control, need
synchronous information processing, while other tasks, for example, production planning, are comple-
tely self-paced. Fourth, especially synchronous human information processing involves different sensory
modalities, which must be considered. Fifth, the autonomy of the manufacturing system requires to
model aspects of human learning and adaptation. Sixth, human errors play an important role for
performance prediction and evaluation of system reliability should be taken into account. Seventh, a
strict modeling formalism is required to anticipate time consumption of relevant cognitive functions.
Eighth, it is required to represent cognitive stress and strain, because equal performance levels may be
assessed differently with regard to “mental costs.” Ninth, it is required to integrate formal aspects of
human communication in terms of semiotic modeling, so that different levels of information exchange
between human and machine can be differentiated (physical, syntactical, semantical, pragmatical).
In addition to these application-driven requirements, two utility-driven requirements must be taken
into account: first, it is useful to have software tools for supporting cognitive analysis, modeling, and
evaluation. Second, widespread models are preferred, because they ease data collection and comparative
assessments.
TABLE 5.1 Evaluation of Cognitive Models (Cells of Matrix Refer to Requirement Fulfillment Level, the Goodness-
of-Fit is the Weighted Sum of Fulfillment Levels)
Model/Requirement COSIMO GOMS SRK UCT ACT-R Priority
Complex tasks 2 2 1 2 3 1
Resource 3 2 3 1 1 2
Different time bases 1 2 1 1 3 1
Multiple modalities 3 1 3 2 3 1
Learning 2 2 1 1 1 2
Human error 2 3 1 3 3 1
Time consumption 1 1 2 1 1 1
Stress/Strain 3 3 1 3 1 3
Semiotics 3 3 1 3 3 1
Software support 2 1 3 2 2 3
Spreading 2 1 2 2 3 2
Goodness-of-fit 0.53 0.61 0.73 0.62 0.30
second step, the corresponding weights of the requirements were defined with the help of an exponential
priority scale (first priority has twice the weight of second priority, second priority has twice the weight of
third priority). The goodness-of-fit for each model was calculated as the sum of the weighted fulfillment
levels (Table 5.1).
With regard to human information processing in complex task situations, the SRK approach rep-
resents the construct of mental model in terms of a detailed means-ends abstraction hierarchy (Rasmussen,
1985) and, therefore, fully fulfills the first requirement. However, COSIMO, UCT, and GOMS define
different levels of cognitive abstraction and, therefore, partially fulfill this requirement. ACT-R uses the
action-rules monolithically and, therefore, does not fulfill this requirement. Concerning the allocation of
mental resources, the UCT, as well as ACT-R provide features to activate and schedule cognitive rules in
parallel and, therefore, cope well with limited cognitive resources. Also GOMS-extensions, for example,
Gray et al. (1993), can model parallel execution threads, but only with a restricted horizon of resource
allocation. The other models offer limited features only and, therefore, do not fulfill this requirement.
Multiple time bases concerning task coupling are represented well in SRK, COSIMO, and UCT, which
rely on a multi-layered architecture of cognition. GOMS has restricted abilities when considering differ-
ent partial models (like unit task and keystroke level). Multiple sensory modalities can be modeled with
GOMS appropriately. UCT differentiates between sensory input and output channels, but is lacking
specific operators. The other models do not fulfill this requirement. Learning processes are represented
well with SRK, ACT-R, and UCT. The SRK-framework interprets human learning as a shift towards lower
levels of cognitive control while ACT-R and UCT include chunking mechanisms. The other approaches
only partially fulfill this requirement.
Aspects of human error and reliability are represented well with extensions of SRK such as Reason
(1987) and Hannaman et al. (1985). COSIMO also copes with human reliability, but is restricted to
error detection and recovery. The other models are lacking sufficient mechanisms for human error mod-
eling. COSIMO, UCT, GOMS, and ACT-R provide strict formalisms for modeling of mental time con-
sumption and, therefore, fulfill this requirement completely. SRK offers limited functions in terms of
rules of thumb. Aspects of mental stress and strain are a fundamental part of ACT-R, which controls
rational behavior. Therefore, this model fulfills this requirement completely. Moreover, the work of
Moray et al. (1988a, b) must be taken into account, which enriches SRK with workload modeling. The
other models are lacking such evaluation mechanisms and do not fulfill this requirement. Semiotic
aspects of human–machine communication are represented well in SRK, which differentiates among
signals, signs, and symbols for information exchange. The other models do not integrate semiotic aspects
and, therefore, do not fulfill this requirement.
Concerning the first utility-driven requirement, the GOMS approach offers a variety of software tools
for analysis, modeling and evaluation and, therefore, fulfills this requirement completely. However,
Humans in Work System Environment 5-37
COSIMO, UCT, and ACT-R also offer specialized software tools for model simulation and, therefore,
fulfill this requirement partially. Finally, GOMS is the most widespread model of human information
processing and is a “de facto” standard for the investigation of human–computer interaction
(Gugerty, 1993) and fulfills this requirement completely. COSIMO, SRK, and UCT are well known in
their corresponding scientific communities and partially fulfill this requirement. ACT-R is a highly
specialized approach.
In conclusion, Rasmussen’s (1983, 1986) model of skills, rules, knowledge, and the abstraction hierar-
chy for knowledge representation (Rasmussen, 1985) in conjunction with additional work on human
reliability modeling (Reason, 1987; Hannaman, 1985), as well as workload assessment (Moray et al.,
1988a, b) represent the framework for cognitive modeling with the highest overall goodness-of-fit in
the application domain of flexible manufacturing. Therefore, this framework was used extensively to
investigate operator requirements for human –machine interfaces of APCs (Schlick et al., 1995), to
design the human–machine interface on a conceptual (Schlick et al., 1996), as well as a detailed level
(Schlick et al., 1997), and to simulate and assess the production system as a whole, which includes
aspects of human error and labor division (Schlick, 1999).
level and refer to general traits in terms of abilities. Process elements allow a more detailed description of
task execution (concrete operations, tools, etc.) and are directly linked with knowledge and skills. Taking
into account the specific nature of process elements, the corresponding library is open and can be modi-
fied by users to their needs. A detailed description of the modeling concept and its advantages for the use
in a CE environment can be found in Stahl (1998) and Stahl et al. (2000). The KSA model and the way in
which job descriptors and KSAs are linked are presented in Stahl and Luczak (2000). Furthermore, a
reference catalog gives access to predefined tasks, relevant for manufacturing processes. Figure 5.18
shows an extract of the graphical user interface (GUI) of Spaceþ and contains an example to clarify
the modeling syntax and the hierarchical structure of job models.
The main goal of Spaceþ is to assist the user-designing jobs, which offer motivation potentialities and
opportunities for learning and personal development to (potential) employees. For assessing the created
job models nine characteristic values are determined concerning the following nine criteria:
1. Regulation requirements (requirements for planning, cogitating, decision-making)
2. Hierarchical completeness
3. Cyclical (or sequential) completeness
4. Cooperation and communication requirements
5. Autonomy (degrees of freedom)
6. Responsibility
7. Feedback
8. Variety of KSA requirements
9. Opportunities for learning and further qualification
Additionally, the job designer or rather the job design team is supported in continuously improving
the modeled jobs by recallable design recommendations.
FIGURE 5.18 Extract of the GUI of Spaceþ with basic elements and its exemplary use.
Humans in Work System Environment 5-39
on the action regulation theory (e.g., Tätigkeitsbewertungssystem, TBS, from Hacker et al., 1995). Taking
these assessment concepts into account, the characteristics of humanitarian jobs had been systematically
worked out, sorted, and selected. Each of the criteria mentioned earlier has been operationalized in an
assessment model in terms of subcriteria, relative characteristic values, equations, and instructions,
necessary for determining attributes. Two of the nine assessment models are presented in Figure 5.19.
It becomes obvious that not only the jobs are assessed and made comparable by the characteristic
values, but also the tasks and subtasks, which is another specialty of Spaceþ to handle the increasing
level of detail during design.
The implemented assessment algorithms evaluate the used descriptors, respectively, the linked KSA
requirements. In case, an evaluation based on these entities is not possible, the algorithms utilize the pre-
attributed reference tasks. For the analytical description of the criteria, a deliberate limitation to the
designable aspects of jobs and working conditions had been made, in order to accommodate for prospec-
tiveness and incompleteness of information. Such characteristics were left out, which result only after the
complete realization of the work system from the interaction of all or at least some system elements and
which can, therefore, not be influenced directly by the job designer (e.g., feedback from superiors,
working atmosphere).
With the exception of set memberships, represented by the blob-notation, and cooperation relation-
ships, no relations between tasks and jobs are evaluated. The inclusion of control-, material-, and infor-
mation-flows into the assessment concept would already necessitate a very detailed modeling in the early
stages and would therewith reduce the tolerance towards incomplete information. In contrast to other
methods, Spaceþ does not require a complete description of the work system. The assessment algorithms
linked directly to the design allow modifying, gradually completing, and recognizing interdependencies
of different jobs within the work system.
(visualized as a histogram)
N
Task (Tk) RRTk = max({RS STn n = 1, 2, , N }) Trs Tk = ∑ TST n RS ST n = rs, rs = 1R,1, 2R, ,5
n =1
(visualized as a histogram)
Subtask (ST) Determination of the regulation stage (RS) using the 10-stage-model of
Oesterreich/Volpert 1991 (not necessary when using preattributed reference tasks)
N = number of subtasks within the job / task; Lrs = limit of the allocated time per regulation stage
hrs = weighing factors; TrsJ,Tk = time allocated for sub tasks related to total time of the job / task
FIGURE 5.19 Two of the nine assessment models for determining the characteristic values in Spaceþ .
5-40 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
this reference catalog into Spaceþ, the process of design and redesign had been significantly shortened
and simplified, which is especially important for the efficient use of the system in a CE environment.
A special methodology for attributing newly designed tasks and subtasks or modified reference tasks
ensures the applicability of the assessment algorithms even in later phases of the design process and hence
allows the user to adapt his hierarchical structure of jobs to the increasing maturity level of information
within the CE process. The system allows, therewith, the representation of different levels of information
quality and quantity.
5.5.6.4 System Evaluation
Standard methods of validation could not be used for the evaluation of Spaceþ, as Spaceþ focuses on the
assessment of incompletely or vaguely described jobs that have not been realized. To get an indication of
reliability, validity, and usability of Spaceþ and, furthermore, to identify shortcomings and sources of
errors, a controlled experiment with 20 experts, from industry and research in the field of industrial
engineering and ergonomics, had been conducted. The results of the study were used in the sense of a
formative evaluation to optimize and improve the system.
The evaluation study has shown that Spaceþ (despite its prototype-character) is a suitable and easy-to-use
instrument for prospective, human-oriented job design. The hierarchical modeling, the atomistic approach
of description, the adapted and linked assessment models, the catalog of described and preattributed refer-
ence tasks, and the methodology for attributing self-designed tasks are the characteristical features and con-
cepts of Spaceþ, making it appropriate for the use in a CE environment. Spaceþ is able to support an early
production system design and, thus, can help to reap the benefits outlined in the introduction.
Due to the uncertainty in a CE environment and, therefore, the required simplifications, the assessment
results should not be interpreted as absolute values, but rather should be seen as decision guidance and
be used as such within a cooperative design process. The Spaceþ system should not dictate, but assist.
Further research refers to the extension of the system in terms of other kinds of jobs as well as further
assessment criteria (e.g., safety, feasibility).
. Completeness of the task: proportion of task elements in the group that can be taken over by a
coworker. The design of jobs in an island should be oriented to a mixture of direct and indirect
tasks: inspection of tools and machines, quality control, documentation or results, etc. give
improved scope for self-determined actions and for building up a feeling of responsibility.
In particular, the analytical and simulation models clearly indicate how important these variables
are for a quantitative ex-ante evaluation of a situation: person-oriented job design depends on labor-
partition and cooperation between persons (group size), the availability and demands of tools and
machines, and the scope of tasks to be performed by a more or less qualified working person.
. Spatial extension: proportion of workplaces that have no direct communication (visual or audi-
tory) with the others. Group coherence and the direct help in solving problems will suffer with
spatial extension.
. Homogeneity of workplaces: differences in qualification demands between workplaces. A high hom-
ogeneity of workplaces eases an exchange of workforce and increases the flexibility of manning
. Continuity in personnel: proportion of works that are not directly bound to the process and, thus,
facilitate disposition of personnel capacities (over and under ). A continuous manning of the
island with a well-defined crew is important for the group coherence. People without a permanent
group membership do not share the responsibility for quality, production times, etc. In cases of
overcapacity in personnel, additional workplaces in the production island should be available to
keep persons within their group.
. Stability of the orders: variations in the time series of loading by a different order quantity. Different
order quantities lead to increased efforts in disposition of personnel. If better workplaces are not
available in the production island, personnel deficits or overflow have to be regulated by external
workplaces. Negative consequences for group coherence and indirect tasks (planning efforts) arise.
These variables determine to what extent procedures of introduction of new forms of work organization,
as well as the procedures in the group can be anticipated with respect to their success and their stability.
Thus, they are derived from ideas stemming from organizational development in theory and combined
in a practicable manner in the procedural and participative approach.
. Continuity of flows: number of interrupts in process chains by external operations (not performed
on the production island), inspections, or intermediate storage. Problems for group work arise,
because of internal responsibility for delivery due date with external (uncontrolled) operations.
. Length of process chains: relation between the number of workplaces in a production island, which
are bound to sequential operations at parts, components, or products to the total of workplaces in
the island. To implement “group thinking” in the production island, a reciprocal interdependency
between the workplaces is useful. This is introduced by process chains in the sequential operations.
Self-control is improved, when a person knows about the working processes before and after him
and, thus, the reciprocal responsibility for quality and deadliness.
. Scope in job shop and order scheduling: proportion of orders (in terms of quantity and time) for
which a planning of the sequence and time of operations and orders is possible. An optimal job
shop scheduling and disposition of times and sequences of orders is possible only near to pro-
duction. Responsibility for delivery dates implies scope, in order scheduling for the group.
. Purity of the production island: relation between completely manufactured parts in the island to the
total sum of parts in the island. Islands should be designed in a way such that the manufacturing
process is complete. External operations cause increased efforts of coordination, the responsibility
for the completed accounting quantity and date becomes weak, and quality deficits are not cor-
rected immediately, but delayed by weak feelings of responsibility.
Figure 5.20, profile in the form of a polar diagram is outlined for a manufacturing area of rotors for
pumps, which signalize a status quo that can be developed to group work with a good prognosis of
successful design efforts.
The development of the variables is an experience- and knowledge-based process to the same extent as
a systematical research process. Nevertheless, the variables themselves show a considerable amount of
face validity for suitability for group work. The scaling and evaluation itself is done normally in a dis-
cussion process between company experts and university staff with the help of visual aids. Thus, the
identification of design possibilities is a premise for the identification of applicable design solutions.
procedural
homogeneity of approach
workplace
spatial continuity in
extension personnel
analytical approach
bottleneck purity of the
machines production island
programmatical
approach
FIGURE 5.20 Polar diagram for the anticipation of design success in the introduction of group work in production
systems.
Humans in Work System Environment 5-43
. Variant manufacturer
. Stock manufacturer
A morphologic characteristic pattern assigns specific characteristics to the respective job execution
types. For instance, one characteristic of an order manufacturer is that the production is initiated by a
customer-specific order (make-to-order production). Whereas, the stock manufacturer can fulfill all cus-
tomer orders ex stock, since he produces order-neutrally and exclusively program-dependent. A goal of
the usage of this typology is to be able to quickly create an expressive, operation-specific process model by
the allocation of a certain production enterprise to one of the job execution types.
5.5.8.5 Outlook
Due to the increasing cross-linking of enterprises, the design range of in-plant aligned PPC expanded to
the inter-plant management of complete supply chains. In this context the questions arise how inter-
plant processes are to be arranged and how the coordination of the distributed production has to take
place. In order to accommodate for the changed requirements, the Research Institute for Operations
Management is currently working on an extension of the Aachener PPC model (Schiegg and Lücke,
2004).
FIGURE 5.22 Phases of cooperation processes (Killich, S. and Luczak, H., Unternehmenskooperation für kleine und
mittelständische Unternehmen-Lösungen für die Praxis, Springer, Berlin, 2003. With Permission).
reasons only regional business associates are chosen as project associates. In this case, the purchase of
information is not that important, since numerous information is already available because of the
long-lasting business connections. The following Sections describe the single phases of the process
model in detail.
of the organizational potential should take place. Hence, fields of cooperation can be deduced and
the cooperation project can be defined.
. With the passive initiation the own organization has already been identified as a potential business
associate and is asked whether it is interested in participating in the cooperation project. In this
context, the following question arises: “Is the predefined cooperation project important for the
own organization, or not?.” In answering this question, again the strategic orientation of the
own organization is highly important. The procedure of identifying and/or judging a cooperation
is identical for the active and passive initiation. Only the actuators of the planning of the cross-
plant cooperation differ.
5.6 Conclusion
The realizations of systems theory particularly were introduced into ergonomics via the work system
approach. This conception acts as an analysis framework. It is conducive to systemic thinking,
because it does not regard individual elements in isolation, but considers the various interdependences
between the elements. Besides, the application of this analysis framework forces the ergonomist to draw
up a system limitation and, thus, make a clear problem definition.
The focus of the application of the work system concept is in operational practice at levels S3 and S4 of
the ordering model. The work system approach can also be applied to the other levels, which are shown
in the project examples in the previous sections. The following example of a professor reveals that the
system borderlines shift from one level to another.
A professor performs his lectures usually standing or walking, while the students follow while sitting.
The professor’s physical load during the lecture is, therefore, higher than that of his students. The pro-
fessor uses, in addition, media during his lecture like an overhead projector, which produces heat.
Thus, the professor’s core body temperature possibly increases, so that his “physiological systems”
require a lower room temperature compared to his students (level S1). During his lecture, the professor
performs a series of coordinated movements. For example, he moves the mouse of a computer to begin
his presentation or writes formulas on the board with chalk. The more these coordinations are
performed, the less is the time taken and the quality of the movement (e.g., better typeface on the
board) increases to a certain level. Work resources are also considered at level S2 (operation
systems), as well as psychological and physical functions like the coordination of movements.
Referring to the example of the professor, this means, for example, that the height of desk in the
lecture room is to be designed corresponding to anthropometric measures. Considering the teaching
work order of a professor, or the redefined task of teaching (“task system,” level S3), it does not
consist of only performing lectures. The professor has to prepare his lectures, for example, to read
up on the adequate literature. Furthermore, the professor applies the continuous improvement
process to the concept of teaching, in integrating the latest research results and takes out the dated
contents. As well as the teaching tasks, further tasks belong to the “job system” of a professor (level
S4); primarily, the research and tasks, which result from the leadership of an institute. Referring to
the leadership, the professor has scientific and nonscientific coworkers at his disposal, in order to
cope with the various activities and subtasks, which are related to the “job system” of a professor.
These employees work together in “group systems” (level S5). The individual groups represent the
main research fields of the institute. The members of the individual groups work together, so that
the typical group characteristics like role differentiation and sense of togetherness are developed.
Besides, an institute can be compared to a “company system” (level S6). The professor takes on the
role of a manager, for example, in setting goals, formulating strategies, and coordinating the research
processes. In doing so, the professor interacts with his employees and represents the institute. There-
fore, his tasks and activities are imbedded in a “societal system” (level S7). In this context the rough
contents of public research are worked out by the ministries. Furthermore, the institute, for instance, is
imbedded in a legal framework (e.g., industrial law, salary law, university law).
5-48 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Bibliography
Alioth, A., Entwicklung und Einführung alternativer Arbeitsformen, in Schriften zur Arbeitspsychologie,
Ulich, E., ed., Vol. 27, Huber, Bern, 1980.
Anderson, J.R., Rules of the Mind. Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc, Hillsdale, 1993.
Antoni, C.A., Teilautonome Arbeitsgruppen - Ein Königsweg zu mehr Produktivität und einer menschenger-
echten Arbeit? Psychologie Verlags Union, Weinheim, 1996.
Ashby, W.R., An Introduction to Cybernetics. Chapman & Hall, London, 1956.
Behling, K., Ein analoges Modell der Thermoregulation des Menschen bei Ruhe und Arbeit aufgrund experi-
menteller Daten. Dissertation Universität Hamburg, 1971.
Bertalanffy, L. von, General systems theory — a new approach to unity of science, in Human Biology,
Vol. 23, Winsor, Ch. et al., eds., Baltimore, Maryland, 1951, pp. 306–361.
Brödner, P., Fabrik 2000 - Alternative Entwicklungspfade in die Zukunft der Fabrik, 2nd ed., Sigma, Berlin, 1986.
Buckley, W., Society as a complex adaptive system, in: Modern Systems Research for the Behavioral
Scientist, Buckley, W., ed., Aldine, Chicago, 1968, pp. 490– 513.
Bullinger, H.-J., Fähnrich, K.-P., and Niemeier, J., Informations- und Kommunikationssysteme für
“schlanke Unternehmungen,” in Office Management, 41(1–2), 1993, pp. 6 –19.
Caccibue, P.C., Modelling and Simulation of Human Behaviour in System Control, Berlin, Springer, 1998.
Carayon, P., and Smith, M.J., Work organization and ergonomics, in Appl. Ergon., 31, 2000, pp. 649–662.
Card, S.K., Moran, T.P., and Newell, A., The Psychology of Human –Computer Interaction, Lawrence
Erlbaum Assoc., Hillsdale, 1983.
Clausing, D.P., World-class concurrent engineering, in Concurrent Engineering: Tools and Technologies for
Mechanical System Design, Haug, E.J., ed., NATO ASI Series, Series F: Computer and Systems
Sciences, Vol. 108, Springer, Berlin, 1993, pp. 3 –40.
Compes, P., Sicherheitstechnisches Gestalten - Gedanken zur Methodik und Systematik im projektiven und
konstruktiven Maschinenschutz, Habilitationsschrift RWTH Aachen, 1970.
Döring, B., Systemergonomie bei komplexen Arbeitssystemen, in Arbeitsorganisation und Neue Techno-
logien, Hackstein, R., Heeg, F.-J., and Below, F.v., eds., Springer, Berlin, 1986, pp. 399ff.
Eisenführ, F., and Weber, M., Rationales Entscheiden, 2nd ed., Springer, Berlin, 1994.
Emery, F.E., Characteristics of socio-technical Systems, in Design of Jobs, Davis, L.E. and Taylor, J.C., eds.,
Penguin, Harmondsworth, 1972, pp. 177 –198.
Foley, J.D., and van Dam, A., Fundamentals of Interactive Computer Graphics, Addison Wesley, Menlo
Park, CA, 1982.
Frank, H., Was ist Kybernetik? in Kybernetik - Brücke zwischen den Wissenschaften, Frank, H., ed., 4th ed.,
Umschau, Frankfurt a.M., 1964, pp. 9–20.
Fricker, A. R., Eine Methodik zur Modellierung, Analyse und Gestaltung komplexer Produktionsstrukturen,
Augustinus, Aachen, 1996.
Fürstenberg, F., Humane Arbeitsgestaltung als Partizipations problem, in Menschengerechte Gestaltung
der Arbeit, Fürstenberg, F., Hanau, P., Kreikelbaum, H., and Rohmert, W., eds., BI Wissenschafts-
verlag, Mannheim, 1983.
Gell-Mann, M., Das Quark und der Jaguar - Vom Einfachen zum Komplexen - Die Suche nach einer neuen
Erklärung der Welt, Piper, München, 1994.
Gray, W.D., John, B.E., and Atwood, M.E., Project Ernestine: a validation of GOMS for prediction and
explanation of real-world task performance, in Hum. –Comput. Interact., 8(3), 1993, pp. 237–259.
Grochla, E., Einführung in die Organisationstheorie, Poeschel, Stuttgart, 1978.
Gugerty, L., The use of analytical models in human–computer interface design, in Int. J. Man –Machine
Stud., 38(1), 1993, pp. 625 –660.
Hacker, W., Projektieren von Arbeitstätigkeiten: Möglichkeiten-Probleme-Grenzen, in Zeitschrift für
Arbeitswissenschaft, 45(4), 1991, pp. 193 –198.
Hacker, W., Arbeitstätigkeitsanalyse: Analyse und Bewertung psychischer Arbeitsanforderungen, Asanger,
Heidelberg, 1995.
Humans in Work System Environment 5-49
Hacker, W., Fritsche, B., Richter, P., and Iwanowa, A., Tätigkeitsbewertungssystem (TBS) - Verfahren zur
Analyse, Bewertung und Gestaltung von Arbeitstätigkeiten. Schriftenreihe Mensch-Technik-Organ-
isation, Vol. 7, Ulich, E., ed., Zürich: vdf, 1995.
Hackman, J.R., Toward understanding the role of tasks in behavioral research, in Acta Psychol., 31, 1969,
pp. 97–128.
Hannaman, G.W., Spurgin, A.J., and Lukic, Y., A Model for Assessing Human Cognitive Reliability in
PRA Studies. Third IEEE Conference on Human Factors and Nuclear Power Plants, Monterey,
CA, 1985, pp. 343–353.
Harel, D., On visual formalisms, Commun. ACM, 31(5), 1988, pp. 514– 530.
Heeg, F.J., Moderne Arbeitsorganisation - Grundlagen der organisatorischen Gestaltung von Arbeitssystemen
bei Einsatz neuer Technologien, München, Hanser, Wien, 1988.
Hendrick, H.W., Organizational design and management (ODAM) in HCI, in Luczak, H., Cakir, A., and
Cakir, G. eds., Work with Display Units 92, Amsterdam, North-Holland: 1993, pp. 30–34.
Hendrick, H.W., Sociotechnical systems theory: the sociotechnical systems model of work systems, in
International Encyclopedia of Ergonomics and Human Factors, Karwowski, W., ed., Taylor &
Francis, New York, 2001, pp. 1720–1722.
Hettinger, T., and Wobbe, G., Kompendium der Arbeitswissenschaft - Optimierungsmöglichkeiten zur
Arbeitsgestaltung und Arbeitsorganisation, Kiehl, Ludwigshafen, 1993.
Hill, W., Fehlbaum, R., and Ulrich, P., Organisationslehre 2 - Ziele, Instrumente und Bedingungen der
Organisation sozialer Systeme, 3rd ed., Paul Haupt, Bern, 1981.
Kahnemann, D., Attention and Effort, Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, 1973.
Kaminsky, G., Praktikum der Arbeitswissenschaft - Analytische Untersuchungen beim Studium der
menschlichen Arbeit, 2nd ed., Hanser, Darmstadt 1979.
Karwowski, W., Complexity, fuzziness and ergonomic incompatibility issues in the control of dynamic
work environments, in Ergonomics, 34(6), 1991, pp. 671–686.
Kelly, J.E., A reappraisal of sociotechnical systems theory, in Hum. Relations, 31(12), 1978,
pp. 1069–1099.
Kieras, D.E., A guide to GOMS model usability evaluation using NGOMSL, in Handbook of Human
Computer Interaction, 2nd ed., Helander, M., Landauer, T.K., and Prabhu, P., eds., Elsevier
Science, Amsterdam 1997.
Killich, S., and Luczak, H., Unternehmenskooperation für kleine und mittelständische Unternehmen -
Lösungen für die Praxis, Springer, Berlin, 2003.
Kirchner, J.H., Arbeitswissenschaftlicher Beitrag zur Automatisierung - Analyse und Synthese von Arbeits-
systemen, Beuth, Berlin, 1972.
Kirchner, J.H., Arbeitswissenschaft - Entwicklung eines Grundkonzeptes, in Zeitschrift für Arbeitswis-
senschaft, 47, 1993, pp. 85 –92.
Kirchner, J.H., Integrative Arbeitssystemgestaltung, in Handbuch Arbeitswissenschaft, Luczak, H., and
Volpert, W., eds., Schäffer-Poeschel, Stuttgart, 1997, pp. 805–810.
Kleinbeck, U., Arbeitsmotivation: Entstehung, Wirkung und Förderung, Weinheim, Juventa, München,
1996.
Kraiss, K.-F., Rechnergestützte Methoden zum Entwurf und zur Bewertung von Mensch-Maschine-
Systemen, in Arbeitsorganisation und Neue Technologien, Hackstein, R., Heeg, F.-J., and Below, F.V.,
eds., Springer, Berlin, 1986, pp. 435f.
Krieger, D.J., Einführung in die allgemeine Systemtheorie, Fink, München, 1996.
Laakmann, J., and Much, D., Das Aachener PPS-Modell - FIR entwickelt Grundlage für organisatorische
und informationstechnische PPS-Gestaltung, in firþiaw-Mitteilungen, 27(1), 1995, pp. 14/15.
Locke, E.A., and Latham, G.P., A theory of goal setting and task performance, Prentice-Hall, Englewood
Cliffs, 1990.
Luczak, H., Arbeitswissenschaftliche Untersuchungen von maximaler Arbeitsdauer und Erholzeiten bei
informatorisch-mentaler Arbeit nach dem Kanal- und Regler-Mensch-Modell sowie superponier-
ten Belastungen am Beispiel Hitzearbeit, in VDI-Z, Vol. 10, No. 6, VDI, Düsseldorf, 1979.
5-50 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Luczak, H., Koordination der Bewegungen, in Praktische Arbeitsphysiologie, 3rd ed., Rohmert, W., and
Rutenfranz, J. eds., Thieme, Stuttgart, 1983.
Luczak, H., Wesen menschlicher Leistung, in Institut für angewandte Arbeitswissenschaft, ed., Arbeitsges-
taltung in Produktion und Verwaltung, Bachem, Cologne, 1989, pp. 39 –65.
Luczak, H., Volpert, W., Raeithel, A., and Schwier, W., Arbeitswissenschaft - Kerndefinition, Gegenstands-
katalog, Forschungsgebiete, 3rd ed., RKW, TÜV Rheinland, Cologne, 1989.
Luczak, H., Macroergonomic anticipatory evaluation of work organization in production systems, in
Ergonomics, 38(8), 1995, pp. 1571–1599.
Luczak, H., Arbeitssysteme und Arbeitstechnologien. in Produktion und Management - Betriebshütte.
Part 2, 7th ed., Eversheim, W., and Schuh, G., eds., Springer, Berlin, 1996, chap. 12, pp. 11–39.
Luczak, H., and Fricker, A.R., Komplexitätsbewältigung - Bedingung zur ganzheitlichen Rationalisierung,
in Wie rational ist Rationalisierung heute? Ein öffentlicher Diskurs anlässlich des 75-jährigen Jubi-
läums des RKW e.V., Hoß, D. ed., Raabe, Stuttgart, 1996.
Luczak, H., and Metz, A., Development of a decision support system for the selection of a pilot section to
implement self-directed work teams in manufacturing and assembling, in Human Factors in
Organizational Design and Management — V. Proceedings of the Fifth International Symposium
on Human Factors in Organizational Design and Management held in Breckenridge, CO.,
U.S.A., Brown, Jr., ed., Elsevier Science B.V., Amsterdam, 1996, pp. 383–389.
Luczak, H., Kerndefinition und Systematiken der Arbeitswissenschaft, in Handbuch Arbeitswissenschaft,
Luczak, H., and Volpert, W., eds., Schäffer-Poeschel, Stuttgart, 1997, pp. 11 –19.
Luczak, H., and Rohmert, W., Belastungs-Beanspruchungs-Konzepte, in Handbuch Arbeitswissenschaft,
Luczak, H., and Volpert, W., eds., Schäffer-Poeschel, Stuttgart, 1997, pp. 326 –332.
Luczak, H., Arbeitswissenschaft, 2nd ed., Springer, Berlin, 1998.
Luczak, H., and Eversheim, W., eds., Produktionsplanung und -steuerung - Grundlagen, Gestaltung und
Konzepte, Springer, Berlin, 1998.
Luczak, H., and Kees, A., Das Aachener PPS-Modell, in Referenzmodellierung ’98 - Anwendungsfelder in
Theorie und Praxis, Conference, Aachen, 1998.
Luczak, H., and Schlick, C., Utility of Cognitive Models, in Proceedings of the IEA 2000/HFES 2000
Congress, eds., Human Factors and Ergonomics Society,. San Diego, 2000, S. 1/585–1/588.
Luczak, H., and Schlick, C., An essay about the theory-to-practice relationship of ergo-
nomics, in International Congress on Humanizing Work and Work Environment, Bombay,
2001, pp. 1–10.
Luczak, H. Reuth, R., and Schmidt, L., Development of error-compensating UI for autonomous pro-
duction cells, in Ergonomics, 46(1–3), 2003, pp. 19 –40.
Luhmann, N., Soziologische Aufklärung I, 2nd ed., Westdeutscher Verlag, Opladen, 1971.
Luhmann, N., Zweckbegriff und Systemrationalität - Über die Funktion von Zwecken in sozialen Systemen,
Suhrkamp, Frankfurt a.M., 1973.
Luhmann, N., Soziale Systeme - Grundriß einer allgemeinen Theorie, Suhrkamp, Frankfurt a. M., 1984.
Malik, F., Strategie des Managements komplexer Systeme. Ein Beitrag zur Management-Kybernetik evolutio-
närer Systeme, 4th ed., Haupt, Bern, 1992.
Malik, F., Systemisches Management, Evolution, Selbstorganisation: Grundprobleme, Funktionsmechanis-
men und Lösungsansätze für komplexe Systeme, Haupt, Bern, 1993.
Maturana, H.R., Erkennen - Die Organisation und Verkörperung von Wirklichkeit, 2nd ed., Vieweg,
Wiesbaden, 1985.
Maturana, H., and Varela, F., Der Baum der Erkenntnis - Die biologischen Wurzeln des menschlichen Erken-
nens, Scherz, Bern, 1987.
McCormick, E.J., Jeanneret, P.R., and Mecham, R.C., Position Analysis Questionnaire (PAQ) Form
C. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, 1989.
Metz, A., Entscheidungshilfe für die Auswahl von Pilotbereichen zur Einführung von Gruppenarbeit in
der Produktion. Dissertation RWTH Aachen, 1997.
Miller, E.J., and Rice, A.K., Systems of Organization, London: Tavistock, 1967.
Humans in Work System Environment 5-51
Moray, N., Eisen, P., Money, L., and Turksen, I.B., Fuzzy analysis of skill- and rule-based workload, in
Human Mental Workload, Hancock, P., and Meshkati, L., eds., North-Holland, Amsterdam, 1988a.
Moray, N., Kruschelnicky, E., Money, L., and Turksen, I.B., On measuring the combined workload of
skill-, rule- and knowledge-based workload, Proceedings of the Human Factors Society — 32nd
Annual Meeting — Anaheim (CA), 1988b.
Mütze-Niewöhner, S., and Luczak, H., Prospective job design and evaluation in early stages of production
system design, in Human Factors in Organizational Design and Management — VII, Proceedings of
the Seventh International Symposium on Human Factors in Organizational Design and Manage-
ment held in Aachen, Luczak, H. and Zink, K.J., eds., IEA Press, Santa Monica, 2003, pp. 323 –328.
Newell, A., Unified Theories of Cognition, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, 1990.
Newell, A., Precis of unified theories of cognition, Behav. Brain Sci., 15, 3, 1992, pp. 425–492.
Pahl, G., Grundlagen der Konstruktionstechnik, in Dubbel - Taschenbuch für den Maschinenbau, 18th ed.,
Beitz, W., and Küttner, K.-H., eds., Springer, Berlin, 1995.
Pew, R.W., and Baron, S., Perspectives on human performance modeling, Automatica, 19(6), 1983,
pp. 663–676.
Pfeifer, T., Sack, D., Stemmer, M., and Orth, A., Sensor/Actuator Network — The Nervous System of a
New Production Concept: The Autonomous Production Cells, Congresso Brasileiro de Automá-
tica, Florianópolis, Brasil, 2000.
Prasad, B., Concurrent Engineering Fundamentals — Integrated Product and Process Organization, Vol. 1,
Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, 1996.
Rasmussen, J., Skills, rules, knowledge: signals, signs, and symbols and other distinctions in human per-
formance models, IEEE Trans. Sys., Man and Cybern., 13(3), 1983, pp. 257 –267.
Rasmussen, J., The role of hierarchical knowledge representation in decision making and system manage-
ment, IEEE Trans. Syst., Man and Cybern., 15(2), 1985, pp. 234 –243.
Rasmussen, J., Information Processing and Human-Machine Interaction: An Approach to Cognitive
Engineering, North-Holland, New York, 1986.
Reason, J., Generic error-modeling system: a cognitive framework for locating common human error
forms, in Rasmussen, J., Duncan, K., and Leplat, J., eds., New Technology and Human Error,
John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1987.
REFA — Verband für Arbeitsstudien und Betriebsorganisation e.V. ed., Methodenlehre der Betriebsorga-
nisation. Part 1 Grundlagen der Arbeitsgestaltung, 2nd ed., Hanser, München, 1993.
Reuth, R., Schlick, C., and Luczak, H., A simulation approach: comparative assessment of knowledge,
skill and abilities in autonomous production cells, in Proceedings of HCI International 2001,
Vol. 2, Systems, Social and Internationalization Design Aspects of Human –Computer Interaction,
2001 in New Orleans, Smith, J.M., and Salvendy, G., eds., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah,
NJ, pp. 207 –211.
Rice, A.K., Productivity and Social Organization — The Ahmedabad Experiment, Tavistock, London, 1958.
Rohmert, W., and Rutenfranz, J., Arbeitswissenschaftliche Beurteilung der Belastung und Beanspruchung
an unterschiedlichen industriellen Arbeitsplätzen. Der Bundesminister für Arbeit und Sozialord-
nung ed., Referat Öffentlichkeitsarbeit, Bonn, 1975.
Rohmert, W., and Weg, F.J., Organisation teilautonomer Gruppenarbeit - betriebliche Projekte, Leitregeln
zur Gestaltung, ed., RKW (Beiträge zur Arbeitswissenschaft, Reihe 1, Bd. 1), Hanser, München,
1976.
Ropohl, G., Eine Systemtheorie der Technik - zur Grundlegung der allgemeinen Technologie, Hanser,
München, 1979.
Scarperi, M., Scarperi, S., Behling, K., Bleichert, A., and Kitzing, J., Antriebe und effektorische Maßnah-
men der Thermoregulation bei Ruhe und während körperlicher Arbeit. III. Über den Einfluss des
Wasserhaushaltes auf die Thermoregulation bei Ruhe und Arbeit, Int. Z. f. angew. Physiol. 1972, 30,
pp. 186–192.
Schiegg, P., and Lücke, T., Referenzmodell der Produktionsplanung und -steuerung, in Betriebsorganisa-
tion im Unternehmen der Zukunft, Luczak, H., and Stich, V., eds., Springer, Berlin, 2004.
5-52 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
6-1
6-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
more is learned about the process through quality methodologies (e.g., Statistical Process Control: Grant
and Leavenworth, 1995; Designed Experiments: Taguchi, 1986), so the process can change from closed
loop control using performance feedback, to open loop control using valid prediction models (Drury
and Prabhu, 1994). Predictive control allows the process to operate with minimum setup time after a
product change, thus facilitating moves toward just-in-time manufacturing.
The other company response to the quality imperative is the active management of quality. This has
comprised both the realization that managerial leadership is important (Witcher, 1995) and specific pol-
icies for managing quality (Deming, 1986). An obvious policy is the use of teams as both a change agent
(Blest, Hunt, and Shadle, 1992) and as a natural group for controlling a process (Brennan, 1990).
At the same time that these strategic-driven changes in quality have been taking place, there have been
simultaneous programs at other levels. Thus, new technology has been introduced to reduce labor costs
and improve process capability. In response to both rising costs and public/government pressure, there
has been a movement toward managing the costs associated with human errors and injuries. Company
responses here have been injury reduction/safety programs (Rahimi, 1995), ergonomics programs
(Liker, Joseph and Armstrong, 1984) focusing on workforce injury reduction, and similar programs
for reduction of the consequences of human error (e.g., Taylor, 1990). These latter are usually termed
“human factors” programs but have many characteristics in common with “industrial ergonomics”
programs. Indeed, programs incorporating both injury and error reduction are possible (Drury,
1995). In this chapter the terms “ergonomics” and “human factors” will be used interchangeably.
With few exceptions, programs arising from the quality movement and the human factors movement
have been simultaneous but unrelated in industry. They have many similarities and some obvious differ-
ences, but there is no a priori reason for them to be separate. The remainder of this chapter takes up the
managerial challenge of integrating these largely parallel programs so as to gain additional benefits. For a
more detailed comparison and discussion of their linkages, see Drury (1996). In particular, that paper
looks at many facets of the quality movement, such as TQM, Quality awards, the ISO-9000 series,
and justin-time manufacturing, while in the current chapter we concentrate on just one (TQM) for
simplicity.
Source: Adapted from Hackman, J. R. and Wageman, R. 1995. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40: 308–342.
its basic beliefs. Thus, societies within the International Ergonomics Association have their own defi-
nitions of ergonomics, as do textbooks and journals. Although some authors have begun to consider
the underpinnings of the discipline (e.g., Karwowski, Marek, and Noworol, 1988; Meister, 1996), there
is no simple list of tenets similar to Table 6.1. As a working list, Table 6.2 is proposed, keeping the struc-
ture of the equivalent TQM list to facilitate comparison. Note that this listing is biased toward design
ergonomics, rather than more overtly sociotechnical systems approaches (e.g., Taylor and Felten, 1993).
As is obvious from Table 6.2, ergonomics is a human-oriented process, using detailed knowledge of
human functioning as a basis for designing high-performance, safe systems. Indeed, the current book
gives many examples of both the detailed human knowledge, and its use in design. We now need to consider
the linkages between TQM and ergonomics explicitly, to find how to manage both programs together.
Two recent papers show how these ideas have progressed in Finland. Vainio and Mattila (1996) inte-
grated safety concerns within the TQM system for an electrical utility. They made safety and health an
integral part of TQM largely by addressing safety and health issues within the total quality handbook.
More evaluation data were provided by Saari and Laitinen (1996) in a manufacturing setting. They set
up continuous improvement teams for safety, defining best work practices in each area. Then, using
the measurement-based TUTTAVA system, the teams set goals, made continuous improvements, and
validated the results. A posture survey across the whole plant showed considerable improvement over
the course of the project. In addition, injury and illness days lost were reduced by about 90% over
three years.
Beyond safety is the safety role of ergonomics, typically designing to avoid injury. Here also a consider-
able literature is developing. Stuebbe and Houshmand (1995) characterize the production system as an
inadvertent injury-producing system and advocate applying quality control approaches such as control
charting, Pareto analysis, etc., to an “integrated ergonomic-quality system.” This consists of analysis of
the task, worker, and environment using these quality control techniques. Getty, Abbott, and Getty
(1995) link quality initiatives to ergonomic projects, showing how an intervention to control cumulative
trauma disorder in a panel drilling task also had a substantial effect on quality and productivity.
A major program in Sweden, the Quality, Working Environment and Productivity (QPEP) project
(Axelsson, 1994; Eklund, 1995) examined specifically the linkages between quality and ergonomics in
a car assembly plant. In eight departments, they produced an inventory of ergonomically demanding
jobs, both those which were physically demanding and those causing production problems. Two different
measures of quality showed significant differences between ergonomically good and ergonomically poor
tasks, indicating the close link between ergonomics and quality.
One of the most integrated quality ergonomics efforts so far appears to be the implementation of ergo-
nomic change within the TQM philosophy at the mail order clothing manufacturer and distributor, L. L.
Bean (Rooney and Morency, 1992; Rooney, Morency, and Herrick, 1993). Their ergonomic objective was
initially to eliminate the cumulative trauma disorder exposures of repetitive sewing production in a 400-
person manufacturing plant. TQM was seen as defining the mission, objectives, and responsibility for
safety with line management. Ergonomics moved over a six-year period from reacting to employee inju-
ries, through proactive job design using teams, to now become part of the management and employee
performance expectations and rewards.
In a follow-on paper, Rooney et al. (1993) were able to tackle some of the more deep-seated problems
of repetitive work. They redesigned payment systems (with active operator involvement), replacing direct
piece-rates with an annual appraisal system in which units produced were only 35 to 33% of the weight-
ing. More complexity was built into jobs, by using cross-training and team work. Management and
supervisor commitment for the ergonomics program was shown by their active support. Rooney et al.
(1993) see these changes as a way of incorporating the musculoskeletal injury reduction aspects of ergo-
nomics into a wider framework based upon macroergonomics (Hendrick, 1992) and TQM principles.
We can, however, go beyond these examples to provide managerial advice on TQM/human factors
interactions, making use of similarities where they exist and exploiting differences to enhance each
program. Starting with the similarities between the tenets of TQM and ergonomics (Table 6.1 and
Table 6.2) we can suggest, with some combining of categories:
1. Study and Measure the Process. Start from a systems focus rather than the current process (also
advocated in Business Process Reengineering, Hammer and Champy, 1990). Use this as the
basis for a detailed quantitative understanding of the process. Standard quality techniques
should be used to measure process parameters, and models of human performance and well-
being to measure and understand the role of the operator in the system. Use these measurements
as the basis for directing and quantifying continuous improvement.
2. Honor Thy User. (To quote Kantowitz and Sorkin, 1987). Respect the operators in the system as
people trying to do their best, and having an inherent stake in performing well. Do not necessarily
blame the operator alone for poor quality/productivity/safety. Tap the potential for
6-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
operatorempowered improvement by giving real power to small teams which include operators.
The rewards will be improvements in performance, safety, and job satisfaction.
From differences between TQM and ergonomics we can show first how ergonomics can learn from
TQM practice. These represent largely a shift from a technical process level of intervention to a more
strategic, managerial level. (Longer discussions of each issue are presented in Drury, 1996.)
3. Consider the Strategic Level. Understand the forces beyond the process within the factory, such as
requirements of the ultimate customer, and active management of the supply chain. Ensure that
ergonomic interventions are truly customer-driven by explicitly measuring customer needs.
4. Understand Leadership. Any change activity needs responsibility of managers, up to the highest
level. Do not take the mechanistic view of an organization which defines each manager by func-
tion. Understand the principles of leadership, recognize leaders, and practice leadership. All
change projects need a powerful champion.
5. Use Well-Developed Team Skills. TQM, and many other change disciplines have standard methods
of starting, organizing, and running successful teams. Use these methods where they are appro-
priate. At least understand these methods so that you can build on the teamwork training existing
within the organization from TQM programs.
Where TQM can learn from ergonomics is in the area of technical knowledge of the human operator,
and how to incorporate this in process design.
6. Use Allocation of Function Techniques. A basic building block of human factors is the concept of
function allocation, i.e., permanent or flexible assignment of logical functions between human
and machine. This has been used by ergonomists at levels ranging from the whole complex
system (Older, Clegg, and Waterson, 1996) to a single human–machine system (Drury, 1994).
Without an explicit treatment of function allocation, technology can easily fail. For example, con-
sider the baggage handling system at the Denver International Airport.
7. Error-Free Manufacturing/Service. While TQM is calling for drastic reductions in error rates,
human factors is coming to grips with the causes of human error (e.g., Reason, 1990). In
airline flight operations (Wiener and Nagel, 1988) and maintenance (Wenner and Drury, 1997)
we have classified errors and derived logical interventions, moving from a consideration only
of the accident-precipitating event to a study of root causes and latent pathogens.
8. Interface Design. From physical workplace layout to reduce injuries (e.g., Kroemer, Kroemer, and
Kroemer-Elbert, 1994) to the interface between software and the user (Helander, 1988), human
factors engineers have been designing less error-prone interfaces between people and systems.
This set of techniques is largely ignored in the TQM literature, despite the latter’s emphasis on
error reduction, parts per million, and six-sigma processes.
6.4 Summary
In this chapter we have examined the relationship between quality programs, specifically TQM, on the
one hand and ergonomics/human factors programs on the other. Simple listing of their tenets, although
these may still be arguable, led to recognition of the similarities and differences between the programs.
Examples of use of ergonomics within a TQM context showed that sensible linkages had already been
reported.
The aim of the chapter was to find prescriptions which would help the manager exploit the similarities
and differences, so as to find new linkages between human factors and TQM. Seven prescriptions are
given which can lead to greater integration between the two programs in the future. Readers who do
use these for successful integration of the human factors and the quality imperative are urged to continue
to report their work in the open literature and continue the integration process for the benefit of all.
Human Factors and TQM 6-7
References
Axelsson, J. R. C. 1994. Ergonomic aspects on design and quality, IEA’94, Vol. 4: 18 –21.
Bjorkman, T. 1996. The rationalisation movement in perspective and some ergonomic implications,
Applied Ergonomics, 27(2): 71–77.
Blest, J. P., Hunt, R. G., and Shadle, C. C. 1992. Action teams in the total quality process: experience in a
job shop, National Productivity Review/Spring 1992, 195–202.
Brennan, L. 1990. The human dimension to statistical process control within advanced manufacturing
systems, in Ergonomics of Hybrid Automated Systems II, Eds. W. Karwowski and M. Rahimi,
pp. 527–534, Elsevier Science Publishers, London.
Costigan, R. D. 1995. Adaptation of traditional human resources processes for total quality environ-
ments, Quality Management Journal 95 Spring, 7–23.
Crosby, 1979. Quality is Free, The Art of Making Quality Certain, McGraw-Hill, New York.
Deming, W. E. 1986. Out of the Crisis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Mass.
Dobyns, L. and Crawford-Mason, C. 1991. Quality or Else. The Revolution in World Business, Houghton
Mifflin Company, Boston.
Drury, C. G. 1992. Ergonomics of job and equipment design, Impact of Science on Society, 165:
41–52.
Drury, C. G. 1994. Function allocation in manufacturing, in Proceedings of the Ergonomics Society 1994
Annual Conference, Ed. S. A. Robertston, University of Warwick, 19–22 April 1994, pp. 2–16.
Drury, C. G. 1995. Work design, in Human Factors Guide for Aviation Maintenance, Ed. M. Maddox,
Chapter 6, Federal Aviation Administration/DOT, Washington, D.C.
Drury, C. G. 1996. Ergonomics and the quality movement. The 1996 Ergonomics Society Lecture, Lei-
cester, U.K., 10 –12 April 1996.
Drury, C. G. and Prabhu, P. V. 1994. Human factors in test and inspection, in Design of Work and Devel-
opment of Personnel in Advanced Manufacturing, Eds. G. Salvendy and W. Karwowski, Chapter 13,
pp. 355–492, John Wiley & Sons, New York.
Eklund, J. A. E. 1995. Relationships between ergonomics and quality in assembly work. Applied Ergo-
nomics, 26(1): 15–20.
Evans, J. R. and Lindsay, W. M. (1995). The Management and Control of Quality, Third Edition, West
Publishing Company, Minneapolis/St. Paul, MN, 143.
Getty, R. L., Abbott, W. L., and Getty, J. M. 1995. ISO 9000 methodology enhances ergonomics effort:
ergonomics becomes a tool for continuous improvement, ASQC 49th Annual Quality Congress
Proceedings, 904–913.
Grant, E. L. and Leavenworth, R. S. 1995. Statistical Quality Control, McGraw-Hill, 1984, New York.
Grant, R. M., Shari, R. and Krishnan, R. 1994. TQM’s challenge to management theory and practice,
Sloan Management Review/Winter, 25 –35.
Hackman, J. R. and Wageman, R. 1995. Total quality management: empirical, conceptual, and practical
issues, Administrative Science Quarterly, 40: 308–342.
Hammer, M. and Champy, J. 1993. Reengineering the Corporation, Harper Business, New York.
Helander, M. Ed. 1988. Handbook of Human-Computer Interaction, Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.,
Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
Hendrick, H. W. 1992. A macroergonomic approach to work organization for improved safety and pro-
ductivity, in Advances in Industrial Ergonomics and Safety IV, Ed. S. Kumar, pp. 3–10, Taylor &
Francis, Hampshire.
Kantowitz, B. H. and Sorkin, R. D. 1987. Allocation of function, in Handbook of Human Factors, Ed.
G. Salvendy, pp. 355 –369, John Wiley & Sons, New York.
Karwowski, W., Marek, T., and Noworol, C. 1988. Theoretical basis of the science of ergonomics.
Ergonomics International 88, Proceedings of the 10th Congress of the International Ergonomics Associ-
ation, Sydney, Australia, 1 –5 August 1988, Eds. A. S. Adams, R. R. Hall, B. J. McPhee and M. S.
Oxenburgh, pp. 756–758, Taylor & Francis, London.
6-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Kleiner, B. M. and Drury, C. G. 1996. Macroergonomics in regional planning and economic development
in O. Brown, Jr. and H.W. Hendrick (Eds.), Human Factors in Organizational Design and Manage-
ment, 523– 528.
Krause, T. R. 1993. Safety and quality: two sides of the same coin, Occupational Hazards, April 1993, 47–50.
Kroemer, K.H.E., Kroemer, H.J., and Kroemer-Elbert, K.E. 1994. Ergonomics: How to Design for Ease and
Efficiency, Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ, 430 –441.
Larson, P. D. and Sinha, A. 1995. The TQM impact: a study of quality managers’ perceptions, Quality
Management Journal 95 Spring, 53 –66.
Liker, J. K., Joseph, B. S., and Armstrong, T. J. 1984. From ergonomic theory to practice: Organizational
factors affecting the utilization of ergonomic knowledge, in Human Factors in Organizational
Design and Management, Eds. H. W. Hendrick and O. Brown, Proc. 1st Symp., Honolulu, HI,
August 1984, North-Holland, Amsterdam.
Meister, D. 1996. A new theoretical structure for developmental ergonomics. Paper to 4th Pan-Pacific
Conference on Occupational Ergonomics, Taipei 1996.
Mize. J. H. 1992. Constant change, constant challenge, in Manufacturing Systems, Foundations of World-
Class Practice, Eds. J. A. Heim and W. D. Compton, pp. 196–203, National Academy Press,
Washington, D.C.
Older, M., Clegg, C. W. and Waterson, P. E. 1996. Task allocation in complex systems, in Advances in
Applied Ergonomics, Eds. A. F. Ozok and G. Salvendy, pp. 471 –474, U.S.A. Publishing Corp,
West Lafayette, IN.
Rahimi, M. 1995. Merging strategic safety, health and environment into total quality management, Inter-
national Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 16: 83 –94, Elsevier, London.
Reason, J. 1990. Human Error, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K.
Rooney, E. F. and Morency, R. R. 1992. A practical evaluation method for quantifying ergonomic changes
at L. L. Bean, in Advances in Industrial Ergonomics and Safety IV, Ed. S. Kumar, pp. 475 –481, Taylor
& Francis, New York.
Rooney, E. F., Morency, R. R., and Herrick, D. R. 1993. Macroergonomics and total quality management
at L. L. Bean: a case study, in Advances in Industrial Ergonomics and Safety V, Eds. R. Nielsen and
K. Jorgensen, pp. 493 –498, Taylor & Francis, New York.
Roughton, J. 1993. TQM Integrating a total quality management system into safety and health programs.
American Society of Safety Engineers, June 1993, 32 –37.
Russell, S. 1991. Employee involvement aspects of total quality management, Pþ European Participation
Monitor, (2): 29–32.
Saari, J. and Laitinen, H. 1996. Towards continuous improvement of workplace, in Advances in
Applied Ergonomics, Eds. A. F. Ozok and G. Salvendy, pp. 82 –87, U.S.A. Publishing Corp,
West Lafayette, IN.
Smith, T. J. and Larson, T. L. 1991. Integrating quality management and hazard management: a beha-
vioral cybernetic perspective, Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 35th Annual Meeting —
1991, 903–907.
Stuebbe, P. A. and Houshmand, A. A. 1995. Quality and ergonomics, Quality Management Journal 95
Winter, 52 –64.
Taguchi, G. 1986. Introduction to Quality Engineering, UNIPUB, White Plains, NY.
Taylor, J. C. and Felten, D. F., 1993. Performance by Design, Prentice Hall, NJ.
Taylor, J.C. 1990. Organizational context for aircraft maintenance and inspection, in Proceedings of the
Human Factors Society 34th Annual Meeting, Volume 2, 1176 –1180.
Vainio, P. and Mattila, M. 1996. Development of a safety and ergonomics oriented total quality system for
an electricity company, in Advances in Applied Ergonomics, Eds. A. F. Ozok and G. Salvendy,
pp. 43–46, U.S.A. Publishing Corp, West Lafayette, IN.
Waldman, D. A. 1994. The contributions of total quality management to a theory of work performance,
Academy of Management Review 1994, 19(3): 510 –536.
Human Factors and TQM 6-9
Wenner, C. and Drury, C. G. 1997. Deriving targeted interventions for ground damage, Proceedings of the
1997 SAE Airframe Engine Maintenance & Repair Conference (AEMC ’97) August 1997, SAE Tech-
nical Paper Series 972591, Warrendale, PA.
Wiener, E. L. and Nagel, D. C. Eds. 1988. Human Factors in Aviation. Academic Press, Inc., San Diego.
Wilson, J. R., Neely, A. D., and Chew, T. 1993. Human and production requirements in modern
manufacturing: complementary or contradictory? Journal of Design and Manufacturing (1993),
3: 167–175.
Witcher, B. 1995. The changing scale of total quality management, Quality Management Journal 95
Summer, 9 –29.
Zink, K. J., Hauer, R., and Schmidt, A. 1994. Quality assessment: instruments for the analysis of quality
concepts based on EN 29000, the Malcolm Baldridge Award and the European Quality Award, in
Total Quality Management, Ed. G. K. Kanji, 5(5): 329– 343, Carfax Publishing Company, UK.
V. Kathlene Leonard 7.3 Common Tools and Techniques for UCD . . . . . . . 7-8
Overview † Understanding Users, Their Needs, and Their
Kevin P. Moloney Work † Testing and Evaluation of Proposed Design Solutions
Julie A. Jacko 7.4 Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7-34
Georgia Institute of Technology 7.5 Message from the Authors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7-35
7-1
7-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools For Occupational Ergonomics
FIGURE 7.1 The punch card voting system, as experienced by voters in Palm Beach County, FL, for the 2000
presidential election. (From Psephos Corporation, 2005. With permission.)
Cranor (2001) outlines the difficulties with the two-page layout, even for someone without vision
impairment with healthy hand-eye motor skills and cognition. The population of Palm Beach County
is comprised of mostly adults over 50. The population commonly experiences age-related declines in
their sensory, motor, and cognitive abilities. Thus, it is easy to understand how the insufficient spatial
mapping resulting from this design could prove highly problematic for users. In addition to the poor
layout, there were several problems with the actual reading of the cards to count votes. Often at times
the hole was not punched out completely leaving a ballot that was “dimpled” or not completely perfo-
rated through (i.e., the infamous hanging chads), and henceforth not readable by the machine. This led
to thousands of work hours dedicated to manual sorting and classifying these paper cards, and ultimately
delayed the outcome of the election.
While hindsight is 20/20, had the decision to implement the butterfly ballot taken into consideration
abilities of the actual voters, the demands presented by the task, and the costs of recounting these
ballots, officials may not have been inclined to use such a system, or (at least) they may have been
more deliberate in the layout. The core problem with the butterfly ballot system was its narrow
focus on the automation and functionality of counting votes. The users of the system (the voters)
and all of their inherent variability were not considered as an operational component of this
system. Instead, they were given tasks for which the vote counting system was operationally incapable
of completing. That is, the people in this system took on the leftover work, such as inserting the cards
into the butterfly ballot, selecting the candidate, and in case of the 2000 election, manually counting
chads and dimples.
Critical features implicating the voting system/voter interaction were overlooked, such as the
cognitive and physical abilities of the users, voter trust in the system, and verification that the
intended vote was cast. Because the design of the system was remiss in addressing these critical
components, impediments emerged to the ultimate goal of electing a president. It is critical in
the design process to consider how to allocate the functions of work between people and
systems, in order to more readily facilitate when the user is considered a component of that
system (Noyes and Barber, 1999).
Quality in systems and products is not an accident; it’s an outcome of judicious design (Ostroff, 2001).
Furthermore, good design does not happen by chance. While the tools and systems integrated into the
workplace can be effective and beneficial to work goals and a pleasure to use, potential always exists for
these tools to be ineffective and cumbersome to the goals and needs of the user, work, and organization.
Additionally, the impact of an ill-fit system in the workplace can propagate throughout an organization
to seriously implicate total quality, worker satisfaction, worker retention, productivity, and downtime.
User-Centered Design of Information Technology 7-3
In extreme cases, users abandon technology intended to augment their work, or abandon the job out of
frustration when the use of a poorly designed system is mandatory. This can ultimately influence the
bottom line and long-term organizational endurance.
New tools and systems introduce change from many perspectives (Karat et al., 2000). Underestimating
the impact of a new system can have negative implications. For example, the introduction of electronic
purchasing reports may be aimed at improving productivity by removing the need for face-to-face
meetings with a purchasing manager. This organizational productivity may come at the cost of indi-
vidual productivity, as the purchasing manager may obtain information beyond what the electronic
system records provide, such as the immediacy of the needs of different orders. Furthermore, the
elimination of personal contact with other employees may prompt a feeling of isolation and a signifi-
cant decrease in job satisfaction. Providing tools that enable users through directing and facilitating
activity and not by restricting and controlling their activity should always be a consideration (Karat
et al., 2000).
The design process entails innumerable assumptions and decisions by the designer(s) about the work
and the user. Good designs are more likely to emerge when those assumptions and decisions are well-
informed. The quality, quantity, and accuracy of the information gathered directly impact the efficacy
of the final design product. Good designs represent a symphonic integration of characteristics of both
the users and the technology. That is, when the decision makers who orchestrate the design process
possess an adequate understanding of the people using the system, the technology, and what drives
the interactions between the two, the resulting designs are more liable to promote harmonious inter-
actions. Good designs incorporate consideration of the user(s), the organization, the environment,
and the technology at several points throughout the design and implementation process. This process
should incorporate communication, analyses, and design techniques, which are creative, yet grounded
in knowledge (Hackos and Redish, 1998).
According to Karwowski (2003), the ergonomics approach to information technology (IT) “advocates
the systematic use of knowledge concerning relevant human characteristics to achieve compatibility in the
design of interactive systems among people, computers, and outside environments . . . to achieve other
specific goals, such as system effectiveness, safety, ease of performance and to contribute to overall user
well-being” (p. 1227). This compatibility is representative of the overarching goal of human factors and
ergonomics, and is to be considered at all levels in the design (e.g., physical, emotional, perceptual, cog-
nitive, social, organization, environmental, etc.) to optimize the user-interface system, and its subsequent
output. The integration of IT into the workplace creates a paradox. As technologies evolve to include more
sophisticated features and provide richer information to the users, an increase in the complexity of the
system results, as does a corresponding propensity for usability problems (Hackos and Redish, 1998;
Karwowski, 2003; Norman, 1988). Norman termed this concept creeping featurism and suggested that
the square additional functionality, which is incorporated in a design materializes as additional complexity
is imparted upon then required interaction (Norman, 1988). This is also known as the complexity-
compatibility paradigm, which states that the added complexity of a system decreases the potential for
efficacy in ergonomic interventions and overall design efficacy (Karwowski, 2003). These things considered,
a deliberate approach to the incorporation of user factors is essential for the design of usable, useful
systems.
justification for the importance of UCD and some common strategies for incorporating accurate infor-
mation about users and their work.
The term user-centered design first emerged in the mid 1980s and is often attributed to Norman’s
(1988) renowned book, The Psychology of Everyday Things (later reissued with the title: The Design of
Everyday Things) (Norman, 1988, 2002). UCD was embraced by the human –computer interaction
(HCI) discipline as the foundation for usable, useful, and successful interfaces. The Berkshire Encyclopedia
of Human Computer Interaction, more recently, defines UCD as a “broad term, used to describe the
process in which end users influence how a design takes shape” (Bainbridge, 2004, p. 763). UCD
directs focus of the design process to the users’ role in the interactions between man and machine,
and the resulting dynamics that ensue with the work, environment, and organization.
Changes and modifications are introduced to the UCD process by designers in reaction to an evolving
network of users, goals, environments, technologies, and associated constraints with which the design
must operate. This improvisational approach aims to achieve a balanced harmony of interactions
between users, goals, context and constraints, and to avoid dissonance within the network that could
impede productivity. UCD embodies a set of design strategies and information gathering techniques
and tools that are perceived as personalized rather than controlling. The vehicle to achieve this level
of usability and usefulness is the multifaceted task of understanding the users, the work context, and
the tasks to be accomplished (Karat et al., 2000).
In truth, the specific set of techniques used for UCD depends on the specific combination of circum-
stances and constraints related to the users, work, and organization associated with the given project
(Mao et al., 2001; Marcus, 2005). It is only through practice in developing and hands on experience in
implementing these techniques that designers acquire their own combinations of tools and techniques
optimal for the constraints and requirements of the target problem and goals. Those who are highly
skilled in UCD demonstrate the ability to adapt techniques to the constraints of a given domain
and situation. UCD is an exercise in improvization of the well-established, tested techniques used to
acquire and integrate user characteristics, in order to adapt these techniques concurrently as the
requirements of the design evolve. As such, this chapter provides a high-level roadmap for the UCD
process, and introduces readers to fundamental methods for incorporating the user into the UCD
process. An operational definition is provided of UCD, followed by two sections, which emphasize
common methods of user involvement in the design process. The first section details requirements
gathering, and the second underscores user involvement in the testing and evaluation of designs
and prototypes.
Readers unfamiliar with this vision of product development should refer to Mayhew (1999, 2003) for
further details.
UCD focuses on incorporating the user, task, and environment into the design process. While the user
lies at the center of the design process, they are part of an intricate network consisting of workplace-,
environment-, and organization-related factors. Critical information about the users is gathered and
applied in the formulation of design requirements, as well as the continued evaluation of design alterna-
tives and final implementation. Figure 7.2 illustrates this high level conceptualization of UCD.
Eason (1995) describes three overarching goals of UCD, which include: (1) the translation of articu-
lated user/context/work needs into overall product requirements and design specifications; (2) the pro-
duction of several design options; and (3) the evaluation of the degree to which the design options fulfill
the requirements. With this view of the system design (or redesign) and development process in mind,
the primary activities of UCD can be divided into two phases. The first phase involves the front-end
development of an appropriate understanding and representation of the system users, their goals,
their need, their work tools, and their work (i.e., “understanding the work system”). The second
phase involves the back-end testing and evaluation of proposed design solutions to examine and validate
this front-end work and its interpretation (i.e., “testing and evaluation”). These two primary phases of
the UCD process are shaded in Figure 7.2.
UCD mandates that inquiries be made into user and work conditions. In order to create appropriate
systems for users, the needs of the users should be at the forefront of the process (Sugar, 2001). This
FIGURE 7.2 Illustration of the UCD process and the key steps of user input or feedback.
User-Centered Design of Information Technology 7-7
facilitates the derivation of design goals, creates a formal account of the design’s requirements, and trans-
lates these into a tangible list of specifications, or action items to inform design. Intuitively, the next step
in UCD is the application of the specifications to the actual system design. Once system designs have been
developed through a process of prototyping, these potential designs will be tested and evaluated for their
appropriateness, utility, and usability. Finally, when a good design results, a full working system will be
implemented and released.
Additionally, as can be seen in Figure 7.2, this process is highly iterative with continual verification and
validation of decisions and progress along the path to implementing designs. As has been commonly out-
lined in the usability engineering or design “lifecycles” (see Mayhew, 1999; e.g., Nielsen, 1993; Samaras
and Horst, 2005), there are several feedback loops in the process, often heavily involving users as sources
for validation and testing. While this concept of iterative design will not be heavily discussed in this
chapter, it is one of the cornerstone principles of UCD and the software and usability engineering
lifecycles.
The UCD approach just looks beyond principles of physical design — beyond traditional ergonomics
— to consider more fundamental issues such as the structure of information presented, the degree of
process automation, implications of skill transfer and retention, and training requirements. If operators
and supervisors are involved only at the later phases of the design process, then the final product may be
difficult to learn, and incompatible with the existing well-established working practices, while also trig-
gering increases in vigilance tasks and diminished user acceptance and trust (Kontogiannis and Embrey,
1997).
The result is a set of integrated techniques, which combine users’ assertions with observations across
several levels of the organization, and several classes of users and stakeholders. Damodaran (1996) has
suggested a well-defined taxonomy of the roles of individuals involved in the design process and the
identification and selection of the most representative users from the organization. In fact, users
should be identified at several levels from within an organization, ranging from end users to middle-
and top-level management. This focus on all stakeholders is because the outcomes of technical design
decisions may have profound implications on job design, and subsequently working life (Damodaran,
1996; Damodaran et al., 1980).
7.2.2 Summary
This chapter will focus on the inclusion of users at the two phases in the design process during which user
feedback can have the greatest influence on its eventual success. This includes, as illustrated by Figure 7.2,
understanding the work system and the testing and evaluation of designs and prototypes. The integration
of specifications into tangible designs will briefly be touched on, as a more complete explanation is out of
the scope of a manuscript of this length. The actual design production step is best learnt through practice
and case studies of the experiences of others. In a study of the efficacy of novice designers to UCD, Sugar
(2001) investigated the challenges that novice designers face in the application of UCD principles and
guidelines. These designers were observed to have incomplete mental models of the translation of
requirements into creative design solutions and tended to focus on overt observations of user needs.
In this chapter, readers will be provided with guidance on how to incorporate users into and leverage
their knowledge and insight during the UCD process. UCD is partly a structured methodology and partly
a skillful improvization. Readers should recognize the importance of practical experience and reviewing
actual design case studies, both grounded in empirically validated guidelines and underlying philos-
ophies of UCD in order to achieve effective designs. In UCD, those designing the system are responsible
for: (1) facilitating the task or work for the user; (2) ensuring that the user can use the product as it was
intended to be; and (3) making certain that the training and learning required to use the product is
minimized.
7.3.1 Overview
There are, in both literature and practice, several well-documented and commonly applied methods for
gathering user information and including users in the design and evaluation processes to achieve UCD.
The International Organization for Standardization (ISO) in an attempt to consolidate these methods
has published principles of UCD and steps required in the UCD development cycle (ISO 13407,
1999). In his article entitled “Methods to support Human-Centered Design,” Maguire (2001) provides
an overview of the main UCD principles and an extensive list of methodologies appropriate for each
step in the UCD process.
There are a seemingly infinite number of approaches and frameworks for UCD. These range from
general principles (e.g., active user participation or iterative design and testing) to specific tools [e.g.,
the Unified Modeling Language (Booch et al., 1998; Rumbaugh et al., 2004) or hierarchical task analysis
(Shepherd, 2000, 2001)] or techniques [e.g., contextual design (Beyer and Holtzblatt, 1998; Holtzblatt,
User-Centered Design of Information Technology 7-9
2003) or ISO 13407 (ISO, 1999) or 18529 (ISO, 2000)] to amalgamations of any and all approaches that
suit the needs and purposes of the UCD team. However, despite this considerable variability in the tech-
niques and approaches one might use or the design philosophy one might subscribe to, there are
common threads linking all of these methods and techniques to one common goal. This goal, of
course, is ensuring that the customers’, users’, and stakeholders’ needs and goals are met in the design
of a system or tool.
One of the reasons that the UCD process is such a nebulous topic to summarize is the variability
among approaches and the differences imposed by context disparities. As is well known by all those
who practice UCD, the ideal circumstances for UCD, such as complete user buy-in, unlimited resources,
unlimited time, and unlimited access to users and their work never actually exist in practice. As such,
UCD practitioners often pick their preferred techniques or tools and go about the business of designing
or redesigning work systems for users. This being said, this chapter will discuss the UCD process in fairly
broad terms, focusing more on the general ideas, principles, purposes, and methods or tools of UCD.
As a preface to this discussion, readers should note that UCD practitioners (e.g., ergonomists, human
factors engineers, psychologists, ethnographers, designers, developers, etc.) often deal with a whole host
of constraints, limitations, and obstacles to overcome in real-world domains. This being said, we begin
the discussion of UCD and the process by which practitioners can translate an understanding of users,
their needs, and their work into appropriate and successful system design solutions.
involves a number of activities, such as discovering the purpose for which the system is intended,
identifying the system stakeholders and their needs, and (finally) translating and documenting
these needs into a form that can be communicated, validated, and implemented (Nuseibeh and
Easterbrook, 2000). RE is, arguably the seminal activity in the UCD process, because it represents the
initial step in involving the users (and their needs) into the design process and, ultimately, serves as
the blueprints for the designs that are to be tested and evaluated in later phases of the system develop-
ment process.
As alluded to, RE is a process, meaning that it is (necessarily) somewhat ill-defined, fluid, and complex.
This process involves any number of activities, ranging from simply defining the purpose of the system or
goals of the customer or organization to diligently observing and modeling the actual work of the end-
users. As all of the experts agree, there is no best way of conducting the RE process, just as there is no
single or best way to define and document the requirements that result from the process.
While there are considerable issues regarding the practice of RE, such as requirements validation,
change management, documentation styles, and traceability, this discussion falls outside of the scope
of this chapter. Readers can be directed to any number of books and reports that deal with these delicate
issues (e.g., Christel and Kang, 1992; Ransley, 2003; Rosenberg, 1999; Young, 2001). Some of these issues,
such as insuring that representative users are chosen, will be discussed in relation to the activities at the
various stages depicted in Figure 7.3. There are a number of good texts detailing the process of and con-
siderations associated with the RE process, including those by Wiegers (2003), Robertson and Robertson
(1999), and Young (2001, 2004).
The real purpose of the RE process is to minimize problems resulting from costly and destructive mis-
understandings or misinterpretation between the stakeholders or users and the design and development
team (Ransley, 2003; Sommerville and Sawyer, 1997). The RE process is able to limit these types of pro-
blems by utilizing systematic and structured process from the start of the system development process
before making definitive judgments or assumptions about design alternatives or work system needs.
Overall, RE is able to provide a better understanding of the system to be developed, help communication
between users and the design and development team, minimize risk and maximize acceptance through
good structuring, clear language, clear linkages between requirements and documented work system
needs, and direct user involvement.
In more concrete terms, RE is the process of translating knowledge of users, their needs, and their
work, and so on into specific, tangible requirements of the system to be designed and developed. As a
simple summary, the RE process is utilized to produce system requirements in such a way that represen-
tative, accurate, and complete work system data are provided in a feasible, affordable, and ethical manner.
We will now turn to the primary component or outcome of the RE process — the system and design
requirements.
organizational culture, and work domain (e.g., medicine versus commercial logistics versus fast-food),
the nature of the requirements and the language used to express them can vary considerably.
Regardless of the method of creation or language, there are some established criteria for system
requirements. Good system requirements possess the qualities or properties of specificity, feasibility,
testability, clarity, comprehensiveness, consistency, exclusivity, traceability, accuracy, and so on (Ransley,
2003). The properties comprise the basis for good systems requirements as they specify the guidelines to
ensure that the requirements are appropriate and effective for guiding UCD. It should also be mentioned
that there are also a number of different types of requirements.
Design requirements can range from general requirements, which set out what the system should do in
broad terms, to performance requirements, which specify a minimum acceptable performance level of the
system (Kotonya and Sommerville, 1998; Ransley, 2003; Sommerville and Sawyer, 1997). Traditionally,
system requirements govern functional capabilities rather than nonfunctional requirements such as issues
of human cognition and capabilities, usability, individual differences and preferences, and social and organ-
izational concerns (see Dix et al., 1998, for a good overview of this philosophy and appropriate techniques).
Not surprisingly, there is little consensus on the number and nomenclature regarding the classification
of requirements. Generally speaking, there are two major classes of requirements: (1) functional require-
ments, which outline the things that the system must do (e.g., services and functionality to provide); and
(2) nonfunctional requirements, which outline the properties that the system must have (e.g., usability,
efficiency, reliability, etc.) (Robertson and Robertson, 1999; Young, 2004). Table 7.2 outlines some of
these broad classes of system requirements. For a more comprehensive examination of requirement
classification and delineation, readers can refer to texts by Young (2001, 2004) and Kovitz (1998).
As can be seen, requirements represent the collective set of issues, problems, needs, and constraints of the
work system that need to be met by the new design or tool. With the concepts of the RE process and the
requirements established, it is appropriate to discuss the specific activities of this process and how to equate
it to requirements. These activities effectively supply the data on which the requirements are constructed.
While this division of approaches is not absolute, it does distinguish that there are clear differences
between these two types of approaches. For example, elicitation techniques are generally (arguably)
quick preliminary techniques, which involve direct interaction with the users to yield rough data that
often requires considerable interpretation, refinement, and validation. On the other hand, analysis
and modeling techniques are often more extensive follow-up efforts that are used to represent this
rough data in a manner that can be more easily communicated and interpreted and applied. For these
reasons, the following discussion will consider elicitation techniques (Stage 2: Examining the work
system) and analysis and modeling techniques (Stage 3: Representing the work system and its needs)
as independent stages in the RE process as shown in Figure 7.3.
Before talking about these approaches and techniques in greater detail, Table 7.3 outlines many of the
popular techniques of requirements elicitation and analysis/modeling commonly employed by UCD
professionals. For greater detail on these (and other) methods and techniques, readers should
examine some of the literature (e.g., Beyer and Holtzblatt, 1998; Maguire, 2001; Sommerville and
Sawyer, 1997) and Websites (http://www.usabilitynet.org; http://usability.jameshom.com/index.htm)
that provide detailed insight into different UCD techniques.
7.3.2.1.5 Selection of UCD Methods and Techniques
It is well-known that the UCD approach taken and the methods and tools chosen heavily depend on the
constraints of a given project-time, money, staffing, and accessibility to users. Readers will find that even
expert practitioners adopt their own unique combination of the techniques, as suits the needs and work
within the constraints of a project. In a recent article, Marcus (2005) summarized the factors that mostly
affect collection of UCD techniques used. These factors include: (1) availability of testing labs and equip-
ment; (2) availability of usability professionals; (3) availability of users; (4) budget; and (5) calendar
schedule.
A survey of UCD professionals showed that the cost-benefit tradeoff was instrumental in the consider-
ation of which UCD method, if any would be implemented in a given design process (Mao et al., 2001).
TABLE 7.3 Common Requirements Elicitation and Requirements Analysis and Modeling Techniques
Requirements Elicitation Requirements Analysis and Modeling
RE stage
Stage 2: Examining the work system Stage 3: Representing the work system and its needs
Approach summary
Gathering or capturing system requirements through Defining system requirements through the development of
direct observation or interaction with stakeholders models based on the data collected using elicitation
or end-users techniques and documentation
Characteristics
Ethnographic in nature Analytical in nature
Direct interaction with users Direct involvement with data
Results in rough data Refined representations of users, needs and tasks
Relatively quick and simple Time and resource intensive
Requires more interpretation Requires less interpretation
Preliminary activities
Common techniques
Surveys and questionnaires Use cases/scenarios
Interviews Visioning
Observation/ethnography Personas/user profiles
Contextual inquiry/user interview Error and critical incident analysis
Focus groups and brainstorming Affinity diagramming
Think aloud Work modeling
Laddering and card sorting Decision/action diagrams
Documentation and product review Cognitive work/task analysis
7-14 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools For Occupational Ergonomics
According to the 103 survey respondents, the top five UCD methods easily applied in practice included:
(1) informal usability testing, (2) user analysis and profiling, (3) evaluations of existing systems, (4)
low-fidelity prototyping, (5) heuristic evaluations, (6) task identification, (7) navigation design, and (8)
scenario-based design. To summarize, practitioners tend to use those methods that are most flexible, infor-
mal, and least structured. The use of these “low hanging fruit” UCD methods reflects that in practice,
actives are in fact driven by time, money, and the attention, which the process is given by key stakeholders.
7.3.2.1.6 Summary
While not all of these techniques and tools will be discussed in detail, representative methods that are
particularly popular with UCD practitioners will be chosen for review. These tools and techniques
will be summarized, followed by a summary section discussing how ergonomics, human factors, and
HCI researchers and practitioners can get from this initial phase of understanding users and their
work to leveraging and incorporating this new knowledge into the actual design of work tools.
As will be seen, all of these methodologies and techniques, while somewhat interrelated and in some
ways redundant, comprise the initial activities of the UCD process (i.e., understanding the work system).
All of these methods can be used to help examine, understand, and model the users, their needs, and their
work. It is this intimate knowledge of the work system that helps to drive ergonomic design. These user-
centered methods, techniques, and activities directly concern the documentation or modeling of the user
and their work. At the heart of all of the aforementioned methods and techniques is the focus on human
users — their abilities, needs, work context, and work.
7.3.2.2.4 Summary
Arguably, the contextual design process will not always be the best or most appropriate method of RE and
UCD. For example, contextual design is particularly well suited for software-focused, computer-based
systems, which are fairly radical upgrades to the previous legacy systems that exist within the organiz-
ation. However, it is a good example of a work system design process that stresses the need to incorporate
the user into the design process and ensure that the needs and work processes of the user will be appro-
priately addressed in design solutions.
define some classes of system requirements as discussed in Table 7.2, primarily including organizational
requirements, general constraints on the system or process, and definition of the overall purpose and
business goals to be met by the system. Thus, the project planning activities are important not only
for ensuring project success, but also help to define the requirements.
Stakeholder Analysis Stakeholder analysis, or the stakeholder meeting refers to the general process of
identifying those individuals within the work system that are affected by the development of a new
system and getting their input and buy- in on the development process. In the systems development
(or RE) process, stakeholders generally refer to any individuals who affect or are affected by the
system of interest (see Sharp et al., 1999, for a good review of stakeholder theory). In practice, this
often includes business managers, project managers, user representatives, training and support staff,
developers, etc. Stakeholder analysis is a technique used to identify, assess, and prioritize the needs,
goals, and requirements of the stakeholders (Damodaran et al., 1980). As stakeholders, by definition,
have the ability to significantly influence the success of the project, stakeholder analysis is also an
opportunity to establish buy- in and agreement at the onset of the project.
Stakeholder analysis helps to define the work system through the identification of the overall goals,
requirements, constraints, and needs related to the system to be developed. This process helps to
define some of the overarching organizational goals and business requirements that will help to scope
and define the system requirements. Additionally, usability analysis and the establishment and agreement
of usability priorities and goals, which are translated into usability requirements, can also be conducted
during this time (Maguire, 2001). The overall goal of stakeholder analysis is to bring together all relevant
parties to create a common vision of the overall purpose, scope, constraints, and high-level requirements
of the system to be developed.
Context and Domain Analysis Context or domain analysis refers to the general process of collecting
information about the context of use in which the system will be used. This context of use idea is similar
to our idea of the work system, as described previously. The basic goal is to gather stakeholders and
domain experts together to identify information such as identifying the users and work tasks, as well as
laying out the technical and environmental constraints of the work system (Bevan et al., 1996; Maguire,
2001). Context (of use) and domain analysis is also helpful in familiarizing the UCD team with
important background knowledge of application domain considerations and terminology. As
previously discussed, a firm understanding of domain-specific issues is necessary for the development
of meaningful system requirements.
More specifically with respect to the development and delineation of usability needs and requirements,
Bevan et al. (1996) and Bevan and Macleod (1994) have developed a structured method, called Usability
Context Analysis, which is used to elicit details from stakeholders about a potential system and how it
will be used in context. The method results in a list of important characteristics of systems’ users,
their work tasks, and the context of work. This list serves as a framework to ensure that all factors affect-
ing system usability have been identified. Context, or context of use, analysis is also often the spawning
ground for associated RE activities such as analysis of competitive or existing system (Preece et al., 1994)
and usability planning (Mayhew, 1999).
User-Centered Design of Information Technology 7-17
7.3.2.3.3 Summary
As suggested by the term UCD, the key to incorporating the needs of users into the design of systems,
tools, or interfaces lies in developing an understanding of the needs of the users being designed for.
This includes examination of their work, their objectives, their activities, their (cap)abilities, their differ-
ences, their environment, their constraints, etc. In essence, this is developing a picture of the work
system as previously discussed. While the techniques discussed in the initial stage of defining the
work system needs do serve to information system requirements, this is not the complete picture. As
is well-acknowledged in the scientific and practical knowledge base, there are limitations to merely
collecting a group of experts or stakeholders together and asking questions, as users cannot necessarily
vocalize or make their understanding of their own work explicit — especially out of context. Karen
Holtzblatt, cocreator of the popular contextual design approach to system design, elucidates this point
well: “. . . requirements gathering is not simply a matter of asking people what they need. A product is
always part of a larger work practice. It is used in the context of other tools and manual processes.
Product design is fundamentally about the redesign of work or life practice, given the technological
possibility. Work practice cannot be designed well if it is not understood in detail” (2003, p. 944). In
response to this phenomenon, researchers and practitioners in ergonomics, human factors, psychology,
cognitive science, systems engineering, and HCI have developed knowledge elicitation techniques to help
augment this understanding of the work system.
An important outcome of this stage is defining the problem that is to be solved, which is often done
through the definition of boundaries. This is an important activity within Stage 1 efforts as the definition
of the problem space with boundaries helps to define the scope of the design to be implemented. As noted
by Nuseibeh and Easterbrook (2000), the nature of the boundaries chosen affects all subsequent require-
ments elicitation efforts (Stage 2), as it affects the identification and selection of stakeholders and user
cases, the identification of applicable goals, and the choice of tasks to observe. Once the boundaries
are drawn, various goals and constraints settled upon by the primary stakeholders and the general
model of the work system (including user, context, and work) envision, the process of examining the
work system and collecting data begins. Generally, this process begins with the use of requirements
elicitation techniques.
Focus Groups and Brainstorming Focus groups and brainstorming are two cheap, easy, and popular
ways that UCD professionals directly interact with stakeholders or users to collect data that can be
used to understand the work system and define appropriate requirements. The basic concept of focus
groups and brainstorming is to gain insight into the real events of work by providing a loosely
moderated forum in which stakeholders or users can collectively discuss aspects of their work. Focus
groups tend to be more rigidly designed and moderated to help keep the participants on the focus
(Caplan, 1990; Kontio et al., 2004), while brainstorming techniques tend to allow for more leeway in
the scope of the discussion — as long as it remains on the focus. Additionally, these techniques are
also good for achieving internal buy-in and support by providing stakeholders with the forum to
provide input and collectively agree on what aspects of the work system are important.
Another derivative of focus groups and brainstorming, which is commonly used, are requirements
workshops. These techniques use the “whole is more than the sum of its parts” principle of collecting
stakeholders and the design team together in a shared space with a shared purpose of defining design
requirements (Gottesdiener, 2002). The output of these meetings includes use cases, business rules,
system criteria, and various models of the work system. These requirements meetings and workshops
are often very efficient and useful methods of data collection and requirements gathering.
Contextual Inquiry: User Interviews in Context It is likely that the majority of readers will be familiar
with the concept of contextual inquiry, or will have been introduced already with the previous
discussion of contextual design. Contextual inquiry is a well-formulated interview technique
popularized by Beyer and Holtzblatt (1998) as the primary data collection tools of their Contextual
Design approach to system design. As the authors explain, “The core premise of Contextual Inquiry is
very simple: go where the customer works, observe the customer as he or she works, and talk to the
customer about the work” (1998, p. 41).
While this method is effectively a user interview (another common requirements elicitation tech-
nique), it is actually more akin to ethnography (Blomberg et al., 2003; Saferstein, 1998) applying the
principles of being anchored in natural settings, focusing on the holistic context, providing descriptive
(rather than prescriptive) understanding of events, and using the point-of-view of the individual
being observed. While there is interaction between the interviewer and the interviewee, this is largely
to clarify the interpretations of the interviewee’s words and actions and to help keep the interviewee
vocal and focused on the project scope.
In addition, contextual inquiry is both structured and unstructured as the creators have outlined
several principles and procedures to follow, yet the bulk of the process is the naturalistic observation
of user work. As has been outlined by its creators, contextual inquiry has defined philosophies including
the “Master/Apprentice” model of the relationship between the customer and the designer as well as the
four steps of the interview process — the conventional interview, the transition, the contextual interview
proper, and the wrap-up, wherein each has its own prescribed methods and suggestions (Beyer and
Holtzblatt, 1995, 1998; Holtzblatt and Jones, 1993). The end result of the contextual inquiry process
is a very rich set of notes and observations, which are then used to build models to represent this
interpretation of the work system. These analyses and modeling techniques along with other popular
tools for representing the work system will be discussed in the next section.
7.3.2.4.2 Summary
As can be understood from the discussion, requirements elicitation techniques are used to gather inform-
ation on the work system including the users, their goals, their tasks, their work context, their abilities,
their needs, etc. However, as discussed, the information gathered through requirements elicitation tech-
niques is often rough, verbose, and overbearing for immediate use. As such, this data needs to be
arranged, interpreted, analyzed, and modeled to achieve the end goal of creating system requirements
(Nuseibeh and Easterbrook, 2000). Thus, researchers and practitioners developed analysis and modeling
methods to help wrangle this work system data into a form that was more amenable to communication,
study, interpretation, and translation. We refer to these techniques as requirements analysis and model-
ing tools.
User-Centered Design of Information Technology 7-19
Contextual Design Work Models and the User Environment Design (UED) As previously mentioned,
contextual design includes a fairly well-defined set of analysis of modeling tools, which are used to
represent the free-form data and observations that are collected during the contextual inquiry (Beyer
and Holtzblatt, 1998). According to the contextual design protocol, five work models are generated
during an interpretation session from the collective observation data. These models include:
. The artifact model, which represents the composition, structure, and usage of physical work
artifacts
. The cultural model that represents the influences on the user including all organizational partner-
ships, the hierarchical structure of power, business policies, and standards, etc
. The flow model that represents the users’ responsibilities and duties and the communication and
collaboration related to carrying out these duties
. The physical model that represents the actual work environment including the physical layout, the
environmental constraints, the organization of business objects, etc
. The sequence model that represents the procedures and actions as well as their sequence required to
perform specific work tasks
In addition to these work models, the raw data from the contextual inquiry is also used to build an
affinity diagram, which uses a bottom-up approach to link all of the insights gained during the interview
into one hierarchical representation that can be used to reveal common issues and themes to be addressed
(Beyer and Holtzblatt, 1998). This affinity diagram and the work models are then used to develop an idea
of the work redesign necessary for the work system and, finally, through a process of visioning and story-
boards, develops the UED (Beyer and Holtzblatt, 1998). The UED is composed of focus areas represent-
ing the system components that support specific work activities (the purpose), in which the system
functions and links to other focus areas are linked. Using the UED and the previous models and materials
constructed, the team can derive system requirements and specifications through linking the sequence
and structure of the new work system model (the UED and other models) to needed system functions
and behaviors.
7-20 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools For Occupational Ergonomics
Personas and Use Case Models Two more recent requirements analysis and modeling techniques that are
commonly used are personas and use cases (or use case models). Personas, which were first introduced in
a remarkable book by Alan Cooper (2004), are becoming a wildly popular technique for UCD. Personas
are typically built from data collected in ethnographic interviews (e.g., contextual inquiry) and are
expressed in 1 to 2 page descriptions that include a general profile, behavior patterns, goals, skills,
attitudes, and environment. A persona is a user archetype that is based on a collection of job
responsibilities and goals (Pruitt and Grudin, 2003). Personas help to enhance the utility of scenarios
and other user-centered techniques such as participatory design by strengthening the focus on
realistic users and work contexts — albeit through a fictionalized setting (Grudin and Pruitt, 2002;
Pruitt and Grudin, 2003). In this way, personas are really user models, which can then be used to
guide and enhance other techniques such as use case models.
Use case models are largely popularized through their use in the UML method (Booch et al., 1998;
Rumbaugh et al., 2004) and are used to capture the system requirements through a narrative-like
format of the steps to perform the tasks, the tasks to be performed, and the system’s behavior for
each task (Cockburn, 2000; Kulak and Guiney, 2004). The use cases are generated from work scenarios
created through the requirements elicitation processes such as user interviews and brainstorming. Use
cases essentially represent a goal-oriented set of interactions between the users (actors) and the system
to be used and describe the sequence of the user’s actions and the system’s reactions. In this way, each
use case presents the system functionality needed to support user work for a given task and goal.
7.3.2.5.2 Summary
If any reader happens to already know UCD or system engineering practitioners, there are a lot of other
popular methods, many of which include structural diagramming methods such as task analysis or hier-
archical task analysis (Hackos and Redish, 1998; Shepherd, 2000, 2001) and work domain or cognitive
work analysis (Rasmussen, 1986; Vicente, 1999), just to name a few. Researchers and practitioners
have developed analysis and modeling methods and tools to represent nearly all imaginable aspects of
the work system from models representing the organizational culture and policies of the work system
[the cultural model of contextual design (Beyer and Holtzblatt, 1998)] to models representing
complex tasks in terms of goals and subgoals with associated plans for carrying out those subgoals [hier-
archical task analysis (Shepherd, 2001)].
However, a discussion of all of these work system analyses and modeling techniques could itself
produce a multi-volume handbook. As can be seen, work system analysis and modeling techniques
are used to create fairly concise, yet complex representations of the work system. While each of these
analysis and modeling tools might represent completely different aspects of the work system, they are
all used to provide a representation of the work system that can be used to generate ideas of (primarily)
what the system is supposed to do and (to some extent) how the system should go about supporting these
goals and needs. In other words, work system analyses and modeling tools are used to provide a basis to
form system requirements.
requirements. The translation of this collected information into an actual list of requirements is very
complex and nuanced. It is often an art as much as it is a science (although RE theoreticians may
cringe at the thought). There is no prescribed or preferred methodology for this process, rather
several useful summaries of this process (Nuseibeh and Easterbrook, 2000; Ransley, 2003; Wiegers,
2003; Young, 2004).
Fortunately, however, even Karl Wiegers (a noted RE heavyweight) acknowledges that RE is primarily a
communication activity rather than a technical activity (Wiegers, 2000). A number of guidelines can help
make the requirements formulation considerably less random and more manageable. Many of these
guidelines lay out general principles and processes for generating requirements (Alexander and
Stevens, 2002; Robertson and Robertson, 1999), while others actually propose standardized guidelines
for the process and specification (IEEE, 1998a, b).
As a reminder, requirements are descriptions or statements of system services, functions, properties,
attributes, or constraints laying out how a system should behave within its context of use to help
perform work (Kotonya and Sommerville, 1998; Nuseibeh and Easterbrook, 2000; Ransley, 2003;
Sommerville and Sawyer, 1997). Requirements are typically a mixture of some problem information,
statements of system behavior and properties, and design and manufacturing constraints (Sommerville
and Sawyer, 1997). In a somewhat sensible, yet magical way, these requirements naturally “drop out” of
the RE process as the requirements elicitation and analysis and modeling tools are used.
For example, the project planning and stakeholders’ meetings generally set out the overall business
goals to be met by the system as well as any business requirements and constraints and general usability
requirements. The actual nascence of a requirement lies in an idea that is generated from the study of the
work system (e.g., activities of Stages 1 and 2). Once the goals and needs of the work system are elicited
and modeled, the defining goals (e.g., the new system should be usable) or needs (e.g., the new system
needs to interface with legacy systems) should be broken down into atomic, defined problems, put in a
natural language format, described, and referenced back to the actual data or model that spawned the
requirement. For example, in the contextual design process (Beyer and Holtzblatt, 1998; Holtzblatt,
2003), it is suggested that the requirements be referenced back to the part of the UED or storyboard
that outlines the need for a system function, component, or property and also attaches a list of any
relevant data used (e.g., sections of the affinity diagram or pieces of the consolidated models).
These statements or descriptions serve as specifications of what should be implemented. As the RE
process, or even how the results are communicated, is often a function of the nature and culture of
the audience of the RE results (i.e., the requirements) and the domain in which the system is being
implemented (Kotonya and Sommerville, 1998; Sommerville and Sawyer, 1997), the establishment of
the actual system requirement is a highly variable and flexible process. However, as Ransley (2003)
notes, having an actual process is prerequisite to control and repeatability allowing the ability to
refine the process, improve the knowledge, and increase the skill in turning the identified problems
into appropriate solutions. The RE process also provides a clear auditing trail allowing the traceability
of design decisions back to system requirements and, ultimately, to the identified problems that
needed to be addressed. Arguably, this concept of traceability is of utmost importance as it allows the
design decisions, which have been based on system requirements and specifications to be traced back
to actual work system needs and goals, which were explained and enumerated in the system requirements
and specifications.
Along with the requirements document, which explicitly lays out the system specifications, the models
and representations of the work system are used to guide the potential design alternatives. For example,
personas, story boards, use cases, and decision/action diagrams could be used to help envision the order
and organization in which the system provides content or functionality in order to support the natural
workflow of the end-user. However, this process requires a clear and accurate interpretation of the data
and creative ingenuity to meet all of the functional and nonfunctional requirements, while working
within the constraints of the organization or environment. In fact, the boundaries and limitations
created by constraints on the system help design by reducing the number of possible design alternatives
(e.g., if the system needs to be implemented in JavaTM). If a design solution meets all of these require-
ments and can be elegant and appealing to users, then designers have truly earned their keep.
As mentioned previously, the transition from requirements to tangible design is somewhat nebulous.
Design solutions arise from the requirements in many ways, but progress and mature through iterative
development. Designers and their teams should accept a process of iterative prototyping, developing first
a low-fidelity mockup, and iteratively integrating feedback from evaluations of the prototype into
redesigns incrementally until a final working simulation is produced. Ideally, the design process
should gradually iterate on several prototypes, although this may necessarily be realistically feasible or
affordable given the constraints and resources associated with the project. At the minimum, however,
the process should incorporate at least one low-cost low- fidelity prototype in the design of a final
operational system. In fact, the value of design information garnered from the testing and evaluation
of low-fidelity, low-cost prototypes lies in the considerable positive influence of this collected insight
on the ultimate production of usable, useful, attractive final designs (Hall, 2001).
have resources available for and, which will generate the type of information needed to inform the itera-
tive design.
The other methods mentioned by Maguire (2001), which relate more to processes for generating initial
prototypes include brainstorming, parallel design, and the use of design guidelines and standards.
Table 7.4 provides operational definitions for these nine design activities.
7.3.2.6.4 For Consideration in UCD: Standards and Guidelines
Perhaps more clear than the typical interpretation of the work system models to guide design decisions,
designers can also use established standards and guidelines to help constrain and define design decisions.
Standards are documented agreements on technical specifications, aimed at ensuring quality control and
abiding legislation or regulations related to design (Sherehiy et al., 2005). Standards may be international,
national and regional with international standards being the most expansive (Sherehiy et al., 2005).
Sherehiy et al. (2005) in their extensive account of human factors and ergonomics standards, summarize
ergonomic standards put forth by the ISO, the European Committee for Standardization (CEN), the
International Labor Organization, (ILO), the U.S. Government, the Occupation Safety and Health
Administration (OSHA), the American National Standards Institute (ANSI), and other standards
created at the state level as well as by individual organizations.
These standards, while all focus on human factors and ergonomics also deal with a range of human
capabilities and environments providing instructions for dealing with the physical world and interface
design. For example, in terms of user interface standards, a number of conventions have been developed.
The ISO has also fairly recently developed checklists based on standards and principles of user guidance
and dialog as subsets of the overarching ISO 9241 Standard on Ergonomics Requirements for Visual
Display Terminals (ISO, 1998). Sample checklists associated with these standards are provided by
Stewart and Travis (2003) in their chapter on “Guidelines, Standards, and Style Guides” in Jacko and
Sears’ (2003), The Handbook of Human –Computer Interaction.
In addition to these international standards, there are several other sets of guidelines that have been
established by developers, HCI researchers, and usability professionals (e.g., Apple Computer, 2003;
Microsoft Corporation, 2004; W3C, 2004). With so many standards and legal requirements promoting
accessibility, designers must have guidance in how to incorporate these requirements into the main-
stream UCD process. A comprehensive review of accessibility, universal design standards, and techniques
for how to meet these requirements can be found in work by Preiser and Ostroff (2001).
However, unless the design team has a substantial portfolio of experience in a specific design domain,
the translation of specifications into tangible systems is typically a challenge, for which there is little gui-
dance (Ostroff, 2001). That being said, a more effective approach is the generation of several design
alternatives and creating low-fidelity mock-ups of these alternatives. Through testing and evaluation,
the design alternatives are evaluated to verify the degree to which they address the initially derived
requirements. Based on the evaluation, designs are altered and iteratively evaluated with prototypes of
increased fidelity until the final optimal design is ready for implementation.
TABLE 7.5 Summary of Three Popular Collections of Design Principles and Guidelines
Eight Golden Rules of Design Design Heuristics (Nielsen, 1994; Seven Principles of Design
(Shneiderman, 1998) Nielsen and Molich, 1990) (Norman, 2002)
Strive for consistency Visibility of system status Use of knowledge in the world and
knowledge in the head
Enable frequent users to use shortcuts Match between system and the real Use mental aides and consistency in
world information presentation
Offer informative feedback User control and freedom Make things visible, enabling the user to
figure out the use of an object by
seeing the buttons/devices required
for operations
Design dialog to yield closure Consistency and standards Get mappings right and easily
understood
Offer simple error handling Error prevention Use constraints to direct users’ actions
Permit easy reversal of actions Recognition rather than recall Design for error, planning for the
recovery from any possible error
Support internal locus of control Flexibility and efficiency of use Standardize (when everything
else fails) in order to avoid
arbitrary mappings
Reduce short-term memory load Aesthetic and minimalist design help
users to recognize, diagnose, and
recover from errors help and
documentation
user, work, organization, and context to generate a prioritization of usability goals, which then informs
the relative importance of guidelines and leads to the integration of the most appropriate guidelines for
the situation at hand.
7.3.2.7 Summary
As has been discussed, the process of translating the understanding of the work system and its needs into
tangible system designs can be best characterized as highly variable or nebulous. A considerable amount
of interpretation goes into this process of first creating the system requirements and specifications along
with considerable creativity and ingenuity to translate these specifications into actual designs. While
there are several documented methods for this procedure along with design standards and guidelines
and design principles to help guide this process, it is still an art in further need or standardization
through advances in research and science.
This section has provided a cursory overview of some of the popular methods and techniques for the
elicitation, representation, and development of system requirements. It is from this collected knowledge
represented in lists, diagrams, and models that UCD emerges. All of the methods and techniques dis-
cussed, whether they be ethnographic in nature (e.g., contextual inquiry) or modeling tools (e.g., hier-
archical task analysis), have one common link — an implicit focus on users, their needs, and their work.
All of these methods and techniques also have a common goal — the development of the system require-
ments, which will be used to guide the development of UCD alternatives.
As discussed, these methods and techniques used to understand the work system fall within the pre-
liminary phase of the UCD lifecycle (i.e., defining, examining, and representing the work system and its
needs). The knowledge gained from these initial efforts is the fuel used to elicit, understand, and develop
the functional and nonfunctional system requirements. These requirements are then translated into
system engineering specifications, which represent the blueprints for the design and development
process of work tools (i.e., systems, applications, interfaces, etc.). However, it is this understanding of
users and their work that ensures that the system requirements generated are appropriate and useful.
If these requirements are incorrect, then the specifications used to guide the system design and
7-26 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools For Occupational Ergonomics
development will be incorrect and the system or tool will ultimately fall short of good UCD. Thus, the
importance of these initial stages of UCD should be clear.
However, simply performing the user and work analysis and modeling stages is not enough. For
example, the “requirements analysis” stage (Mayhew, 2003) is a necessary, but not sufficient step to
ensure effective and useful designs within the usability engineering lifecycle. In a way, this initial
process of UCD is focused on solving the correct problem. That is, the RE process is directed at identifying
the needs and problems within the work system that the new system is being designed to solve. The fol-
lowing section, which presents some representative and popular testing and evaluation methods for UCD
is focused on solving the problem correctly.
evaluation techniques appropriate at different levels of design fidelity from concept through implemen-
tation. From this study, it is presumed that the ease with which an evaluation method is applied is inter-
related with the level of abstraction required of those conducting the assessment to conceptualize the
design and tasks interactions. Those prototypes that have a physical presence and are highly tangible
maintain more flexibility in applicable evaluation techniques than prototypes of a more conceptual
nature (e.g., Wizard-of-Oz methods or paper-based prototypes).
TABLE 7.6 Questions for Consideration in the Selection of UCD Testing and Evaluation Techniques
Resource-Specific Criteria Method-Specific Criteria
What is the cost-benefit ratio of using this method? What is the purpose of the evaluation?
How much time is available for the study? What is the state of the product/system?
How much money is available for the study? What is the intended outcome of the evaluation?
How many staffs are available for the implementation
and analysis of the study?
How can the users be involved in the evaluation?
How can the designers be involved in the evaluation?
7-28 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools For Occupational Ergonomics
3. Controlled, taking place in a test environment controlling external factors to assess effectiveness,
efficiency, and satisfaction with defined qualitative and quantitative metrics
7.3.3.2 Summary
Evaluations are typically user-based or expert-based in reference to who conducts the assessment of the
design(s). Again, the generalizability trade-offs of using either class should be considered. User-based
methods have been found to derive more practically realistic and legitimate problems. Expert-driven
evaluations, however, provide a means to extract valid, unbiased issues more accurately than a user
group sampling that is insufficiently small (Maguire, 2001). It is not uncommon in evaluations to
have a shortage of actual users to work with.
Recently, Bainbridge (2004) identified user-based evaluations as consisting of the identification of
representative users and tasks and the observation of problems arising out of the use of a design to
create a task. The evaluations can also be formative in their role in design selection and prototype iter-
ation or alternatively summative in the documentation of efficacy, efficiency, and satisfaction at the com-
pletion of the design iterations. Applicable methods, prototype robustness, measures, and purpose are
decidedly different between summative and formative methods (Bainbridge, 2004). As formative
methods are intimately linked to the iterative portion of UCD, evaluations must be timely, in order
not to delay the overall design approach.
7.3.3.3 Some Examples of Methods and Tools
The testing used in UCD is typically categorized under the umbrella of methods classified as usability
evaluation methods. However, there are several categories of methods that inform the evaluation of the
design process and usability testing is one of those categories. Rubin (1994) asserts ten basic techniques
relevant to the UCD process and specifically concerning the evaluation of potential design choices.
Table 7.7 summarizes several of the methods mentioned by Rubin (1994) including focus group research,
surveys, design walk-through, paper-and-pencil evaluations, expert evaluations, usability audits, usabil-
ity testing, field studies, and follow-up studies. Again, note that usability testing is just one of the several
classes of evaluation methodologies.
Even so, usability testing is probably the most common approach used in implementing UCD and has
been a practice since the early 1980s (Dumas, 2003). It has received significant attention in the last 15 yr
as a valid, reliable, and efficient means of assessment. Several texts portray usability testing methods in
great detail providing extensive instructions for the planning, design, and management of these evalu-
ations (e.g., Dix et al., 1998; Nielsen and Mack, 1994; Rubin, 1994). These texts and others alike
provide step-by-step instructions for specific types of evaluations. Like the entire UCD process, the
use of evaluation techniques is affected by several contextual factors, to which the technique must be
adapted. In this section, three approaches to testing and evaluation are discussed. Two of these fall
under the category of expert-based, inspection methods and the other technique discussed is user-
observation.
Heuristic evaluation and cognitive walkthroughs are two usability evaluation methods that are classi-
fied as expert-based inspection methods. These techniques represent two of the original usability inspec-
tion methods introduced to the HCI community. As a result, several of the subsequently emerging
methods were grounded on heuristic evaluation and cognitive walkthrough (Nielsen and Mack, 1994).
Inspection methods are often lauded for their quick turnaround, limited training requirements, and
ability to generate suitable usability problems without a great deal of involvement from actual users
(Sears and Hess, 1999). To contrast, user-based observation techniques will also be introduced. Intui-
tively, a class of techniques requires significant participation from actual users. User-based observations
may be anchored in usability evaluations, but can in fact range from basic observations to highly con-
trolled empirical methods using special equipment to measure interactions. Typically, because they
involve actual users (or at least representative users) they yield a higher degree of validity in their assess-
ment of the ability of a design to match the identified requirement. User-based methods can sometimes
provide surprising design flaws and strengths not observable through expert-based methods.
User-Centered Design of Information Technology 7-29
Nielsen derived these usability principles from a factor analysis of 249 potential usability problems.
Additionally, evaluators can consider other usability principles and it is not uncommon to develop cat-
egory/domain specific heuristics to supplement the original ten (Nielsen, 1994).The true power in the
7-30 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools For Occupational Ergonomics
heuristic evaluation method is the involvement of several evaluators. Evaluators tend to find different
usability problems and when their findings are aggregated (and duplicates reconciled), they can
account for the majority of the usability flaws. Nielsen typically recommends 3 to 5 evaluators
because there is a point of diminishing returns with a large number of evaluators (Nielsen, 1994).
Evaluators may require a training/familiarization session depending on the target user group and
domain and other aspects of the work system (a definite minus to this method as there is no documented
means by which to train the experts). For example, to evaluate an information kiosk, a walk-up and use
system to be used by the general public, an evaluator would not typically receive training or prompting to
explain the use of the system. In contrast, in the evaluation of a nurses’ scheduling tool, a system for
which nurses receive specialized training evaluators should really be provided by giving training for
that system as well as preparation on the subject matter expertise possessed by the nurses. In addition,
it may be useful to provide the evaluators with typical usage scenarios for the design to help them antici-
pate the realistic demands on the design’s functionality. To formally account for these factors, Muller et al.
(1995) introduced three additional heuristics to encourage evaluators to consider the context of use
(arguably ignored in the introductions to heuristic evaluation). These heuristics include:
First, each evaluator steps themselves through the interface several times independently, record any
potential and any usability problems they identify with the interface for target users and tasks. Each
usability problem identified should be accompanied by a sufficiently explicatory description. The
more specific evaluators are in the vindication of the issues observed, the better, as it isolates target pro-
blems and their priority in the subsequent redesign activities.
Following the independent evaluation, evaluators’ problems and descriptions are aggregated and
duplicates are accounted for. Then evaluators discuss their findings eliminating any duplicate problems
and resolving any issues that may be contradictory. In this discussion, the evaluators often work to form
consensus on the severity levels for each issue. The outcome of the heuristic evaluation is a list of specific
problems and reference to which heuristics are violated and a severity level, which provides guidance as to
which issues take priority in redesign. While the outcomes of heuristic evaluations are not recipes that
explicitly direct redesign activities to achieve “correct” design, solutions that emerge from the evaluation
are often intuitive, because of the heuristics’ connection to fundamental usability principles.
A shortcoming of heuristic evaluations is that evaluators are not fully prepared for inspection in terms
of applying the heuristics and relative to the target domain. Direction is typically not provided for the
specific approaches taken up in the validation of design in terms of each heuristic (and even experienced
evaluators may have inconsistent approaches to this). An additional limitation, some argue, is the pro-
pensity for heuristic evaluation to generate several false positive usability issues. This is especially true in
circumstances when the evaluators have inconsistent and unreliable knowledge of the domain and
context (Cockton et al., 2003). In their assessment of heuristic evaluation, Cockton et al. (2003) noted
a trend for evaluators to underestimate users’ capabilities in display interactions. Still, the convenience
and efficiency afforded by the heuristic evaluation technique prompts its use in situations requiring a
quick turnaround in the iterative design process and in situations proving impractical or impossible
for the inclusion of end-users directly in the evaluation and iteration stages of UCD.
The following key points summarize the heuristic evaluation technique:
. Prototype requirements: Compatible with a range of fidelity — from paper through operational
systems
. Number of evaluators: 3 to 5, but possibly more in complex design situations.
. Testing environment: Flexible
. Time involved: 2 to 3 h, but longer if the system is highly complex
User-Centered Design of Information Technology 7-31
. Output: A document delineating specific design problems and explanations of why they are pro-
blems in terms of usability principles
. Special equipment needed: None
in recovering from unforeseen mishaps with equipment or protocols. As in the design process, it is much
more feasible to make changes prior to the full implementation of the assessment and the implications of
any “hiccups” in the protocol will have limited impact. Human behavior is by nature variable and hard to
predict; pilot testing can control some of the negative impacts of this irregularity.
Outcomes of user-observation based studies are typically an indication of usability problems, what
triggers them, and sometimes recommendations for solutions. While it used to be a standard protocol
to develop a formal written report on the finding, a more common trend is a debriefing, during
which the problems, possible solutions, and prioritizations are communicated (sometimes in combi-
nation with a report). Additionally, video footage collected can truly supplement the degree to which
the development team is on board with the suggested problems and fixes (Dumas, 2003; Rubin,
1994). In some cases, just having the development and design team observe the interactions proved to
be sufficient to inspire the necessary requirements (Wixon, 2003).
The focus of UCD evaluation is to include the target end-users or potential end-users in the protocol.
Working with actual users is both a challenging and rewarding venture. The evaluation team has to
decide in many cases the most relevant population of users to test. Dumas (2003) recommends that
this should be based on the priorities of management (which was identified in requirements gathering),
not the ease with which subjects are recruited. Because usability studies typically require 5 to 10 partici-
pants to uncover the majority of problems with a design, subject recruitment is typically not difficult (Dix
et al., 1998; Dumas, 2003). While there is some contention in the literature regarding the optimal number
of participants (Wixon, 2003) there is a point of diminishing return in including additional participants.
Despite the challenges facing user-observations, the discerning power of the results, when gathered
with attention to these details can transform a problematic design into a product that is usable and plea-
surable to use. Table 7.9 introduces specific methods for user observation and a description of each along
with advantages and disadvantages.
The following key points summarize the user evaluations:
. Prototype requirements: Can be completed with prototypes of any level, but the more detailed the
prototype, the better the representation of the user’s sequence of actions and subsequent extraction
of usability issues
. Number of evaluators: Varies depending on the types of analyses and statistical power that is man-
dated; some suggest that between 8 to 25 users be evaluated depending on the power requirements
and user variability (Maguire, 2001)
. Testing environment: Typically controlled laboratory setting, but can sometimes be in the field (or
both in the case of remote testing)
. Time involved: Varies greatly, dependent on prototype fidelity, complexity of the design, and the
number of subjects to run; evaluators coordinating assessment sessions should be sensitive to
time required of the users
. Output: A document delineating specific design problems with justification from observation and
recommended solutions
. Special equipment needed: Video cameras, one-way mirrors/observation area, tape recorders, video
editing equipment, information collection forms, software for logging interaction dialogs, and even
eye tracking to assess the gaze paths of the user
7.3.3.4 Summary
In this section, we discussed both advantages and limitations to the fundamental methods and provided
classification of types of methods for consideration in the evaluation process. Several breeds of testing
methods have spawned from the ones mentioned in this section. The set of methods presented just
scratches the surface on the available UCD methods. Researchers and practitioners are continually devel-
oping new techniques, assessing and improving upon the current steadfast favorites (e.g., heuristic evalu-
ation). As is the case with the overall UCD process, the selection and utilization of testing and evaluation
7-34 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools For Occupational Ergonomics
methods is subject to changes — based on the given constraints of a study. In a perfect world, evaluators
would apply a combination of these techniques to account for as many design problems as possible. What
has become more feasible, however, has been the hybridization of different techniques to improve on
the benefits generated by each [e.g., “heuristic walkthroughs”; see Cockton et al. (2003); Cockton and
Woolrych (2001). The challenge for evaluators, designers, and developers is how to best match the evalu-
ation method with the identified goals of the design within the organizational constraints of funding,
lead-time, and availability of evaluators.
7.4 Conclusions
UCD is just not a methodology for design; its a philosophy under which the entire organization and
design/development process must operate in order to realize the goals of the new system. There are
organizational-wide implications in terms of the commitment to, support for, and acceptance of the
UCD process. Rubin (1994) has identified common characteristics of organizations, which successfully
carry out UCD. These include:
. Use a phase-based approach for development that integrates incremental evaluations from user and
expert feedback at critical stages in the design process
. Utilize multidisciplinary teams to provide the variety of skills, knowledge, and information about
the target users and their activities, including engineering, marketing, training, interface design,
human factors, and multimedia
User-Centered Design of Information Technology 7-35
. Facilitate an involved and concerned management committed to following and promoting the
UCD activities
As discussed, within the realm of UCD there are countless variations of techniques and tools to include
end-users and their needs in the design process, to generate data and knowledge of the work system, and
to test and evaluate the usability, appropriateness, and of system design alternatives. This chapter pro-
vided a high level overview of the most prevailing methods as well as guidance in the selection and appli-
cation of UCD methods and tools validated to be effective through both research and practice.
Overall, UCD is the principle of including users and stakeholders in the design process, an idea that
was unheard of (or ignored) just a few decades ago. However, with the steadily increasing ubiquity of
computer-based technologies within the personal and work lives of the general populace, UCD is becom-
ing not only a “good practice,” but a legal mandate and a practical necessity to ensure the success and
acceptability of technologies. Despite the relatively long history and continual development of UCD
tools and procedures, the UCD philosophy is still far from reaching a saturation point in today’s
world of product and technology design. Champions of UCD are needed to propel this ideology and
techniques in a variety of domains to produce systems that uphold high standards of safety, quality,
and efficiency and support a high level of user satisfaction. Users and the various aspects of the work
system must be deliberately and judiciously woven into the design process to emerge a usable, useful
appealing design.
In truth, considering the needs of users with disabilities merits integration into the priorities of UCD
in order to create opportunity for users of all abilities and operate under a variety of contexts to be gain-
fully employed and be productive members of society. This is especially true in light of the significant
population of aging adults, who will experience the normally anticipated age-related declines in physical,
cognitive, and sensory functions. If systems and tools are not usable by a percentage of the population,
then this percentage of individuals can never be trained to serve the roles typically associated with a given
set of tasks. The design itself may become an impediment to the successful completion of related tasks.
The initiatives driving accessibility agendas such as the Americans with Disabilities Act clearly states
the minimum requirements that must be met in the design of systems and products. However, the
actual challenge lies in the improvement of existing UCD methods and development of new UCD
method that will allow UCD researchers and practitioners to meet these needs and expectations of a
highly variable and important population of end-users. This directive should guide the future of UCD
and, will hopefully, result in more clearly defined, systematic, and acceptable ways for designers and
developers to incorporate the principles of UCD and inclusive design into tangible, practical design
solutions.
References
Alexander, I.F. and Stevens, R., Writing Better Requirements. Addison-Wesley Professional, Boston, MA,
2002.
Apple Computer, Introduction to the Apple Human Interface Guidelines. Retrieved March 8, 2004, from
http://developer.apple.com/documentation/UserExperience/Conceptual/OSXHIGuidelines/
index.html#//apple_ref/doc/uid/20000957.
Bainbridge, W.S., 2004 User – centered design, in Berkshire Encyclopedia of Human – Computer Inter-
action, Bainbridge, W.S., Ed., Vol. 2, Berkshire Publishing Group, Great Barrington, 2004,
pp. 763 – 768.
Bekker, M., Beusman, J., Keyson, D., and Lloyd, P., KidReporter: a user requirements gathering technique
for designing with children. Interact. Comput., 15 (2), 187 –202, 2003.
Bevan, N., International standards for HCI and usability. Int. J. Hum. –Comput. Stud., 55 (4), 533–552,
2001.
Bevan, N., Bowden, R., Corcoran, R., Curson, I., Macleod, M., Maissel, J. et al., Usability Context Analy-
sis—A practical guide, v4.02. Middlesex: NPL Usability Services, National Physical Laboratory,
1996.
Bevan, N. and Macleod, M., Usability measurement in context. Behav. Inf. Technol., 13, 132–145, 1994.
Beyer, H. and Holtzblatt, K., Apprenticing with the customer. Commun. ACM, 38 (5), 45–52, 1995.
Beyer, H. and Holtzblatt, K., Contextual Design: Defining Customer-Centered Systems. Morgan Kaufmann
Publishers, San Francisco, CA, 1998.
Blomberg, J., Burrell, M., and Guest, G., An ethnographic approach to design, in The Human –Computer
Interaction Handbook: Fundamentals, Evolving Technologies and Emerging Applications, Jacko, J.A.
and Sears, A. Eds., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Mahwah, NJ, 2003, pp. 964 –986.
Booch, G., Rumbaugh, J., and Jacobson, I., The Unified Modeling Language User Guide, 1st ed., Addison-
Wesley Professional, Boston, MA, 1998.
Caplan, S., Using focus groups methodology for ergonomic design. Ergonomics, 33 (5), 527 –533, 1990.
Cassim, J. and Dong, H., Critical users in design innovation, in Inclusive Design: Design for the whole
Population. Clarkson, J., Coleman, R., Keates, S., and Lebbon, C. Eds., Springer, New York, 2003.
Christel, M.G. and Kang, K.C., Issues in Requirements Elicitation (Technical Report; CMU/SEI-92-TR-12;
ESC-TR-92 –012). Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, 1992.
Cockburn, A., Writing Effective Use Cases (1st ed.). Addison-Wesley Professional, Boston, MA, 2000.
Cockton, G., Lavery, D. and Woolrych, A., Inspection-Based Evaluation, in The Human –Computer Inter-
action Handbook: Fundamentals, Evolving Technologies and Emerging Applications Jacko, J. A.
and Sears, A. Eds., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc, Mahwah, NJ, 2003, pp. 1118 –1138.
User-Centered Design of Information Technology 7-37
Cockton, G. and Woolrych, A., Understanding inspection methods: Lessons from an assessment of heur-
istic evaluation, in People and Computers XV, Blandford, A. and Vanderdonckt, J. Eds., Springer-
Verlag, Berlin, 2001, pp. 171–192.
Coleman, R., Clarkson, J., Lebbon, C., and Keates, S., Introduction: from margin to mainstream, in
Inclusive Design: Design for the Whole Population, Clarkson, J., Coleman, R., Keates, S.,
and Lebbon, C. Eds., Springer, New York, 2003.
Cooper, A., The Inmates Are Running the Asylum: Why High Tech Products Drive Us Crazy and How to
Restore the Sanity (2nd ed.). SAMS Publishing, Pearson Education, Indianapolis, IN, 2004.
Damodaran, L., User involvement in the systems design process–a practical guide for users. Behav. Inf.
Technol., 15 (6), 363–377, 1996.
Damodaran, L., Simpson, A., and Wilson, P., Designing systems for people. NCC Publications, Manchester,
1980.
Dix, A.J., Finlay, J.E., Abowd, G.D., and Beale, R., Human –Computer Interaction (2nd ed.). Prentice Hall,
London, UK, 1998.
Dourish, P., What we talk about when we talk about context. Personal Ubiquitous Comput., 8 (1), 19 –30,
2004.
Dumas, J.S., User-Based Evaluations, in The Human-Computer Interaction Handbook: Fundamentals,
Evolving Technologies and Emerging Applications, Jacko, J. A., and Sears, A. Eds., Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Mahwah, NJ, 2003, pp. 1093 –1117.
Eason, K.D., User-centred design: For users or by users? Ergonomics, 38 (8), 1667– 1673, 1995.
Emery, V.K., Jacko, J.A., Sainfort, F. and Moloney, K., Intranet and intra-organizational communication,
in The Handbook of Human Factors in Web Design, Proctor, R.W. and Vu, K.P.L. Eds., Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Mahwah, NJ, 2004, pp. 528 –550.
Gottesdiener, E., 2002 Requirements by Collaboration: Workshops for Defining Needs (1st ed.). Addison-
Wesley Professional, Boston, MA, 2002.
Grudin, J. and Pruitt, J., Personas, participatory design and product development: An infrastructure for
engagement. Proceedings of the Participatory Design Conference (PDC 2002) Computer Pro-
fessionals for Social Responsibility (CPSR), Malmö, pp. 144–161, 2002.
Hackos, J.T. and Redish, J.C., User and Task Analysis for Interface Design. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.,
New York, NY, 1998.
Hall, R., Prototyping for usability of new technology. Int. J. Hum. –Comput. Stud., 55, 485 –501, 2001.
Holtzblatt, K., Contextual Design, in The Human-Computer Interaction Handbook: Fundamentals, Evol-
ving Technologies and Emerging Applications, Jacko, J. A., and Sears, A. Eds., Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Inc., Mahwah, NJ, 2003, pp. 941–963.
Holtzblatt, K. and Jones, S., Contextual inquiry: A participatory technique for system design, in Partici-
patory design: Principles and practices, Schuler, D. and Namioka, A. Eds., Lawrence Erlbaum Associ-
ates, Inc., Hillsdale, NJ, 1993, pp. 177 –210.
IEEE., EEE Recommended Practice for Software Requirements Specifications (IEEE Std 830 –1998). IEE,
New York, 1998a.
IEEE., IEEE Guide for Developing System Requirements Specifications (IEEE Std 1233). IEEE, New York,
1998b.
International Organization for Standardization, ISO 9241 –11: Ergonomic requirements for office work
with visual display terminals (VDTs). Part 11: Guidance on Usability, 1998.
International Organization for Standardization (ISO), ISO 13407: Human-Centred Design Processes for
Interactive Systems. International Organization for Standardization, Geneva, 1999.
International Organization for Standardization (ISO), ISO/TR 18529: Ergonomics—Ergonomics of
Human –System Interaction—Human-Centred Lifecycle Process Descriptions. International Organ-
ization for Standardization, Geneva, 2000.
Jacko, J.A. and Sears, A., The Handbook of Human-Computer Interaction Handbook: Fundamentals,
Evolving Technologies and Emerging Applications. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ,
2003.
7-38 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools For Occupational Ergonomics
Javahery, H., Seffah, A., and Radhakrishnan, T., Beyond power: Making bioinformatics tools user-
centered. Commun. ACM, 47 (11), 58– 63, 2004.
Johnson, C.M., Johnson, T.R. and Zhang, J., A user-centered framework for redesigning health care inter-
faces. J. Biomed. Informatics, 38 (1), 75 –87, 2005.
Karat, J., Karat, C.-M. and Ukelson, J., Affordances, motivations, and the design of the user interface.
Commun. ACM, 43, 49 –51, 2000.
Karwowski, W., Achieving compatibility in human-computer interface design and evaluation, in The
Human–Computer Interaction Handbook, Jacko, J.A. and Sears, A. Eds., Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Inc., Mahwah, NJ, 2003, pp. 1226– 1238.
Kirlik, A., The design of everyday life environments, in A Companion to Cognitive Science, Bechtel, W. and
Graham, G. Eds., Blackwell Publishers, Oxford, UK, 1998, pp. 702 –712.
Kontio, J., Lehtola, L., and Bragge, J., Using the Focus Group Method in Software Engineering: Obtaining
Practitioner and User Experiences. Proceedings of the ACM-IEEE International Symposium on
Empirical Software Engineering (ISESE 2004), Redondo Beach, CA, pp. 271 –280, 2004.
Kontogiannis, T. and Embrey, D., A user-centred design approach for introducing computer-based
process information systems. Appl. Ergon., 28 (2), 109 –119, 1997.
Kotonya, G. and Sommerville, I., Requirements Engineering: Processes and Techniques. John Wiley & Sons,
Inc., New York, 1998.
Koubek, R., Benysh, D., Buck, M., Harvey, C., and Reynolds, M., The development of a theoretical frame-
work and deisgn tool for process usability assessment. Ergonomics, 46 (1– 3), 220 –241, 2003.
Kovitz, B.L., Practical Software Requirements: A Manual of Content and Style. Manning Publications Co.,
Greenwich, CT, 1998.
Kulak, D. and Guiney, E., Use Cases: Requirements in Context (2nd ed.). Pearson Education, Inc.,
(Addison-Wesley), Boston, MA, 2004.
Law, C., Keeping designers up with the times . . . projects for enabling the rapid development of univer-
sally accessible IT by industry. Proceedings of the Inclusion by Design — Planning a Barrier — Free
World, An International World Congress Montreal, Canada, 2001.
Lebbon, C. and Coleman, R., A design-centered approach, in Inclusive design: Design for the Whole Popu-
lation, Clarkson, J., Coleman, R., Keates, S. and Lebbon, C. Eds., Springer, New York, 2003,
pp. 500–518.
Leonard, V.K., Jacko, J.A., Yi, J., and Sainfort, F., Human Factors & Ergonomic Methods, in Handbook of
Human Factors & Ergonomics, Salvendy, G. Ed., (3rd ed., pp. in press). John Wiley and Sons, Inc.,
New York, 2005.
Lidwell, W., Holden, K., and Butler, J., Universal Principles of Design: 100 Ways to Enhance Usability, Influ-
ence Perception, Increase Appeal, Make Better Design Decisions, and Teach through Design. Rockport
Publishers, Inc., Gloucester, MA, 2003.
Maguire, M., Methods to support human-centred design. Int. J. Hum. –Comput. Stud., 55 (4), 587–634,
2001.
Mao, J.-Y., Vrendenberg, K., Smith, P.W., and Carey, T., User-centered design methods in practice: A
survey of the state of the art. Proceedings of the 2001 Conference of the IBM Centre for Advanced
Studies on Collaborative Research (CASCON ’01), IBM Press, Toronto, pp. 12 –24, 2001.
Marcus, A., User-centered design in the enterprise. Interactions, 12, 18–23, 2005.
Mayhew, D.J., The Usability Engineering Lifecycle: A Practitioner’s Handbook for User Interface Design (1st
ed.). Morgan Kaufmann Publishers, San Francisco, CA, 1999.
Mayhew, D.J., Requirements Specifications Within the Usability Engineering Lifecycle, in The
Human-Computer Interaction Handbook: Fundamentals, Evolving Technologies and Emerging Appli-
cations, Jacko, J.A. and Sears, A. Eds., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Mahwah, NJ, 2003,
pp. 913–921.
Microsoft Corporation, Microsoft Tablet PC — Visual Design Guidelines (Microsoft Tablet PC). Retrieved
March 8, 2004, from http://msdn.microsoft.com/library/default.asp?url ¼ /library/en-us/
tpcsdk10/html/whitepapers/designguide/tbcontpcvisualdesignguidelines.asp, 2004.
User-Centered Design of Information Technology 7-39
Muller, M.J., McClard, A., Bell, B., Dooley, S., Meisky, L., Meskill, J.A. et al., Validating an extension to
participatory heuristic evaluation: Quality of work and quality of life. Proceedings of the ACM
SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ’95), ACM Press, Denver,
CO, pp. 115 –116, 1995.
Newell, A.F. and Gregor, P., “User sensitive inclusive design” — In search of a new paradigm. Proceedings
of the ACM Conference on Universal Usability (CUU ’00), ACM Press, Arlington, VA, pp. 39– 44,
2000.
Nielsen, J., Usability Engineering (1st ed.). Morgan Kaufmann, San Francisco, CA, 1993.
Nielsen, J., Heuristic Evaluation, in Usability Inspection Methods Nielsen, J. and Mack, R.L. Eds., John
Wiley & Sons, New York, 1994, pp. 25 –62.
Nielsen, J., Durability of Usability Guidelines. Retrieved February 20, 2005, from http://www.useit.com, 2005.
Nielsen, J. and Mack, R.L., Usability Inspection Methods. John Wiley & Sons, Inc., New York, 1994.
Nielsen, J. and Molich, R., Heuristic evaluation of user interfaces. Proceedings of the ACM SIGCHI Con-
ference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ’90), ACM Press, Seattle, WA, pp. 249 –256,
1990.
Norman, D.A., The Psychology of Everyday Things (reprint ed.). Basic Books (Perseus), New York, 1988.
Norman, D.A., The Design of Everyday Things (1st ed.). Basic Books (Perseus), New York, 2002.
Noyes, J. and Barber, C., User-Centred Design of Systems. Springer-Verlag, London, UK, 1999.
Nuseibeh, B. and Easterbrook, S., Requirements Engineering: A Roadmap. Proceedings of the Inter-
national Conference on Software Engineering (ICSE 00), ACM Press, Limerick, pp. 35 –46, 2000.
Ostroff, E., Universal Design: The New Paradigm, in Universal Design Handbook, Preiser, W.F.E. and
Ostroff, E. Eds., McGraw Hill, Boston, MA, 2001, pp. 1.3 –1.12.
Oulasvirta, A., Finding Meaningful Uses for Context-Aware Technologies: The Humanistic Research
Strategy. CHI Lett., 6 (1), 247 –254, 2004.
Polson, P., Lewis, C., Rieman, J. and Wharton, C., Cognitive walkthroughs: A method for theory-based
evaluation of user interfaces. Int. J. Man–Machine Stud., 36, 741 –773, 1992.
Preece, J., Rogers, Y., Sharp, H., Benyon, D., Holland, S. and Carey, T., Human-Computer Interaction:
Concepts And Design (1st ed.). Addison-Wesley, Reading, MA, 1994.
Preiser, W.F.E. and Ostroff, E., Universal Design: The New Paradigm. McGraw Hill, MA, Boston, 2001.
Pruitt, J. and Grudin, J., Personas: Practice and Theory. Proceedings of the 2003 Conference on Designing
for User Experiences (DUX2003), ACM Press, San Francisco, pp. 1– 15, 2003.
Psephos Corporation., 2005 Butterfly ballot voting machine. Retrieved February 22, 2005, from http://
www.xeniashopper.com/psephos_voting_machine.htm
Ransley, P., White Paper: A Primer on Requirements Engineering, 2003.
Introducing Beavers Requirements Engineering Services Capability, Beaver Computer Consultants, Ltd,
Essex, UK.
Rasmussen, J., Information Processing and Human –Machine Interaction: An Approach to Cognitive Engin-
eering. North-Holland, Amsterdam, 1986.
Robertson, S. and Robertson, J., Mastering the Requirements Process. Addison-Wesley Professional,
Boston, MA, 1999.
Rosenberg, L., Requirements Engineering: A Methodology for Writing High Quality Requirement Specifica-
tion and for Evaluating Existing Ones. Retrieved February 17, 2005, from http://www.stc-onli-
ne.org/cd-rom/1999/slides/MethWrit.pdf, 1999.
Rouse, W.B., Design for Success: A Human-Centered Approach to Designing Successful Products and
Systems. John Wiley & Sons, Inc., New York, 1991.
Rubin, J., Handbook of Usability Testing: How to Plan, Design, and Conduct Effective Tests. John Wiley &
Sons., New York, 1994.
Rumbaugh, J., Jacobson, I. and Booch, G., The Unified Modeling Language Reference Manual (2nd ed.).
Addison-Wesley Professional, Boston, MA, 2004.
Saferstein, B., Ethnomethodology, in A Companion to Cognitive Science, Bechtel, W. and Graham, G. Eds.,
Blackwell Publishers, Oxford, UK, 1998, pp. 391 –401.
7-40 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools For Occupational Ergonomics
Samaras, G.M. and Horst, R.L., A systems engineering perspective on the human-centered design of
health information systems. J. Biomed. Informatics, 38(1), 61–74, 2005.
Sears, A. and Hess, D., Cognitive walkthroughs: Understanding the effect of task description detail on
evaluator performance. Int. J. Hum. –Comput. Interact., 11 (3), 185– 200, 1999.
Sears, A., Lin, M., Jacko, J.A., and Xiao, Y., When Computers Fade. . . Pervasive Computing and Situa-
tionally-Induced Impairments and Disabilities, in Human –Computer Interaction: Theory and
Practice (Part II), Stephanidis, C. and Jacko, J.A. Eds., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.,
Mahwah, NJ, 2003, pp. 1298 –1302.
Sharp, H., Finkelstein, A. and Galal, G., Stakeholder Identification in the Requirements Engineering
Process. Proceedings of the 10th International Workshop on Database & Expert Systems Applications
(DEXA), IEEE Computer Society, Florence, pp. 387 –391, 1999.
Shepherd, A., HTA as a framework for task analysis, in Task Analysis, Annett, J. and Stanton, N.A. Eds.,
Taylor and Francis, New York, 2000, pp. 9 –24.
Shepherd, A., Hierarchical Task Analysis. Taylor & Francis, Inc., New York, 2001.
Sherehiy, B., Karwowski, W., and Rodrick, D., Human Factors and Ergonomics Standards, in Handbook
of Human Factors and Ergonomics, Salvendy, G. Ed., (3rd ed., pp. in press). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.,
New York, 2005.
Shneiderman, B., Designing the User Interface: Strategies for Effective Human-Computer Interaction (3rd
ed.). Addison-Wesley Longman, Inc., Reading, MA, 1998.
Sommerville, I. and Sawyer, P., Requirements Engineering: A Good Practice Guide. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.,
New York, 1997.
Stanton, N.A. and Young, M.S., A Guide to Methodology in Ergonomics. Taylor & Francis., New York, 1999.
Stewart, T. and Travis, D., Guidelines, Standards, and Style Guides, in The Human –Computer Interaction
Handbook: Fundamentals, Evolving Technologies and Emerging Applications, Jacko, J.A. and
Sears, A. Eds., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Mahwah, NJ, 2003, pp. 991 –1005.
Sugar, W.A., What is so good about user-centered design: Documenting the effect of usability sessions on
novice software designers. J. Res. Comput. Educ., 33 (3), 235– 250, 2001.
U.S. Department of Defense (DOD), Section 8.3: Methods, in MIL-HDBK-46855A: Department of
Defense Handbook: Human Engineering Program Processes and Procedures 1999, pp. 113 –174.
Van Natta, D., Jr. and Canedy, D., The 2000 Elections: The Palm Beach Ballot. Florida Democrats Say
Ballot’s Design Hurt Gore. The New York Times, p. A1, 2000.
Vicente, K.J., Cognitive work analysis: Toward safe, productive, and healthy computer-based work. Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Inc, Mahwah, NJ, 1999.
Vredenburg, K., Isensee, S., and Righi, C., User-Centered Design: An Integrated Approach. Prentice-Hall,
Inc., Upper Saddle River, NJ, 2002.
W3C., 2004 Authoring Tool Accessibility Guidelines 2.0 (W3C Working Draft). Retrieved March 8, 2004,
from http://www.w3.org/TR/2004/WD-ATAG20–20040224/
Wharton, C., Rieman, J., Lewis, C. and Polson, P., The Cognitive Walkthrough Method: A Practitioner’s
Guide, in Usability Inspection Methods, Nielsen, J. and Mack, R.L. Eds., John Wiley & Sons,
New York, 1994, pp. 105–141.
Wiegers, K.E., 2000 When Telepathy Won’t Do: Requirements Engineering Key Practices. Retrieved
February 22, 2005, from http://www.processimpact.com/articles/telepathy.pdf
Wiegers, K.E., Software Requirements (2nd ed.). Microsoft Press, Redmond, WA, 2003.
Wixon, D., Evaluating usability methods: Why the current literature fails the practitioner. ACM Interact.,
10 (4), 28 –34, 2003.
Young, R.R., Effective Requirements Practices. Addison-Wesley Professional, Boston, MA, 2001.
Young, R.R., The Requirements Engineering Handbook. Artech House, Inc., Norwood, MA, 2004.
8
Application of Risk
Theory in
Man– Machine –
Environment Systems
All activities in the man –machine –environment system also create the risk state. Risk state influences
health and safety, for example, health injury due to accident or due to long-time influences to the
health. At the same time risk causes economical loses due to failures of machines or due to mechanical
accidents/explosions, fire, destructions. The main goal of all activities of risk management is to control
the risk, to minimize all negative influences, for example, illness, health injury, death and also technical
consequences or major industrial accidents.
All necessary demands on the technical safety are integrated into two basic legislation products
of EU:
– Direction 391/89/EU:1 Increasing safety and health protection in working conditions
– Direction 392/89/EU:2 together with 93/44/EU, 93/68/EU — Machine safety, approximation of
member states legislation
According to the first Direction, it is necessary to evaluate all possible risks in the working process
to perform measures for protection. This is the duty of the employer, who must take into consider-
ation all-important influences and conditions, that is, organization of working process, working
conditions, hazardous situations and many others. For such a process, one must know all methods
for the identification, analysis and classification of risk, which is an integrated part of the complex
system of risk.
In the Slovak Republic these directions are integrated into the Law “Safety and Health Protection at
Work” (Law No. 330/1996).
8-1
8-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
R¼PD (8:1)
where P is a probability of risk situation occurrence, D is a consequence after risk situation occurrence
(damage in the whole man — machine — environment system).
Also it is possible to write:
R¼PDW E (8:2)
8.2.1 Example 1
The example considered here are big gearboxes with a power output of about 3.6 to 5.4 MW. We analyzed
two basic conditions for failure in risk evaluation of parts of gearboxes:
– Current operation with all suitable parameters
– Operation with improper maintenance
FIGURE 8.1 Gearboxes are one- or two-stage types with helical gears and with a synchronous motor.
8.2.2 Example 2
8.2.2.1 Risk Evaluation in the Pipe Mill Hall
In the next part the example considered is the pipe mill hall. It is a big technological complex from the
technical risk point of view. Products of this pipe mill are pipes with diameters from 500 to 1420 mm,
R = P . D
TABLE 8.1
Part of Gearbox Sign
Pinion bearing Rlp
Pinion Rp
Bearing and pinion Rl þ p
Countershaft bearing Rlpr
Countershaft gearing Rpr
Countershaft shaft Rhpr
Whole block Rcb
Big gear Rlvk
Output gear Rvk
Bearing and output gear Rlvk þ vk
with thickness from 5.6 to 12.5 mm and with length from 9 to 12 m. The arrangement of pipe mill hallis
shown in Figure 8.4.
0.016
optimal parameters
0.014
unsufficient maintenance
0.012
Risk Values
0.01
0.008
0.006
0.004
0.002
0
Rlp Rp Rl+p Rlpr Rpr Rhpr Rcb Rlvk Rvk Rlvk+vk
Parts of Gearbox
grinding of crane
defects
mechanical
tapering machine
RTG 1 RTG 2
ultrasonic
welding 1 binding
Therefore, we obtained the technical –human combined risk values or levels. These levels are only the
primary and preliminary information for the designer. But this information is very important for all
the successive steps of detailed technical risk analysis.
charging truck
table shears
calibration shears
edge shears
scrap shears
plasma
Segment of
Aggregate
drifting bridge
welding
cutter
calibre
blowing
ultrasound
centring
strip lead
scraper
0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14
R / points
FIGURE 8.5 Technical risk analysis for all components of machine for ultrasonic welding 1.
risk. It is recommended to perform this evaluation of technical risk always by the same person/team, to
reduce the human subjectivity factor. This method is based on the inductive principles.
R ¼ M U P (M=30) (8:3)
8.3.3 Example 3
Determination of acceptable risk level is the most important step. If it is not exactly determined, it will
limit the value for the acceptance of technical risk. It depends on various influences, for example, state of
science, working relations, legislation and social policy. Acceptable risk is such a risk, which can be
accepted from the human and technical point of view.
Metallurgy is always a very important factor of economical development of each economy. Branches
like chemistry, electrotechnics, power engineering and metallurgy create the base for the fast, successful
and continuous economical growth. Therefore, it is necessary to analyze the technical risk in the
metallurgical technologies.
There is one most serious problem in the day-to-day metallurgical operation from the technical risk
point of view: this problem is the transport of liquid metal and their manipulation.
8-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Material flow
FIGURE 8.6 Representation of one chosen material flow in the steel plant.
Dangerous manipulations in the new and well-developed technologies are quite reduced or eliminated.
But there are always lot of very dangerous manipulations with the liquid iron and liquid steel in many old
steel plants.
The main source of the technical risk in this part of metallurgical technology is the improper manipu-
lation with the liquid metal. There is a representation of one chosen material flow in the steel plant shown
in Figure 8.6. The whole transport of material is performed by means of bridge crane and transport truck.
In connection with the hazard identification it is necessary to distinguish two criteria above all:
1. Exposition on risk
2. Cause of risk situation
After determination of all necessary values we obtain the final result: risk values for each element,
which takes part in the working process in the chosen segment of metallurgical technology. Values
and results are given in Table 8.3.
If we determine that the acceptable risk level is R 10, we can discuss the results from various points
of view.
This process of technical risk analysis may be very useful if computerized. The flow diagram developed
for this purpose is shown in Figure 8.7.
START
2. Identification of hazard
3. Identification of danger
A
Necessary measures
B
4. Are respected
NO
requirements of
laws, directions and
standards ?
YES
C
5. Computation of risk:
probability, consequence
Acceptable risk
6. Evaluation of END
system safety
D
Unacceptable risk
A – risk analysis
B – risk evaluation
C – risk management
D – risk control
application for data collection and evaluation for crane 250 t. The first step was creation of crane function
structures. On the base of these structures, possible failures were defined — causes and consequences,
predominately from the point of view of safety, environment influence, time losses of individual
system components (also taken into consideration were failures, which process quality).
By means of creation of such database it was enabled to analyze and evaluate risks. Applying Pareto-
analysis, such failures (their causes) were specified, whose risk value was unacceptable (for safety).
8-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Suggestion of new measurements led to minimization of safety failure influences towards residual or
acceptable risk levels. Of course, failures failures with impact such as great time losses of device were
also taken into consideration. Evaluation of the present maintenance strategy and suggestion of new
steps for increasing efficiency were the result of the analysis.
After the introduction of device into operation, it was evident that the value of operation costs was
dependent on the maintenance quality. RCM (reliability centered maintenance) helps to master
problems of incomplete data collection about machines and devices, which leads to incorrect decisions
and the result is often not profit able, but is a unreasonable loss. The flexible maintenance strategy
requires elimination of the most risk factors to apply new methods of personal management such as
the basic principle of total production maintenance. It enables flexible response to “requirements” of
own operation (devices).
All changes require initial investment. But predominately in the area of maintenance, it is important to
reduce costs that lead to output profit because of time loss and higher production quality.
Bibliography
1. Direction 391/89/EU.
2. Direction 392/89/EU.
3. Sinay, J., Pačaiová, H., Kopas, M., Tomková, M., Application of technical risk theory for evaluation of
gearbox damaging processes, in IEA 97, Helsinki, s.559 –561.
4. Sinay, J., Oravec, M., Kopas, M., Juris, M., New development trends in the application of risk theory of
man–machine –environment systems, in Advances in Occupational Ergonomics and Safety, IOS Press,
Washington, 1997, s.573– 575.
Application of Risk Theory in Man– Machine– Environmnet System 8-11
5. Sinay, J., Oravec, M., Pačaiová, H., Kopas, M., Risk evaluation and working conditions – ergonomical
point of view, in HAAMAHA 98, Hong-Kong, 1998, s.547 –550.
6. Sinay, J., Pačaiová, H., Kopas, M., Maintenance and risks during maintenance operation, in Int. Conf.
on Occupational Risk Prevention – ORP 2000, Tennerife, 2000, CD.
7. Sinay, J., Kopas, M., Tomková, M., Safety and risk analysis of steel wire ropes, in CAES 99, Barcelona,
1999, CD-ROM.
9
Engineering
Anthropometry
9.1 Overview
People come in a great variety of sizes and the proportions of their body parts are not the same. Thus,
devising tools, gear, and workstations to fit their bodies requires careful consideration; design for the
statistical “average” will not do. Instead, for each body segment to be fitted, the designer must determine
what dimension(s) is (are) critical: this may be a minimal or a maximal value, or a range. Often, a series
of such decisions is necessary to accommodate body segments or the whole body by clothing, workspace,
and equipment.
The following text describes the steps involved, provides statistical tools, and supplies
anthropometric data.
9.2 Terminology
While all humans have heads and trunks, arms and legs, the body parts come in various sizes, assembled
in different proportions. Anthropometry is the name of the science of measuring human bodies.
Table 9.1 lists special terms often used in anthropometry. Together with the reference planes shown
in Figure 9.1, they describe major aspects of anthropometric information useful to designers and
engineers.
9-1
9-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Note: For terms related to body reference planes, see Figure 9.1.
a
Distal and proximal usually refer to limbs with the point of reference at the attachment to the next larger section of the
body.
STEPS IN DESIGN THAT FIT CLOTHING , TOOLS , WORKSTATIONS, AND EQUIPMENT TO THE BODY
(Adapted from Kroemer et al., 1997, 2001)
Step 1: Select those anthropometric measures that directly relate to defined design dimensions.
Examples are: hand length related to handle size; shoulder and hip breadth related to escape-hatch
diameter; head length and breadth related to helmet size; eye height related to the heights of
windows and displays; knee height and hip breadth related to the leg room in a console.
Engineering Anthropometry 9-3
Step 2: For each of these pairings, determine whether the design must fit only one given percen-
tile (minimal or maximal) of the body dimension, or a range along that body dimension. Examples
are: the escape hatch must be big enough to accommodate the very largest value of shoulder
breadth and hip breadth, with clothing and equipment worn; the handle size of pliers is probably
selected to fit the smallest hand; the leg room of a console must accommodate the tallest knee
heights; the height of a seat should be adjustable to fit persons with short and with long lower
legs. (The explanation on how to use and calculate percentiles follows.)
Step 3: Combine all selected design values in a careful drawing, computer model or mock-up to
ascertain that they are compatible. For example, the required height of the leg-room clearance that
sitting persons with long lower legs need, may be very close to or may even overlap with the height
of the working surface determined from the elbow height.
Step 4: Determine whether one design will fit all users. If not, several sizes or adjustment features
must be provided to fit all users. Examples are: one extra-large bed size fits all sleepers; gloves and
shoes must come in different sizes; seat heights should be adjustable.
Number of Persons
FIGURE 9.2 Frequency distribution of body height (stature) in Americans. About 95% of all males are between 162 and
188 cm tall, about 2.5% are shorter, another 2.5% taller. (From Kroemer et al., Engineering Physiology. Bases of Human
Factors/Ergonomics, 3rd ed., Van Nostraud Reinhold — John Wiley & Sons, New York, NY 1997. With permission.)
male Americans; only 2.5% are shorter than approximately 1,620 mm, and another about 2.5% are taller
than 1,880 mm. In other words, about 95% of all men are in the height range of 1,620 to 1,880 mm,
because the 2.5th percentile value is at 1,620 mm and the 97.5th percentile is at 1,880 mm. The 50th
percentile is at 1,750 mm.
(In a normal data distribution, mean [m], average, median, and mode coincide with the 50th percen-
tile. The standard deviation [S] describes the peak or the flatness of the data set. More details on these
statistical descriptors are discussed later in this chapter under “Estimation by Probability Statistics.”)
There are two ways to determine the given percentile values. One is simply to take a distribution of
data, such as that shown in Figure 9.2, and to determine from the graph (measure, count, or estimate)
critical percentile values. This works whether the distribution is normal, skewed, binomial, or in any
other form. Fortunately, most anthropometric data are normally distributed, which allows the second,
even easier (and usually more exact) approach to calculate percentile values. This involves the standard
deviation, S. If the distribution is flat (the data are widely scattered), the value of S is larger than when the
data cluster is close to the mean.
To calculate the percentile value, one simply multiplies the standard deviation S by a factor k, selected
from Table 9.2. Then one adds the product to the mean m:
p¼mþkS (9:1)
If the desired percentile is above the 50th percentile, the factor k has a positive sign and the product
k S is added to the mean; if the p-value is below average, k is negative and hence the product k S is
subtracted from the mean m.
Examples
1st percentile is at m þ k S with k ¼ 2:33 (see Table 9.2; note the negative value of k)
2nd percentile is at m þ k S with k ¼ 2:01
2.5th percentile is at m þ k S with k ¼ 1:96
5th percentile is at m þ k S with k ¼ 1:64
10th percentile is at m þ k S with k ¼ 1:28
Engineering Anthropometry 9-5
Note: Any percentile value p can be calculated from the mean m and the
standard deviation SD (normal distribution assumed) by p ¼ m þ kD.
9-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
correlations among these dimensions. More details on correlations follow but for detailed information
see McConville’s chapter in NASA (1978) and the book by Roebuck in 1995.
FIGURE 9.3 The EE line serves as a reference to describe head posture and the angle of the line-of-sight. The EE line
is easier to use than the older “Frankfurt Plane,” which is about 158 more declined. (From Kroemer, K.H.E.,
Ergonomics in Design, pp. 7– 8, 1993; p. 40, 1994. With permission.)
9-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
TABLE 9.3 Guidelines for the Conversion of Standard Measuring Postures to Functional Stances and Motions
To consider Do the following
Slumped standing or sitting Deduct 5 to 10% from appropriate height measurements
Relaxed trunk Add 5 to 10% to trunk circumferences and depths
Wearing shoes Add approximately 25 mm to standing and sitting heights: more for “height heels”
Wearing light clothing Add about 5% to appropriate dimensions
Wearing heavy clothing Add 15% or more to appropriate dimensions (note that heavy clothing may
severely reduce mobility)
Extended reaches Add 10% or more for extensive motions of the trunk
Use of hand tools Center of handle is at about 40% hand length, measured from the wrist
Forward bent head (and neck) EE line close to horizontal
posture
Comfortable seat height Add or subtract upto 10% to or from standard seat height
Source: Adapted from Kroemer et al., Engineering Physiology. Bases of Human Factors/Ergonomics, 3rd ed., Van Nostrand
Reinhold — John Wiley & Sons, New York, NY 1997. With permission.
In spite of the military sampling bias, the data in Table 9.4 are the best available information on the
civilian North American adult population as Kroemer et al. argued in 1997. Their main reservation con-
cerns body weight, which is obviously more variable in the civilian population than in the military and,
given the current trend toward general obesity, on average is likely to be larger among the civilians. Head,
foot, and hand sizes should not differ appreciably between soldiers and civilians.
Table 9.4 gives the mean (50th percentile), as well as the standard deviation, for 36 body segments and
for body weight. This allows calculation of any percentile value of interest, as discussed earlier.
Table 9.5, Table 9.6, and Table 9.7 present, in similar fashion, descriptive data from Taiwan and Japan,
Great Britain and France, Germany and Russia.
Reliable and comprehensive information on body sizes is at hand, unfortunately, for only the few
populations listed previously. For other groups, some limited anthropometric information are avail-
able, which are compiled in Table 9.8. However, these data resulted from widely varying techniques of
data gathering, often on only small samples. This makes it doubtful that the listed statistics for mean
and standard deviation truly represent the underlying population. For most humans on earth, only
gross estimates exist that are listed in Table 9.9. The data in Table 9.8 and Table 9.9 may serve as
rough approximations of regional anthropometry, but they cannot replace exact measurements.
Engineering Anthropometry
Brackets. These Data also Describe the U.S. Civilian Population — see text
Men Women
Dimension Mean Std. Dev. Mean Std. Dev. Applications
1. Stature 1756 67 1629 64 A main measure for comparing population samples. Reference for
The vertical distance from the floor to the top of the head when the minimal height of overhead obstructions Add height for more
standing [99] clearance, hat, shoes stride
2. Eye height, standing 1634 66 1516 63 Origin the visual field of a standing person. Reference location of
The vertical distance from the floor to the outer corner of the visual obstructions and of targets such as displays; consider
right eye when standing [D19] slump and motion
3. Shoulder height (acromion), standing 1443 62 1334 58 Starting point for arm length measurements; near the center of
The vertical distance from the floor to the tip (acromion) of the rotation of the upper arm. Reference point for hand reaches;
shoulder when standing [2] consider slump and motion
4. Elbow height, standing 1073 48 998 45 Reference for height and distance of the work area of the hand and
The vertical distance from the floor to the lowest point of the location of controls and fixtures; consider slump and motion
right elbow when standing with the elbow flexed at 908 [D16]
5. Hip height (trochanter), standing 928 48 862 45 Traditional anthropometric measure, indicator of leg length and the
The vertical distance from the floor to the trochanter landmark height of the hip joint. Used for comparing population samples
on the upper side of the right thigh when standing [107]
6. Knuckle height, standing nd nd nd nd Reference for low locations of controls, handles, and handrails;
The vertical distance from the floor to the tip of the extended consider slump and motion of the standing person
index finger of the right thigh when standing [107]
7. Fingertip height, standing 653 40 610 36 Reference for lowest location of controls, handles, and handrails;
The vertical distance from the floor to the tip of the extended consider slump and motion of the standing person
index finger of the right hand when standing [D13]
8. Sitting height 914 36 852 35 Reference for the minimal height of overhead obstructions. Add
The vertical distance from the sitting surface to the top of the height for more clearance, hat, trunk motion of the seated person
head when sitting [93]
9. Sitting eye height 792 34 739 33 Origin of the visual field of a seated person. Reference point for the
The vertical distance from the sitting surface to the outer corner location of visual obstructions and of target such as displays;
of the right eye when sitting [49] consider slump and motion
10. Sitting shoulder height (acromion) 598 30 556 29 Starting point for arm length measurements; near the center of
The vertical distance from the sitting surface to the tip rotation of the upper arm. Reference for hand reaches; consider
(acromion) of the shoulder when sitting [3] slump and motion
(Table continued)
9-9
TABLE 9.4 Continued
9-10
Men Women
Dimension Mean Std. Dev. Mean Std. Dev. Applications
11. Sitting elbow height 231 27 221 27 Reference for the height of an armrest, of the work area of the hand,
The vertical distance from the sitting surface to the lowest point and of keyboard and controls; consider slump and motion of the
of the right elbow when sitting with the elbow flexed at 908 [48] seated person
12. Sitting thigh height (clearance) 168 13 160 12 Reference for the minimal clearance needed between see pan and the
The vertical distance from the sitting surface to the highest point underside of a structure such as a table or desk; add clearance for
on the top of the horizontal right thigh with the knee flexed at clothing and motions
908 [104]
(Table continued)
9-11
9-12
TABLE 9.4 Continued
Men Women
Dimension Mean Std. Dev. Mean Std. Dev. Applications
31. Head length 197 7 187 6 A traditional measure for comparing population sample. Reference
Note: Measurements were taken in 1987/8 on U.S. Army soldiers, 1774 men and 2208 women. nd: no data available.
Engineering Anthropometry 9-13
FIGURE 9.4 Illustrations of standard anthropometric measurements. (From Kroemer et al., Engineering Physiology.
Bases of Human Factors/Ergonomics, 3rd ed., Van Nostraud Reinhold — John Wiley & Sons, New York, NY 1997.
With permission.)
assumption that, though people vary greatly in size, they are likely to be relatively similar in proportions.
This premise holds true for body components that are related to each other in size. For example,
many body “lengths” highly correlate with each other; also, groups of body “breadths” are interrelated,
as are “circumferences.” However, it is not true that all body lengths (or breadths, or circumferences) are
tightly associated with each other, and certainly many lengths are rather independent of breadths and
depths and circumferences. Thus, one should better be very careful in deriving one set of data from
another.
9-14 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Note: Chinese (Taiwan) Adults, 25 to 34 yr of age, according to Wang et al (2000a,b). Data measured during 1996-
2000 on civilians. Japanese Adults 18 to 35 yr of age, according to Kagimoto (1990). Data measured in 1988. nda: no
data available.
A basic rule for ratio scaling is to use only pairings of data that are closely related in a statistical way to
each other. Even if the coefficient of correlation is 0.7, for example, the variability of the predictor deter-
mines the variability of the derived information by only about 50% (the correlation coefficient 0.7
squared is 0.49). Ratio scaling should never be used if one has to assume that the sample to be scaled
has body proportions different from those of the other set. For example, many Asian populations
have proportionally shorter legs and longer trunks than the Europeans or North-Americans.
Engineering Anthropometry 9-15
TABLE 9.6 Anthropometric Data (mm) of Adults in Great Britain and France
Britain France
Men Women Men Women
Dimension Mean Std. Dev. Mean Std. Dev. Mean Std. Dev. Mean Std. Dev.
1. Stature 1740 70 1610 62 1747 nda 1620 nda
2. Eye height, standing 1630 69 1505 61 nda nda nda nda
3. Shoulder height (acromion), 1425 66 1310 58 1434 nda 1331 nda
standing
4. Elbow height standing 1090 52 1005 46 nda nda nda nda
5. Hip height (trochanter) 920 50 810 43 918 nda 844 nda
6. Knuckle height, standing 755 41 720 36 nda nda nda nda
7. Fingertip height, standing 655 38 625 38 nda nda nda nda
8. Sitting height 910 36 850 35 918 nda 867 nda
9. Sitting eye height 790 35 740 33 819 nda 772 nda
10. Sitting shoulder height 595 32 555 31 nda nda nda nda
(acromion)
11. Sitting elbow height 245 31 235 29 nda nda nda nda
12. Sitting thigh height 160 15 155 17 nda nda nda nda
(clearance)
13. Sitting knee height 545 32 500 27 533 nda 487 nda
14. Sitting popliteal height 440 29 400 27 nda nda nda nda
15. Shoulder-elbow length 365 20 330 17 365 nda 332 nda
16. Elbow-fingertip length 475 21 430 19 472 nda 427 nda
17. Overhead grip reach, sitting 1245 60 1150 53 nda nda nda nda
18. Overhead grip reach, standing 2060 80 1905 71 nda nda nda nda
19. Forward grip reach 780 34 705 31 nda nda nda nda
20. Arm length, vertical 780 36 705 32 nda nda nda nda
21. Downward grip reach 665 32 600 29 nda nda nda nda
22. Chest depth 250 22 250 27 nda nda nda nda
23. Abdominal depth, sitting 270 32 255 30 nda nda nda nda
24. Buttock-knee depth, sitting 595 31 570 30 595 nda 569 nda
25. Buttock-popliteal depth, 495 32 480 30 nda nda nda nda
sitting
26. Shoulder breadth 400 20 355 18 382 nda 340 nda
(biacromial)
27. Shoulder breadth (bideltoid) 465 28 395 24 457 nda 410 nda
28. Hip breadth, sitting 360 29 370 38 342 nda 346 nda
29. Span 1790 83 1605 71 nda nda nda nda
30. Elbow span 945 47 850 43 nda nda nda nda
31. Head length 195 8 180 7 155 nda 148 nda
32. Head breadth 155 6 145 6 142 nda 134 nda
33. Hand length 190 10 175 9 190 nda 173 nda
34. Hand breadth 85 5 75 4 86 nda 76 nda
35. Foot length 265 14 235 12 264 nda 237 nda
36. Foot breadth 95 6 90 6 101 nda 91 nda
37. Weight (in kg) 75 12 63 11 70 nda 58 nda
Note: British Adults, 19 to 35 yr of age, according to Pheasant (1986, 1996) who estimated the data in or before
1986. French Adults, 18 to 51 yr of age, according to Coblentz, A (1997), personal communication of 22 April 1997
regarding ERGODATA taken on 1015 French Soldiers (687 males, 328 females). Data measured in or before 1997.
nda: no data available.
For sets of highly correlated data, one can establish an estimate E of a ratio scaling factor for a desired
dimension (dy) in the population sample Y if:
Note: East German Adults, 18 to 59 yr of age, according to Fluegel et al. (1986). Data measured between 1979 (some 1967)
and 1982. Ethnic Russian Factory Workers: 192 males between 18 and 29 yr old and 205 females between 20 and 29 yr old, all
from Moscow, according to Strokina and Pakhomova (1999). Data measured during 1895 and 1986. nda: no data available.
Since the basic assumption is that the two samples are similar in proportion, the same scaling factor
applies to both samples X and Y:
TABLE 9.8 International Anthropometry: Adults, Measured; Averages (and Standard Deviations)
Sample Stature Sitting Knee Height Weight
Size (mm) Height (mm) Sitting (mm) (kg)
ALGERIA
Females (Mebarki and Davies, 1990) 666 1576 (56) 795 (50) 487 (36) 61 (1)
AUSTRALIA
Females, 77 (8) yr old 138 1521 (70 775 (40) — 61 (13)
Males 76 (7) yr old (Kothiyal and Tettey, 2000) 33 1658 (79) 843 (56) — 72 (11)
BRAZIL
Males (Ferreira, 1988; cited by Al-Haboubi, 3076 1699 (67) — — —
1991)
CHINA
Females (Hong Kong) 69 1607 (54) 838 (45) 510 (31) —
Females (Taiwan) (Huang and You, 1994) 300 1582 (49) — — 51 (7)
Males (Hong Kong) (Chan, So and Ng, 2000) 286 1737 (49) 884 (42) 552 (29) —
Males (Canton) (Evans, 1990) 41 1720 (63) — — 60 (6)
EGYPT
Females (Moustafa, Davies, Darwich and 4960 1606 (72) 838 (43) 499 (25) 63 (4)
Ibraheem, 1987)
FRANCE
Females 328 1620 867 487 58
Males (Coblentz, personal communication, 687 1747 918 533 70
1997)
GERMANY (East)
Females 123 1608 (59) 854 (31) 497 (24) —
Males (Fluegel, Greil and Sommer, 1986) 30 1715 (66) 903 (34) 531 (27) —
INDIA
Females 251 1523 (66) 775 (39) 483 (28) 50 (10)
Males (Chakarbarti, 1997) 710 1650 (70) 937 (45) 520 (30) 57 (11)
East-Ctr. India male farm workers (Victor, Nath 300 1638 (56) 775 (40) — 57 (7)
and Verma, 2002)
Central India male farm workers (Gite and 39 1620 (50) 739 (26) 509(30) 49 (6)
Yadav, 1989)
South India male workers (Fernandez and 128 1607 (60) 791 (40) 542 (38) 57 (5)
Uppugonduri, 1992)
East Indian male farm workers (Yadav, Tewari 134 1621 (58) 809 (22) 515 (29) 54 (67)
and Prasad, 1997)
INDONESIA
Females 468 1516 (54) 719 (34) — —
Males (Sama’mur, 1985; cited by Intaranont, 949 1613 (56) 872 (37) — —
1991)
IRAN
Female students 74 1597 (58) 861 (36) 488 (23) 56 (10)
Male students (Mououdi, 1997) 105 1725 (58) 912 (26) 531 (24) 66 (10)
IRELAND
Males (Gallwey and Fitzgibbon, 1991) 164 1731 (58) 911 (30) 508 (28) 74 (9)
ITALY
Females 753 1610 (64) 850 (34) 495 (30) 58 (8)
Females 386 1611 (62) — — 58 (9)
Males 913 1733 (71) 896 (36) 541 (30) 75 (10)
Males 410 1736 (67) — — 73 (11)
(Coniglio, Fubini, Masali et al., 1991)
(Robinette, Blackwell, Daanen et al., 2002)
(Table continued)
9-18 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
JAMAICA
Females 123 1648 832 — 61
Males (Lamey, Aghazadeh, and Nye, 1991) 30 1749 856 — 68
JAPAN
Females 240 1584 (50) 855 (28) 475 (20) 54 (6)
Males (Kajimito, 1990) 248 1688 (55) 910 (30) 509 (22) 66 (8)
KOREA (South)
Female workers (Fernandez, Malzahn, Eyada, 101 1580 (57) 833 (32) 460 (22) 54 (7)
and Kim, 1989)
MALASIA
Females (Ong, Koh, Phoon, and Low, 1988) 32 1559 (66) 831 (39) — —
NETHERLANDS
Females, 20 –30 yr old 68 1686 (66) — — 67 (10)
Females (18–65 yr old) 691 1679 (75) — — 73 (16)
Males (20–30 yr old) 55 1848 (80) — — 81 (14)
Males (18–65 yr old) 564 1813 (90) — — 84 (16)
(Steenbekkers and Beijsterveldt, 1998)
(Robinette, Blackwell, Daanen et al., 2002)
RUSSIA
Female herders (ethnic Asians) 246 1588 (55) — — —
Female students (ethnic Russians) 207 1637 (57) 859 (32) 527 (24) 61 (8)
Female students (ethnic Usbeks) 164 1578 (49) 839 (28) 487 (25) 56 (7)
Female factory workers (ethnic Russians) 205 1606 (53) 849 (30) 494 (26) 61 (8)
Female factory workers (ethnic Usbeks) 301 1580 (54) 845 (31) 484 (26) 58 (9)
Male students (ethnic Russians) 166 1757 (56) 912 (32) 562 (25) 71 (9)
Male students (ethnic Usbeks) 150 1700 (52) 905 (29) 531 (23) 65 (7)
Male factory workers (ethnic Russians) 192 1736 (61) 909 (32) 550 (25) 72 (10)
Male factory workers (ethnic mix) 150 1700 (59) 896 (32) 541 (24) 68 (8)
Male farm mechanics (ethnic Asians) 520 1704 (58) 902 (31) 530 (25) 64 (8)
Male coal miners (ethnic Russians) 150 1801 (61) 978 (33) 572 (25) —
Male construction workers (ethnic Russians) 150 1707 (69) — — —
(Strokina and Pakhomova, 1999)
SAUDI ARABIA
Males (Dairi, 1986; cited by Al-Haboubi, 1991) 1440 1675 (61) — — —
SINGAPORE
Females (Ong, Koh, Poon and Low, 1988) 46 1598 (58) 855 (31) — —
Males (pilot trainees) (Singh, Peng, Lim, and 832 1685 (53) 894 (32) — —
Ong, 1995)
SRI LANKA
Females 287 1523 (59) 774 (22) — —
Males (Abeysekera, 1985; cited by Intaranont, 435 1639 (63) 833 (27) — —
1991)
SUDAN
Males
Villagers 37 1687 (63) — — 57 (8)
City dwellers 16 1704 (72) — — 62 (13)
City dwellers 48 1668 — — 51
Soldiers 21 1735 (71) — — 71 (8)
Soldiers 104 1728 — — 60
(ElKarim, Sukkar, Collins and Doré, 1981)
(Ballal et al., 1982; cited by Intaranont, 1991)
(Table continued)
Engineering Anthropometry 9-19
THAILAND
Females 250 1512 (48) — — —
Females 711 1540 (50) 817 (27) — —
Males 250 1607 (20) — — —
Males 1478 1654 (59) 872 (32) — —
(Intaranont, 1991)
(NICE; cited by Intaranont, 1991)
TURKEY
Females
Villagers 47 1567 (52) 792 (38) 486 (27) 69 (14)
City dwellers (Goenen, Kalinkara, and Oezgen, 1991) 53 1563 (55) 786 (05) 471 (05) 66 (13)
Male Soldiers (Kayis and Oezok, 1991) 5108 1702 (60) 888 (34) 513 (280 63 (7)
U.S.A.
Note: Last updated 15 January 2004. Contact the author for source references.
dy ¼ E Dy (9:4)
TABLE 9.9 Estimates of Average Anthropometric Data (mm) for 20 Regions of the Earth
Stature Sitting Height Knee Height, Sitting
Females Males Females Males Females Males
North America 1650 1790 880 930 500 550
LATIN AMERICA
Indian Population 1480 1620 800 850 445 495
European and Negroid Population 1620 1750 860 930 480 540
EUROPE
North 1690 1810 900 950 500 550
Central 1660 1770 880 940 500 550
East 1630 1750 870 910 510 550
Southeast 1620 1730 860 900 460 535
France 1630 1770 860 930 490 540
Iberia 1600 1710 850 890 480 520
AFRICA
North 1610 1690 840 870 500 535
West 1530 1670 790 820 480 530
Southeast 1570 1680 820 860 495 540
Near east 1610 1710 850 890 490 520
INDIA
North 1540 1670 820 870 490 530
South 1500 1620 800 820 470 510
ASIA
North 1590 1690 850 900 475 515
Southeast 1530 1630 800 840 460 495
South China 1520 1660 790 840 460 505
Japan 1590 1720 860 920 395 515
AUSTRALIA
European extraction 1670 1770 880 930 525 570
Source: Adapted from Juergens et al., International Data on Anthropometry, International Labour Office, Geneva,
Switzerland, 1990.
or weight (as discussed earlier). Thus, ratio scaling requires careful consideration of the circumstances,
especially taking into account statistical correlations.
y ¼aþbx (9:5)
where x is the known mean value and y the predicted mean. The constants a (the “intercept”) and b (the
“slope”) must be determined (known) for the data set of interest. A recent example of this procedure is
the estimation of body dimensions of American soldiers by Cheverud et al. (1990).
Engineering Anthropometry 9-21
If mean value of y is predicted (for any value of x) using the regression Equation 9.5, the actual values
of y are scattered about the mean in a normal (Gaussian) probability distribution. The standard error
(SE) of the estimate depends on the correlation r between x and y, and on the standard deviation of y
(Sy) according to
Roebuck (1995) discussed the implications in some detail, including its extension to develop multi-
variate regression equations, principal component analyses, and boundary description analyses.
m ¼ (Sx)=n (9:7)
where m is the mean (average), x is the individual measurement, and n is the number of measured indi-
viduals. The distribution of the data is described by the equation
with S called the standard deviation of the sample (often designated SD in statistics texts). If the sample
size is small (conventionally, 30 or less) one usually makes an arbitrary correction by using (n 1)
instead of n:
The smaller the n in the sample, the larger is the standard error SE. The standard error SE of the mean
is determined from
As the number n increases, the mean m and the standard deviation S become more reliable estimates of
the underlying general population, mu and sigma.
A useful way to describe the variability of a sample is to divide the standard deviation S by its mean m
(and multiplying the result by 100). This yields the coefficient of variation
This expression is independent of the magnitude of the measurement and of the unit of measurement.
Groups of human dimensions show characteristic variabilities. Table 9.10 lists typical coefficients of
variation.
9-22 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Use of the CV is often of great help in assessing the credibility of data published in the literature;
unusually large or small CV values indicate that, either, the distribution of the measured population
is indeed different from the other populations, or that irregularities occurred in measuring, or in data
treatment or reporting. The CV is also useful for estimating the standard deviation of an unknown
data set.
COV(x,y) ¼ rx ,y Sx Sy (9:12)
After calculating the sum of the two mean values, msum, of the x and y distributions from
msum ¼ mX þ my (9:13)
mdiff ¼ mX my (9:15)
Engineering Anthropometry 9-23
Examples
Example 1: What is the 95p shoulder-to-fingertip length? Assume the mean lower arm (LA) link length
(with the hand) to be 442.9 mm with a standard deviation of 23.4 mm. Also assume the mean upper arm
(UA) link length to be 335.8 mm and its standard deviation 17.4 mm.
The multiplication factor of k ¼ 1:64 (from Table 9.2) leads to the 95th percentile. But one cannot
calculate the sum of the two 95p lengths because this would disregard their covariance; instead, one
should calculate the sum of the mean values first, using Equation (9.13):
Inserting the standard deviations for LA and UA into Equation (9.14), and using an assumed
coefficient of correlation of 0.4, one can obtain:
Now, one can calculate the 95p total arm length (AL), using Equation (9.1):
Example 2: What is the average arm (acromion to wrist) length of an American pilot? it is known that for
a standing pilot, the 90th percentile acromial (shoulder) height is 1532.0 mm and the wrist height is
905.6 mm; for the 10th percentile, the values are 1379.5 and 808.6 mm, respectively. The correlation
between shoulder and wrist heights is estimated to be 0.3.
One must first calculate the mean acromion (A) and wrist (W) heights to be able to estimate the
standard deviations. For the 90th percentile this is, as per Equation (9.7):
Using Equation (9.1), with k ¼ 1:28 taken from Table 9.2, one obtains:
Likewise,
Now Equation (9.15) can be used to calculate the average arm length (acromion to wrist, AW):
Example 3: What is the mass of the head of a 75p Japanese female? The mass (weight) of the total body
has a mean of 54.0 kg and a standard deviation of 6.0 kg — see Table 9.5. The estimated mass of the head
is 6.2% of the total body mass (from data compiled by Kroemer et al. 1997). One can assume that the
correlation coefficient between the masses of head and total body to be 1. (That assumption may be
challenged, though.)
The mean head mass is, of course,
The standard deviation of the head mass is calculated from Equation (9.4):
According to Equation (9.1), the mass of a 75th percentile head is (with k ¼ 0:67 taken from Table 9.2)
TABLE 9.11 Comparison of Mobility Data (in degrees) for Females and Males
5th percentile 50th percentile 95th percentile Differencea
Joint Movement Female Male Female Male Female Male Female Male
Neck Ventral flexion 34.0 25.0 51.5 43.0 69.0 60.0 þ8.5
Dorsal flexion 47.5 38.0 70.5 56.5 93.5 74.0 þ14.0
Right rotation 67.0 56.0 81.0 74.0 95.0 85.0 þ7.0
Left rotation 64.0 67.5 77.0 77.0 90.0 85.0 NS
Shoulder Flexion 169.5 161.0 184.5 178.0 199.5 193.5 þ6.5
Extension 47.0 41.5 66.0 57.5 85.0 76.0 þ8.5
Adduction 37.5 36.0 52.5 50.5 67.5 63.0 NS
Abduction 106.0 106.0 122.5 123.5 139.0 140.0 NS
Medical rotation 94.0 68.5 110.5 95.0 127.0 114.0 þ15.5
Laternal rotation 19.5 16.0 37.0 31.5 54.5 46.0 þ5.5
Elbow-forearm Flexion 135.5 122.51 148.0 138.0 160.5 150.0 þ10.0
Supination 87.0 86.0 108.5 107.5 130.0 135.0 NS
Pronation 63.0 42.5 81.0 65.0 99.0 86.5 þ16.0
Wrist Extension 56.5 47.0 72.0 62.0 87.5 76.0 þ10.0
Flexion 53.5 50.5 71.5 67.5 89.5 85.0 þ4.0
Adduction 16.5 14.0 26.5 22.0 36.5 30.0 þ4.5
Abduction 19.0 22.0 28.0 30.5 37.0 40.0 22.5
Hip Flexion 103.0 95.0 125.0 109.5 147.0 130.0 þ15.5
Adduction 27.0 15.5 38.5 26.0 50.0 39.0 þ12.5
Abduction 47.0 38.0 66.0 59.0 85.0 81.0 þ7.0
Medical rotation (prone) 30.5 30.5 44.5 46.0 58.5 62.5 NS
Lateral rotation (prone) 29.0 21.5 45.5 33.0 62.0 46.0 þ12.5
Medical rotation (sitting) 20.5 18.0 32.0 28.0 43.5 43.0 þ4.0
Lateral rotation (sitting) 20.5 18.0 33.0 26.5 45.5 37.0 þ6.5
Knee Flexion (standing) 99.5 87.0 113.5 103.5 127.5 122.0 þ10.0
Flexion (prone) 116.0 99.5 130.0 117.0 144.0 130.0 þ143.0
Medical rotation 18.5 14.5 31.5 23.0 44.5 35.0 þ8.5
Lateral rotation 28.5 21.0 43.5 33.5 58.5 48.0 þ10.0
Ankle Flexion 13.0 18.0 23.0 29.0 33.0 34.0 26.0
Extension 30.5 21.0 41.0 35.5 51.5 51.5 þ5.5
Adduction 13.0 15.0 23.5 25.0 34.0 38.0 NS
Abduction 11.5 11.0 24.0 19.0 36.5 30.0 þ5.0
a
Listed are only differences at the 50th percentile, and if significant (a , 0.5).
Source: Adapted from Houy, D.A., Proceedings of the Human Factors Society’s 27th Annual meeting, Human Factors Society,
Santa Monica, CA, pp. 374–378, 1983; Staff, K.R., A Comparison of Range of Joint Mobility in College Females and Males,
Unpublished Master’s Thesis, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, 1983. With permission.
9-26 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
at the lower back. However, these bending and unbending lumbar motions (in the medial plane) are
rather limited, and often lead to overexertions, especially if combined with lateral twisting of the
torso; low back pain has been reported throughout the history of humans (Snook, 2000). Wrist problems
have been associated with excessive motion requirements since the early 1700s (Kroemer, 2001). Our
head and neck have limited mobility in bending and twisting. Our thumbs and fingers, as well as the
eyes, have limited but finely controlled motion capability.
Ranges of motion (also called mobility or flexibility) depend much on age, health, fitness, training, and
skill. Diverse measuring instructions and techniques have been applied to dissimilar groups of people to
assess their mobility; hence, there is much diversity in reported ranges of motion. However, one set of
mobility measurements has been taken on groups of 100 females and of 100 males each by the same
researchers using the same techniques (Houy, 1983; Staff, 1983). Excerpts from these data appear in
Table 9.11. Note that the differences in mobility between males and females are generally negligible.
Designing to fit motion ranges, instead of fixed postures, is not difficult. The articulations in the
human body have varying degrees of freedom for movement. These are shown in Figure 9.5 for major
body joints and the motion ranges are listed in Table 9.11. These maximal ranges were measured on
students of physical education; hence, many people will have slightly less mobility than shown. “Con-
venient” mobility is within the range of maximal values shown in Table 9.11, but not always in the
middle of the ranges. Occasionally, convenient motions are near the limits of mobility. Habits and
skill as well as strength requirements may make different ranges preferred.
Design for motions starts by establishing the actual movement ranges. Convenient motions may
cluster around the mean of mobility in a body joint or may be close to the limits of flexibility. For
example, a person walking about, or standing, has the knees most of the time nearly extended, that is,
the knee angle — in the sagittal view — is close to its extreme value of about 1808. The sagittal hip
angle (between trunk and thigh) is also in the neighborhood of 1808. Of course, when sitting, both
hip and knee angles change and cluster to about 908 when sitting — see Table 9.12. While sitting or
moving about, the trunk is normally nearly erect, as are the neck and head. In most work situations,
the upper arm hangs from the shoulder while the elbow angle tends to be near 908; but the wrist is
best held straight i.e., at about 1808.
Preferred work areas of the hands and feet are in front of the body, within curved envelopes that reflect
the mobility of the forearm in the elbow joint, or of the total arm in the shoulder joint, of the lower leg in
the knee joint, and of the total leg in the hip joint. Thus, these envelopes may be described as (partial)
spheres around the presumed locations of the body joints. However, the preferred ranges within the poss-
ible motion zones differ depending on whether the main requirements are strength, or speed, or accuracy,
or vision — as discussed in some detail, by Kroemer et al. (1997, 2001). Thus, there is not one reach
envelope, but different preferred envelopes, depending on the task.
For each job situation, the ergonomic designer determines the dominant requirements of the task, for
example, whether the operator
FIGURE 9.5 Maximal displacements in body joints. (From Kroemer et al., Engineering Physiology. Bases of Human
Factors/Ergonomics, 3rd ed., Van Nostraud Reinhold — John Wiley & Sons, New York, NY 1997. With permission.)
9-28 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Source: Adapted from Juergens et al., International Data on Anthropometry, International Labour Office,
Geneva, Switzerland, 1990.
Engineering Anthropometry 9-29
9.10 Summary
It is inexcusable to design tasks, tools, or workstations for the phantom of “the average person” in a static
position. No such persons exist and design for the average fits nobody well. Instead, ranges of body sizes,
of motions, and of strengths (see elsewhere in this book) establish the design criteria. This is easy to do
for the designer and engineer who starts with proper anthropometric information and applies it ergo-
nomically, that is, with “ease and efficiency” as the guiding principles.
References
Annis, J.F. and McConville, J.T., Anthropometry, in Occupational Ergonomics, Bhattacharya, A. and
McGlothlin, J.D. Eds., Dekker, New York, NY, 1996, chap. 1, pp. 1 –46.
Bradtmiller, B., Anthropometry for Persons with Disabilities: Needs in the Twenty-First Century. Paper pre-
sented at RESNA 2000 Annual Conference and Research Symposium, 28 –30 June 2000, Orlando,
FL. Rehabilitation Engineering and Assistive Technology Society of North America, Arlington, VA,
2000.
Cheverud, J., Gordon, C.C.,Walker, R.A., Jacquish, C., Kohn, L., Moore, A., and Yamashita, N., 1988
Anthropometric Survey of U.S. Army Personnel: Correlation Coefficients and Regression Equations
(NATICK TR 90/032-6), U.S. Army Research, Development and Engineering Center, Natick,
MA, 1990.
Fluegel, F., Greil, H., and Sommer, K., Anthropologischer Atlas, Tribuene, Berlin, Germany, 1986.
Gordon, C.C., Churchill, T., Clauser, C.E., Bradtmiller, B., McConville, J.T., Tebbetts, I., and Walker, R.A.,
1988 Anthropometric Survey of U.S. Army Personnel: Summary Statistics Interim Report (Natick-TR-
89/027), U.S. Army Natick Research, Development and Engineering Center, Natick, MA, 1989.
Houy, D.A., Range of joint motion in college males, in Proceedings of the Human Factors Society’s 27th
Annual Meeting, Human Factors Society, Santa Monica, CA, pp. 374–378, 1983.
Juergens, H.W., Aune, I.A., and Pieper, U., International Data on Anthropometry. (Occupational Safety
and Health Series No. 65), International Labour Office, Geneva, Switzerland, 1990.
Kagimoto,Y., ed., Anthropometry of JASDF Personnel and its Applications for Human Engineering, Aero-
medical Laboratory, Air Development and Test Wing JASDF, Tokyo, Japan, 1990.
9-30 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Kroemer, K.H.E., Locating the computer screen: how high, how far? Ergon. Design, October issue, 7 –8,
1993; and January issue, p. 40, 1994.
Kroemer, K.H.E., Keyboards and keying: an annotated bibliography of the Literature from 1878 to 1999.
Int. J. Universal Access Inf. Soc. UAIS, 1/2, pp. 99–160, 2001.
Kroemer, K.H.E., Extra-ordinary Ergonomics: Designing for Small and Big Persons, The Disabled and
Elderly, Expectant Mothers and Children, CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL, 2006.
Kroemer, K.H.E. and Kroemer, A.D., Office Ergonomics, Taylor & Francis, London, UK, 2001.
Kroemer, K.H.E., Kroemer, H.B., and Kroemer-Elbert, K.E., Ergonomics: How to Design for Ease and
Efficiency, 1st ed. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ, 1994.
Kroemer, K.H.E., Kroemer, H.B., and Kroemer-Elbert, K.E., Ergonomics: How to Design for Ease and
Efficiency, 2nd ed., Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle Rivert, NJ, 2001.
Kroemer, K.H.E., Kroemer, H.J., and Kroemer-Elbert, K.E., Engineering Physiology. Bases of Human
Factors/Ergonomics, 3rd ed., Van Nostrand Reinhold — John Wiley & Sons, New York, NY, 1997.
McConville, J.T., Anthropometry in sizing and design, in Anthropometric Sourcebook, Vol. 1, NASA/
Webb, eds., NASA Reference Publication 1024, LBJ Space Center, Houston, TX, chap. 8, 1978,
pp. 8.1–8.23.
Pheasant, S.T., A technique for estimating anthropometric data from the parameters of the distribution
of stature, Ergonomics, 25, pp. 981–992, 1982.
Pheasant, S., Bodyspace: Anthropometry, Ergonomics and Design, Taylor & Francis, London, UK., 1986.
Pheasant, S., Bodyspace: Anthropometry, Ergonomics and the Design of Work, 2nd ed., Taylor & Francis,
London, UK, 1996.
Robinette, K.M., Blackwell, S., Daanen, H., Boehmer, M, Fleming, S., Brill, T., Hoeferlin, D., and
Burnsides, D., Civilian American and European Surface Anthropometry Resource (CAESAR) Final
Report, Volume 1: Summary (AFRL-HE-WP-TR-2002-0169), United States Air Force Research
Laboratory, Wright-Patterson AFB, OH, 2002.
Robinette, K.M. and Daanen, H., Lessons learned from CAESAR: a 3-D anthropometric survey, in Pro-
ceedings of the 15th Triennial Congress of the International Ergonomics Association, August 24–29,
Paper No. 00730, 2003.
Roebuck, J.A., Anthropometric Methods, Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Santa Monica, CA,
1995.
Snook, S.H., Back risk factors: an overview, in Violante, F., Armstrong, T., and Kilbom, A., eds., Muskulo-
skeletal Disorders of the Upper Limb and Back, Taylor & Francis, London, UK, 2000, chap. 11,
pp. 129–148.
Staff, K.R., A Comparison of Range of Joint Mobility in College Females and Males, Unpublished Master’s
Thesis, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, 1983.
Strokina, A.N. and Pakhomova, B.A., Anthropo-Ergonomic Atlas (in Russian), Moscow State University
Publishing House, Moscow, Russia, 1999.
Wang, M.J.J., Wang, E.M.Y., and Lin, Y.C., Anthropometric Data Book of the Chinese People in Taiwan,
The Ergonomics Society of Taiwan, Hsinchu, ROC, 2002.
10
Human Strength
Evaluation
10.1 Overview
Skeletal muscles are able to move body segments with respect to each other against internal and external
resistances. Muscle components can shorten dynamically, statically retain their length, or be lengthened.
Various methods and techniques are available for assessing muscular strength. The engineering appli-
cation of data on available body strength requires the determination of whether minimal or maximal
exertions, static or dynamic, are critical. Data on body strength are available for the design of tools,
equipment, and work tasks.
10-1
10-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Physiology books published until the middle of the twentieth century often divided muscle activities
into either dynamic efforts lasting for minutes or hours, with work, energy, and endurance typical topics;
or short bursts of contractile exertion. Much research on muscle effort concerned the “isometric” con-
dition in which muscle length (and hence body segment position) did not change. Consequently, much
information on muscle strength applies to such static exertion. All other muscle activities were typically
called “anisometric,” often even falsely labeled “isotonic” or “kinetic,” meant to cover all the many poss-
ible dynamic muscle uses. Chaffin et al. (1999), Marras et al. (1993), Kroemer (1999), and Kumar (2004)
discuss proper terminology: Table 10.1 lists and explains terms that correctly describe muscular events.
For the engineer, skeletal muscles are of primary interest since they pull on segments of the human
body and generate energy for exertion to outside objects. Skeletal muscles connect two body links
across their joint, as shown in Figure 10.1; in some cases muscles cross even two joints. Muscles are
usually arranged in “functional pairs” so that contracting muscles counteract each other. The muscle,
or the group of synergistic muscles, pulling in the intended direction is the agonist (also called protago-
nist) and the opposite is the antagonist. Cocontraction, the simultaneous activation of paired opposing
muscles, serves to control speed and strength exertion.
There are several hundred skeletal muscles in the human body, known by their Latin names. Connec-
tive tissue (fascia) enwraps them; it imbeds nerves and blood vessels. At the ends of the muscle, the tissues
combine to form tendons, which usually attach to bones.
Thousands of individual muscle fibers run, more or less parallel to the length of the muscle. Seen via a
microscope, skeletal muscle fibers appear striped (striated) crosswise: thin and thick, light and dark
bands run across the fiber in regular patterns, which repeat along the length of the fiber. One such
thick dark stripe appears to penetrate the fiber like a membrane or disc: this is the so-called z-disk
(from the German zwischen, between). The distance between two adjacent z-lines defines the sarcomere.
Its length at rest is approximately 250 Å (1 Å ¼ 10210 m), meaning that there are about 40,000 sarco-
meres in series within 1 mm of muscle fiber length.
Within each muscle fiber, thread-like myofibrils (from the Greek mys, muscle) lie in parallel by the
hundreds or thousands. Each of these, in turn, consists of bundles of myofilaments. A network of
tubular channels, sacs and cisterns, which connect with a larger tubular system in the z-disks, fill the
spaces between the filaments. All of this is part of the networks of blood vessels and nerves in the fascia.
This is the “plumbing and control” system of the muscle, the sarcoplasmic reticulum. It provides fluid
transport between the cells inside and outside the muscle and carries chemical and electrical messages.
Two of the myofibrils, myosin and actin, have the ability to slide along each other; this is the source of
muscular contraction. Small projections, called cross-bridges, protrude from the myosin filaments towards
neighboring actins. The actin filaments are twisted double-stranded protein molecules, wrapped in a
double helix around the myosin molecules. This is the “contracting microstructure” of the muscle.
The only active action that a muscle can take is contraction; external forces that stretch the muscle
bring about passive elongation. According the “sliding filament theory,” the heads of adjacent actin
rods moving toward each other cause contraction. This pulls the z-disks closer together: sarcomeres
in series (and those parallel) shorten, and as a result, the whole muscle shortens. After a contraction,
the muscle returns to its resting length, primarily through a recoiling of its shortened filaments,
fibrils, fibers, and other connective tissues. Force external to the muscle can stretch the muscle beyond
its resting length, either by gravity or other force acting from outside the body, or by the action of antag-
onistic muscle. (Refer to texts by Asimov, 1963; Astrand and Rodahl, 1977, 1986; Chaffin et al., 1999;
Enoka, 2002; Kroemer et al., 1997, 2001; Schneck 1990, 1992; and Winter, 1990, among others, for
more information.)
AL
100% TOT
Tension
AC
T IVE
E
IV
SS
PA
0
60% 100% 200%
Resting length
FIGURE 10.1 The biceps muscle reduces the elbow angle as agonist, counteracted by the triceps muscle and
antagonist. Note the simplification of the actual conditions in modeling: in addition to the biceps, two other
muscles (radialis and brachioradialis) also contribute to flexion about the elbow joint.
curl completely around the myosin rods. This is the shortest possible length of the sarcomeres, below
which the muscle cannot develop any additional active contraction force.
Near resting length, the cross-bridges between the actin and myosin rods are in an optimal position to
generate contact for contractile pull force. If the muscle is elongated further, the actin and myosin fibrils
slide along each other, reducing the cross-bridge overlap between the protein rods. At about 150% of
resting length, very little overlap remains that the proteins cannot actively resist the elongation
anymore and only passive stretch resistance remains. Thus, the curve of active contractile tension devel-
oped within a muscle in isometric twitch is near zero at approximately half resting length, then rises to a
maximum at about resting length, and finally falls back to a minimum at about 150% resting length.
Figure 10.2 indicates this schematically while Figure 10.5 in Marras’ chapter on “Occupational Biome-
chanics” in this book shows more detail.
The muscle also passively resists stretch like a rubber band. This passive resistance becomes stronger as
the muscle is pulled more beyond its resting length and is strongest near the point of muscle or tendon
(attachment) breakage. Thus, above the resting length, the tension in the muscle is the summation of
active and passive strains. The summation effect explains why we stretch muscles for a strong exertion,
like in bringing the arm behind the shoulder before throwing a rock. This “preloading” tenses the muscle
for a strong exertion – see Figure 10.2 and Marras’ Figure 10.5.
In engineering terms, the muscles exhibit “viscoelastic” qualities. They are viscous in that their beha-
vior depends both on the amount by which they are deformed, and on the rate of deformation. They are
elastic in that they return to the original length and shape after deformation. These behaviors help to
explain why the tension that can be developed isometrically (“statically,” especially in a state of eccentric
stretch) is the highest possible one, while in active shortening (in a “dynamic” concentric movement)
muscle tension is decidedly lower (Schneck, 1990, 1992).
scapula
humerus
biceps
triceps
radius
ulna
FIGURE 10.2 Active, passive and, total tension within a muscle at different lengths. (With permission by the
publisher from Kroemer et al., Engineering Physiology. Bases of Human Factors/Ergonomics, 3rd ed., Van Nostrand
Reinhold — John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1997. All rights reserved.)
Paradoxically, by contracting the muscle can compress its own fine blood vessels that permeate the
muscle tissues. A strongly contracting muscle generates pressure within itself, as can be felt by touching
a tightened biceps or calf muscle. By such strong pressure, the muscle reduces the cross-section of the
vessels, diminishing or even shutting off its own blood circulation. The interruption of blood flow
through a muscle stops the metabolic energy conversion and allows metabolic by-products to be
accumulated in the muscle tissues; this quickly leads to muscle fatigue, forcing relaxation. Experiencing
such fatigue is painful as it occurs slowly when the muscle is not contracting to its maximum, when
working overhead with raised arms, for example, while fastening a screw in the ceiling of a room.
Muscle fatigue in the shoulder muscles makes it impossible to keep one’s arms raised even after a
minute or so, even though nerve impulses from the CNS still arrive at the neuromuscular junctions,
and the resulting action potentials continue to spread over the muscle fibers.
The operational definition of muscle fatigue is “a state of reduced physical ability that can be restored
by rest.” Figure 10.3 shows the relation between static exertion and muscle endurance schematically: a
maximal exertion can be maintained for just a few seconds; 50% of the tension is present for about
1 min; but less than 20% can be applied for long endurance periods.
100%
Tension 50%
0
0 5 10 min
Endurance
FIGURE 10.3 Muscle exertion and endurance. (With permission by the publisher from Kroemer et al., Engineering
Physiology. Bases of Human Factors/Ergonomics, 3rd ed., Van Nostrand Reinhold — John Wiley & Sons, 1997.
New York, All rights reserved.)
origin
−M
H Muscle
M Link U
Link L insertion
a
h
FIGURE 10.4 The muscle-tendon unit exerts pull forces M to links L and U at origin and insertion. Note the
simplification in modeling the actual conditions at the elbow: in reality, the biceps muscle has its origin proximal
to the shoulder joint: two other muscles (radialis and brachioradialis) contribute to flexion about the elbow joint.
Torque T ¼ m M sin a must be transmitted across the wrist joint to generate hand force H. (With permission
by the publisher from Kroemer et al., Engineering Physiology. Bases of Human Factors/Ergonomics, 3rd ed., Van
Nostrand Reinhold — John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1997. All rights reserved.)
Human Strength Evaluation 10-7
T ¼ m M sin a (10:1)
This torque T then counteracts an external force H, acting perpendicularly at its lever arm h,
according to
T ¼ m M sin a ¼ h H: (10:2)
DEFINITIONS
To help distinguish among muscle tension, its internal transmission, and the final exertion to an
outside object, it is useful to define terms as follows:
Muscle strength is the maximal tension (or force) that muscle can develop voluntarily between its
origin and insertion.
The best word to refer to this is “muscle tension” (in N/mm2 or N/cm2) but the term strength
(force in N) is commonly used. If the variables m, h, a and H in Equation (10.2) are known, one
can solve for muscle force as follows:
Internal transmission is the manner in which muscle tension transfers in the form of torque
inside the body along links and across joint(s) to the point of application to a resisting object.
If several link-joint systems in series constitute the internal path of torque (in N m or N cm)
transmission, each transfers the arriving torque by the existent ratio of lever arms (m and h in
the example shown earlier) until resistance is met, which is usually the point where the body
interfaces with an outside object. This transfer of torques is more complicated under dynamic
conditions than in the static case because of changes in muscle functions with motion, changes in
lever arms and pull angles, and because of the effects of accelerations and decelerations of masses.
10-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Body segment strength is the force or torque that a body segment can apply to an object that is
external to the body.
Hand, shoulder, back, and foot are the body segments with which we commonly apply our
“strength” as force (in N) or torque (in N m or N cm) to an object.
The quality and quantity of the force or torque transmitted to an outside object depends on many bio-
mechanical and physical conditions, which are as follows:
. Body segment employed, for example, hand or foot
. Type of body object attachment, for example, by a touch or grasp
. Coupling type, for example, by friction or interlocking
. Direction of force/torque vector
. Static posture or body motions with dynamic exertion
Within the field of ergonomics (aka human factors aka human engineering)
. Muscle strength draws in particular, the attention of the engineering physiologist
. Internal transmission is of concern to both the biomechanist and the designer because of the
implications for body segment posture and motion
. Body segment strength is of great practical interest to the designer of tools, equipment, and work
tasks
Figure 10.5 shows, in the form of a flow diagram, the feed-forward of excitation signals sent from the
CNS to the muscle in order to generate tension. The diagram also shows that one can record associated
signals (EEGs, electroencephalograms), muscle activation (EMGs, electromyograms), and calculate
muscle tension and developed torques (via biomechanical modeling). Resulting torque and force that
are available for application by a body segment to a resisting object (often a handle or pedal) can be
measured together with body posture and motion.
Figure 10.5 also identifies three feedback paths, although at present they do not provide convenient
avenues for measurements. The first is a reflex loop F1 that originates at interoceptors. The other two
loops start at exteroceptors and lead to a comparator, where they modify the input to the CNS. F2 pro-
vides kinesthetic signals related to touch, body position, and motion; F3 is similar but relates specifically
to task execution. F3 commonly involves sound and vision. (Note that the experimenter can easily
manipulate these feedback signals, for example, via verbal exhortation, or with an instrument that
shows the magnitude of the applied force, or a desired value.)
Feedback Measures
CNS Programs
Activate Executive Programs
Subroutines ?
Motivation
Generate
F1 Efferent Signals EEG (measured)
MUSCLES(S)
Activate EMG (measured)
Motor Units
F2 Generate
Tension (calculated)
Muscle Tension
INTERNAL TRANSMISSION
F3 Posture, motion
Activate
(measured)
Pull Angles, Lever Arms
about Joint(s)
SEGMENT STRENGTH
TASK EXECUTION
Incomplete Successful
END
FIGURE 10.5 A conceptual model of the feed-forward and feed-back loops employed in generating and controlling
muscle strength exertion. (Modified from Kroemer K. H. E. In Proceedings, Annual Conference of the Human Factors
Society, Santa Monica, CA, pp. 19–20, 1979; Kroemer et al. Ergonomics: How to Design for Erase and Efficiency,
Prentice Hall, Englewood cliffs, NJ, 1994; Kroemer K. H. E. et al. Bases of Human Factors/Ergonomics, 3rd ed., Van
Nostrand Reinhold — John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1997. With permission.)
The static condition is theoretically simple and experimentally well controllable. It allows a rather easy
measurement of muscular effort. Therefore, most of the information currently available on “human strength”
describes the outcomes of static (isometric) testing. Accordingly, most of the tables on body segment
strength in this chapter, and in other human engineering or physiologic literature, contain static data.
10-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Besides the simple convenience of dealing with statics, measurement of isometric strength yields
for most cases of practical design interest a reasonable estimate of the maximally possible exertion.
That estimate also applies to slow body segment movements, especially if they are eccentric. However,
the data do not estimate fast exertions well, especially if they are concentric and of the ballistic-
impulse type, such as throwing or hammering.
strongest operator cannot break a handle or pedal. Accordingly, we set the design value with a
safety margin above the highest perceivable strength application.
. Minimal user strength is that expected from the weakest operator, which still yields the desired
result, so that a door handle or brake pedal can be operated successfully or a heavy object be
moved.
Human Strength Evaluation
TABLE 10.2 Techniques to Measure Muscle Performance by Selecting Specific Independent and Dependent Variables
Isoinertia
Names of Isometric Isovelocity Isoacceleration Isojerk Isoforce (Static (Static or
Technique (Static) Indep. (Dynamic) (Dynamic) (Dynamic) or Dynamic) Dynamic) Free Dynamic
Variables Dep. Indep. Dep. Indep. Dep. Indep. Dep. Indep. Dep. Indep. Dep. Indep. Dep.
Displacement, constant ðzeroÞ C or X C or X C or X C or X C or X X
linear/
angular
Velocity, 0 constant C or X C or X C or X C or X X
linear/
angular
Acceleration, 0 0 constant C or X C or X C or X X
linear/
angular
Jerk, linear/ 0 0 0 constant C or X C or X X
angular
Force, torque C or X C or X C or X C or X constant C or X X
Mass, moment C C C C C constant C or X
of inertia
Repetition C or X C or X C or X C or X C or X C or X C or X
Note: Indep: independent; Dep: dependent; C: variable can be controlled; : set to zero; 0: variable is not present (zero); X: can be dependent variables. The boxed constant variable provides
the descriptive name.
Source: [With permission by the publisher from Kroemer, et al., Engineering Physiology. Bases of Human Factors/Ergonomics. 3rd ed., Van Nostrand Reinhold — John Wiley & Sons,
New York, 1997. All rights reserved.]
10-11
10-12 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
. A range of expected strength exertions is, obviously, that between the considered minimum and
maximum. The infamous “average user” strength is usually of no design value — see the
Chapter 9 on “Engineering Anthropometry” in this handbook.
Most data on body segment strength apply to static (isometric) exertions. They provide reasonable
guidance also for slow motions, although they are probably too high for concentric motions and a bit
too low for eccentric motions. Of the little information available for dynamic strength exertions,
much is limited to isokinematic (constant velocity) cases. As a rule, strength exerted in motion is less
than that measured in static positions located on the path of motion.
The usual statistical treatment of measured strength data assumed that they fall into a normal distri-
bution, which allows describing them in terms of averages (means) and standard deviations. This also
allows the use of common statistical techniques to determine data points of special interest to the designer
— as discussed in detail in the Chapter 9 on “Engineering Anthropometry” in this book. In reality, data
describing body segment strength often appear in a skewed rather than in a bell-shaped distribution. The
actual shape of the distribution is not of great concern, however, if the data points of special interest are
the extremes. We can determine the maximal forces or torques, which the equipment must be able to bear
without breaking, as those above the strongest measured data points. We can identify the minimal exer-
tions, which even “weak” persons are able to generate at the low end of the distribution; again, see the
“Anthropometry” Chapter 9 for procedures to calculate or estimate the design values.
TABLE 10.3 Average Forces and Standard Deviations (N) Exerted by Nine Subjects with their Fingertips, Depend-
ing on the Angle of the Proximal Interphalangeal Joint (PIP)
PIP at 308 PIP at 608
Direction Direction
Digit Fore Aft Down Fore Aft Down
2, index finger 5.4 (2.0) 5.5 (2.2) 27.4 (13.0) 5.2 (2.4) 6.8 (2.8) 24.4 (13.6)
2, non- 4.8 (2.2) 6.1 (2.2) 21.7 (11.7) 5.6 (2.9) 5.3 (2.1) 25.1 (13.7)
preferred
3, middle finger 4.8 (2.5) 5.4 (2.4) 24.0 (12.6) 4.2 (1.9) 6.5 (2.2) 21.3 (10.9)
4, ring finger 4.3 (2.4) 5.2 (2.0) 19.1 (10.4) 3.7 (1.7) 5.2 (1.9) 19.5 (10.9)
5, little finger 4.8 (1.9) 4.1 (1.6) 15.1 (8.0) 3.5 (1.6) 3.5 (2.2) 15.5 (8.5)
Source: [With permission by the publisher from Kroemer, et al., Ergonomics: How to Design for Ease and Efficiency, 2nd ed.,
Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ, 2001. All rights reserved.]
TABLE 10.4 Mean Poke Forces, and Standard Deviations (N) Exerted by 30 Subjects in the Direction of the Straight
Digits
10 Male 10 Male 10 Female
Digit Mechanics Students Students
1, Thumb 83.8 (25.2) A 46.7 (29.2) C 32.4 (15.4) D
2, index finger 60.4 (25.8) B 45.0 (30.0) C 25.4 (9.6) DE
3, middle finger 55.9 (31.9) B 41.3 (21.6) C 21.5 (6.5) E
Note: Entries followed by different letters are different from each other (p 0.05).
Source: [With permission by the publisher from Kroemer, et al., Ergonomics: How to Design for Ease and Efficiency, 2nd ed.
Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ, 2001. All rights reserved.]
10-14
TABLE 10.5 Mean Forces and Standard Deviations (N) Exerted by 21 Male students and by 12 Male Machinists
Digit 3 Digit 4 Digit 5
Couplings (see Figure 8–26) Digit 1 (Thumb) Digit 2 (Index) (Middle) (Ring) (Little)
Push with digit tip in 91 (39) 52 (16) 51 (20) 35 (12) 30(10) See also Table 11.4
direction of the extended 138 (41) 84 (35) 86 (28) 66 (22) 52 (14)
digit (“Poke”)
Digit touch (Coupling #1) 84 (33) 43 (14) 36 (13) 30 (13) 25 (10) —
perpendicular to 131 (42) 70 (17) 76 (20) 57 (17) 55 (16)
extended digit
Source: [With permission by the publisher from Kroemer, et al., Ergonomics: How to Design for Ease and Efficiency 2nd ed., Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ, 2001. All rights reserved.]
Human Strength Evaluation 10-15
FIGURE 10.6 Couplings between hand and handle. (Adapted from Kroemer, Hum. Factors, 28, pp, 337 –339. With
permission. All rights reserved.)
segment” is present in the sequence of torques about body joints. Starting at the point of external exer-
tion, for instance, at the hand, one assesses the strength requirements joint-by-joint along the arm,
shoulder, and back. Often, the lumbar back area is the “weak link.”
A walking or standing person receives all support from the ground, of course, and not seldom hip or
knee joint strength limits the ability to do hard efforts, such as lifting a load on the back. A slippery
10-16 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
100
(Schematic)
90
70
60
50
40
FIGURE 10.7 Relation of elbow angle and elbow torque. (With permission by the publisher from Kroemer et al.,
Ergonomics: How to Design for Ease and Efficiency, Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ, 2001. All rights reserved.)
2 3
180° 4
1 150°
60° 120°
90°
FIGURE 10.8 Fifth-percentile arm forces (N) exerted by sitting men. (Adapted from MIL HDBK 759.)
Human Strength Evaluation 10-17
TABLE 10.6 Horizontal Push and Pull Forces (N) that Male Soldiers can Exert Intermittently or
for Short Periods of Time
Condition (m is the
Horizontal Forcea coefficient of friction
at least Applied withb at the floor)
100 N push or pull Both hands, or one With low traction, m
shoulder, or the back between 0.2 and 0.3
200 N push or pull Both hands, or one With medium traction, m
shoulder, or the back about 0.6
250 N push One hand If braced against a vertical
wall, 50 to 150 cm from
the push panel and
parallel to it
300 N push or pull Both hands, or one With high traction, m above
shoulder, or the back 0.9
500 N push or pull Both hands, or one If braced against a vertical
shoulder, or the back wall, 50 to 150 cm from
the push panel and
parallel to it, or if
anchoring the feet on a
perfectly nonslip ground,
such as at footrest
750 N push The back If braced against a vertical
wall, 60 to 110 cm from
the push panel and
parallel to it, or if
anchoring the feet on a
perfectly non slip
ground, such as at
footrest
a
May be nearly doubled for two and less than tripled for three operators pushing simultaneously. For
the fourth and each additional operator, add about 75% of their push capabilities.
b
See Figure 11.9 for example.
Source: Adapted from MIL-STD 1472.
surface may make it impossible to push a heavy object sideways with the shoulder; one can experience
this in the winter on an icy ground when trying to push a car out of the ditch. To a sitting person, the seat
provides most of the reaction that counters the forces actively exerted through the upper body and arms,
although some support may be gathered from the floor via the legs.
Force-plate(1) Force, N
Distance(2)
height Mean SD
50 80 664 177
50 100 772 216
50 120 780 165
70 80 716 162
70 100 731 233
70 120 820 138
90 80 625 147
90 100 678 195
90 120 863 141
Percent of shoulder height Both hands
60 70 761 172
60 80 854 177
60 90 792 141
70 60 580 110
70 70 698 124
70 80 729 140
80 60 521 130
80 70 620 129
80 80 636 133
Percent of shoulder height
70 70 623 147
70 80 688 154
70 90 586 132
80 70 545 127
80 80 543 123
80 90 533 81
90 70 433 95
90 80 448 93
90 90 485 80
Percent of shoulder height Both hands
Both hands
Force plate 100 percent 50 581 143
of shoulder 60 667 160
height 70 981 271
80 1285 398
90 980 302
100 646 254
Preferred hand
50 262 67
60 298 71
70 360 98
80 520 142
90 494 169
100 427 173
Percent of
thumb-tip reach*
(1)Height of the center of the force plate – 20 cm high by 25 cm long – upon which force is applied.
(2)Horizontal distance between the vertical surface of the force plate and the opposing vertical surface (wall or
footrest, respectively) against which the subjects brace themselves.
*Thumb-tip reach – distance from backrest to tip of subject’s thumb as arm and hand are extended forward.
**Span – the maximal distance between a person’s fingertips when arms and hands are extended to each side.
FIGURE 10.9 Mean horizontal push forces and standard deviations (N) exerted by standing men with their hands,
the shoulder, and the back. (Adapted from NASA STD. 3000 A, 1989.)
Human Strength Evaluation 10-19
Finger
Manipulation
Thumb Use of hand tools
Carrying by hand
Hand
Wrist
Elbow
Shoulder Carrying on
shoulder or
Back back
Hip
Knee
Ankle
FIGURE 10.10 Determining the critical body segment strength for manipulating and carrying. (With permission
by the publisher from Kroemer et al., Engineering Physiology. Bases of Human Factors/Ergonomics, 3rd ed., Van
Nostrand Reinhold — John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1997. All rights reserved.)
but the downward or down-and-fore directions are preferred, especially for forceful exertions. Nearly
fully extended legs can generate very large forces roughly in the direction of the lower leg. A fully
extended knee angle (b in Figure 10.11), body inertia, and buttock and back support all limit leg
force. Current automobile designs illustrate these principles. For example, under normal conditions,
the driver an easily operate clutch and brake pedals with a knee angle of about 908. However, a failure
of the power-assist system unexpectedly requires very large forces from the feet; in this case, the
driver must thrust the backside against a strong backrest and extend the legs to generate the needed
pedal force.
Figure 10.11 through Figure 10.15 provide information about the forces that can be applied with legs
and feet to a pedal. Of course, the forces depend on body support and hip and knee angles. The largest
forward thrust force can be exerted with the nearly extended legs, which leaves very little room to move
the foot control further away from the hip.
Of course, the strength that a foot can exert to an object such as a pedal, depends on the existent
muscle strength, the means by which it can be transmitted along the “joint chain” ankle-knee-hip,
and the way in which the seat provides the needed reactive support — see Figure 10.16. The diagram
shows that an improper seat, a frail hip, knee, or ankle, or bad coupling of the shoe with the object
may all make for a “weak kick.”
Information on body strengths has been compiled in NASA and U.S. Military Standards; for
example, by Chengular et al. (2003), Kroemer et al. (1997, 2001); Peebles and Norris (1998, 2000,
2002), Weimer (1993), and Woodson et al. (1991). However, caution is necessary when applying
these data because they were measured with various techniques on different populations under
varying conditions.
10-20 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
FIGURE 10.11 Body segment angles, seat dimensions, and pedal location affecting foot force exertion. (With
permission by the publisher from Kroemer et al., Ergonomics: How to Design for Ease and Efficiency, Prentice Hall,
Upper Saddle River, NJ, 2001. All rights reserved.)
100
R = 14cm
90 r = 12cm
80
70
% Average max. leg force
60
50
40
5° ≤ α ≤ 10°
30 15° ≤ α ≤19°
20
10 – 6° ≤ α ≤ –15°
0
60 80 100 120 140 160 180 deg.
β : Knee angle
FIGURE 10.12 Effects of thigh angle a and knee angle b — see Figure 10.11 — on pedal push force. (With
permission by the publisher from Kroemer et al., Ergonomics: How to Design for Ease and Efficiency, Prentice Hall,
Upper Saddle River, NJ, 2001. All rights reserved.)
Human Strength Evaluation 10-21
100
90
70
D = 93 ± 3 cm
H = –9 cm
60
50
30 40 50 60 70 80 90 deg.
δ : Pedal angle
FIGURE 10.13 Effects of ankle (pedal) angle d — see Figure 10.11— on foot force generated by ankle rotation.
(With permission by the publisher from Kroemer, et al., Ergonomics: How to Design for Ease and Efficiency,
Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ, 2001. All rights reserved.)
10.8 Summary
Muscle contraction is brought about by active shortening of muscle substructures. Elongation of the
muscle is due to external forces. Maximal muscle tension depends on the individual’s muscle size and
exertion skill.
Prolonged strong contraction leads to muscular fatigue, which hinders the continuation of the effort
and finally cuts it off. Hence, maximal voluntary contraction can be maintained only for a few seconds.
100
0.95 reach
% Average max. leg force
90
80
0.85 reach
70
FIGURE 10.14 Effects of pedal height H and leg extension — see Figure 10.11 — on pedal push force. (With
permission by the publisher from Kroemer et al., Ergonomics: How to Design for Ease and Efficiency, Prentice Hall,
Upper Saddle River, NJ, 2001. All rights reserved.)
10-22 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
100
α = 15°
90 β = 160°
60
50 r = 13 cm
0 10 20 30 40 cm
R : Height of backrest above seat (SRP)
FIGURE 10.15 Effects of backrest height R — see Figure 10.11 — on pedal push force. (With permission by the
publisher from Kroemer et al., Ergonomics: How to Design for Ease and Efficiency, Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle
River, NJ, 2001. All rights reserved.)
In isometric contraction, muscle length remains constant, which establishes a static condition for the
body segments affected by the muscle. In an isotonic effort, the muscle tension remains constant, which
usually coincides with a static (isometric) effort.
Dynamic activities result from changes in muscle length, which bring about motion of body segments.
In an isokinematic effort, speed remains unchanged. In an isoinertial test, the mass properties remain
constant.
Human body (segment) strength is measured routinely as the force (or torque) exerted to an instru-
ment external to the body. This is information of great importance to the ergonomic designer/engineer.
Design of equipment and work tasks to match human body strength capabilities considers these
aspects:
. Determine whether the exertion is static or dynamic
. Establish the part of the body on which the force or torque is exerted
. Select the segment strength percentile (minimum and maximum) that is critical for the operation
. Follow the chain of strength vectors through the involved body segments to find the “weak link”
and to improve and rearrange the conditions if possible
Foot
Ankle
Knee
Hip
Reaction Force
Standing Sitting: provided by
Not Recommended Seat design proper seat interface
FIGURE 10.16 Determining the “critical body segment strength” for foot operation.
Human Strength Evaluation 10-23
References
Asimov, I., The Human Body. Its Structure and Operation, The New American Library/Signet, New York,
1963.
Astrand, P.O. and Rodahl, K., Textbook of Work Physiology, 2nd, 3rd ed., McGraw-Hill, New York, 1977,
1986.
Chaffin, D.B., Andersson, G.B.J., and Martin, B.J., Occupational Biomechanics, 3rd ed., John Wiley &
Sons, New York, 1999.
Chengular, S.N., Rodgers, S.H., and Bernard, T.E., Kodak’s Ergonomic Design for People at Work, 2nd ed.,
John Wiley & Sons, New York, 2003.
Enoka, R.M., Neuromechanical Basis of Kinesiology, Human Kinetics Books, Champaign, IL, 1988.
Enoka, R.M., Neuromechanics of Human Movement, 3rd. ed., Human Kinetics Books, Champaign, IL,
2002.
Karwowski, W., ed., International Encyclopedia of Ergonomics and Human Factors, Taylor & Francis,
London, UK, 2001.
Kroemer, K.H.E., A new model of muscle strength regulation, in Proceedings, Annual Conference of the
Human Factors Society, Human Factors Society Santa Monica, CA, pp. 19–20, 1979.
Kroemer, K.H.E., Assessment of human muscle strength for engineering purposes: basics and definitions,
Ergonomics, 42, pp. 74 –93, 1999.
Kroemer, K.H.E., Kroemer, H.B., and Kroemer-Elbert, K.E., Ergonomics: How to Design for Ease and
Efficiency, 2nd ed., Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ, 2001.
Kroemer, K.H.E., Kroemer, H.J., and Kroemer-Elbert, K.E., Engineering Physiology. Bases of Human
Factors/Ergonomics, 3rd ed., Van Nostrand Reinhold — John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1997.
Kroemer, K.H.E., Marras, W.S., McGlothlin, J.D., McIntyre, D.R., and Nordin, M., Assessing Human
Dynamic Muscle Strength (Technical Report, 8-30-89), Virginia Tech, Industrial Ergonomics
Laboratory Blacksburg, VA, 1989. Also published in Int. J. Indus. Ergon., 6, pp. 199–210, 1990.
Kumar, S., Ed., Muscle Strength, CRC Press — Taylor & Francis, Boca Raton, FL, 2004.
Marras, W.S., McGlothlin, J.D., McIntyre, D.R., Nordin, M., and Kroemer, K.H.E., Dynamic Measures of
Low Back Performance, American Industrial Hygiene Association, Fairfax, VA, 1993.
Peebles, L. and Norris, B., Adultdata. The Handbook of Adult Anthropometric and Strength Measurements
— Data for Design Safety. (DTI/Pub 2917/3k/6/98/NP), Department of Trade and Industry,
London, UK, 1998.
Peebles, L. and Norris, B., Strength Data (DTI/URN 00/1070), Department of Trade and Industry,
London, UK, 2000.
Peebles, L. and Norris, B., Filling “gaps” in strength data for design, Appl. Ergon., 34, pp. 73–88, 2003.
Rodgers, S.H., Work physiology — fatigue and recovery, in G. Salvendy, Ed., Handbook of Human Factors
and Ergonomics, 2nd ed., John Wiley & Sons, New York, Chap. 10, pp. 268 –297, 1997.
Salvendy, G., Ed., Handbook of Human Factors and Ergonomics, 2nd ed., John Wiley & Sons, New York,
1997.
Schneck, D.J., Engineering Principles of Physiologic Function, New York University Press, New York, 1990.
Schneck, D.J., Mechanics of Muscle, 2nd ed., New York University Press, New York, 1992.
Weimer, J., Handbook of Ergonomic and Human Factors Tables, Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ, 1993.
Winter, D.A., Biomechanics and Motor Control of Human Movement, 2nd ed., John Wiley & Sons,
New York, 1990.
Woodson, W.E., Tillman, B., and Tillman, P., Human Factors Design Handbook, 2nd ed., McGraw-Hill,
New York, 1991.
11
Biomechanical Basis
for Ergonomics
11.1.1 Definitions
Biomechanics can be defined as an interdisciplinary field in which information from both the biological
sciences and engineering mechanics is used to assess the function of the body. A major assumption of
occupational biomechanics is that the body behaves according to the laws of Newtonian mechanics.
By definition, “mechanics is the study of forces and their effects on masses” (Kroemer, 1987). The
object of interest in an occupational ergonomics context is most often a quantitative assessment of mech-
anical loading that occurs within the musculoskeletal system. The goal of an occupational biomechanics
11-1
11-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
assessment is to quantitatively describe the musculoskeletal loading that occurs during work so that one
can derive an appreciation for the degree of risk associated with an occupationally related task.
The characteristic that distinguishes occupational biomechanics analyses from other types of ergonomic
analyses is that the comparison is quantitative in nature. The quantitative nature of occupational biome-
chanics permits ergonomists to address the question of “how much exposure to the occupational risk
factors is too much exposure?”
The portion of biomechanics dealing with ergonomics issues is often labeled industrial or occupational
biomechanics. Chaffin et al. (1999) have defined occupational biomechanics as “the study of the physical
interaction of workers with their tools, machines, and materials so as to enhance the worker’s perform-
ance while minimizing the risk of musculoskeletal disorders.” This chapter will address occupational bio-
mechanical issues exclusively in this ergonomics framework.
Tolerance
Safety
Spinal Load
Margin
Loading
Pattern
Time
results in tissue damage, some ergonomists are beginning to expand the concept of tolerance to include
not only mechanical tolerance of the tissue, but also the point at which the tissue exhibits an inflamma-
tory reaction.
Industrial tasks are becoming more repetitive involving lighter loads. The conceptual load tolerance
model can also be adjusted to also account for this type of risk exposure. As shown in Figure 11.2, occu-
pational biomechanics logic can account for the fact that with repetitive loading the tolerance of the struc-
ture tissue may decrease over time to the point where it is more likely that the structure loading will exceed
the structure tolerance and result in injury or illness. Thus, occupational biomechanical models and logic
are moving towards systems that consider manufacturing and work trends in the workplace and attempt
to represent these observations (such as cumulative trauma disorders) in the model logic.
Tolerance
Loading Pattern
Time
in Figure 11.1. Cumulative trauma, on the other hand, refers to the repeated application of force to a
structure that tends to wear down a structure, thus, lowering the structure tolerance to the point
where the tolerance is exceeded through a reduction of the tolerance limit. This situation was illustrated
in Figure 11.2. Cumulative trauma represents more of a “wear and tear” on the structure. This type of
trauma is becoming far more common in the workplace since more repetitive jobs are becoming
common in industry and is the mechanism of concern for many ergonomics evaluations.
Cumulative trauma can initiate a process that can result in a tissue reactive cycle that is extremely dif-
ficult to break. This process is illustrated in Figure 11.3. The cumulative trauma process begins by expos-
ing the worker to manual exertions that are either frequent (repetitive) or prolonged. This repetitive
application of force can affect either the tendons or the muscles of the body. If the tendons are affected,
the following sequence occurs. The tendons are subject to mechanical irritation when they are repeatedly
exposed to high levels of tension and groups of tendons may rub against each other. The physiologic reac-
tion to this mechanical irritation can result in inflammation and swelling of the tendon. This swelling will
stimulate the nociceptors surrounding the structure and signal the central control mechanism (brain) via
pain perception that a problem exists. In response to this pain, the body will attempt to control the
problem via two mechanisms. First, the muscles surrounding the irritated area will coactivate in an
attempt to stabilize the motion of the tendons or stiffen the structure. Since motion will further stimulate
the nociceptors and result in further pain, motion avoidance is often indicative of the start of a cumu-
lative trauma disorder. Second, in an attempt to reduce the friction occurring within the tendon, the
body will increase its production of synovial fluid within the tendon sheath. However, given the
limited space available between the tendon and the tendon sheath the increased production of synovial
fluid often exacerbates the problem by further expanding the tendon sheath and, in thus, further stimu-
lating the surrounding nociceptors. As indicated in the figure, this initiates a viscous cycle where the
response of the tendon to the increased friction results in a reaction (inflammation and the increased
production of synovial fluid) that exacerbates the problem. Once this cycle is initiated it is very difficult
to stop and often anti-inflammatory agents are required. This process results in chronic joint pain and a
series of musculoskeletal reactions such as reduced strength, reduced tendon motion, and reduced mobi-
lity. Collectively, these reactions result in a functional disability.
Functional Disability
A similar process occurs if the muscles are affected by cumulative trauma as opposed to the tendons.
Muscles can be easily overloaded when they become fatigued. Fatigue can lower the tolerance to stress
and can result in microtrauma to the muscle fibers. This microtrauma typically means that the
muscle is partially torn and the tear will cause capillaries to rupture and result in swelling, edema, or
inflammation near the site of the tear. This process can stimulate nociceptors and result in pain. As
with cumulative trauma to the tendons, the body reacts by cocontracting the surrounding musculature
and thereby minimizing the motion of the joint. Since the tendons are not involved with cumulative
trauma to the muscles there is no increased production of synovial fluid. However, the end result is
the same series of musculoskeletal reactions resulting from tendon irritation (i.e., reduced strength,
reduced tendon motion, and reduced mobility). The ultimate result of this process is once again a func-
tional disability.
Even though the stimulus associated with the cumulative trauma process is somewhat similar between
tendons and muscles there is a significant difference in the time required to heal from the damage to a
tendon compared to a muscle. The mechanism of repair for both the tendons and muscles is dependent
upon blood flow to the damaged structure. Blood provides nutrients for repair as well as dissipates waste
materials. However, the blood supply to a tendon is just a fraction (typically about 5% in an adult) of that
supplied to a muscle. Thus, given an equivalent strain to a muscle and a tendon, the muscle will heal
rapidly (in about 10 days if not reinjured), whereas the tendon could take months (20 times longer)
to accomplish the same level of repair. For this reason, ergonomists must be particularly vigilant in
the assessment of workplaces that could pose a danger to the tendons of the body. This lengthy repair
process also explains why most ergonomics processes place a high value on identifying potentially
risky jobs prior to a lost time incident through mechanisms such as discomfort surveys.
(a) (b)
Internal Load (F) Internal Load (F)
F ..0254 m = 44.5 N .305 m F ..0127 m = 89 N .1525 m
44.5 N .305 m 89 N .1525 m
F= F=
.0254 m .0127 m
F = 534.35 N (120 lbs.) Internal F = 1068 N (240 lbs.)
Load (F)
Internal .0127 m
Load (F) External Load
External Load
.0254 m
.305 m
44.5 N .1525 m
89 N
FIGURE 11.4 An example of an anatomical third-class lever (a) demonstrating how the mechanical advantage
changes as the elbow position changes (b).
force acts in between the two. An example of this system in the human body is the elbow joint and is
shown in Figure 11.4.
length –strength relationships, and temporal relationships) can be manipulated in order to facilitate this
goal and serve as the basis for many ergonomic recommendations.
Active
+
Passive
Passive
Tension
Active
400
355
310
Force (Nt)
265
220
175
130
85
40
30 45 60 75 90 105 120
Elbow Angle (deg)
FIGURE 11.6 Position-force diagram produced by flexion of the forearm in pronation. “Angle” refers to included
angle between the longitudinal axes of the forearm and upper arm. The highest parts of the curve indicate the
configurations where the biomechanical lever system is most effective. (Adapted from Chaffin, D.B. and
Andersson, G.B., Occupational Biomechanics, John Wiley & Sons, Inc. New York, 1991. With permission.)
muscle (and ligaments) can generate tension when muscles are stretched. Thus, the orientation of the muscle
fibers during a task can greatly influence the force available to perform work and can, therefore, influence
risk by altering the available internal force within the system. A given tension on a muscle can either tax the
muscle greatly or be a minimum burden on the muscle. What might be considered a moderate force for a
muscle at the resting length can become the maximum force a muscle can produce when it is in a stretched or
contracted position, thus, increasing the risk of muscle strain. When this relationship is considered in con-
junction with the mechanical load placed on the muscle and tendon via the arrangement of the lever system,
the position of the joint arrangement becomes a major factor in the design of the work environment. It is
typically the case that the length–strength relationship interacts synergistically with the lever system.
Figure 11.6 shows the effect of elbow position on the force generation capability of the elbow. This figure
indicates that position can have a dramatic effect on force generation. As already discussed this position
can also have a great effect on internal loading of the joint and the subsequent risk of cumulative trauma.
FORCE
1.8
1.4
Maximal
1.0 isometric
force
0.6
0.2
FIGURE 11.7 Influence of velocity upon muscle force (Adapted from The Textbook of Work Physiology, McGraw-
Hill, 1977. With permission.)
repeated dynamic conditions. This indicates that if it is determined that a task requires a large portion of
a workers’ strength, one must consider how long that portion of the strength is required in order to
ensure that the work does not strain the musculoskeletal system.
100
Load as a % of Muscle Strength
80
60
200 sec
100 sec
50 sec
40 25 sec Rest
10 sec
20
No Rest
0
10 20 30 40 50 60
Endurance Time (Min)
FIGURE 11.8 Forearm flexor muscle endurance times in consecutive static contractions of 2.5 sec duration with
varied rest periods. (Adapted from Chaffin, D.B. and Andersson, G.B., Occupational Biomechanics, John Wiley &
Sons, Inc. New York, 1991. With permission.)
11-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Adenosine Triphosphate
(ATP)
O2 Debt Glucose High-Energy Phosphate
Regeneration
Lactic Acid Pyruvic Muscular
(Fatigue) Acid Contractions
+O2
O2 Payback
Adenosine Diphosphate
H2O
(ADP)
CO2
Low-Energy Phosphate
FIGURE 11.9 The body’s energy system during work. (Adapted from Grandjean, E., Fitting the Task to the Man: An
Ergonomic Approach, Taylor & Francis, Ltd., London, 1982. With permission.)
shows how energy for a muscular contraction is regenerated during work. Adenosine triphosphate (ATP) is
required to produce a significant muscular contraction. ATP changes to adenosine diphosphate (ADP) once
a muscular contraction has occurred. This ADP must be converted to ATP in order to enable another
muscular contraction. This conversion can occur with the addition of oxygen to the system. If oxygen is
not present, then the system goes into oxygen debt and there is insufficient ATP for a muscular contraction.
Thus, this flow chart indicates that oxygen is a key ingredient to maintain a high level of muscular exertion.
Oxygen is delivered to the target muscles via the blood flow. However, under static exertions the blood
flow is reduced and there is a subsequent reduction in the blood flow to the muscle. This restriction of
blood flow and subsequent oxygen deficit are responsible for the rapid decrease in force generation over
time as shown in Figure 11.8. The solid lines shown in Figure 11.8 indicate how the force generation capacity
of the muscles increase when different amounts of rest are permitted in a fatiguing exertion. As more and
more rest time is permitted, increases in force generation are achieved when more oxygen is delivered to
the muscle and more ADP can be converted to ATP. This relationship also shows that any more than
about 50 sec of rest, under these conditions, does not result in a significant increase in force generation
capacity of the muscle. Practically, this indicates that in order to optimize the strength capacity of the
worker and minimize the risk of muscle strain, a schedule of frequent and brief rest periods would be
more beneficial than lengthy infrequent rest periods.
10000
Level (N)
6000
4000
2000
0
<40 40- 50 50-60 >60
Age
FIGURE 11.10 Mean and range of disc compression failures by age. (Adapted from National Institute for
Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH) Work practices guide for manual lifting, Department of Health and
Human Services (DHHS), NIOSH, Cincinnati, OH, 81–122, 1981. With permission.)
the compressive load on the spine reaches a value of 9317 N, almost all of those exposed to the loading
will experience a vertebral endplate microfracture. It should also be noted that the tolerance distribution
shifts to lower levels with increasing age (Adams et al., 2000). In addition, it should be emphasized that
this tolerance is based upon compression of the vertebral endplate alone. Shear and torsional forces in
combination with compressive loading would be expected to further lower the tolerance of the end plate.
This distribution of risk has been widely used as the tolerance limits of the spine. However, it should be
noted that others have identified different limits of vertebral endplate tolerance. Jager et al. (1991) have
reviewed 13 studies of spine compressive strength and suggested different compression value limits. Their
summary of these spine tolerance limits are shown in Table 11.2. These researchers have also been able to
describe the vertebral compressive strength based upon an analysis of 262 values collected from 120
samples. They have related the compressive strength of the lumbar spine according to a regression equation:
Source: Adapted from Jager, Luhman, and Laurig, Int. J. Indust. Ergo., 1991.
With permission.
Biomechanical Basis for Ergonomics 11-13
100
Probability of Failure
80
60-70%
60
50-60%
Lo 40
ad 40-50%
Ra 30-40% 20
ng
e 20-30%
0
10 100 500 1000 5000
Load Cycles
FIGURE 11.11 Probability of a motion segment to be fractured in dependence on the load range and the number of
load cycles. (Adapted from Brinckmann, et al., Clin. Biomech., 3(Suppl. 1), S1– S23, 1988. With permission.)
where A is the age in decade; G is the gender coded as 0 for female or 1 for male; C is the cross-sectional area of
the vertebrae in cm2; L is the the lumbar level unit where 0 is the L5/S1 disc, 1 represents the L5 vertebrae, etc.
through 10, which represents the T10/L1 disc; S is the structure of interest where 0 is a disc and 1 is a vertebra.
This analysis suggests that the decrease in strength within a lumbar level is about 0.15 kN of that of
the adjacent vertebrae and that the strength of the vertebrae is about 0.8 kN lower than the strength
of the discs (Jager et al., 1991). Using this equation these researchers were able to account for 62% of
the variability among the samples.
It has also been suggested that the tolerance limits of the spine varies as a function of frequency of
loading (Brinkmann et al., 1988). Figure 11.11 indicates that spine tolerance varies as a function of
spine load level and frequency of loading.
11.3.1.1 Shoulder
Shoulder pain is suspected of being one of the most under-recognized musculoskeletal disorders in the
workplace. Second only to low back injury and neck pain, shoulder disorders are increasingly being
recognized as a major workplace problem by those organizations that have reporting systems sensitive
enough to detect such trends. The shoulder is one of the more complex structures of the body with
numerous muscles and ligaments crossing the shoulder joint-girdle complex. Because of its biomecha-
nical complexity surgical repair of the shoulder can be problematic. During many shoulder surgeries
it is often necessary to damage much of the surrounding tissue in an attempt to reach the structure in
need of repair. Often the target structure is small in size and difficult to reach. Thus, often at times,
more damage is done to surrounding tissues than the benefits derived to the target tissue. Therefore,
the best course of action is to ergonomically design work stations so that the risk of initial injury is
minimized.
Since the shoulder joint is so biomechanically complex, much of our biomechanical knowledge is
derived from empirical evidence. The shoulder represents a statically indeterminate system in that we
can typically measure six external moments and forces acting about the point of rotation, yet, there
are far more internal forces (over 30 muscles and ligaments) that must counteract the external
moments. Thus, quantitative estimates of shoulder joint loading are rare.
With respect to the shoulder, optimal workplace design is typically defined in terms of preferred
posture during work. Shoulder abduction, defined as the elevation of the shoulder in the lateral direction,
is of concern when work is performed overhead. Figure 11.12 indicates shoulder performance measures
in terms of both available strength and perceived fatigue while the shoulder is held in varying degrees of
abduction. This figure indicates that shoulder can produce a considerable amount of strength through-
out shoulder abduction angles of between 30 and 908. However, when comparing fatigue characteristics
at these same abduction angles it is apparent that fatigue increases rapidly as the shoulder is abducted
above 308. Thus, even though strength is not a problem at shoulder abduction angles upto 908,
fatigue becomes the limiting factor. Therefore, the only position of the shoulder that is acceptable
from both a strength and fatigue standpoint is a shoulder abduction of at most 308.
Shoulder flexion has been examined almost exclusively as a function of fatigue. Chaffin (1973) has
shown that even slight shoulder flexion can influence fatigue characteristics of the shoulder musculature.
Figure 11.13 and Figure 11.14 indicate the effects of vertical height of the work and horizontal distance,
respectively, during shoulder flexion while seated upon fatigability of the shoulder musculature. During
vertical flexion/extension (Figure 11.13), fatigue occurs more rapidly as the workers’ arm becomes more
elevated. This trend is most likely due to the fact that the muscles are farther from the neutral position as
120 80
Strength (% of Max) ( --- )
100 70
60
80
50
60 40
40 30
20
20
10
)
0 0
30 60 90 120 150
Shoulder Abduction Angle (Deg)
FIGURE 11.12 Shoulder abduction strength and fatigue time as a function of shoulder abducted from the torso.
(Adapted from Chaffin, D.B. and Andersson, G.B., Occupational Biomechanics, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., New York,
1991. With permission.)
Biomechanical Basis for Ergonomics 11-15
Significant Muscle
15
10
Vertical Height
5 5 cm
30cm
50cm
0
5 10
Weight Held in Hand (N)
50cm
50cm
30cm
5 cm
75 cm
FIGURE 11.13 Expected time to reach significant shoulder muscle fatigue for varied arm flexion postures.
(Adapted from Chaffin, D.B. and Andersson, G.B., Occupational Biomechanics, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., New York,
1991. With permission.)
the shoulder becomes more elevated thus affecting the length–strength relationship (Figure 11.5) of the
shoulder muscles. Figure 11.14 shows that as the horizontal distance between the work and the body is
increased, the time to reach significant fatigue is decreased. This trend is due to the fact that as a load is
held further from the body, more of the external moment (force . distance) must be supported by the
shoulder. Thus, the shoulder muscles must produce a greater internal force when the load is held
further from the body and they fatigue quicker. Elbow supports have been shown to significantly increase
the endurance time in these postures. In addition an elbow support has the effect of changing the bio-
mechanical situation by providing a fulcrum at the elbow. Thus, the axis is rotation becomes the elbow
instead of the shoulder and this makes the external moment much shorter. As shown in Figure 11.15, this
not only increase the time one can maintain a posture, but also significantly increases the external load
one can hold in the hand.
11.3.1.2 Neck
Neck disorders can also be associated with sustained work postures. In general, the more upright posture
of the head, the less muscle activity and neck strength is required to maintain the posture. Upright neck
postures also have the advantage of reducing the extent of fatigue perceived in the neck region. This
relationship is shown in Figure 11.16. This trend indicates that when the head is tilted forward by 308
or more from the vertical position, the time to experience significant neck fatigue decreases rapidly.
From a biomechanical standpoint, as the head is flexed forward the center of mass of the head moves
forward relative to the base of support of the head (spine). Therefore, as the head is moved forward,
11-16 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
30
Average Time (Min) for
Young Males to Reach
Horizontal
15 Distance
10 30 cm
5 40 cm
50 cm
0
5 10
Weight Held in Hand (N)
50 cm
40 cm
30 cm
105 cm
FIGURE 11.14 Expected time to reach significant shoulder muscle fatigue for different forward arm reach postures.
(Adapted from Chaffin, D.B. and Andersson, G.B., Occupational Biomechanics, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., New York,
1991. With permission.)
more of a moment is imposed about the spine, which necessitates increased activation of the neck mus-
culature and greater risk (probability of fatigue) since a static posture is maintained by the neck muscles.
When the head is not flexed forward and is relatively upright, the neck can be positioned in such a way
that minimal muscle activity is required of the neck muscles and thus fatigue is minimized.
20
0
0 20 40 60 80 100
Weight Held in Hand (N)
Elbow
Supports
FIGURE 11.15 Expected time to reach significant shoulder and arm muscle fatigue for different arm postures and hand
loads with the elbow supported. The greater the reach, the shorter the endurance time. (Adapted from Chaffin, D.B., and
Andersson, G.B., Occupational Biomechanics, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., New York, 1991. With permission.)
disadvantageous for another part of the body. Thus, ergonomic design of the workplace requires one to
consider the various trade-offs and rationales for various design options.
One common trade-off encountered in ergonomic design is the trade-off between accommodating the
shoulders and accommodating the neck. This trade-off is resolved by considering the hierarchy of needs
required by the task. Figure 11.17 illustrates this reasoning. The recommended height of the work is a
function of the type of work that is to be performed. Precision work requires a high level of visual
acuity, which becomes the greatest need in order to perform the work task. However, if the work is per-
formed at too low of a level the head must be flexed in order to accommodate the visual requirements of
the job and this becomes a problem for the neck. Therefore, in this circumstance, visual accommodation
is at the top of the hierarchy of task needs, so that the work is raised to a relatively high level (95 to 110 cm
above the floor) in order to accommodate vision and the neck posture. This posture accommodates the
neck but creates a problem for the shoulders since they must be abducted when the work level is high.
Thus, a trade-off should be considered. In this instance, ideal shoulder posture is sacrificed in order to
accommodate the neck since the visual requirements of the job represent the greater priority for work
performance, whereas, the minimal shoulder strength is required for precision work and, thus, represents
a lower priority. Thus, visual accommodation is given a higher priority in the hierarchy of task needs and
this criterion must be given priority over any other criteria. Besides, the shoulder problems can be mini-
mized by providing wrist or elbow supports at the workplace.
The other extreme example of the working height situation involves heavy work. The greatest demand
on the worker during heavy work involves a high degree of arm strength, whereas, visual requirements in
11-18 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
180 10
120 5
0
0 15 30 45 60
Head Tilt ∝ (Degrees)
∝
FIGURE 11.16 Neck extensor fatigue and muscle strength required versus head tilt angle. (Adapted from
Chaffin, D.B. and Andersson, G.B., Occupational Biomechanics, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., New York, 1991. With
permission.)
+20 cm
+10 cm
0
–10 cm
–20 cm
–30 cm
FIGURE 11.17 Recommended heights of bench for standing work. The reference line (þ0) is the height of the
elbows above the floor. (Adapted From Grandjean, E., Fitting the Task to the Man: An Ergonomic Approach, Taylor &
Francis, Ltd., London, 1982. With permission.)
Biomechanical Basis for Ergonomics 11-19
this type of work are often minimal. Thus, shoulder position represents a higher priority in the hierarchy
of task needs in this situation. In this situation, ideal neck posture is typically sacrificed in favor of more
favorable shoulder and arm postures. For this reason, heavy work is performed at a height of 70 to 90 cm
above floor level. With the work set at this height, the position wherein the elbows are close to 908 maxi-
mizes strength (Figure 11.6). In addition, the shoulders are close to 308 of abduction, which minimizes
fatigue. In this situation, the neck is not in an optimal position but the hierarchy logic dictates that the
visual demands of a heavy task would not be substantial and thus the neck would not be flexed for pro-
longed periods of time and, therefore, do not pose much of a risk.
The third work height situation involves light work. Light work is a mix of moderate visual demands
with moderate strength requirements. In this situation, work is a compromise between shoulder position
and visual accommodation and neither visual nor strength demands dominate the hierarchy of work
needs. Thus, the height of the work is set at a height between those of the precision work height level
and the heavy work height level. In this manner, a compromise between the benefits and costs associated
with accommodating the neck versus the shoulder is resolved. This situation dictates that the work is
performed at a level of between 85 and 95 cm off the floor under light work conditions.
5 cm 1m
F • 5 cm = 222 N • 1 m
222 Nm
F=
0.05 m
FIGURE 11.18 Internal muscle force required to counterbalance an external load during lifting.
motion of the body is considered (since force is a product of mass and acceleration). Thus, the most
important concept to consider in workplace design from a back protection standpoint is to keep the
moment arm at a minimum.
N
700
600
500
Lumbar Support
400
(cm)
0
300 1
2
3
200 4
FIGURE 11.19 Disc pressures measured with different backrest inclinations and different size lumbar supports.
(Adapted from Chaffin, D.B. and Andersson, G.B., Occupational Biomechanics, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., New York,
1991. With permission.)
the back angle of the chair and magnitude of lumbar support are varied. Since it is infeasible to directly
measure the forces in the spine in vivo, disc pressure measures have traditionally been used as a rough
approximation of loads imposed upon the spine. This figure indicates that both the seat back angle
and lumbar support features have a significant effect on disc pressure. Disc pressure is observed to
decrease as the backrest angle is increased. However, increasing the backrest angle in the workplace is
often not practical, since it also has the effect of moving the worker away from the work and thereby
increasing external moment. The figure also indicates that increasing lumbar support can also signifi-
cantly reduce disc pressure. This reduction in pressure is most likely due to the fact that as lumbar cur-
vature (lordosis) is reestablished (with lumbar support) the posterior elements play more of a role in
providing an alternative load path as is the case when standing in the upright position.
Less is known about risk to the low back associated with prolonged standing. It is known that the
muscles experience low level static exertions and may be subject to the static overload through the
muscle static fatigue process described in Figure 11.9. This fatigue can result in lowered muscle force gen-
eration capacity and can, thus, initiate the cumulative trauma sequence of events (Figure 11.3). It has
been demonstrated that this fatigue and cumulative trauma sequence can be minimized by two
actions. First, foot rails provide a mechanism to allow relaxation of the large back muscles and thus
increased blood flow to the muscle. This reduces the static load and fatigue in the muscle by the
process described in Figure 11.9. When a leg is lifted and rested on the foot rest the large back
muscles are relaxed on one side of the body and the muscle can be supplied with oxygen. Alternating
legs on the foot rest provides a mechanism to minimize back muscle fatigue throughout the day.
Second, floor mats have been shown to decrease the fatigue in the back muscles provided that the
mats have proper compression characteristics (Kim et al., 1994). Floor mats are believed to induce
body sway, which facilitate the pumping of blood through back muscles, thereby, minimizing fatigue.
Our knowledge of when standing workplaces are preferable is dictated mainly by work performance
criteria. In general, standing workplaces are preferred when: (1) the task required a high degree of mobi-
lity (reaching and monitoring in positions that exceed the reach envelope or when performing tasks at
11-22 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
different heights or different locations), (2) precise manual control actions are not required, (3) leg room
is not available (when leg room is not available, the moment arm distance between the external load and
the back is increased and thus greater internal back muscle force and spinal load result), and (4) heavy
weights are handled or large forces are applied. When jobs must accommodate both sitting and standing,
it is important to ensure that the positions and orientations of the body, especially the upper extremity,
are in the same location under both standing and sitting conditions.
11.3.1.5 Wrists
The wrist has been of increased interest to ergonomists in the past three decades. The Bureau of Labor
Statistics reports that repetitive trauma has increase from 18% of occupational illnesses in 1981 to 63% of
occupational illnesses in 1993. Based upon these figures, repetitive trauma has been described as the
fastest growing occupational problem. Even though these numbers and statements appear alarming
one must acknowledge that occupational illnesses represent 6% of all occupational injuries and illnesses.
Furthermore, these figures for illness include illnesses unrelated to musculoskeletal disorders such as
noise-induced hearing loss. Thus, the magnitude of the cumulative trauma problem must not be over-
stated. Nonetheless, there are specific industries (i.e., meat packing, poultry processing, etc.) where
cumulative trauma to the wrist is a major problem and this problem has reached epidemic proportions
within these industries.
Pisiform bone
Median nerve
Tendons
120
100
Grip Strength (% of max)
80
60
40
20
0
Neutral Flexion Extension Radial Deviation Ulnar Deviation
Wrist Position
Supination (palm up) Mid-position Pronation (palm down)
FIGURE 11.21 Grip strength as a function of wrist and forearm position. (Adapted from Sanders, M.S.
and McCormick, E.F., Human Factors in Engineering and Design, McGraw-Hill Inc., New York, 1993. With
permission.)
11-24 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
motions requiring a cycle time of less than 30 sec is considered a candidate for cumulative trauma dis-
order risk.
Third, the force applied by the hands and fingers during a work cycle has been identified as a risk
factor. In general, the greater the force required by the work the greater the risk of CTD. Greater hand
forces result in greater tension within the tendons and result in greater tendon friction and tendon
travel. Another factor related to force is wrist acceleration. Industrial surveillance studies have reported
that repetitive jobs resulting in greater wrist acceleration are associated with greater CTD incident rates
(Marras and Schoenmarklin, 1993; Schoenmarklin et al., 1994). Since force is a product of mass and
acceleration, jobs that increase the angular acceleration of the wrist joint result in greater tension and
force transmitted through the tendons. Thus, wrist acceleration can be another mechanism of imposing
force on the wrist structures.
Fourth, as shown in Figure 11.20, the anatomy of the hand is such that the median nerve becomes very
superficial at the palm. Direct impact to the palm of the hand through pounding or striking an object
with the palm, as is done often in assembly work, can directly stimulate the median nerve and initiate
symptoms of cumulative trauma even though the work may not be repetitive.
kg lb
54 120
35 80 Grip axis
27 60
5th percentile male
18 40
50th percentile female
9 20
5th percentile female
0 0
0 1 2 3 4 5 in
0 25 51 76 101 127 mm
Grip axis opening
Grip strength
FIGURE 11.22 Grip strength as a function of grip opening and hand anthropometry. (Adapted from Sanders, M.S.
and McCormick, E.J., Human Factors in Engineering and Design, McGraw-Hill Inc., New York, 1993. With
permission.)
Biomechanical Basis for Ergonomics 11-25
can influence grip strength and risk. Therefore, proper design of the handles is crucial in ergonomic
workplace design.
Handle shape can also affect the strength of the wrist. Figure 11.23 shows how changes in the design of
screwdriver handles can affect the maximum force that can be exerted. The biomechanical origin of these
differences in strength capacity is most likely related to the length –strength relationship of the forearm
muscles as well as contact area with the tool. The handle designs that result in less strength permit the
wrist to twist or permit the grip to slip resulting in a deviation from the ideal length– strength position
in the forearm muscles.
11.3.1.5.4 Gloves
The use of gloves can significantly influence the generation of grip strength and may play a role in the
development of CTDs. When gloves are worn during work three effects must be considered. First, the
grip strength generated is often reduced. There is typically a 10 to 20% reduction in grip strength
when gloves are worn. When using gloves the coefficient of friction between the hand and the tool
can be reduced which, in turn, permits some slippage of the hand upon the tool surface. This slippage
can result in a deviation from the ideal muscle length and thus a reduction in available strength. The
degree of slippage and the degree of strength loss depends upon how well the gloves fit the hand and
the type of material used in the glove. Poorly fitting gloves result in greater strength loss. Figure 11.24
indicates how the glove material and glove fit can dramatically influence grip force application.
Second, when wearing gloves, even though the externally applied force (grip strength) is often reduced,
the internal forces are often very large compared to not using a glove. For a given grip strength the muscle
activity is significantly greater when using gloves compared to a bare-handed condition (Kovacs et al.,
2002). Thus, the musculoskeletal system is less efficient when wearing a glove.
Third, the ability to perform a work task is affected negatively when wearing gloves. Figure 11.25 shows
the increase in time required to perform work tasks when wearing gloves composed of different materials
compared to performing the task bare-handed. The
figure indicates that task performance can increase
upto 70% when wearing gloves.
These effects have indicated that there are bio- 9 100%
mechanical costs associated with glove usage. Less
strength capacity is available to the worker, more
internal force is generated, and worker productivity 10 104%
is affected. These negative effects of gloves do not
mean that gloves should never be worn at work.
When hand protection is needed gloves should be
2 95%
considered as a potential solution. However, pro-
tection should only be provided to the parts of
the hand that require protection. For example, if
3 85%
the palm of the hand requires protection, fingerless
gloves might provide an acceptable solution. If the
fingers require protection, but there is little risk to
4 84%
the palm of the hand, then grip tape wrapped
around the fingers might be considered. In
addition, different styles, materials, and sizes of
5 83%
gloves will fit workers differently. Thus, gloves pro-
duced by various manufacturers and of different
sizes should be available to the worker to minimize
the negative effects mentioned before. FIGURE 11.23 Maximum force, which could be
exerted on a screwdriver as a function of handle shape.
11.3.1.5.5 Design Guidelines (From Konz, S.A., Work Design: Industrial Ergonomics,
This discussion has indicated that there are many 2nd ed., Grid Publishing, Inc., Columbus, OH, 1983.
factors that can affect the biomechanics of the With permission.)
11-26 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
35 C C C C C
30
Force (kg)
25
20
15
10
0
Barehand Surgical Jersey OJersey OSurg PVC Cotton Oleather Nephr Leather
Glove Type
FIGURE 11.24 Peak grip force shown as a function of type of glove. Different letters above the columns indicate
statistically significant differences.
wrist and the subsequent risk of CTDs. This suggests that proper ergonomic design of a work task cannot
be accomplished by simply providing the worker with an “ergonomically designed” tool. Since ergo-
nomics is associated with matching the workplace design to the workers’ capabilities it is not possible
to design an “ergonomic tool” without considering the workplace design and task requirements simul-
taneously. What might be an “ergonomic” tool for one work situation may be improper for use while a
worker is assuming another work posture. For example, using an in-line tool may keep the wrist straight
when inserting a bolt into a horizontal surface. However, if the bolt is to be inserted into a vertical surface
a pistol grip tool may be more appropriate. Using the in-line tool in this situation (inserting a bolt into a
vertical surface) may cause the wrist to be significantly deviated. Hence, there are no ergonomic tools.
Glove material
Neogrene Terry Cloth Leather PVC
Percentage increase in time compared with
100
bare-handed performance
75
50
25
0
1 2 3 4 5 Total
FIGURE 11.25 Performance (time to complete) on a maintenance-type task while wearing gloves constructed of
five different materials. (From Sanders, M.S. and McCormick, E.J., Human Factors in Engineering and Design,
McGraw-Hill Inc., New York, 1993. With permission.)
Biomechanical Basis for Ergonomics 11-27
There are just ergonomic situations. What may be an ergonomically correct tool in one situation may not
be ergonomically correct in another work situation.
Workplace design should be performed with care and trade-offs between different parts of the body
must be considered by taking into consideration the various biomechanical trade-offs. Given these con-
siderations, the following components of the workplace should be considered when designing a work-
place so that cumulative trauma risk is minimized. First, maintain a neutral wrist posture. Second,
minimize tissue compression. Third, avoid actions that repeatedly impose force on the internal struc-
tures. Fourth, minimize required wrist accelerations and motions. Fifth, consider the impact of glove
use, hand size, and left-handed workers.
study has shown that lifting belts can significantly increase blood pressure (Rafacz and McGill, 1996).
This could become problematic for workers who have a compromised cardiovascular system.
The brief review indicates that there is a large amount of conflicting evidence as to the benefits or
liabilities associated with the use of back belts. There appears to be little biomechanical benefit to belt
usage and some negative physiological consequences. Recent epidemiologic studies have not been able
to find any evidence of benefit. A consistent finding among the studies is that if there is a benefit to
back belts, it is probably for those who have previously experienced an LBD. The literature also suggests
that belts should only be used for a limited period of time. Until more definitive studies are available it is
prudent to use caution when recommending the use of back belts in a work environment. This includes a
screening by an occupational physician who is familiar with the literature so that potential cardiovascular
problems can be assessed.
AL ¼ k(HF)(VF)(DF)(FF), (11:1)
where AL is the action limit in kg or lb; k is the load constant (40 kg or 90 lb), which is the greatest weight
a subject could lift if all lifting conditions are optimal; HF is the horizontal factor defined as the horizon-
tal distance from a point bisecting the ankles to the center of gravity of the load at the lift origin. Defined
algebraically as 15/H (metric) or 6/H (US units); VF is the vertical factor or height of the load at lift
origin. Defined algebraically as (0.004) jV 2 75j(metric) or 1-(0.01)jV 2 30j(US units); DF is the dis-
tance factor or the vertical travel distance of the load. Defined algebraically as 0.7 þ 7.5/D (metric) or
0.7 þ 3/D (US units); FF is the frequency factor or lifting rate defined algebraically as 1 2 F/Fmax
F ¼ average frequency of lift, Fmax is shown in Table 11.3.
The logic associated with this equation assumes that if the lifting conditions are ideal a worker could
safely hold (and implies lift) the load constant, k (40 kg or 90 lb). If the lifting conditions are not ideal the
allowable weight is discounted according to the four factors HF, VF, DF, and FF. These four factors are
shown in monogram form in Figure 11.26 through Figure 11.29. According to the load discounting
Source: Reprinted from NIOSH, Work Practices Guide for Manual Lifting,
Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS) NIOSH, Cincinnati,
OH, 81–122, 1981. With permission.
Biomechanical Basis for Ergonomics 11-29
1.0
0.9
0.8
0.7
0.6
Horizontal factor
0.5
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
20 40 60 80 (cm)
0 10 20 30 (in.)
Horizontal location
FIGURE 11.26 Horizontal factor, (HF) varies between the body interference limit and the limit of functional reach.
(Adapted from National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH), Work practices guide for manual
lifting, Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS), NIOSH, Cincinnati, OH, No. 81–122, 1981. With
permission.)
associated with these figures, the HF, which is associated with the external moment has the most dramatic
effect on acceptable lifting conditions. VF and DF are associated with the back muscle’s length–strength
relationship. FF attempts to account for the cumulative effects of repetitive lifting.
The second benchmark associated with this guide is the maximum permissible limit or MPL. The MPL
represents the point at which significant risk, defined in part, as a significant risk of vertebral endplate
microfracture (Figure 11.10). The MPL is associated with a compressive load on the spine of 6400 N,
which corresponds to a point at which 50% of the people would be expected to suffer a vertebral endplate
1.0
0.8
0.6
Vertical factor
0.4
0.2
0 20 40 60 80 (in.)
Vertical location
FIGURE 11.27 Vertical factor, (VF) varies both ways from knuckle height. (Adapted from National Institute for
Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH), Work practices guide for material lifting, Department of Health and
Human Services (DHHS), NIOSH, Cincinnati, OH, 81 –122, 1981. With permission.)
11-30 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
1.0
0.9
0.8
0.7
0.6
Distance factor
0.5
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
20 40 60 80 100 120 (cm)
0 10 20 30 40 50 (in.)
Lift distance
FIGURE 11.28 Distance factor, (DF) varies between a minimum vertical distance of 25 cm (10 in.) that was moved
to a maximum distance of 200 cm (80 in.). (Adapted from National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health
(NIOSH), Work practices guide for manual lifting. Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS), National
Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH), Cincinnati, OH, 81– 122, 1981. With permission.)
microfracture. Equation (11.2) indicates that the MPL is a function of the AL and is defined as follows:
The weight that the worker expected to lift in a work situation is compared to the AL and MPL. If the
magnitude of weight falls below the AL the work is considered safe and no adjustments are necessary. If
the magnitude of the weight falls above the MPL then the work is considered risky and engineering
changes involving the adjustment of HF, VF, and/or DF are required to reduce the AL and MPL. If
the weight falls between the AL and MPL then either engineering changes or administrative changes,
defined as selecting workers who are less likely to be injured or rotating workers, are recommended.
The AL and MPL were also indexed to nonbiomechanical benchmarks. According to NIOSH (1981)
these limits also correspond to strength, energy expenditure, and psychophysical acceptance points.
L
LI ¼ , (11:3)
RWL
where L is the load weight or the weight of the object to be lifted; RWL is the recommended weight limit
for the particular lifting situation; LI is the lifting index used to estimate relative magnitude of physical
stress for a particular job.
If the LI is greater than 1.0, an increased risk for suffering a lifting-related LBD exists. The RWL is
similar to the 1981 lifting guide AL equation [Equation (11.1)] in that it contains factors that discount
the allowable load according to the horizontal distance, vertical location of the load, vertical travel dis-
tance, and frequency of lift. However, the form of these discounting factors was changed. Moreover, two
additional discounting factors have been included. These additional factors include a lift asymmetry
Biomechanical Basis for Ergonomics 11-31
factor to account for asymmetric lifting conditions and a coupling factor that accounts for whether or not
the load lifted has handles. The RWL is represented algebraically in Equation (11.4) (metric units) and
Equation (11.5) (US units).
where H is the horizontal location forward of the midpoint between the ankles at the origin of the lift. If
significant control is required at the destination then H should be measured both at the origin and des-
tination of the lift; V is the vertical location at the origin of the lift; D is the vertical travel distance
between origin and destination of the lift; FM is the frequency multiplier shown in Table 11.4; A is
the angle between the midpoint of the ankles and the midpoint between the hands at the origin of
the lift; CM is the coupling multiplier ranked as either food, fair, or poor as described in Table 11.5.
In this revised equation the load constant has been significantly reduced compared to the 1981
equation. The adjustments for load moment, muscle length –strength relationships, and cumulative
loading are still integral parts of this equation. However, these adjustments or discounting factors
have been changed (compared to the 1981 Guide) to reflect the most conservative value of the biome-
chanical, physiological, psychophysical, or strength data upon which they are based. Recent studies
report that the 1993 revised equation yields a more conservative (protective) prediction of work-
related LBD risk (Marras et al., 1999).
1.0
0.9
Occasional,
0.8 Bench Height and Above
0.7
Frequency factor
0.6 Continuous,
Bench Height and Above
0.5
Occasional,
0.4 Low Lifting
0.3
Continuous,
0.2 Low Lifting
0.1
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
FIGURE 11.29 Frequency factor (FF) varies with lifts/minute and the Fmax curve. The Fmax depends upon lifting
posture and lifting time. (Adapted from National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH), Work
practices guide for manual lifting. Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS), National Institute for
Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH), Cincinnati, OH, 81– 122, 1981. With permission.)
11-32 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
assess the risk of injury during manual materials handling activities. In both models the moments
imposed upon the various joints of the body due to the object lifted are evaluated assuming that a
static posture is representative of the instantaneous loading of the body. These models then compare
the imposed moments about each joint with the static strength capacity derived from a working popu-
lation. The static strength capacity of the major articulations (assessed by this model) have been docu-
mented in a database of over 3000 workers. In this manner the proportion of the population capable of
performing a particular static exertion is predicted. In addition, the joint that limits the capacity to
perform the task can be identified via this method. These models assume that a single equivalent
muscle (internal force) supports the external moment about each joint. By considering the contribution
of the externally applied load and the internally generated single muscle equivalent, spine compression
acting on the lumbar discs is predicted. The predicted compression can then be compared to the toler-
ance limits of the vertebral endplate (Figure 11.10). An important assumption of these models is that no
significant motion occurs during the exertion since it is a static model. The implications of these assump-
tions are discussed further in Chapter 28. Figure 11.30 shows the output screen for this computer model
where the lifting posture, lifting distances, strength predictions, and spine compression are shown.
Source: Reprinted from NIOSH, Application Manual for Revised NIOSH Equation,
Cincinnati, OH, Publication No. 94–122, 1994. With permission.
Biomechanical Basis for Ergonomics 11-33
VIDEO STOPPED-FRAME,
OR FILM
−81°
25°
125°
−51°
VORIGIN
78° −90° 38 cm
200N
HORIGIN
53cm
FIGURE 11.30 The 2D-static strength prediction model. (Adapted from Chaffin, D.B. and Andersson, G.B.,
Occupational Biomechanics, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., New York, 1991. With permission.)
INOR EXOR
b d
RCAR
LATR
IAP
RCAL ERSR
C
S
a
ERSL γ
INOL
LATL
EXOL
FIGURE 11.31 Cross-sectional view of the human trunk at the lumbrosacral junction. (Adapted from Schultz, A.B.
and Andersson, G.B.J., Spine, 6, pp. 76 – 82, 1981. With permission.)
during work. Because of the variability in muscle recruitment patterns it has been virtually impossible
to predict the instantaneous coactivation and resultant loading on the spine during dynamic trunk
exertions. One of the few means to accurately account for the effect of the trunk muscle system
coactivation upon spine loading is through the use of biologically assisted models. The most
common of these models are electromyographic or EMG-assisted models. These models take into
account the individual recruitment patterns of the muscles during a specific lift for a specific individ-
ual. By directly monitoring muscle activity the EMG-assisted model can determine individual muscle
force and the subsequent spine loading. These models have been developed and tested under bending
and twisting dynamic motion conditions and have been validated (McGill and Norman, 1985, 1986;
Marras and Reilly, 1988; Reilly and Marras, 1989; Marras and Sommerich, 1991a, b; Granata and
Marras, 1993, 1995b; Marras and Granata, 1995, 1997a, b; Marras et al., 2001). Figure 11.32 shows
how such models can assess the effects of lifting dynamics upon spine loading. These models are
the only ones that can predict the multi-dimensional loads on the lumbar spine under many 3D
complex dynamic lifting conditions. The limitation of such models is that they require significant
instrumentation of the worker.
directed towards static response of the trunk as well as sudden loading responses (Cholewicki et al.,
2000a, b; Granata and Orishimo, 2001; Granata et al., 2001; Granata and Wilson, 2001; Cholewicki
and VanVliet, 2002). While these analyses may consider muscle coactivation beneficial from a stability
point of view, the point at which the stability benefits of coactivation are overcome by the increased
loading remains yet to be determined.
has been shown to have a high degree of predictability (odds ratio ¼ 10.7) compared to previous
attempts to assess work-related LBD risk. The advantage of this assessment is that the evaluation pro-
vides information about risk that would take years to derive from historical accounts of incidence rates.
The model has also been validated in a prospective study (Marras et al., 2000a, b). Chapter 49 further
explains the logic and validity of this tool.
11.4.7 TLVs
Threshold Limit Values or TLVs have been recently introduced for controlling biomechanical risk to
the back in the workplace. These limits have been introduced through the American Conference of
Governmental Industrial Hygienists (ACGIH) and provide lifting weight limits as a function of lift
origin “zones” and repetitions associated with occupational tasks. The lift origin zones are defined
by the lift height off the ground and lift distance from the spine associated with the lift origin.
Twelve zones are defined that related to lifts within +308 of asymmetry from the sagittal plane.
These zones are represented in three figures with each figure corresponding to different lift frequency
and time exposures. Within each zone limits are specified based upon the best information available
from several sources, which include: (1) EMG-assisted biomechanical models, (2) the 1993 revised
lifting equation, and (3) the historical risk data associated with the LMM database. This tool is
further described in Chapter 50.
TABLE 11.6 Descriptive Statistics of the Workplace and Trunk Motion Factors in Each of the Risk Groups
11-37
(Table continued)
11-38
TABLE 11.6 Continued
Twisting Plane
Maximum left twist (8) 1.21 9.08 227.56 29.54 21.92 5.36 230.00 11.44 3.2b
Maximum right twist (8) 13.95 8.69 213.45 30.00 10.83 6.08 211.20 30.00 2.2a
Range of motion (8) 20.71 10.61 3.28 53.30 17.08 8.13 1.74 38.59 2.9b
Average velocity (8/sec) 8.71 6.61 1.02 34.77 5.44 3.19 0.66 17.44 3.8b
Maximum velocity (8/sec) 46.36 25.61 8.06 136.72 38.04 17.51 5.93 91.97 4.7a
Maximum acceleration (8/sec2) 304.55 175.31 54.48 853.93 269.49 146.65 44.17 940.27 2.9b
Maximum deceleration (8/sec2) 288.52 70.30 2428.94 25.84 2100.32 72.40 2325.93 22.74 1.6a
a
Significant at a 0.05 (two-sided).
b
Significant at a 0.01 (two-sided).
Source: Adapted from Marras et al., Spine 18, pp. 617–628, 1993. With permission.
Biomechanical Basis for Ergonomics 11-39
11.5 Summary
This chapter has shown that biomechanics provides one of the few means to quantitatively consider the
implications of workplace design. Biomechanical design is important when a particular job is suspected
of imposing large or repetitive forces on a particular structure of the body. It is particularly important to
recognize that the internal structures of the body such as muscles are the primary loaders of the joint and
tendon structures. In order to evaluate the risk of injury from a particular task, one must consider the
contribution of both the external loads and internal loads upon the structure. Several quantitative models
and assessment methods have been developed that systematically consider the internal loading imposed
on the worker due to workplace layout and task requirements. Proper use of these models and methods
involves recognizing the limitations and assumptions of each technique so that they are not applied inap-
propriately. When properly used, these assessments can help assess the risk of work-related injury and
illness.
References
Adams, M.A., Freeman, B.J., Morrison, H.P., Nelson, I.W., and Dolan, P. Mechanical initiation of inter-
vertebral disc degeneration, Spine., 25, pp. 1625–1636, 2000.
Anderson, C.K., Chaffin, D.B., Herrin, G.D., and Matthews, L.S. A biomechanical model of the lumbo-
sacral joint during lifting activities, J. Biomech., 18, pp. 571 –584, 1985.
Andersson, B.J., Ortengren, R., Nachemson, A.L., Elfstrom, G., and Broman, H. The sitting posture: an
electromyographic and discometric study, Orthop. Clin. North Am., 6, pp. 105 –120, 1975.
Andersson, G.B. The epidemiology of spinal disorders, in The Adult Spine: Principles and Practice, Fry-
moyer, J.W., Ed., Lippincott-Raven Publishers, Philadelphia, 1997, 93– 141.
Basmajian, J.V. and De Luca, C.J. Muscles Alive: Their Functions Revealed by Electromyography, 5th ed.,
Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore, MD, 1985.
Brinkmann, P., Biggermann, M., and Hilweg, D. Fatigue fracture of human lumbar vertebrae, Clin
Biomech (Bristol, Avon)., 3, pp. S1–S23, 1988.
Cavanaugh, J.M. Neural mechanisms of lumbar pain, Spine, 20, pp. 1804– 1809, 1995.
Cavanaugh, J.M., Ozaktay, A.C., Yamashita, T., Avramov, A., Getchell, T.V., and King, A.I. Mechanisms of
low back pain: a neurophysiologic and neuroanatomic study, Clin. Orthop., pp. 166–180, 1997.
Chaffin, D.B. A computerized biomechanical model: development of and use in studying gross body
actions, J. Biomech., 2, pp. 429 –441, 1969.
Chaffin, D.B., Localized muscle fatigue — definiton and measurement, J. Occup. Med., 15, pp. 346 –354,
1973.
Chaffin, D.B. and Muzaffer, E. Three-dimensional biomechanical static strength predcition model sensi-
tivity to postural and anthropometric innaccuracies, IIE Transac., 23, pp. 215 –227, 1991.
Chaffin, D.B., Andersson, G.B.J., and Martin, B.J. Occupational Biomechanics, New York, 1999.
Cholewicki, J. and S. McGill. Mechanical stability of the in vivo lumbar spine: Implications ofr injury and
chronic low back pain, Clin. Biomech. (Bristol, Avon), 11, pp. 1 –15, 1996.
Cholewicki, J. and I.J. VanVliet. Relative contribution of trunk muscles to the stability of the lumbar spine
during isometric exertions, Clin. Biomech. (Bristol, Avon), 17, pp. 99 –105, 2002.
Cholewicki, J., Polzhofer, G.K., and Radebold, A. Postural control of trunk during unstable sitting, J
Biomech., 33, pp. 1733– 1737, 2000a.
Cholewicki, J., Simons, A.P., and Radebold, A. Effects of external trunk loads on lumbar spine stability, J
Biomech 33, pp. 1377 –1385, 2000b.
Gedalia, U., Solomonow, M., Zhou, B.H., Baratta, R.V., Lu, Y., and Harris, M. Biomechanics of increased
exposure to lumbar injury caused by cyclic loading. Part 2. Recovery of reflexive muscular stability
with rest, Spine, 24, pp. 2461–2467, 1999.
Granata, K.P. and Marras, W.S. An EMG-assisted model of loads on the lumbar spine during asymmetric
trunk extensions, J. Biomech., 26, pp. 1429 –1438, 1993.
11-40 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Granata, K.P. and Marras, W.S. The influence of trunk muscle coactivity on dynamic spinal loads, Spine,
20, pp. 913–919, 1995a.
Granata, K.P. and Marras, W.S. An EMG-assisted model of trunk loading during free-dynamic lifting,
J. Biomech., 28, pp. 1309 –1317, 1995b.
Granata, K.P. and Marras, W.S. Cost-benefit of muscle cocontraction in protecting against spinal instabil-
ity [In Process Citation], Spine, 25, pp. 1398 –1404, 2000.
Granata, K.P. and Orishimo, K.F. Response of trunk muscle coactivation to changes in spinal stability, J.
Biomech., 34, pp. 1117 –1123, 2001.
Granata, K.P. and Wilson, S.E. Trunk posture and spinal stability, Clin. Biomech. (Bristol, Avon), 16,
pp. 650–659, 2001.
Granata, K.P., Marras, W.S. and Davis, K.G. Biomechanical assessment of lifting dynamics, muscle
activity and spinal loads while using three different styles of lifting belt, Clin. Biomech. (Bristol,
Avon), 12, pp. 107 –115, 1997.
Granata, K.P., Orishimo, K.F., and Sanford, A.H. Trunk muscle coactivation in preparation for sudden
load, J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol., 11, pp. 247 –254, 2001.
Grandjean, E. Fitting the Task to the Man: An Ergonomic Approach, Taylor & Francis, Ltd., London, 1982.
Guo, H.R. Back Pain and U.S. Workers, American Occupational Health Conference, 1993.
Guo, H.R., Tanaka, S., Halperin, W.E., and Cameron, L.L. Back pain prevalence in US industry and esti-
mates of lost workdays, Am. J. Public Health., 89, pp. 1029–1035, 1999.
Hollbrook, T.L., Grazier, K., Kelsey, J.L., and Stauffer, R.N. The Frequency of Occurrence, Impact and
Cost of Selected Musculoskeletal Conditions in the United States, American Academy of Ortho-
paedic Surgeons, Chicago, IL, 1984.
Hoy, M.G., Zajac, F.E., and Gordon, M.E. A musculoskeletal model of the human lower extremity: the
effect of muscle, tendon, and moment arm on the moment-angle relationship of musculotendon
actuators at the hip, knee, and ankle, J. Biomech., 23, pp. 157–169, 1990.
Jager, M., Luttmann, A. and Laurig, W. Lumbar load during one-hand bricklaying, Int. J. Indust. Ergo., 8,
pp. 261–277, 1991.
Khalsa, P.S. Biomechanics of musculoskeletal pain: dynamics of the neuromatrix, J. Electromyogr.
Kinesiol., 14, pp. 109 –120, 2004.
Kim, J., Stuart-Buttle C., and Marras, W.S. The Effects of Mats on Back and Leg Fatigue, Applied
Ergonomics 25, pp. 29–34, 1994.
Konz, S.A. Work Design: Industrial Ergonomics, 2nd ed., Grid Publishing, Inc., Columbus, OH, 1983.
With permission.)
Kovacs, K., Splittstoesser, R., Maronitis, A., and Marras, W.S. Grip force and muscle activity differences
due to glove type, AIHA J (Fairfax, Va) 63, pp. 269 –274, 2002.
Kroemer, K.H.E. Biomechanics of the Human Body, In G. Salvendy ed., Handbook of Human Factors.
John Wiley and Sons, Inc., New York, 1987.
Lantz, S.A. and Schultz, A.B. Lumbar spine orthosis wearing. I. Restriction of gross body motions. Spine
11, pp. 834–837, 1986.
Lavender, S.A., Thomas, J.S., Chang, D., and Andersson, G.B. Effect of lifting belts, foot movement, and
lift asymmetry on trunk motions, Hum Factors, 37, pp. 844 –853, 1995.
Luo, X., Pietrobon, R., X.S. S, Liu, G.G. and Hey, L. Estimates and patterns of direct health care expen-
ditures among individuals with back pain in the United States, Spine, 29, pp. 79 –86, 2004.
Marras, W.S. and Granata, K.P. A biomechanical assessment and model of axial twisting in the thoraco-
lumbar spine, Spine, 20, pp. 1440 –1451, 1995.
Marras, W.S. Spine loading during trunk lateral bending motions, J. Biomech., 30, pp. 697– 703, 1997a.
Marras, W.S. The Development of an EMG-Assisted Model to Assess Spine Loading during Whole-Body
Free-Dynamic Lifting, J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol., 7, pp. 259–268, 1997b.
Marras, W.S. and Mirka, G.A. Electromyographic studies of the lumbar trunk musculature during the
generation of low-level trunk acceleration, J. Orthop. Res., 11, pp. 811 –817, 1993.
Biomechanical Basis for Ergonomics 11-41
Marras, W.S. and Reilly, C.H. Networks of internal trunk-loading activities under controlled trunk-
motion conditions, Spine, 13, pp. 661 –667, 1988.
Marras, W.S. and Schoenmarklin, R.W. Wrist motions in industry, Ergonomics, 36, pp. 341 –351, 1993.
Marras, W.S. and Sommerich, C.M. A three-dimensional motion model of loads on the lumbar spine:
I. Model structure, Hum Factors, 33, pp. 123–137, 1991a.
Marras, W.S. A three-dimensional motion model of loads on the lumbar spine: II. Model validation,
Hum Factors, 33, pp. 139– 149, 1991b.
Marras, W.S., Davis, K.G., and Splittstoesser, R.E. Spine loading during whole body free dynamic lifting,
The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, Grant #:R01 OH03289, 2001.
Marras, W.S., Fine, L.J., Ferguson, S.A. and Waters, T.R. The effectiveness of commonly used lifting
assessment methods to identify industrial jobs associated with elevated risk of low-back disorders,
Ergonomics, 42, pp. 229 –245, 1999.
Marras, W.S., Jorgensen, M.J. and Davis, K.G. Effect of foot movement and an elastic lumbar back
support on spinal loading during free-dynamic symmetric and asymmetric lifting exertions, Ergo-
nomics, 43, pp. 653 –668, 2000a.
Marras, W.S., Allread, W.G., Burr, D.L., and Fathallah, F.A. Prospective validation of a low-back disorder
risk model and assessment of ergonomic interventions associated with manual materials handling
tasks [In Process Citation]. Ergonomics, 43, pp. 1866– 1886, 2000b.
Marras, W.S., Lavender, S.A., Leurgans, S.E., Fathallah, F.A., Ferguson, S.A., Allread, W.G., and Rajulu, S.L.
Biomechanical risk factors for occupationally related low back disorders, Ergonomics, 38, pp.
377–410, 1995.
Marras, W.S., Lavender, S.A., Leurgans, S.E., Rajulu, S.L., Allread, W.G., Fathallah, F.A., and Ferguson, S.A.
The role of dynamic three-dimensional trunk motion in occupationally-related low back dis-
orders. The effects of workplace factors, trunk position, and trunk motion characteristics on
risk of injury, Spine, 18, pp. 617–628, 1993.
McCoy, M., Congleton, J., Johnson, W. and Jaing, B. The role of lifting belts in manual lifting, Int.
J. Indus. Ergon., 2, pp. 256 –259, 1988.
McGill, S. Low back disorder, Human Kinetics, 2002a.
McGill, S. Low back disorders: evidence-based prevention and rehabilitation, Human Kinetics,
Champaign, IL, 2002b.
McGill, S., Seguin, J., and Bennett, G. Passive stiffness of the lumbar torso in flexion, extension, lateral
bending, and axial rotation. Effect of belt wearing and breath holding, Spine, 19, pp. 696– 704,
1994.
McGill, S.M. Abdominal belts in industry: a position paper on their assets, liabilities and use, Am. Ind.
Hyg. Assoc. J., 54, pp. 752 –754, 1993.
McGill, S.M. The biomechanics of low back injury: implications on current practice in industry and the
clinic, J. Biomech., 30, pp. 465–475, 1997.
McGill, S.M. and Norman, R.W. Dynamically and statically determined low back moments during lifting,
J. Biomech., 18, pp. 877– 885, 1985.
McGill, S.M. and Norman, R.W. Partitioning of the L4-L5 dynamic moment into disc, ligamentous, and
muscular components during lifting [see comments], Spine, 11, pp. 666 –678, 1986.
McGill, S.M., Hughson, R.L. and Parks, K. Changes in lumbar lordosis modify the role of the extensor
muscles [In Process Citation], Clin. Biomech. (Bristol, Avon), 15, pp. 777 –780, 2000.
Mitchell, L.V., Lawler, F.H., Bowen, D., Mote, W., Asundi, P., and Purswell, J. Effectiveness and cost-
effectiveness of employer-issued back belts in areas of high risk for back injury, J. Occup. Med.,
36, pp. 90–94, 1994.
Nachemson, A. Towards a better understanding of low-back pain: a review of the mechanics of the
lumbar disc, Rheumatol Rehabil, 14, pp. 129–143, 1975.
NIOSH 1981. Work Practices Guide for Manual Lifting, Department of Health and Human Services
(DHHS) NIOSH, Cincinnati, OH, 81 –122.
NIOSH Workplace use of back belts, Cincinnati, OH, Publication No. 94 –122, 1994.
11-42 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Nordin, M., and Frankel, V. Basic Biomechanics of the Musculoskeletal System. Lea and Febiger,
Philadelphia, PA, 1989.
NRC Work-related musculokeletal disorders: report, workshop summary, and workshop papers.
National Academy Press, Washington D. C, 1999.
NRC Musculoskeletal disorders and the workplace: low back and upper extremity. National Academy
Press, Washington D. C, 2001.
Panjabi, M.M. The stabilizing system of the spine. Part II. Neutral zone and instability hypothesis,
J. Spinal. Disord., 5, pp. 390 –396; discussion 397, 1992a.
Panjabi, M.M. The stabilizing system of the spine. Part I. Function, dysfunction, adaptation, and
enhancement, J. Spinal. Disord., 5, pp. 383– 389; discussion 397, 1992b.
Park, K. and Chaffin, D. A biomechanical evaluation of two methods of manual load lifting, AIIE Trans. 6,
pp. 105–113, 1974.
Pope, M.H. Muybridge Lecture, International Society of Biomechanics XIVth Congress, Paris, France, 1993.
Praemer, A., Furner, S. and Rice, D.P. Musculoskeletal Conditions in the United States, pp. 23 –33. Amer-
ican Academy of Orthopaedic Surgeons, Park Ridge, IL, 1992.
Rafacz, W. and McGill, S.M. Wearing an abdominal belt increases diastolic blood pressure, J. Occup.
Environ. Med., 38, pp. 925 –927, 1996.
Reddell, C., Congleton, J., Hutchingson, R. and Montgomery, J. An evaluation of weight-lifting belt and
back injury prevention training class for airline baggage handlers, Appl. Ergon., 22, pp. 319–329,
1992.
Reilly, C.H. and Marras, W.S. Simulift: a simulation model of human trunk motion, Spine, 14, pp. 5 –11,
1989.
Sanders, M.S. and McCormick, E.F. Human Factors in Engineering and Design, McGraw-Hill Inc.,
New York, 1993.
Schoenmarklin, R.W., Marras, W.S., and Leurgans, S.E. Industrial wrist motions and incidence of hand/
wrist cumulative trauma disorders, Ergonomics, 37, pp. 1449–1459, 1994.
Schultz, A.B. and Andersson, G.B. Analysis of loads on the lumbar spine, Spine, 6, pp. 76 –82, 1981.
Silverstein, M.A., Silverstein, B.A., and Franklin, G.M. Evidence for work-related musculoskeletal dis-
orders: a scientific counterargument, J. Occup. Environ. Med., 38, pp. 477 –484, 1996.
Silverstein, B.A., Stetson, D.S., Keyserling, W.M., and Fine, L.J. Work-related musculoskeletal disorders:
comparison of data sources for surveillance, Am. J. Ind. Med., 31, pp. 600– 608, 1997.
Solomonow, M. Ligaments: a source of work-related musculoskeletal disorders, J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol.,
14, pp. 49–60, 2004.
Solomonow, M., Zhou, B.H., Harris, M., Lu, Y. and Baratta, R.V. The ligamento-muscular stabilizing
system of the spine, Spine, 23, pp. 2552– 2562, 1998.
Solomonow, M., Zhou, B.H., Baratta, R.V., Lu, Y. and Harris, M. Biomechanics of increased exposure to
lumbar injury caused by cyclic loading: Part 1. Loss of reflexive muscular stabilization, Spine, 24,
pp. 2426–2434, 1999.
Solomonow, M., Zhou, B., Baratta, R.V., Zhu, M. and Lu, Y. Neuromuscular disorders associated with
static lumbar flexion: a feline model, J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol., 12, pp. 81 –90, 2002.
Solomonow, M., He Zhou, B., Baratta, R.V., Lu, Y., Zhu, M. and Harris, M. Biexponential recovery model
of lumbar viscoelastic laxity and reflexive muscular activity after prolonged cyclic loading, Clin.
Biomech., (Bristol, Avon) 15, pp. 167–175, 2000.
Stubbs, M., Harris, M., Solomonow, M., Zhou, B., Lu, Y., and Baratta, R.V. Ligamento-muscular protec-
tive reflex in the lumbar spine of the feline, J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol., 8, pp. 197–204, 1998.
Thelen, D.G., Schultz, A.B. and Ashton-Miller J.A. Co-contraction of lumbar muscles during the devel-
opment of time-varying triaxial moments, J. Orthop. Res., 13, pp. 390– 398, 1995.
Tichauer, E.R. The Biomechanical Basis of Ergonomics: Anatomy applied to the design of work situ-
ations, John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1978.
Van Dieen, J.H., Hoozemans, M.J., and Toussaint, H.M. Stoop or squat: a review of biomechanical
studies on lifting technique, Clin. Biomech., (Bristol, Avon) 14, pp. 685 –696, 1999.
Biomechanical Basis for Ergonomics 11-43
Videman, T., Nurminen, M., and Troup, J.D. 1990 Volvo Award in clinical sciences. Lumbar spinal path-
ology in cadaveric material in relation to history of back pain, occupation, and physical loading,
Spine, 15, pp. 728– 740, 1990.
Walsh, N.E. and Schwartz, R.K. The influence of prophylactic orthoses on abdominal strength and low
back injury in the workplace [see comments], Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil., 69, pp. 245 –250, 1990.
Wang, J.L., Parnianpour, M., Shirazi-Adl, A., and Engin, A.E. Viscoelastic finite-element analysis of a
lumbar motion segment in combined compression and sagittal flexion. Effect of loading rate,
Spine, 25, pp. 310– 318, 2000.
Wassell, J.T., Gardner, L.I., Landsittel, D.P., Johnston, J.J., and Johnston, J.M. A Prospective Study of Back
Belts for Prevention of Back Pain and Injury, J. Am. Me. Assoc., 284, pp. 2727 –2732, 2000.
Waters, T.R., Putz-Anderson, V., Garg, A., and Fine, L.J. Revised NIOSH equation for the design and
evaluation of manual lifting tasks, Ergonomics, 36, pp. 749–776, 1993.
12
Fundamentals of
Manual Control
12.1 Introduction
For their work, for transportation or simply for entertainment, human beings are often involved
in the manual control of devices. Vehicles such as cars, bicycles, ships, and airplanes are some
examples, but also video games and many work situations involve manual control. Normally,
after learning the task, the human operator in such a control situation behaves like a well-designed
controller. In fact, in their paper on “Quasi-linear pilot models,” McRuer and Jex make the remark
that data of measured pilot behavior matches very well with the Primary Rule of Thumb for
Frequency Domain Synthesis, a design rule for automatic controllers. It is not surprising, therefore,
that many of the models used in modeling manual control situations are based on various control
system design techniques. Two of the most commonly applied are the frequency domain design
methods, which serve as the basis for the cross-over model (COM) and variants thereof, the
precision model and the simplified precision model (McRuer and Jex, 1967), and optimal
control theory, which lies at the basis of the optimal control model (OCM; Kleinman et al.,
1970b).
The theories on human control behavior have matured by now, and its applications, particularly
human vehicle control, have been extensively studied. However, for many application areas these theories
and their applications are still very relevant today. Some examples are:
12-1
12-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
. Investigation of the roles of multi-modal (visual, vestibular, tactile) feedback on human manual
control behavior in virtual environments such as flight and driving simulators (Mulder et al., 2004).
. The design of haptic manipulators in applications like tele-operation or the development of force-
feedback systems in vehicular control (Van Paassen, 1994).
. Investigation of vehicular control (aircraft, automobile) in general, including handling qualities
research.
. Investigation and evaluation of augmented systems, that is, the study of the interim between fully
manual and automated control.
. Studying human perception and action cycles in active psychophysics, for example, in the
determination and identification of a human’s use of visual cues in multi-cue displays (Flach,
1991; Mulder, 1999).
In this chapter we will provide a short introduction into some fundamentals of control theory. This
introduction is very limited, however, and for further study the reader is referred to the many good
textbooks that are available. A textbook geared towards human control is “Control Theory for
Humans,” by Jagacinski and Flach (2003). Others that are recommended for their “human engineering”
perspective are “Man-Machine Systems” by Sheridan and Ferrell (1974), and “Engineering Psychology and
Human Performance” by Wickens (1992). More engineering-oriented textbooks are “Control Systems
Engineering” by Nise (1995), “Control System Design” by Goodwin et al. (2001) “Modern Control
Systems” by Dorf and Bishop (2005) and “Feedback Control of Dynamics Systems” by Franklin et al.
(2002). Note that this selection is not exhaustive, and that many more excellent textbooks are available.
Furthermore, a historic overview of the modeling of human control behavior is given, followed by a
more detailed description of two of the most common and widely used approaches to describe human
behavior in control-theoretical terms, the COM, based on classical control theory, and the OCM, based
on optimal control theory.
environment
inputs
u1 outputs
y1
u2 system
y2
u3
FIGURE 12.1 Representation of a system, with inputs and outputs, and a system boundary that separates the
system from the environment.
This definition stresses the fact that the choice of what is considered to be the system and what belongs to
the environment (surroundings) is often a subjective one. However, once the system boundaries are
defined, the interaction between the system and its environment, by means of input and output
signals, also becomes clear, see Figure 12.1.
A system is linear with respect to the inputs and outputs, if the output to a sum of two or more input
signals, is the sum of the outputs each of these inputs would give individually. Most real-world systems
are not linear, but in many control situations, where there are only small excursions around an “operating
point,” they can be well approximated with linear models. If also the properties of the system do not
change over time, thus if we neglect processes such as wear and tear, the change of mass due to use of
fuel, etc., one obtains an LTI system. The behavior of an LTI system can be described with an ordinary
differential equation (ODE), with constant coefficients. For instance, consider the following ODE,
describing the relationship between the input signal of a system, u(t) and the output signal of that
system, y(t), see Figure 12.2:
As in this example, the ODE generally consists of time-derivatives of the input and output signals,
characterizing the dynamic response of that system to the input signals.
U( jw) Y( jw)
u(t ) y(t)
h(t) or H( jw)
FIGURE 12.2 LTI systems in the time domain (left) and the frequency domain (right).
12-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
to transform a differential equation to the Laplace domain. Assuming that the system is at rest at t ¼ 0, so
f (0) ¼ 0 and df (0)/dt ¼ 0, one obtains:
This yields a constant relation between the input U(s) and the output Y(s):
Y(s) b0 þ b1 s
H(s) ¼ ¼ , (12:4)
U(s) a0 þ a1 s þ a2 s2
which is known as the system’s transfer function. For an LTI system with one input and one output,
the transfer function completely defines the system. The transfer function is a convenient format for
manipulation of system models. For example, the transfer function for two systems placed in series,
that is, the output of the first system is the input of the second, is simply the product of the two transfer
functions, and, likewise, the transfer function for the total of the two systems placed in parallel is the sum
of the transfer functions.
The stability of an LTI system can be determined from its transfer function by determining the poles
of the transfer function, which are the (complex) numbers for which the denominator equals zero (these
solutions are referred to as the “roots” of the denominator). Any poles with a positive real part are
associated with an unstable response of the system to an input signal. This response may be oscillating
for a pair of complex poles with positive real part, or it may be a-periodic when the corresponding poles
are on the positive real axis. A system with such poles is unstable. Poles with negative real part produce
responses with exponentially decreasing amplitude. Pole pairs on the imaginary axis produce an
undamped oscillatory response of the system, a pole in the origin produces an integration. An experi-
enced control engineer can interpret the location of the poles and zeros of a transfer function in
terms of a system’s dynamical behavior.
1
amplitude response
0.5
0
phase response
−0.5
−1
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
FIGURE 12.3 Input sinusoid (black) and system output (grey) as a function of time.
Fundamentals of Manual Control 12-5
The frequency response can be determined from the Laplace transfer function, by substituting jv for
the Laplace variable s; proof for this is given in most control engineering textbooks. Here j is the ima-
ginary number, and v is the frequency of the input sine signal in radians per second. The frequency
response H( jv) is a complex function of v. The magnitude of that function, jH( jv)j, is the gain of
the frequency response and the angle of the complex number H( jv) is the phase shift. In most cases
the phase shift is negative and is called a lag. For LTI systems, the frequency response completely
defines the system.
Consider an LTI system with the following transfer function:
K
H(s) ¼ : (12:5)
(1 þ ts)
This is known as a ‘first order system’ with time constant t. Now consider two values of t (0.1 and
10 sec) and look at the system response y(t) for sinusoidal input signals that have various frequencies
v. The result is illustrated in Figure 12.4. The output of both LTI systems are sinusoids as well, but
the amplitudes and phases of these sinusoids are different from the input sinusoids. Whereas the
system with t equal to 0.1 sec hardly changes the amplitude and phase and the input and output
signals are almost the same, the system with t ¼ 10 sec, does not (completely) “pass” the sinusoidal
input signals that have a higher frequency v. It “filters out” these higher-frequency signals, and is
therefore known as a low-pass filter system. The value of t determines which frequencies are “passed”
and which frequencies are not.
0 0 0
−1 −1 −1
0 50 100 0 50 100 0 50 100
1 1 1
0.32rad/s
0 0 0
−1 −1 −1
0 50 100 0 50 100 0 50 100
1 1 1
1.00 rad/s
0 0 0
−1 −1 −1
0 50 100 0 50 100 0 50 100
1 1 1
ω0=3.16 rad/s
0 0 0
−1 −1 −1
0 50 100 0 50 100 0 50 100
time,s time,s time,s
FIGURE 12.4 LTI response to sinusoidal input signal with four frequencies v0 (left column), for two values of the
variable t : t ¼ 0.1 sec (middle column) and t ¼ 10 sec (right column).
12-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Such frequency responses are often shown as Bode plots. The magnitude and phase for different
input signal frequencies are plotted separately. The magnitude is plotted on a logarithmic scale,
against the frequency, which is also plotted on a logarithmic scale. At the whim of the maker of the
plot, the frequency may be given in rad/sec, in Hertz, or sometimes in octaves. For the magnitude,
often a decibel (dB) scale is used. The relation between a magnification M and its equivalent in dB
m is:
Figure 12.5 shows the Bode plot for this system. One can see that for low frequencies the gain of
the system is 1 (0 dB). For high frequencies the gain decreases 20 dB per decade. The asymptotes for
the low and high frequency behavior cross at the corner frequency 1/t, in this case, with t ¼ 10 sec,
at 0.1 rad/sec.
12.2.4 Control
12.2.4.1 Feed-Forward and Feedback
In a control system, a controlling element (controller) provides input to a system, often called a
plant, normally with the aim of producing outputs of the plant that are equal to given reference
values. Unknown disturbances may be acting on the plant, as for example, the turbulence acting
on an aircraft. The controller may be an automatic device, such as an autopilot, or it may be a
human.
In feed-forward control, or open-loop control, the controller measures the disturbances on the plant,
and based on the knowledge about the plant’s dynamics, creates inputs that produce plant outputs as
close to the reference values as possible, see Figure 12.6a.
Amplitude Response
101
100
|H (jw)|
10−1
corner frequency
10−2 −2
10 10−1 100 101
Phase Response
0
−20
|H (jw)|
−40
−60
FIGURE 12.5 Bode plot for LTI system H(jv) ¼ 1/(1 þ 10jv). The circles show the system response for the four
frequencies shown in Figure 12.4.
Fundamentals of Manual Control 12-7
disturbance
Sensor reference output
Controller Controlled
System
reference Controlled output
Controller System
Sensor
In feedback, or closed-loop control, the controller measures the output of the plant, and compares that
to the reference values. Control input to the plant is calculated on the basis of this comparison, see
Figure 12.6b. The advantage of closed-loop control over open-loop control is that in most cases
“modeling errors,” that is, mismatches in the plant model used to tune the controller and the real
plant, have little effect on the control performance. Closed-loop control is thus said to be more
robust, meaning that it is insensitive to variations in the controlled system and influences from the
environment. Robustness is an important property of control systems and tools exist for the design
and tuning of robust controllers.
Feedback is a fundamental property of many control systems. The closed-loop system can itself be
considered as a new system, with an input (in most cases the desired output) and an output. If they are
part of a larger whole, such systems are often called a servo. For example, the motor, “plant” and feedback
system for moving an aircraft’s elevator forms a servo system and the servo system itself is part of the auto-
matic pilot.
The choice for a type of controller applied in a servo system is normally made on the basis of knowl-
edge on the plant dynamics and on the desired properties of the closed loop system. Common controller
types are the proportional (P) controllers that generate a control signal proportional to the error; pro-
portional and differentiating controllers (PD) generate a control signal on the basis of a sum of error and
the derivative of the error; proportional and integrating controllers (PI) generate a control signal on the
basis of a sum of error and the integrated error, and the combination of the above, are the PID
controllers.
After a choice for a particular type of controller has been made, its parameters need to be tuned. Several
tuning methods are in use in control system design, as an example, and since it forms the basis for the
commonly used COM, tuning in the frequency domain with Bode diagrams will be treated here.
Consider an LTI system, G(s), Figure 12.7. The main design requirement is to design the controller K(s)
in such a way that the system output z(t) follows the reference signal r (t) as closely as possible.
Solving this problem in the Laplace domain is not too difficult, since all combinations of signals
and systems can be obtained by algebraic manipulation. From Figure 12.7, one can derive the following
basic equations:
controller system
+ e (t ) c (t) z (t )
r (t) K (s) G (s)
−
so
1
E(s) ¼ R(s) (12:11)
1 þ G(s)K(s)
and
G(s)K(s)
Z(s) ¼ R(s): (12:12)
1 þ G(s)K(s)
Thus,
Z(s) K(s)G(s)
¼ (12:13)
R(s) 1 þ K(s)G(s)
One can consider this solution in the frequency domain, by substituting jv for s, which yields:
Z(jv) K(jv)G(jv)
¼ : (12:14)
R(jv) 1 þ K(jv)G(jv)
The design requirement is that the system output Z( jv) equals the reference signal R( jv), and so
(Z(jv)=R(jv) 1: The solution to this problem would be to achieve a high “open-loop gain”
K( jv)G( jv). When K( jv)G( jv) is very large, one can see that Z( jv) R( jv) [Equation (12.14) and
that E( jv) 0 Equation (12.11)].
However, one should bear in mind that K( jv)G( jv) is still a function of v, with a complex-valued
outcome. In general, it is not possible, and for many practical reasons not desirable to obtain a large
value for K( jv)G( jv) for all v. Essentially K( jv)G( jv) determines the “speed” of reaction to an error
signal. When time delays are small then a faster response will yield a lower tracking error. When time
delays are large, however, it is possible to respond too quickly and cause the system to become unstable.
Hence, as will be discussed in more detail, in the following paras, generally there is a trade-off between
response speed and accuracy.
Just as transfer functions can be considered in terms of their frequency response, that is, what (sine
signal) frequencies they pass and what frequencies they block, signals can be considered in terms of
their frequency content. A reference signal such as a block or sawtooth signal can be seen as constructed
from an infinitely large sum of sine signals (see Figure 12.8), a much smoother signal has less high-
frequency components.
0 0 0
−3 −3 −3
0 5 10 0 5 10 0 5 10
FIGURE 12.8 Triangular function approximated by sums of sine functions, base frequency 0.1 Hz, following sine
components at 0.3, 0.5, 0.7 Hz, etc. (a) one sine function; (b) three sine functions; (c) ten sine functions.
Fundamentals of Manual Control 12-9
In most cases, a smooth response is acceptable and even desirable from a mechanics standpoint,
and thus responses to the high-frequency components in an input signal are not needed. Summarizing,
at low frequencies, the gain of K( jv)G( jv), called the open-loop gain, should be high, while at high fre-
quencies it may be low. Note that K( jv)G( jv) is a complex-valued function, and that it can have a value
equal to or close to 21. To assess the behavior of the closed loop, the frequency response of the open-
loop, K( jv)G( jv), is studied near this point where the magnitude of the open-loop response,
jK( jv)G( jv)j, is 1 or 0 dB. This point is the cross-over point, and the corresponding frequency is
called the cross-over frequency.
The phase shift of the open-loop at the cross-over frequency determines what the gain of the closed-
loop system will be at the cross-over frequency. A phase shift near 1808 will cause the magnitude of
the denominator of the closed-loop system in Equation 12.14 to become very small (while the numer-
ator’s magnitude is 1), and the closed-loop frequency response will have what is called an oscillatory
peak. The response of the closed-loop system to, for example, a step change in the reference signal
will show an oscillation with approximately the cross-over frequency. Investigation of the phase at
cross-over is important for the assessment of the stability of the closed loop. The difference between
the phase shift at cross-over and a phase of 21808 (i.e., the point 21) is called the phase margin. For
stability of the closed-loop system, the phase margin must be positive, that is, the phase shift of the
system is less negative than 21808. Usually, a phase margin larger than 408 is chosen.
For any feedback system, where the open-loop transfer function is not unstable, a positive phase margin
is a guarantee for closed-loop stability. Stability for a system that is open-loop unstable, that is, has open-
loop poles in the right-half complex plane, can be studied by means of the Nyquist stability theorem, which
also constitutes more formal proof of stability by means of the frequency response. Proof of and expla-
nation on the Nyquist stability theorem can be found in the engineering textbooks already mentioned.
An exemplary open-loop system, which, when used in a closed-loop feedback, is the “single
in-tegrator”. For a single integrator, K( jv)G( jv) ¼ 1/( jv). The single integrator has an infinitely high
gain at v ¼ 0, thus the output of the closed-loop system will perfectly follow the input for low frequen-
cies. The phase margin is always 908, whatever gain is chosen for the controller.
20
|H | |H| 20
[dB] [dB]
10 10
1+t 2s 1+t1s
0 H (s)= 0 H (s)=
1+t 1s 1+t2s
−10 −10
−20 −20
1/t1 1/t 2 ω[rad/sec] 1/t1 1/t2 ω [rad/sec]
∠H 90 ∠H 90
[deg] [deg]
45 45
0 0
−45 −45
−90 −90
1/t 1 1/t 2 ω[rad/sec] 1/t1 1/t2 ω [rad/sec]
FIGURE 12.9 Bode diagram of a lag-lead compensating network (left) and of a lead-lag compensating network
(right).
12-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Of course, most systems are not single integrator systems. However, the same principles can be
applied, that is, that a high gain at low frequencies is needed, and a sufficiently wide frequency range,
with a frequency response locally resembling a single integrator and having an acceptable phase
margin, can then be chosen for the cross-over frequency. This is summarized in the Primary Rule
of Thumb for frequency domain design, which says that near the cross-over frequency, the following
must hold:
vc
Y OL (jv) ¼ K(jv)G(jv) ¼ : (12:15)
jv
For the controlling element, when design in the frequency domain is used, a lead/lag or a lag/lead
network is usually chosen. Since a logarithmic scale is used in a Bode diagram, the frequency response
of the controller K( jv) can simply be added to the frequency response of the controlled system
G( jv). Diagrams of the most common “compensating networks” are given in Figure 12.9.
12.3.1 Motivation
The motivation for obtaining mathematical models of human control behavior has evolved from the
need to explain the behavior of human-vehicle control systems to understanding human behavior in
general. The analytical descriptions desired are in control-theoretical terms. The main purposes of the
engineering models are:
1. To summarize behavioral data
2. To provide a basis for rationalization and understanding of human control behavior
3. To be used in conjunction with vehicle dynamics in forming predictions or in explaining the
behavior of combined closed loop human-machine systems
Modeling humans using systems theory has proved to be a tremendous challenge. Humans are complex
control and information processing-systems: they are time-varying, adaptive, nonlinear, and their beha-
vior is essentially stochastic in nature (McRuer and Jex, 1967). Such systems are difficult to be charac-
terized in mathematical terms because most of the mathematical tools are applied strictly to
stationary, linear, and nonadaptive systems.
variables. The report provided convincing empirical evidence that humans systematically adapt their
control behavior to the task variables.
and grasp it into a mathematical model, one encounters a nontrivial problem, as most of the available
techniques only work out well in the time-invariant case.
The earliest attempts in describing human behavior with control-theoretical models failed to pay
much attention to the adaptivity of human behavior. Research showed that when experiments are not
done under (almost) exactly the same circumstances, the human will adapt and the observed control
behavior will be different. The lack of a systematic approach to this problem resulted in scattered data
and many different and unexpected findings, and theory progressed only slowly.
This changed in the late 1950s and early 1960s when McRuer et al. started working on this problem.
Learning from past experience, they first determined and classified a list of variables that could possibly
have an effect on human behavior. These included environmental variables (e.g., conducting the task in
real flight or in a fixed-base simulator), procedural variables (e.g., subject instruction, practice), and
operator-centered variables (e.g., subject motivation, workload).
Most important to understand the adaptation of the human operator are the task variables, however,
which include:
. The dynamics of the system to be controlled
. The properties (bandwidth) of the forcing function, that is, the signal to be followed (in a following
task) or the disturbance signal acting on the system (in a disturbance task)
. The type of display (e.g., a compensatory display or a pursuit display)
. The type of manipulator
McRuer et al. conducted a massive number of tracking task experiments. In their approach they tried to
very closely control all the variables that could have an effect on human behavior and systematically
varied two of the task variables, that is, the dynamics of the system and the bandwidth of the forcing
function signal. As a result of this approach, human variability decreased significantly and for the first
time insight was gained into how and why humans adapt to changing circumstances. In the following
paragraphs the main results of this research will be elaborated on.
In short, the human will establish, in a learning process, a control system that aims at establishing a
trade-off between the requirements of performance and stability, in the same fashion as a control engin-
eer would design an automatic control system.
Compensatory Tracking Tasks Extensive research has been conducted on the problem of modeling
human control behavior in elementary single-axis compensatory tracking tasks, see Figure 12.10a. In
this task the operator must minimize the difference (error E) between the output (Z) of the system to
be controlled and a reference signal (R). This is the same control situation as described earlier,
Figure 12.7. The double-lined block in Figure 12.10a illustrates that pilot behavior in this closed loop
is essentially nonlinear.
Fundamentals of Manual Control 12-13
(a) system
R( jw) + E( jw) U( jw) Z( jw)
PILOT Hc( jw)
-
describing function
FIGURE 12.10 The quasi-linear pilot model in the elementary single-axis compensatory tracking task. In this
figure, Hc depicts the dynamics of the system to be controlled, and R, E, U, and Y the reference signal, the
displayed error signal, the pilot control signal, and the system output signal, respectively. The quasi-linear pilot
model consists of a describing function Hp and a remnant N, (a) single-axis compensatory tracking task; (b)
quasi-linear pilot model.
Experimental studies concerning the compensatory tracking task showed that, as long as the task
variables remain constant, the operator control behavior remains fairly constant too. In this case, it
can be described by a deterministic model — a linear differential equation with constant coefficients
and a time delay (the describing function) — and a remnant model — a stationary noise process.
The result is a quasi-linear pilot model (Figure 12.10b): the describing function accounts for the portion of
the human controller’s output that is linearly related to his input and the remnant represents the difference
between the linear model output and the experimentally measured output of the human controller.
The application of quasi-linear theory allowed human manual control behavior in single-axis com-
pensatory control tasks to be identified. The insights gained led to the postulation of the COM
theorem.
wc
10−1 10−1 10−1
10−1 100 101 10−1 100 101 10−1 100 101
101 101 101
k/s
magnitude
wc
10−1 10−1 10−1
10−1 100 101 10−1 100 101 10−1 100 101
101 101 101
k/s 2
magnitude
wc
10−1 10−1 10−1
−1 0 1 −1 0 1 −1
10 10 10 10 10 10 10 100 101
frequency, rad/sec frequency, rad/sec frequency, rad/sec
FIGURE 12.11 Identification of pilot control behavior when controlling the three basic systems k, k/s, and k/s 2.
The crosses indicate the frequencies of the sinusoids in the forcing function R( jv). The dashed lines in the right
column indicate integrator-like dynamics.
column shows the magnitude of the estimated open-loop, that is, the product of the estimated pilot FRF
and the system FRF: Y^ OL (jv) ¼ H^ p (jv)Hc (jv):
The dynamics of the center column show the proportional (top), integrator (center), and double inte-
grator (bottom) properties of the system to be controlled. The left column shows that the pilot FRF is
different for all three systems, a clear indication that the pilot is adapting to the dynamics of the
system to be controlled. The right column, however, shows that the shape of the open-loop is the
same. Independent of the system dynamics, the open-loop FRF resembles “integrator- like” dynamics
near the frequency where the open-loop equals one, that is, near the cross-over frequency. As will be
discussed later, the value of the cross-over frequency depends, among others, on the dynamics of the
system and the bandwidth of the forcing function.
Apparently, human operators adapt to the system to be controlled in such a way that the open-loop,
that is, the human dynamics times the system dynamics, becomes an integrator. McRuer et al. generalized
this systematic adaptation of human control behavior with the postulation of their COM theorem
(McRuer et al., 1965).
The COM theorem states that human controllers adjust their control behavior to the dynamics of the
controlled element in such a way that the dynamic characteristics of the open-loop transfer function in
the cross-over region can be described by:
vc jvte
Y OL (jv) ¼ Hp (jv)Hc (jv) e , (12:16)
jv
where vc is the cross-over frequency and te is a time delay lumping the information-processing delays of
the human operator.
Fundamentals of Manual Control 12-15
The COM is a mathematical statement of the empirical observation that human controllers adjust their
control characteristics so that the closed-loop system dynamics mimic those of a well-designed feedback
system (McRuer and Jex, 1967). Then, when the dynamics of the system to be controlled are known, the
COM allows a prediction of the human controller characteristics via:
vc
Hp (jv) ejvte : (12:17)
jv H c (jv)
The parameters vc and te are task-dependent and can be selected on the basis of the so-called verbal
adjustment rules that are in turn based on an immense amount of experimental data (McRuer and
Krendel, 1974).
The parameters in this model reflect the pilot adaptation characteristics as well as the limitations such
as the time delay and the neuromuscular system. The equalization parameters are adjusted by the pilot in
such a way that the open-loop dynamics satisfy the COM (Equation (12.16)).
The most commonly used approximation of the precision model is the simplified precision model
(McRuer and Jex, 1967):
1 þ t L jv
Hp (jv) ¼ Kp ejvte
|ffl{zffl} (12:19)
|{z} 1 þ tI jv
gain |fflfflfflfflffl{zfflfflfflfflffl} time delay
pilot equalization
with:
. Kp the pilot gain
. tL the lead time constant (in sec)
. tI the lag time constant (in sec)
. te the effective time delay (in sec)
The simplified precision model (SPM) is widely used and will be discussed in the following.
1
Skipping the so-called ‘a’-term (McRuer and Jex, 1967).
12-16 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
adjustment rules enable us to predict the parameters used in the precision or simplified precision models,
including their numerical values, for a specific combination of system dynamics and forcing function
signal bandwidth.
There are six rules, only the discussion of the first and the last will follow. The reader should keep in
mind that the adjustment rules have an empirical basis and should be used with care.
1/tI wc
10−1 10−1 10−1
10−1 100 101 10−1 100 101 10−1 100 101
101 101 101
k/s
magnitude
wc
10−1 10−1 10−1
10−1 100 101 10−1 100 101 10−1 100 101
101 101 101
k/s 2
magnitude
1/tL wc
10−1 10−1 10−1
10−1 100 101 10−1 100 101 10−1 100 101
frequency, rad/sec frequency, rad/sec frequency, rad/sec
FIGURE 12.12 Pilot equalization when controlling the three basic systems k, k/s, and k/s 2.
Fundamentals of Manual Control 12-17
For the velocity control system (k/s), the pilot equalization parameters can be zero or equal (tI ¼ tL),
and the pilot can act like a proportional gain with a time delay:
Hp ¼ Kp ejvte : (12:20)
There is no need to “fix” the system dynamics through adaptation because the system dynamics itself
already mimics the integrator-like dynamics that are predicted by the COM.
For the proportional control system (k), the pilot must use the lag equalization (tI) to make the open-
loop an integrator:
1
Hp ¼ Kp ejvte : (12:21)
(1 þ jvtI )
The lag parameter needs to be chosen such that it creates the integrator-like characteristic well before
the cross-over frequency, that is, 1=tI
vc . An intuitive interpretation for the lag is that the human inte-
grates or smoothers the input, responding to trends rather than to moment-to-moment variations. In
essence, the human is responding to the “averages” of error over moving windows of observations
rather than to the instantaneous error, values.
Finally, for the acceleration control system (k/s 2), the pilot needs to use the lead equalization (tL) to
make the open-loop an integrator:
The lead parameter needs to be chosen such that it creates the integrator-like characteristic well before
the cross-over frequency, that is, 1=tL
vc . An intuitive interpretation for the lead is that the human
responds to both the position and velocity of the error signal. In essence, attending to velocity allows
the human to effectively anticipate future position errors.
Similarly, the experiments showed that vc increases with vi with a slope that is also practically inde-
pendent of the controlled element, and again the basic level of vc depends primarily on the controlled
element and is independent of vi:
TABLE 12.1 Values for t0 ,v0, and Dt for the three basic Systems
(McRuer and Jex, 1967).
v0 (rad/sec) t0 (sec) Dt (sec)
k 5 to 6 0.33 0.070
k/s 4.3 0.36 0.065
k/s2 3.3 0.50 0.065
12.4.6 Remnant
Much, if not all of the research into the applicability of quasi-linear pilot models focused on the describ-
ing function element. The reader should note that in the discussion on the COM, no mention was made of
the remnant part in the quasi-linear model. The whole discussion focused on the adaptation that takes
place in the pilot describing function, that is, the pilot frequency response function.
Numerous efforts to obtain insight into the origin and characteristics of the remnant have been
conducted (Levison et al., 1969; Jex et al., 1971), and models exist for some of the more structural
aspects of remnant (McRuer and Krendel, 1974; Jagacinski and Flach, 2003). In general, remnant increases
when the task becomes more difficult, that is, for more difficult systems like the double integrator k/s 2,
when workload increases, when more tasks have to be conducted simultaneously, and so forth.
12.5.1 Introduction
The COM and the models originating from it operate primarily in the frequency domain. This is not
surprising, since the techniques involved come from the “classical” frequency-domain approach to
Fundamentals of Manual Control 12-19
designing feedback control systems, dating back to the early 1950s. With the advent of optimal control
and estimation theory in the 1960s, a second approach to modeling human control behavior became
popular, operating primarily in the time domain, resulting in the human OCM.
The basic assumption of the OCM is that “the well-trained, well-motivated human operator behaves
in an optimal manner, subject to his inherent limitations and to the requirements of the control task”
(Kleinman et al., 1970b). It is a normative model in the sense that parameters in the model (the observer
and control gains) are set to minimize errors of observation and control based on the analytical solution
of optimal algorithms. The model describes what operators should do given their inherent constraints
limiting their behavior and the extent to which they understand the task objectives. The OCM structure
is shown in Figure 12.13.
The operator task is to control a dynamic system (state x) perturbed by external disturbances (w).
The outputs of the system (y) are presented with a display. The operator uses the information gathered
about the system state to generate a control signal (u), which maintains a system reference state,
compensating for the effects of the disturbances. The modeling through optimal control of the
stationary input–output relation of the operator has the following starting points (Kok and Van Wijk,
1978):
. The system to be controlled can be described as an LTI system
. The observed outputs are an LTI combination of the system states
. The operator minimizes a quadratic cost functional
. The limitations of the operator can be modeled with:
(i) A single time delay t, lumping the operator information-processing lags
(ii) An observation noise vy , a Gaussian white noise signal, which represents operator uncer-
tainties concerning the observed variables
(iii) A motor noise vu , a Gaussian white noise signal, which represents operator uncertainties in
generating their control input uðtÞ
(iv) A neuromuscular lag tN, representing the operator neuromuscular system
. Operators have internal models of:
(i) The dynamics of the system to be controlled
(ii) The statistics of the system disturbances
(iii) The relation between the observed outputs and the system state
w(t) SYSTEM
system
disturbances
system observed
control state outputs
inputs system display
x (t) y (t)
u (t)
uc(t)
where x(t) is the (n 1) state vector and u(t) are the ‘ inputs to the system. The (k 1) disturbance
vector is defined as an independent, zero-mean, Gaussian white noise vector with auto-covariance:
E{w(t)wT (t þ t)} ¼ W d(t). It is assumed that several system outputs are presented to the human in a
continuous way via some instrument panel:
where y(t) is the (m 1) observation vector representing the information set upon which the operator
bases the control actions. It is assumed that if a quantity yi is displayed, the derivative of that quantity —
y_ i — is also perceived; the observation vector contains pairs of variables explicitly displayed to, as well as
those implicitly derived by the operator.
The perceived output is processed by the operator who generates a commanded control input uc (t) that
is considered optimal for the task at hand. A motor noise vu (t) — representing errors in executing the
intended control movements and the fact that the operator does not have perfect knowledge of the
Fundamentals of Manual Control 12-21
The (‘ 1) motor noise vector is defined as an independent, zero-mean, Gaussian white noise vector
with auto-covariance: E{vu (t)vTu (t þ t)} ¼ diag(Vu )d(t).
Vyi ¼ p ryi E{yi2 }ji¼1,2,...,m and Vui ¼ p rui E{u2ci }ji¼1,2,...,‘ : (12:30)
The observation noise ratio ryi , defined as the noise intensity Vyi normalized with respect to the signal
variance has a typical value of 0.01. In other words, the normalized observation noise intensity has a
power density level of 220 dB. The motor noise ratio rui , defined similarly as ryi , has a typical
value of 0.003. Thus, the normalized motor noise intensity has a power density level of 225 dB. The
value of the motor noise ratio has not been validated as extensively as the observation noise ratio, for
which the value of 220 [dB] is substantiated in several psychophysical studies (Kleinman et al., 1971;
Baron, 1976).
Iterative OCM Solution Due to the separation theorem, the OCM can be computed in two iterations as
shown in Figure 12.14 (Thompson, 1987). First, the OCM regulator is computed. The ‘ diagonal
elements gi of G are computed iteratively until the resulting first-order lag time constants tNi equal
those defined by the user (or, when matching experimental data, the value of tNi as exhibited by the
tracker). Second, the OCM observer is computed. The observation and motor noise ratios are defined,
and the model iteratively changes Vyi and Vyi — resulting in different variances — to obtain the
defined ratios.
iteration I iteration II
tNi correct ryi , rui correct
r0 X
i¼m
ryi ¼ (i ¼ 1, 2, . . . , m), subject to fi ¼ 1 (12:31)
fi i¼1
The nominal observation noise ratio is defined as the observation noise ratio when attention is not
shared and fi represents the fraction of attention allocated to a display variable. Then, ryi is the observation
noise ratio associated with the ith display variable when attention is being shared. In accordance with the
fundamental assumption of optimality it can be hypothesized that the operator divides attention among
the displayed variables in an optimal manner. Then, only the nominal level of attention r0 needs to be
defined and the model iteratively computes the optimal set fi (i ¼ 1, . . ., m), yielding the observation
noise ratios ryi for all displayed quantities (Kleinman, 1976). The optimal allocation set depends strongly
on the specification of the control task. It shows the relative importance of the displayed quantities to the
operator in performing the task and can be informative when designing and analyzing displays (Baron,
1976; Korn et al., 1982).
1
r0 ¼ , (12:32)
1
r1 þ 1
r2 þ . . . þ r1
m
References
Baron, S., A model for human control and monitoring based on modern control theory, J. Cyber. Inf. Sci.,
4(1), pp. 3 –18, 1976.
Baron, S. and Levison, W.H., An optimal control methodology for analyzing the effects of display
parameters on performance and workload in manual flight control. IEEE Trans. Sys., Man,
Cyber., SMC-5(4), pp. 423– 430, 1975.
Dorf, R.C. and Bishop, R.H., Modern Control Systems, 10th ed., Pearson Education, Upper Saddle River,
NJ, 2005.
Flach, J.M., Control with an eye for perception: precursors to an active psychophysics, Proceedings of a
workshop held at NASA Ames Research Center, Moffett Field (CA), in Johnson W.W. and Kaiser,
M.K. eds., Visually Guided Control of Movement, NASA Ames Research Center, 1991, pp. 121–149,
June 26 to July 14, 1989.
Franklin, G.F., Powell, J.D., and Emami-Naeini, A., Feedback Control of Dynamic Systems, Prentice Hall,
Upper Saddle River, NJ, 2002.
Goodwin, G.C., Graebe, S.F., and Salgado, M.E., Control System Design, Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle
River, NJ, 2001.
Hess, R.A., Analytical display design for flight tasks conducted under instrument meteorological
conditions, IEEE Trans. Sys., Man, Cyber., SMC-7(6), pp. 453 –462, 1977.
Hess, R.A., Aircraft control-display analysis and design using the optimal control model of the human
pilot, IEEE Trans. Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, SMC-11(7), pp. 465–480, 1981.
Fundamentals of Manual Control 12-25
Hess, R.A., Feedback Control Models: Manual control and tracking, in Handbook of Human Factors and
Ergonomics Salvendy, G., ed., John Wiley & Sons, New York, pp. 1249 –1294, 1997.
Hess, R.A. and Wheat, L.W., A model-based analysis of a display for helicopter landing approach, IEEE
Trans. Sys., Man, Cyber., SMC-6(7), pp. 505 –511, 1976.
Jagacinski, R.J. and Flach, J.M., Control Theory for Humans — Quantitative Approaches to Modeling
Performance, Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, 2003.
Jex, H.R., Allen, R.W., and Magdaleno, R.E., Effects of display format on pilot describing function
and remnant, Proc. Seventh Annual Conference on Manual Control, NASA SP-281, pp. 155 –159,
1971.
Kleinman, D.L., Solving the optimal attention allocation problem in manual control, IEEE Trans.
Automatic Control, AC-21(6), pp. 813 –821, 1976.
Kleinman, D.L. and Baron, S., Manned-Vehicle Systems Analysis by Means of Modern Control Theory
(NASA Contractor Report No. CR-1753), Washington, D.C., 1971.
Kleinman, D.L. and Baron, S., A control theoretic model for piloted approach to landing, Automatica, 9,
pp. 339–347, 1973.
Kleinman, D.L., Baron, S., and Levison, W.H., An optimal control model of human response. Part II:
prediction of human performance in a complex task, Automatica, 6, pp. 371 –383, 1970a.
Kleinman, D.L., Baron, S., and Levison, W.H., An optimal control model of human response. Part I:
theory and validation, Automatica, 6, pp. 357 –369, 1970b.
Kleinman, D.L., Baron, S., and Levison, W.H., A control theoretic approach to manned-vehicle systems
analysis, IEEE Trans. Automatic Control, AC-16(6), pp. 824 –832, 1971.
Kleinman, D.L. and Korn, J., Information and display requirements for aircraft terrain following, Proc.
Seventeenth Annual Conference Manual Control, pp. 329 –331, 1981.
Kok, J.J. and Van Wijk, R.A., Evaluation of Models Describing Human Operator Control of Slowly Respond-
ing Complex Systems. Ph.D. dissertation, Faculty of Mechanical Engineering, Delft University of
Technology, 1978.
Korn, J., Gully, S.W., and Kleinman, D.L., Validation of an advanced cockpit design methodology via a
workload/monitoring tradeoff analysis, Proc. Eighteenth Annual Conference on Manual Control,
pp. 268–292, 1982.
Krendel, E.S. and McRuer, D.T., A servomechanics approach to skill development, J. Franklin Institute,
269(1), pp. 24–42, 1960.
Kwakernaak, H. and Sivan, R., Linear Optimal Control Systems, John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1972.
Levison, W.H., Baron, S., and Kleinman, D.L., A model for human controller remnant, IEEE Trans.
Man-Machine Sys., MMS-10(4), pp. 101–108, 1969.
McRuer, D.T., Allen, R.W., Weir, D.H., and Klein, R.H., New Results in Driver Steering Control Models.
Hum. Factors, 19(4), pp. 381 –397, 1977.
McRuer, D.T., Graham, D., Krendel, E.S., and Reisener W. Jr., Human Pilot Dynamics in Compensatory
Systems. Theory, Models, and Experiments with Controlled Element and Forcing Function Variations
(Technical Report No. AFFDL-TR-65-15), Air Force Flight Dynamics Laboratory, Wright-
Patterson AFB, OH, 1965.
McRuer, D.T. and Jex, H.R., A review of quasi-linear pilot models, IEEE Trans. Hum. Factors Electron.,
HFE-8(3), pp. 231– 249, 1967.
McRuer, D.T. and Krendel, E.S., Mathematical Models of Human Pilot Behaviour (Agardograph No. 88),
AGARD, 1974.
McRuer, D.T. and Schmidt, D.K., Pilot-vehicle analysis of multiaxis tasks, J. Guidance, Control Dynamics,
13(2), pp. 348–355, 1990.
Mulder, M., Cybernetics of Tunnel-in-the-Sky Displays, Ph.D. dissertation, Faculty of Aerospace Engineer-
ing, Delft University of Technology, 1999.
Nise, N.S., Control Systems Engineering, 2nd ed., Benjamin/Cummings, Redwood City, CA, 1995.
Olsder, G.J. and van der Woude, J.W., Mathematical Systems Theory., Delftse Uitgevers Maatschappij,
Delft, 1994.
12-26 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Phatak, A.V., Weinert, H., Segall, I., and Day, C.N., Identification of a modified optimal control model for
the human operator, Automatica, 12, pp. 31–41, 1976.
Sheridan, T.B. and Ferrell, W.R. Man-Machine Systems, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1974.
Stapleford, R.L., Craig, S.J., and Tennant, J.A., Measurement of Pilot Describing Functions in
Single-Controller Multiloop Tasks (NASA Contractor Report No. CR-1238), Washington, D.C., 1969.
Stapleford, R.L., McRuer, D.T., and Magdaleno, R. Pilot describing function measurements in a multi-
loop task. IEEE Trans. Hum. Factors Electron., HFE-8(2), pp. 113–125, 1967.
Stassen, H.G., Johannsen, G., and Moray, N., Internal representation, internal model, human perform-
ance and mental workload, Automatica, 26(4), pp. 811–820, 1990.
Thompson, P.M. Program CC’s implementation of the human optimal control model, Proc. AIAA
Guidance, Navigation and Control Conference, Monterey, CA, August 17 –19, 1987.
Van Lunteren, A., Identification of Human Operator Describing Function Models with One or Two Inputs in
Closed Loop Systems, Ph.D. dissertation, Faculty of Mechanical Engineering, Delft University of
Technology, 1979.
Van Paassen, M.M. Biophysics in Aircraft Control. A Model of the Neuromuscular System of the Pilot’s Arm,
Ph.D. dissertation, Faculty of Aerospace Engineering, Delft University of Technology, 1994.
Van Wijk, R.A. and Kok, J.J., Theoretic aspects of the identification of the parameters in the optimal
control model, Proc. Thirteenth Annual Conference on Manual Control, pp. 27 –34, 1977.
Weir, D.H. and McRuer, D.T., Models for steering control of motor vehicles, Proc. of the Fourth Annual
Conference on Manual Control, NASA SP-192, pp. 135–169, 1968.
Weir, D.H. and McRuer, D.T., Pilot Dynamics for Instrument Approach Tasks: Full Panel Multiloop and
Flight Director Operations (NASA Contractor Report No. CR-2019), Washington, D.C., 1972.
Wewerinke, P.H., An Analysis of In-Flight Helicopter Pilot Control Behaviour and Work-load (NLR Tech-
nical Report No. TR 76146 C), National Aerospace Laboratory, Amsterdam, 1976.
Wewerinke, P.H., Visual Scene Perception — Frequency-Domain Data and Model Para-meter Estimation
Procedure (NLR Memorandum No. MP 79009 U), National Aerospace Laboratory, Amsterdam, 1979.
Wewerinke, P.H., Models of the Human Observer and Controller of a Dynamic System, Ph.D. dissertation,
Department of Applied Mathematics, University of Twente, 1989.
Wickens, C.D., Engineering Psychology and Human Performance, 2nd ed., Harper-Collins, 1992.
13
Cumulative Spine
Loading
13.1 Introduction
Cumulative loading is an approach to assess the potential for injury or pain reporting that is garnering a
great deal of attention in both industry and ergonomic communities. This chapter will examine the
theoretical foundations linking cumulative exposure and injury, review the studies that have linked
cumulative exposure and pain reporting, detail current barriers to the wide spread implementation of
cumulative loading assessments in industrial settings and present the author’s opinions on the future
directions of the field.
It is important to realize that cumulative loading at its core is an extension of acute loading
(Figure 13.1) analyses employed to document the time varying exposures that workers experience. It
is encumbered with the same errors and limitations that biomechanical models of the low back have,
with many added factors that do not occur when assessing peak task exposures. Cumulative loading
assessments are utilized to capture a better representation of the factors that can contribute to injury
and as such it promises a much greater ability to examine interworker variability in task performance
13-1
13-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Injury
Tolerance
Value
Force
Tissue Load
Time
FIGURE 13.1 Acute injury scenario where a single load application exceeds the tolerance of a tissue resulting in an
injury.
and injury reporting. In other words the individual methods used by different workers performing the
same job would result in similar risk assessments with many acute tools that only incorporate task
demands (i.e., rate of lifting, height of lift, etc.) or examine maximum demands (postures or forces).
In contrast by examining the full work cycle of a task the individual differences between workers
would be revealed in loading and postural demands both within a single cycle and over an extended
period, typically expressed as shift exposure.
While cumulative loading is a relatively new terminology in spine biomechanics or low back ergo-
nomics it has a relation to cumulative trauma disorders (CTD) of the upper extremity that warrants
a brief examination. CTDs can be traced back in the ergonomics literature to the 1980s. It appears
that Armstrong et al. (1982) were among the first to use the specific terminology in the peer
reviewed literature. While the underlying rationale between CTD and cumulative loading are
similar, the two terms have some very clear differences. CTD is defined as pathology to soft
tissues as a result of exposure to excessively frequent use of biological tissue, compounded by exces-
sive loads, awkward joint positions and inadequate recovery (Stramler, 1993). In contrast cumulative
loading is a direct quantification of the magnitude of loading that a joint experiences over a given
period of time, which is influenced by posture, repetition, duration and force. Evidently, both CTD
and cumulative loading are tied to injury through common risk factors such as force, posture and
rate of repetition or duration. However, the important difference between the two terms is that
cumulative loading is a direct quantification of loading exposure in response to these risk factors
whereas CTD is an injury definition. The origin of the terminology “cumulative loading” in the
spine can be traced back to the 1990s when it first appeared in the research literature (Kumar,
1990).
The ideas of cumulative loading, repetition rate and duration of exposure, while being related, have
very different meanings, which will be brought to light in this chapter. Since the publication of the
phrase “cumulative loading” the idea has been quickly adopted by the industrial community, including
ergonomists, health care providers, workers and management. The most frequent question asked by these
groups is invariably “How much cumulative exposure is too much?” Unfortunately the question is not
readily answerable with the currently available knowledge base.
This chapter will focus exclusively on cumulative loading of the lumbar spine and associated issues.
Evidence will be presented in support of a cumulative injury mechanism and the hurdles that need to
be addressed before cumulative loading will be usable as an injury prevention approach in industry
will be discussed.
Cumulative Spine Loading 13-3
X
n
Shift Cumulative Loading ¼ Cumulative Task Loading (i) (13:1)
i¼1
Often this is simplified by taking the cumulative loading in one cycle and extrapolating the data to
represent what the equivalent exposure would be for an 8-h shift. The example presented in
Figure 13.2 has a cycle time of 5 sec. To extrapolate this value to a shift dosage would require a multi-
plication by 5760 (12 times per minute 60 minutes per hour 8 hours per shift). This would yield
a shift cumulative compression value of 37.5 MN s. An understanding of how cumulative loading is
calculated is fundamental to appreciate the unique challenges associated with developing a method to
assess the risk of injury from cumulative loading exposure. This will be explored in the following sections.
2500
2000
Compression (N)
1500
1000
500
0
0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 5
Time (s)
FIGURE 13.2 A biomechanical model was used to calculate the compression value for each frame of video for a
sagittal lifting task. The area under the curve (solid area) is integrated to determine the amount of compression
to which the L4/L5 joint is exposed over the course of the task. The cumulative compression for this lift is the
value of this area (6514 N s).
13-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
(a) (b)
Tolerance Tolerance
Value Value
Time Time
FIGURE 13.3 Cumulative loading scenarios, sustained (a) and repetitive (b) exposures that result in a tissue injury
at sub-acute force values.
Cumulative Spine Loading 13-5
10
1 Hz
Area = 4 kNs
9 10 Hz
6
Force (kN)
0
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1
Time (s)
FIGURE 13.4 In vitro loading profiles of two different rates. The cumulative loading for the two different
frequencies is equal due to the same percentage of time spent at any given loading magnitude.
that repetition is not an injury risk factor. However, these findings have only been illustrated for bone in
tension. While bone in compression (Caler and Carter, 1989) and muscle tendon (Wang et al., 1995;
Wren et al., 2003) both respond to different frequencies of exposure supporting repetition as a risk
factor for developing CTDs.
Specific evidence of the time varying response of the spine to repetitive loading has also been
documented in the research literature. At a fundamental level, research linking biological response to
prolonged exposures has shown that cell death in the intervertebral disc of a rat model, tested under
sustained compressive loads, was directly linked to the magnitude and duration of the loading (Lotz
and Chin, 2000). The loads that were employed were comparable in relative magnitude to human
spine loading of 500 to 1500 N, which are representative of the range of compressive spine loads experi-
enced in upright standing to very light industrial lifting tasks. A porcine model of the spine exhibited
morphological changes to the intervertebral disc, such as shape changes, in response to repetitive com-
pressive loading in the elastic range of the motion segments employed (Yu et al., 2003). These changes are
thought to be indicative of early stages of disc degeneration. Furthermore, an in situ rabbit spine model
with a higher number of loading cycles in a fixed time frame (equivalent to larger cumulative exposure)
revealed that annular damage was elevated with increased loading cycles (Wada et al., 1992). These
studies, which provide primary evidence of sub-acute injury pathways of the spine are still secondary
evidence of cumulative loading as they have not directly quantified injury due to cumulative loading
magnitude.
A final property of the spine, with primary focus on the intervertebral disc, that merits consideration is
its somewhat unique response to cumulative exposure and its poor ability for self-repair once an injury
pathway has been initiated. Known bone healing times are in the order of several weeks or months, so it is
fair to assume that even micro-fractures to trabeculae would require weeks to repair while the tissue is
continually being loaded. It is hard or near impossible to isolate the spine from loading, similar to the
immobilization that would be applied to an injured appendage. The avascular nature of the interior
margins of the intervertebral disc is a factor that constrains its ability for self-repair. The proteoglygan
13-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
turnover of the intervertebral disc has been shown to take 500 days in a canine model (Urban et al., 1978)
and collagen production is thought to take even longer (Adams and Hutton, 1982; Porter et al., 1989).
This is supported in an in situ animal study using rats that revealed that intervertebral disc annular
damage was still present after a month of recovery (Lotz et al., 1998). Clearly once an injury is initiated
in the spine, cumulative loading has the potential to outpace the repair mechanism. This pathway
is further compounded by the lack of pain sensing fibers in the interior of the intervertebral disc.
Only the outer third of the annulus is innervated with pain sensing fibers (Bogduk, 1983; Cavanaugh
et al., 1995), so an injury can progress from the interior margin to the exterior boundary before there
is the potential for direct pain generation. The mechanical changes in this process are not well under-
stood and secondary pathways for pain generation could occur due to disc height change or altered
mechanics.
100
90
Hansson et al.
Compression (% Max) 80 Brinckmann et al.
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
0 500 1000 1500 2000 2500 3000 3500 4000 4500
Cycles
FIGURE 13.5 Logarithmic trend lines fit to the data demonstrating the nonlinear relationship between magnitude
of compressive loading and cycles to failure. (From Brinckmann, P., Biggemann, M., and Hilweg, D., Clin. Biomech., 3
(Suppl. 1), pp. s1– s23, 1988; Hansson, T.H., Keller, T.S., and Spengler, D.M., J. Orthop. Res., 5, pp. 479– 487, 1987.)
discussed earlier in the chapter supports this relationship for bone in compression (Caler and Carter,
1989) and muscle tendon (Wang et al., 1995; Wren et al., 2003). This lack of a single cumulative
loading failure threshold was substantiated in work that examined the in vitro generation of interverteb-
ral disc herniations from combined loading (Callaghan and McGill, 2001). Varying levels of compression
were applied in conjunction with repetitive flexion/extension motions. The cumulative compression to
injury was affected by the magnitude of the load applied (Figure 13.7).
60
y = 0.0119x + 0.117
R2 = 0.9508
Cumulative Compression (MN s)
50
40
30
20
10
0
0 500 1000 1500 2000 2500 3000 3500 4000 4500 5000
Cycles
FIGURE 13.6 The linear relationship of cumulative loading sustained to failure based on the data.
(From Brinckmann, P., Biggemann, M., and Hilweg, D., Clin. Biomech., 3 (Suppl. 1), pp. s1 –s23, 1988.)
13-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
30
26400 Cycles
81000 Cycles (Complete Herniation)
Cumulative Compression (MN s) 25 (No Damage)
Specimen 1
20 Specimen 2
15
Initiation of
10
Herniation 5870
3850 Cycles
5 Cycles
0
300 1000 1000 1000
Compression Group (N)
FIGURE 13.7 Cumulative compression for two in vitro spine specimens tested in combined flexion/extension
motion with static compression. Specimen 1 (300 N) exhibited no injury. The three columns representing
specimen 2 (1000 N) demonstrate the development of initiation of an injury and complete intervertebral disc
herniation (based on data from Callaghan and McGill, Clin. Biomech., 16, pp. 28–37, 2001. With permission.)
In summary, the idea of cumulative exposure being linked to injury is well supported by both repeti-
tive testing on isolated tissues at sub-acute levels and existing in vitro spine biomechanics research.
However, the clear influence of loading factors (i.e., load magnitude) as modifiers of cumulative
loading to failure that the spine can sustain raises the question of whether a single tolerance value can
be employed to assess a worker’s risk of injury from cumulative loading exposure.
(a) (b)
Increased Increased
chance of chance of
Risk Injury Risk Injury
FIGURE 13.8 Injury exposure relationships. Simple models (a) that represent increasing risk with increasing
exposure, either linearly or nonlinearly. The “J” or “U” nonlinear risk model (b), where there is an optimum
loading that results in minimal risk of injury.
Cumulative Spine Loading 13-9
and remodeling behaviors are considered it does not appear to be a plausible model of the in vivo
response to loading.
Increased levels of motion have been shown to be beneficial in providing nutrition to the structures of
the intervertebral disc (Holm and Nachemson, 1983). This contrasts with the in vitro research and the
linear injury models that have demonstrated that increased loading cycles resulted in an increased
probability of injury. Intervertebral disc degeneration has been associated with decreased nutrition
(Buckwalter, 1995). In an examination of work demands and injury risk (Videman et al., 1990), seden-
tary work characterized by small spinal motions and loading was shown to result in intervertebral
disc injury. Additionally, workers who performed heavy work were also at increased risk of developing
a spinal injury. Workers who were involved in varying or mixed work had the lowest risk of developing
a spine injury. This presents the idea that too little or too much motion or load can both present an
increased risk of spinal injury. An in situ rat intervertebral disc study (Iatridis et al., 1999) appears to
support this “U”- or “J”-shaped relationship between exposure and risk of injury (Figure 13.8b).
Three groups of rats were used that underwent different mechanical loading: a control group (equivalent
to mixed exposure), an immobilized group (sedentary) and a chronic compression group (increased
loading). The control group had the fewest biomechanical changes to the intervertebral disc with the
immobilized and chronic compression group exhibiting similar degenerative responses. However, the
chronic compression degenerative changes occurred earlier and were more pronounced (Iatridis et al.,
1999) supporting a “J”-shaped injury risk model.
The importance of these injury models to cumulative loading is paramount in how cumulative loading
will be used in the future and its potential success at preventing low back injuries. If the scenario in
Figure 13.8B is the response of the spine to cumulative loading then a simple tolerance value cannot
be considered without at least considering the type of work to which an individual is exposed.
Further there are likely other factors that will modify the relationship between cumulative loading
exposure and risk of injury that will be examined later in this chapter.
Threshold
Injury Rate
Cases
Controls
Cumulative Loading
FIGURE 13.9 Representation of the unknown location where a tolerance limit value would lie when observing case
control cumulative loading studies.
used in these studies will be reviewed briefly with the remainder of the chapter addressing issues that are
critical to the development of a cumulative threshold limit value and measurement method.
Methods to document cumulative loading have involved questionnaires (Kumar, 1990), self-recall and
report (Seidler et al., 2001, 2003) and varying forms of video documentation (Godin et al., 2003; Jager
et al., 2000; Norman et al., 1998). Each of these approaches possess strengths and weaknesses and it is
beyond the scope of this work to debate these inherent properties in any depth. Two studies have
relied on 2D regression-based equations to yield lumbar spine compression from position of the external
load and weight of the object lifted (Seidler et al., 2001, 2003). It is difficult to assess the validity of this
approach as the studies do not report validation data of the modified Mainz-Dortmund dose model
(MDD) used. Two studies use 2D biomechanical rigid link models with a single muscle equivalent
model and examined joint compression and shear (Kumar, 1990) as well as net joint flexion/extension
moment (Norman et al., 1998). The final two studies used relatively sophisticated 3D biomechanical
models to yield the forces and moments on the lumbar spine (Godin et al., 2003; Jager et al., 2000).
The two studies employing the regression approach examined the total cumulative compression from
lifting and carrying over the individual’s work life prior to spinal injury diagnosis (Seidler et al., 2001,
2003). These values are obviously extremely large, on the order of 106 N h or 3600 MN s. These values
hold little value for ergonomists in evaluating a shift dosage in order to assess risk of injury. However,
these studies do provide valuable information about the relationship of injury, diagnosed medically,
and cumulative compression as stated previously. The remaining industrial studies have evaluated
cumulative loading over varying periods but all included a shift exposure. These values are presented
in Table 13.1.
Briefly expanding on the methodologies used in these studies, the most common approach has been to
incorporate video analysis to record the working activities and then assess the exposures later. Norman
et al. (1998) reduced data collection and processing time by using the peak static spinal load multiplied
by the number of repeats and duration of each task to estimate the shift cumulative loading. Kumar
(1990) used a similar static approach; however, it was based on subject recall of end point postures of
tasks and then the generation of intervening frames to represent data collected at 5 frames/sec. The
task data was then combined and similarly extrapolated out to shift exposures and longer periods.
The DOLLY study (Jager et al., 2000) used a posture sampling approach and a 3D-biomechanical
model to assess cumulative exposure for a large number of tasks over entire shifts of eight individuals.
The posture sampling approach utilized results with a lower level of fidelity of documented postures
(i.e., trunk flexion was divided into seven gradations of 158) and had equivalent sampling rates of 0.4
to 1.8 frames/sec. Our own approach to calculate cumulative compression also involves a posture match-
ing approach allowing for 3D assessments from a single 2D video recording (Callaghan et al., 2003;
Jackson et al., 2003). Figure 13.10 illustrates the main user interface of 3DMatch, which allows 3D
Cumulative Spine Loading 13-11
TABLE 13.1 Cumulative Loading Magnitudes Reported in the Research Literature. Values
Given Represent Averages +1 Standard Deviation Unless Noted Otherwise.
Cases Controls
Compression (MN s)
Jager et al. (2000) n/a 10.8 to 36.0 (range) (n ¼ 8)
Kumar (1990) F 15.6 + 5.0 (n ¼ 6) F 6.6 + 5.5 (n ¼ 8)
C 13.5 + 12.1 (n ¼ 52) C 9.3 + 7.7 (n ¼ 95)
Norman et al. (1998) 21.0 +4.72 (n ¼ 104) 19.5 +3.84 (n ¼ 130)
Shear (MN s)
Jager et al. (2000) n/a n/a
Kumar (1990) F 2.5 + 0.9 F 1.0 + 1.0
C 2.2 + 4.2 C 1.6 + 1.5
Norman et al. (1998) 1.52 +0.64 1.32 +0.45
Moment (MN m s)
Jager et al. (2000) n/a n/a
Kumar (1990) n/a n/a
Norman et al. (1998) 0.55 + 0.24 0.47 + 0.15
hand forces and uses postures of the elbow (3), shoulder (10), neck (8) and trunk (18) as inputs to a 3D-
biomechanical model. The video recordings with rates of 2 to 3 frames/sec are opened directly into the
software and evaluated. The justification for this sample rate will be presented later in this chapter.
Alternative approaches have been used to yield cumulative loading. Electromyography (EMG) to com-
pression relationships (Mientjes et al., 1999; Potvin et al., 1990) have been examined as one approach to
provide estimates of cumulative loading. Electromagnetic tracking devices have also been used to provide
valid real-time cumulative compression in 2D (Agnew et al., 2002, 2003). The relationship between heart
FIGURE 13.10 3DMatch user interface for using postural matching and external forces to calculate 3D spine loads
using a biomechanical model.
13-12 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
rate determined physical activity level (HR-PAL) and cumulative low back loads has also been explored
by our group (Azar, 2004). None of these approaches have been used in large-scale studies and all possess
limitations. EMG to compression approaches to date have not been able to track shear and joint moment
or other 3D variables. The electromagnetic approaches are very susceptible to metallic or electrical noise
interference, both features that are prevalent in industry. Self-report type measures are promising for the
great reduction in quantified data collection time that they provide. These have been employed to docu-
ment cumulative loading in large-scale studies (Seidler et al., 2001, 2003) and have been investigated by
our research group (Azar et al., 2005). Our own work revealed a strong agreement between loading cal-
culated with a biomechanical model and the LOG method, where individuals recorded the demands of a
task immediately after completing it (r ¼ 0.989) or a RECALL at the end of a 2-h session (r ¼ 0.403).
Accuracy of recall estimates, particularly task durations are strongly affected by the length of time
(Akesson et al., 2001) from the event to recall time and is a major limitation of this approach.
HR-PAL accounted for over 80% of the variance in cumulative compression forces during nonoccupa-
tional tasks estimated over a period of 2 h (Azar, 2004). Relationships between HR-PAL and other cumu-
lative loading variables were not as strong, but it was concluded that the use of physiological variables
such as heart rate might provide a way to document cumulative load exposure in highly nonrepetitive
activities, at a much reduced processing cost.
25
Pain
No Pain
Cumulative Compression (MN*s)
20
15
10
0
Norman et al. (1998) Kumar (1990)
Study
FIGURE 13.11 Shift cumulative compression values for two case control studies.
Cumulative Spine Loading 13-13
If a single TLV was to be implemented it would not isolate the pain from the no pain groups across the
two studies shown in Figure 13.11. In other words the pain group in the Kumar study (1990) had less
cumulative compression than the control or no pain group in the Norman et al. (1998) study. This
can be attributed to many factors including different industrial samples (institutional aides versus auto-
motive assembly workers) and the methods employed to document cumulative compression. The mag-
nitude of peak spine compression exposure in the nursing and patient handling population tends to be
quite large whereas an automotive plant with ergonomic staff and programs, works to eliminate large
peak exposures. This is supported by the peak data of these two studies, where the institutional aides
(Kumar, 1990) had mean acute loads of sample tasks exceeding the NIOSH MPL (6400 N) and the auto-
motive group (Norman et al., 1998) had mean peak compression below the NIOSH AL (3400 N). This
links well with the tissue-based rationale for cumulative injuries and the nonlinear relationship between
magnitude of loading and cumulative loading to failure. This issue has also been raised by the research
group in Germany (Jager et al., 2000) and different weighting values for the force-time exposures have
been implemented in part based on the work of Brinckmann et al. (1988). The most used weighting
factor appears to involve squaring of the spine forces, which would effectively weight higher forces
more heavily as the time base is unaltered. Unfortunately, returning briefly to tissue evidence, weighting
approaches while sound in theory are poorly supported by actual data at present. While this author
strongly supports the idea and need for weighting functions, and not only for magnitude of loading,
the scatter of the tissue results from Brinkmann et al. (1988) show no clear relationship between magni-
tude of loading and cumulative loading to failure (Figure 13.12). Regardless of the curve fitting approach
employed the R 2 values were on the order of 0.1, which would not support a weighting factor being
applied to compressive magnitudes prior to integrating exposures. In fact if weighting schemes such
as the force squaring or quadrupling approaches as suggested by Jager et al. (2000) are applied to this
data set the R 2 values approach zero. The use of weighting functions is important and clearly
warranted based on the tissue mechanisms of injury.
Recent work that we have completed (Parkinson & Callaghan, 2004) has examined the association
between cumulative exposure and compression injuries in vitro. The study modified the magnitude of
the peak cyclic compression as a percentage of estimated ultimate strength. Specifically, the spine speci-
mens were cyclically loaded at 40, 50, 70, and 90% of their estimated compressive strength up to a
maximum of 21600 cycles and injuries were recorded. The magnitude of loading had a strong non-linear
100
90
80
Compression (%Max)
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50
Cumulative Loading (MN s)
FIGURE 13.12 Cumulative loading sustained to failure calculated for different magnitudes of in vitro repetitive
compressive testing. Only specimens were used in the reanalysis that sustained failure in the 5000-cycle test
window. (From Brinckmann, P., Biggemann, M., and Hilweg, D., Clin. Biomech., 3 (Suppl. 1), pp. s1 –s23, 1988.
With permission.)
13-14 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
90
80
70
Cumulative Load (MN*s)
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Loading Magnitude (% of maximum compressive strength)
FIGURE 13.13 Scatter plot of cumulative load tolerated at failure (MN s) vs. loading magnitude (% of maximum
compressive strength), with the power curve fit to the data (r 2 ¼ 0.9024). Symbols indicate the cumulative magnitude
sustained until failure by specimens in the different compressive exposure groups.
relationship (r 2 ¼ 0.9024) with the cumulative loading a specimen could withstand prior to failure
(Figure 13.13). Based on this relationship a weighting factor was developed that created an equal cumu-
lative exposure magnitude for a given risk of injury. The equation effectively increases the cumulative
exposure for higher acute exposures to represent the same risk of injury that would result from a
longer exposure at a lower force magnitude. This nonlinear weighting equation (Equation 13.2) incor-
porates the magnitude of exposure as a percentage of maximum strength. This can be implemented in
biomechanical models by using regression approaches to predict an individual’s lumbar spine compres-
sive strength. An example of such an equation is developed by Genaidy et al. (1993) which includes age,
gender, body weight and spinal level as input variables and is based on the work of Hansson et al. (1987).
This work provides an initial estimate and a usable equation of the force weighting relationship when
calculating cumulative exposure. However, this theoretical foundation should be assessed on a popu-
lation data set of worker exposure and corresponding injury reporting to determine if it strengthens
this association.
A final issue related to setting a dose exposure limit is the length of standardized exposure period that
should be adopted. Should an hourly dose be set, a shift, a day, a week or an even longer period? The most
common reported exposure dose is a shift exposure. This will allow for a point exposure to be assessed
and intervention decisions to be made without requiring information on long periods of exposure.
However, biologically, injury pathways can progress over long periods of time and an injury is in response
to the loading history prior to the injury or pain event. This extended cumulative exposure was the
rationale behind the two studies linking medically documented injuries and cumulative loading exposure
(Seidler et al., 2001, 2003). An additional factor is that loading exposure does not end when a worker
leaves the work place. Cumulative and peak loading documented in activities external to the industrial
Cumulative Spine Loading 13-15
24
22 Cases (Norman et al., 1998)
14
12
10
8
6
4
2
0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Subject
FIGURE 13.14 Cumulative spine compression forces from the work of Godin et al. (2003) for 2 h of at home
activities. The horizontal bars represent the pain groups’ (cases) cumulative compression shift exposure averages
for industrial workers.
work environment resulted in substantial peak and cumulative loading (Godin et al., 2003; Lauder et al.,
2002). In following individuals for a 2-h period around their homes, the magnitude of peak spinal
compression forces exceeded the NIOSH AL in 60% of the subjects (Godin et al., 2003). The cumulative
compression over this same 2-h period was also substantial (Figure 13.14). In fact if the 2 h of exposure is
extrapolated out to represent a shift dose, the values would surpass the cumulative exposure reported for
back pain cases in industrial workers (Kumar, 1990; Norman et al., 1998). Also noteworthy is the varia-
bility of the cumulative loading that was present in the ten subjects. This is not surprising when differ-
ences in activity levels and lifestyles are considered. However, it does raise the interesting question of
whether this loading, when combined with workplace exposure, would explain any of the variance in
which workers report an injury from a group experiencing homogeneous external task demands.
While a shift dose limit will likely prove to be the desirable and most useful standard for workplace ergo-
nomic assessments, a daily exposure may be more insightful into risk of developing an injury or pain
given the significant magnitudes of cumulative loads documented outside of work.
sufficient sampling rate — 30 frames/sec, in three dimensions for every second of the shift. This would
represent almost 1 million frames of data that would need to be analyzed! Even in a controlled research
environment this would be a fairly daunting task. A further consideration is the end-user of cumulative
loading, the ergonomists and other safety personnel performing job assessments. It would be both
impractical and infeasible to require a job assessment to track an entire shift of a job and spend weeks
analyzing the data to assess risk, and then do it again after any intervention had been performed.
Clearly, documenting true cumulative loading is nearly impossible with current measurement tech-
niques. In order for ergonomists to be able to perform cumulative exposure assessments and research
studies examining cumulative loading to be able to collect a sufficient sample size, more efficient
means to document cumulative loading are required. While secondary approaches such as regression
approaches or subject self-reports, are very appealing metrics, they are ultimately compared against
quantified spine force-time histories as the criterion measure. In order to validate these and other
secondary approaches, documentation of cumulative exposure using biomechanical modeling is
required for comparison. To perform biomechanical modeling on a sample of any reasonable size,
various strategies to reduce the quantity of data required to calculate cumulative exposure have been
employed by researchers. These data reduction approaches and their validity will be addressed in the
remainder of this chapter.
static load magnitude dependent on lifting speed and load magnitude (Danz and Ayoub, 1992). None of
the published cumulative loading studies have used a dynamic modeling approach. Another data
reduction method to reduce the quantity of data precludes dynamic analyses and will be discussed
shortly. Several studies appear to have used quasi-dynamic approaches with representative peak measures
taken of hand loads (Daynard et al., 2001; Godin et al., 2003; Norman et al., 1998) with all other studies
using static models (Jager et al., 2000; Kumar, 1990) and recall of static loads (Seidler et al., 2001, 2003).
The use of a static assumption in cumulative analyses does not induce the same magnitude of errors seen
in peak analyses. Cumulative exposure average errors across varying tasks, masses and lifting speeds were
approximately 10% compared to a dynamic model (Callaghan et al., 2005). The quasi-dynamic model
faired much better with average errors well below 5% compared to a dynamic model. These
lower errors in cumulative loading compared to peak loading can be attributed to the changing accelera-
tion of hand loads when being lifted and lowered. When being lifted the hand load will exceed
the actual static load of the object. But when being lowered the effective hand load will be less than
the static load of the object thereby letting over- and underprediction errors cancel out when dynamic
components are not included. This study used a model with sampled hand loads so that at any
instant in time the hand load was known. This is impractical in an industrial application and the
studies that have employed quasi-dynamic models have utilized a representative hand load for the
entire lifting period or fraction of the task when the load is in contact with the body. This is effectively
a static or nonchanging load and may prove to be no better than simply using the mass of the objects
lifted or manipulated.
A reduction in the quantity of data used to describe cumulative loading on the low back substantially
increased the amount of error. The approaches that used a representative posture to quantify cumulative
loading for a few portions of a cycle resulted in substantial error (square — 70%, work/ rest — 27%,
component — 39%, work only — 35%) relative to the complete data set at 30 Hz (Figure 13.15).
(a) (b)
2500 2500
2000 2000
Compression (N)
Compression (N)
1500 1500
1000 1000
500 500
Gold standard 5 Hz
0 0
0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0 3.5 4.0 0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0 3.5 4.0
Time (s) Time (s)
(c) (d)
2500 2500
2000 2000
Compression (N)
Compression (N)
1500 1500
1000 1000
500 500
Square
Work/Rest
0 0
0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0 3.5 4.0 0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0 3.5 4.0
Time (s) Time (s)
(e) (f)
2500 2500
get lift place return
2000 2000
Compression (N)
Compression (N)
1500 1500
1000 1000
500 500
Work only Component
0 0
0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0 3.5 4.0 0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0 3.5 4.0
Time (s) Time (s)
FIGURE 13.15 A schematic representation of actual data demonstrating the six approaches used to calculate the
cumulative loading at L4/L5. (a) Gold standard — rectangular integration of all frames collected at 30 Hz. (b)
5 Hz - rectangular integration of data sampled at 5 Hz based on the methods of Kumar (1990). (c) Square —
loading at the beginning of lift multiplied by the duration of the task based on the methods of Norman et al.
(1998). (d) Work/rest — work, loading at the beginning of lift multiplied by the time the mass was in subjects
hands, and rest, loading during upright standing multiplied by remaining time of the cycle. (e) Work only — the
work component of work/rest alone. (f) Component — cycle divided into four components of the task: get load,
lift load, place load, and return. The † in (a) indicates the start point of the lifting phase. (From Callaghan et al.
(2001) Ergonomics, Taylor & Francis, 44 (9), 825 – 837. With permission.) http://www.tandf/co.uk/journals.
Cumulative Spine Loading 13-19
With errors between different calculation methods using the same data set approaching more than 100%
in some cases (Callaghan et al., 2001), it presents the problem that researchers will not be able to combine
exposure data from different studies or adopt a universal exposure TLV. Arguably, the absolute error in
cumulative load estimates might be of little concern when comparing relative exposure between two
groups (i.e., low back pain and controls). The assumption that error would be equal in the two
groups is also brought into question as error of these reduction approaches was influenced by the
type of task performed. The use of a reduced sampling rate (5 Hz approach) resulted in very small
errors across all tasks and subjects. This suggests that significant digitizing time might be saved
without compromising the accuracy of cumulative loading estimates. The sensitivity of some of the
approaches employed, in particular the square, work only and work/rest approaches, to the type of
task examined further brings into question the validity of using a single point in time to represent
cumulative exposure. Further reductions in sample rate were deemed to still produce accurate results
(Andrews and Callaghan, 2003). In fact, representing a task with kinetics calculated from data
sampled at 2 frames/sec only introduced an average of 3% error compared to kinetics at 60 frames/
sec. While this represents a great reduction in the data required to document a task, processing
this volume of data is still quite labor-intensive. The reduction of sample rate to document
exposure has a further impact on model usage. As alluded to earlier in the discussion of dynamic
models, if data is reduced to a point where it does not sufficiently represent the frame by frame worker
movement, dynamic calculations of segmental accelerations cannot be performed or they produce erro-
neous values. The use of 2 frames/sec is well below the sample rates required for even slow moving
activities.
TABLE 13.2 Kinetic Variables that can be Calculated Using Two and Three-Dimensional Rigid
Link Segment Models Partnered with a Lumbar Joint Biomechanical Model
Model Variable Type Anatomical Description
Two-dimensional (2D) Moment (Nm s) Flexion/Extension
Reaction forces (N s) Compression
Anterior/Posterior shear
Joint forces (N s) Compression
Anterior/Posterior shear
Three-dimensional (3D) Moments (Nm s) Flexion/Extension
Lateral bend
Axial twist
Reaction forces (N s) Compression
Anterior/Posterior shear
Medial/Lateral shear
Joint forces (N s) Compression
Anterior/Posterior shear
Medial/Lateral shear
models partition net joint moments into tissue forces that are anatomically and biomechanically
modeled.
While the number of variables has been listed as 5 and 9, for two- and three-dimensional models,
respectively, if the initially stated rationale for choosing a variable is considered, namely that the variable
should be linked to injury mechanics, the number of variables becomes eight for a two-dimensional
FIGURE 13.16 Sample output from 3DMatch, (Callaghan et al., 2003) which provides a frame by frame force-
time history for net joint moments (b), joint shear (c) and compression (d) for a task as well as the
cumulative exposure (a). The horizontal bars on the shear (c) and compression (d) outputs represent acute
exposure TLVs.
Cumulative Spine Loading 13-21
model and 16 for a three-dimensional model. As an example, let us consider the anterior/posterior shear
forces. When spinal motion segments are tested to failure in anterior shear, the primary site of injury is
the pars interarticularis (Yingling and McGill, 1999a) whereas when tested in posterior shear the end-
plate is the most common injury site (Yingling and McGill, 1999b). This would indicate that the vari-
ables with a directional component should have each direction accounted for or accumulated separately.
This reasoning would also hold for the net joint moments where the musculature to generate an exten-
sion moment is completely different from the musculature for a flexor moment. Unfortunately this
greatly increases the number of variables and raises the potential problem that some variables could
show little or zero cumulative exposure depending on the task demands. The calculation of this
increased number of variables is a relatively simple addition to any biomechanical model used for asses-
sing cumulative loading. Modeling approaches that use sufficient data to generate force-time histories
can provide outputs for both acute and cumulative exposure, an example is presented in Figure 13.16.
The real challenge with generating so many variables is interpreting their relationship with the devel-
opment of pain. As mentioned previously, cumulative compression has been the most commonly used
variable in published studies and is also the most used peak exposure variable in ergonomic assessments.
It also has the added benefit of being unidirectional for joint compression. This is a direct result of the
imposed joint compression associated with activating trunk musculature to generate movement in any
dimension. These factors in combination with the epidemiological evidence and widespread use would
make it the forerunner in becoming the variable of choice when setting and implementing a cumulative
exposure TLV.
13.9 Summary
Taking into account all of the potential barriers and processing difficulties associated with documenting
cumulative exposure, the fact that all of the studies that have examined the relationship between cumu-
lative loading and low back pain have found a significant positive relationship (Kumar, 1990; Norman
et al., 1998; Seidler et al., 2001, 2003) demonstrates the strength and potential of this approach. The posi-
tive association was independent of whether primary or secondary assessment methods between
exposure and developing injury/pain were used.
This chapter has focused on spinal cumulative loading exposure. This approach is not meant to replace
assessments of peak exposure. In fact, peak exposure is really just an instantaneous component of a
cumulative exposure dosage. Both peak and cumulative evaluations of work provide complimentary
but unique pieces of information. This is supported by the fact that in the calculation of multivariate
odds ratios for the reporting of low back pain, peak and cumulative exposure were independent com-
ponents in a factor analysis (Norman et al., 1998).
One of the primary barriers to the transfer of usable techniques for the assessment of cumulative
exposure to practicing ergonomists is the lack of consensus in data sets. Clearly, there is a need to deter-
mine a common method, or at the least provide an estimate of the error inherent in the approach used,
before any progress can be made towards developing a standard for assessing risk of injury from exposure
to cumulative loading. This chapter has attempted to highlight and where possible quantify the errors in
documenting cumulative loading. The knowledge presented spans from the theoretical foundations of
the link between injury and cumulative loading to applied techniques such as 3DMatch for quantifying
a worker’s exposure. There is a strong evidence base to support the implementation of cumulative
loading assessments and highlights the need for a TLV and supporting assessment techniques for practi-
cing ergonomists.
Acknowledgments
The author’s research work reported in this chapter was funded by the Natural Sciences and Engineering
Research Council of Canada, and the AUTO21 Network Centers of Excellence whose funding is provided
by the Canadian federal government.
13-22 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
The author would like to acknowledge the contribution of Dave Andrews, Ph.D. and Wayne Albert,
Ph.D., who have collaborated and shared their ideas on cumulative loading. Much of the work presented
here would not have been possible without a very dedicated group of graduate students: Diana Sullivan,
M.Sc., Kiera Keown, M.Sc. and Jennie Jackson, M.Sc. who was instrumental in the development of
3DMatch. Jack Callaghan, Robert Parkinson, M.Sc. a Canada Research Chair in Spine Biomechanics
and Injury Prevention.
References
Adams, M.A. and Hutton, W.C., Prolapsed intervertebral disc: a hyperflexion injury, Spine, 7, pp. 184 –
191, 1982.
Adams, M.A. and Hutton, W.C., The effect of fatigue on the lumbar intervertebral disc, J. Bone Joint Surg.
(B), 65, pp. 199–203, 1983.
Agnew, M.J., Andrews, D.M., and Callaghan, J.P., Using a magnetic tracking device as a means of measur-
ing cumulative spine loading, in XVI International Annual Occupational Ergonomics and Safety
Conference, University of Windsor, Toronto, 2002.
Agnew, M.J., Andrews, D.M., Potvin, J.R., and Callaghan, J.P., Dynamic 2-d measurements of cumulative
spine loading using an electromagnetic tracking device, in 34th Annual Conference of the
Association of Canadian Ergonomists, Association of Canadian Ergonomists, London, 2003.
Akesson, I., Balogh, I., and Skerfving, S., Self-reported and measured time of vibration exposure at
ultrasonic scaling in dental hygienists, Appl. Ergon., 32, pp. 47 –51, 2001.
Allread, W.G., Marras, W.S., and Burr, D.L., Measuring trunk motions in industry: variability due to task
factors, individual differences, and the amount of data collected, Ergonomics, 43, pp. 691 –701,
2000.
Andrews, D.M. and Callaghan, J.P., Determining the minimum sampling rate needed to accura-
tely quantify cumulative spine loading from digitized video, Appl. Ergon., 34, pp. 589 –
595, 2003.
Armstrong, T.J., Foulke, J.A., Joseph, B.S., and Goldstein, S.A., Investigation of cumulative trauma
disorders in a poultry processing plant, Am. Ind. Hyg. Assoc. J., 43, pp. 103 –116, 1982.
Azar, N., The relationship between cumulative low back loads and heart rate determined physical activity
level during non-occupational tasks, M.Sc. Thesis Dissertation, University of Windsor, Windsor,
ON, Canada, 2004.
Azar, N., Andrews, D.M., Callaghan, J.P., Accuracy of spine cumulative loading using self-reported dur-
ation and frequency information during non-occupational tasks, International Journal of Industrial
Ergonomics, 43(8), pp. 459–461, 2005.
Bogduk, N., The innervation of the lumbar spine, Spine, 8, pp. 286 –293, 1983.
Brinckmann, P., Biggemann, M., and Hilweg, D., Fatigue fracture of human lumbar vertebrae,
Clin. Biomech., 3 (Suppl. 1), pp. s1– s23, 1988.
Buckwalter, J.A., Spine update: aging and degeneration of the human intervertebral disc, Spine, 20,
pp. 1307–1314, 1995.
Caler, W.E. and Carter, D.R., Bone creep-fatigue damage accumulation, J. Biomech., 22, pp. 625 –635,
1989.
Callaghan, J.P., Cumulative loading as an injury mechanism: tissue considerations, in Proceedings of the
IV World Congress of Biomechanics, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada, 2002.
Callaghan, J.P., Jackson, J., Andrews, D.M., Albert, W.J., and Potvin, J.R., The design and preliminary
validation of ‘3D-Match’ — a posture matching tool for estimating three dimensional cumulative
loading on the low back, in 34th Annual Conference of the Association of Canadian Ergonomists,
Association of Canadian Ergonomists, London, 2003.
Callaghan, J.P., Keown, K., and Andrews, D.M., Influence of dynamic factors on calculating cumulative
low back loads, Occupational Ergonomics, In Press, 2005.
Cumulative Spine Loading 13-23
Callaghan, J.P. and McGill, S.M., Intervertebral disc herniation: studies on a porcine model exposed to
highly repetitive flexion/extension motion with compressive force, Clin. Biomech., 16, pp. 28 –37,
2001.
Callaghan, J.P., Salewytsch, A.J., and Andrews, D.M., An evaluation of predictive methods for estimating
cumulative spinal loading, Ergonomics, 44, pp. 825–837, 2001.
Carter, D.R. and Caler, W.E., Cycle-dependent and time-dependent bone fracture with repeated loading,
J. Biomech. Eng., 105, pp. 166 –170, 1983.
Carter, D.R. and Hayes, W.C., Fatigue life of compact bone — I. Effects of stress amplitude, temperature
and density, J. Biomech., 9, pp. 27– 34, 1976.
Cavanaugh, J.M., Kallakuri, S., and Özaktay, A.C., Innervation of the rabbit lumbar intervertebral disc
and posterior longitudinal ligament, Spine, 20, pp. 2080 –2085, 1995.
Danz, M.E. and Ayoub, M.M., The effects of speed, frequency, and load on measured hand forces for a
floor to knuckle lifting task, Ergonomics, 35, pp. 833 –843, 1992.
Daynard, D., Yassi, A., Cooper, J.E., Tate, R., Norman, R., and Wells, R., Biomechanical analysis of peak
and cumulative spinal loads during simulated patient-handling activities: a substudy of a random-
ized controlled trial to prevent lift and transfer injury of health care workers, App. Ergon., 32,
pp. 199–214, 2001.
Genaidy, A.M., Waly, S.M., Khalil, T.M., and Hidalgo, J., Spinal compression tolerance limits for the
design of manual material handling operations in the workplace, Ergonomics, 36, pp. 415– 434,
1993.
Godin, C.A., Andrews, D.M., and Callaghan, J.P., Cumulative low back loads of non-occupational tasks
using “3DMatch”, a 3-dimensional video-based posture sampling approach, in 34th Annual
Conference of the Association of Canadian Ergonomists, Association of Canadian Ergonomists,
London, 2003.
Granata, K.P., Marras, W.S., and Davis, K.G., Variation in spinal load and trunk dynamics during
repeated lifting exertions, Clin. Biomech., 14, pp. 367–375, 1999.
Gunning, J.L., Callaghan, J.P., and McGill, S.M., Spinal posture and prior loading history modulate com-
pressive strength and type of failure in the spine: a biomechanical study using a porcine cervical
spine model, Clin. Biomech., 16, pp. 471–480, 2001.
Hansson, T.H., Keller, T.S., and Spengler, D.M., Mechanical behavior of the human lumbar
spine. II. Fatigue strength during dynamic compressive loading, J. Orthop. Res., 5, pp.
479–487, 1987.
Hardy, W.G., Lissner, H.R., Webster, J.E., and Gurdjian, E.S., Repeated loading tests of the lumbar spine,
Surg. Forum, 9, pp. 690– 695, 1958.
Holm, S. and Nachemson, A., Variations in the nutrition of the canine intervertebral disc induced by
motion, Spine, 8, pp. 866–874, 1983.
Iatridis, J.C., Mente, P.L., Stokes, I.A., Aronsson, D.D., and Alini, M., Compression-induced changes in
intervertebral disc properties in a rat tail model, Spine, 24, pp. 996 –1002, 1999.
Jackson, J., Reed, B., Andrews, D.M., Albert, W.J., and Callaghan, J.P., Usability of 3D-Match — An evalu-
ation of the inter- and intra-observer reliability of posture matching to calculate cumulative low
back loading, in 34th Annual Conference of the Association of Canadian Ergonomists, Association
of Canadian Ergonomists, London, 2003.
Jager, M., Jordan, C., Luttmann, A., and Laurig, W., Evaluation and assessment of lumbar load during
total shifts for occupational manual materials handling jobs within the Dortmund Lumbar
Load Study — DOLLY, Int. J. Ind. Ergon., 25, pp. 553–571, 2000.
Keown, K.J., Andrews, D.M., and Callaghan, J.P., Predicting cumulative low back loading during
repetitive lifting: determining the number of trials needed to represent workday conditions, in
XVI International Annual Occupational Ergonomics and Safety Conference, University of
Windsor, Toronto, ON, Canada, 2002.
Kumar, S., Cumulative load as a risk factor for back pain, Spine, 15, pp. 1311–1316, 1990.
13-24 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Lauder, C.L., Andrews, D.M., and Callaghan, J.P., Peak and cumulative loading of the lumbar spine
during simulated non-work activities, in XVI International Annual Occupational Ergonomics and
Safety Conference, University of Windsor, Toronto, ON, Canada, 2002.
Lindbeck, L. and Arborelius, U.P., Inertial effects from single body segments in dynamic analysis of
lifting, Ergonomics, 34, pp. 421 –433, 1991.
Liu, Y.K., Njus, G., Buckwalter, J.A., and Wakano, K., Fatigue response of lumbar intervertebral joints
under axial cyclic loading, Spine, 8, pp. 857–865, 1983.
Lotz, J.C. and Chin, J.R., Intervertebral disc cell death is dependent on the magnitude and duration of
spinal loading, Spine, 25, pp. 1477 –1483, 2000.
Lotz, J.C., Colliou, O.K., Chin, J.R., Duncan, N.A., and Liebenberg, E., Compression-induced degener-
ation of the intervertebral disc: an in vivo mouse model and finite-element study, Spine, 23,
pp. 2493–2506, 1998.
Marras, W.S. and Granata, K.P., Changes in trunk dynamics and spine loading during repeated trunk
exertions, Spine., 22, pp. 2564 –2570, 1997.
McGill, S.M. and Norman, R.W., Dynamically and statically determined low back moments during
lifting, J. Biomech., 18, pp. 877–885, 1985.
Mientjes, M.I., Norman, R.W., Wells, R.P., and McGill, S.M., Assessment of an EMG-based method
for continuous estimates of low back compression during asymmetrical occupational tasks,
Ergonomics, 42, pp. 868–879, 1999.
Muir, P., Johnson, K.A., and Ruaux-Mason, C.P., In vivo matrix microdamage in a naturally occurring
canine fatigue fracture, Bone, 25, pp. 571 –576, 1999.
NIOSH, Work Practices Guide for Manual Lifting (Rep. No. NIOSH Technical Report No. 81–122),
National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health, Cincinnati, OH, 1981.
Norman, R.W. and McGill, S.M., Selection od 2-D and 3-D biomechanical spine models: Issues for con-
sideration by the ergonomist, In The Occupational Ergonomics Handbook (edited by Karwowski,W.
and Marras,W.S.), pp. 967 –984, CRC Press, New York, 2002.
Norman, R.W., Wells, R.P., Neumann, P., Frank, J., Shannon, H.S., Kerr, M.S., and Ontario Universities
Back Pain Study (OUBPS) Group, A comparison of peak vs cumulative physical work exposure
risk factors for the reporting of low back pain in the automotive industry, Clin. Biomech., 13,
pp. 561–573, 1998.
Parkinson, R. and Callaghan, J.P., The role of load magnitude as a modifier of the cumulative load
tolerance of porcine cervical spinal units: Progress towards a force weighting approach, Theoretical
Issues In Ergonomics Science, In Press, 2005.
Porter, R.W., Adams, M.A., and Hutton, W.C., Physical activity and strength of the lumbar spine, Spine,
14, pp. 201–203, 1989.
Potvin, J.R., Norman, R.W., and Wells, R., A field method for continuous estimation of dynamic
compressive forces on the L4/L5 disc during the performance of repetitive tasks, in 23rd Annual
Conference of the Human Factors Associations of Canada Ottawa, Ontario, Human Factors
Association of Canada, Ottawa, ON, Canada, pp. 51 –55, 1990.
Seidler, A., Bolm-Audorff, U., Heiskel, H., Henkel, N., Roth-Kuver, B., Kaiser, U., Bickeboller, R.,
Willingstorfer, W.J., Beck, W., and Elsner, G., The role of cumulative physical work load in
lumbar spine disease: risk factors for lumbar osteochondrosis and spondylosis associated with
chronic complaints, Occup. Environ. Med., 58, pp. 735–746, 2001.
Seidler, A., Bolm-Audorff, U., Siol, T., Henkel, N., Fuchs, C., Schug, H., Leheta, F., Marquardt, G.,
Schmitt, E., Ulrich, P.T., Beck, W., Missalla, A., and Elsner, G., Occupational risk factors
for symptomatic lumbar disc herniation; a case-control study, Occup. Environ. Med., 60,
pp. 821–830, 2003.
Stramler, J.H., The Dictionary for Human Factors and Ergonomics, CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL, 1993.
Urban, J.P.G., Holm, S., and Maroudas, A., Diffusion of small solutes into the intervertebral disc: an
in vivo study, Biorheology, 15, pp. 203 –223, 1978.
Cumulative Spine Loading 13-25
Videman, T., Nurminen, M., and Troup, J.D.G., Lumbar spinal pathology in cadaveric material in
relation to history of back pain, occupation, and physical loading, Spine, 15, pp. 728–740, 1990.
Wada, E., Ebara, S., Saito, S., and Ono, K., Experimental spondylosis in the rabbit spine. Overuse could
accelerate the spondylosis, Spine, 17, pp. S1–S6, 1992.
Wang, X.T., Ker, R.F., and Alexander, R.M., Fatigue rupture of wallaby tail tendons, J. Exp. Biol., 198,
pp. 847–852, 1995.
Waters, T.R., Putz-Anderson, V., Garg, A., and Fine, L.J., Revised NIOSH equation for the design and
evaluation of manual lifting tasks, Ergonomics, 36, pp. 749–776, 1993.
Wren, T.A., Lindsey, D.P., Beaupre, G.S., and Carter, D.R., Effects of creep and cyclic loading on the mech-
anical properties and failure of human Achilles tendons, Ann. Biomed. Eng., 31, pp. 710–717, 2003.
Yingling, V.R., Callaghan, J.P., and McGill, S.M., Dynamic loading affects the mechanical properties and
failure site of porcine spines, Clin. Biomech., 15, pp. 301 –305, 1997.
Yingling, V.R. and McGill, S.M., Anterior shear of spinal motion segments. Kinematics, kinetics, and
resultant injuries observed in a porcine model, Spine, 24, pp. 1882 –1889, 1999a.
Yingling, V.R. and McGill, S.M., Mechanical properties and failure mechanics of the spine under pos-
terior shear load: observations from a porcine model, J. Spinal Disord., 12, pp. 501 –508, 1999b.
Yu, C.Y., Tsai, K.H., Hu, W.P., Lin, R.M., Song, H.W., and Chang, G.L., Geometric and morphological
changes of the intervertebral disc under fatigue testing, Clin. Biomech., 18, pp. S3 –S9, 2003.
Zioupos, P., Currey, J.D., and Casinos, A., Tensile fatigue in bone: are cycles-, or time to failure, or both,
important? J. Theor. Biol., 210, pp. 389– 399, 2001.
Zioupos, P., Wang, X.T., and Currey, J.D., Experimental and theoretical quantification of the develop-
ment of damage in fatigue tests of bone and antler, J. Biomech., 29, pp. 989 –1002, 1996a.
Zioupos, P., Wang, X.T., and Currey, J.D., The accumulation of fatigue microdamage in human cortical
bone of two different ages in vitro, Clin. Biomech., 11, pp. 365 –375, 1996b.
14
Low-Level Static
Exertions
14-1
14-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
TABLE 14.1 Electromyographic Data on Static (P ¼ 0.1), Mean (P ¼ 0.5), and Peak (P ¼ 0.9) Muscle Load in the
Shoulder Region During Different Work Tasks Expressed in Percentage of Maximal Electromyographic Activity or
Percentage of Maximal Voluntary Force Development (%MAX)
Job Muscles P ¼ 0.1 (%MAX) P ¼ 0.5 (%MAX) P ¼ 0.9 (%MAX) References
Typewriting m. trapezius 4 7 10 8
Office work m. trapezius 1 4 — 6,9
Computer mouse work m. trapezius (r) 1/2 1/3 2/5 10
(young/elderly) m. trapezius (l) 1/2 1/3 3/7
m. deltoideus 1/2 2/3 3/5
m. neck extensors 2/3 4/5 6/7
CAD work (mouse side/ m. trapezius 2/1 5/2 9/6 11,12
other side)
Industrial sewing m. trapezius (r) 9 14 21 13
m. trapezius (l) 9 16 25
m. infraspinatus 4 9 20
Floor cleaning m. trapezius 10 25 54 14
Assembly plantelectronic m. trapezius 8 16 27 15
work m. deltoideus 7 13 28
m. infraspinatus 13 20 33
Meat cutting m. trapezius 6 10 17 16
Dental work m. trapezius 9 13 18 17
Flight loading/unloading m. trapezius 5 14 45 18
Letter sorting m. trapezius 5 10 27 19
m. deltoideus 5 14 19
m. infraspinatus 5 10 16
Industrial production work m. trapezius 1 5 15 11
Chocolate manufacturing m. trapezius 2 5 — 6
work
TABLE 14.2 One-Year Prevalence of Musculoskeletal Symptoms in Neck, Shoulder, Elbow/Forearm, and Hand/
Wrist According to the Standardized Nordic Questionnaire23
Number of Elbow/ Hand/Wrist
Job Sex Workers Neck (%) Shoulder (%) Forearm (%) (%) References
Office work F 643 48 48 12 22 24
M 35 18 18 6 9
Computer work F 1745 53 42 — 30 25
M 834 27 23 — 19
CAD work F 106 79 52 41 55 12
M 43 68 49 20 30
Sewing machine F 77 55 51 7 26 26
operators F 303 57 53 7 28
Cleaners F 737 63 63 27 46 27
Assembly plant FþM 25 64 56 — — 15
electronic work
Meat cutting F 16 67 54 7 47 24
M 114 39 62 15 47
Meat cutting M, 8% F 2463 52 66 28 60 16
Dental work F 43 49 40 19 40 17
M 56 57 39 13 5
Flight loading M 808 30 31 14 22 18
Source: Kuorinka, I., et al. Appl. Ergon. 18, pp. 233–234, 1987.
Low-Level Static Exertions 14-3
be causally related to the identified risk aspect. The term “low-level static exertions” will be discussed, fol-
lowed by a presentation of possible short- and long-term physiological responses. Based on this, preven-
tive strategies are presented.
breaking point
Force
tolerance limit
acute
FIGURE 14.1 Tolerance limit for maximum force at breaking point depends on the type of muscle, for example,
cross-sectional area, state of training, and age. Further, the contraction mode, that is, static or dynamic, is
significant. For submaximal forces, the tolerance limit decreases with time, the rate of decrease depending on
force magnitude and repetition frequency.
of time before being exhausted. Different muscles show highly different endurance capacity depending on
muscle fiber type, anatomy, and state of training. But for every muscle there is a limit.
Third, in industry many low-level exertions include repeated static exertions or movements at quite
high speed but with little displacement. When observing such tasks, often little attention is paid to
the displacement, which is the cause for such exertions to be assessed as static. Also for intermittent
static as well as dynamic contractions, endurance time curves exist.2,20 It is for the dynamic contractions
(a) (b)
8 800
Endurance time (hours)
6 600
Velocity (%S)
4 400
2 200
x x
20 40 60 80 100 20 40 60 80 100
Force (% MVC) Force (% MVC)
FIGURE 14.2 (a) Shows an endurance time curve for static contractions, which are defined as contractions at
constant muscle length. Of note is the large range of endurance time at low forces, where the end-point of
exhaustion varies significantly, for example, due to the level of motivation. Examples are given for handgrip (D),
shoulder abduction (x), and trunk extension (†). (b) Lower part shows a force–velocity curve for dynamic
contractions, only shown for concentric contractions, that is, during muscle shortening. The maximum muscle
force decreases with increasing velocity of shortening. Examples are given for shoulder movements during floor
cleaning (†), shoulder movements during forking in agriculture (P), and wrist movements during meat cutting
(x). For the latter, it is seen that although the load is only 10% MVC, it is about 20% of the dynamic strength,
since the maximal dynamic strength at this velocity is approximately 50% MVC.
Low-Level Static Exertions 14-5
that the contraction level cannot be described only by the force in N or percent MVC. The force –velocity
relationship must also be taken into account, the relative load being higher when a specific force is devel-
oped with increasing speed (Figure 14.2). For instance, keyboard operators may press 200,000 keys a day
or 500 per minute and a piano player strikes the tangents with finger movements at very high, maybe
sometimes maximum, speed. For the unloaded limb, the EMG activity increases linearly with the velocity
of the movement.21 If, in addition to the movement velocity, there is an external force to overcome
during the work tasks, this could imply maximum effort at high velocities even if the force level is
low. Thus, low-level cannot be assessed only in terms of percent MVC, but the mode of contractions
must also be taken into account. Ideally, the maximum dynamic voluntary contraction forces should
be assessed and the work task evaluated in relation to the corresponding maximum force–velocity
relationship.
In short, the term “low-level” in the context of work-related static exertions refers to a working con-
dition in which a muscle is activated at a level that can be maintained for a long period. This may be a
true static contraction, sustaining a constant force and posture or varying in force within a limited range
and without any movement. But even performing intermittent static or dynamic contractions (con-
centric/eccentric) at submaximal force velocities with small displacements and at intensities that can
be maintained for a long time may be considered low-level static exertions in occupational settings.22
Actually, when such exertions are measured by electromyography and analyzed by the previously men-
tioned APDF of the EMG, “static” levels of 5% MVC or more may be found. This means that a low-level
static exertion is to be considered in the time domain and is characterized by workers being able to
perform it for hours. The main feature is that the exertion is sufficiently low so that it can be sustained
for a prolonged time and the duration probably implies the risk.
sufficient recovery periods are allowed during such work tasks. A more informative term for the related
risk factor would be prolonged sustained or repeated muscle contractions. According to the endurance
curve, it is possible to sustain low-level exertions for a longer time than high-level exertions. It is
likely, that this time factor is the risk. This hypothesis is supported by the physiological responses to
such exertions, which constitute the plausibility. Standardized muscle contractions have been studied
in combination with detailed physiological responses. In the following discussion, focus will be on mech-
anisms, which may induce muscle damage.
An example of a standardized setup for studying muscle contractions is shown in Figure 14.3. The test
chair can be regulated for the subject to adopt any working posture and the force transducers connected
to the handles allow for three-dimensional recordings. During specific work tasks, biomechanical calcu-
lations may then assess the relative load on various muscles or muscle parts/groups based on maximum
contractions performed in identical postures and directions.33
Intramuscular Pressure and Blood Flow With each muscle contraction, the tissue pressure (hydrostatic
pressure) in the muscle increases in proportion to the force development. The absolute level in terms
FIGURE 14.3 Experimental chair, where arm posture can be adjusted in any position of abduction (a) and flexion
(b). The hands are grasping handles connected to three-dimensional force transducers (c). Professor Bjørn Quistorff,
University of Copenhagen, is acknowledged for the design.
Low-Level Static Exertions 14-7
(a) (b)
0.8
IMP 0.6
300
0.4
200
MABP
0.2
100
20 40 60 80 100 20 40 60 80 100
FIGURE 14.4 (a) Shows mean arterial blood pressure (MABP) and intramuscular pressure (IMP) with increasing
contraction force. (b) Shows corresponding blood flow.
of mmHg varies widely between muscles and depends, among other things on the anatomy of the muscle
itself as well as its surroundings. A bulky muscle attains higher pressures than a thin muscle, and a muscle
with bony surroundings or tight fascia shows relatively large increases because of the low compliance of
these surroundings. At high contraction forces, the intramuscular pressure may attain values far above
blood pressure (Figure 14.4) and obviously cause muscle blood flow to be occluded in areas where intra-
muscular pressure exceeds blood pressure, the highest pressures normally occurring deep in the muscles.
However, even at low-level contractions, the complex microcirculatory regulation may become impeded.
First of all, at low blood flow velocities it is not the mean blood pressure but the diastolic pressure that is
decisive for maintenance of blood flow.34 Further, with prolonged contractions, the muscle water content
will increase35 and correspondingly, the thickness of the muscle has been shown to increase.36 Such a state
of edematic tissue with increased volume will per se increase tissue pressure in a deliminated closed
muscle compartment with low compliance. At contraction levels in the order of 5 to 10% MVC, intra-
muscular pressures of 40 to 60 mmHg or more have been reported in muscles such as the m. supraspi-
natus in the shoulder.37,38 Of note is that intramuscular pressure may increase to higher levels when
dynamic contractions are performed at a force corresponding to the static contractions.39 However, it
may rather be the duration of increased intramuscular pressure than the absolute level that may be dele-
terious for the muscle and therefore static and not dynamic muscle contractions constituting a risk
factor. Thus, causal relationships between prolonged moderately increased tissue pressure and patho-
genic changes have been studied extensively in relation to compartment syndromes.40 Pressures above
30 mmHg maintained for 8 h have been shown to induce necrotic changes in the muscle even if no
active contraction was performed and energy demand therefore was minimal.41 One possible mechanism
is that although initially blood flow is sufficient during low-level contractions, this may not be the case
when the contraction is maintained for prolonged periods. Conditions with low flow and low perfusion
pressure may provoke granulocyte plugging in the capillaries, which effects microcirculation and may
also facilitate formation of free radicals, which have a highly toxic effect.42,43 Not only muscle tissue
but also the peripheral nerves are sensitive to prolonged or repetitive elevated mechanical pressure. Dys-
function of the sensory perception indicating entrapment of n. medianus and n. ulnaris has been found
among computer users with pain in the hand/wrist and forearm/elbow regions.44
Metabolism Adequate muscle blood flow is essential for muscle function because force development
relies on the conversion of chemically bound energy to mechanical energy, a process also called
energy turnover or metabolism. Some chemically bound energy or substrate is located in the muscle
tissue (especially glycogen), but these may become depleted during prolonged activities. Therefore,
14-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
the supply of substrates (including oxygen) to the muscle is crucial for such activities. A decrease in
muscle oxygenation in response to a certain exertion indicates an acute imbalance between oxygen
supply and oxygen demand. Oxygenation of the muscle is significantly reduced at an intramuscular
pressure of 30–40 mmHg45 and a 7% or greater reduction in muscle oxygenation relative to resting
values is related to a reduced force generating capacity of the muscle.46 The ultimate substrate in the con-
version of chemical energy to mechanical energy is ATP, which is broken down in the myofibrils during
the actin –myosin reaction. ATP is significant for the detachment of actin and myosin, and insufficiency
of this process may cause rigor or contracture with massive pain. In normal muscle contractions, the
actin –myosin reaction is initiated by the release of Ca2þ from the sarcoplasmic reticulum into the
cytosol and has been the focus in a number of studies on muscle fatigue. However, during the last
decade, attention has been drawn also to the pathogenesis of Ca2þ-induced damage of muscle
cells.47,48 The reuptake of Ca2þ into the sarcoplasmic reticulum is an ATP-dependent process, which
may be insufficient during prolonged activity since it accounts for upto 30% of the energy turnover
during muscle activity. Further, energy crisis may result in an influx of Ca2þ from the extracellular
space. Consequently, the cytosolic-free Ca2þ is likely to be increased above normal for a prolonged
time. This has serious implications for the phospholipids, including those in the muscle membrane.
Ca2þ has a direct effect on phospholipase activity and, in addition, increases the susceptibility of the
membrane lipids to free radicals, which have a highly toxic effect as mentioned earlier. Both these pro-
cesses promote breakdown of the muscle membrane.49 Finally, prolonged increased cytosolic Ca2þ con-
centration induces a Ca2þ load on the mitochondria and may eventually impair ATP formation, a
sufficient concentration of which is a prerequisite for active force production (for more details
see Ref. 50).
Motor Control Another important aspect during low force development is that although the muscle as a
whole may not be metabolically exhausted, this may well be the case for single-muscle fibers. The muscles
are composed of different muscle fiber types and motor units with different recruitment thresholds. A
stereotype recruitment order has been documented, which means that with increasing force, the low
threshold motor units are always being recruited first.51 Within a motor unit pool, various motor
units may be alternating in activity pattern during a submaximal muscle contraction postponing
fatigue to develop in each of the involved fibers.34 However, performing highly skilled movements
and accurate manipulations, it is likely that the very same motor units are being recruited continuously.
This holds true for pure static as well as slow force-varying and low-velocity dynamic contractions.52,53
Additionally, contractions may be elicited due to reflexes, causing even more stereotype recruitment than
during voluntary contractions. Mental load has been demonstrated to generate nonpostural muscle
tension in shoulder and forearm muscles,54 – 60 and the same holds true for visual demands and neck
muscles and even during breaks, for example, in computer work, muscle activity may stay above
resting value.61 Also reflexes originating in the muscle itself from chemo- as well as mechanoreceptors
may play a role and recently the gamma-loop has been proposed to play a role in developing a potentially
vicious circle.62,63 The muscle fibers being continuously activated have been termed Cinderella fibers,
because they are working from early to late.64 A high energy turnover occurs in these fibers and most
likely, they receive the least blood flow because tissue pressure increases in their vicinity due to the mech-
anical contraction impeding blood flow.34 The pathogenic mechanisms described above regarding
accumulation of Ca2þ and free radicals may be a concern, especially at the single-muscle fiber level. Pro-
longed activity of specific motor units throughout an 8-h working day may cause insufficient time for full
recovery of these motor units due to a long-lasting element of fatigue,65 which has been shown to occur
in simulated occupational settings.66 This may cause necrosis and, finally, cell destruction in these fibers.
In line with this, fibers with marked degenerative characteristics have been found more frequently in
muscle biopsies from patients with work-related chronic myalgia than in normal subjects in the trapezius
muscle.67 Interestingly, the degenerative fibers identified are slow twitch fibers, which connect with low-
threshold motor nerves.68
Perception of Fatigue When muscular work is performed over a prolonged period, fatigue develops.
Fatigue may cause the work to be performed with less care or precision and an accident can result. A
Low-Level Static Exertions 14-9
fatigued worker is more likely to make a wrong movement, such as a slip and fall, leading to injury.
However, even if an accident does not occur, prolonged fatigue without adequate time for recovery
can lead to the development of musculoskeletal disorders. From the beginning of every muscle activity,
the muscle is fatiguing and muscle function is decreasing.69 This condition is normally perceived as
muscle fatigue. The perception of fatigue is a very useful mechanism for protecting the muscle against
overload. Among other factors, the work-induced increase in potassium concentration in the interstitial
space can help mediate the perception of fatigue to the central nervous system (CNS).70 However, during
very low-level contractions, the accumulated increase in interstitial potassium may be subliminal to the
threshold of the sensory afferents mediating the information to the CNS.71 Also, in situations of
machine-controlled work or heavy work pressure, the fatigue message is depressed, when it is not possible
for the employee to take a rest. In other words, fatigue is ignored — consciously or subconsciously —
which in the long-term can have serious consequences.
The processes that take place in relation to fatigue are normally reversible for biological tissues, which
is in contrast to inert tissues and a reason why we normally do not consider fatigue as dangerous. This
means that muscles recover when resting after exertion. A rest period following muscular activity is there-
fore essential to enable the muscle to recover its full functional potential with regard to strength and
endurance. Even an improvement or training effect of these variables may be obtained if optimal per-
formance of activity and recovery periods is planned. There is no simple time equation for length of
work and adequate length of a subsequent resting period. The process of recovery depends on the
type of work that caused the muscle fatigue. For instance, the so-called low-frequency fatigue and
high-frequency fatigue are caused by fundamentally different biochemical changes in the muscle.71 If
fatigue is due to relatively high loads over a short time, the necessary recovery will be quicker than if
fatigue is due to prolonged working at low-load levels. Thus, if the same muscle group or group of
fibers are activated continuously for a full working day of 7 or 8 h, there is a risk that the muscle will
not even be fully recovered by the next day. If such conditions persist for months or even years, they
can ultimately inflict irreversible or chronic changes that may result in pain and impaired function.
Muscle function
training
injury
FIGURE 14.5 Muscle function in relation to work and recovery (duty circle).
we need guidelines for maximum acceptable time limits for prolonged sustained or repeated muscle con-
tractions. This means that we must focus our attention on how long a time a muscle or group of muscles
can tolerate maintaining or repeating the same low-level exertions. This is especially true when these
same low-level exertions are imposed on a muscle day after day.
1. The workplace must allow for variation in working postures. This means, for example, that table
and chair are easily adjustable. For instance, shifting frequently between sitting and standing is
recommended and instructions should be given to implement adjustments at frequent time
intervals.
2. The workplace must be designed based on principles of optimization and not minimization of
mechanical workload. Therefore, it is recommended that work cycles include loads ranging from
complete relaxation to moderately high contraction forces and velocities. Workers should be
given the possibility to optimize the phases of a duty cycle according to their capacity. It is of import-
ance that the level or intensity of exertion changes over a wide range continuously over time.
3. The job profile must allow for performing a variety of different tasks. The task variation should
include variation regarding mental load as well as physical (mechanical) load on the musculoske-
letal system. If only specialized work tasks can be performed at each workstation, a job must
include tasks at different workstations. For the use of tools, it is recommended that a variety of
tools with different designs are used interchangeably. In combination, these variations should
cause loading of different body regions and muscle groups regularly over time.
References
1. Rohmert, W. Problems of determination of rest allowances, App. Ergon., 4(3), pp. 158–162, 1973.
2. Björkstén, M. and Jonsson, B. Endurance limit of force in long-term intermittent static contractions,
Scand. J. Work. Environ. Health, 3, pp. 23 –27, 1977.
3. Jonsson, B. Kinesiology. With special reference to electromyographic kinesiology, in Contemporary
Clinical Neurophysiology., Cobb, W.A. and Van Duijn H., Eds., Elsevier Scientific Publishing
Company, Amsterdam, pp. 417 –428, 1978.
4. Jonsson, B. The static load component in muscle work, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 57, pp. 305–310, 1988.
5. Veiersted, K.B., Westgaard, R.H., and Andersen, P. Pattern of muscle activity during stereotyped
work and its relation to muscle pain, Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health., 62, pp. 31 –41, 1990.
Low-Level Static Exertions 14-11
6. Jensen, C., Nilsen, K., and Hansen, K., et al., Trapezius muscle load as a risk indicator for occu-
pational shoulder-neck complaints, Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health., 64, pp. 415 –423, 1993.
7. Wærsted, M. and Westgaard, R.H., Working hours as a risk factor in the development of musculos-
keletal complaints, Ergonomics, 34(3), pp. 265 –276, 1991.
8. Björkstén, M., Itani, T., and Jonsson, B. et al. Evaluation of muscular load in shoulder and forearm
muscles among medical secretaries during occupational typing and some nonoccupational activities,
in: Biomechanics X-A, Jonsson, B., Ed., pp. 35–39, 1987.
9. Blangsted, A.K., Hansen, K., and Jensen, C. Muscle activity during computer-based office work in
relation to self-reported job demands and gender, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 89, pp. 352 –358, 2003.
10. Laursen, B., Jensen, B.R., and Ratkevicius, A. Performance and muscle activity during computer
mouse tasks in young and elderly adults, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 84, pp. 329 –336, 2001.
11. Jensen, C., Finsen, L., and Hansen, K. et al. Upper trapezius muscle activity patterns during
repetitive manual material handling and work with a computer mouse, J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol.,
9, pp. 317 –325, 1999.
12. Jensen, C., Borg, V., and Finsen, L. et al. Job demands, muscle activity and musculoskeletal
symptoms in relation to work with the computer mouse, Scand. J. Work Environ. Health., 24,
pp. 418 –424, 1998.
13. Jensen, B.R., Schibye, B., and Søgaard, K. et al. Shoulder muscle load and muscle fatigue among
industrial sewing-machine operators, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 67, pp. 467–475, 1993.
14. Søgaard, K., Fallentin, N., and Nielsen, J. Work load during floor cleaning, The effect of cleaning
methods and work technique, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 73, pp. 73 –81, 1996.
15. Christensen, H. Muscle activity and fatigue in the shoulder muscles of assembly-plant employees,
Scand. J. Work Environ. Health., 12, pp. 582 –587, 1986.
16. Udbeningsarbejde i svineslagterier, 1 ed. København: Arbejdsmiljøinstituttet, 1996.
17. Finsen, L., Christensen, H., and Bakke, M. Musculoskeletal disorders among dentists and variation in
the dental work, Appl. Ergon., 29, pp. 119–125, 1998.
18. Jørgensen, K., Jensen, B., and Stokholm, J. Postural strain and discomfort during loading and
unloading flight. An ergonomic intervention study, in Trends in Ergonomics/Human factors IV,
Asfour, S.S., Ed. Elsevier Science Publishers B.V., North-Holland, pp. 663– 673, 1987.
19. Jørgensen, K., Fallentin, N., and Sidenius, B. The strain on the shoulder and neck muscles during
letter sorting, Int J Ind Erg., 3, pp. 243 –248, 1989.
20. Sjøgaard, G., Sejersted, O.M., and Winkel, J. et al. Exposure assessment and mechanisms of patho-
genesis in work-related musculoskeletal disorders: Significant aspects in the documentation of risk
factors, in Work and health, Scientific Basis of Progress in the Working Environment, Svane, O. and
Johansen, C., Ed. European Commission, Directorate-General V, Luxembourg, 1995, pp. 75 –87.
21. Carpentier, A., Duchateau, J., and Hainaut, K. Velocity-dependent muscle strategy during plantar-
flexion in humans, J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol., 6, pp. 225 –233, 1996.
22. Jørgensen, K., Fallentin, N., and Krogh-Lund, C., et al. Electromyography and fatigue during
prolonged, low-level static contractions, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 57, pp. 316 –321, 1988.
23. Kuorinka, I., Jonsson, B., and Kilbom, Å. et al. Standardised Nordic questionnaires for the analysis of
musculoskeletal symptoms, Appl. Ergon., 18, pp. 233 –237, 1987.
24. Ydreborg, B., Bryngelsson, I.-L., and Gustafsson, C. Referensdata till Örebroformulären FHV 001 D
(200 D), 002 D (202 D), 003 D, 004 D och 007 D, Data från 95 yrkesgrupper insamlade åren 1984 –
1989. Örebro: Stiftelsen för yrkes- och miljömedicinsk forskning och utveckling i Örebro, 1989.
25. Jensen, C., Finsen, L., and Søgaard, K. et al. Musculoskeletal symptoms and duration of computer
and mouse use, Int. J. Ind. Ergon., 30, pp. 265– 275, 2002.
26. Schibye, B., Skov, T., and Ekner, D. et al. Musculoskeletal symptoms among sewing machine
operators, Scand. J. Work Environ Health., 21, pp. 427 –434, 1995.
27. Nielsen, J., Occupational health among cleaners. (In Danish with English summary.) Ph.D. thesis,
University of Copenhagen, National Institute of Occupational Health, Copenhagen, 1995.
14-12 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
28. Armstrong, T.J., Buckle, P., and Fine, L.J., et al. A conceptual model for work-related neck and upper-
limb musculoskeletal disorders, Scand. J. Work Environ. Health., 19, pp. 73 –84, 1993.
29. Sjøgaard, G., Lundberg, U., and Kadefors, R. The role of muscle activity and mental load in the
development of pain and degenerative processes at the muscle cell level during computer work,
Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 83, pp. 99–105, 2000.
30. Kunita, K. and Fujiwara, K. Relationship between reaction time of eye movement and activity of the
neck extensors, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 74, pp. 553–557, 1996.
31. Laursen, B., Jensen, B.R., and Sjøgaard, G. Effect of speed and precision demands on human shoulder
muscle electromyography during a repetitive task, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 78, pp. 544 –548, 1998.
32. Birch, L., Juul-Kristensen, B., and Jensen, C. et al. Acute response to precision, time pressure and
mental demand during simulated computer work, Scand. J. Work Environ. Health., 26,
pp. 299 –305, 2000.
33. Laursen, B., Jensen, B.R., and Németh, G., et al. A model predicting individual shoulder muscle
forces based on relationship between EMG and 3D external forces in static position, J. Biomech.,
31, pp. 731 –739, 1998.
34. Sjøgaard, G., Kiens B., and Jørgensen K., et al. Intramuscular pressure, EMG and blood flow during
low-level prolonged static contraction in man, Acta. Physiol. Scand., 128, pp. 475 –484, 1986.
35. Sjøgaard, G. Muscle energy metabolism and electrolyte shifts during low-level prolonged static con-
traction in man, Acta. Physiol. Scand., 134, pp. 181 –187, 1988.
36. Jensen, B.R., Jørgensen, K., and Sjøgaard, G. The effect of prolonged isometric contractions on
muscle fluid balance, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 69, pp. 439 –444, 1994.
37. Jensen, B.R., Jørgensen, K., and Huijing, P.A., et al. Soft tissue architecture and intramuscular
pressure in the shoulder region, Eur. J. Morphol., 33, pp. 205–220, 1995.
38. Järvholm, U., Palmerud, G., and Herberts, P., et al. Intramuscular pressure and electromyography in
the supraspinatus muscle at shoulder abduction, Clin. Orthop., 245, pp. 102 –109, 1989.
39. Sjøgaard, G., Jensen, B.R., and Hargens, A.R., et al. Intramuscular pressure and EMG relate during
static contractions but dissociate with movement and fatigue, J. Appl. Physiol., 96, pp. 1522 –1529,
2004.
40. Pedowitz, R.A., Hargens, A.R., and Mubarak, S.J. et al. Modified criteria for the objective diagnosis of
chronic compartment syndrome of the leg, Am. J. Sports Med., 18(1), pp. 35 –40, 1990.
41. Hargens, A.R., Schmidt, D.A., and Evans, K.L. et al. Quantitation of skeletal-muscle necrosis in a
model compartment syndrome, Bone Joint Surg. (Am)., 63-A, pp. 631 –636, 1981.
42. Schmid-Schönbein, G.W. Capillary plugging by granulocytes and the no-reflow phenomenon in the
microcirculation, Fed. Proc., 46(7), pp. 2397–2401, 1987.
43. Jensen, B.R., Sjøgaard, G., and Bornmyr, S., et al. Intramuscular laser-Doppler flowmetry in the
supraspinatus muscle during isometric contractions, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 71, pp. 373 –378, 1995.
44. Jensen, B.R., Pilegaard, M., and Momsen, A. Vibrotactile sense and mechanical functional state of the
arm and hand among computer users compared with a control group, Int. Arch. Occup. Environ.
Health., 75, pp. 332–340, 2002.
45. Jensen, B.R., Jørgensen, K., and Hargens, A., et al. Physiological response to submaximal isometric
contractions of the paravertebral muscles, Spine, 24, pp. 2332–2338, 1999.
46. Murthy, G., Hargens, A.R., and Lehman, S., et al. Ischemia causes muscle fatigue, J. Orthop. Res., 19,
pp. 436 –440, 2001.
47. Jackson, M.J., Jones, D.A., and Edwards, R.H.T. Experimental skeletal muscle damage: The nature of
the calcium-activated degenerative processes, Eur. J. Clin. Invest., 14, pp. 369 –374, 1984.
48. Gissel, H. Ca2þ accumulation and cell damage in skeletal muscle during low frequency stimulation,
Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 83, pp. 175–180, 2000.
49. Das, D.K. and Essman, W.B. Oxygen Radicals: Systemic Events and Disease Processes, Karger, 1990.
50. Sjøgaard, G. and Jensen, B.R. Muscle pathology with overuse, in Chronic musculoskeletal injuries in
the workplace, Ranney, D., Ed., W.B. Saunders Company, Philadelphia, 1997, pp. 17–40.
Low-Level Static Exertions 14-13
51. Henneman, E. and Olson, C.B. Relations between structure and function in the design of skeletal
muscles, J. Neurophysiol., 28, pp. 581–598, 1965.
52. Søgaard, K., Christensen, H. and Jensen, B.R., et al. Motor control and kinetics during low level con-
centric and eccentric contractions in man, Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol., 101, pp. 453 –460, 1996.
53. Christensen, H., Søgaard, K. and Jensen, B.R., et al. Intramuscular and surface EMG power spectrum
from dynamic and static contractions, J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol., 5, pp. 27– 36, 1995.
54. Westgaard, R.H. and Bjørklund. R. Generation of muscle tension additional to postural muscle load,
Ergonomics, 30(6), pp. 911 –923, 1987.
55. Wærsted, M. and Westgaard, R.H. Attention-related muscle activity in different body regions during
VDU work with minimal physical activity, Ergonomics, 39, pp. 661–676, 1996.
56. Wærsted, M., Eken, T. and Westgaard, R.H. Activity of single motor unit in attention-demanding
tasks: firing pattern in the human trapezius muscle, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 72, pp. 323 –329, 1996.
57. Finsen, L., Jensen, C. and Søgaard, K., et al. Muscle activity and cardiovascular response during com-
puter mouse work with and without memory demands, Ergonomics, 44, pp. 1312–1329, 2001.
58. Finsen, L., Søgaard, K. and Christensen, H. Influence of memory demand and contra lateral activity
on muscle activity, J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol., 11, pp. 373 –380, 2001.
59. Laursen, B., Jensen, B.R. and Garde, A.H., et al. Effect of mental and physical demands on muscular
activity during the use of a computer mouse and a keyboard, Scand. J. Work Environ. Health., 28,
pp. 215–221, 2002.
60. Søgaard, K., Sjøgaard, G. and Finsen, L., et al. Motor unit activity during stereotyped finger tasks and
computer mouse work, J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol., 11, pp. 197–206, 2001.
61. Blangsted, A.K., Søgaard, K. and Christensen, H., et al. The effect of physical and psychosocial loads
on the trapezius muscle activity during computer keying tasks and rest periods, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol.,
91, pp. 253 –258, 2004.
62. Johansson, H. and Sojka, P. Pathophysiological mechanisms involved in genesis and spread of
muscular tension in occupational muscle pain and in chronic musculoskeletal pain syndromes: a
hypothesis, Med. Hypotheses., 35, pp. 196– 203, 1991.
63. Mense, S. Considerations concerning the neurobiological basis of muscle pain, Can. J. Physiol.
Pharmacol., 69, pp. 610 –616, 1991.
64. Hägg, G.M. Static work loads and occupational myalgia — a new explanation model, in Electromyo-
graphical Kinesiology, Anderson, P.A., Hobart, D.J., Danoff, J.V., Eds., Amsterdam, Elsevier Science
Publishers B.V., 1991, pp. 141–144.
65. Edwards, R.H.T., Hill, D.K., and Jones, D.A., et al. Fatigue of long duration in human skeletal muscle
after exercise, J. Physiol. (Lond)., 272, pp. 769–778, 1977.
66. Byström, S. Physiological response and acceptability of isometric intermittent handgrip contractions,
Arbete. och. Hälsa., 38, pp. 1 –108, 1991.
67. Larsson, S.-E., Bengtsson, A., and Bodegård, L. et al. Muscle changes in work-related chronic
myalgia, Acta. Orthop. Scand., 59(5), pp. 552–556, 1988.
68. Henriksson, K.G. Muscle pain in neuromuscular disorders and primary fibromyalgia, Eur. J. Appl.
Physiol., 57, pp. 348–352, 1988.
69. Bigland-Ritchie, B., Cafarelli, E., and Vøllestad, N.K. Fatigue of submaximal static contractions, Acta.
Physiol. Scand., 128 (Suppl 556), pp. 137 –148, 1986.
70. Sjøgaard, G. Exercise-induced muscle fatigue: The significance of potassium, Acta. Physiol. Scand.,
140(suppl.593), pp. 1 –64, 1990.
71. Sjøgaard, G. Potassium and fatigue: the pros and cons, Acta Physiol Scand., 156, pp. 257 –264, 1996.
72. Sjøgaard, G. Intramuscular changes during long-term contraction, in The Ergonomics of Working Pos-
tures: Models, Methods and Cases, Corlett, N., Wilson, J., and Manenica, I., Eds. Taylor & Francis,
London and Philadelphia, 1986, pp. 136–143.
73. Kadefors, R., Christensen, H., and Sjøgaard, G., et al. PROCID Recommendations for healthier
computer work, Work & Stress., 16, pp. 91 –93, 2004.
15
Soft-Tissue
Pathomechanics
15-1
15-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
studied independently, separate treatments are warranted. This review will focus specifically on the
pathomechanics of contraction-induced muscle injury.
The field of pathomechanics is not new; however, much of relevant basic and applied research has been
conducted under more established but related scientific disciplines. For example, much of the research on
skeletal muscle pathomechanics comes from the fields of muscle physiology, exercise physiology,
and sports medicine where the study of the risk factors and physiological mechanisms associated with
contraction-induced muscle injury have long been investigated. It is therefore a goal of this chapter to
integrate this vast, diverse body of knowledge into a brief, yet helpful resource for applied ergonomists.
This chapter is organized into three sections. The first section introduces and illustrates the fundamen-
tal terms and concepts pertaining to basic muscle anatomy and physiology. The second section reviews
the various methodological approaches that have been used with both human and nonhuman subjects.
The third section examines the body of research that sheds light on the underlying molecular, cellular,
and functional mechanisms associated with muscle injury caused by the major work-related risk
factors such as force, repetition, posture, and vibration. The chapter concludes with a discussion of
the current knowledge gaps and possible directions for further research.
FIGURE 15.1 (a) Cross-sectional view of whole muscle and the attached tendon. (b) Enlarged view of myofibrils
within a muscle fiber. (c) View of the thick filament (Myosin). (d) View of the thin filament (Actin). (e) Cytoskeletal
components of a myofibril showing cross-bridge arrangement. a–e (Copyright Brooks/Cole — Thomson Learning.)
the terminal button of the NMJ. This facilitates the release of the chemical messenger Acetylcholine
(ACH) that crosses the space to the motor end plate. This causes an ionic shift, which results in the
propagation of the action potential down the basement membrane of the muscle fiber and then down
the T-tubules of the muscle cell (Figure 15.2). The action potential activates the voltage-gated dihydro-
pyridine receptors in the T-tubule. This change in the T-tubules triggers the opening of Ca2þ release
channels (ryanodine receptors) on the SR. Ca2þ leaves the SR through the ryanodine receptors, enters
the cytoplasm, and binds to troponin, one of the proteins on the thin filaments.
Troponin has three polypeptide units; one binds to tropomyosin, one binds to actin, and a third one
binds to Ca2þ. Under resting conditions, tropomyosin is bound to actin and it blocks the myosin-binding
site on the actin protein, preventing the formation of cross-bridges (Figure 15.3). However, when free
Ca2þ rises in the cytoplasm of a myofiber, it binds to troponin and tropomyosin is pulled away
from the myosin-binding site on actin, leaving it open for cross-bridge formation. Once cross-bridges
are formed, the ATPase located on the myosin head increases its activity and hydrolyzes ATP. This
causes the cross-bridge to break, and Ca2þ then dissociates from its binding site on tropomyosin.
When Ca2þ is removed, tropomyosin slides back into the blocking position and the muscle relaxes.
Thus, troponin and tropomyosin are referred to as regulatory proteins in muscle contraction
(Figure 15.3).
FIGURE 15.2 The T-tubules and SR in relationship to the myofibrils. (Copyright Brooks/Cole — Thomson
Learning, 2001.)
Soft-Tissue Pathomechanics 15-5
FIGURE 15.3 The role of Ca2þ in activating the cross-bridges. (Copyright Brooks/Cole — Thomson Learning, 2001.)
the area over which thin and thick filaments overlap is reduced, and thus there is a decrease in the
number of cross-bridges that can be formed resulting in a reduction in force (Figure 15.5). Thus,
maximal force is generated when sarcomeres are at a length that produces the optimal overlap
between thin and thick fibers.
FIGURE 15.4 Muscle twitch, twitch summation, and tetanus. (a) If a muscle fiber is restimulated after complete
relaxation, the second response is the same as the initial response. (b) If the muscle fiber is restimulated before
complete relaxation takes place, the second twitch is added to the first twitch. (c) If the muscle fiber is stimulated
rapidly such that it does not have the opportunity to relax, a maximal contraction or tetanus occurs. (Copyright
Brooks/Cole — Thomson Learning, 2001.)
15-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
FIGURE 15.5 Length– tension relationship of muscle. At point A optimal overlap of thick and thin filaments results
in maximal tension developed. This is referred to as the normal resting length in the body (lo). As the muscle
lengthens (point B), less cross-bridges are attached, which results in a decrease in tension. Further increases in
length correspond with less cross-bridge attachment and further declines in tension (Point C). The response from
point A to point C is usually referred to as the descending limb of the length – tension relationship. If shortening
occurs at less than lo, fewer filament-binding sites are exposed to filament cross-bridges, thus tension decreases
(Point D). (Copyright Brooks/Cole — Thomson Learning, 2001.)
Force is generated at the cross-bridges, but it is transmitted longitudinally and radially along myofi-
brils. The longitudinal transmission of force occurs down the thick myosin filament to the Z-disk, and
on to the next serial set of myofibrils. Two proteins titin and nebulin, maintain length registry of the
sarcomere and aid in axial transmission of contractile forces. The actions of titin and nebulin maintain
registry of the A-band with the Z-disk, which is important for sarcomere integrity. Nebulin maintains
length registry of the thin filaments49,306 by interacting with tropomyosin and troponin to form a
lateral network with actin to regulate thin filament length. Titin functions as a two part spring to transmit
force from the thick filaments to the corresponding Z-disk.
Radial forces are transmitted via lateral stabilization of adjacent myofibrils. The protein responsible
for maintaining lateral registry of adjacent myofibrils at the Z-line is desmin.160 The Z-disk structure
is thought to be three-dimensional in nature and comprised of the proteins desmin, actin, and
a-actinin. The radial enclosure of these three proteins also extends longitudinally along the myofibrils
to provide both radial and longitudinal stability.207 These proteins are thought to be anchored to the
Z-disk via intermediate filament-associated proteins (IFAP). The cytoskeletal lattice extends radially
from the Z-disk to the sarcolemma via the transmembrane proteins. The transmembrane proteins
are thought to anchor the myofilaments to the sarcolemma via focal adhesions.205 These adhesions or
“costameres” are made up of a variety of transmembrane proteins. The basement membrane is then
attached to the sarcolemma via the dystroglycan complex.205,207 Radial transmission of forces occurs
through structural proteins located inside and outside of the sarcomeric region via the intermediate
filament network, and to the sarcolemma via the transmembrane proteins.207 Capability of radial
force transmission is necessary for redundancy in case of fiber injury. Thus, force can be transmitted
Soft-Tissue Pathomechanics 15-7
in any direction in relation to the axis of the muscle fibers via endosarcomeric and exosarcomeric
protein lattices.
flexors,193 – 197,221,222,231 knee flexors,173,174,294 hamstring,179 pectoralis and anterior deltoids (chest
press),245 and calf and biceps.136 Whole body movements such as downhill stepping exercise,187,189
downhill running,199 or cycling175 have also been used to create muscle injuries. The amount of acute
resistance for most human studies ranged from 10 to 180 repetitions for the segmental exercise
studies, and as little as 20 min of downhill stepping exercise187 to produce muscle soreness and evidence
of myofiber injury. Thus, even a low number of repetitions or short exposure to lengthening contractions
can result in a strain injury.
It is interesting to note that exposures that involve both whole body movements and segmental
loading result in muscle damage, loss of force, and delayed onset muscle soreness. Different types of
information can be attained from these two types of exposures. Whole body exercise typically involves
closed kinetic chain movements where the level of exercise (treadmill or cycling speed, metabolic load,
angle of inclination) is controlled but muscle forces or torques, velocity, range of motion, and
number of repetitions are not typically measured. In contrast, most segmental exercise models are
open kinetic chain movements involving isolated loading of the limbs, and are administered by either
isokinetic or isotonic dynamometry (computer-controlled strength testing equipment that operates in
either constant velocity or constant torque mode), or by isoinertial loads (using either free weight or
weight attached to an apparatus). Joint torques or forces, as well as velocity, range of motion, and
number of repetitions are measured.
Prolonged strength loss, as measured by maximal isometric force, is considered to be the best method
to quantify the degree and time course of muscle injury and recovery after exposure to damaging
lengthening contractions.289 It is also the primary means to quantify muscle function in humans
where muscle function is defined as the ability of the muscle group of interest to generate force over a
prescribed range of motion, or fixed length at a given level of muscle activation.289 Other measures,
such as biochemical markers in the blood and urine, and level of histological damage as obtained
from biopsy, are not well associated temporally with functional performance.289
Muscle injury studies with humans have been very beneficial in elucidating the type and intensity of
exercise and muscle actions that produce injury. Those studies have also been beneficial for examining
the resultant myofiber changes after injurious exposure and the recovery time after injury. The corre-
sponding levels of pain perception and muscle soreness after exposure was also consonant with the
degree of performance deficit.58 However, though many studies of muscle injury with humans have
been conducted in controlled laboratory settings, some experimental questions cannot be fully addressed
using human subjects. Confounding factors such as lifestyle, level of psychological stress, pre-existing
disease states such as diabetes and hypertension, and genetic polymorphisms are difficult to control.
In addition, key issues such as the amount of fiber strain and muscle biomechanics necessary for
injury, the amount and site of resultant muscle injury from longitudinal and cross-sectional tissue
analysis, the effect of structural protein knockouts and inflammatory mediator blockade on muscle
injury and repair, and biochemical analyses of muscle tissue, is either difficult or impossible with
human subjects.
Animal models have been developed that reduce or eliminate these confounding factors as well as
provide for control of the degree of motivation and muscle activation strategies. In depth physiological
questions about the pathways involved in muscle injury and repair, the site and extent of injury, the role
of structural proteins in muscle force transmission and injurious response, and biomechanical loading
signature necessary for injury can be more easily addressed in animal models. Animal models that
have been developed allow for more controlled study from the level of isolated muscle fibers to fully
intact muscle groups that contain intact neural and vascular supplies.
rodents. The majority of studies of acute injury in animals have focused on the temporal force response
of muscle to a single exposure (1 to 1800 repetitions) of high force isometric (muscle generating force at a
fixed length), concentric contractions, or lengthening contractions.13,35,36,39,72,74,75,129,161,163 – 165
The similarities between the micro-architecture of rodent and human skeletal muscle and the ability
to precisely control the biomechanics of contractile activity in rodents through various in vitro, in situ, or
in vivo methods, as well as the ability to investigate in depth physiological questions such as injury
pathways, are major advantages using animal preparations. Each type of animal model has inherent
advantages and disadvantages for the study of muscle injury. The models that have been used to
investigate muscle pathomechanics are described here.
FIGURE 15.6 In vivo dynamometer with an anaesthetized rat on the heated X –Y positioning table. The left foot is
secured in the load cell fixture and the knee is secured in 908 flexion.
servomotor (in the dynamometer) controls the movement kinematics and measures the resultant
muscle response during those movements. In the Wong and Booth model, the muscle forces produced
the shortening-only movement about the joint axis where the kinematics was not controlled.
The first reported in vivo rodent dynamometer was developed by Ashton-Miller for the study of
biomechanical behavior of the plantar and dorsi flexor muscles of the mouse hindlimb.12 This approach
also has been used to study rabbit dorsiflexors,26,86,163,164,206 rat dorsiflexors,62,97,116,152,154,156,157 mouse
dorsiflexors,132,168,283 and rat plantar flexors.63,296 – 299 Typical exposures ranged from 20 repetitions of
the rat plantar flexors298 to 900 repetitions in the rabbit dorsiflexors.86,163,164,206 Typical angular velocities
were based on the animal being tested: 75 deg/sec for rabbit dorsiflexors,163 upto 500 deg/sec for rat
dorsiflexors,97,152 and upto 2000 deg/sec for mouse dorsiflexors.132,168,283 Ranges of angular velocities
were selected based on the volitional capability of the muscle group and animal species in order to be
physiologically representative.
The major benefit of nonvolitional in vivo models is the ability to study muscle function and injury
mechanics about the joint axis of the target muscles. Thus, the normal muscle, tendon, and bone attach-
ments are intact as well as the neural and vascular supplies. The synergistic function of muscle agonists
and lateral transmission of adjacent muscle forces is also preserved.
One major limitation of nonvolitional models is the use of artificial electrical stimulation to invoke
muscle contractions. Unlike voluntary contractile activity, which is submaximal and characterized by
a selective recruitment of motor units, nonvolitional contractile activity is typically supramaximal
because electrical stimulation involves the activation of all motor units of the target muscle. Thus,
caution must be exercised when making inferences from comparisons between muscle responses from
supramaximal electrical stimulation and voluntary submaximal contractions.
models of volitional lifting was developed by Gordon in 1967 using weights attached to the back of rats
during vertical crawling and other exercises.104 – 107 This type of work was furthered by Stone et al.261,262
and Ho et al.120 in rats. Gonyea et al. extended this model to cats, also using weight lifting exercise.100 – 102
Weights also have been added to wing muscles of chickens, roosters, and other birds to produce an
overload model designed to study the skeletal muscle response to persistent overload.3,83,166,246
Animal treadmill models were developed in the early 1980s as a way to invoke voluntary repetitive
eccentric muscle actions capable of producing muscle injury.10,234 It was found that downhill treadmill
locomotion produced an eccentric bias on the soleus muscle in the plantar flexor group that resulted in
distinguishable signs of injury.10,66,202,234 Treadmill exposure has been used to study a wide variety of
physiological variables and typical exposures range from 30 to 150 min per session for a single session
in rats10,66,153,202,277 upto five sessions per week for 10 weeks duration,16,109 and upto 9 h exposure in
a single session for mice185 to study acute injury response and adaptation and reduction of injury
susceptibility after repetitive exposure. The treadmill studies are similar to those conducted in humans
although the animal exposures are typically longer in duration.
In volitional treadmill and resistance training models, the exposure biomechanic, such as muscle forces
or torques, or the number of muscle contractions (repetitions) are not controlled or quantified during
the activity. This lack of quantitation makes it difficult to relate physiological outcomes to specific
parameters of performance and loading history, which can differ widely across individual animals.
Some researchers have employed operant conditioning procedures to produce the kinds of repetitive
muscle loadings that are relevant for the study of exercise-induced physiological responses. In these
approaches, voluntary responses were motivated by various consequences, such as food
rewards,102,141,308 intracranial stimulation,94 or electric shock to the tail or feet.71,84,120,266 The species,
target muscle groups, and training protocols, however, differed widely among these models. For
example, Gonyea et al.98,101,102 trained cats to grasp and move a weighted bar with the forelimb repeat-
edly in 30-min sessions conducted 5 days per week for upto 87 weeks. Barbe et al.19 trained rats upto 8
weeks to repeatedly reach their forelimbs into a small tube to retrieve food pellets. Yarasheski et al.308
trained rats over an 8-week period to climb a wire-mesh ladder with weights secured to their tails,
and Klitgaard141 trained rats over a 36-week period to enter a vertical tube and use their hindlimbs to
lift a weighted ring. In other approaches, rats wore weighted jackets and were trained in sessions
conducted 8 to 16 weeks to rear up on their hindlimbs to avoid an electric shock71,120,266 or to receive
brain stimulation.94
Many of these approaches have been developed for the study of adaptive or regenerative processes.
For example, there is considerable evidence that under some conditions, voluntary repetitive exertions
performed over several weeks or months can lead to muscle hypertrophy as evidenced by increases in
either myofiber number or size.71,101,266,301 Under other conditions, however, similar patterns of repeti-
tive exertions have resulted in degenerative morphology98 and inflammatory responses.19 Unfortunately,
many of these approaches lacked the necessary control and quantitation of the biomechanics of the
movements to allow for thorough assessments of external and internal loadings that would be necessary
to characterize dose –response relationships. In addition, few studies have specifically examined the
effects of external loads on both internal loads and tolerances (as measured by biomechanical per-
formance and physiological force tremor) and physiological or biochemical processes. Thus, little is
known about the relation between specific parameters or changes in performance or physiological
force tremor and physiological or biochemical processes.
for movement of the limbs and external work, as well as the absorption of work, it can produce loads on
other tissues such as tendons, joints, and nerves. There have been extensive studies to date on acute
contraction-induced muscle injury using both animal and human models. Studies of soft-tissue injury
resulting from acute strain overload have been conducted using animal and human models. Indeed, a
number of studies on contraction-induced injury have been conducted in rodents and rabbits. These
studies have used in vivo, in situ, and in vitro preparations to investigate muscle injury.
contractions were maximal. The results of their study showed that the loss of force was due to changes in
contractile elements, not the level of muscle activation. This was the first study to suggest that the
force decrement resulting from eccentric muscle actions is not the result of less muscle activation, but
instead may have a mechanical etiology. Thus, the force deficit seen after eccentric contraction-
induced injury in both humans and animals is due to damage of the contractile proteins and supporting
structures, not central nervous system activation level.
Once eccentric contractions were identified as causing muscle injury, it was important to investigate
how the injurious response could be modified by mechanical exposure factors (e.g., force, strain,
strain rate, number of repetitions, and velocity). The primary factors that have been studied have
some generalizability to occupational physical exposures.
decreased protein synthesis, and thus an increased population of weak sarcomeres. In contrast, Willems
and Stauber did not find a difference in isometric force deficit after 30 eccentric stretches in old (39%)
and young rats (35%).297 While muscle weights were similar in the young (4 months) and old (24
months) groups, peak force was lower in the old group during the eccentric stretches. Thus, injury
studies using rodents should be cognizant of age as a possible contributing or confounding factor in
the response to contraction-induced injury. The effect of age on the adaptive or pathological response
to a chronic administration of high force eccentric muscle actions has not been studied thus far. The
age of the animals selected for study is an important factor in acute injury and may affect the response
to repetitive exposure.
FIGURE 15.7 Fifteen sets of injurious SSCs performed on active dorsiflexor muscles at 1-min intervals. The curves
are the force response (N) of the dorsi flexor muscles during sets 1, 3, 5, 7, and 15. The SSCs were conducted at a range
of motion of 70 – 120– 708 in a reciprocal fashion at 500 deg/sec.
15-20 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
FIGURE 15.8 Time course of change in isometric force resultant from exposure to SSCs (oscillatory group) and
isometric contractions (isometric control) over a 10-day period.
pronounced reduction in soreness, loss of strength, and release of enzymes, such as creatine kinase
after the second exposure than the initial exposure.17,58,60 Also, a second exposure of eccentric con-
tractions does not appear to delay the recovery from the first exposure. Typically, recovery from eccentric
contraction-induced injury takes at least 10 days for recovery of performance and remodeling of
muscle fiber disruption.96 If the second session is administered within 6 days after the first exposure,
the recovery time was unaffected.194 Thus, the normal recovery process is not affected by at least
one intervening injurious exposure. Clearly there is an adaptation that takes place after one exposure
to injurious eccentric contractions that ameliorates the injurious response to subsequent exposures.
The mechanism by which this takes place is not clearly understood at this time. Exposure to a mild
session of eccentric contractions still provides a protective effect to further sessions of more intense
eccentric exposure.40,60
Some authors postulate that the adaptive mechanism could be at the level of the central nervous
system where recruitment patterns could be adapting to recruit in a different fashion such that motor
unit recruitment would be appropriately synchronized to reduce asymmetric stresses in the muscle
fibers.126,194 However, the repeated bout effect has also been found in electrically stimulated animal
models, which would argue against the hypothesis that neural factors provide the adaptive effect.179
Thus, the mechanism for adaptation must be in the muscle fibers themselves. There could be weak sar-
comeres as a result of deconditioning that are more susceptible to stresses generated during eccentric
contractions. The notion that stress-susceptible fibers exist was first postulated by Armstrong et al.8 If
fragile fibers were compromised after an initial session of eccentric muscle actions, the target muscle
should lose muscle volume as a result of the loss of those fragile fibers. This hypothesis was supported
by findings in human elbow flexors where there was a loss of muscle volume (approximately 10%) 14
days after an injurious exposure to eccentric muscle actions. In contrast, after a second session performed
8 weeks later, there was no loss of muscle volume. The authors concluded that the fragile fibers were
lost after the first session and are repaired over time. As repaired fibers replace the fragile fibers, the
muscle becomes more resistant to eccentric contraction-induced damage.85 However, if muscle is not
used, fragile fibers may again develop that can be injured at some juncture. There is also evidence
that one session of injurious eccentric contractions can ameliorate the resultant damage from the
Soft-Tissue Pathomechanics 15-21
FIGURE 15.9 Cross-sections of TA muscle (H&E stain, 40 objective). After exposure to SSCs: (a) 6 h, (c) 72 h,
and (e) 240 h. After isometric contractions: (b), 6 h, (d) 72 h, and (f) 240 h.
second session, even if the sessions are spaced 6 months apart.198 This indicates that in normal subjects,
the protective effect can last for some months, but the protective effect predictably dissipates with time.
Another explanation for this protective effect is the notion that muscles can add sarcomeres after
an initial injurious exposure as a means to reduce the number that are at their extreme length, thus redu-
cing injury.58 The addition of sarcomeres after an injurious session of eccentric-biased exercise has been
shown in both human292 and animal170 studies. Apparently, concentric muscle training reduces the
number of sarcomeres and does not provide the level of protection in repeated sessions that eccentric
training does.292
Few studies have investigated muscle response to the chronic administration of eccentric, concentric,
and isometric muscle actions. The limited number of studies which have been conducted did not control
the dynamics of movement, quantify the forces during the movement, or quantify the changes in
performance longitudinally throughout the protocol. Investigation of in vivo muscle functional,
15-22 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
FIGURE 15.10 Histology of control muscle with no injury (a) and exposure to SSCs of 30 repetitions
(b), 70 repetitions (c), and 150 repetitions (d).
histological, and biochemical responses to a chronic administration of muscle actions will further
elucidate the factors, which predispose or mitigate contraction-induced injury. It will also further the
understanding of the functional changes that result, characterize the inflammatory and repair kinetics
longitudinally, and determine whether biochemical markers are representative of muscle performance
or injury.
In summary, the study of different factors and their effect on muscle injury indicates that increasing
force, task duration, number of repetitions, and range of motion exacerbate muscle injury. Older
animals also have an increased susceptibility to injury. Training, particularly if involves eccentric
muscle actions, can provide a protective effect from contraction-induced muscle injury. These findings
should be of use to those attempting to ameliorate occupational muscle injury incidence.
may mediate the acute reduction in isometric force, and it may initiate other cellular mechanisms
that exacerbate myofiber damage.
Increases in intracellular Ca2þ can also alter myofiber structure and integrity by modifying proteins
and lipids in damaged cells.163,304 For example, calpain proteases, which are activated by increases in
intracellular Ca2þ, are elevated in muscle after exercise.21 Calpain can cleave signaling, cytoskeletal,
and myofibrillar proteins.228,229 This cleavage may act to target proteins for degradation by other
proteases including ubiquitin.228 In vitro studies have demonstrated that activated calpain degrades
desmin and stimulates the release of a-actinin, thereby inducing Z-line streaming.22 Calpain-mediated
modifications of proteins can also stimulate the activity of a number of intracellular signaling proteins
important for mediating cellular responses to damage. For example, the signaling molecule, protein
kinase C (PKC), is activated by calpain cleavage.228 This active form of PKC can affect myofiber function
by modifying proteins that are already present in the cell or by acting upon transcriptional pathways
to regulate gene expression.232 Calpain may also act as a chemotactic signal to enhance neutrophil
infiltration into the damaged tissue.22
A number of other intracellular signaling systems are also activated by increased intracellular Ca2þ
levels in myofibers. For example, intracellular Ca2þ liberates phospholipase A2 (PLA2) from the
extracellular membrane. PLA2 acts to increase arachidonic acid production and the synthesis of
prostaglandins, which can stimulate inflammation and cause pain.204,244,279,280
Other signaling systems that may be indirectly activated in response to contraction-induced injury
and increases in intracellular Ca2þ include the extracellular receptor kinase 1-2 (ERK1-2), p38
mitogen activating protein kinase,51 and c-JUN NH2-terminal kinase pathways.47,117,200 These pathways
are stimulated in response to growth factors, cellular stress, and injury. Activation of these pathways
regulates transcriptional and translational activity in many cell types including muscles. Although the
increase in intracellular Ca2þ and associated proteolysis and lipolysis have been traditionally thought
to exacerbate muscle injury, Ca2þ-induced activation of cell signaling pathways and proteolysis might
also activate cell systems necessary for initiating cellular repair and regeneration.
in damaged tissues enhance local pathways mediating tissue inflammation and may act to exacerbate
damage during the first 5 days after muscle injury.
to assess contraction-induced damage have been reviewed previously.289 In humans, these approaches
include examining force production, histology, blood levels of muscle-associated proteins, and reports
of pain.
Force production has commonly been used to assess muscle function because reductions in force are
often correlated with injury. Changes in force are immediate, and maintained for a number of days
following the exposure that caused the injury.10,61,87,96,291 Although muscle damage is often associated
with a force deficit, force measurements alone should not be used to diagnose injury because muscle
fatigue can also result in reductions in force.80 – 82
Overuse disorders can also be associated with losses in force, however a better marker for these dis-
orders is muscle fatigue.30,133 Although injured muscle does demonstrate signs of fatigue,80,299 fatigue
is also seen prior to the generation of injury, and it is believed that fatigue and the cellular changes
associated with this physiological state, may be in part responsible for generating overuse disorders.243
Thus, depending on when measurements are taken, fatigue can be used as a biomarker for predicting
the development of a disorder, or for diagnosing a disorder.
Increases in the circulating concentrations of certain muscle-associated proteins, including creatine
kinase, lactate dehydrogenase, and myoglobin are often associated with strain-induced muscle inju-
ries.54,195,223,227,248 Increases in these circulating proteins do not correlate well with injury-induced force
deficits during the first 24 h of injury,56,60,186 and thus, the concentrations of these proteins do not serve
as good markers for the early diagnosis of muscle injury. In addition, certain proteins, such as creatine
kinase, fluctuate in response to muscle activity, and not solely in response to muscle injury.55,187,289
Histological examination of muscle biopsies can be used to diagnose muscle injury. As mentioned pre-
viously, infiltration of immune cells, disruptions of the sarcolemma, and necrosis can be seen in muscles
with strain-induced injuries.90,163,255 However, these changes are not usually apparent until 24–48 h after
the injury,90 and thus, they cannot be used to identify injuries early during the process. With overuse
disorders, ragged red fibers are found by histological examination of biopsy tissue.159 These ragged
red fibers appear to be strongly immunopositive for cytochrome C oxidase (an enzyme involved in mito-
chondrial function), or immunonegative for this marker.158 Thus, the presence of ragged red fibers, and
their cytochrome C oxidase phenotype, can be used as a marker of muscle disorders.
Pain or muscle soreness is one of the most common symptoms used to determine if there is muscle
damage. However, with strain-induced injuries, muscle soreness is not apparent until 24 –48 h after
the injury occurred,55,90 and it does not correlate well with injury-induced force deficits.50,69,136 There-
fore, pain is not a good marker to use for early diagnoses of strain injuries. Pain is also common in
overuse disorders and is associated with muscle fatigue,159,240 the presence of ragged red fibers,158,159
and reductions in blood flow.158 Pain has also been correlated with biochemical changes associated
with fatigue.30,240,242 Because fatigue often precedes actual damage, pain and muscle fatigue may serve
as biomarkers for predicting muscle damage.
Based on our current knowledge the diagnoses of muscle strains can most quickly be done by checking
for force deficits. Reports of muscle soreness, or the presence of myofiber proteins in the blood can be
used to confirm that the force deficit is due to an injury. Depending on the timing of events, reports
of pain and measurements of muscle fatigue can also be used to indicate that injury may occur, or to
diagnose an injury. Although most biomarkers are currently used for diagnosis, research focusing on
pain, fatigue, and their association with biomarkers may provide a means for determining when an indi-
vidual is at risk for developing a muscle strain or disorder.
In vivo dynamometry is well suited for the study of chronic muscle injury and adaptation and studies of
this kind would be beneficial in elucidating the physiological response to repetitive biomechanical
loading. Biomechanical loading parameters, such as number of repetitions per day, the velocity and
acceleration of the movement, the range of motion of the movement, muscle force or torque during
each movement, and the rest interval between sets, should be controlled during experiments. Controlling
and quantifying the biomechanical loading profile is essential for rigorous study of muscle injury and
adaptation for both acute and chronic exposure studies. Also, it is important to have the ability to
vary the biomechanical inputs to study the effects of different inputs, and how the level of those
inputs, such as more or less repetitions, higher or lower velocities or acceleration, and so on, affects
the physiological response after a single exposure and/or multiple exposures.
In addition to controlling the biomechanical inputs, it is also important in volitional models to not
only provide an apparatus to facilitate controlled movement, but to appropriately instrument the appar-
atus to monitor movement dynamics in real time. This will allow for quantitation of the biomechanical
loading profile within each exposure session for the study of both acute and chronic muscle response.
This approach is similar to in vivo dynamometry in that the biomechanical loading signature is recorded
during each exposure, but different from in vivo dynamometry in that the movement profile is not
controlled by an external source like a servomotor, but controlled by the animal.
In vivo models, whether volitional or nonvolitional, can provide a wealth of information about the
effects of biomechanical loading inputs on acute and chronic physiological responses. Refinement of
these models to control and monitor the biomechanical loading signature has been done in nonvolitional
models and is currently being accomplished in volitional models.
15.6.3 Summary
There has been much work in the area of exercise-induced muscle injury over the past 30 years. The
relation between factors such as force, range of motion, number of repetitions, exposure duration,
velocity, age, gender, and training on muscle injury have been studied and have a clear occupational
relevance. Also, the cellular mechanisms responsible for the injury and repair processes have been well
studied to date. The relation between muscle function, myofiber damage, pain, and molecular indicators
Soft-Tissue Pathomechanics 15-29
of injury have also been studied. Clearly, more work needs to be done in this area, particularly regarding
the physiological response to long-term repetitive loading. The findings to date indicate that eccentric
muscle actions result in muscle damage and recovery from this injury can require upto 1 month.
Increasing the biomechanical exposure such as force, number of repetitions, and range of motion can
exacerbate the magnitude of injury response. Increased age can also increase injury susceptibility.
The encouraging news is that training can reduce the injurious response and adaptation can take
place, particularly if the appropriate rest intervals are included. There are biomarkers currently being
studied that may indicate the evidence of myofiber injury that have the appropriate level of sensitivity
and specificity needed for occupational monitoring. The area of soft-tissue pathomechanics can
provide a wealth of information that will be of value to ergonomists and occupational health
professionals in the quest to reduce the incidence of occupational musculoskeletal disorders.
Acknowledgments
The author would like to thank Kris Krajnak for her contributions in cellular biology, Oliver Wirth for
his contributions in volitional models, and Steve Alway, Robert Mercer, and Aaron Schopper for their
constructive comments.
References
1. Adams, G.R., Autocrine and/or paracrine insulin-like growth factor-I activity in skeletal muscle,
Clin. Orthop., (403 Suppl), S188 –S196, 2002.
2. Alvarez, B., Quinn, L.S., Busquets, S., Lopez-Soriano, F.J., and Argiles, J.M., Direct effects of tumor
necrosis factor alpha (TNF-alpha) on murine skeletal muscle cell lines. Bimodal effects on protein
metabolism, Eur. Cytokine Netw., 12(3), 399 –410, 2001.
3. Alway, S.E., Attenuation of Ca(2þ)-activated ATPase and shortening velocity in hypertrophied fast
twitch skeletal muscle from aged Japanese quail, Exp. Gerontol., 37(5), 665–678, 2002.
4. Amelink, G.J., Kamp, H.H., and Bar, P.R., Creatine kinase isoenzyme profiles after exercise in the
rat: sex-linked differences in leakage of CK-MM, Pflugers Arch., 412(4), 417– 421, 1988.
5. Amelink, G.J., Koot, R.W., Erich, W.B., Van Gijn, J., and Bar, P.R., Sex-linked variation in creatine
kinase release, and its dependence on oestradiol, can be demonstrated in an in-vitro rat skeletal
muscle preparation, Acta Physiol. Scand., 138(2), 115–124, 1990.
6. Armstrong, R.B., Mechanisms of exercise-induced delayed onset muscular soreness: a brief review,
Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 16(6), 529–538, 1984.
7. Armstrong, R.B., Muscle damage and endurance events, Sports Med., 3(5), 370–381, 1986.
8. Armstrong, R.B., Initial events in exercise-induced muscular injury, Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 22(4),
429 –435, 1990.
9. Armstrong, R.B., Duan, C., Delp, M.D., Hayes, D.A., Glenn, G.M., and Allen, G.D., Elevations in rat
soleus muscle [Ca2þ] with passive stretch, J. Appl. Physiol., 74(6), 2990 –2997, 1993.
10. Armstrong, R.B., Ogilvie, R.W., and Schwane, J.A., Eccentric exercise-induced injury to rat skeletal
muscle, J. Appl. Physiol., 54(1), 80– 93, 1983.
11. Armstrong, R.B., Warren, G.L., and Warren, J.A., Mechanisms of exercise-induced muscle fibre
injury, Sports Med., 12(3), 184– 207, 1991.
12. Ashton-Miller, J.A., He, Y., Kadhiresan, V.A., McCubbrey, D.A., and Faulkner, J.A., An apparatus to
measure in vivo biomechanical behavior of dorsi- and plantarflexors of mouse ankle, J. Appl.
Physiol., 72(3), 1205 –1211, 1992.
13. Asmussen E. Positive and Negative Muscular Work. ACTA Physiol. Scand., 28, 365–382, 1953.
14. Avela, J. and Komi, P.V., Reduced stretch reflex sensitivity and muscle stiffness after long-lasting
stretch-shortening cycle exercise in humans, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol., 78(5), 403–
410, 1998.
15-30 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
15. Baggiolini, M., Dewald, B., Moser B., Chemokines., In: Gallin, J.I., Snyderman, R., editors. Inflam-
mation: Basic Principles and Clinical Correlates. 3rd ed., Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Philadelphia,
1999, pp. 419–431.
16. Ballor, D.L., Tommerup, L.J., Smith, D.B., and Thomas, D.P., Body composition, muscle and fat pad
changes following two levels of dietary restriction and/or exercise training in male rats, Int. J. Obes.,
14(8), 711– 722, 1990.
17. Balnave, C.D. and Thompson, M.W., Effect of training on eccentric exercise-induced muscle
damage, J. Appl. Physiol., 75(4), 1545 –1551, 1993.
18. Bar, P.R., Amelink, G.J., Oldenburg, B., and Blankenstein, M.A., Prevention of exercise-induced
muscle membrane damage by oestradiol, Life Sci., 42(26), 2677–2681, 1988.
19. Barbe, M.F., Barr, A.E., Gorzelany, I., Amin, M., Gaughan, J.P., and Safadi, F.F., Chronic repetitive
reaching and grasping results in decreased motor performance and widespread tissue responses in a
rat model of MSD, J. of Orthopaed. Res., 21(1), 167 –176, 2003.
20. Barr, A.E. and Barbe, M.F., Pathophysiological tissue changes associated with repetitive movement:
a review of the evidence, Phys. Ther., 82(2), 173 –187, 2002.
21. Belcastro, A.N., Skeletal muscle calcium-activated neutral protease (calpain) with exercise, J. Appl.
Physiol., 74(3), 1381–1386, 1993.
22. Belcastro, A.N., Shewchuk, L.D., and Raj, D.A., Exercise-induced muscle injury: a calpain hypoth-
esis, Mol. Cell. Biochem., 179(1–2), 135–145, 1998.
23. Benz, R.J., Friden, J., and Lieber, R.L., Simultaneous stiffness and force measurements reveal subtle
injury to rabbit soleus muscles, Mol. Cell. Biochem., 179(1–2), 147 –158, 1998.
24. Bernard, B.P., Putz-Anderson, V., Burt, S.E., Cole, L.L., Fairfield-Estill, C., Fine, L.J. et al., Muscu-
loskeletal disorders and workplace factors. A critical review of epidemiologic evidence for work-
related musculoskeletal disorders of the neck, upper extremity, and low back, Cincinnati:
NIOSH; 1997. Report No.: DHHS (NIOSH) Pub no. 97 –141.
25. Best, T.M. and Hunter, K.D., Muscle injury and repair, Phys. Med. Rehabil. Clin. N. Am., 11(2),
251– 266, 2000.
26. Best, T.M., McCabe, R.P., Corr, D., Vanderby, R., Jr., Evaluation of a new method to create a stan-
dardized muscle stretch injury, Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 30(2), 200–205, 1998.
27. Best, T.M., Shehadeh, S.E., Leverson, G., Michel, J.T., Corr, D.T., Aeschlimann, D., Analysis of changes
in mRNA levels of myoblast- and fibroblast-derived gene products in healing skeletal muscle using
quantitative reverse transcription-polymerase chain reaction, J. Orthop. Res., 19(4), 565 –572, 2001.
28. Bigland-Ritchie, B., Graichen, H., and Woods, J.J., A variable-speed motorized bicycle ergometer for
positive and negative work exercise, J. Appl. Physiol., 35(5), 739 –740, 1973.
29. Bigland-Ritchie, B. and Woods, J., Proceedings: Oxygen consumption and integrated electrical
activity of muscle during positive and negative work, J. Physiol., 234(2), 39P– 40P, 1973.
30. Blangsted, A.K., Sogaard, K., Christensen, H., and Sjogaard G., The effect of physical and psychosocial
loads on the trapezius muscle activity during computer keying tasks and rest periods, Eur. J. Appl.
Physiol., 2003.
31. Bobbert, M.F., Huijing, P.A., and van Ingen Schenau, G.J., A model of the human triceps surae
muscle-tendon complex applied to jumping, J. Biomech., 19(11), 887 –898, 1986.
32. Bonde-Petersen, F., Henriksson, J., and Knuttgen, H.G., Effect of training with eccentric muscle
contractions on skeletal muscle metabolites, Acta. Physiol. Scand., 88(4), 564 –570, 1973.
33. Bonde-Petersen, F., Knuttgen, H.G., and Henriksson, J., Muscle metabolism during exercise with
concentric and eccentric contractions, J. Appl. Physiol., 33(6), 792 –795, 1972.
34. Borsa, P.A. and Sauers, E.L., The importance of gender on myokinetic deficits before and after
microinjury, Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 32(5), 891 –896, 2000.
35. Brooks, S.V. and Faulkner, J.A., Contraction-induced injury: recovery of skeletal muscles in young
and old mice, Am. J. Physiol., 258(3 Pt 1), C436–C442, 1990.
36. Brooks, S.V. and Faulkner, J.A., Isometric, shortening, and lengthening contractions of muscle fiber
segments from adult and old mice, Am. J. Physiol., 267(2 Pt 1), C507–C513, 1994.
Soft-Tissue Pathomechanics 15-31
37. Brooks, S.V. and Faulkner, J.A., The magnitude of the initial injury induced by stretches of maxi-
mally activated muscle fibres of mice and rats increases in old age, J. Physiol., 497(Pt 2), 573 –580,
1996.
38. Brooks, S.V. and Faulkner, J.A., Severity of contraction-induced injury is affected by velocity only
during stretches of large strain, J. Appl. Physiol., 91(2), 661 –666, 2001.
39. Brooks, S.V., Zerba, E., and Faulkner, J.A., Injury to muscle fibres after single stretches of passive
and maximally stimulated muscles in mice, J. Physiol., 488(Pt 2), 459 –469, 1995.
40. Brown, S.J., Child, R.B., Day, S.H., and Donnelly, A.E., Exercise-induced skeletal muscle damage
and adaptation following repeated bouts of eccentric muscle contractions, J. Sports Sci., 15(2),
215–222, 1997.
41. Byl, N., Wilson, F., Merzenich, M., Melnick, M., Scott, P., Oakes, A. et al., Sensory dysfunction
associated with repetitive strain injuries of tendinitis and focal hand dystonia: a comparative
study, J. Orthop. Sports Phys. Ther., 23(4), 234–244, 1996.
42. Byrd, S.K., Alterations in the sarcoplasmic reticulum: a possible link to exercise-induced muscle
damage, Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 24(5), 531 –536, 1992.
43. Cantini, M., Massimino, M.L., Bruson, A., Catani, C., Dalla Libera, L., and Carraro U.,
Macrophages regulate proliferation and differentiation of satellite cells, Biochem. Biophys. Res.
Commun., 202(3), 1688– 1696, 1994.
44. Cantini, M., Massimino, M.L., Rapizzi, E., Rossini, K., Catani, C., Dalla Libera, L. et al., Human
satellite cell proliferation in vitro is regulated by autocrine secretion of IL-6 stimulated by a
soluble factor(s) released by activated monocytes, Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun., 216(1), 49–
53, 1995.
45. Carbo, N., Lopez-Soriano, J., Costelli, P., Busquets, S., Alvarez, B., Baccino, F.M. et al., Interleukin-
15 antagonizes muscle protein waste in tumour-bearing rats, Br. J. Cancer, 83(4), 526 –531, 2000.
46. Carpenter, J.E., Flanagan, C.L., Thomopoulos, S., Yian, E.H., and Soslowsky, L.J., The effects of
overuse combined with intrinsic or extrinsic alterations in an animal model of rotator cuff tendi-
nosis, Am. J. Sports Med., 26(6), 801–807, 1998.
47. Carrasco, M.A., Riveros, N., Rios, J., Muller, M., Torres, F., Pineda, J. et al., Depolarization-induced
slow calcium transients activate early genes in skeletal muscle cells, Am. J. Physiol. Cell Physiol.,
284(6), C1438–C1447, 2003.
48. Cavanagh, P., On “muscle action” vs “muscle contraction”, J. Biomech., 21, 69, 1988.
49. Chen, M.J., Shih, C.L., and Wang, K., Nebulin as an actin zipper. A two-module nebulin fragment
promotes actin nucleation and stabilizes actin filaments, J. Biol. Chem., 268(27), 20327–20334, 1993.
50. Chen, T.C. and Hsieh, S.S., Effects of a 7-day eccentric training period on muscle damage and
inflammation, Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 33(10), 1732– 1738, 2001.
51. Childs, T.E., Spangenburg, E.E., Vyas, D.R., and Booth, F.W., Temporal alterations in protein sig-
naling cascades during recovery from muscle atrophy, Am. J. Physiol. Cell Physiol., 285(2),
C391–C398, 2003.
52. Chou, S.M. and Mizuno, Y., Induction of spheroid cytoplasmic bodies in a rat muscle by local
tetanus, Muscle Nerve, 9(5), 455 –464, 1986.
53. Clarkson, P.M., Eccentric exercise and muscle damage. International Journal of Sports Medicine,
18(Suppl 4), S314 –S317, 1997.
54. Clarkson, P.M., Apple, F.S., Byrnes, W.C., McCormick, K.M., and Triffletti, P., Creatine kinase
isoforms following isometric exercise, Muscle Nerve, 10(1), 41 –44, 1987.
55. Clarkson, P.M., Byrnes, W.C., McCormick, K.M., Turcotte, L.P., and White, J.S., Muscle soreness
and serum creatine kinase activity following isometric, eccentric, and concentric exercise, Int.
J. Sports Med., 7(3), 152–155, 1986.
56. Clarkson, P.M. and Ebbeling, C., Investigation of serum creatine kinase variability after muscle-
damaging exercise, Clin. Sci. (Lond.), 75(3), 257 –261, 1988.
57. Clarkson, P.M. and Hubal, M.J., Are women less susceptible to exercise-induced muscle damage?,
Curr. Opin. Clin. Nutr. Metab. Care, 4(6), 527–531, 2001.
15-32 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
58. Clarkson, P.M. and Hubal, M.J., Exercise-induced muscle damage in humans. Am. J. Phys. Med.
Rehabil., 81(11 Suppl), S52 –S69, 2002.
59. Clarkson, P.M., Nosaka, K., and Braun, B., Muscle function after exercise-induced muscle damage
and rapid adaptation, Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 24(5), 512–520, 1992.
60. Clarkson, P.M. and Tremblay, I., Exercise-induced muscle damage, repair, and adaptation in
humans, J. Appl. Physiol., 65(1), 1 –6, 1988.
61. Cutlip, R.G. The Dynamic Parameters that Affect Eccentric-Contraction Induced Injury in an In
Vivo Rodent Model [Doctoral Dissertation]: West Virginia University; 1995.
62. Cutlip, R.G., Geronilla, K.B., Baker, B.A., Kashon, M.L., Miller, G.R., and Schopper, A.W., Impact of
muscle length during stretch-shortening contractions on real-time and temporal muscle perform-
ance measures in rats in vivo, J. Appl. Physiol., 96(2), 507– 516, 2004.
63. Cutlip, R.G., Stauber, W.T., Willison, R.H., McIntosh, T.A., and Means, K.H., Dynamometer for rat
plantar flexor muscles in vivo, Med. Biol. Eng. Comput., 35(5), 540– 543, 1997.
64. De Bleecker, J.L., Meire, V.I., Declercq, W., and Van Aken, E.H., Immunolocalization of tumor
necrosis factor-alpha and its receptors in inflammatory myopathies, Neuromuscul. Disord., 9(4),
239– 246, 1999.
65. Devor, S.T. and Faulkner, J.A., Regeneration of new fibers in muscles of old rats reduces contrac-
tion-induced injury, J. Appl. Physiol., 87(2), 750–756, 1999.
66. Duan, C., Delp, M.D., Hayes, D.A., Delp, P.D., and Armstrong, R.B., Rat skeletal muscle mitochon-
drial [Ca2þ] and injury from downhill walking, J. Appl. Physiol., 68(3), 1241– 1251, 1990.
67. Dubravec, D.B., Spriggs, D.R., Mannick, J.A., and Rodrick, M.L., Circulating human peripheral
blood granulocytes synthesize and secrete tumor necrosis factor alpha, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
U.S.A., 87(17), 6758 –6761, 1990.
68. Edman, K.A., The velocity of unloaded shortening and its relation to sarcomere length and iso-
metric force in vertebrate muscle fibres, J. Physiol., 291, 143 –159, 1979.
69. Eston, R.G., Finney, S., Baker, S., and Baltzopoulos, V., Muscle tenderness and peak torque changes
after downhill running following a prior bout of isokinetic eccentric exercise, J. Sports Sci., 14(4),
291– 299, 1996.
70. Eston, R.G., Lemmey, A.B., McHugh, P., Byrne, C., and Walsh, S.E., Effect of stride length on symp-
toms of exercise-induced muscle damage during a repeated bout of downhill running, Scand.
J. Med. Sci. Sports, 10(4), 199–204, 2000.
71. Farrell, P.A., Fedele, M.J., Hernandez, J., Fluckey, J.D., Miller, J.L., 3rd, Lang, C.H. et al., Hypertro-
phy of skeletal muscle in diabetic rats in response to chronic resistance exercise, J. Appl. Physiol.,
87(3), 1075– 1082, 1999.
72. Faulkner, J.A., Brooks, S.V., and Zerba, E., Muscle atrophy and weakness with aging: contraction-
induced injury as an underlying mechanism, J. Gerontol. A. Biol. Sci. Med. Sci., 50 Spec No:124 –
129, 1995.
73. Faulkner, J.A., Jones, D.A., and Round, J.M., An Apparatus for Unilateral Exercise of the Lower
Limb Muscles of Small Mammals, J. Physiol. Lond., 365, 11P, 1985.
74. Faulkner, J.A., Jones, D.A., and Round, J.M., Injury to Skeletal Muscle of Mice by Lengthening
Contractions. J. of Physiol., Lond., 365, 75P, 1985.
75. Faulkner, J.A., Jones, D.A., and Round, J.M., Injury to skeletal muscles of mice by forced lengthen-
ing during contractions, Q. J. Exp. Physiol., 74(5), 661 –670, 1989.
76. Faulkner, J.A., Opiteck, J.A., and Brooks, S.V., Injury to skeletal muscle during altitude training:
induction and prevention, Int. J. Sports Med., 13 Suppl 1, S160 –S162, 1992.
77. Fibbi, G., D’Alessio, S., Pucci, M., Cerletti, M., and Del Rosso, M., Growth factor-dependent pro-
liferation and invasion of muscle satellite cells require the cell-associated fibrinolytic system, Biol.
Chem., 383(1), 127–136, 2002.
78. Fielding, R.A., Evans, W.J., Hughes, V.A., Moldawer, L.L., and Bistrian, B.R., The effects of high
intensity exercise on muscle and plasma levels of alpha-ketoisocaproic acid, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol.
Occup. Physiol., 55(5), 482 –485, 1986.
Soft-Tissue Pathomechanics 15-33
79. Fielding, R.A., Manfredi, T.J., Ding, W., Fiatarone, M.A., Evans, W.J., and Cannon, J.G., Acute phase
response in exercise. III. Neutrophil and IL-1 beta accumulation in skeletal muscle, Am. J. Physiol.,
265(1 Pt 2), R166 –R172, 1993.
80. Fitts, R.H., Cellular mechanisms of muscle fatigue, Physiol. Rev., 74(1), 49 –94, 1994.
81. Fitts, R.H., Muscle fatigue: the cellular aspects, Am. J. Sports Med., 24(6 Suppl), S9–S13, 1996.
82. Fitts, R.H. and Holloszy, J.O., Effects of fatigue and recovery on contractile properties of frog
muscle, J. Appl. Physiol., 45(6), 899–902, 1978.
83. Fluck, M., Carson, J.A., Schwartz, R.J., and Booth, F.W., SRF protein is upregulated during stretch-
induced hypertrophy of rooster ALD muscle, J. Appl. Physiol., 86(6), 1793 –1799, 1999.
84. Fluckey, J.D., Kraemer, W.J., and Farrell, P.A., Pancreatic islet insulin secretion is increased after
resistance exercise in rats, J. Appl. Physiol., 79(4), 1100 –1105, 1995.
85. Foley, J.M., Jayaraman, R.C., Prior, B.M., Pivarnik, J.M., and Meyer, R.A., MR measurements of
muscle damage and adaptation after eccentric exercise, J. of Appl. Physiol., 87(6), 2311–2318, 1999.
86. Friden, J. and Lieber, R.L., Segmental muscle fiber lesions after repetitive eccentric contractions, Cell
Tissue Res., 293(1), 165–171, 1998.
87. Friden, J., Lieber, R.L., and Thornell, L.E., Subtle indications of muscle damage following eccentric
contractions, Acta Physiol. Scand., 142(4), 523–524, 1991.
88. Friden, J., Seger, J., Sjostrom, M., and Ekblom B. Adaptive response in human skeletal muscle sub-
jected to prolonged eccentric training, Int. J. Sports Med., 4(3), 177– 183, 1983.
89. Friden, J., Sfakianos, P.N., and Hargens, A.R., Muscle soreness and intramuscular fluid pressure:
comparison between eccentric and concentric load, J. Appl. Physiol., 61(6), 2175–2179, 1986.
90. Friden, J., Sjostrom, M., and Ekblom, B., A morphological study of delayed muscle soreness, Experi-
entia, 37(5), 506–507, 1981.
91. Friden, J., Sjostrom, M., and Ekblom, B., Myofibrillar damage following intense eccentric exercise in
man, Int. J. Sports Med., 4(3), 170–176, 1983.
92. Fritz, V.K. and Stauber, W.T., Characterization of muscles injured by forced lengthening, II. Proteo-
glycans. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 20(4), 354– 361, 1988.
93. Gallucci, S., Provenzano, C., Mazzarelli, P., Scuderi, F., and Bartoccioni, E., Myoblasts produce IL-6
in response to inflammatory stimuli, Int. Immunol., 10(3), 267–273, 1998.
94. Garner, R.P., Terracio, L., Borg, T.K., and Buggy, J., Intracranial self-stimulation motivates weight-
lifting exercise in rats, J. Appl. Physiol., 71(4), 1627 –1631, 1991.
95. Garrett, W.E., Jr., Muscle strain injuries. Am. J. Sports Med., 24(6 Suppl), S2 –S8, 1996.
96. Geronilla, K.B., Miller, G.R., Mowrey, K., Kashon, M.L., and Cutlip, R.G., The Mechanical and
Histological Response of Rat Skeletal Muscle to Oscillatory Contractions, In: American College
of Sports Medicine; 2001, Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, Baltimore, MD 2001,
p. 474.
97. Geronilla, K.B., Miller, G.R., Mowrey, K.F., Wu, J.Z., Kashon, M.L., Brumbaugh, K. et al., Dynamic
force responses of skeletal muscle during stretch-shortening cycles, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 90(1–2),
144–153, 2003.
98. Giddings, C.J., Neaves, W.B., and Gonyea, W.J., Muscle fiber necrosis and regeneration induced by
prolonged weight-lifting exercise in the cat, Anat. Rec., 211(2), 133–141, 1985.
99. Gillis, J.M., Membrane abnormalities and ca homeostasis in muscles of the mdx mouse, an animal
model of the duchenne muscular dystrophy: a review, Acta Physiol. Scand., 156(3), 397–406, 1996.
100. Gonyea, W. and Bonde-Petersen, F., Alterations in muscle contractile properties and fiber compo-
sition after weight-lifting exercise in cats, Exp. Neurol., 59(1), 75 –84, 1978.
101. Gonyea, W.J., Role of exercise in inducing increases in skeletal muscle fiber number, J. Appl. Physiol.,
48(3), 421 –426, 1980.
102. Gonyea, W.J. and Ericson, G.C., An experimental model for the study of exercise-induced skeletal
muscle hypertrophy, J. Appl. Physiol., 40(4), 630–633, 1976.
103. Gordon, A.M., Huxley, A.F., and Julian, F.J., The variation in isometric tension with sarcomere
length in vertebrate muscle fibres, J. Physiol., 184(1), 170–192, 1966.
15-34 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
104. Gordon, E.E. Anatomical and biochemical adaptations of muscle to different exercises, Jama.,
201(10), 755 –758, 1967.
105. Gordon, E.E., Kowalski, K., and Fritts M., Adaptations of muscle to various exercises, Studies in rats.
Jama., 199(2), 103 –108, 1967.
106. Gordon, E.E., Kowalski, K., and Fritts, M., Changes in rat muscle fiber with forceful exercises, Arch.
Phys. Med. Rehabil., 48(11), 577 –582, 1967.
107. Gordon, E.E., Kowalski, K., and Fritts, M. Protein changes in quadriceps muscle of rat with repeti-
tive exercises, Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil., 48(6), 296– 303, 1967.
108. Gorospe, J.R., Tharp, M.D., Hinckley, J., Kornegay, J.N., and Hoffman, E.P., A role for mast cells in
the progression of Duchenne muscular dystrophy? Correlations in dystrophin-deficient humans,
dogs, and mice, J. Neurol. Sci., 122(1), 44 –56, 1994.
109. Gosselin, L.E., Attenuation of force deficit after lengthening contractions in soleus muscle from
trained rats, J. Appl. Physiol., 88(4), 1254–1258, 2000.
110. Gosselin, L.E. and Burton H., Impact of initial muscle length on force deficit following lengthening
contractions in mammalian skeletal muscle, Muscle Nerve, 25(6), 822 –827, 2002.
111. Hales, T.R. and Bernard, B.P., Epidemiology of work-related musculoskeletal disorders, Orthop.
Clin. North Am., 27(4), 679 –709, 1996.
112. Hasselgren, P.O., Role of the ubiquitin-proteasome pathway in sepsis-induced muscle catabolism,
Mol. Biol. Rep., 26(1– 2), 71 –76, 1999.
113. Hasselgren, P.O., Pedersen, P., Sax, H.C., Warner, B.W., and Fischer, J.E., Current concepts of
protein turnover and amino acid transport in liver and skeletal muscle during sepsis, Arch. Surg.,
123(8), 992 –999, 1988.
114. Hasselman, C.T., Best, T.M., Seaber, A.V., and Garrett, W.E., Jr. A threshold and continuum of
injury during active stretch of rabbit skeletal muscle, Am. J. Sports Med., 23(1), 65–73, 1995.
115. Henriksson, J., Knuttgen, H.G., and Bonde-Petersen, F., Perceived exertion during exercise with
concentric and eccentric muscle contractions, Ergonomics, 15(5), 537–544, 1972.
116. Hesselink, M.K., Kuipers, H., Geurten, P., and Van Straaten, H., Structural muscle damage and
muscle strength after incremental number of isometric and forced lengthening contractions, J.
Muscle Res. Cell. Motil., 17(3), 335 –341, 1996.
117. Hilder, T.L., Tou, J.C., Grindeland, R.E., Wade, C.E., and Graves, L.M., Phosphorylation of insulin
receptor substrate-1 serine 307 correlates with JNK activity in atrophic skeletal muscle, FEBS Lett.,
553(1– 2), 63 –67, 2003.
118. Hill, A.V., The heat of shortening and the dynamic constants of muscle, Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B,
126, 1938.
119. Hill, M., Wernig, A., and Goldspink, G., Muscle satellite (stem) cell activation during local tissue
injury and repair, J. Anat., 203(1), 89 –99, 2003.
120. Ho, K.W., Roy, R.R., Tweedle, C.D., Heusner, W.W., Van Huss, W.D., and Carrow, R.E., Skeletal
muscle fiber splitting with weight-lifting exercise in rats. Am. J. Anatomy, 157, 433–440, 1980.
121. Hoffman, E.P., Brown, R.H., Jr., and Kunkel, L.M., Dystrophin: the protein product of the
Duchenne muscular dystrophy locus, Cell, 51(6), 919– 928, 1987.
122. Hoffman, E.P., Knudson, C.M., Campbell, K.P., and Kunkel, L.M., Subcellular fractionation of dys-
trophin to the triads of skeletal muscle, Nature, 330(6150), 754–758, 1987.
123. Hopf, F.W., Reddy, P., Hong, J., and Steinhardt, R.A., A capacitative calcium current in cultured
skeletal muscle cells is mediated by the calcium-specific leak channel and inhibited by dihydropyr-
idine compounds, J. Biol. Chem., 271(37), 22358–22367, 1996.
124. Hopf, F.W., Turner, P.R., Denetclaw, W.F., Jr., Reddy, P., and Steinhardt, R.A., A critical evaluation
of resting intracellular free calcium regulation in dystrophic mdx muscle. Am. J. Physiol.,
271(4 Pt 1), C1325–C1339, 1996.
125. Horita, T., Komi, P.V., Nicol, C., and Kyrolainen, H., Effect of exhausting stretch-shortening cycle
exercise on the time course of mechanical behaviour in the drop jump: possible role of muscle
damage, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol., 79(2), 160 –167, 1999.
Soft-Tissue Pathomechanics 15-35
126. Hough, T., Ergographic Studies in Muscular Soreness. Am. J. Physiol., 7, 76 –92, 1902.
127. Howell, J.N., Chleboun, G., and Conatser R., Muscle stiffness, strength loss, swelling and soreness
following exercise-induced injury in humans. J. Physiol., 464, 183 –196, 1993.
128. Huijing, P.A., Architecture of the human gastrocnemius muscle and some functional consequences,
Acta Anat. (Basel), 123(2), 101–107, 1985.
129. Hunter, K.D. and Faulkner, J.A., Pliometric contraction-induced injury of mouse skeletal muscle:
effect of initial length, J. Appl. Physiol., 82(1), 278 –283, 1997.
130. Ingalls, C.P., Warren, G.L., and Armstrong, R.B., Dissociation of force production from MHC
and actin contents in muscles injured by eccentric contractions, J. Muscle Res. Cell Motil., 19(3),
215 –224, 1998.
131. Ingalls, C.P., Warren, G.L., and Armstrong, R.B., Intracellular Ca2þ transients in mouse soleus
muscle after hindlimb unloading and reloading, J. Appl. Physiol., 87(1), 386– 390, 1999.
132. Ingalls, C.P., Warren, G.L., Williams, J.H., Ward, C.W., and Armstrong, R.B., E-C coupling failure in
mouse EDL muscle after in vivo eccentric contractions, J. Appl. Physiol., 85(1), 58 –67, 1998.
133. Jensen, B.R., Schibye, B., Sogaard, K., Simonsen, E.B., and Sjogaard, G., Shoulder muscle load and
muscle fatigue among industrial sewing-machine operators, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol.,
67(5), 467 –475, 1993.
134. Jensen, B.R., Sjogaard, G., Bornmyr, S., Arborelius, M., and Jorgensen, K., Intramuscular laser-
Doppler flowmetry in the supraspinatus muscle during isometric contractions, Eur. J. Appl.
Physiol. Occup. Physiol., 71(4), 373 –378, 1995.
135. Jones, D.A., Jackson, M.J., McPhail, G., and Edwards, R.H., Experimental mouse muscle damage:
the importance of external calcium, Clin. Sci. (Lond), 66(3), 317 –322, 1984.
136. Jones, D.A., Newham, D.J., Round, J.M., and Tolfree, S.E., Experimental human muscle damage:
morphological changes in relation to other indices of damage, J. Physiol., 375, 435–448, 1986.
137. Kadi, F., Hagg, G., Hakansson, R., Holmner, S., Butler-Browne, G.S., and Thornell, L.-E., Structural
changes in male trapezius muscle with work-related myalgia, Acta Neurophathol., 95, 352– 360,
1998.
138. Keller, K., Corbett, J., and Nichols, D., Repetitive strain injury in computer keyboard users: Patho-
mechanics and treatment principles in individual and group intervention, J. Hand Therapy, 11,
9 –26, 1998.
139. Kissel, J.T., Lynn, D.J., Rammohan, K.W., Klein, J.P., Griggs, R.C., and Moxley, R.T., 3rd, et al.,
Mononuclear cell analysis of muscle biopsies in prednisone- and azathioprine-treated Duchenne
muscular dystrophy, Neurology, 43(3 Pt 1), 532 –536, 1993.
140. Klausen, K. and Knuttgen, H.G., Effect of training on oxygen consumption in negative muscular
work, Acta. Physiol. Scand., 83(3), 319 –323, 1971.
141. Klitgaard, H., A model for quantitative strength training of hindlimb muscles of the rat, J. Appl.
Physiol., 64(4), 1740–1745, 1988.
142. Knutsson, E., Analysis of spastic paresis. In: Proceedings of the Tenth International Congress of the
World Confederation for Physical Therapy, 1987, Sydney; 1987. p. 629– 633.
143. Knuttgen, H.G., Oxygen debt, lactate, pyruvate, and excess lactate after muscular work, J. Appl.
Physiol., 17, 639–644, 1962.
144. Knuttgen, H.G., Human performance in high-intensity exercise with concentric and eccentric
muscle contractions. Int. J. Sports Med., 7 Suppl 1, 6 –9, 1986.
145. Knuttgen, H.G. and Klausen K., Oxygen debt in short-term exercise with concentric and eccentric
muscle contractions, J. Appl. Physiol., 30(5), 632–635, 1971.
146. Knuttgen, H.G., Nadel, E.R., Pandolf, K.B., and Patton, J.F., Effects of training with eccentric muscle
contractions on exercise performance, energy expenditure, and body temperature. Int. J. Sports
Med., 3(1), 13 –17, 1982.
147. Knuttgen, H.G., Petersen, F.B., and Klausen K., Oxygen uptake and heart rate responses to
exercise performed with concentric and eccentric muscle contractions. Med. Sci. Sports, 3(1),
1 – 5, 1971.
15-36 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
148. Komi, P.V., Measurement of the force-velocity relationship in human muscle under concentric and
eccentric contractions, Medicine and Sport, 8, 224–229, 1973.
149. Komi, P.V., Stretch-shortening cycle: a powerful model to study normal and fatigued muscle,
J. Biomech., 33(10), 1197–1206, 2000.
150. Komi, P.V. and Buskirk, E.R., Effect of eccentric and concentric muscle conditioning on tension and
electrical activity of human muscle, Ergonomics, 15(4), 417 –434, 1972.
151. Komi, P.V. and Rusko, H., Quantitative evaluation of mechanical and electrical changes during
fatigue loading of eccentric and concentric work. Scand. J. Rehabil. Med., Suppl 3, 121–126, 1974.
152. Komulainen, J., Kalliokoski, R., Koskinen, S.O., Drost, M.R., Kuipers, H., and Hesselink, M.K.,
Controlled lengthening or shortening contraction-induced damage is followed by fiber hypertrophy
in rat skeletal muscle, Int. J. Sports Med., 21(2), 107–112, 2000.
153. Komulainen, J., Koskinen, S.O., Kalliokoski, R., Takala, T.E., and Vihko, V., Gender differences in
skeletal muscle fibre damage after eccentrically biased downhill running in rats, Acta. Physiol.
Scand., 165(1), 57–63, 1999.
154. Komulainen, J., Takala, T.E., Kuipers, H., and Hesselink, M.K., The disruption of myofibre
structures in rat skeletal muscle after forced lengthening contractions, Pflugers Arch., 436(5),
735 –741, 1998.
155. Kuipers, H. and Van der Meulen, J.H., Increased Passive Stiffness of Rat Muscle after Eccentric and
Isometric Work, Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 24(5), S1007, 1992.
156. Lapointe, B.M., Fremont, P., and Cote, C.H., Adaptation to lengthening contractions is indepen-
dent of voluntary muscle recruitment but relies on inflammation. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr.
Comp. Physiol., 282(1), R323– R329, 2002.
157. Lapointe, B.M., Frenette, J., and Cote, C.H., Lengthening contraction-induced inflammation is
linked to secondary damage but devoid of neutrophil invasion, J. Appl. Physiol., 92(5), 1995–
2004, 2002.
158. Larsson, B., Bjork, J., Henriksson, K.G., Gerdle, B. and Lindman, R., The prevalences of cytochrome
c oxidase negative and superpositive fibres and ragged-red fibres in the trapezius muscle of female
cleaners with and without myalgia and of female healthy controls, Pain, 84(2–3), 379 –387, 2000.
159. Larsson, S.E., Bodegard, L., Henriksson, K.G., and Oberg, P.A., Chronic trapezius myalgia.
Morphology and blood flow studied in 17 patients, Acta Orthop. Scand., 61(5), 394 –398, 1990.
160. Lazarides, E., Intermediate filaments as mechanical integrators of cellular space, Nature, 283(5744),
249 –256, 1980.
161. Lieber, R.L. and Friden, J., Selective damage of fast glycolytic muscle fibres with eccentric contrac-
tion of the rabbit tibialis anterior, Acta Physiol. Scand., 133(4), 587 –588, 1988.
162. Lieber, R.L. and Friden, J., Muscle damage is not a function of muscle force but active muscle strain,
J. Appl. Physiol., 74(2), 520 –526, 1993.
163. Lieber, R.L., Schmitz, M.C., Mishra, D.K., and Friden, J., Contractile and cellular remodeling in
rabbit skeletal muscle after cyclic eccentric contractions, J. Appl. Physiol., 77(4), 1926– 1934, 1994.
164. Lieber, R.L., Thornell, L.E., and Friden, J., Muscle cytoskeletal disruption occurs within the first
15 min of cyclic eccentric contraction, J. Appl. Physiol., 80(1), 278 –284, 1996.
165. Lieber, R.L., Woodburn, T.M., and Friden, J., Muscle damage induced by eccentric contractions of
25% strain, J. Appl. Physiol., 70(6), 2498 –2507, 1991.
166. Lowe, D.A., and Alway, S.E., Stretch-induced myogenin, MyoD, and MRF4 expression and acute
hypertrophy in quail slow-tonic muscle are not dependent upon satellite cell proliferation, Cell
Tissue Res., 296(3), 531 –539, 1999.
167. Lowe, D.A., Warren, G.L., Hayes, D.A., Farmer, M.A., and Armstrong, R.B., Eccentric contraction-
induced injury of mouse soleus muscle: effect of varying [Ca2þ]o, J. Appl. Physiol., 76(4), 1445 –
1453, 1994.
168. Lowe, D.A., Warren, G.L., Ingalls, C.P., Boorstein, D.B., and Armstrong, R.B., Muscle function and
protein metabolism after initiation of eccentric contraction-induced injury, Journal of Applied
Physiology, 79(4), 1260 –1270, 1995.
Soft-Tissue Pathomechanics 15-37
169. Lynch, G.S. and Faulkner, J.A., Contraction-induced injury to single muscle fibers: velocity of
stretch does not influence the force deficit, Am. J. Physiol., 275(6 Pt 1), C1548–C1554, 1998.
170. Lynn, R. and Morgan, D.L., Decline running produces more sarcomeres in rat vastus intermedius
muscle fibers than does incline running, J. Appl. Physiol., 77(3), 1439 –1444, 1994.
171. Macpherson, P.C., Dennis, R.G., and Faulkner, J.A., Sarcomere dynamics and contraction-induced
injury to maximally activated single muscle fibres from soleus muscles of rats, J. Physiol., 500 (Pt 2),
523 –533, 1997.
172. Macpherson, P.C., Schork, M.A., and Faulkner, J.A., Contraction-induced injury to single fiber seg-
ments from fast and slow muscles of rats by single stretches, Am. J. Physiol., 271(5 Pt 1), C1438–
C1446, 1996.
173. Mair, J., Koller, A., Artner-Dworzak, E., Haid, C., Wicke, K., Judmaier, W. et al., Effects of exercise
on plasma myosin heavy chain fragments and MRI of skeletal muscle, J. Appl. Physiol., 72(2),
656 –663, 1992.
174. Mair, J., Mayr, M., Muller, E., Koller, A., Haid, C., Artner-Dworzak, E. et al., Rapid adaptation to
eccentric exercise-induced muscle damage, Int. J. Sports Med., 16(6), 352– 356, 1995.
175. Malm, C., Nyberg, P., Engstrom, M., Sjodin, B., Lenkei, R., Ekblom, B. et al., Immunological
changes in human skeletal muscle and blood after eccentric exercise and multiple biopsies,
J. Physiol., 529 Pt 1, 243–262, 2000.
176. McArdle, A., van der Meulen, J.H., Catapano, M., Symons, M.C., Faulkner, J.A., and Jackson, M.J.,
Free radical activity following contraction-induced injury to the extensor digitorum longus muscles
of rats, Free Radic. Biol. Med., 26(9-10), 1085 –1091, 1999.
177. McCully, K.K. and Faulkner, J.A., Injury to skeletal muscle fibers of mice following lengthening con-
tractions, J. Appl. Physiol., 59(1), 119 –126, 1985.
178. McCully, K.K. and Faulkner, J.A., Characteristics of lengthening contractions associated with injury
to skeletal muscle fibers, J. Appl. Physiol., 61(1), 293 –299, 1986.
179. McHugh, M.P., Connolly, D.A., Eston, R.G., and Gleim, G.W., Exercise-induced muscle damage and
potential mechanisms for the repeated bout effect, Sports Medicine, 27(3), 157 –170, 1999.
180. Medina, R., Wing, S.S., and Goldberg, A.L., Increase in levels of polyubiquitin and proteasome
mRNA in skeletal muscle during starvation and denervation atrophy, Biochem. J., 307 (Pt 3),
631 –637, 1995.
181. Mishra, D.K., Friden, J., Schmitz, M.C., and Lieber, R.L., Anti-inflammatory medication after
muscle injury. A treatment resulting in short-term improvement but subsequent loss of muscle
function, J. Bone Joint Surg. Am., 77(10), 1510 –1519, 1995.
182. Moldawer, L.L., Svaninger, G., Gelin, J., and Lundholm, K.G., Interleukin 1 and tumor necrosis
factor do not regulate protein balance in skeletal muscle, Am. J. Physiol., 253(6 Pt 1), C766 –
C773, 1987.
183. Morgan, D.L., New insights into the behavior of muscle during active lengthening, Biophys. J.,
57(2), 209 –221, 1990.
184. Mutsaers, S.E., Bishop, J.E., McGrouther, G., and Laurent, G.J., Mechanisms of tissue repair: from
wound healing to fibrosis, International Journal of Biochemistry & Cell Biology, 29(1), 5 –17, 1997.
185. Myllyla, R., Salminen, A., Peltonen, L., Takala, T.E., and Vihko, V., Collagen metabolism of mouse
skeletal muscle during the repair of exercise injuries, Pflugers Arch., 407(1), 64 –70, 1986.
186. Newham, D.J., Jones, D.A., and Clarkson, P.M., Repeated high-force eccentric exercise: effects on
muscle pain and damage, J. Appl. Physiol., 63(4), 1381–1386, 1987.
187. Newham, D.J., Jones, D.A., and Edwards, R.H., Large delayed plasma creatine kinase changes
after stepping exercise, Muscle Nerve, 6(5), 380– 385, 1983.
188. Newham, D.J., Jones, D.A., Ghosh, G., and Aurora, P., Muscle fatigue and pain after eccentric
contractions at long and short length, Clin. Sci. (Lond), 74(5), 553 –557, 1988.
189. Newham, D.J., Jones, D.A., Tolfree, S.E., and Edwards, R.H., Skeletal muscle damage: a study of
isotope uptake, enzyme efflux and pain after stepping, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol.,
55(1), 106 –112, 1986.
15-38 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
190. Newham, D.J., McPhail, G., Mills, K.R., and Edwards, R.H., Ultrastructural changes after concentric
and eccentric contractions of human muscle, J. Neurol. Sci., 61(1), 109– 122, 1983.
191. Nikolaou, P.K., Macdonald, B.L., Glisson, R.R., Seaber, A.V., and Garrett, W.E., Jr., Biomechanical
and histological evaluation of muscle after controlled strain injury, Am. J. Sports Med., 15(1), 9 –14,
1987.
192. Noonan, T.J., Best, T.M., Seaber, A.V., and Garrett, W.E. Jr., Identification of a threshold for skeletal
muscle injury, Am. J. Sports Med., 22(2), 257 –261, 1994.
193. Nosaka, K. and Clarkson, P.M., Effect of eccentric exercise on plasma enzyme activities previously
elevated by eccentric exercise, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol., 69(6), 492–497, 1994.
194. Nosaka, K. and Clarkson, P.M., Muscle damage following repeated bouts of high force eccentric
exercise, Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 27(9), 1263 –1269, 1995.
195. Nosaka, K. and Clarkson, P.M., Changes in indicators of inflammation after eccentric exercise of the
elbow flexors, Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 28(8), 953 –961, 1996.
196. Nosaka, K. and Clarkson, P.M., Variability in serum creatine kinase response after eccentric exercise
of the elbow flexors, International Journal of Sports Medicine, 17(2), 120 –127, 1996.
197. Nosaka, K., Clarkson, P.M., and Apple, F.S., Time course of serum protein changes after strenuous
exercise of the forearm flexors, Journal of Laboratory & Clinical Medicine, 119(2), 183 –188, 1992.
198. Nosaka, K., Clarkson, P.M., McGuiggin, M.E., and Byrne, J.M., Time course of muscle adaptation
after high force eccentric exercise, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol., 63(1), 70 –76, 1991.
199. Nurenberg, P., Giddings, C.J., Stray-Gundersen, J., Fleckenstein, J.L., Gonyea, W.J., and Peshock,
R.M., MR imaging-guided muscle biopsy for correlation of increased signal intensity with ultra-
structural change and delayed-onset muscle soreness after exercise, Radiology, 184(3), 865 –869,
1992.
200. Oak, S.A., Zhou, Y.W., and Jarrett, H.W., Skeletal muscle signaling pathway through the dystrophin
glycoprotein complex and Rac1, J. Biol. Chem., 278(41), 39287 –39295, 2003.
201. Obremsky, W.T., Seaber, A.V., Ribbeck, B.M., and Garrett, W.E., Jr., Biomechanical and histologic
assessment of a controlled muscle strain injury treated with piroxicam, Am. J. Sports Med., 22(4),
558 –561, 1994.
202. Ogilvie, R.W., Armstrong, R.B., Baird, K.E., and Bottoms, C.L., Lesions in the rat soleus muscle fol-
lowing eccentrically biased exercise, Am. J. Anat., 182(4), 335 –346, 1988.
203. Ortenblad, N., Sjogaard, G., and Madsen, K., Impaired sarcoplasmic reticulum Ca(2þ) release rate
after fatiguing stimulation in rat skeletal muscle, J. Appl. Physiol., 89(1), 210–217, 2000.
204. Ortenblad, N., Young, J.F., Oksbjerg, N., Nielsen, J.H., and Lambert, I.H., Reactive oxygen species
are important mediators of taurine release from skeletal muscle cells, Am. J. Physiol. Cell Physiol.,
284(6), C1362– C1373, 2003.
205. Pardo, J.V., Siliciano, J.D., and Craig, S.W., A vinculin-containing cortical lattice in skeletal muscle:
transverse lattice elements (“costameres”) mark sites of attachment between myofibrils and sarco-
lemma, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 80(4), 1008 –1012, 1983.
206. Patel, T.J., Cuizon, D., Mathieu-Costello, O., Friden, J., and Lieber, R.L., Increased oxidative
capacity does not protect skeletal muscle fibers from eccentric contraction-induced injury, Am.
J. Physiol., 274(5 Pt 2), R1300 –R1308, 1998.
207. Patel, T.J. and Lieber, R.L., Force transmission in skeletal muscle: from actomyosin to external
tendons, Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev., 25, 321–363, 1997.
208. Petersen, F.B. and Knuttgen, H.G., Effect of training with eccentric muscle contractions on human
skeletal muscle metabolites, Acta. Physiol. Scand., 80(4), 16A– 17A, 1970.
209. Peterson, J.M., Trappe, T.A., Mylona, E., White, F., Lambert, C.P., Evans, W.J. et al., Ibuprofen and
acetaminophen: effect on muscle inflammation after eccentric exercise, Med. Sci. Sports Exerc.,
35(6), 892 –896, 2003.
210. Petrof, B.J., Shrager, J.B., Stedman, H.H., Kelly, A.M., and Sweeney, H.L., Dystrophin protects the
sarcolemma from stresses developed during muscle contraction, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 90(8),
3710 –3714, 1993.
Soft-Tissue Pathomechanics 15-39
211. Piligian, G., Herbert, R., Hearns, M., Dropkin, J., Landsbergis, P., and Cherniack, M., Evaluation
and management of chronic work-related musculoskeletal disorders of the distal upper extremity,
American Journal of Industrial Medicine, 37(1), 75 –93, 2000.
212. Porter, J.D., Khanna, S., Kaminski, H.J., Rao, J.S., Merriam, A.P., Richmonds, C.R. et al., A chronic
inflammatory response dominates the skeletal muscle molecular signature in dystrophin-deficient
mdx mice, Hum. Mol. Genet., 11(3), 263 –272, 2002.
213. Prisk, V. and Huard, J., Muscle injuries and repair: the role of prostaglandins and inflammation,
Histol. Histopathol., 18(4), 1243– 1256, 2003.
214. Quinn, L.S., Anderson, B.G., Drivdahl, R.H., Alvarez, B., and Argiles, J.M., Overexpression of inter-
leukin-15 induces skeletal muscle hypertrophy in vitro: implications for treatment of muscle
wasting disorders, Exp. Cell Res., 280(1), 55 –63, 2002.
215. Quinn, L.S., Haugk, K.L., and Grabstein, K.H., Interleukin-15: a novel anabolic cytokine for skeletal
muscle, Endocrinology, 136(8), 3669 –3672, 1995.
216. Rabinovsky, E.D., Gelir, E., Gelir, S., Lui, H., Kattash, M., DeMayo, F.J. et al., Targeted expression
of IGF-1 transgene to skeletal muscle accelerates muscle and motor neuron regeneration, Faseb
J., 17(1), 53 –55, 2003.
217. Rafael, J.A. and Brown, S.C., Dystrophin and utrophin: genetic analyses of their role in skeletal
muscle, Microsc. Res. Tech., 48(3–4), 155 –166, 2000.
218. Rafael, J.A., Townsend, E.R., Squire, S.E., Potter, A.C., Chamberlain, J.S., and Davies, K.E., Dystro-
phin and utrophin influence fiber type composition and post-synaptic membrane structure, Hum.
Mol. Genet., 9(9), 1357– 1367, 2000.
219. Reddy, A.S., Reedy, M.K., Best, T.M., Seaber, A.V., Garrett, W.E., Jr. Restriction of the injury
response following an acute muscle strain, Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 25(3), 321 –327, 1993.
220. Rinard, J., Clarkson, P.M., Smith, L.L., and Grossman, M., Response of males and females to high-
force eccentric exercise, J. Sports Sci., 18(4), 229 –236, 2000.
221. Rodenburg, J.B., Bar, P.R., and De Boer, R.W., Relations between muscle soreness and biochemical
and functional outcomes of eccentric exercise, J. Appl. Physiol., 74(6), 2976 –2983, 1993.
222. Rodenburg, J.B., Steenbeek, D., Schiereck, P., and Bar, P.R., Warm-up, stretching and massage
diminish harmful effects of eccentric exercise, Int. J. Sports Med., 15(7), 414 –419, 1994.
223. Rogers, M.A., Stull, G.A., and Apple, F.S., Creatine kinase isoenzyme activities in men and women
following a marathon race, Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 17(6), 679 –682, 1985.
224. Round, J.M., Jones, D.A., and Cambridge, G., Cellular infiltrates in human skeletal muscle: exercise
induced damage as a model for inflammatory muscle disease? J. Neurol. Sci., 82(1–3), 1–11, 1987.
225. Sadeh, M., Czyewski, K., and Stern, L.Z., Chronic myopathy induced by repeated bupivacaine injec-
tions, J. Neurol. Sci., 67(2), 229– 238, 1985.
226. Sam, M., Shah, S., Friden, J., Milner, D.J., Capetanaki, Y., and Lieber, R.L., Desmin knockout
muscles generate lower stress and are less vulnerable to injury compared with wild-type muscles,
Am. J. Physiol. Cell Physiol., 279(4), C1116– C1122, 2000.
227. Sargeant, A.J. and Dolan, P., Human muscle function following prolonged eccentric exercise, Eur.
J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol., 56(6), 704 –711, 1987.
228. Sato, K. and Kawashima, S., Calpain function in the modulation of signal transduction molecules,
Biol. Chem., 382(5), 743 –751, 2001.
229. Sato, S., Yanagisawa, K., and Miyatake, T., Conversion of myelin-associated glycoprotein (MAG) to
a smaller derivative by calcium activated neutral protease (CANP)-like enzyme in myelin and
inhibition by E-64 analogue, Neurochem. Res., 9(5), 629 –635, 1984.
230. Savage, K.J. and Clarkson, P.M., Oral contraceptive use and exercise-induced muscle damage and
recovery, Contraception, 66(1), 67 –71, 2002.
231. Saxton, J.M., Clarkson, P.M., James, R., Miles, M., Westerfer, M., Clark, S., et al. Neuromuscular
dysfunction following eccentric exercise, Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 27(8), 1185– 1193, 1995.
232. Sayeed, M.M., Alterations in calcium signaling and cellular responses in septic injury, New Horiz.,
4(1), 72 –86, 1996.
15-40 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
233. Sayers, S.P. and Clarkson, P.M., Force recovery after eccentric exercise in males and females. Eur.
J. Appl. Physiol., 84(1–2), 122–126, 2001.
234. Schwane, J.A. and Armstrong, R.B., Effect of training on skeletal muscle injury from downhill
running in rats, J. Appl. Physiol., 55(3), 969–975, 1983.
235. Scifres, J.C. and Martin, D.T., Velocity of Eccentric Contraction affects the Magnitude of Delayed
Onset Muscle Soreness. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 24(5), S851, 1992.
236. Sheehan, S.M. and Allen, R.E., Skeletal muscle satellite cell proliferation in response to members of
the fibroblast growth factor family and hepatocyte growth factor, J. Cell Physiol., 181(3), 499–506,
1999.
237. Sherwood, L., Human Physiology: From cells to systems. 4th ed., Brooks –Cole, Pacific Grove, CA,
2001.
238. Singh, M. and Karpovich, P.V., Isotonic and isometric forces of forearm flexors and extensors, J.
Appl. Physiol., 21(4), 1435 –1437, 1966.
239. Sjogaard, G., Muscle energy metabolism and electrolyte shifts during low-level prolonged static
contraction in man, Acta Physiol. Scand., 134(2), 181–187, 1988.
240. Sjogaard, G. and Jensen, B.R., Muscle Pathology with Overuse. In: Ranney, D., editor. Chronic Mus-
culoskeletal Injuries in the Workplace. Philadelphia, PA: W.B. Saunders Company; 1997. p. 17 –40.
241. Sjogaard, G., Lundberg, U., and Kadefors, R., The role of muscle activity and mental load in the
development of pain and degenerative processes at the muscle cell level during computer work,
Eur. J. Appl. Physiol., 83(2–3), 99 –105, 2000.
242. Sjogaard, G. and McComas, A.J., Role of interstitial potassium, Adv. Exp. Med. Biol., 384, 69 –80,
1995.
243. Sjogaard, G. and Sogaard, K., Muscle injury in repetitive motion disorders, Clin. Orthop., (351),
21 –31, 1998.
244. Smith, H.J. and Tisdale, M.J., Signal transduction pathways involved in proteolysis-inducing factor
induced proteasome expression in murine myotubes, Br. J. Cancer., 89(9), 1783–1788, 2003.
245. Smith, L.L., Fulmer, M.G., Holbert, D., McCammon, M.R., Houmard, J.A., Frazer, D.D. et al., The
impact of a repeated bout of eccentric exercise on muscular strength, muscle soreness and creatine
kinase, Br. J. Sports Med., 28(4), 267 –271, 1994.
246. Sola, O.M., Christensen, D.L., and Martin, A.W., Hypertrophy and hyperplasia of adult chicken
anterior latissimus dorsi muscles following stretch with and without denervation, Exp. Neurol.,
41(1), 76 –100, 1973.
247. Sorichter, S., Mair, J., Koller, A., Calzolari, C., Huonker, M., Pau, B. et al., Release of muscle
proteins after downhill running in male and female subjects, Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports, 11(1),
28 –32, 2001.
248. Sorichter, S., Puschendorf, B., and Mair, J., Skeletal muscle injury induced by eccentric muscle
action: muscle proteins as markers of muscle fiber injury, Exercise Immunology Review, 5, 5 –21,
1999.
249. Spencer, M.J., Montecino-Rodriguez, E., Dorshkind, K., and Tidball, J.G., Helper (CD4(þ)) and
cytotoxic (CD8(þ)) T cells promote the pathology of dystrophin-deficient muscle, Clin.
Immunol., 98(2), 235 –243, 2001.
250. Spencer, M.J. and Tidball, J.G., Do immune cells promote the pathology of dystrophin-deficient
myopathies? Neuromuscul. Disord., 11(6–7), 556 –564, 2001.
251. St. Pierre, B.A. and Tidball, J.G., Macrophage activation and muscle remodeling at myotendinous
junctions after modifications in muscle loading, Am. J. Pathol., 145(6), 1463–1471, 1994.
252. St. Pierre Schneider, B., Correia, L.A., and Cannon, J.G., Sex differences in leukocyte invasion in
injured murine skeletal muscle, Res. Nurs. Health, 22(3), 243 –250, 1999.
253. Stauber, W.T., Eccentric action of muscles: physiology, injury, and adaptation, Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev.,
17, 157 –185, 1989.
254. Stauber, W.T., Measurement of Muscle Function in Man. In: Sports Injuries, International Perspec-
tives in Physical Therapy: Churchhill-Livingstone; 1989.
Soft-Tissue Pathomechanics 15-41
255. Stauber, W.T., Fritz, V.K., Vogelbach, D.W., and Dahlmann, B., Characterization of
muscles injured by forced lengthening I. Cellular infiltrates, Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 20(4), 345–
353, 1988.
256. Stauber, W.T., Knack, K.K., Miller, G.R., and Grimmett, J.G., Fibrosis and intercellular collagen
connections from four weeks of muscle strains, Muscle Nerve, 19(4), 423 –430, 1996.
257. Stauber, W.T., and Smith, C.A., Cellular responses in exertion-induced skeletal muscle injury, Mol.
Cell Biochem., 179(1–2), 189 –196, 1998.
258. Stevens, E.D., Relation between work and power calculated from force-velocity curves to that done
during oscillatory work, J. Muscle. Res. Cell. Motil., 14(5), 518– 526, 1993.
259. Stevens, E.D., Effect of phase of stimulation on acute damage caused by eccentric contractions in
mouse soleus muscle, J. Appl. Physiol., 80(6), 1958 –1962, 1996.
260. Stevens, E.D., and Syme, D.A., Effect of stimulus duty cycle and cycle frequency on power output
during fatigue in rat diaphragm muscle doing oscillatory work, Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol., 71(12),
910 –916, 1993.
261. Stone, M.H. and Lipner, H., Responses to intensive training and methandrostenelone adminis-
tration. I. Contractile and performance variables. Pflugers Arch., 375(2), 141 –146, 1978.
262. Stone, M.H., Rush, M.E., and Lipner, H., Responses to intensive training and methandrostenelone
administration: II. Hormonal, organ weights, muscle weights and body composition, Pflugers Arch.,
375(2), 147 –151, 1978.
263. Stupka, N., Lowther, S., Chorneyko, K., Bourgeois, J.M., Hogben, C., and Tarnopolsky, M.A.,
Gender differences in muscle inflammation after eccentric exercise, J. Appl. Physiol., 89(6),
2325 –2332, 2000.
264. Sun, J.S., Tsuang, Y.H., Hang, Y.S., Liu, T.K., Lee, W.W., and Cheng, C.K., The deleterious effect of
tetanic contraction on rabbit’s triceps surae muscle during cyclic loading, Clin. Biomech. (Bristol,
Avon), 11(1), 46– 50, 1996.
265. Talbot, J.A. and Morgan, D.L., Quantitative analysis of sarcomere non-uniformities in active muscle
following a stretch, J. Muscle Res. Cell Motil., 17(2), 261 –268, 1996.
266. Tamaki, T., Uchiyama, S., and Nakano, S., A weight-lifting exercise model for inducing hypertrophy
in the hindlimb muscles of rats, Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 24(8), 881 –886, 1992.
267. Tate, C.A., Bonner, H.W., and Leslie, S.W., Calcium uptake in skeletal muscle mitochondria. I. The
effects of chelating agents on the mitochondria from fatigued rats, Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup.
Physiol., 39(2), 111– 116, 1978.
268. Thompson, C.B., Choi, C., Youn, J.H., and McDonough, A.A., Temporal responses of oxidative vs.
glycolytic skeletal muscles to Kþ deprivation: Naþ pumps and cell cations, Am. J. Physiol., 276(6 Pt
1), C1411–C1419, 1999.
269. Tidball, J.G., Inflammatory cell response to acute muscle injury, Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 27(7),
1022 –1032, 1995.
270. Tiidus, P.M., Can estrogens diminish exercise induced muscle damage? Can. J. Appl. Physiol., 20(1),
26 –38, 1995.
271. Tiidus, P.M. and Bombardier, E., Oestrogen attenuates post-exercise myeloperoxidase activity in
skeletal muscle of male rats, Acta. Physiol. Scand., 166(2), 85–90, 1999.
272. Tiku, K., Tiku, M.L., Liu, S., and Skosey, J.L., Normal human neutrophils are a source of a specific
interleukin 1 inhibitor, J. Immunol., 136(10), 3686 –3692, 1986.
273. Trappe, T.A., Fluckey, J.D., White, F., Lambert, C.P., and Evans, W.J., Skeletal muscle PGF(2)(alpha)
and PGE(2) in response to eccentric resistance exercise: influence of ibuprofen acetaminophen,
J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab., 86(10), 5067 –5070, 2001.
274. Trappe, T.A., White, F., Lambert, C.P., Cesar, D., Hellerstein, M., and Evans, W.J., Effect of ibuprofen
and acetaminophen on postexercise muscle protein synthesis, Am. J. Physiol. Endocrinol. Metab.,
282(3), E551 –E556, 2002.
275. Triolo, R., Robinson, D., Gardner, E., and Betz, R., The eccentric strength of electrically stimulated
paralyzed muscle, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 651 –652, 1987.
15-42 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
276. van der Meulen, J.H., Exercise Induced Muscle Damage: Morphological, Biochemical, and Func-
tional Aspects [Doctoral Dissertation]. Amsterdam: Universiteit van Amsterdam; 1991.
277. van der Meulen, J.H., Kuipers, H., and Drukker, J., Relationship between exercise-induced muscle
damage and enzyme release in rats, J. Appl. Physiol., 71(3), 999–1004, 1991.
278. van der Meulen, J.H., McArdle, A., Jackson, M.J., and Faulkner, J.A., Contraction-induced injury
to the extensor digitorum longus muscles of rats: the role of vitamin E, J. Appl. Physiol., 83(3),
817 –823, 1997.
279. van der Vusse, G.J., de Groot, M.J., Willemsen, P.H., van Bilsen, M., Schrijvers, A.H., and Reneman, R.S.,
Degradation of phospholipids and triacylglycerol, and accumulation of fatty acids in anoxic
myocardial tissue, disrupted by freeze-thawing, Mol. Cell Biochem., 88(1 – 2), 83 – 90, 1989.
280. van der Vusse, G.J., van Bilsen, M., and Reneman, R.S., Is Phospholipid Degradation a Critical
Event in Ischemia and Reperfusion-Induced Damage? News in Physiological Sciences, 49 –53, 1989.
281. Vijayan, K., Thompson, J.L., Norenberg, K.M., Fitts, R.H., and Riley, D.A., Fiber-type susceptibility
to eccentric contraction-induced damage of hindlimb-unloaded rat AL muscles, J. Appl. Physiol.,
90(3), 770 –776, 2001.
282. Vollestad, N.K. and Sejersted, O.M., Biochemical correlates of fatigue. A brief review, Eur. J. Appl.
Physiol. Occup. Physiol., 57(3), 336 –347, 1988.
283. Warren, G.L., 3rd, Williams, J.H., Ward, C.W., Matoba, H., Ingalls, C.P., Hermann, K.M., et al.
Decreased contraction economy in mouse EDL muscle injured by eccentric contractions, J. Appl.
Physiol., 81(6), 2555 –2564, 1996.
284. Warren, G.L., Hayes, D.A., Lowe, D.A., and Armstrong, R.B., Mechanical factors in the initiation of
eccentric contraction-induced injury in rat soleus muscle, J. Physiol., 464, 457 –475, 1993.
285. Warren, G.L., Hayes, D.A., Lowe, D.A., Prior, B.M., and Armstrong, R.B., Materials fatigue initiates
eccentric contraction-induced injury in rat soleus muscle, J. Physiol., 464, 477 –489, 1993.
286. Warren, G.L., Hayes, D.A., Lowe, D.A., Williams, J.H., and Armstrong, R.B., Eccentric contraction-
induced injury in normal and hindlimb-suspended mouse soleus and EDL muscles, J. Appl.
Physiol., 77(3), 1421 –1430, 1994.
287. Warren, G.L., Ingalls, C.P., and Armstrong, R.B., Temperature dependency of force loss and Ca(2þ)
homeostasis in mouse EDL muscle after eccentric contractions, Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp.
Physiol., 282(4), R1122 –R1132, 2002.
288. Warren, G.L., Ingalls, C.P., Shah, S.J., and Armstrong, R.B., Uncoupling of in vivo torque pro-
duction from EMG in mouse muscles injured by eccentric contractions, J. Physiol., 515 (Pt 2),
609 –619, 1999.
289. Warren, G.L., Lowe, D.A., and Armstrong, R.B., Measurement tools used in the study of eccentric
contraction-induced injury, Sports Medicine, 27(1), 43 –59, 1999.
290. Warren, G.L., Lowe, D.A., Hayes, D.A., Farmer, M.A., and Armstrong, R.B., Redistribution of cell
membrane probes following contraction-induced injury of mouse soleus muscle, Cell & Tissue
Research, 282(2), 311–320, 1995.
291. Warren, G.L., Lowe, D.A., Hayes, D.A., Karwoski, C.J., Prior, B.M., and Armstrong, R.B., Excitation
failure in eccentric contraction-induced injury of mouse soleus muscle, J. Physiol., 468, 487 –499,
1993.
292. Whitehead, N.P., Allen, T.J., Morgan, D.L., and Proske, U., Damage to human muscle from eccentric
exercise after training with concentric exercise, J. Physiol., 512 (Pt 2), 615– 620, 1998.
293. Wickiewicz, T.L., Roy, R.R., Powell, P.L., and Edgerton, V.R., Muscle architecture of the human
lower limb, Clin. Orthop., (179), 275 –583, 1983.
294. Widrick, J.J., Costill, D.L., McConell, G.K., Anderson, D.E., Pearson, D.R., and Zachwieja, J.J., Time
course of glycogen accumulation after eccentric exercise, J. Appl. Physiol., 72(5), 1999–2004, 1992.
295. Wilkie, D.R., The relation between force and velocity in human muscle, J. Physiol., 110,
249 –280, 1950.
296. Willems, M.E. and Stauber, W.T., Isometric and concentric performance of electrically stimulated
ankle plantar flexor muscles in intact rat, Exp. Physiol., 84(2), 379–389, 1999.
Soft-Tissue Pathomechanics 15-43
297. Willems, M.E. and Stauber, W.T., Changes in force by repeated stretches of skeletal muscle in young
and old female Sprague Dawley rats, Aging (Milano), 12(6), 478– 481, 2000.
298. Willems, M.E. and Stauber, W.T., Force deficits after repeated stretches of activated skeletal muscles
in female and male rats, Acta Physiol. Scand., 172(1), 63 –67, 2001.
299. Willems, M.E. and Stauber, W.T., Fatigue and recovery at long and short muscle lengths after
eccentric training, Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 34(11), 1738–43, 2002.
300. Williams, P., Kyberd, P., Simpson, H., Kenwright, J., and Goldspink, G., The morphological basis of
increased stiffness of rabbit tibialis anterior muscles during surgical limb-lengthening, J. Anat., 193
(Pt 1), 131 –138, 1998.
301. Wirth, O., Gregory, E.W., Cutlip, R.G., and Miller, G.R., Control and quantitation of voluntary
weight-lifting performance of rats, J. Appl. Physiol., 95(1), 402 –412, 2003.
302. Wong, T.S. and Booth, F.W., Skeletal muscle enlargement with weight-lifting exercise by rats,
J. Appl. Physiol., 65(2), 950–954, 1988.
303. Wong, T.S. and Booth, F.W., Protein metabolism in rat gastrocnemius muscle after stimulated
chronic concentric exercise, J. Appl. Physiol., 69(5), 1709 –1717, 1990.
304. Wong, T.S. and Booth, F.W., Protein metabolism in rat tibialis anterior muscle after stimulated
chronic eccentric exercise, J. Appl. Physiol., 69(5), 1718–1724, 1990.
305. Wood, S.A., Morgan, D.L., and Proske, U., Effects of repeated eccentric contractions on structure
and mechanical properties of toad sartorius muscle, Am. J. Physiol., 265(3 Pt 1), C792–C800, 1993.
306. Wright, J., Huang, Q.Q., and Wang, K., Nebulin is a full-length template of actin filaments in the
skeletal muscle sarcomere: an immunoelectron microscopic study of its orientation and span with
site-specific monoclonal antibodies, J. Muscle. Res. Cell Motil., 14(5), 476 –483, 1993.
307. Wung, B.S., Cheng, J.J., Hsieh, H.J., Shyy, Y.J., and Wang, D.L., Cyclic strain-induced monocyte che-
motactic protein-1 gene expression in endothelial cells involves reactive oxygen species activation of
activator protein 1, Circ. Res., 81(1), 1 –7, 1997.
308. Yarasheski, K.E., Lemon, P.W., and Gilloteaux, J., Effect of heavy-resistance exercise training on
muscle fiber composition in young rats, J. Appl. Physiol., 69(2), 434 –437, 1990.
309. Yeung, E.W., Bourreau, J.P., Allen, D.G., and Ballard, H.J., Effect of eccentric contraction-induced
injury on force and intracellular pH in rat skeletal muscles, J. Appl. Physiol., 92(1), 93 –99, 2002.
310. Zador, E., Mendler, L., Takacs, V., de Bleecker, J., and Wuytack, F., Regenerating soleus and extensor
digitorum longus muscles of the rat show elevated levels of TNF-alpha and its receptors, TNFR-60
and TNFR-80, Muscle & Nerve, 24(8), 1058 –1067, 2001.
311. Zajac, F.E., Muscle and tendon: properties, models, scaling, and application to biomechanics and
motor control, Crit. Rev. Biomed. Eng., 17(4), 359– 411, 1989.
312. Zeng, C., Pesall, J.E., Gilkerson, K.K., and McFarland, D.C., The effect of hepatocyte growth factor
on turkey satellite cell proliferation and differentiation, Poult. Sci., 81(8), 1191–1198, 2002.
313. Zerba, E. and Faulkner, J.A., A Single Lengthening Contraction can Induce Injury to Skeletal Muscle
Fibers, Physiologist, 33, A122, 1990.
314. Zerba, E., Komorowski, T.E., and Faulkner, J.A., Free radical injury to skeletal muscles of young,
adult, and old mice, Am. J. Physiol., 258(3 Pt 1), C429–C435, 1990.
16
Mechanisms for
Pain and Injury in
Musculoskeletal
Disorders
Painful musculoskeletal disorders are a common problem in today’s society, affecting an estimated one-
third of the population. The societal costs (including litigation, work-lost, treatment and disability) for
painful musculoskeletal disorders are staggering. Financially, considering all chronic pain syndromes
together, there is an annual cost of $90 billion.2 For example, the cost of low back pain alone has
been estimated between $40 –50 billion annually.20,21 Until a better understanding of the pathomechan-
isms in chronic pain and the injuries which cause them is defined, the effective prevention and treatment
of these disorders and their symptoms will remain elusive. It is the intent of this chapter to review and
highlight traditional and more recently emerging theories explaining pain sensation, signaling and trans-
mission in the context of injury and musculoskeletal disorders.
This review offers an overview of the mechanistic pathways of persistent (chronic) pain associated with
musculoskeletal disorders (MSDs) and injuries. A discussion of the neurophysiology of pain addresses
concepts of injury and pain processing and describes more recent hypotheses of the central nervous
system’s (CNS) neuroimmunologic involvement in persistent pain. These concepts, together with the
associated neurochemical nociceptive responses are addressed and discussed in the context of findings
from animal models of persistent pain with behavioral hypersensitivity. Physiologic responses in the
CNS are addressed as they pertain to the interplay of the electrophysiological and immune research
areas, and also as they relate to MSDs and biomechanics of injury. In particular, one such injury
16-1
16-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
leading to persistent pain is radiculopathy, which results from nerve root compression or impingement
and is a common source of low back pain. This painful syndrome is used here as an example to provide a
context for presenting immune mechanisms of chronic pain and their relationship to specific injury
parameters. Measures of injury biomechanics are presented for work with these models, including beha-
vioral sensitivity, local structural changes, and cellular and molecular changes in the CNS, as they apply
to kinematics, kinetics, and injury. Incorporating effects of injury parameters on mechanisms of persist-
ent pain, the text discusses implication of these and other factors confounding pain in MSD. Lastly, based
on these findings and others, a discussion is provided highlighting areas of future work to help elucidate
methods of injury prevention, diagnosis and development of therapeutic treatments.
It is important to define, at the outset, relevant distinctions in terminology. “Pain” is a complex per-
ception that is influenced by prior experience and by the context within which the noxious stimulus
occurs. Likewise, “nociception” is the physiologic response to tissue damage or prior tissue damage.
Similarly, for discussion in this chapter, “hyperalgesia” is defined as enhanced pain to a noxious stimu-
lus.55 Strictly speaking, this is a leftward-shift of the stimulus-response function relating pain to intensity.
The corresponding pain threshold is lowered and there is enhanced response to a given stimulus. Hyper-
algesia is mediated by nociceptor sensitization, where “sensitization” describes a corresponding shift in
the neural response curve for stimulation. Sensitization is characterized by a decrease in threshold, an
increased response to suprathreshold stimulus and spontaneous neural activity.
For this chapter, many of the examples are drawn from painful injuries related to the spine. These
include both low back and neck pain from radiculopathy. While it is recognized that these examples
are by no means all-inclusive of the painful musculoskeletal injuries, they do provide an ideal context
for discussing many of the pain mechanisms presented here. Certainly, syndromes such as those affecting
the carpal tunnel are important injuries in their own right, yet many of the same issues apply regarding
neural tissue damage, mechanical injury and inflammation. In addition, in this regard, within these
examples comments are made in this text to delineate the differences between persistent and resolving
(acute) pain syndromes.
Tract of
Lissauer Dorsal column
Dorsal root Dorsal horn
(primary afferents)
I
II Gray matter
Lateral column X
Ventrolateral column
FIGURE 16.1 Schematic illustrating the spinal nerve roots, spinal cord and its regions of white and gray matter.
Also indicated are columns of the neuronal tracts in the white matter. Those regions of the gray matter (laminae)
of particular relevance to pain sensation and transmission are also labeled.
spinal nerve, which communicates with the peripheral nervous system. Spinal nerves further branch into
smaller nerves, which innervate various tissues and different organs of the body. These peripheral nerves
branch out into the periphery to innervate bones, ligaments, joints, discs, muscles, organs, and many
other tissue-types (Figure 16.2).
The spinal cord is anatomically composed of two regions (Figure 16.1) based on appearance, function
and cell populations. The gray matter, which has a darker appearance, contains the cell bodies of spinal
neurons and comprises the central region of the spinal cord. The white matter surrounds the gray matter
and contains the axons of the spinal neurons. The spinal columnar tracts are regionally specialized
according to the information they carry, with the lateral column containing motor neurons, the
dorsal column carrying information related to mechanoreception and the ventrolateral column
housing neurons which communicate information regarding pain, temperature and motor signals
(Figure 16.1). In general, the sensory system is an ascending pathway that comprises the dorsal
portion of the spinal cord, while the descending pathway of the motor control system comprises the
ventral aspect of the spinal cord.
Afferents of the dorsal root enter the spinal cord dorsolaterally and branch in the white matter, with
collaterals terminating in the gray matter. Nerve fibers mediating pain pass through the tract of Lissauer
and have branches termining in the most superficial regions of the dorsal horn, laminae I and II
(Figure 16.1). Neurons in these laminae synapse on secondary neurons in laminae IV–VI of the
dorsal horn. These secondary neurons then cross the midline of the cord and ascend to the brain con-
tralaterally in the anterolateral region of the cord. Also of note, lamina X, which is located in the gray
matter region closest to the central canal, also receives pain sensory inputs. Nociceptive information
is transmitted from the spinal cord to supraspinal sites, primarily in the pons, medulla, and thalamus.
The anterolateral ascending system has three tracts: spinothalamic, spinoreticular, and spinomesencephalic.
The spinothalamic is the most prominent of the tracts. Briefly, the spinothalamic and spinoreticular
tracts mediate noxious sensations, with axons terminating on neurons in the reticular formation of
the medulla and pons. From there, signals are relayed to the thalamus, and then, neurons project to
the somatosensory cortex.
16-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
articular bone
joint
intervertebral
disc
dorsal root
ganglion
nerve
FIGURE 16.2 Schematic illustrating varied tissue components capable of sensing pain, particularly following
mechanical or inflammatory injuries. While this schematic is based on the spinal column and highlights tissues
relevant to its anatomy, it highlights all such tissues in the body, including neural tissue, hard and soft tissue,
ligaments and bone. Muscle tissue is not shown here, but is innervated and is the tissue component particularly
relevant to pain sensation.
PAIN
SYMPTOMS
Afferent
fibers
INJURY
CENTRAL
LOCAL (CNS)
(periphery) Dorsal Horn Excitability
Electrophysiologic Decreased threshold,
Structural & Material Signaling Increased response to stimuli,
Responses Ongoing spontaneous activity
FIGURE 16.3 This schematic illustrates the collection of physiologic mechanisms following an injury in
the periphery, which initiates and contributes to pain. Nociceptive responses are complicated and involve a
host of changes both locally and in the CNS. While the schematic suggests a simple linear cascade (from left to
right) of events following injury, which lead to pain, events are quite dynamic in nature and involve aspects of
electrophysiology, immunology and an interplay between both. Some changes occur both at the site of injury and
in the spinal cord and CNS. However, the degree to which these alterations occur is variable and dependent on
both the nature of the injury and the type of response. Moreover, many of these individual responses affect each
other (small arrows) and are themselves directly altered by confounding physiologic, anatomical and mechanical
factors.
by nociceptors, the nerve endings of the Ad and C fibers. Conduction velocities of Ad fibers are approxi-
mately 10 times those of C fibers (5–30 and 0.5– 2 m/sec, respectively) due to the saltatory conduction
resulting from the myelin sheathing. As a consequence of this difference in conduction velocity, a sharp
pain is first detected in response to a stimulus, followed by a second, longer lasting dull and burning pain
mediated by the C fibers.
Pain sensation and signaling begin at the injury site where Ad and C fibers are activated by thermal,
chemical, mechanical, and electrical stimuli. These nociceptors can become sensitized; this both lowers
their thresholds for firing and increases their firing rates when stimulated at levels similar to those before
injury.4 Following injury, inflammation is induced in an effort to promote healing and recovery. In this
process, inflammatory mediators such as prostaglandin E, serotonin, bradykinin and histamine, among
others, can have altering effects on fiber responses for a given mechanical stimulation. These and other
mediators activate nociceptors or further sensitize those nociceptors which are already responding to
stimuli — inducing increased activity. Specifically, bradykinin, serotonin, excitatory amino acids, and
hydrogen ions are all responsible for directly activating afferents.4,31 Similarly, prostaglandins, serotonin,
noradrenaline, adenosine, NO, and nerve growth factor sensitize nociceptors.
In addition to altered electrical activity, injury initiates the local synthesis and release of inflammatory
mediators that induce inflammation and edema as part of the healing process (Figure 16.3). However,
these same processes that provide healing also sensitize nociceptors and recruit new nociceptors that
enhance pain.17,19 Cytokines are also released in the periphery in association with tissue injury and
inflammation. These small proteins, in turn, contribute to the local inflammatory response, while
further affecting the electrophysiologic responses of nerve fibers in the region and altering nociception.
16-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Additional details regarding specific biochemical mediators of pain and their mechanisms of action
throughout the pain signaling pathway (i.e., injury site, spinal cord, brain) are provided in the following
section specifically addressing biochemical mediators of pain.
Primary afferents communicate with spinal neurons via synaptic transmission. A variety of neuro-
transmitters (i.e., glutamate, NMDA, substance P; see following section for details) modulate postsyn-
aptic responses, with further transmission to postsynaptic spinal neurons and supraspinal sites via the
ascending pathways.4 Tissue damage (injury) generates an increased neuronal excitability in the spinal
cord.64 Associated with this sensitization is also a decreased activation threshold, increased response
magnitude and increased recruitment of receptive fields.55 The continuous input from nociceptive affer-
ents can drive the spinal circuits, leading to central sensitization, and maintaining a chronic pain state.15
These neuroplastic changes are accompanied by other electrophysiological manifestations that cause
neurons to fire with increased frequency or even spontaneously.58 In addition, spinal processing is
further directly altered by descending inhibitory and facilitory pathways that provide additional modu-
lation of spinal interneurons.54
Ultimately, persistent pain results from the sensitization of the CNS. While the exact mechanism by
which the spinal cord reaches a “hyperexcitable” state of sensitization is not fully known at present,
many hypotheses have emerged. Only the highlights of these theories are provided here as an overview.
More extensive and detailed discussions can be found elsewhere in the literature.5,16,18,64 Central hyper-
excitability is characterized by a “windup” response of repetitive C fiber stimulation, expanding receptive
field areas and spinal neurons taking on properties of wide dynamic range neurons.7 Low threshold Ab
afferents, which normally do not serve to transmit pain, become recruited to signal spontaneous and
movement-induced pain.16 Ultimately, Ab fibers stimulate postsynaptic neurons to transmit pain,
where these Ab fibers previously had no effect. This plasticity of neuronal function in the spinal cord
contributes to central sensitization.
periphery. The release of prostaglandin E2, which occurs in response to the upregulation of the enzyme
causes a shift in the peripheral terminal of the nociceptor lowering its threshold and increasing its
excitability.
Likewise, there are also a host of other inflammatory-related mediators, which also have direct and
indirect effects on pain and nociception. Serotonin (5HT) is elevated in inflammation. It is released
from platelets downstream from mast cell degranulation, as occurs during inflammatory responses. Ser-
totonin can cause pain by locally activating primary afferents and can further enhance sensitivity of
sensory responses to bradykinin. Also, leukotriene B4 is a neutrophil attractant that can directly sensitize
afferents. Certainly, each of these mediators has a complicated mechanism of action far more compli-
cated than has been reviewed here.
Substance P is a potent pro-nociceptive neurotransmitter, which is transported to the periphery after
afferent activation. It has inflammatory effects by causing vasodilation and release of prostaglandin E2
and cytokines. These protein releases further impact the local responses of inflammation as well as
nociception. Substance P can cause a release of calcium from intracellular stores and in turn lead to
NO production and neuronal excitability and long-term sensitization.34,36 Substance P is also shown
to have a role in pain in the CNS. Also contributing to sensitization, CGRP, a neuropeptide often
colocalized with substance P in the spinal cord, regulates nociceptive responses by further promoting
the release of substance P as well as glutamate from primary afferents and retarding the metabolism
of substance P.1,35 Antibodies to substance P and CGRP have been demonstrated to attenuate pain symp-
toms in inflammatory models of carageenan-induced hyperalgesia and painful nerve injury.34,46 These
results strongly implicate both neuropeptides in the transmission of pain. Additionally, application of
antagonists to the substance P receptor, NK-1, has induced antinociception in the CNS after chronic
nerve constriction,35 as well as globally in rodent models of inflammatory arthritis.28 However,
despite research indicating the potent role of substance P and CGRP in many types of pain, little is
known about the relative contributions of these neuropeptides to the onset or maintenance of pain
symptoms, either in persistent or resolving pain.
Excitatory amino acids, such as glutamate, have potent roles in pain, both locally at the site of injury
and in the CNS. Indeed, glutamate is produced by both non-neuronal and neuronal cells in the peri-
phery. Both of these sources act on primary afferents by activating them when bound to any of the
NMDA, kainite, AMPA or metabotropic glutamate receptors.55 Certainly, this positive feedback mech-
anism of nociceptor excitation leads to peripheral sensitization and correspondingly to altered afferent
signaling into the spinal cord. Glutamate receptors are expressed in dorsal root ganglion cells,46
suggesting its direct involvement in afferent signaling to the cord. As the NMDA glutamate receptor
has a key role in regulating synaptic efficacy, there is also a direct role of this receptor in the plasticity
changes, which occur in the spinal cord associated with central sensitization changes of persistent pain.
Cytokines can directly or indirectly regulate cascades that can lead to the transmission and modulation
of pain. The broad collection of cytokines includes both pro-inflammatory and anti inflammatory pro-
teins, both of which are upregulated in painful injuries.10,11,25,56,57 In particular, in models of neural
injury either distal (peripheral injury) or proximal (nerve root injury) to the DRG, spinal IL-1b, IL-6,
IL-10 and TNF mRNA are all significantly elevated.61 However, not all of these factors are pro-
inflammatory. In fact, IL-10 has been shown to suppress NO production in cultured astrocytes and
also suppress proliferation in macrophages,66 providing an anti-inflammatory mechanism. Additionally,
the presence of cytokines often stimulates a positive feedback loop as demonstrated in the case of IL-1,
which can stimulate its own production.56 Cytokines mediate cellular processes through the production
or suppression of NO. NO has an immunoregluatory role in the CNS. Its production leads directly to the
hyperalgesic NMDA pathway as discussed earlier. Cytokines regulate NO by interfering with the
production of NOS (nitric oxide synthase). Of the two main forms of NOS, inducible and constitutive,
astrocytes can produce both types, whereas microglia are only responsible for inducible NOS.66 TNF-a
and IL-1b control the stimulation of the production of iNOS in both astrocytes and glia therefore indu-
cing the production of NO from those cell types. Alternatively, TGF-b suppresses NO production in both
astrocytes and microglia, whereas IL-10 only affects NO production in astrocytes.
16-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
compressive deformation thresholds for pain behaviors and hypersensitivity were defined based on
the amount of nerve root compression.60 More recent work examining cervical nerve root compression
magnitude and behavioral sensitivity in the forepaw also suggests a force-based threshold for pain
symptoms, below 10gf applied to the dorsal nerve root.26 These mechanical parameters defining
painful injuries provide added utility for clinicians in diagnosing painful injuries, directly linking the
injury event to the likelihood of pain symptoms. Moreover, in future, it will hopefully provide insight
into predicting clinical outcomes for this class of injuries.
While defining the relationship between injury events and pain is necessary for understanding the
clinical context of these pathologies, defining the relationship between injury and specific and relevant
nociceptive responses is crucial for understanding the central mechanisms of persistent pain in MSD.
Using RNase Protection Assays to detect spinal mRNA of a panel of cytokines (TNFa, IL-1a/b, IL-6,
IL-10), a statistically significant correlation was found between mRNA levels at postoperative day 7
and the degree of tissue deformation at injury.62 This suggests a modulatory effect of injury magnitude
on one aspect of spinal nociception. Using immunohistochemistry, lumbar spinal expression of the pro-
inflammatory cytokine IL-1b was previously found to depend on nerve root compression intensity;23
suggesting preservation of these changes at both the message and protein levels for the spinal cytokines
involved in chronic low back pain responses. Further, in comparative models of cervical nerve root
injury, either from compression or transection, with different symptom presentation, spinal IL-6
protein at day 1 was elevated over sham levels, suggesting a potential relationship to hypersensitivity
on the day of assay.25 While continued research into these and other cytokine responses is needed for
understanding cervical nerve root injuries and pain mechanisms, findings suggest that similar cytokine
responses may be evident throughout the spine (i.e., lumbar versus cervical injuries).
Consistent with the grading of behavioral responses and spinal cytokine expression according to injury
severity,23,62,63 spinal microglial activation has been demonstrated to be more intense for greater nerve
root deformation in lumbar injuries.23,59 Yet, in these same studies, astrocytic activation did not follow
injury magnitude, highlighting that biomechanics at injury in lumbar radiculopathy models may differ-
entially modulate some neuroimmune responses and not others (Figure 16.3). Recent work from our
laboratory has examined these same spinal glial responses in two different cervical spine injuries;26,32
in these studies of nerve root compression and facet joint tension, different astrocytic responses were
observed. Specifically, for the nerve root compression, spinal astrocytic expression was elevated over
sham (Figure 16.4), and followed the behavioral hypersensitivity patterns. Despite this, astrocytic acti-
vation did not show a dependence on mechanical force magnitude. This is similar to the earlier findings
of nerve root injury in the lumbar spine.59 In contrast, in our mechanical facet-mediated painful injury
model, spinal astrocytic activation did demonstrate a significant correlation with injury magnitude
(Figure 16.4).32 This finding may suggest that different spinal immune response cascades exist for mech-
anical injuries to different tissue types. It further highlights the need for continued integrative research to
identify common and different physiologic mechanisms for injuries within the musculoskeletal system.
(a) (b)
(c) (d)
(e) (f)
FIGURE 17.4 Glial cell activation responses (as detected by GFAP) in the ipsilateral dorsal horn of the spinal cord at
the level of injury for facet joint (a, b, c) and nerve root (d, e, f) injuries. Sham levels (a, d) are the same as normal
naı̈ve responses. For the facet injury, astrocytic activation was increased for 0.89 mm of distraction (c) with
significant increases in behavioral sensitivity, compared to 0.17 mm of distraction (b), which produced no
symptoms of hypersensitivity. In contrast, while both 10gf (e) and 60gf (f) nerve root compression produced
behavioral hypersensitivity, there was no difference in GFAP reactivity for such six-fold difference in injury force.
Bar in c is 200 mm, and applies to all. Images are modified from those published in Lee, K.E., Davis, M.B.,
Mejilla, R.M., Winkelstein, B.A. Proceedings of 48th Stapp Car Crash Conference, Paper pp. 2004-22-0016, 48,
pp. 373 – 393, 2004 and Hubbard and Winkelstein Spine, 30, pp. 1924– 1932, 2005. With permission.
injury produces the local inflammatory changes discussed earlier, which in turn may lower that tissue’s
threshold for mechanical injury. In fact, it has been shown that the mechanical insult required to produce
behavioral hypersensitivity in the presence of an inflammatory insult is nearly one-half that required in
its absence, despite resulting in the same degree of sensitivity.13
Mechanisms for Pain and Injury in Musculoskeletal Disorders 16-11
Inferences can be made in light of these findings for painful MSDs. For example, epidemiologic studies
indicate that patients with pre-existing spinal degeneration at the time of injury, experience more severe
and longer lasting neck pain symptoms.41,42,49 It is possible that such degeneration can contribute to
inflammatory changes in the facet joint, which may increase this joint’s susceptibility to mechanical
injury. Therefore, when undergoing motions or loading, which may not normally elicit nociceptive
changes, the pain fibers may be sensitized and fire under mechanical conditions, which are much less
severe than previously required to initiate nociception. The same may be true for degenerative
changes in other tissues. In this light, it may further be possible that repetitive loading of tissues at nor-
mally noninjurious level, when sustained over many times, can lead to painful outcomes or lowered
thresholds for stimulating painful injury.
Additional geometric and anatomic factors contributing to pain risk are gender, existing spinal
degeneration, stenosis, and genetics48 (Table 16.1). For example, for the case of whiplash, females
experience increased symptom persistence when compared to males.48 The anatomical considerations
specifically related to gender, which include decreased neck muscle strength and spinal canal size, add
support to the role of neck mechanics in affecting a pain mechanism. Moreover, anecdotal evidence
has shown that smaller spinal canal size is associated with more symptomatic responses in whiplash.41
Similar geometric constraints of anatomy also apply to regions of the body such as the wrist (i.e.,
carpal tunnel syndrome). Finally, more recent clinical research into spinal pain in general has shown
that genetics may play a very key role in pain persistence for a given injury, accounting for many discre-
pancies observed among different patients for seemingly similar injuries.13 Future research into neck pain
mechanisms would be strengthened if it considered the role of genetics in many of the issues discussed in
this chapter.
many aspects of injury, physiology, and cellular mechanisms contribute to chronic pain in MSDs. In this
context, then, it is possible to synthesize these findings to discuss preventing these injuries and treating
them. As continued biomechanical research is performed to determine conditions under which tissue
injury occurs and initiates physiologic responses, it becomes clear that findings can help guide preventive
strategies to protect some of these anatomic structures and tissues from undergoing kinematically and
kinetically risky situations and injury. In addition, the cellular findings presented here highlight the
need for defining the relationship of an injury event, its physiologic responses, and their relationship
to behavioral manifestations of pain symptoms.
As the understanding of the mechanisms of persistent pain expands, increased research is being
focused on development of effective treatment modalities. A broad variety of approaches exist for offer-
ing pain relief: joint blocks, TENS, manipulation, pharmacology, and many others.13 However, the exact
mechanisms of injury often remain elusive, making it extremely challenging to act at the structural site of
injury for therapy. Pharmacologic treatment options offer a promising approach for manipulating those
aspects of the CNS response, which contribute to chronic nociception. For example, global immunosup-
pressants have been shown to ameliorate pain behaviors in both neuropathic and radiculopathic rodent
pain models.61 Likewise, manipulation of specific spinal cytokines to alter sensory processing and other
select agents have been effective in reducing allodynia in a variety of pain models.12,24,51,52,61 Pharmaco-
logic antagonists to and inhibitors of particular pro-inflammatory cytokines and other algesic mediators
(IL-1, TNF, COX-2) have shown effectiveness in animal pain models for attenuating both behavioral
hypersensitivity and elements of the CNS neuroimmune cascade.12,24,51,61 Indeed, combinations of
some of these agents may have promise for effectiveness in reducing pain. As continued research ident-
ifies the specific physiologic pathways (both electrophysiologic and immunologic), which are responsible
for chronic pain, it will become more feasible and even more tractable to target specific sites along these
pathways for selectively manipulating and modulating a persistent pain response. With continued inte-
grative efforts, progress will be made in this area.
16.5 Summary
It is recognized that spinal injuries are by no means the only chronically painful MSDs. As such, it should
be noted that many of the theories described previously may assist with developing a more broad under-
standing in the context of other painful MSDs, such as carpal tunnel syndrome and other repetitive
motion injuries. While magnitude, rate, and duration of loading all modulate electrical signaling patterns
(amplitude, frequency) and local tissue changes (edema, pressure), and the neuroimmune cascade for
painful radiculopathy, their effects for other painful syndromes may be similar. Continued integration
of multidisciplinary approaches applied to a broader class of MSDs will help define nociceptive responses
in these disorders.
In the typical response of an acutely painful episode, the balance of injury, repair, and healing is
achieved and the cascade of electrophysiologic and chemical events resolves following inflammation
and injury. However, for persistent pain, the local, spinal, and even supraspinal, responses are undoubt-
edly altered from that described previously. Based on the discussion presented in the previous sections
regarding persistent pain, a comprehensive picture is emerging for neural injury and CNS responses
of nociception: spinal cytokine upregulation, microglial, and astrocytic activation, altered neuronal–
glial interactions, cellular adhesion molecule upregulation, and immune cell infiltration into the
spinal cord.13,14,50,53,56 These aspects of neuroimmune activation induce the expression and release of
pain mediators (substance P, glutamate, NO) and also lead to neuronal hypersensitivity.
In this context, it is important to consider novel methods for preventing and treating painful injuries.
Clinical emphasis has largely been focused on local interventions at the injury site. However, the previous
discussion points to the spinal cord physiology as having equal, if not stronger, contribution for main-
tenance of pain. Continued understanding of spinal and supraspinal mechanisms and mediation of
central sensitization can hopefully provide valuable contributions to this understanding. It is the hope
Mechanisms for Pain and Injury in Musculoskeletal Disorders 16-13
that this review has provided a summary of current thinking in pain mechanisms with a particular
emphasis on how these mechanisms relate to musculoskeletal injury. Likewise, it was the intent to illu-
minate interesting new work within the study of pain, highlighting the complications and intricacies of
its nature. Finally, through this presentation, areas of future work have been indicated. It is only through
continual efforts that meaningful advances will be made in preventing and treating painful musculoske-
letal disorders.
Acknowledgments
The author gratefully acknowledges the following for financial support: National Institute of Arthritis
and Musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases (AR47564), the Whitaker Foundation and the Catharine
Sharpe Foundation.
References
1. Allen, B., Li, J., Menning, P., Rogers, S., Ghilardi, J., Mantyh, P., and Simone, D. Primary afferent
fibers that contribute to increased substance P receptor internalization in the spinal cord after
injury. J. Neurophysiol., 81(3), pp. 1379–1390, 1999.
2. American Chronic Pain Association, http://www.theacpa.org/.
3. Black, J., Langworthy, K., Hinson, A., Dib-Hajj, S., and Waxman, S. NGF has opposing effects on
Naþ channel III and SNS gene expression in spinal sensory neurons. Neuroreport 8, pp. 2331 –
2335, 1997.
4. Cavanaugh, J.M. Neurophysiology and neuroanatomy of neck pain. In: Frontiers in Whiplash
Trauma: Clinical and Biomechanical. Yoganandan, N., Pintar, F.A., Eds. IOS Press. Amsterdam,
2000, pp. 79 –96.
5. Coderre, T.J., Katz, J., Vaccarino, A., and Melzack, R. Contribution of central neuroplasticity to
pathological pain: review of clinical and experimental evidence. Pain, 52, pp. 259– 285, 1993.
6. Colburn, R., Rickman, A., and DeLeo, J. The effect of site and type of nerve injury on spinal glial
activation and neuropathic pain behavior. Exp. Neurol., 157, pp. 289 –304, 1999.
7. Cook, A.J., Woolf, C.J., Wall, P.D., and McMahon, S.B. Dynamic receptive field plasticity in rat spinal
cord dorsal horn following C-primary afferent input, Nature, 325, pp. 151 –153, 1987.
8. Cornefjord, M., Sato, K., Olmarker, K., Rydevik, B., and Nordborg, C. A model for chronic nerve
root compression studies. Presentation of a porcine model for controlled slow-onset compression
with analyses of anatomic aspects, compression onset rate, and morphologic and neurophysiologic
effects. Spine, 22, pp. 946 –957, 1997.
9. Couture, R., Harrisson, M., Vianna, R.M., and Cloutier, F. Kinin receptors in pain and inflammation.
Eur. J. Pharmacol., 429, pp. 161 –176, 2001.
10. DeLeo, J. and Colburn, R. Low Back Pain: A Scientific and Clinical Overview. The role of cytokines in
nociception and chronic pain, in: Weinstein, J., Gordon, S., Eds. Rosemont, IL: AAOS, pp. 163 –185.
11. DeLeo, J., Colburn, R., Nichols, M., and Malhotra, A. Interleukin (IL)-6 mediated hyperalgesia/
allodynia and increased spinal IL-6 in two distinct mononeuropathy models in the rat. J. Interferon
Cytokine Res., 16, pp. 695–700, 1996.
12. DeLeo, J., Hashizume, H., Rutkowski, M., and Weinstein, J. Cyclooxygenase –2 inhibitor SC-236
attenuates mechanical allodynia following nerve root injury in rats. J. Orthop. Res., 18(6), pp.
977 –982, 2000.
13. DeLeo, J. and Winkelstein, B. Physiology of chronic spinal pain syndromes: From animal models to
biomechanics. Spine, 27(22), pp. 2526– 2537, 2002.
14. DeLeo, J.A. and Yezierski, R.P. The role of neuroinflammation and neuroimmune activation in
persistent pain. Pain, 91, pp. 1 –6, 2001.
15. Devor, M. Neuropathic pain and injured nerve: peripheral mechanisms. Br. Med. Bull., 47,
pp. 619 –630, 1991.
16-14 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
16. Devor, M. Pain arising from the nerve root and the dorsal root ganglion. in: Low Back Pain: A
Scientific and Clinical Overview. Weinstein, J., Gordon, S., Eds. Rosemont, IL: AAOS, 1986,
pp. 187 –208.
17. Dray, A. and Perkins, M. Bradykinin and inflammatory pain. Trends Neurosci., 16(3): pp. 99 –104,
1993.
18. Dubner, R. and Basbaum, A.I. Spinal dorsal horn plasticity following tissue or nerve injury. in:
Textbook of Pain. Wall, P.D., Melzak, R., Eds. Churchill-Livingstone: Edinburgh, 1994, pp. 225–241.
19. Dubner, R. and Hargreaves, K.M. The neurobiology of pain and its modulation. Clin. J. Pain, S1–S6,
1989.
20. Frymoyer, J. and Cats-Baril, W. An overview of the incidences and costs of low back pain. Orthop.
Clin. North Am., 22, pp. 263 –271, 1991.
21. Frymoyer, J. and Durett, C. The economics of spinal disorders. In: The Adult Spine: Principles and
Practice. Frymoyer, J.W., Eds. Lippincott-Raven Publishers: Philadelphia 1997.
22. Hanai, F., Matsui, N., and Hongo, N. Changes in responses of wide dynamic range neurons in the
spinal dorsal horn after dorsal root or dorsal root ganglion compression. Spine, 21, pp. 1408 –
1415, 1996.
23. Hashizume, H., DeLeo, J., Colburn, R., and Weinstein, J. Spinal glial activation and cytokine
expression following lumbar root injury in the rat. Spine, 25, pp. 1206 –1217, 2000.
24. Hashizume, H., Rutkowski, M., Weinstein, J., and DeLeo, J. Central administration of methotrexate
reduces mechanical allodynia in an animal model of radiculopathy/sciatica. Pain, 87(2), pp. 159 –
169, 2000.
25. Hubbard, R., Rothman, S., and Winkelstein, B. Mechanisms of persistent neck pain following nerve
root compression injury: understanding behavioral hypersensitivity in the context of spinal cytokine
responses and tissue biomechanics. North American Spine Society 19th Annual Meeting, #P49,
Chicago, IL, October, 2004.
26. Hubbard, R.D. and Winkelstein, B.A. Transient cervical nerve root compression in the rat induces
bilateral forepaw allodynia and spinal glial activation: mechanical factors in painful neck injuries.
Spine, 30, pp. 1924–1932, 2005.
27. Hunt, J.L., Winkelstein, B.A., Rutkowski, M.D., Weinstein, J.N., and DeLeo, J.A. Repeated injury to
the lumbar nerve roots produces enhanced mechanical allodynia and persistent spinal neuroinflam-
mation. Spine, 26, pp. 2073–2079, 2001.
28. Hong, S., Han, J., Min, S., Hwang, J., Kim, Y., Na, H., Yoon, Y., and Han, H. Local neurokinin-1
receptor in the knee joint contributes to the induction, but not maintenance, of arthritic pain in
the rat. Neurosci. Lett., 322(1), pp. 21 –24, 2002.
29. Junger, H. and Sorkin, L.S. Nociceptive and inflammatory effects of subcutaneous TNF alpha. Pain,
85(1 –2), 145 –151, 2000.
30. Kandel, E.R., Schwartz, J.H., and Jessell, T.M. Principles of Neural Science. 3rd edn, Elsevier:
New York, 1991.
31. Kawakami, M. and Weinstein, J.N. Associated neurogenic and nonneurogenic pain mediators
that probably are activated and responsible for nociceptive input. in: Low Back Pain: A Scientific
and Clinical Overview. Weinstein, J., Gordon, S., Eds. Rosemont, IL: AAOS, pp. 265 –273, 1986.
32. Lee, K.E., Davis, M.B., Mejilla, R.M., and Winkelstein, B.A. In vivo cervical facet capsule distraction:
mechanical implications for whiplash and neck pain. Proceedings of 48th Stapp Car Crash Confer-
ence, Paper #pp. 2004-22-0016, 48, pp. 373 –393, 2004.
33. Lundborg, G., Myers, R., and Powell, H. Nerve compression injury and increased endoneurial fluid
pressure: a “miniature compartment syndrome.” J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry, 46, pp. 1119–1124,
1983.
34. Ma, W. and Eisenach, J. Intraplantar injection of a cyclooxygenase inhibitor ketorolac reduces
immunoreactivities of substance P, calcitonin gene-related peptide, and dynorphin in the dorsal
horn of rats with nerve injury or inflammation. Neuroscience, 121, pp. 681– 90, 2003.
Mechanisms for Pain and Injury in Musculoskeletal Disorders 16-15
35. Meert, T., Vissers, K., Geenan, F., and Kontinen, V. Functional role of exogenous administration of
substance P in chronic constriction injury model of neuropathic pain in gerbils. Pharmacol. Biochem.
Behav., 76(1), pp. 17–25, 2003.
36. Millan, M. The induction of pain: An integrative review. Prog. Neurobiol., 57, pp. 1 –164, 1999.
37. Olmarker, K., Holm, S., and Rydevik, B. Importance of compression onset rate for the degree of
impairment of impulse propagation in experimental compression injury of the porcine cauda
equina. Spine, 15, pp. 416 –419, 1990.
38. Olmarker, K., Rydevik, B., and Holm, S. Edema formation in spinal nerve roots induced by exper-
imental, graded compression. An experimental study on the pig cauda equina with special reference
to differences in effects between rapid and slow onset of compression. Spine, 14, pp. 569 –573, 1989.
39. Ozaktay, A.C., Cavanaugh, J.M., Asik, I., DeLeo, J.A., and Weinstein, J.N. Dorsal root sensitivity to
interleukin-1 beta, interleukin-6 and tumor necrosis factor in rats. Eur. Spine J., 11(5), pp. 467 –475,
2002.
40. Pedowitz, R., Garfin, S., Massie, J., Hargens, A., Swenson, M., Myers, R., and Rydevik, B. Effects of
magnitude and duration of compression on spinal nerve root conduction. Spine, 17, pp.194 –199,
1992.
41. Pettersson, K., Karrholm, J., Toolanen, G., and Hidlingsson, C. Decreased width of the spinal canal in
patients with chronic symptoms after whiplash injury. Spine, 20, pp. 1664–1667, 1995.
42. Radanov, B.P., Sturzenegger, M., and DiStefano, G. Long-term outcome after whiplash injury.
Medicine, 74, pp.281 –297, 1995.
43. Rutkowski, M., Pahl, J., Sweitzer, S., and DeLeo, J. Limited role of macrophages in generation of
nerve injury-induced mechanical allodynia. Physiol. Behavi., 71, pp. 225– 235, 2000.
44. Rutkowski, M.D., Winkelstein, B.A., Hickey, W.F., Pahl, J.L., and DeLeo, J.A. Lumbar nerve root
injury induces CNS neuroimmune activation and neuroinflammation in the rat: Relationship to
painful radiculopathy. Spine, 27(15): pp. 1604–1613, 2002.
45. Rydevik, B., Myers, R., and Powell, H. Pressure increase in the dorsal root ganglion following mech-
anical compression. Closed compartment syndrome in nerve roots. Spine, 14, pp. 574 –576, 1989.
46. Satoh, M., Kuraishi, Y., and Kawamura, M. Effects of intrathecal antibodies to substance P, calcitonin
gene-related peptide and galanin on repeated cold stress-induced hyperalgesia: comparison with
carrageenan-induced hyperalgesia. Pain, 49(2), pp. 273 –278, 1992.
47. Skouen, J., Brisby, H., Otami, K., Olmarker, K., Rosengren, L., and Rydevik, B. Protein markers in
cerebrospinal fluid experimental nerve root injury. A study of slow-onset chronic compression
effects or the biochemical effects of nucleus pulposus on sacral nerve roots. Spine, 24,
pp. 2195 –2200, 1999.
48. Spitzer, W.O., Skovron, M.L., Salmi, L.R., Cassidy, J.D., Duranceau, J., Suissa, S., and Zeiss, E.
Scientific monograph of the Quebec Task Force on whiplash-associated disorders: redefining
“whiplash” and its management. Spine, 20, 1S –73S, 1995.
49. Sturtzenegger, M., Radanov, B.P., and DiStefano, G. The effect of accident mechanisms and initial
findings on the long-term course of whiplash injury. J. Neurol., 242, pp. 443–439, 1995.
50. Sweitzer, S., Arruda, J., and DeLeo, J. The cytokine challenge: methods for the detection of central
cytokines in rodent models of persistent pain. in: Methods in Pain Research. Kruger, L., Ed. CRC
Press, 2001, pp. 109– 132.
51. Sweitzer, S., Martin, D., and DeLeo, J. IL-1ra and sTNFr reduces mechanical allodynia and spinal
cytokine expression in a model of neuropathic pain. Neuroscience, 103, pp. 529–39, 2001.
52. Sweitzer, S., Schubert, P., and DeLeo, J. Propentofylline, a glial modulating agent, exhibits anti-
allodynic properties in a rat model of neuropathic pain. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther., 297(3),
pp. 1210 –1217, 2001.
53. Sweitzer, S., White, K.A., Dutta, C., and DeLeo, J. The differential role of spinal MHC class II
and cellular adhesion molecules in peripheral inflammatory versus neuropathic pain in rodents.
J. Neuroimmunol., 125(1–2): pp. 82 –93, 2002.
16-16 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
54. Vanderah, T.W., Ossipov, M.H., Lai, J., Malan, T., and Porreca, F. Mechanisms of opioid-induced
pain and antinociceptive tolerance: descending facilitation and spinal dynorphin. Pain, 92,
pp. 5 –9, 2001.
55. Wall, P. and Melzack, R. Textbook of Pain. 3rd edn., Churchill Livingstone: London, 1994.
56. Watkins, L., Maier, S., and Goehler, L. Immune activation: the role of pro-inflammatory cytokines in
inflammation, illness responses, and pathological pain states. Pain, 63, pp. 289 –302, 1995.
57. Watkins, L., Wiertelak, E., Goehler, L., Smith, K., Martin, D., Maier, S. Characterization of cytokine-
induced hyperalgesia. Brain Res., 654, pp. 15– 26, 1994.
58. Waxman, S., Dib-Hajj, S., Cummins, T., and Black, J. Sodium channels and pain. Proc. Natl. Acad.
Sci., U.S.A. 96, pp. 7635 –7639, 1999.
59. Winkelstein, B.A. and DeLeo, J.A. Nerve root tissue injury severity differentially modulates spinal
glial activation in a rat lumbar radiculopathy model: Considerations for persistent pain. Brain
Res., 956(2), pp. 294–301, 2002.
60. Winkelstein, B.A. and DeLeo, J.A. Mechanical thresholds for initiation and persistence of pain
following nerve root injury: Mechanical and chemical contributions at injury. J. Biomech. Eng.,
126, pp.258 –263, 2004.
61. Winkelstein, B., Rutkowski, M., Sweitzer, S., Pahl, J., and DeLeo, J. Nerve injury proximal or distal to
the DRG induces florid spinal neuroimmune activation related to enhanced behavioral sensitivity.
J. Comp. Neurol., 439, pp.127 –139, 2001.
62. Winkelstein, B., Rutkowski, M., Weinstein, J., and DeLeo, J. Quantification of neural tissue injury in
a rat radiculopathy model: comparison of local deformation, behavioral outcomes, and spinal
cytokine mRNA for two surgeons. J. Neurosci. Methods, 111, pp. 49 –57, 2001.
63. Winkelstein, B., Weinstein, J., and DeLeo, J. Local biomechanical factors in lumbar radiculopathy: An
in vivo model approach. Spine, 27, pp. 27 –33, 2001.
64. Woolf, C.J. Evidence for a central component of post-injury pain hypersensitivity. Nature, 306,
pp.686 –688, 1983.
65. Woolf, C., Safieh-Garabedian, B., Ma, Q., Crilly, P., and Winter, J. Nerve growth factor contributes to
the generation of inflammatory sensory hypersensitivity. Neuroscience, 62, pp. 277–331, 1994.
66. Xiao, B.G. and Link, H. Immune regulation within the central nervous system. J. Neurol. Sci., 157,
pp. 1 –12, 1998.
67. Yoshizawa, H., Kobayashi, S., and Kubota, K. Effects of compression on intraradicular blood flow in
dogs. Spine, 14, pp. 1220–1225, 1989.
17
Ergonomics and Aging
The rate of population aging is unprecedented, pervasive, enduring, and it has profound implications
for most aspects of society, including the workforce (Klinger, 2002). Early retirement pushes that
occurred in the early twentieth century, leveled off in the middle twentieth century, and recently
have been increasing (Burtless and Quinn, 2000), which has led to a growing number of older
adults in the workforce. On a global scale, approximately 1 in 5 adults are 65 years and older and
are in the workforce (Klinger, 2002). In more developed countries approximately 30% of adults
aged 60 years or older are in the workforce; the percentage is more than 60% in less developed
countries (Klinger, 2002). In addition, some unemployed older adults report wanting employment.
For example, 5% of Americans aged 55 to 74 who are unemployed or retired report a desire to be
employed (U.S. General Accounting Office, 2001). The U.S. older adult workforce is projected to
rise by 37% by 2015, which means the older adult workforce could make up almost 20% of the
total workforce by 2015 (U.S. General Accounting Office, 2001). Such growth trends have spawned
a renewed interest in the older adult worker.
Defining the typical older adult worker, however, is not a simple task because the older adult workforce
is an extremely diverse group. Currently men outnumber women in the workforce, however, the ratio of
men to women is moving toward 1:1. In more developed countries, women make up 41% of the older
adult workforce. In the U.S., women constituted approximately 52% of the older adult workforce in 2000
and this percentage is expected to grow to 61% by 2015 (U.S. Government Accounting Office, 2001).
17-1
17-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Source: U.S. General Accounting Office. Older Workers, GAO-02-85-95-152. Washington, D.C.
GAO 2001. With permission.
The U.S. older adult workforce is also racially and ethnically diverse. About the same percentage of
Caucasians (14.1%), African Americans (12.4%), Hispanics (13.6%), and Asians (12.5%) continue to
work after age 65 (U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2004).
Older adults are employed in a wide variety of positions including executive, professional, sales, and
service (Table 17.1), but they are more likely to be white-collar managers or professionals (U.S. General
Accounting Office, 2001). Also, the proportion of older adults in certain occupations such as teaching
and nursing is expected to grow in the future (U.S. General Accounting Office, 2001). Moreover,
certain workers, such as farmers, are particularly more likely to remain in the workforce after the
minimum retirement age (Table 17.1). One survey found that farmers were twice as likely to continue
working past the age of 65 compared to their peers in other occupations (Sofranko, 2000). Hence,
although older adults are employed in a wide variety of occupations, the proportion of older adults in
any particular occupation may vary.
Economic, societal, cultural, and social factors have contributed to the growth of the older adult workforce.
The decision to delay retirement is strongly based on financial need (68%) and the desire to build up income
(64%) (Parkinson, 2002). Certain individuals are more likely to delay retirement based on their financial
need, such as those who have low-paying jobs that do not offer sufficient pensions, divorced or widowed
women who depend on their husband’s pensions, and people who have dependent children, spouses, and/
or parents (Szinovácz et al., 1992). Another financial factor that has grown in importance in the last two
decades is healthcare cost. Rising healthcare costs along with increased life expectancy and an age-related
increase in healthcare needs has made healthcare coverage especially important for older adults. In fact,
many American workers may delay retirement to retain their health insurance coverage (Gruber et al., 1995).
Some older adults may delay retirement to retain the salary increase they have attained from reaching a
level of tenure. Not surprisingly, median years of tenure tend to increase with age, such that workers aged
55 to 64 have more than three times the median tenure of workers aged 25 to 34 (Bureau of Labor
Statistics, 2002). In 2002, older adult workers aged 55 and above had been at their job for more than
9 years. In these situations, individuals may be more inclined to delay their retirement to benefit from
the economic rewards of tenure.
Societal trends and laws concerning aging and retirement also influence the likelihood that an older
adult will remain in the workforce. Norms regarding retirement vary from country to country,
with American, Japanese, and Scandinavian workers leaving the workforce later than most other indus-
trialized countries (Burtless and Quinn, 2000). In spite of such variation, there is a strong relationship
across countries between the incentives for continued work provided by social security and labor par-
ticipation of older adults. That is, individuals are less likely to retire when additional work results in
larger increases in “social security wealth” (Gruber et al., 1995). In the U.S., the Social Security Act
mandates that 67 is the minimum age for the receipt of full benefits. It is likely that the Social Security
Act along with the Age Discrimination in Employment Act, which eliminated a mandatory retirement
Ergonomics and Aging 17-3
age for most occupations, have contributed substantially to the growing older adult workforce in
the U.S. In contrast, under current Italian law individuals, who have worked for 35 years can retire
at 57. Hence, the age at which individuals are eligible for benefits varies from country to country,
which may contribute to a certain extent to differences in older adult worker participation rates
(Table 17.2).
Whereas some people work out of financial necessity, others work because they enjoy their jobs
(AARP, 2002). Even people who have retired from their careers may return to work because they
desire to be productive. That is, they may enjoy the satisfaction they get from accomplishing tasks.
Atchley (1976) noted several stages of adjustment to retirement, two of which are often accompanied
by feelings that may be related to an individual’s desire to return to work. For example, a “honeymoon
stage” often occurs immediately following retirement, which involves feeling excited to be retired
but also feeling a sense of loss about leaving a job. During the “reorientation stage” an individual
re-evaluates their decision to retire after which some individuals may decide to return to work. In
addition, for financial need and work enjoyment, AARP’s (2002) Work and Career Study found
that older adults are motivated to work past typical retirement age to have something interesting to
do and to stay physically active.
In sum, the demographic characteristics of older adult workers are quite diverse. Moreover, there are a
number of factors that have contributed to the growing older adult workforce. Given that many older
adults need and/or desire to remain in the workplace, their demographic is important to those concerned
with occupational ergonomics. Aging is associated with a variety of changes in abilities that may affect the
older adult worker, including sensation and perception, cognition, and motor control. To ensure the
safety and effectiveness of older workers, it is essential to understand these changes and the impact
they may have on older adult workers.
on older workers. Age-related anatomic and physiologic changes have been well-documented.
For example, pupil size decreases with age, which is associated with a condition called senile miosis
(Loewenfeld, 1979). Senile miosis is most severe and disruptive in low-illumination conditions
(Loewenfeld, 1979). Age-associated yellowing and opacification of the lens can also lead to vision diffi-
culties in low-illumination conditions (Weale, 1992).
In addition to anatomic and physiologic changes, aging is associated with changes in visual func-
tioning, the most notable of which are declines in visual acuity, which occur progressively after 50
years of age (Schieber and Baldwin, 1996). Presbyopia, a condition associated with loss of flexibility
in the lens resulting in a decreased ability to accommodate (ciliary muscles are not strong enough to
focus thickened lens on either near or far targets in the environment), is the major factor that affects
visual acuity (Fozard and Gordon-Salant, 2001). Visual acuity declines are amplified in low-illumina-
tion and with low-contrast objects (Sturr et al., 1990). The acuity of peripheral vision is also reduced
with age (Kline and Scialfa, 1996). Similarly, the useful field of view, which is the functional visual
field, declines with age (Cerella, 1985). Age-related declines in visual acuity may affect the reading
speed of older adults. However, Akutsu (1991) found that older adults read as fast as younger
adults, unless the characters are very small (less than 0.38) or very large (1.08). The same range of
character sizes that maximized reading speed for older adults was optimal for younger adults
as well. Hence, older adults may benefit from adjusting the size of icons and text presented on
computers. In addition, printed materials should be designed such that the character size is
between 0.3 and 1.08.
The ability of the eye to adapt to darkness is reduced with age, which may contribute to night vision
problems, which are frequently experienced by older adults (Jackson et al., 1999). Contrast sensitivity
refers to the amount of contrast that is necessary between a target and its background for the target
to be perceived. Contrast sensitivity loss is associated with aging, especially at higher spatial frequencies
(Owsley et al., 1983). Glare is also much more disruptive for older adults than for young adults and older
adults’ eyes take longer to recover from glare (Pulling, 1980). Increased sensitivity to glare may make
reading from a computer screen particularly difficult for older adults (Park, 1992). Aging is also associ-
ated with loss of color discrimination (Fiorentini et al., 1996), which may make color discrimination
more difficult for older adults, particularly blue/green comparisons (Charness and Bosman, 1992).
Visual declines associated with aging may impact the work performance of older adults, particularly if
tasks rely heavily on processing visual information or if task parameters are not changed to
accommodate age-related declines in vision. Glare-reducing monitors and individually adjusted contrast
levels on monitors could aid in reducing the effects of age-related contrast sensitivity loss and glare
sensitivity.
Age-related hearing loss is one of the most prevalent chronic disorders reported by older adults
(Willott, 1991). Physiological changes in the inner ear that cooccur with chronological age often
result in presbycusis, a condition associated with decreased sensitivity to higher frequency sounds
(i.e., over 6000 Hz). Accompanying presbycusis, a lifetime of previous work experience in noisy work
environments may result in noise-induced hearing loss such that sensitivity to mid-range frequencies
(i.e., 4000 Hz) is also reduced. Because presbycusis and noise-induced hearing are additive, older
workers may demonstrate decreased sensitivity to much of the auditory spectrum. As a consequence,
older adults often experience difficulty in processing auditory information such as speech and demon-
strate a reduced ability to filter out background noise (Kline and Scialfa, 1997). Deficits in speech per-
ception are quite small when older adults are presented with auditory stimuli in a quiet environment,
however, these deficits become substantial as background noise increases (Helfer, 1992). Furthermore,
older adults tend to miss information from multiple speakers in a noisy environment because they
have to selectively attend to pertinent information received by one ear while ignoring competing infor-
mation from the other (Barr and Giambra, 1990). Many of these factors associated with age-related
auditory decline may interact in the work environment to reduce older adults’ abilities to meet
job-related demands.
Ergonomics and Aging 17-5
17.2 Intelligence
Current theories view intelligence as having multiple aspects, rather than being a unitary ability. One
conceptualization of intelligence incorporates two types of abilities: fluid intelligence, which relate to
understanding new, complex relationships and making inferences and conclusions that resolve complex-
ities (Masunaga and Horn, 2001) and crystallized intelligence, which includes breadth and depth of
knowledge (Cattell, 1941, 1943). Fluid abilities are more likely to show age-related declines, whereas
crystallized abilities are less likely to show age-related declines (e.g., Salthouse, 1991; Smith et al.,
1989) (see Figure 17.1 for theoretical representation).
One drawback of traditional measures of intelligence is their inability to accurately gauge individual-
specific intelligence, such as that obtained by work and life experience. Many researchers have argued that
traditional conceptualizations of intelligence are not comprehensive because they do not incorporate
real-world intelligence, sometimes called practical intelligence (Colonia-Willner, 1998; Dixon and
Baltes, 1988; Wagner and Sternberg, 1988; Willis, 1987). An aspect of practical intelligence is tacit knowl-
edge, which is knowledge about procedures that are relevant for everyday life functioning (Sternberg and
Caruso, 1985). Tacit knowledge is a major contributing factor to achieving success in the workplace
(Wagner and Sternberg, 1991). For example, in a study that investigated managerial practical intelligence
of older bank managers, Colonia-Willner (1998) found stabilization of practical intelligence with aging,
in spite of age-related declines in standard reasoning test performance. It is possible that preserved prac-
tical intelligence may serve as a compensatory mechanism that older adults use to maintain functioning
(Colonia-Willner, 1998).
Another nontraditional aspect of intelligence that is related to real-world knowledge is wisdom.
Wisdom has been conceptualized as “expert knowledge in the pragmatics of life” (Smith et al., 1994,
p. 989), recognition and response to human limitations (Taranto, 1989), “the ability to understand
human nature” (Clayton, 1982, p. 315). That is, wisdom is thought to be related to fundamental ques-
tions about the conduct, interpretation, and meaning of life (Baltes et al., 1995). Many studies of wisdom
used real-world dilemmas, to which participants responded. Participants’ answers were then scored with
20
18
16
Fluid Intelligence
14
Crystallized
Intelligence
12
Mental age
10
0
5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70
Years of Age
FIGURE 17.1 Theoretical description of age changes in fluid and crystallized intelligence (Cattell, 1987).
17-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
respect to criteria, such as knowledge about the conditions of life and its variations, knowledge about strat-
egies of judgment and advice about life matters, knowledge about the contexts of life and their developmen-
tal relations, knowledge about differences in values, goals, and priorities, and knowledge about the relative
indeterminism and unpredictability in life (Baltes et al., 1995; Smith et al., 1994). These studies and others
found age invariance in wisdom (e.g., Smith et al., 1994; Staudinger et al., 1992). One specific characteristic
of wisdom is theory of mind ability. That is, “the ability to attribute independent mental states to self and
others to predict and explain behavior” (Happe et al., 1998, p. 358). Research suggests that theory of mind is
also preserved with age and, moreover, that older adults may have superior theory of mind compared to
younger adults. Hence, while older adults may experience declines on traditional intelligence measures,
particularly those that reflect fluid abilities, they appear to have preserved practical intelligence and wisdom
with which they can compensate.
Spoken language comprehension may also be a significant job-related skill depending on the occu-
pation. Given that spoken language comprehension heavily involves hearing ability, which declines
with age, one would expect significant difficulties related to listening comprehension. However, older
adults rarely complain of difficulties understanding speech (Burke and Harrold, 1988). Yet, research
has found some age-associated decrements with respect to listening comprehension. For example,
Titone et al. (2000) found evidence that older adults do not adjust their listening behavior to meet
the demands of increased difficulty during language processing. In contrast, studies have shown that
older adults do adjust their language processing strategies when other text demands increase, such as per-
ceptual difficulty (e.g., Pichora-Fuller et al., 1995). For a task identifying sentence-final words presented
with varying degrees of noise where words were either predictable or unpredictable, older adults com-
pensated for difficult listening conditions by taking advantage of supportive contextual information to
a greater extent compared to younger adults (Pichora-Fuller et al., 1995). Additional research exploring
the effects of listening in distracting environments, demonstrated that older adults’ listening comprehen-
sion declines are primarily related to hearing decrement (Schneider et al., 2000). Hence, while older
adults can use compensatory mechanisms to maintain listening comprehension, they would benefit
from working in environments that do not have noise distractions.
The ability to understand written discourse, such as legal documentation, contractual materials, and
training manuals, is essential in the work environment. To successfully apply for a job, potential employ-
ees must complete the application and when offered a position, they must be able to read and compre-
hend the terms of employment. When faced with learning to use new office equipment, workers must
comprehend instructional materials. According to the situation model approach to comprehension
(van Dijk and Kintsch, 1983), readers determine the meaning of text by interpreting it in terms of
what they already know and drawing inferences (i.e., making conclusions that are not explicitly
stated). Recent evidence suggests that older adults may be at a disadvantage when they have to draw infer-
ences in novel situations where they cannot utilize their prior knowledge (Hancock et al., 2005).
Factors that might improve older adults’ comprehension of written instructions such as those pre-
sented on office equipment include the use of large font sizes to accommodate visual deficits, simplified
sentence structure, and nontechnical terminology presented at the maximum of a sixth-grade reading
level (Wickens and Hollands, 1999). Other solutions that might facilitate comprehension and retention
of written materials include the use of elaborative memory strategies (Qualls et al., 2001) and the use of
explicit signals that highlight the main ideas and relations in the text (Meyer et al., 1998). Consistent with
the work of Hancock et al. (2005), procedures should be explicitly stated so that older adults do not have
to rely on inferential information.
17.4 Memory
Coupled with the ability to comprehend instructional materials, memory is another age-sensitive cogni-
tive factor that might impact many aspects of an older worker’s performance. For example, to use office
technology a worker must remember the procedures involved in operating a device, but he or she may
also be required to initiate use at specific times or simultaneously store and manipulate incoming infor-
mation during use. As the next sections illustrate, specific types of memory decline with age whereas
others are spared (Smith, 2002).
Working memory tasks require temporary storage and manipulation of information in memory
(Baddeley and Hitch, 1974). The mental calculation required during a visit to the warehouse for office
supplies is an example of a working memory task because a person must mentally calculate the cost
of the items being purchased by constantly updating the total for the items placed on and removed
from the corporate invoice. Age-related differences in working memory are well documented (Craik,
2000) and there is some evidence that working memory decrements increase with task complexity
(Craik et al., 1990).
One instance where age-related differences in working memory might impact an older adult’s inter-
action with office technology is in the use of telephone voicemail menu systems. Older workers using
17-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
telephone voicemail menu systems to check on work-related responsibilities or to inquire about the
status of their paycheck are required to store and process the menu options while attempting to make
navigational decisions. If the structure of the menu system is very broad such that a large number of
options must be considered before a choice can be made, older adults may find themselves forgetting
the content of the options because their working memory capacity is being taxed. As only one option
can be chosen at a time, it must be appropriate for the goals of the older adult. Thus, all options
other than the desired option must be considered as unwanted information and should be ignored.
Furthermore, the speed of menu option presentation is another factor to consider when designing
phone menu systems because older workers may be slower to process information than younger
coworkers.
System modifications used to combat the age-related decline in working memory should result in
increased usability of the telephone menu system. Reducing the number of menu items that have to
be considered at each level of menu hierarchy should reduce working memory demands (Reynolds
et al., 2002). Yet another solution might be to present the most commonly requested menu items
first, thereby reducing the need to ignore unwanted options. Slowing the speed of menu item presen-
tation may also result in a more usable menu system for older adults because they will be given the oppor-
tunity to process all of the available menu options (Reynolds et al., 2002).
Semantic memory refers to the store of factual information that accrues through a lifetime of learning.
Remembering the meaning of symbolic codes used in an account invoice, the telephone number for the
employee lounge, and the route to the purchasing department are all examples of semantic memory
because this is information that was acquired through experience. Age-related differences in the organ-
ization and use of semantic information are slight or nonexistent, although it may take older adults
longer to learn new information. Thus, semantic memory remains intact throughout the lifespan
(Light, 1992). When new information is encountered, it is often interpreted in the context of the pre-
existing knowledge base.
Working memory and semantic memory are forms of retrospective memory or memory for past
events. Prospective memory refers to remembering to do things in the future. For example, remembering
to check a gauge at a certain time or to attend a meeting requires one to remember a task that has to be
performed at some time in the future. Event-based and time-based prospective memory tasks vary by the
demands of the task characteristics (Einstein and McDaniel, 1990). For an event-based task, a cue in the
environment reminds one to perform a prospective task (e.g., placing a note next to the computer as a
reminder to write an office memo). In this context, cues in the environment act as mnemonic or environ-
mental support that increases the likelihood of remembering the prospective task. In contrast, time-based
tasks lack environmental support because they have few external cues. Time-based tasks are largely self-
initiated and require one to perform an action at a certain time or after a specified amount of time has
elapsed (e.g., remembering to make a phone call at 4:30). Age differences in prospective memory are
usually much greater for time-based than event-based tasks (Park et al., 1997). Hence, older workers
are less likely to exhibit declines when performing event-based tasks.
17.5 Decision-Making
On a daily basis, workers encounter situations that require them to make decisions such as when to sche-
dule a meeting or which lever to use to make a particular adjustment. While these decision contexts seem
quite different, they both rely on a decision-making process that requires people to consider multiple
pieces of information, interpret that information in relation to some goal, then select the best course
of action from the alternatives available. Although decision-making is obviously an important cognitive
factor in the workplace, relatively little is known about the decision-making capacity of older adults
because it has been neglected in research on aging (Sanfey and Hastie, 2000).
Of the few studies that have been conducted, it appears that older adults consider fewer pieces of infor-
mation so as not to tax working memory when determining courses of action (Johnson, 1990) and that
Ergonomics and Aging 17-9
they are susceptible to biases such as the framing effect that also influence younger adults’ decisions
(Mayhorn et al., 2002). While some suggest that older adults are more cautious decision-makers than
younger adults (Green et al., 1994), the speed and quality of decision-making is often found to be age
invariant (Dror et al., 1998).
17.6 Attention
Attention is a ubiquitous component to information processing; it involves selection, vigilance, and
control (Parasuraman, 1998). In the workplace, attention ability would be most pertinent for tasks
that involve finding given information among similar information, doing two tasks at the same time,
selecting relevant information while inhibiting irrelevant information, switching from one task to
another, and detecting rarely occurring information over a prolonged period of time. The ability to
apply mental effort to something for a sustained period of time appears to be relatively preserved
with age, unless the task involves perceptual discrimination (Deaton and Parasuraman, 1993).
However, there is evidence that older adults are less able to select relevant information (e.g., Allen
et al., 1994) and to inhibit irrelevant information compared to younger adults (e.g., Hasher and
Zacks, 1988). Furthermore, some research suggests that visual selective attention, which involves select-
ing relevant visual information while inhibiting irrelevant visual information, is influenced by aging (e.g.,
Carlson et al., 1995; Connelly et al., 1991; Hasher et al., 1991; McDowd and Oseas Kreger, 1991).
However, simple displays (Humphrey and Kramer, 1997), low-perceptual load (Madden and Langley,
2003) and adequate practice (Fisk and Rogers, 1991) minimize age differences.
Research has also examined selective attention to auditory information. Although some research has
demonstrated an age-related decline with regard to auditory selective attention (e.g., Barr and Giambra,
1990; Panek and McGown, 1981), more recent research showed that once age-differences in auditory per-
ception were controlled older and younger adults were equally affected by irrelevant information
(Murphy et al., 1999).
Furthermore, in a recent meta-analysis of dual task performance, older adults’ speed and accuracy
were compromised to a greater extent compared to younger adults (Verhaeghen et al., 2003). The rami-
fications of these findings for the workplace are that older adults may perform attention-demanding
tasks better if they are less complex and rely less on working memory. Furthermore, age-related
declines in attention may be minimized when older adults are given adequate practice, when they
are not distracted by irrelevant information, and if they perform complex tasks separately rather
than concurrently.
using a computer mouse to click on a target icon (Card et al., 1978) to manipulating a microscope
(Langolf et al., 1976). However, older adults’ motor control deficits are not universal and performance
among individuals is highly variable given differences in aerobic fitness and cardiovascular status
(Spirduso and MacRae, 1990).
(Brandimonte et al., 1996). For instance, older workers may remind themselves to attend a late afternoon
meeting by leaving their umbrella by the door or placing the appointment in a planner and leaving it
in view on their desk. The use of an alarm or automated reminder is similarly beneficial. The ability
to effectively use these reminder cues as memory aids should decrease the likelihood of forgetting
work-related prospective tasks.
include altering the speed of the mouse clicks or making use of the keyboard to compensate for slow
clicking. Should these accommodations with computer mice fail, older adults might also benefit from
the use of other input devices such as touchscreens that allow direct input or a rotary encoder that
allows precise control (Rogers et al., 2005).
Cognitive changes that influence computer usage include declines in working memory as well as
retention of semantic memory. Working memory declines appear to explain why older adults have
difficulty performing complex computerized tasks such as Internet searches. While searching for
information within a website, older adults must remember what information they are searching
for, where they have looked for it, and where they are currently located within the site (Stronge
et al., 2002). As a result of declining working memory capacity, older adults have been observed to
revisit pages within a website that they have already browsed and have trouble keeping track of the
current page that they are visiting (Mead et al., 1997). Thus, poor working memory might result in
an inaccurate mental representation of the hierarchical structure of a website or more generally, a computer
system.
As semantic memory is preserved in older adults, well-learned tasks that require the use of familiar
computer systems should be minimally impacted by the aging process. Problems arise when technology
and systems are upgraded, thereby forcing older workers to acquire new skills through training. When
new information is encountered during training, it is often interpreted in the context of the pre-existing
knowledge base. For this reason, instructional materials should be presented using familiar terminol-
ogy that makes reference to earlier technology. Because metaphors tap well-developed crystallized
knowledge, they may be an effective means of simulating experience when teaching older adults
(Bowles et al., 2002).
17.8.10 Training
Although eluded to in previous sections of this chapter, training is an aspect of the workplace that should
not be underrated. Computers and other technology have revolutionized the way almost all workers do
their jobs, from the office to the factory to the farm. Particularly with this ever changing technology,
comes a need for workers to be willing and able to learn new skills. Occupations that require extensive
computer skills tend to hire fewer older adult workers possibly because of perceptions that older workers
have difficulty learning new skills. Indeed, changing technology can be especially difficult for older adults
because they were educated and trained for a different work culture. However, some research has shown
that older adults are able and willing to learn new skills (for a comprehensive review, see Sterns and
Doverspike, 1989). In contrast, one meta-analysis of studies related to aging and training concluded
that older adults demonstrate poorer performance overall, less mastery of relevant material, and
slower performance compared to younger adults (Kubeck et al., 1996). Although the evidence is not con-
clusive whether one type of training is more beneficial for older adults compared to younger adults
Ergonomics and Aging 17-15
(Charness and Bosman, 1997), evidence suggests that older adults are more likely to benefit from training
when they can go at their own pace and when they learn with their age peers (Gist et al., 1988; Knowles,
1987; Shea, 1991). Older adults also benefit from training when it is goal-oriented, allows exploratory
learning, involves a task that is interesting to the learner, permits active participation in the learning
process, involves adequate practice, and provides prompt feedback (Belbin and Belbin, 1968; Bolton,
1978; Fisk and Rogers, 1991; Hiemstra, 1972; Hollis-Sawyer and Sterns, 1999; McGehee and Thayer,
1961; Neale et al., 1968). Moreover, the match between training format and task demands is more critical
to successful performance for older adults (Jamieson and Rogers, 2000; Mea and Fisk, 1998; McLaughlin
et al., in press).
Bibliography
AARP, Staying Ahead of the Curve: The AARP Work and Career Study, American Association of Retired
Persons, Washington, D.C., 2002.
Ahuja, K., Sounds of silence: “Quiet curtains” combine audio privacy and aesthetics for nursing homes,
hospitals, hotels, and offices, Georgia Institute of Technology News Release, http://gtresearch-
news.gatech.edu/newsrelease/CURTAINS.html, 1999.
Akutsu, H. Psychophysics of reading: X. Effects of age-related changes in vision, J. Gerontol, 46(6), pp.
P325–P331, 1991.
Allen, P.A., Weber, T.A. and Madden, D.J. Adult age differences in attention: filtering or selection?,
J. Gerontol., 49(5), pp. P213 –P222, 1994.
Atchley, R.C. The Sociology of Retirement, Schenkman, Cambridge, MA, 1976.
17-16 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Baddeley, A.D. and Hitch, G.J. Working memory, in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Bower,
G.H. ed., Vol. 8, Academic Press, New York, 1974, pp. 47 –90.
Baltes, P.B., Staudinger, U.M., Maercker, A., Smith, J. People nominated as wise: a comparative study of
wisdom-related knowledge, Psychol. Aging, 10, pp. 155–166, 1995.
Barr, R.A. and Giambra, L.M. Age-related decrement in auditory selective attention, Psychol. Aging, 5(4),
pp. 597–599, 1990.
Belbin, E. and Belbin, R.M. New careers in middle age, in Middle Age and Aging: A Reader in Social
Psychology, Neugarten B. ed., University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1968, pp. 341 –350.
Bolton, E.B. Cognitive and noncognitive factors that affect learning in older adults and their implications
for instruction, Educ. Gerontol., 3, pp. 331 –344, 1978
Bowles, C.T., Fisk, A.D. and Rogers, W.A. Inference and the use of similes and metaphors in warnings.
Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Santa Monica, CA: HFES, 2002, pp.
1703– 1707.
Boyce, P.R. Human factors in lighting, Essex, England: Applied Science, 1981.
Brandimonte, M., Einstein, G.O. and McDaniel, M.A. Prospective memory: Theory and applications,
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Mahwah, NJ, 1996.
Bromley, D.B. Aspects of written language production over adult life. Psychol. Aging, 6(2), pp. 296–308,
1991.
Bureau of Labor Statistics (2002). ftp://ftp.bls.gov/pub/news.release/tenure.txt.
Burke, D. Language, aging, and inhibitory deficits: Evaluation of a theory. J. Gerontol. Psychol. Sci., 52B,
pp. P254–P264, 1997.
Burke, D.M. and Harrold, R.M. Automatic and effortful semantic processes in old age; Experimental and
naturalistic approaches, in Language, Memory, and Aging, L.L. Light and D.M. Burke eds.,
Cambridge University Press, New York, 1988, (pp. 100– 116),
Burke, D.M. and Peters, L. Word associations in old age: Evidence for consistency in semantic encoding
during adulthood. Psychol. Aging, 1(4), pp. 283 –291, 1986.
Burtless, G. and Quinn, J.F. Retirement trends and policies to encourage work among older Americans.
Presented at the Annual Conference of the National Academy of Social Insurance, Washington, DC,
2000.
Card, S.K., English, W.K., and Burr, B.J. Evaluation of mouse, rate-controlled isometric joystick, step
keys, and task keys for text selection on a CRT. Ergonomics, 21(8), pp. 601 –613, 1978.
Carlson, M.C., Hasher, L., Connelly, S.L., and Zacks, R.T. Aging, distraction, and the benefits of predict-
able location. Psychol. Aging, 10, pp. 427–436, 1995.
Cattell, R.B. Some theoretical issues in adult intelligence testing. Psychol. Bull., 38, 592, 1941.
Cattell, R.B. The measurement of adult intelligence. Psychol. Bull., 40, pp. 153–193, 1943.
Cerella, J. Age-related decline in extrafoveal letter perception. J. Gerontol., 40(6), pp. 727–736, 1985.
Charness, N. and Bosman, E.A. Human factors and age, in The Handbook of Aging and Cognition,
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc, F.I.M. Craik and T.A. Salthouse, eds., Hillsdale, NJ, 1992,
pp. 495–551.
Charness, N. and Dijkstra, K. Age, luminance, and print legibility in homes, offices, and public places.
Hum. Factors, 41(2), pp. 173 –193, 1999.
Clayton, V. Wisdom and intelligence: the nature and function of knowledge in the later years. Int. J. Aging
Hum. Dev., 15, pp. 315–321, 1982.
Colonia-Willner, R. Practical intelligence at work: Relationship between aging and cognitive efficiency
among managers in a bank environment. Psychol. Aging, 13(1), pp. 45 –57, 1998.
Connelly, S.L., Hasher, L., and Zacks, R.T. Age and reading: The impact of distraction. Psychol. Aging, 6,
pp. 533–541, 1991.
Conzola, V.C. and Wogalter, M.S. Using voice and print directives and warnings to supplement product
manual instructions. Int. J. Ind. Ergon., 23, pp. 549 –556, 1999.
Cooper, P.V. Discourse production and normal aging: Performance on oral picture description tasks.
J. Gerontol. Psychol. Sci., 45, pp. P210–P214, 1990.
Ergonomics and Aging 17-17
Craik, F.I.M. Age-related changes in human memory. in Cognitive Aging: A Primer D.C. Park and
N. Schwartz eds., PA: Taylor and Francis, Philadelphia, 2000, pp. 75 –92.
Craik, F.I.M. and Byrd, M. Aging and cognitive deficits: The role of attentional resources. In Aging and
Cognitive Processes, F.I.M. Craik and S.E. Trehub eds., Plenum, New York, 1982, pp. 191–211.
Craik, F.I.M., Morris, R.G., and Gick, M.L. Adult age differences in working memory. in Neuropsycholo-
gical Impairments of Short-term Memory, G. Vallar, and T. Shallice eds., Cambridge University
Press, Cambridge, England,1990, pp. 247–267.
Davies, D.R. and Sparrow, P.R. Age and work behaviour. in Aging and Human Performance, Charness ed.,
England Wiley, Chichester, 1985, pp. 293 –332.
Deaton, J.E. and Parasuraman, R. Sensory and cognitive vigilance: Effects of age on performance and sub-
jective workload. Hum. Perform., 6, pp. 71 –97, 1993.
Dixon, R.A. and Baltes, P.B. Toward life-span research on the functions and pragmatics of intelligence. in
Practical Intelligence: Nature and Origins of Competence in the Everyday World, R.J. Sternberg and
R.K. Wagner eds., Cambridge University Press, New York, 1988, pp. 203 –235.
Dror, I.E., Katona, M., and Mungur, K. Age differences in decision-making: To take a risk or not?
Gerontology, 44, pp. 67–71, 1998.
Einstein, G.O. and McDaniel, M.A. Normal aging and prospective memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Mem., Learn.,
and Cogn., 16, pp. 717 –726, 1990.
Fiorentini, A., Porciatti, V., Morrone, M.C., and Burr, D.C. Visual ageing: unspecific decline of the
response to luminance and colour. Vision Research, 36, pp. 3557– 3566, 1996.
Fisk, A.D. and Rogers, W.A. Toward an understanding of age-related memory and visual-search effects.
J. Exp. Psychol: General, 120(2), pp. 131–149, 1991.
Fitts, P.M. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of
movement. J. Exp. Psychol., 47, pp. 381 –391, 1954.
Fitts, P.M. and Posner, M.I. Human Performance, Brooks/Cole, Belmont, CA, 1967.
Fozard, J.L. and Popkin, S.J. Optimizing adult development: Ends and means of an applied psychology.
Am. Psychol., 33, pp. 975 –989, 1978.
Fozard, J.L. and Gordon-Salant, S. Changes in vision and hearing with aging. in Handbook of the Psychol-
ogy of Aging. J.E. Birren and K.W. Schaie, eds., 5th ed., Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 2001,
pp. 241–266.
Fullerton H.N. and Toossi, M. Labor force projections to 2010: Steady growth and changing composition.
Mon. Labor Rev., 11, pp. 21 –38, 2001.
Gist, M., Rosen, B., and Schwoerer, C. The influence of training method and trainee age on the acqui-
sition of computer skills. Personnel Psychol., 41, pp. 255 –265, 1988.
Green, L., Fry, A.F., and Myerson, J. Discounting of delayed rewards: A lifespan comparison. Psychol. Sci.,
5, pp. 33–36, 1994.
Gruber, J., Madrian, B.C., and National Bureau of Economic Research. Non-employment and Health
Insurance Coverage, National Bureau of Economic Research, Cambridge, MA, 1995.
Hancock, H.E., Rogers, W.A., and Fisk, A.D. An evaluation of warning habits and beliefs across the adult
lifespan. Hum. Factors, 43(3), pp. 343 –354, 2001.
Hancock, H.E., Fisk, A.D., and Rogers, W.A. Comprehending product warning information: Age-related
effects of memory, inferencing, and knowledge. Human Factors, 47, pp. 219 –234, 2005.
Happe, F.G.E., Brownell, H., and Winner, E. The getting of wisdom: theory of mind in old age. Dev.
Psychol., 34(2), pp. 358– 362, 1998.
Hasher, L. and Zacks, R.T. Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. in
The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, G.H. Bower, ed., Vol. 22, Academic, New York,
1988, pp. 193 –226.
Hasher, L., Stoltzfus, E.R., Zacks, R.T., and Rypma, B. Age and inhibition. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn., Mem.
and Cogn., 17, pp. 163 –169, 1991.
Helfer, K.S. Aging and the binaural advantage in reverberation and noise. J. Speech Hear. Res., 35,
pp. 1394–1401, 1992.
17-18 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Heller, R.B. and Dobbs, A.R. Age differences in word finding in discourse and nondiscourse situations.
Psychol. Aging, 8, pp. 443 –450, 1993.
Hiemstra, R.P. Continuing education for the aged: A survey of needs and interests of older people. Adult
Educ., 22, pp. 100 –109, 1972.
Hollis-Sawyer, L.A. and Sterns, H.L. A novel goal-oriented approach for training older adult computer
novices: Beyond the effects of individual-difference factors. Educ. Gerontol., 25(7), pp. 661 –684,
1999.
Hughes, P.C. and McNelis, J.F. Lighting, productivity, and the work environment. Lighting Res. Des., 8,
pp. 32–40, 1978.
Humphrey, D.G. and Kramer, A.F. Age differences in visual search for feature, conjunction, and triple-
conjunction targets. Psychol. Aging, 12(4), pp. 704 –717, 1997.
Jackson, G.R., Owsley, C., and McGwin, G. Aging and dark adaptation. Vision Research, 39,
pp. 3975–3982, 1999.
James, L.E., Burke, D.M., Austin, A., and Hulme, E. Production and perception of “verbosity” in younger
and older adults. Psychol. Aging, 13(3), pp. 355 –367, 1998.
Jamieson, B.A. and Rogers, W.A. Age-related effects of blocked and random practice schedules on learn-
ing a new technology. J. Gerontol.: Psychol. Sci., 55B, pp. P343 –P353, 2000.
Jaschinski, W. Conditions of emergency lighting. Ergonomics, 25(5), pp. 363 –372, 1982.
Johnson, M.M.S. Age differences in decision making: A process methodology for examining strategic
information processing. J. Gerontol. Psychol. Sci., 45(2), pp. 75–78, 1990.
Kelley, C.L. and Charness, N. Issues in training older adults to use computers. Behav. Inf. Technol., 14(2),
pp. 107–120, 1995.
Kemper, S. Adults’ diaries: Changes made to written narratives across the life-span. Discourse Process., 13,
pp. 207–223, 1990.
Kemper, S. Adults’ sentence fragments: Who, what, when, where, and why. Commun. Res., 19, pp. 332 –
346, 1992.
Kemper, S., Kynette, D., Rash, S., O’Brien, K., and Sprott, R. Life-span changes to adults’ language: Effects
of memory and genre. Appl. Psycholing., 10, pp. 49–66, 1989.
Kemper, S., Herman, R.E., and Lian, C.H.T. The costs of doing two things at once for young and older
adults: Talking while walking, finger tapping, and ignoring speech of noise. Psychol. Aging, 18(2),
pp. 181–192, 2003.
Klein, G. Sources of Power: How People Make Decisions. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1998.
Kline, D.W. and Scialfa, C.T. Visual and auditory aging. in Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, J. Birren
and K.W. Schaie, eds., 4th ed., Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 1996, pp. 181 –203.
Kline, D.W. and Scialfa, C.T. Sensory and perceptual functioning: Basic research and human factors
implications. in Handbook of Human Factors and the Older Adult, A.D. Fisk and W.A. Rogers,
eds., Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 1997, pp. 27 –54.
Klinger, A. Issue Paper: Labour market response to population ageing and other socio-demographic change,
United Nations Economic Commission: United Nations, 2002.
Knowles, M. Adult learning. in Training and Development Handbook, Craig, R., ed., McGraw-Hill,
New York, 1987, pp. 168–179.
Kubeck, J.E., Delp, N.D., Haslett, T.K., and McDaniel, M.A. Does job-related training performance
decline with age? Psychol. Aging, 11(1), pp. 92 –107, 1996.
Langolf, C.D., Chaffin, D.B., and Foulke, S.A. An investigation of Fitts’ Law using a wide range of move-
ment amplitudes. J. Mot. Behav., 8, pp. 113–128, 1976.
Light, L.L. The organization of memory in old age. in The Handbook of Aging and Cognition, F.I.M. Craik
and T.A. Salthouse, Eds., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, 1992, pp. 111 –165.
Light, L.L. and Anderson, P.A. Memory for scripts in young and older adults. Memory, 11, pp. 435–444,
1983.
Light, L.L., Valencia-Laver, D. and Zavis, D. Instantiation of general terms in young and old adults.
Psychol. Aging, 6, pp. 337 –351, 1991.
Ergonomics and Aging 17-19
Loewenfeld, I.E. Pupillary changes related to age. in Topics in Neuro-Opthalmology, H.S. Thompson, ed.,
Williams and Wilkens, Baltimore, MD, 1979.
MacKay, D.G. and Abrams, L. Age-linked declines in retrieving orthographic knowledge: Empirical, prac-
tical, and theoretical implications. Psychol. Aging, 13(4), pp. 647–662, 1998.
Madden, D.J. and Langley, L.K. Age-related changes in selective attention and perceptual load during
visual search. Psychol. Aging, 18(1), pp. 54–67, 2003.
Masunaga, H. and Horn, J. Expertise and age-related changes in components of intelligence. Psychol.
Aging, 16(2), pp. 293–311, 2001.
Mayhorn, C.B., Fisk, A.D. and Whittle, J.D. Decisions, decisions: Analysis of age, cohort, and time of
testing on framing of risky decision options. Hum. Factors, 44(4), pp. 515–521, 2002.
Mayhorn, C.B., Nichols, T.A., Rogers, W.A., and Fisk, A.D. Hazards in the home: Using older adults’
perceptions to inform warning design. Injury Control and Safety Promotion, 11, pp. 211 –218,
2004b.
Mayhorn, C.B. and Podany, K.I. Older adults and warnings. in The Handbook of Warnings, M.S. Wogalter,
ed., (in press).
Mayhorn, C.B., Stronge, A.J., McLaughlin, A.C., and Rogers, W.R. Older adults, computer training, and
the systems approach: A formula for success. Educ. Gerontol., 30, pp. 185 –203, 2004a.
McDowd, J.M. and Oseas Kreger, D.M. Aging, inhibitory processes, and negative priming. J.Gerontol.,
46(6), pp. 340–P345, 1991.
McEvoy, G.M. and Cascio, W.F. Cumulative evidence of the relationship between employee age and job
performance. J. Appl. Psychol., 74(1), pp. 11 –17, 1989.
McGehee, W. and Thayer, P.W. Training in Business and Industry, John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1961.
McLaughlin, A.C., Rogers, W.A. and Fisk, A.D. (in press). Helping patients follow their doctor’s instruc-
tions: Matching instructional media to task demands. in Social and Cognitive Perspectives on
Medical Adherence, D.C. Park and L. Liu, eds., American Psychological Association, Washington,
D.C., (in press).
Mead, S.E. and Fisk, A.D. Measuring skill acquisition and retention with an ATM simulator: The need for
age-specific training. Hum. Factors, 40, pp. 516–523, 1998.
Mead, S.E., Spaulding, V.A., Sit, R.A., Meyer, B. and Walker, N. Effects of age and training on World Wide
Web navigation strategies. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society (pp. 152–156).
HFES, Santa Monica, CA, 1997.
Meyer, B.J.F., Talbot, A., Stubblefield, R.A., and Poon, L.W. Interests and strategies of young and old
readers differentially interact with characteristics of texts. Educ. Gerontol., 24, pp. 747– 771, 1998.
Morrell, R.W., Park, D.C., Mayhorn, C.B., and Kelley, C.L. The effects of age and instructional format on
teaching older adults how to use ELDERCOMM: An electronic bulletin board system. Educ.
Gerontol., 26, pp. 221 –236, 2000.
Morrow, D.G., Yesavage, J., Leirer, V. and Tinklenberg, J. Influence of age and practice on piloting tasks.
Exp. Aging Res., 19(1), pp. 53–70, 1993.
Morrow, D.G., Menard, W.E., Stine-Morrow, E.A.L., Teller, T. and Bryant, D. The influence of expertise
and task factors on age differences in pilot communication. Psychol. Aging, 16(1), pp. 31 –46, 2001.
Morrow, D.G., Ridolfo, H.E., Menard, W.E., Sanborn, A., Stine-Morrow, E.A.L, Magnor, C., Herman, L.,
Teller, T., and Bryant, D. Environmental support promotes the expertise-based mitigation of age
differences on pilot communication tasks. Psychol. Aging, 18(2), pp. 268– 284, 2003.
Murphy, D.R., McDowd, J.M., and Wilcox, K.A. Inhibition and aging: similarities between younger and
older adults as revealed by the processing of unattended auditory information. Psychol. Aging,
14(1), pp. 44–59, 1999.
Neale, J.C., Toye, M.H., and Belbin, E. Adult training: The use of programmed instruction. Occup.
Psychol., 42(1), pp. 23– 31, 1968.
Norman, D.A. The Psychology of Everyday Things, Basic Books, New York, 1988.
Owsley, C., Sekuler, R., and Siemsen, D. Contrast sensitivity throughout adulthood. Vis. Res., 23(7),
pp. 689–699, 1983.
17-20 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Panek, P.E. The older worker. in Handbook of Human Factors and the Older Adult, A.D. Fisk and W.A.
Rogers, eds., Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 1997, pp. 363 –394.
Panek, P.E. and McGown, W.P. Risk-taking across the life-span as measured by an intrusion-omission
ratio on a selective attention task. Percep. Mot. Skills, 52(3), pp. 733 –734, 1981.
Parasuraman, R. The attentive brain: Issues and prospects. in The Attentive Brain R. Parasuraman, ed.,
MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1998, (pp. 3 –15).
Park, D.C. Aging, cognition, and work. Hum. Performance, 7, pp. 181 –205, 1994.
Park, D.C. Applied cognitive aging research. in The Handbook of Aging and Cognition, F.I.M. Craik and
T.A. Salthouse, eds., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc, Hillsdale, NJ, 1992, (pp. 449–493).
Park, D.C., Hertzog, C., Kidder, D.P., Morrell, R.W. and Mayhorn, C.B. The effect of age on event-based
and time-based prospective memory. Psychol. Aging, 12(2), pp. 314 –327, 1997.
Parkinson, D. Voices of experience: mature workers in the future workforce, (Report No. R-1319-02-RR).
The Conference Board. 2002.
Pichora-Fuller, M.K., Schneider, B.A., and Daneman, M. How young and old adults listen to and remem-
ber speech heard in noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 97, pp. 593 –608, 1995.
Pratt, M.W., Boyes, C., Robins, S.. and Manchester, J. Telling tales: Aging, working memory, and the nar-
rative cohesion of storytellers. Dev. Psychol., 25, pp. 628 –635, 1989.
Pratt, M.W. and Robins, S.L. That’s the way it was: Age differences in the structure and quality of adults’
personal narratives. Discourse Process., 14, pp. 73–85, 1991.
Pulling, N.H. Headlight glare resistance and driver age. Hum. Factors, 22(1), pp. 103–112, 1980.
Qualls, C.D., Harris, J.L., and Rogers, W.A. Cognitive-linguistic aging: Considerations for home health
care environments. in Human Factors Interventions for the Health Care of Older Adults, W.A.
Rogers and A.D. Fisk, eds., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, 2001, pp. 47– 67.
Reynolds, C., Czaja, S.J., and Sharit, J. Age and perceptions of usability on telephone menu systems.
Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 46th Annual Meeting HFES, Santa
Monica, CA, 2002, pp. 175 –179.
Rogers, W.A., Fisk, A.D., McLaughlin, A.C., and Pak, R. Touch a screen or turn a knob: Choosing the best
device for the job. Hum. Factors, 47, pp. 271–288, 2005.
Root, W. Injuries at work are fewer among older employees. Mon. Labor Rev., 104, pp. 30–34, 1981.
Rousseau, G.K., Lamson, N., and Rogers, W.A. Designing warnings to compensate for age-related
changes in perceptual and cognitive abilities. Psychol. Market., 15, pp. 643 –662, 1998.
Ryan, E.B., See, S.K., Meneer, W.B., and Trovato, D. Age-based perceptions of conversational skills among
younger and older adults. in Interpersonal Communication in Older Adulthood: Interdisciplinary
Theory and Research, M.L. Hummert, J.M. Wieman and J.F. Nussbaum, eds., Sage, Thousand
Oaks, CA, 1994, pp. 15 –39.
Salthouse, T.A. Effects of age and skill in typing. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 113, pp. 345 –371, 1984.
Salthouse, T.A. Theoretical Perspectives on Cognitive Aging, L. Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, 1991.
Salthouse, T.A. and Mauer, Aging, job performance, and career development. In Handbook of the Psychol-
ogy of Aging, 4th ed., J. Birren and K.W. Schaie, eds., Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 1996.
pp. 353–364.
Sanfey, A.G. and Hastie, R. Judgment and decision-making across the adult life span: A tutorial review of
psychological research. in Cognitive Aging: A Primer, D.C. Park and N. Schwartz, eds., Taylor and
Francis, Philadelphia, 2000, pp. 253– 273.
Schieber, F. and Baldwin, C.L. Vision, audition, and aging research. In Perspectives on Cognitive Change in
Adulthood and Aging, F. Blanchard-Fields and T.M. Hess, eds., McGraw-Hill, New York, 1996,
pp. 122–162.
Schneider, B.A., Daneman, M., Murphy, D.R., and Kwong See, S. Listening to discourse in distracting
settings: The effects of aging. Psychol. Aging, 15(1), pp. 110–125, 2000.
Shea, G. Managing Older Employees, Jossey-Bass Inc, San Francisco, 1991.
Sit, R.A. and Fisk, A.D. Age-related performance in a multiple-task environment. Hum. Factors, 41(1),
pp. 26–34, 1999.
Ergonomics and Aging 17-21
Smith, A.D. Consideration of memory functioning in healthcare intervention with older adults. in
Human Factors Interventions for the Healthcare of Older Adults, W.A. Rogers and A.D. Fisk, eds.,
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, 2002, (pp. 31–46).
Smith, J. Dixon, R.A., and Baltes, P.B. Expertise in life planning: A new research approach to investigating
aspects of wisdom. in Adult development, Vol. 1: Comparisons and Applications of Developmental
Models, M.L. Commons and J.D. Sinnott, eds., England: Praeger Publishers, New York, 1989,
(pp. 307 –331).
Smith, J., Staudinger, U.M., and Baltes, P.B. Occupational settings facilitating wisdom-related knowledge:
The sample case of clinical psychologists. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol,, 62(5), pp. 989 –999, 1994.
Smith, M.W., Sharit, J., and Czaja, S.J. Aging, motor control, and the performance of computer mouse
tasks. Hum. Factors, 41(3), pp. 389 – 396, 1999.
Sofranko, A. “Rocking chair remains idle for many Illinois farmers.” ACES News, July 12, 2000. College of
Agricultural, Consumer and Environmental Sciences. University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign,
(2000)
Sparrow, P.R. and Davies, D.R. Effects of age, tenure, training, and job complexity on technical perform-
ance. Psychol. Aging, 3, pp. 307–314, 1988.
Spirduso, W.W. and MacRae, P.G. Motor performance and aging. in Handbook of the Psychology of
Aging 3rd ed., J.E. Birren and K.W. Schaie, eds., Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 1990,
pp. 183–200.
Staudinger, U.M., Smith, J., and Baltes, P.B. Wisdom-related knowledge in a life review task: age differ-
ences and the role of professional specialization. Psychol. Aging, 7, pp. 271–281, 1992.
Sternberg, R.J. and Caruso, D. Practical modes of knowing. in Learning the Ways of Knowing E. Eisner,
ed., Chicago University Press, Chicago, 1985, pp. 133–158.
Sterns, H.L. Training and retraining adult and older adult workers. n Age, Health, and Employment
J.E. Birren, P.K. Robinson, and J.E. Livingston, eds., Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ, 1986,
pp. 93–113.
Sterns, H.L. and Doverspike, D. Aging and the training and learning process. in Training and
Development in Organizations I.L. Goldstein, ed., Jossey-Bass/Pfeiffer, San Francisco, 1989,
pp. 299–332.
Stronge, A.J., Walker, N., and Rogers, W.A. Searching the World Wide Web: Can older adults get what
they need? in Human Factors Interventions for the Healthcare of Older Adults, W.A. Rogers and
A.D. Fisk, eds., Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, 2002, (pp. 255 –269).
Sturr, J.F., Kline, G.E., and Taub, H.A. Performance of young and older drivers on a static acuity test
under photopic and mesopic luminance conditions. Hum. Factors, 32(1), pp. 1 –8, 1990.
Szinovácz, M., Ekerdt, D.J., and Vinick, B.H. Families and Retirement, Newbury Park: Sage Publications,
1992.
Taranto, M.A. Facets of wisdom: A theoretical synthesis. Int. Aging Hum. Dev., 29(1), pp. 1 –21, 1989.
Taylor, H.J. Older Workers: A valuable resource for the workplace. in Working Through Demographic
Change: How OlderAamericans Can Sustain the Nation’s Prosperity, W. Zinke and S. Tattershall,
eds., Human Resource Services, Inc, Boulder, CO, 2000, pp. 5 –14.
Titone, D., Prentice, K.J., and Wingfield, A. Resource allocation during spoken discourse processing:
Effects of age and passage difficulty as revealed by self-paced listening. Mem. Cogn., 28(6), pp.
1029–1040, 2000.
Ulatowska, H.K., Hayashi, M.M., Cannito, M.P., and Fleming, S. Disruption of reference in aging. Brain
Lang., 28, pp. 24– 41, 1986.
U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics (2004). Employment Status of the Civilian Noninstitutional Population by
Age, Sex, and Race. Retrieved September 18, 2005, from http://www.bls.gov/cps/cpsaat3.pdf.
U.S. General Accounting Office. Older Workers, GAO-02-85-95-152. Washington, D.C. GAO, 2001.
van Dijk, T. and Kintsch, W. Strategies of Discourse Comprehension, Academic Press, New York, NY, 1983.
Vercrucyssen, M. Movement control and speed of behavior. in Handbook of Human Factors and the Older
Adult, A.D. Fisk and W.A. Rogers, eds., Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 1997, (pp. 55–86).
17-22 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Verhaeghen, P., Steitz, D.W., Sliwinski, M.J., and Cerella, J. Aging and dual-task performance: A meta-
analysis. Psychol. Aging, 18(3), pp. 443–460, 2003.
Wagner, R.K. and Sternberg, R.J. Tacit knowledge and intelligence in the everyday world. in Practical
Intelligence: Nature and Origins of Competence in the Everyday World, R.J. Sternberg and R.K.
Wagner, eds., Cambridge University Press, New York, 1988, (pp. 51 –83).
Wagner, R.K. and Sternberg, R.J. Tacit knowledge: its uses in identifying, assessing, and developing man-
agerial talent. in Applying Psychology in Business: The Manager’s Handbook, J. Jones, B. Steffy, and
D. Bray, eds., Human Sciences Press, New York, 1991, (pp. 333 –344).
Walker, N., Philbin, D.A., and Fisk, A.D. Age-related differences in movement control: Adjusting
submovement structure to optimize performance. J. Gerontol. Psychol. Sci., 52B, pp. P40–P52,
1997.
Walker, N., Philbin, D.A., and Spruell, C. The use of signal detection theory on age-related differences
in movement control. in Aging and Skilled Performance: Advances in Theory and Application,
W.A. Rogers, A.D. Fisk and N. Walker, eds., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, 1996,
pp. 45–64.
Weale, R.A. The Senescence of Human Vision, Oxford University Press, Oxford, England, 1992.
Wechsler, D. WAIS-R Manual, Psychological Corporation, New York, 1981.
Welford, A.T. Signal noise, performance, and age. Hum. Factors, 23, pp. 97–109, 1981.
Wickens, C.D. and Hollands, J.G. Engineering Psychology and Human Performance 3rd ed.. Prentice Hall,
Upper Saddle River, NJ, 1999.
Willott, J. Aging and the Auditory System, Singular Publishing Group, San Diego, CA, 1991.
Wogalter, M.S., Dejoy, D.M., and Laughery, K.R. Warnings and Risk Communication, Taylor and Francis,
London, 1999.
Wogalter, M.S. and Mayhorn, C.B. Providing cognitive support with technology-based warning systems.
Ergonomics, 48(5), pp. 522–533, 2005.
Zsambok, C.E. and Klein, G. Naturalistic Decision Making, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ,
1997.
18
Vision and Work
Vision is an integral part of nearly every job. Since vision is integral to nearly every job, a corollary is that
each job has a minimum level of vision or visual skills that is required for its proper performance.1
For some jobs the minimum level of vision is specified as a job requirement. Such vision requirements
are strictly applied in situations where job performance is specifically dependent upon particular visual
characteristics or skills and where the cost of nonperformance is high in terms of safety and/or clearly
identifiable costs. A common example is driving, a job (or task) for which a minimum level of corrected
visual acuity is required. Some states also have visual field (peripheral vision) requirements for driver
licensure. Other critical occupations such as law enforcement officers, firefighters, pilots, and military
among others have minimum vision requirements to obtain and/or retain employment. These vision
requirements can include corrected visual acuity, uncorrected visual acuity, limits on refractive error
magnitude, binocular vision, depth perception, color discrimination, visual fields, and limits on patho-
logical conditions. Another example is requiring normal color vision for a job such as quality control
inspection where good color discrimination is important. In many cases minimum visual requirements
have been challenged legally on the grounds that they are discriminative. If the visual requirements can be
shown to be bonafide as related to critical task performance then they are usually defensible.
Although relatively few jobs have formal vision standards, good vision is a prerequisite for optimal
performance and visual comfort for most jobs. Vision is often screened at the time of application or
hire as a means to determine if the applicants have the necessary visual skills to perform certain tasks
and/or to identify those applicants who may be in need of better visual correction prior to hire.
Besides poor performance, inadequate visual skills and/or improper visual correction can result in
eye-related symptoms such as eyestrain, headache, irritated eyes, blurred vision, and fatigue. These pro-
blems are common when performing tasks at near working distances because they place great demands
upon accommodation (eye focusing mechanism) and binocular alignment. Such symptoms have been
particularly common among computer workers where specific workplace ergonomic factors often
increase stress upon the visual system. The costs of sub-optimally corrected vision can be quite high
with respect to the costs of providing eye care (Daum et al., 2004).
1
Although people without vision can also work, considerable adaptations to the job are usually required. This
chapter addresses the visual aspects of work for those who are able to attain relatively normal visual acuity either
with or without optical correction. Adaptation of the work environment to the blind or those with low vision is
not covered in this chapter.
18-1
18-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
The objectives of this chapter are to describe and identify the parameters of vision that are important
to job performance, and to provide information about those parameters that, based upon experience, will
be useful to ergonomic practitioners and researchers.
Myopia and astigmatism will certainly reduce visual acuity. Uncorrected visual acuity for different
levels of myopia is shown in Table 18.1 (Pincus, 1946; Crawford et al., 1945; Peters, 1961). Hyperopia
will reduce visual acuity less predictably because younger individuals can use their accommodative mech-
anism (eye focusing ability) to add power to the eye in order to eliminate or reduce the blur caused by the
hyperopia. Accommodative ability predictably declines with age (thus necessitating reading glasses or
bifocals), therefore the visual acuity losses due to myopia and hyperopia approach one another in
older age groups. Proper spectacle and/or contact lens correction will significantly improve visual
acuity in most eyes with a refractive error. It was reported (Zerbe and Hofstetter, 1958) that 98% of
the population between the ages of 14 and 40 can be corrected to 20/20 vision in at least one eye.
Section 18.2.3.1 on Presbyopia) in which it is complicated with the need for optical assistance at near
viewing distances.
Driving is a task for which there is a minimum visual acuity requirement. Most studies on the relation-
ships between driving and visual measurements have shown weak relationships at best (Bailey and
Sheedy, 1988). The reasons for these poor relationships are many, including the facts that licensure
screening eliminates people with poor vision from the driving population and individuals with
poorer vision self-limit their driving. Most (41 of 50) states require a visual acuity of 20/40 in order
to obtain a regular driver’s license. Most states also will consider issuing a driver’s license (usually
restricted) for reduced levels of visual acuity. Most states also have stricter acuity requirements for
commercial driver licenses. Another aspect of visual acuity and driving is the standard to which
highway signs are designed, which is 1 in. of letter height for every 50 ft of recognition distance
(Mace, 1988). Mathematically this transforms to a requirement for 20/23 visual acuity.
Many occupations have specific visual acuity requirements (Mahlman, 1982). The requirements are
usually for corrected visual acuity, that is, the worker can meet the requirement with spectacles or
contact lenses. For example, a corrected visual acuity standard of 20/20 for police officers was justified
on the basis of the need to identify city street signs at appropriate distances when driving in emergency
conditions (Sheedy, 1980). Corrected visual acuity standards apply to occupations such as pilots (20/20
each eye), FBI (20/20 in one eye, 20/40 in the other) U.S. Border Patrol (20/20 each eye), firefighters
(20/30 binocular), and commercial truck drivers (20/40 in each eye), among others.
Some occupations also have requirements for uncorrected visual acuity, that is, a minimum level of
visual acuity is required without optical correction. Uncorrected visual acuity standards typically
apply for occupations where performance is critical and especially if it is feasible that glasses or
contact lenses could be dislodged during work such as for police officers. Examples of uncorrected
acuity requirements include firefighters (20/100 binocular), police officers (usually between 20/40–
20/200 dependent on jurisdiction), FBI (20/200), and U.S. Secret Service (20/60 each eye). In many
occupations with an uncorrected visual acuity standard, job applicants are allowed to have refractive
surgery to meet the standard.
The effects of visual acuity upon performance of only a few tasks have been studied. One study (Good
and Augsburger, 1987) analyzed the level of visual acuity required for adequate performance with a
firearm. Fifty subjects performed a “friend or foe” task under different levels of visual acuity (20 ft
viewing distance, 10 cd/M2), analysis determined that 20/45 vision was required to perform at the
threshold performance level. Facial recognition is a complex visual task, but one that can be related to
visual acuity (Bullimore et al., 1991). With photopic (daytime) lighting (100 cd/M2) subjects could
effectively identify faces at 20 ft with 20/72 vision and identify facial expressions with 20/80 vision.
The equivalent acuities required at 20 ft viewing distance for lower light levels were 20/44 and 20/52.
Another means to assess visual capabilities associated with different levels of visual acuity is to utilize
accepted medical or insurance categorizations. For example, best-corrected vision of 20/200 or worse is
defined as legal blindness in nearly all states and allows income tax advantage. Another useful categori-
zation of abilities related to visual acuity is provided by the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-
9-CM, 2004) that is used for insurance reimbursements, shown in Table 18.2. Functionality of different
visual acuity levels can also be assessed with the visual efficiencies accepted by the American Medical
Association (PDR, 1998) used to determine post-trauma compensation, shown in Table 18.3.
The acuity level at which individuals decide to wear glasses or contact lenses habitually is an indication
of the acuity level at which people feel compromised in the performance of everyday tasks. Numerous
variables can influence the acuity level at which ophthalmic correction is worn habitually: age,
demands of visual tasks, illumination levels (day or night), individual tolerance to blur, and cosmetic
concerns among others. In a survey (U.S., 1964) of 7710 persons, only 2% of those not wearing
glasses had a visual acuity of less than 20/100. Unfortunately, they did not provide any details
between 20/20 and 20/100. An analysis of 9468 screening records of West German factory workers,
reported the habitual binocular visual acuities (with usual eye correction if worn) at work (Schober,
4 –6 April, 1968). Only 3.62% of the workers had distance visual acuity of 20/50 or worse, and only
2.29% had near acuity of 20/50 or worse. Acuities between 20/29 and 20/40 at distance were measured
in 13.8% of the workers and at near in 9.09%. These data indicate that very few factory workers have
habitual acuities of 20/50 or worse — an indication that for these acuity levels most workers choose
to habitually wear ophthalmic correction. It appears that most people with acuity in the range of
20/32 to 20/50 will choose to habitually wear optical correction.
will blur distance visual acuity for most people, but a person with hyperopia will relax accommodation
and hence still attain good visual acuity through the þ1.00 lens. Hence, a worker fails the screening test if
visual acuity is not reduced with the þ1.00 D lens.
18.2.2.1 Accommodation
Accommodation is the mechanism by which the eye changes its focus to look at near objects (see
Figure 18.1). The maximum amount by which the eye can change its power is termed the “amplitude
of accommodation.” This is measured for a corrected eye by slowly moving an object with small detail
towards the eye and noting the distance at which the subject reports first noticeable blur. The distance
(in meters) is measured from the spectacle plane (14 mm in front of the corneal apex) to the point of
first blur. The inverse of this distance is the amplitude of accommodation in Diopters. The amplitude
of accommodation quite predictably decreases with age (Donder, 1864; Duane, 1922; Turner, 1958) as
shown in Table 18.4. Some workers have amplitudes that are reduced compared to age-expected
values — often causing eye-related symptoms.
The near point of accommodation represents the closest distance to which a worker can bring an object
and keep it focused without optical assistance. However, accommodative fatigue limits a worker from
viewing at the near point of accommodation for very long. Clinical experience indicates that individuals
can sustain only approximately 50% of their maximum amplitude of accommodation. The 50% accom-
modative effort distance is also shown in Table 18.4.
Most people under the age of 40 can comfortably use accommodation to meet typical near viewing
needs. However, some workers have accommodative disorders such as reduced amplitude of accommo-
dation for their age or accommodative infacility — a condition in which the accommodative state is slow
to change. Such disorders are very common in clinical practice (Sheedy and Parsons, 1990) and clinical
studies have shown that workers with these conditions have symptoms of discomfort such as eyestrain
(Hennessey et al., 1984; Levine et al., 1985). Intermittent blurring of near objects is caused by inability
of the accommodative mechanism to maintain steady focus on near objects. Blurring of distant objects
occurs when, after extended near work, the ciliary muscle remains somewhat contracted in the near pos-
ition. This effectively makes the eye myopic when looking at distance and can last several hours after
extended near work, affecting after-work driving. These disorders are also associated with generalized
symptoms such as general fatigue, concentration difficulties, dizziness, and headaches (Jaschinski-
Kruza and Schweflinghaus, 1992). Workers with accommodative disorders can usually be treated with
TABLE 18.4 The Normal Relationships of Amplitude of Accommodation and Sustainable Near Working Distance
to Age
Max Amp of Near Point of Sustainable Sustainable Near Sustainable Near
Age Accom. (D) Accom. (cm) Accom. (D) Distance (cm) Distance (in.)
10 14.0 7.1 7.0 14.3 5.6
15 12.0 8.3 6.0 16.7 6.6
20 10.0 10.0 5.0 20.0 7.9
25 8.5 11.8 4.3 23.5 9.3
30 7.0 14.3 3.5 28.6 11.2
35 5.5 18.2 2.8 36.4 14.3
40 4.5 22.2 2.3 44.4 17.5
45 3.5 28.6 1.8 57.1 22.5
50 2.5 40.0 1.3 80.0 31.5
55 1.8 57.1 0.9 114.3 45.0
60 1.0 100.0 0.5 200.0 78.7
65 0.5 200.0 0.3 400.0 157.5
70 0.3 400.0 0.1 800.0 315.0
75 0.0 1 0 1 1
18-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
low power plus spectacle lenses that relieve the accommodative effort required to see clearly at near
working distances.
18.2.3.1 Presbyopia
As shown in Table 18.4, the amplitude of accommodation gradually decreases throughout life and the
sustainable near working distance recedes with age. A person has presbyopia when, as result, they can
no longer comfortably see their near work. Workers typically begin to have problems with normal
near working distances of 40 –50 cm between the ages of 40 to 45. The primary symptoms are intermit-
tent blur at near working distances, although the problem may also express as eyestrain.
Presbyopia is most commonly corrected with plus power lenses in the form of single-vision lenses or
multifocal lenses, discussed later in this section. The lens prescribed for a presbyopic individual depends
upon the amount of remaining accommodation and the working distance. Once a person reaches the age
of 60, accommodation has essentially reduced to zero and the lens power is nearly totally based upon the
required viewing distance.
Add
FIGURE 18.4 Lens designs for correction of presbyopia (a) flat top bifocal; (b) executive bifocal; (c) flat top trifocal;
(d) executive trifocal; (e) double executive; (f) double flat top.
The most common general usage presbyopic lens designs are the flat top bifocal (Figure 18.4a) and the
progressive addition lens (PAL — Figure 18.5a). As usually prescribed and fitted, the bifocal provides a
wide clear field of vision at 40 cm viewing distance. The near field of vision is typically attained with
20 –258 of downward gaze. The usual width of 28 mm provides a wide fixation field of approximately
508, however, wider bifocal segments are available.
Computer workers commonly have problems with flat top bifocals because the computer is at an inter-
mediate viewing distance (50–70 cm) and intermediate gaze angle (10 –158 downward). The bifocal
wearer must tilt their head backward and lean inward, resulting in awkward posture and musculoskeletal
discomfort. The worker can either see the computer display clearly or have comfortable posture — but
not both. Solutions include a trifocal lens (Figure 18.4c) that includes the intermediate power in the
intermediate section (usually with a greater than normal height of the intermediate segment), or a
bifocal design in which the intermediate power is placed on the top of the lens and the near power in
the bottom. These lenses are occupational glasses and will meet vision needs at work, but they do not
meet the general visual needs outside of the workplace. The occupational progressive lens
(Figure 18.5b) could also be a solution, however, if the person wears a segmented bifocal for general
wear it is usually more acceptable to also wear one for work. With proper selection of powers in the
(a) (b)
Far I
I
I
N
N
FIGURE 18.5 (a) Typical progressive addition lens and (b) typical occupational progressive lens. D, distance, I,
intermediate, N, near. Contours show increasing levels of unwanted aberrations.
18-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
work lenses, the computer worker can easily navigate the workplace while wearing the computer glasses.
This is important for most computer users and other workers with intermediate viewing needs —
because it is inconvenient to change glasses each time they move to or from the workstation.
Progressive addition lenses (Figure 18.5a) are another common lens design used to correct presbyopia.
These lenses are characterized by a smooth continuous change of power across the lens surface but with
undesired aberrations in the lower quadrants that limit viewing through those areas. PALs have cosmetic
advantages and also optical advantages compared to standard bifocals and are generally preferred over
bifocals by most wearers. However, even though progressive addition lenses provide a region with inter-
mediate power, this is the portion of the lens with the narrowest field of clear vision. If the worker
requires significant viewing at intermediate distances such as at a computer, then they must continually
find the small sweet spot on the lens and use their neck to move the head rather than moving their eyes.
This is inefficient and results in blur and musculoskeletal stress.
Occupational progressive lenses (OPL — Figure 18.5b) can very successfully meet the needs of pres-
byopic workers with extensive intermediate viewing needs such as computer users, assembly line workers,
assemblers, clerks, janitorial, general office work, etc. The lens design provides a reasonably large inter-
mediate viewing zone straight ahead, a wide near viewing zone in the bottom of the lens, and far-
intermediate vision on the top of the lens to enable the worker to navigate the workplace. The magnitude
of unwanted aberrations in an OPL is significantly less than in a PAL because the total power change is
less and the power poles are farther apart, resulting in wider viewing zones compared to the PAL. OPLs
are also a work pair of spectacles that do not meet general viewing needs outside of the workplace.
Workers with extensive near or intermediate viewing needs, especially at a wide workstation, benefit
from an Executive bifocal or trifocal (Figure 18.4b and Figure 18.4d). This design provides very wide
fields of clear, near, and intermediate vision, however, the complete near correction in the bottom of
the lens can make workplace navigation difficult.
Many presbyopic workers with near or intermediate viewing needs and superior gaze angles need
double segment lens designs such as in Figure 18.4e and Figure 18.4f. These jobs include pharmacists,
librarians, mechanics, drapery hangers, painters, etc. The double segment design enables better job per-
formance and posture.
Presbyopic workers are generally unaware of the mismatch between their visual needs at work and the
vision provided by their general wear glasses. Likewise such mismatches are not frequently identified
during routine eye examinations. The problems discussed above are not common among younger pres-
byopic individuals who still have some remaining accommodation because the lens power prescribed for
near viewing distances is relatively small and they have greater ranges of viewing distances through the
distance and near portions of their lenses. The problems noted earlier become most common for workers
aged 50 and above. When awkward postures are noted in presbyopic workers, the typical viewing dis-
tances and gaze angles should be noted and conveyed to the eye doctor for optimal occupational prescrip-
tion and lens design. In the situations discussed earlier, the spectacles are prescribed and designed to meet
the specific visual tasks in the workplace and should be provided by the employer.
TABLE 18.5 Initial and Final Resting Position of the Eyes When Fixating
Peripheral Stimuli at Different Angles (degress)
Lateral stimulus amount 10 20 30 40 50
Initial eye rotation 10 20 28 33 41
Final eye rotation 2 5 11 15 19
Because of the strong preference to use head rather than eye movements, workers primarily adjust
body posture to a peripheral target in order that the eyes can operate comfortably. Clearly, “the eyes
lead the body.” If the worker must fixate a peripheral target for extended periods, the awkward
posture can result in musculoskeletal symptoms. It has been shown that optimal performance and
comfort are attained with a downward gaze angle of 10 –158 and straight ahead in the lateral meridian
(Sheedy and Shaw-McMinn, 2002). Ideally visual tasks should be located so the eyes are depressed by
approximately 108 with the worker in a neutral musculoskeletal posture. For example, computer displays
should be located straight in front of the worker laterally, and the height adjusted so that screen center is
approximately 10 cm lower than the eyes (60 cm viewing distance) — resulting in 108 downward gaze.
60
40
20
Vertical (degrees)
0
−100 −80 −60 −40 −20 0 20 40 60 80 100
−20
−40
−60
−80
Horizontal (degrees)
FIGURE 18.6 Peripheral vision (visual fields) with eyes fixating straight ahead. Monocular fields overlap to create a
larger binocular visual field.
for injury and caution should be used in placing monocular workers. Monocular workers should always
wear eye protection — they don’t have a spare.
compared to both eyes opened (Sheedy et al., 1986). Stereopsis provides the worker with enhanced ability
to properly maneuver objects with respect to one another. Stereopsis can also be important for some
workers with critical intermediate distance tasks — such as forklift drivers.
Individuals without stereopsis or with significantly reduced stereopsis should probably not be placed
in jobs that benefit highly from it. Monocular individuals, of course, have a total loss of stereopsis. The
effects of the loss of stereopsis in everyday task performance have been described by Brady, who wrote a
book based upon his traumatic loss of an eye (Brady, 1972). Likewise, individuals who have two eyes but
are unable to keep them aligned (a condition named “strabismus”) do not have stereopsis. Also, some
individuals are able to keep their eyes aligned but have amblyopia (reduced acuity in one eye) and
hence significantly reduced stereopsis. Likewise, stereopsis is reduced if a worker has reduced acuity in
one eye due to uncorrected refractive error, this situation can normally be resolved by obtaining
proper correction. Various vision screening devices are available to identify individuals with reduced
or absent stereopsis.
18.2.3.5.2 Binocular Alignment — Phoria and Convergence
Even though most workers keep both eyes aligned when viewing an object, many have difficulty main-
taining this ocular alignment resulting in symptoms such as fatigue, headache, blur, double vision, and
general ocular discomfort. These problems are much more common among workers with demanding
near and intermediate visual needs such as viewing
printed text or computer displays. Binocular
alignment at near viewing distances is more
complex than for distance viewing because of the
required ocular convergence and the interaction
between ocular convergence and accommodation.
Phoria Binocular fusion, the sensory process by
which the images from each eye are combined to
form a single percept, requires sensory input from
each of the two eyes. If the eyes are not properly
aligned on the fixation object, double vision will
result. The brain continuously feeds information
back to the eye alignment muscles in order to
maintain ocular alignment and avoid double
vision. Without sensory feedback from each eye
(e.g., occluding an eye as in Figure 18.7), the eyes
assume their “position of rest” with respect to one
another. If the position of rest is outward or
diverged, the patient has exophoria. If it is inward
or converged, the condition is esophoria.
Most workers have at least a small phoria.
Whether a worker experiences symptoms depends
upon the amount of the misalignment, the ability
of the worker to overcome that misalignment,
and the task demands. The symptoms associated
with phoria can be eyestrain, double vision, head-
ache, eye irritation, and general fatigue. Clinical
studies have documented the relationships
between the visual clinical measurements and the
symptoms that are experienced (Sheedy and
Saladin, 1978). Various vision screening devices FIGURE 18.7 Binocular alignment when fusion is
enable measurement of the phoria and provide denied by occluding an eye. (a) Orthophoria. (b)
guidelines for eye care referral. Esophoria. (c) Exophoria.
18-14 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
2
Color vision tests are available at Richmond Products, Richmond, CA.
Vision and Work 18-15
test, more exact testing requires examination with an anomaloscope available only in large clinics. In
some cases such as airline pilots, boat captains, and train engineers, it is critical to identify the color
of small lights. Job-related color vision testing can be performed with the Farnsworth Lantern test.
The testing distance from the chart to the subjects’ eyes should be maintained and specified. For
each acuity measurement it is important to note the eye being tested (right or left), corrective con-
dition of the eye (uncorrected, spectacles, contact lenses), and testing distance. Most commonly, visual
acuity is measured with each eye individually (monocular visual acuity), but may also be measured
with both eyes open (binocular visual acuity). The subject should be prompted to guess at letters
near the size threshold, many subjects will identify 1–2 additional lines with encouragement. Subjects
should be instructed to not squint the eyelids during testing and the examiner should enforce
nonsquinting.
Dependent upon the refractive condition, visual acuity can be different at far distances compared
to near distances. Hence acuity charts are also available for testing of near visual acuity. For
near visual acuity testing, it is particularly important to maintain the correct distance of the chart to
the eye.
Source: Sorsby, A., Refractive Anomalies of the Eye, Washington, DC: U.S. Dept of HEW, 1967.
References
ANSI, American national standard for ophthalmics — instruments — general purpose clinical visual
acuity charts, American National Standards Institute, 1992.
Bailey, I.L. and Lovie, J.E., New design principles for visual acuity letter charts, Am. J. Optom. Physiol.
Opt., 53(11), pp. 740–745, 1976.
Bailey, I.L. and Sheedy, J.E., Vision screening and driver licensure, in Transportation in an Aging Society:
Improving Mobility and Safety for Older Persons. TRB Special Report 218, Transportation Research
Board, Washington, D.C., 1988.
Brady, F.B., A Singular View: The Art of Seeing with One Eye, Medical Economics, Oradell, NJ, 1972.
Bullimore, M.A., Bailey, I.L., and Wacker, R.T., Face recognition in age-related maculopathy, Invest.
Ophthalmol. Visual Sci., 32(7), pp. 2020–2029, 1991.
Butzon, S.P., Sheedy, J.E., and Nilsen, E., The efficacy of computer glasses in reduction of computer
worker symptoms, Optometry, 73(4), pp. 221 –230, 2002.
Vision and Work 18-17
Crawford, J.S., Shagass, C, and Pashby, T.J., Relationship between visual acuity and refractive error in
myopia, Amer. J. Ophth., 28, pp. 1220 –1225, 1945.
Daum, K.M., Clore, K.A., Simms, S.S., Vesely, J.W., Wilczek, D.D., Spittle, B.M., and Good, G.W.,
Productivity associated with visual status of computer users, Optometry, 75(1), pp. 33 –47,
2004.
Donder, F.C., On the Anomalies of Accommodation and Refraction of the Eye. The New Sydenham Society,
London, 1864.
Duane, A., Studies in monocular and bincocular accommodation with their clinical applications,
Am. J. Ophthalmol., 5, p. 865, 1922.
Good, G.W. and Augsburger, A.R., Uncorrected visual acuity requirements for police applicants, J. Police.
Sci. Admin., 15, pp. 18 –23, 1987.
Good, G.W. and Fogt, N., Dynamic visual fields of one-eyed observers, Opto. Vis. Sci., 75(12S),
p. 104, 1998.
Grisham, J.D., Visual therapy results for convergence insufficiency: a literature review, Am. J. Optom.
Physiol. Opt., 65(6), pp. 448 –454, 1988.
Hennessey, D., Iosue, R.A., and Rouse, M.W., Relation of symptoms to accommodative infacility of
school-aged children, Am. J. Optom. Physiol. Opt., 61(3), pp. 177–183, 1984.
ICD-9-CM, International Classification of Diseases, 9th Revision, Clinical Modification, American Medical
Association, Ann Arbor, MI, 2004.
ISO, Ophthalmic optics — visual acuity testing — standard optotype and its presentation, in Inter-
national Standards Organization, American National Standards Institute, New York, 1994.
Jaschinski-Kruza, W. and Schweflinghaus, W., Relations between dark accommodation and psycho-
somatic symptoms, Ophthal. Physiol Opt., 12(1), pp. 103 –105, 1992.
Johnson, C.A. and Keltner, J.L., Incidence of visual field loss in 20,000 eyes and its relationship to driving
performance, Arch. Ophthalmol., 101(3), pp. 371– 375, 1983.
Levine, S., Ciuffreda, K.J., Selenow, A. and Flax, N., Clinical assessment of accommodative facility in
symptomatic and asymptomatic individuals, J. Am. Optom. Assoc., 56(4), pp. 286 –290, 1985.
Mace, D.J., Sign legibility and conspicuity, in Transportation in an Aging Society: Improving Mobility and
Safety for Older Persons. TRB Special Report 218, Washington, D.C., 1988.
Mahlman, H.E., Handbook of federal vision requirements, Chicago: Professional Press, Inc, 1982.
PDR., Physicians Desk Reference for Ophthalmology, Montvale, NJ: Medical Economics Company,
Inc, 1998.
Peters, H.B., The relationship between refractive error and visual acuity at three age levels, Amer.
J. Optom. Arch. Amer. Acad. Optom., 38, pp. 194–198, 1961.
Pincus, M.H., Unaided visual acuities correlated with refractive errors, Am. J. Ophth., 29, pp. 853–
858, 1946.
Schober, H.A.W., April 4-6, The role of exact eye correction in industrial hygiene. Paper read at First
International Seminar Vision Science, at Bloomington, IN, 1968.
Sheedy, J.E., Police vision standards, J. Police. Sci. Admin., 8(3), pp. 275–285, 1980.
Sheedy, J.E. and Parsons, S.D., The Video Display Terminal Eye Clinic: clinical report, Optom. Vis. Sci.,
67(8), pp. 622–626, 1990.
Sheedy, J.E. and Saladin, J.J., Association of symptoms with measures of oculomotor deficiencies, Am.
J. Optom. Physiol. Opt., 55(10), pp. 670 –676, 1978.
Sheedy, J.E. and Shaw-McMinn, P., Diagnosing and treating computer-related vision problems, Woburn,
MA: Butterworth-Heinemann, 2002.
Sheedy, J.E. and Stocker, E.G., Surrogate color vision by luster discrimination, Am. J. Optom. Physiol.
Opt., 61(8), pp. 499–505, 1984.
Sheedy, J.E., Bailey, I.L., Buri, M., and Bass, E., Binocular vs. monocular task performance, Am. J. Optom.
Physiol. Opt., 63(10), pp. 839 –846, 1986.
Sorsby, A., The nature of spherical refractive errors, in Refractive Anomalies of the Eye, Washington, D.C.:
U.S. Dept of HEW, 1967.
18-18 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Turner, M.J., Observations of the normal subjective amplitude of accommodation, Bri. J. Physiol. Opt.,
15(2), pp. 70– 100, 1958.
Uemura, T., Arai, Y., and Shimazaki, C., Eye-head coordination during lateral gaze in normal subjects,
Acta. Otolaryngol., 90(3–4), pp. 191 –198, 1980.
U.S., Binocular visual acuity of adults. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Dept of Health Education and Welfare,
National Center for Health Statistics, 1964.
Villanueva, M. B., Jonai, H., Sotoyama, M., Hisanaga, N., Takeuchi, Y., and Saito, S., Sitting posture and
neck and shoulder muscle activities at different screen height settings of the visual display terminal,
Ind. Health., 35(3), pp. 330 –336, 1997.
Wiggins, N. P., Visual discomfort and astigmatic refractive errors in VDT use. J. Am. Optom. Assoc., 62(9),
pp. 680–684, 1991.
Yamashiro, M., Objective measurement of the limit of uniocular movement, Japanese J. Ophthalmol., 1,
pp. 130–136, 1957.
Zeltzer, H.I., Use of modified X-Chrom for relief of light dazzlement and color blindness of a rod mono-
chromat, J. Am. Optom. Assoc., 50(7), pp. 813 –818, 1979.
Zerbe, L.B. and Hofstetter, H.W., Prevalence of 20/20 with best, previous and no lens correction, J. Am.
Optom. Assoc., 29, pp. 772–776, 1958.
19
Individual Factors and
Musculoskeletal
Disorders
19.1 Introduction
Musculoskeletal (MSK) disorders are common experiences in the lives of many people worldwide
(Volinn, 1997). They are also an important source of disability in both Canada (Cole et al., 2001) and
the U.S. The latter is well described in the chapter “Dimensions of the Problem” of a recent report by
the National Research Council/Institute of Medicine (NRC/IOM) Panel on Musculoskeletal Disorders
(NRC/IOM Panel, 2001). Yet not everyone has an MSK disorder and many with MSK disorders do
not suffer much disability, leading the public, practitioners, and researchers to ask: what factors increase
the likelihood of experiencing an MSK disorder and suffering an MSK disability?
Among the factors considered are individual factors, although the term may mean different things to
different practitioners, policy-makers, and researchers. From a social epidemiology perspective
(Berkman and Kawachi, 2000), individuals are nested within families, schools, neighborhoods, work-
places, and other social institutions, which are themselves located in different cities, states, provinces,
and countries. Individual factors are therefore attributes of individual persons rather than the social
organizations or geographic entities of which they are a part. As with most health outcomes, MSK
disorders are likely multi-factorial in origin, with influences at multiple levels spinning webs of causation
(Krieger, 1994). For occupational epidemiologists, such as those that participated in the U.S. National
Institute of Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH) review (Bernard, 1997), individual factors
are often construed as nonworkplace factors in contrast to workplace factors that contribute to
19-1
19-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
work-related MSK disorders (see appendix B in NIOSH report). In the more resent NRC/IOM Panel
report, individual factors are thought to affect personal responses to workplace exposures and tend to
be thought of as physiological and psychological attributes in contrast to biomechanical characteristics
(NRC/IOM Panel on Musculoskeletal Disorders, 2001).
Conceptualizing the nature and meaning of individual factors therefore remains a challenge. The aim
of this article is to share an approach to consideration of individual factors in MSK disorders. It draws on
experience of the first author as a primary care physician, occupational medicine/community medicine
specialist, and clinical/population epidemiologist. Given the multiple specific questions that might be
involved, our citation of evidence is illustrative rather than comprehensive, making reference to systema-
tic reviews when available or individual studies when useful, and pointing to complementary treatment
in other articles in this volume when appropriate. We start by exploring what individual factors rep-
resent. We go on to consider the similar and different roles of individual factors as they operate in the
course of an MSK disorder and the ways that their contribution can be estimated. We then note both
the rationale for consideration of individual factors and the directions we might go in considering indi-
vidual factors in our joint efforts to reduce the burden of MSK disability.
Physical differences between workers, such as variability in body size and height, can give rise
to anthropometric mismatches between individuals and their job demands, such as when taller or
shorter people are placed in “average” nonadjustable workstations (Chung et al., 1997). Equipment
usability problems may be exacerbated by anthropometric differences (Botha and Bridger, 1998).
Variation in muscle bulk or physical capacity may influence the biomechanical exposures from tools
such as a hand-held powered nutrunner, with small women experiencing greater demands than large
men (Oh and Radwin, 1998). Psychological differences can also result in different biomechanical
exposures at the tissue level. Those scoring higher on different personality traits, as measured by the
Meyer-Briggs Trait Inventory, showed differential trunk kinematics while carrying out standard lifting
tasks (Marras et al., 2000).
19.2.3 Vulnerabilities
Closely related are underlying genetic factors with their resultant contribution to both physical structure
and chemical environments in ways that make individuals more vulnerable to MSK disorders. Familial
19-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
tendencies have long been recognized for different forms of arthritis (Hirsch et al., 1998) but
recently, researchers investigating CTS among twin pairs, estimated that upto half of the variability in
prevalence among women may be genetically determined (Hakim et al., 2002).
Acquired vulnerabilities are more commonly recognized, particularly the contribution of one MSK
episode to the risk of subsequent ones. Among a cohort of soldiers, the observed risk of injury was
seven times greater among previously injured individuals (Schneider et al., 2000). Earlier distress or
depression, often called individual psychosocial factors in contrast to workplace or job psychosocial
factors, have been shown to predict subsequent MSK pain or poorer recovery in an episode of pain in
a wide range of studies, as set out in “Table 4.8 of the NRC/IOM Panel report.”
Finally, indicators of social vulnerability have also been associated with MSK disorders. Divorced and
widowed injured workers took considerably longer to return to work or go off with temporary total
workers’ compensation benefits than those who were single or married (Clarke et al., 1999). Racial dis-
crimination may affect subordinate groups, as suggested by the higher levels of MSK pain but lower
intensity of diagnostic assessment of low back pain among black patients in the U.S. (Carey and
Garret, 2003). Further evidence of complex socio-economic and health relationships comes from exam-
ination of healthcare and income support utilization prior to and subsequent to workers’ compensation
claims in British Columbia, Canada. It appeared that a particularly vulnerable group had consistently
higher utilization both before and after the accident date than the rest of the population cohort
(Hertzman et al., 1999).
19.3.1 Etiology
Etiology was the main focus of the NIOSH systematic review (Bernard, 1997) and the NRC/IOM Panel
report. Particularly consistent in those reviews were the role of age and gender in contributing to expla-
nation of variations in prevalence or incidence of MSK disorders. Multiple reviews of etiology of MSK
disorders have included individual factors, for example, age, personality, and work technique, were all
factors, which were found to modify the burden of shoulder-neck complaints (Winkel and Westgaard,
1992).
EXPOSURE OUTCOMES
RISK FACTORS
FIGURE 19.1 Where do individual factors operate in the course of a MSK disorder?
Individual Factors and Musculoskeletal Disorders 19-5
Linked to etiology is the role of individual factors in reporting and recognizing MSK disorders. Gender
has been shown to influence both reporting and recognition of MSK disorders in relation to workplace
exposures (Chung et al., 2000). Similarly, those of lower socio-economic status or only temporarily
employed may feel anxious about reporting an MSK disorder to their workplace.
19.3.2 Prognosis
Prognostic studies examine the factors that affect recovery from (or conversely chronicity of) an MSK
disorder, either in a clinical setting or using workers’ compensation claim (WC) databases. Clinical
studies are often quite specific as to the kind of disorder, while the WC studies may group heterogeneous
soft tissue MSK conditions. Studies using WC claimants often have cost figures associated with them. The
review papers by Baldwin (2004) and Gatchel (2004) comment extensively on such studies, with the latter
including a number of individual factors that predict prognosis.
Across both kinds of studies, condition-related factors are among the most important. These include
duration of symptoms and initial pain severity. The former is important for low back pain (Frank et al.,
1996; Carey et al., 2003; van Tulder et al., 2002) and upper extremity disorders (Cole and Hudak, 1996),
while the latter is also important for low back pain and for neck pain (Coté et al., 2001). Closely allied to
these are specific signs such as radiation below knee for low back pain or location of pain for lateral elbow
pain (Hudak et al., 1996).
The way individuals respond to their MSK disorder is a second important group of characteristic.
Baseline mental health status explained variation in recovery from CTS in the main cohort health
(Katz et al., 1997), while depression is a common predictor of poor recovery among low back pain
patients. Patient or claimant expectations of recovery were found to explain about 15% of the variation
in time on total temporary WC benefits for MSK soft-tissue disorders, with those having more positive
expectations of returning to work or going off with benefits sooner (Cole et al., 2002a).
Some might regard both condition characteristics and individual responses as clinical factors rather
than “individual” factors, though both would be nonworkplace and nonbiomechanical. As noted
earlier, age and gender also influence prognosis for MSK disorders including a broad group of WC
claimants (Hogg-Johnson and Cole, 2003) as well as more specific conditions such as whiplash (Coté
et al., 2001).
Of note, is it that individual factors may make similar or different contributions, depending on where
in the course of a disorder they are operating? For example, in modeling change in symptom and dis-
ability levels between two surveys of newspaper workers, 1 yr apart, we found that gender and job
tenure acted in different ways for those who were not cases at one time and those who had more
severe problems (Cole et al., 2002b). Caseness was defined on the basis of frequency, intensity, and dur-
ation of MSK symptoms or the extent of limitation of activities. Being a women was protective for non-
cases, that is, they were less likely than men to become cases between surveys, yet if they were more severe
cases, they had reduced chances of recovery, both in symptoms and disability. Similarly, those with longer
tenure at the newspaper were less likely to become cases over the year (survivors), but if they already had
a more severe problem, they were less likely to get better between the surveys. Hence the importance of
clarifying where in the course individual factors are operating.
pain (Brisson et al., 1999) and that shorter assembly line workers experienced greater reductions in dis-
comfort in legs and low back, when flooring was modified do be more absorbent (King, 2002).
19.5 Directions
Measurement of individual factors will remain important in research on MSK disorders, particularly for
the growing area of genetic factors. We face considerable challenges in understanding what individual
factors represent and the mechanisms by which individual factors affect MSK disorders. We need to
better link individual factors to the different periods in the course of MSK disorders, comparing etiolo-
gical and prognostic roles. In keeping with notions of a web of causation (Krieger, 1994), we need to
maintain the perspective of multiple versus single causes linked over time in pathways of causation.
The latter would emphasize independent and combined contributions to explanation of variance and
the role of heterogeneity, not only within and across populations but also across effect measures.
Finally, we must incorporate our understandings into practice and policy in ethical ways, improving pro-
tection for the vulnerable through prevention and accommodation and better matching of contexts,
people and interventions, to improve intervention effectiveness.
Acknowledgments
Epidemiological, clinical, biomechanical, and other colleagues for ongoing discussions of these ideas. Bill
Marras, Tom Waters, and STAR-MSK participants for stimulating the development of the framework and
providing a forum for discussion of these ideas. Participating workplaces, practitioners, organizations,
and individuals in studies cited. Workplace Safety and Insurance Board of Ontario funding to the Insti-
tute for Work and Health. The U.S. National Institute of Occupational Safety and Health and other
funders for support.
References
Balci, N., Balci, M.K., and Tuzuner, S., Shoulder adhesive capsulitis and shoulder range of motion in type
II diabetes mellitus: association with diabetic complications, J. Diabetes Complications, 13(3),
pp. 135–140, 1999.
Baldwin, M.L., Reducing the costs of work-related musculoskeletal disorders: targeting strategies to
chronic disability cases, J. Electromyog. Kinesiol., 14(1), pp. 33 –41, 2004.
Berkman, L.F. and Kawachi, I., eds., Social Epidemiology, Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2000.
Bernard, B.P. ed., Musculoskeletal disorders and workplace factors. A critical review of epidemiologic
evidence for work-related musculoskeletal disorders of the neck, upper extremity and low back,
National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health, Cincinnati, OH, DHHS (NIOSH)
Publication No. 97-141, 1997.
Bland, J.D.P., Do nerve conduction studies predict the outcome of carpal tunnel decompression?, Muscle
Nerve, 24(7), pp. 935 –940, 2001.
19-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Bongers, P.M., Kremer, A.M., and ter Laak, J. Are psychosocial factors, risk factors for symptoms of the
shoulder, elbow or hand/wrist?: a review of the epidemiological literature, Ame. J. Ind. Med., 41(5),
pp. 315–342, 2002.
Botha, W.E. and Bridger, R.S. Anthropometric variability, equipment usability and musculoskeletal pain in
a group of nurses in the Western Cape. Occup. Health Ind. Med., 39(6), 252, 1998.
Brisson, C., Montreuil, S., and Punnett, L. Effects of an ergonomic training program on workers with
video display units. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health, 25(3), pp. 255 –263, 1999.
Brisson, C., Larocque, B., and Bourbonnais, R., Les contraintes psychosociales au travail chez les
Canadiennes et les Canadiens, Canadian Journal of Public Health 92(6), 460–467, 2001.
Buckle, P. Upper limb disorders and work: The importance of physical and psychosocial factors,
J. Psychosom. Res., 43(1), pp. 17–25, 1997.
Carey, T.S. and Garrett, J.M. The relation of race to outcome and the use of health care services for acute
low back pain. Spine, 28(4), pp. 390–394, 2003.
Chung, J., Cole, D.C., Clarke, and J. Women, work and injury. Chapter 4 in: T. Sullivan ed., Injury and the
New World of Work. University of British Columbia Press: Vancouver 2000, pp. 69 –90.
Chung, M.K. and Kyungim, C. Ergonomic anlaysis of musculoskeletal discomforts among conversational
V.D.T. operators, Comput. Ind. Eng., 33(3– 4), pp. 521 –524, 1997.
Clarke, J., Chung, J., Cole, D.C., Hogg-Johnson, S., Haidar and the ECC Prognosis Working Group. Gender
and benefit duration in lost time work-related soft tissue disorders: relationships with work and social
factors. I.W.H. Working Paper #85. Toronto, O.N., Canada: Institute for Work & Health 1999.
Cole, D.C. and Hudak, P.L. Understanding prognosis of non-specific work related musculoskeletal dis-
orders of the neck and upper extremity, Am. J. Ind. Med., 29, pp. 657 –668, 1996.
Cole, D.C., Ibrahim, S.A., Shannon, H.S., Scott, F., and Eyles, J. Work correlates of back problems and
activity restriction due to musculoskeletal disorders in the Canadian National Population
Health Survey (NPHS) 1994/95 Data, Occup. Environ. Med., 58, pp. 728 –734, 2001.
Cole, D.C., Mondloch, M.V., and Hogg-Johnson, S., ECC Prognostic Modeling Group. Listening to injured
workers: how recovery expectations predict outcomes?, Can. Med. Assoc. J. 166, pp. 749–754, 2002a.
Cole, D.C., Manno, M., Beaton, D., and Swift, M. Transitions in self-reported musculoskeletal pain and
interference with activities among newspaper workers. J. Occup. Rehab., 12(3), pp. 163–174, 2002b.
Coté P, Cassidy, J.D., Carroll, L., Frank, J.W., and Bombardier, C. A systematic review of the prognosis of
acute whiplash and a new conceptual framework to synthesize the literature. Spine, 26(19),
pp. 445–458, 2001.
Elders, L.A.M., van der Beek, A.J., and Burdorf, A. Return to work after sickness absence due to back
disorders-a systematic review on intervention strategies. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health,
73(5), pp. 339–348, 2000.
Evers, A.W.M., Kraaimaat, F.W., van Riel, P.L.C.M, and Bijlsma, J.W.J. Cognitive, behavioral and phys-
iological reactivity to pain as a predictor of long-term pain in rheumatoid arthritis patients.
Pain, 93, pp. 139 –146, 2001.
Feuerstein, M. Workstyle: Definition, empirical support and implications for prevention, evaluation and
rehabilitation of occupational upper-extremity disorders. In: Moon, S.D., Sauter, S.L. eds. Beyond
Biomechanics: Psychosocial Aspects of Musculoskeletal Disorders in Office Work. Taylor and Francis:-
Bristo, PA, pp. 177 –206, 1996.
Feuerstein, M., Carosella, A.M., and Burrell, L.M. Occupational upper extremity symptoms in sign
language interpreters: Prevalence and correlates of pain, function, and work disability, J. Occup.
Rehab., 7(4), pp. 187 –205, 1997.
Frank, J.W., Brooker, A.S., Demaio, S.E., Kerr, M.S., Maetzel, A., Shannon, H.S., Sullivan, T., Norman,
R.W., and Wells, R.P. Disability resulting from occupational low back pain. Part, II: What do we
know about secondary prevention? A review of the scientific literature on prevention after disabil-
ity begins. Spine, 21(24), pp. 2918– 2929, 1996.
Gatchel, R.J. Musculoskeletal disorders: primary and secondary interventions, J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol.,
14(1), pp. 161–170, 2004.
Individual Factors and Musculoskeletal Disorders 19-9
Gatchel, R.J., Polatin, P.B., and Mayer, T.G. The dominant role of psychosocial risk factors in the devel-
opment of chronic low back pain disability. Spine, 20(24), pp. 2702 –2709, 1995.
Hakim, A.J., Cherkas, L., El Zayat, S.E., Macgregor, A.J., and Spector, T.D. The genetic contribution to
carpal tunnel syndrom in women: a twin study. Arthritis Rheum., 47(3), pp. 275 –279, 2002.
Hertzman, C., McGrail, K., and Hirtle, B. Overall pattern of health care and social welfare use by injured
workers in the British Columbia cohort. Int. J. Law Psychiatry, 22 (5–6), pp. 581–601, 1999.
Hirsch, R., Lin, J.P., Scott, W.W. Jr, Ma, L.D., Pillemer, S.R., Kastner, D.L., Jacobsson, L.T., Bloch, D.A.,
Knowler, W.C., Bennett, P.H., and Bale, S.J. Rheumatoid arthritis in the Pima Indians: the
intersection of epidemiologic, demographic and genealogic data. Arthritis Rheum., 41(8),
pp. 1464–1469, 1998.
Hogg-Johnson, S. and Cole, D.C., Early prognostic factors for duration on temporary total benefits in the
first year among workers with compensated occupational soft tissue injuries, Occupational and
Environmental Medicine 60, 244–253, 2003.
Hudak, P.L., Cole, D.C., and Haines, A.T. Understanding prognosis to improve rehabilitation: the case of
lateral elbow pain. Arch. Phys. Medi. Rehab., 77, pp. 586 –593, 1996.
Katz, J.N., Keller, R.B., Fossel, A.H., and Punnett, L. Predictors of return to work following carpal tunnel
release, Am. J. Ind. Med., 31(1), pp. 85–91, 1997.
Kerr, M. The importance of psychosocial risk factors in injury. Ch 5 in: T. Sullivan ed., Injury and the new
world of work. University of British Columbia Press:Vancouver, 2000 pp. 93–114.
Kerr, M.S., Frank, J.W., Shannon, H.S., Norman, R.W., Wells, R.P., and Neumann, W.P. Examining the
overlap between measures of the psychosocial and physical demands of work: data from case-
control study of risk factors for low back pain. Institute for Work & Health working paper #181. 2001.
King, P.M., A comparison of the effects of floor mats and show in-soles on standing fatigue, Appl. Ergon.,
33, pp. 477–484, 2002.
Krieger, N., Epidemiology and the web of causation: has anyone seen the spider? Social Sci. Med., 39(7),
pp. 887–903, 1994.
Leino-Arjas, P. Smoking and musculoskeletal disorders in the metal industry: A prospective study. Occup.
Health Ind. Med., 40(2), 99, 1999.
Marras, W.S., Davis, K.G., Heaney, C.A., Maronitis, A., and Allread, W.G. The influence of psychosocial
stress, gender, and personality on mechanical loading of the lumber spine. Spine, 25(23),
pp. 3045–3054, 2000.
McGill, S.M. Low back stability: from formal description to issues for performance and rehabilitation.
Exerc. Sports Sci. Rev., 29(1), pp. 26– 31, 2001.
Mergler, D., Brabant, C., Vézina N., and Messing, D. The weaker sex? Men in women’s working conditions
report similar health symptoms. J. Occup. Med., 29, pp. 417 –421, 1987.
Messing, K., Chatigny, C., and Courville, J. ‘Light’ and ‘heavy’ work in the housekeeping service of a hos-
pital. Appl. Ergon., 29(6), pp. 451 –459, 1998.
Messing, K., Neis, B., and Dumais, L., eds. Invisible: Issues in women’s occupational health. Charlotte-
town, PEI: Gynergy Books, 1995.
Montreuil, S., Laflamme, L., and Tellier, C. Profile of the musculoskeletal pain suffered by textile tufting
workers handling thread cones according to work, age and employment duration, Ergonomics
39(1), 76–91, 1996.
National Research Council and Institute of Medicine Panel on Musculoskeletal Disorders and the Work-
place (NRC/IDM), Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Musculoske-
letal disorders and the workplace: low back and upper extremities, National Academy Press:
Washington, D.C., U.S.A. 2001. Available at http://www.nap.edu
Oh, S.A. and Radwin, R.G. The influence of target torque and torque build-up time on physical stress in
right angle nutrunner operation. Ergonomics, 41(2), pp. 188–206, 1998.
Punnett, L. and van der Beek, A.J. A comparison of approaches to modelling the relationship between
ergonomic exposures and upper extremity disorders, American Journal of Industrial Medicine
37(6), 645–655, 2000.
19-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Rothman, K.J. and Greenland, S., eds., Modern epidemiology, 2nd Edn., Lippincott Williams & Wilkins,
Philadelphia, 1998.
Schneider, G.A., Bigelow, C., and Amoroso, P.J. Evaluating risk of re-injury among 1214 army airborne
soldiers using a stratified survival model. Am. J. Prev. Med., 18(3 Suppl), pp. 156 –163, 2000.
Spector, T.D. The menopause, oestrogens and arthritis. Maturitas, 27(1), 15, 1997.
Szabo, R.M. Dissent. Appendix, B in: National Research Council and Institute of Medicine Panel on
Musculoskeletal Disorders and the Workplace, Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and
Education. Musculoskeletal Disorders and the Workplace: Low Back and Upper Extremities. National
Academy Press, Washington, D.C., 2001, pp. 439–457.
Van der Windt, D.A.W.M., Thomas, E., Pope, D.P., de Winter, A.F., Macfarlane, G.J., Bouter, L.M., and
Silman, A.J. Occupational risk factors for shoulder pain: a systematic review. Occup. Environ. Med.,
57, pp. 433–442, 2000.
Van Tulder, M., Koes, B., and Bombardier, C. Low back pain. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Rheumatol., 16(5),
pp. 761–775, 2002.
Vézina, N., Prévost, J., and Lajoie, A. Élaboration d’une formation à l’affilage des couteaux dans six usines
d’abattage et de transformation du porc: une étude ergonomique. Montréal, Québec: Institut de
Recherche en Santé et Securité au Travail, 2000. 48 pages
Vezina, N. and Chatigny, C. Training in factories: A case study of knife-sharpening. Saf. Sci., 23(2/3), 195,
1996.
Volinn, E. The epidemiology of low back pain in the rest of the world. A review of surveys in low- and
middle-income countries. Spine, 22, pp. 1747 –1754, 1997.
Weimer, L.H., Yin, J., Lovelace, R.E., and Gooch, C.L. Serial studies of carpal tunnel syndrome during and
after pregnancy. Muscle Nerve, 25(6), pp. 914 –917, 2002.
Winkel, J. and Westgard, R. Occupational and individual risk factors for shoulder-neck complaints:
part II — the scientific basis (literature review) for the guide, Int. J. Ind. Ergon., 10:
pp. 85–104, 1992.
20
Rehabilitating Low
Back Disorders
REDUCING THE IMPACT OF LOW BACK DISORDERS (LBD) requires the best efforts of those experts in pre-
vention together with those in rehabilitation. Rehabilitation approaches will not work well if the
patient/worker remains exposed to the cause — hence, optional rehabilitation must incorporate excel-
lent prevention or ergonomics. By the same token, prevention involves not only primary prevention but,
since a problem often pre-exists, secondary level prevention is also required. Thus the best prevention
efforts must understand and involve evidence-based principles of rehabilitation. The purpose of this
chapter is to illustrate this symbiosis and compliment the ergonomics concepts explained in other chap-
ters with some notion of rehabilitation exercise for LBDs.
Injury to low back tissues leads to joint instability and very well documented changes in motor/
motion patterns. Because of the strong link to instability and the effective use of specific exercise
approaches to restore stability, reduce pain, and enhance function, this chapter will focus on the
injury process and develop a synthesis of the scientific foundation and formalization of the notion of
stability as it pertains to the lumbar spine, and then provide specific guidelines for enhancing stability
to advance spine rehabilitation. While a large book could be written to describe ideal exercise programs
for the entire population including chronic low back pain sufferers, adolescents to geriatrics, through to
elite athletes, the focus of the exercises discussed here is more towards the beginner’s program — developing
the safest exercise for enhancing stability and for acquiring and maintaining low back health. For the
interested reader, more extensive references together with tabulated data of specific muscle activation
20-1
20-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
profiles, resultant spine loads, etc., can be found in the authors’ review chapters and original papers listed
at www.ahs.uwaterloo.ca/kin/kinfac/mcgill.html or in my recent textbooks. “Low Back Disorders:
Evidence Based Prevention and Rehabilitation” and “Ultimate Backfitness and Performance.”35
In most traditional approaches to designing low back exercise, an emphasis has been placed on
the immediate restoration, or enhancement, of spine range of motion and muscle strength. Generally,
this approach has not been sufficiently efficacious in reducing back troubles, in fact a review of the
evidence suggests only a weak link with improving back symptoms while some studies suggest a
link with negative outcome in significant numbers of people.31 It appears that the emphasis on
early restoration of spine range of motion continues to be driven by legislative definitions of low
back disability — namely loss of range of motion (ROM). Thus, therapeutic success is often judged
on motion restored. Most recent work suggests little correlation between ROM and work versatility
ratings.39 The underlying theme of this chapter, and in fact book, reflects the developing philosophy
based on mechanisms of injury and stability — that a spine must first be stable before moments
and forces are produced to enhance performance but to do so in a way that spares the spine from
potentially injurious load. Preliminary field evidence (although not yet definitive) suggests that the
approach has promise.
then the dominant low back extensors with their unique force vector direction (specifically longisimus
thoracis and iliocostalis lumborum) support the shear reaction forces caused by the action of
gravity on the flexed torso, resulting in a lowering of the shear load experienced by the joint. These
forces would normally be borne by the disc and facet joints. However, if the individual elects to flex
the spine itself when bending forward sufficiently so as to stretch the posterior ligaments with full
spinal flexion, then the architecture of the interspinous ligaments cause anterior shear forces21 to
add to the shearing reaction from gravity. Furthermore, ligamentous involvement disables the
lumbar muscles (specifically noted above) from supporting the reaction shear as they reorientate
to a line of action more parallel to the compressive axis33(see Figure 20.1). With full spine flexion
and a modest amount of gravitational reaction shear, it is not difficult to exceed shear failure
loads of the spine, which have been found to be in the neighborhood of 2000 to 2800 N in adult
FIGURE 20.1 Subjects rotated about the hips while maintaining a neutral lordosis to activate the longissimus/
iliocostalis complex — note the oblique angle of the fibers in the ultrasound image with respect to the compressive
axis suggesting these muscles produce large shear forces. This angle is lost, together with the protective shear forces,
when the spine is flexed.
20-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
cadavers.15 This paragraph suggests that personal work technique, or more specifically, spine motion
can effect the risk of spine damage. Recent work by Yingling et al.46 on pig spines has shown that load
rate is not a major modulator of shear tolerance unless the load is very ballistic such as what might
occur during a slip and fall. Summarizing the lumbar sagittal motion and shear issue, evidence
from tissue-specific injury studies generally supports the notion of avoiding full lumbar flexion
when performing loading tasks to minimize the risk of low back injury. There is no evidence to
support a conscious effort to perform “pelvic tilts” (i.e., hyperlordosis or lumbar flexion) during
lifting or exertion.
While twisting has been named in several studies as a risk factor for low back injury, the literature
appears confused by not making the distinction between the kinematic variable of twisting and the
kinetic variable of generating twisting torque. While many epidemiological surveillance studies link a
higher risk of LBD with twisting, twisting with low twist moment results in a relatively low muscle
activity and correspondingly low spine load.26,27 Further, passive tissue loading is not substantial until
the end of the twist range of motion.17 However, developing twisting moment places very large compres-
sive loads on the spine due to the enormous coactivation of the spine musculature27 and this can occur
when the spine is not twisted, but in a neutral posture where the ability to tolerate loads is higher. It
would appear that either single variable (the kinematic act of twisting or generating the kinetic variable
of twist torque while not twisting) is less dangerous than may be suggested by epidemiological surveys.
However, it would appear that elevated risk from very high tissue loading occurs when the spine is fully
twisted at the same time where there is a need to generate high twisting torque.27
There are many personal factors, which appear to affect spine tissue tolerance, for example, age and
gender. Jager et al.22 compiled the available literature that passed their inclusion criteria on the tolerance
of lumbar motion units to bear compressive load. Their results revealed that if males and females are
matched for age, females were able to sustain only approximately two-thirds of the compressive loads
of males. Furthermore, Jager et al.’s data showed that within a given gender, the 60-yr-old spine was
able to tolerate only about two-thirds of that tolerated by a 20-yr-old. There are other personal
factors such as motor control system fitness where it appears that a motor control error can lead to a
back injury during very benign tasks such as picking up a pencil from the floor. (This will be explained
in a subsequent section.)
Many factors appear to modulate the risk of specific low back tissues damage other than load magni-
tude and loading mode. While disc herniations have been produced under controlled conditions,19
Callaghan and McGill9 have been able to consistently produce disc herniations by mimicking spine
motion and load patterns seen in workers and in replicating the motion and loads of some lumber exten-
sion exercise machines. Specifically, it appears that only a very modest amount of spine compression
force is required (only 800 –1000 N) but the spine specimen must be repeatedly flexed — mimicking
repeated torso-spine flexion from continual bending to a fully flexed posture. In these experiments,
the progressive tracking of disc nucleus material travelling posteriorly through the annulus of the disc
was documented with sequestration of the nucleus material around 18,000–25,000 cycles of flexion
(fewer cycles were required for herniation with higher simultaneous compressive loads). This study
included the utilization of a pig degenerative trauma model, which on one hand was an animal model
but on the other, control over age, diet, physical activity provided a unique opportunity. Spines and
discs obtained from humans are typically older and have lost sufficient disc hydration to match the
hydration levels, and potential for herniation, seen in the age groups of workers at risk for this specific
type of event (typically 30–50-yr-olds). But the important point here is that herniation appears to be
more strongly linked to repeated flexion motion rather than load.
Another modulator for tissue damage appears to be the posture of the joint resulting from the curva-
ture of the spine in vivo. For example, Adams et al.1 showed that a fully flexed spine is weaker than the one
that is moderately flexed. In a most recent study, Gunning and McGill20 have shown that a fully flexed
spine (using a controlled porcine spine model) is 20 to 40% weaker than if it were in a neutral posture,
and that hydration levels matched to the changes seen in peoples’ discs throughout the work day also
modulate the tolerance. For example, the spinal discs are more easily damaged first thing in the
Rehabilitating Low Back Disorders 20-5
morning upon rising from bed when they are fully hydrated. A fascinating study, reported by Snook
et al.43 demonstrated that of 85 patients randomly assigned to a group that controlled the amount of
early morning lumbar flexion, had significant reduction in pain intensity, compared to a control
group. Then, when the control group received the experimental treatment they responded with
similar reductions.
Collectively, the evidence suggests that the risk of spine tissue damage is a function of load magnitude,
directional mode of the applied load, motion repetition, spine posture, hydration level and time of day,
motor control and instantaneous stability, and individual age and gender. Injury history and tissue
damage is an overlaying modulator. Collectively this data supports the notion of an envelope of
motion and loading for optimal tissue health. In addition it is well known that tissues adapt and
remodel with load (e.g., Bone — Carter,11 Ligament — Woo et al.,45 Disc — Porter,40 Vertebrae —
Brinckmann et al.8), which is at the core of any rehabilitation program. However, biological variability
prevents the identification of specific levels of loading, which either build tissue or initiate breakdown,
together with the optimal rest periods and days off, which promote healthy tissue adaption for a given
individual. Thus it would appear that the wisest philosophical approach for the optimal design of
activity, either during the activities of daily living or during rehabilitation efforts, may be to adopt the
notion that “too much of any single activity is problematic.” No rehabilitation program can be fully effec-
tive if patients undo the beneficial responses of therapy with inappropriate activities of daily living.
flexion calibrated angle and exceeded it by one-half a degree, while all other lumbar joints maintained
their static positions (not fully flexed).12 The spine buckled! Sophisticated modeling analysis revealed
that buckling can occur from a motor control error where a short and temporary reduction in activation
to one, or more, of the intersegmental muscles would cause rotation of just a single joint so that passive
or other tissues become irritated or possibly injured.13
Adams and Dolan2 have noted that passive tissues begin to damage with bending moments of 60 Nm
— this occurs simply with the weight of the torso when bending over and a temporary loss of muscular
support. This scenario is not an excessive task, but it is often reported to clinicians by patients as the event
that caused their injury (i.e., picking up a pencil). However, reporting of such an event will not be found
in the scientific literature. Medical personnel would not record this event since in many jurisdictions it
would not be deemed as a compensable injury — the medical report attributes the cause elsewhere.
Other evidence linking poor motor coordination with higher risks for the lumbar spine reaching
critical points of instability exists and is revealing. Cholewicki and McGill13 have identified through a
modeling analysis, the nodal points, or specific spinal joint, where buckling could occur from specific
motor control errors. Such inappropriate muscle sequencing has been observed in men who are chal-
lenged by holding a load in the hands while breathing 10% CO2 to elevate breathing. On one hand,
the muscles must cocontract to ensure sufficient spine stability, but on the other, challenged breathing
is often characterized by rythmic/contraction/relaxation of the abdominal wall.30 Thus, the motor
system is presented with a conflict — should the torso muscles remain active isometrically to maintain
spine stability or will they rhythmically relax and contract to assist with active expiration (but sacrifice
spine stability). Fit motor systems appear to meet the simultaneous breathing and spine support chal-
lenge — unfit ones may not. All of these deficient motor control mechanisms will heighten biomecha-
nical susceptibility to injury or reinjury.13,14
In vitro, a ligamentous lumbar spine buckles under compressive loading at about 90 N (about 20 lb)
highlighting the critical role of the musculature to stiffen the spine against buckling (the critical work and
analysis of the passive tissues being performed by Crisco and Panjabi16). Anatomical arrangement of
muscle around the spine, coupled with critically important patterns of activation, enables the spine to
bear a much higher compressive load as it stiffens and becomes more resistant to buckling but in so
doing, the spine bears even more load due to the “stiffening” muscle activity. As noted previously, aber-
rant patterns of activation can result in instantaneous spine instability18 and acute tissue overload. But
over the longer term, the Queensland group42 have developed a tissue damage model, which suggests that
chronically poor motor control (and motion patterns) initiates microtrauma in tissues, which accumu-
lates leading to symptomatic injury. Injury leads to further deleterious change in motor patterns such
that chronicity can only be broken with specific techniques to re-educate the local muscle-motor
control system. Both acute and chronic instability-tissue models have been proposed. But given the
wide range of individuals and physical demands, question remain as to what is the optimal balance in
terms of stability, motion facilitation and moment generation — if stability is achieved through muscular
cocontraction, how much is necessary and how is it best achieved?
The concept of stability begins with potential energy, which for the purposes here, is of two basic
forms. In the first form, objects have potential energy by virtue of their height above a datum.
Critical to measuring stability are the notions of energy “wells” and minimum potential energy. If a
ball is placed into a bowl it is stable; if a force is applied to the ball (or a perturbation) the ball will
rise up the side of the bowl but then come to rest again in the position of least potential energy at the
bottom of the bowl — or the “energy well.” As noted by Bergmark, “stable equilibrium prevails when
the potential energy of the system is minimum.” The system is made more stable by deepening the
bowl and/or by increasing the steepness of the sides of the bowl (see Figure 20.2). Thus, the notion
of stability requires the specification of the unperturbed energy state of a system followed by the
study of the system following perturbation — if the “joules” of work done by the perturbation is less
than the “joules” of potential energy inherent to the system then the system will remain stable (i.e.,
the ball will not roll out of the bowl). The corollary is that the mechanical system will collapse if the
applied load exceeds a critical value (determined by potential energy and stiffness).
The previous ball analogy is a two-dimensional example. This would be analogous to a hinged skeletal
joint that only has the capacity for flexion/extension. Spinal joints can rotate in three planes and translate
along three axes requiring a six-dimensional bowl for each joint — mathematics enables the examination
of a 36-dimensional bowl (six lumbar joints with six degrees of freedom) representing the whole lumbar
spine. If the height of the bowl were decreased in any one of these 36 dimensions, the ball could roll out.
In clinical terms, a single muscle having an inappropriate force (and thus stiffness), or a damaged passive
tissue, which has lost stiffness can cause instability that is both predictable and quantifiable.
While potential energy by virtue of height is useful for illustrating the concept, potential energy as a
function of stiffness and storage of elastic energy is actually used for musculoskeletal application. Elastic
(a) (b)
(c) (d)
FIGURE 20.2 The continuum of stability — (a) is most stable through the continuum (b and c) to (d) which is least
stable. The ball in the bowl seeks the “energy well” or position of minimum potential energy (m g h). Deepening the
bowl or increasing the steepness of the sides increases the ability to survive perturbation — this increases stability.
20-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
potential energy is calculated from stiffness (k) and deformation (x) in the elastic element:
PE ¼ 1=2 k x2 :
In other words the greater the stiffness (k) the greater the steepness of the sides of the bowl (from the
previous analogy), and the more stable the structure. Thus stiffness creates stability (see Figure 20.3).
Active muscle produces a stiff member and in fact the greater the activation of the muscle, the greater
this stiffness — it has long been known that joint stiffness increases rapidly and nonlinearly with
muscle activation such that only very modest levels of muscle activity create sufficiently stiff and stable
joints. Furthermore, joints possess inherent joint stiffness as the passive capsules and ligaments contribute
stiffness particularly at the end range of motion. The motor control system is able to control stability of the
joints through coordinated muscle coactivation and to a lesser degree by placing joints in positions,
which modulate passive stiffness contribution. However, a faulty motor control system can lead to
inappropriate magnitudes of muscle force and stiffness, allowing a “valley” for the “ball to roll out” or
clinically, for a joint to buckle or undergo shear translation. But mechanical systems and particularly mus-
culoskeletal linkages, are limited to the analysis of “local stability” since the energy wells are not infinitely
deep and the many anatomical components contribute force and stiffness in synchrony to create “surfaces”
of potential energy where there are many local wells. Thus local minima are located from examination of
the derivative of the energy surface (see Reference 13 for mathematical details). Spine stability then,
is quantified by forming a matrix where the total “stiffness energy” for each degree of freedom of joint
motion is represented by a number (or eigenvalue) and the magnitude of that number represents its
contribution to forming the “height of the bowl” in that particular dimension. Eigenvalues less than
zero indicate the potential for instability. The eigenvector (different from the eigenvalue) can then identify
the mode in which the instability occurred while sensitivity analysis may reveal the possible contributors
allowing unstable behavior. Gardner-Morse et al.18 have initiated interesting investigations into eigenvectors
by predicting patterns of spine deformation due to impaired muscular intersegmental control) or for
clinical relevance — what muscular pattern would have prevented the instability?
Activating a group of muscle synergists and antagonists in the optimal way now becomes a critical
issue. In clinical terms the full complement of the stabilizing musculature must work harmoniously to
both ensure stability together with generation of the required moment and desired joint movement.
But only one muscle with inappropriate activation amplitude may produce instability, or at least unstable
behavior could result at lower applied loads.
How much stability is necessary — obviously insufficient stiffness renders the joint unstable but too
much stiffness and coactivation imposes massive load penalties on the joints and prevents motion.
FIGURE 20.3 (a) Increasing the stiffness of the cables (muscles) increases the stability (or deepens the bowl) and
increases the ability to support larger applied loads “p” without falling. (b) Spine stiffness (and stability) is achieved
by a complex interaction of stiffening structures along the spine and (c) those forming the torso wall (right panel).
Rehabilitating Low Back Disorders 20-9
“Sufficient stability” is a concept that involves the determination of how much muscular stiffness is
necessary for stability together with a modest amount of extra stability to form a margin of safety. Inter-
estingly enough, given the rapid increase in joint stiffness with modest muscle force, large muscular
forces are rarely required. In our recent papers, stabilization exercises were quantified and ranked for
muscle activation magnitudes together with the resultant spine load.35 (Quantification of individual
tissue loads in the spine is a complex procedure and an issue outside the constraints of this article —
the interested reader is directed to Reference 28.) Furthermore, Cholewicki’s work14 has demonstrated
that sufficient stability of the lumbar spine is achieved, in an undeviated spine, in most people with
modest levels of coactivation of the paraspinal and abdominal wall muscles. This means that people,
from patients to athletes, must be able to maintain sufficient stability in all activities — with low, but
continuous, muscle activation. Thus, maintaining a stability “margin of safety” when performing
tasks, particularly the tasks of daily living, is not compromised by insufficient strength but rather
insufficient endurance. We are now beginning to understand the mechanistic pathway of those studies
showing the efficacy of endurance training for the muscles that stabilize the spine. Having strong
abdominals does not necessarily provide the prophylactic effect that had been hoped for — but
several works suggest that endurable muscles reduce the risk of future back troubles.7
robust spine models to document the ability of each component to stiffen and stabilize. Second,
electromyographic recordings of all muscles (even deep muscles requiring intramuscular electrodes)
are necessary to confirm the extent that the motor control system involves each muscle to ensure
sufficient stability. For some time our limited intramuscular EMG and modeling studies, and those
of others, suggested that virtually all torso muscles play a role in stabilization. (Our most recent
quantification breakthroughs appears at the end of this section.) However, while multifidus,
the other extensors, and the abdominal wall, have been highlighted before, the architecture of
quadratus lumborum (QL) suggests that it can be a stabilizer. This notion is further strengthened
by some earlier observation that the motor control system involves this muscle together with the
abdominal wall when stability is required in the absence of major moment demands. The fibers
of QL cross-link the vertebrae, they have a large lateral moment arm via the transverse process
attachments, and traverse to the rib cage and iliac crests. Thus, the quadratus could buttress
shear instability, and be effective in all loading modes, by design. Typically, the first mode of
buckling is lateral — the quadratus can play a significant role in local lateral buttressing.
Further, activation profiles support the notion of the stabilizing role of quadratus. It is active
during a variety of flexion dominant, extensor dominant and lateral bending tasks. Specifically,
Andersson et al.4 found that the QL did not relax with the extensors during the flexion –relaxation
phenomonon. The flexion –relaxation phenomonon is an interesting task since there is no substan-
tial lateral or twisting torques and the extensor torque appears to be supported passively —
suggesting some stabilizing role for QL. Other very limited data suggest (our laboratory techniques
to obtain QL activation were rather imprecise at the time) that in an experiment where subjects
stood upright, but held buckets in either hand, where load was incrementally added to each
bucket, the QL appeared to increase its activation level (together with the obliques) as more
stability was required. This task forms a special situation since only compressive loading is
applied to the spine in the absence of any bending moments. The three layers of the abdominal
wall are also important for stability together with muscles, which attach directly to vertebra —
the multisegmented longissimus and iliocostalis and the unisegmental multifidii. Cholewicki18 has
also presented an argument for the role of the small intertransversarii in producing small
but critical stabilizing forces. On the other hand, psoas activation appears to have little relationship
with low back demands — the motor control system activates it when hip flexor moment is
required (data is presented by Andersson et al.3 and Juker et al.23).
Most recently we have completed evolution of our model to quantify the role of individual muscles
to contribute to stability. Once again the conclusion is that all muscles are important and that the
most important muscle at any instant or task is a transient variable — they continually change their
relative contribution (see Figure 20.4 and Figure 20.5 for the ranking in a selected group of exercises).
So which are the wisest ways to challenge and train these identified stabilizers?
20.6 Training QL
Given the architectural and electromyographic evidence for QL as a spine stabilizer, the optimal
technique to maximize activation but minimize the spine load appears to be the side-bridge
(Figure 20.6) — beginners bridge from the knees while advanced bridges are from the feet.
When supported with the feet and elbow the lumbar compression is a modest 2500 N, but the
quadratus closest to the floor, appears to be active upto 50% of MVC (the obliques experience
similar challenge). Advanced technique to enhance the motor challenge is to roll from one elbow to
the other while abdominally bracing (Figure 20.7) rather than repeated “hiking” of the hips off the
floor into the bridge position. Higher levels of activation would be reached with the feet on a labile
surface.35
Rehabilitating Low Back Disorders 20-11
300
250
RectAbd
ExtObl
IntObl
200 ParsLum
IlioLum
LongThor
QuadLum
150
LatDors
Mult
Trans
100
50
0
Abdcurl Ball Fpn_leg/arm
Exercise Posture
FIGURE 20.4 In an attempt to understand the contributions of each muscle pair on spine stability, the increase in
the stability index is shown as a function of setting each muscle pair activation in turn to 100% MVC — in this way
their relative contrive could be assessed. Note that the relative order of muscles that increase stability changes across
exercises. As well, in flexion tasks, the pars lumborum (in this example) plays a larger stabilizing role over the rectus
abdominis. In contrast, during the extension tasks the opposite holds true suggesting a task-dependent role reversal
between moment generation and stability. The exercises were: Abcurl — curl-up on the stable floor, Ball — sitting on
a gym ball, Fpn_leg/arm — four-point kneeling while extending one leg and the opposite arm.
1600 3000
2800
1400
Stability
2600
Compression
1200
2400
Stability Index (nm/rad/rad)
1000
Compression (N)
2200
800 2000
1800
600
1600
400
1400
200
1200
0 1000
Ball Chair Abdcurl Fpn_leg Bridge SideBridge Bridge_leg Fpn_arm/leg
Exercise Posture
FIGURE 20.5 Stability versus L4-L5 compression for eight different stabilization exercises. All exercises were
performed with the abdominal wall active and were as follows: Ball — sitting on a gym ball, Chair — sitting on a
chair, Abcurl — curl-up on the floor, Fpn_leg — four-point kneeling while extending one leg at the hip,
Bridge — back bridge on the floor, SideBridge — side-bridge with the elbow and feet on the floor, Bridge_leg —
back-bridge but extending the knee and holding one leg against gravity, Fpn_arm/leg — four-point kneeling
while extending one leg and the opposite arm. Exercises are rank ordered based on increasing lumbar spine stability.
20-12 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
FIGURE 20.6 The horizontal isometric side-bridge. Supporting the lower body with the knees on the floor reduces
the demand further for those who are more concerned with safety while supporting the body with the feet increases
the muscle challenge, but also the spine load.
FIGURE 20.7 Advanced side-bridge — following each side-bridge hold, one “rolls” from one elbow to the other
while abdominally bracing locks the pelvis and the rib cage.
Rehabilitating Low Back Disorders 20-13
low. Sit-ups (both straight-leg and bent-knee) are characterized by higher psoas activation and higher
low back compressive loads that exceed NIOSH occupational guidelines, while leg raises cause even
higher activation and also spine compression (the interested reader is directed to Reference 8 for
actual data). It is also interesting that myoelectric evidence suggests that there is no functional distinction
between an “upper” and “lower” rectus abdominis in most people but, in contrast, the obliques are
regionally activated with “upper” and “lower” motor point areas together with medial and lateral com-
ponents. Transverse abdominis is selectively activated by dynamically “hollowing” in the abdominal
wall,42 while an isometric abdominal brace coactivates transverse abdominis together with the external
and internal obliques to ensure stability in virtually all modes of possible instability.13
Several relevant observations were made regarding abdominal exercises in our investigations. The chal-
lenge to psoas is lowest during curl-ups, followed by higher levels during the horizontal side-bridge, while
bent-knee sit-ups were characterized by larger psoas activation than straight-leg sit-ups, through to the
highest psoas activity observed during leg raises and hand-on-knee flexor isometric exertions. It is interesting
to note that the often recommended “press-heels” sit-up, which has been hypothesized to activate hamstrings
and neurally inhibit psoas was actually confirmed to increase psoas activation! (Original data can be found in
Reference 23 — we note here that some clinicans and coaches who intentionally wish to train psoas and will
find this data informative). Once again the horizontal side support appears to have merit as it challenges the
lateral obliques and transverse abdominis without high lumbar compressive loading.
Clearly, curl-ups excel at activating the rectus abdominis but produce relatively low oblique activity.
Curl-ups with a twisting motion is expensive in terms of lumbar compression due to the additional
oblique challenge. A wise choice for abdominal exercises, in the early stages of training or rehabilitation,
for simple low back health objectives, would consist of several variations of curl-ups for rectus abdominis
and the side-bridge for the obliques and quadratus, the variation of which is chosen commensurate with
patient/athlete status and goals.
FIGURE 20.8 The flexion –extension stretch is performed by slowly cycling through full spine flexion to full
extension. Spine mobility is emphasized rather than “pressing” at the end range of motion. This exercise
facilitates motion for the spine with very low loading of the intervertebral joints, reduces viscous stresses for
subsequent exercise and “flosses” the nerve roots through the foramenae at each spine joint (hence coordination
of full cervical, thoracic and lumbar flexion – extension).
the other leg straight to lock the pelvis-lumbar spine and minimize the loss of a neutral lumbar posture.
Then, lateral musculature exercises are performed — namely the side-bridge, for QL and muscles of the
abdominal wall for optimal stability (Figure 20.6). Advanced variations involve placing the upper leg-
foot in front of the lower leg-foot to facilitate longitudinal “rolling” of the torso to challenge both anterior
and posterior portions of the wall. The extensor program consists of leg extensions and the “birddog”
(Figure 20.10). In general, we recommend that these isometric holds be held no longer than 7 to 8 sec
given recent evidence from near infrared spectroscopy indicating rapid loss of available oxygen in the
torso muscles contracting at these levels — short relaxation of the muscle restores oxygen.34
Motivated by the evidence for the superiority of extensor endurance over strength as a benchmark for
good back health, we have recently documented “normal” ratios of endurance times for the torso flexors
relative to the extensors (e.g., it is “normal” to hold a flexor posture — see Reference 32, about 0.98 of the
maximum time holding a reference extensor posture) and for the lateral musculature relative to the
extensors (0.5) to assist clinicians to identify endurance deficits — both absolute values and for one
muscle group relative to another. Our most recent evidence suggests that these endurance ratios (both
right to left sides and flexor to extensor) are significantly “out of balance” in those who have had a
history of low back troubles with work loss.36 Finally, as patients progress with these isometric stabiliz-
ation exercises, we recommend conscious simultaneous contraction of the abdominals (i.e., bracing —
simply isometrically activating the abdominals for maximum stability).
FIGURE 20.9 The curl-up, where the head and shoulders are raised off the ground with the hands under the lumbar
region to help stabilize the pelvis and support the neutral spine. Only one leg is bent to assist in pelvic stabilization
and preservation of a “neutral” lumbar curve. Additional challenge can be created by raising the elbows from the floor
and generating an abdominal brace or cocontraction.
Rehabilitating Low Back Disorders 20-15
FIGURE 20.10 Single leg extension holds, while on the hands and knees, produces mild extensor activity and lower
spine compression (,2500 N). Raising the contralateral arm increases extensor muscle activity but also spine
compression to levels over 3000 N. Sufficient stability is ensured with mild abdominal bracing.
FIGURE 20.11 A challenge for the torsional components that produces low spine loads is to support the extensor
moment of the flexed torso with one hand while the other raises a modest weight. The lumbar torso is braced
(including all layers of the abdominal wall) in a neutral posture resisting the twisting moments generated by the weight.
Acknowledgments
The continual financial support from the Natural Science and Engineering Research Council, Canada is
gratefully acknowledged and has made this series of work possible.
References
1. Adams, M.A., McNally, D.S., Chinn, H., and Dolan, P., Posture and the compressive strength of the
lumbar spine, Clin. Biomech., 9, pp. 5 –14, 1994.
2. Adams, M.A. and Dolan, P., Recent advances in lumbar spine mechanics and their clinical signifi-
cance, Clin. Biomech., 10, pp. 3 –19, 1995.
Rehabilitating Low Back Disorders 20-17
3. Andersson, E., Oddsson, L., Grundstrom, H., and Thorstensson, A., The role of the psoas and iliacus
muscles for stability and movement of the lumbar spine, pelvis and hip, Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports, 5,
pp. 10–16, 1995.
4. Andersson, E.A., Oddsson, L.I.E., Grundström, H., and Nilsson, J., EMG activities of the quadratus
lumborum and erector spinae muscles during flexion-relaxation and other motor tasks,
Clin. Biomech., 11(7), pp. 392–400, 1996.
5. Axler, C.T., and McGill, S.M., Low back loads over a variety of abdominal exercises: Searching for the
safest abdominal challenge, Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 29, pp. 804 –811, 1997.
6. Bergmark, A., Mechanical stability of the human lumbar spine, Doctoral Dissertation, Department
of Solid Mechanics, Lund University, Sweden, 1987.
7. Biering-Sorensen, F., Physical measurements as risk indicators for low back trouble over a one year
period, Spine, 9, pp. 106–119, 1984.
8. Brinckmann, P., Biggemann, M., and Hilweg, D., Prediction of the compressive strength of human
lumbar vertebrae, Clin. Biomech., 4, Suppl 2, pp. s1– s27, 1989.
9. Callaghan, J. and McGill, S.M., Intervertebral disc herniation: Studies on a porcine spine exposed
to highly repetitive flexion/extension motion with compressive force, Clin. Biomech., 16(1): 28 –37.
10. Callaghan, J.P., Gunning, J.L., and McGill, S.M., Relationship between lumbar spine load and muscle
activity during extensor exercises, Phys. Ther., 78, pp. 8 –18, 1998.
11. Carter, D.R., Biomechanics of bone, in Biomechanics of Trauma, Nahum, H.M., and Melvin, J., eds.,
Appleton Century Crofts, Norwalk, Connecticut, 1985, pp. 135 –165.
12. Cholewicki, J. and McGill, S.M., Lumbar posterior ligament involvement during extremely heavy lifts
estimated from flouroscopic measurements, J. Biomech., 25(1), pp. 17 –28, 1992.
13. Cholewicki, J. and McGill, S.M., Mechanical stability of the in vivo lumbar spine: Implications for
injury and chronic low back pain, Clin. Biomech., 11(1), pp. 1 –15, 1996.
14. Cholewicki, J., Simons, A.P.D., Radebold, A., Effects of external trunk loads on lumbar spine stability,
J. Biomech., 33(11), pp. 1377–1385, 2000.
15. Cripton P., Berleman U., and Visarino H., et al., Response of the lumbar spine due to shear loading,
in Injury prevention through biomechanics, Symposium proceedings, May, Wayne State University,
1995, pp. 4 –5.
16. Crisco, J.J. and Panjabi, M.M., Euler stability of the human ligamentous lumbar spine, Part I Theory
7, pp. 19 –26 and Part II, Experiment, Clin. Biomech., 7, pp. 27–32, 1992.
17. Duncan, N.A. and Ahmed, A.M., The role of axial rotation in the etiology of unilateral disc prolapse:
An experimental and finite-element analysis, Spine, 16, pp. 1089 –1098, 1991.
18. Gardner-Morse, M., Stokes, IAF., and Laible, JP., Role of the muscles in lumbar spine stability in
maximum extension efforts, J. Orthop. Res., 13, pp. 802 –808, 1995.
19. Gordon, S.J., Yang, K.H., Mayer, P.J., et al., Mechanism of disc rupture-a preliminary report, Spine,
1991, 16, pp. 450– 456.
20. Gunning, J., Callaghan, J.P., and McGill, S.M., The role of prior loading history and spinal posture
on the compressive tolerance and type of failure in the spine using a porcine trauma model,
Clin. Biomech., 16(6), pp. 471 –480, 2001.
21. Heylings, D.J., Supraspinous and interspinous ligaments of the human lumbar spine, J. Anat., 123,
pp. 127 –131, 1978.
22. Jäger, M. and Luttmann, A., The load on the lumbar spine during asymmetrical bi-manual materials
handling, Ergonomics, 35, pp. 783 –805, 1992.
23. Juker, D., McGill, S.M., Kropf, P., and Steffen T., Quantitative intramuscular myoelectric activity of
lumbar portions of psoas and the abdominal wall during a wide variety of tasks, Med. Sci. Sports
Exerc., 30(2), pp. 301–310, 1998.
24. Marras, W.S. Occuaptional low back disorders causation and control, Ergonomics, 2000, 43, pp. 880 –
902.
25. McGill, S.M. and Norman, R.W., Effects of an anatomically detailed erector spinae model on L4/L5
disc compression and shear, J. Biomech., 20, pp. 591–600, 1987a.
20-18 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
26. McGill, S.M., Kinetic potential of the lumbar trunk musculature about three orthogonal orthopaedic
axes in extreme postures, Spine, 16, pp. 809 –815, 1991a.
27. McGill, S.M., Electromyographic activity of the abdominal and low back musculature during the
generation of isometric and dynamic axial trunk torque: Implications for lumbar mechanics,
J. Orthop. Res. 9, pp. 91 –103, 1991b.
28. McGill, S.M., A myoelectrically based dynamic three dimensional model to predict loads on lumbar
spine tissues during lateral bending, J. Biomech., 25, pp. 395 –414, 1992.
29. McGill, S.M. and Kippers, V., Transfer of loads between lumbar tissues during the flexion-relaxation
phenomenon, Spine 19, pp. 2190 –2196, 1994.
30. McGill, S.M., ISB Keynote Lecture-The biomechanics of low back injury: Implications on current
practice in industry and the clinic, J. Biomech., 30, pp. 465 –475, 1997.
31. McGill, S.M., Low Back Exercises: Evidence for improving exercise regimens, Phys. Ther., 78,
pp. 754 –765, 1998.
32. McGill, S.M., Childs, A., and Liebenson, C., Endurance times for stabilization exercises: Clinical
targets for testing and training from a normal database, Arch. Phys. Med. Rehab., 80, pp. 941 –
944, 1999.
33. McGill, S.M., Parks, K., and Hughson, R., Changes in lumbar lordosis modify the role of the extensor
muscles, Clin. Biomech., 15(10), pp. 777 –780, 2000.
34. McGill, S.M., Hughson, R., and Parks, K., Erector spinae oxygenation during prolonged contrac-
tions: Implications for prolonged work, Ergonomics, 43, pp. 486 –493, 2000.
35. McGill, S.M., Low back disorders: Evidence based prevention and rehabilitation, Human Kinetics
Publishers, Champaign, Illinois, 2002 and “Ultimate back fitness and performance” at
www.backfitpro.com.
36. McGill, S.M., Grenier, S., Bluhm, M., Preuss, R., Brown, S., and Russell, C., Previous history of
LBP with work loss is related to lingering effects in biomechanical, physiological, personal and
psychosocial characteristics, Ergonomics, 46(7): 731 –746.
37. Norman, R.W., Wells, R., Neumann, P., Frank, J., Shannon, H., and Kerr, M., A comparison of peak
vs cumulative physical work exposure risk factors for the reporting of low back pain in the auto-
motive industry, Clin. Biomech., 13, pp. 561–573, 1998.
38. Oxland, T.R., Panjabi, M.M., Southern, E.P., and Duranceau, J.S., An anatomic basis for spinal
instability: A porcine trauma model, J. Orthop. Res., 9, pp. 452– 462, 1991.
39. Parks, K.A., Crichton, K.S., Goldford, R.J., and McGill, S.M., On the validity of ratings of impair-
ment for low back disorders, Spine, 28(4), pp. 380– 384, 2003.
40. Porter, R.W., Is hard work good for the back? The relationship between hard work and low back
pain-related disorders, Int. J. Ind. Ergon., 9, pp. 157–160, 1992.
41. Potvin, J., McGill, S.M., and Norman, R.W., Trunk muscle and lumbar ligament contributions to
dynamic lifts with varying degrees of trunk flexion, Spine, 16(9), pp. 1099–1107, 1991.
42. Richardson, C., Jull, G., Hodges, P., and Hides, J., Therapeutic exercise for spinal segmental stabili-
zation in low back pain. Churchill-Livingston, Edinburgh, 1999.
43. Snook, S.H., Webster, B.S., McGorry, R.W., Fogleman, M.T., and McCann, K.B., The reduction of
chronic nonspecific low back pain through the control of early morning lumbar flexion, Spine,
23, pp. 2601 –2607, 1998.
44. Vera Garcia, F.J., Grenier, S.G., and McGill, S.M., Abdominal response during curl-ups on both stable
and labile surfaces, Phys. Ther., 80(6), pp. 564 –569, 2000.
45. Woo, S.L.-Y., Gomez, M.A., and Akeson, W.H., Mechanical behaviors of soft tissues: Measurements,
modifications, injuries, and treatment, in Biomechanics of trauma, H.M. Nahum and J. Melvin, eds.,
Appleton Century Crofts, Norwalk, Connecticut, pp. 109 –133, 1985.
46. Yingling, V.R., Callaghan, J.P., and McGill, S.M., Dynamic loading affects the mechanical properties
and failure site of porcine spines, Clin. Biomech., 12, pp. 301 –305, 1997.
21
Human Adaptation
in the Workplace
21.1 Introduction
The concept of individual health, defined as the overall condition of an individual or a condition of well-
being, has been broadly expanded in the past two decades from that of an individual to that of a group of
individuals and general systems. The rising costs of healthcare particularly in the workplace have
prompted a recent move towards the concept of “healthy workforce” as advocated by the National Insti-
tute for Occupational Safety and Health.
Very recently, Genaidy and Karwowski (2006) have coined the term “health engineering” to signify
“the identification, improvement and maintenance of the well-being of general systems (e.g., individual,
technologies, facilities), through the application of engineering, medicine, management, and human
sciences knowledge and methodologies”. This definition emphasizes the multi-dimensional nature of
health and its continuum as a function of time. Furthermore, the application of different disciplines
to health acknowledges the complex web of factors influencing this issue.
In light of the above, one can deduce that health is the product of an internal dynamic process that is
consistently updated (if possible) to narrow the gap with reference to the state of an individual’s
optimum well-being. As such, systems “adapt” or “adjust” to the challenges emanating from the environ-
ment. The objectives of this chapter are to define human adaptation, describe the occupational adap-
tation process and workplace factors impacting adaptation and discuss the needs for future research
on the subject.
21.2 Definition
According to the American Heritage Dictionary (2000) adaptation is “the act or process of adapting.” In
biological terms, this term indicates an alteration or adjustment in structure or behavior by which the
21-1
21-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Occupational Challenge
Occupational
Person
Environment
Adaptive Response
Generation Subprocess
Occupational
Response
FIGURE 21.1 Occupational adaptation process (Adapted from Schkade, J.K. and Schultz, S., in Perspectives in
Human Occupation: Participation in Life, Kramer, P., Hinojosa, J., Royeen, C.B., Eds., Lippincott Williams &
Wilkins, Philadelphia, PA, 2003, pp.181 – 221. With permission.)
Human Adaptation in the Workplace 21-3
the occupational environment also contributes to those expectations through its physical, social, and cul-
tural subsystems. The person and environment contributions to the role expectations are reflected by the
arrows leading to the expectations. The arrows returning to the person indicate that the individual has a
perception of the interactive internal and external expectations, which may be fairly accurate or faulty. It
is this mix of expectations that prompts the person to create an occupational response, perform it, assess
its efficacy, and integrate the information surrounding this occupational event into the person for sub-
sequent use. The creation, assessment, and integration occur through the adaptive response generation
subprocess, the relative response evaluation subprocess and the adaptive response integration subprocess,
respectively.
demands (the extent to which the work elements exert negative impact upon the person) and ener-
gizers (the extent to which the work elements induce positive effect upon the person). Genaidy and
Karwowski defined work compatibility as an integration of all work demands and energizers. It is
expected that the greater the incompatibility between the demands and the energizers, the greater
the challenge to which the person is exposed during the “Occupational Process” and the more difficult
is the human adaptation.
21.6 Discussion
Traditionally, technological advancements have outpaced research and development in the field of
human systems (Paez et al., 2004). This may be attributed to, among other things, the assumptions
that humans are replaceable machines and too complex to be configured, and the narrow specializ-
ations engaged in the study of human systems. As a result, the costs of injuries and illnesses in the
workplace are still prohibitive. Therefore, the National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health
has initiated the concept of U.S. healthy workforce in order to adopt a holistic approach based on
both health protection and promotion in order to advance research and development in the field of
human systems.
It was the objective of this chapter to research the literature on occupational adaptation. In general, a
significant portion of occupational injuries and illnesses is attributed to maladaptation in the response of
the individual to the challenges arising from the workplace as well as maladaptation of the workplace to
the needs and capabilities of human systems. In this chapter, we reviewed the state-of-the-art research on
human adaptation in the workplace. Although a comprehensive framework has been established, there is
a lack of quantitative methodology to address human adaptation within the context of work. This meth-
odology should take into account both health protection and promotion. Similarly, a quantitative meth-
odology is needed to tackle the issue of workplace adaptation to the needs and capabilities of human
systems.
References
Abdallah, S., Genaidy, A., Salem, O., Karwowski, W., Shell, R., The concept of work compatibility: an
integrated work design criterion for improving workplace human performance in manufacturing
systems, Hum. Factors Ergon. Manufacturing, 14(4), pp. 1– 24, 2004.
American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language, Houghton Mifflin Company, 2000.
Genaidy, A.M. and Karwowski, W., The emerging field of health engineering, Theor. Issues Ergon. Sci.,
2005, in press.
Genaidy, A.M. and Karwowski, W., Human performance in lean production environment: critical
assessment and research framework, Hum. Factors Ergon. Manufacturing, 13(4), pp. 317 –330,
2003.
Paez, O., Dewees, J., Genaidy, A., Tuncel, S., Karwowski, W., Zurada, J., Lean enterprise: an emerging
socio-technological system integration, Hum. Factors Ergon. Manufacturing, 14(3), pp. 285–306,
2004.
Salem, O., Paez, O., Holley, M.B., Tuncel, S., Genaidy, A.M., Karwowski, W., Performance tracking
through the work compatibility model, Hum. Factors Ergon. Manufacturing, 2005, in press.
Human Adaptation in the Workplace 21-5
Shoaf, C., Genaidy, A.M., Haartz, J., Karwowski, W., Waters, T., Hancock, P.A., Huston, R., Shell, R., An
adaptive control model for assessment of work-related musculoskeletal hazards and risks, Theor.
Issues Ergon. Sci., 1, pp. 34 –61, 2000.
Schkade, J.K. and Schultz, S., Occupational adaptation: toward a holistic approach of a contemporary
practice, part I, Am. J. Occup. Ther., 46, pp. 829– 837, 1992.
Schkade, J.K. and Schultz, S., Occupational adaptation, in Perspectives in Human Occupation:
Participation in Life, Kramer, P., Hinojosa, J., Royeen, C.B., Eds., Lippincott Williams & Wilkins,
Philadelphia, PA, 2003, pp. 181–221.
22
Rehabilitation
Ergonomics
22.1 Definition
Rehabilitation Ergonomics is the practice of applying scientific and functional principles to provide a match
of work and worker that prevents injury or assists in the return to work process. The practitioners are thera-
pists and other specialists whose backgrounds include anatomy, physiology, kinesiology and pathology and
ergonomics. They must analyze both the humans who perform work activities and the setting in which they
work. Rehabilitation ergonomists specialize in functional evaluation, improvement of functional work per-
formance, education of the worker and redesign of work to reduce musculoskeletal stressors.
22-1
22-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
A. The American Physical Therapy Association developed guidelines for the practice of occupational
health. The diversity of the practices parallels the diversity of the needs of those with potential or
actual musculoskeletal problems related to work. Both management of the injured worker and preven-
tion of injury are covered.
1. Occupational Health Physical Therapy Guidelines: work-related injury/illness prevention and
ergonomics.1
2. Occupational Health Physical Therapy Guidelines: physical therapist management of the acutely
injured worker.2
3. Occupation Health Physical Therapy Guidelines: work conditioning and work hardening
programs.3
4. Occupational Health Physical Therapy Guidelines: evaluating functional capacity.4
The guidelines provide professional structure, are informational to educational programs for curricula
development and are available to other professions and referral sources for delineation of the services
and their goals.
B. The American Occupational Therapy Association published guidelines, “Occupational Therapy: Ser-
vices for Ergonomics.”5 Within the guidelines, there are statements on ergonomics definition, ergonomic
services provided by occupational therapists, goals of ergonomics, referral and payment mechanisms and
occupational therapist background in relation to ergonomics. These serve as a guide for the occupational
therapist and those who utilize the services.
C. In 1994, a representative group of sixteen professionals who performed rehabilitation ergonomics defined
their work and standards.6 This provided the specialty with information on training, standards, methods,
and goals. The group acknowledged that the specialty of rehabilitation ergonomics was held by more than
one profession. The guidelines bridged the gap of specific professional background and, instead, emphasized
the commonalities of the practitioner and the practice. It has served as a guide for definition and use.
Figure 22.1 is a summary of professional and practice goals from all three groups of references.
A. The U.S. Occupational Safety and Health Administration (OSHA), in its ergonomic regulations, sets a
goal of prevention of work-related musculoskeletal disorders. Its earliest published ergonomic guide for
the meatpacking industry was published in 1993.7 Its current guidelines, available on the internet,8
support and enhance many of the original provisions. These guidelines call for healthcare providers to
be part of the team, early intervention to be provided and job modification to be utilized as a means
2. Maintain the health and improve the safety of the worker in the workplace
to allow workers to stay at work as they improve. Other aspects of work such as education, work redesign
and selection of appropriate tools are part of the scope of the rehabilitation ergonomist, although also
practiced by other professionals.
B. The National Institute for Occupational Safety Health (NIOSH) discusses the roles of the healthcare
provider in returning injured workers to work. In Elements of Ergonomics Programs,9 it lists employer
responsibilities that lead to involvement of rehabilitation ergonomists:
1. Provide in-house training
2. Promote early reporting and prompt evaluation by healthcare provider
3. Have healthcare provider become familiar with jobs and job tasks
4. Modify jobs or accommodate as determined by healthcare provider
C. Antidiscrimination laws such as the U.S. Americans Disabilities Act,10 as well as Canadian and Euro-
pean human rights legislation, mandate accommodations for disabled persons. These allow qualified dis-
abled persons to perform the essential functions of the job. When the disability affects physical function,
ergonomic enhancements of work, work tools or work methods facilitate functional performance of the
specific work.
D. Workers’ compensation laws provide wage replacement while a worker is off work with an injury. To
facilitate return to work, specific work-related evaluation and rehabilitation are authorized. Onsite visits
to ensure that the worker can perform the job safely and provide job modifications, are part of the reha-
bilitation plan that leads to return to work. At the treatment and ergonomic modification level, rehabi-
litation ergonomists are utilized.
Following the logic of the regulations, guidelines and laws, rehabilitation ergonomists enhance the
match of work and worker. This is used for primary prevention, modification of work, early return to
work, prevention of disability and avoidance of discrimination in the workplace.
Figure 22.2 summarizes the structure of rehabilitation ergonomics in the return to work process. It
describes the attributes of the professional specialty derived from guidelines, regulations, professional
definitions, and actual practice.
As a result, rehabilitation professionals developed four specialties that bridge the gap between treatment
and return to work. In brief, they are:
. Functional capacity evaluation (FCE): FCE adds work relevance to testing by using functional
activities such as lifting, pushing, pulling, carrying, gripping, climbing, walking, balancing, reach-
ing, sitting, and standing.
. Work rehabilitation: Work-related rehabilitation provides a structured regime that allows the
injured worker to increase function and regain work capabilities.
. Job modification: At the worksite, modifications match the work to the capacity of the worker to
promote return to work and prevention of reinjury.
. Early intervention: Immediate intervention when a work injury or illness threatens work ability,
reduces the lost time for the worker and increases healing and functional work capability.
5. Relationship with the team of medical manager, case manager, human resource
professional, safety manager, and worksite supervisors
However, validity and real-world usefulness can be enhanced by insuring that the job description is accu-
rate and designing the test to match the functional job description.27 Occupations, which lend themselves
to work-specific tests have unique physical demands. Examples are nurse and nurse assistants when
transfers are required, specific repetitive assembly processes, lineman who climbs power poles, etc.
Choosing a standardized FCE versus a work-related functional test depends on the questions that are
being asked. If there is no specific job description available for the worker, or if the worker could be con-
sidered for several jobs, then a standardized FCE is most effective. It can be compared to several job
descriptions in order to prioritize which jobs match the worker more closely. If only one job is being
considered, however, a specific functional test for that job can be developed from the functional job
description. This provides greater opportunities for work-specific tests, but the evaluators must be
careful to determine that the job description is valid and that the functional tests replicate the job
demands.
While functional testing is a specialty for therapists, the resultant findings and recommendations are
stronger when the therapist is also a rehabilitation ergonomist. The knowledge of the worksite, the jobs
and the job modification opportunities provide information for a stronger resolution of the return to
work objective.
. An atmosphere is created in which the clients are responsible for their own progress. The therapist
is a guide and assists, but the worker does the work to accomplish the goals.
. The focus is on function, not perfection. For those with injury or illnesses that are beyond the acute
phase, improving the residual function is the focus of the program. While increase in motion and
strength along with decrease of discomfort are important, they only build a foundation for which
function can be obtained.
. The goals are related to ability to function outside the clinic. In work rehabilitation, the return to
work is always the target. The program is designed for sequential improvement until full duty or
the highest level of safe work is gained.
The Commission on Accreditation of Rehabilitation Facilities (CARF),31 developed the first work
hardening standards. It defined work hardening as a multi-disciplinary program including physical,
psychological and vocational components.
In response to CARF, the American Physical Therapy Association determined that there were two types
of work rehabilitation programs. Work hardening was defined in similar terms as CARF. A more direct
program was additionally defined and described. Work conditioning emphasizes physical and functional
strengthening for return to work. It is often performed by a single discipline rather than being multi-
disciplinary. The psychological and vocational aspects are not part of the program, although they may
be contracted separately.
Both types of work rehabilitation require full participation from the worker in a setting that empha-
sizes safe work behaviors, use work-related tasks for rehabilitation and have return to work as the goal.
22-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
FIGURE 22.4 The description of both the worker and supervisor how to handle the progression.
Rehabilitation Ergonomics 22-7
FIGURE 22.5 Modification of specific essential functions in the return to work process.
programs were designed to bring an injured worker very early for evaluation and treatment of their
musculoskeletal injuries.33 – 35
In many cases, the early intervention, in conjunction with modified work, keeps the injured person in
the workplace without lost days. For those who do have lost time, treatment is underway at once and an
early return to work is more likely. In either case, the workers retain their self-image for work and avoid
the “patient” mindset. The coworkers see that the worker is retained and supervisors continue to see the
worker as productive. The intervention is directed at healing, protecting the injured part while healing is
taking place, using the uninjured portions of the worker’s body in work tasks and focusing on function
rather than pain.
In order to develop early intervention, measures must be in place before an injury is reported. Industry
management must approve the constructs of the program and facilitate education and responsibility of
the injury team. These include the claims manager, human resource coordinator, safety officer and any
other onsite medical personnel. In addition, all employees must understand that early reporting is
desired. It will be met with positive response, not negative. At times it takes many reminders, as
workers often fear that a report of a problem will result in negative action toward them. This is especially
true when reward systems for “no OSHA recordables” is in place. Also, if the culture is to disdain a
worker with an injury, or the “light” duty that might be a result, employees will be reluctant to report
until the problem is severe.
Once the early intervention process is in place, education for workers and supervisors is necessary for
early symptoms of musculoskeletal disorders to be recognized. Workers must be aware of the early stages
of carpal tunnel syndrome, tendonitis, strains, and others, in order for the system to work.
When injury, illness or early symptoms are reported, the onsite rehabilitation ergonomist works with
the employer and the medical team. Intervention includes evaluation of the condition, assessment of
current functional capacity to determine if the worker can continue to work, institution of functional
treatment and modification of the job when early return to work can be accomplished. Early intervention
is best done onsite or in a clinic that is close to the worksite.
Outcome measurement identifies the effectiveness of early intervention compared to previous tra-
ditional treatment. Analysis can also identify jobs or job tasks in the workplace where problems occur
most frequently. The safety department, with the rehabilitation ergonomist as part of the team, can
then institute prevention measures. These may be ergonomic redesign, new tools, education, improved
job training, stretching, ergonomic postures and problem solving. Early intervention is a bridge between
injury prevention and injury management.
22-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
22.6.1 Overview
Three models of rehabilitation ergonomics provide return to work:
1. The clinical model utilizes an occupational rehabilitation specialist in the clinic and a rehabilita-
tion ergonomist onsite for the re-entry to work phase. The clinical model is utilized in three situ-
ations. First, if a worker has an illness or injury that requires strict safety standards, the adherence
to medical safety in the clinic provides security. This can include those with heart rate or blood
pressure problems, as well as those in the healing stage of a severe sprain, fracture, etc. Second, if a
worker is at a level of job readiness that is too low for even modified work, conditioning will need
to be provided to raise the worker to a level that would sustain half or full day of work. Third,
some occupations do not provide for anything but full duty. This could include trucking, con-
struction work, heavy manufacturing or heavy patient handling.
2. The mixed clinical and work model combines the benefit of safe structured rehabilitation with
worksite experience that prevents the worker from becoming alienated from the work and
coworkers.
3. The onsite work model is utilized either when there is a clinic at the worksite or the employer
approves using actual work for rehabilitation.
Once John returns to work, he is monitored on a regular basis to insure safety and provided with reas-
surance and assistance by his supervisor and coworkers. Sequential upgrading of activity takes place until
full duty is reached. A satisfactory final evaluation by the therapist and physician formally releases John to
full duty.
Two carry-over changes will impact John:
1. John is interested in maintaining his new fitness level. He expressed concern that he had never
recovered his preoperative work state until he participated in the work rehabilitation. His
employer pays for a local health club membership. John’s program will be transferred to this facil-
ity for maintenance and improvement in fitness and function.
2. The employer and medical providers became aware that workers who have been out for prolonged
periods, with any medical condition, would benefit from functional testing prior to returning to
heavy work. If there is a deficiency, a suitable rehabilitation program will be provided and the
rehabilitation ergonomist will be able to serve such workers at the worksite. The employer has
identified a high reinjury rate for returning employees and has targeted this for reduction with
these policies and programs.
This combines the best of clinical rehabilitation with the use of a rehabilitation ergonomist in return
to work.
At discharge, she does have considerations that were not in place previously. They are contained in a
new agreement with her supervisor and employer:
1. When faced with pushing/lifting that is beyond her specified job requirements, she will seek help.
This will prevent the sprain/strain she suffered previously.
2. If myofascial symptoms are high, she will be allowed short breaks for her stretching. She and her
supervisor will work out what is acceptable. This will prevent her suffering in silence and being
absent because of another physical problem.
3. The equipment she has to move has been ergonomically redesigned with larger and more movable
wheels. Force required for pushing and pulling has been reduced from 100# previously to 30# with
the new wheels. If other equipment or material creates high stresses, she is to report them for
analysing an ergonomic modification. To this point, the employer now has a relationship with
the rehabilitation ergonomist for more work to reduce stressors in the entire facility.
4. Interest in stretches, ergonomic education and fitness increased after there was interaction with
the rehabilitation ergonomist. Preventive programs were discussed and are being put into place
for all employees.
His resumption of full duty is accompanied by removal of restrictions on home activity. The following
ergonomic home and work guidelines remain:
1. He is to continue to use the pneumatic impact wrench at work to reduce work stressors perma-
nently. He will make similar accommodations at home. He now realizes tool use and heavy repeti-
tive gripping must be monitored in his recreation and home activities. New sensibilities are
present in his home carpentry, sports and chores. They particularly revolve around ergonomic
positions, proper tools, reduction of stressors and implementation of stretching as appropriate.
2. If he has symptoms again, he is to report these at work as soon as possible. The health department
at work has determined that causation (work-related or not work-related) will not stop the
process of working with him to avoid a reinjury. The rehabilitation ergonomist also can be
brought in any time that there is an issue.
3. He participates in safety meetings to work on early intervention and ergonomic guidelines for all
employees.
Note: This case was resolved with no lost time and 4 weeks of modified duty. This result was superior to
former cases of workers with similar problems. In addition to the good metrics (decreased lost time and
medical costs) both the worker and supervisor rated their satisfaction as high.
FIGURE 22.6 Working onsite, a rehabilitation ergonomist analyzes how the worker’s neck and upper extremities
function during work activity. This forms a base for tasks described in a job function description.
. Assist in creation of a model that defines work health as a continuum, using the team
approach to problem solving for all workers whether currently healthy or temporarily
unable to perform work. Minimize the need for separate systems for injured and noninjured
workers
FIGURE 22.7 In the return to work process, the rehabilitation ergonomist blends knowledge of the worker’s
functional capacity with the demands of the job to ensure productivity and prevent reinjury.
Rehabilitation Ergonomics 22-15
Rehabilitation ergonomics is one of several models of broader ergonomics. With continued dialog,
research and interaction across professions, it will continue to provide value for the workers and
employers it serves.
References
1. Occupational Health Guidelines: evaluating functional capacity, American Physical Therapy Associ-
ation, Alexandria VA, 1999.
2. Occupational Health Guidelines: physical therapist management of the acutely injured worker,
American Physical Therapy Association, Alexandria VA, 2000.
3. Occupational Health Guidelines: work conditioning and work hardening programs, American Phys-
ical Therapy Association, Alexandria VA, 1997.
4. Occupational Health Guidelines: work related injury/illness prevention and ergonomics, American
Physical Therapy Association, Alexandria VA, 2001.
5. Occupational therapy services in ergonomics, American Occupational Therapy Association,
Bethesda MD, 1998.
6. Isernhagen, S.J., Hart, D.L., and Matheson, L.N., Rehabilitation ergonomists: standards for develop-
ment, Work, 10, 199, 1998.
7. Ergonomic program management guidelines for meatpacking plants, U.S. Department of Labor,
Occupational Safety and Health Administration, OSHA 3123,1993.
8. Effective Ergonomics: strategy for success, www.OSHA.gov/ergonomics, current.
9. Elements of Ergonomics Programs. NIOSH, DHHS Publications 97, 1997.
10. A technical assistance manual on the employment provision of the Americans with Disabilities
Act, Equal Employment Opportunity Commission, U.S. Government Printing Office, Washington,
DC, 1992.
11. Isernhagen, S., Functional capacity evaluation, in Work Injury Management and Prevention, Isernhagen, S.,
Ed., Aspen, Gaithersburg MD, 1988, Chap. 14.
12. Hart, D.L., Isernhagen, S.J., and Matheson, L.N., Guidelines for functional capacity evaluation of
people with medical conditions, J. Ortho. Sports Phys. Ther., 18, 682, 1993.
13. Key, G., Work capacity analysis, in Physical Therapy, Scully, R. and Barnes, M., Eds., J.B. Lippincott,
Philadelphia PA, 304, 1989.
14. Selected characteristics of occupations as defined in the revised dictionary of occupational titles, U.S.
Department of Labor, National Technical Information Service, Washington, DC, 94, 1993.
15. Reneman, M.F. et al., Test-retest reliability of lifting and carrying in a 2 day functional capacity evalu-
ation, J. Occup. Rehab., 12, 269, 2002.
16. Reneman, M.F. et al., The reliability of determining effort level of lifting and carrying in a functional
capacity evaluation, J. Occup. Rehab., 18, 23, 2002.
17. Brouwer, S. et al., Test-retest reliability of a modified Isernhagen Work Systems functional capacity
evaluation in patients with chronic low back pain, J. Occup. Rehab., 13, 207, 2003.
18. Gross, D.P. and Battie, M.C., Reliability of safe maximum lifting determinations of a functional
capacity, Phys. Ther., 4, 364, 2002.
19. Isernhagen, S., Hart, D.L., and Matheson, L.N., Reliability of independent observer judgments of
level of lift effort in a kinesiophysical functional capacity evaluation, Work, 12, 145, 1999.
20. Lechner, D.E. et al., Reliability and validity of a newly developed test of physical work performance,
J. Occup. Med., 38, 997, 1994.
21. Matheson, L.N. et al., A test to measure lift capacity of physically impaired adults, Part 1, develop-
ment and reliability testing, Spine, 20, 2119, 1995.
22. Smith, R.L., Therapists’ ability to identify safe maximum lifting in low back pain patients during
functional capacity evaluation, J. Ortho. Sports Phys. Ther., 19, 277, 1994.
23. Gross, D.P. and Battie, M.C., The construct validity of a kinesiophysical functional capacity evalu-
ation administered within a worker’s compensation environment. J. Occup. Rehab., 13, 287, 2003.
22-16 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
24. Matheson, L.N., Isernhagen, S.J., and Hart, L., Relationships among lifting ability, grip force and
return to work, Phys. Ther., 82, 249, 2002.
25. Reneman, M.F. et al., Concurrent validity of questionnaire and performance-based disability
measurements in patients with chronic nonspecific low back pain, J. Occup. Rehab., 12, 119, 2002.
26. Innes, E. and Straker, L., Reliability of work-related assessments, Work, 13, 107, 1999.
27. Innes, E. and Straker, L., Validity of work-related assessments, Work, 13, 125, 1999.
28. Lett, C.F., Work hardening in Work Injury Management and Prevention, Isernhagen, S., Ed., Aspen,
Gaithersburg MD, 1988, Chap. 15.
29. Matheson, L.N. et al., Work hardening: occupational therapy in industrial rehabilitation, Am.
J. Occup. Ther., 39, 314, 1985.
30. Matheson, L., Work Capacity Evaluation Manual, Employment and Rehabilitation Institute of
California, 1987.
31. Work Hardening Standards, Commission on Accreditation of Rehabilitation Facilities, Tucson AZ,
1992.
32. Mayer, T. et al., Objective assessment of spine function following industrial injury: a prospective
study with comparison group and one year followup, Spine, 10, 482, 1985.
33. Isernhagen, S., Primary and secondary therapy for acute musculoskeletal disorders, in Occupational
Musculoskeletal Disorders, Mayer, T.G., Gatchel, R.J., and Polatin, P.B., Lippincott, Williams and
Wilkins, Philadelphia, 2000, Chap. 19.
34. Vance, S.R., Brown, A.M., On-site medical care and physical therapy impact, in Comprehensive Guide
to Work Injury Management, Isernhagen, S., Ed., Aspen, Gaithersburg MD, 1995, Chap. 13.
35. Pransky, G. et al., Work-related outcomes in occupational low back pain, Spine, 27, 864, 2002.
36. Feuerstein, M. et al., Clinical and workplace factors associated with a return to modified duty in
work-related upper extremity disorders, Pain, 102, 51, 2003.
37. Staal, J.B., Return to work interventions for low back pain: a descriptive review of contents and con-
cepts of working mechanisms, Sports Med., 32, 251, 2002.
38. Loisel, P. et al., Management of occupational back pain: the Sherbrooke model: results of a pilot and
feasibility study, Occup. Environ. Med., 51, 597, 1994.
39. Loisel, P. et al., A population-based, randomized clinical trial on back pain management, Spine, 22,
2911, 1997.
40. Loisel, P. et al., Cost-benefit and cost-effectiveness of a disability prevention model for back pain
management: six year follow up study, Occup. Environ. Med., 59, 807, 2002.
41. Durand, M.J. et al., Constructing the program impact theory for an evidence-based work rehabilita-
tion program for workers with low back pain, Work, 21, 233, 2003.
42. Anema, J.R. et al., Participatory ergonomics as a return to work intervention: a future challenge?
Am. J. Ind. Med., 44, 273, 2003.
43. Lemstra, M. and Olszynski, W.P., The effectiveness of standard care, early intervention, and occu-
pational management in workers compensation claims, Spine, 25, 299, 2003.
44. Isernhagen, S.J., Functional capacities assessment after rehabilitation, in Ergonomics, Bullock, M.,
Ed., Churchill Livingstone, London, 1990, Chap. 11.
45. Nygard, C.H., Pikkanen, M., and Arola, Hl, Promotion of health and work ability through ergo-
nomics among aging workers, Proceedings of the 34th Congress of the Nordic Ergonomics
Society, Humans in a Complex Environment Vol II, Kalmarden, Sweden, 611, 2002.
46. Job Accommodation Network Website, www.jan
23
Visual, Tactile, and
Multimodal
Information
Processing
23.1 Introduction
This chapter will discuss the most, and one of the least, frequently employed sensory channels in current
interface design: vision and touch. Foveal vision1 in particular continues to be relied on heavily in display
design, mostly because it affords a higher rate of information transfer than other sensory channels
(Sorkin, 1987). Visual representations appear to be well-suited for conveying large amounts of
complex detailed information, especially in the spatial domain. They also allow for permanent presen-
tation, which affords delayed and prolonged attending. However, in many environments, overreliance
on, and the inappropriate design of, foveal visual displays has resulted in data overload and related break-
downs in attention management and human–machine interaction.
One promising way to overcome these problems was suggested by an early version of Multiple
Resource Theory (MRT; Wickens, 1984), which assumed that different modalities draw from separate
pools of attentional resources and that therefore more information could be processed effectively if it
was distributed across sensory channels. This assumption seems to be supported by experiences in
many real-world domains where a combination of visual and auditory information presentation is
1
In contrast to peripheral vision, foveal vision involves the perception of cues that are presented in the central 28 of
visual angle.
23-1
23-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
being employed rather successfully. In some cases, and under certain circumstances, however, even the
use of these two channels is no longer sufficient nor appropriate to handle the ever increasing
amounts and complexity of available data. This has recently sparked considerable interest in haptic
interfaces as an alternate or additional channel for conveying information (Sarter, 2002).
Haptic sensory information can take various forms, including proprioceptive, kinesthetic, and tactile
cues. Tactile feedback — the focus of this chapter — is presented to the skin in the form of force, texture,
vibration, and thermal sensations. One advantage of cues presented in the tactile modality is that they do
not require a particular body or head orientation in order to be perceived. They are transient in nature yet
rather difficult to miss, which makes them appropriate for indicating unexpected events (but not necess-
arily critical events that warrant warnings and alerts for which the more intrusive auditory channel tends
to be reserved). Also, tactile cues are well-suited for providing spatial guidance.
First, we will present separate brief overviews of the anatomy and physiology of the visual and tactile
systems, which focus primarily on the physical basis for the registration and early processing of sensory
stimuli, rather than the phenomenal experience of perception. The chapter will also consider different
approaches to perception, such as Gestalt theory, which are concerned with the question how we
succeed in disambiguating retinal images and thus “why we see what we see.” These perspectives reject
the idea that physiology can ultimately explain perception. Instead, strong top-down influences are con-
sidered necessary to be able to “make sense” of the world around us. Perceptual phenomena and limit-
ations that are associated with the two modalities and that are of particular interest to human factors
professionals, will be described, and affordances associated with both modalities will be reviewed. The
last part of the chapter is concerned with the benefits and limitations of combining vision, audition,
and touch in multimodal interfaces. In this context, recently identified crossmodal constraints on atten-
tion will be discussed.
Because this chapter will provide an overview of both visual, tactile, and multimodal information pro-
cessing, the level of detail in which each area can be covered is necessarily limited. The interested reader
is referred to Soderquist (2002), Wade and Swanson (2001), Purves and Lotto (2003), and Gordon (1989)
for more indepth accounts of the neurophysiological basis for, as well as empiricist accounts and
theories of vision. More detailed descriptions of various aspects of touch can be found, for example,
in Cholewiak and Collins (1991), Loomis and Lederman (1986), and Kruger (1996). Finally, multimodal
information presentation and processing is examined in more detail in Stein and Meredith (1993),
Spence and Driver (1997), Oviatt (2002), and Sarter (2002).
23.2 Vision
We will begin this account of vision by ignoring, for now, the process by which objects in our surround-
ings are selected for in-depth processing. Instead, we will assume that the process of visual perception
simply starts with light passing through the cornea, a protective surface that surrounds the eyeball.
The cornea covers the pigmented iris, which adjusts in diameter to control the amount of light entering
the pupil, the round opening at the front of the eye. Next, the light passes through the lens, a transparent
oval structure that is located directly behind the pupil and iris. Ciliary muscles adjust the shape of the lens
to bring the object in focus on the retina at the back of the eye — a process called accommodation. When
the eyes are at rest, the shape of the lens is relatively flat and allows us to view distant objects. To view
nearer objects, the shape of the lens becomes more spherical and therefore has greater refractive
power (e.g., Soderquist, 2002).
The process and limits of accomodation are of interest to human factors practitioners for various
reasons. First, accomodation contributes to depth perception (as will be discussed later), which is
important for a variety of real-world tasks. Also, accomodation is affected by aging, which
often leads to reduced elasticity of the lens and thus a limited range over which objects can be
brought into focus. The result can be myopia (nearsightedness where distant objects cannot be
Visual, Tactile, and Multimodal Information Processing 23-3
3
Subjective measurements
2
1
Objective measurements
0 40 45 50 55 60
Figure 27 Age (years)
FIGURE 23.1 Average magnitude of available accommodation by age, measured subjectively and objectively.
(Adapted from Hamasaki, D., Ong, J. and Marg, E., Am. J. Optom. Physiol. Opt., 33, pp. 3–14, 1956.)
focused) or presbyopia (farsightnedness where near stimuli cannot be brought in focus) (e.g.,
Soderquist, 2002).
For example, a 15-yr-old healthy user normally has approximately 10 diopters of accommodation
(or is able to focus at 1/10 of a meter). However, that same person at age 40 may show less than one-
third of that accommodation. Figure 23.1 illustrates how the ability to accommodate deteriorates
even further for people over the age of 40 yr, both objectively and according to subjective ratings.
This reduced accommodation ability can be compensated for, to a limited extent, by applying more
force to the muscles that control accommodation. Still, it cannot be overcome completely and therefore
needs to be considered in the design of interfaces that are intended to be used by a range of diverse users.
After passing through the lens, visual stimuli reach the retina at the back of the eye. The retina consists
of layers of cells that contain two major types of photoreceptors: rods and cones. These two types of recep-
tors contain different light-processing chemicals, called photopigments. Rods, which contain the photo-
pigment rhodopsin, are much more numerous than cones. There are approximately 100 to 120 million
rods in the retina, compared to only 6 to 7 million cones.
Rods and cones differ considerably with respect to the perceptual functions they support. Rods are
highly sensitive to light but they are not sensitive to color nor do they support high-acuity vision.
Thus, rods are particularly important for night vision and mostly ineffective during daylight because
of saturation. Rods also support contrast sensitivity, a perceptual phenomenon that is of critical import-
ance to human factors professionals. Contrast sensitivity has been defined as “the reciprocal of minimum
contrast between lighter and darker spatial areas that can just be detected” (Wickens et al., 1998). Contrast
sensitivity thus is a prerequisite for detecting and recognizing shapes. Several factors influence contrast
sensitivity, including the illumination of an object where lower illumination reduces sensitivity, as illus-
trated by our difficulties with reading under low lighting conditions or detecting objects at night. Contrast
sensitivity is also reduced when the object of interest is moving and with increasing age due to factors
such as cataracts (increased clouding of the lens).
When stimulated, rods rapidly lose their sensitivity to light and require a long time to regain it. For
example, when entering a dark room, we cannot distinguish any objects around us at first. Over
several minutes, the visual system adapts to the ambient light and objects become increasingly visible.
This dark adaptation can take as long as 20 to 30 min if a transition from photopic (cone-based
vision) to scotopic (rod-based vision) conditions is required. Note that during the first 7 min in dark-
ness, the cones require less light to perceive a visual stimulus. After that time period, the cones are
more sensitive. This point is referred to as the rod-cone break. The reverse process, light adaptation
from darkness to bright light, occurs significantly faster than dark adaptation. It requires only about
2 to 3 min. Dark and light adaptation need to be considered in the design of workspaces that involve
rapid changes in illumination (see Purves and Lotto, 2003).
23-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
One way to overcome difficulties with dark adaptation through design is being suggested by the fact
that, while rods are highly sensitive to light, their sensitivity to color is very limited. In particular, they are
insensitive to long wavelengths (which, as we will discuss later, lead to the perception of a red hue). We
can turn this apparent limitation into an advantage by illuminating objects in red light and thus mini-
mize the stimulation of rods. By doing so, the need for dark adaptation is minimized, which can be very
useful, for example, when briefing pilots before a night mission.
Rods do not only exhibit high sensitivity to light; they are also highly effective for perceiving orientation
and motion (e.g., Leibowitz, 1988). One often cited illustration of how to utilize this affordance of photo-
pic vision is the so-called Malcolm Horizon that helps pilots notice changes in an airplane’s roll and pitch
without having to look directly at a foveal visual display (Stokes et al., 1990). The Malcolm Horizon is a line
of red laser light that is projected onto the instrument panel to the left and right of the traditional attitude
indicator. The Malcolm Horizon moves along
with the artificial horizon relative to the earth’s
surface as the plane moves through space. (a)
Lightness or
Unlike the attitude indicator, the Malcolm brightness
n
Hu
io
differ in terms of the type of light absorbing pigment they contain. These cones are “tuned” to different
portions of the visible spectrum (e.g., Soderquist, 2002; Purves and Lotto, 2003):
. Cones that absorb best at the relatively long wavelengths peaking at 575 nm (leading to the percep-
tion of red)
. Cones with a peak absorption at 535 nm (leading to the perception of green)
. Cones with a peak absorption at 445 nm (leading to the perception of blue)
A number of predictable contrast and fatigue effects are associated with color vision. These effects can
be demonstrated, for example, by fixating a red square that is placed on a green surrounding. After a few
moments, we begin to see a greenish tinge surrounding the red. Also, an intense green light induces a
reddish afterimage. The same effect can be observed also for a blue square on a yellow background,
or, more generally, for any complementary hues, that is, hues that, when mixed, form neutral grays.
While the fovea contains almost exclusively cones, the periphery of the retina is inhabited by both rods
and cones, with the number of cones declining rapidly with increasing eccentricity (Figure 23.3). As a
result, color discrimination is degraded at eccentricities beyond 20 to 308. For example, the relative
brightness and perceived hues change, causing red and green to appear yellow. Also, colors that are per-
ceived in the periphery tend to be less saturated. This gradual reduction in the ability to discriminate
colors in the periphery can be compensated for, to some extent, by increasing the size, luminance, or sat-
uration of the stimulus. However, complete color blindness starts at around 40 to 508 of visual angle.
Another factor that can limit color vision is color blindness or color deficiency. Complete color blind-
ness in people is extremely rare. Only about 0.005% of the population is truly color blind, that is, they
completely lack at least one of the photopigments used to transmit color information (Cornsweet, 1970).
However, for about 8% of males and 0.5% of females, some color distinctions are absent or at least not as
pronounced. In these cases, all photopigments are present but their responses are slightly altered. People
with color deficiencies most often have trouble distinguishing between red and green, a form of color
deficiency that is referred to as “protanopia”. They discriminate between the two hues based on perceived
brightness instead. For example, red colors appear darker to a person with a deficiency of “red” photo-
pigment cones (Murch, 1984). The need to account for users with color blindness or deficiency can be
considered one of the reasons for the general design recommendation to create monochrome displays
first and add color only at a later stage (Shneiderman, 1998).
As mentioned earlier, cones are not only responsible for color vision; they also support high acuity
vision. Their ability to resolve detail is much greater than for rods which, in turn, display greater
FIGURE 23.3 Distribution of rods and cones in the retina. (Adapted from Rosenzweig, M.R., Leiman, A.L., and
Breedlove, S.M., Biological Psychology, Sunderland, MA, Sinauer Associates Inc., 1996.)
23-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
sensitivity to light. To understand this trade-off, we need to examine the structure of the retina in more
detail. As shown in Figure 23.4, the retina consists not only of rods and cones but also includes amacrine,
bipolar, horizontal, and ganglion cells.
A detailed treatment of the functions of these cells is beyond the scope of this chapter. Basically, both rods
and cones connect to bipolar cells which, in turn, transfer impulses to a second set of neurons, called
ganglion cells. Horizontal cells synapse with both bipolar cells and receptor cells as well as with other
horizontal cells. And amacrine cells synapse with both bipolar cells and ganglion cells as well as with
other amacrine cells. The horizontal and amacrine cells, thus, help transfer information laterally
among different elements of the retina. In contrast, bipolar and ganglion cells play a role in determining
the sensitivity and the acuity of the photopic (cone-based) and scotopic (rod-based) visual systems.
To understand this phenomenon, remember that there are approximately 100 to 120 million rods and
4 to 6 million cones in the retina but only approximately 1 million ganglion cells. This implies that a
considerable amount of convergence and compression of information must occur as information is
passed from the receptor cells via the bipolar cells to the ganglion cells. Convergence, that is, the sum-
mation of input from several receptors to a ganglion cell, supports increased sensitivity because a
weak stimulus can activate several receptors to a limited extent and, once their input is combined and
sent off to the ganglion cell, it may exceed the threshold for neural activity, that is, for an action potential
to occur. As many as 1000 rods may pass information via their bipolar cells to a single ganglion cell, thus
exhibiting a high degree of convergence.
In contrast, cones show very little convergence. There is typically a 1:1 relationship and ratio between
cones in the fovea and the corresponding ganglion cells. For cones outside the fovea, the ratio is some-
what larger but never reaches that of rods. This explains the high acuity of cones, which have smaller
receptive fields than rods. A receptive field can be defined as “a circumscribed area on the retina that pro-
vides the input to a ganglion cell” (Soderquist, 2002).
Table 23.1 summarizes the main differences between rods and cones that have been discussed so far.
So far, we have focused on monocular vision, that is, the structures and perceptual processes associated
with the individual eye. To understand other important visual functions, such as depth perception, we
need to consider binocular vision and its affordances. For example, binocular vision grants us a larger
field of view, it reduces the risk of becoming disabled following damage to one eye, and it supports stereo-
scopic vision and thus depth perception.
Depth perception is important for a variety of tasks (e.g., flying an aircraft, driving a car), where a
person needs to be able to judge distance from and between objects in the environment. It is supported
by three main classes of depth cues: (a) oculomotor cues, (b) visual binocular cues, and (c) visual
monocular cues. Oculomotor cues include accomodation (discussed earlier), which provides depth
information by informing higher-level brain regions about the extent to which the ciliary muscles
had to change the lens shape in order to bring the object of interest in focus. This information
indirectly indicates the distance of an object from the observer. Convergence, that is, the amount to
which inward rotation of the two eyeballs is necessary to bring an image to rest on corresponding
areas of the retina of both eyes, is an example of a binocular depth cue. If an object is 6 m or more
away from the observer, the line of sight is parallel. If the object moves closer, the eyeballs begin to
turn inward progressively. Binocular disparity, that is, the disparity between the views obtained by
each eyeball, is another example of such cues and also provides information on distance. These
three mechanisms relate, for the most part, to depth perception for objects that are close to the obser-
ver (within a few meters).
Depth perception for more distant objects requires so-called “pictorial” cues, which are based on
past experience and thus represent a top-down influence on perception. They include, but are not
limited to:
. Linear perspective, that is, convergence of parallel lines toward a more distant point (Figure 23.5)
. Relative size, that is, if two objects that are known to be of the same size occupy different visual
angles, then the one occupying a smaller angle is perceived to be farther away
Visual, Tactile, and Multimodal Information Processing 23-7
FIGURE 23.4 Basic structures of the human eye and configuration of various cell types in the human retina.
(Adapted from Rosenzweig, M.R., Leiman, A.L., and Breedlove, S.M., Biological Psychology, Sunderland, MA,
Sinauer Associates Inc., 1996.)
23-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
. Parallax or relative motion, that is, more distant objects show smaller movement across visual field
as the observer moves
. Occlusion, that is, if one object is in front of another (with respect to the viewer), it “over-writes”
the other object in the image (Figure 23.6)
It is important to note that depth perception is affected by illumination and texture. For example, objects
showing less texture tend to be perceived as being further away. This explained the difficulties that pilots
experience when they are flying at night and/or over an extended body of snow or calm water. The lack of
surface texture creates the risk that pilots overestimate their altitude.
Visual perception beyond the retinal level is carried by the axons of the ganglion cells, which form the
optic nerve. The optic nerve leaves the eye through the retina in the area of the optic disk (better known
as the “blind spot” because of the absence of receptor cells, and thus the inability to perceive stimuli, in
this area). Part of each branch of the optic nerve crosses over in an area in front of the pituitary gland at
the optic chiasm and then reaches the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), a walnut-sized nuclear complex
in the thalamus.
The LGN comprises several layers, including two magnocellular layers and four parvocellular layers.
The magnocellular layers receive input from larger retinal ganglion cells. They are characterized by
high sensitivity, low spatial resolution, high temporal resolution, and little or no color selectivity.
These cells and layers are concerned primarily with the perception of motion. The parvocellular layers
are innervated by smaller ganglion cells and exhibit low sensitivity, high spatial resolution, low temporal
resolution, and color selectivity. Thus, their role is to transmit spatially detailed visual information such
as form and color sensations.
The neurons in both the magno- and parvocellular layers are innervated by axons descending
from the cortex, which support the top-down modulation of perception through factors such as
expectations and the perceived importance of a signal. Also, the retinal ganglion cells project to other
brain regions, such as brainstem cells that control pupil diameter as a function of light intensity, the
superior colliculus (mediates the organization of eye movements to keep objects in focus), and
the hypothalamus (involved in organizing circadian rhythm based on normal cycles of light and dark).
From the LGN, projections lead to the primary visual cortex where objects in the left visual field are
represented in the right hemisphere and vice versa. At this level, only simple visual sensations are avail-
able, which maintain a topographical representation of the pattern of retinal activity. This representation
is abandoned once the signals are sent to higher-order visual processing areas in the occipital, parietal,
and temporal lobes of the brain (Figure 23.7).
One major difference between neurons in the primary and higher-order visual areas is the size of
their receptive fields. Receptive fields of neurons in the primary cortex serve foveal vision and cover
less than 18 of visual angle. In contrast, receptive fields of neurons serving peripheral vision are a
few degrees of visual angle across. The overall visual field extends 1808 horizontally and 1308 vertically.
But why do we see what we see?
“Whilst part of what we perceive comes through our senses from the object before us, another part
(and it may be the larger part) always comes out of our head.”
(William James, 1890)
The previous brief overview of the morphology and physiology of the visual system and of selected
perceptual phenomena does not answer two very important questions about visual perception:
(a) “What will the user look at?” and (b) “What will things look like to the user?”. The first question
is concerned with the selection of visual objects for further processing, which, as mentioned earlier, is
affected both by top-down influences (such as operator expectations) and bottom-up factors (such
as the salience of objects). It has been suggested that an initial organization of the visual field that
supports figure-ground perception occurs at a preattentive level. In other words, the entire visual
field is processed automatically with the goal to detect basic features of objects such as colors, contrast,
or size (Treisman, 1986) and determine which objects should undergo further processing (Broadbent,
1958; Neisser, 1976).
Interface design can capitalize on this bottom-up process for the purpose of attention guidance
through the presentation of highly salient or conspicuous objects, that is, objects that are large,
bright, colorful, or flashing (Wickens, 1984) and thus are likely to capture attention.
It is important to note that these tendencies are easily overridden by top-down influences on the
selection of visual objects for in-depth processing. For example, people tend to start scanning in
23-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Optic
nerve Optic
Optic tract
chiasm
Hypothalamus:
Dorsal lateral
circadian rhythm
geniculate
nucleus Edinger-Westphal nucleus:
pupillary light reflex
Optic
radiation
Superior colliculus:
orienting the
Primary movements
visual cortex of head and eyes
FIGURE 23.7 The pathways of visual information throughout the visual system. (Adapted from Purves, D. and
Lotto, R.B., Why We See What We Do: An Empirical Theory of Vision, Sunderland, MA, Sinauer Associates Inc.,
2003. With permission.)
the upper left corner of a display and focus on the center regions of the interface while avoiding its
edges (Wickens, 1984). Another top-down influence on visual selection is expectations and schemata.
The interplay between top-down and bottom-up influences on the selection of visual stimuli is
captured by Neisser’s (1976) perceptual cycle (Figure 23.8).
One starting point for perception in this cycle is internal schemata (mental models or expectations)
that direct sensory exploration which, in turn, samples the environment for relevant information. The
result of this exploration is either confirmation or modification of the existing schemata, which leads
to the beginning of a new cycle, possibly involving redirection of attention. Alternatively, as mentioned
earlier, highly salient stimuli in the environment can serve as the starting point for a perceptual cycle
where the person’s attention is captured externally. The signal that attracted attention may lead to
changes of expectations or schemata and result in a subsequent search for additional related information.
The second important question that remains unanswered by any account of the neurophysiological
basis of vision is “Why do things look as they do?” One critical question in this context is how the
visual system deals with the inherent ambiguity of retinal stimuli. Answering this question was one of
the driving forces behind the Gestaltist movement, which proposed that “the whole is greater than the
sum of its parts.” In other words, Gestalt theorists insisted that what matters is not isolated stimuli
but entire patterns and configurations. Gestaltists also emphasize that perception is not passive.
Instead, humans impose structure on observed visual stimuli and scenes. Without this ability to organize
and interpret sensations, we would feel surrounded by a meaningless mishmash of colors and shapes.
One of the main questions that Gestaltists (including Wertheimer, 1880 –1943, Koehler; 1887–1964;
and Koffka, 1886–1941) studied was how we organize our percepts to be able to distinguish between
figures and the background against which they are seen. Some of the general organizational tendencies
Visual, Tactile, and Multimodal Information Processing 23-11
Object
available
information
Modifies Samples
Schema Exploration
Directs
that have been observed by Gestaltists in this context are: (a) that symmetric patterns tend to be seen as a
figure, (b) a region that is completely surrounded by another is perceived as a figure, (c) the smaller of
two regions tends to be seen as a figure, and (d) vertically or horizontally organized region tends to be
seen as figure. A number of Gestalt laws were formulated, including the laws of proximity, similarity,
closure (gaps in figures are filled in), and common fate (elements with common motion or orientation
are perceived as belonging together).
The law of proximity predicts that elements that are closer together will be perceived as a coherent
object. This tendency is illustrated in Figure 23.9. On the left (a), the gauges in each column are likely
to be perceived as belonging together whereas on the right (b), the gauges in each row are spaced
more closely and therefore seem to form groups. Consideration of the law of proximity is important
in interface design to achieve an appropriate system image, that is, to create a physical structure of a
system (in this case, the arrangement of gauges) that suggests the correct relationship between elements.
which also includes proprioception and kinesthesis (sensory input from joint, muscles, and internal
organs) and pain (tissue damaging high intensity stimuli). Touch is, for the most part, a proximal
sense. In other words, we tend to feel stimuli that are in contact with, or at least in close proximity
to, our body (Cholewiak and Collins, 1991). Touch is also our only bi-directional sense, that is, it sup-
ports both perception and acting on the environment.
We will start our overview of touch by examining its medium, the skin, which is a multi-layered sheet
of 1.8 m2 in area and approximately 4 kg in weight in an average adult. There are three types of skin: (a)
glabrous skin, that is, hairless skin such as the skin of our palms, (b) hairy skin, and (c) mucocutaneous
skin, that is, skin that borders the entrances to the body’s interior (Greenspan and Bolanowski, 1996).
The most active role in tactual perception is played by the glabrous skin, especially in the palmar and
fingertip regions of the hand. The ridges and valleys of the skin in this area have been implied in the per-
ception of texture and in the tactile identification of objects. Most studies on tactual perception have
focused on these regions, and they are most often used to present tactile stimuli in current interfaces
(e.g., CyberTouch, Tactools, and Touchmaster). In general, the skin is composed of the epidermis (its
outer layer) and the dermis (the inner layer), both of which contain several types of receptors (see
Figure 23.12).
In our overview of touch, we will focus on tactile sensations resulting from mechanical stimulation of
the skin, which forms the basis of most current tactile displays. Mechanoreceptors can be divided into
four major types (e.g., Burdea, 1996):
1. Meissner corpuscles: these receptors represent approximately 43% of all tactile receptors in the
hand. They are found only in glabrous skin and are sensitive to stimuli such as velocity and
skin curvature.
2. Merkel’s disks: merkel’s disks represent 25% of all mechanoreceptors in the hand and sense gentle
localized pressure and vibration information.
3. Pacinian corpuscles: these receptors are located deeper in both hairy and glabrous skin. Approxi-
mately 13% of all mechanoreceptors are Pacinian corpuscles. They sense rapid variations of
deformation, acceleration, and vibration.
4. Ruffini corpuscles: 19% of all mechanoreceptors in the hand are Ruffini corpuscles. They are
located deep under the skin and are sensitive to vibrations, stretching of the skin, and thermal
changes.
The distribution of these receptors varies considerably across different body regions. For example,
there is a total of approximately 17,000 mechanoreceptors in the human hand (Johansson and Vallbo,
1983), which exceeds by far the number of these receptors in other body regions. Also, certain types
of receptors are not represented in some body regions. For example, there are no Pacinian corpuscles
in the skin of the cheek (Cholewiak and Collins, 1991).
The four types of mechanoreceptors can be classified according to the following two criteria (Konta-
niris and Howe, 1995; Johansson and Vallbo, 1983):
1. The receptor’s active area: small well-defined receptive fields (Type I units) and larger receptive
fields with obscure borders (Type II).
2. The receptor’s response to static stimuli: approximately 45% of all mechanoreceptors respond to
static stimuli with a sustained discharge and are called slowly adapting (SA). The remaining recep-
tors respond to the onset and offset of stimuli with bursts of impulses and are called fast or rapidly
adapting (FA or RA).
Table 23.2 summarizes important characteristics of the four receptor types.
For the purpose of this chapter, it is not necessary to examine in detail the process of transformation of
mechanical stimuli into neural events. This process is still poorly understood and, more importantly, it is
of limited relevance for human factors practitioner. Ultimately, all tactile information is relayed to the
somatosensory cortex, which is laid out in the form of a homunculus representing the opposite side
of the body. In this representation, areas of greater sensitivity occupy larger cortex areas. This suggests
23-14 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
FIGURE 23.12 Cross-section of the human glabrous (a) and hairy (b) skin showing various haptic receptors. (Adapted
from Kruger, L. eds., Handbook of Perception and Cognition: Pain and Touch, 2nd edn., Academic Press, New York, 1996.
With permission.)
the most promising areas for applying tactile feedback such as the fingertips or lips. However, since it is
often not feasible to use these regions for tactile displays in real-world environments where tactile devices
could interfere with the performance of communication or manipulation tasks, designers need to make
trade-off decisions.
Visual, Tactile, and Multimodal Information Processing
TABLE 23.2 Functional Features of Cutaneous Mechanoreceptors
Meissner Pacinian
Characteristic Corpuscles Corpuscles Merkel’s Disks Ruffini Endings
Receptor type FAI FAII SAI SAII
Rate of adaptation Rapid Rapid Slow Slow
Location Shallow; superficial Deep; dermis and Shallow; superficial Deep; dermis and
dermis subcutaneous dermis subcutaneous
Mean size of receptive field 13 mm2 101 mm2 11 mm2 59 mm2
Spatial resolution Poor Very poor Good Fair
Percent of all mechanoreceptors 43% 13% 25% 19%
Response frequency range 10 to 200 Hz 70 to 1000 Hz 0.4 to 100 Hz 0.4 to 100 Hz
Spatial summation Yes No No Unknown
Temporal summation Yes No No Yes
Source: Adopted from Shimoga, K. A survey of perceptual feedback issues in dextrous telemanipulation: Part II. Finger touch feedback. Proceeding of IEEE
Virtual Reality Annual International Symposium, pp. 271–279. New York: IEEE, 1993. With permission.
23-15
23-16 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
The absolute threshold for perceiving tactile stimuli varies not only between different locations
on a person’s body but also depends on a variety of other factors (Cholewiak and Collins, 1991),
including:
1. Frequency of the stimulus: for example, sensitivity for tactile stimuli reaches a maximum at 200 to
300 Hz for Pacinian corpuscles, which respond to vibratory stimulation.
2. Temperature: temperature decreases have been shown to affect the sensitivity of Pacinian corpus-
cles but not other skin receptors. For example, the thresholds for 100, 256, and 900 Hz vibrations
were shown to vary with skin temperature, with a maximum sensitivity at 378C (Weitz, 1941).
However, for vibrations in the 30 Hz range, the effect of skin temperature was negligible
(Green, 1977). When measured on hairless skin, cooling impairs sensitivity to high-frequency
vibration. Warming the skin improves, to a limited extent, the sensitivity of all receptor types
and a wide range of frequencies (Green, 1977).
3. Area of contact: the larger the contact area of a tactile stimulus is, the lower is the threshold for
perceiving the stimulus due to spatial summation. However, this rule does not seem to apply at
frequencies below 40 Hz.
4. Duration of the stimulus: temporal summation of tactile stimuli has been observed for upto 300
to 500 m sec. If the stimulus is applied for an even longer time, then adaptation may occur.
5. Age of the observer: sensitivity for vibrotactile signals deteriorates throughout aging (Verrillo,
1980). However, some studies suggest that this effect does not occur with respect to the fingertips
(Stuart et al., 1999).
Variations in hormone levels can also affect sensitivity for tactile cues. In general, females appear to be
slightly more sensitive to tactile stimuli although the overall pattern of sensitivity across body locations is
similar to that of males (Weinstein, 1968).
In terms of the temporal resolution for tactile stimuli, it has been shown that people can distinguish
between two tactile stimuli that are presented at least 5 m sec apart. Note that this represents a much
higher temporal resolution than for vision where stimuli have to be presented 25 m sec apart to be per-
ceived as two separate signals. At the other extreme, two sounds can be distinguished even if they are
separated by as little as 0.01 m sec. The sequence in which signals were presented to the skin can be deter-
mined reliably for stimuli that are presented 20 or more meter seconds apart.
If a tactile stimulus is presented for a long time, adaptation, that is, an increase in threshold or a
reduction in the perceived intensity of the stimulus, can occur. The time course of adaptation to
tactile stimuli is not well known. In general, it appears that the adaptation period increases with increas-
ing stimulus intensity, decreases with increasing stimulus area, and varies inversely with the sensitivity of
different body regions (Greenspan and Bolanowski, 1996). Recovery from adaptation occurs quickly once
the particular stimulus is removed. Note that adaptation to one type of tactile stimulus does not lead to
increased thresholds for other types of tactile stimuli.
This independence of tactile channels is also supported by the observation that masking only occurs
for stimuli that are presented in the same channel. Masking refers to an increased threshold or reduced
perceived intensity of a stimulus as a result of a second stimulus being presented in close spatial or tem-
poral proximity to another stimulus. For example, a 250-Hz vibratory stimulus will not mask a 20-Hz
stimulus, except under conditions of very high stimulus intensity (Gescheider et al., 1983). The potential
for masking creates a problem for designers of tactile interfaces who would like to achieve as high infor-
mation transfer rates as possible through fast patterns and small spatial separation — the same thing that
is likely to result in masking. This is an important consideration in the design of multicontactor dynamic
devices that may be used to convey texture or motion.
Overall, the spatial resolution for tactile stimuli is determined by the size of the receptive field of the
respective receptor. For example, the receptive fields of mechanoreceptors vary from 1 to 2 mm2 upto
45 cm2. The larger the receptive field of a receptor is, the lower is its spatial resolution. For example, on
average, the skin on the thigh cannot discriminate two points that are closer than 67 mm whereas the
palm can distinguish between two points that are as little as 11 mm apart. As indicated earlier, the fingertip
Visual, Tactile, and Multimodal Information Processing 23-17
region has the highest resolution with a two-point discrimination of 2.5 mm (Shimoga, 1993). The
fingertips thus play a similar role in touch as the fovea of the retina does in visual perception (Krueger,
1982).
One important distinction that has been made with respect to tactile perception is between active and
passive touch (Gibson, 1962). Passive touch refers to situations where a tactile stimulus is applied to the
passive body. In contrast, active touch involves an individual moving their hands to manipulate and/or
explore an object. Gibson emphasized that, similar to vision, some important phenomena are experi-
enced only when a person actively explores an object by touch, thus adding kinesthetic information.
According to Gibson, active touch enables a person to integrate over time and space the invariances
in the stimulation that characterize an object; in that sense, “active touch is an exploratory rather
than a merely receptive sense” (Gibson, 1962). This is of importance, for example, when trying to identify
a relatively large object. It appears, however, that for other purposes, such as the perception of texture,
active touch is not necessarily more useful than passive touch (see Kruger, 1996).
Clearly, there are connections between vision and touch, the two senses that we have discussed so far.
Tactile sensing tends to be combined and aided by vision and vice versa. Vision, touch, and propriocep-
tion operate together to explore the environment. In some cases, we use touch to substitute for vision if it
has become unavailable or lost completely. There is considerable empirical evidence for interactions
between these two (and other) senses at the neuronal level. For example, spatially coincident stimuli
in the two modalities tend to produce response enhancement. In contrast, disparate stimuli often lead
to response depression (Stein and Meredith, 1993). The following sections will discuss in some detail
multimodal information processing and presentation, both in terms of their benefits, limitations, and
possible implications for interface design.
1993) and modern flight decks (e.g., Brickman et al., 2000; Latorella, 1999; Sklar and Sarter, 1999). Given
the increasing information demands on operators in these environments, the trend towards multimodal
output systems is likely to continue.
As mentioned earlier, the design of multimodal interfaces tends to be based (albeit implicitly) on the
assumption of an early version of MRT that modalities represent separate attentional resources and that
the distribution of information therefore enhances our ability to share time. More recently, Wickens and
colleagues have added some qualifications to the modality-specific aspects of MRT. First, they empha-
sized that MRT was intended to apply to the performance of continuous tasks only, and that information
distribution across sensory channels may not matter in the context of discrete tasks where a person can
switch rather than share time (Wickens, 1991). Also, Wickens and Liu (1988) pointed out that benefits
that have been observed for crossmodal time sharing may not result from using different pools of atten-
tional resources but may instead be related to peripheral factors such as visual scanning costs. More
recently, additional behavioral and neurophysiological evidence has emerged that suggests the existence
of crossmodal constraints on performance.
2
ERP ¼ Evoked Response Potential ¼ electroencephalographic signals reflecting the operations of neuronal
systems when transmitting and processing responses to sensory stimuli.
23-20 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
23.4.3.2 MSE
MSE (e.g., Ferstl et al., 1994; Zubin, 1975) is another example of a crossmodal constraint on attention. It
describes the strong tendency of people to “respond more slowly to a target in one modality if the pre-
ceding target was presented in a different modality than if the preceding target was presented in the same
modality” (Spence and Driver, 1997). This implies that, in general, responses will be slower to targets in
less frequent modalities. It appears to be particularly difficult (time-consuming) to shift attention to the
visual or auditory channel away from rare events that are presented in the tactile modality (Spence et al.,
2000).
MSE likely plays a role in real-world domains where signals tend to be assigned to modalities based on
appropriateness (rather than experimental control) considerations. For example, auditory signals are
often reserved for warning and alerts; given the rare occurrence of these events, the frequency of auditory
cues is likely lower than that of visual indications, which are considered appropriate for presenting a wide
range of diverse and detailed information.
3
The production of voice in such a way that the sound seems to come from a source other than the vocal organs of
the speaker (often a puppet).
Visual, Tactile, and Multimodal Information Processing 23-21
Soto-Faraco et al. (2002) and Arnell and Jolicoeur (1999) have found that, if a stimulus is presented in
one modality, then the ability to process stimuli in a different modality is limited if the latter appear
within 50 m sec of the first signal. This phenomenon has been referred to as the “crossmodal attentional
blink.” At the neurophysiological level, it has been confirmed that stimuli that occur in close temporal
proximity affect one another, while those separated by long-time intervals are processed separately by
neurons in the superior colliculus (Stein and Meredith, 1993).
References
Arnell, K.M. and Jolicoeur, P., The attentional blink across stimulus modalities: evidence for a central
processing limitation, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 25, pp. 630–648, 1999.
Benoit, C. and Le Goff, B., Audio-visual speech synthesis from French text: eight years of models, designs
and evaluation at the ICP, Speech Commun., 26, pp. 117 –129, 1998.
Boulter, L.R., Attention and reaction times to signals of uncertain modality, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept.
Perform., 3, pp. 379–388, 1977.
Broadbent, D.E., Perception and Communication, Pergamon, Oxford, England, 1958.
Brickman, B.J., Hettinger, L.J. and Haas, M.W., Multisensory interface design for complex task domains:
replacing information overload with meaning in tactical crew stations, Int. J. Aviat. Psychol., 10(3),
pp. 273–290, 2000.
Bundesen, C., A theory of visual attention, Psychol. Rev., 97(4), pp. 523 –547, 1990.
Burdea, G.C., Force and Touch Feedback for Virtual Reality, John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1996.
Calvert, G.A., Brammer, M.J., Bullmore, E.T., Campbell, R., Iversen, S.D. and David, A.S., Response
amplification in sensory-specific cortices during crossmodal binding, Neuroreport, 10,
pp. 2619–2623, 1999.
Cholewiak, R. and Collins, A., Sensory and physiological basis of touch, in The Psychology of Touch
Heller, M. and Schiff, W., eds., LEA, Mahwah, NJ, 1991, pp. 23 –60.
23-22 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Cohen, P.R., Johnston, M., McGee, S., Oviatt, S. and Pittman, J., Quickset: Multimodal Interaction for
Simulation Set-up and Control. Proceedings of the 5th Applied Natural Language Processing
Meeting, Washington, DC, 1997.
Cornsweet, T.N., Visual Perception, Academic Press, New York, 1970.
Driver, J. and Frith, C., Shifting baselines in attention research, Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 1, pp. 147 –148, 2000.
Driver, J. and Grossenbacher, P.G., Multimodal spatial constraints on tactile selective attention, in
Attention and Performance, Vol. 16, Information Integration in Perception and Communication,
Innui, T. and McClelland, J.L. eds., MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1996, pp. 209–235.
Eimer, M., Crossmodal links in spatial attention between vision, audition, and touch: evidence from
event-related brain potentials, Neuropsychologia, 39, pp. 1292–1303, 2001.
Eimer, M., Can attention be directed to opposite locations in different modalities? An ERP study, Clin.
Neurophysiol., 110, pp. 1252–1259, 1999.
Ferstl, R., Hanewinkel, R. and Krag, P., Is the modality-shift effect specific for schizophrenic patients?
Schizophr. Bull., 2, pp. 367 –373, 1994.
Folk, C.L., Remington, R.W. and Johnston, J.C., Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional
control settings, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 18, pp. 1030–1044, 1992.
Gescheider, G.A., O’Malley, M.J. and Verrillo, R.T., Vibrotactile forward masking: Evidence for channel
independence, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 74(2), pp. 474 –485, 1983.
Gibson, J.J., Observations on active touch, Psychol. Rev., 69, pp. 477 –490, 1962.
Gordon, I.E., Theories of Visual Perception, John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1989.
Gray, R., Tan, H.Z. and Young, J.J., Do Multimodal Signals Need to Come from the Same Place? Crossmodal
Attentional Links between Proximal and Distal Surfaces, Proceedings of the Fourth IEEE
International Conference on Multimodal Interfaces (ICMI ’02). IEEE, 2002.
Green, B.G., The effect of skin temperature on vibrotactile sensitivity, Percept. Psychophys., 21(3),
pp. 243–248, 1977.
Greenspan, J.D. and Bolanowski, S.J., The psychophysics of tactile perception and its peripheral physio-
logical basis, in Pain and Touch, Kruger, L., ed., Academic Press, San Diego, 1996, pp. 25– 104.
Hamasaki, D., Ong, J. and Marg. E., The amplitude of accommodation in presbyopia, Am. J. Optom.
Physiol. Opt., 33, pp. 3 –14, 1956.
Hohnsbein, J., Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J. and Blanke, L., Effects of crossmodal divided attention on
late ERP components: I. Simple and choice reaction tasks, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.,
78, pp. 438–446, 1991.
Ho, C-Y., Nikolic, M.I. and Sarter, N.B., Supporting Timesharing and Interruption Management Through
Multimodal Information Presentation, Proceedings of the 45th Annual Meeting of the Human
Factors and Ergonomics Society, Minneapolis, MN, October, 2001.
James, W., Principles of Psychology (Vol. 1), Holt, New York, 1890.
Johansson, R.S. and Vallbo, A.B., Tactile sensory coding in the glabrous skin of the human hand, Trends
Neurosci., 6(1), pp. 27 –32, 1983.
Kawashima, R., O’Sullivan, B.T. and Roland, P.E., Positron Emission Tomography Studies of Cross-Modality
Inhibition in Selective Attentional Tasks: Closing of the “mind’s eye”, Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences USA, 92, pp. 5969 –5972, 1995.
Kennett, S., Eimer, M., Spence, C. and Driver, J., Tactile-visual links in exogenous spatial attention under
different postures: convergent evidence from psychophysics and ERPs, J. Cogn. Neurosci., 13(4),
pp. 462–478, 2001.
Kontaniris, D.A. and Howe, R.D., Tactile display of vibratory information in teleoperation and virtual
environments, Presence, 4(4), pp. 387 –402, 1995.
Kruger, L. eds., Handbook of Perception and Cognition: Pain and Touch, 2nd edn. Academic Press,
New York, 1996.
Krueger, L.E., Historical perspective, in Schiff, W. and Foulke, E. eds., Tactual Perception: A Sourcebook,
pp. 1– 54, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1982.
Visual, Tactile, and Multimodal Information Processing 23-23
LaBerge, D., Attentional Processing: The Brain’s Art of Mindfulness, Harvard University Press, Cambridge,
MA, 1995.
Latorella, K.A., Investigating Interruptions: Implications for Flightdeck Performance, NASA/Technical
Memorandum (TM)-1999-209707, Virginia, Hampton, 1999.
Lederman, S.J. and Browse, R.A., The physiology and psychophysics of touch, in Dario, P. eds., Sensors and
Sensory Systems for Advanced Robots, NATO ASI Series F43, Springer Verlag, Berlin, 1988.
Leibowitz, H., The human senses in flight, in Wiener, E. and Nagel, D. eds., Human Factors in Aviation,
Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 1988.
Loomis, J.M. and Lederman, S.J., Tactual perception, in Boff, K.R., Kaufman, L., Thomas, J.P. eds.,
Handbook of Perception and Human Performance: Cognitive Processes and Performance, 2(31),
pp. 1–41, Wiley & Sons, New York, 1986.
Macaluso, E., Frith, C.D. and Driver, J., Directing attention to locations and to sensory modalities:
Multiple levels of selective processing revealed with PET. Cereb. Cortex, 12, pp. 357–368, 2002.
MacIntyre, B., Mynatt, E., Voida, S., Hansen, K., Tullio, J., Corso, G., Support For Multitasking and
Background Awareness Using Interactive Peripheral Displays, Proceedings of ACM User Interface
Software and Technology (UIST’01). Orlando, FL, November, 2001.
McConkie, G.W., Eye movements and perception during reading, in Raynor K. ed., Eye Movements in
Reading, Academic Press, New York, 1983.
McDonald, J.J., Teder-Saelejaervi, W.A. and Hillyard, S.A., Involuntary orienting to sound improves
visual perception. Nature, 407, pp. 906– 908, 2000.
Means, L.G., Fleischman, R.N., Carpenter, J.T., Szczublewski, F.E., Dingus, T.A. and Krage, M.K., Design
of TravTek Auditory Interface, In NRC-TRB: Driver performance: measurement and modeling,
National Academy Press, Washington, D.C., 1993.
Murch, G.M., Physiological principles for the effective use of color, IEEE Comput. Graph. Appl.,
November, pp. 49 –54, 1984.
Neisser, U. Cognition and Reality, W.H. Freeman, San Francisco, 1976.
Nikolic, M.I. and Sarter, N.B., Peripheral visual feedback: A powerful means of supporting attention
allocation and human-automation coordination in highly dynamic data-rich environments,
Hum. Factors, 43(1), pp. 30 –38, 2001.
Oviatt, S., Multimodal interfaces, in Jacko, J. and Sears, A. eds., Handbook of Human-Computer Inter-
action, LEA: Hillsdale, NJ, 2002.
Oviatt, S.L. and Cohen, P.R., Multimodal systems that process what comes naturally, Commun. ACM,
43(3), pp. 45–53, ACM Press, New York, March, 2000.
Pentland, A., Smart room, Sci. Am., pp. 54 –62, April, 1996.
Posner, M.I., Chronometric Explorations of Mind, LEA, Hillsdale, NJ, 1978.
Posner, M.I. and Petersen, S.E., The attention system of the human brain, Ann. Rev. Neurosci., 13,
pp. 25–42, 1990.
Post, L.J. and Chapman, C.E., The effects of cross-modal manipulations of attention on the detection of
vibrotactile stimuli in humans, Somatosen. Mot. Res., 8, pp. 149 –157, 1991.
Pratt, J. and Hommel, B., Symbolic control of visual attention: The role of working memory and
attentional control settings, J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform., 29(5), pp. 835 – 845,
2003.
Purves, D. and Lotto, R.B., Why We See What We Do: An Empirical Theory of Vision, Sinauer Associates
Inc., Sunderland, MA, 2003.
Quinlan, P.T. and Bailey, P.J., An examination of attentional control in the auditory modality – Further
evidence for auditory orienting. Percept. Psychophys., 57(5), pp. 614– 628, 1995.
Sarter, N.B., Multimodal information presentation in support of human-automation communication
and coordination, in Eduardo, S. eds., Advances in Human Performance and Cognitive Engineering
Research, pp. 13–36, JAI Press, New York, 2002.
23-24 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Shimoga, K., A Survey of Perceptual Feedback Issues in Dextrous Telemanipulation: Part II. Finger Touch
Feedback, Proceeding of IEEE Virtual Reality Annual International Symposium, pp. 271 –279,
IEEE, New York, 1993.
Shneiderman, B., Designing the User Interface: Strategies for Effective Human-Computer Interaction,
Addison-Wesley, Reading, MA, 1998.
Sklar, A.E. and Sarter, N.B., “Good vibrations”: The use of tactile feedback in support of mode awareness
on advanced technology aircraft. Hum. Factors., 41(4), pp. 543 –552, 1999.
Soderquist, D.R., Sensory processes, London, U.K.: Sage Publications, (2002).
Sorkin, R.D. Design of auditory and tactile displays, in Salvendy, G. eds., Handbook of Human Factors
John Wiley & Sons, New York, pp. 549–576, 1987.
Soto-Faraco, S., Spence, C., Fairbank, K., Kingstone, A., Hillstrom, A.P. and Shapiro, K., A crossmodal
attentional blink between vision and touch, Psychon. Bull. Rev., 9(4), pp. 731–738, 2002.
Spence, C., Multisensory attention and tactile information processing. Behav. Brain Res., 135,
pp. 57 – 64, 2002.
Spence, C. and Driver, J., Crossmodal Links in Attention Between Audition, Vision, and Touch: Impli-
cations for Interface Design, Int. J. Cogn. Ergon., 1(4), pp. 351– 373, 1997.
Spence, C., Pavani, F. and Driver, J., Crossmodal links between vision and touch in covert endogenous
spatial attention, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 26, pp. 1298 –1319, 2000.
Spence, C., Nicholls, M.E.R., Gillespie, N. and Driver, J., Cross-modal links in exogenous covert spatial
orienting between touch, audition, and vision, Percept. Psychophy., 60(4), pp. 544–557, 1998.
Stein, B.E. and Meredith, M.A., The Merging of the Senses, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1993.
Stork, D.G. and Hennecke, M.E. eds., Speechreading by Humans and Machines, Springer Verlag,
New York, 1995.
Stokes, A.F., Wickens, C.D. and Kite, K., Display Technology: Human Factors Concepts, Society of
Automotive Engineers, Inc., Warrendale, PA, 1990.
Stuart, M., Shaw, J.A., Walsh, N., Nguyen, V. and Turman, A.B. 1999. Vibration detection thersholds at
several body sites: influence of gender, preferred side and assessment method. Proc. Aust. Neuro-
science Soc, 10, 173ff.
Treisman, A. Properties, parts, and objects, in Boff, K.R., Kaufman, L. and Thomas, J.P. eds., Handbook of
Perception and Human Performance, John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1986.
Turk, M. and Robertson, G. eds., Perceptual user Interfaces, Commun. ACM [Special Issue], 43(3),
pp. 32–70, 2000.
Verrillo, R.T., Age related changes in the sensitivity to vibration, J. Gerontol., 35, pp. 185–193, 1980.
Vo, M.T. and Wood, C., Building an Application Framework for Speech and Pen Input Integration In
Multimodal Learning Interfaces, Proceedings of the International Conference on Acoustics,
Speech, and Signal Processing., pp. 3545– 3548, 1996.
Wade, N. and Swanson, M., Visual Perception: An Introduction 2nd edition, Psychology Press, 2001.
Wang, J., Integration of Eye-gaze, Voice and Manual Response in Multimodal user Interface, Proceedings of
the IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics., pp. 3938–3942, 1995.
Weinstein, S., Intensive and extensive aspects of tactile sensitivity as a function of body part, sex, and
laterality, in Kenshalo D.R. eds., The Skin Senses, Thomas Springfield, IL, pp. 195 –222, 1968.
Weitz, J., Vibratory sensitivity as a function of skin temperature. J. Exp. Psychol., 28, pp. 21 –36, 1941.
Wickens, C.D., Processing resources in attention, in Parasuraman, R. and Davies, D.R. eds., Varieties of
Attention, Academic Press, pp. 63–102, 1984.
Wickens, C.D., Processing resources and attention, in Damos D.L. eds., Multiple Task Performance,
Taylor&Francis, London, pp. 3 –34, 1991.
Wickens, C.D., Gordon, S.E. and Liu, Y., An Introduction to Human Factors Engineering, Addison Wesley
Longman, New York, 1998.
Wickens, C.D. and Liu, Y., Codes and modalities in multiple resources: A success and qualification, Hum.
Factors, 30(5), pp. 599 –616, 1988.
Visual, Tactile, and Multimodal Information Processing 23-25
Wu, S-C. and Remington, R., Characteristics of covert and overt visual orienting: Evidence from atten-
tional and oculomotor capture, J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform., 29(5), pp. 1050–1067, 2003.
Zhai, S., Morimoto, C. and Ihde, S., Manual and Gaze input Cascaded (MAGIC) Pointing, Proceedings
of the Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI’99), pp. 246–253, ACM Press,
New York, 1999.
Zlotnik, M. A., Applying Electro-Tactile Display Technology to Fighter Aircraft — Flying with Feeling Again,
In Proceedings of the IEEE 1988 National Aerospace and Electronics Conference NAECON 1988,
pp. 191–197, IEEE Aerospace and Electronics Systems Society, New York, 1988.
Zubin, J., Problems of attention in schizophrenia, in Kietzman, M.L., Sutton, S. and Zubin, J. eds., Exper-
imental Approaches to Psychopathology, pp. 139 –166, Academic Press, New York, 1975.
24
Applying Cognitive
Psychology to System
Development
24.1 Introduction
Product designers need to wear many hats during the development process. These different perspectives
range from that of the product designer who is trying to invent and integrate new concepts within a
product, to the hardware/software engineer who is trying to create the physical realization of these
design concepts, to the manufacturing engineer who is trying to manufacture the product, to the market-
ing specialist who is trying to sell the product (Bralla, 1998).
The successful designer needs to wear each of these hats in order to make sure that critical questions
are asked during the design process, or make sure that a broader design team has been assembled that
coordinates to deal with the implications and interactions of each of these perspectives regarding
design decisions. As an example, it is not enough to design from a user’s perspective. It is equally import-
ant to consider the viewpoint of the manufacturing engineer who must cope with the constraints
imposed by the design when developing a manufacturing process that will be effective in terms of its
impact on productivity, quality, and ergonomic/health costs.
The broad theme of this chapter is that a multidisciplinary perspective (Carroll, 2003) is needed to
identify and answer all of the questions relevant to developing a cost-effective product that is useful,
usable, and actually used. A failure to consider any one of these perspectives can increase costs, decrease
usefulness or usability, or create barriers to the actual marketing and use of the product.
This broad theme will be illustrated by considering one of these perspectives in detail, that of the
psychologist who must predict how alternative design concepts and features will influence the perform-
ances of users. This is not to say that every design team needs to include a psychologist. Rather, the point
is that every design team needs to consider the psychology of the user (Norman, 2002), looking at design
as a prediction task.
24-1
24-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
. What needs am I trying to serve? From a problem-driven design perspective, what are the short-
comings of the existing products and methods that I am trying to improve upon? What benchmark
tasks should my product support? See Witkin and Altshuld (1995) for a discussion of methods for
conducting such needs assessments
. Who are the potential users I am trying to serve? What are the important defining dimensions that
identify the different populations, such as:
– expected frequency of use
– knowledge of the task domain
– familiarity with other products used for the same or similar tasks
– relevant physical, perceptual, psychomotor, or cognitive abilities
– the importance/value of the needs to be served by the product
– the resources the user may be willing to expend to meet these needs (time, money, etc.)
– personal preferences and beliefs that influence the likelihood of purchasing and using the
product
– individual differences that exist along these dimensions?
. For those needs or tasks that are met by some existing product, how is the task currently
performed? See Rubenstein and Hersh (1984) and Preece (1994) for a discussion of how to
complete a descriptive task analysis or cognitive task analysis.
. What are the broader physical, organizational, social, and legal contexts in which the benchmark
tasks will be performed (Flach, 1998)?
. What different combinations of users, needs/tasks, and contexts actually exist? How can this
representative set of use cases or scenarios be used to avoid cognitive narrowing (Smith and
Geddes, 2003) when making design decisions, making sure that the design accommodates all of
the use cases satisfactorily, rather than focusing on satisfying only a subset? See Carroll (1995)
and Rosson and Carroll (2001) for a discussion of scenario-based design.
. What key constraints should be considered during the design process, including acceptable levels for
– learning rates
– error rates
– productivity
– development time
– manufacturing costs
– marketing constraints
– development and operational costs?
. In terms of design for manufacturability (Helander and Nagamachi, 1992; Bralla, 1998) what
constraints does the design place on the production process?
. As I consider design alternatives, what are the key discriminators that could differentiate me from
my competitors, including possible differences in:
– usefulness
– usability
– aesthetic appeal
– cost
– quality
– durability
– service/maintenance
– marketing strategy (including market entry time)
– product evolution plan?
. What are the alternative business plans for sustaining this product in the market? What impli-
cations does this have for the design?
Applying Cognitive Psychology to System Development 24-3
Although answering these conceptual questions about the nature of the design problem is always an
iterative process, using a top-down approach in which they are addressed early in the development
process can help to ensure that the real needs of the potential users are being addressed in an effective
fashion. Once the design problem is well understood, then alternative conceptual solutions can be
generated and evaluated, ultimately leading to a specific design.
Clearly, in order to answer these questions during the initial user studies for a design, an understanding
of cognitive, organizational, and social psychology is very useful, both in terms of the research methods
developed by these fields and in terms of the models of human performance (Wickens et al., 2004) and
group dynamics (Brehm et al., 1999) that they provide.
To illustrate applications of psychology to design in more detail, below we discuss a second component
of the design process where psychology is involved, the evaluation of a specific product design proposal.
Step 2. Specify the normative (correct) paths for this use case (represented as a goal hierarchy), thus
indicating the alternative sequences of steps that the user could take to successfully achieve the specified
goal. Note that there could be more than one correct path for completing a given task.
Step 3. Identify the state of the product and the associated “world” at each node in the goal hierarchy.
In the case of a software product, the state of the product would be the current appearance of the interface
and any associated internal states (such as the queue of recently completed actions that would be used
should an undo function be applied). The state of the “world” applies if the product or the user actually
changes something in the world as part of an action, such as changing the temperature of a glass man-
ufacturing system when using the interface to a process control system.
Step 4. Generate predictions. For each correct action (node in hierarchy):
. Predict all the relevant success stories.
. Predict all the relevant failure stories.
. Record the reasons and assumptions made in generating these stories.
. Identify potential fixes to avoid or assist in recovery from failure stories. (Keep in mind that these
fixes could be local patches or bandaids or they could involve proposing a major change in the
design concept.)
Note that, in developing a specific set of predictions, it may be more efficient to consider the use cases
for the full range of users in parallel while stepping through the normative goal hierarchy. For instance,
predictions might be generated for both a first-time user and a frequent user of the product while looking
at each node in the goal hierarchy.
To make this discussion clearer, a sample cognitive walkthrough is provided next. For this illustration,
we will consider the design of a specific online library search system.
Example of Step 1. Select a use case for evaluation. In completing this sample walkthrough, assume as
the use case that we are dealing with a college student in the United States who is a first-time user who has
used other online library search systems before (but is not an expert at library searches), and who also
regularly searches for material on the Web. Abstractly, this means that the user:
. Is reasonably literate in the English language
. Is familiar with the use of a keyword entry box and the use of hot links on web pages
. Knows that menus are often shown along the top of the page as short phrases and that a given
menu item can be selected by clicking on it
. Knows that library searches are often structured in terms of author, title, and subject searches
. Is not familiar with the layout and navigation of this specific library system
Assume the user is looking for wedding songs to be performed at her own wedding, that she is con-
ducting the search at home on her own computer over a high-speed connection, and that help from a
librarian is available only by phone. Assume further that she wants CDs to listen to (she only has a
CD player), rather than sheet music, tapes, etc.
Example of Step 2. Specify the normative (correct) paths for this use case. Such a normative model is gen-
erally best represented as a goal/subgoal hierarchy (Preece, 1994). The high-level goal or task is represented
as the top node in the hierarchy, and each level below a node represents the subgoals that will achieve the
goal represented by that higher level node. Relationships among subgoals can be indicated by OR (com-
pletion of either subgoal alone is sufficient to achieve the higher level goal), AND (both subgoals must
be completed to achieve the higher level goal, but in any order) or sequence (both subgoals must be
completed to achieve the higher level goal, and they must be completed in a specified order) operators.
In this example, the initial screen that this student will access is shown in Figure 24.1 and Figure 24.2.
For this user the top section of the normative model or goal hierarchy is shown in Figure 24.3. Note that:
. The highest node is the student’s goal or task in this use case (finding wedding songs on CDs)
. The nodes below this highest node represent four different ways the student could successfully
begin (and are therefore marked with OR)
Applying Cognitive Psychology to System Development 24-5
FIGURE 24.1 Initial screen seen when accessing this library search system.
Or Or Or
“Enter term,” “Select type,” or “Choose format” was accessed (using either the mouse or the tab key). We
decided that such lower level details would not add any important insights into our predictions for this
type of Web literate user. (If we were concerned with the time required to complete the task, we might
want to include such details.)
Note also that we did not include all possible paths (such as “click on AUTHOR”), but only included
those paths that could lead to a successful search. Since, when dealing with information retrieval,
“success” is a relative concept that must be defined in terms of a tradeoff between recall and precision
(Baeza-Yates and Ribeiro-Neto, 1999), we chose to define “success” as any path that leads to at least
some useful retrievals. (For this example, a title search using the search term “Wedding songs” does
not retrieve any CDs. Because such a search therefore represents a failure story, it is not represented
in the normative/correct model.)
We could also go on to add additional nodes to this goal hierarchy to indicate iterations in the search
process (e.g., capturing how the user might also try a Keyword search after trying a Subject search).
However, for the purposes of this illustration, we will assume that, for each of the alternative paths,
the user stops after following that one search path.
Note that, in terms of this general method of conducting a cognitive walkthrough, three points have
been illustrated earlier:
. Branches to subgoals under a given node can be marked with AND, OR, or a sequence arrow to
indicate the relationships among these subgoals
. To make the analysis process more efficient, only the normative paths (those paths that lead to a
successful completion of the task) are shown in the goal hierarchy
And And
Or Or
FIGURE 24.4 Subgoals showing alternative ways to successfully complete the desired search.
Applying Cognitive Psychology to System Development 24-7
. Also for efficiency, decisions can be made about how deep to go in the hierarchy. In some cases,
lower levels can be left out if they are judged to be unimportant in terms of predicting critical
behaviors
Two final ways to increase efficiency in completing such a cognitive walkthough are to:
. Only represent those portions of the goal hierarchy that are judged to be critical by the
analyst
. Limit the number of use cases to be considered based on some judgment of importance, for
instance, considering only those use cases that are likely to occur frequently, that could result in
some highly undesirable outcome, or that are relevant to some design decision about which the
developers are uncertain (Mitta et al., 1995)
Example of Step 3. Identify the state of the product and the associated “world” at each node. Previously,
we have illustrated the completion of Step 1 (Select a use case for evaluation) and Step 2 (Specify the
normative paths for this use case). To complete the third step, we need to identify the screen displays
associated with each node in the hierarchy represented in Figure 24.3 and Figure 24.4. (Note that the
rest of the context — the associated “world” — is assumed to stay constant in this illustration, as the
student is accessing the library system at home on her own computer over a high-speed connection,
and that help from a librarian is available only by phone.)
Analysis of a Sample Path. Suppose the student enters “wedding songs” and changes the search type
from the default (Title) to Subject, but uses the default format (All). Figure 24.5 shows the appearance
of the Basic Catalog Search screen for this path, and Figure 24.6 shows the appearance of the search
results displayed for this path.
To complete Step 3, we would normally identify the state of the software interface associated with all
possible paths leading to success in the normative goal hierarchy. For the purposes of this example,
however, we will limit our focus to this one sample path in which the information seeker has started
by entering the term “wedding songs” in a subject search for all possible formats.
Example of Step 4. Generate predictions. The final step is to walk through the hierarchy along each path
that would lead to success, and to play psychologist, generating predicted behaviors at each node (for
FIGURE 24.5 Basic catalog search screen for the sample path: searching for “Wedding songs” with the search type
Subject and the default format (all).
24-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
FIGURE 24.6 (a) Initial search results displayed for sample path: searching for “Wedding songs” with subject as the
search type and all as the default format. (b) Next level of detail when the user clicks on the “Wedding music” link
shown in (a).
each associated screen display). Lewis and Wharton (1997) suggest asking four questions to help guide
this prediction process:
More detailed questions can be identified by considering how different cognitive processes could influ-
ence performance (Card et al., 1983; Eysenck and Keane, 1990; Wickens and Hollands, 1999; Ashcraft,
2002; Wickens et al., 2004), such as:
. Selective attention: What are the determinants of attention? What is most salient in the display? Where
will the user’s focus of attention be drawn? (See Johnston and Dark, 1986; Pashler et al., 2001.)
. Perception: How will perceptual processes influence the user’s interpretation? How, for instance,
will the proximity of various items on the screen influence judgments of “relatedness” as predicted
Applying Cognitive Psychology to System Development 24-9
by the Gestalt Law of Proximity? (See Tufte, 1983, 1990, 1997; Gibson, 1986; Bennett and Flach,
1992; Goldstein, 1996; Card et al., 1999; Vincente, 2002; Watzman, 2003.)
. Memory: How will the user’s prior knowledge influence selective attention and interpretation? Does
the knowledge necessary to perform tasks reside in the world or in the user’s mind? (See Bransford
and Johnson, 1972; Hutchins, 1995; Baddeley, 1998.)
. Information Processing, mental models, and situation awareness: What inferences/assumptions will
the user make? (See Johnson et al., 1981; Gentner and Stevens, 1983; Plous, 1993; Endsley, 2003.)
. Design-induced error: How could the product design and the context of use influence performance
and induce errors? (See Bainbridge, 1983; Roth et al., 1987; Reason, 1991, 1997; Parasuraman and
Riley, 1997; Smith et al., 1997; Skirka et al., 1999; Sheridan, 2002; Smith and Geddes, 2003.)
. Motor performance: Can the controls be used efficiently and without error? (See Jagacinski and
Flach, 2003.)
. Group dynamics: How will the system influence patterns of interaction among people? (See Horton
and Lewis, 1991; Baecker et al., 1995; Olson and Olson, 2003.)
These predictions could be generated by members of the design team, independent experts in inter-
action design, and experts in the relevant domain (such as expert librarians). Because of their different
backgrounds relevant to the design and the domain of interest, individuals from each group are likely to
generate somewhat different predictions.
For the sample path discussed in the example for Step 3, some sample predictions include the
following.
Success story. The user enters the term “Wedding songs.” The user also recognizes that it is important to
change the search type from the default (Title) to either Keyword or Subject, and selects Subject. The user
either does not notice or does not choose to change the format, but by leaving it as the default (All), she
completes a search with high recall but low precision in terms of the desired format (music CD). Because
“wedding songs” happens to be a synonym that triggers search results for the controlled vocabulary term
“Wedding music,” the user completes a search that, in terms of her intended semantic concept, “Wedding
songs,” leads to 337 retrievals (Figure 24.6a) with high precision and high recall for that semantic cat-
egory (plus one other relevant retrieval that is a CD under the controlled vocabulary term “Wedding
songs, English Ireland”).
Assuming she understands the need to click on the “SEE Wedding music” link (Figure 24.6a), the
searcher will then access a page that provides access to all 337 items (Figure 24.6b). If she further recog-
nizes that she can still limit the retrievals by format at this point by clicking on the CD icon in the column
labeled “Click to limit by format,” she will further improve the precision of her search by limiting the
retrievals to music CDs. After viewing those retrievals, she could then back up and look at the relevant
retrieval indexed under “Wedding songs, English Ireland.”
Failure story 1. A very plausible failure story would be for the searcher to simply enter “Wedding
songs” as the search term and hit return or click on the Search button (Figure 24.7a). Either
action would result in a search with only 13 document records, none of which are music CDs
(Figure 24.7b).
This failure is very plausible for two reasons. Based on their past experience with other websites, many
experienced Web users are in the habit of just entering a keyword and hitting the Enter key or clicking on
a “search” or “go” button in order to run a search. This prior knowledge, combined with the salience of
the Search Term entry box, makes it quite likely that a number of users will make this mistake.
Note further that, because the default for this search system is a Title search, and because that search
does generate retrievals, it is also quite possible that the user will not recognize that she could have run
a different type of search that would have generated more retrievals. The page of retrievals for this
search, shown in Figure 24.7b, does not provide any feedback on the type of search run. It merely
states that “9 titles match your search for wedding songs.” This is probably the worst kind of flaw
in the design of an information retrieval system, as the user does not even realize that she should
try a different search.
24-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
FIGURE 24.7 (a) Search definition display for failure story 1: searching for “Wedding songs” using the defaults for
search type (title) and format (all). (b) Search results display for failure story 1: searching for “Wedding songs” using
the defaults for search type (title) and format (all).
To avoid this problem, it would be better to pick a default for search type that is more encompassing
(erring on the side of high recall at the expense of low precision). One way to accomplish this would be to
use a Keyword search as the default. The designer would have to further decide whether to require the use
of logical operators (AND; OR; NOT), and whether to allow phrases to be entered without separating
words with logical operators.
It would probably be better to not require the use of logical operators, given this is not typical in most
websites with keyword searches, and given people are known to be poor at correctly using such operators.
Instead, the search engine could:
. Run the search using the broader query Wedding AND Songs.
. Sort the retrievals using some type of relevance ranking (e.g., ranking highest those retrievals
indexed with the controlled vocabulary term “Wedding Music” — which has “Wedding songs”
as a synonym, followed by those retrievals containing “Wedding songs” as a phrase somewhere
in the document representation (but not indexed as “Wedding music”), followed by those
Applying Cognitive Psychology to System Development 24-11
containing the words “Wedding” AND “Songs” but not containing the phrase “Wedding Songs”
and not indexed as “Wedding Music”)
. The search engine should also automatically deal with alternative spellings, cognates, etc. so that
“Wedding songs” and “Wedding song” would generate the same set of retrievals
If this list is presented ungrouped, then it should probably be designed so the user can sort based on
format in order to support more rapid scanning of the results of most interest in use cases such as this
one.
Failure story 2. In the success story mentioned earlier, the searcher failed to use the format menu to
limit the retrievals to Music CDs. This is very plausible, as it is last in the list of steps, is labeled optional,
and has a label that may not be very suggestive of its function for some users. If the user also fails to
recognize that icons in the column labeled “Click to limit by format” indicate the formats of the
items (book vs. sheet music vs. music CD, etc.), then she might wind up having to look through the
full set of 338 retrievals instead of only the subset which consists of music CDs.
Since there is a lot of unused space on the search window (Figure 24.6a), the different formats could be
presented directly instead of hidden within a pull-down window. Alternatively, the ability to limit the
retrievals to specific formats could be made more salient and understandable on the retrieval page
(Figure 24.6b). A third alternative would to be automatically sort retrievals by format as the default
display when the user clicks on “Wedding Music,” showing them organized in a hierarchy such as:
Wedding Music
Books
Music CDs
Sheet Music
Music Tapes
Failure story 3. Most users accessing the display shown in Figure 24.6a would probably click on the
link for “Wedding music.” However, a few users might look at the column labeled “Catalog Records”
and see the indication that there is 1 item for “Wedding songs, English Ireland” and blank items for
“Wedding music.” This could be misinterpreted as indicating that there are no items available on
“Wedding music.”
Failure story 4. In this example, the information seeker was shown entering a term that triggered the
appropriate controlled vocabulary term. It is well known, however, that people are remarkably varied in
the labels that they choose to express a concept of interest, and that they often do not understand the
need to explore the use of alternative synonyms and related terms in order to conduct a thorough search.
To avoid such a failure, the thesaurus used for triggering the controlled vocabulary terms therefore
needs to be carefully constructed. In addition, the search system could suggest a search strategy (entering
synonyms, broader terms or related terms) when the user completes a search the doesn’t generate any
retrievals (Shute and Smith, 1992).
Example of a Cognitive Walkthrough — Summary. The sample analysis above serves to illustrate how a
cognitive walkthrough can be conducted. There are several important considerations surrounding such
an analysis:
. A person’s performance is strongly influenced by his or her immediate goal context. Developing the
normative goal hierarchies for the different use cases of interest, and identifying the product state
(and the associated context) corresponding to each node in a goal hierarchy, helps to focus the
analyst’s attention on that immediate goal context when making predictions about success and
failure stories
. The analyst is more likely to generate a thorough set of predictions by explicitly considering a list of
questions like those suggested previously:
– What is most salient in the display?
– Where will the user’s focus of attention be drawn?
24-12 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
– How will the user’s prior knowledge influence selective attention and interpretation?
– Will the user be trying to achieve the right effect?
. Tokeep the time required for the analysis manageable
– Carefully select a subset of the overall use cases for analysis, based on a judgment of which ones
are most likely to produce insights of value
– Develop the goal hierarchy for the normative paths rather than for all possible (correct and
incorrect) paths
– Make decisions regarding how deep to go in building the goal hierarchy based on judgments
about how useful the inclusion of additional levels of detail will be
– Build goal hierarchies for subtasks rather than covering a complete use case when there are only
specific subtasks that have raised questions or uncertainties about the current design
In addition, it is not always necessary to formally draw the goal hierarchies. The goal is to identify
sequences of steps involved in using the product for a given scenario, and to predict potential errors
at each step. Unless the results need to be communicated formally to other people, it is often sufficient
to just make rough sketches of the hierarchy in order to help the analyst think through the sequence of
states for the product for each step (such as a sequence of screen displays for a software product).
However, there are also times when it is useful to formally develop the hierarchies, either to help
ensure that an analysis is sufficiently accurate and complete, or to help communicate the methods
used and the findings of an analysis to other people.
ratings may be based on consideration of the probability that the defect will be encountered along with its
expected impact.
Sample Heuristics. The following set of heuristics, although not exhaustive, is useful in its versatility of
application:
Does the design support the users’ task flows?
Look at the sequence of steps necessary to complete a given task with the new product and determine
whether this sequence is efficient, effective, and clear, and whether it fits into the user’s broader task flow.
Are metaphors appropriately used?
There are situations where an interface can be more easily understood by the use of a metaphor. By
resembling a commonplace system such as the controls on a tape deck, an interface’s functionality
may be quickly ascertained (if the user is familiar with and recognizes the metaphor). Any metaphors
that have been used should be consistently and clearly suggested through the product’s interface.
Areas where the metaphors break down should be carefully assessed to determine how to deal with
such concerns.
What is the hierarchical focus of attention in the display? Does it support completion of the alternative
goals that users may have when looking at that display?
Our eyes are drawn to animated areas of an interface or product display more readily than static areas.
Determine whether the use of motion or a flashing cursor helps users notice important state changes, or
whether it becomes a distraction to what is important. Size, font style, display format/layout, and color
also have a big impact on the focus of attention.
Are the product’s key functions and features salient?
Determine whether important features and functions are visibly presented. Many users prefer to see
“up-front” the set of functions that are available to them. Important features and functions buried
deep in an interface structure may go unrecognized. Users should be able to understand the functions
afforded by the product interface through a quick visual scan of the interface.
Do the labels and icons for buttons, menu items, etc. clearly indicate their meanings, telling the user what
action will be performed or what information will be accessed if that item is selected?
Whether a graphical or textual label, it is important that this label clearly indicate its meaning. In many
cases, if a graphic is used, it helps to include a text label along with the graphic or icon.
Are external memory aids provided to help the user remember what actions to take or to remember what
steps in some process have already been completed?
Knowledge and information can be stored in the head or in the world (Norman, 2002). While access to
knowledge in the head can be very efficient, that assumes that the person has committed it to memory,
and will be able to retrieve it at the right time. Often, by showing critical information within some
display, this memory load can be reduced, making it easier for the person to remember how to
perform various functions with the product.
When information is presented as text, has it been organized using subheadings as advance organizers, thus
structuring the content to make it easy for the user to scan for specific information?
The use of subheadings and bulleted lists both make it easier for the user to scan text and to identify
those portions that are relevant to his or her interests. Subheadings further serve to inform the user about
the topic of the associated content, making it easier for the reader to quickly and accurately interpret that
content as the details are read (Bransford and Johnson, 1972).
Are the relationships among associated controls and displays indicated through some form of functional
grouping?
There are often relationships among information displays and controls based on specific tasks or
subtasks that users need to perform. When tasks create such functional relationships, they should be
supported by grouping or integrating the displays (Wickens et al., 2004), clearly indicating this
24-14 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
relationship. This can be done by integrating related information into a single graphical display (Tufte,
1983, 1990, 1997), by placing the related displays and controls in close proximity to one another, or by
indicating relationships among controls and display elements using techniques like color coding. It can
also be done in a virtual space when all of the related information cannot be displayed at the same time,
by providing links to all of the related information in the appropriate location so that the user has a
reminder of its relevance and availability, and so that the information can be quickly accessed by pressing
a button or clicking on a link (hot spot).
Does the product look and behave consistently?
Displays and controls should look and behave in a consistent and coherent manner. These behaviors
should make sense to the user from one part of the product’s interface to the next.
Does the product design take into consideration users’ past experiences with related products or tasks, either
to reduce resistance to change (Brehm et al., 1999), to reduce the time required to learn this new product, or to
differentiate it from its competitors?
Just as consistency within a product is important, consistency with the users’ past experiences can be
very important, as users develop expectancies for certain design features. If there are actual or implicit
standards for the design of a given type of product, or if users are likely to have had significant experience
with similar products, the resultant expectancies that they have developed from these past experiences
need to be considered.
Is the navigation robust enough to support the easy completion of alternative tasks, while still clear enough
to help the user navigate along the correct paths without getting lost? Are landmarks provided to help the user
remember where he or she is within the system (relative to the overall navigational structure), and to under-
stand where he or she can go next to complete different tasks?
Most products are meant to support more than one goal or task. Thus, the interface needs to support
navigation along a variety of different paths to support these different goals. This represents a fundamen-
tal design challenge, as the flexibility offered by multiple paths to support different user goals makes it
more difficult to develop a design that ensures that the user will know which path to select and will
be able to navigate along this path successfully.
Is the navigation flexible enough to accommodate novice users (for whom quickly learning to use the system
for limited purposes may be most important), while also meeting the needs of frequent, experienced users, for
whom shortcuts to ensure speed of use may be most important?
One of the challenges in designing a product is the accommodation of users with a variety of needs,
and with different experience levels. If possible it is desirable to embed training in the interface to help
novices learn shortcuts as they use the system. A good example of this is often provided in software by
including a description of the shortcut (Control-P) along with the label for a specific function such as
Print in the pull-down menu used to select the print function.
Does the system support different mechanisms for searching for relevant information in order to accom-
modate users with levels of knowledge about their topics and with different preferences regarding how to
conduct a search?
Hierarchical menus are useful to support browsing when searching for information. However, in some
cases, the information seeker may not recognize the correct path to follow through the menu and may
therefore need to have a more direct keyword search function as an alternative to find the desired infor-
mation. Keyword searches provide faster, more direct access to information if the information seeker
knows the “correct” terminology. However, since different information seekers may enter a variety of
different terms when searching for the same content, it is useful for the search engine to map synonyms
to the same underlying information content.
Users may also have difficulty selecting the right level of detail to use when searching for a topic. They
may enter a keyword or keyword phrase that is too narrow or too broad (Shute and Smith, 1992).
Embedding a thesaurus function in the search engine can be very helpful for both of these cases.
Applying Cognitive Psychology to System Development 24-15
While trained searchers often like the specificity enabled by search systems that allow the entry of logical
operators (AND, OR, NOT, ADJACENT, etc.), many people have difficulty understanding the meanings
of such operators. A well-designed relevance ranking algorithm is often an effective way to avoid requiring
the use of logical operators when entering a keyword phrase, helping to direct the information seeker’s
attention to those retrievals that are likely to be most focused on the topic of interest by putting them
at the top of the list of retrievals, while still providing access to the broader set of retrievals lower in
the list in case the search engine’s relevance judgments do not fully reflect the interests of that user.
Finally, structured keyword entry systems can be an effective compromise between browsing and a
simple keyword search. If the information available in a system can be categorized along several dimen-
sions, (such as the model, year, color, and cost of used cars in a database), it may be helpful to present the
user with these categories so he or she can pick from the menus of items available for each category. In
some cases, such menus can even be made context specific, so that as the user fills in one category, the
remaining choices in the other categories are limited.
Are “cause and effect” actions represented in the interface?
Each input by the user or change in the behavior of the program should be accompanied by a correspond-
ing change of its representation within the interface. The “current selection” (active field or object) should be
indicated with some visual contrast. The current state of the product should be displayed in a consistent,
clear, and unambiguous manner. If the product acts directly upon the “world” (such as with a process
control system), the current state of the “world” as known to the product should also be displayed clearly.
Will users be able to detect and recover from errors?
It is inevitable that someone will hit the wrong button due to a slip or mistake. Potential errors need to
be predicted, and measures to assure detection and recovery need to be identified if design changes
cannot be made to prevent these errors. Minimally, it should be possible to undo or cancel actions.
Actions that are irreversible like “saving over an existing file” should require a user to make an explicit
commitment. These safety nets will give novice users a sense of comfort, as well as supporting more effec-
tive use of the product.
Is the interface esthetically pleasing?
Keeping in mind that beauty is in the mind of the beholder, the interface should have a sense of beauty,
or at the very least not be perceived by the intended users as crude or awkward. Ask questions like:
. Are the colors harmonious?
. Is the type legible?
. Are the graphics consistent?
Also note whether users have the ability to customize the display of the interface, that is, change
the colors or type font to suit their own esthetic judgments. Factors that influence the esthetics of a
product also often have an effect on usability because of their impact on the salience of key information.
24.4 Conclusions
As suggested at the beginning of this chapter, one of the hats that the designer needs to wear is that of a
psychologist, conducting initial user studies and needs assessments to define the design problem, and
attempting to determine how a given design will influence user performance as part of its evaluation.
In terms of the evaluation of a specific design, both analytical and empirical evaluations are valuable. Both
of these forms of evaluation call for expertise in cognitive and social psychology, as patterns of performance
need to be predicted a priori for analytical evaluations, and need to be detected in empirical evaluations.
To improve analytical predictions, two general approaches have been developed:
. Cognitive walkthroughs
. Heuristic evaluations
24-16 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Both types of reviews can be conducted using paper or functional prototypes, thus making it possible
to complete them at different times during the design process. In addition, designers, experts in human-
centered design and subject matter experts can all participate in such evaluations, each providing their
particular insights into potential user behaviors.
The strength of these two complementary approaches is that they guide evaluators to focus on context-
specific predictions of user behaviors, and they make explicit important questions to ask as part of the
prediction process. By doing so, they provide structure to the evaluation process, helping to ensure that
the users’ underlying cognitive processes are considered and that important principles for effective design
are followed. Thus, the theme of this chapter is that one important perspective is to view design as a
prediction task, using an understanding of the psychology of the user to guide the identification of poten-
tial weaknesses in a design, so that they can be corrected during the design and implementation process.
References
Annett, J. and Stanton, N. (2000). Task Analysis. London: Taylor & Francis.
Ashcraft, M.H. (2002). Cognition, 3rd ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Baddeley, A. (1998). Human Memory: Theory and Practice. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Badre, A. N. (2002). Shaping Web Usability, Interaction Design in Context. Boston, MA: Pearson
Education, Inc.
Baecker, R., Grudin, J. Buxton, W., and Greenberg, S. (1995). Readings in Groupware and Computer-Supported
Cooperative Work: Assisting Human–Human Collaboration. San Francisco, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
Baeza-Yates, R. and Ribeiro-Neto, B. (eds.) (1999). Modern Information Retrieval. Harlow, UK: Addison-
Wesley.
Bainbridge, L. (1983). Ironies of automation. Automatica, 19, 775 –779.
Bennett, K.B. and Flach, J.M. (1992). Graphical displays: implications for divided attention, focused
attention, and problem solving. Human Factors, 34(5), 513 –533.
Bralla, J. (1998). Design for Manufacturability Handbook, 2nd ed. New York: McGraw Hill.
Bransford, J.D. and Johnson, M.K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investi-
gations of comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 717–726.
Brehm, S., Kassin, S. M., and Fein, S. (1999). Social Psychology, (4th ed) Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin
Company.
Card, S., Moran, T., and Newell, A. (1983). The Psychology of Human –Computer Interaction. San
Francisco, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
Card, S., Mackinlay, J. and Shneiderman, B. (1999). Readings in Information Visualization: Using Vision to
Think. San Francisco, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
Carroll, J.M. (1995). Scenario-Based Design: Envisioning Work and Technology in System Development.
New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Carroll, J.M. (2003). HCI Models, Theories and Frameworks: Toward a Multidisciplinary Science.
San Francisco, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
Diaper, D. and Stanton, N. (eds.) (2004). The Handbook of Task Analysis for Human –Computer
Interaction. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Endsley, M. (2003). Designing for Situation Awareness. London: Taylor and Francis.
Eysenck, M. and Keane, M. (1990). Cognitive Psychology: A Student’s Handbook. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Flach, J.M. (1998). Cognitive systems engineering: putting things in context. Ergonomics, 41(2), 163–167.
Gentner, D. and Stevens, A. (1983). Mental Models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Gibson, J.J. (1986). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Goldstein, B.E. (1996). Sensation and Perception. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Gordon, S.E. and Gill, R.T. (1997). Cognitive task analysis. In: C. Zsambok and G. Klein (eds.),
Naturalistic Decision Making. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 131–140.
Applying Cognitive Psychology to System Development 24-17
Helander, M. and Nagamachi, M. (1992). Design for Manufacturability: A Systems Approach to Concurrent
Engineering and Ergonomics. London: Taylor and Francis.
Hollnagel, E. (2003). Handbook of Cognitive Task Design. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Horton, F. and Lewis, D. (eds.) (1991). Great Information Disasters. London: Association for Information
Management.
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Jagacinski, R.J. and Flach, J.M. (2003). Control Theory for Humans: Quantitative Approaches to Modelling
Performance. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Johnson, P., Duran, A., Hassebrock, F., Moller, J. Prietulla, M., Feltovich, P., and Swanson, D. (1981).
Expertise and error in diagnostic reasoning. Cognitive Science, 5, 235–283.
Johnston, W.A. and Dark, V.J. (1986). Selective attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 37, 43–75.
Jonassen, D., Tessmer, M., and Hannum, W. (1999). Task Analysis Methods for Instructional Design.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Kirwan, B. and Ainsworth, L. (1992). A Guide to Task Analysis. London: Taylor and Francis.
Klein, G. (2000). Cognitive task analysis of teams. In: J.M. Schraagen, S. Chipman, and V. Shalin (eds.),
Cognitive Task Analysis. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 417 –430.
Krug, S. (2000). Don’t Make Me Think — A Common Sense Approach to Web Usability. Indianapolis, IN:
New Riders.
Lewis, C. and Wharton, C. (1997). Cognitive walkthroughs. In: M. Helander, T. Landauer, and P. Prabhu
(eds.), Handbook of Human –Computer Interaction, 2nd edn. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 717– 731.
Mayhew, D. (1999). The Usability Engineering Lifestyle: A Practitioner’s Handbook for User Interface
Design. San Francisco, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
Mirel, B. (2003). Interaction Design for Complex Problem Solving: Developing Useful and Usable Software.
San Francisco, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
Mitta, D., Delk, C., and Lively, W. (1995). Selecting system functionalities for interface evaluation.
Human Factors, 37(4), 817 –834.
Nielsen, J. (1994). Usability Engineering. San Francisco, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
Nielsen, J. (1997). Usability testing. In: G. Salvendy (ed.), Handbook of Human Factors and Ergonomics.
New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1543–1567.
Neilsen, J. (2000). Designing Web Usability: The Practice of Simplicity. Indianapolis, IN: New Riders.
Nielsen, J. and Mack, R. (1994). Usability Inspection Methods. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Norman, D. (2002). The Design of Everyday Things. New York: Doubleday.
Olson, G.M. and Olson, J.S. (2003). Human –computer interaction: psychological aspects of the human
use of computing. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 491–516.
Parasuraman, R., and Riley, V. (1997). Humans and automation: use, misuse, disuse, abuse. Human
Factors, 39(2), 230 –253.
Pashler, H., Johnston, J.C. and Ruthruff, E. (2001). Attention and performance. Annual Review of
Psychology, 52, 629 –651.
Plous, S. (1993). The Psychology of Judgment and Decision Making. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Preece, J. (1994). Human– Computer Interaction. Harlow, UK: Addison-Wesley.
Reason, J. (1991). Human Error. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Reason, J. (1997). Managing the Risks of Organizational Accidents. Aldershot, Hampshire, UK: Ashgate.
Rosenfeld, L. and Morville, P. (2002). Information Architecture for the World Wide Web, 2nd ed.
Sebastopol, CA: O’Reilly.
Rosson, M.B. and Carroll, J.M. (2001). Usability Engineering: Scenario-Based Development of Human–
Computer Interaction. San Francisco, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
Roth, E.M., Bennett, K., and Woods, D.D. (1987). Human interaction with an “intelligent” machine.
International Journal of Man–Machine Studies, 27, 479 –525.
Rubenstein, R. amd Hersh, H. (1984). The Human Factor. Boston, MA: Digital Press.
Rubin, T. (1994). Handbook of Usability Testing: How to Plan, Design and Conduct Effective Tests.
New York: John Wiley & Sons.
24-18 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Schraagen, J. M., Chipman, S. and Shalin, V. (eds.) (2000). Cognitive Task Analysis. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Sheridan, T. (2002). Humans and Automation: System Design and Research Issues. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Shepherd, A. (2000). Hierarchical Task Analysis. London: Taylor & Francis.
Shute, S., and Smith, P. J. (1992). Knowledge-based search tactics. Information Processing and Management,
29, 29–45.
Silver, M. (2005). Exploring Interface Design. Clifton Park, NY: Delmar Learning.
Skirka, L., Mosier, K., and Burdick, M. (1999). Does automation bias decision making? International
Journal of Human –Computer Systems, 51, 991 –1006.
Smith, P.J. and Geddes, N. (2003). A cognitive systems engineering approach to the design of decision support
systems. In: J. Jacko and A. Sears (eds.), The Human–Computer Interaction Handbook: Fundamentals,
Evolving Technologies and Emerging Applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 656–675.
Smith, P.J., McCoy, E., and Layton, C. (1997). Brittleness in the design of cooperative problem-solving
systems: the effects on user performance. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics,
27(3), 360– 370.
Spool, J., Scanlon, T., Snyder, C., DeAngelo, T. and Schroeder, W. (1998). Web Site Usability: A Designer’s
Guide. San Francisco, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
Tufte, E.R. (1983). The Visual Display of Quantitative Information. Chesire, CT: Graphics Press.
Tufte, E.R. (1990). Envisioning Information. Chesire, CT: Graphics Press.
Tufte, E.R. (1997). Visual Explanations. Cheshire, CT: Graphics Press.
Van Duyne, D., Landay, J., and Hong, J. (2003). The Design of Sites. Boston, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Vincente, K. (1999). Computer Work Analysis: Toward Safe, Productive and Healthy Computer-Based
Work. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Vicente, K.J. (2002). Ecological interface design: progress and challenges. Human Factors, 44(1), 62–78.
Watzman, S. (2003). Visual design principles for usable interfaces. In: J. Jacko and A. Sears (eds.), The
Human–Computer Interaction Handbook: Fundamentals, Evolving Technologies and Emerging
Applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 263– 285.
Wickens, C.D. and Hollands, J. (1999). Engineering Psychology and Human Performance, 3rd edition.
Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Wickens, C.D., Lee, J., Liu, Y. and Gordon-Becker, S. (2004). An Introduction to Human Factors Engine-
ering, 2nd edition. Upper Saddle River NJ: Pearson/Prentice Hall.
Witkin, B.R. and Altshuld, J.S. (1995). Planning and Conducting Needs Assessments. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage Publications.
Yantis, S. (1998). Control of visual attention. In: H. Pashler (ed.), Attention. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum, 223 –256.
25
The Role Personality
in Ergonomics
25.1 Background
It is not surprising that the underlying causes of musculoskeletal disorders (MSDs) are complex, multi-
factorial, and difficult to determine. Despite decades of research, the numbers of individuals developing
MSDs are still quite high. Yelin et al. (1999) reported that 90% of disabled older workers had MSDs.
Further, it was estimated that 18.4% of the U.S. population (nearly 60 million individuals) will suffer
from one or more chronic MSDs by the year 2020 (Lawrence et al., 1998). Treatment of these disorders
is in the tens of billions of dollars (Praemer et al., 1999). These injury statistics suggest that the current
methods for identifying and reducing or eliminating MSDs in today’s industries have not been
completely effective.
Research on MSD causation typically has focused on either of two primary groups of possible
factors — biomechanical demands in the workplace or psychosocial influences. Biomechanical
demands most often arise from performing physical work, which impact the body’s tissues. These
types of factors include, but are not limited to, loads handled, work repetition, the duration of the
activity, joint kinematics, and contact with sources of vibration. Psychosocial influences are less
clearly defined, but they typically involve social aspects of the work environment, how a job is organized,
and the content of the job tasks performed (Sauter et al., 1990), in addition to the environment outside of
work and individual traits (Bernard, 1997).
Each of these group’s contributions to injury risk has not yet been established, and estimates have
varied considerably. Regardless, neither research area alone appears capable of explaining MSD causa-
tion. Davis et al.’s (2000) review of the literature concluded that neither biomechanical nor psychosocial
factors could individually explain all the variabilities in MSD reporting.
25-1
25-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Further, research on MSD causation has not yet provided a clear picture of how physical workplace
and psychosocial factors together contribute to MSDs. Leaders in the field, including ergonomists, phys-
icians, and epidemiologists, generated a report suggesting that several types of factors likely are involved
and interact with one another to produce an MSD (National Research Council and Institute of Medicine,
2001). Here, these experts reviewed hundreds of research studies and determined that a variety of factors
must be considered to understand injury mechanisms. They developed a conceptual model of factors that
may affect MSD risk in the workplace, which is shown in Figure 25.1. This model approaches MSD risk
from a “whole person” perspective, meaning that it includes aspects both of the workplace (e.g., loads
imposed on the body, organizational factors, and social aspects of jobs) and the individual (e.g., phys-
iological responses to external loading, physical tissue tolerances, health outcomes, and personal
factors). The model also underscores the complex nature of MSD causation. These researchers concluded
that, “Because workplace disorders and individual risk and outcomes are inextricably bound, musculo-
skeletal disorders should be approached in the context of the whole person rather than focusing on body
regions in isolation.” Thus, it would appear that only by studying the entire work system (i.e., the inter-
action of physical, psychosocial, and individual factors) can we derive the root causes of MSDs.
Clearly, the range of factors considered psychosocial in nature is large and diverse. They include per-
sonal aspects of individuals as well as their perceptions of the work environment. Undoubtedly, different
individuals can view the same environment and work situation in a number of ways. Some may view
these environments as suitable to their preferred method of operating, or they may find the work organ-
ization too stressful. Thus, the psychosocial nature of an environment should perhaps be viewed from the
perspective of those evaluating and working within it. An understanding of personality and personality
theory may aid in this comprehension.
One’s personality preferences can be included in the group of individual factors interacting with others
to affect MSD risk. There is general consensus that one’s personality is, at least, in part, biologically based.
Jung (1923) proposed that human behavior was predictable, not random, that one’s preferences emerge
early in life, and that these preferences form the basis for our attractions to and aversions from people,
Internal Tolerances
Mechanical
Organizational Strain
Factors
Fatigue
Outcomes
Pain
Social Discomfort
Context
Impairment
Disability
FIGURE 25.1 Conceptual model of likely groups of musculoskeletal disorder risk factors and their interactions.
(From National Research Council and Institute of Medicine, 2001, Musculoskeletal Disorders and the Workplace:
Low Back and Upper Extremities (Washington, DC: National Academy Press). With permission.)
The Role Personality in Ergonomics 25-3
tasks, events, etc. throughout our lifetimes. Recent research has suggested that several traits important in
understanding personality have an inherited or genetic component (Bouchard et al., 1990; Plomin and
Caspi, 1999; Krueger, 2000). Loehlin (1992) estimated that as much 40% of the variability in personality
could be attributed to inherited traits. As further evidence that personality preferences are at least partly
“hard wired,” Canli et al. (2001) found clear relationships between personality and reactivity in the brain
to emotional stimuli, particularly with regard to extraversion and neuroticism. These results suggest that
personality is a stable trait that can be measured or observed as part of the environment that may
influence how work is perceived and reacted to.
By understanding one’s personality, we may be better able to understand the influence that psychoso-
cial factors have on individuals and, as a result, on the potential for MSD development. Our personalities
dictate our preferences for many life situations, including work. Theorist Allport (1937) wrote that per-
sonality is “. . . the dynamic organization within the individual of those psychophysical systems that
determine his unique adjustments to his environment.” This implies that personality is a system, that
it integrates both the physical and mental aspects of an individual, and that people are active in adapting
to their surroundings.
Personality theory has been applied extensively to occupational work. Several researchers have studied
individuals’ personalities and how these interact with one’s jobs. In situations where an individual’s per-
sonality preferences were less matched with the nature of their work, higher rates of turnover (Myers and
Myers, 1980) and job dissatisfaction (Smart et al., 1986; Smith, 1988; Karras, 1990; Holland, 1996) were
found. Such incongruence also has created communication problems, inefficiency, and, at times, health
concerns (Shelton, 1996). Thus, there appears to be a link between the job environment and individuals’
reactions to it, depending on their personality preferences.
Understanding and determining one’s personality is not an exact science. There is a wide range of com-
peting personality theories. The aim of this chapter is not to compare and contrast these theories or to
detail the specifics of all types of personality assessments that have been used historically. The goal,
however, is twofold. The first is to identify those personality theories that have a solid research foun-
dation. The second goal is to detail those personality assessment techniques that may best be suited
for use in studying occupational risk factors for the development of MSDs. Those listed have established
acceptable levels of reliability and validity, and could be used to address the interaction of individual
factors with risk factors for the development of MSDs.
and attitudes. Among the objectives of scale 7 (psychasthenia or obsessive –compulsive disorder) is to
determine short- and long-term anxieties. Items on the schizophrenia scale (8) relate to one’s perceptions
and thought processes. In addition to mood and activity levels, scale 9 (hypomania) assesses family
relationships and moral values. Finally, social introversion (scale 10) relates to one’s comfort level is
group situations, from shy and uncomfortable (introverted) to friendly and talkative (extraverted).
TABLE 25.2 Descriptions and Occupationally Related Traits of the Eight MBTI Personality Preferences
Personality Types
Extraversion (E) is a preference for active involvement and Introversion (I) is a preference for inner reflection of thoughts
quick action. Extraverts: and ideas. Introverts:
Like variety, action Like quiet and periods of uninterrupted work
Are impatient with long, slow jobs Are content working alone
Like to have people around Have some problems communicating
Sensing (S) reflects a preference for dealing with physical Intuition (N) is a preference for deriving meaning from
reality and facts. Sensors: personal insight. Intuitors:
Like established ways of doing things Dislike doing the same thing repeatedly
Enjoy using current skills than learning new ones Enjoy learning new skills than using them
Work more steadily Work in bursts of energy, with slack periods in between
Are patient with routine details Dislike taking time with precision
Are good at precision work
Thinking (T) is a preference for making decisions based on Feeling (F) is a preference for decision-making based on
logic. Thinkers: personal values. Feelers:
Are uncomfortable dealing with people’s feelings Are people-oriented and aware of others’ feelings
Like putting things into a logical order Enjoy pleasing people
Are sometimes impersonal Like harmony
Can reprimand and fire people when necessary Need praise
Judging (J) is a preference to lead an orderly, planned life. Perceiving (P) is a preference to lead a more spontaneous,
Judgers: flexible life. Perceivers:
Like to keep and follow a plan of work Adapt well to changing situations
Prefer to settle and finish things May have trouble making decisions
Dislike interruptions, even for more urgent matters Want to know all about a new job
Are curious about new work situation
Research generally supports the validity of Eysenck’s personality questionnaires. Early studies provided
evidence of construct validity of the Eysenck Personality Inventory (White et al., 1968; Platt et al., 1971),
a precursor to the EPQ. Comparisons with other personality measures found the EPQ to be valid
and reliable on the extraversion – introversion and neuroticism scales but less so on psychoticism
(Wakefield et al., 1976; Goh et al., 1982; Caruso et al., 2001). Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for these
scales range from 0.66 to 0.86 (Eysenck and Eysenck, 1994).
Eysenck’s questionnaires were designed to be completed quickly. The revised version of the EPQ con-
tains 73 questions and takes about 10 to 15 min to complete. A short form contains only 57 items in
which respondents answer “yes” or “no.” It can be completed in 3 to 5 min.
The extraversion –introversion and neuroticism traits, in particular, have been linked to physical
response outcomes. As summarized by Pervin (2003), introverts tend to be more sensitive to pain and
are more easily fatigued. Those scoring high on the neuroticism scale are more likely to be anxious
and to experience body aches. These characteristics suggest the possible interaction between job
design and physical outcomes due to one’s personality.
(a) Aggressive
Cold
Egocentric
Impersonal
Psychoticism Impulsive
Antisocial
Unempathic
Creative
Tough-Minded
(b) Sociable
Lively
Active
Assertive
Extraversion–
Introversion Sensation-Seeking
Carefree
Dominant
Surgent
Venturesome
(c) Anxious
Depressed
Guilt Feelings
Low Self-Esteem
Neuroticism Tense
Irrational
Shy
Moody
Emotional
FIGURE 25.2 Eysenck’s three basic dimensions of personality — psychoticism (a), extraversion– introversion (b),
and neuroticism (c). (Adapted from Eysenck, H.J., 1990, Biological dimensions of personality, in Pervin, L.A. (ed.),
Handbook of Personality: Theory on Research [New York: Guilford]. With permission.)
25-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
levels) alone did not account for the increases seen in this disease (Friedman and Rosenman, 1957).
Results from the Framingham Heart Study data (Haynes et al., 1982) and other research (e.g.,
Rosenman et al., 1975) established a significant link between Type A behavior and increased incidence
of CHD. However, this positive association has not been found in some follow-up studies, such as the
Aspirin Myocardian Infarction Study (Shekelle et al., 1985a) and the Multiple Risk Factor Intervention
Trial (Shekelle et al., 1985b).
A limited number of studies have focused on links between Type A personality and MSDs. Flodmark
and Aase’s (1992) survey of blue-collar workers found that those reporting symptoms of MSDs had more
pronounced Type A behaviors than those who did not report any symptoms. Further, tenderness in the
neck and shoulder regions was found by Salminen et al. (1991) to be greater in those exhibiting Type A
behavior, leading these researchers to postulate that this was due to higher levels of muscle activity exhib-
ited by this group. Finally, Glasscock et al. (2003) found significantly higher levels of elbow flexor
muscle activity in those with Type A behavior, though the effect was a function of gender. Collectively,
these studies suggest that a link may exist between behavior due to personality preferences and muscle
functioning.
The Jenkins Activity Survey, or JAS, was developed to measure Type A behavior (Jenkins et al., 1979).
This assessment resulted from a study in which a group of males having had experienced a heart attack
scored significantly higher on the survey than did the control group (Jenkins et al., 1971). The JAS is a
self-administered, multiple-choice questionnaire that can be completed in 20 to 30 min. In addition to a
rating on the Type A scale, it yields scores on three subscales: speed and impatience, job involvement, and
being hard-driving and competitive. Estimates of test –retest reliability for the JAS, for intervals from 6
months to 4 yr, ranged from between 0.60 and 0.70.
TABLE 25.4 Categories of the Five Factor Model and their Descriptions
Factor Description
Extraversion Relates to the level of comfort one has with sensory stimulation. Those with high levels of
extraversion tend to prefer larger numbers of relationships and more social interactions,
while low levels (i.e., introversion) reflect a preference for being more independent and
spending time alone
Agreeableness This trait involves the quality of one’s interpersonal communications. Those at one end of
the spectrum on this dimension (sometimes referred to as adapters) select their norms
for behavior from a number of sources. “Challengers” typically are on the other end of
this continuum, as they base their behavior on their own personal beliefs
Conscientiousness This construct refers to the focus one has on goals. On one end of this spectrum are
individuals who are more self-disciplined and concentrate on fewer goals, while on the
other end are individuals who pursue many goals but are more easily distracted
Neuroticism Neuroticism is akin to anxiety. Those considered to be more reactive are bothered by a
greater variety of stimuli that may be of lower strength, while more resilient individuals
are affected by fewer types of stimuli that are at more intense levels.
Openness to experience This factor refers to one’s interest level in the unfamiliar. Those with “high” openness tend
to have more interests but less depth of knowledge about them, while those with “low”
openness prefer to focus more in-depth on fewer interests
assessment have been constructed, included the Big Five Marker Scales (taking about 10 min to com-
plete) and the Traits Personality Questionnaire (25 min). The Hogan Personality Inventory, or HPI
(Hogan and Hogan, 1995), was constructed using data from adult workers and is geared toward use
in occupational environments. A complete list and descriptions of inventories, questionnaires, adjective
scales, and other related instruments using FFM methodology has been provided by De Raad and Per-
ugini (2002).
Many researchers have provided evidence of this model’s validity. McCrae and Costa (1990) found
considerable agreement between self-ratings using the FFM and those ratings put forth by colleagues
and significant others. Significant associations between self-ratings and observer ratings using the
FFM also were reported by Funder et al. (1995), Riemann et al. (1997), and Watson et al. (2000). De
Raad and Perugini (2002) provided reviews of validity studies for several instruments using the FFM.
25.4 Conclusions
Researchers still are learning a great deal about factors related to musculoskeletal discomfort and the
causes of MSDs. However, the evidence is increasingly clear that the origins of MSDs are multifactorial
in nature. As conceptualized in Figure 25.1, important risk factors relate to both the physical and psy-
chosocial aspects of an individual’s working environment, as well as to aspects of the individuals them-
selves. One of the many characteristics of individuals that can influence their (real or perceived) reactions
to the working environment is personality.
The study of human personality has created a multitude of theories aimed at describing one’s behavior.
From these models arose a variety of inventories developed to assess personality factors relevant to that
theory. Some of the more commonly used models were described here. These have produced generally
The Role Personality in Ergonomics 25-11
acceptable levels of validity and have been used by researchers and practitioners to study human behavior
in a wide range of settings.
This chapter summarized personality assessments that may be useful in understanding how individual
influences interact with workplace factors in contributing to discomfort and MSD risk. The study of per-
sonality in occupational settings is not new, but the focus has traditionally been on improving communi-
cation among coworkers or assisting individuals to better understand themselves and their actions. Some
recent research highlighted here has studied personality in terms of the possibility that a “mind –body”
injury pathway exists. In other words, it may be that individuals’ personalities influence how they
respond to the physical and psychosocial demands of their workplace. This may translate to the creation
of physical symptoms (e.g., increased muscle activity, more joint loading) that, cumulatively, lead to pain
or injury.
This chapter has also made clear the fact that much work still remains in understanding the complex-
ities of MSD causation. Finally, it suggests that new discoveries may be made by integrating knowledge of
human behavior and individuals’ perceptions of their workplace with the actual, measurable workplace
factors that have been more commonly studied.
References
Allport, G. W., 1937, Personality: A Psychological Interpretation (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston).
Bayne, R., 1995, The Myers– Briggs Type Indicator — A Critical Review and Practical Guide (London:
Chapman and Hall).
Bernard, B. P. (ed.), 1997, Musculoskeletal Disorders and Workplace Factors: A Critical Review of Epidemio-
logical Evidence for Work-Related Musculoskeletal Disorders of the Neck, Upper Extremity, and Low
Back (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service, Centers for Disease
Control, National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health).
Bigos, S. J., Battie, M. C., Spengler, D. M., Fisher, L. D., Fordyce, W. E., Hansson, T. H., Nachemson, A. L.,
and Wortley, M. D., 1991, A prospective study of work perceptions and psychosocial factors affect-
ing the report of back injury, Spine, 16(1), 1– 6.
Block, J., 1993, Studying personality the long way, in Funder, D. C., Parke, R. D., Tomlinson-Keasey, C.,
and Widaman, K. (eds.), Studying Lives through Time (Washington DC: American Psychological
Association), pp. 9 –41.
Bongers, P. M., De Winter, C., Kompier, M. A. J., and Hildebrandt, V. H., 1993, Psychosocial factors at
work and musculoskeletal disease, Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment & Health, 19(5),
297–312.
Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Lykken, D. T., McGue, M. Segal, N. L., and Tellegen, A., 1990, Sources of human
psychological differences: The Minnesota study of twins reared apart, Science, 250, 223 –250.
Butcher, J. N., and Rouse, S. V., 1996, Personality: individual differences and clinical assessment, Annual
Review of Psychology, 47, 87–111.
Butcher, J. N., Dahlstrom, W. G., Graham, J. R., Tellegen, A., and Kaemmer, B., 1989, MMPI-2: Manual
for Administration and Scoring (Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press).
Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E., Gross, J., and Gabrieli, J. D., E., 2001, An fMRI study of
personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli, Behavioral Neuroscience, 115,
33–42.
Capraro, R. M. and Capraro, M. M., 2002, Myers-Briggs Type Indicator score reliability across studies: a
meta-analytic reliability generalization study, Educational and Psychological Measurement, 62(4),
590–602.
Carlson, J. G., 1985, Recent assessments of the MBTI, Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 356–365.
Caruso, J. C., Witkiewitz, K., Belcourt-Dittloff, A., and Gottlieb, J., 2001, Reliability of scores from the
Eysenck Personality Questionnaire: a reliability generalization (RG) study, Educational and Psycho-
logical Measurement, 61, 675– 682.
25-12 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc., 1998, Myers–Briggs Type Indicator, Form M (3803 E. Bayshore Road,
Palo Alto, CA, 94303).
Costa, P. T., Jr., and McCrae, R. R., 1992, NEO PI-R: Professional Manual (Odessa, FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources).
Davis, K. G., Marras, W. S., and Heaney, C. A., 2000, The relationship between psychosocial work
characteristics and low back pain: underlying methodological issues, Clinical Biomechanics, 15,
389– 406.
De Raad, B., and Perugini, M. (eds.), 2002, Big Five Assessment (Goettingen, Germany: Hogrefe &
Huber).
Druckman, D., and Bjork, R. A. (eds.), 1991, In the Mind’s Eye: Enhancing Human Performance
(Washington, DC: National Academy Press).
Eysenck, H. J., 1990, Biological dimensions of personality, in Pervin, L. A. (ed.), Handbook of Personality:
Theory and Research (New York: Guilford), p. 246.
Eysenck, H. J., and Eysenck, S. B. G., 1975, Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (London:
Hodder & Stoughton).
Eysenck, H. J., and Eysenck, S. B. G., 1994, Manual of the Eysenck Personality Scales – Revised (San Diego,
CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service).
Flodmark, B. T., and Aase, G., 1992, Musculoskeletal symptoms and type A behavior in blue collar
workers, British Journal of Industrial Medicine, 49(10), 683–687.
Friedman, M., and Rosenman, R., 1974, Type A Behavior and Your Heart (New York: Knopf).
Friedman, M., and Rosenman, R. H., 1957, Comparison of fat intake of American men and women: poss-
ible relationship to incidence of clinical coronary artery disease, Circulation, 16, 339–347.
Funder, D. C., Kolar, D. C., and Blackman, M. C., 1995, Agreement among judges of personality: inter-
personal relations, similarity, and acquaintanceship, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
69, 656– 672.
Gatchel, R. J., Polatin, P. B., and Kinney, R. K., 1995, Predicting outcome of chronic back pain using clini-
cal predictors of psychopathology: a prospective analysis, Health Psychology, 14(5), 415 –420.
Glasscock, N. F., Mirka, G. A., Sommerich, C. M., and Klein, K. W., 2003, The effects of personality type
and stress on muscle activity during simulated work tasks, Proceedings of the Human Factors and
Ergonomics Society 47th Annual Meeting, pp. 1159–1163.
Goh, D. S., King, D. W., and King, L. A., 1982, Psychometric evaluation of the Eysenck Personality
Questionnaire, Educational and Psychological Measurement, 42(1), 297 –309.
Goldberg, L. R., 1993, The structure of phenotypic personality traits, American Psychologist, 48(1), 26–34.
Greene, R. L., 2000, The MMPI-2: An Interpretive Manual, 2nd edition (Boston: Allyn and Bacon).
Haynes, S. G., Feinleib, M., and Eaker, E. D., 1982, Type A behavior and the ten year incidence of coronary
heart disease in the framingham Heart Study, Activitas Nervosa Superior, Suppl. 3(1), 57 –77.
Hogan, R., and Hogan, J., 1995, The Hogan Personality Inventory Manual, 2nd edition (Tulsa, OK: Hogan
Assessment Systems).
Holland, J. L., 1996, Exploring careers with a typology: what we have learned and some new directions,
American Psychologist, 51, 397– 406.
Jenkins, C. D., Zyzanski, S. J., and Rosenman, R. H., 1971, Progress toward validation of a computer-
scored test for the type A coronary-prone behavior pattern, Psychosomatic Medicine, 33(3),
193– 202.
Jenkins, C. D., Zyzanski, S. J., and Rosenman, R. H., 1979, Jenkins Activity Survey: JAS Manual (New York:
The Psychological Corporation).
Johnson, D. A., 1992, Test –retest reliabilities of the Myers –Briggs Type Indicator and the type differen-
tiation indicator over a 30-month period, Journal of Psychological Type, 24, 54–58.
Jung, C. G., 1923, Psychological Types (New York: Harcourt & Brace).
Karras, J. E., 1990, Psychological and environmental types: implications of degrees of congruence in
work, Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH.
The Role Personality in Ergonomics 25-13
Krueger, R. F., 2000, Phenotypic, genetic, and nonshared environmental parallels in the structure of per-
sonality: a view from the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire, Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 79, 1057 –1067.
Kummerow, J. M., Barger, N. J., and Kirby, L. K., 1997, Work Types (New York: Warner Books).
Lawrence, R. C., Helmick, C. G., Arnett, F. C., Deyo, R. A., Felson, D. T., Giannini, E. H., Heyse, S. P.,
Hirsch, R., Hochberg, M. C., Hunder, G. G., Liang, M. H., Pillemer, S. R., Steen, V. D., and
Wolfe, F., 1998, Estimates of the prevalence of arthritis and selected musculoskeletal disorders
in the United States. Arthritis and Rheumatism, 41(5), 778–799.
Loehlin, J. C., 1992, Genes and Environment in Personality Development (Newbury Park, CA: Sage).
Marras, W. S., Davis, K. G., Heaney, C. A., Maronitis, A. B., and Allread, W.G., 2000, The influence of
psychosocial stress, gender, and personality on mechanical loading of the lumbar spine, Spine,
25(23), 3045–3054.
Matthews, K. A., and Haynes, S. G., 1986, Type A behavior pattern and coronary disease risk: Update and
critical evaluation, American Journal of Epidemiology, 123, 923 –960.
McCrae, R. R., and Costa, P. T., Jr., 1990, Personality Adulthood (New York: Guilford).
McCrae, R. R., and Costa, P. T., Jr., 1999, A five-factor theory of personality, in Pervin, L. A., and
John, O. P. (eds.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (New York: Guilford), pp. 139–153
Myers, I. B. and Myers, P., 1980, Gifts Differing (Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.).
Myers, I. B., McCaulley, M. H., Quenk, N. L., and Hammer, A. L., 1998, MBTI Manual: A Guide to the
Development and Use of the Myers –Briggs Type Indicator, 3rd edn (Palo Alto: Consulting Psychol-
ogists Press, Inc.).
National Research Council, Institute of Medicine, 2001, Musculoskeletal Disorders and the Workplace: Low
Back and Upper Extremities (Washington, DC: National Academy Press).
Pervin, L. A., 2003, The Science of Personality (New York: Oxford University Press).
Platt, J. J., Pomeranz, D., and Eisenman, R., 1971, Validation of the Eysenck Personality Inventory by the
MMPI and Internal –External Control Scale, Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27(1), 104 –105.
Plomin, R., and Caspi, A., 1999, Behavioral genetics and personality, in Pervin, L. A., and John, O. P.
(eds.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (New York: Guilford), pp. 251–276.
Praemer, A., Furner, S., and Rice, D. P., 1999, Musculoskeletal Conditions in the United States (Rosemont,
IL: American Academy of Orthopaedic Surgeons).
Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., and Strelau, J., 1997, Genetic and environmental influences on personality: a
study of twins reared together using the self- and peer report NEO-FFI scales, Journal of Personality,
65, 449–476.
Rosenman, R. H., Brand, J. H., Jenkins, C. D., Friedman, M., Straus, R., and Wurm, M., 1975, Coronary
heart disease in the Western Collaborative Group Study: final follow-up experience of 8.5 years,
Journal of the American Medical Association, 233, 872 –877.
Salminen, J. J., Pentti, J., and Wickstrom, G., 1991, Tenderness and pain in neck and shoulders in relation
to type A behaviour, Scandinavian Journal of Rheumatology, 20(5), 344–350.
Sauter, S. L., Murphy, L. R., and Hurrell, J. J., Jr., 1990, Prevention of work-related psychological dis-
orders: a national strategy proposed by the National Institute for Occupational Safety and
Health, American Psychologist, 45, 1146–1158.
Scheier, M. F., and Carver, C. S., 1987, Dispositional optimism and physical well-being: the influence of
generalized outcome expectancies on health, Journal of Personality, 55(2), 169 –210.
Shelton, J., 1996, Health, stress, and coping, in Hammer, A. L. (ed.), MBTI Applications (Palo Alto, CA:
Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc).
Shekelle, R. B., Gale, M., and NORUSIS, M., 1985a, Type A score (JAS) and risk of recurrent coronary heart
disease in the Aspirin Myocardial Infarction Study, American Journal of Cardiology, 56, 221–225.
Shekelle, R. B., Hulley, S. B., Neaton, J. D., Billings, J. H., Borhani, N. O., Gerace, T. A., Jacobs, D. R.,
Lasser, N. L., Mittlemark, M. B., and Stamler, J., 1985b, The MRFIT behavior pattern study II:
Type A behavior and incidence of coronary heart disease, American Journal of Epidemiology,
122, 559–570.
25-14 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Smart, J. C, Elton, C. F., and McLaughlin, G. W., 1986, Person –environment congruence and job satis-
faction, Journal of Vocational Behavior, 29, 216–225.
Smith, C. E., 1988, A comparative study of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator and the Minnesota Import-
ance Questionnaire in the prediction of job satisfaction. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Ball State
University, Muncie, IN.
Wakefield, J. A., Sasek, J., Brubaker, M. L., and Friedman, A. F., 1976, Validity study of the Eysenck Per-
sonality Questionnaire, Psychological Reports, 39(1), 115 –120.
Watkins, C. E., Jr., Campbell, V. L., Nieberding, R., and Hallmark, R., 1995, Contemporary practice of
psychological assessment by clinical psychologists, Professional Psychology: Research and Practice,
26, 54– 60.
Watson, D., Hubbard, B., and Wiese, D., 2000, Self-other agreement in personality and affectivity: the
role of acquaintanceship, trait visibility, and assumed similarity, Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 78, 546 –558.
White, J. H., Stephenson, G. M., and Child, S. E., 1968, Validation studies of the Eysenck Personality
Inventory, British Journal of Psychiatry, 114(506), 63– 68.
Willis, C. G., 1984, Review of Myers-Briggs Type Indicator, in Keyser, D. J., and Sweetland, R.C. (eds.),
Test Critiques, Vol. 1 (Austin, TX: Pro-Ed).
Yelin, E. H., Trupin, L. S., and Sebesta, D. S., 1999, Transitions in employment, morbidity, and disability
among persons aged 51 to 61 with musculoskeletal and non-musculoskeletal conditions in the
U. S., 1992–1994, Arthritis and Rheumatism, 42, 769–779.
26
Psychosocial Work
Factors
26.1 Introduction
This chapter examines the concept of psychosocial work factors and its relationship to occupational ergo-
nomics. First, we provide a brief historical perspective of the development of theories and models of work
organization and psychosocial work factors. Definitions and examples are then presented. Several expla-
nations are given for the importance of psychosocial work factors in occupational ergonomics. Finally,
measurement issues and methods for controlling and managing psychosocial work factors are discussed.
The role of “psychosocial work factors” in influencing individual and organizational health can be
traced back to the early days of work mechanization and specialization, and the emergence of the
concept of division of labor. Taylor (1911) expanded the principle of division of labor by designing effi-
cient work systems accounting for proper job design, providing the right tools, motivating the indivi-
duals, and sharing of responsibilities between management and labor, and sharing of profits. This is known
as the era of scientific management in which scientific methods are used to objectively measure work with
the aim of improving its efficiency. These scientific methods involved breaking the tasks into small com-
ponents or units, thus making work requirements and performance evaluations easy to define and
monitor. Under these methods, work is simplified and standardized, therefore having a great impact
on job and work processes. An analysis of psychosocial work factors in a job in this system would
reveal that skill variety is minimal, workers have no control of the work processes, and the job is
highly repetitive and monotonous. Such work system design can still be found in numerous workplaces.
As the workforce became more educated, individuals became more aware of their working conditions
and environment, and began to seek avenues for improving their quality of working life. This is when the
human relations movement emerged (Mayo, 1945), which raised the issue of the potential influence of
the work environment on an individual’s motivation, productivity, and well-being. Individual needs and
wants were emphasized (Maslow, 1970). Thus, job design theorists incorporated worker behavior and
work factors in their theories. The two theories of job enlargement and job enrichment formed the
26-1
26-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
basis for many job design theories thereafter. These theories conceptualize the role of worker behavior
and perception of the work environment in influencing personal and organizational outcomes. Job enlar-
gement theory emphasized giving a larger variety of tasks or activities to the worker. While this was an
improvement from the era of scientific management, the additional tasks or activities could be of a
similar skill level and content: workers were performing multiple tasks of the same “kind.” This has
been called “horizontal loading” of the job, and is the opposite of job enrichment, which focused on
the “vertical loading” of the job. Job enrichment aims at expanding the skills used by workers, while
at the same time increasing their responsibility. Herzberg (1966), the father of the job enrichment
theory, defined intrinsic and extrinsic factors (or motivation versus hygiene factors) that are important
to worker motivation, thus leading to satisfaction or dissatisfaction, and psychological well-being. Intrin-
sic factors are related to the work (or job) conditions, such as having additional control over work sche-
dules or resources, feedback, client relationships, skill use and development, better work content, direct
communications, and personal accountability (Herzberg, 1974). Extrinsic factors are related to aspects of
financial rewards and benefits and also to the physical environment. Herzberg indicated that extrinsic
factors could lead to dissatisfaction with work, but not to satisfaction, while intrinsic factors could
increase satisfaction with work. Herzberg’s work demonstrated the complex relationships of job con-
ditions, the individual’s motivation, satisfaction, dissatisfaction, and psychological well-being. In a
way similar to Herzberg’s job enrichment theory, the Job Characteristics Theory (Hackman and
Oldham, 1976) focused on the idea that specific characteristics of the job (i.e., skill variety, task identity,
task significance, autonomy, and feedback) in combination with individual characteristics (growth need
strength) would determine personal and work outcomes.
The Sociotechnical Systems Theory recognized two inter-related systems in an organization: the
social system and the technical system. The main principle of the Sociotechnical Systems Theory is
that the social and technical systems interact with each other, and that the joint optimization of
both systems can lead to increased satisfaction and performance. The social system focused on the
workers’ perception of the work environment (i.e., job design factors) and the technical system empha-
sized the technology and the work processes used in the work (for example, automation, paced
systems, and monitoring systems). In a study of coal mining (Trist and Bamforth, 1951), it was
demonstrated that the technical system could impact the social system. In this study where semi-
autonomous work groups were set up, workers were given opportunities to make decisions related
to their work, and experienced better interactions with workers in their group, as well as task signifi-
cance and completeness (see also Trist, 1981). Work by Trist and his colleagues showed that techno-
logical factors could influence both organizational and job factors. However, it was Davis (Davis
1980) who provided a conceptual framework and a set of principles that formulated the Sociotechnical
theory. His framework called for a flattened management structure that would promote participation,
interaction between and across groups of workers, enriched jobs, and most important, meeting indi-
vidual needs. The Sociotechnical Systems Theory laid down the groundwork for the current under-
standing of how psychosocial work factors can be related to ergonomic factors by examining the
interplay between the social and technical systems in organizations. Other recent theories and
models of psychosocial work factors will be discussed later.
This rapid overview of the development of job design theories in the 20th century demonstrates the
increasing role of psychological, social, and organizational factors in the design of work.
26.2 Definitions
Within the last decade, the role of psychosocial work factors on worker health has gained much popu-
larity. However, the term of “psychosocial work factors” has been used loosely to define and represent
many factors that are a part of, attached to or associated with the individuals. Some would consider
what has been traditionally termed socioeconomic factors such as income, education level, and demo-
graphic or individual factors (e.g., age and marital status) as part of the psychosocial factors (Hogstedt,
Psychosocial Work Factors 26-3
Vingard et al., 1995; Ong, Jeyaratnam et al., 1995). In order to understand psychosocial factors in the
workplace, one needs to take into account the ability of an individual to make a psychological connection
to his or her job, thus formulating the relationship between the person and the job. For instance, the
International Labour Office (ILO, 1986) defines psychosocial work factors as “interactions between
and among work environment, job content, organizational conditions and workers’ capacities, needs,
culture, personal extra-job considerations that may, through perceptions and experience, influence
health, work performance, and job satisfaction.” Thus, the underlying premise in defining psychosocial
work factors is the inclusion of the behavioral and psychological components of job factors. In the rest of
the chapter, we will use the definitions proposed by Hagberg and his colleagues (Hagberg et al., 1995)
because they are most highly relevant for occupational ergonomics.
Work organization is defined as the way work is structured, distributed, processed, and supervised
(Hagberg et al., 1995). It is an “objective” characteristic of the work environment, and depends on
many factors, including management style, type of product or service, characteristics of the workforce,
level and type of technology, and market conditions. Psychosocial work factors are “perceived” charac-
teristics of the work environment that have an emotional connotation for workers and managers, and
that can result in stress and strain (Hagberg et al., 1995). Examples of psychosocial work factors
include overload, lack of control, social support, and job future ambiguity. Other examples are described
in the following section.
The concept of psychosocial work factors raises the issue of objectivity– subjectivity. Objectivity has
multiple meanings and levels in the literature. According to Kasl (1978), objective data is not supplied
by the self-same respondent who is also describing his distress, strain, or discomfort. At another level,
Kasl (1987) feels that “psychosocial factor perception” can be less subjective when the main source of
information is the employee but that this self-reported exposure is devoid of evaluation and reaction.
Similarly, Frese and Zapf (1988) conceptualize and operationalize “objective stressors” (i.e., work organ-
ization) as not being influenced by an individual’s cognitive and emotional processing. Based on this, it is
more appropriate to conceptualize a continuum of objectivity and subjectivity. Work organization can be
placed at one extreme of the continuum (that is the objective nature of work) whereas psychosocial work
factors have some degree of subjectivity (see definitions above).
Psychosocial work factors result from the interplay between the work organization and the individual.
Given our definitions, psychosocial work factors have a subjective, perceptual dimension, which is related
to the objective dimension of work organization. Different work organizations will ‘produce’ different
psychosocial work factors. The work organization determines to a large extent the type and degree of
psychosocial work factors experienced by workers. For instance, electronic performance monitoring,
or the on-line, continuous computer recording of employee performance-related activities, is a type
of work organization that has been related to a range of negative psychosocial work factors, including
lack of control, high work pressure, and low social support (Smith et al., 1992). In a study of office
workers, information on psychosocial work factors was related to objective information on job title
(Sainfort, 1990). Therefore, psychosocial work factors are very much anchored in the objective work
situation, and are related to the work organization.
and theories of job design reviewed at the beginning of the chapter tended to emphasize a small set of
psychosocial work factors. For instance, the human relations movement (Mayo, 1945) focused on the
social aspects of work, whereas the job characteristics theory (Hackman and Oldham, 1976) lists five
job characteristics, i.e., skill variety, task identity, task significance, autonomy, and feedback. However,
research and practice in the field of work organization has demonstrated that considering only a small
number of work factors can be misleading and inefficient in solving job design problems. The balance
theory proposes a systematic, global approach to the diagnosis and design or redesign of work
systems that does not emphasize any one aspect of work. According to the balance theory, psychosocial
work factors are multiple and of diverse nature.
Table 26.1 lists eight categories of psychosocial work factors and specific facets in each category. This
list cannot be considered as exhaustive, but is representative of the most often studied psychosocial work
factors.
The study of psychosocial work factors needs to be tuned in to the changes in society. Changes in the
economic, social, technological, legal, and physical environment can produce new psychosocial work
factors. For instance, in the context of office automation, four emerging issues are appearing
(Carayon and Lim, 1994): (1) electronic monitoring of worker performance, (2) computer-supported
work groups, (3) links between the physical and psychosocial aspects of work in automated offices,
and (4) technological changes. The issue of technological changes applies nowadays to a large
segment of the work population. Employees are asked to learn new technologies on a frequent, some-
times continuous, basis. Other trends in work organization include the development of teamwork and
other work arrangements, such as telecommuting. These new trends may produce new psychosocial
work factors, such as high dependency on technology, lack of socialization on the job and identity
with the organization, and pressures from teamwork. Two APA publications review psychosocial stress
issues related to changes in the workforce in terms of gender, diversity, and family issues (Keita and
Hurrell, 1994), and some of the emergent psychosocial risk factors and selected occupations at risk of
psychosocial stress (Sauter and Murphy, 1995).
1. Physical and psychosocial ergonomics are interested in the same job factors
2. Physical and psychosocial work factors are related to each other
3. Psychosocial work factors play an important role in physical ergonomics interventions
4. Physical and psychosocial work factors are related to the same outcome, for instance, work-related
musculoskeletal disorders
First, some of the concepts examined in the physical ergonomics literature are similar to concepts
examined in the psychosocial ergonomics literature. For instance, the degree of repetitiveness of a task
is very important from both physical and psychosocial points of view. Physical ergonomists are
more interested in the effect of the task repetitiveness on motions and force exerted on certain body
parts, such as hands; whereas psychosocial ergonomists are concerned about the effect of task
repetitiveness on monotony, boredom, and dissatisfaction with one’s work (Cox 1985). In the physical
ergonomics literature, an important job redesign strategy for dealing with repetitiveness is job
rotation: workers are rotated between tasks which require effort from different body parts and
muscles, therefore reducing the negative effects of repetitiveness of motions in a single task. From a
psychosocial point of view, job rotation is one form of job enlargement (see above for a discussion of
job enlargement). However, as discussed earlier, the psychosocial benefits of job rotation are limited
because workers may be simply performing a range of similar, nonchallenging tasks. From a physical
ergonomics point of view, job rotation is effective only if the physical variety of the tasks is increased;
whereas from a psychosocial ergonomics point of view, job rotation is effective only to the extent of the
content and meaningfulness of the tasks.
Second, physical and psychosocial work factors can be related to each other. For instance, the model
proposed by Lim (1994) states that the psychosocial factor of work pressure can influence the physical
factors of force and speed of motions. According to this model, workers may change their behaviors
under the influence of work pressure, and therefore, tend to exert more force or to speed up their
work. Empirical evidence tends to confirm this relationship between work pressure (i.e., a psychosocial
work factor) and physical work factors (Lim 1994). Another form of relationship between physical and
psychosocial work factors is evident in the literature on control over one’s physical environment. In this
case, the psychosocial work factor of control is applied to one particular facet of the work, that is the
physical environment. Control over one’s physical environment can, therefore, have benefits from a phy-
sical point of view (i.e., being able to adapt one’s physical environment to one’s physical characteristics
and task requirements), but also from a psychosocial point of view (i.e., having control is known to have
many psychosocial benefits [Sauter et al., 1989]).
26-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Third, psychosocial work factors are a crucial component of physical ergonomics interventions. In par-
ticular, the concept of participatory ergonomics uses the benefits of one psychosocial work factor, that is
participation, in the process of implementing physical ergonomics changes (Noro and Imada, 1991).
From a psychosocial point of view, using participation is important to improve the process and outcomes
of ergonomic interventions. In addition, any type of organizational interventions, including ergonomic
interventions, can be stressful because of the emergence of negative psychosocial work factors, such as
uncertainty and increased workload (i.e., having more work during the intervention or the transitory
period). Therefore, in any physical ergonomics intervention, attention should be paid to psychosocial
work factors in order to improve the effectiveness of the intervention and to reduce or minimize its nega-
tive effects on workers.
Fourth, physical and psychosocial work factors can be related to the same outcome. One of these out-
comes is work-related musculoskeletal disorders (WMSDs). There is increasing theoretical and empirical
evidence that both physical and psychosocial work factors play a role in the experience and development
of WMSDs (Hagberg et al. 1995; Moon and Sauter, 1996). Several mechanisms for the joint influence of
physical and psychosocial work factors on WMSDs have been presented (Smith and Carayon, 1996).
Therefore, in order to fully prevent or reduce WMSDs, both physical and psychosocial work factors
need to be considered.
psychosocial work factors can be more objective when devoid of evaluation and reaction (Kasl and
Cooper, 1987). As discussed earlier, any kind of data can be placed somewhere on this objectivity/sub-
jectivity continuum from “low in dependency on cognitive and emotional processing” (e.g., objective) to
“high in dependency on cognitive and emotional processing” (e.g., subjective).
We discuss three different questionnaires which include numerous scales of psychosocial work factors.
In addition, validity and reliability analyses have been performed on all three questionnaires. Two of these
questionnaires have been developed and used to measure psychosocial work factors in various groups of
workers or large samples of workers: (1) the NIOSH Job Stress questionnaire (Hurrell and McLaney
1988), and (2) the Job Content Questionnaire (JCQ) (Karasek, 1979). The NIOSH Job Stress question-
naire is often used in the Health Hazard Evaluations performed by NIOSH. Translations of Karasek’s JCQ
exist in many different languages, including Dutch and French. The University of Wisconsin Office
Worker Survey (OWS) is a questionnaire developed to measure psychosocial work factors in office/com-
puter work (Carayon, 1991). This questionnaire covers a wide range of psychosocial work factors of
importance in office and computer work. In addition to many of the psychosocial work factors measured
by the NIOSH Job Stress Questionnaire or Karasek’s JCQ, the OWS measures psychosocial work factors
related to computer technology, such as computer-related problems (Carayon-Sainfort, 1992). The OWS
questionnaire has been translated into Finnish, Swedish, and German. For all three questionnaires, data
exist for various groups of workers in numerous organizations of multiple countries. This data can serve
as a comparison to newly collected data and for benchmarking. Numerous other questionnaires for
measuring psychosocial work factors exist, such as the Occupational Stress Questionnaire in Finland
(Elo et al., 1994) and the Occupational Stress Indicator in England (Cooper et al., 1988). Other question-
naires are listed in Cook et al. (1981).
systems approach, negative psychosocial work factors can be balanced out or compensated by positive
work factors.
Some trends in the field of organizational design and management may have positive characteristics
from a psychosocial point of view. For instance, under certain conditions, the use of quality engineering
and management methods can positively affect the psychosocial work environment, such as increased
opportunity for participation, and learning and development of quality-related skills (Smith et al.,
1989). However, other trends in the business world can have negative effects on the psychosocial work
environment. For instance, downsizing and other organizational restructuring and reengineering may
create highly stressful situations of uncertainty and loss of control (DOL 1995).
26.7 Conclusion
This chapter has demonstrated the importance of psychosocial work factors in the research and practice
of occupational ergonomics. In order to clarify the issue at hand, we presented definitions of work
organization and psychosocial work factors. It is important to understand the long research tradition
on psychosocial work factors that has produced numerous models and theories, but also valid and
reliable methods for measuring psychosocial work factors. At the end of the chapter, we presented
examples of methods for managing and controlling psychosocial work factors.
Psychosocial work factors need to be taken into account in the research on and practice of occu-
pational ergonomics. We have discussed the important role of psychosocial work factors with regard
to physical ergonomics. In addition, given the constantly changing world of work and organizations,
we need to pay even more attention to the multiple aspects of people at work, including psychosocial
work factors.
References
Carayon, P. (1991). The Office Worker Survey. Madison, WI, Department of Industrial Engineering,
University of Wisconsin-Madison.
Carayon, P. and Lim, S.-Y. (1994). Stress in automated offices. The Encyclopedia of Library and Infor-
mation Science. A. Kent. New York, Marcel Dekker. Vol. 53, Supplement 16: 314–354.
Carayon-Sainfort, P. (1992). The use of computers in offices: impact on task characteristics and worker
stress. International Journal of Human Computer Interaction 4(3): 245 –261.
Cook, J. D., Hepworth, S. J. et al. (1981). The Experience of Work. London, Academic Press.
Cooper, C. L., Sloan, S. J. et al. (1988). Occupational Stress Indicator. Windsor, England, NFER-Nelson.
Cox, T. (1985). Repetitive work: Occupational stress and health. Job Stress and Blue-Collar Work. C. L.
Cooper and M. J. Smith. New York, John Wiley & Sons: 85–112.
Cox, T. and Ferguson, E. (1994). Measurement of the subjective work environment. Work and Stress
8(2): 98 –109.
Davis, L. E. (1980). Individuals and the organization. California Management Review 22(2): 5 –14.
DOL (1995). Guide to responsible restructuring. Washington, D.C. 20210, U.S. Department of Labor,
Office of the American Workplace.
Elo, A.-L., Leppanen, A., et al. (1994). The Occupational Stress Questionnaire. Occupational Medicine.
C. Zenz, O. B. Dickerson and E. P. Horvarth. St. Louis, Mosby: 1234–1237.
Frese, M. and Zapf, D. (1988). Methodological issues in the study of work stress. Causes, Coping and
Consequences of Stress at Work. C. L. Cooper and R. Payne. Chichester, John Wiley & Sons.
Hackman, J. R. and Oldham, G. R. (1976). Motivation through the design of work: test of a theory.
Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 16: 250–279.
Hagberg, M., Silverstein, B., et al. (1995). Work-Related Musculoskeletal Disorders (WMSDs): A Reference
Book for Prevention. London, Taylor & Francis.
Hendrick, H. W. (1991). Human Factors in organizational design and management. Ergonomics 34:
743– 756.
Psychosocial Work Factors 26-9
Hendrick, H. W. (1996). Human factors in ODAM: an historical perspective. Human Factors in Organi-
zational Design and Management -V. O. J. Brown and H. W. Hendrick. Amsterdam, The Netherlands,
Elsevier Science Publishers: 429–434.
Herzberg, F. (1966). Work and the Nature of Man. New York, Thomas Y. Crowell Company.
Herzberg, F. (1974). The wise old turk. Harvard Business Review (September/October): 70–80.
Hogstedt, C., E. Vingard, et al. (1995). The Norrtalje-MUSIC Study — An ongoing epidemiological study on risk
and health factors for low back and neck-shoulder disorders. PREMUS’95-Second International
Scientific Conference and Prevention of Work-Related Musculoskeletal Disorders, Montreal, Canada.
Hurrell, J. J. J. and M. A. McLaney (1988). “Exposure to job stress — A new psychometric instrument.”
Scandinavian Journal of Work Environment and Health 14(Suppl. 1): 27 –28.
ILO (1986). Psychosocial Factors at Work: Recognition and Control. Geneva, Switzerland, International
Labour Office.
Jackson, W. E. (1983). Participation in decision-making as a strategy for reducing job-related strain.
Journal of Applied Psychology 68: 3 –19.
Karasek, R. A. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude, and mental strain: implications for job
redesign. Administrative Science Quarterly 24: 285– 308.
Kasl, S. V. (1987). Methodologies in stress and health: past difficulties, present dilemmas, future direc-
tions. Stress and Health: Issues in Research and Methodology. S. V. Kasl and C. L. Cooper. Chichester,
John Wiley & Sons: 307–318.
Kasl, S. V. and C. L. Cooper, Eds. (1987). Stress and Health: Issues in Research and Methodology.
Chichester, John Wiley & Sons.
Keita, G. P. and Hurrell, J. J. J. (1994). Job Stress in a Changing Workforce — Investigating Gender, Diversity,
and Family Issues. Washington, D.C., APA.
Lim, S. (1994). An integrated approach to cumulative trauma disorders in computerized offices: the
role of psychosocial work factors, psychological stress and ergonomic risk factors. IE. Madison,
WI, University of Wisconsin-Madison.
Maslow, A. H. (1970). Motivation and Personality. New York, Harper and Row.
Mayo, E. (1945). The Social Problems of an Industrial Civilization. Andover, MA, The Andover Press.
Moon, S. D. and Sauter, S. L. Eds. (1996). Beyond Biomechanics — Psychosocial Aspects of Musculoskeletal
Disorders in Office Work. London, Taylor & Francis.
Noro, K. and Imada, A. (1991). Participatory Ergonomics. London, Taylor & Francis.
Ong, C. N., Jeyaratnam, J., et al. (1995). “Musculoskeletal disorders among operators of video display
terminals.” Scandinavian Journal of Work Environment and Health 21(1): 60–64.
Sainfort, P. C. (1990). Perceptions of Work Environment and Psychological Strain Across Categories of Office
Jobs. The Human Factors Society 34th Annual Meeting.
Sauter, S. L., Hurrell, J. J. Jr., et al., Eds. (1989). Job Control and Worker Health. Chichester, John Wiley & Sons.
Sauter, S. L. and Murphy, L. R. (1995). Organizational Risk Factors for Job Stress. Washington, D.C., APA.
Smith, M. J. and Carayon, P. (1996). Work organization, stress, and cumulative trauma disorders. Beyond
Biomechanics — Psychosocial Aspects of Musculoskeletal Disorders in Office Work. S. D. Moon and
S. L. Sauter. London, Taylor & Francis: 23 –41.
Smith, M. J., Carayon, P., et al. (1992). Employee stress and health complaints in jobs with and without
electronic performance monitoring. Applied Ergonomics 23(1): 17 –27.
Smith, M. J. and Carayon-Sainfort, P. (1989). A balance theory of job design for stress reduction. Inter-
national Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 4: 67 –79.
Smith, M. J., Sainfort, F., et al., Eds. (1989). Efforts to Solve Quality Problems, Secretary’s Commission on
Workforce Quality and Labor Market Efficiency, U.S. Department of Labor, Washington, D.C.
Taylor, F. (1911). The Principles of Scientific Management. New York, Norton and Company.
Trist, E. (1981). The Evaluation of Sociotechnical Systems. Toronto, Quality of Working Life Center.
Trist, E. L. and Bamforth, K. (1951). Some social and psychological consequences of the long-wall
method of coal getting.
27
Biomechanical
Modeling of the
Shoulder
27.1 Introduction
At present, it is difficult to establish a unifying integrative approach in the study of glenohumeral stability
and upper extremity strength. The glenohumeral joint is the most mobile articulation of the human
body, involving interacting and interrelated geometric variables relating bones, muscles, and ligaments,
that needs further exploration and understanding. The purpose of the current study was to propose
a method of geometrical description of the glenoid fossa in order to deduce an unrecognized inter-
relationship between glenoid concavity and the deltoid attachment on the glenohumeral head position.
It supports the concept of a new biomechanical parameter, the dynamic glenohumeral stability index
proposed by Lee and An,16 which considers not only the force vectors generated by the deltoid muscle
but also the concavity compression mechanism. Recent analyses of the glenohumeral joint have not
focused on the contribution of geometric parameters like the shape of the glenoid fossa, radius of the
humerus, the attachment of the deltoideus lateral part in relation to the glenoid fossa, and the angular
measure of the articular surface of the humerus head.
The concavity of the glenoid fossa plays a significant role in the stability of the glenohumeral joint
as was revealed in recent experiments by Lee and An.16 A labrum may not contribute to glenohumeral
stability as much as was previously assumed, as also was found by Halder et al.8 It has been shown that the
glenohumeral joint exhibits ball-and-socket kinematics; however, its motion is coupled with translation
of the humeral head on the glenoid which requires a certain degree of mismatch of the articulating
surfaces and leads to variations in the joint-contact area.8,9,11,17
Gagey and Hue5,6 recently analyzed the mechanics of the deltoid muscle and suggested that the deltoid
acts on the humeral head like a cable on a pulley. They pointed out “the downward-oriented force applied
27-1
27-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
by the deltoid to the head depends on the angle of reflection around the humerus.” These observations
suggest that there exist more geometric details which never were analyzed.
Saha18 pointed out the variance in the congruity of the glenoid and humeral head. Gielo-Perczak7 con-
firmed in theoretical studies the influence of the variance in the congruity on physical capabilities.
Additionally, Lee and An16 reported the strong relationship between glenoid depth and stability of a
joint. The study provided by Halder et al.8 revealed “the degree of stability of the glenohumeral joint
depends on the perpendicular component of the rotator-cuff muscle forces as well as on the radius of
the articular surface.” Halder et al.8 hypothesized that “the degree of stability through concavity-com-
pression is position-dependent.” Generally, the translations in the inferior–superior direction (more
than 4 mm) are larger than in the anterior–posterior direction.3 These translations are a function of
glenoid concavity, which in all previous studies was described as a part of a sphere or ellipse. Are these
adequate descriptions to explain the variety of the glenohumeral head positions and the strength of the
joint during abduction of the arm?
The glenoid concavity is irregular and less marked than the convexity of the humeral head,14 thus it
needs adequate radiographic data and analysis. There are two major controversies relating to the shape of
the glenoid which is considered with or without the cartilage surfaces of the glenohumeral joint. One
represents the concept that the glenoid is flatter18 or apparently shallow3 if taking into account the
bone surfaces beneath the articulating surfaces as determined from radiograph. The second represents
that the glenoid can be described as a part of a sphere. Bigliani and his coworkers2 observed that
out of nine fresh frozen human cadaveric shoulders (average age 50 yr; range 42 –59 yr), the glenoid
bone surfaces for eight had a larger radius of curvature than the matching humeral head bone surfaces
(difference was less than 2.5 mm), whereas there was only one joint in which the humeral head had a
larger radius of curvature than the glenoid (difference was 1.2 mm). Iannotti and his coworkers13
(1992) shared similar observations. Are these inconsistencies and lack of consensus in the descriptions
of the glenohumeral joint the result of conceptually omitting the geometric parameters in the modeling
of its structure and, as a next step, disregarding them in the experiments?
A few studies have focused on measuring the glenoid geometry by:
. The lateral humeral displacement during translation across the glenoid8,15
. The glenoid articular surface angles in the superior –inferior and anterior – posterior planes1
. The depths in the superoinferior and anteroposterior directions10
However, none of these terms adequately provides a description of the glenoid fossa concavity which
can be used in mechanical studies.
The purpose of the study was twofold. (1) Theoretically explain the influence of the glenoid concavity
(without the labrum) and the middle deltoid attachment on humeral head positions in the superior –
inferior direction. (2) By applying the theoretical results, propose a method of geometrical description
of the glenoid and formulate a necessary number of segments tangent to the glenoid, which can be
used to determine the planar shape of the glenoid fossa.
27.2 Methods
z
O f
Z
z'
y'
A y
b
b
Q γ3
90−b γ2
R
α S
90−f
RM RS
γ1
L1
L2
FIGURE 27.1 A planar model of the glenohumeral joint with the glenoid inclination and the distance of the
deltoideus lateral part attachment.
glenohumeral joint was restrained only by the deltoid lateral part, (4) the glenohumeral joint reaction
force acts perpendicular to the articular surface, (5) friction at the joint was omitted. Reference axes
Y–Z were fixed in the scapula at the center of the lateral margin of the acromion, where the Y-axis
was parallel to the longitudinal axis of the thorax.
The main feature of the approach was combining the possible mutual geometric relations, including
the position of the humerus, the inclination of the tangents to the glenoid surface, the radius of the
humerus, the angle between the middle deltoid attachment, and the muscle contact with the
humerus, with the conditions for equilibrium of the structure during static loading.
These relations are:
R 1
g1 ¼ sin (27:1)
L1
y
!
sin f z cos f
g2 ¼ tan1 (27:2)
R z sin f
27-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
0 1
B R C
g3 ¼ cos1 @qffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiA (27:3)
y
( sin f z cos f) þ (R z sin f)
2 2
P 9
P MA ¼ P L2 sin a RM R ¼ 0 =
P F Z ¼ R S sin f R M sin ( g a ) Q cos ( b g g f) ¼ 0 (27:6)
1 2 3
;
FY ¼ P þ RS cos f þ RM cos (g1 a) þ Q sin (b g2 g3 f) ¼ 0
The known variables are: a — glenohumeral joint angle defined as the angle between the axis of the
humerus and the Y-axis of body; f — an angle at which a tangent to a given curve of the glenoid fossa at
the point S in the frontal plane crosses the Z-axis; z — distance measured along the Z-axis between the
middle deltoid attachment and a point at which a tangent to the glenoid fossa crosses the Z-axis; g1 —
angle between the distal tendon of the deltoid fiber and the axis of the humerus; P — external load and
reduced weight of upper limb applied at point C; L1 — distance between the distal insertion of the middle
deltoid and center of the humerus head considered as a sphere; L2 — distance between the center of the
humerus head and point of hand where an external load and reduced weight of upper limb were applied;
R — radius of the humerus head.
The unknown variables are: RS — the glenohumeral reaction force between the articular surface of the
glenoid fossa and the head of the humerus which is perpendicular to the glenoid articular surface
described by a tangent at its midpoint; RM — the force in a single muscle fiber of the deltoideus
lateral par, applied on the humeral insertion of the deltoideus; Q — the reaction force of the deltoideus
on the curved humerus area, which is a function of RM; y — distance along the Y-axis at which a humerus
is balanced; b — angle of “glenohumeral pulley” defined as an angle of the curved contact area of muscle
fiber with the head of the humerus; g2 — angle at the center of the humerus formed by the perpendicular
line of reaction force and line connected with the distal insertion of the middle deltoid fiber muscle; g3 —
angle at the center of the humerus formed by the perpendicular line to the muscle fiber at the curved
contact area and line connected with the distal insertion of muscle. This geometric model should be
applied to each fiber of the lateral part of the deltoideus.
The variable y was calculated as a distance along the Y-axis at which a humerus was in balance. The
detailed trigonometric analysis revealed the relations into analytic form and the geometric equations were:
z 0 ¼ z sin f
Ez ¼ (R z 0 )2
Bz ¼ (R z 0 )2 R2
Cz ¼ (R z 0 )R
R sin f
Gz ¼
L2 sin a (27:7)
Dz ¼ Gz Ez þ Cz
qffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
Iz ¼ 4D2z 4Bz Gz Dz þ B2z
Bz 2Gz Dz Iz
Jz ¼
2G2z 1
pffiffiffiffi
y ¼ Jz sin f þ z sin f cos f
Biomechanical Modeling of the Shoulder 27-5
Parametric analyses have been provided for changing values f, z, and function of a. The positions of the
humerus head were calculated for each angle of elevation/abduction a ¼ 08 –1708, and for the four para-
metric conditions: (1) f ¼ 908, z ¼ 0 mm (Figure 27.2a); (2) f ¼ 908, z ¼ 0– 32 mm (Figure 27.2b); (3)
f ¼ 108 –1708, z ¼ 0 mm (Figure 27.2c and e); (4) f ¼ 108 –1708, z ¼ 0–32 mm (Figure 27.2d and f). In
the four geometrical structures related to joint articulations, the concavity of the glenoid fossa was reduced
to a tangent to the glenoid surface. The deltoid lateral part was attached directly to the glenoid fossa or at
the certain distance (Figure 27.2).
For the first conditions (Figure 27.2a) where f ¼ 908 (a tangent to the glenoid fossa was a vertical line)
and z ¼ 0 mm (the deltoid lateral part was attached directly to the glenoid fossa), the distance along the
Y-axis was:
rffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
L2 sin a þ R
y¼R (27:8)
L2 sin a R
For the second condition (Figure 27.2b) where f ¼ 908 (a tangent to the glenoid fossa was a vertical
line) and z ranged from 0 to 32 mm (the deltoid lateral part was attached at a certain distance from the
Z z f Z
Z
FIGURE 27.2 The geometrical structures related to joint articulations. (a) The conditions where f ¼ 908,
z ¼ 0 mm. (b) The conditions where f ¼ 908, z ¼ 0– 32 mm. (c) The conditions where f ¼ 08– 908, z ¼ 0 mm.
(d) The conditions where f ¼ 08– 908, z ¼ 0 –32 mm. (e) The conditions where f ¼ 908 –1808, z ¼ 0 mm.
(f) The conditions where f ¼ 908– 1808, z ¼ 0 –32 mm.
27-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
E ¼ (R z)2
B ¼ (R z)2 R2
C ¼ (R z)R
R
G¼
L2 sin a
(27:9)
D ¼ GE þ C
pffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
I ¼ 4D2 4BGD þ B2
B 2GD I
J¼
2G2 1
pffiffi
y¼ J
For the third conditions (Figure 27.2c and e) where f ¼ 108 to 1708 (a tangent to the glenoid fossa was
a line crossing the Z-axis in the range from 108 to 1708) and z ¼ 0 mm (the deltoid lateral part was
attached to the glenoid fossa which was an inclined articular surface), the conditions were represented
by both an acute angle (Figure 27.2c) and an obtuse angle (Figure 27.2e). The geometric relations were:
sffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
L2 sin a þ R sin f
y ¼ R sin f (27:10)
L2 sin a R sin f
For the fourth conditions (Figure 27.2d and f) where f ¼ 108 to 1708 (a tangent to the glenoid fossa
was a line crossing the Z-axis in the range from 108 to 1708) and z ranged from 0 to 32 mm (the deltoid
lateral part was attached at a certain distance from the glenoid fossa), the conditions are represented by
both an acute angle (Figure 27.2d) and obtuse angle (Figure 27.2f). The geometric relations were as
presented in Equation (27.7).
where gi is a subgradient of f in the point zi (if f is differentiable). The nodes zi, . . . , zk on the curve of the
glenoid in the frontal plane were determined when the maximal linearization error 1(z) was minimal.
The tangents to a given glenoid fossa in the frontal plane f (zi) are the straight lines at the points
(zi, f (zi)) which cross the Z-axis at angles f1, . . . , fi. Applying the smallest error of the polyhedral
model of a convex function,2 it was found that the minimum number of tangents necessary to
Biomechanical Modeling of the Shoulder 27-7
c
k ¼ pffiffiffi (27:12)
2 2e
where c is the height of glenoid fossa measured along the Y-axis and e is the tolerance of measurement.
Both terms are expressed in millimeters. For example, if the height of glenoid fossa c was 39.1 mm and
the humerus magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) slices were 2 mm in thickness, then the number of
tangents was seven (Figure 27.3). For thinner MRI slices, the minimum number of tangents needed to
fit the glenoid fossa geometric surface would be larger.
Y
z1
z7
z2
z6
z3
z5
z4
f1 f2 f3 f4 f5 f6 f7
FIGURE 27.3 The group of seven tangents approximated the bone surface data that fit to the glenoid fossa
geometric surface from MRI slice of 2 mm thickness.
27-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
for all 12 male participants. Data were recorded by using the 1,5-T Sigma system (GE Medical Systems,
Milwaukee, WI). From 120 images taken in the frontal plane, a total of 12 characteristic images were
identified. The humerus MRI slices were 2 mm in thickness. Data were digitized by using AutoCAD
R14 and Alice v.4.4.9 software.
30
Distance
Translations [mm]
of muscle
attachment
z = 0 mm
z = 8 mm
25
z = 16 mm
z = 24 mm
z = 32 mm
20
0 50 100 150
FIGURE 27.4 The translation of the humeral head center during arm abduction a ¼ 08– 1708 and the conditions
f ¼ 908 and z ¼ 0 – 32 mm.
attachment of the lateral part of the deltoideus increased, the humeral head translations decreased.
Generally, the increased distance of muscle attachment from the glenoid fossa and the increased
inclination from 1108 to 1708 shortened the translations. However, these translations were all
smaller than for the third parametric condition. For example, during abduction of 108 with muscle
attachment of 24 and 28 mm, and the angle of tangent inclination is 1508, the translations were 2.4
and 0.6 mm, respectively (Figure 27.6a).
The smallest translations for any position of an arm during abduction were for tangent f ¼ 1708 and
muscle attachment z ranging from 16 to 28 mm. Additional analysis has been provided for the angles of
abduction a ¼ 1108, 1408, 1608, and 1708. The calculations revealed the same results as for the
supplementary angles a ¼ 708, 408, 208, and 108, respectively.
It was observed from MRI measurements of the glenoid fossa concavity for the 12 participants that
the tangents crossed the Z-axis at angles in a range from f1 ¼ 67.2 + 12.18 to f 7 ¼ 116.1 + 14.18
with a maximum of 136.08 and a minimum of 50.28. The distances at which the tangents crossed the
35
30
Tangent
Translations [mm]
25 inclination
20 φ = 10°, 170°
φ = 30°, 150°
15 φ = 50°, 130°
φ = 70°, 110°
10 φ = 90°
5
0
0 20 40 60 80 100 120 140 160 180
Angle of arm abduction [°]
FIGURE 27.5 The translation of the humeral head contact point with the glenohumeral fossa along the Y-axis
during arm abduction a ¼ 08– 1708 and the conditions f ¼ 108 – 1708 and z ¼ 0 mm.
27-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
(a)
Tangent
30 inclination
70°
φ = 10°
110°
50° φ = 30°
Translations [mm] 20 φ = 50°
130°
30° φ = 70°
150° φ = 110°
10
φ = 130°
10° φ = 150°
170°
φ = 170°
0
0 4 8 12 16 20 24 28 32
Distance of muscle attachment [mm]
(b) Tangent
30 inclination
70° φ = 10°
110° φ = 30°
Translations [mm]
50°
20 φ = 50°
130°
30° φ = 70°
150° φ = 110°
10
φ = 130°
10°
170° φ = 150°
0 φ = 170°
0 4 8 12 16 20 24 28 32
Distance of muscle attachment [mm]
(c)
Tangent
30 inclination
φ = 10°
70°
110° φ = 30°
Translations [mm]
50°
20 φ = 50°
130°
φ = 70°
30°
150° φ = 110°
10
φ = 130°
10°
170° φ = 150°
0 φ = 170°
0 4 8 12 16 20 24 28 32
(d) Tangent
30 inclination
φ = 10°
70°
110° φ = 30°
Translations [mm]
50°
20 φ = 50°
130°
φ = 70°
30°
150° φ = 110°
10
φ = 130°
10°
170° φ = 150°
0 φ = 170°
0 4 8 12 16 20 24 28 32
FIGURE 27.6 The translation of the humeral head contact point with the glenohumeral fossa along the Y-axis
during (a) arm abduction 108 and 1708 and the conditions f ¼ 108 –1708 and z ¼ 0– 32 mm; (b) arm abduction
208 and 1608 and the conditions f ¼ 108 – 1708 and z ¼ 0 –32 mm; (c) arm abduction 408 and 1408 and the
conditions f ¼ 108– 1708 and z ¼ 0– 32 mm; and (d) arm abduction 708 and 1108 and the conditions f ¼ 108–
1708 and z ¼ 0 –32 mm.
Biomechanical Modeling of the Shoulder 27-11
TABLE 27.1 Geometric Parameters of the Glenoid Fossa Shapes at the Seven Nodes (i ¼ 7) for 12 Subjects
Subjects
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
z1 mm 67.2 14.2 30.8 28.6 27.8 34.3 10.6 37.9 49.4 8.2 67.1 20.8
z2 mm 56.4 11.8 24.1 30.1 33.1 23.7 16 23.7 38.3 26.5 37.3 13.4
z3 mm 38.2 8 18.9 14.4 30.5 14.2 7.7 10.1 30.4 15.1 17.6 12.3
z4 mm 30.3 1.1 8.9 9.4 16.5 4.7 4.6 8.3 22.4 34 11.5 10.1
z5 mm 16.3 21.2 2.6 5.6 8.8 21.2 20.7 1.7 15.1 0.9 5.3 8.8
z6 mm 8.4 2.2 23.4 24 0.7 25.6 22.8 25.8 5.4 21.9 27.2 3
z7 mm 5.7 6 28 23.7 1.7 214.6 25.5 212.5 211.4 2.8 220 26.5
f1 8 50.2 78.5 70 72.8 69.1 63.3 75 66 58 93.3 53 57
f2 8 56.4 81.7 76 71.5 64.7 73 64 78 66 74 73 72
f3 8 70.7 87.3 81 87.1 67 83.5 84 95 73 87.5 92 74
f4 8 78.7 98.5 93 92.9 82.4 95.7 93 97 81 103.6 98 81
f5 8 95.4 102.7 101 97.8 92.2 103.9 109 106 90 107.5 105 85
f6 8 105.8 95.9 109 110.9 104.4 110.5 116 116 103 112.1 120 109
f7 8 109.6 87.3 116 110.4 102.5 123.6 125 124 124 102.4 132 136
Note: zi is the distance between the middle deltoid attachment and a point at which a tangent to the glenoid foosa crosses
the Z-axis and fi is the tangent inclination.
Z-axis from the lateral margin of the acromion, the lateral deltoid muscle attachment, were respectively
from z1 ¼ 33.1 + 18.9 mm to z7 ¼ 25.5 + 8.0 mm with a maximum of 67.2 mm and a minimum of
220.0 mm. The results of the measurements are summarized in Table 27.1.
27.5 Conclusions
1. The study responds to the recent studies12 confirming an emerging need to include glenoid incli-
nation, mechanics of the deltoid muscle, and glenoid labrum concavity into stability analyses of
the glenohumeral joint
2. The study implies the practicability of a geometric description of the glenoid fossa which should
no longer be considered as a straight line but as a series of tangents
3. The current study proposed considering the whole planar shape of the glenoid fossa as a mean-
ingful geometric component of the glenohumeral joint
4. Migration of the humeral head along the glenoid fossa is related to the glenoid fossa inclination
and the distance from the glenoid fossa of the deltoid muscle attachment
5. The results for the tangents with supplementary angular values (i.e., in Figure 27.6a f ¼ 308 and
1508; f ¼ 508 and 1308; f ¼ 708 and 1108) demonstrate symmetry because the translations of
the glenoid head along the Y-axis oscillate around the same balance position
6. A tangent to the glenoid fossa, formed by a larger than 908 angle to the z-axis improves the
glenohumeral stability significantly
7. The MRI results for 12 subjects confirm variability in tangent inclinations and distances of
tangents from the lateral deltoid attachment
8. Considering and measuring the angles of tangents as the parameters of the glenoid fossa will
provide new information on the interface shape sensitivity of glenohumeral joint stability
9. This method may help in early diagnosis of mechanical instability, which can be considered as a
condition predisposing the glenohumeral joint to degenerative change especially for a subject
returning to work after work absence resulting from a work-related injury
10. These analytical mechanical considerations may provide a better biomechanical basis for joint
modeling and lead to consideration of articular geometry during designing of workplaces
27-12 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
11. The geometrical parameters distinguished in mechanical glenohumeral joint analysis will help to
modify the physical strength standards which are still too high as reflected in the high frequency
of recorded shoulder injuries
12. Additional research is necessary combining geometries of the glenoid fossa with strength tests
performed by the participants in order to find a functional relationship which would be
applied in work environments
References
1. F.T. Ballmer, S.B. Lippitt, A.A. Romeo, and F.A. Matsen 3rd, Total shoulder arthroplasty: Some
considerations related to glenoid surface contact, Journal of Shoulder and Elbow Surgery 3 (1994),
299– 306.
2. D.P. Bertsekas, Nonlinear Programming, Athena Scientific, Belmont, MA, 1995.
3. L.U. Bigliani, R. Kelkar, E.L. Flatow, R.G. Pollock, and V.C. Mow, Glenohumeral stability: biomecha-
nical properties of passive and active stabilizers, Clinical Orthpaedics and Related Research 330
(1996), 13 –30.
4. R. Fletcher, Practical Methods of Optimization, John Wiley & Sons, Chichester, 1987.
5. O. Gagey and E. Hue, Mechanics of the deltoid muscle. A new approach, Clinical Orthpaedics and
Related Research 375 (2000), 250 –257.
6. O. Gagey and E. Hue, Reply to Blasier et al. comments on: Clin. Orthop. 2000 Jun 375:250–257,
Clinical Orthpaedics and Related Research 388 (2001), 259.
7. K. Gielo-Perczak, Analysis and modeling of the glenohumeral joint mechanism structure, SOMA
Engineering for the Human Body 3 (1989), 35 –46.
8. A.M. Halder, S.G. Kuhl, M.E. Zobitz, D. Larson, and K.N. An, Effects of the glenoid labrum and
glenohumeral abduction on stability of the shoulder joint through concavity-compression: an in
vitro study, The Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery 83-A (2001), 1062 –1069.
9. D.T. Harryman 2nd, J.A. Sidles, J.M. Clark, K.J. Mcquade, T.D. Gibb, and F.A. Matsen 3rd, Trans-
lation of the humeral head on the glenoid with passive glenohumeral motion, The Journal of Bone
and Joint Surgery 72 (1990), 1334– 1343.
10. S.M. Howell and B.J. Galinat, The glenoid-labral socket: a constrained articular surface, Clinical
Orthpaedics and Related Research 243 (1989), 122 –125.
11. S.M. Howell, B.J. Galinat, A.J. Renzi and P.J. Marone, Normal and abnormal mechanics of the
glenohumeral joint in the horizontal plane, The Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery 70 (1988),
227 –232.
12. R.E. Hughes, C.R. Bryant, J.M. Hall, J. Wening, L.J. Huston, J.E. Kuhn, J.E. Carpenter, and R.B.
Blasier, Glenoid inclination is associated with full-thickness rotator cuff tears, Clinical Orthpaedics
and Related Research 407 (2003), 86 –91.
13. Iannotti, JP, Gabriel JP, Schneck SL, Evans BG, Misra S. The normal glenohumeral relationships. An
anatomical study of one hundred and forty shoulders. The Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery 1992;
74:491 –500.
14. I.A. Kapandji, The Physiology of the Joints, 5th ed., vol. 1 Upper Limb, Churchill Livingstone,
Edinburgh, 1982.
15. M.D. Lazarus, J.A. Sidles, D.T. Harryman 2nd, and F.A. Matsen 3rd, Effect of a chondral-labral defect
on glenoid concavity and glenohumeral stability. A cadaveric model, The Journal of Bone and Joint
Surgery 78 (1996), 94 –102.
16. S.-B. Lee and K.-N. An, Dynamic glenohumeral stability provided by three heads of the deltoid
muscle, Clinical Orthpaedics and Related Research 400 (2002), 40 –47.
17. N.K. Poppen and P.S. Walker, Normal and abnormal motion of the shoulder, The Journal of Bone and
Joint Surgery 58-A (1976), 195–201.
18. A.K. Saha, Theory of Shoulder Mechanism: Descriptive and Applied, Charles C Thomas Publishing
Company, Springfield, IL, 1961.
28
Application of
Ergonomics to the
Low Back
28.1 Overview
Before an individual can accurately assess the workplace, there must be a basic understanding of the prin-
ciples of ergonomics, particularly with respect to how they relate to risk of injury. When evaluating the
risk of low back injury, it is important to start with a load –tolerance relationship, whether that is a phys-
ical, social, or psychological load. In other words, exposures in the workplace exert loads on the body,
which the body must respond to, and, at some point, a tolerance is exceeded causing the injury and
resulting in an injury (McGill, 1997). The most likely tolerance relating to low back injuries is structural
tolerance — loading on the spine. Another potential tolerance that may also contribute to risk assessment
is pain sensation, particularly with respect to psychosocial work factors (this will be discussed further
later). Based on this load –tolerance viewpoint, one must understand what factors contribute to the
loading on the spine structures and their corresponding tolerances. Once these factors are identified,
the proper assessment tools can be identified and developed to measure the levels of the specific risk
factors that would exceed the tolerances of the spine structures. Thus, assessment tools can only be
effective if they can accurately identify when tolerances are exceed either cumulatively or acutely
and, often times, must be multifactorial in nature.
Many factors in the workplace may contribute to the loading on the lower back. Some of these factors
are the weight of the object being lifted, height of origin and destination, horizontal distance (moment arm)
at origin and destination, task asymmetry at origin and destination, repetition (lift rate), mode of
exertion (lifting, squat lifting, stoop lifting, lowering, pushing, pulling, carrying, etc.), feet position,
28-1
28-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
coupling (handles), and box size (Kelsey et al., 1984; Kelsey and Golden, 1988; Marras et al., 1993, 1995;
Hales and Bernard, 1996; Burdorf and Sorock, 1997; Ferguson and Marras, 1997; National Academy of
Science, 2001; Neumann et al., 2001). Muscles generate internal forces that must offset the loads placed
on low back by these workplace (external) factors. Several factors relating to the internal response
(muscle force) impact the ability to counteract the forces and moments such as strength, the ability to
exert sufficient force; posture, the length–strength relationship; motion, the force–velocity relationship;
and endurance, the ability to resist fatigue. Together, the external demands and the internal muscle
response produce loads on the spine structures that ultimately produce the injury. Thus, a successful
low back risk assessment tool must account for many of these factors either by direct assessment or
using surrogate measures. The next seven sections of this chapter will discuss in detail the relationship
of various factors and the resulting loads on the spine and how these specific factors can be evaluated.
Finally, the role of psychosocial work characteristics and psychological responses will also be discussed
with respect to injury mechanism, assessment, and interaction with other factors.
FIGURE 28.2 Compression and anterior –posterior shear force is impacted by moment arm (horizontal distance)
and height of origin. (Marras et al. 2001, Spine.)
28-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
with low back pain in industry (Punnet et al., 1991; Marras et al., 1993, 1995; Ono et al., 1997; Brulin
et al., 1998; Josephson et al., 1998; Norman et al., 1998; Wickstrom and Pentti, 1998; Ozguler et al.,
2000). Further, Figure 28.2 also provides indirect evidence of the importance of trunk flexion by having
greater compression and anterior–posterior shear forces as the lift origin becomes lower (e.g., knee or
floor height). It would be expected that these lower regions would have more trunk flexion (based on pre-
vious literature).
Awkward postures (e.g., lateral flexion, twisting, and combination of flexion with twisting) also pose a
risk to workers with respect to low back pain (Andersson, 1981; Kelsey and Golden, 1988; Hales and
Bernard, 1996; Ferguson and Marras, 1997; Hoogendoorn et al., 1999). One factor that influences the
awkward postures is task asymmetry — producing greater lateral flexion and twisting (Ferguson et al.,
1992; Plamondon et al., 1995; Allread et al., 1996; Kingma et al., 1998; Marras and Davis, 1998;
Granata et al., 1999). Task asymmetry is the location of the object (or box) being lifted relative to the
midsagittal plane of the individual at the beginning or at the end of the lift (NIOSH, 1993). Increases
in nonsagittal plane motion (lateral and twist) will typically accompany more asymmetric tasks since
the box is located away from the midline of the sagittal plane, causing the individual to twist and
bend sideways. However, the magnitude of off-plane motion may be minimized by lower extremity
compensation (e.g., moving of feet or twisting of hips).
The posture of the worker can be quantified by several methods with varying levels of accuracy and
resource requirements (cost, time, effort, and knowledge) (Burdorf et al., 1997). The simplest method
is using a checklist or questionnaire that identifies designated regions (see Figure 28.3 for an
example). The basic checklist method uses diagrams as a reference to identify how far an individual
flexes forward, flexes laterally, or twists. One useful aid is to videotape the worker performing the job
so that repeated evaluations of the postures may be made. The drawback of this method is the subjectivity
or the ability to distinguish the actual posture of the individual, particularly twist postures. These
methods are effective for more gross assessment of posture.
For more accurate quantification of the posture, goniometers (also referred to as electrogoniometers
or potentiometers) measure joint position; in this case, the lower back. There are several forms of low
back goniometric systems (e.g., lumbar motion monitor, Isotechnologies Back Tracker) that are commer-
cially available, but each requires substantial monetary resources and expertise in application. One
system, the lumbar motion monitor (LMM), has been validated with a large industrial database that
1
2 2
0°
20° 20°
3
3
60°
L3/L4
FIGURE 28.3 One example of checklist diagrams for posture: Trunk flexion diagram from REBA checklist.
(Adapted from Hignett and McAtamney, 2000. With permission.)
Application of Ergonomics to the Low Back 28-5
is linked to actual injuries (Marras et al., 1993, 1995, 2000a). The LMM has also been validated with
respect to posture in three dimensions (Marras et al., 1992). Thus, the LMM is a potentially powerful
tool with respect to measuring the three-dimensional posture of the trunk but requires significant exper-
tise and monetary resources.
Snook and Ciriello, 1991). The Snook tables provide benchmarks for both sustained and initial push and
pull forces as a function of height of applied force (handle height), distance of object movement, and
frequency of push/pull.
The impact of carrying will depend upon the weight lifted and the length of time carrying (or dis-
tance). While it would be expected that the impact of carrying would be less than lifting, the risk may
increase with large amounts of weight or long distances. Again, the only current method to evaluate
carrying is the Snook tables (Snook and Ciriello, 1991).
One factor that may play an increasing role in the development of low back pain may be prolonged
standing. Since major lifting has been designed out of many workplaces, more static and upright tasks
have become prevalent. These types of tasks will include standing with limited walking or in a restricted
pattern causing static postures in the low back. One example of a prolonged standing is working on a drill
press or other machinery for long periods of time throughout the shift while an example of walking
in a restricted pattern is when a worker performs a task while walking along with the moving assembly
line. In both cases, the trunk oftentimes remains in a slightly flexed posture (static) for long durations.
As a result, muscles remain on for long durations, potentially eliciting pain from fatigue failure. Since
prolonged standing has been typically neglected as a potential risk factor, there are few methods to
assess this risk factor. One method is to use an ergonomic dosimeter (one device is the ActivPAL —
PAL Technologies Ltd, Glasgow, U.K.) that measures the amount of time standing without moving,
time walking, and time sitting.
and muscle activity (Flor et al., 1985; Bongers et al., 1993; Davis and Heaney, 2000); two recent studies
have actually documented such changes in response to mental stress (Marras et al., 2000b) and mental
concentration (Davis et al., 2002). A second potential mechanism is that the psychosocial factor changes
chemical reactions such as accumulation of metabolites in the muscles (Backus and Dudley, 1974) or
increased cortisol levels that make the muscle vulnerable to mechanical load (Theorell et al., 1993).
Finally, psychosocial factors may alter the reporting of low back pain in two ways: (1) by altering the
pain tolerance or threshold — increasing the likelihood of reporting (Theorell et al., 1991, 1993);
(2) by avoiding the work situation — report an injury when low back pain is not even severe or call
in sick (Frank et al., 1995). No matter what the reason for reporting, these variables must be accounted
for in any comprehensive evaluation.
There are many well-known questionnaires in literature that assess the various components of the psy-
chosocial environment and can be easily administrated to workers. Some examples, although not exhaus-
tive, are: work APGAR by Bigos et al. (1991), Generic Job Stress Questionnaire (Hurrell and McLaney,
1988), and Job Content Questionnaire designed by Karasek and Theorell (Karasek et al., 1998). With
any questionnaire, the reliability and validity of the questionnaire is dependent upon the questions
used (e.g., degree of subjectivity, ability to delineate the levels of exposure), the number of questions
assessing specific item (e.g., one question less reliable than multiple questions), and complexity of the
questions (e.g., language issues). Oftentimes, the effectiveness of the questionnaire is related to the
balance between the length and extensiveness and the time it takes to complete it. In most cases of
job assessment, time required away from the work must be minimized so the selection of which
factors, how many factors, and which questionnaire will be important since different questionnaires
require different amounts of time to complete.
References
ACGIH, 1996, Threshold Limit Values for Chemical and Physical Agents and Biological Exposure Indices:
Whole-Body Vibration. Cincinnati, OH: American Conference of Governmental Industrial
Hygienists, pp. 123 –131.
Allread, W.G., Marras, W.S., Parnianpour, M., 1996, Trunk kinematics of one-handed lifting, and the
effects of asymmetry and load weight. Ergonomics, 39(2), 322– 334.
Andersson, G.B.J., 1981, Epidemiologic aspects on low-back pain in industry. Spine, 6(1), 53–60.
Backus, F.I., Dudley, D.L., 1974, Observations of psychosocial factors and their relationship to organic
disease. International Journal of Psychiatry Medicine, 5, 499– 515.
Bigos, S.J., Battie, M.C., Spengler, D.M., Fisher, L.D., Fordyce, W.E., Hansson, T.H., Nachemson, A.L.,
Wortley, M.D., 1991, A prospective study of work perceptions and psychosocial factors affecting
the report of back injury. Spine, 16, 1– 6.
Bongers, P.M., de Winter, C.R., Kompier, M.A.J., Wildebrandt, V.H., 1993, Psychosocial factors at work
and musculoskeletal disease. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and Health, 19, 297–312.
Bovenzi, M. and Holshof C.J. 1999, An updated review of epidemiologic studies on the relationship
between exposure to whole-body vibration and low back pain (1986 –1997). International Archives
of Occupational and Environmental Health, 72(6), 351–365.
Bovenzi, M., Zadini, A., 1992, Self-reported low-back symptoms in urban bus drivers exposed to whole-
body vibration. Spine, 17, 1048–1059.
Brown, R., Li, G., 2003, The development of action levels for the “Quick Exposure Check” (QEC) system.
In: Contemporary Ergonomics, McCabe P.T., Ed., London: Taylor & Francis, pp. 41–46.
Brulin, C., Gerdle, B., Granlund B., Hoog, J., Knutson, A., Sundelin, G., 1998, Physical and psychosocial
work-related risk factors associated with musculoskeletal symptoms among home care personnel.
Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences, 12(2), 104–110.
Burdorf, A., Sorock, G., 1997, Positive and negative evidence of risk factors for back disorders.
Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and Health, 23, 243– 256.
Burdorf, A., Rossignol, M., Fathallah, F.A., Snook, S.H., Herrick, R.F., 1997, Challenges in assessing risk
factors in epidemiologic studies on back disorders. American Journal of Industrial Medicine, 32,
142– 152.
Buseck, M., Schipplein, O.D., Andersson, G.B.J., Andriachi, T.P., 1988, Influence of dynamic factors and
external loads on the moment at the lumbar spine in lifting. Spine, 13(8), 918–921.
Chaffin, D.B., Park, K.S., 1973, A longitudinal study of low-back pain as associated with occupational
weight lifting factors. American Industrial Hygiene Association Journal, 34, 513– 525.
Ciriello, V.M., Snook, S.H., Blick, A.C., Wilkenson, P.L., 1990, The effects of task duration on
psychophysically-determined maximum acceptable weights and forces. Ergonomics, 33(2),
187– 200.
Davis, K.G., Heaney, C.A., 2000, The relationship between psychosocial work characteristics and low
back pain: underlying methodological issues. Clinical Biomechanics, 15(6), 389 –406.
Davis, K.G., Marras, W.S., 2000a, Assessment of the relationship between box weight and trunk
kinematics: does a reduction in box weight necessarily correspond to a decrease in spinal loading?
Human Factors, 42(2), 195–208.
Davis, K.G., Marras, W.S., 2000b, The effects of motion on trunk biomechanics. Clinical Biomechanics,
15, 703– 717.
Davis, K.G., Marras, W.S., Waters, T.R., 1998, Evaluation of the spinal loading during lowering and
lifting, Clinical Biomechanics, 13(3), 141 –152.
Davis, K.G., Marras, W.S., Heaney, C.A., Waters, T.R., Gupta, P., 2002, The impact of mental processing
and pacing on spine loading, Spine, 27(23), 2645 –2653.
de Looze, M.P., Stassen, A.R.A., Markslag, A.M.T., Borst, M.J., Wooning, M.M., Toussaint, H.M.,
1995, Mechanical loading on the low-back in 3 methods of refuse collecting. Ergonomics, 38,
1993– 2006.
Application of Ergonomics to the Low Back 28-9
Dolan, P., Kingma, I., van Dieen, J., de Looze, M.P., Toussaint, H.M., Baten, C.T.M., Adams, M.A., 1999,
Dynamic forces acting on the lumbar spine during manual handling — can they be estimated
using electromyographic techniques alone? Spine, 24(7), 698–703.
Drury, C.G., Deeb, J.M., Hartman, B., Wooley, S., Drury, C.E., Gallagher, S., 1989, Symmetric and asym-
metric manual materials handling. Part 2: biomechanics. Ergonomics, 32(6), 565 –583.
Fathallah, F.A., Marras, W.S., Parnianpour, M., 1998, The role of complex, simultaneous trunk motions
in the risk of occupational-related low back disorders. Spine, 23(9), 1035 –1042.
Ferguson, S.A., Marras, W.S., 1997, A literature review of low back disorder surveillance measures and
risk factors. Clinical Biomechanics, 12(4), 211 –226.
Ferguson, S.A., Marras, W.S., Waters, T.R., 1992, Quantification of back motion during asymmetric
lifting. Ergonomics, 35(7/8), 845–859.
Flor, H., Turk, D.C., Birbaumer, N., 1985, Assessment of stress-related psychophysiological reactions in
chronic back pain patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 354 –364.
Frank, J.W., Pilcins, I.R., Kerr, M.S., Shannon, H.S., Stansfeld, S.A., 1995, Occupational back pain — an
unhelpful polemic. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and Health, 21, 3–14.
Freivalds, A., Chaffin, D.B., Garg, A., Lee, K.S., 1983, A dynamic biomechanical evaluation of lifting
maximum acceptable loads. Journal of Biomechanics, 18(8), 571 –584.
Gagnon, M., Beaugrand, S., Authier, M., 1992, The dynamics of pushing loads onto shelves of different
heights, International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 9, 1– 13.
Garg, A., Moore, J.S., 1992, Epidemiology of low-back pain in industry. Occupational Medicine: State of
the Art Review, 7, 593–608.
Granata, K.P., Marras, W.S., 1995, An EMG-assisted model of trunk loading during free-dynamic lifting.
Journal of Biomechanics, 28(11), 1309 –1317.
Granata, K.P., Marras, W.S., Davis, K.G., 1999, Variation in spinal load and trunk dynamics during
repeated lifting exertions. Clinical Biomechanics, 14(6), 367 –375.
Hales, T.R., Bernard, B.P., 1996, Epidemiology of work-related musculoskeletal disorders. Journal of
Orthopaedic Clinics of North of America, 27(4), 679–709.
Hoogendoorn, W.E., van Poppel, M.N.M., Bongers, P.M., Koes, B.W., Bouter, L.M., 1999, Physical load
during work and leisure time as risk factors for back pain. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environ-
ment and Health, 25(5), 387 –403.
Hoozemans, M.J.M., Van der Beek, A.J., Frings-Dresen, M.H.W., Van Dijk, F.J.H., Van der Woude, L.H.V.,
1998. Pushing and pulling in relation to musculoskeletal disorders: a review of risk factors.
Ergonomics, 41, 757–781.
Hurrell, J.J., McLaney, M.A., 1988, Exposure to job stress — a new psychometric instrument. Scandina-
vian Journal of Work, Environment and Health, 14, 27 –28.
International Standards Organization, 1985, Evaluation of Human Exposure to Whole-Body Vibration.
Geneva, Switzerland: ISO. Report No. ISO-2631.
Jager, M., Luttmann, A., 1989, Biomechanical analysis and assessment of lumbar stress during load lifting
using a dynamic 19-segment human model. Ergonomics, 32(1), 93 –112.
Josephson, M., Vingard, E., MUSIC-Norrtalje Study Group, 1998, Workplace factors and care seeking for
low-back pain among female nursing personnel. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and
Health, 24(6), 465 –472.
Karasek, R.A., Brisson, C., Kawakami, N., Houtman, I., Bongers, P., Amick, B., 1998, The Job Content
Questionnaire (JCQ): an instrument for internationally comparative assessments of psychosocial
job characteristics. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 3(4), 322–354.
Kelsey, J.L., Golden, A.L., 1988, Occupational and workplace factors associated with low back pain.
Journal of Occupational Medicine, 3(1), 7–16.
Kelsey, J.L., Githens, P.B., White, A.A., Holford, T.R., Walter, S.D., O’Conner, T., Ostfeld, A.M.,
Weil, U., Southwick, W.O., Calogero, J.A., 1984, An epidemiological study of lifting and twisting
on the job and risk for acute prolapsed lumbar intervertebral disc. Journal of Orthopaedic Research,
2, 61–66.
28-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Kingma, I., van Dieen, J.H., de Looze, M., Toussaint, H.M., Dolan, P., Baten, C.T.M., 1998, Asymmetric
low back loading in asymmetric lifting movements is not prevented by pelvic twist. Journal of
Biomechanics, 31, 527 –534.
Lings, S., Leboeuf-Yde, C., 2000, Whole-body vibration and low back pain: a systematic critical review of
the epidemiological literature 1992–1999. International Archives of Occupational and Environ-
mental Health, 73, 290– 297.
Marras, W.S., Davis, K.G., 1998, Spine loading during asymmetric lifting using one vs. two hands.
Ergonomics, 41(6), 817– 834.
Marras, W.S., Sommerich, C.M., 1991, A three dimensional motion model of loads on the lumbar spine:
II. Model validation. Human Factors, 33(2), 139 –149.
Marras, W.S., Fathallah, F.A., Miller, R.J., Davis, S.W., and Mirka, G.A., 1992, Accuracy of a three dimen-
sional lumbar motion monitor for recording dynamic trunk motion characteristics. International
Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 9, 75 –87.
Marras, W.S., Lavender, S.A., Leurgans, S.E., Rajulu, S.L., Allread, W.G., Fathallah, F.A., Ferguson, S.A.,
1993, The role of dynamic three-dimensional motion in occupationally-related low back disorders.
The effects of workplace factors, trunk position, and trunk motion characteristics on risk of injury.
Spine, 18(5), 617 –628.
Marras, W.S., Lavender, S.A., Leurgans, S.E., Fathallah, F.A., Ferguson, S.A., Allread, W.G., Rajulu, S.L.,
1995, Biomechanical risk factors for occupationally related low back disorders. Ergonomics, 38(2),
377– 410.
Marras, W.S., Granata, K.P., Davis, K.G., Allread, W.G., Jorgensen, M.J., 1997, Spine loading and prob-
ability of low back disorder risk as a function of box location on a pallet. Human Factors in Man-
ufacturing, 7(4), 323 –336.
Marras, W.S., Granata, K.P., Davis, K.G., Allread, W.G., Jorgensen, M.J., 1999b, The effects of box features
on spinal loading during warehouse order selecting. Ergonomics 42(7), 980 –996.
Marras, W.S., Allread, W.G., Burr, D.L., Fathallah, F.A., 2000a, Prospective validation of a low-back dis-
order risk model and assessment of ergonomic interventions associated with manual materials
handling tasks. Ergonomics, 43(11), 1866 –1886.
Marras, W.S., Davis, K.G., Heaney, C.A., Maronitis, A.B., Allread, W.G., 2000b, The influence of psycho-
social stress, gender, and personality on mechanical loading of the lumbar spine, Spine, 25(23),
3045– 3054.
Marras, W.S., Davis, K.G., Ferguson, S.A., Lucas, B.R., Gupta, P., 2001, Spine loading characteristics of
low back pain patients compared to asymptomatic individuals, Spine, 26(23), 2566 –2574.
McGill, S.M., 1997, The biomechanics of low back injury: implications on current practice in industry
and the clinic, Journal of Biomechanics, 30(5), 465–475.
National Academy of Science, 2001, Musculoskeletal Disorders and the Workplace: Low Back and Upper
Extremities. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health, 1981, Work Practices Guide for Manual Lifting.
NIOSH Technical Report 81 –122. [Cincinnati, OH:] U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services, National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health.
National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health, 1993, Application Manual for the Revised Lifting
Equation. NIOSH Technical Report 94 –110. [Cincinnati, OH:] U.S. Department of Health and
Human Services, National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health.
Neumann, W.P., Wells, R.P., Norman, R.W., Frank, J., Shannon, H., Kerr, M.S., OUBPS Working Group,
2001, A posture load sampling approach to determining low-back pain risk in occupational set-
tings. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 27, 65–77.
Norman, R., Wells, R., Neumann, P., Frank, J., Shannon, H., Kerr, M., The Ontario Universities Back Pain
Study (OUBPS) Group, 1998, A comparison of peak vs cumulative physical work exposure risk
factors for the reporting of low back pain in the automotive industry, Clinical Biomechanics, 13,
561– 573.
Ono, Y., Shimaoka, M., Hiruta, S., Takeuchi, Y., 1997, Low back pain among cooks in nursery schools.
Industrial Health, 35(2), 194 –201.
Application of Ergonomics to the Low Back 28-11
Ozguler, A., Leclerc, A., Landre, M.-F., Pietri-Taleb, F., Niedhammer, I., 2000, Individual and
occupational determinants of low back pain according to various definitions of low back
pain. Epidemiology and Community Health, 54, 215 – 220.
Plamondon, A., Gagnon, A., Gravel, D., 1995, Moments at the L5/S1 joint during asymmetrical lifting:
effects of different load trajectories and initial positions. Clinical Biomechanics, 10, 128 –136.
Punnet, L., Fine, L.J., Keyserling, W.M., Herrin, G.D., Chaffin, D.B., 1991, Back disorders and non-
neutral trunk postures of automobile assembly workers. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environ-
ment and Health, 17, 337 –346.
Resnick, M.L., Chaffin, D.B., 1996, Kinematics, kinetics, and psychophysical perceptions in symmetric
and twisting pushing and pulling tasks, Human Factors, 38, 114–129.
Schultz, A.B., Andersson, G.B.J., Ortengren, R., Haderspeck, K.K., Nachemson, A.L., 1982a, Loads on the
lumbar spine — validation of a biomechanical analysis by measurements of intradiscal pressures
and myoelectric signals. Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery — American Volume, 64(5), 713–720.
Schultz, A.B., Andersson, G.B.J., Ortengren, R., Bjork, R., Nordin, M., 1982b, Analysis and quantitative
myoelectric measurements of loads on the lumbar spine when holding weights in standing pos-
tures. Spine 7(4), 390 –397.
Snook, S.H., Ciriello, V.M., 1991, The design of manual handling tasks: revised tables of maximum accep-
table weights and forces. Ergonomics, 34(9), 1197 –1213.
Snook, S.H., Campanelli, R.A., Hart, J.W., 1978. A study of three preventive approaches to low back
injury. Journal of Occupational Medicine, 20, 478–481.
Theorell, T., Harms-Ringdahl, K., Ahiberg-Hulten, G., Westin, B., 1991, Psychosocial job factors and
symptoms from the locomotor system — a multicausal analysis. Scandinavian Journal of Rehabi-
litation Medicine, 23, 165 –173.
Theorell, T., Nordemar, R., Michelsen, H., Stockholm Music I Study Group, 1993, Pain thresholds during
standardized psychological stress in relation to perceived psychosocial work situation. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 37(3), 299– 305.
Washington State Ergonomics Standard 296-62-051, http://www.lni.wa.gov/wisha/Rules/
generaloccupationalhealth/HTML/ergowac.htm
Wickstrom, G.J., Pentti, J., 1998, Occupational factors affecting sick leave attributed to low-back pain.
Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and Health, 24(3), 145–152.
29
Application of
Ergonomics to the Legs
29.1 Introduction
Most of the ergonomics literature dealing with the prevention and control of musculoskeletal disorders
in the workplace has focused on the upper extremity and the back. Comparatively, little attention has
been given to lower extremity musculoskeletal disorders that occur in the workplace. One could argue
that since lower extremity problems are not well documented in ergonomics journals, the problems
may not be of much practical significance. The first objective of this chapter is to review the current lit-
erature regarding occupational musculoskeletal disorders affecting the lower extremities and to demon-
strate the significance of the problem. The second objective is to describe what types of intervention
strategies are available to minimize the likelihood of future or recurrent injuries to the feet, angles,
knees, and hips.
29-1
29-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
injury (Kohatsu and Schurman, 1990; Holmberg et al., 2004). Sahlstrom and Montgomery (1997) report
that knee OA was weakly associated with weight-bearing knee bending, which increases the dynamic load
on the knee when bending. In fact, when corrected for confounders, weight-bearing knee bending was no
longer significant. Being overweight was a significant risk factor, however. Likewise, Coggon et al. (2000)
found that obesity, defined as a BMI greater than or equal to 30 kg/m2, and prolonged kneeling or squat-
ting on the job combined to substantially increase the risk of knee OA. Kohatsu and Schurman (1990)
found no relationship between leisure time activities and knee OA. Baker et al. (2003) report that while
occupational activities contribute to the hospital referrals for knee symptoms, participation in soccer
substantially increases the risk of knee cartilage injury.
Torner et al. (1990) reported that chronic prepatellar bursitis was the predominant knee disorder in
120 fishermen who underwent an orthopedic physical examination. Forty-eight percent of the men
examined showed this disorder. Interestingly, the finding was as common among the younger men as
in the older men. The authors believe that this disorder is a secondary effect of the boat’s motion.
The knees are used to stabilize the body by pressing against gunwales or machinery as tasks are performed
with the upper extremities. Furthermore, just standing in mild sea conditions (maximum roll angles
of 88) has been shown to considerably elevate the moments at the knees as the motion in the lower extre-
mities and the trunk are the primary means for counteracting a ship’s motions (Torner et al., 1991).
Kivimaki (1992a) reported an increased thickness in the prepatellar or infrapatellar bursa was much
more common in carpet and floor layers than in a reference group of house painters. Carpet and
floor layers also experienced greater laxity in the knee joint (Kivimaki et al., 1994a), had more osteo-
phytes of the patella (Kivimaki et al., 1992b), and more frequently reported prior knee conditions
than house painters (Kivimaki, 1994b).
Several musculoskeletal colloquialisms have been used to describe occupationally related knee con-
ditions including “beat knee,” “carpet-layer knee,” “preacher knee,” and “housemaid knee” (Lee et al.,
2004). Housemaid knee is an inflammation of the prepatellar bursa whereas preacher knee is an infra-
patellar bursitis that is associated with excessive kneeling (Lee et al., 2004). The etiology of “beat
knee” was described by Sharrard (1963), who reported on the examination of 579 coal miners. Forty
percent of those examined were symptomatic or had previously experienced symptoms. Most of the inju-
ries could be characterized as acute simple bursitis or chronic simple bursitis. The majority of the affected
miners were colliers whose job requires constant kneeling at the mine face. There was a strong relation-
ship between the coal seam height (directly related to roof height in a mine) and the incidence of beat
knee. The incidence rates were much higher in mines with a roof height under 4 ft as compared with
those with greater roof heights. Obviously, this factor greatly affects the work posture of the miners.
With higher roof heights miners can alternate between stooped and kneeling postures but when
seams are 1 m or less, the stooped posture is no longer an alternative. Gallagher and Unger (1990),
for example, present recommendations for weight limits of handled materials in underground mines.
Below 1.02 m these are based on miners in kneeling postures. Sharrard (1963) also speculated on the
individual factors attributable to the disorder and found a higher incidence among younger men.
However, this may be due to the “healthy worker effect” (Andersson, 1991) in which older miners
with severe “beat knee” have left the mining occupation.
Tanaka et al. (1982) reported that the occupational morbidity ratios for workers’ compensation claims
of knee-joint inflammation among carpet installers was twice that found in tile setters and floor layers,
and was over 13 times greater than that of carpenters, sheet metal workers, and tinsmiths. Others have
shown the knees of those involved with carpet and flooring installation were more likely to have fluid
collections in the superficial infrapatellar bursa, have a subcutaneous thickening in the anterior wall
of the superficial infrapatellar bursa, and have an increased thickness in the subcutaneous prepatellar
region (Myllymaki et al., 1993).
Thun et al. (1987) determined the incidence of repetitive knee trauma in the flooring installation pro-
fessions. While all flooring installers spend a large amount of time kneeling, the authors divided the 154
survey respondents into two groups, “tilesetters” and “floor layers,” based on their use of a “knee kicker.”
This device is used to stretch the carpet during the installation process. These respondents were
29-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
compared with a group of millwrights and brick layers whose jobs did not require extended kneeling or
the use of a knee kicker. Of the 112 floor layers (those who used the knee kicker), the prevalence rate of
bursitis was approximately twice that found in the 42 tilesetters, and over three times that found in the
243 millwrights and brick layers. However, the prevalence in both groups of flooring workers of having
required needle aspiration of the knee was almost five times that of millwrights and bricklayers. These
results suggest that long durations of occupational kneeling is related to fluid accumulation, yet the
bursitis is due to the repetitive trauma endured by the floor layers using the knee kicker. Similar findings
were obtained by Jensen et al. (2000), who reported that the percentage of time performing “knee strain-
ing work” for floor layers, carpenters, and compositors (56, 26, and 0%, respectively) was positively
correlated with knee complaints. It is also important to note that these authors found age, seniority,
weight, BMI, smoking, and knee-straining sports were not significant covariates in the their analyses.
Village et al. (1991, 1993) found that the peak impulse forces generated in the knees of carpetlayers
when using the “knee kicker” were on the order of 3000 N. The opposite knee that was supporting
the body during this action had an average peak force of 893 N. Bhattacharya et al. (1985) reported
knee impact forces of 2469 N (about three times body weight) for a light kick and 3019 N (or about
four times body weight) for a hard kick. These light and hard kicks resulted in impact decelerations
of 12.3 and 20 g, respectively. The authors observed that the knee kicking action during flooring instal-
lation occurred at a rate of 141 kicks per hour. However, putting the knee injuries in perspective, pain was
reported by 22% of questionnaire respondents in the tufting job in a carpet manufacturer. However,
knees were only listed in 2.4% of the accident records. Thus, the knee is frequently the site of discomfort,
although there may be few lost days associated with knee pain (Tellier and Montreuil, 1991).
In summary, several occupational risk factors have been identified which place an employee at
increased risk for disorders in the lower extremity. The literature has shown that heavy physical labor
and frequent knee bending are factors, especially in the older component of the workforce, thereby
suggesting an interaction between the age degenerative processes and cumulative work experience.
And clearly, the role of direct cumulative trauma in those employees who must maintain kneeling pos-
tures and use their knees to strike objects (knee kicker) cannot be overlooked when considering preven-
tive measures.
frequencies in the gastrocnemius and anterior tibialis muscle over the trial period, the EMG median fre-
quencies in these muscles were not affected by the use of floor mats. The median frequency shift in the
erector spinae was reduced when subjects stood on the thinner and more compressible mat. The authors
hypothesized that greater compressibility would have made for a less stable base of support, thereby
requiring more frequent postural changes in the trunk to overcome the destabilization associated with
postural sway. Thus, the dynamic use of erector spinae muscles to correct for postural sway would facili-
tate the oxygen delivery and the removal of contractile byproducts through increased blood flow. A
further test of this hypothesis would evaluate whether this motion occurred only in the trunk, or if it
occurred in the lower extremities that did not show the spectral shift due to the floor condition.
Redfern and Cham’s (2000) review of the data on antifatigue floor mats suggests that there is a con-
sistent reduction of discomfort with matting when compared with hard floor conditions. Unfortunately,
the objective measures are less conclusive across studies. In part this may be due to variations in the phys-
ical properties of the mats tested. Cham and Redfern (2001) report that fatigue and discomfort were
reduced when floor mats had increased elasticity, decreased energy absorption, and increased stiffness;
however, the changes in these subjective responses were only detectable after 3 h of standing.
polymer insole and a standard mesh insole that were issued by platoon to over 3000 marine recruits.
While the polymer insole had good shock absorbing properties, the incidence rate of lower extremity
stress injuries over the 12 week basic training program were unaffected by the insole used. Schwellnus
et al. (1990) found that the mean weekly incidence of total overuse injuries and tibial stress syndrome
decreased significantly in the 237 military recruits provided with neoprene insoles as compared to
1151 controls. These authors also found a trend (p , 0.10) suggesting that the incidence of stress fractures
was reduced with the insoles. However, Jones et al. (2002), in their review of the military intervention
studies investigating insoles, do not find convincing evidence that shock absorbent insoles prevent stress
fractures, thus suggesting they are of questionable value if the intended purpose is solely stress fracture
prevention. Rome et al. (2005), in their review of the research examining the potential for shock
absorbing shoe insoles to prevent injuries in military recruits, conclude that there is evidence to
support their use; however, the strongest supporting trial included in their review had some methodo-
logical issues.
Several studies have been conducted to evaluate variations in insole materials. Leber and Evanski
(1986) describe the characteristics of the following seven insole materials: Plastazote, Latex foam,
Dynafoam, Ortho felt, Spenco, Molo, and PPT. These authors measured the plantar pressures in 26
patients with forefoot pain. All insole materials reduced the plantar pressure by between 28 and 53%
relative to a control condition; however, PPT, Plastazote, and Spenco were the superior products. Viscolas
and Poron were found to have the best shock absorbency of the five insole materials tested by Pratt et al.
(1986). Maximum plantar pressures were found to be significantly reduced in the forefoot region with
PPT, Spenco, and Viscolas, although the three materials were not significantly different (McPoil and
Cornwall, 1992). In the rear foot region, however, McPoil and Cornwall (1992) report that only the
PPT and the Spenco insoles reduced the maximum plantar pressure relative to the barefoot condition.
The plantar pressure in the rearfoot region was not significantly reduced with the Viscolas. Interestingly,
based on the shock absorbency data from Pratt’s (1988) 30-day durability test, the resilience of Viscolas,
PPT, and Plastazote could be described as excellent, good, and poor, respectively. Sanfilippo et al. (1992)
also reported the change in foot to ground contact area as a function of insole material. Plastazote,
Spenco, and PPT led to a significantly greater contact area than the other materials tested.
In summary, insoles appear to be effective at modifying the lower extremity kinematics and reducing
the peak plantar pressures, although their effectiveness is dependent upon the material used. Additional
research is needed to clarify the effectiveness of insoles in controlling lower extremity stress injuries.
Based on the previous discussion it should be clear that the effectiveness of this control strategy will
be dependent upon shock absorbing capacity, the pressure dispersion, and the durability properties of
the insole materials selected.
have proposed using imaging methods to derive knee anthropometric features that could be used to
optimize knee pad design (Pellmann and Thumler, 1992). Moreover, the pads also have to be evaluated
for their impact on comfort and performance of nonkneeling tasks; for example, tasks that require
standing and walking. This is important for individuals who work in a variety of low extremity
postures throughout the day. If the knee pads do not accommodate walking and standing, for example,
they will likely not be used where they should be.
Ringen et al. (1995) report of a new tool to reduce the knee and back trauma in those who tie rebar
rods together in preparation for pouring concrete. No longer will concrete workers need to kneel or stoop
for extended periods to interconnect the iron rods as this tool allows the operator to work in a standing
posture.
Powered carpet stretching tools are available to remove the repeated trauma experienced by carpet
layers. Village et al. (1993) have provided design guidelines for improved carpet stretching devices.
However, the widespread implementation of improved devices depends upon educating flooring
workers on the trade-offs between the additional time necessary to operate the tool and the knee dis-
orders associated with the conventional technique.
29.4 Summary
Ergonomics texts historically have focused relatively little attention on the prevention of lower extremity
disorders or the accommodation of individuals returning to work whom have experienced a lower extre-
mity disorder. In part this may be due to lesser appreciation of the frequency and severity of occupational
lower extremity disorders. Unlike many back or upper extremity disorders, which have their origins in
the repeated stresses placed on muscular, tendinous, and ligamentous tissues, many of the occupational
lower extremity disorders occur through direct compression of the body tissues by a surface in the
environment. As a result the occupational lower extremity disorders often involve cartilaginous tissue
and bone. Therefore, accommodation and prevention of these disorders occur primarily through the
optimization of the body’s contact with surfaces in the environment. This chapter, in addition to high-
lighting some of the epidemiological findings relevant to occupational lower extremity disorders, has
reviewed some of the more common intervention pathways available.
References
Anderson, E.G. (1990). Fatigue fractures of the foot. Injury, 21, 275– 279.
Anderson, J.J., Felson, D.T. (1988). Factors associated with osteoarthritis of the knee in the first national
health and nutrition examination survey (HANES I). American Journal of Epidemiology, 128, 179 –
189.
Andersson, G.B.J. (1991). The epidemiology of spinal disorders. In: J.W. Frymoyer (ed.) The Adult Spine:
Principles and Practice. New York: Raven, pp. 107 –146.
Andres, R.O., O’Conner, D., Eng, T. (1992). A practical synthesis of biomechanical results to prevent slips
and falls in the workplace. In: S. Kumar (ed.) Advances in Industrial Ergonomics and Safety IV.
London: Taylor & Francis, pp. 1001 –1006.
Baker, P., Reading, I., Cooper, C., Coggon, D. (2003). Knee disorders in the general population and their
relation to occupation. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 60, 794–797.
Bhattacharya, A., Mueller, M., Putz-Anderson, V. (1985). Traumatogenic factors affecting the knees of
carpet installers. Applied Ergonomics, 16, 243 –250.
Cham, R., Redfern, M.S. (2001). Effects of flooring on standing comfort and fatigue. Human Factors,
43, 381– 391.
Chen, J.C., Dennerlein, J.T., Shih, T.S., Chen, C.J., Cheng, Y., Chang, W.P., Ryan, L.M., Christiani, D.C.
(2004). Knee pain and driving duration: a secondary analysis of the taxi drivers’ health study.
American Journal of Public Health, 94, 575 –581.
Application of Ergonomics to the Legs 29-9
Coggon, D., Croft, P., Kellingray, S., Barrett, D., McLaren, M., Cooper, C. (2000). Occupational physical
activities and osteoarthritis of the knee. Arthritis & Rheumatism, 43, 1443 –1449.
Cook, J., Branch, T.P., Baranowski, T.J., Hutton,W.C. (1993). The effect of surgical floor mats in pro-
longed standing: an EMG study of the lumbar paraspinal and anterior tibialis muscles. Journal
of Biomedical Engineering, 15, 247–250.
Cowan, D.N., Jones, B.H., Frykman, P.N., Polly, D.W., Harman, E.A., Rosenstein, R.M., Rosenstein, M.T.
(1996). Lower limb morphology and risk of overuse injury among male infantry trainees. Medicine
and Science in Sports and Exercise, 28, 945 –952.
Craig, B.N., Congleton, J.J., Kerk, C.J., Amendola, A.A., Gaines, W.G., Jenkins, O.C. (2003). A prospective
field study of the relationship of potential occupational risk factors with occupational injury/
illness. AIHA Journal, 64, 376–387.
Crenshaw, S., Fabian, E., Calton, E. (2000). Effects of lateral-wedged insoles on kinetics at the knee.
Clincial Orthopaedics and Related Research, 375, 185 –192.
Felson, D.T., Nannan, M.T., Naimark, A., Berkeley, J., Gordon, G., Wilson, P.W., Andersson, J. (1991).
Occupational physical demands, knee bending, and knee osteoarthritis: results from the Framingham
study. Journal of Rheumatology, 18, 1587–1592.
Frederick, E.C. (1986). Kinematically mediated effects of sport shoe design: a review. Journal of Sports
Sciences, 4, 169–184.
Gallagher, S., Unger, R.L. (1990). Lifting in four restricted lifting conditions. Applied Ergonomics, 21,
237–245.
Gardner, L.I., Dziados, J.E., Jones, B.H., Brundage, J.F. (1988). Prevention of lower extremity stress
fractures: a controlled trial of a shock absorbent insole. American Journal of Public Health, 78,
1663–1567.
Giladi, M., Ahronson, Z., Stein, M., Danon, Y.L., Milgrom, C. (1985). Unusual distribution and onset of
stress fractures in soldiers. Clinical Orthopaedics and Related Research, 192, 142– 146.
Giladi, M., Milgrom, C., Simkin, A., Danon, Y. (1991). Stress fractures. Identifiable risk factors. American
Journal of Sports Medicine, 19, 647–652.
Greaney, R.B., Gerber, F.H., Laughlin, R.L., Kmet, J.P., Metz, C.D., Kilsheski, T.S., Rao, B.R., Silverman, E.D.
(1983). Distribution and natural history of stress fractures in U.S. Marine recruits. Radiology, 146,
339–346.
Hinnen, P., Konz, S. (1994). Fatigue mats. In: F. Aghazadeh (ed.) Advances in Industrial Ergonomics and
Safety VI. London: Taylor & Francis, pp. 323–327.
Holmberg, S., Thelin, A., Thelin, N. (2004). Is there an increased risk of knee osteoarthritis among
farmers? A population-based case –control study. International Archives of Occupational and
Environmental Health, 77, 345–350.
Jarvholm, B., Lundstrom, R., Malchu, H., Rehn, B., Vingard, E. (2004). Osteoarthritis in the hip and
whole-body vibration in heavy vehicles. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental
Health, 77, 424– 426.
Jensen, L.K., Mikkelsen, S., Loft, I.P., Eenberg, W. (2000). Work-related knee disorders in floor layers and
carpenters. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 42, 835 –842.
Jones, B.H., Thacker, S.B., Gilchrist, J., Kimsey, C.D. Jr., Sosin, D.M. (2002). Prevention of lower extremity
stress fractures in athletes and soldiers: a systematic review. Epidemiologic Reviews, 24, 228–247.
Jordaan, G., Schwellnus, M.P. (1994). The incidence of overuse injuries in military recruits during basic
military training. Military Medicine, 159, 421 –426.
Kim, J.Y., Stuart-Buttle C., Marras, W.S. (1994). The effects of mats on back and leg fatigue. Applied
Ergonomics, 25, 29–34.
King, P.M. (2002). A comparison of the effects of floor mats and shoe in-soles on standing fatigue.
Applied Ergonomics, 33, 477–484.
Kiuru, M.J., Niva, M., Reponen, A., Pihlajamaki, H.K. (2005). Bone stress injuries in asymtomatic elite
recruits: a clinical and magnetic resonance imaging study. The American Journal of Sports Medicine,
33, 272–276.
29-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Kivimaki, J. (1992a). Occupationally related ultrasonic findings in carpet and floor layers’ knees.
Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and Health, 18, 400– 402.
Kivimaki, J. (1992b). Knee disorders in carpet and floor layers and painters. Scandinavian Journal of
Work, Environment and Health, 18, 310 –316.
Kivimaki, J., Hanninen, K., Kujala, U.M., Osterman, K., Riihimaki, H. (1994a). Knee laxity in carpet and
floor layers and painters. Annales Chirurgiae et Gynaecologie, 83, 229 –233.
Kivimaki, J., Riihimaki, H., Hanninen, K. (1994b). Knee disorders in carpet and floor layers and painters.
Part II: knee symptoms and patellofemoral indicies. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment
and Health, 26, 97 –101.
Kohatsu, N.D., Schurman, D.J. (1990). Risk factors for the development of osteoarthrosis of the knee.
Clinical Orthopaedics and Related Research, 261, 242– 246.
Kuorinka, I., Hakkanen, S., Nieminen, K., Saari, J. (1978). Comparison of floor surfaces for standing
work. In: E. Asmussen and K. Jorgensen (eds.) Biomechanics VI-B, Proceedings of the Sixth
International Congress of Biomechanics. Baltimore: University Park Press, pp. 207 –211.
Leber, C., Evanski, P.M. (1986). A comparison of shoe insole materials in plantar pressure relief. Prosthetics
and Orthotics International, 10, 135–138.
Lee, P., Hunter, T.B., Taljanovic, M. (2004). Musculoskeletal Colloquialisms: How did we come up with
these names? RadioGraphics, 23, 1009–1027.
Lehman, W.L. (1984). Overuse syndromes in runners. American Family Physician, 29, 157 –161.
Lindberg, H., Axmacher, B. (1988). Coxarthrosis in farmers. Acta Orthopaedica Scandinauica, 59, 607.
Lindberg H., Montgomery, F. (1987). Heavy labor and the occurrence of gonarthrosis. Clinical Orthopae-
dics and Related Research, 214, 235 –236.
Linenger, J.M., Shwayhat, A.F. (1992). Epidemiology of podiatric injuries in US Marine recruits
undergoing basic training. Journal of the American Podiatric Medical Association, 82, 269 –271.
Loy, D.J., Voloshin, A.S. (1991). Biomechanics of stair walking and jumping. Journal of Sports Sciences, 9,
136– 149.
Madeleine, P., Voigt, M., Arendt-Nielsen, L. (1997). Subjective, physiological, and biomechanical
response to prolonged manual work performed standing on hard and soft surfaces. European
Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology, 77, 1 –9.
Manninen, P., Heliovaara, M., Riihimaki, H., Suoma-Iainen, O. (2002). Physical workload and the risk
factors of severe knee osteoarthritis. Scandinavian Journal Work, Environment and Health, 28, 25–32.
McGlothlin, J.D. (1996). Ergonomic Interventions for the Soft Drink Beverage Delivery Industry. U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services (NIOSH) Publication No. 96 –109.
McKenzie, D.C., Clement, D.B., Taunton, J.E. (1985). Running shoes, orthotics, and injuries. Sports
Medicine, 2, 334 –347.
McPoil, T.G., Cornwall, M.W. (1992). Effect of insole material on force and plantar pressures during
walking. Journal of the American Podiatric Medical Association, 82, 412– 416.
Milgrom, C., Finestone, A., Shlamkovitch, N., Wosk, J., Laor, A., Voloshin, A., Eldad, A. (1992). Preven-
tion of overuse injuries of the foot by improved shoe shock attenuation. A randomized prospective
study. Clinical Orthopaedics and Related Research, 281, 189 –192.
Myllymaki, T., Tikkakoski, T., Typpo, T., Kivimaki, J., Suramo, I. (1993). Carpet-layer’s knee. An ultra-
sonographic study. Acta Radiologica, 34, 496–499.
Orlando, A.R., King, M. (2004). Relationship of demographic variables on perception of fatigue and dis-
comfort following prolonged standing under various flooring conditions. Journal of Occupational
Rehabilitation, 14, 63–76.
Pellmann, P., Thumler, P. (1992). Ergonomic design of knee pads. Wirtschaftsverlag NW, Postfach 10 11 10,
Am Alten Hafen 113–115, D-W-2850 Bremerhaven 1, Germany.
Pinshaw, R., Atlas, V., Noakes, T.D. (1984). The nature and response to therapy of 196 consecutive
injuries seen at a runners’ clinic. Sourth African Medical Journal, 65, 291 –298.
Pratt, D.J. (1988). Medium term comparison of shock attenuating insoles using a spectral analysis tech-
nique. Journal of Biomedical Engineering, 10, 426 –429.
Application of Ergonomics to the Legs 29-11
Pratt, D.J., Rees, P.H., Rodgers, C. (1986). Assessment of some shock absorbing insoles. Prosthetics and
Orthotics International, 19, 43–45.
Redfern, M.S., Cham, R. (2000). The influence of flooring on standing comfort and fatigue. AIHA
Journal, 61, 700 –708.
Ringen, K., Englund, A., Seegal, J. (1995). Construction workers. In: B.S. Levey and D.H. Wegman (eds.)
Occupational Health: Recognizing and Preventing Work-Related Disease, Boston: Little, Brown, and
Company, pp. 685– 701.
Rome, K, Handoll, H.H.G., Ashford, R. (2005). Interventions for preventing and treating stress fractures
and stress reactions of bone of the lower limbs in young adults. The Cochrane Database of Systema-
tic Reviews, Issue 2. Art. No.: CD000450.pub2. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD000450.pub2.
Rys, M., Konz, S. (1989). An evaluation of floor surfaces. In: The Proceedings of the Human Factors Society
33th Annual Meeting, Vol. 1, pp. 517 –520.
Rys, M., Konz, S. (1990). Floor mats. In: The Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 34th Annual
Meeting, Vol. 1, pp. 575–579.
Sahlstrom, A., Montgomery, F. (1997). Risk analysis of occupational factors influencing the development
of arthrosis of the knee. European Journal of Epidemiology, 13, 675 –679.
Sanfilippo, P.B., Stess, R.M., Moss, K.M. (1992). Dynamic plantar pressure analysis. Comparing common
insole materials. Journal of the American Podiatric Medical Association, 82, 502 –513.
Sasaki. T., Yasuda, K. (1987). Clinical evaluation of the treatment of osteoarthritic knees using a newly
designed wedged insole. Clinical Orthopaedics and Related Research, 221, 181 –187.
Schwellnus, M.P., Jordaan, G., Noakes, T.D. (1990). Prevention of common overuse injuries by the use of
shock absorbing insoles. A prospective study. The American Journal of Sports Medicine, 18, 636 –641.
Sharrard, W.J.W. (1963). Aetiology and pathology of beat knee. British Journal of Industrial Medicine, 20,
24–31.
Tanaka, S., Smith, A.B., Halperin, W., Jensen, R. (1982). Carpet-layers knee. The New England Journal of
Medicine, 307, 1276–1277.
Tellier, C., Montreuil, S. (1991) Pain felt by workers and musculoskeletal injuries: assessment relating to
tufting shops in the carpet industry. In: Y. Queinnec and F. Daniellou (eds.) Designing for Everyone,
Taylor and Francis, Vol. 1, pp. 287 –289.
Thun, M., Tanaka, S., Smith, A.B., Haperin, W.E., Lee, S.T., Luggen, M.E., Hess, E.V. (1987). Morbidity
from repetitive knee trauma in carpet and floor layers. British Journal of Medicine, 44, 611–620.
Torner, M., Zetterberg, C., Hansson, T., Lindell, V. (1990). Musculo-skeletal symptoms and signs and
isometric strength among fishermen. Ergonomics, 33, 1155–1170.
Torner, M., Almstrom, C., Karlsson, R., Kadefors, R. (1991). Biomechanical calculations of musculo-
skeletal load caused by ship motions, in combination with work, on board a fishing vessel.
In: Y. Queinnec and F. Daniellou (eds.) Designing for Everyone, Taylor & Francis, London,
Vol. 1, pp. 293 –295.
Village, J., Morrison, J.B., Leyland, A. (1991). Carpetlayers and typesetters ergonomic analysis of work
procedures and equipment. In: Y. Queinnec and F. Daniellou (eds.) Designing for Everyone,
Taylor & Francis, London, Vol. 1, pp. 320–322.
Village, J., Morrison, J.B., Leyland, A. (1993). Biomechanical comparison of carpet stretching devices.
Ergonomics, 36, 899–909.
Vingard, E., Alfredsson, L., Goldie, I., Hogstedt, C. (1991). Occupation and osteoarthrosis of the hip and
knee: a register-based cohort study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 20, 1025 –1031.
Vingard, E., Alfredsson, L., Fellenius, E., Christer, H. (1992). Disability pensions due to musculo-skeletal
disorders among men in heavy occupations. Scandinavian Journal of Social Medicine, 20, 31–36.
Yasuda, K., Sasaki, T. (1987). The mechanics of treatment of the osteoarthritic knee with a wedged insole.
Clinical Orthopaedics and Related Research, 215, 162–172.
Yooshimura, N., Sasaki, S., Iwasaki, K., Danjoh, S., Kinoshita, H., Yasuda, T., Tamaki, T., Hashimoto, T.,
Kellingray, S., Croft, P., Coggon, D., Cooper, C. (2000). Occupational lifting is associated with hip
osteoarthritis: a Japanese case –control study. Journal of Rheumatology, 27, 434 –440.
30
Application of
Ergonomics of the
Foot
30.1 Foot/Leg
30.1.1 Anatomy
Figure 30.1 shows the bones of the foot and ankle. The toes (foot fingers) are divided into metatarsals and
three phalanges (except for the big toe, which only has two phalanges). In supporting the body, the calcaneus
(heel) supports 50% of the weight, the first and second metatarsals 25%, and the third, fourth, and fifth meta-
tarsals 25%. In between are two arches: (1) the medial arch (calcaneus, the talus, the navicular, the cuneiform
bones, and the first, second, and third metatarsals) and (2) the lateral arch (calcaneus, talus, cuboid, and the
fourth and fifth metatarsals). The plantar facia is a fibrous tissue that forms the arch underneath your foot
from the heel to the toes. If it weakens, the facia can cause pain to either the heel end or the toe end.
Under the heel (calcaneus) is a very important shock absorber, the heel pad (about 1.8 cm thick). The
bottom of the calcaneus is not spherical but has two small “mountains”; the pad reduces the pressure on
these mountains, and thus on the ankle, knee, and back.
The foot is connected to the ankle with a mortise and tenon joint. The vertical leg of the mortise is short
on the outside (lateral side); in addition, the ligaments holding the bottom of the fibula (lateral malleo-
lus) to the talus and calcaneus are relatively weak. In contrast, the vertical leg of the inside (medial)
mortise is longer, and the ligaments holding the bottom of the tibia (medial malleolus) to the talus
are relatively strong.
This chapter is a concise version of material in Konz and Johnson (2004).
30-1
30-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Cuboid Tibla
Ankle joint
Navicular
Talus (astragalus)
Tubercle, Base, Sub-Talar Joint
Fifth Metatarsal Navicular
Cuniform
Metatarsal Calcaneus (os calcis)
Phalanges (proximal) Metatarsals
Phanlanges (middle) Phalanges
Phalanges (distal)
Mortise
Fibula
Tibia
Tenon Tibio-Fibular
Ligament
Ankle Joint
Medial Malleolus Talus
Medial Collateral Lateral Malleolus
Ligament Lateral Collateral
Sub-Talar Joint Ligament (3 bands)
Calcaneus
FIGURE 30.1 The foot and ankle. The right foot is viewed from below (top left) and the outside (top right); the left
ankle (bottom) is viewed from the front.
Inward rotation (inversion) of the foot tends to pull the ligaments from the bone. With proper treat-
ment, healing is usually complete in about 3 weeks. There is a danger that the injured person may not
seek medical advice — even with a complete tear of the ligaments (connecting either the malleolus
and the talus or the tibia). Then surgical repair and rigid fixation in a cast is needed for 2–3 months.
External rotation (eversion) of the foot tends to break one of the malleoli bones (vertical part of the
mortise). The person tends to recognize this serious injury and go to a physician.
Approximately 80% of all foot fractures involve the toes; almost all of them could be prevented by
safety shoes since they lie within the area protected by the metal toe cap.
Three venous systems drain the lower limbs: (1) a deep central system drains the muscles, (2) a super-
ficial system drains the foot and the skin of the leg, and (3) a perforating system connects the deep and
superficial systems.
30.1.2 Physiology
The veins store the body’s blood. If the legs do not move, the blood from the heart tends to go down to
the legs and stay there (venous pooling). This causes more work for the heart, as, for a constant supply of
blood, when the blood/beat is lower, then there must be more beats. Venous pooling causes swelling of
the legs (edema) and varicose veins. The foot swelling during stationary seated work can be overcome by
modest leg activity (such as by rolling the chair about the workstation) (Winkel and Jorgensen, 1986).
Venous pressure in the ankle of sedentary people is approximately equal to hydrostatic pressure from
the right auricle. Pollack and Wood (1949) gave a mean ankle venous pressure of 56 mmHg for sitting
and 87 mmHg for standing. Nodeland et al. (1983) gave 48 mmHg for sitting and 80 mmHg for stand-
ing. Pollack and Wood reported walking drops ankle venous pressure to about 23 mmHg (Nodeland
et al. reported 21 mmHg) in about 10 steps.
Application of Ergonomics of the Foot 30-3
Note: The percentages show the dimension as a percent of stature height. If only male anthropometric data for a popu-
lation are available, the female dimension can be estimated as 93% of the corresponding male dimension. Shoes add
25 mm height for males and 15 mm for females. Shoes add 0.9 kg to body weight.
Source: From Konz, S. and Johnson, S. 2004. Work Design: Occupational Ergonomics, 6th ed. Holcomb-Hathaway,
Scottsdale, AZ. With permission.
The fall occurs as the calf muscles contract in taking the next step before venous filling is complete.
Thus additional blood is pumped out of the leg, causing a further drop in pressure when the calf
muscles relax. The drop stabilizes in about 10 steps when the incoming flow to the vein from the capil-
laries equals the flow out of the leg. Thus, walking can partially compensate for posture. For example,
Nodeland et al. reported standing bench work (i.e., with occasional steps around the area) had ankle
pressure approximately equal to sitting at a desk (48 mmHg). Walking aids blood circulation through
the “milking action” of the leg muscles; this reduces the work of the heart. Active standing (walking
2 –4 min every 15 min) results in less discomfort than standing without walking.
Because of vasoconstriction, foot skin temperature (without shoes) usually is the lowest body skin
temperature. Normal skin foot temperature ¼ 33.38C for males but 30.28 for females (Oleson and
Fanger, 1973).
30.1.3 Dimensions
Table 30.1 gives some dimensions for U.S. adults. Also see Chapter 9 — Engineering Anthropometry. The
torso is relatively constant in height; most of the variation in standing height is due to differences in leg
length. Figure 30.2 shows the mean difference, when standing, between the inside of the two feet is about
6
8
106 94
95 108 84
85
34
249
227
229
251
29
35
29
109.4
Area left 104
90.8
112
105.8 9.1°
Area right
87.8
7.3° 6.8°
7.3°
FIGURE 30.2 Footprint dimensions in mm (males above the line; females below line); areas in mm2; angles in
degrees. Toe area is 10% of contact area. They stood with the right foot slightly (6 – 8 mm) ahead of the left foot.
The left foot (for males) averaged 78 to the left of the medial plane; the right foot averaged 98 to the right. (From
Rys, M. and Konz, S. 1994. Ergonomics, 37(4):677 –687. With permission.)
30-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
107 mm. The distance between foot centerlines is about 107 þ 90 ¼ 197 mm (200 mm in round numbers).
The distance between outside edges is 107 þ 90 þ 89 ¼ 286 mm (300 mm in round numbers). Yet mean
height for males is 1756 mm! Thus there is only a base of 200–300 mm for a structure of 1756 mm.
There is no significant average difference between the left and right feet. However, for specific individ-
uals, that there often is considerable difference between the left and right foot.
The technical name for differences in leg length in the same person is leg length discrepancy (LLD).
Contreras et al. (1993), summarizing studies with N ¼ 2377, reported that 40% of people had LLD
5 mm, 30% had LLD 11 mm, and 10% had LLD 14 mm.
Weight of leg segments, as a percent of body weight, are 1.47 for foot, 4.35 for calf, and 10.27 for thigh; a
total leg is 16.1 and both legs are 32.2. Thus the leg weight of a 70 kg person would be 0.322 (70) ¼ 22.5 kg.
When people stand at a work surface, there needs to be an indentation (150 mm deep, 150 mm high,
and 500 mm wide) so they can stand close to the work surface.
30.2.1 Standing
During standing, the legs will generally move occasionally. Figure 30.3 shows how to support the stand-
ing body. The changed height of the foot rotates the thigh bone forward, enabling the hip on that side to
be fixed; it also flattens the lumbar curve and relaxes the iliopsoas muscle. When a leg is on the rail/foot-
rest, the large back muscles on that side of the body are relaxed and receive a flow of blood (bringing
nutrients and removing waste products).
30.2.2 Walking
When walking, the activity of one leg has a shorter swing phase (when the foot is being passed forward)
and the longer support (stepping, contact) phase (when the foot is on the ground). The support phase
starts at heel strike and ends at toe-off; it has an early, passive section and a later active (propulsion)
section.
At heel strike, the forward-moving heel hits the
ground (causing deceleration). Continued forward
motion of the body results in the forefoot contact-
ing the ground; propulsion (acceleration) begins.
The heel rises and the foot is pushed backward
under the body. This tendency is resisted by fric-
tion under the sole; the body is propelled
forward. The foot is everted, increasing forefoot
contact area on the inner side, until only the skin
around the big toe is in ground contact. Finally,
contact ceases and the cycle repeats.
Because the swing phase is shorter than the
support phase, heel strike on the opposite limb
occurs during the propulsion part of the support
phase. At heel strike, horizontal velocity decreases
FIGURE 30.3 Bar footrests along a work station or
from about 450 to 20 cm/sec; heel angle to the
conveyor can reduce standing stress. (From Rodgers,
S. 1984. Working with Backache. Perinton Press, Fairport, floor changes from about 208 prior to heel
NY. With permission.) A small platform is another contact to 08 at 100 m/sec after contact (Redfern
alternative. (From Konz, S. and Johnson, S. 2004. Work and Rhoades, 1996). During a slip, instead of stop-
Design: Occupational Ergonomics, 6th ed. Holcomb- ping, the heel continues to move and the leading
Hathaway, Scottsdale, AZ. With permission.) foot moves out in front of the body.
Application of Ergonomics of the Foot 30-5
30.2.3 Running
Walking changes to running, for normal size adults, at about 2.5 m/sec (6 miles/h), since it uses less
energy (for the same speed). Running differs from walking in that both feet are off the ground for
part of the stride. In addition, the heel strike should be renamed the “foot strike,” since the initial
contact probably will be forward of the heel. After foot strike (usually on the outside edge of the
foot), the foot rolls inward and flattens out (pronation). Then the foot rolls through the ball and
rotates outward (supination).
Peak force is about three times body weight at about 0.1 sec after contact. For running, the average
contact duration is 0.29 sec. In contrast, it is 0.48 sec for walking (Scanton and McMaster, 1976).
30.2.4 Stepping
Descending stairs demands a gait quite different from ascent (Templer, 1992).
For descent, the leading foot swings forward over the nosing edge and stops its forward motion when it
is directly over the tread below; the toe is pointed downward. Meanwhile, the heel of the rear foot begins
to rise, starting a controlled fall downward toward the tread. The heel of the forward foot then is lowered
and the weight is transferred to the forward foot. The rear foot then begins to swing forward.
We tend to hold our center of gravity as far back as possible by leaning backward. Problems are over-
stepping the nosing with the forward foot, catching the toe of the forward foot, and snagging the heel of
the rear foot on the nosing as it swings past. Falls tend to be down the stairs.
For ascent, the leading foot has a toe-off, swing, and first contact with the upper step. The foot is
approximately horizontal. The ball of the foot is well forward of the tread; the heel may or may not
be on the tread. The rear foot then rises on tiptoes, pushing down and back. The rear foot then
begins the swing phase. The primary problem is catching the toe, foot or heel of either foot on the
stair nosing. Another problem is the rear foot slipping when it pushes backward. Falls tend to be upward.
30.3 Accidents
30.3.2.1 Slips
Slips occur primarily during foot push off and heel strike. During pushoff, the person falls forward (less
common and less dangerous); in addition, during pushoff, most of the weight has already been trans-
ferred to the other foot.
During heel strike, the person falls backward.
Slips also can occur when the “ground” slopes (front to back or side to side). Examples are ramps and
ladder rungs. Slips also can occur, with stationary feet, during pushing and pulling, but there does not
tend to be a fall and injury.
During a slip, a lubricant (water, oil, grease, dust, ice, snow) usually is present, either on the surface or
on the shoe heel. Table 30.2 has some coefficients of friction. In the special outdoors circumstances of
snow and ice, slipping can be common. The most danger occurs when the ice is “wet” (i.e., close to
the freezing point). Chang et al. (2001) give 0.67 for the static coefficient of friction for shoes on ice
at 2408C, but 0.01 as the dynamic coefficient at 218C (when there is a thin film of water).
Table 30.3 summarizes how to reduce slips.
30.3.2.2 Trips
Trips occur during foot swing. As the foot swings forward, it hits an obstacle and the person falls forward;
the problem is lack of leg movement. Usually there is a visual problem. Indoor trips tend to be from
obstacles on the floor. Outdoor trips often occur from uneven surfaces (e.g., walkways, parking lots)
that the person expects to be even.
30.3.2.3 Stepping-on-air
Unexpected vertical movement can occur on stair steps when the distance between steps is not equal;
when there is a hole in the ground; or when there is no ground (e.g., “cliff,” edge of scaffold, unexpected
step, step on spiral stairs, unexpected curb or ramp). “Single steps” (small changes in elevation such as
curbs or one-step changes in floor level) are dangerous. Steps descending from large trucks and off-road
vehicles can present problems; for such vehicles use the “three-contact rule” (at least three limbs should
be in contact with steps or handles at all times). On steps, the fall usually occurs when descending; the fall
can be for a considerable distance.
Coefficient Floor
of Friction Floors Clean Soiled Shoe: Soles
1.0 Soft rubber pad 0.8 0.6 Rubber-cork
0.8 End grain wood 0.75 0.55 U.S. Army/
Air Force
standard
0.7 Concrete, rough finish 0.7 0.5 Rubber-crepe
0.65 Working decorative, dry 0.6 0.4 Neoprene
0.5 Working decorative, 0.5 0.3 Leather
soiled
0.4 Steel
Shoes: Heels
0.7 0.5 Neoprene
0.65 0.55 Nylon
In some cases, the surface is there initially but breaks or moves (e.g., step breaks, a chair used as a step-
stool moves, ladder feet move, a roof gives way under the foot).
30.4 Fatigue/Comfort
The discussion is divided into walking and standing.
30.4.1 Walking/Running
The primary problem is the shock of heel strike being transmitted up the foot, leg, and back. For shoe
solutions, see Figure 30.4.
The energy cost of walking depends on the terrain, with a hard surface giving the minimum cost
(Pandolf et al., 1976):
where:
WLKMET ¼ walking metabolism, W/kg of body weight
30-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
C ¼ terrain coefficient
¼ 1.0 for treadmill, blacktop road
¼ 1.1 for dirt road
¼ 1.2 for light brush
¼ 1.3 for hard packed snow; C ¼ 1.3 þ 0.082 (foot depression, cm)
¼ 1.5 for heavy brush
¼ 1.8 for swamp
¼ 2.1 for sand
v ¼ velocity, m/sec [for v .0.7 m/sec (2.5 km/h)]
The metabolic cost of carrying (walking with a load) depends on the load location. Soule and
Goldman (1980) reported that loads on the head used 1.2 times the energy of carrying a 1 kg of your
own body weight; in the hands, loads required 1.4 –1.9 times as much; on the feet, loads required
4.2 –6.3 times as much.
The energy cost of running is:
where RUNMET is the running metabolism (total), W/kg, V is the velocity (km/h), and WT is the
weight (body), in kg.
30.4.2 Standing
Although there are some problems with static electricity and with floor temperature, the primary
problem is lack of circulation in the leg.
Static electricity. Static electricity solutions include:
. Raise humidity above 40% (Not only permits voltage to flow to ground but also moisture is a lubri-
cator, reducing friction between moving parts.)
. Make carpets conductive (Carbon fibers added to carpets.)
. Put an antistatic floor mat under the operator’s chair. Also can ground operator with a static-bleed
wrist strap
. Encourage cotton clothing; discourage nylons, slips, and polyester clothing. Use shoes with
static-dissipating soles
. Remove dust by blowing deionized air from an air gun
Application of Ergonomics of the Foot 30-9
Floor temperature. When wearing normal shoes, ASHRAE recommends 238C for standing and
walking but 258C for sedentary standing. Heavy carpet will save about 1% of the total energy used to
heat the building (Hager, 1977).
Floor comfort. Teitelman et al. (1990) reported preterm births occurred more often (7.7%) when
women had jobs with prolonged standing; the rate for sitting jobs was 4.2% and for active jobs
was 2.8%. There are four alternatives: (1) replace standing with sitting; (2) supplement static
standing with occasional walking; (3) shift posture while standing; and (4) cushion the floor. See
Figure 30.5.
Replace standing with sitting. In general, heart rates are lower when sitting than standing. However,
in an experiment of checkout workstations, Lehman et al. (2001) reported workstations with a standing
0operator had lower EMG than sitting. Sitting does restrict torso movement and thus reach distances.
A possible alternative is a sit –stand stool, where the person’s legs are almost vertical, supporting about
2/3 of body weight. The adjustable height seat (65– 85 cm) should tilt forward 158 – 308. Chester et al.
(2002) recommend a footrest resulting in a 908 footrest – calf angle.
Supplement static standing with occasional walking. As discussed in Section 30.1.2 at the start of this
chapter, there are cardiovascular benefits of occasional walking.
Shift posture while standing. As discussed in Section 30.2.1 and Figure 30.3, there are benefits of shifting
standing posture using standing aids such as bar rails and foot rests.
Cushion the floor. See Figure 30.5.
FIGURE 30.5 Standing Aids. (From: Adapted from Konz, S. and Rys, M. 2003. Occupational Ergonomics, 3:165 –
172. With permission.)
30-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
30.5.1 Pedals
Pedals can be used for power and control. Power generation can be continuous (bicycle) or discrete (non-
powered automobile brake pedal). For information on continuous power, see Whitt and Wilson (1982)
and Brooks et al. (1986).
Discrete power generally is applied by one leg; there does not seem to be any advantage to using the left
or right leg. Force using both feet is about 10% higher than using just one foot.
A control example is an auto accelerator pedal. Bend the ankle (808 –1158) by depressing the toe rather
than depressing the heel or moving the entire foot.
30.5.2 Switches
A foot switch can actuate a machine (such as a punch press). Generally the foot remains on the switch so
the time and effort of moving the foot/leg is not important. On –off controls (such as faucets, clamping
fixtures) can be actuated by lateral motion of the knee as well as vertical motion of the foot. The knee
should not have to move more than 75 –100 mm; force requirements should be light. Hospitals use
knee switches to actuate faucets to improve germ control of the hands.
Avoid foot pedals/switches which are operated while standing as they tend to distort posture and cause
back problems.
References
Brooks, A., Abbott, A., and Wilson, D. 1986. Human-powered watercraft. Scientific American,
256(12):120–130.
Chang, W-R, Gronqvist, R., Leclercq, S., Myung, R., Makkonen, L., Strandberg, L. Brungraber, R.,
Mattke, U., and Thorpe, S. 2001. The role of friction in the measurement of slipperiness.
Ergonomics, 44(13):1217–1232.
Application of Ergonomics of the Foot 30-11
Chester, M., Rys, M., and Konz, S. 2002. Leg swelling, comfort, and fatigue when sit/standing using a 08,
158 and 308 footrest. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 29, 289 –296.
Contreras, R., Rys, M., and Konz, S. 1993. Leg length discrepancy. In The Ergonomics of Manual Work,
eds. W. Marras, W. Karwowski, and L. Pacholski, 199 –202, Taylor & Francis, London.
Courtney, T., Sorock, G., Manning, D., Collins, J., and Holbein-Jenny, M. 2001. Occupational slip, trip,
and fall-related injuries — can the contribution of slipperiness be isolated? Ergonomics,
44(13):1118–1137.
Hager, N. 1977. Energy conservation and floor covering materials. ASHRAE Journal, 34 –39.
Hanna, S. and Konz, S., 2004, Facility Design and Engineering, 3rd edn. Holcomb Hathaway, Scottsdale, AZ.
Konz, S. and Johnson, S. 2004. Work Design: Occupational Ergonomics, 6th ed. Holcomb-Hathaway,
Scottsdale, AZ.
Konz, S. and Rys, M. 2003. An ergonomic approach to standing aids. Occupational Ergonomics, 3:165 –172.
Kroemer, K. 1974. Horizontal push and pull forces. Applied Ergonomics, 5(2):94 –102.
Krumweide, D., Konz, S., and Hinnen, P. 1998. Floor mat comfort. In Advances in Occupational
Ergonomics and Safety, ed. S. Kumar, 159 –162, IOS Press.
Lehman, K., Psihogios, J., and Meulenbroek, R., 2001, Effects of sitting versus standing and scanner type
on cashiers. Ergonomics, 44(7): 719 –38.
Nodeland, H., Ingemansen, R., Reed, R., and Aukland, K., 1983. A telemetric technique for studies of
venous pressure in the human leg during different positions and activities. Clinical Physiology,
3:573–576.
Oleson, B. and Fanger, P. 1973. The skin temperature distribution for resting man in comfort. Archives
des Sciences Physiologigues, 27(4):A385–A393.
Pandolf, K., Haisman, M., and Goldman, R. 1976. Metabolic energy expenditure and terrain coefficients
for walking on snow. Ergonomics, 19:683 –690.
Pollack, A. and Wood, E. 1949. Venous pressure in the saphenous vein at the ankle in man during exercise
and change of posture. Journal of Applied Physiology, 1:649 –662.
Redfern, M. and Rhoades, T., 1996, Fall prevention in industry using slip resistance testing. In Occu-
pational Ergonomics, Bhattacharya, A. and McGloughlin, J. (eds.), 463 –476, M. Decker, New York.
Rodgers, S. 1984. Working with Backache. Perinton Press, Fairport, NY.
Rys, M. and Konz, S. 1994. Standing. Ergonomics, (37)4:677 –687.
Scanton, P. and McMaster, J. 1976. Momentary distribution of forces under the foot. Journal of Biome-
chanics, 9:45–48.
Satzler, L., Satzler, C., and Konz, S. 1993. Standing aids. Proceedings of Ayoub Symposium, 29 –31, Texas
Tech University, Lubbock, TX.
Soule, R. and Goldman, R. 1980. Energy cost of loads carried on the head, hands, or feet. Journal of
Applied Physiology, 27:687–690.
Teitelman, A., Welch, L., Hellenbrand, K., and Bracken, M. 1990. Effect of maternal work activity on
preterm birth and low birth weight. American Journal of Epidemiology, 131:104–113.
Templer, J. 1992. The Staircase: Studies of Hazards, Falls, and Safer Design. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Whitt, F. and Wilson, D. 1982. Bicycling Science. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Winkel, J. and Jorgensen, K., 1986, Evaluation of foot swelling and lower-limb temperature in relation to
leg activity during long-term seated office work. Ergonomics, 29(2):313 –328.
Further Reading
Konz, S. and Johnson, S. 2004. Work Design: Occupational Ergonomics, 6th ed. Holcomb Hathaway,
Scottsdale, AZ. This popular textbook concisely summarizes many aspects of job design and
gives detailed design guidelines.
Ergonomics. This journal publishes articles on ergonomics from authors around the world.
International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics. This journal publishes articles on ergonomics from
authors around the world.
31
Noise in Industry
31.1 Introduction
The din of noise emanating from industrial processes pervades many occupational settings, and its effects
on workers range from minor annoyance to major risk of hearing damage. Unfortunately, at least within
the current limits of technology, noise is a by-product of many industries, such as manufacturing,
especially those which use high-energy or impact processes such as metal cutting and mineral refinement,
and service-related industries, such as air transport, construction, and farming. Workers complain about
the negative effects of noise on their abilities to communicate, hear warning and other signals, and
concentrate on tasks at hand. However, the effect that has been of most concern to industry has been
permanent noise-induced hearing loss, or NIHL.
The primary intent of this chapter is to provide an introductory overview of the basic properties,
measurement, effects on hearing, government regulations, and abatement of industrial noise, with a
particular focus on reducing the physiological damage potential of noise as it impacts the human
hearing organ. While the effects of noise exposure are serious and must be reckoned with by the
hearing conservationist or safety professional, one fact is encouraging: process/machine-produced
noise is a physical stimulus that can be avoided, reduced, or eliminated; therefore, occupationally
related NIHL in workers is completely preventable with effective abatement and protection strategies.
Total elimination of NIHL should thus be the only acceptable goal.
31-1
31-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
l ¼ c=f (31:1)
Noise can be loosely defined as a subset of sound; that is, noise is sound that is undesirable or offensive in
some aspect. However, the distinction is largely situation-and listener-specific, as perhaps best stated in
the old adage “one person’s music is another’s noise.”
Unlike some common ergonomics-related stressors such as repetitive motions or awkward lifting
maneuvers, noise is a physical stimulus that is readily measurable and quantifiable using transducers
(microphones) and instrumentation (sound level meters) that are commonly available. Aural exposure
to noise, and the damage potential therefrom, is a function of the total energy transmitted to the ear. In
other words, the energy is equivalent to the product of the noise intensity and duration of the exposure.
Several metrics that relate to the energy of the noise exposure have been developed, most with an eye
toward expressing the exposures that occur in industrial or community settings. These metrics are
covered later in this chapter. But first, the most basic unit of measurement must be understood,
namely the decibel.
where Pw1 is the acoustic power of the sound in Watts, or other power unit, and Pwr is the acoustic power
of a reference sound in Watts, usually taken to be the acoustic power at hearing threshold for a young,
healthy ear at the frequency of maximum sensitivity, or the quantity 10212 W.
Noise in Industry 31-3
Sound intensity level, following from power level, is typically expressed in dB, and is defined as
where I1 is the acoustic intensity of the sound in W/m2, or other intensity unit, and Ir is the acoustic
intensity of a reference sound in W/m2, usually taken to be the acoustic intensity at hearing threshold,
or the quantity 10212 W/m2.
Within the last decade, sound measurement instruments to measure sound intensity level have become
commonplace, albeit expensive and relatively complex. Sound power level, on the other hand, is not
directly measurable but can be computed from empirical measures of sound intensity level or sound
pressure level.
Sound pressure level (SPL) is also typically expressed in dB. Since power is directly proportional to the
square of the pressure, SPL is defined as
where P1 is the pressure level of the sound in mPa, or other pressure unit, and Pr is the pressure level
of a reference sound in mPa, usually taken to be the pressure at hearing threshold, or the quantity
20 mPa, or 0.00002 Pa. Other equivalent reference quantities are: 0.0002 dyn/cm2, 20 mN/m2, and
20 mbar.
The application of the decibel scale to acoustical measurements yields a convenient means of collap-
sing the vast range of sound pressures that would be required to accommodate sounds that can be
encountered into a more manageable, compact range. As shown in Figure 31.1, using the logarithmic
compression produced by the decibel scale, the range of typical sounds is 120 dB, while the same
FIGURE 31.1 For typical sounds, sound pressure level values in decibels and sound pressure values in pascals.
31-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
range measured in pressure units (Pa) would be 1,000,000. Of course, sounds do occur that are higher
than 120 dB (for instance, artillery fire) or lower than 0 dB (below the normal threshold on an audio-
meter). A comparison of decibel values of example sounds to their pressure values (in Pa) is also depicted
in Figure 31.1.
In considering changes in sound level measured in decibels, a few numerical relationships emanating
from the decibel formulae given earlier are often helpful in practice. An increase (decrease) in SPL by
6 dB is equivalent to a doubling (halving) of the sound pressure. Similarly, on power or intensity
scales, an increase (decrease) of 3 dB is equivalent to a doubling (halving) of the sound power or inten-
sity. This latter relationship gives rise to what is known as the “equal energy rule or trading relationship.”
Because sound represents energy that is itself a product of intensity and duration, an original sound that
increases (decreases) by 3 dB is equivalent in total energy to the same original sound that does not change
in decibels but decreases (increases) in its duration by half (twice).
31.2.3.1 Phons
The decibel level of a 1000-Hz tone that is judged by human listeners to be equally loud to a sound in
question is the phon level of the sound. The phon levels of sounds of different intensities are shown in the
top panel of Figure 31.2; this family of curves is referred to as the equal loudness contours. On any given
curve, the combinations of sound level and frequency along the curve produce sound experiences of
equal loudness to the normal-hearing listener. Note that at 1000 Hz on each curve the phon level is
equal to the decibel level. The threshold of hearing for a young, healthy ear is represented by the 0
phon level curve. The young, healthy ear is sensitive to sounds between about 20 and 20,000 Hz,
although, as shown by the curve, it is not equally sensitive to all frequencies. At low- and mid-level
sound intensities, low-frequency sounds and to a lesser extent high-frequency sounds are perceived as
less intense than sounds in the 1000 to 4000 Hz range, where the undamaged ear is most sensitive.
But as phon levels move to higher values, the ear becomes more linear in its loudness perception for
sounds of different frequencies.
Because the ear exhibits this nonlinear behavior, several frequency weighting functions have been stan-
dardized for use with sound level meters. The most common curves are the A, B, and C curves, with the
corresponding decibel measurement denoted as dBA, dBB, and dBC, respectively. If no weighting func-
tion is selected on the meter, the notation dB or dB(linear) is used, and all frequencies are processed
without weighting factors. The actual weighting functions for the three suffix notations A, B, and C
are superimposed on the phon contours of the top panel of Figure 31.2, and also depicted as actual
frequency weighting functions in the bottom panel. In nearly all U.S. measurements of industrial
noise made for assessment of exposure risk to workers, the dBA scale is used, and the meter is set
on the “slow” dynamic response setting, which produces slow exponential averaging of a 1-sec
window. For determination of the adequacy of hearing protection for a particular noise, and for
application of noise control measures, the C-weighted level is often taken in addition to the A-weighted
level.
31.2.3.2 Sones
While the phon scale provides the ability to equate the loudness of sounds of different frequencies, it does
not afford an ability to describe how much louder one sound is compared with another. For this, the sone
scale is needed.1 One sone is defined as the loudness of a 1000 Hz tone of 40 dB SPL. In relation to
one sone, two sones are twice as loud, three sones are three times as loud, one-half sone is half as
Noise in Industry 31-5
Threshold of Feeling
120 Phons
120
110
C
100
100
90
80
80 B
70
spl, dB re 20 µPa
60
60
50
A
40
40 Th
res 30
ho
ld
of 20
Au
20 dibi
lity 10
0
0
+10
Relative Response–Decibels
0
C A
–10
B B+C
–20
–30
A
–40
–50
20 50 100 200 500 1000 2000 5000 10,000 20,000
Frequency In Hertz
FIGURE 31.2 Top: Equal loudness contours based on the psychophysical phon scale, with sound level meter
frequency weighting curves superimposed. Bottom: Decibel versus frequency values of A, B, and C sound level
meter weighting curves. (Adapted from Earshen, J. J. (1986). Sound measurement: instrumentation and noise
descriptors. In Berger, E. H., Ward, W. D., Morrill, J. C., and Royster, L. H. (Eds.), Noise and Hearing
Conservation Manual (pp. 38 – 95). Akron, OH: American Industrial Hygiene Association. With permission.)
loud, and so on. Phon level (LP) and sones are related by the following formula for sounds at or above a
40-phon level:
According to formula 31.5, 1 sone equals 40 phons and the number of sones doubles with each 10-phon
increase; therefore, it is straightforward to conduct a comparative estimate of loudness levels of sounds
with different decibel levels. The “rule-of-thumb” is that each 10-dB increase in a sound (i.e., one that is
above 40 dB to begin with) will result in a doubling of its loudness. For instance, a conference room that
31-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
is at 45 dBA may currently be comfortable for communication. However, if a new ventilation system
increases the noise level in the room by 10 dBA, the occupants will experience a doubling of loudness
and will likely complain about the effects of the background noise on conversation in the room. Once
again, the compression effect of the decibel scale yields a measure that does not reflect the much
larger influence that an increase in sound level will have on the human perception of loudness. Although
the sone scale is not widely used (one exception is that household ventilation fans typically have
voluntary sone ratings), it is a very useful scale for comparing different sounds as to their perceived
loudness.
It should be evident that phon levels can be calculated directly from psychological measurements in
sones, but not from physical measurements of SPL in decibels. This is because the phon-based loudness
and SPL relationship changes as a function of the sound frequency and the magnitude of this change
depends on the intensity of the sound.
31.3.2.2 Masking
Masking is technically defined as the tendency for the threshold of a desired signal or speech (the masked
sound) to be raised in the presence of an interfering sound (the masker). As an example, in a noisy airport
waiting area, a pay telephone’s earphone volume must often be increased to enable the listener to hear the
party on the line, whereas a lower volume will be more comfortable while affording audibility when there
is no crowd or public address system noise present. The masked threshold is defined as the SPL required
for 75% correct detection of a signal when that signal is presented in a two-interval task wherein, on a
random basis, one of the two intervals of each task trial contains the signal and the noise and the other
contains only noise. In a controlled laboratory test scenario, a signal that is about 6 dB above the masked
threshold will result in near perfect detection performance.7 Analytical prediction (as opposed to actual
experimentation with human subjects) of the interfering effects of noise on speech communications may
31-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
be conducted using the Speech Intelligibility Index (SII) technique defined in ANSI S3.5-1997 (R2002).8
Essentially, this relatively complex technique utilizes a weighted sum of the speech-to-noise ratios in
specified frequency bands to compute an SII score ranging between 0.0 and 1.0, with higher scores indica-
tive of greater predicted speech intelligibility. (The SII actually represents the proportion of the speech
cues that would be available to the listener for “average speech” under the noise/speech conditions
for which the calculations were performed. Hence, intelligibility would be greatest when the SII ¼ 1.0,
indicating that all of the speech cues are reaching the listener, and poorest when the SII ¼ 0.0, indicating
that none of the speech cues are reaching the listener.) This method is extremely flexible and can account
for factors such as differences in speaker vocal effort, room reverberation, monaural and binaural listen-
ing, hearing loss, varying message content, and insertion loss (HPD use) or gain (amplified communi-
cations systems), as well as the existence of external masking noise. Nonverbal signal detectability
predictions can also be made analytically, with the most comprehensive computational technique,
based on a spectral analysis of the noise, appearing in ISO 7731 –2003.9 While a full discussion of
these analytical procedures is beyond the scope of this chapter, the reader is referred to the individual
ANSI and ISO standards for detail. The SII and masked threshold computational techniques provide
better resolution and accuracy for speech intelligibility and signal detectability predictions than a
simple evaluation of broadband S/N ratios because the techniques incorporate the frequency-specific
information that simple S/N ratios do not reflect. However, the following general principles regarding
masking effects on nonverbal signals and speech can be used for general guidance:
1. The greatest increase in masked threshold occurs for nonverbal signal frequencies that are equal
or near to the predominant frequencies of the masking noise; this is called direct masking. There-
fore, warning signals should not utilize tonal frequencies equivalent to those of the masker. Pre-
ferably, the signal should be in the most sensitive range of human hearing, approximately 1000 to
4000 Hz, unless the noise energy is intense at these frequencies.
2. If the signal and masker are tonal in nature, the primary masking effect is at the fundamental
frequency of the masker and at its harmonics. For instance, if a masking noise has primary fre-
quency content at 1000 Hz, this frequency and its harmonics (2000, 3000, 4000, etc.) should be
avoided as signal frequencies.
3. The greater the SPL of the masker, the more the increase in masked threshold of the signal. A
general rule-of-thumb is that the S/N ratio at the listener’s ear should at a minimum be about
15 dB above the masked threshold for reliable signal detection. However, in noise levels above
about 80 dBA, the signal levels required to maintain an S/N ratio of 15 dB above the masked
threshold may increase the hearing exposure risk, especially if signal presentation occurs fre-
quently. Therefore, if lower S/Ns become necessary, it is best to construct signals that are
unlike the masker in frequency and have modulated or alternating frequencies to grab attention.
4. Warning signals should not exceed the masked threshold by more than 30 dB to avoid verbal
communications interference and operator annoyance.7
5. As the SPL of the masker increases, the primary change in the masking effect is that it spreads
upward in frequency, often causing signal frequencies that are higher than the masker to be
missed. This is termed upward masking. Since most warning signal guidelines recommend
that the midrange and high-frequency signals (about 1000 to 4000 Hz) be used for detectability,
it is important to consider that the masking effects of industrial noise of lower frequencies can
spread upward and cause interference in this range. Therefore, if the noise has its most significant
energy in this range, a lower frequency signal, say 500 Hz, may be necessary. However, it must be
kept in mind that the ear is not as sensitive to low frequencies, so the signal level must be care-
fully set to ensure reliable audibility.
6. Masking effects can also spread downward in frequency, causing signal frequencies below those
of the masker to be raised in threshold. This is called remote masking and the effect is most pro-
minent at signal frequencies that are subharmonics of the masker. With typical industrial noise
sources, remote masking is generally less of a problem than direct or upward masking.
Noise in Industry 31-9
7. In extremely loud environments of about 110 dB and above, nonauditory signal channels such as
visual and vibrotactile should be considered as alternatives to auditory displays.
8. Speech intelligibility in noise depends on a combination of complex factors and, as such, predic-
tions based on simple S/N ratios should not be relied upon. However, in very general terms, S/N
ratios of 15 dB or higher should result in intelligibility performance above about 80% words
correct for normal-hearing individuals in broadband noise.10 Above speech levels of about
85 dBA, there is some decline in intelligibility even if the S/N ratio is held constant.11 In very
high noise levels, it is impractical and may pose additional hearing hazard risk to amplify the
voice to maintain the high S/N ratios necessary for good intelligibility performance. The S/N
ratio required for reliable intelligibility may be reduced via the use of certain techniques such
as reduction of speaker-to-listener distances, use of smaller vocabularies, provision of contextual
cues in the message, use of the phonetic alphabet, and use of noise-attenuating headphones and
noise-canceling microphones in electronic systems.
9. Electronic speech communications systems should reproduce speech frequencies in the range of
500 to 5000 Hz, which encompasses the most sensitive range of hearing and includes the speech
sounds important for message understandability. More specifically, because much of the infor-
mation required for word discrimination lies in the consonants, which are in the higher end of
the frequency range and of low power (while the power of the vowels is in the peaks of the speech
waveform), the use of electronic peak-clipping and reamplification of the waveform may
improve intelligibility because the power of the consonants is thereby boosted relative to the
vowels. Furthermore, it is critical that frequencies in the region of 1000 to 4000 Hz be faithfully
reproduced in electronic communication systems to maintain intelligibility. Filtering out of fre-
quencies outside this range will not appreciably affect word intelligibility, but will influence the
quality of the speech.
10. Actual human speech results in higher intelligibility in noise than computer-generated speech;
therefore, especially for critical message displays and annunciators, live, recorded, or digitized
human speech is preferable over synthesized speech.12
prominent member; exposure to certain chemicals and industrial solvents; hereditary factors; head
trauma; sudden hyperbaric- or altitude-induced pressure changes; and aging of the ear (presbycusis).
Furthermore, not all noise exposure occurs on the job. Many workers are exposed to hazardous levels
during leisure activities, from such sources as automobile/motorcycle racing, personal stereo headsets
and car stereos, firearms, and power tools. The effects of noise on hearing are generally subdivided
into the following three categories.15
10
Median Presumed Noise-
Induced Threshold Shift
in Decibels 20
30
40
EXPOSURE
50 5 – 9 Yr
15 – 19 Yr
25 – 29 Yr
60
35 – 39 Yr
40 – 52 Yr
70
125 250 500 1000 2000 3000 4000 6000
Frequency in Hertz
FIGURE 31.3 Cumulative auditory effects of years of noise exposure in a jute weaving industry. (Adapted from
Taylor, W., Pearson, J., Mair, A., and Burns, W., (1964). Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 38, 113– 120.
With permission.)
three primary instruments (sound level meters, dosimeters, and real-time spectrum analyzers) and their
data output will suffice. In instances where noise is highly impulsive in nature or selection and develop-
ment of situation-specific engineering noise control solutions is anticipated, more specialized instru-
ments may be necessary.
Because sound is propagated as pressure waves that vary over space and in time, a complete quanti-
fication would require simultaneous measurements over the continuous time periods (representing com-
plete operator exposure durations) at all points of an occupational sound field to exhaustively document
the noise level in the space. Clearly, this is typically cost- and time-prohibitive, so one must resort to
sampling strategies for establishing the observation points and intervals. The hearing conservationist
must also decide whether detailed, discrete time histories are needed (such as with a noise-logging dosi-
meter, discussed later), if averaging over time and space with long data records is required (with an aver-
aging/integrating dosimeter), whether discrete samples taken with a short-duration moving time average
(with a basic sound level meter) will suffice, or if frequency-band-specific SPLs are needed for selecting
noise abatement materials (with a spectrum analyzer). Following is a brief discussion of the three primary
types of sound measurement instruments and the noise descriptors that can be obtained therefrom.
10
PERPENDICULAR
0
GRAZING
PERPENDICULAR
–5
GRAZING
–10
–15
–20
20 50 100 200 500 1k 2k 5k 10k 20k
Frequency in Hertz
FIGURE 31.5 Frequency response of an hypothetical microphone for three angles of incidence. (Adapted from
Peterson, A.P.G. (1979). Noise measurements: Instruments. In Harris, C. M. (Ed.), Handbook of Noise Control
[pp. 5-1 – 5-19]. New York: McGraw-Hill. With permission.)
incidence of approximately 708. This will produce a measurement most closely corresponding to the
random-incidence response. Care must be taken to avoid shielding the microphone with the body or
other structures. The response of microphones can also vary with temperature, atmospheric pressure,
and humidity, with temperature being the most critical factor. Correction factors for variations in
decibel readout due to temperature effects are supplied by most microphone manufacturers. Atmos-
pheric effects are generally only significant when measurements are made in aircraft or at very high alti-
tudes, and humidity has a negligible effect except at very high levels. In any case, microphones must not
be exposed to moisture or large magnetic fields, such as those produced by transformers. When used in
windy conditions, a foam windscreen should be placed over the microphone. This will reduce the con-
taminating effects of wind noise, while only slightly influencing the frequency response of the micro-
phone at primarily high frequencies. In an industrial setting, the windscreen offers the additional
benefit of protection of the microphone from damage due to striking and/or airborne foreign matter.
pulse. It is important to note that the rms-based IMPULSE dynamics setting is unsuitable for measure-
ment of TRUE PEAK SPLs.
31.4.1.2 Dosimeter
The “audio-dosimeter” or more simply, “dosimeter,” is a battery-powered, highly portable device that is
derived directly from an SLM but also features the ability to obtain special measures of noise exposure
(discussed later) that relate to regulatory compliance and hearing hazard risk. Dosimeters are very
compact and are generally worn on the belt or in the pocket of an employee, with the microphone gen-
erally clipped to the lapel or shoulder of a shirt or blouse. The intent is to obtain a noise exposure log or
record over the course of a full or partial workshift, and to obtain, at a minimum, a readout of the time-
weighted average (TWA) exposure and noise dose for the period measured. Depending upon the fea-
tures, the dosimeter can log the time history of exposure, providing a running histogram of noise
levels on a short time interval (such as 1 min) basis, compute statistical distributions of the noise
exposures for the period, flag and record exposures that exceed OSHA maxima of 115 dBA continuous
or 140 dB TRUE PEAK, and compute average metrics using 3 dB, 5 dB, or even other time-versus-level
exchange rates. The dosimeter eliminates the need for the observer to set up a discrete sampling scheme
or follow the worker, both of which are necessary with a conventional SLM. However, it is important that
the observer establish rapport and gain the confidence of the worker wearing the dosimeter, and convey
at least the following information: (1) to behave normally as to the work activity, (2) to not tamper with
the dosimeter or microphone, (3) to return the device when visiting restrooms or entering damp areas,
(4) to return the device if there is a need to approach large transformers or other magnetic fields, and (5)
to understand the purpose of the dosimetry. Since they are designed to be worn on the noise-exposed
employee, dosimeters are typically thought of as devices for personal measurements, but they may
also be tripod-mounted or held by an observer for area or survey measurements and are very useful
for obtaining community noise measurements as well.
31-16 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
More precise spectral resolution can be obtained with other center frequency proportional filter sets with
narrower bandwidths, the most common being the 1/3 octave, and with constant percentage bandwidth
filter sets, such as 1 or 2% filters. Note that in both types the filter bandwidth increases as the center fre-
quency increases. Still other analyzers have constant bandwidth filters, such as 20-Hz-wide bandwidths
that are of constant width regardless of center frequency. While in the past most spectrum analyzer filters
have been analog devices with “skirts” or overshoots extending slightly beyond the cutoff frequencies,
digital computer-based analyzers are now very common. These “computational” filters use fast
Fourier transform (FFT) algorithms to compute sound level in a prespecified band of fixed resolution.
FFT devices can be used to obtain very high resolutions of noise spectral characteristics using bandwidths
as low as 1 Hz. However, in most industrial noise applications, a 1/1- or 1/3-octave analyzer will suffice
unless the noise has considerable power in near-tonal components that must be isolated. One caution is
in order: if a noise fluctuates in time or frequency, an integrating/averaging analyzer should be used to
achieve good accuracy of measurements. It is important that the averaging period be long in comparison
to the variability of the noise being sampled.
Inexpensive spectrum analyzers sometimes have filter sets that must be addressed individually in
obtaining a measurement. Such devices are called sequential analyzers and the operator must manually
(or via computer control) step through each filter separately and then read the result. Obviously, sequen-
tial filters are problematic when applied to the measurement of a fluctuating noise. On the other hand,
real-time analyzers incorporate parallel banks of filters that can process all frequency bands simul-
taneously, and the signal output may be controlled by a SLOW, FAST, or other time constant setting,
or it may be integrated or averaged over a fixed time period to provide LOSHA, Leq, or other average-
type data.
While occupational noise is monitored with a dosimeter or SLM for the purpose of noise exposure
compliance (using A-weighted broadband measurement), or assessment of hearing protection adequacy
(using C-weighted broadband measurement), both of these applications can also be addressed (in some
cases more accurately) with the use of spectral measurements of the noise level. For instance, the OSHA
occupational noise exposure standard.19 allows the use of octave band measurements reduced to broad-
band dBA values to determine if noise exposures exceed dBA limits defined in Table G-9 of the standard.
Furthermore, Appendix B of the standard concerns hearing protector adequacy and allows the use of an
octave band method for determining, on a spectral rather than a broadband basis, whether a hearing pro-
tector is adequate for a particular noise spectrum. It is also noteworthy that spectral analysis can help the
hearing conservationist discriminate noises as to their hazard potential even though they may have
similar A-weighted SPLs. This is illustrated in Figure 31.6, where both noises would be considered to
Noise in Industry 31-17
120
90 dBA
100
90
60
40
120
100
90 dBA
90
60
40
125 1000 8000
1/3 octave band center, Hz
FIGURE 31.6 Spectral differences for two different noises that have the same dBA value.
be of equal hazard by the OSHA-required dBA measurements (since they both are 90 dBA), but the 1/3-
octave analysis demonstrates that the lowermost noise is more hazardous as evidenced by the heavy con-
centration of energy in the midrange and high frequencies.
Perhaps the most important application of the spectrum analyzer is to obtain data that will provide the
basis for engineering noise control solutions. For instance, in order to select an absorption material for
lining interior surfaces of a workplace, the spectral content of the noise must be known so that the appro-
priate density and thickness of material may be identified. If the noise is found to be primarily of low
frequency, the absorption techniques may not provide adequate reduction because low frequencies are
more difficult to absorb than high frequencies.
Lacking a spectrum analyzer, the hearing conservationist can obtain a very rough indication of the
dominant spectral content of a noise by using an SLM and taking measurements in both dBA and
dBC for the same noise. If the (dBC 2 dBA) value is large, that is, about 5 dB or more, then it can be
concluded that the noise has considerable low frequency content. If, on the other hand, the
(dBC 2 dBA) value is negative, then the noise clearly has strong midrange components, since the A-
weighting curve exhibits slight amplification in the 2000 to 4000 Hz range. Such rules-of-thumb rely
on the differences in the C- and A-weighting curves shown in Figure 31.2. However, they should not
be relied upon in lieu of a spectrum analysis if the noise is believed to have high frequency or narrow
band components that need noise control attention.
correct SPL for a standard reference calibrator output at a specified SPL and frequency (most often 94 dB
at 1000 Hz). The posttest calibration is done to determine if the instrumentation, including the micro-
phone, has drifted during the measurement and if so, if the drift is large enough to invalidate the data
obtained. Calibrators may be electronic transducer-type devices with loudspeaker outputs from an
internal oscillator, or “pistonphones,” which use a reciprocating piston in a closed cavity to produce sinu-
soidal pressure variations as the cylinder volume changes. Both types include adapters that allow the
device to be mated to microphones of different diameters. Calibrators should be sent to the factory
for annual calibration. SLMs and dosimeters used for occupational noise measurements should also
be factory calibrated on an annual basis.
There are many other issues that bear on the proper application of sound level measurement equip-
ment, such as microphone selection and placement, averaging time and sampling schemes, and statistical
data reduction techniques, all of which are beyond the scope of this chapter. Further coverage of measure-
ment and instrumentation appears in Harris.20
8
< for 3-dB exchange, q ¼ 10:0
q ¼ Q= log10 (2) for 4-dB exchange, q ¼ 13:3
:
for 5-dB exchange, q ¼ 16:6
" #
1X N
The equivalent continuous sound level, or Leq, equals the continuous sound level, which, when integrated
or averaged over a specific time, would result in the same energy as a variable sound level over the same
time period. The equation for Leq, which uses a 3-dB exchange rate, is
" #
1X N
In applying the Leq, usually the individual Li values are in dBA. Equation (31.9) may also be used to
compute the overall equivalent continuous sound level (for a single site or worker) from individual
Leq’s that are obtained over contiguous time intervals by substituting the Leq values in the Li variable.
Leq values are often expressed with the time period over which the average is obtained, for instance,
Leq (24) is an equivalent continuous level measured over a 24-h period. Another average measure that
is derived from the Leq and often used for community noise quantification is the Ldn. The Ldn is
simply a 24-h Leq measurement with a 10-dB penalty added to all nighttime noise levels from 10 p.m.
31-20 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
to 7 a.m. The rationale for the penalty is that humans are more disturbed by noise, especially due to sleep
arousal, during nighttime periods.
The equation for the OSHA average noise level, or LOSHA, which uses a 5-dB exchange rate, is
" #
1X N
OSHA’s TWA is a special case of LOSHA, which requires that the total time period always be 8 h, that time
is expressed in hours, and that sound levels below 80 dBA, termed the threshold level, are not included in
the measurement:
" #
1X N
OSHA’s noise dose is a percentage representation of the noise exposure, where 100% is the maximum
allowable dose, corresponding to a 90-dBA TWA referenced to 8 h. Dose utilizes a criterion sound
level, which is presently 90 dBA, and a criterion exposure period, which is presently 8 h. A noise dose
of 50% corresponds to a TWA of 85 dBA, and this is known as the OSHA action level. Calculation of
dose, D, is as follows:
100 X
N
Noise dose, D, can also be expressed as follows, for a constant sound level over the workday:
C1 C2 Cn
D ¼ 100 þ þ þ
T1 T2 Tn
(where Ci is the total time (hours) of actual exposure at
Li ; Ti is total time (hours) of reference allowed exposure at
Li , from TableG-16a of OSHA19 ;
Ci =Ti represents a partial dose at sound level i) (31:13)
T, the reference allowable exposure for a given sound level, can also, in lieu of consulting Table G-16a in
OSHA,19 be computed as
8
T¼ (where L is the measured dBA level) (31:14)
2(L90)=5
Noise in Industry 31-21
Two other useful equations to compute dose, D, from TWA and vice versa are
TWA can also be found for each value of dose, D, in Table A-1 of OSHA.19
A final measure that is particularly useful for quantifying the exposure due to single or multiple occur-
rences of an acoustical event (such as a complete operating cycle of a machine, a vehicle drive-by, or air-
craft flyover), is the sound exposure level, or SEL. It has also been suggested for use in exposure regulations
for industry, but to date has not been incorporated into OSHA requirements. The SEL represents a sound
of one second length that imparts the same acoustical energy as a varying or constant sound that is inte-
grated over a specified time interval, ti, in seconds. Over ti, an Leq is obtained, which indicates that SEL is
used only with a 3-dB exchange rate. A reference duration of 1 sec is applied for t0 in the following
equation for SEL:
ti
SEL ¼ Leq þ 10 log10 (where Leq is the equivalent sound pressure
t0
level measured over time period ti ) (31:17)
Equation (31.16) (or OSHA, Table A-119) is then used to compute the TWA:
75
TWA ¼ 16:61 log10 þ 90 ¼ 87:9 dBA per 8-h day
100
Note: As shown in this example, regardless of the total workday, the OSHA method references everything
to an 8-h criterion, with PEL of 90 dBA TWA. This problem could also have been solved by application of
Equations (31.11) and (31.12).
an 80 dBA threshold sound level, and a 5-dB exchange rate, then the OSHA dose may be read directly
from the meter regardless of the fact that the total measurement period is 12 h. The TWA can be then
be computed using Equation (31.16) (or OSHA code, Table A-119):
300
TWA ¼ 16:61 log10 þ 90 ¼ 97:9 dBA
100
Next, Equation (31.15) is used to compute the dose from the TWA:
D ¼ 100 10((97:990)=16:61)
D ¼ 299%
Example 4. Workshift greater than 8-h, dosimeter measurement for only partial workshift
A dosimeter is worn by an employee for 7 h of a 12-h workshift. It was not possible to apply the dosi-
meter for the full shift, but it has been determined, based on discussion with employees and direct obser-
vation that the entire workshift is consistent in regard to work activity. The dose measured for the 7-h
period is 115%. Note that this dose is based on only 7 h of data and that the OSHA criterion exposure
period of 8 h is reflected in the dose calculation from the meter. Since only 7 h of data are included, the
dose is lower than that which would occur during a full 12-h shift.
Because the entire workshift is consistent with respect to noise-producing work activity, it is reasonable
to assume that the same rate of dose per hour would continue through the complete shift.
The 7-h sampling period included: (1) one 15-min rest break and (2) one 30-min meal break. The
remaining 5-h period that was not sampled does include one 15-min break
7 h sampled less the total of meal/breaks of 45 min ¼ 375 min in noise
Total 12-h shift ¼ (12 60) 2 60 min of meal/breaks ¼ 660 min in noise
The 12-h shift dose can be computed via either of the following methods:
1. Set up a proportional relationship as follows:
115% dose
¼ 0:3067% dose per minute
375 minutes
D ¼ 660 minutes 0:3067% dose=minute ¼ 202:4%
and products provided by external noise control consultants, audiology or medical personnel who
conduct the hearing measurement program, and vendors (e.g., hearing protection suppliers). Further-
more, government regulatory agencies, such as OSHA and MSHA, have a responsibility to maintain
and disseminate up-to-date noise exposure regulations and HCP guidance, to conduct regular in-
plant monitoring of noise exposure and quality of HCPs, and to provide strict enforcement where
inadequate noise control and hearing protection exists. And finally, the “end user” of the HCP, that is,
the worker himself/herself, must be an informed and motivated participant. For instance, if a fundamen-
tal component of the HCP is the personal use of HPDs, the effectiveness of the program in preventing
NIHL will depend most heavily on the worker’s commitment to properly and consistently wear the HPD.
Failure by any of these groups to carry out their responsibilities can result in HCP failure and worker
hearing loss.
ACOSTICAL
ENGINEER ENGINEERING
CONTROLS
WARNING HEARING
SIGNALS PROTECTORS
SAFETY
ENGINEER
AUDIOMETRIC
FB
RECORDKEEPING
AND DATABASE
AUDIOLOGIST,
NURSE, OR M.D. HCP FB
FB DEVELOPMENT
AND
AUDIOMETRY NOISE
FB MANAGEMENT
MONITORING
AND
EMPLOYEE EVALUATION
NOISE
EXPOSURE
JOB ROTATION RECORDS
AND
SCHEDULING
MOTIVATION
AWARENESS
WORK
SUPERVISOR EDUCATION
AND
TRAINING
EQUIPMENT
MANAGEMENT PROCUREMENT
NOISE CRITERIA
HCP
COORDINATOR
KEY:
FB
All OSHA-related audiograms must include 500, 1000, 2000, 3000, 4000, and 6000 Hz, in comparison to
most clinical audiograms that extend from 125 to 8000 Hz. If an STS is revealed, a licensed physician or
audiologist must review the audiogram and determine the need for further audiological or otological
evaluation, the employee must be notified of the STS, and the selection and proper use of HPDs must
be revisited. An annual audiogram is substituted for the original baseline when the STS is determined
to be persistent or when the annual audiogram indicates significant improvement over the baseline.
headband is useful for storing the device around the neck when the user moves out of the noise. Earmuffs
consist of earcups, usually of a rigid plastic material with an absorptive liner, that completely enclose the
outer ear and seal around it with foam- or fluid-filled cushions. A headband connects the earcups, and on
some models this band is adjustable so that it can be worn over the head, behind the neck, or under the
chin, depending upon the presence of other headgear, such as a welder’s mask. In general terms, as a
group, earplugs provide better attenuation than earmuffs below about 500 Hz and equivalent or
greater protection above 2000 Hz. At intermediate frequencies, earmuffs typically have the advantage
in attenuation. Earmuffs are generally more easily fit by the user than earplugs or canal caps, and depend-
ing on the temperature and humidity of the environment, the earmuff can be uncomfortable (in hot or
high humidity environments) or a welcome ear insulator (in a cold environment). Semi-inserts generally
offer less attenuation and comfort than earplugs or earmuffs, but because they are readily storable around
the neck, they are convenient for those workers who frequently move in and out of noise. A thorough
review of HPDs and their application may be found in Berger and Casali.21 Recent new technologies
in hearing protection have emerged, including electronic devices offering active noise cancellation, com-
munications capabilities, and noise-level-dependent attenuation, as well as passive, mechanical HPDs
that offer level-dependent attenuation and near-flat or uniform attenuation spectra; these devices are
reviewed in Casali and Berger.22
Regardless of its general type, HPD effectiveness depends heavily on the proper fitting and use of the
devices.23 Therefore, the employer is required to provide training in the fitting, care, and use of HPDs to
all affected employees.19 Hearing protector use becomes mandatory when the worker has not undergone
the baseline audiogram, has experienced an STS, or has a TWA exposure that meets or exceeds 90 dBA. In
the case of the worker with an STS, the HPD must attenuate the noise to 85 dBA TWA or below. Other-
wise, the HPD must reduce the noise to at least 90 dBA TWA.
The protective effectiveness or adequacy of an HPD for a given noise exposure must be determined by
applying the attenuation data required by the EPA24 to be included on protector packaging. These data
are obtained from psychophysical threshold tests at nine 1/3-octave bands with centers from 125 to
8000 Hz that are performed on human subjects, and the difference between the thresholds with the
HPD on and without it constitutes the attenuation at a given frequency. Spectral attenuation statistics
(means and standard deviations) and the single number noise reduction rating (NRR), which is com-
puted therefrom, are provided. The ratings are the primary means by which end users compare different
HPDs on a common basis and make determinations of whether adequate protection and OSHA compli-
ance will be attained for a given noise environment.
The most accurate method of determining HPD adequacy is to use octave band measurements of the
noise and the spectral mean and standard deviation attenuation data to determine the ‘protected exposure
level’ under the HPD. This is called the ‘NIOSH long method’ or the ‘octave band’ method. Computational
procedures appear in NIOSH.25 Because this method requires octave band measurements of the noise,
preferably with each noise band’s data in TWA form, the data requirements are large and the method
is not widely applied in industry. However, because the noise spectrum is compared against the attenu-
ation spectrum of the HPD, a “matching” of exposure to protector can be obtained; therefore, the
method is considered to be the most accurate available.
The NRR represents a means of collapsing the spectral attenuation data into one broadband attenu-
ation estimate that can easily be applied against broadband dBC or dBA TWA noise exposure measure-
ments. In the calculation of the NRR, the mean attenuation is reduced by two standard deviations; this
translates into an estimate of protection theoretically achievable by 98% of the population.24 The NRR is
primarily intended to be subtracted from the dBC exposure TWA to estimate the protected exposure level
in dBA, as via the following equation:
Unfortunately, because OSHA regulations require that noise exposure monitoring be performed in dBA,
the dBC values may not be readily available to the hearing conservationist. In the case where the TWA
Noise in Industry 31-27
values are in dBA, the NRR can still be applied, albeit with some loss of accuracy. With dBA data, a 7-dB
“safety” correction is applied to the NRR to account for the largest typical differences between C- and A-
weighted measurements of industrial noise, and the equation is as follows:
While these methods are promulgated by OSHA19 for determining HPD adequacy for a given noise situ-
ation, a word of caution is needed. The data appearing on HPD packaging are obtained under optimal
laboratory conditions with properly fitted protectors and trained human subjects. In no way does the
“experimenter-fit” protocol and other aspects of the current test procedure (ANSI S3.19-1974)26 rep-
resent the conditions under which HPDs are selected, fit, and used in the workplace.23 Therefore, the
attenuation data used in the octave band or NRR formulae are highly inflated and cannot be assumed
as representative of the protection that will be achieved in the field. The results of a review of research
studies in which manufacturers’ on-package NRRs were compared against NRRs computed from
actual subjects taken with their HPDs from field settings are shown in Figure 31.8.27 Clearly, the differ-
ences between laboratory and field estimates of HPD attenuation are large and the hearing conservation-
ist must take this into account when selecting protectors. Efforts by ANSI Working Group S12/WG11
focused on the development of a new testing standard, ANSI S12.6-1997(R2002),28 which utilizes
subject (not experimenter) fitting of the HPD and relatively naive (not trained) subjects to yield attenu-
ation data that are more representative of those achievable under workplace conditions wherein an HCP
is operated (described in Royster, Ref. 29). However, at the time of working this new standard had not
been adopted into law promulgating its use in producing the data to be utilized in labeling HPD per-
formance (although it is likely to happen in the future).
If the currently available HPD attenuation data are inaccurate, what steps should be taken to gain a
more accurate estimate of the NRR for use in determining protected exposure levels? The OSHA30
Field Operations Manual of the Office of General Industry Compliance Assistance indicates: “Citations
for violations of 29CFR 1910.95(b)(1) shall be issued when engineering and/or administrative controls
are feasible, both technically and economically; and (1) Employee exposure levels are so high that hearing
30
Laboratory
Noise Reduction Rating (dB)
25
20
15
10 Field
0
3E
He M c. m A
R e
am
23 e
So Cus wn
d- m
g Mk s
13
-1
E- -fla 00
P/ 1R
PO V-5 t
V
U n
EP n
i
A- ng
on is
is H9
aF
llb SA uf
Ba
un to
So
No I
7P
UF
o
ls o
3 1
fo
D
Bi N
ltr
rH
lto
to
Pe
lso
l
M
Pe
Bi
er
Plugs Muffs
FIGURE 31.8 Comparison of hearing protection device NRRs by device type: manufacturers’ laboratory data
versus real-world “field” data. (Adapted from Berger, E. H., Franks, J. R., and Lindgren, F. (1996). International
review of field studies of hearing protector attenuation. In Axelsson, A., Borchgrevink, H., Hamernik, R. P.,
Hellstrom, P., Henderson, D., and Salvi, R. J. (Eds.), Scientific Basis of Noice-Induced Hearing Loss (pp. 361– 377).
New York: Thiesse Medical Publishers, Inc. With permission.)
31-28 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
protectors alone may not reliably reduce noise levels received by the employee’s ear to the levels specified
in Tables G-16 or G-16a of the Standard. Given the present state of the art, hearing protectors which offer
the greatest attenuation may not reliably be used when employees exposure levels border on 100 dBA.”
This guideline alludes to the importance of engineering controls as a primary countermeasure against
high noise levels. The OSHA31 Technical Manual of the Directorate of Technical Support states:
“OSHA experience and the published scientific literature indicate that laboratory-obtained real-ear
attenuation data for hearing protectors are seldom achieved in the workplace.” . . . Under “Field Attenu-
ation of Hearing Protection”: “When analyzing the attenuation a personal hearing protector may afford a
noise-exposed employee in an actual work environment, the hearing protector shall be evaluated as
follows: . . . 2) To adjust for the lack of attainment of the laboratory-based noise reduction calculated
according to Appendix B [laboratory ratings] estimating techniques, apply a safety factor of 50
percent; that is, divide the calculated laboratory-based attenuation by two. 3) For dual protection
(i.e., earplugs and muffs) add 5 dB to the NRR of the higher-rated protector.” For case (2), the derating
factor may appear to be a reasonable strategy; however, these authors and others have argued that a con-
stant derating factor is not appropriate because certain protectors (e.g., earmuffs) are easier to fit prop-
erly than others (e.g., user-formed earplugs), and thus the differences between laboratory and actual in-
workplace performance will not be the same for all devices. In perusing Figure 31.8, this becomes quite
apparent in that the laboratory NRRs for earplugs overestimate the field NRRs by an average of about
75%, while the laboratory NRRs for earmuffs overestimate the field NRRs by an average of only about
40%. These data would argue for the use of derating factors that differ by device type, not a constant
derating such as the 50% OSHA recommendation. But in any case, the use of derating factors or
other modifications of the NRR to adjust it for field applications is tenuous at best and should not be
expected of the end user. The best solution is to establish a testing standard (and attenuation rating there-
from) that accurately predicts workplace protection achieved by HPDs, and this is the ANSI standard
work described in Royster et al.29
Noise exposure records may be used as feedback to identify machines that need maintenance attention,
to assist in the relocation of noisy equipment during plant layout efforts, to provide information for
future equipment procurement decisions, and to target plant areas that are in need of noise control inter-
vention. Some employers plot noise levels on a “contour map,” delineating floor areas by their decibel
levels. When monitoring indicates that the noise level in a particular contour has changed, it is taken
as a sign that the machinery or work process has changed in the area and that further evaluation may
be needed.
Sound shield,
absorbing
Flexible
pipe
Control
room
Door with
sealing
strips
Vibration
isolation
Double glass with
large interval
between, with
stripping
Noisy equipment
in basement
Sound Placement of heavy,
insulating vibrating equipment
joints on separate plates
with pillars
FIGURE 31.9 Examples of noise control implementation in an industrial plant. (Adapted from OSHA (1980) Noise
Control, A Guide for Workers and Employers. Occupational Safety and Health Administration Report No. 3048.
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Labor. With permission.)
creates difficulties for the operators in carrying out their jobs, they may tend to modify or remove it,
rendering it ineffective.
31.5.2.2.7 Personnel
As shown in Figure 31.7, multiple individuals play important roles in an industrial HCP, and the program
should filter down from management personnel who must demonstrably support it. The key individual
is the HCP coordinator (at the lower left in Figure 31.7), typically a permanent employee of the company
but sometimes an outside consultant, who serves as the responsible individual and overseer for the
program as well as its internal “champion.” This individual, if properly qualified, may also be responsible
for implementation of certain aspects of the program, including noise monitoring, audiometry on
employees, selection and purchase of hearing protection devices (HPDs), and other functions. The
HCP coordinator often heads a hearing conservation committee with representatives from labor, man-
agement, plant engineering, and safety. The coordinator also serves as a link between management and
Noise in Industry 31-31
the workforce, and generally participates in management decisions that impact the noise environment
or the HCP itself. For instance, one means of noise control is to establish a procurement policy that
limits the decibel output of new equipment to a prespecified level; the HCP coordinator should be
involved in such purchase decisions and in ensuring that criteria for noise emissions are met.
An audiologist, nurse, otolaryngologist, or other physician may conduct audiometric tests on employ-
ees and maintain a database for the test records. Industrial audiometry for OSHA purposes may also be
conducted by a technician who is certified by the Council of Accreditation in Occupational Hearing Con-
servation (CAOHC), but this individual must ultimately be responsible to a professional audiologist or
physician. The person who performs the audiometric test function may also be involved in helping the
worker select an appropriate HPD (with input from the noise exposure records) and in educating and
training the worker about the hazards of noise and the proper use of protection.
The work supervisor or foreman may also provide input to the HCP. For instance, in cases where
workers are rotated on and off noisy machines to limit their exposures (a type of administrative counter-
measure), the supervisor should be consulted to determine feasible rotation schemes. Furthermore, it is
imperative that the supervisor exhibits good hearing conservation practice himself/herself and provide
specific feedback to the HCP coordinator about occurrences that impact the success of the HCP, such as a
machine that has become noisy due to lack of maintenance or a worker who is uncomfortable with his/
her assigned HPD and therefore repeatedly takes it on and off. Because of his/her close relationship and
proximity to production employees, the foreman or supervisor can serve as a key individual in helping to
motivate the workers to exercise good hearing conservation practice, both by serving as a role model and
an information resource.
Some large companies have an acoustical engineer on staff while others may need to hire such an indi-
vidual when engineering noise control becomes necessary. The acoustical engineer can perform in-depth
spectral analyses of specific noise sources and design noise control solutions. Furthermore, acoustical
engineers can be helpful in the overall design of the HCP, in that the specialized knowledge they
possess will be useful in considering tradeoffs in dollar cost-to-decibel reduction benefits when compar-
ing various countermeasure strategies.
If the company has a safety engineer on staff, this individual should serve on the hearing conservation
committee and participate in noise-related decisions that impact safety in other ways. For instance, if
noise levels increase in an area where acoustic alarms signal the approach of an automated material trans-
port vehicle, the safety engineer will need to work to increase the alarm’s output to maintain detectability
or use an alternate warning system, such as a flashing strobe, to maintain vehicle conspicuity. The safety
engineer may also work with the HCP coordinator in selecting appropriate hearing protection for
employees who must maintain communications in hazardous areas. In some small companies, the
safety engineer may, in fact, have responsibility for the HCP itself.
Involvement and commitment of the proper hearing conservation and safety personnel, support of
company management, and a trained and motivated workforce are all important to the success of a prop-
erly designed and implemented industrial hearing conservation program. Such a program can markedly
reduce noise-induced distractions and interference on the job, and, above all, prevent the tragic and irre-
coverable occurrence of occupational hearing loss in workers.
References
1. Stevens, S. S. (1936). A scale for the measurement of a psychological magnitude: loudness. Psycho-
logical Review, 43, 405 –416.
2. Stevens, S. S. (1972). Perceived level of noise by Mark VII and decibels (E). Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America, 51(2, pt. 2), 575 –601.
3. Zwicker, E. (1960). En verfahren zur berechnung der lautstarke. Acustica, 10, 304 –308.
4. Kryter, K. D. (1994). The Handbook of Hearing and the Effects of Noise. New York: Academic Press.
5. Sanders, M. S. and McCormick, E. J. (1993). Human Factors in Engineering and Design, 7th edition.
New York: McGraw-Hill.
31-32 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
6. Poulton, E. (1978). A new look at the effects of noise: a rejoinder. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 1068 –
1079.
7. Sorkin, R. D. (1987). Design of auditory and tactile displays. In Salvendy, G. (Ed.), Handbook of
Human Factors (pp. 549–576). New York: McGraw-Hill.
8. ANSI S3.5 –1997 (R2002). Methods for the Calculation of the Speech Intelligibility Index. New York:
American National Standards Institute, Inc.
9. ISO 7731:2003 (2003). Ergonomics — Danger Signals for Public and Work Places — Auditory Danger
Signals. Geneva, Switzerland: International Organization for Standardization.
10. Acton, W. I. (1970). Speech intelligibility in a background noise and noise-induced hearing loss.
Ergonomics, 13(5), 546 –554.
11. Pollack, I. (1958). Speech intelligibility at high noise levels: effects of short-term exposure. Journal of
the Acoustical Society of America, 30, 282 –285.
12. Morrison, H. B. and Casali, J. G. (1994). Intelligibility of synthesized voice messages in commercial
truck cab noise for normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. Proceedings of the 1994 Human
Factors and Ergonomics Society 38th Annual Conference, Nashville, Tennessee, October 24 –28,
pp. 801 –805.
13. EPA (1981). Noise in America: the Extent of the Noise Problem. Environmental Protection Agency
Report No. 550/9-81-101. Washington, DC: EPA.
14. National Institutes of Health (NIH) Consensus Development Panel (1990). Noise and hearing loss.
Journal of the American Medical Association, 263(23), 3185–3190.
15. Melnick, W. (1991). Hearing loss from noise exposure. In Harris, C. M. (Ed.), Handbook of Acoustical
Measurements and Noise Control (pp. 18.1–18.19). New York: McGraw-Hill.
16. Taylor, W., Pearson, J., Mair, A., and Burns, W. (1964). Study of noise and hearing in jute weavers.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 38, 113–120.
17. ANSI S1.4–1983 (R2001). Specification for Sound Level Meters. New York: American National Stan-
dards Institute, Inc.
18. Peterson, A. P. G. (1979). Noise measurements: instruments. In Harris, C. M. (Ed.), Handbook of
Noise Control (pp. 5-1–5-19). New York: McGraw-Hill.
19. OSHA (1983). 29CFR1910.95. Occupational Noise Exposure; Hearing Conservation Amendment; Final
Rule. Occupational Safety and Health Administration. Code of Federal Regulations, Title 29, Chapter
XVII, Part 1910, Subpart G, 48 FR 9776–9785. Washington, DC: Federal Register.
20. Harris, C. M. (1991), Handbook of Acoustical Measurements and Noise Control. New York: McGraw-
Hill.
21. Berger, E. H. and Casali, J. G. (1997). Hearing protection devices. In Crocker, M. J. (Ed.), Encyclo-
pedia of Acoustics. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
22. Casali, J. G. and Berger, E. H. (1996). Technology advancements in hearing protection: active noise
reduction, frequency/amplitude-sensitivity, and uniform attenuation. American Industrial Hygiene
Association Journal, 57, 175 –185.
23. Park, M. Y. and Casali, J. G. (1991). A controlled investigation of in-field attenuation performance of
selected insert, earmuff, and canal cap hearing protectors. Human Factors, 33(6), 693 –714.
24. EPA (1979). 40CFR211, Noise labeling requirements for hearing protectors. Environmental Protec-
tion Agency, Federal Register, 44(190), 56130 –56147.
25. NIOSH (1975). List of Personal Hearing Protectors and Attenuation Data. National Institute for
Occupational Safety and Health-HEW Publication No. 76-120, pp. 21–37. Washington, DC:
NIOSH.
26. ANSI S3.19-1974 (1974). Method for the Measurement of Real-Ear Protection of Hearing Protectors
and Physical Attenuation of Earmuffs. New York: American National Standards Institute, Inc.
27. Berger, E. H., Franks, J. R., and Lindgren, F. (1996). International review of field studies of hearing
protector attenuation. In Axelsson, A., Borchgrevink, H., Hamernik, R. P., Hellstrom, P., Henderson,
D., and Salvi, R. J. (Eds.), Scientific Basis of Noise-Induced Hearing Loss (pp. 361–377). New York:
Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc.
Noise in Industry 31-33
28. ANSI S12.6-1997 (R2002) Methods for Measuring the Real-Ear Attenuation of Hearing Protectors.
New York: American National Standards Institute, Inc.
29. Royster, J. D., Berger, E. H., Merry, C. J., Nixon, C. W, Franks, J. R., Behar, A., Casali, J. G., Dixon-
Ernst, C., Kieper, R. W., Mozo, B. T., Ohlin, D., and Royster, L. H. (1996). Development of a new
standard laboratory protocol for estimating the field attenuation of hearing protection devices.
Part I. Research of Working Group 11, Accredited Standards Committee S12, Noise. Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America, 99(3), 1506–1526.
30. OSHA (1989). OSHA Instruction CPL 2.45B, June 15. Field Operations Manual (pp. IV-33–IV-35).
Rockville, MD: Government Institutes, Inc.
31. OSHA (1990). OSHA Instruction CPL 2-2.20B, February 5. Field Technical Manual (pp. 4-1– 4-15).
Rockville, MD: Government Institutes, Inc.
32. Royster, J. D. and Royster, L. H. (1986). Audiometric data base analysis. In Berger, E. H., Ward, W. D.,
Morrill, J. C., and Royster, L. H. (Eds.), Noise and Hearing Conservation Manual (pp. 293–317).
Akron, OH: American Industrial Hygiene Association.
33. Zohar, D., Cohen, A., and Azar, N. (1980). Promoting increased use of ear protectors in noise
through information feedback. Human Factors, 22(1), 69 –79.
34. Bruce, R. D. and Toothman, E. H. (1986). Engineering controls. In Berger, E. H., Ward, W. D.,
Morrill, J. C., and Royster, L. H. (Eds.), Noise and Hearing Conservation Manual (pp. 417–521).
Akron, OH: American Industrial Hygiene Association.
35. Earshen, J. J. (1986). Sound measurement: instrumentation and noise descriptors. In Berger, E. H.,
Ward, W. D., Morrill, J. C., and Royster, L. H. (Eds.), Noise and Hearing Conservation Manual
(pp. 38–95). Akron, OH: American Industrial Hygiene Association.
36. OSHA (1980). Noise Control, A Guide for Workers and Employers. Occupational Safety and Health
Administration Report No. 3048. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Labor.
32
Shiftwork
32.1 Introduction
The ergonomic problem with regard to shiftwork is that of enabling the individual to work at abnor-
mal hours, an activity which runs both counter to his or her own biology (Homo sapiens is a diurnal
species) and counter to the surrounding society which is structured to protect the sleep of day
workers, but not that of night workers, and expects evenings and weekends to be free for social, reli-
gious, athletic, and cultural events. Moreover, shiftwork is not simply restricted to a very small group
of people who can be carefully selected or self-selected to experience minimal problems. Neither is it
restricted to the youngest and fittest of workers who can bid their way out of abnormal hours when
they advance into their middle age. Employment trends, particularly in the manufacturing sector, now
dictate that the “bidding out of shiftwork by seniority” option often no longer applies. For many
middle-aged and late middle-aged workers the only option is between shiftwork and no work.
Thus, approximately one fifth of all employees are engaged in some form of work that requires
their presence outside of the “standard” 7 am to 6 pm working day on a regular basis, and can
thus be regarded as “shiftworkers.”
The proportion of the working population engaged in shiftwork can be expected to rise as second
jobbing and mandatory overtime increase.1 The fastest growing sector of most Western economies is
the service sector, and people are increasingly demanding and receiving around the clock availability
of such services. Even in the production sector, plant machinery has become so expensive and so
quickly obsolete that it has to be run 24 h per day, 7 days per week, in order for it to be profitable. Also,
many nations have adopted taxation and business evaluation strategies (e.g., in assessing profitability
and providing health insurance) that encourage employers to squeeze as many work hours per year as
possible from their existing employees, rather than hiring new ones, as the volume of business increases.
This may lead to extended work weeks and fewer different work teams covering each 24-h day.
Some people cope well with shiftwork, others poorly. Moore-Ede2 and others have referred to a shift-
work maladaptation syndrome in those failing to cope. As noted above, shiftwork intolerance stems pri-
marily from the fact that we are a diurnal species, designed to be asleep at night and alert and active
during the day, and that we have constructed a society which is built around this biological reality.
32-1
32-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
However, shiftwork intolerance is a problem that should not be regarded as solely a circadian rhythms
(“biological clock”) issue, or, indeed, solely a sleep disorders issue, or solely a social and domestic
issue.3 Rather, it is a complex interaction of the three factors, with each factor influencing both of the
other factors and the final outcome of shiftwork tolerance.4 The long-term health consequences of shift-
work have been reviewed extensively elsewhere.5 – 7 In addition to sleep disorders, gastrointestinal dys-
function, cancer, and cardiovascular disease have been the major complaints implicated, and
shiftworkers should be encouraged to abstain from behaviors (such as unwise dietary choices) that
might further exacerbate such risks. There may also be psychiatric disorders such as depression and sub-
stance abuse resulting from prolonged exposure to shiftwork. The aim of this chapter is to introduce a
general conceptual framework within which shiftwork coping ability may be considered, so that the ergo-
nomist can better understand the various factors that are involved.
As noted earlier, shiftwork coping ability can be considered to be the product of a mutually interactive
triad of factors: (1) circadian, (2) sleep, and (3) social/domestic. Circadian factors stem from the indi-
vidual’s biological clock, which has been shown to be endogenous and self-sustaining under conditions of
temporal isolation.8 Sleep factors are, of course, intimately bound up with the circadian ones, but have a
greater significance for the shiftworkers themselves and are thus more likely to appear in shiftworker
complaints.9 Domestic factors (including social and community aspects) are often neglected in terms
of empirical research10 but can be equally important as determinants of shiftwork coping ability, and cer-
tainly influence the behavior of the shiftworker in relation to the other two factors.11 The three factors are
discussed in the following sections, with emphasis placed on interactions and interrelationships.
the realignment of the circadian system. For the shiftworker, however, the physical zeitgebers are
resolutely opposed to a nocturnal alignment, as are most of the social zeitgebers stemming from a day-
oriented society. Much research has thus focused on enhancing the zeitgebers that may encourage a noc-
turnal circadian orientation in night workers. The discovery of the strong zeitgeber effects of bright lights
led to a series of studies using very bright artificial light to assist in changing the phase of the circadian
system. Typically, the bright light exposure regimens required at least 3 h of .3000 Lux exposure from a
bank of florescent tubes in a light box. Eastman15 conducted a careful series of experiments using both
student volunteers and real shiftworkers to assess the utility of bright artificial light in helping shift-
workers to cope. As with other investigations, however, (e.g., Czeisler and colleagues16), these studies
indicated that darkness during the sleep period was almost as important as the light at work. Thus,
complete bedroom light proofing was required, and Eastman and colleagues17 also showed that dark
sunglasses or welder’s goggles usually needed to be worn during the morning commute home from
night work for the required circadian system phase delay to be accomplished.
The case can still be made quite strongly, however, for nighttime workplace lighting to be increased
in brightness by whatever amount is financially and operationally possible. Several studies have shown
that bright light on the night shift definitely increases alertness even when the light is of insufficient
intensity to induce a strong resetting of the circadian system. Thus, the improvement in nighttime alert-
ness rendered by the light appears to be mediated by some other alerting mechanism than an enhanced
phase resetting of the circadian system. Having said that, it should be noted that several authors (e.g.,
Boivin and collegues,18 Martin and Eastman19) have shown that even moderate levels of night shift
illumination can phase shift the circadian system, albeit less strongly than is achieved by very bright
light.
Another way of enhancing circadian adjustment is by taking melatonin pills — a strategy used by many
night workers in the United States following the attention given to that hormone in the popular press,
and its availability there without a prescription. While there is some laboratory evidence for the effective-
ness of melatonin as a chronobiotic,20 its effects are comparatively weak compared to those of daylight
and are likely to be washed out for many shiftworkers. Slightly different is the concept of using melatonin
pills to facilitate daytime sleep (without necessarily changing the timing of the circadian pacemaker).
However, although there is good laboratory evidence for such facilitation, double-blind studies of
melatonin effects in actual shiftworkers have resulted in few definitive improvements in the quality
and duration of daytime sleep.21,22 Concerns also remain regarding chronic use of melatonin pills
whose safety and purity cannot always be guaranteed since their production and sale in the United
States is unregulated.
In all but the most socially isolated shiftworkers, attention must be paid to behavior during off-duty
(“weekend type”) breaks. The process of circadian realignment for the night worker can be likened to a
salmon leaping up a waterfall; it is difficult to achieve a nocturnal orientation (i.e., reach the top of the
waterfall), but easy indeed to fall back down to a diurnal orientation, since that is the natural state for the
human organism. That asymmetry becomes vitally important when social and domestic influences
during days off lead to daytime activity, particularly when it is outdoors, resulting in daylight exposure.
Few parents would forgo attending their child’s Saturday morning soccer game simply to preserve their
nocturnal orientation. Thus, although a worker may be a permanent night worker as far as the company
is concerned, in reality the individual may be alternating between nocturnal and diurnal orientations,
simply because of the social and domestic concerns. On the first night after a weekend break, even
permanent night workers may have a totally diurnal circadian orientation in their temperature and
subjective alertness rhythms.23
During the process of circadian realignment, there are three mechanisms by which mood, well-being,
and performance efficiency can be adversely affected. First, sleep will be disrupted, and the individual will
be in a state of partial sleep deprivation.24 Second, the new time of wakefulness is likely to tap into the
“down phases” of various psychological functions that are normally coincident with sleep in the day-
oriented individual.7,25 Third, the various individual components of the circadian system will be in a
state of disarray, with the normal harmony of appropriate phase relationships destroyed.26 An analogy
32-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
of the circadian system under these conditions is that it is like a symphony orchestra, with a conductor on
the rostrum making sure that the various instruments are brought in at the right time. For the night
worker, it is as if a second conductor appears on the rostrum, beating at a different time. The rate at
which the different instruments switch to the new conductor varies, and until they all do, there is a
cacophony, with all harmony lost. In circadian terms, we speak of this cacophony as “desynchronosis”
or “internal dissociation” because the component circadian rhythms no longer have appropriate phase
relationships to each other. In addition to poor sleep, the symptoms of desynchronosis include
malaise, gastrointestinal dysfunction, and performance decrements.
Individual differences in circadian system characteristics may also have a role in determining shiftwork
coping ability. Individuals who are “night owls,” or “late phasers,” in their circadian system often find
shiftwork considerably easier to cope with than do “morning larks,” or “early phasers.”27 Phase differ-
ences may also explain why late-middle-aged people often find shiftwork difficult. A typical case is
that of a 50-yr-old patient who has hitherto been fairly happy with shiftwork but now finds it increasingly
difficult to cope with. In some ways, this is paradoxical, given that he has had many decades of learning
shiftwork coping strategies and that he probably has a quieter house now that his children have grown up
and he can afford better housing. The reason for the problem may be that he has become more of a
“morning lark” in circadian phase orientation. Carrier and colleagues28 have shown that many of the
sleep decrements seen in the progression through the middle years of life (even in day workers) can
be attributed to age-related changes in morningness – eveningness which can occur through a person’s
forties and fifties. Also, Campbell29 has shown that circadian manipulations designed to improve
night work tolerance may work much better for young adults than for those in middle age.
Before the discussion of circadian factors is concluded, the question must be addressed whether
circadian realignment is actually desirable, given all the caveats regarding the weekend regression to a
diurnal orientation mentioned before.12 In Europe, many companies use “rapidly rotating” systems in
which only one or two shifts are worked at a time, before a different one is worked.30 Thus, for example,
on the “continental” rotation, employees work two morning shifts, two evening shifts, and two night
shifts, followed by two days off. Most European experts favor such systems because they allow the circa-
dian system to retain its diurnal orientation, thus eliminating problems of desynchronosis. Because only
one or two night shifts are worked before time off is given, sleep loss and fatigue are minimized. The
drawbacks of rapid rotation are the circadian-related fatigue experienced during the night shifts,
which, for some tasks, may render the approach undesirable, and the workers’ difficulties in predicting
when they will be at work. However, there are undoubtedly many situations in which rapid rotation is
worthy of consideration.
and performance abilities of the worker. There are now several well-controlled studies which document
the pathological sleepiness levels exhibited by many shiftworkers, both at work35 and on the drive home
after work.36 Indeed, one could argue that the latter represents the most dangerous activity that most
shiftworkers ever engage in, and one which, in aggregate, represents a major public safety concern invol-
ving significant loss of life.37 Prophylactic naps have shown to be beneficial in reducing sleepiness in night
workers before starting a run of night duty (a 2-h nap after lunch is recommended), and some experts
favor short naps during the night shift itself, although controls have to be in place for the worker to
recover from the grogginess of sleep inertia, before operating dangerous machinery or monitoring
equipment.
Many shiftworkers assert that if only they could solve their sleep problem, then everything else
would be quite tolerable. However, because of the impact of the circadian system on sleep, disrupted
sleep may be as much a symptom of shiftwork maladjustment as a cause of it. This idea is demonstrated
clearly in a study by Walsh and colleagues38 who brought actual shiftworkers into a sound-attenuated,
electrically shielded bedroom for their sleep periods, with the subjects commuting to their work from
the laboratory rather than from home. Even in this closely protected environment, there was a highly
significant difference in duration between the day sleep of night workers and the night sleep of day
workers (306 vs. 401 min). In addition, there were reliable differences between the polysomnographic
characteristics of the sleep, with a smaller amount of REM sleep and a greater proportion of slow-
wave sleep for the night workers. Thus, even if it were economically feasible, the complete soundproofing
and lightproofing of all shiftworkers’ bedrooms would not eradicate the problem of sleep for
shiftworkers.
Circadian factors are not the only ones having an impact on a shiftworker’s sleep, however. Domestic
and social factors (see Section 33.4) are also crucial in determining the patient’s sleep quality and dur-
ation. First, the sleep of the shiftworker is not as protected by society’s taboos as that of a day worker; for
example, no one would think of phoning a day worker at 2 am, but few would have qualms about
phoning a night worker at 2 pm. Similarly, unless the shiftworker is in a well-adjusted household, his
(and more especially her) sleep is liable to be truncated by the demands of child care, shopping, and
household management. In viewing the sleep of shiftworkers, one must therefore consider both endogen-
ous and exogenous factors that are going to limit sleep time.
Sleep demands may also be as much of an influence on the other two factors in the triad as a product of
their influence. Much domestic disharmony can be attributed to the shiftworker’s need for sleep at a time
when households are usually rather noisy, and impaired mood is a classic symptom of partial sleep loss.39
Prescribed circadian rhythm coping strategies may not work because the weary shiftworker may be asleep
when he or she would ideally be experiencing bright light and activity.
Finally, in discussing the sleep of shiftworkers, one must address the issue of caffeine to promote alert-
ness, and hypnotics (sleeping pills) to promote sleep. In a study of rotating shiftworkers, Walsh and col-
leagues40 found that triazolam 0.5 mg could improve the quality and duration of day sleeps. However, the
study was also important in demonstrating that the drug had no significant “phase-resetting” effects.
Thus, on the third- and fourth-day sleeps in a run of night duty, for which no medication was given,
there were no significant differences between those who had been given triazolam on day sleeps 1 and
2 and those who had been given placebo. Moreover, when drug and placebo groups were compared in
terms of nighttime alertness and performance, no reliable differences emerged, even on the days in
which medication was given.41 One must therefore recognize that hypnotics will probably ameliorate
only the sleep factor of the triad. As a general rule, the use of hypnotics is thus inadvisable for shiftworkers
because problems of tolerance and dependence are likely to occur. It is noteworthy that most hypnotics
are intended only for “occasional transitory insomnia,” and are thus not intended to be taken daily for
months or years at a stretch. The recently available short-acting hypnotics such as zaleplon are unlikely to
be helpful to the shiftworker who is usually suffering from a problem of remaining asleep, rather than
falling asleep. One situation in which hypnotics might be sometimes appropriate is in rapidly rotating
shift systems, in which the occasional day sleep may be improved by hypnotics, and no phase resetting
is required.
32-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Caffeine is widely used by shiftworkers in order to stay awake at work, and there is empirical evidence
from the laboratory that it is effective in improving alertness. However, it is important that shiftworkers
recognize that caffeine has a half-life of between 3 and 5 h. Because of the fragility of day sleeps discussed
earlier, it is important that night workers do not further worsen their day sleep by having caffeine still in
their bloodstream when attempting a day sleep. Thus, night workers should not consume any caffeine
after 4 am.12
32.5 Solutions
The best approach to the challenge of shiftwork is one that involves both management and the work
force.3 Corporate safety officers and medical officers can be extremely helpful in this regard because
they are skilled at bridging the gap between workers and managers, and at developing long-running
education and awareness programs. Management should realize that it has not only a moral but also a
financial obligation to be sensitive to issues of shiftwork tolerance in the training of its employees and
in the selection of shift schedules. Increasing medical, recruiting, and retraining costs dictate that
poor employee morale, higher job turnover, and increased accident, ill-health and absenteeism rates
resulting from shiftwork intolerance can become a financial burden to the company or organization.
Employee education programs should emphasize the way in which circadian, sleep, and domestic
factors can influence shiftwork coping ability. Workers should be taught good sleep hygiene practice
Shiftwork 32-7
and advised how they can manipulate zeitgebers to their advantage, enhancing those that are acting in
their favor and attenuating those acting against them. They should also be taught the benefit of prophy-
lactic naps and caveats about the use of caffeine. In some cases, family counseling may be indicated to
discuss solutions to some of the social and domestic problems. The creation of self-help networks can
often be of benefit, lessening some of the social and community isolation that many shiftworkers feel.
When educational strategies fail, and the shift schedule cannot be changed, the patient may require a
change to a day-working job.
The main task with regard to management education is that of first convincing managers that there is a
problem and that shiftwork concerns cannot simply be swept under the carpet or dismissed as a problem
confined to sick or disgruntled employees who are simply not trying hard enough. Second, management
must be informed of the wide range of different shift systems that are available, including the rapidly
rotating systems so popular in Europe. Third, managers must be taught to recognize the factors (e.g.,
type of job, nature of work force, average commuting time, male –female ratio, and preponderance of
moonlighting) that should influence the selection of the optimal schedule for that work group in that
situation. For management, the “carrot” is a happy, healthy, and productive work force; the “stick” is
the specter of human error failures, such as that at the Three Mile Island nuclear power plant, and of
litigation from a work force that might consider inappropriately selected work schedules to have
adversely affected their health or their safety.
A recent tool that may help management in creating a more “shiftworker tolerant” environment is the
mathematic model. Several authors (e.g., Folkard and Akerstedt45) have developed models incorporating
both circadian and sleep loss effects as determinants of “on shift” alertness and performance. Such
models are currently in the early stages of development and need considerable refinement. Eventually,
though, they might allow for the effects of different shift schedule choices to be evaluated in computer
simulations, before they are actually imposed on the hapless shiftworker.
32.6 Conclusions
Although some people cope well with shiftwork, many others have significant problems that can
adversely affect their health and well-being. These problems can become a “shiftwork sleep disorder,”
which may be quite debilitating to the individual. Shiftwork problems can be usefully understood
using a multifaceted approach that recognizes the interaction of circadian rhythms, sleep, and social
and domestic factors in determining shiftwork coping ability.
References
1. U.S. Congress, Office of Technology Assessment, (OTA). Biological Rhythms: Implications for the
Worker (OTA-BA-463). 1991. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.
2. Moore-Ede MC. Jet lag, shift work, and maladaption. News in Physiological Sciences. 1986. 1:156 –
160.
3. Knauth P, Hornberger S. Preventive and compensatory measures for shift workers. Occupational
Medicine (London). 2003. 53:109–116.
4. Monk TH. Coping with the stress of shift work. Work & Stress. 1988. 2:169– 172.
5. Rutenfranz J, Colquhoun WP, Knauth P, et al. Biomedical and psychosocial aspects of shift work:
a review. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and Health. 1977. 3:165 –182.
6. Scott AJ, LaDou J. Shiftwork. Effects on sleep and health with recommendations for medical surveil-
lance and screening. Occupational Medicine. 1990. 5:273 –299.
7. Monk TH, Carrier J. Shift worker performance. Occupational and Environmental Medicine. 2003.
3:209–229.
8. Moore RY. The suprachiasmatic nucleus and the organization of a circadian system. Trends in Neuro-
sciences. 1982. 5(11):404–407.
9. Tepas DI, Carvalhais AB. Sleep patterns of shiftworkers. Occupational Medicine. 1990.5:199– 208.
32-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
10. Akerstedt T, Gillberg M. Night and Shift Work: Biological and Social Aspects. 1990. Oxford: Pergamon
Press.
11. Tepas DI. Shift worker sleep strategies. Journal of Human Ergology. 1982. 11(Suppl):325–326.
12. Monk TH. What can the chronobiologist do to help the shift worker? Journal of Biological Rhythms.
2000. 15:86 –94.
13. Aschoff J, Hoffman K, Pohl H, et al. Re-entrainment of circadian rhythms after phase-shifts of the
zeitgeber. Chronobiologia. 1975. 2:23–78.
14. Barnes RG, Deacon SJ, Forbes MJ, et al. Adaptation of the 6-sulphatoxymelatonin rhythm in shift-
workers on offshore oil installations during a 2-week 12-h night shift. Neuroscience Letters. 1998.
241:9 –12.
15. Eastman CI. Squashing versus nudging circadian rhythms with artificial bright light: solutions for
shift work? Perspectives in Biology and Medicine. 1991. 34,2:181 –195.
16. Czeisler CA, Johnson MP, Duffy JF, et al. Exposure to bright light and darkness to treat physiologic
maladaptation to night work. New England Journal of Medicine. 1990. 322(18):1253–1259.
17. Eastman CI, Stewart KT, Mahoney MP, et al. Dark goggles and bright light improve circadian rhythm
adaptation to night-shift work. Sleep. 1994. 17:535 –543.
18. Boivin DB, Duffy JF, Kronauer RE, et al. Dose –response relationships for resetting of human circa-
dian clock by light. Nature. 1996. 379:540 –542.
19. Martin SK, Eastman CI. Medium-intensity light produces circadian rhythm adaptation to simulated
night-shift work. Sleep. 1998. 21:154– 165.
20. Sack RL, Lewy AJ. Melatonin as a chronobiotic: treatment of circadian desynchrony in night workers
and the blind. Journal of Biological Rhythms 1997. 12:595–603.
21. Jorgensen KM, Witting MD. Does exogenous melatonin improve day sleep or night alertness in
emergency physicians working night shifts? Annals of Emergency Medicine. 1998. 31:699–704.
22. James M, Tremea MO, Jones JS, et al. Can melatonin improve adaptation to night shift? American
Journal of Emergency Medicine. 1998. 16:367 –370.
23. Monk TH. Advantages and disadvantages of rapidly rotating shift schedules — a circadian view-
point. Human Factors. 1986. 28:553–557.
24. Weitzman ED, Kripke DF, Goldmacher D, et al. Acute reversal of the sleep-waking cycle in man.
Archives of Neurology. 1970. 22:483–489.
25. Folkard S, Monk TH. Shiftwork and performance. Human Factors. 1979. 21:483–492.
26. Wever RA. The Circadian System of Man: Results of Experiments under Temporal Isolation. 1979.
New York: Springer-Verlag.
27. Monk TH, Folkard S. Individual differences in shiftwork adjustment. In: Folkard S, Monk TH, eds.
Hours of Work — Temporal Factors in Work Scheduling. 1985. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
28. Carrier J, Monk TH, Buysse DJ, et al. Sleep and morningness-eveningness in the “middle” years of life
(20y –59y). Journal of Sleep Research. 1997. 6:230–237.
29. Campbell SS. Effects of timed bright-light exposure on shift-work adaptation in middle-aged
subjects. Sleep. 1995. 18:408–416.
30. Knauth P, Rutenfranz J, Schulz H, et al. Experimental shift work studies of permanent night, and
rapidly rotating, shift systems. II. Behaviour of various characteristics of sleep. Internal Journal of
Occupational and Environmental Health. 1980. 46:111– 125.
31. Knauth P, Landau K, Droge C, et al. Duration of sleep depending on the type of shift work. Inter-
national Journal of Occupational and Environmental Health 1980. 46:167 –177.
32. Tasto DL, Colligan MJ. Health Consequences of Shift Work (Project UR11-4426). 1978. Menlo
Park,CA.: Stanford Research Institute.
33. Akerstedt T. Adjustment of physiological circadian rhythms and the sleep-wake cycle to shiftwork.
In: Folkard S, Monk TH, eds. Hours of Work: Temporal Factors in Work Scheduling. 1985.
New York: John Wiley & Sons.
34. Akerstedt T. Shift work and disturbed sleep/wakefulness. Occupational Medicine (London). 2003.
53:89–94.
Shiftwork 32-9
35. Akerstedt T, Torsvall L, Gillberg M. Sleepiness and shift work: field studies. Sleep. 1982.
5(Suppl 2):S95–S106.
36. Richardson GS, Miner JD, Czeisler CA. Impaired driving performance in shiftworkers: the role of the
circadian system in a multifactorial model. Alcohol, Drugs and Driving. 1990;5(4) and 6(1):265 –273.
37. Pack AI, Pack AM, Rodgman E, et al. Characteristics of crashes attributed to the driver having fallen
asleep. Accidental Analysis and Prevention 1995. 27:769–775.
38. Walsh JK, Tepas DI, Moss PD. The EEG sleep of night and rotating shift workers. In: Johnson LC,
Tepas DI, Colquhoun WP et al., eds. The Twenty-four Hour Workday: Proceedings of a Symposium
on Variations in Work –Sleep Schedules. 1981. Cincinnati, OH: Department of Health and Human
Services (NIOSH).
39. Horne J. Why We Sleep: The Functions of Sleep in Humans and Other Mammals. 1988. Oxford,
England: Oxford University Press.
40. Walsh JK, Muehlbach MJ, Scweitzer PK. Acute administration of triazolam for the daytime sleep of
rotating shift workers. Sleep. 1984. 7:223–229.
41. Walsh JK, Schweitzer PK, Anch AM, et al. Sleepiness/alertness on a simulated night shift following
sleep at home with triazolam. Sleep. 1991. 14 (2):140– 146.
42. Walker JM. Social problems of shift work. In: Folkard S, Monk TH, eds. Hours of Work — Temporal
Factors in Work Scheduling. 1985. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
43. Colligan MJ, Rosa RR. Shiftwork effects on social and family life. Occupational Medicine. 1990.
5:315 –322.
44. White L, Keith B. The effect of shift work on the quality and stability of marital relations. Journal of
Marriage & Family. 1990. 52(May):453 –462.
45. Folkard S, Akerstedt T. Trends in the risk of accidents and injuries and their implications for models
of fatigue and performance. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine. 2004. 75:A161–A167.
33
Vibrometry
33.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33-1
33.2 Vibration Basics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33-1
Donald E. Wasserman 33.3 Vibration Measurements Basics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33-4
D.E. Wasserman, Inc.
33.4 Occupational Vibration Standards/Guides . . . . . . . 33-7
Whole-Body Vibration Standards/Guides Used in the U.S.
David G. Wilder † Hand-Arm Vibration Standards/Guides Used in the U.S.
University of Iowa 33.5 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33-12
33.1 Introduction
There are some 8 million workers1 in the U.S. exposed to occupational whole-body vibration (WBV) or
hand-arm vibration (HAV) with resulting severe medical consequences of WBV or HAV exposures (see
text Chapter: Occupational Vibration and Cumulative Trauma Disorders). The ability to measure, quan-
tify, and evaluate the vibration impinging on the human body and relating these results to the disease
processes it produces is essential to understanding both dose– response relationships and methods for
controlling human vibration exposure. The purpose of this chapter is thus threefold: (1) To provide
an introduction to the occupational vibration measurement process; (2) to provide a basic understand-
ing of the occupational WBV and HAV health and safety standards/guides currently in use in the U.S.;
and (3) to demonstrate the interrelationships between these measurements and their respective WBV and
HAV standards/guides.
33-1
33-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
x
x
y y
ax, ay, az = acceleration in the x
directions of the x, y, z axes
x axis = back-to-chest
y axis = right-to-left side
z axis = foot (or buttocks)-to-head
FIGURE 33.1 Whole-body vibration measurements coordinate system. (ANSI S3.18, ISO 2631, ACGIH-TLV
(WBV), EU).
“X axis” motion is in the front-to-back direction (through the sternum) for WBV, and for HAV measure-
ments the motion is through the palm of the hand.
Having defined the directions of motion, the vibration magnitude or intensity parameter(s) must be
specified. We can choose between three mathematically interrelated quantities: displacement, velocity, or
acceleration. Displacement is merely the distance moved away from some reference position. Velocity (or
speed) is the time-rate-of-change of displacement. Acceleration is the time-rate-of-change of velocity.
Acceleration is usually the magnitude/intensity parameter of choice for several reasons which include
ease of measurement and the belief that acceleration is both a hard and soft tissue stressor. Acceleration
is expressed in units of meters/sec/sec or in terms of gravitational g units, where 1g ¼ 9.81 m/sec/sec.
The “peak” acceleration or maximum values are not usually evaluated, rather an average acceleration par-
ameter called root-mean-squared or rms acceleration is measured and evaluated and is relatable directly to
zh
zh
xh
Biodynamic yh
coordinate system
Basicentric
coordinate system
FIGURE 33.2 Hand-arm vibration measurements coordinate systems. (ANSI S3.34, ACGIH-TLV (HAV), EU,
NIOSH #89-106).
Vibrometry 33-3
the human vibration standards. In the rms process the measured values of acceleration are squared and
subsequently averaged to get its mean value. Finally, the square root is determined resulting in an accel-
eration value proportional to the vibration signal’s energy content (see equation 33.1).
sffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
ð
1 T 2
arms ¼ a (t) dt (33:1)
T 0
where a and b ¼ amplitude values of each sinusoid at specific frequencies composing the spectrum;
a0 ¼ dc term or zero Hertz value.
Specialized computers called Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) analyzers or real time analyzers (RTA) are
used to transform the vibration mix into its discrete frequencies.4 Each such frequency is graphically dis-
played as a series of vertical lines or spectra; the position of each line identifies its vibration frequency in
Hz and thus its place in the spectrum; the height of each line is a measure of its individual vibration accel-
eration intensity in g units or meters/sec/sec. The entire spectrum is the sum of all these lines. Vibrating
tool spectra are quite unique, depending on the tool. Vehicle spectra from trucks, buses, trains, etc., are
also unique.
The final concept to be discussed is called resonance (or natural frequency) which is an unwelcome
situation where the conditions for transferring vibration from its source (i.e., tools, vehicles, etc.) to
the human receiver are optimal. Thus, a very small magnitude of vibration impinging on a human or
a structure (such as a bridge) causes an uncontrollably amplified response by the human or structure.
This is the reason why bridges collapse if soldiers march in cadence across them. Unfortunately, we
humans have resonances too, namely, in WBV 4 to 8 Hz for Z axis vertical vibration, 1 to 2 Hz in
both the X and Y axes. Spinal resonance is 4.5 to 5.5 Hz.5 The hand-arm system seems to resonate in
the 150 to 250 Hz range.3 In general the larger the mass or weight of a structure, the lower the resonant
frequency. Equation II is the resonance equation for a simple single-degree of freedom system consisting
of motion in one direction consisting of a mass, spring, and damping element.
rffiffiffiffi
k
W¼ (33:3)
m
Resonance thus represents the Achilles heel of human response to vibration. As is the auditory system
to sound, human response to vibration is therefore frequency dependent and nonlinear because at res-
onance the impinging vibration finds its easiest pathway to the person; at other vibration frequencies, the
vibration pathway is not as easy and thus it requires more acceleration at a nonresonant frequency to
produce the same level of human response.
Accelerometers
Multichannel Fourier
Conditioning
X Monitoring Spectrum
Preamplifier
Oscilloscope Analyzer
Z YX
Selector
Conditioning Chart
Y Switch
Preamplifier Recorder
Multichannel
Conditioning DAT Tape
Z Preamplifier Recorder
X
Y Z Voice Input
Microphone
Fourier spectrum analysis can be performed separately on each data channel. Each spectrum is next
separately evaluated using the appropriate standard(s). A key element in these measurements is the
triaxial accelerometer.
Accelerometers are devices which convert mechanical motion into a corresponding electrical signal.
Two distinctly different devices are used for occupational vibration measurements. For WBV measure-
ments piezoresistive accelerometers are used; for HAV measurements piezoelectric or crystal acceler-
ometers are used.
The former device works on the principle of an electronic four arm electronic balanced bridge; P or N
semiconductors form each of the bridge arms. All of these arms are bonded to one end of a tiny metal
beam. The other end of this beam is bonded to a tiny metal mass or weight to which the force of vibration
is applied. With no vibration present, the bridge is “balanced,” yielding zero output voltage. When
vibration is applied to this tiny mass/beam combination, we obtain the acceleration of this mass
against the beam because of the bending motion of the beam compressing some of the beam arms,
thereby unbalancing the bridge, resulting in an electrical voltage proportional to acceleration; in effect
we are calculating Newton’s second law, F ¼ ma, with a vibration force and a known mass bonded to
the metal beam. The acceleration signal is very small (millivolts) and needs to be amplified using
a so-called difference or differential amplifier which measures and amplifies the voltage or potential
difference across the arms of the Wheatstone bridge. The amplifier’s output voltage is next recorded
by the DAT.
HAV measurements require a different type of accelerometer called a crystal, which is commonly
found in nature. The crystal has a phenomenon called the piezoelectric effect; if a moving force is
applied to the crystal, the crystal responds by generating a small electrical voltage across its face. The
more intense the force, the larger the voltage generated. A crystal is a force-measuring device found in
nature, not an accelerometer per se. If, however, a small weight or mass is bonded to the motion-sensitive
surface of the crystal and the force of vibration is applied to this tiny mass, we have once again created an
accelerometer, since, as before, it is the vibration force accelerating this mass against the crystal face which
results in a corresponding voltage and charge proportional to acceleration. The acceleration signal is very
small and needs to be amplified by a special charge amplifier whose output can then be recorded on a
DAT recorder.
In either WBV or HAV measurements the accelerometers must be of very light weight (less than 15
grams) and small; if not, measurement errors called mass loading result, the rule is that the total
weight of the triaxial accelerometers, mounting fixture, cables, etc., must collectively be less than 10%
of the weight of the object whose vibration is to be measured — a tool handle, for example. In all
cases great care must be taken to avoid cable entanglement and/or breakage; the shortest possible
cable length and integrity must be maintained especially from accelerometers to preamplifiers to
ensure high quality and low electrical noise signals. In the case of HAV measurements, some crystal accel-
erometers can be purchased with built-in charge amplifiers to avoid some of these problems.
WBV measurements are usually made using an instrumentated hard rubber disc about the size of a pie
plate (Figure 33.4). The disc is placed between the top of the driver’s seat cushion and the buttocks. The
center of the disc is hollow and contains three tiny accelerometers, mounted mutually perpendicular
to each other to a small metal cube. The three piezoresistive accelerometer cables lead from the disc
to preamplifiers and on to a DAT.
HAV measurements use three small lightweight crystal accelerometers mounted to a small metal cube,
which in turn is usually welded to an inexpensive automotive hose clamp as shown in Figure 33.5. This
hose clamp/accelerometer assembly is next clamped around the vibrating tool handle with the acceler-
ometers placed very close to where the operator grasps the tool. Once again great care must be taken in
arranging the three accelerometer cables such that the tool operator is free to perform the job safely
during the vibration measurements.
To summarize, piezoresistive accelerometers are best suited to performing WBV measurements which
are inherently very low frequency, low acceleration level measurements. Piezoelectric or crystal acceler-
ometers are best suited to performing HAV measurements which require a wide bandwidth from a low of
33-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
FIGURE 33.4 Whole-body vibration vehicle measurements using an instrumentated seat disc. (Adapted from
Wasserman, D. 1987. Human Aspects of Occupational Vibration, Elsevier Publishers, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
With permission.)
about 6 Hz to as high as 5,000 Hz; tool acceleration levels can be very high (several hundred g’s or more)
and these devices must be able to measure these high g levels; these devices must be rugged too. In all
cases care with the accelerometer cabling must be taken. It is certainly not advisable to drop any of
these devices on the ground or else they can be severely damaged and/or lose their calibration. Generally
the manufacturer will supply a calibration sheet with a newly purchased accelerometer. It is advisable to
Steel Mounting
Block
.625 in (1.6 cm)
cube
Weld
Hose Clamp
FIGURE 33.5 Hand-arm vibration tool measurements using an instrumentated automotive hose clamp. (Adapted
from Wasserman, D. 1987. Human Aspects of Occupational Vibration, Elsevier Publishers, Amsterdam, The
Netherlands. With permission.)
Vibrometry 33-7
use a portable calibrator as an added calibration safety measure just in case the accelerometer has been
unknowingly damaged.
Obtaining vibration measurements requires careful planning and first performing a walk-through
tour of the worksite to be measured, or the course a vehicle takes if its vibration is to be measured.
Many times, first using a video camcorder is a good way to record the details of how workers
function on the job. Using the camcorder in real time while vibration measurements are obtained is
also very useful for recalling the chronology of events of the test day. Finally, the minimum test time
that vibration data are gathered and recorded is usually specified by the standard(s) which will be
used. For example, most HAV standards require that a minimum time of one minute of continuous triax-
ial vibration acceleration data be collected and tape recorded per tool tested. The differences in WBV
work situations and the so-called duty cycle to a large extent determine the minimum vibration measure-
ment time. For example, the length of a complete work cycle for a delivery truck, or the duty cycle of a
large vibrating metal stamping machine in a plant are quite different and should be considered
individually.
Finally, a word about handheld portable human vibration meters. We have briefly described measure-
ment methods which will yield maximum usable information for the time and expense spent in gather-
ing, recording, and analyzing vibration data and then applying it to the human vibration standards (to be
discussed next). These methods provide: (1) a permanent tape recording of the vibration data; (2) a com-
puter spectrum analysis which provides a graphical picture of the vibration frequencies which comprise
the spectrum; (3) the interaction and comparison of these spectra with these standards; and (4) numeri-
cal results indicating the total rms acceleration of the spectra. However, if only a single number total rms
acceleration value for each axis is required, then there are handheld instruments available from two com-
mercial manufacturers at this writing. The problem is that some of these instruments measure only one
acceleration axis and the testing is stopped. The one accelerometer is reoriented in another axis and the
testing is resumed. This is repeated until all three axes are recorded. This is not desirable since vibration
virtually always moves simultaneously in all three axes; thus data can be lost as the one accelerometer is
reoriented over and over again. One of the available commercial instruments has in a single handheld
meter the desirable three accelerometers for simultaneous measurements of either WBV or HAV and
also has triple output jacks for DAT recording and later spectrum analysis of the data. Thus, the
reader should be very careful in the selection of a handheld vibration meter. Further, be aware that
with the advent of miniaturized, high-density/high-speed, surface-mounted electronics technology,
many of the above-mentioned functions (i.e., triaxial accelerometer and signal conditioning, data collec-
tion, analog-to-digital conversion, data storage, initial unweighted spectrum analysis/display, radio
frequency remote control of functions) can all be performed onsite using rugged, battery operated/
stackable, miniature solid-state modules.
There are four whole-body vibration standards/guides and four hand-arm vibration standards/guides
now in use in the U.S.:
1. International Standards Organization (Geneva, Switzerland), ISO 2631: Guide to the Evaluation of
Human Exposure to Whole-Body Vibration, 1972 –85
2. American National Standards Institute (New York, NY), ANSI S3.18: Guide to the Evaluation of
Human Exposure to Whole-Body Vibration, 1979
Because of the differences and complexity of each occupational vibration standard/guide, the reader is encour-
aged to obtain, read, and understand the standard(s) which are to be used before collecting vibration data. Herein we
can only discuss some of the major elements contained in these standards.
33-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
m/s2
20
16
1.6
12.5
10
1.0
8.0 xgn 0.707gn
6.3
0.63
5.0
4.0
0.4
3.15
2.5 1 min
Acceleration oz (rms)
0.25
2.0 16 min
1.6 25 min
0.16
1.25
1.0 1h
0.1
0.8
0.63 2.5h
10 dB
0.063
0.50
4h
0.40
0.04
0.315
8h TO OBTAIN
0.25 −“EXPOSURE LIMITS” MULTIPLY
0.025
0.20 ACCELERATION VALUES BY 2(6 dB
HIGHER);
16 h
0.16 −“REDUCED COMFORT BOUNDARY”:
0.016 DIVIDE ACCELERATION VALUES BY
0.125 24 h
3.15 (10 dB LOWER).
0.10
0.016 0.4 0.5 0.63 0.8 1.0 1.25 1.6 2.0 2.5 3.15 4.0 5.0 6.3 8.0 10 12.5 16 20 25 31.5 40 50 63 80
FIGURE 33.6 (FDP) Whole-body vibration curves for Z axis rms acceleration evaluation. (ANSI S3.18,
ISO 2631, EU.)
peaks touches and/or exceeds an FDP weighted curve, then the standard has been exceeded for that daily
exposure time. The most severe axis is defined by the highest spectral peak(s) which intersect the FDP
curves. As a matter of practice, FDP curves are mostly used for both health and safety, and the EL
curves are not used because researchers believe these curves are not protective enough.2,6 Thus, the
ACGIH-TLV for WBV uses only the FDP curves for health and safety, and they totally eliminate both
the EL and RC curves. Further, the ACGIHTLV for WBV then recommends using a weighted vector-
sum calculation for all three axes to obtain a single number which is then compared to the 0.5 m/
sec./sec. action level established by the EU. In all of the cited WBV standards if in any of the three
axes, the vibration crest factor (defined as the peak acceleration divided by the rms acceleration in the
same direction) is less than or equal to six, the standard can be used; values greater than six cause the
standard to underestimate the true severity of the vibration hazard. This is particularly troublesome
when a vehicle, for example, goes off road and traverses numerous very steep bumps at fast speeds.
There are other methods for evaluating WBV exposure. For example, there are those who believe that
the WBV severity is best described by equations raised to the fourth power of acceleration;7 actual data
support the notion that mostly subjective discomfort is best described by this fourth power concept since
there is little hard epidemiological evidence at this writing to show that this concept applies to worker
health. Finally, the reader should be aware that there are various proposals to revise ISO 2631, which
may occur in the future.
2
m/s
20
16
1.6
12.5
10
1.0
8.0
xgn
6.3
0.63
5.0
4.0
0.4
3.15
2.5
Acceleration ox oy (rms)
0.25
2.0
1 min
1.6
0.16 16 min
1.25
25 min
1.0
0.1
0.8 1h
10 dB
0.63
0.063
0.50
2.5h
0.40 TO OBTAIN
0.04
0.315 4h −“EXPOSURE LIMITS”: MULTIPLY
ACCELERATION VALUES BY 2(6 dB
0.25 HIGHER);
0.025
0.20 −“REDUCED COMFORT BOUNDARY” :
8h
DIVIDE ACCELERATION VALUES BY
0.16 3.15 (10 dB LOWER).
0.016 16 h
0.125
24 h
0.10
0.016 0.4 0.5 0.63 0.8 1.0 1.25 1.6 2.0 2.5 3.15 4.0 5.0 6.3 8.0 10 12.5 16 20 25 31.5 40 50 63 80
FIGURE 33.7 (FDP) Whole-body vibration curves separately used for X, Yaxes rms acceleration evaluations. (ANSI
S3.18, ISO 2631, EU)
standard is exceeded if one or more spectral peaks in any of the axes touches or exceeds one or more of
the exposure time dependent curves; the ACGIH-TLV for HAV uses the same “shape” weighted curve but
requires that each axis yield a numerical weighted sum, each of which is next compared to the acceptable
values of HAV daily exposure given in Table 33.1. The EU standard also requires that each of these
numerical weighted values or their weighted sum be compared to the 2.5 meters/sec./sec. “action
level.” Notice that the format of Figure 33.8 is similar to the formats of Figure 33.6 and Figure 33.7,
where the abscissa is vibration frequency, in 1/3 octave bands, from 5.6 to 1250 Hz and the ordinate
is vibration intensity in acceleration. All standards use the HAV measurement coordinate system pre-
viously shown in Figure 33.2 with the “basicentric system” the method of choice. Except for NIOSH
#89-106, all of the above standards use this same “elbow shaped” weighting given in Figure 33.8.
The NIOSH standard is an interim standard without stating any acceptable acceleration level limit at
any frequency; this standard asks for each axis that: (1) weighted HAV acceleration values from 5.6 to
1250 Hz be calculated, (2) unweighted acceleration values from 5.6 to 5000 Hz be calculated, and (3)
the weighted and unweighted be compared in view of the severity of the prevalence of the hand-arm
vibration syndrome (HAVS) determined by using the tool(s) from whence these acceleration measure-
ments were made. NIOSH has chosen to issue this interim standard because there is an anomaly in
the other HAV standards, namely that the HAV weighting network shown in Figure 34.4 was originally
developed using older vibrating tool types commonly found in the workplace. Over the last few years,
some very high speed vibrating hand tools have been introduced, some of which have spectral peaks
extending to 5000 Hz and above. Current standards end at 1250 Hz, and hence in a few instances the
current standards would rule these very high-speed tools as acceptable when that may not be the case.
NIOSH has chosen to keep their interim standard, until this anomaly is resolved.
Vibrometry 33-11
1000
500
ay
/d
100 u rs ay
d
ho s/ y
1 ur /da y
to ho rs a
. 5 2 hou rs/d
50 0 to 4 u
1 to ho
2 o8
t
4
20
10
1
8 16 31.5 63 125 250 500 1000
Third-Octave Band Center Frequency, Hz
FIGURE 33.8 Hand-arm vibration curves for the separate evaluation of X,Y,Z axes rms accelerations (see text).
(ANSI S3.34.)
The architects of the other HAV standards are carefully making adjustments to their standards with
regard to the special case of these very high-speed tools as the vibration data and corresponding
HAVS health data become available. We recommend the use of ANSI S3.34, ACGIH-TLV for HAV
and the EU criteria since all provide good overall guidance, and in the special case where very high-speed
tools are to be tested caution is advised when evaluating the triaxial vibration test data.
TABLE 33.1 ACGIH Threshold Limit Values for Exposure of the Hand to Vibration
in X, Y, Z Directions
Values of the Dominant,b Frequency-
Weighted, rms, Component Acceleration Which
Shall Not Be Exceeded
aK, (aKeq)
Total Daily Exposure Durationa m/s2 g
4 hours and less than 8 4 0.40
2 hours and less than 4 6 0.61
1 hour and less than 2 8 0.81
Less than 1 hour 12 1.22
33.5 Summary
In this chapter the basic concepts of displacement, velocity, acceleration, resonance, coordinate systems
for measurements, and spectrum analysis are presented and integrated for an understanding of their
application to whole-body and hand-arm occupational vibration. Generic acceleration measurement
systems and methods are discussed, which included piezoelectric and piezoresistive accelerometers, con-
ditioning preamplifiers, data recording systems, and Fourier spectrum computers. The chapter concludes
with a discussion of the various occupational whole-body and hand-arm vibration standards/guides cur-
rently used in the U.S. and their application to the evaluation of triaxial acceleration data from the work-
place; because of the complexity of each standard, users are encouraged to obtain copies of standards
which are to be used before obtaining vibration data.
Defining Terms
Acceleration: The time rate of change of velocity of a moving object.
Accelerometer: A device designed to convert mechanical motion into a corresponding electrical
analog voltage, charge, or current proportional to acceleration.
Conditioning Preamplifier: An electronic solid state amplifier designed to faithfully amplify, both in
amplitude and frequency bandwidth, the minute electrical signals emanating from an accelero-
meter. Some preamplifiers are called “charge amplifiers” and convert the voltage generated
across the face of a crystal (piezoelectric) accelerometer into corresponding charge, thereby allow-
ing long cables to be used for measurements without loss of signal. Other preamplifiers are called
“differential amplifiers,” which act as an amplifying voltmeter when used with “piezoresistive”
type accelerometers.
DAT or Digital Audio Tape: A new type of instrumentation tape system with large dynamic input
range and wide frequency bandwidth, whereby an analog input signal is converted and stored
on a cassette tape in digital format. The original signal so stored can be retrieved either in
digital format or reconverted again into its original analog version.
Displacement: Movement traversed away from a reference position.
Fourier Spectrum Analysis: The analysis of vibration data by mathematically converting time
domain information into its corresponding frequency domain; the underlying assumptions are
that the data are linear and that time domain information can be dissected and represented as
a mathematical series of elemental sines and cosines. Computers which perform this function
are called Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) analyzers or Real Time Analyzers (RTA).
Resonance: The tendency of an object to (1) move in concert with an external vibrating source and
(2) to internally amplify the impinging vibration from that source; resonance is the optimum
energy transfer condition between the source and the receiver.
Vector Coordinate System: A mutually perpendicular set of vectors, originating at the same motion
point, which define the vector motion of that point. Typically, there are three linear and three
rotational vectors which comprise motion at a point.
Vector: A mathematical quantity defined by both its magnitude and direction.
Velocity: The time rate of change of displacement of a moving object. Also called speed.
Vibration: At any one point, vibration is motion defined by six vectors, three mutually perpendicular
linear vectors and three rotational vectors moving around these linear vectors (pitch, yaw, roll).
References
1. Wasserman, D., Badger, D., Doyle, T., and Margolies, L. 1974. Industrial vibration — An overview.
J. Am. Soc. Safety Engrs. 19(6):38 –43.
2. Wasserman, D. 1987. Human Aspects of Occupational Vibration, Elsevier Publishers, Amsterdam, The
Netherlands.
Vibrometry 33-13
3. Pelmear, P. and Wasserman, D. 1998. Hand-Arm Vibration: A Comprehensive Guide for Occupational
Health Professionals, Second edition, OEM Press, Beverly Farms, MA.
4. Thalheimer, E. 1996. Practical Approach To Measurement and Evaluation of Exposure to Whole-Body
Vibration in the Workplace. Conference Proceedings: Seminars in Perinatology, International Confer-
ence on Pregnant Women in the Workplace Sound and Vibration Exposure, Univ. of Florida,
Gainesville, FL.
5. Wilder, D., Wasserman, D., Pope, M., Pelmear, P., and Taylor, W. 1994. Chapter 4: Vibration, in
Physical and Biological Hazards of the Workplace, Eds. P. Wald and G. Stave, p. 64–83. Van Nostrand
Reinhold, New York, NY.
6. Bovenzi, M. and Betta, A. 1994. Low-back disorders in agricultural tractor drivers exposed to whole-
body vibration and postural stress. Appl. Ergonomics 25(4): 231– 241.
7. Griffin, M. 1990. Handbook of Human Vibration. Academic Press, London, U.K.
II-1
II-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
41 The ACGIH TLVw for Hand Activity Level Thomas J. Armstrong . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41-1
Introduction † ACGIH TLVs † The Basis for a TLV on Hand Activity Level † Applying
the ACGIH TLV † Applying the TLV † Determining Compliance with the TLV †
Summary
42 REBA and RULA: Whole Body and Upper Limb Rapid Assessment Tools
Sue Hignett and Lynn McAtamney . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42-1
Introduction † REBA: The Postures † Examples † Reliability and Validity † Using
RULA † Example
43 An Assessment Technique for Postural Loading on the Upper Body (LUBA)
Dohyung Kee and Waldemar Karwowski . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43-1
Posture Classification Scheme † Objectives † LUBA † Application
Example † Conclusions
44 The Washington State SHARP Approach to Exposure Assessment
Stephen Bao, Barbara Silverstein, Ninica Howard, and Peregrin Spielholz . . . . . . . . . . 44-1
Introduction † Exposure Parameters, Measurement Strategy, and
Measurement Methods † SHARP Study Exposure Assessment Methods † Summary
45 Upper Extremity Analysis of the Wrist Andris Freivalds . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45-1
Anatomy of the Hand and Wrist † Models of the Hand and Wrist † Direct
Measurement Studies † Data-Driven WRMSD Risk Index † Conclusions
46 Revised NIOSH Lifting Equation Thomas R. Waters . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46-1
Introduction † Definition of Terms † Limitations of Equation † Obtaining and
Using the Data † Procedures † Applying the Equations † Example Problems †
Validation of the Revised NIOSH Lifting Equation
47 Psychophysical Approach to Task Analysis Patrick G. Dempsey . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47-1
Introduction † The Psychophysical Approach to Designing Manual Materials Handling
Tasks † The Psychophysical Approach to Designing Upper Extremity Tasks † Advantages
and Disadvantages of the Psychophysical Approach † Conclusions
48 Static Biomechanical Modeling in Manual Lifting Don B. Chaffin and
Charles B. Woolley . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 48-1
Introduction † Development of Static Strength Prediction Programs † Computerization
of Strength Prediction and Back Force Prediction Models † Validation of Strength and
Back Force Prediction Models † Final Comments
49 Industrial Lumbar Motion Monitor William S. Marras
and W. Gary Allread . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49-1
Introduction † Development of the Industrial Lumbar Motion Monitor † Development
of the LBD Risk Model † Benefits of the iLMM and the LBD Risk Model † Applications:
How to Use the iLMM and LBD Risk Model † Selecting the Job(s) to Monitor † Defining
the Major Components of the Job through a Task Analysis † Collecting and Recording
Workplace Data for Risk Assessment † Setting Up the iLMM for Data
Collection † Collecting the Data † Analyzing and Interpreting the iLMM Data
50 The ACGIH TLVw for Low Back Risk William S. Marras
and Chris Hamrick . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 50-1
Overview † TLV Development Background † Using the Lifting TLV
34
Overview of
Ergonomic
Assessment
34.1 Introduction
The statistician George P. E. Box has been quoted as saying, “All models are wrong; some models are
useful.” Such is the case with the models that ergonomics practitioners use in the field to perform ergo-
nomic assessments. No single assessment tool is perfect. However, some assessment tools can be of value
to the ergonomist in the field.
The purpose of this chapter is to provide an overview of some of the practical assessment tools that are
readily available to ergonomics practitioners. The tools that will be discussed can be used in the field to
assess the relative risk of musculoskeletal disorders (MSDs) resulting from the job or task being evalu-
ated. Each of the models presented here have drawbacks, but if used properly each can play a key role
as part of a comprehensive ergonomics process.
Organizations have great incentive to identify workplace factors contributing to MSDs; work-related
MSDs are prevalent and costly. In the United States in 2001, there were 216,400 “repeated trauma” cases
reported by private industry, which translates to a rate of 23.8 cases per 10,000 worker hours of exposure
(United States Department of Labor, 2003). Data from the Ohio Bureau of Workers’ Compensation
(Hamrick, 2000) showed that in Ohio in 1996, MSDs accounted for 15.5% of all claims in the workplace,
and 48.5% of all workers’ compensation dollars paid went toward MSDs. Those data also indicated that
approximately 40% of these claim costs resulted from back pain claims, and 9% resulted from upper
extremity claims.
The ideal field assessment tool would possess the following attributes:
. Predictive — the tool would be able to provide a valid predictive measure of the risk of musculo-
skeletal injury that would occur to a population performing the task being assessed
34-1
34-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
on these logs includes the type of injury or illness, the number of days away from work, and the number
of days of job transfer or restriction as a result of the injury/illness.
When using this information, it is imperative to normalize any loss data to worker exposure hours, so
that the injuries/illnesses can be expressed in terms of an incidence rate. OSHA recommends the follow-
ing formula for calculating incidence rates (United States Department of Labor, 2004):
Expressing injuries and illnesses in terms of a rate allows comparisons to be made between exposure
groups (e.g., job titles, departments, and facilities) and for different time periods, even if those time
periods are of different duration. Additionally, the number of days away from work and the number of
job transfer or job restriction days can be similarly normalized to get an indication of severity. When
looking at these results, we must realize that transitional work programs can have an effect on these rates.
Cost of injuries can also provide a measure of impact of MSDs. Workers’ compensation records can be
used to determine medical and indemnity costs. It should be noted that most workers’ compensation
systems were not created for the purpose of surveillance; rather, they were created to track payments
to those injured at work. So, information obtained from this source is often limited. Further investigation
may be required to determine the nature and cause of the injury. Additionally, workers’ compensation
laws vary considerably by state, so comparing results between workers’ compensation systems is not advi-
sable. Other passive surveillance sources can include employee medical visits and employee turnover rates.
Passive surveillance data sources are generally inexpensive to obtain — they are usually already being
kept so no additional cost is involved. The information obtained is also valuable at pointing out real
problems and their costs. Hence, any ergonomics effort should, at a minimum, incorporate passive
surveillance. However, a major drawback to relying solely on passive surveillance is that although they
may provide an indication of future injuries, the measurements are reactive; they occur after problems
are reported. A proactive approach is preferred.
A more proactive type of surveillance method is referred to as “active surveillance” (Saldaña, 1996).
These types of information gathering involve actively seeking information from the workforce on
MSDs before they are reported on OSHA logs or become a workers’ compensation claim. Examples
of these types of methods can include questionnaires, symptoms surveys, and “discomfort surveys.”
To gather information, worker input is solicited concerning their work environment, health problems
they are experiencing, and ways to eliminate the health problems. Many practitioners use the survey,
or a variation thereof, developed by Corlett and Bishop (1976).
An advantage to using active surveillance is that the input of those closest to the task, the worker, is
obtained. Worker involvement is a critical part of any comprehensive ergonomics program (NIOSH,
1997). Advantages of soliciting worker input include more feasible recommendations and better
worker acceptance. Also, the tools provide a picture of the current situation. Health indicators are
measured before they are recorded as workers’ compensation claims or recordable injuries/illnesses.
Care must be taken, however, when developing and administering these tools. Design of ques-
tionnaires and surveys requires skill; a more detailed description of what is involved in administering
questionnaires and surveys can be found in Sinclair (1995). The author cautions, “unless the information
required is strictly factual and fairly easily checked, their reliability and validity can be quite low, so
considerable care must be taken.” Design and administration of active surveillance tools can be costly;
skill and time are required for development, and workers must take time to fill out the instrument.
Furthermore, it is advisable that the use of active surveillance technique not be undertaken by an
organization unless management is committed to acting upon the results. Soliciting worker input
and then failing to act can be counterproductive to ergonomic efforts.
It should also be noted that very few questionnaires have been demonstrated to be valid, unbiased esti-
mators of true exposure to risk factors. Burdorf (1992) stated, “several studies have cast doubt on the
determination of exposure to risk factors through questionnaire assessments. Comparisons of
34-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
questionnaire assessments with observational data have shown that reports on the time spent in specific
activities like walking, standing, and kneeling are not very reliable.” So, questionnaires may have an
appropriate use as a screening tool to direct the practitioner for appropriate follow-up analyses, but
the specific results should be interpreted with caution.
own psychophysical study for their specific MMH conditions, but doing so would be costly, time
consuming, and require expertise, so conducting a study is usually not feasible by practitioners.
When using the tables, some limitations of the approach must be considered. During the testing, the
load limits were based on subjective assessments by the test subject. So, although the limits may provide a
realistic assessment of what a worker feels he or she can tolerate, it is questionable whether a subject can
anticipate how much can be tolerated over the long term without incurring an injury. Marras et al.
(1999), in an investigation of measurement effectiveness of different MMH assessment tools, found
that in nearly two thirds of jobs that could be classified as high and medium risk the psychophysical
criteria would indicate that those jobs are acceptable. So, a psychophysical approach appears to under-
estimate the actual level of risk.
The user should also keep in mind that the model also assumes good coupling (between the load and
the hands and between the feet and the floor), two-handed symmetrical material handling, moderate
load lifts, unrestricted working postures, and a favorable physical environment.
angles was generally misestimated by 4.58 less than the reference value at the origin of lift and by 7.98
greater than the reference value at the destination. Also, the reference point location is a critical measure-
ment because it has a large impact on the determination of the horizontal distance factor, which, in turn,
carries great mathematical weight in the LI equation. It is apparent, then, that the NIOSH guide should
really only be used by experts or by those given training on proper measurement and calculation of
the LI.
Another investigation into use of the 1991 equation asymmetry multiplier in industry was reported by
Dempsey and Fathallah (1999). The authors stated that problems with measuring asymmetry in practice
occur because: (1) the definitions provided by NIOSH do not provide a consistent, objective method,
(2) the analyst often must measure while the worker is in motion, and (3) there are frequently
obstructions in the workplace which compound the problem. They then concluded that, in practice,
field measurements of the asymmetry angle are essentially qualitative.
So, in practice, the NIOSH Lifting Equation can provide a valid assessment of the risk associated with
MMH activities. However, only those who have received training on the tool should take the measure-
ments use the equation, and interpret the results. Those using the equation should be aware of the limit-
ations of the model. Despite the limitations, the NIOSH Lifting Equation can provide the ergonomics
practitioner with a to that can be used set priorities for job design. The ergonomist can also determine
which attributes of the job are good candidates for redesign by looking at the specific components of the
equation. For example, if the horizontal multiplier is the lowest of the multipliers in the equation, the
ergonomist would want to employ design strategies to reduce the horizontal distance of the lift. In
summary, the NIOSH Lifting Equation appears to be predictive, fairly robust (when used within the
limitations explicitly stated in the applications manual), inexpensive (the tool is available for free, but
its use takes expert time), and relatively noninvasive. However, the tool is not necessarily easy to use
and should be reserved for use by those who have been adequately trained.
10
Maximum
15 Maximum
Vertical lift origin
FIGURE 34.1 Guidelines for lifts involving trunk twisting angle of asymmetry between 0 and 308 (The angle
between the front of the body, when facing forward, and the load being lifted).
34-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
FIGURE 34.2 Guidelines for lifts involving trunk twisting angle of asymmetry between 30 and 608 (The angle
between the front of the body, when facing forward, and the load being lifted).
ACGIH TLV. In addition, the guidelines can be used by medical professionals to develop realistic rec-
ommendations for injured workers’ capabilities, since capabilities can be related to characteristics in
the workplace. Also, transitional work providers can use the guidelines to place employees recovering
from LBDs in the right tasks at the right time. The tool can help return an injured worker to work as
soon as possible while minimizing the risk of aggravating an existing LBD.
10
15 Maximum
Vertical lift origin
FIGURE 34.3 Guidelines for lifts involving trunk twisting angle of asymmetry between 60 and 908 (The angle
between the front of the body, when facing forward, and the load being lifted).
Overview of Ergonomic Assessment 34-9
are intensity of exertion, duration of exertion, efforts per minute, wrist posture, speed of exertion, and
duration of task per day. After data collection, the analyst uses a table to assign rating values for each of
the measures. These ratings values are then converted to multipliers by the analyst using the provided
table. When the multipliers are multiplied together, a final index is obtained. The authors stress that
this assessment tool is designed to assess jobs, not individual workers.
A validation study of the strain index was reported by Rucker and Moore (2002), who measured 28
jobs in two manufacturing facilities. The authors suggested using a strain index score of 9 as a criterion
for classifying jobs as “hazardous” (.9) or “safe” (,9). They found that the sensitivity of the index was
0.92, meaning that the tool correctly identified 92% of the jobs associated with morbidity data. The sen-
sitivity reported in the study was 1.00, meaning that the tool correctly classified all jobs not associated
with morbidity. These numbers suggest that the strain index is effective at identifying jobs that do
and do not expose workers to an increased risk of developing upper extremity MSDs.
The developers also point out some limitations of the index that users should be aware of as they
collect data and interpret results. Notably, the index only applies to the distal upper extremity; it predicts
a spectrum of upper extremity MSDs, but not specific disorders; and the training and experience needed
to use the tool effectively has not been determined.
From a practitioner’s standpoint, an advantage of the strain index is the flexibility in the types of
measurement that can be used to obtain some of the task variables. For example, the hand/wrist
posture rating value can be obtained by actually measuring wrist angles in flexion, extension, and
ulnar deviation. An ergonomist could measure the posture with a goniometer. On the other hand, if
an ergonomics team is using the tool and equipment is not available, then they can use the anchors pro-
vided for subjective ratings of “perceived posture.” Of course, it is preferable to use the quantitative
measures, but it is not always feasible in practice due to time and money constraints.
It should also be noted that the tool requires qualitative ratings for three of the input variables, and no
quantitative alternative is given. Consequently, it was recommended by the developers that any subjective
ratings be reached by consensus to minimize interrater reliability. To date, test –retest reliability has not
yet been formally measured for this tool.
In all, however, the Strain Index can be a valuable tool for ergonomics practitioners and ergonomics
teams to prioritize intervention efforts, to determine which attributes of the task to address in order to
reduce worker exposure to MSD risk factors, and as a tool for follow-up analysis to ensure that any inter-
vention efforts resulted in the desired effect.
The tool does not account for some workplace factors that may increase the likelihood of MSDs,
namely sustained non-neutral postures, contact stresses, low temperatures, and vibration exposure.
If these factors are present, then professional judgment should be used to determine if exposure is
above the TLV. Also, the TLV is not meant to apply to work conditions that include multiple tasks.
The TLV does, however, provide a useful tool to quickly assess mono-tasks. It can be quick, easy to
use, and easy to interpret. It also allows the user flexibility to be more accurate with an additional invest-
ment of time and expertise. Appropriate uses of the TLV include identifying potential jobs as candidates
for ergonomic interventions and for setting priorities for resource allocation. The TLV can also be used as
a metric to determine if interventions have had the desired effect by performing an assessment before and
after implementation and comparing the results.
focus on eliminating hazards and risk factors; administrative solutions such as worker training,
recommended procedures, and job rotation should be considered as solutions only until engineering
changes eliminate worker exposure to hazards can be implemented. Solutions should also be specific.
According to Peterson (2003), “Solutions that merely state, ‘Be more alert’ or ‘Use more caution’ or
something similar, are worthless.”
Once the JSA is completed, it is important to make them readily available to workers performing the
job — they do little good if filed in a drawer, which is all to often the case. Also, as the job demands
change (i.e., after a recommended control is implemented), the JSA should be updated. It should be
noted that although quantitative methods may be used to describe risk factors, JSAs do not provide a
quantitative measure of risk factors. Therefore, JSAs alone cannot be used to predict injury, nor can
they alone be used to prioritize tasks. However, JSAs do provide a systematic method to identify
hazards, including MSD risk factors, and to develop potential solutions.
34.4.3.3 Checklists
Many checklists are available as an aid to identify MSD risk factors. Checklists, when used in ergonomics,
are essentially lists of risk factors or workplace conditions that are believed to increase the risk of MSDs.
They help the user to ensure that they have addressed areas of concern, and have not overlooked potential
risk factors. Hence, they are particularly useful for the novice. In general, checklists indicate the presence
of a risk, or if the level of risk is above a certain threshold level (e.g., if the weight lifted is greater
than 20 lbs or if the neck angle is greater than 208). Consequently, checklists generally do not provide
a quantitative level of risk and, therefore, cannot be used to predict risk of injury.
In their draft ergonomics standard for prevention of work-related MSDs developed in 1995, OSHA
provided a checklist to be used as a screening tool (Schneider, 1995). In addition to indicating the
presence of a risk factor, the tool also assigns a score between 0 and 3, depending on the duration of
worker exposure and the severity of the identified risk factor. The tool is divided into “upper extremity,”
“back and legs,” and “environmental” sections. Score totals are obtained for each section, and then
combined for a total score. A slightly modified version of this MSD Risk Factor Screening Tool is
shown in Table 34.1.
Although the tool has not been validated for its original intended use of providing a threshold value,
the screening tool can provide a relative measure of worker exposure to MSDs. It should also be noted
that interrater reliability is relatively poor, so comparisons made between raters may not be valid. The
numerical score obtained from the screening tool can help prioritize tasks and can also be used to
ensure that risk of injury has been reduced after a design change.
As an example, the tool was used as to provide an assessment of a window assembly task before and
after ergonomic intervention. Figure 34.4 shows the window assembly task being performed before the
ergonomic interventions were implemented. The upper extremity score was 19, the back and legs score
was 6, and the environmental score was 0, for a total score of 25. Ergonomic interventions focusing on
reducing upper extremity MSD risk factors were then implemented. The interventions consisted of: (1)
providing automated screw feeders, (2) using in-line tools, and (3) providing an arm to counteract the
tool torque. The redesigned task is shown in Figure 34.5. The follow-up screening tool analysis score was
11 for upper extremity, 6 for back and legs, and 0 for environmental. Thus, the screening tool indicated
that we have reduced the upper extremity MSD risk factors as intended.
34.5 Conclusions
In order to get the most from data collected using ergonomic assessment tools, the analysis tool user
must understand some fundamental issues associated with data collection. First, there may be some
intersubject variation in the way tasks are performed, either due to variances in processes or variances
in worker performance. So, it is important to observe the task for a long enough period to account
for these variations. The length of time will depend on the task cycle time and any variation cycles
times. Second, there will also likely be intrasubject variation; work practices can vary between
34-12 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Upper Extremity
A B C D E F
Risk Factor Risk 2 to 4 4+ to 8 8+ Score
Category Factors Hours Hours Hours
Add 0.5
per
hour
Repetition 1. Identical or Similar Motions Performed Every Few
(Finger, Seconds
Wrist, Motions or motion patterns that are repeated every 15 seconds 1 3
Elbow, or less. (Keyboard use is scored below as a separate risk
Shoulder, factor.)
or Neck
Motions)
2. Intensive Keying
Scored separately from other repetitive tasks in the repetition
category; includes steady pace, as in data entry. 1 3
3. Intermittent Keying
Scored separately from other repetitive tasks. Keyboard or
other input activity is regularly alternated with other activities 0 1
for 50 to 75 percent of the work.
Hand Force 1. Grip More Than 10-Pound Load
(Repetitive Holding an object weighing more than 10
or Static) pounds or squeezing hard with hand in a power 1 3
grip.
2. Pinch More Than 2 Pounds
Pinch force of 2+ pounds as in the pinch used to 2 3
open a small binder clip with the tips of fingers.
Awkward 1. Neck: Twist / Bend
Postures Twisting neck to either side more that 20°,
bending neck forward more than 20° as in
viewing a monitor, or bending neck
backward more than 5°. 1 2
2. Shoulder: Unsupported Arm or Elbow
Above Mid-Torso Height
Arm is unsupported if there is not an arm 2 3
rest when doing precision finger work, or
when the elbow is above mid-torso height.
3. Forearm: Rapid Rotation
Rotating the forearm or resisting rotation from a tool. An 1 2
example of forearm rotation is using a manual screwdriver.
4. Wrist: Bend / Deviate
Wrist bends that involve more than 20° of flexion (bending
the wrist palm down) or more than 30° of extension (bending 2 3
the wrist back). Bending can occur during manual assembly
and data entry.
5. Fingers
Forceful gripping to control or hold an object, such as click- 0 1
and-drag operations with a computer mouse or deboning with a
knife.
(Table Continued)
Overview of Ergonomic Assessment 34-13
(Table Continued)
34-14 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Environmental Worksheet
A B C D E F
Risk Risk 2 to 4 4+ to 8 8+ Score
Factor Factors Hours Hours Hours
Category Add 0.5
per
hour
Environ- 1. Lighting (Poor Illumination / Glare) 0 1
ment Inability to see clearly (e.g. glare on a computer monitor).
2. Cold Temperature
Air temperature less than 60ºF for sedentary work, 40ºF for 0 1
light work, 20ºF for moderate/heavy work; cold exhaust blowing
on hands.
Total Score:
(Upper Extremity + Back and Legs + Environmental)
workers. Therefore, the observer should sample enough workers to make a valid assessment. Oftentimes,
it is feasible to collect data for each person performing the task. Other times a judgment on the sample
size must be made based upon the amount of variability between workers and the number of workers
performing the task. Third, there is likely to be interrater (or observer) variability, and this variability
will increase the more subjective the measure being used. If the interrater variability is high, then com-
parison of analysis results performed by different raters is not advised. One way to reduce the amount of
interrater variability is to gain consensus from a group or ergonomics team when recording subjective
measures. Doing so will tend to make assessments more consistent and will facilitate communication
within the team. A more complete description of the issues associated with making ergonomic assess-
ments in the field can be found in Haines and McAtamney (1995).
Another useful tool that can be used to supplement each of the analysis tools mentioned above is
videotaping. Videotaping provides a visual record of the task; this record is particularly useful when com-
paring jobs before and after implementing ergonomic interventions. Use of videotape also allows ergo-
nomics teams to make assessments and discuss issues together, with minimal disruption of the work
environment. Videotape also allows slow motion playback, zoom, and multiple viewing, enabling the
observer to pick up on quick or subtle motions that may not be seen in real time. Some guidelines
for recording work activities on videotape are presented in Grant (1996).
A myriad of analysis tools exist to help the ergonomic practitioner to identify MSD risk factors, to
prioritize candidate tasks for resources, and to measure whether ergonomic intervention strategies
have had the desired effect. Some tools that have been demonstrated to be practical for the ergonomics
practitioner were presented in this chapter. None of the tools is perfect; each has its advantages and its
drawbacks. In order to use the tools appropriately, the practitioner must understand the limitations of
the assessment tools that they are using. As long as the strengths and weaknesses of the tools are kept in
mind, they can provide the ergonomics team and the ergonomics practitioner with valuable data to
prioritize projects, measure successes, and gain support from management to make the workplace
safer for the worker.
References
ACGIH (2003). 2003 TLVs and BEIs: Threshold Limit Values for Chemical Substances and Physical Agents
& Biological Exposure Indices, American Conference of Governmental Industrial Hygienists,
Cincinnati, Ohio.
Bernard, B.B. (1997). Musculoskeletal disorders and workplace risk factors: a critical review of epidemio-
logic evidence for work-related musculoskeletal disorders of the neck, upper extremity, and low
back, DHHS (NIOSH) Publication #97-141, NIOSH, Cincinnati, Ohio.
Burdorf, A. (1992). Exposure assessment of risk factors for disorders of the back in occupational epide-
miology, Scand J Work Environ Health, 18:1 –9.
Corlett, E.N. and Bishop, R.P. (1976). A technique for assessing postural discomfort, Ergonomics,
19:175–182.
34-16 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Dempsey, P.G. and Fathallah, F.A. (1999). Application issues and theoretical concerns regarding the 1991
NIOSH equation asymmetry multiplier, Int Appl Ergon, 23:181–191.
Grant, K.A. (1996). Job analysis, in Occupational Ergonomics: Theory and Applications Ed. by
Battacharya A. and McGlothlin J.D., Marcel Dekker, Inc., New York.
Haines, H. and McAtamney, L. (1995). Undertaking an ergonomics study in industry, in Evaluation of
Human Work: A Practical Ergonomics Methodology, 2nd edn. Ed. by Wilson, J.R. and Corlett,
E.N., Taylor & Francis, London.
Hamrick, C.A. (2000). CTDs and ergonomics in Ohio, Proceedings of the IEA 2000/HFES 2000 Congress,
5-111–5-114.
Marras, W.S., Lavender, S.A., Leurgans, S., Rajulu, S.L., Allread, W.G., Fathallah, F.A., and Ferguson, S.A.
(1993). The role of dynamic three dimensional trunk motion in occupationally-related low back
disorders: the effects of workplace factors, trunk position and trunk motion characteristics on
injury, Spine, 18(5):617 –628.
Marras, W.S., Fine, L.J., Ferguson, S.A., and Waters, T.R. (1999). The effectiveness of commonly used
lifting assessment methods to identify industrial jobs associated with elevated risk of low-back
disorders, Ergonomics, 42(1):229– 245.
Marras, W.S., Allread, W.G., Burr, D.L., and Fathallah, F.A. (2000). Validation of a low back disorder risk
model: a prospective study of ergonomic interventions associated with manual materials handling
jobs, Ergonomics, 43(11):1866–1886.
Marras, W.S., Davis, K.G., Ferguson, S.A., Lucas, B.R., and Gupta, P. (2001). Spine loading characteristics
of patients with low back pain compared to asymptomatic individuals, Spine, 26(23):2566–2574.
McAtamney, L. and Corlett, E.N. (1993). RULA: a survey method for the investigation of work-related
upper limb disorders, Appl Ergon, 24(2):91 –99.
Moore, J.S. and Garg, A. (1995). The Strain Index: a proposed method to analyze jobs for risk of distal
upper extremity disorders, Am Ind Hyg Assoc J, 56:443–458.
NIOSH (National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health) (1981). Work Practices Guide for Manual
Lifting, DHHS (NIOSH) Publication No. 81 –122, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services
(NIOSH), Cincinnati, Ohio.
NIOSH (National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health) (1994). Applications Manual for the
Revised NIOSH Lifting Equation, DHHS (NIOSH) Publication No. 94-110, U.S. Department of
Health and Human Services (NIOSH), Cincinnati, Ohio.
NIOSH (National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health) (1997). Elements of Ergonomics Programs:
A Primer based on Workplace Evaluations of Musculoskeletal Disorders, DHHS (NIOSH) Publication
No. 97-117, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (NIOSH), Cincinnati, Ohio.
Peterson, D. (2003). Techniques of Safety Management: A Systems Approach, 4th Edition, American
Society of Safety Engineers, Des Plaines, Illinois.
Rucker, N. and Moore, J.S. (2002). Predictive validity of the strain index in manufacturing facilities, Appl
Occup Environ Hyg, 17(1):63 –73.
Saldaña, N. (1996). Active surveillance of work-related musculoskeletal disorders: an essential com-
ponent in ergonomics programs, in Occupational Ergonomics: Theory and Applications Ed. by
Battacharya A. and McGlothlin J.D., Marcel Dekker, Inc., New York.
Schneider, S. (ed.) (1995). OSHA’s draft standard for prevention of work-related musculoskeletal
disorders, Appl Occup Environ Hyg, 10(8): 665–674.
Sinclair, M.A. (1995). Subjective assessment, in Evaluation of Human Work: A Practical Ergonomics
Methodology, 2nd edn. Ed. by Wilson, J.R. and Corlett, E.N., Taylor & Francis, London.
Snook, S.H. (1978). The design of manual handling tasks, Ergonomics, 21:963 –985.
Snook, S.H. and Ciriello, V.M. (1991). The design of manual handling tasks: revised tables of maximum
acceptable weights and forces, Ergonomics, 34(9): 1197 –1213.
Tanaka, S. (1996). Record-based (passive) surveillance for cumulative trauma disorders, in Occupational
Ergonomics: Theory and Applications Ed. by Battacharya A. and McGlothlin J.D., Marcel Dekker,
Inc., New York.
Overview of Ergonomic Assessment 34-17
United States Department of Labor (2003). Bureau of Labor Statistics (www.bls.com), Public Data
Query, December 16, 2003.
United States Department of Labor (2004). OSHA Forms for Recording Work-Related Injuries and
Illnesses, U.S. Department of Labor, Washington, D.C.
Waters, T.R., Putz-Anderson, V., Garg, A., and Fine, L.J. (1993). Revised NIOSH equation for the design
and evaluation of manual lifting tasks, Ergonomics, 36(7):749– 776.
Waters, T.R., Baron, S.L., and Kemmlert, K. (1998). Accuracy of measurements for the revised NIOSH
lifting equation, Appl Ergon, 29(6):433 –438.
Waters, T.R., Baron, S.L., Piacitelli, L.A., Anderson, V.P., Skov, T, Haring-Sweeney, M., Wall, D.K., and
Fine, L.J. (1999). Evaluation of the revised NIOSH lifting equation: a cross-sectional epidemiologic
study, Spine, 24(4):386 –395.
35
Low Back Injury Risk
Assessment Tools
35.1 Introduction
Over the last three decades, several low back injury risk assessment tools have been developed to provide
ergonomics practitioners the ability to evaluate the relative risk posed by manual materials handling
(MMH) tasks. The Work Practices Guide for Manual Lifting (NIOSH, 1981) and the Revised NIOSH
Lifting Equation (Waters et al., 1993, 1994) are two well-established methods developed by the National
Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH). The Lumbar Motion Monitor (LMM) risk assess-
ment model (Marras et al., 1993) and the Three-Dimensional Static Strength Prediction ProgramTM
(3DSSPP) were developed by researchers at The Ohio State University and the University of Michigan,
respectively. The goals of this paper are to compare and contrast these existing assessment models,
develop the motivation for a hybrid modeling technique and identify gaps in our current low back
injury risk assessment techniques for other high-risk activities.
35-1
35-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
level, the task is deemed acceptable. If the actual weight exceeds the MPL (calculated as three times the
AL), significant risk exists and the task should be carefully evaluated and redesigned.
After over a decade of using this assessment tool, researchers at NIOSH recognized the limitations of
this tool in assessing the variety of realistic working conditions faced by those performing manual
materials handling tasks. Specifically, they identified the inability of the 1981 equation to assess the
risk of asymmetric work postures and varied kinds of coupling between the lifter and the load being
lifted. So in 1993 they published a Revised NIOSH Lifting Equation (Waters et al., 1993). The workplace
variables considered in this revised equation include: vertical position of load, horizontal distance
between the load and the spine, frequency of lifting, vertical travel distance of the load, asymmetric
posture of torso, and coupling quality between the lifter and the object being lifted. As in the 1981
equation, these measures are then combined in a multiplicative model but instead of calculating an
AL this model calculated a value called the recommended weight limit, or RWL, which describes a
weight that can be lifted safely by a majority of the working population. The ratio of the actual
weight being lifted in the job to this RWL is a value called the lifting index (LI). LI values greater
than 1 are said to place some workers at increased risk, while values greater than 3 are said to be a poten-
tial problem for a majority of healthy industrial workers (Waters et al., 1994). There are a number of
stated assumptions that should be considered when applying this assessment technique. Among these
assumptions are that the workers perform only two-handed lifts, they work for no more than 8 h, they
are not lifting or lowering objects faster than 75 cm/sec, and they are lifting in a relatively unrestricted
work environment.
There have been several studies that have been conducted to evaluate the effectiveness of the NIOSH
method in predicting the reporting of low back pain/discomfort. In a study of 97 MMH jobs, Wang
et al. (1998) report a monotonically increasing relationship between the severity ratings of low back dis-
comfort and the NIOSH LI. Their results showed that for jobs with a severity rating of 0 (on a 0–5 point
scale) the mean LI was 0.8 while for jobs with a mean severity rating of 5 the mean LI was 4.1 with
intermediate points following the trend. Another significant result from this study was that 42 of the
97 evaluated jobs had an RWL ¼ 0, a result that the authors attribute to having tasks wherein lifting
frequencies and/or horizontal distances exceeded those allowed by the NIOSH modeling methodology.
In another study (Waters et al., 1999), 50 jobs in four different industrial facilities, were evaluated and
the authors report that the unadjusted prevalence odds ratio for reported low back pain were 1.14, 1.54,
and 2.54 for LIs of 0–1, 1 –2, and 2– 3, respectively. Interestingly, they report an unadjusted prevalence
odds ratio of 1.63 for jobs with an LI of .3, and note potential selection and survivor effects may have
influenced the results of their analysis. In addition to these studies that have considered the relationships
between the NIOSH assessments and reporting of discomfort there have also been several studies that
have considered “usability” aspects of the assessment tools. The reader is referred to the following
articles for further information: Dempsey (2002), Dempsey and Fathallah (1999), and Waters et al.
(1998).
While the NIOSH method is straightforward in application and has great utility in many industrial
environments the static representation of the workplace does not take into account some of the
human performance issues that have been implicated in the low back injury process. Specifically, in per-
forming an MMH task, the three-dimensional postures, velocities and accelerations have been shown to
play a role in the development of low back injuries (Marras et al., 1995). It should be noted that the
NIOSH modeling approach did consider dynamics from physiological and psychophysical perspectives,
but in many cases trunk motion plays a direct role in biomechanical loading and therefore is a facet of
risk that is not directly addressed in the NIOSH approach.
the Lumbar Motion Monitor (LMM) (Figure 35.1) that was developed to capture the instantaneous pos-
ition, velocity, and acceleration of the lumbar spine in the three cardinal planes of human motion. Their
approach to developing an assessment tool was to use this tri-axial goniometric device to capture the
trunk kinematic profiles of workers performing their normal work tasks and then to relate these kin-
ematic characteristics (along with a cadre of other task descriptors such as lifting frequency, moments
about the spine created by the load, job satisfaction, the static workplace variables from the NIOSH
Lifting Equation, etc.) to the historical incidence of low back injuries. They sampled 403 industrial
jobs and then used multiple logistic regression techniques to form a relationship between historical
injury data and the task parameters. Their results showed that five parameters were adequate to dis-
tinguish between the high- and low-risk jobs in this data set: lift rate, maximum sagittal angle,
average twisting velocity, maximum lateral velocity, and maximum moment (model odds ratio of
10.7). The result of their work is a low back injury risk assessment model that takes as inputs these
five task variables and the output is a single value that describes the probability of high-risk group
membership (PHRGM) for that job.
The principal strength of this model is that it is based on the empirical relationship between outcome
measures (injury and job turnover rates) with quantifiable job characteristics, including human perform-
ance-related variables. With this approach comes the ability to begin to consider the role that individual
differences (i.e., lifting and MMH techniques) may play in the etiology of low back injury. While this
model was able to overcome the static biomechanical modeling limitations of the NIOSH Lifting
Guides, a limitation to the generalizabilty of this model is that it was developed using data collected
from a sample of jobs where workers performed “repetitive jobs without job rotation.” Since this was
an empirical model, the specific job dataset that was used to develop the relationship between work
characteristics and risk will have a great influence on the model output. Since nonrepetitive jobs were
not included in the dataset, certain characteristics of these types of jobs may not be represented in
this model’s predictions. Further, because of the special emphasis placed on the variables describing
trunk dynamics that resulted from this sample of jobs, some high-risk activities, such as lifting heavy
loads in awkward, static postures will often escape identification.
FIGURE 35.2 A three-dimensional model using the 3DSSPP. (Provided by the Center for Ergonomics, University of
Michigan.)
Low Back Injury Risk Assessment Tools 35-5
similar analysis but used the strength capacity of a large/strong man in the denominator of the LSR, thus
normalizing the data such that it would be very unlikely to encounter a job have lifting requirements with
an LSR exceeding 1.0. Their results showed that for jobs with a LSR between 0.8 and 1.0 the job-related
low back incidence rate approached 4 while the incidence rates associated with lower LSRs were less
than 2.
The strength of the 3DSSPP approach is in its ability to assess risks associated with one time exertions,
because it compares directly the required moments of the task with population strength data. Another
strength of the model is its ability to estimate spine compression values that can be compared with estab-
lished load limits to assess relative risk. The limitations of this approach are in its ability to quantify risk
in jobs that are highly repetitive in nature but do not have torque or spine compression forces that
approach human strength capabilities or spine compression load limits.
In their study these three assessment tools were used in the analysis of construction workers in the home
building industry to identify specific work tasks to be addressed through subsequent intervention
research (Mirka et al., 2003). In this assessment a set of jobs was identified by each technique and the
results showed little overlap in the specific tasks that were identified for intervention. In their discussion
of these differences, the focus was less on the acute vs. cumulative loading distinction, but more on those
task characteristics that the individual assessment tools keyed on in making their risk assessments.
Specifically, it was noted that the NIOSH equation is very sensitive to the three-dimensional location
of a hand-held load while spine compression is more sensitive to the deviation of the center of mass
of the torso from its neutral position. The two extremes of this spectrum would be: (1) a person
bending to the ground with little or no load in the hands (such as using a tape measure to mark off a
wall location) and (2) a person holding a moderate weight in his or her hands at shoulder height
with arms extended. In the first case, the LI would be very near zero due to the light hand-held load
while there would be a significant amount of spine compression due to the body mass. In the second
case, the LI could be very high due to the extreme position of the load relative to the lifter while the
spine compression is relatively modest due to the neutral position of the torso. It is believed that the
origin of these differences can be found in the differences between the pure biomechanical approach
of the spine compression assessment and the contributions of the physiological and psychophysical
aspects of lifting which played a role in the development of the NIOSH Lifting Equation. Finally, the
LMM model approach is unique in its ability to consider the dynamic nature of the work demands in
developing its assessment in overall risk. Since force is equal to mass times acceleration, it is logical
to consider the additional forces encountered during dynamic lifting activities and this model is the
only one that captures these important characteristics. This “human performance” aspect of this tool
is somewhat unique among the three, however, the 3DSSPP does allow for the human performance
aspect to be considered through the posture assumed by the worker.
FIGURE 35.3 Three phases of modeling: (a) video capture, (b) stick figure representation for the 3DSSPP and
NIOSH models, and (c) laboratory LMM simulations. (Sawing at ground level.)
spent in these postures that these activities move into the priority rankings. This illustrates that the time-
weighted probabilistic representation of the biomechanical stresses gives an important insight into some
of the activities performed by these workers that would have gone unnoticed with more traditional task
analysis procedures.
FIGURE 35.4 Three phases of modeling: (a) video capture, (b) stick figure representation for the 3DSSPP and
NIOSH models, and (c) laboratory LMM simulations. (Lifting a crab pot into the deck of a boat.)
receiving more attention in the research literature is the prolonged stooped posture. There are a number
of industries (agriculture and construction to name two) that require these prolonged stooped postures
as part of the standard work activity and our current ability to quantify the risk associated with these
postures is quite limited. In the stooped posture, the mass of the upper body is often supported
through a passive mechanism that includes tension in the posterior spinal ligaments and fascia as well
as compression in the spine itself. If the stooped posture is maintained, the viscoelastic properties of
these passive tissues needs to be considered, which means that the time spent in the posture and the
Low Back Injury Risk Assessment Tools 35-9
45
40
35
30
Percent of Time
25
20
15
10
5
0
0
20
0
40 40
00 00
60 60
20 20
80 80
40 40
0
66
98
60
13
26 26
33 33
39 39
46 46
52 52
59 59
0–
–1
–6
0–
–
20
80
66
13
19
FIGURE 35.5 Distribution of probability of high-risk group membership values for framing carpenters.
required trunk posture are two important variables to consider when evaluating these work activities.
Further, as stooping period continues, the passive tissues in the lumbar spine lose their stiffness and
change their original dimensions, and micro-damage can occur in these tissues. These responses can
result in the increase of the laxity and range of motion of the lumbar spine, and consequently,
degrade the spinal stability.
A number of injury mechanisms have been proposed to understand the relationship between stooped
postures and low back injury risk. A mechanism of lumbar spine instability has been suggested by in vivo
experiments and biomechanical modeling studies. Cholewicki and McGill (1996) studied the lumbar
spine stability during three-dimensional dynamic tasks and observed that there was a sufficient stability
safety margin during tasks that demand a high muscular effort, whereas lighter tasks had a potential
hazard of spine buckling and the risk increased if passive tissues lose their stiffness, which is the response
of prolonged stooping. Another mechanism for low back disorder related to prolonged stooping has been
recently introduced by Solomonow et al. (2003). These authors examined muscle activity patterns of
multifidus and micro-damage in the L4/L5 supraspinous ligaments of in vivo feline lumbar spine
during 20 min constant creep loading and 7 h recovery period. In the creep loading period, the multi-
fidus showed exponential decrease over time and random spasms, suggesting possible decrease in the
stability of the lumbar spine due to reduced muscle force and the development of inflammation in liga-
ments. The damage and reduced stiffness in the ligaments were not fully recovered even after 24 h. This
indicates that the lumbar spine requires greater activation of multifidus muscles to maintain the stability
and protect the damaged tissues even after a full day’s rest. The concern might be that a similar task per-
formed on the following day may cause continued creep deformation and severe tissue damage in the
ligaments because of cumulative exposures to creep loading.
Risks associated with prolonged stooping include decreasing stability in lumbar spine structure,
increasing muscle exertion level, and micro-damage in passive tissues, and these are time-dependent
(stooping time and recovery time) and mainly driven by changes in physical characteristics of the pos-
terior spinal ligaments. Assessment of these risks is quite essential to understand and prevent low back
disorder from work-related prolonged stooping. No risk assessment tool of prolonged stooping has yet
been developed but is the subject of on-going development research in our laboratory. The approach
being pursued is to use finite element analysis (FEA) to develop a time-dependent biomechanical evalu-
ation of the system (Figure 35.6). Mechanical changes of passive tissues in the lumbar spine under creep
loading (prolonged stooping and recovery) can be simulated by modeling the lumbar spine using three-
dimensional FEA technique. Nonlinear and viscoelastic material properties of ligaments and disc com-
ponents have been investigated in in vitro experiments, and those data can be input into the FEA model.
35-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
(a)
Posterior Bony Elements
Vertebral Body
Intervertebral Disc
Spinal Ligaments
(b)
4.5
4
Flexion Angle (deg)
3.5
2.5
2
0 500 1000 1500 2000 2500 3000 3500
Time (sec)
FIGURE 35.6 Finite element analysis: (a) three-dimensional FEA model of a L3/L4 motion segment; (b) predicted
creep response of sagittal flexion angle during 1 h static moment loading.
The FEA model can simulate prolonged stooping and recovery, and the results (deformation and stress of
passive tissues) can be used to estimate the changes in lumbar spinal stability and micro-damage as a
function of time and posture. In the final assessment model the user would input the duty cycle charac-
teristics of the alternating stooping and upright postures as well as the trunk flexion angles assumed
during the stooping phase and the output would be a measure of the changes in a spinal stability
index and predicted degrees of micro-damage occurring in the passive tissues.
References
Adams, M. and Dolan, P. (1995). Recent advances in lumbar spinal mechanics and their clinical signifi-
cance, Clinical Biomechanics, 10(1):3– 19.
Chaffin, D. (1974). Human strength capability and low-back pain, Journal of Occupational Medicine,
16:248–254.
Chaffin, D. and Park, K. (1973). A longitudinal study of low-back pain as associated with occupational
weight lifting factors, American Industrial Hygiene Association Journal, 34:513–524.
Cholewicki, J. and McGill, S. (1996). Mechanical stability of the in vivo lumbar spine: implications for
injury and chronic low back pain, Clinical Biomechanics, 11:1 –15.
Low Back Injury Risk Assessment Tools 35-11
Dempsey, P. (2002). Usability of the Revised NIOSH Lifting Equation, Ergonomics, 45(12):817–828.
Dempsey, P. and Fathallah, F. (1999). Application issues and theoretical concerns regarding the 1991
NIOSH Equation Asymmetry Multiplier, International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics,
23(3):181–191.
Lavender, S., Oleske, D., Nicholson, L., Andersson, G., and Hahn, J. (1999). Comparison of five methods
used to determine low back disorder risk in a manufacturing environment, Spine, 24:1441–1448.
Marras, W., Lavender, S., Leurgans, S., Rajulu, S., Alread, G., Fathallah, F., and Ferguson, S. (1993). The
role of dynamic three-dimensional trunk motion in occupationally related low back disorders: the
effects of workplace factors, trunk position and trunk motion characteristics on risk of injury,
Spine, 18:617– 628.
Marras, W., Lavender, S., Leurgans, S., Fathallah, F., Ferguson, S., Allread, G., and Rajulu, S. (1995). Bio-
mechanical risk factors for occupationally related low back disorders, Ergonomics, 38:377 –410.
McGill, S. (1997). The biomechanics of low back injury: implications on current practice in industry and
the clinic, Journal of Biomechanics, 30(5):465 –476.
Mirka, G., Kelaher, D., Nay, D., and Lawrence, B. (2000). Continuous assessment of back stress (CABS): a
new approach to quantifying acute and cumulative low back stress, Human Factors, 42(2):
209–225.
Mirka, G., Monroe, M., Nay, D., Lipscomb, H., and Kelaher, D. (2003). Ergonomic interventions for the
reduction of low back stress in framing carpenters in the home building industry, International
Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 31(6):397 –409.
National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (1981) Work Practices Guide For Manual Lifting,
DHHS (NIOSH) Publication No. 81-122.
Solomonow, M., Baratta, R., Zhou, B., Burger, E., Zieske, A., and Gedalia, A. (2003). Muscular dysfunc-
tion elicited by creep of lumbar viscoelastic tissue, Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology,
13(4):381–396.
Wang, M., Garg, A., Chang, Y., Shih, Y., Yeh, W., and Lee, C. (1998). The relationship between low back
discomfort ratings and the NIOSH Lifting Index, Human Factors, 40:509–515.
Waters, T., Putz-Anderson, V., Garg, A., and Fine, L. (1993). Revised NIOSH equation for the design and
evaluation of manual lifting tasks, Ergonomics, 36:749–776.
Waters, T., Putz-Anderson, V., and Garg, A. (1994). Applications Manual for the Revised NIOSH Lifting
Equation, DHHS (NIOSH) Publication No. 94–110.
Waters, T., Baron, S., and Kemmlert, K. (1998). Accuracy of measurements for the Revised NIOSH
Lifting Equation, Applied Ergonomics, 29(6):433–438.
Waters, T., Baron, S., Piacitelli, L., Andedrson, V., Skov, T., Haring-Sweeney, M., Wall, D., and Fine, L.
(1999). Evaluation of the Revised NIOSH Lifting Equation: a cross-sectional epidemiologic
study, Spine, 24:386 –394.
36
Cognitive Task
Analysis — A Review
36-1
36-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
procedural knowledge of similar components, and specifying the conditions that best facilitate
progression from one knowledge state to another. CTA techniques are used for a number of different pur-
poses, and CTA outputs are used, among other things, to inform the design of procedures and processes,
the design of new technology and systems, allocation of functions, the development of training pro-
cedures and interventions, and the evaluation of individual and team performance within complex
systems.
Flanagan (1954) first probed the decisions and actions made by pilots in near accidents using the criti-
cal incident technique (CIT). However, the term “cognitive task analysis” did not appear until the early
1980s when it began to be used in research texts. According to Hollnagel (2003), the term was first used in
1981 to describe approaches to the understanding of the cognitive activities required of man– machine
systems. Since then, the focus on the cognitive processes employed by system operators has increased,
and CTA applications are now on the increase, particularly in complex, dynamic environments such
as those seen in the nuclear power, defence, and emergency services domains. Various CTA techniques
have been subject to widespread use over the past two decades, with applications in a number of
domains, such as fire fighting (Militello and Hutton, 2000), aviation (O’Hare et al., 2000), emergency
services (O’Hare et al., 2000), command and control (Chin et al., 1999), military operations (Klein,
2000), naval maintenance (Schaafstal and Schraagen, 2000), and even white-water rafting (O’Hare
et al., 2000).
There are a plethora of CTA approaches available to the HF practitioner. The Cognitive Task
Analysis Resource Website (www.ctaresource.com) lists over 100 CTA-related techniques designed
to evaluate and describe the cognitive aspects of task performance. Roth et al. (2002) suggest that
there are three different approaches to CTA. The first approach involves analyzing the domain in
question in terms of goals and functions, in order to determine the cognitive demands imposed
by the tasks performed. The second approach involves the use of empirical techniques, such as obser-
vation and interview techniques, in order to determine how the users perform the tasks under analy-
sis, allowing a specification of the knowledge requirements and strategies involved. The third and
more recent approach involves developing computer models that can be used to simulate the cogni-
tive activities required during the task under analysis. Traditional CTA approaches use a combination
of traditional knowledge elicitation methods such as observation, semistructured and structured inter-
views, and questionnaires in order to retrospectively elicit data regarding the mental processes used by
system operators during task performance. For example, the cognitively orientated task analysis
(COTA) framework is a collection of procedures, including verbal protocol analysis and interviews,
that are used to describe the expertise involved during task performance (DuBois and Shalin,
2000). The critical decision method (CDM) (Klein et al., 1989) uses direct observation and semistruc-
tured interviews in order to analyze the cognitive processes underlying decision-making in complex
environments. The applied cognitive task analysis (ACTA) approach (Militello and Hutton, 2000) uses
sets of specific cognitive probes designed to elicit information regarding the cognitive processes
employed during task performance. Finally, a more recent theme within CTA is the development
of software packages that are designed to automate large portions of the typically lengthy CTA
process and also to simulate the cognitive activities required during task performance. Software
techniques such as the man–machine integration design and analysis system (MIDAS) attempt to
simulate the cognitive processes required during task performance. There are a number of CTA
software packages available, such as MIDAS, MicroSaint, the work domain analysis workbench
(WDAW), the cognitive activity analysis toolset (CAATS), and the decompose, network, and assess
(DNA) tool.
during collaborative, team-based scenarios. According to Savoie (1998) (cited by Salas, 2004), the
use of teams has risen dramatically with reports of “team presence” by workers rising from 5% in
1980 to 50% in the mid-1990s. Cooke (2004) suggests that an increase in task cognitive complexity
caused by an increased use of technology has led to an increased requirement for teamwork. Salas
(2004) defines a team as consisting of two or more people, dealing with multiple information
resources, who work to accomplish some shared goal and suggests that while there are a number
of advantages associated with the use of teams, there are also a number of disadvantages. Cooke
(2004) suggests that teams are required to detect and interpret cues, remember, reason, plan, solve
problems, acquire knowledge, and make decisions as an integrated and coordinated unit. Team per-
formance comprises two components of behavior, taskwork and teamwork. Teamwork represents
those instances where individuals interact or coordinate behavior in order to achieve tasks that are
important to the team’s goals, while taskwork describes those instances where individuals in a
team setting are performing individual tasks separate from their team counterparts. Consequently,
various analyses of team performance in industrial settings has led to the conclusion that team per-
formance is extremely complex to understand and often flawed (Salas, 2004). Traditional CTA
approaches such as CDM and GOMS tend to focus upon individual task performance, whereas
team cognitive task analysis (TCTA) techniques focus on the cognitive components required
during collaborative performance. The output of TCTA techniques is typically used to aid the
design of team-based technology, the development of team training procedures, task allocation
within teams, and also the organization of teams and selection of team members. Klein (2000)
suggests that the main reason for conducting a TCTA is to improve the team in questions perform-
ance, and that this is achieved through using the CTA outputs to restructure the team, change the size
of the team, to design better information technology, information management strategies, human
computer interfaces, decision support systems, and communications, and also to develop novel
team training methods.
According to Blickensderfer et al. (2000), TCTA differs from individual CTA in two ways. Firstly, a
TCTA must identify, define, and describe the cognitive processes and knowledge associated with the
teamwork processes (e.g., communication and coordination) involved in the task or scenario under
analysis. Secondly, a TCTA technique must be able to determine the team knowledge required during
the task or scenario under analysis. Klein (2000) identified a set of cognitive processes that are required
during the performance of teamwork tasks. The team cognitive processes are outlined as follows (Klein,
2000):
. Control of attention — refers to the way a team engages in information management; for example,
information seeking, information communication, and allocation of attention
. Shared situation awareness — refers to the degree to which the members have the same interpret-
ation of ongoing events
. Shared mental models — refers to the extent that members have the same understanding of for the
dynamics of key processes; for example, roles and functions of each team member, nature of the
task, and use of equipment
. Application of strategies and heuristics to make decisions, solve problems, and plan
. Metacognition — refers to the process of self-monitoring in terms of difficulties encountered and
also the limitations and vulnerabilities of the team. Effective teams are able to self-monitor their
performance and shift strategies as appropriate
According to Klein (2000), a TCTA should capture and describe these five processes and then
represent the findings to others so that they can be effectively used. Blickensderfer et al. (2000)
conducted a review of existing CTA techniques that could potentially be applied to the analysis
of team performance and highlighted a number of techniques that showed promise for use in
the CTA of team performance. However, Blickensderfer et al. (2000) suggest that a thorough
analysis of the cognitive components underlying team performance is lacking. Although a
36-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
number of TCTA techniques do exist, a universally accepted approach to team CTA is yet to
emerge.
normally require the provision of a multidisciplinary team over a lengthy period of time. For example,
a CTA of a military-based task would, at least, require the following personnel:
. Cognitive psychologists
. Human factors engineers
. System designers
. Research staff
. Various military subject matter experts
Of course, it is often difficult to assemble such multidisciplinary teams, let alone gather them together at
one location for any period of time. The problems of cost and personnel shortages ensure that the process
of merely getting to the stage where a CTA analysis can actually commence is a very difficult one. As a
result, the problems associated with the cost, time invested, and personnel required may far outweigh
the benefits associated with conducting the CTA in the first place. Consequently, organizations may
be put off conducting CTA-type analyses by the cost-effectiveness issue alone, let alone the other
problems associated with it.
Alongside the resource intensiveness of CTA techniques, there are also a number of associated meth-
odological concerns. An analysis of the literature reveals a common problem associated with the format
and presentation of the results of cognitive task analyses. It is apparent that once a CTA has been con-
ducted and the results obtained, exactly what the results mean in relation to the problem goals is often
difficult to understand or is often misinterpreted. It is also evident that it is often not clear what to do
with the results of a CTA. Shute et al. (2000) highlight the imprecise and vague nature of CTA techniques.
It seems that the great amount of resources that are invested in a CTA effort are often wasted as the output
fails to be interpreted adequately by the system designers and their counterparts. Potter et al. (2000)
describe a bottleneck that occurs during the transition from CTA to system design, and suggest that
the information gained during the CTA must be effectively translated into design requirements and
specifications.
Potter et al. (2000) conducted a review of CTA techniques in order to evaluate the current state of prac-
tice in CTA. They discovered that there was a wide diversity in the CTA techniques employed, the type of
information generated, and also the manner in which the information is presented. According to Potter
et al. (2000), this diversity has led to confusion as to what CTA actually refers to, what results are expected
from CTA, and how the results will effect system development and evaluation. It was also concluded that
typical CTA approaches are labor intensive and generate huge amounts of data, which, of course, leads to
a lengthy transcription process.
Schraagen et al. (2000) conducted a review of existing CTA techniques and computer-based CTA tools.
The review indicated that although there were a large number of CTA techniques available, they were
generally limited. It was also concluded that there is limited guidance available in assisting practitioners
in the selection of the most appropriate CTA techniques, in how to use the available CTA techniques, and
also how to use the output of the CTA. As a result of the review of existing CTA techniques, Schraagen
et al. (2000) identified the following issues surrounding CTA that require further investigation:
techniques and their associated source(s), author(s), and availability. For the purposes of this review,
the following categories were used to determine the availability of the techniques identified in the
literature review:
1. Off the shelf — includes CTA techniques that can be purchased at a financial cost. Once purchased
the technique can be used freely by the owner(s)
2. Proprietary — includes CTA techniques that have been developed by HF consultancies and other
organizations. For a financial cost, the creators will conduct a CTA analysis using the technique in
question
3. Free — includes CTA techniques that are freely available in the public domain and can be used
without the author’s permission
4. Software — includes software-based CTA techniques and add-ons that can be purchased at a
financial cost
The result of this initial literature review was a database of over 50 CTA techniques. The CTA technique
database is presented in Table 36.1.
(Table continued)
36-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
of the appropriate methods. The output of the analysis is designed to act as a CTA techniques manual,
aiding practitioners in the use of the CTA techniques reviewed:
1. Name and acronym — The name of the technique and its associated acronym
2. Author(s), affiliations(s) and address(es) — The names, affiliations, and addresses of the authors
are provided to assist with citation and requesting any further help in using the technique
3. Availability — The availability of the technique is specified. Techniques are classed as free, off the
shelf, proprietary, or as software add-ons or plug-ins
4. Background and applications — This section introduces the method, its origins and development,
the domain of application of the method, and also application areas that it has been used in
5. Domain of application — Describes the domain that the technique was originally developed for
and applied in
6. Team/individual technique — Denotes which aspects of performance the technique caters for
7. Experts required — This section attempts to clarify whether SMEs are required as either analysts
or participants
8. Procedure and advice — This section describes the procedure for applying the method as well as
general points of expert advice
9. Flowchart — A flowchart is provided, depicting the methods procedure
10. Advantages — Lists the advantages associated with using the method in the design of C4i
systems
11. Disadvantages — Lists the disadvantages associated with using the method in the design of C4i
systems
12. Example — An example, or examples, of the application of the method are provided to show the
methods output
13. Related methods — Any closely related methods are listed, including contributory and similar
methods
14. Approximate training and application times — Estimates of the training and application times are
provided to give the reader an idea of the commitment required when using the technique
15. Reliability and validity — Any evidence on the reliability or validity of the method are cited
16. Tools needed — Describes any additional tools required when using the method
17. Bibliography — A bibliography lists recommended further reading on the method and the
surrounding topic area
36-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
A summary of the CTA methods review is presented in Table 36.3. A brief description of the methods
reviewed is presented below.
36-11
(Table continued)
TABLE 36.3 Continued
36-12
Team or Experts Training Related Validation
Method Domain Individual Required Time App. Time Methods Tools Needed Studies Advantages Disadvantages
Critical incident Generic Individual Yes Med – high High Critical decision Pen and paper Yes Can be used to elicit specific Reliability is questionable
technique method Audio recording information regarding There are numerous problems
equipment decision-making in associated with recalling
complex environments past events, such as
Seems suited to C4i analysis memory degradation
Great skill is required on
behalf
of the analyst for the
technique
36-13
(Table continued)
TABLE 36.3 Continued
36-14
Team or Experts Training Related Validation
Method Domain Individual Required Time App. Time Methods Tools Needed Studies Advantages Disadvantages
HTA — Generic Individual No Med Med HEI Pen and paper Yes HTA output feeds into Provides mainly
Hierarchical Task analysis numerous HF techniques descriptive information
task analysis Has been used extensively Cannot cater for the
in a variety of domains cognitive components
Provides an accurate of task performance
description of task activity Can be time consuming to
conduct for large, complex
tasks
Source: Militello, L. G., and Hutton, J. B. (2000). In J. Annett and N. A. Stanton (Eds), Task Analysis, pp. 90 –113. London:
Taylor & Francis. With permission.
Table 36.6. From the CDM analyses, it is possible to develop a propositional network that represents the
ideal collection of knowledge objects for the scenario. A propositional network for the scenario is pre-
sented in Figure 36.1.
Source: O’Hare, D., Wiggins, M., Williams, A., and Wong, W. (2000). In J. Annett and N. A. Stanton (Eds), Task Analysis,
pp. 170–190. London: Taylor & Francis. With permission.
activities. The COTA approach uses interviews with relevant SMEs in order to achieve these aims. In
describing job expertise, the COTA approach uses videotaped protocol analysis of task performance in
order to determine and describe the knowledge required during task performance. The final stage of a
COTA analysis, developing CTA products, involves transforming the knowledge representation into
appropriate inputs for the specified application.
Source: Salmon, P. M., Stanton, N. A., and Walker, G. (2004c). Work Package 1.1.3: NGT Switching Scenario Report.
Defence Technology Centre for Human Factors Integration Report.
variables (time occupied, level of information processing, and task set switches) are combined to deter-
mine the level of cognitive load imposed by the task. High ratings for the three variables equal a high
cognitive load imposed on the operator by the task.
Gas
Insulated Airblast NOC
Notices/
Locks System State
Certificates
Circuit
Breakers Check Open
Location
Lock & Caution
Isolation Wokingham
Open Lock & Shutters
Caution
Control
Engineer Switching
Instructions Time
Accept
Dressed
Rear
Log Sheet Procedures
Refuse Switching
Log
Displays Earth
Switches Isolators
Front
WE1000 Points of
Isolation
Closed
Voltage
Electrical
Contacts
Current
Phone
Equipment
DBI Switching Lables
Faulty
Indication Phone
Substations
Identity
SAP
FIGURE 36.1 Propositional network for energy distribution scenario. (Source: Salmon, P. M., Stanton, N. A., and
Walker, G. (2004c). Work Package 1.1.3: NGT Switching Scenario Report. Defence Technology Centre for Human
Factors Integation Report.)
question (moving from left to right across the decomposition hierarchy is the equivalent of zooming
into the system). An ADS can be used to develop so-called problem solving trajectories (Vicente, 1999)
for tasks. Problem solving trajectories demonstrate how actors switch between different models of a
particular work system in order to match their task demands. For example, Vicente (1999) describes
how verbal protocol analysis (VPA) data of electronic technicians engaged in troubleshooting computer
equipment were mapped onto an ADS in order to indicate the different ways (e.g. abstractions and
decompositions) in which technicians were thinking about the work domain. An example of a task
problem solving trajectory for a rail signaling task is presented in Figure 36.2 (Walker and Stanton,
2004). Each component task step was mapped onto an ADS to demonstrate how the platform staff and
signaler were thinking about the system when performing each task.
Task list for rail signaler task:
further analysis of the subgoal hierarchy that it offers to the HF practitioner. The majority of HF analysis
methods either require an initial HTA of the tasks under analysis as their input, or at least are made sig-
nificantly easier through the provision of an HTA. HTA acts as an input into various HF analyses, such
human error identification (HEI), allocation of function, workload assessment, interface design and
evaluation, and many more. Consequently, HTA has been applied in a number of domains, including
the process control and power generation industries (Annett, 2004), emergency services (Baber et al.,
2004) military applications (Kirwan and Ainsworth, 1992; Ainsworth and Marshall, 2000), civil aviation
(Marshall et al., 2003), driving (Walker, 2005) public technology (Stanton and Stevenage, 1998) and even
retail (Shepherd, 2001). A HTA of the fire brigade training scenario “Hazardous chemical spillage at
remote farmhouse” (Baber et al, 2004) is presented in Figure 36.3. Annett (2004) describes the appli-
cation of HTA to a team-based naval warfare task, the purpose of which was to identify and measure
team skills critical to successful antisubmarine warfare. An extract from the team HTA is presented in
Figure 36.4. The analysis is also presented in tabular format in Table 36.7.
1. Initiate 4. Resolve
2. Proceed to scene 3. Deal with
response to incident
of incident incident
incident
Plan 1: Do in order
Plan 4: Do in order
1.1 (Police 2.1.2 (Hospital) Inform 2.1.3 (Police 2.1.4 (Police officer) 2.1.5 (PO) Respond 2.2.1 (FC) Contact 2.2.2 (FC) 2.2.3 (FC)
officer) Proceed to police control of officer) Capture Gather information to situation involving station Request Dispatch to
incident casualty suffering from suspects from suspects chemicals attendance scene
respiratory problems
36-21
FIGURE 36.3 HTA for the fire brigade training scenario “Deal with chemical incident.”
36-22
(b) 3. Deal with
incident
Plan 3: Do in order
1. ProtectHVU
[1+2]
1.2.1. Urgent 1.2.3. Report 1.2.4. Input data 1.2.5. Deliberate 1.2.6. Follow up
attack 1.2.2. Step aside Contact to AIO system attack lost contact
[1>2]
1.2.5.1.1.2.
1.2.5.1.1.1.
Confirm
Issue SITREP
assessment
FIGURE 36.4 Extract from an analysis of an antisubmarine warfare team task. (Source: Annett, J. (2004). In
N. A. Stanton, A. Hedge, K. Brookhuis, E. Salas, and H. Hendrick (Eds), Handbook of Human Factors Methods.
Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. With permission.)
Source: Annett, J. (2004). In N. A. Stanton, A. Hedge, K. Brookhuis, E. Salas, and H. Hendrick (Eds), Handbook of Human
Factors Methods. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. With permission.
According to Klein (2000), a TCTA is provides a way of capturing each of these processes and repre-
senting the findings to others. The output of TCTA can be used to improve team performance through
informing team training, team design, and team procedures. A study of marine corps command posts
was conducted by Klein et al. (1996) as part of an exercise to improve the decision-making process in
command posts. Three data collection phases were used during the exercise. Firstly, four regimental exer-
cises were observed and any decision-making related incidents were recorded. As a result, over 200 critical
decision-making incidents were recorded. Secondly, interviews with command post personnel were con-
ducted in order to gather more specific information regarding the incidents recorded during the obser-
vation. Thirdly, a simulated decision-making scenario was used to test participant responses. Klein et al.
(1996) presented 40 decision requirements, including details regarding the decision, reasons for difficulty
in making the decision, errors and cues and strategies used for effective decision-making. The decision
requirements were categorized into the following groups: building and maintaining situational aware-
ness, managing information, and deciding on a plan. Furthermore, a list of 30 “barriers” to effective
decision-making were also presented. A summary of the barriers identified is presented in Table 36.11.
Cognitive Task Analysis 36-25
Source: Ormerod, T. C. (2000). In J. M. Schraagen, S. F. Chipman, and V. L. Shalin (Eds), Cognitive Task Analysis,
pp. 181–200. Hillsdale; NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. With permission.
From the simulated decision-making exercise, it was found that the experienced personnel (colonels
and lieutenant colonels) required only 5 to 10 min to understand a situation. However, majors took over
45 min to study and understand the same situation (Klein et al., 1996). In conclusion, Klein et al. (1996)
reported that there were too many personnel in the command post, which made it more difficult to
36-26 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Source: Ormerod, T. C. and Shepherd, A. (2003). In D. Diaper and N. A. Stanton (Eds), The Handbook of Task Analysis for
Human–Computer Interaction, pp. 347– 366. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. With permission.
complete the job in hand. Klein et al. (1996) suggested that reduced staffing at the command posts would
contribute to speed and quality improvements in the decisions made.
36-27
36-28 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Source: Adapted from Klein, G. (2000). In J. M. Schraagen, S. F. Chipman, and V. L. Shalin (Eds), Cognitive
Task Analysis, pp. 417–431. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. With permission.
normally conducted prior to interviews with the system personnel involved. Techniques such as CDM,
ACTA, and CWA utilize interview and observational techniques during their application. Interview and
observational techniques can also be used either alone or in conjunction with other techniques as part of
a CTA effort.
7. Poor interpretation of CTA outputs — It is apparent that there are problems associated with the
interpretation of the outputs offered from CTA analyses. Exactly what the outputs mean in
relation to the project goals is often misinterpreted. As a result, the applicability and benefits
of the approaches are often misunderstood. Potter et al. (2000) describe a bottleneck that
occurs during the transition from CTA to system design, and suggest that the information
gained during the CTA must be effectively translated into design requirements and
specifications.
8. Limited reliability and validity evidence associated with CTA techniques — The problem of the
validation of HF techniques remains a primary concern when considering HF method appli-
cations. As with other method types such as human error identification (HEI), situation aware-
ness (SA) measurement, and mental workload (MWL) assessment, the literature revealed a
distinct lack of CTA validation studies.
9. Lack of TCTA techniques — At the moment, there appears to be only a limited number of CTA
techniques that can be applied in team settings. The provision of TCTA techniques is a critical
requirement when considering the application of CTA in C4i environments. Exactly how to
measure the cognitive processes involved in team performance and collaboration requires
further investigation.
10. Lengthy training requirements associated with CTA techniques — Typical CTA approaches require
a lengthy training procedure, and practitioners are required to be proficient in a number of
different techniques (e.g., knowledge elicitation, task analysis, etc.) in order to use the techniques
efficiently.
11. Requirement of a multidisciplinary team in order to conduct CTA analyses effectively — A typical
CTA analysis effort requires a multidisciplinary team. According to Shute et al. (2000), a vast
number of appropriately skilled personnel are required if a CTA is to be conducted properly.
It is often difficult to assemble such multidisciplinary teams, let alone gather them together at
one location for any period of time.
Despite the problems associated with the use of CTA techniques, their provision during the design life-
cycle is essential. The original aim of the CTA methods review was to evaluate the suitability of existing
CTA methods for use during the design, development, and evaluation of C4i systems. The methods
review highlighted a subset of CTA techniques that appear to be suitable for use in C4i environments.
These techniques are presented in Table 36.12.
Furthermore, the methods review highlighted that the different CTA approaches are more suitable at
different stages of the CADMID design lifecycle. Thus, it is tentatively concluded that the selection of the
most appropriate CTA approach is dependent upon the stage in the CADMID (Concept, Assessment,
Demonstration, Manufacture, In-service, and Disposal) lifecycle that the CTA analysis is required. The
CADMID cycle (SEA Technologies, 2002) refers to the series of stages that are designed to take the
project in question through its whole life, from the initial concept stage to its disposal. There is a require-
ment for human factors integration (HFI) input at each stage in the CADMID cycle. A brief description
of each of the CADMID stages is given subsequentyly (adapted from Anon, 2002):
. Concept — The concept stage represents the beginning of any project, and requires an analysis of
the human issues related to the proposed project, and also an assessment of the associated risks and
requirements. The concept process is normally referred to as early human factors analysis (EHFA).
. Assessment — During the assessment stage, more detailed work is conducted in order to quantify
the HFI issues and risks. Information regarding the user tasks, working conditions, and expected
performance is normally required during this stage. Issues such as manpower reduction, workload,
and performance shortfalls are also assessed during this stage. The main objective during the assess-
ment stage is to focus on the human-related issues arising from the project in question.
. Demonstration — During the demonstration stage, specifications are refined to ensure robust HFI
content, and clear human performance targets are specified.
36-30 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
. Manufacture — During the manufacture stage, the MOD ensures integration with training devel-
opment, tactics development, support strategy, etc. End-user trials are also conducted in order to
build confidence in equipment operability, which leads to acceptance and subsequent hand over.
. In-Service — Declaration of in-service date (ISD) follows the demonstration of effective integration
of the equipment with the human component (personnel, procedures, support, and training
games) under operational conditions. While in-service, HFI evaluation helps to identify any
human-related performance shortfalls or failures of human– equipment integration.
. Disposal — The aim of the disposal stage is to dispose of the equipment safely, efficiently, and
effectively.
Table 36.13 presents the CADMID lifecycle phases, their associated HFI aims, objectives and activities,
and recommended CTA techniques for each stage.
The concept stage involves the development of a description of the users of the proposed system and
also a high-level task description. The CWA approach (Rasmussen et al., 1994) is useful at this stage as it
involves conducting an analysis of the tasks involved in the work domain, an analysis of the allocation of
work, and also worker competency and mental strategies involved. HTA and HTA(T) offers a description
of the tasks involved in the system under analysis. The assessment stage involves identifying the human-
related system requirements, analyzing human performance, reviewing the human issues involved, and
conducting user interface modeling. Again, the CWA approach is useful at this stage as it offers an
exhaustive breakdown of the work involved, the competencies and strategies required, and also the allo-
cation of work within the system. The CWA approach has also been commonly used during the design of
user interfaces in the past. HTA and HTA(T) are also required here in order to determine the key
human-related system requirements. The demonstration stage is perhaps the CADMID stage where
there is the most scope for CTA applications. The demonstration phase involves the analysis and evalu-
ation of design concepts, the definition of human performance and operability targets, the coordination
of the human aspects of system design, training analysis and support analysis, and dealing with HFI
issues as they arise during the design process. Thus, there is potential for the applications of various
CTA approaches during the demonstration stage. HTA, HTA(T), CWA, CDM, TCTA, TTA, and the
DRX could all potentially be applied during the demonstration stage, particularly for the analysis and
evaluation of the design concepts. Techniques such as the CDM and TCTA are particularly useful
when considering the evaluation and definition of human performance and also the specification of
training and support procedures. The manufacture stage also has scope for CTA applications, as it
involves further HFI evaluation and analysis. As such, numerous CTA techniques could potentially be
applied, including CDM, TCTA, TTA, DRX, interviews, and observation. The in-service stage involves
the demonstration of effective integration of the equipment with the human component (personnel,
procedures, support, and training games) under operational conditions. To support further performance
TABLE 36.13 Recommended CTA Techniques for Each CADMID Stage
36-31
Disposal Assess HF issues of the proposed disposal process. Take action Assess HF issues of the proposed disposal process. Take action as CWA
as required required HTA, HTA(T)
36-32 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
evaluation, techniques such as CDM, TCTA, TTA, and DRX are required. Finally, the disposal stage
involves the assessment of the HF issues involved during the disposal of the system in question, in
order to ensure its efficient and safe disposal. HTA and HTA(T) could potentially be used to offer a
step-by-step description of the proposed disposal process, while the CWA technique could be used to
determine the work, organization, competencies, and strategies involved during the proposed disposal
process.
It is apparent that there is scope for much further work in the area of CTA. Further investigation into
the development of guidelines for the selection and usage of the various CTA techniques, the validation of
CTA techniques, the interpretation of CTA outputs, and the development of software assistance is
required. It is clear that for the future of CTA to be assured, a great deal of work is required. Without
this, the usefulness of CTA techniques may well be questioned.
Acknowledgment
The work reported in this paper was carried out under the U.K. Defence Technology Centre (DTC) for
Human Factors Integration (HFI), funded by the U.K. Ministry of Defence and the Defence Science and
Technology Laboratory (DSTL).
References
Ainsworth, L. and Marshall, E. (2000). Issues of quality and practicability in task analysis: preliminary
results from two surveys. In J. Annett and N. A. Stanton (Eds), Task Analysis pp. 79 –90. Boca
Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Annett, J. (2005). Hierarchical task analysis (HTA). In N. A. Stanton, A. Hedge, K. Brookhuis, E. Salas,
and H. Hendrick (Eds), Handbook of Human Factors Methods. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Annett, J., Duncan, K. D., Stammers, R. B., and Gray, M. (1971). Task Analysis. London: HMSO.
Baber, C. (2005). Repertory grid for product evaluation. In N. A. Stanton, A. Hedge, K. Brookhuis,
E. Salas, and H. Hendrick (Eds), Handbook of Human Factors Methods. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Baber, C., Walker, G., Salmon, P., and Stanton, N. A. (2004). Observation Study Conducted at the Fire
Service Training College. Human Factors Integration Defence Technology Centre Report.
Bass, A., Aspinal, J., Walter, G., and Stanton, N. A. (1995). A software toolkit for hierarchical task
analysis. Applied Ergonomics, 26 (2) 147–151.
Blickensderfer, E., Cannon-Bowers, J. A., Salas, E., and Baker, D. P. (2000). Analysing knowledge require-
ments in team tasks. In J. M. Schraagen, S. F. Chipman, V. L. Shalin, (Eds), Cognitive Task Analysis,
pp. 431–447. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Burke, C. S. (2005). Team task analysis. In N. A. Stanton, A. Hedge, K. Brookhuis, E. Salas, and
H. Hendrick (Eds), Handbook of Human Factors Methods. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Card, S. K., Moran, T. P., and Newell, A. (1983). The Psychology of Human Computer Interaction.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Chin, M., Sanderson, P., and Watson, M. (1999). Cognitive work analysis of the command and control
work domain. Proceedings of the 1999 Command and Control Research and Technology Symposium
(CCRTS), June 29–July 1, Newport, RI, Volume 1, pp. 233–248.
Cooke, N. J. (2004). Measuring team knowledge. In N. A. Stanton, A. Hedge, K. Brookhuis, E. Salas, and
H. Hendrick (Eds), Handbook of Human Factors Methods. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Diaper, D. and Johnson, P. (1989). Task analysis for knowledge descriptions: Theory and application in
training. In J. Long and A. Whitefield (Eds), Cognitive Ergonomics in Human Computer Interaction,
pp. 191–224. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
DuBois, D. and Shalin, V. L. (2000). Describing job expertise using cognitively-oriented task analysis.
In J. M. Schraagen, S. F. Chipman, and V. L. Shalin (Eds), Cognitive Task Analysis, pp. 41 –56.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Flanagan, J. C. (1954). The critical incident technique. Psychological Bulletin, 51, 327 –358.
Cognitive Task Analysis 36-33
Hall, E., Gott, S., and Pokomy, R. (1995). A Procedural Guide to Cognitive Task Analysis: The
PARI Methodology. Technical Report AL/HR-TR-1995-0108. Brooks AFB, TX: Armstrong
Laboratory.
Hollnagel, E. (2003). Handbook of Cognitive Task Design. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Kirwan, B. (1998a). Human error identification techniques for risk assessment of high-risk systems —
Part 1: review and evaluation of techniques. Applied Ergonomics, 29, 157 –177.
Kirwan, B. (1998b). Human error identification techniques for risk assessment of high-risk systems —
Part 2: towards a framework approach. Applied Ergonomics, 5, 299 –319.
Kirwan, B. and Ainsworth, L. K. (1992). A Guide to Task Analysis. London: Taylor & Francis.
Klein, G. (2000). Cognitive task analysis of teams. In J. M. Schraagen, S. F. Chipman, and V. L. Shalin
(Eds), Cognitive Task Analysis, pp. 417–431. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Klein, G. A., Calderwood, R., and MacGregor, D. (1989). Critical decision method for eliciting knowl-
edge. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man and Cybernetics, 19 (3), 462–472.
Klein, G., Schmitt, J., McCloskey, M., Heaton, J., Klinger, D., and Wolf, S. (1996). A Decision-Centred
Study of the Regimental Command Post. Fairborn, OH: Klein Associates.
Klinger, D. W. and Hahn, B. B. (2005). Team decision requirement exercise: making team decision
requirements explicit. In N. A. Stanton, A. Hedge, K. Brookhuis, E. Salas, and H. Hendrick.
(Eds), Handbook of Human Factors Methods. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Lesgold, A. (2000). On the future of cognitive task analysis. In J. M., Schraagen, S. F. Chipman, and V. L.
Shalin (Eds.), Cognitive Task Analysis, pp. 135–146. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Marshall, A., Stanton, N., Young, M., Salmon, P., Harris, D., Demagalski, J., Waldmann, T., and
Dekker, S. (2003). Development of the human error template — a new methodology for assessing
design induced errors on aircraft flight decks. DTI ErrorPred Report.
Militello, L. G. and Hutton, J. B. (2000). Applied cognitive task analysis (ACTA): a practitioner’s toolkit
for understanding cognitive task demands. In J. Annett & N. A Stanton (Eds), Task Analysis,
pp. 90–113. London: Taylor & Francis.
Neerincx, M. A. (2003). Cognitive task load analysis: allocating tasks and designing support. In
E. Hollnagel (Ed), Handbook of Cognitive Task Design, pp. 281–305. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
O’Hare, D., Wiggins, M., Williams, A., and Wong, W. (2000). Cognitive task analyses for decision centred
design and training. In J. Annett and N. Stanton (Eds), Task Analysis, pp. 170 –190. London: Taylor
& Francis.
Ormerod, T. C. (2000). Using task analysis as a primary design method: the SGT approach. In J. M.
Schraagen, S. F. Chipman, and V. L. Shalin (Eds), Cognitive Task Analysis, pp. 181–200. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Ormerod, T. C. and Shepherd, A. (2004). Using task analysis for information requirements specification:
the sub-goal template (SGT) method. In D. Diaper and N. Stanton (Eds), The Handbook of Task
Analysis for Human –Computer Interaction, pp. 347 –366. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Polson, P. G. and Lewis, C. (1990). Theory-based design for easily learned interfaces. Human Computer
Interaction, 5 (2 & 3), 191–220.
Polson, P. G., Lewis, C., Rieman, J., and Wharton, C. (1992). Cognitive walkthroughs: a method for
theory based evaluation of user interfaces. International Journal of Man– Machine Studies, 36,
741–773.
Potter, S. S., Roth, E. M., Woods, D. D., and Elm, W. (2000). In J. M. Schraagen, S. F. Chipman, and V. L.
Shalin (Eds), Cognitive Task Analysis, pp. 317–340. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Rasmussen, J., Pejtersen, A., and Goodstein, L. P. (1994). Cognitive Systems Engineering. New York: John
Wiley & Sons.
Redding, R. E. (1989). Perspectives on cognitive task-analysis: the state of the state of the art. In Proceed-
ings of the Human Factors Society, Vol. 33, pp. 1353 –1357. Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors
Society.
36-34 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Roth, E. M., Patterson, E. S., and Mumaw, R. J. (2002). Cognitive engineering: issues in user-
centred system design. In J. J. Marciniak (Ed), Enclyopedia of Software Engineering, 2nd edn,
pp. 163–179. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Salas, E. (2005). Team methods. In N. A. Stanton, A. Hedge, K. Brookhuis, E. Salas, and H. Hendrick.
(Eds), Handbook of Human Factors Methods. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Salmon, P. M., Stanton, N. A., Walker, G., and Green, D. (2004a). Situation awareness measurement:
A review of applicability for C4i environments. The Journal of Applied Ergonomics (In Press).
Salmon, P. M., Stanton, N. A., Walker, G., and Green, D. (2004b). Human Factors Design and Evaluation
Methods Review. Defence Technology Centre for Human Factors Integration, Report No.
HFIDTC/WP1.3.2/1.
Salmon, P. M., Stanton, N. A., and Walker, G. (2004c). Work Package 1.1.3: NGT Switching Scenario
Report. Defence Technology Centre for Human Factors Integration Report.
Savoie, E. J. (1998). Tapping the power of teams. In R. S. Tindale, L. Heath, et al. (Eds), Theory and
Research on Small Groups. Social Psychological Applications to Social Issues, vol. 4, pp. 229 –244.
New York: Plenum Press.
Schaafstal, A. and Schraagen, J. M. (2000). Training of troubleshooting: a structured, task analytical
approach. In J. M. Schraagen, S. F. Chipman, and V. L. Shalin (Eds), Cognitive Task Analysis,
pp. 57–71. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Schraagen, J. M., Chipman, S. F., and Shalin, V. L. (2000). Cognitive Task Analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Schraagen, J. M., Ruisseau, J. I., Graff, N., Annett, J., Strub, M. H., Sheppard, C., Chipman, S. E., Shalin,
V. L., and Shute, V. L. (2000). Cognitive Task Analysis. RTO Technical Report 24.
Seamster, T. L., Redding, R. E., and Kaempf, G. L. (2000). A skill-based cognitive task analysis framework.
In J. M. Schraagen, S. F. Chipman, and V. L. Shalin (Eds), Cognitive Task Analysis, pp. 135–146.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
SEA Technologies (2002). STGP 10 HFI Management Guide. Bristol: SEA.
Shepherd, A. (2001). Hierarchical Task Analysis. London: Taylor & Francis.
Shute, V. J., Torreano, L. A., and Willis, R. E. (2000). DNA: providing the blueprint for instruction.
In J. M. Schraagen, S. F. Chipman, and V. L. Shalin (Eds), Cognitive Task Analysis, pp. 71 –86.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Stanton, N. A. and Stevenage, S. V. (1998). Learning to predict human error: issues of acceptability,
reliability and validity. Ergonomics, 41 (11), 1737 –1756.
Stanton, N. A., Hedge, A., Brookhuis, K., Salas, E., and Hendrick, H. (2005). Handbook of Human Factors
Methods. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Vicente, K. J. (1999). Cognitive Work Analysis: Towards Safe, Productive, and Healthy Computer-Based
Work. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Walker, G. H. (2005). Verbal protocol analysis. In N. A. Stanton, A. Hedge, K. Brookhuis, E. Salas, and
H. Hendrick (Eds), Handbook of Human Factors Methods. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Walker, G. H. and Stanton, N. A. (2004). Work Package 1.1.3: EAST Analysis of a Railway Signalling Task.
Defence Technology Centre for Human Factors Integration Report.
Walker, G. H., Stanton, N. A., and Young, M. S. (2001). An on-road investigation of vehicle feedback and
its role in driver cognition: Implications for cognitive ergonomics. International Journal of
Cognitive Ergonomics, 5 (4), 421–444.
Zachary, W. W., Ryder, J. M., and Hicinbothom, J. H. (2000). Building Cognitive Task Analyses and
Models of a Decision-Making Team in a Complex Real-Time Environment.
37
Subjective Scales of
Effort and Workload
Assessment
37-1
37-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Paired Comparison Technique, and the Likert Scale (Sinclair, 1995). The simple rating method consists of
a set of questions related to workload or task attributes, and a scale on which those questions are rated.
The rating scale is represented by the 100 mm line, usually with subdivisions and labels at the end of the
scale. The intermediate labels or numbers are often assigned to some or all subdivisions. Thurstone’s
Paired Comparison Technique asks the subject to compare two entities (e.g., two tasks) or every combi-
nation of all measured entities, and decide which one is larger or smaller. Each pair of compared entities
can be also evaluated on the rating scale with assigned numbers. The Likert Scale has an odd number of
discrete options and consisting of a range from 1 to 5 (or 7), with labels from “strongly disagree” to
“strongly agree,” respectively.
There are several common approaches applied in subjective workload measurements (Tsang and
Vidulich, 1994): (1) unidimensional versus multidimensional rating scale; (2) immediate versus retro-
spective assessment procedure; and (3) absolute estimation versus relative judgments approach. Unidi-
mensional scale assesses only one dimension of workload or focuses on the overall workload level, while
multidimensional scale evaluates several aspects or components of workload. The ratings can be obtained
immediately after performance of each condition or retrospectively after experiencing all task conditions.
The simple or direct rating approach is called the absolute magnitude estimation, in contrast to the rela-
tive judgment approach where as in Thurstone’s Paired Comparison Technique, the subject is asked to
assess the task condition in reference to the single standard or multiple task conditions.
Each subjective method should demonstrate several properties in order to be accepted as good
measurement tool. These properties are: validity, reliability, sensitivity, diagnosticity, intrusiveness, trans-
ferability, and ease of implementation and subject acceptability (Eggemeier et al., 1991; Wierwille and
Eggemeier, 1993). These criteria should be applied when the suitability of workload assessment
method is considered for evaluation of a particular type of workload, or for a particular type of
environment, task, or system.
Reliability is the degree of precision to which a method or instrument is able to measure what it
measures. Reliability can be assessed as homogeneity, consistency or stability of measurement, or in
the case of two or more raters as the interrater reliability (ISO/FDIS 10075-3, 2002).
Validity is the degree to which a method or instrument is able to measure what it is intended to
measure (ISO/FDIS 10075-3, 2002). Thus, in case of mental workload, the measures should reflect differ-
ences in cognitive demands, but not changes in other variables such as physical workload, which are not
necessarily associated with mental workload.
Sensitivity refers to how well a technique can distinguish the differences in levels of load required to
accomplish a task (Wierwille and Eggemeier, 1993). With regard to workload, this criterion is primary
among other criteria, since it is important to access differences in the workload imposed by a task or
system.
Diagnosticity is the ability to distinguish the type of workload, or ability to attribute it to a particular
aspect of a performed task (Tsang and Wilson, 1997). Thus, the diagnosticity can help to determine
which elements or aspects of the task caused workload.
Intrusiveness is related to the fact that application of measurement technique can interfere with
the task performance, which is evaluated, and can cause performance changes that are not related to
the task itself (Wierwille and Eggemeier, 1993). In order to avoid contamination of workload measures,
it is desirable to minimize intrusiveness of the measurement method.
Transferability refers to the possibility of applying a given technique in different environments or tasks.
Implementation requirements related to such issues are: ease of data collection, robustness of the
measurement instruments, and overall data quality control (Wierwille and Eggemeier, 1993). Finally,
subject acceptability refers to the subject’s perception of the measurement procedure.
In reference to criteria presented earlier, the subjective workload methods are described as sensitive to
different levels of the workload. However, since many research studies showed rather low diagnosticity of
the measures, it was concluded that the subjective rating scales can be used only as global measure of
workload (Eggemeier and Wilson, 1991). There is some evidence that the multidimensional workload
rating scales present better diagnostic properties (Tsang, 2001). Subjective measures appear to be reliable
Subjective Scales of Effort and Workload Assessment 37-3
and have concurrent validity with performance measures. In most applications, the rating scales are
completed after the task performance and, therefore, are considered as nonintrusive methods.
Furthermore, subjective methods have high subject acceptance and are easy to implement.
S ¼ k log I
where S is the strength of sensation, k is a constant, and I is the intensity of the stimulus.
Stevens (1974) argued that the approach of Fechner is “indirect” because it defines ability to discriminate
among stimuli as the basic unit of sensation. Stevens developed a ratio scaling technique, for example,
magnitude estimation (ME), where the subjects were asked to “directly” assign numbers to stimuli to rep-
resent the magnitudes of their sensations. Stevens (1974) modified the Fechner Law and discovered more
general psychophysical law which holds in all sense modalities. Stevens’s power law states that the strength
of sensation (S) and the intensity of its physical stimulus (I) is related by the power function:
S ¼ k In
where S is the strength of sensation, k is a constant, I is the intensity of the stimulus, and n is the slope of the
line that represents the power function when plotted on log–log coordinates. According to the
aforementioned power law, equal stimulus ratios produce equal sensation ratios.
The RPE scale was developed according to the principles of quantitative semantics, which deals with the
meaning of words and the quantitative relationship between verbal expressions (Noble and
Robertson, 1996; Borg and Borg, 2002). Borg chose verbal expressions for the scale that were not only
well defined but also had a comparatively equal meaning for subjects. The original RPE scale was developed
to be linearly related to heart rate, and based on subjective force estimates provided by subjects while per-
forming short-time work on a bicycle ergometer with a stepwise increase in workload. In order to relate the
scale linearly with heart rate, the scale number were arranged to conform to the absolute heart rate quan-
tities. Since heart rate ranges from 60 to 200 in a healthy group of adults, the scale numbers were set from 6
to 20 (Table 37.1). Close relationship with the pulse rate and application of quantitative semantics support
the notion that the RPE scale is a category scale with interval properties, that is, the scale not only rank
sensations but, also satisfies the equal interval criterion (Noble and Robertson, 1996).
Since the original 15-point RPE scale does not reflect the more exponential growth of perceptual
responses as intensity increases, Borg (1982) developed a Category– Ratio (CR10) scale for perceived
exertion (Table 37.2). This scale follows the growth of perceptual responses, and the exponential
growth of nonlinear physiological responses such as lactate accumulation and pulmonary ventilation.
This scale also combined advantages of both scales, that is, category-rating and ratio scale (Borg,
2002). CR scale can be useful for identifying local sensations of effort during high-intensity exercise.
However, a valid determination of direct intensity levels with this scale is not possible. Thus, Borg
(1982) did not recommend use of this scale in place of the 15-point RPE scale for general exercise
testing. The CR10 scale has 12 categories, with values ranging from 0 to 10, with “0” for “nothing at
all” and 0.5 for “extremely weak (just noticeable)”. In order to prevent a ceiling affect, the term
“maximal” was placed outside of the scale, to which any number can be assigned by subject (Noble
and Robertson, 1996; Borg and Borg, 2002).
The original Borg RPE scale has been shown to be an accurate and reliable instrument to measure per-
ceived exertion in a number of investigations. The use of the Borg scale was validated on several tasks
such as bicycle ergometer, walking, stool stepping, and walking treadmill. Various physiological measures
of physical exertion: heart rate, blood and muscle lactate concentration, oxygen uptake, ventilation and
respiration rate have been used as criterion in the validation research (Noble and Robertson, 1996;
Russell, 1997; Chen et al., 2002). The validation against heart rate showed correlation coefficients in
the range of r ¼ 0.80 –0.90 (Noble and Robertson, 1996; Hampson et al., 2001; Chen, 2002). The
reliability coefficients established by test –retest method were 0.78 and 0.90 for bicycle ergometer tasks
(Skinner et al., 1973; Noble and Robertson, 1996), 0.76 for the oscillating test, 0.76 for stool stepping
exercise, and 0.76 for treadmill walking test (Noble and Robertson, 1996). In addition, validation
Subjective Scales of Effort and Workload Assessment 37-5
Source: Adapted from Borg, G.A. (1982). Med Sci Sports Exerc, 14, 377– 381.
With permission.
studies showed that different levels of physical activity, gender, age and conditions such as asthma, blind-
ness, and cardiac disease do not affect the validity of this scale (Hampson et al., 2001). The CR10 scale has
also been correlated with physiological measures, such as heart rate, blood lactate, and muscle lactate,
supporting the validity of the scale (Borg, 1985; Borg et al., 1987; Noble and Robertson, 1996).
Most of the differences in RPE responses are attributed to physiological factors (oxygen uptake and
heart rate, ventilation and respiration rate, electromyographic (EMG) measures, perspiration rate,
blood lactate) (Borg, 2001). However, quite large variance in RPE responses may be explained by psycho-
logical factors such as personality and rating behavior, emotional and motivational factors. Morgan
(1973) demonstrated that subjects who are neurotic, anxious, and depressed have difficulties with accu-
rate rating of perceived exertion. It was also revealed that preferred exercise intensity was higher for extra-
verted subjects than for introverted subjects, and supported the hypothesis that extraverts suppress
painful stimuli (Morgan, 1973).
The overwhelming focus on these scales application has been the use of RPE for regulation of exercise
intensity and exercise prescription (Russell, 1997). The main application of scales of perceived exertion in
human factors research is in the assessment of physical workload (Borg, 2001). The RPE scale is often used
to evaluate the exertion and difficulty of a work task in order to compare different performance techniques
or to adjust the task to specific population group (age, gender, or handicapped). Perceived exertion
has been used in different context to evaluate the level of physical effort required to complete different
tasks (Shepard, 1994). The Borg CR10 scale was also used to rate physical exertions of truck drivers
during heavy operations (Johansson and Borg, 1993). In a package delivery industry, the scale of perceived
exertion was used to estimate the amounts of load that correspond to various levels of load heaviness
(Genaidy et al., 1998). Psychophysical assessments were also applied to investigate the perceived physical
effects of resident-transferring methods on nursing assistants, and to determine which method minimizes
psychophysical stress (Zhuang et al., 2000). Ratings of perceived exertions also have been used to
determine appropriate work design parameters, for example, efficient working postures, task frequency,
work capacity, and tool assessment (Pandolf et al., 1978; Legg et al., 1997; Olendorf and Drury, 2001).
seven-point category scale with values from 1 to 7, each number anchored with verbal descriptions from
1 ¼ very, very light, to 7 ¼ very, very hard, and 4 ¼ somewhat hard. In the first study (Hogan and
Fleishman, 1979) the proposed index was applied to assessment of 37 occupational and 62 recreational
tasks whose metabolic costs had been previously determined. In the second study (Hogan et al., 1980)
energy expenditure for 24 manual handling tasks were measured and perceived effort were assessed.
Both studies supported validity and reliability of the physical effort scale. The results showed high
interrater agreement (0.83) among subjects concerning the perceived effort needed to perform the
assessed tasks. The ratings of perceived effort were highly related (0.83–0.88) to estimated metabolic
costs. Subjects were also able to make distinction between tasks at all ranges of physical effort. Similarities
in ratings of perceived exertion suggest that group ratings of perceived effort can be used to accurately
reflect metabolic costs of task performance (Fleishman et al., 1984), as opposed to individual ratings that
only represent the effort perceived by one person and do not always accurately represent perception of
population.
for as long as possible, and if estimates of discomfort level were made at intervals during the holding
time, the growth of discomfort was linearly related to holding time regardless of the level of force
being exerted (Corrlet, 1995). Therefore, it was concluded that perception of discomfort level can be
used as a linear scale. In this technique the severity of discomfort is evaluated on a five- or seven-
point ordinal scale anchored at 0 and 5(7) categories by “no discomfort” and “extreme discomfort.”
In order to identify the body areas where discomfort is experienced the body map divided into seg-
ments is used. The original body chart used in Corlett and Bishop (1976) has been modified by many
researchers to fit their particular purposes (Olendorf and Drury 2001; Drury et al., 1989). Coury and
Drury (1982) and Drury et al. (1989) developed three summary measures for the Body Part Discomfort
Scale: BPD frequency (BPDF), BPD severity (BPDS), and BDP frequency severity (BPDFS). BPDF is cal-
culated as the number of body parts rated greater than zero, whereas BPDS is the average of all nonzero
ratings. BPDFS is the product of the BPDF and BPDS.
The Body Part Discomfort Scale (Corrlett and Bishop, 1976) has been validated in a many studies. Sig-
nificant relationship was found between the applied biomechanical torque and discomfort (Boussenna
et al., 1982). The Boussenna et al. (1982) study also supported relationship between holding time and
perceived discomfort level, that is postures that caused greater discomfort than others led to shorter
holding times. Jung and Choe (1996) demonstrated that a psychophysical scale of discomfort correctly
reflects physiological muscle activity obtained with EMG recordings. The cross-validation of this tech-
nique with RPE scale and OWAS showed high intercorrelation of results (Drury et al., 1989; Liao and
Drury, 2000; Olendorf and Drury, 2001). The reliability of body discomfort scale was demonstrated
with high intersubject agreement correlation in the study on the postural loading effect at joints
(Boussenna et al., 1982). However, the research results on sensitivity of the Body Part Discomfort
Scale are equivocal. Bonney et al. (1990) revealed that perceived discomfort discriminate 208changes
in back postures from one another. In the study by Kumar et al. (1999), the BDPR was unable to differ-
entiate between task variables, in comparison to RPE and visual scale (VS) that were sensitive to changes
in the lifting tasks, which were of a short duration, continuous, and not biomechanically demanding.
from paired comparisons, is used for calculation of the weights for each dimension. These weights rep-
resent relative importance associated with each dimension for the workload of rated task. During the
event scoring procedure, subjects rate performed task on each of the six workload scales. Workload
evaluation with TLX is conducted immediately after completion of task.
Several studies reported that NASA TLX was a valid and reliable measure of workload (Hart and
Staveland, 1988; Hill et al., 1992). Hart and Staveland (1988) stated that the TLX provides a sensitive
indicator of the overall workload as it differentiates among tasks of various cognitive and physical
demands. They also concluded that the weights determined for each TLX dimension reflect diagnostic
information about the sources of loading within a task. Instrument sensitivity has been tested in a
wide variety of multitask environments, including flight simulators (Battiste and Bortolussi, 1988;
Tsang and Johnson, 1989), actual flight (Shively et al., 1987), air combat (Hill et al., 1989; Bittner
et al., 1989). These studies showed that TLX ratings significantly discriminated the flight segments
(Shively et al., 1987; Battiste and Bortolussi, 1988) and between low- and high-workload scenarios
(Battiste and Bortolussi, 1988; Tsang and Johnson, 1989).
Low High
FIGURE 37.1 Rating scale used in NASA task load index.
Subjective Scales of Effort and Workload Assessment 37-9
TABLE 37.6 The Description of Workload Dimensions of Subjective Workload Assessment Technique (SWAT)
I. Time load
1. Often have spare time. Interruptions or overlap among activities occur infrequently or not at all
2. Occasionally have spare time. Interruptions or overlap among activities occur frequently
3. Almost never have spare time. Interruptions or overlap among activities are very frequent, or occur all the time
II. Mental effort load
1. Very little conscious mental effort or concentration required. Activity is almost automatic, requiring little or no attention
2. Moderate conscious mental effort or concentration required. Complexity of activity is moderately high due to uncertainty,
unpredictability, or unfamiliarity. Considerable attention required
3. Extensive mental effort and concentration are necessary. Very complex activity requiring total attention
III. Psychological stress load
1. Little confusion, risk, frustration, or anxiety exists and can be easily accommodated
2. Moderate stress due to confusion, frustration, or anxiety noticeably adds to workload. Significant compensation is required
to maintain adequate performance
3. High to very intense stress due to confusion, frustration, or anxiety
4. High to extreme determination and self-control required
measurement methodology is applied to develop the best-fitting scale for perceived workload. SWAT has
the capability to account for individual differences by grouping the subjects according to the dimensions
they emphasize most in their ratings (Meshkati et al., 1992). A separate workload scale can be derived for
each subgroup. Although the individuals are not asked to evaluate the importance of each of the three
SWAT dimensions, the estimates of the relative importance are obtained as a function of rescaling of
rank-ordered data by conjoint scaling procedure (Nygren, 1991). The obtained scale is used in the
event scoring phase to assess the workload associated with performed task.
Mental workload assessment with SWAT was extensively tested in diverse environments, including
military flight scenarios and commercial air travel (Nataupsky and Abbott, 1987; Battiste and Bortolussi,
1988), nuclear plant simulations (Beare and Dorris, 1984), military tank simulators (Whitaker, Peters
and Garinther, 1989), different systems of air defense (Bittner et al., 1989), and remote control vehicles
(Byers et al., 1988). SWAT demonstrated sensitivity to variations in mental workload during a variety of
tasks, including visual display monitoring, memory tasks, and manual control (Rubio, et al., 2004). It was
also found that the three SWAT rating scales are differently sensitive to the tasks demands. Therefore,
it was suggested that the individual scales have differential diagnosticity in assessing workload, and
individual scale information should be retained and separately examined as workload components
(Moroney et al., 1995).
estimates of mental workload. Validation research showed that the MCH scale reflects differences in both
performance and workload, and is sensitive to variations in controls, displays, and aircraft stability
(Rehman, 1995). The MCH was successfully applied to workload evaluation in many flight simulation
experiments (Casali and Wierwille, 1983, 1984; Wierwille et al., 1985; Skipper et al., 1986). The MCH
scale was able to discriminate between the low, moderate, and high communication loads and mental
loads in such tasks as different hazard detection conditions were also observed (Casali and Wierwille,
1984) and navigation dilemmas (Wierwille et al., 1985). Significant increase of MSH ratings with
increased danger conditions (Casali and Wierwille, 1984). The applications of the MCH scale in such
environments as remotely piloted vehicle system (Byers et al., 1988) and generic air defense system
(Bittner et al., 1989) confirmed sensitivity of the scale. It was concluded that the MCH scale provides
consistent and sensitive ratings of workload across a range of tasks (Wierwille et al., 1985; Skipper
et al., 1996). However, some studies showed that the MCH is less sensitive than the NASA TLX or the
overall workload scale (Hill et al., 1992).
dimensions described in multiple resource model of Wickens (Rubio et al., 2004). The workload dimen-
sions represent demands that can be imposed by a task, including perceptual/central processing,
response selection and execution, spatial processing, verbal processing, visual processing, auditory
processing, manual output, and speech output. This instrument aims to combine the advantages of sec-
ondary task performance based procedures (high diagnosticity) and subjective techniques (high subject
acceptability and low implementation requirements and intrusiveness) (Rubio et al., 2004). During
workload assessment with WP, the subjects are asked to provide the proportion of attentional resources
used after experiencing all of the tasks to be evaluated. Subjects are provided with the definition of each
dimension at the time of their rating. Each dimension is rated with the number between 0 and 1 to reflect
the proportion of attentional resources used in each task. A rating 0 means that tasks placed no demands
on the dimension rated, and 1 means the maximum attentional demands (Tsang and Velazquez, 1996).
The WP procedure performance was investigated by two studies (Tsang and Velazquez, 1996; Rubio
et al., 2004). Tsang and Velazquez (1996) established instrument reliability with test –retest method
and concurrent validity in reference to task performance. Both studies showed that WP ratings are
sensitive to the task demand manipulations. However, the properties of WP demand more detailed
and extensive research (Tsang and Velazquez, 1996; Rubio et al., 2004).
Output demand is the load associated with the responses required by the task. Time pressure is the load
associated with the speed at which tasks must be performed. The DRAWS tool requires people to provide
ratings for each description of demand on a scale of 0 –100, where 0 means no demand and 100 the
maximum demand (HIFAdata, 2002).
37.4.5.4 Rating Scale Mental Effort
The Rating Scale Mental Effort (RSME) is a unidimensional rating scale. This scale ranges from 0 to 150
and has nine descriptive indicators along its axis (e.g., “not effortful” and “awfully effortful”). Validation
of this technique ensured that the meanings of the verbal labels are the same for different people (Verwey
and Veltman, 1996).
37.4.5.5 Analytical Hierarchy Process
The Analytical Hierarchy Process (AHP) (Saaty, 1980) is a relative, retrospective, and redundant tech-
nique of mental workload assessment. The AHP procedure based on the Gopher’s psychophysical
scaling approach and uses relative judgments for workload assessment (Vidulich et al., 1991).
However, there is no single reference task, each task is compared with all other tasks. It is also a fully
retrospective technique, where all comparisons are made after the rater completed all tasks. A
17-point rating scale is used to evaluate all possible pairs of tasks comparisons. The scores are inputs,
the in-judgment matrix, in which each row or column represents the workload dominance of one
task relative to all of the other. Research showed that AHP ratings were sensitive to changes in difficulty
among 10 procedural elements performed during in-flight aircraft testing (Vidulich et al., 1991).
37.4.5.6 Subjective Workload Dominance Technique
The Subjective Workload Dominance (SWORD) technique uses a series of relative judgments comparing
the workload of different task conditions (Vidulich et al., 1991). The SWORD technique is an implemen-
tation of the AHP approach designed specifically for subjective workload assessment. There are three
required steps: (1) a rating scale listing all possible pairwise comparisons of the tasks performed must
be completed, (2) a judgment matrix comparing each task to every other task must be filled in with
each subject’s evaluation, and (3) ratings must be calculated using a geometric means approach. Vidulich
and Tsang (1986) showed that SWORD is a sensitive and reliable workload measure.
37.4.5.7 Comparison between Different Subjective Rating Scales for Mental Workload
The most common approach applied in subjective workload assessment has been the combination of
absolute magnitude estimation with immediate presentation (Tsang and Vidulich, 1994). Two most
popular techniques — the NASA TLX and SWAT — use the absolute estimation approach, that is,
both techniques require independent assessments of each condition on abstract scale dimension. Also,
both techniques (the NASA TLX and SWAT) are designed to be used immediately after the performance
of rated task. Immediate procedure has been promoted as protection against loss of information from
short-term memory (Reid and Nygren, 1988). The raw ratings from these instruments are intended to
be based on fresh memory and free from contamination from the raters hypothesis concerning tasks
ordering. Relative judgment approaches (SWORD, AHP) to subjective mental workload assessments
are rather uncommon. Some research (Vidulich and Tsang, 1986; Tsang and Vidulich, 1994) compared
the relative-retrospective method (AHP) to the absolute-immediate evaluation method (NASA TLX). It
was found that the retrospective rating has higher test –retest reliability and sensitivity than the immedi-
ate ratings (Vidulich et al., 1991). The investigation of the relative-immediate and relative–respective
approach in SWORD technique (Tsang and Vidulich, 1994) showed that relative-immediate approach
had the lowest reliability, concurrent validity, and sensitivity.
Vidulich and Tsang (1986) compared the SWAT and the NASA-Bipolar methods ratings with respect to
performance ratings. These methods were applied to workload assessment in the tracking and spatial
transformation tasks with different levels of difficulty, different input/output configurations, and
various degree of resources competition. The evaluation of construct validity of the scales was based
on their ability to differentiate the levels of task difficulty. The concurrent validity was established by
Subjective Scales of Effort and Workload Assessment 37-13
the correlation between the subjective workload and performance ratings. Both techniques showed
similar sensitivity to different tasks manipulations and task difficulty. However, both techniques were
not able to detect resource competition effects in dual tasks performance, demands of response execution
processing, and the dynamics of the difficulty changes.
Hill et al. (1992) compared four subjective workload ratings scales: NASA TLX, OW, SWAT, and MCH
scale. These techniques were compared in reference to the following criteria: (1) sensitivity (measured by
factor validity), (2) operator acceptance, (3) resource requirements, and (4) special procedures. The
results showed that NASA TLX had the highest factors validity (the greatest correlation with the operator
workload factor), while the OW had the second highest average factor validity. The TLX was liked best by
the operators, and OW was the easiest to complete. Verwey and Veltman (1996) compared sensitivity and
diagnosticity of several workload assessment methods such as SWAT, RMSE, workload secondary task,
ratings, heart rate, and eyebinks. The workload assessment techniques were compared for the short
periods of elevated visual and mental workload during driving. The results revealed that the secondary
performance technique, SWAT, and RSME were sensitive to the visual workload peaks. Secondary per-
formance technique and RSME were also sensitive to the mental workload peaks, while SWAT was less
sensitive to the mental workload peaks.
Rubio et al. (2004) evaluated psychometric properties (sensitivity, diagnosticity, and validity) of three
instruments: the NASA TLX, SWAT, and WP. Two laboratory tasks were evaluated: Sternberg’s memory
searching tasks and tracking tasks. The sensitivity and diagnosticity of WP ratings were higher than
NASA TLX and SWAT ratings. The assessment of concurrent validity by correlation of ratings with
performance showed that the NASA TLX ratings had higher correlation than SWAT and WP.
Nygren (1991) made a theoretical analysis of the psychometric properties of NASA TLX and SWAT. He
concluded that psychometric properties of the unidimesional scale (such as Cooper – Harper or OW)
make them less sensitive to differences in workload than either TLX or SWAT, and relatively more vari-
able than TLX and SWAT scores. According to Nygren (1991), the advantage of SWAT over TLX is that it
is a psychological model of subjective judgment, which may be oversimplified, but may also reflect
cognitive mechanisms and biases that actually affect the process of mental workload judgments. The
advantage of TLX over SWAT is based on the general linear model, where six dimensions derived
from extensive multivariate analyses based on numerous studies and different workload domains.
SWAT is more relevant for empirical testing of the appropriateness of particular additive model of work-
load assessment. TLX has greater potential for solving workload problems in many applied settings, by
accurately predicting operator workload levels across a variety of tasks (Nygren, 1991). The results of
numerous validation studies indicate that MCH, Bedford scale, SWAT, and TLX procedures represent
globally sensitive measures of operator workload (Wierwille and Eggemeier, 1993). Since, both SWAT
and TLX are multidimensional scale and they can therefore provide some diagnostic information
concerning causes of workload represented by the subscales (Moroney et al., 1995).
37.5 Summary
Several authors concluded that subjective ratings scales are among the simplest and most efficient of
workload estimation instruments that can be used for ergonomics applications (Skipper et al. 1986;
Nygren, 1991; Wierwille and Eggemeier, 1993; Tsang, 2001). These scales are the most sensitive, most
transferable, and least intrusive techniques for workload estimation, are easy to administer, and
require little effort or no equipment. Several scales demonstrated global sensitivity and thus can
provide appropriate workload indicators in test and evaluation situations. Some authors stated that sub-
jective rating scales constitute the most relevant method of mental workload assessment, since” subjective
scaling is the most direct measure of such subjective experience” (Sheridan, 1980). However, the subjec-
tive rating scales have also some serious disadvantages. The source of the resource demands is hard to
introspectively diagnose within a dimensional framework. Subjects may not be able to distinguish
mental demands from other type of demands such as physical or manipulative (O’Donnell and
37-14 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Eggemeier, 1986). Additionally, the levels of fatigue and emotional states can have significant effect on the
workload ratings. Furthermore, operators who provide ratings may have been adapted to particular
system and learned to compensate for its deficiencies, thus the rating will not reflect properly the work-
load imposed by a task or a system (Skipper et al., 1986). Finally, all subjective methods are prone to
biases due to central tendency, halo, and leniency effects. Careful instruction and properly performed
measurement procedures can minimize the possibility of bias occurrence and ensure accurate ratings.
References
Annett, J. 2002. Subjective rating scales: science or art. Ergonomics, 45, 966–987.
Battiste, V. and Bortolussi, M. 1988. Transport pilot workload: a comparison of two objective techniques.
In Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 32nd Annual Meeting, 150– 154.
Beare, A. and Dorris, R. 1984. The effects of supervisor experience and the presence of a shift technical
advisor on the performance of two-man crews in a nuclear power plant simulator. In Proceedings of
the Human Factors Society, 28th Annual Meeting, 242–246. Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors
Society.
Bittner, A.V., Byers, J.C., Hill, S.G., Zaklad, A.L., and Christ, R.E. 1989. Generic workload ratings of a
mobile air defense system (LOS-F-H). In Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Human
Factors Society, 1476 –1480. Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.
Bonney, R., Weisman, G., Haugh, L.D., and Finkelstein, J. 1990. Assessment of postural discomfort. In
Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 34th Annual Meeting, 684–687. Orlando, FL, 2 –5
October.
Borg, G.A. 1982. Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med Sci Sports Exerc, 14, 377 –381.
Borg, G. 1985. An Introduction to Borg’s RPE-scale. Ithaca, NY: Mouvement.
Borg, G. 2001. Rating scales for perceived physical effort and exertion. In International Encyclopedia of
Ergonomics and Human Factors, Karwowski, W. (ed.), 538 –542. New York: Taylor & Francis.
Borg, G., Hassmen, P., and Lagerstrom, M. 1987. Perceived exertion related to heart rate and blood lactate
during arm and leg exercise. Eur J App Physiol, 65, 679 –685.
Borg, E. and Borg, G. 2002. A comparison of AME and CR100 for scaling perceived exertion.
Acta Psychol, 109, 157– 175.
Boussenna, M., Corlett, E.N., and Pheasant, S.T. 1982. The relation between discomfort and postural
loading at the joints. Ergonomics, 25, 315 –322.
Byers, J.C., Bittner, A.C., Hill, S.G., Zaklad, A.L., and Christ, R.E. 1988. Workload assessment of a remo-
tely piloted vehicle (RPV) system. In Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 32nd Annual
Meeting, 1145–1149. Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.
Casali, J.G. and Wierwille, W.W. 1983. A comparison of rating scale, secondary task, physiological, and
primary task workload estimation techniques in a simulated flight emphasizing communications
load. Human Factors, 25(6), 623–641.
Casali, J.G. and Wierwille, W.W. 1984. On the measurement of pilot perceptual workload: a comparison
of assessment techniques addressing sensitivity and intrusion issues. Ergonomics, 27, 1033–1050.
Chen, M.J., Fan, X., and Moe, S.T. 2002. Criterion-related validity of the Borg ratings of perceived
exertion scale in healthy individuals: a meta-analysis. J Sports Sci, 20, 873– 899.
Cooper, G.E. and Harper, R.P. 1969. The use of pilot ratings in the evaluation of aircraft handling qual-
ities. NASA Ames Technical Report NASA TN-D-5153. Moffett Field, CA: NASA Ames Research
Center.
Corlett, E.N. and Bishop, R.P. 1976. A technique for measuring postural discomfort. Ergonomics, 9,
175– 182.
Corlett, E.N. 1995. The evaluation of posture and its effects. In Wilson J.R. and Corlett E.N. (eds),
662– 713, London: Taylor and Francis.
Coury, B.G. and Drury, C.G. 1982. Optimum handle positions in a box-holding task. Ergonomics, 25,
645– 662.
Subjective Scales of Effort and Workload Assessment 37-15
Dickinson, C.E., Campion, K., Foster, A.F., Newman, S.J., O’Rourke, A.M.T., and Thomas, P.G. 1992.
Questionnaire development: an examination of the Nordic Musculoskeletal Questionnaire. Appl
Ergon, 23, 197–201.
Drury, C.G., Deeb, J.M., Hartman, B., Woolley, S., Drury, C.E., and Gallagher, S. 1989. Symmetric and
asymmetric manual materials handling. Part 1: Physiology and psychophysics. Ergonomics, 32,
467–489.
Eggemeier, F.T., Wilson, G.F., Kramer, A.F., and Damos, D.L. 1991. General considerations concerning
workload assessment in multi-task environments. In Multiple Task Performance Damos, D.L.
(ed.), 207 –216. London: Taylor & Francis.
Eggemeier, F.T. and Wilson, G.F. 1991. Performance-based and subjective assessment of workload in
multi-task environments. In Multiple Task Performance, Damos, D.L. (ed.), 217 –278. Washington,
DC: Taylor & Francis.
Fleishman, E.A., Gebhardt, D.L., and Hogan, J.C. 1984. The measurement of effort. Ergonomics, 27(9),
947–954.
Genaidy, A.M., Karwowski, W., Christensen, D.M., Vogiatzis, C., Deraiseh, N., and Prins, A. 1998. What
is ‘heavy’? Ergonomics, 41(4), 420–432.
Hampson, D.B., Gibson, A.S.C., Lambert, M.I., and Noakes, T.D. 2001. The Influence of sensory cues on
the perception of exertion during exercise and central regulation of exercise performance. Sports
Med, 31, 935–952.
Hart, S.G. and Staveland, L.E. 1988. Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): results of empirical
and theoretical research. In Human Mental Workload, Hancock P.A. and Meshkati, N. (eds.),
139–183. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.
HIFAdata. 2002. Human Factor Integration in Future database website. http://www.eurocontrol.int/
eatmp/hifa/hifa/HIFAdata.html.
Hill, S.G., Byers, J.C., Zaklad, A.L., and Christ, R.E. 1989. Subjective workload assessment during 48
continuous hours of LOS-F-H operations. In Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 33rd
Annual Meeting, 1129–1133. Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.
Hill, S.G., Iavecchia, H.P., Byers, J.C., Bittner, A.C., Zaklad, A.L., and Christ, R.E. 1992. Comparison of
four subjective workload rating scales. Human Factors, 34, 429– 439.
Hogan, J.C. and Fleishman, E.A. 1979. An index of the physical effort required in human task
performance. J Appl Psychol 64, 197– 204.
Hogan, J.C., Ogden, G., and Fleishman, E.A. 1980. Reliability and validity of methods for evaluating
perceived physical effort. J Appl Psychol 65, 672–679.
ISO/FDIS 10075-3, 2002, Ergonomic principles related to mental workload — Part 3: Principles and
requirements concerning methods for measuring and assessing mental workload. International
Standardization Organization.
Johansson, S.-E. and Borg, G. 1993. Perception of heavy work operations by tank truck drivers. Appl
Ergon, 24, 421–426.
Jung, E.S. and Choe, J. 1996. Human reach posture prediction based on psychophysical discomfort. In J
Ind Ergon, 18, 173– 179.
Kroemer, K.H.E., Kroemer, H.B., and Kroemer-Elbert, K.E. 1994. Ergonomics: How to Design for Ease and
Efficiency. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Kumar, S., Narayan, Y., and Bjornsdottir, S. 1999. Comparison of the sensitivity of three psychophysical
techniques to three manual materials handling task variables. Ergonomics, 42, 61–73.
Kuorinka, I., Jonsson, B., Kilbom, A., Vinterberg, H., Biering-Sorensen, F., Andersson, G., and
Jorgensen, K. 1987. Standardized Nordic Questionnaires for the analysis of musculoskeletal
symptoms. Appl Ergon, 18, 233–237.
Legg, S.J., Perko, L., and Campbell, P. 1997. Subjective perceptual methods for comparing backpacks.
Ergonomics, 40, 809 –817.
Liao, M.-H. and Drury, C.G. 2000. Posture, discomfort and performance on a VDT task. Ergonomics, 43,
345–359.
37-16 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Meshkati, N., Hancock, P., and Rahimi, M. 1992. Techniques in mental workload assessment. In Evalu-
ation of Human Work. A Practical Ergonomics Methodology, Wilson, J. and Corlett, E. (eds.),
605– 627. London: Taylor and Francis.
Moray, N. 1982. Subjective mental workload. Human Factors, 24, 25 –40.
Morgan, W.P. 1994. Psychological components of effort sense. Med Sci Sports Exerc, 26, 1071–1077.
Moroney, W.F., Biers, D.W., and Eggemeier, F.T. 1995. Some measurement and methodological consider-
ations in the application of subjective workload measurement techniques. Int J Aviat Psychol, 5,
87– 106.
Nataupsky, M. and Abbott, T.S. 1987. Comparison of workload measures on computer-generated
primary flight displays. In Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 31st Annual Meeting,
548– 552. Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.
Noble, B.J. and Robertson, R.J., 1996. Perceived Exertion. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Nygren, T.E.
1991. Psychometric properties of subjective workload measurement techniques: implications for
their use in the assessment of perceived mental workload. Human Factors, 33, 17–33.
O’Donnell, R.D. and Eggemeier, F.T. 1986. Workload assessment methodology. In Boff, K.R., Kaufman,
L. and Thomas, J.P. (eds.). Handbook of perception and human performance, Volume II: Cognitive
Processes and Performance, 42/1 – 42/49. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Olendorf, M.R. and Drury, C.G. 2001. Postural discomfort and perceived exertion in standardized
box-holding postures. Ergonomics, 44, 1341 –1367.
Pandolf, K.B. 1983. Advances in the study and application of perceived exertion. Exerc Sport Sci Rev, 11,
118– 158.
Reid, G.B. and Nygren, T.E. 1988. The subjective workload assessment technique: a scaling procedure for
measuring mental workload. In Hancock, P.A. and Meshkati, N. (eds.), Human Mental Workload,
185– 218. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.
Rehman, A.J. 1995. Handbook of Human Performance Measures and Crew Requirements for Flightdeck
Research. Technical report, DOT/FAA/CT-TN95/49.
Rubio, S., Diaz, E., Martin, J., and Puente, J.M. 2004. Evaluation of subjective mental workload: a
comparison of SWAT, NASA-TLX, and Workload Profile methods. Appl Psychol Int Rev, 53, 61 –86.
Russell, W.D. 1997. On the current status of rated perceived exertion. Percept Motor Skills, 84, 799–808.
Saaty, T.L. 1980. Multicriteria Decision Making: The Analytic Hierarchy Process. New York: McGraw Hill
Company.
Shephard, R.J. 1994. Perception of effort in the assessment of work capacity and the regulation of the
intensity of effort. Int J Ind Ergon, 13, 67 –80.
Sheridan, T.B. 1980. Mental workload, what is it? Why bother with it? Human Factor Soc Bull, 23, 1 –2.
Shively, R., Battiste, V., Matsumoto, J., Pepiton, D., Bortolussi, M., and Hart, S.G. (1987). In-flight evalu-
ation of pilot workload measures for rotorcraft research. In Proceedings of the Fourth Symposium on
Aviation Psychology, 637– 643. Columbus, OH: Department of Aviation, The Ohio State University.
Sinclair, M.A. 1995. Subjective assessment. In Wilson, J.R. and Corlett, E.N. (eds.), Evaluation of Human
Work, 69–100. London: Taylor & Francis.
Skinner, J.S., Hustler, R., Bergsteinova, V., and Buskirk, E.R. 1973. The validity and reliability of a rating
scale of perceived exertion. Med Sci Sports, 5, 94 –96.
Skipper, J.H., Rieger, C.A., and Wierwille, W.W. 1986. Evaluation of decision tree rating scales for mental
workload estimation. Ergonomics, 29, 585–599.
Stevens, S.S. (1974). perceptual magnitude and its measurement. In Handbook of Perception: Psychophysi-
cal Judgment and Measurement, Carterette C. and Friedman, M.P. (eds.), 361 –389. New York:
Academic Press.
Tsang, P.S. and Johnson, W.W. 1989. Cognitive demands in automation. Aviat Space Environ Med, 60,
130– 135.
Tsang, P.S. and Velazquez, V.L. 1996. Diagnosticity and multidimensional subjective workload ratings.
Ergonomics, 39, 358–381.
Subjective Scales of Effort and Workload Assessment 37-17
Tsang, P.S. and Vidulich, M.A. 1994. The roles of immediacy and redundancy in relative subjective
workload assessment. Human Factors, 36, 503 –513.
Tsang, P.S. and Wilson, G.F. 1997. Mental workload. In Handbook of Human Factors and Ergonomics,
Salvendy, G. (ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Tsang, P.S., 2001. Mental workload. In International Encyclopedia of Ergonomics and Human Factors,
Karwowski, W. (ed.). New York, Taylor & Francis.
Verwey, W.B. and Veltman, H.A. 1996. Detecting short periods of elevated workload: a comparison of
nine workload assessment techniques. J Exp Psychol Appl, 2, 270 –285.
Vidulich, M.A. and Tsang, P.S. 1986. Techniques of subjective workload assessment: a comparison of
SWAT and the NASA-Bipolar methods. Ergonomics, 29(11), 1385 –1398.
Vidulich, M.A., Ward, G.F., and Schueren, J. 1991. Using subjective Workload Dominance (SWORD)
technique for projective workload assessment. Human Factors, 33, 677 –692.
Whitaker, L., Peters, L., and Garinther, G. 1989. Tank crew performance: effects of speech intelligibility in
target acquisition and subjective workload assessment. In Proceedings of the Human Factors Society
33rd Annual Meeting, 1411– 1413. Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.
Wierwille, W. W. and Eggemeier, F.T. 1993. Recommendations for mental workload measurement in a
test and evaluation environment. Human Factors, 35, 263 –281.
Wierwille, W. W., Rahimi, M., and Casali, J.G. 1985. Evaluation of 16 measures of mental workload using
a simulated flight task emphasizing mediational workload. Human Factors, 21, 499 –502.
Wierwille, W.W. and Casali, J.G. 1983. A validated rating scale for global mental workload measurement
applications. In Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 27th Annual Meeting, 129 –133. Santa
Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.
Zhuang, Z., Stobbe, T.J., Collins, J.W., Hsiao, H., and Hobbs, G.R. 2000. Psychophysical assessment of
assistive devices for transferring patients/residents. Appl Ergon, 31, 35 –44.
38
Rest Allowances
38.1 Introduction
The chapter is divided into two major parts. The first part (Section 38.2 to Section 38.6) gives numerical
allowances for time standards for various tasks from the International Labor Organization (ILO).
Although the ILO values are logical, the values are subject to many questions.
The second part (Section 38.7 and Section 38.8) gives general guidelines for fatigue.
That is, the time study technician recorded the time the worker took to do the job (observed time) and
multiplied the observed time by a pace rating to determine the normal time of an experienced operator to
do the job. Standard data tables from systems such as MTM are normal time.
This chapter is a concise version of material from Konz and Johnson (2004).
38-1
38-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
However, it was recognized that, in addition to normal time, additional time was needed for “allow-
ances” — typically divided into machine allowances, personal allowances, delay allowances, and fatigue
allowances. Some firms express the allowances as a percentage of shift time and some as a percentage of
work time.
For shift time:
Normal time
Standard time ¼
1 Allowances
For work time:
The use of the ILO values is complex. Remembering that fatigue allowances are given only for work
time (not machine time), the applicable fatigue allowance points are to be totaled. Then, using
Table 38.1, the points are converted to percentage of time. A large fatigue allowance is an indication
that there is a large potential for improved ergonomics.
Fatigue allowances will be divided into physical, mental, and environmental.
TABLE 38.1 Conversion from Points Allowance to Percentage Allowance for ILO
Points
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
0 10 10 10 10 10 10 10 11 11 11
10 11 11 11 11 11 12 12 12 12 12
20 13 13 13 13 14 14 14 14 15 15
30 15 16 16 16 17 17 17 18 18 18
40 19 19 20 20 21 21 22 22 23 23
50 24 24 25 26 26 27 27 28 28 29
60 30 30 31 32 32 33 34 34 35 36
70 37 37 38 39 40 40 41 42 43 44
80 45 46 47 48 48 49 50 51 52 53
90 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63
100 64 65 66 68 69 70 71 72 73 74
110 75 77 78 79 80 82 83 84 85 87
120 88 89 91 92 93 95 96 97 99 100
130 101 103 105 106 107 109 110 112 113 115
140 116 118 119 121 122 123 125 126 128 130
Note: The second column (0) gives the 10 s, and the remaining columns give the units. Thus, 30 points (0 column) ¼ 15%;
31 points (1 column) ¼ 16%; 34 points ¼ 17%.
The percentage allowance is for manual work time (not machine time) and includes 5% personal time for coffee breaks.
Note that 0 points gives 10% allowance. In addition, at “low” points (say 0 to 20), it takes about six points to get an additional
1% allowance while at “high” points (say 100), one point gives about an additional 1% allowance.
Source: International Labor Office. 1992. Introduction to Work Study, 4th ed. Geneva, Switzerland: ILO. With permission.
38-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Note: Push includes foot pedal push and carry on the back. Arm carry includes
hand carry and swinging arm movements. Weight is averaged over time. A 15-kg
load lifted for 33% of a cycle is 5 kg.
Source: International Labor Office. 1992. Introduction to Work Study, 4th ed.
Geneva, Switzerland: ILO. With permission.
a load, and various major body movements such as climbing a ladder, lifting, and shoveling. For
an ergonomics analysis of the effects of posture, see the Rapid Upper Limb Assessment (RULA)
technique in Chapter 42.
3. Short cycle. Table 38.4 gives short-cycle allowances to permit muscles some time to recover from
highly repetitive movements. Since it is for cycle times of 3 to 10 sec, this really involves only
hand– arm movements and probably, even more narrowly, hand–finger movements. In a
factory, this would be “highly repetitive bench assembly.”
4. Restrictive clothing. Table 38.5 gives allowances due to clothing restrictions.
Source: International Labor Office. 1992. Introduction to Work Study, 4th ed.
Geneva, Switzerland: ILO. With permission.
Rest Allowances 38-5
1. Climate. Table 38.8 gives allowances for climate, subdivided into temperature/humidity, wet, and
ventilation. For more on temperature/humidity, see Konz and Johnson (2004)
2. Dirt, dust, and fumes. Table 38.9 gives allowances for dirt, dust, and fumes
3. Noise and vibration. Table 38.10 gives allowances for noise and vibration. For more on noise and
vibration, see Chapter 31 and Chapter 33
4. Eye strain. Table 38.11 gives allowance for eye strain. For more on eyes, vision, and illumination,
see Konz and Johnson (2004)
Notes: ILO (1992) considers clothing effects on dexterity as well as clothing weight
in relation to effort and movement. Also consider whether the clothing affects vision or
breathing.
Source: International Labor Office. 1992. Introduction to Work Study, 4th ed. Geneva,
Switzerland: ILO. With permission.
38-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Note: ILO (1992) considers what would happen if the operator were to relax attention,
responsibility, need for exact timing, and accuracy or precision required.
Source: International Labor Office. 1992. Introduction to Work Study, 4th ed. Geneva,
Switzerland: ILO. With permission.
Note: ILO (1972) considers the degree of mental stimulation and if there is
companionship, competitive spirit, music, and so on.
Source: International Labor Office. 1992. Introduction to Work Study, 4th ed.
Geneva, Switzerland: ILO. With permission.
Rest Allowances 38-7
Note: ILO (1992) considers temperature/humidity, wet, and ventilation. For temperature/humidity, use the average
environmental temperature. For wet, consider the cumulative effect over a long period. For ventilation, consider quality/
freshness of air and its circulation by air conditioning or natural movement.
Source: International Labor Office. 1992. Introduction to Work Study, 4th ed. Geneva, Switzerland: ILO. With permission.
allowances were developed during a time in which Europeans worked longer hours per year than at
present; presently they work 1500 to 1600 h/yr.
Finally, the ILO allowances consider only the duration of the rest and not what happens during the
rest. Each minute of rest is considered equally valuable.
The following will give some general guidelines to the temporal aspects of fatigue — especially from
Chapter 21, Temporal Ergonomics, of Konz and Johnson (2004).
Fumes
0 Lathe turning with coolants
1 Emulsion paint, gas cutting, soldering with resin
5 Motor vehicle exhaust in small commercial garage
6 Cellulose painting
10 Molder procuring metal and filling mold
Notes: For dust, consider both volume and nature of the dust. The dirt
allowance covers “washing time” where this is paid for (e.g., 3 min for
washing). Do not allow both time and points. For fumes, consider the nature
and concentration, odor, whether toxic or injurious to the health, irritating
to the eyes, nose, and throat.
Source: International Labor Office. 1992. Introduction to Work Study, 4th ed.
Geneva, Switzerland: ILO. With permission.
Finkleman (1994) analyzed 3700 people who had reported fatigue in their work. Physically demanding
jobs had less fatigue than jobs with low physical demand! Significant predictors of fatigue included job
pay, job control, and supervisor quality — emphasizing the importance of lack of motivation.
Fatigue generally can be overcome by rest (recovery). Resting time can be “off work” (evenings, week-
ends, holidays, vacations) and “at work.” “At work” is further divided into “formal breaks” (lunch, coffee)
and “informal breaks” (work interruptions, training), microbreaks (short breaks of a minute or less), and
“working rest” (doing a different task, using a different part of the body, such as answering the phone
instead of keying data).
The following are given as axioms:
. Most jobs have peaks and valleys of demand within the shift — that is, the “load” is not constant
. Fatigue increases exponentially with time
. Rest is more beneficial if it occurs before the muscle (cardiovascular system, brain) has “too much”
fatigue
. The value of a rest declines exponentially with time
. Different parts of the body have different recovery rates
The “body” will be divided into three parts: (1) cardiovascular system, (2) musculoskeletal system, and
(3) the brain.
Rest Allowances 38-9
Note: For noise, ILO (1992) considers whether the noise affects communication, is a steady
hum or a background noise, is regular or occurs unexpectedly, and is irritating or soothing. For
vibration, consider the impact of vibration on the body, limbs, or hands and the addition to
mental effort as a result, or to a series of jars and shocks.
Source: International Labor Office. 1992. Introduction to Work Study, 4th ed. Geneva, Swit-
zerland: ILO. With permission.
Notes: ILO (1992) considers the lighting conditions, glare, flicker, illumina-
tion, color, and closeness of work and for how long the strain is endured.
Source: International Labor Office. 1992. Introduction to Work Study, 4th ed.
Geneva, Switzerland: ILO. With permission.
38-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
38.7.3 Brain
Two aspects involving the brain are optimum stimulation and concentration and attention.
38.7.3.1 Optimum Stimulation
Many people have pointed out that fatigue is not just physiological but has a strong psychological (lack of
motivation) aspect (Finkleman, 1994). Finkleman concluded that an important predictor of fatigue was
processing either too much information (overload, and thus fatigue) or too little information (under-
load, and thus boredom).
38.7.3.2 Concentration and Attention
There are some sedentary jobs with mental activity; three examples are simultaneous translation,
gambling, and education. Two examples of mental underload are monitoring/vigilance and controlling
a vehicle at night.
Simultaneous translation usually is done by pairs of translators — one translates while the other rests.
In Quebec (French/English), they switch every 30 min; in Japan (Japanese/English), they switch every
20 min. Sign language translators for the hearing-impaired switch every 20 min. In casinos, blackjack
dealers (standing work with intense concentration and finger activity) work for 60 min and then have
a 20 min nonworking rest. The typical schedule at universities in the United States is 50 min of
lecture with a 10 min break before the next class. However, the typical student schedule is 15 class
hours per week, so there is considerable recovery time between classes. Professors tend to have 6 to 12
Rest Allowances 38-11
teaching “hours” (each of 50 min) each week; typically they teach for 32 weeks/yr. U.S. high school stu-
dents typically have six to seven periods/day; each period is 60 min, which includes a 5 min break. Often
one period is physical education; lunch is an additional break. Typically they go to school ,190 days/yr.
Example underload tasks (monitoring/vigilance) are process control monitoring, hospital monitor-
ing, and industrial inspection. The Accreditation Council for Graduate Medical Education, in an
effort to reduce medical errors, set new standards for doctors-in-training. They are now limited to
80 h workweeks and they must get at least 10 h of rest between shifts. They are not be to be on duty
for more than 24 h continuously. Craig (1985) summarized some vigilance studies. Changes that
reduced boredom were beneficial; one example was loading/unloading (10 min) as well as inspecting
coins (14 min). Another example was 30 min of inspection followed by 60 min of other tasks.
Driving/piloting at night is an underload problem. Buck and Lamonde (1993) reported locomotive
drivers had sleep problems — especially close to 0300 and 1500 h. Fatigue may be more of a problem
when the driver is “externally scheduled” — for example, when tired, a bus driver or a pilot cannot
stop and take a break, whereas a car or truck driver, being self-paced, can stop. Japanese taxi drivers typi-
cally work a 16 h day and then have the following 1 or 2 days off.
38.7.4.1 Sleep
Sleep restores the functions of the brain. Only sleep allows some form of cerebral shutdown. Females and
people over 50 yr are more prone to night-sleep problems. Sleep deprivation of 24 to 48 h primarily
affects motivation to perform rather than ability to perform; thus uninteresting, undemanding,
simple tasks are the most affected.
38.7.4.3 Countermeasures
Three sleep-deprivation countermeasures are rest, drugs/food, and environmental stimulation.
Naps are a potential supplement to sleep at night. However, people tend to think of naps as inadequate
opportunity to sleep rather than an opportunity for partial recuperation. A nap problem is sleep
inertia — poor performance for about 15 to 30 min after being awakened.
The most common drug to decrease sleepiness/increase alertness is caffeine. Amphetamines also work
well. Caffeine has a metabolic half-life of 3 to 7 h. Pregnancy increases half-life (to as much as 18 h) while
smoking decreases caffeine’s half-life. A pharmacologically active dose, depending on the individual, is
about 3 mg/kg of body weight. Truck drivers often recommend eating — possibly while still driving.
Eat a moderate intake of carbohydrates rather than fats or proteins. Another possibility is having a
drink with caffeine.
Environmental stimulation can be exposure to bright light such as daylight. (Normal indoor lighting is
too dim to stimulate.) In a vehicle, modify the ventilation or decrease isolation by listening to a radio/CD
or talking on a cell phone. Physical activity (say walking 100 m) can be useful.
Source: Konz, S. and Johnson, S. 2004. Work Design: Occupational Ergonomics, 6th ed. Scottsdale, AZ: Holcomb-Hathaway.
With permission.
Rest Allowances 38-13
38.8.3.1 Intensity
Good ergonomics practice reduces high stress on the individual. For example, use machines and devices
to reduce hold-and-carry activities. Static work is especially stressful, as specific muscles are activated
continuously and the alternation of muscles that occurs in dynamic work is not present.
cell layout and having the operator do the material handling for the workstation (obtaining supplies or
disposal of finished components).
References
Ahsberg, E. and Gamberale, F. 1998. Perceived fatigue during physical work: an experimental evaluation
of a fatigue inventory. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 21: 177–131.
Bhatia, N. and Murrell, K. 1969. An industrial experiment in organized rest pauses. Human Factors,
11 (2): 167 –174.
Bigland-Ritchie, B., Furbach, F., and Woods, J. 1986. Fatigue of intermittent submaximal voluntary con-
tractions: central and peripheral factors. Journal of Applied Physiology, 61 (2): 421 –429.
Buck, L. and Lamonde, F. 1993. Critical incidents and fatigue among locomotive engineers. Safety
Science, 16: 1 –18.
Craig, A. 1985. Field studies of human inspection: the application of vigilance research. In Hours of
Work, Folkard, S. and Monk, T. Eds., John Wiley & Sons: New York.
Finkleman, J. 1994. A large database study of the factors associated with work-induced fatigue. Human
Factors, 36 (2): 232 –243.
International Labor Office. 1992. Introduction to Work Study, 4th ed. Geneva, Switzerland: ILO.
Kadefors, R. and Laubli, T. 2002. Muscular disorders in computer users: an introduction. International
Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 30: 203 –210.
Konz, S. and Johnson, S. 2004. Work Design: Occupational Ergonomics, 6th ed. Scottsdale, AZ: Holcomb-
Hathaway.
Mital, A. 1984a. Comprehensive maximum acceptable weights of lift database for regular 8-hour work
shifts. Ergonomics, 27: 1127–1138.
Mital, A. 1984b. Maximum weights of lift acceptable to male and female industrial workers for extended
work shifts. Ergonomics, 27: 1115 –1126.
Mital, A., Hamid, F., and Brown, M. 1994. Physical fatigue in high and very high frequency manual
materials handling: perceived exertion and physiological factors. Human Factors, 36 (2): 219–231.
Mital, A., Nicholson, A., and Ayoub, M.M. 1993. A Guide to Manual Material Handling. London:
Taylor & Francis.
Further Reading
Konz, S. and Johnson, S. 2004. Work Design: Occupational Ergonomics, 6th ed. Scottsdale, AZ: Holcomb-
Hathaway. This popular textbook has an extensive coverage of fatigue and allowances as well as design
guidelines for occupational ergonomics.
39
Wrist Posture in
Office Work
39-1
39-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
of work-related CTS reported in 1994 were attributed to repetitive typing or key entry of data (Szabo,
1998). The loss in productivity occurs before (less typing speed), during, and after (days of hospitali-
zation) the treatment of CTS (Moore, 1992).
The electrogoniometer was adjusted across the wrist with the goniometer’s arms aligned to the long
axes of the hand and the lower arm. For radial and ulnar deviation assessment, the electrogoniometer’s
fulcrum was centered over the middle of the dorsal aspect of the wrist over the capitate. The proximal
arm was aligned with the dorsal midline of the forearm, using the lateral epicondyle of the humerus
for reference and the distal arm was aligned with the dorsal midline of the third metacarpal bone. For
flexion and extension measurements, the fulcrum of the electrogoniometer was centered over the
radial aspect of the wrist (trapezium level) with the proximal arm aligned with the medial side of the
radius and the distal arm aligned with the midline of the second metacarpal bone. The device was
adhered using Velcro closures. For EMG recording, after forearm preparation, the EMG electrodes
were applied 5 –7 cm distal to the line connecting the medial epicondyle and biceps tendon for flexor
carpi radialis (FCR), above the shaft of ulna in the middle of forearm for extensor carpi ulnaris
(ECU), at 2– 3 cm volar to ulna at the junction of the upper and middle thirds of the forearm
for flexor carpi ulnaris (FCU), and at 3 cm medio-distal to lateral epicondyle for extensor carpi
radialis (ECR).
Subjects were blindfolded. After completing the maximal isometric contractions (MICs), volunteers
were asked to bring the passively deviated wrist (458 flexion, 458 extension, 308 ulnar deviation, and
at the end of range of motion for radial deviation) in the subjective neutral position. The sequence
was randomized in order to avoid the carry-over effect. Each condition was repeated once (two trials).
Between conditions, a 2-min resting period was given. The forearm muscles’ EMG activity was measured
in both deviated and neutral wrist positions.
similar pattern with ECR being the most active (9.2–11.1% of MVC), followed by ECU, FCR, and FCU
with normalized average EMG values varying between 7.7 –9.3%, 6.9 –8.4%, and 4.8 –8.5% of MVC,
respectively (Figure 39.2). Hence, a drop of up to 75% in the muscle activity in the neutral zone
when compared to normalized average EMG values in wrist-deviated postures (16.5–38.3% of MVC)
was found. Although not significant, females presented higher %MVC EMG values for all muscles.
While keeping the wrist in ulnar deviation, the maximum activity was observed for ECU (26.9–35.7%
of MVC) and FCU (16.5–29.1% of MVC). ECR was the most active muscle in both wrist radial deviation
(25.5–36.8% of MVC) and extension (29.4–38.3% of MVC). FCR (19.9–26.8% of MVC) and FCU
(18.3–23.6% of MVC) were the most active muscles while the wrist was maintained in flexion. FCR
in wrist radial deviation (19.8–24.2% of MVC) and ECU in wrist extension (17.3–34.1% of MVC)
were the second most active muscles after FCU.
The proven coactivation of wrist muscles was also noted by Hoffman and Strick (1999). This coactiva-
tion included both synergists and antagonist muscles. Since wrist extensors have smaller moment arms
compared to flexors, larger forces will be required by extensors to maintain the wrist posture (Keir et al.,
1996) posing this group of muscles to elevated risk of injury while performing with in wrist flexion
posture. Passive muscle forces in antagonist muscles may further increase the risk. The deviated joints
cause muscle overstretch, thus pose a greater risk for musculoskeletal injury.
EMG activity levels between 8% and 17% of MVC were recorded for muscles acting as secondary
effectors (FCU in extension and radial deviation, FCR in extension and ulnar deviation, ECR in
flexion and ulnar deviation, and ECU in flexion and radial deviation). These levels demonstrate their
concomitant dual role in wrist stabilization and force exertion. Prolonged muscle loading promoted
fatigue. As a result, due to lack of rest, the risk of musculoskeletal injury is increased (Kumar, 2001).
Also, Drury et al. (1985) noted an important increase in EMG at extreme wrist deviations, whereas
the muscle activity for wrist angles between 58 radial deviation and 108 ulnar deviation was low and
almost constant.
ECR was the most active muscle in both radial deviation (25.5–36.8%) and extension (29.4–38.3%)
making it at risk in activities that require this wrist deviation concomitantly (e.g., use of computer
mouse). The higher prevalence of epicondylitis on the extensor side can be explained by the ECR’s
role as wrist stabilizer and primary effector in wrist extension and radial deviation. These values were
obtained in passively deviated wrist postures and any active contractions would require significantly
greater muscle activity, increasing the risk for musculoskeletal injuries even more.
Simultaneous recordings of forearm muscle activity and CTP are needed in order to see if the selected
wrist posture corresponds to the lowest values for both EMG and CTP. Although during rest, forearm
muscle activity and CTP are low, some office tasks require awkward upper extremity postures, signifi-
cantly changing the required muscle activity.
Males Females
30 30
FCR FCR
20 20
MVC %
MVC %
ECR ECR
10 10
0 0
Left Right Left Right
FIGURE 39.2 The forearm muscles normalized average EMG (% isometric MVC) for both genders in the self-
selected neutral position (FCU, flexor carpi ulnaris; FCR, flexor carpi radialis; ECR, extensor carpi radialis; ECU,
extensor carpi ulnaris).
Wrist Posture in Office Work 39-5
Training work sessions in a particular wrist position should be made after the wrist neutral position
has been determined. Owing to the fact that static load is an important factor for musculoskeletal dis-
orders development, even after the safe margins for wrist deviation are known (neutral zone), wrist pos-
itions should be alternated within its limits. Serina et al. (1999), studying the typists’ posture when using
a standard flat QWERTY keyboard, noted that typing on a keyboard in an adjusted workstation, forces
the users to spend about 75% of the working time with the wrist in greater than 158 extension and 28%
and 9% of their time with a wrist extension greater than 308 for the left and right hand, respectively.
Ergonomic assessment of hazardous postures should precede the design and introduction of alternate
keyboards. Otherwise, elevated CTS prevalence and complaints will follow.
39.4.1.2 Repetition
Extreme postures and high-repetitive actions (38–40 per minute per finger) are frequently required
during computer tasks. This value exceeds the highest acceptable frequency in a repetitive motion (fre-
quency of 30 per minute) (Bergamasco et al., 1998). Cumulative load is a risk factor for causation of mus-
culoskeletal injuries (Kumar, 1990, 2001). The adjacent tendons are sliding one against the other with the
friction force being proportional to the tension in the tendon and inversely proportional to the radius
curvature (Hadler, 1987). Serina et al. (1999) noted that velocities during typing in flexion/extension
Wrist Posture in Office Work 39-7
plane are similar to velocities measured in workers involved in industrial activities with great risk for
CTS. Nerve compression due to thickening of the flexor tendon sheaths has been proven by Yamaguchi
et al. (1965) who found greater fibrosis and edema in the tendon sheaths in CTS patients compared with
controls. Highest velocity and accelerations occurred in flexion/extension and radial/ulnar deviation
movements (Serina et al., 1999).
FIGURE 39.3 The keyboards that have been used in the experiment: (a) conventional; (b) maltron; (c) prosper
street technologies; and (d) goldtouch.
force, and performance were measured during a standardized typing task. Four different keyboards
(one conventional and three alternative) were used in this study (Figure 39.3). The conventional key-
board was a Fujitsu 105-keys traditional QWERTY layout with 58 positive slope. The alternative key-
boards were: Maltron E-Type (fixed split design, tilted keys and pads, straight vertical key columns,
central number pad, and slightly modified layout such as thumb keys for Enter, Space, and Backspace),
Goldtouch Adjustable Ergonomic Keyboard (adjustable split angle and lateral slope with lacking ball
and socket latch mechanism), and Prosper Street Technologies (PST) LLC Wave Keyboard (QWERTY
slim design with row vertical curves for longer fingers). Although the Goldtouchw keyboard lateral incli-
nation could have influenced both pronation and ulnar deviation, in order to be able to assess the impact
of split angle design on typing posture, the authors chose a fixed split angle of 258 and a 08 lateral slope.
Also, wrist motion and number of wrist repetition greater than 108 (changes in wrist movement) were
measured bilaterally using two SG 65 Biometrics Ltd electrogoniometers. The EMG forearm muscle
activity was measured using DelSys BagnolyTM EMG system. In order to record the overall applied
typing force, an AMTI force plate was placed under the keyboards.
The results of the study indicate that for all four keyboards, the wrist was ulnarly deviated and
extended for typing. Table 39.1 presents the wrist deviation angles for extension/flexion and ulnar/
radial deviation planes while typing on each of the four tested keyboards. When compared to conven-
tional keyboard, Maltron and Goldtouch keyboards significantly reduced the wrist ulnar deviation for
TABLE 39.1 Means and Standard Deviations for Average Wrist Angles on Four Different Keyboard Designs
Extension (þ)/Flexion (2) Ulnar (þ)/Radial (2) Deviation
Keyboard Left Right Left Right
Conventional 21.80 (4.89) 21.73 (6.12) 15.67 (3.63) 16.91 (4.33)
Goldtouch 25.76 (4.67) 23.56 (5.87) 0.55 (6.44) 3.62 (5.61)
Maltron 15.01 (5.55) 13.29 (5.45) 4.69 (4.11) 7.00 (4.80)
PST 21.00 (5.46) 21.78 (4.45) 14.04 (4.81) 15.58 (3.32)
Wrist Posture in Office Work 39-9
both left and right sides (p , 0.001). The PST keyboard required 98–138 more ulnar deviation than
Maltron and Goldtouch keyboards for both LUR and RUR planes ( p , 0.001). The Goldtouch keyboard
design forced 80% of subjects to type with the left or right wrist in greater than 208 extension as
compared to 70% for the conventional and PST and 30% for Maltron. Within keyboards, no significant
differences were found between sides for all four planes. In order to reduce the risk of musculoskeletal
problems at this level, wrist extension should be lowered at 58 –108. A split design QWERTY layout
with 258– 308 split angle, 08 lateral slope, and horizontal or negative slope is needed. While lateral
slope tends to decrease forearm pronation, it also reduces typing productivity and user’s acceptance.
The reduction in both ulnar deviation and shoulder external rotation due to split angle design
promote a safe forearm pronation while typing.
In terms of wrist excursions greater than 108, Maltron caused decreased wrist repetition for REF, LUR,
and RUR planes. Table 39.2 presents the repetitive values for all four keyboards tested. No significant
differences were found between the conventional, Goldtouch, and PST keyboards for all four movement
planes. For the Maltron keyboard, the key-column vertical curvature and the thumb keys for Enter, Back-
space, Delete, and other frequently used keys reduced the hand movement in the extension –flexion plane.
For the ulnar – radial deviation plane, the wrist repetitive movements over 108 were reduced by the pre-
sence of the central numeric pad, which could be used by either hand, as preferred. Also, straight vertical
key-columns reduced wrist excursions. Some of these design features should be further evaluated and, if
valid, adopted by other keyboards.
Compared with the conventional keyboard, only Maltron had a statistically significant difference in
applied force (p , 0.001). The mean typing force for participants using the conventional keyboard
was 1.91 N (SD ¼ 1.05), as compared to Maltron (M ¼ 5.84; SD ¼ 4.16). The mean applied typing
forces for Goldtouch and PST were 0.97 N (SD ¼ 0.52) and 1.28 N (SD ¼ 0.85), in that order, which
were 4.87 and 4.56 N, respectively, lower than the Maltron’s average force (p , 0.001). The overall
applied typing force recorded while using the conventional and Maltron keyboards exceeded the
ANSI/HFS recommendations (0.5–1.5 N). These results are similar to those of Rempel et al. (1994),
who using a piezoelectric load cell determined that the subject’s mean peak force ranged between 1.6
and 5.3 N. The differences in overall typing force can be explained by important variations in keyswitch
characteristics (key travel distance, over travel distance, stiffness, and keyswitch make force).
In terms of EMG muscle activity while typing, no significant differences were found between the six
recorded muscles (ECU, FCR, FCU bilaterally) for all four keyboards.
Table 39.3 presents the average values for typing speed and accuracy for each keyboard. The Maltron
keyboard was associated with significantly lower performance compared to other three keyboards for
both typing speed and error rate (p , 0.001). While the conventional and PST keyboard were statistically
similar in terms of accuracy, Goldtouch keyboard showed significantly higher error rate than the conven-
tional, with 89% level of confidence. For the Maltron keyboard, the productivity was significantly
reduced (58% decrease in typing speed and 149% increase in error rate, when compared to the conven-
tional keyboard). On the Goldtouch keyboard, subjects reached 86% and on the PST keyboard 90%, of
their typing speed on the traditional keyboard. Also, the error rate for these keyboards was statistically
identical when compared to the conventional design. The decrease in productivity of 58% for the
TABLE 39.2 Mean Values and Standard Deviations for Wrist Repetition .108 Per Minute for All
Planes and Keyboards
Movement Plane
Keyboard LEF REF LUR RUR
Conventional 51 (13) 44 (15) 9 (3) 12 (5)
Goldtouch 43 (10) 39 (14) 12 (6) 10 (5)
Maltron 26 (8) 29 (12) 7 (3) 10 (5)
PST 46 (14) 41 (12) 9 (6) 12 (4)
39-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
TABLE 39.3 Typing Speed Words Per Minute (WPM) and Accuracy (Backspace Strokes per
100 Typed Words) for Different Keyboards
Keyboard Typing speed Typing accuracy
Conventional 69.67 (19.61) 7.61 (4.09)
Goldtouch 58.92 (21.40) 11.38 (5.37)
Maltron 29.26 (8.86) 19.29 (5.88)
PST 62.37 (17.28) 8.39 (3.72)
Maltron keyboard represents an important impediment for its acceptance. Table 39.4 summarizes the
effect of alternative keyboard designs on the tested typing variables.
The current data demonstrate that after a relative short practice session typists were able to adjust their
posture, performing as well with some of the tested alternative keyboards as with the conventional key-
board. This study indicated that keyboard design had an important effect on typing in terms of musculos-
keletal diseases risk factors.
TABLE 39.4 Changes in Typing Parameters for the Tested Alternative Keyboards when Compared with the
Conventional Design
Wrist posture Wrist Applied Muscle Typing Performance
Keyboard Ulnar deviation Extension Repetition Force Activity Pm Error Rate
PST $ $ $ # $ $ $
Goldtouch # $ $ # $ $ $
Maltron # # # " $ # "
Note: $ ¼ no statistically significant difference; # ¼ statistically significant decrease; " ¼ statistically significant
increase.
Wrist Posture in Office Work 39-11
group was 11.3 errors per 100 typed words (SD ¼ 5.37), as compared to 15.3 (SD ¼ 3.14) for the
untrained subjects (p , 0.039). Training on the Maltron keyboard significantly reduced the error
rate from 26.5 (SD ¼ 7.40) to 19.2 (SD ¼ 5.88) (p , 0.007). The fact that trained participants were
able to type at 89% of their baseline typing speed when using the Goldtouch keyboard constitutes
strong evidence that with additional experience alternative keyboards could easily replace the widespread
traditional design without any loss in productivity, or perhaps even a gain. Our results differ from those
reported by Treaster and Marras (2000), who noted a decrease of only 14% when typing on the KinesisTM
keyboard (similar design to Maltron). With regard to typing performance, Smith et al. (2000) noted
values similar to those recorded for the traditional keyboard after only 2 h of training on a split angle
keyboard. Previous research indicated that the initial decline in typing productivity has been recuperated
after 2 days of training.
For the Maltron keyboard, the more ergonomic working postures found in untrained participants (less
wrist ulnar deviation and extension) when compared with the traditional design, were maintained after
the training session. Working on the Goldtouch keyboard promoted mitigated ulnar deviation in both
untrained and trained groups. Although training decreased wrist repetition (number of wrist excursions
greater than 108) with two to six repetitions per minute, the difference was not significant ( p . 0.05).
The 10% decrease in wrist repetition represents an important decrease in risk factors associated with
prolonged typing (e.g., tendon travel, tendon sheaths friction). After training, for one day of work the
decline in wrist repetition would be of 2400 movements per movement plane (5 repetitions/
min 60 min per hour 8 h of work). For one hand (both ulnar –radial deviation and flexion –
extension planes) a total of approximately 4800 unnecessary wrist movements would be avoided
through training. For the Maltron keyboard, training induced values below 30 per minute, which is
the recommended highest acceptable frequency in a repetitive motion.
In terms of applied force, training significantly reduced the overall applied force for both Goldtouch
(p , 0.022) and Maltron (p , 0.031) keyboards. The mean typing force was reduced by 58% for Gold-
touch and by 42% for the Maltron keyboard. For the Goldtouch keyboard the training session was
enough in order to reduce the typing force below the values for the conventional design (from 2.27 to
0.97 N, compared with 2.17 N recorded for the conventional one).
The decrease in overall applied force following training could be explained by hesitancy alleviation.
Working under time pressure, especially with keyboard designs totally different than the one subjects
are used to, spending more time in order to find the right keys leads to higher key stroke force (increased
finger velocities) when the key is found. Training makes devices more familiar, eliminating unnecessary
actions.
A synthesis of the effects of training on the studied variables is presented in Table 39.5.
postures. Encouraging workers to perform with wrist positions within neutral zone as it could reduce
job-associated musculoskeletal disorders risks.
Typing on alternative keyboards improves upper extremity posture, thus eliminating, or at least redu-
cing the risk factors associated with awkward posture while typing. Further, wrist repetition and overall
applied typing force are reduced without a significant effect on typing performance. When designing
alternative keyboards, one should keep in mind the trade-off between drastic design modifications
and typing performance. In any new design, a balance between new features and their effect on work per-
formance should be in the designer’s mind. Not only are ergonomic keyboards able to meet the immedi-
ate requirements such as performance, typing speed, and short training time, but they also promote safer
hand postures. Additional research is mandatory in order to see if prolonged office work on alternative
keyboards supports these findings. The current results show that with additional experience, alternative
keyboards represent a valid alternative for the conventional keyboard design.
References
Armstrong TJ, Castelli WA, Evans FG, Diaz-Perez R. 1984. Some histological changes in carpal tunnel
contents and their biomechanical implications. Journal of Occupational Medicine 26: 197– 201.
Armstrong TJ, Chaffin DB. 1979. Carpal tunnel syndrome and selected personal attributes. Journal of
Occupational Medicine 21: 481 –486.
Bergamasco R, Girola C, Colombini D. 1998. Guidelines for designing jobs featuring repetitive tasks.
Ergonomics 41: 1364–1383.
Braun RM. 1988. The dynamic diagnosis of carpal tunnel syndrome. 4th Congress of Peripheral Neuro-
pathy Association of America, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada.
Bureau of Labor Statistics. 1994. CTD News. http://ctdnews.com/bls.html.
De Krom MCTFM, Kester ADM, Knipschild PG, Spaans F. 1990. Risk factors for carpal tunnel syndrome.
American Journal of Epidemiology 132: 1102– 1110.
Drury CG, Begbie K, Ulate C, Deeb JM. 1985. Experiments on wrist deviation in manual materials hand-
ling. Ergonomics 28: 577 –589.
Dvorak A. 1943. There is a better typewriter keyboard. National Business Education Quarterly 12:
51– 58, 66.
Fagarasanu M, Kumar S, Narayan Y. 2004a. Measurement of angular wrist neutral zone and forearm
muscle activity. Journal of Clinical Biomechanics (in press).
Fagarasanu M, Kumar S, Narayan Y. 2004b. An ergonomic comparison of four computer keyboards.
Applied Ergonomics (in press).
Fagarasanu M, Kumar S, Narayan Y. 2004c. The training effect of typing on two ergonomic keyboards.
International Journal of Human –Computer Studies (pending).
Hadler NM. 1987. Clinical Concepts in Regional Musculoskeletal Illness. Grune & Stratton, Inc., Harcourt
Brace Jovanovich, Publishers.
Harvey R, Peper E. 1997. Surface electromyography and mouse use position. Ergonomics 40: 781–789.
Hedge A, Powers JR. 1995. Wrist postures while keyboarding: effects of a negative slope keyboard system
and full motion forearm supports. Ergonomics 38: 508 –517.
Hoffman DS, Strick PL. 1999. Step-tracking movements of the wrist. IV. Muscle activity associated with
movements in different directions. Journal of Neurophysiology. 81: 319 –333.
Keir PJ, Bach JM, Rempel DM. 1996. Effects of finger posture on carpal tunnel pressure during wrist
motion. Journal of Hand Surgery 23A(6): 1004–1009.
Keir PJ, Bach JM, Rempel DM. 1998. Effects of finger posture on carpal tunnel pressure during wrist
motion. The Journal of Hand Surgery 23A: 1004 –1009.
Keir P, Bach J, Rempel D. 1999. Effects of computer mouse design and task on carpal tunnel pressure.
Ergonomics 42: 1350–1360.
Kumar S. 1990. Analysis of selected high risk operations in a garmet industry. In: Advances in Industrial
Ergonomics and Safety II, Das B. Ed. 227 –236. Taylor & Francis, London.
Wrist Posture in Office Work 39-13
Werner R, Armstrong TJ, Aylard MK. 1997. Intracarpal canal presure: the role of finger, hand, wrist and
forearm position. Clinical Biomechanics 12: 44 –51.
Werner RA, Armstrong TJ. 1997. Carpal tunnel syndrome — ergonomic risk factors and intracarpal canal
pressure. Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation Clinics of North America 8: 555 –569.
Yamaguchi DM, Lipscomb PR, Soule EH. 1965. Carpal tunnel syndrome. Minnesota Medicine January:
22– 23.
Zecevic A, Miller DI, Harburn K. 2000. An evaluation of the ergonomics of three computer keyboards.
Ergonomics 43: 55– 72.
Further Reading
Armstrong TJ, Castelli WA, Evans FG, Diaz-Perez R. 1984. Some histological changes in carpal tunnel
contents and their biomechanical implications. Journal of Occupational Medicine 26: 197– 201.
Chen C, Burastero S, Tittiranonda P, Hollerbach K, Shih M, Denhov R, 1994. Quantitative evaluation of
four computer keyboards: wrist posture and typing performance. Proceedings of the Human Factors
and Ergonomics Society 38th Annual Meeting. Santa Monica, CA, pp. 1094–1098.
Fernandez JE, Dahalan JB, Halpern CA, Viswanath V, 1991. The effect of wrist posture on pinch strength.
Proceedings of the Human Factors 35th Annual Meeting, Human Factors Society, San Francisco, CA,
pp. 748–752.
Hertting-Thomasius R, Steidel F, Prokop M, Lettow H. 1992. On the introduction of ergonomically
designed keyboards. Abstracts Book: Work with Display Units WWDU ’92. Luczak, H Cakir, A
Cakir G Eds. Tachnische Universitat Berlin, Berlin p. P-2.
Kapandji IA, 1982. The Physiology of the Joints, Annotated Diagrams of the Mechanics of the Human Joints.
Churchill Livingstone, Edinburgh.
Pryce J. 1980. The wrist position between neutral and ulnar deviation that facilitates the maximum power
grip strength. Journal of Biomechanics 13: 505 –511.
Tittiranonda P, Rempel D, Armstrong T, Burastero S, 1999. Workplace use of an adjustable keyboard:
Adjustment preferences and effect on wrist posture. American Industrial Hygiene Association
Journal, 60(3): 340 –349.
Yoshitake R, Ise N, Yamada S, Tsuchiya K. 1997. An analysis of users’ preference on keyboards through
ergonomic comparison among four keyboards. Applied Human Science 16(5): 205–211.
Weiss ND, Gordon L, Bloon T, So Y, Rempel DM, 1995. Position of the wrist associated with the lowest
carpal tunnel pressure: implications for splint design. Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery 77A(11):
1695– 1699.
40
PLIBEL — A Method
Assigned for
Identification of
Ergonomics Hazards
40.1 Introduction
The Swedish Work Environment Act stipulates that the employer shall investigate occupational injuries,
draw up action plans, and organize and evaluate job modifications. Hence, it is also of interest for the
Labor Inspectorate to study conditions and improvements at workplaces.
A method for the identification of musculoskeletal stress factors that may have injurious effects
(PLIBEL) was designed to meet such needs3 – 5,7 (Figure 40.1). PLIBEL has been used in several
studies, in practical on-site ergonomics work, and also in education. It has been presented in various
parts of the world and translated into several languages.3,6,8,9,11
PLIBEL is a simple checklist screening tool intended to highlight musculoskeletal risks in connection
with workplace investigations. Time aspects, environmental factors, and organizational factors also have
to be considered as modifying factors.
The checklist was designed so that items ordinarily checked in a workplace assessment of
ergonomics hazards would be listed and linked to five body regions (Figure 40.1). Only specific
work characteristics, defined and documented as ergonomics hazards in scientific papers or
textbooks, are listed (Figure 40.2 and Figure 40.3). Whenever a question is irrelevant to a certain
body region, as documentation has not been found in the literature, it is represented by a gray field in
the checklist.
The list was made in 1986 and new references have since then been read continuously and the
list updated. Mostly, these only add knowledge to the primary list, which accordingly has not
been changed. Only one, concerning hips, knees, feet, and the lower spinal region, has the kind of
new information searched for and has therefore been added to the documented background
(Figure 40.2).
40-1
40-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
The PLIBEL form.
FIGURE 40.1
PLIBEL — A Method Assigned for Identification of Ergonomics Hazards 40-3
FIGURE 40.2 Documented background for PLIBEL. References, as numbered in the footnote, are given for each
risk factor in relation to body regions as in the PLIBEL form. Note, however, that in this presentation the
distribution is by four body regions. Hips, knees, and feet are combined in the table.
40-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
FIGURE 40.3 Documented background for modifying factors (for references, see footnote to Figure 40.2).
40.2 Procedure
A workplace assessment using PLIBEL starts with an introductory interview with the employee and
preliminary observation. Representative parts of the job, the tasks that are conducted for most of the
working hours, and tasks that the worker or the observer look upon as particularly stressful to the
musculoskeletal system are chosen for the assessments. Thus, several PLIBEL forms may have to be
filled in for each employee.
The assessments should be related to the capacity of the individual observed. Unusual or personal ways
of doing a task are also recorded.
When an ergonomics hazard is observed, the numbered area on the form is checked or a short note
is made. In the concluding report, where the crude dichotomous answers are arranged in order
of importance, quotations from the list of ergonomics hazards may be used. Modifying factors,
duration, and quantities of environmental or organizational factors are then taken into consideration
(Figure 40.1).
Usually PLIBEL is used to identify musculoskeletal injury risk factors for a specific body region, and
only questions relevant to that body region need be answered. A more general application may also be
feasible. Here, the whole list is used, and the result can be referred to one or more body regions.
To use PLIBEL, first locate the injured body region, then follow the white fields to the right and check
any observed risk factors for the work task. The continued assessment is more difficult, as it requires
consideration of questions a–f. These can either upgrade or minimize the problem. Additional
evident risks, not mentioned in the checklist, are noted and addressed. For example, there are no dur-
ation criteria for a PLIBEL record, and so cumbersome but short-lasting or rare events can also be
recorded. In fact, the purpose of the interview with the worker, which precedes the observation, is to
make such aspects of the task manifest.
A participatory approach of this kind has also been suggested by other authors, for example, Drury2,
who recommend that observes talk to operators to get a feel for what is important. If only “normal” sub-
jects and work periods are chosen for assessments, many of the unusual conditions, which may constitute
main hazards, may be missed.
A handbook (unpublished material) has been compiled to provide the scientific background for each
item and help identify the cut-off point for “yes” or “no” answers. This facilitates the assessments, which
are to be performed by knowledgable and experienced observers. To make the checklist easy to handle
and applicable in many different situations, the questions are basic.
The analysis of possible ergonomics hazards is done at the workplace and only relevant risk infor-
mation from the assessment is considered. The issues identified as risks are arranged in order of
PLIBEL — A Method Assigned for Identification of Ergonomics Hazards 40-5
importance. The concluding report gives an interpretation of the ergonomics working conditions,
starting with the most tiresome movements and postures.
The PLIBEL method is a general assessment method and is not intended for any specific occupation or
task. It observes a part, or the whole, of the body and summarizes the actual identification of ergonomics
hazards in a few sentences.
It is simple and designed for primary checking. For labor inspectors and others looking at many tasks
every day, it is certainly enough to be equipped and well acquainted with the checklist.
PLIBEL is an initial investigative method for the workplace observer to identify ergonomics hazards,
and it can be supplemented by other measurements, for instance, weight and time, or quotations/
observations from other studies.
Although it is tempting to add up items, to obtain a simple and quantitative measure of ergonomics
conditions after a workplace assessment, PLIBEL should not be modified or used in this way. Different
ergonomics hazards do not have an equal influence, and certain problems can appear with more than one
hazardous factor in the checklist.
Many other methods are intended for a specific occupation or body region and can record more detailed
answers. If necessary, these more specific data can easily be used to supplement the PLIBEL questions.
40.3 Example
PLIBEL analysis of the task shown in Figure 40.4 reveals that it entails a risk of musculoskeletal stress
to the lower region of the back, due to the nonresilient walking surface, the unsuitably designed tools
and equipment, and the lack of any possibility to sit and rest. Repetitive and sustained work is performed
with the back flexed slightly forward, bent sideways, and slightly twisted. Loads are repeatedly lifted
manually and often above shoulder height. Note that the text order has been expressed by giving the
most exposed body region and the environmental and instrumental conditions first. The following
phrase gives “the answers” from the body, followed by a description of the tiresome, and perhaps
individual, way of performing the task.
FIGURE 40.4 Example of a task posing ergonomics hazards that was analyzed using PLIBEL. (From Prevention of
Occupational Musculosketetal Injuries. Scand Journ. Of Rehab. Medicine. 35, 1996. With premission.)
40-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
between matching items was considerable and the interobserver reliability yielded kappa values
expressing a fair to moderate agreement on the following questions:
1. Is the content of PLIBEL and the set of items consistent with theoretically derived expectations?
2. Can occurrence of the criterion (ergonomics hazard) be validated by comparison with another
method?
3. Are the results from different users of the PLIBEL method consistent when observing the same
working situation?
4. How has the method been used? What are the experiences?
PLIBEL was written in Swedish, but has also been translated into several languages: Dutch,9 English,3
French6 Greek,8 and Spanish.11
To provide a reference instrument for PLIBEL, an inventory of available scientific literature on
occupational risk factors for musculoskeletal disorders was performed. Original papers, review papers,
and textbooks were studied.
After a thorough review of the literature, the German ergonomics job analysis procedure AET
(Arbeitswissenshaftliche Erhebungsverfahren zur Tätigkeitsanalyse) was chosen as the referent instru-
ment for field testing.10
Like PLIBEL, AET is applicable to all sorts of occupational tasks, and covers workplaces, tools and
objects, degree of repetitiveness, work organization, cognitive demands, and also environmental
factors such as visual conditions, noise, and vibration.
However, while AET analyzes all components in a man-at-work system, PLIBEL focuses on one
extreme phenomenon, that is, the occurrence of an ergonomics hazard.
Two researchers, each of whom had been practising AET and PLIBEL, respectively, clearing very many
workplace assessments, identified 18 matching items in the two methods. For PLIBEL only dichotomous
answers are used, whereas multilevel codes, in steps zero to five, are applied in AET. For each of the items,
the corresponding level between the two methods was identified. The two observation methods were then
used simultaneously for observations on a total of 25 workers, men and women in different tasks.
When comparing the results of PLIBEL and AET, the agreement between matching items was con-
siderable. However, the modifications of AET scores for a dichotomous coding could not completely
eliminate the differences between the methods. In concordance with its purposes, PLIBEL was more
sensitive to ergonomics hazards.
40.5 Conclusion
PLIBEL was designed to meet the need of a standardized and practical method for the identification of
ergonomics hazards and for a preliminary assessment of risk factors. An ergonomics screening tool, for
the assessment of ergonomics conditions at workplaces, has been suggested as a feasible instrument by
other researchers.
Moreover, it is valuable to have a systematic way of assessment when doing follow-ups and when
analyzing how intervention after occupational musculoskeletal injuries could be made more effective.
PLIBEL follows standards and regulations of the day, and though it is a self-explanatory, subjective
assessment method, registering only at a dichotomous level, it requires a solid understanding of
ergonomics. For using the method skilfully, a certain degree of practice is firmly recommended.
To see that a situation is awkward is not difficult, nor is it difficult to find such a situation with the aid
of the checklist. PLIBEL is quick to use and easy to understand, and users will become familiar with this
within hours. However, although PLIBEL is a self-explanatory subjective assessment method, making
dichotomous judgments about risks, it requires a solid ergonomics understanding, and using the
method skilfully requires practice.
Paper, pencil, a folding rule, and a camera are sufficient for ordinary workplace observations and for
initial identification of ergonomics hazards.
PLIBEL — A Method Assigned for Identification of Ergonomics Hazards 40-7
Observational findings have provided a base for recommended improvements, for discussion of
ergonomics problems, and for worksite education. Moreover, PLIBEL has been used for ergonomics
education both in industry and in the Swedish school system.
References
1. Carmines, E.G. and Seller, R.A. (1979). Reliability and Validity Assessment, Sage Publications,
London.
2. Drury, C.G. (1990). Methods for direct observation of performance, in: Wilson, J.R. and Corlett,
E.N. (eds), Evaluation of Human Work, Taylor & Francis, London, pp. 35–57.
3. Kemmlert, K. (1995). A method assigned for the identification of ergonomic hazards — PLIBEL,
Applied Ergonomics, 26(3), 199–211.
4. Kemmlert, K. (1996a). Prevention of occupational musculo-skeletal injuries. Labour Inspectorate
investigation, Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitaiton Medicine, 35 (Suppl.), 1 –34.
5. Kemmlert, K. (1996b). New analytic methods for the prevention of work-related musculoskeletal
injuries, in: Fifteen Years of Occupational Accident Research in Sweden, Rådet för arbetslivsforskning,
Stockholm, pp. 176 –186.
6. Kemmlert, K. and Kilbom, Å. (1996c). La check-list, in: D’Hertefelt, H, Bentein, K, and Willcox, M,
Le corps au travail, Bruxelles, INRCT, pp. 224 –226.
7. Kemmlert, K. (1997), On the Identification and Prevention of Ergonomic Risk Factors, Solna, Arbet-
slivsinstitutet. Doctoral thesis, Luleå University of Technology.
8. Lomi, C. (2002). MEQODOSGIA TON PPOSDIOPISMO TVN PAPAGONTVN KATA-
PONHSHS TOY MYOSKELETIKOY SYSTHMATOS POY MHOPEI NA EINAI BLAPTIKH
— PLIBEL. [Method for the identification of musculo-skeletal stress factors which may have injur-
ious effects—PLIBEL] YGIEINH KAI ASFALEIA THS EPGASIAS, 9, 5–12 [Occupational hygiene
and safety, Vol. 9, pp. 5 –12].
9. Ollongren, G., Debout, J., and Desiron, H. (1990). Van klacht naar problemformulering [From com-
plaints to the wording of a problem], Postuniversitaire opleiding bedrijfsergonomie en arbeidshy-
giëne, Universiteit Antwerpen [University training in occupational health and hygiene, University
of Antwerp].
10. Rohmert, W. and Landau, K. (1983). A New Technique for Job Analysis, Taylor & Francis, London.
11. Serratos-Perés, N. and Kemmlert, K. (1998). Assessing ergonomic conditions in industrial oper-
ations, Asian-Pacific Newsletter on Occupational Health and Safety, 5(3), 67 –69.
41
The ACGIH TLVw for
Hand Activity Level
41.1 Introduction
Musculoskeletal disorders (MSDs) continue to be a major cause of disability and lost work in many
industries involving hand-intensive activities (BLS, 2003). MSDs are “multifactorial,” which means
that they may be caused or aggravated by multiple factors. Factors that pertain to the individual, such
as weight, age, and leisure activities, are referred to as “personal” factors. Factors that pertain to the
job, such as work posture, force required to handle materials and use tools, and recovery time, are
referred to as “work-related” factors. There is a growing body of literature that demonstrates exposure
to work factors results in an increased risk of MSDs (NRC, 1999; NRC and IOM, 2001). MSDs that
result from exposure to work factors are referred to as “work-related” MSDs or “WMSDs.” This
chapter describes the American Council of Industrial Hygienists Threshold Limit Value (ACGIH
TLVw) for monotask hand work and reviews the basic concepts underlying general development of
TLVs. It discusses specifically some of the studies on which the TLV for monotask handwork was
based and some studies of the TLV that have been reported since it was proposed. The chapter concludes
with the concepts and methods that are used to apply this TLV.
41-1
41-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
The TLV for hand activity is intended to protect most workers from MSDs for the hand, wrist, and
forearm. Workers with predisposing health conditions such as arthritis, endocrinological disorders,
obesity, pregnancy, old age, or previous injuries may be affected by exposures below the TLV. It is
likely that exposures below the TLV may produce some discomfort, but it should not persist from day
to day or interfere with activities of work or daily living. The ACGIH TLV is designed to prevent only
work-related cases. Employers should educate workers about non-work-related causes of musculoskeletal
disorders and how they can minimize their risk.
0 2 4 6 8 10
hands idle consistent, slow steady rapid rapid steady
most of conspicuous, steady motion/ steady motion or
the time; long pauses; motion/ exertion motion/ continuous
no regular slow motions exertion; infrequent exertions exertion
exertions frequent pauses infrequent difficulty
brief pauses pauses keeping up
0 10
none at all greatest imaginable
FIGURE 41.1 (a) Visual analog scale for estimating HAL from observations (Latko et al., 1997; ACGIH, 2005).
(b) Peak finger force can be estimated from observations of a job and using the visual analog scale given
in (a). (From Latko, et al., 1999; ACGIH, 2005.)
close to a 97.5 percentile. For purposes of the TLV, peak finger force is defined as a 90th percentile value.
Peak forces also are consistent with the measurements used by Roquelaure et al. (1997). Assessment of
90th percentile peak finger forces is discussed next.
The maximum acceptable hand force was established as zero for an HAL of ten. By definition an
HAL of ten is the “fastest possible: continuous exertions, rapid motion difficulty keeping up.” At
this point, external forces are assumed to be zero. However, there are likely to be very high inertial
forces on tendons and muscles due to the rapid motions (Marras and Schonmarklin, 1993). The cycli-
cal motions of the wrist translate into constant accelerations and decelerations. Wrist motions can be
described as a series of sine functions (Radwin and Lin, 1993). The angular velocity of the joints is
proportional to the amplitude and the frequency of motion, and the angular acceleration is pro-
portional to the amplitude and the frequency squared. The force required to overcome the inertia of
the fingers and the wrist is equal to the product of angular acceleration and the moment of inertia
of the hand segments. Thus, “rapid steady motion or continuous exertion difficulty keeping up” is
associated with high inertial forces that would be transmitted to the finger and wrist tendons and
the forearm muscles.
The TLV assumes a simple linear relationship in which peak finger force decreases from an value of
seven for a HAL value of one to a peak finger force of zero for an HAL value of zero and is shown in
Figure 41.2. The relationship between peak finger force and HAL can be described mathematically as
A nonlinear relationship is possible between peak finger force and HAL, but at the time the TLV was
developed there were insufficient epidemiological data to rationalize a more complex function.
There was concern that a significant number of workers might still be at risk at exposure levels
below the TLV, so an action limit was recommended. In contrast to the TLV, which should not be
exceeded, the action limit may be exceeded, but it triggers a proactive program that includes: training,
job and health surveillance, and medical management. The action limit goes from an HAL and
peak finger force of one and five to ten and zero, respectively. The action limit can be described
mathematically as
10
6
Peak Hand Force
0
0 2 4 6 8 10
Hand Activity Level
FIGURE 41.2 The ACGIH TLV for monotask hand work. The solid line depicts combinations of hand activity level
and peak finger force that should not be exceeded to prevent excessive risk of hand and wrist MSDs. The dashed line is
an action limit at which some people may be elevated risk. The action limit should trigger additional job analysis and
worker training. (From ACGIH (2005a). Documentation of the TLVs and BEI with Other Worldwide Occupational
Exposure Values 2005. Cincinnati, OH: ACGIH Worldwide; ACGIH (2005b). Hand Activity Level. 2005 Threshold
Limit Values for Chemical Substances and Physical Agents and Biological Exposure Limits. Cincinnati, OH: ACGIH,
pp. 112 – 117. With permission.)
There is strong biomechanical support for the contribution of non-neutral wrist postures to hand–
wrist –forearm MSDs (Armstrong and Chaffin, 1979; Moore et al., 1991; Armstrong et al., 1993; Moore,
2002; NRC, 1999; NRC and IOM 2001; Clark et al., 2004). In contrast, Marras and Schoenmarklin
(1993) showed that angular wrist velocity and acceleration may be more important than posture.
Angular velocity and acceleration are captured with the speed consideration in HAL (see
Figure 41.1a). Presently, posture is deferred to professional judgment.
There is a growing body of literature concerned with organizational issues (NRC, 1999; NRC and IOM,
2001). The mechanism by which organizational factors contribute to MSDs is not clear or at least not
clear enough to specify a TLV for these factors at this time.
Several studies of the TLV have been reported since it was proposed. Franzblau et al. (2005) examined
the prevalence of symptoms and specific disorders among 908 workers from seven different job sites in
relation to the TLV. Worker exposures were categorized as below the action limit, between TLV and the
action limit, or below the action limit. Elbow/forearm tendonitis was found to be significantly related to
TLV category as were all measures of carpal tunnel syndrome. Still, there was a substantial prevalence of
symptoms and specific disorders below the TLV action limit. These results suggest that adherence to the
TLV and action limit will reduce, but not eliminate symptoms and/or upper extremity MSDs and that a
control program may still be necessary.
Gell et al. (2005) conducted a prospective study of 432 industrial and clerical workers over a period of
5.4 yr. Incident cases were defined as diagnosed with CTS in workers who had no history of CTS at the
beginning of the study. There was elevated incidence of new CTS cases among workers whose jobs exceed
the TLV (relative risk 1.6); however, the relationship was not statistically significant at p , 0.05.
Werner et al. (2005) studied upper extremity pain in a cohort of 501 active workers from four indus-
trial and three clerical work sites for an average of 5.4 yr. Cases were defined as workers who were asymp-
tomatic or had a discomfort score of two or less at baseline testing, but reported a discomfort score of
The ACGIH TLVw for Hand Activity Level 41-5
four or above on a ten point visual analog scale at follow-up. Controls were defined as those who reported
pain of two or less at follow-up. The peak finger force was close to or exceeded the TLV for 65% of cases
versus only 39% of control (p ¼ 0.01). The peak finger force was higher among cases (3.4 versus 2.9, p
¼ 0.04). Significant relationships were also reported between upper extremity discomfort and both peak
and average finger forces and both peak and average wrist postures.
These studies provide additional support for the TLV, but suggest that the action limit for a proactive
control program may be set too high. Users should consider lowering it to a peak finger force of 3 for an
HAL value of 1. The Werner et al. (2005) study also supports the concern about non-neutral wrist
postures.
TABLE 41.1 HAL can be Estimated from Exertion Frequency or Period and Work Cycle Recovery Time
Recovery
Freq Period 0–20% 20 –40% 40–60% 60 –80% 80–100%
0.12/s (0.09–0.18) 8.0s (5.66–11.31) 6 4 3 1 1
0.25/s (0.18–0.35) 4.0s (2.83–5.66) 6 4 3 2 2
0.5/s (0.35–0.71) 2.0s (1.41–2.83) 6 5 5 4 3
1.0/s (0.71–1.41) 1.0s (0.71–1.41) 7 6 5 5 4
2.0/s (1.41–2.83) 0.5s (0.35–0.71) 8 7 6 5 4
Values Extrapolated by author – not From ACGIH.
Source: Adapted from ACGIH, 2005.
The concept of an exertion is important in ratings based on observations and on calculations. An exer-
tion is a single movement or exertion of force. Figure 41.3 shows time-based plots of finger forces and
postures for two cycles of a turkey thigh boning job reported by Armstrong et al. (1982). In this case,
an exertion entails positioning the knife and performing the cut. The force plots are estimated from
surface EMG measurements of the forearm. It can be seen from the force plot for the right hand
(Figure 41.3a), that over the course of each 7.6 sec cycle, there are approximately four peaks that corre-
spond with each cut, plus a fifth exertion to control the knife between thighs. It can be seen from the plot
of the wrist posture, in Figure 41.3c and Figure 41.3d, that these cuts also involve flexion and ulnar
deviation of the wrist. The average exertion frequency for the right hand is 0.66 exertions per second,
computed by dividing five exertions by 7.6 sec. The left hand is used to hold the thigh and pull the
(a)
200
Peak
Right Hand Force, N
150
90% tile
100
Average
50
0
0 5 10 15 20
Time (sec)
(b)
200
Left Hand Force, N
150
100
Peak
90% tile
50
Average
0
0 5 10 15 20
Time (sec)
FIGURE 41.3 Time-based plots of hand force, a and b, wrist flexion/extension, c and d, and wrist ulnar/radial
deviation, e and f, for the right (a, c, e) and left (b, d f) hands for two cycles of turkey thing boning. (Adapted
form Armstrong T., J. Foulke, B. Joseph and S. Goldstein (1982). Am Ind Hyg Assoc J 43(2): 103– 116. With
permission.)
The ACGIH TLVw for Hand Activity Level 41-7
(c) 40
Flexion/Extension
20
Right Wrist 0
0 5 10 15 20
–20
–40
Time (sec)
(d) 40
Flexion/Extension
20
Left Hand
0
0 5 10 15 20
–20
–40
Time (sec)
(e) 20
Ulnar-/Radial + Deviation
10
0
Right Wrist
0 5 10 15 20
–10
–20
–30
–40
Time (sec)
(f) 20
Ulnar-/Radial Deviation
10
0
Left Wrist
0 5 10 15 20
–10
–20
–30
–40
Time (sec)
meat away from the bone. Although the force peaks are not as conspicuous, four exertions per thigh are
required for each 7.6 sec cycle. The left hand can rest between successive thighs. The average exertion
frequency for the left hand is 0.5 exertions per second.
Other examples of an exertion include swinging a hammer, twisting a screwdriver, driving a threaded
fastener with a power screwdriver, getting and placing a part, folding the flap of a box, ejecting the con-
tents of a pipette, activating a control on a machine, pressing a key on a keyboard, etc. All of these
examples entail a single movement or exertion of force. It is not always necessary to make a plot as
shown in Figure 41.3. Exertions can be counted from detailed job descriptions used by engineers to estab-
lish production standards or from video recordings.
In addition to exertion frequency, the force plots also provide information about recovery time so that
Table 41.1 can be used to estimate the HAL. There is essentially no recovery time for the right hand
because of the exertion to hold the knife in between successive cuts and thighs. The recovery time can
be calculated by adding up the times that the hand is at rest or estimated from job observations:
Peak finger force (0–10) ¼ 90th percentile finger force (N)=strength (N) 10
The knife and thigh in the thigh boning example are held in a power grip posture. Based on a survey of
40 –45-yr-old suburban and rural females by Mathiowetz et al. (1985), average right and left hand grip
strengths are estimated as 314 + 60 and 278 + 61 N respectively:
Strength can be adjusted from males or females, young or old workers, or higher or lower percentiles,
depending on the population of interest.
The ACGIH TLVw for Hand Activity Level 41-9
(a)
16 120%
14
100%
12
80%
10
Frequency
8 60%
6
40%
4
20%
2
0 0%
10
30
50
70
90
0
0
0
0
0
e
11
13
15
17
19
or
M
Finger Force, N
(b)
30 120%
25 100%
20 80%
Frequency
15 60%
10 40%
5 20%
0 0%
10
30
50
70
90
0
0
e
11
13
15
17
19
or
M
Finger Force, N
FIGURE 41.4 Frequency and cumulative histograms for finger force of (a) right hand and of (b) left hand from
Figure 41.3(a) and Figure 41.3(b). The dashed lines show the 90th percentile forces.
Peak finger force can be estimated by the observer (Latko et al., 1995). Force estimates can also
be obtained from the workers themselves. Marshall et al. (2004) showed that worker force ratings can
be improved significantly if they are first asked to make a maximal exertion on a force gauge. The
maximum force values can be compared with the relative worker ratings to estimate forces in pounds
or Newtons. Bao and Silverstein (2005) described the use of force matching in which workers indicate
the task force requirements by squeezing a force dynamometer in a similar position. They reported
good agreement between actual and observed forces, but that clear instructions are important.
In some cases, forces can be estimated from the weight of a work object using biomechanical analysis.
For example, the finger force required to lift a tool box in a hook grip posture will be equal to the weight
of the tool box. The finger force required to hold a work object in a pinch posture will be equal to the
weight of the object divided by two times the coefficient of friction. The finger force required to one part
against another may be difficult to measure. Biomechanical analysis of these exertions are complex and
beyond the scope of this discussion.
41-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Surface electromyography (EMG) can be used to estimate hand forces in some settings (Armstrong
et al., 1982; Jonsson, 1988; Matiassen and Winkel, 1991). Finally, some tasks may be suitable to the
use of electronic force gauges for measurement of forces. Armstrong et al. (1994) describe the use of
force gauges under a keyboard for measuring reaction forces as subjects type on a keyboard. The
method selected for assessing force and repetition will depend on the desired level of quality, type of
job, and available resources.
(a) 10
0
0 2 4 6 8 10
Hand Activity Level
(b) 10
8
Peak Hand Force
0
0 2 4 6 8 10
Hand Activity Level
FIGURE 41.5 Applications of the TLV for the right hand and left hand for boning 3780 turkey thighs per shift. The
right hand exceeds the TLV; the left hand is below the action limit.
the right hand is clearly above the TLV. In this case, the intersection of the left hand lines is below the
action limit.
The TLV also can be evaluated computationally as shown previously:
For the right hand of the thigh boner, the acceptable finger force is
Since the observed force was four, the TLV is clearly exceeded by the right hand. In addition to the force
and posture, it can be seen from Figure 41.3(a) and Figure 41.3(e) that there is also repeated wrist flexion
and deviation. Engineering controls need to be considered to reduce the finger forces and the postural
stresses on the wrist. Administrative controls also should be implemented as long as the peak finger
force and HAL values are near the action limit.
Similarly, the TLV is not exceeded by the left hand, but was slightly above the action limit.
TLV
Peak finger force 7.77 – 0.77 5
Peak finger force 3.8
Action limit:
Peak finger force 5.55 – 0.55 5
Peak finger force 2.8
This example was based on the average strength of a female in her early 40s. Different results would be
obtained for workers with different strengths. It is important that strength values representative of all of
the workers be considered — male –female, young –old, and lower percentiles in each category.
Administrative controls should be continued as long as exposures are close to the action limit. Admin-
istrative controls should include educating workers about the symptoms of hand–wrist –forearm MSDs
and where to go to get help if symptoms occur. Workers also should be instructed in best work practices,
and how to adjust their equipment. Supervisors should be instructed about the symptoms of MSDs, best
work practices, and work station setup so that they can assist workers. Controls should include surveil-
lance of worker symptoms, injuries, and illnesses to identify both reported and unreported problems so
that appropriate interventions can be implemented. Surveillance should include further analysis of the
job to identify causes for high forces and posture stresses so that they can be addressed. Finally, there
should be some kind of administrative structure to manage these control efforts, for example, an
ergonomics team or safety committee.
41.7 Summary
The ACGIH TLV is a tool for assessing the risk of hand–wrist –forearm MSDs. The TLV applies to mono-
task hand work performed for four or more hours per day. The TLV considers HAL and peak finger force,
but recommends that other work factors, such as wrist posture, contact stresses, and hand– arm
vibration, also be considered — particularly when exposures are close to the TLV. Also, workers
should be advised to discuss personal factors, such as weight, chronic diseases, pregnancy, and past inju-
ries, with their health-care provider. Peak finger force and HAL can be determined from observations or
calculated from time-based measurements. Time-based measurements may be obtained from sequential
analysis of video recordings or electronically from electromyography, force gauges, and goniometry.
Exceeding the TLV should initiate an engineering control change process. Those jobs exceeding the
action limit should have administrative or engineering controls applied.
References
ACGIH (2005a). Documentation of the TLVs and BEI with Other Worldwide Occupational Exposure Values
2005. Cincinnati, OH: ACGIH Worldwide.
The ACGIH TLVw for Hand Activity Level 41-13
ACGIH (2005b). Hand Activity Level. 2005 Threshold Limit Values for Chemical Substances and Physical
Agents and Biological Exposure Limits. Cincinnati, OH: ACGIH, pp. 112– 117.
Armstrong, T. and D. Chaffin (1979). Some biomechanical aspects of the carpal tunnel. J Biomech, 12(7):
567–570.
Armstrong, T., J. Foulke, B. Joseph and S. Goldstein (1982). Investigation of cumulative trauma disorders
in a poultry processing plant. Am Ind Hyg Assoc J 43(2): 103 –116.
Armstrong, T., L. Fine, S. Goldstein, Y. Lifshitz and B. Silverstein (1987). Ergonomics considerations in
hand and wrist tendinitis. J Hand Surg [Am] 12(5 Pt 2): 830 –837.
Armstrong, T., P. Buckle, L. Fine, M. Hagberg, B. Jonsson, A. Kilbom, I. Kuorinka, B. Silverstein,
G. Sjogaard and E. Viikari-Juntura (1993). A conceptual model for work-related neck and
upper-limb musculoskeletal disorders. Scand J Work Environ Health 19(2): 73–84.
Armstrong, T., J. Foulke, B. Martin, J. Gerson and D. Rempel (1994). Investigation of applied forces in
alphanumeric keyboard work. Am Ind Hyg Assoc J 55(1): 30–35.
Armstrong, T., W. Keyserling, D. Grieshaber, M. Ebersole and E. Lo (2003). Time based job analysis
for control of work related musculoskeletal disorders. 15th Triennial Congress of the International
Ergonomics Association, Seoul, South Korea.
Bao, S. and B. Silverstein (2005). Estimation of hand force in ergonomic job evaluations. Ergonomics
48(3): 288 –301.
BLS (2003). Lost-Worktime Injuries and Illnesses: Characteristics and Resulting Time Away from Work,
2001. Washington, DC: Bureau of Labor Statistics, United States Department of Labor.
Borg, G. (1990). Psychophysical scaling with applications in physical work and the perception of exer-
tion. Scand J Work Environ Health 16(Suppl 1): 55 –58.
Bystrom, S. and C. Fransson-Hall (1994). Acceptability of intermittent handgrip contractions based on
physiological response. Hum Factors 36(1): 158 –171.
Chiang, H.-C., Y.-C. Ko, S.-S. Chen, H.-S. Yu and T. Wu (1993). Prevalence of shoulder and upper-limb
disorders among workers in the fish-processing industry. Scand J Work Environ Health 19(2): 126 –
131.
Clark, B., T. Al-Shatti, A. Barr, M. Amin and M. Barbe (2004). Performance of a high-repetition, high-
force task induces carpal tunnel syndrome in rats. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther 34(5): 244 –253.
Ebersole, M. and T. Armstrong (2002). Inter-Rater Reliability for Hand Activity Level (HAL) and Force
Metrics. Baltimore, MD: Human Factor and Ergonomics Society.
Faucett, J. and D. Rempel (1994). VDT-related musculoskeletal symptoms: Interactions between work
posture and psychosocial work factors. Am J Ind Med 26(5): 597 –612.
Fransson-Hall, C., S. Bystrom and A. Kilbom (1995). Self-reported physical exposure and musculoske-
letal symptoms of the forearm-hand among automobile assembly-line workers. J Occup Environ
Med 37(9): 1136 –1144.
Franzblau, A., T. Armstrong, R. Werner and S. Ulin (2005). A cross-sectional assessment of the ACGIH
TLV for hand activity level. J Occup Rehabil 15(1): 57 –67.
Gell, N., R. Werner, A. Franzblau, S. Ulin and T. Armstrong (2005). A longitudinal study of industrial and
clerical workers: incidence of carpal tunnel syndrome and assessment of risk factors. J Occup
Rehabil 15(1): 47 –55.
Hammer, A. (1934). Tenosynovitis. Med Rec 140: 353 –355.
Jonsson, B. (1988). The static load component in muscle work. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol 57(3):
305–310.
Latko, W. (1997). Development and evaluation of an observational method for quantifying exposure to
hand activity and other physical stressors in manual work. Doctoral Dissertation: Industrial and
Operations Engineering. University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, 188.
Latko, W., T. Armstrong, A. Franzblau and S. Ulin (1995). Comparison of three methods for assessing
repetition in manual work. Montreal: PREMUS.
41-14 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Latko, W., T. Armstrong, J. Foulke, G. Herrin, R. Rabourn and S. Ulin (1997). Development and evalu-
ation of an observational method for assessing repetition in hand tasks. Am Ind Hyg Assoc J 58(4):
278– 285.
Latko, W., T. Armstrong, A. Franzblau, S. Ulin, R. Werner and J. Albers (1999). Cross-sectional study
of the relationship between repetitive work and the prevalence of upper limb musculoskeletal
disorders. Am J Ind Med 36(2): 248 –259.
Leclerc, A., P. Franchi, M. Cristofari, B. Delemotte, P. Mereau, C. Teyssier-Cotte and A. Touranchet
(1998). Carpal tunnel syndrome and work organisation in repetitive work: a cross sectional
study in France. Study Group on Repetitive Work. Occup Environ Med 55(3): 180– 187.
Luopajarvi, T., I. Kuorinka, M. Virolainen and M. Holmberg (1979). Prevalence of tenosynovitis and
other injuries of the upper extremities in repetitive work. Scand J Work Environ Health 5 (Suppl
3): 48 –55.
Marshall, M., T. Armstrong and M. Ebersole (2004). Verbal estimation of peak exertion intensity. Human
Factors and Ergonomics 46(4): 697 –710.
Marras, W. and R. Schoenmarklin (1993). Wrist motions in industry. Ergonomics 36(4): 341–351.
Mathiassen, S. and J. Winkel (1991). Quantifying variation in physical load using exposure-vs-time data.
Ergonomics 34(12): 1455 –1468.
Mathiowetz, V., N. Kashman, G. Volland, K. Weber, M. Dowe and S. Rogers (1985). Grip and pinch
strength: normative data for adults. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 66(2): 69 –74.
Moore, J. (2002). Biomechanical models for the pathogenesis of specific distal upper extremity disorders.
Am J Ind Med 41(5): 353 –369.
Moore, A., R. Wells and D. Ranney (1991). Quantifying exposure in occupational manual task with
cumulative trauma disorder potential. Ergonomics 34(12): 1433–1453.
NRC (1999). Work-Related Musculoskeletal Disorders: A Review Of The Evidence. Washington: DC: 4
National Academy Press.
NRC and IOM (2001). Musculoskeletal Disorders and the Workplace: Low Back and Upper Extremities.
Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Obolenskaja, A. and I. Goljanitzki (1927). Die seröse tendovaginitis in der Klinic und im Experiment.
Dtsch Z Chir Leipz 201: 388–399.
Radwin, R. and M. Lin (1993). An analytical method for characterizing repetitive motion and postural
stress using spectral analysis. Ergonomics 36(4): 379– 389.
Rohmert, W. (1973). Problems in determining rest allowances. Appl Ergon 4(2): 91– 95.
Roquelaure, Y., S. Mechali, C. Dano, S. Fanello, F. Benetti, D. Bureau, J. Mariel, Y.H. Martin, F. Derriennic
and D. Penneau-Fontbonne (1997). Occupational and personal risk factors for carpal tunnel
syndrome in industrial workers. Scand J Work Environ Health 23(5): 364–369.
Silverstein, B. (1985). The prevalence of upper extremity cumulative trauma disorders in industry.
Doctoral Disseration: Department of Epidemiology, The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI,
p. 198.
Silverstein, B., L. Fine and T. Armstrong (1987). Occupational factors and carpal tunnel syndrome.
Am J Ind Med 11(3): 343 –358.
Werner, R.A., A. Franzblau, N. Gell, S.S. Ulin and T.J. Armstrong (2005). Predictors of upper extremity
discomfort: a longitudinal study of industrial and clerical workers. J Occup Rehabil 15(1): 27 –35.
42
REBA and RULA:
Whole Body and Upper
Limb Rapid
Assessment Tools
42.1 Introduction
REBA (Rapid Entire Body Assessment) and RULA (Rapid Upper Limb Assessment) provide a quick
analysis of the demands on a person’s musculoskeletal system when performing a specific task. Both
tools are required to be used as part of a full ergonomic workplace assessment and have proved
popular in providing a simple, visual indication as to the level of risk and need for action associated
with the task. REBA was developed to provide a quick and easy observational postural analysis tool
for whole body activities (static and dynamic) giving a musculoskeletal risk action level (Hignett and
McAtamney, 2000). RULA was developed earlier (McAtamney and Corlett, 1993) to provide a rapid
objective measure of musculoskeletal risk caused by mainly sedentary tasks where upper body
demands were high. Both tools use body part diagrams to assist with the coding of joint angles and
body postures, with additional coding for load/force, coupling and muscle activity. They both
produce risk level scores on a given scale to indicate whether the risk is negligible through to very high.
There are several postural analysis tools available for ergonomic and occupational health practitioners,
many of which are included in this publication. In order to differentiate between the tools, consider the
following questions:
1. Task
a. Which area of the body is being assessed, for example, whole body or upper limb?
42-1
42-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
When choosing between REBA and RULA consider the task demands and type of assessment required as
suggested in Table 42.1. RULA is generally used if the person is sitting, standing still or in an otherwise
sedentary position and mainly using the upper body and arms to work, for all other tasks REBA should
be used. In all applications of REBA and RULA, users should receive training or be confidently skilled in
the tool before using it although no previous ergonomic skills are required.
REBA was initially designed to provide a pen-and-paper postural analysis tool that could either be used
in the field by direct observation or with still/video photographs. It has been further developed and there
are now simple computer programmes available which support the coding and analysis (Janik et al.,
2002). As a pen-and-paper tool, it was designed to have wider application than more complex postural
analysis tools (e.g., NIOSH, Waters et al., 1993) and so was developed using examples from electricity,
health care, and manufacturing industries. However, it also has more sensitivity and anatomical (body
part) detail than other postural analysis tools (e.g., OWAS, Karhu et al., 1977).
RULA was developed to provide postural analysis where work placed physical demands on the trunk,
neck, and upper limbs in particular and therefore in tasks where work-related upper limb disorders, cumu-
lative trauma disorders, and similar problems are a concern. RULA assesses the posture, force and move-
ment associated with sedentary tasks such tasks include screen based or computer tasks, manufacturing or
retail tasks where the worker is seated or standing without moving about. The main applications of RULA
are to measure the musculoskeletal risk, usually as part of a broader ergonomic investigation and then:
Group A: trunk, neck, and legs (Figure 42.1 and Table 42.2)
Group B: upper arms, lower arms, and wrists (Figure 42.2 and Table 42.3)
1
2 2
0°
20° 20°
3
3
60°
L3/L4
0°
1
20°
2
1 2 +1 +2
30°− 60°
>60°
FIGURE 42.1 Group A postures. (From Hignett, S. 1998. In: Pitt-Brooke, J., Raid, H., Lockwood, J., and Kerr
K., (Eds.), Rehabilitation of Movement. Theoretical Basis of Clinical Practice. London: W.B. Saunders Company Ltd.
Chapter 13, pp. 480 – 486.)
42-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
The postures are scored by observing the task by video, photograph or in real time and allocating scores for
the body parts (Group A and Group B) load/force, coupling, and activity as shown in Figure 42.3. These
data are recorded on the Score Sheet (Figure 42.4). The choice of right or left arm is usually driven by avail-
ability (what can be observed); however, it is also possible to score both sides and then choose the highest
score to take forward in the score sheet (see examples in Figure 42.5 and Figure 42.6). The scores are then
transformed via Table A (Table 42.4) and Table B (Table 42.5) into SCORE A and SCORE B. At this stage,
the additional scores are added for load/force (Table 42.4), with an additional score for shock or rapid
build-up of force (e.g., catching a load), and coupling (Table 42.5). The coupling score uses four levels
(good, fail, poor, and unacceptable) to give an indication of the interface between the person and the
load and allows for both manual and other body region interfaces.
SCORE A and SCORE B are then entered onto Table C (Table 42.6) to produce SCORE C. At this stage,
the Activity Score (Table 42.7) is added to give additional scores for:
. One or more body parts are static
. Repeated small range actions
. Large range changes in postures or unstable base
This gives a final REBA SCORE, which is then interpreted into an Action Level using Table 42.7.
The five action levels give an indication of the urgency of avoiding or reducing the risk of the assessed
posture.
42.3 Examples
Two examples are given in Figure 42.5 and Figure 42.6. The scoring rationale is outlined in the following
sections. Both of these examples were part of an initial ergonomic workplace assessment and have been
subsequently changed. The medical notes area was relocated in a specially designed building, with appro-
priate height racking and safety stepladders. The pediatric cot was redesigned with a U.K. manufacturer
and has been replaced with an electric cot with redesigned cot sides.
Group B
Upper Arms ¼ 4 þ 1 þ 1 (greater than 908 flexion with medial rotation and a raised shoulder)
Lower arms ¼ 2 (less than 608 flexion)
Wrist ¼ 2 (greater than 158 extension, with no deviation/twist)
Coupling ¼ 2 (poor, not acceptable although possible)
SCORE A ¼ 3
SCORE B ¼ 11 (9 þ 2)
4
90°
45° 3
20°
20°
2 2
1 1
0°
2
100°
2
60°
2 15°
1
0°
1
2 15°
FIGURE 42.2 Group B postures. (From Hignett, S. 1998. In: Pitt-Brooke, J., Raid, H., Lockwood, J., and Kerr
K., (Eds.), Rehabilitation of Movement. Theoretical Basis of Clinical Practice. London: W.B. Saunders Company Ltd.
Chapter 13, pp. 480 – 486.)
42-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
SCORE C ¼ 8
Activity Score ¼ þ1 (unstable base)
REBA SCORE ¼ 9 (Action level 3. High risk level, action is necessary soon)
Score A Score B
Use Table A Use Table B
REBA SCORE
(Add Activity score to Score C)
Legs ¼ 1þ1 (bilateral weight bearing with knees flexed between 30 and 608)
Load/Force ¼ 1 (less than 5 kg force exerted but with a rapid build-up of force (jerk))
Group B
Upper arms ¼ 3 þ 121 (between 45 and 908 flexion with medial rotation and a gravity-assisted posture
(21))
Lower arms ¼ 2 (less than 608 flexion)
Wrist ¼ 1 (between 0 and 158 extension, with no deviation/twist)
Coupling ¼ 1 (fair; hand hold acceptable, but not ideal)
SCORE A ¼ 9 (8 þ 1)
SCORE B ¼ 5 (4 þ 1)
SCORE C ¼ 10
Group A Group B
R U
T Use Table A Use Table B A
R L
N A
+ + R
L W
Load/Force Coupling
Score A Score B
Use Table C
Score C
Activity
Score
REBA Score
42.4.1 Reliability
In the development stages of REBA, the reliability was tested by using over 600 examples of postures from
the electricity, health care, and manufacturing industries. The examples were coded by 14 professionals
(occupational therapists, physiotherapists, nurses, and ergonomists). The reliability of the upper arm
posture score (56%) was excluded due to the addition of the gravity-assisted code during the reliability
testing. The results were analyzed by body part (Figure 42.7) with an agreement rate of between
62 and 85%.
This was felt to be a satisfactory interrater reliability for this stage of development with such a large
group. In comparison, Suurnäkki et al. (1988) reported achieving a reliability range of 74 –99% with
OWAS with only six experts (Corlett, 1998). Hignett (1998) reported a 96% agreement with only two
participants in an interrater reliability study.
42.4.2 Validity
REBA has very good face validity and is widely used internationally. External validity has been achieved
through use in a range of industries and it considered to present generalizable results within the context
of a full ergonomics workplace assessment.
L R
Trunk Use Table A Use Table B Upper Arms
L R
Neck Lower Arms
L R
Legs Wrist
Load/Force Coupling
Score B
Score A
Use Table C
Score C
Activity
Score
REBA Score
L R
Neck Lower Arms
L R
Legs Wrist
Load/Force Coupling
Score A Score B
Use Table C
Score C
Activity
Score
REBA Score
0 20 40 60 80 100
Percentage agreement
(a) (b)
FIGURE 42.8 RULA example. (a) Telephone held on shoulder. RULA Score of 5, (b) Headset used. RULA Score of 1.
42-12 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
42.6 Example
A habitual posture in organizations where headsets are not used is holding a phone on the shoulder. The
nerves and blood vessels in the side of the neck can be placed under high loading and there is a direct
impact on shoulder/neck discomfort after prolonged exposure. The RULA score is five when holding
a phone as shown in Figure 42.8. This changes to a RULA score one simply with the provision of a
headset.
References
American Academy of Orthopedic Surgeons. 1965. Joint Motion: Method of Measuring and Recording.
Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.
Axelsson, J.R.C. 1997. RULA in action: enhancing participation and continuous improvements. In:
Seppala, P., Luopajarvi T., Nygard, C.H., and Mattila, M. (Eds.) From Experience to Innovation
— IEA ’97. Proceedings of the 13th Triennial Congress of the International Ergonomics Association,
Tampere, Finland, June 29–July 4, 1997. Helsinki: Finnish Institute of Occupational Health,
Volume 4, 251–253.
Corlett, E.N. 1998. Personal communication.
Gutierrez, A.M.J.A. 1998. A workstation design for a Philippine semiconductor. In: Bishu, R., Karwowski,
W., and Goonetilleke, R.S. (Eds.), ERGON-AXIA ’98. Proceedings of the First World Congress on
Ergonomics for Global Quality and Productivity, Clear Water Bay, Hong Kong, July 8 –11, 1998,
Hong Kong: Hong Kong University of Science and Technology, pp. 133–136
Hignett, S. 1998. Ergonomics. In: Pitt-Brooke, J., Reid, H., Lockwood J., and Kerr, K. (Eds.) Rehabilitation
of Movement. Theoretical Basis of Clinical Practice. London: W.B Saunders Company Ltd, Chapter
13, pp. 480–486.
Hignett, S. and McAtamney, L. 2000. Rapid Entire Body Assessment (REBA). Applied Ergonomics, 31,
201– 205
Janik, H., Münzbergen, E., and Schultz, K. 2002. REBA-verfahren (Rapid Entire Body Assessment) auf
einem Pocket Computer. Proceedings of 42. Jahrestagung der Deutschen Gesellschaf für Arbeitsmedi-
zin un Umweltmedizin e. V. (DGAUM). April 10 –13, 2002, Munich, V25.
Karhu, O., Kansi, P., and Kuorinka, I. 1977. Correcting working postures in industry: a practical method
for analysis. Applied Ergonomics, 8(4), 199–201.
Leuder, R. 1996. A proposed RULA for computer users. Proceedings of the Ergonomics Summer Workshop,
U.C. Berkeley, August 8– 9.
McAtamney, L. and Corlett, E.N. 1993. RULA: a survey method for the investigation of work-related
upper limb disorders. Applied Ergonomics, 24, 91 –99.
Suurnäkki, T., Louhevaara, V., Karhu, O., Kuorinka, I., Kansi, P., and Peuraniemi, A. 1988. Standardised
observation method for the assessment of working postures: the OWAS method. In: Adams, A.S.,
Hall, R.R., McPhee, B.J., and Oxenburgh, M.S. (Eds.), Designing a Better World. Ergonomics Inter-
national ’88. Proceedings of the 10th Congress of the International Ergonomics Association,
August 1 –5, 1988, Sydney, Australia.
Waters, T.R., Putz-Anderson, V., Garg, A., and Fine, L.J. 1993. Revised NIOSH equation for the design
and evaluation of manual lifting tasks. Ergonomics, 36(7), 749 –776.
43
An Assessment
Technique for Postural
Loading on the Upper
Body (LUBA)
43-1
43-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
production process in industrial sites. Most postural classification schemes developed are the observation
methods. These include the Posture Targeting (Corlett et al., 1979), OWAS (Karhu et al., 1977), PATH
(Buchholz et al., 1996), and RULA (McAtamney and Corlett, 1993).
Depending upon the grouping methods for joint motions involved in a classified posture, Genaidy
et al. (1994) categorized the postural classification approaches used in observational techniques into:
macropostural, micropostural, and postural– work activity classifications. The macropostural classifi-
cation groups more than one non-neutral posture around a joint into one category, while the macro-
postural classification is more detailed than the previous method. The postural –work activity
classification combines postures and work activities (Genaidy et al., 1994).
Although the existing methods have proved useful for quantification of postural stresses in field
studies, and contributed to preventing work-related MSDs, they have many disadvantages. First,
many of the observational classification schemes are not based on experimental data. Second, the exist-
ing methods have been developed for specific application purposes, and, consequently, are not generic in
many respects. Third, many methods deal with only a few representative joint motions, as they focus on
specific joint motions frequently linked to MSDs. Another problem is that only a few schemes (including
OWAS and RULA) utilize specific evaluation criteria for the classified postures, which provide infor-
mation on any corrective actions to be undertaken for reducing postural burden at work. In addition,
the evaluation criteria provided by RULA and OWAS were not based on experimental results, but rather
relied on the rankings provided by ergonomists and occupational physiotherapists using biomechanical
and muscle function criteria (McAtamney and Corlett, 1993), or the subjective rankings provided by
experienced steel workers (Karhu et al., 1977), respectively.
43.2 Objectives
To complement the restrictions of the existing methods, this chapter presents an observational
and macropostural technique for postural loading on the upper body assessment (LUBA). The
method is based on experimental data for perceived discomfort, expressed as numerical ratio scores
for a set of joint motions, including the hand, arm, neck, and back. This technique is applied to
the seated posture or standing posture with the lower limb well supported in an evenly balanced
posture.
Perceived discomforts were gathered for varying postures of five joints in the upper body (Table 43.1),
which included almost every possible joint motion occurring in the sitting and standing postures. Per-
ceived discomforts were measured at five levels of range of joint motion (ROM) in each motion: 0
(neutral), 25, 50, 75, and 100% of ROM, respectively. The magnitude estimation was adopted for measur-
ing discomfort scores. It has the advantage of providing data with the characteristics of the interval or
ratio scale that can be applied to quantitative statistical techniques.
43.3 LUBA
which is 1.0. The developed relative discomfort score scheme is presented in Table 43.2 through
Table 43.6, which are classified by the joints involved in motions.
As shown in Table 43.2 through Table 43.6, the relative discomfort scores are almost identical for both
sitting and standing postures, and increase drastically when the joints approach the limit of their ROM.
The results also showed that back movements were perceived as more stressful than any other joint
motion. Specifically, the back extension of .308 (with a relative discomfort score of 15) in the standing
posture was the most stressful of all the joint motions examined.
or those that the worker itself (or the observer) considers as stressful to the muculoskeletal system. Third,
each joint motion observed in the selected postures is assigned a relative discomfort score according to
the above classification scheme. It is recommended that only the right or left of the upper body, which
seems to be more stressful of two body sides, be assessed at a time.
Figure 43.1 makes it easy and fast for the posture analysts to classify working postures, where they just
tick the corresponding item for each joint motion. After completion of posture classification, the postural
load for the selected posture can be obtained by summing up the respective discomfort score values
ticked in Figure 43.1. Fourth, the following equation is used to calculate the postural load index for
joint motions deviated from their neutral positions in the chosen postures, that is, for joint motions
having relative discomfort scores of two or more. The postural load index is calculated for the left or right
arm/hand, the neck, and back motions. Only the right or left arm/hand is assessed at a time when cal-
culating postural load index. Finally, based on the postural load index, the posture is evaluated using the
criterion of four action categories in the following in terms of whether the posture is acceptable or any
correction actions are needed:
n X
X mj
Postural load index ¼ Sij
j¼1 i¼1
An Assessment Technique for Postural Loading on the Upper Body 43-7
where i is the ith joint motion, j is the jth joint, n is the number of joints involved, mj is the number of
joint motions studied in the jth joint, Sij is the relative discomfort score of the ith joint motion in the jth
joint (here, Sij ¼ 0 if a corresponding relative discomfort score is 1.0).
The four action categories are as follows:
Category I: Postures with the postural load index of 5 or less. This category of postures is acceptable,
except in special situations such as repeating and sustaining them for long periods of time, etc. No
corrective actions are needed
Category II: Postures with the postural load index from 5 to 10. This category of postures requires
further investigation and corrective changes during the next regular check, but immediate inter-
vention is not needed
Category III: Postures with the postural load index from 10 to 15. This category of postures requires
corrective action through redesigning workplaces or working methods soon
Category IV: Postures with the postural load index of 15 or more. This category of postures requires
immediate consideration and corrective action
43.5 Conclusions
A technique for postural loading on the upper body assessment was presented based on the new exper-
imental data for perceived discomfort values for a set of joint motions, including the hand, arm, neck,
and back. Each postural class was assigned a relative discomfort score with the characteristics of the ratio
scale relative to the perceived discomfort for the neutral position of elbow flexion. The ratio discomfort
score makes it easy to quantitatively evaluate postural stresses for varying postures and to compare them
across different postures. It is expected that the postural classification scheme based on consideration of
perceived discomfort can be used as a valuable tool for assessing postural stresses and preventing posture-
related MSDs.
References
Andrews, D.M., Norman, R.W., Wells, R.P., Neumann, P., 1998. Comparison of self-report and observer
methods for repetitive posture and load assessment. Occupational Ergonomics 1(3), 211 –222.
Armstrong, T.J., Buckle, P., Fine, L.J., Hagberg, M., Jonsson, B., Kilbom, A., Kuorinka, I.A.A., Silverstein,
B.A., Sjogaard, G., Viikari-Juntura, E.R.A., 1993. A conceptual model for work-related neck and
43-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
upper-limb musculoskeletal disorders. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment, and Health 19,
73– 74.
Bhatnager, V., Drury, C.G., Schiro, S.G., 1985. Posture, postural discomfort, and performance. Human
Factors 27(2), 189 –199.
Buchholz, B., Paquet, V., Punnett, L., Lee, D., Moir, S., 1996. PATH: a work sampling-based approach to
ergonomics job analysis for construction and other non-repetitive work. Applied Ergonomics 27(3),
177– 187.
Burdorf, F.J., Govaert, G., Elders, L., 1991. Postural load and back pain of workers in the manufacturing
of prefabricated concrete elements. Ergonomics 34(7), 909–918.
Corlett, E.N., Madeley, S.J., Manencia, I., 1979. Posture Targetting: a technique for recording working
postures. Ergonomics 22(3), 357– 366.
Genaidy, A.M., Al-Shedi, A.A., Karwowski, W., 1994. Postural stress analysis in industry. Applied Ergo-
nomics 25, 77– 87.
Heinsalmi, P., 1986. Method to measure working posture loads at working site (OWAS). In: Corlett, E.N.,
Wilson, J., Manencia, I. Eds., The Ergonomics of Working Postures. Talyor & Francis, London,
pp. 100–104.
Karhu, O., Kansi, P., Kuorinka, I., 1977. Correcting working postures in industry: a practical method for
analysis. Applied Ergonomics 8(4), 199 –201.
Kemmlert, K., 1995. A method assigned for the identification of ergonomics hazards — PLIBEL. Applied
Ergonomics 36(3), 199– 211.
Keyserling, W.M., 1986. Postural analysis of trunk and shoulders in simulated real time. Ergonomics
29(4), 569– 583.
McAtamney, L., Corlett, E.N., 1993. RULA: a survey method for the investigation of work-related upper
limb disorders. Applied Ergonomics 24(2), 91 –99.
Priel, V.Z., 1974. A numerical definition of posture. Human Factors 16, 576 –584.
van Wely, P., 1969. Design and disease. Applied Ergonomics 1, 262 –269.
44
The Washington State
SHARP Approach to
Exposure Assessment
44.1 Introduction
Understanding the relationships between workplace exposure parameters and the health outcomes of the
musculoskeletal system is the basis for preventing and reducing work-related musculoskeletal disorders.
Quantification of exposure parameters is critical in epidemiological studies as well as ergonomics appli-
cations. Methods used for the exposure assessment vary depending on the purpose of the applications
and feasibilities of using these methods. This chapter discusses the various exposure parameters at
workplaces related to work-related upper extremity disorders, measurement strategies, and some
exposure assessment methods used in epidemiological studies. The exposure assessment approach
used by SHARP in a large prospective study of upper extremity musculoskeletal disorders is presented
and discussed.
44-1
44-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
work-related musculoskeletal disorders of the neck, upper extremity, and low back and summarized
major findings on the various exposure parameters (NIOSH, 1997). In 2001, the National Research
Council published a comprehensive review of the evidence on work-related musculoskeletal disorders
in which they concluded that repetition, force, and vibration, as well as high job demands and job
stress, were particularly important risk factors for upper extremity disorders (Panel on Musculoskeletal
Disorders and the Workplace, 2001). They also found that modification of these factors could substan-
tially reduce the risk for these disorders.
The way work is organized and performed often determines subsequent physical/mechanical and
psychological job demands on individual workers. Work organization also encompasses the organi-
zational practices and production methods that affect job design. These include the temporal aspects
of work (e.g., work–rest schedules, work shifts, hours of work, work pacing), job content (e.g., repeti-
tiveness of tasks, use of skills, vigilance, participation in decision-making), compensation arrangements
(salary, hourly, quota, piece rate), work status (fulltime, part-time, seasonal, temporary), social inter-
actions (isolated, various levels of team work), task (single, rotating, multiple), and opportunities for
development (Kasl, 1992; Sauter and Swanson, 1996). Consideration of work organization provides
information at the group level and enables multilevel analysis. Work organizational observational
exposure assessment methods that are potentially relevant for assessing relationships with musculo-
skeletal disorders include those of Rohmert and Landau (1983), Ergonomic Workplace Analysis
(Ahonen et al., 1996), Meaning of Work (MOW International Research Team, 1987), and the Occu-
pational Stress Index (Belkic et al., 1995).
Typically, physical exposures identified in workplaces include forceful exertions (Stetson et al., 1993;
Fransson-Hall et al., 1996; Roquelaure et al., 1997), such as gripping a high force demanding hand
tool, lifting a heavy object, pushing a fully loaded cart. Non-neutral postures of hands and upper extre-
mities (Frost and Andersen, 1999; Punnett et al., 2000; Viikari-Juntura et al., 2000), such as bending the
wrist when using a hand tool and raising the hand above the head when performing a task, increase force
requirements. Highly repetitive motions of the hand, wrist, and upper arms (Veiersted and Westgaard,
1993; Blanc et al., 1996; Nordstrom et al., 1997; Punnett, 1998) are found in hand-intensive jobs such
as assembly and data entry. Some other physical demand parameters at workplaces include hand–arm
vibration, wearing gloves, and some environmental conditions such as extremely cold or hot tempera-
tures. Work organization parameters such as work methods, social content, and task pacing may also
influence the development of work-related upper extremity disorders.
Different measurement strategies may be used to meet the various needs of the exposure assessments.
For example, most cross-sectional epidemiological studies measure exposure parameters at a certain
point in time, while most prospective epidemiological studies require the quantification of the exposures
for the days, weeks, and years on the job. Thus, data collected for prospective studies should make it pos-
sible to calculate cumulative exposures. For example, a worker performs two different tasks in a workday.
Exposure from both tasks should be measured and the compound exposure for the whole day should be
calculated depending on the task distribution (time spent on the two tasks). If the worker’s exposure is
changed during the course of the study (e.g., job changes), a new exposure measurement should be per-
formed, and the accumulated exposure is then calculated. If the purpose of the measurement is to assess
exposure differences among two or several conditions, measurement can be done for each of the
conditions.
When considering the measurement strategy to be used, one should also consider the three main
dimensions of physical exposure: amplitude, frequency, and duration, rather only one single dimension.
This is because the physiological significance is dependent on the combination of these exposure dimen-
sions. Therefore, exposure quantification should include the measurement of exposure amplitude (e.g.,
level of hand force, degree of a joint angle), exposure frequency (e.g., number of exertions per minute),
and exposure duration (e.g., length of time in hours). Other aspects of physical exposure may also need
to be quantified, such as duty cycle and speed.
After deciding on the measurement strategy, one should consider the selection of exposure quanti-
fication methods to be used. There are numerous exposure quantification methods available.
The Washington State SHARP Approach to Exposure Assessment 44-3
Cost
Capacity
Versatility
Generality
Accuracy
FIGURE 44.1 General characteristics of the three types of exposure quantification methods. (After Winkel, J. and
Mathiassen, S.E. (1994) Ergonomics 37: 979 – 988. With permission.)
However, different methods may have different specificity, validity, and reproducibility. Depending on
the types of methods, they may also require users with varied expertise. The time and cost required
for data collection and analysis could also vary significantly.
Although risk estimation from job titles has been used as the normal exposure quantification in many
epidemiological studies, they may only give vague or invalid exposure assessment in musculoskeletal
epidemiological studies. This is because workers under the same job title could perform very different
activities due to differing technologies and machines used, individual work techniques, and work organ-
izational differences between different companies. Commonly used exposure quantification methods in
work-related musculoskeletal studies can usually be divided into the following three categories: (1) self-
report questionnaires; (2) observational methods; and (3) direct measurement techniques. Each of these
has its own strengths and weaknesses and may be used in different applications depending on the study
purposes. Figure 44.1 illustrates some general characteristics of the different methods, and can serve as a
guide for selection. In general, direct measurement with instrumentations gives the most specific and
accurate exposure estimation, but involves significant costs. This method would be impractical for
individual exposure assessment in very large populations of large-scale epidemiological studies
because of the significant resources and expertise that would be required. Self-reporting using question-
naires or interview methods can access large populations with reasonable cost, but the data, in general,
have low validity with respect to exposure level and variation. Observational methods are usually
considered to be in between the direct and self-reporting methods in terms of the different characteristics.
The following sections discuss some of the commonly used methods of the three groups.
been used with other methods in industry by several researchers with varying levels of success (Baty et al.,
1986; Burdorf and Laan, 1991; Holmstrom et al., 1992; Wiktorin et al., 1993; Punnett, 1998).
Self-report questionnaires tend to be both reproducible and relatively close to observational and direct
measurement for gross levels of activity (Baty et al., 1986; Wiktorin et al., 1993, 1996). Wiktorin et al.
(1996) reported acceptable reproducibility in the 0.5 to 0.8 range for questions relating to overall physical
activity, whole body working postures, and specific leisure activities. Some success has been reported in
subjective estimates of impulses or forces on the hands (Freivalds and Eklund, 1993). However, both
reproducibility and reliability dropped significantly compared to other measurement methods when
specific questions relating to bent postures and levels of loads were asked (Holmstrom et al., 1992;
Wiktorin et al., 1993). Viikari-Juntura et al. (1996) reported moderate correlation (0.42 to 0.55)
between self-reports and observations of physical workload factors such as frequency of manual hand-
ling, duration of trunk flexion, neck rotation, hands above shoulders, and squatting or kneeling. The cor-
relations, in general, were higher for those without low back pain than for those with low back pain. Pope
et al. (1998) compared results from a self-report questionnaire on physical demands (postures, manual
handling, repetitive upper limb movements) to direct simultaneous observations in six different occu-
pational settings. Agreement was good for most of the manual material-handling activities. However,
minutes of repetitive arm and wrist movements appeared to be the least accurate (overestimates).
These authors concluded that dichotomous recall is satisfactory (i.e., ever, never) and that exposure mag-
nitude recall can also be satisfactory for some risk factors.
Toomingas et al. (1997) tested the hypothesis that those who rate health outcomes high on self-reports
would also rate exposures high on self-reports, thereby biasing risk estimates. Conducting separate
analyses by age, gender, and socioeconomic status, correlations were close to zero for fixed and nonfixed
stimuli, including symptoms and physical exposures, indicating no systematic differences by rating
behavior. Punnett (1998) reported consistent findings of good comparability in estimates, when compar-
ing self-reported physical exposures with observations by researchers blinded to health status. Kerr et al.
(2001) reported good agreement between back injury cases and job-matched controls on self-reported
physical demands of the job (ICC ¼ 0.6), suggesting a lack of symptoms-related bias in estimates.
Bernard et al. (1994) compared observational analysis to self-reports of exposure among symptomatic
newspaper workers and referents. Both groups reported a longer duration of typing time (approximately
50% more) than the observational analysis. Similar results by Spielholz et al. (1999) showed consistent
overestimation of upper extremity risk factors by most individuals. These studies indicate that self-
reports may provide valuable information regarding task duration/frequency and whole body postures
but are generally neither accurate nor reliable for measurement of hand/arm exposure to risk factors in
terms of duration and frequency.
physical exposure for the hand and the wrist. The HAL is applicable to single-task jobs, although some
approaches have been attempted to extend its use in multiple-task situations. The RULA method (McA-
tamney and Corlett, 1993) is used to assess the postures of the neck, trunk, and upper limbs, muscular
effort, and the external loads on the body. This postural exposure assessment system has been used in
several different formats and adopted for use in many different types of industries (Lueder, 1996;
Hignett and McAtamney, 2000).
Time-based methods such as OWAS (Karhu et al., 1981), VIRA (Persson and Kilbom, 1983; and
Kilbom et al., 1986), ARBAN (Holzmann, 1982), and PEO (Fransson et al., 1991) require the analyst
to observe the job performance continuously or at specific time samples during the task performance.
The analyst records the exposure changes based on predefined categories, such as, hand with weight
versus hand without weight, and neck flexion between 08 and 208 versus greater than 208. Observations
can be performed on-site with a computer or off-site where video-tapes are analyzed. Advantages of the
time-based methods are that they more closely represent the true exposure during the task performance.
The disadvantage is that it is time consuming and may also limit the number of exposure parameters that
an analyst can observe if the method is used on-site.
Video-based off-site techniques often use categorical scoring of body positions, movement frequency,
type of grip, and force based on either sampled or real-time recording (Karhu et al., 1977; Corlett et al.,
1979; Holzman-Voigt, 1979; Kemmlert and Kilbom, 1986; Keyserling, 1986; Armstrong et al., 1982). The
method employed by Armstrong et al. (1982), for example, sampled postures several times a second and
classified wrist postures into five categories: (1) neutral, (2) flexion, (3) extreme flexion, (4) extension,
and (5) extreme extension. In general, video-based analysis may be the most appropriate observational
method for risk factor quantification and definition of work activities for large-scale epidemiological
studies because it allows the analysts sufficient time to estimate the postures of the various body parts
and provides the possibility to reanalyze the data for quality control purpose.
With the availability of newer computer technologies, time studies of task performance and postural
analysis can now be carried out on computers. A recently developed multimedia video task analysis
(MVTA) system (Yen and Radwin, 1995) is able to set accurate time codes on videotapes and perform
time analysis on various time-based events (e.g., tasks, postures, and hand exertions). With its flexible
design, users can set their own parameters to be studied (e.g., tasks, wrist flexion and extension postures,
hand exertions) and define their own categories of the different parameters (e.g., for the parameter of task
with two levels: computer keyboarding and writing notes; for the parameter of wrist flexion/extension
with four categories: flexion 0 to 308, flexion .308, extension 0 to 308, and extension .308). A drawback
of this type of analysis is that the analyst has no control on the angles of observation, and has to depend
on the quality of the videotapes. Therefore, to obtain reliable and adequate exposure information, it is
important to take good-quality video. Another disadvantage with the computer-based observation
systems is that one cannot obtain direct measurements such as object weight and forces required to
operate a tool while the analyst is sitting in his or her laboratory. In contrast, when the observation is
done on-site, the analyst can most often communicate or interact with the operator to obtain the infor-
mation. Therefore, if the analysis is performed off-site, it is important to obtain the required information
on-site and be prepared for use in the off-site analysis.
Falling within the scope of observational field methods are methods based on workloads. These
methods define a system of quantifying an overall load score (Helliwell et al., 1992) or classify
workers into classes based on work levels (Nathan et al., 1993). The Strain Index developed by Moore
and Garg (1995) identified six risk factors, each given a categorical 1 to 5 score, that give an overall sever-
ity index (SI) score when multiplied together. This tool has been used in meatpacking and has shown
data that support its validity in predicting morbidity (Moore and Garg, 1995). Although the Strain
Index method was originally designed for single-task jobs, the authors have made attempts to extend
this method to multiple-task jobs.
Force quantification often presents a problem in observational methods. Hand force cannot be seen.
Consequently, it must be estimated, which can be achieved using several methods. A simple dichotomous
classification of either high or low force has commonly been used, typically using manipulation of a
44-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
4-kg object or its equivalent force for power grip or 1-kg object or its equivalent force for pinch grip as
the determinants of class (Silverstein et al., 1987; Stetson et al., 1991). Several researchers have used a
modified Borg scale, which classifies an expert estimate on a 10-point categorical scale ranging from
zero to maximal exertion (Borg et al., 1985; Lloyd et al., 1991). Another approach is to estimate the
tendon force based on hand geometry, assumed friction, and object weight (Helliwell et al., 1992).
Despite the provision of an actual force value, this method relies on the estimation of every factor
and may not be any more reliable than scaling techniques. A continuous method used by Latko
(1997) employs expert consensus rating of average and peak force on 10-cm visual –analog scales. Repro-
ducibility estimates of this method have been between 0.6 and 0.8, showing promise as a continuous
scaling method.
Psychophysical studies use a subject’s perception of sensation to measure a factor of interest. This has
been applied in the field of exposure observation. Snook et al. (1995) developed guidelines for hand/
wrist flexion and extension based on psychophysical studies. Analysts observe the hand/wrist postures
during task performance and give subjective ratings on the postures. Previously, Snook (1978) also
used perception of object weight to develop acceptable guidelines for lifting based on location, lift
frequency, and weight. A more recent method developed by Latko et al. (1997) employed expert
group rating of physical components of work on visual –analog scales. This study evaluated the use of
rating several risk factor exposure metrics on continuous visual –analog line scales. The technique
shows great promise in terms of reproducibility and reliability of quantifying hand activities (Latko
et al., 1997). These scales have been incorporated into primary measurement methods of the American
Conference of Government Industrial Hygienists (ACGIH, 2001) TLV on hand activity.
Although there have been major advances in observational methods and they have been widely used in
musculoskeletal epidemiological studies, some drawbacks exist. One of the major drawbacks is that the
observational methods are based on the subjective judgment of the individual analyst. Some variations
within and between analysts are unavoidable. Measures to reduce such variation should be taken.
Another common problem associated with observational methods in epidemiological studies is that
different researchers have used different predefined exposure categories. This makes it difficult to
compare results from different studies.
Several researchers have evaluated electrogoniometers and the introduction of significant cross-talk
in flexion/extension measurements and deviation measurements from extreme forearm rotation
(Armstrong et al., 1993; Smutz et al., 1994; Buchholz and Wellman, 1997; Roberts, 1997).
Researchers have developed procedures that may reduce errors caused by cross talk (e.g., Smutz et al.,
1994; Buchholz and Wellman, 1997; Roberts, 1997). These results show promise for the use of an electro-
goniometer and electrotorsiometer in tandem to measure motions and allow for error correction.
Researchers appear to agree that an electrotorsiometer and electrogoniometer can be used in a telemetric
system to perform angle measurements with errors less than 58 (Armstrong et al., 1993; Smutz et al.,
1994; Buchholz and Wellman, 1997).
Force exerted by muscle groups is commonly measured by force transducers placed in the line of action
or by the use of surface electromyography (EMG) (Armstrong and Chaffin, 1979). Force transducers may
provide accurate information if specific conditions exist where their placement does not affect the work.
EMG has become relatively easy to perform in the field with the use of disposable surface electrodes
and portable measurement devices (Winkel and Gard, 1988; Hägg et al., 1997). Typically, EMG data may
be measured and analyzed for either physical signs of fatigue or for comparison of static force levels
(NIOSH, 1992). Electrodes transmit motor unit action potentials from the underlying muscles. These
signals, when root-mean-square (RMS) transformed or integrated, have shown a linear or exponential
relationship (r 2 . 0.90) to developed static force (NIOSH, 1992).
Direct estimation, an alternative to EMG for calculation of hand force, can be classified as a direct
measurement method. Field practitioners and consultants commonly use this method to obtain job
force requirements. Direct estimation can be done simply by measuring the force requirements of a
tool or piece of equipment with a force gauge. When this is not possible, estimation of force can also
be accomplished through the reproduction of the exertion on a force gauge in the same orientation
and type of grip as performed by a worker. Kingdon and Wells (2000) conducted a laboratory study
on the accuracy of matching a manual gripping force using a hand dynamometer. Initial findings
have shown that force matching may be relatively accurate and consistent at lower force levels. A
study published by the Safety & Health Assessment & Research for Prevention (SHARP) program on
113 government workers showed that with the use of a hand dynamometer, the force matching
method can be quite accurate and consistent in estimations of power grip force and pinch grip force
(Bao and Silverstein, 2005). These results support the use of this method as an alternative to more
time-consuming and expensive instrumentation techniques for quantifying hand force levels in large
epidemiological studies.
In conclusion, no method is perfect and different methods may be used in different situations for
different purposes. Direct measurement using current techniques represent the most accurate and
reliable exposure assessment method. Work by Spielholz et al. (1999) comparing self-reports, video
observation, and direct measurement showed that video observation may have approximately 30%
more error than direct measurement in some risk factor measurements. Direct measurement would
be the preferred method given unlimited resources; however, modern video observation techniques
have the advantage of providing larger numbers of evaluated participants due to less time-consuming
data collection and analysis.
Due to the population size requirement of most epidemiological studies, direct measurement of all
participants would require resources well beyond what is available from a granting agency. Video-
based observational assessment in combination with direct measurement and estimation of forces is
the only method that would allow measurement of all participants with an acceptable level of accuracy
and reliability. Additionally, discrimination calculations by Spielholz et al. (1999) show that the estimated
tenfold increase in number of measured participants (100 to 1000) possible with video-based techniques
over direct measurement will give a more accurate exposure assessment at the group level despite the
increased measurement error. For these reasons, video-based observation and direct force estimation
techniques used previously by the SHARP (1999) program were chosen as the primary exposure asses-
sment method in a large prospective study of upper extremity musculoskeletal disorders conducted by
SHARP (referred to as the SHARP Study in the subsequent text).
44-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
sampling practice for noncyclic tasks is to take a number of random samples during the task performance
period.
The job sampling approach used in the SHARP Study is based on whether the job is a single-task job or
a multiple-task job, and the tasks are cyclic or noncyclic (Figure 44.2). The first step is to determine
whether the job is a single-task job or a multiple-task job and, secondly, to determine if each task is
cyclic or noncyclic. For a cyclic single-task job, a continuous 15-min job sample is taken for the exposure
measurement purpose. For a noncyclic single-task job, three 5-min job samples are taken randomly
during the workday. This attempts to obtain good equitable exposure measures for this type of job
and capture the fluctuations of the exposure during a workday. The total job sample length for a
single task job is 15 min for both cyclic and noncyclic task jobs.
For multiple-task jobs (more than one task is performed during a workday), a different job sampling
method is used for both cyclic and noncyclic tasks. A 10-min job sample is taken for each of the cyclic
tasks and two 5-min job samples are randomly taken from each of the noncyclic tasks in a multiple-task
Cyclic task
Noncyclic task
max:
max: 4 × 10
4 ×2 ×5 min
min
recording
recording
FIGURE 44.2 Job sampling strategies depending on the type of tasks and jobs.
44-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
job. Depending on the number of tasks, the total length of the job samples is longer in a multiple-task job
than that in a single-task job.
For practical purposes, it may be necessary to limit the number of multiple-task jobs in a large epide-
miological study as the amount of time needed for data collection, processing, and analysis is much
longer in multiple-task jobs than that in single-task jobs. In many industries where manual activities
are dominant, a common practice is to rotate workers between different tasks. Rotations commonly
occur at break times. In the SHARP Study, jobs with more than four tasks were excluded.
interviewing workers and supervisors. Jobs are then video filmed according to the job sampling strategy
discussed previously. Two synchronized cameras are used in order to capture both sides of the body while
the worker is performing tasks. The camera crews should be well coordinated so that when the worker
moves the cameras should be moved accordingly in order for at least one camera to capture both sides of
the body. This will help the off-site data processing in the laboratory.
During the observation period, forces applied in the task are noted and later measured. As it is not
feasible to measure all forces that the worker applies in the task, a subjective determination of “significant
force” is made. Operationally, when one of the ergonomists considers that the force is obvious and may
be of importance to the exposure, the force data will be collected. This is similar to most ergonomics
consultations where an ergonomist takes measurements he or she thinks necessary. Conceptually, a “sig-
nificant force” is defined as a lifting force of 0.9 kg, a pinch grip force of 0.9 kg, a power grip force of
4.4 kg, and a push/pull force of 4.4 kg. The force value is not known until measured. Therefore, in
practice, forces that are lower than the defined levels are sometimes measured. A lifting force is measured
by the object weight. This is typically measured by using a force gauge or a weight scale. Object weights
can also be obtained from the company. A push/pull force is also measured using a force gauge. For prac-
tical purposes, no distinction is made between push and pull forces, though they may have different phys-
iological impacts. Additionally, both lifting weight and push/pull force are also estimated by an
ergonomist using a 1 to 10 rating scale. A pinch or power grip hand force is measured using the force
matching method (Bao and Silverstein, 2005). This is done by asking a subject to recreate the amount
of force he or she uses in the task on a force dynanometer using similar hand/wrist postures. This
process is repeated three times, and the median of the three is used in the analysis. Borg ratings by sub-
jects and researchers are also collected for force applications (CR-10, Borg, 1982). Different measurement
methods are used for the same exposure parameters in order to study the differences and similarities
between the different methods.
Other observed parameters, such as the HAL, and parameters for computing the Strain Index (i.e.,
duration of exertion, efforts/min, hand/wrist posture, speed of work, and duration per day) are also col-
lected during the on-site data collection period. This allows several event-based exposure estimations to
be made.
If the worker uses vibrating tools, the tool information is collected. This is used to crudely estimate
vibration exposure to the worker.
It is important to ensure that the data collection process of the on-site analysis does not interfere with
the normal performance of the task. Some workers may have the tendency to modify their performance
in front of video cameras. This must be discouraged. At the end of the data collection, it is also important
to check the completeness of the data and be sure all data are collected properly.
With the time-study report, a summary statistics report can be produced where average duration, per-
centage of time, and frequency of the various significant forces can be calculated as follows:
P
Duration of individual exertions
Average duration (sec) ¼
Total number of exertion cycles
P
Duration of individual exertions
% Time ¼ 100%
Total recording time
Total number of exertions
Frequency of exertion (times=min) ¼
Total recording time
When the worker is involved in the use of vibrating tools during task performance, the vibration
exposure needs to be quantified. Theoretically, vibration levels should be measured during task perform-
ance. However, vibration measurement requires sophisticated instrumentation and enormous resources.
In the SHARP Study, it is not possible to perform accurate vibration measurements while also collecting a
large amount of other physical exposure parameters for each of the subjects. The alternative is to obtain
the declared vibration values of the tools that the workers use, and then perform a time study on the
video recordings to measure the actual time that the vibration tools are activated. This estimation
may not reflect the real vibration exposure of the workers, as other factors such as tool balancing,
work surface conditions, and individual work techniques can influence the true vibration level, but it
can give an estimation of the vibration exposure.
TABLE 44.2 Predefined Angular Categories for the Different Body Parts
Trunk Trunk flexion–extension Trunk lateral flexion Trunk twisting
,08 (extension) 0 to 108 0 to 108
0 to 208 (flexion) 10 to 308 10 to 458
20 to 608 .308 .458
.608
Neck Neck flexion–extension Neck lateral flexion Neck twisting
,08 (extension) 0 to 108 0 to 108
0 to 208 (flexion) 10 to 308 10 to 458
.208 .308 .458
Upper arms Upper arm flexion –extension Upper arm abduction–adduction Upper arm rotation
,08 (extension) ,08 (adduction) ,08 (outward)
0 to 208 (flexion) 0 to 308 (abduction) 0 to 158 (inward)
20 to 458 30 to 608 15 to 458
45 to 908 60 to 908 .458
.908 .908
Shoulders and Shoulder raise Arm supported Elbow flexion
elbows Yes or no Yes or no ,08 (extension)
0 to 208 (flexion)
20 to 608
60 to 1008
.1008
Forearms and Forearm rotation Wrist flexion–extension Wrist ulnar–radial deviation
wrists 2180 to 2908 (supernation) ,2458 (extension) ,2158 (radial)
2 90 to 08 (supernation) 245 to 2158 215 to 258
0 to 908 (pronation) 215 to 08 25 to 08
90 to 1808 (pronation) 0 to 158 (flexion) 0 to 108 (ulnar)
15 to 458 10 to 208
.458 .208
analyst may assign the worse posture category to the subject when he or she thinks the job is hazardous,
or vice versa. To overcome this problem, in the SHARP Study, a continuous angular scale was used
during data processing (posture estimate) and then the data were categorized later during the analysis.
In order to make the continuous scale for the posture estimate, a special data processing program has
been created. One of the data processing screens is shown in Figure 44.6. In this screen the worker is
shown from two camera angles (pictures are just for illustrative purpose in the figure and does not
represent the actual analysis) at a preselected video frame. The analyst observes the posture and estimates
the approximate locations of the body parts by clicking on the posture diagrams. The continuous angle
data are automatically entered into a database.
From the raw posture data, posture distributions can be computed based on pre-defined angle
categories (e.g., Job A: 15% of time wrist posture is .458 extended, 67% of time wrist posture is
Job B (N = 235)
Job A (N = 216)
0% 10% 20% 30% 40% 50% 60% 70% 80% 90% 100%
FIGURE 44.6 Time-based posture analysis using continuous angular scales (data do not represent the actual angle
measurements).
between 458 extension and 158 flexion, and 18% of time wrist posture is .158 flexion; Job B: 8% of time
wrist posture is .458 extended, 82% of time wrist posture is between 458 extension and 158 flexion, and
10% of time wrist posture is .158 flexion). Depending on the epidemiological analysis needs, alterna-
tively, the descriptive statistics for individual subjects such as mean, median, 95% percentile of the pos-
tures can also be calculated.
or
assessment. Prior to specific physical job demand parameter observations, supervisors and workers are
queried about the number and type of tasks and their usual duration, rotation patterns, and upset
conditions. The ergonomist observes the department that the worker is in for environmental factors,
demographic segregation, work method, social content, pacing, positioning, preparation for action, flexi-
bility, attentiveness demands (adapted from EWA; Ahonen et al., 1996), responsibility for the safety of
FIGURE 44.8 A sample of work organization checklist used in the SHARP study.
44-18 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
others, and job content. During subsequent visits, changes to the work organization are documented
using the checklist.
The annual worker questionnaire interview inquires about duration on the current job, shift, hours and
days of work, overtime schedules, the number of days off in the last week and month, changes in the pre-
vious 4 months in tools/equipment, parts made, workstation/area, tasks, rotation pattern, pace. Changes
in these parameters are queried every 4 months. Annual self-administered questionnaires include ques-
tions about job demands, decision latitude, social support, job satisfaction, job security, and motivation.
The Washington State SHARP Approach to Exposure Assessment 44-19
It should be noted that there is high correlation between this observational assessment of work organi-
zation and worker assessment of psychosocial demands. There is also high correlation between job
content identified on this checklist and HAL.
44.4 Summary
The SHARP Study uses a variety of exposure assessment approaches at the individual level and the more
global level, incorporating worker estimates (force matching, job content, psychosocial demands, social
support, work scheduling), direct measurement (significant push/pull, lifting, pinch and grip forces),
observational methods of physical demands (upper extremity postures and motions in terms of ampli-
tude, frequency, and duration), and departmental-level measures of work organization. These data
collection methods allow us to input variables into existing event-based and time-based exposure assess-
ment methods (RULA, REBA, HAL, Strain Index) as well as to add precision to estimates of risk based on
more quantitative time-based methods. It is hoped that these detailed exposure assessment methods used
by ourselves and others in prospective studies will ultimately lead to more efficient and easy-to-use
exposure assessment methods for practitioners.
References
ACGIH (2001) Hand activity level. In TLVs and BEIs — Threshold Limit Values for Chemical Substances
and Physical Agents. Cincinnati, OH: ACGIH.
Ahonen, A., Launis, M., and Kuorinka, T. (1996) Ergonomic Workplace Analysis. Helsinki, Finland:
Finnish Institute of Occupational Health.
Armstrong, T. J. and Chaffin, D. B. (1979) Carpal tunnel syndrome and selected personal attributes,
J Occup Med 21 (7): 481 –486.
Armstrong, T. J., Dunnigan, J., Ulin, S., and Foulke, J. (1993) Evaluation of a biaxial flexible wire
electrogoniometer for measurement of wrist posture. Paper presented at the 24th Congress of
the International Commission on Occupational Health.
Armstrong, T. J., Foulke, J. A., Joseph, B. S., and Goldstein, S. A. (1982) Investigation of cumulative
trauma disorders in a poultry processing plant, Am Ind Hyg Assoc J 43 (2): 103 –116.
Bao, S. and Silverstein, B. (2005) Estimation of hand force in ergonomic job evaluations, Ergonomics 48:
288–301.
Baty, D., Buckle, P. N., and Stubbs, D. A. (1986) Posture recording by direct observation, questionnaire
assessment and instrumentation: a comparison based on a recent field study. In The Ergonomics of
Working Postures. Edited by N. Corlett and J. Wilson. London: Taylor & Francis.
Belkic, K., Savic, C., Theorell, T., and Cizinsky, S. (1995) Work Stressors and Cardiovasuclar Risk: Assess-
ment for Clinical Practice. Part I. Stockholm (Sweden): Stress Research Reports, National Institute
for Psychosocial Factors and Health. Section for Stress Research, Karolinska Institute, WHO
Psychosocial Center.
Bernard, B. P., Sauter, S. L., Fine, L. J., Petersen, M., and Hales, T. R. (1994) Job task and psychosocial risk
factors for work-related musculoskeletal disorders among newspaper employees, Scand J Work
Environ Health 20: 417 –426.
Blanc, P., Faucett, J., Kennedy, J. J., Cisternas, M., and Yelin, E. (1996) Self-reported carpal tunnel syn-
drome: predictors of work disability form the National Health Interview Survey Occupational
Health Supplement, Am J Ind Med 30 (3): 362– 368.
Borg, G., Ljundggren, G., and Ceci, R. (1985) The increase of perceived exertion, aches and pain in the
legs, heart rate and blood lactate during exercise on a bicycle ergometer, Eur J Appl Physiol 54:
343–349.
Borg, G. A. V. (1982) Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion, Med Sci Sports Exer 14 (5): 377 –381.
Buchholz, B. and Wellman, H. (1997) Practical operation of a biaxial goniometer at the wrist joint,
Hum Factors 39: 119 –129.
Burdorf, A. and Laan, J. (1991) Comparison of methods for the assessment of postural load on the back,
Scand J Work Environ Health 17: 425 –429.
44-20 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Corlett, E. N., Madeley, S. J., and Manenica, I. (1979) Posture targetting: a technique for recording
working postures, Ergonomics 22 (3): 357 –366.
Fallentin, N., Juul-Kristensen, B., Mikkelsen, S., Andersen, J. H., Bonde, J. P., Frost, P., and Endahl, L.
(2001) Physical exposure assessment in monotonous repetitive work — the PRIM study, Scand
J Work Environ Health 27 (1): 21 –29.
Fransson, C., Gloria, R., Kilbom, Å., Karlqvist, L., Nygård, C-H., Wiktorin, C., Winkel, J., and the
Stockholm MUSIC I Study Group. (1991) Presentation and evaluation of a portable ergonomic
observation method (PEO). In 11th Congress International Ergonomics Association: Designing for
Everyone. Edited by Y. Quéinnec and Daniellou. London: Taylor & Francis.
Fransson-Hall, C., Bystrom, S., and Kilbom, A. (1996) Characteristics of forearm-hand exposure in
relation to symptoms among automobile assembly line workers, Am J Ind Med 29 (1): 15 –22.
Freivalds, A. and Eklund, J. (1993) Reaction torques and operator stress while using powered nutrunners,
Appl Ergon 24 (3): 158 –164.
Frost, P. and Andersen, J. H. (1999) Shoulder impingement syndrome in relation to shoulder intensive
work, Occup Environ Med 56 (7): 494 –498.
Helliwell, P. S., Mumford, D. B., Smeathers, J. E., and Wright, V. (1992) Work related upper limb dis-
order: the relationship between pain, cumulative load, disability, and psychological factors, Ann
Rheum Dis 51: 1325 –1329.
Hignett, S. and McAtamney, L. (2000) Rapid entire body assessment (REBA), Appl Ergon 31:
201– 205.
Holmstrom, E. B., Lindell, J., and Mortiz, U. (1992) Low back and neck/shoulder pain in construction
workers: occupational workload and psychosocial risk factors Part 1: relationship to low back pain,
Spine 17 (6): 663 –671.
Holzman-Voigt, P. (1979) ARBAN: a method for ergonomic analysis of work sites, Arh Hig Rada Toksikol
30: 82– 86.
Holzmann, P. (1982) ARBAN — a new method for analysis of ergonomic effort, Appl Ergon 13 (2): 82 –86.
Hägg, G. M., Oster, J., and Bystrom, S. (1997) Forearm muscular load and wrist angle among automobile
assembly line workers in relation to symptoms, Appl Ergon 28 (1): 41– 47.
Karhu, O., Härkönen, R., Sorvali, P., and Vespsäläijnen, P. (1981) Observing working postures in indus-
try: examples of OWAS application, Appl Ergon 12: 13– 17.
Karhu, O., Kansi, P., and Kuorinka, I. (1977) Correcting working postures in industry: a practical method
for analysis, Appl Ergon 8 (4): 199–201.
Kasl, S. V. (1992) Surveillance of psychological disorders in the workplace panel. In Work and Well-being:
An Agenda for the 1990s. Edited by G. P. Keita, and S. L. Sauter. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association, pp. 73 –95.
Kemmlert, K. and Kilbom, Å. (1986) Method for the Identification of Musculo-skeletal Stress Factors which
may Have Injurious Effects — PLIBEL. Solna, Sweden: National Board of Occupational Safety and
Health, Research Department, Work Physiology Unit.
Kerr, M. S., Frank, J. W., Shannon, H. S., Norman, R. W., Wells, R. P., Neumann, W. P., and Bombardier, C.
(2001) Biomechanical and psychosocial risk factors for low back pain at work, Am J Public Health
91 (7): 1069 –1075.
Keyserling, W. M. (1986) Postural analysis of the trunk and shoulders in simulated real time, Ergonomics
29: 569– 583.
Kilbom, Å., Persson, J., and Jonsson, B. (1986) Risk factors for work-related disorders of the neck and
shoulder with special emphasis on working postures and movements. Edited by N. Corlett,
J. Wilson, and I. Manenica. London: Taylor & Francis.
Kingdon, K. and Wells, R. (2000) Accuracy of force matching using a hand dynamometer, Unpublished
work from University of Waterloo, Ontario, Canada.
Latko, W. (1997) Development and evaluation of an observational method for quantifying exposure to hand
activity and other physical stressors in manual work, Ph.D. Dissertation, The University of Michigan.
The Washington State SHARP Approach to Exposure Assessment 44-21
Latko, W. A., Armstrong, R. J., Foulke, J. A., Herrin, G. D., Ranbourn, R. A., and Ulin, S. S. (1997) Devel-
opment and evaluation of an observation method for assessing repetition in hand tasks, Am Ind
Hyg Assoc J 58 (4): 278–285.
Lloyd, A., Gandevia, S., and Hales, J. (1991) Muscle performance, voluntary activation, twitch properties
and perceived effort in normal subjects and patients with the chronic fatigue syndrome, Brain 114:
85–98.
Lueder, R. (1996) A proposed RULA for computer users. Proceedings of the Ergonomics Summer Work-
shop, August 8–9, UC Berkeley Center for Occupational and Environmental Health.
Marras, W. S. and Schoenmarklin, R. W. (1993) Wrist motions in industry, Ergonomics 36: 341 –351.
McAtamney, L. and Corlett, E. N. (1993) RULA: a survey method for the investigation of work-related
upper limb disorders, Appl Ergon 24: 91 –99.
Moore, J. S. and Garg, A. (1995) The strain index: a proposed method to analyze jobs for risk of distal
upper extremity disorders. Am Ind Hyg Assoc J 56: 443 –458.
Moore, A., Wells, R., and Ranney, D. (1991) Quantifying exposure in occupational manual task with
cumulative trauma disorder potential, Ergonomics 34 (12): 1433 –1453.
MOW International Research Team (1987) The Meaning of Working. London: Academic Press.
Nathan, P. A., Keniston, R. C., Meadows, K. D., and Lockwood, R. S. (1993) Validation of occupational
hand use categories, J Occup Med 35: 1034 –1042.
National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH) (1992) Selected Topics in Surface Electro-
myography for Use in the Occupational Setting: Expert Perspectives. DHHS (NIOSH) Publication
No. 91 –100.
National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH) (1997) Musculoskeletal Disorders and
Workplace Factors: A Critical Review of Epidemiologic Evidence for Work-Related Musculoskeletal
Disorders of the Neck, Upper Extremity, and Low Back. DHHS (NIOSH).
National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH) (1994) Revised NIOSH Lifting Equation.
Cincinnati, OH: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service, Centers
for Disease Control, National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health.
Niebel, B. W. (1988) Motion and Time Study, 8th edn. Homewood, IL: Irwin.
Nordstrom, D. L., Vierkant, R. A., DeStefano, F., and Layde, P. M. (1997) Risk factors for carpal tunnel
syndrome in a general population, Occup Environ Med 54: 734 –740.
Panel on Musculoskeletal Disorders and the Workplace; Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and
Education; National Research Council, and Institute of Medicine (2001) Musculoskeletal Disorders
and the Workplace: Low Back and Upper Extremities. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Persson, J. and Kilbom, Å. (1983) VIRA — en enkel videofilmteknik för registrering och analys av arbets-
ställningar och rörelser, Vol. 10, 23 pp.
Pope, D. P., Silman, A. J., Cherry, N. M., Pritchard, C., and Macfarlane, G. J. (1998) Validity of self-completed
questionnaire measuring the physical demands of work, Scand J Work Environ Health 24 (5): 376–385.
Punnett, L. (1998) Ergonomic stressors and upper extremity disorders in vehicle manufacturing: cross
sectional exposure–response trends, Occup Environ Med 55: 414 –420.
Punnett, L., Fine, L. J., Keyserling, W. M., Herrin, G. D., and Chaffin, D. B. (2000) Shoulder disorders and
postural stress in automobile assembly work, Scand J Work Environ Heal 26 (4): 283–291.
Radwin, R. G., Lin, M. L., and Yen, T. Y. (1993) Exposure assessment of biomechanical stress in repetitive
manual work using spectral analysis. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics
Society 37th Annual Meeting, pp. 669–693.
Roberts, R. (1997) Calibration procedure for the Penny and Giles Z110 torsiometer. University of
Massachusetts Lowell, unpublished master’s project.
Rohmert, W. and Landau, K. (1983) A New Technique for Job Analysis. London: Taylor & Francis.
Roquelaure, Y., Mechali, S., Dano, C., Fanello, S., Benetti, F., Bureau, D., Mariel, J., Martin, Y.-H.,
Derriennic, F., and Penneau-Fontbonne, D. (1997) Occupational and personal risk factors for
carpal tunnel syndrome in industrial workers, Scand J Work Environ Health 23: 364 –369.
44-22 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Sauter, S. L. and Swanson, N. G. (1996) An ecological model of musculoskeletal disorders in office work.
In Beyond Biomechanics: Psychosocial Aspects of Musculoskeletal Disorders in Office Work. Edited by
S. D. Moon and S. L. Sauter. London: Taylor & Francis.
SHARP (1999) Ergonomics evaluation report on a trimmer operator’s job. Technical Report 22-4-1999.
Washington State Department of Labor and Industries, Olympia, WA.
Silverstein, B., Fine, L. J., and Armstrong, T. J. (1987) Occupational factors and carpal tunnel syndrome,
Am J Ind Med 11: 343 –358.
Smutz, P., Serina, E., and Rempel, D. (1994) A system for evaluating the effect of keyboard design on
force, posture, comfort, and productivity, Ergonomics 37 (10): 1649 –1660.
Snook, S. H. (1978) The design of manual handling tasks, Ergonomics 21 (12): 963 –985.
Snook, S. H., Vaillancourt, D. R., Ciriello, V. M., and Webster, B. S. (1995) Psychophysical studies of
repetitive wrist flexion and extension, Ergonomics 38 (7): 1488–1507.
Spielholz, P., Silverstein, B. A., and Stuart, M. (1999) Reproducibility of a self-report questionnaire for
upper extremity musculoskeletal disorder risk factors, Appl Ergon 30: 429–433.
Stetson, D. S., Keyserling, W. M., Silverstein, B. A., and Leonard, J. A. (1991) Observational analysis of
the hand and wrist: a pilot study, Appl Occup Environ Hyg 6 (11): 927 –937.
Stetson, D. S., Silverstein, B. A., Keyserling, W. M., Wolfe, R. A., and Albers, J. W. (1993) Median sensory
distal amplitude and latency: comparisons between nonexposed managerial/professional employ-
ees and industrial workers, Am J Ind Med 24: 175 –189.
Toomingas, A., Theorell, T., Michelsen, H., Nordemar, R., and Stockholm MUSIC I Study Group. (1997)
Associations between self-rated psychosocial work conditions and musculoskeletal symptoms and
signs, Scand J Work Environ Health 23: 130–139.
Veiersted, K. B. and Westgaard, R. H. (1993) Development of trapezius myalgia among female workers
performing light manual work, Scand J Work Environ Health 19: 277–283.
Viikari-Juntura, E., Martikainen, R., Luukkonen, R., Mutanen, P., Takala, E., and Riihimaki, H. (2000) A
longitudinal study of work-related and individual risk factors of radiating neck pain. People and
Work Research Report, Helsinki, Finland.
Viikari-Juntura, E., Rauas, S., Marikainen, R., Kuosma, E., Riihimaki, H., Takala, E., and Saarenmaa, K.
(1996) Validity of self-reported physical work load in epidemiologic studies on musculoskeletal
disorders, Scand J Work Environ Health 22: 251 –259.
Volpert, W., Kötter, W., Gohde, H.-E., and Weber, W. G. (1989) Psychological evaluation and design of
work tasks: two examples, Ergonomics 32 (7): 881 –890.
Wiktorin, C., Karlqvist, L., Winkel, J., and Stockholm MUSIC I Study Group, (1993) Validity of self-
reported exposures to work postures and manual materials handling, Scand J Work Environ
Health 19: 208 –214.
Wiktorin, C., Selin, K., Ekenvall, L., Kilbom, A., and Afredsson, L. (1996) Evaluation of perceived and self-
reported manual forces exerted in occupational materials handling, Appl Ergon 27 (4): 231–239.
Winkel, J. and Gard, G. (1988) An EMG-study of work methods and equipment in crane coupling as a
basis for job redesign, Appl Ergon 19 (3): 178– 184.
Winkel, J. and Mathiassen, S. E. (1994) Assessment of physical work load in epidemiologic studies: con-
cepts, issues and operational considerations, Ergonomics 37: 979 –988.
Yen, T. Y. and Radwin, R. G. (1995) A video-based system for acquiring biomechanical data synchronized
wit arbitrary events and activities, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering 42 (9): 944 –948.
45
Upper Extremity
Analysis of the Wrist
45-1
45-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Middle finger
Ring finger III
IV Index finger
Little finger II
V Distal phalanx
Distal interphalangeal
(DIP) joint Medial phalanx
Proximal interphalangeal
(PIP) joint Proximal phalanx
I Thumb
Metacarpophalangeal
(MCP) joint
Metacarpals
Ulna Lunate
Radius
FIGURE 45.1 Bones and joints of the right hand (palmar view).
articulation with the base of the corresponding phalanges. Interosseous muscles and extensor tendons
run along the concave side and the large and smooth dorsal surface of the shaft, respectively, while
the distal ends have a grooved volar surface for the flexor tendons (Nordin and Frankel, 2001).
There are three phalanges for each digit and two for the thumb for a total of 14 bones. They are labeled
proximal, middle, and distal phalanges (with the middle one missing in the thumb), according to their
positions and become progressively smaller. The heads of the proximal and middle phalanges are bicon-
dylar, facilitating flexion, extension, and circumduction. The shafts are semicircular in cross section (the
palmar surface being almost flat), as opposed to the cylindrical metacarpals. The axes of the distal pha-
langes of the index, ring, and little fingers are respectively deviated ulnarly, radially, and radially from the
axes of the middle phalanges (Gigis and Kuczynski, 1982).
(Figure 45.1). The CMC joints are formed by the bases of the four metacarpals and the distal carpal bones
and are stabilized by interosseous ligaments to form a relatively immobile joint. However, a major
function of the CMC joint is to form the hollow of the palm and allow the hand and digits to
conform to the shape of the object being handled (Norkin and Levangie, 1992).
The MCP joints are composed of the convex metacarpal head and the concave base of the proximal
phalanx and stabilized by a joint capsule and ligaments. Flexion of 908 and extension of 208– 308 from
neutral take place in the sagittal plane. The range of flexion differs among fingers (and individuals)
with the index finger having the smallest flexion angle of 708 and the little finger showing the largest
angle of 958 (Batmanabane and Malathi, 1985). Radial and ulnar deviation of approximately 408–608
occurs in the frontal plane, with the index finger showing up to 608 abduction and adduction, middle
and ring fingers up to 458, and the little finger about 508 of mostly abduction (Steindler, 1955). The
range of motion at the MCP joint decreases as the flexion angle increases because of the bicondylar meta-
carpal structure (Youm et al., 1978; Schultz et al. 1987). There is also some axial rotation of the fingers
from a pronated to a supinated position as the fingers are extended. In the reverse motion, the fingers
crowd together as they go into flexion (Steindler, 1955).
The IP joints, being hinge joints, exhibit only flexion and extension. Each finger has two IP joints, the
PIP and the DIP joints, except the thumb, which has only one. Volar and collateral ligaments, connected
with expansion sheets of the extensor tendons, prevent any side to side motion. The largest flexion range
of 1008 –1108 is found in the PIP joints; while a smaller flexion range of 608 –708 is found in the DIP
joints. Hyperextension or extension beyond the neutral position, due to ligament laxity, can also be
found in both DIP and PIP joints (Steindler, 1955).
(a) (b)
Medial head of
triceps brachii Medial Brachialis
Biceps brachii Epicondyle
of humerus
Tendon of
biceps brachii
Pronator teres
Supinator
Brachioradialis
Extensor carpi Extensor carpi
radials longus radialis longus
Flexor carpi radialis
FIGURE 45.2 Anterior muscles of the right hand: (a) superficial layer, (b) middle layer. (Adapted from Spence, A.P.
(1990). Basic Human Anatomy, 3rd ed. Redwood City, CA: Benjamin/Cummings. With permission.)
(0.9–3.4%), while the FDP tendons provide a relatively constant force contribution to each finger
(2.7–3.4%). This results in a relatively large range of force ratios, from 1.5 to 3. Average FDP resting
tendon fiber length is slightly shorter than for the average FDS tendon. Table 45.3 provides a
summary of FDP and FDS tendon characteristics.
The FDP originates from the proximal anterior and medial surface of the ulna and inserts into the base
of the distal phalanx (Figure 45.5). In the midforearm, the muscle divides into two bellies: the radial and
the ulnar. The radial part inserts into the index finger, while the ulnar part inserts into middle, ring, and
little fingers. Consequently, the latter three fingers tend to move together, while the index finger can func-
tion independently of the others. The FDP tendon passes along the finger through a series of pulleys,
which maintain a reasonably constant moment arm for flexing or extending the finger. Before inserting
into the distal phalanx, the FDP passes through a split in the FDS tendon (Fahrer, 1971; Steinberg, 1992;
Brand and Hollister, 1993).
(a) (b)
Olecranon
process Brachioradialis
of ulna
Lateral Extensor carpi
Head of radius epicondyle radialis longus
of humerus
Anconeus Anconeus
Extensor
Supinator digiti minimi
Extensor carpi
radialis brevis
Abductor
pollicis Flexor Extensor
longus Carpi ulnaris digitorum
Extensor Abductor
pollicis Extensor pollicis longus
brevis carpi ulnaris Extensor
Extensor pollicis brevis
pollicis Tendons of
longus Extensor
extensor carpi
pollicis longus
Extensor radialis brevis
indicis and longus
Tendon of
extensor
digitorum
FIGURE 45.3 Posterior muscles of the right hand: (a) deep layer, (b) superficial layer. (Adapted from Spence,
A.P. (1990). Basic Human Anatomy, 3rd ed. Redwood City, CA: Benjamin/Cummings. With permission.)
A5 pulleys are located at the palmar surface of the MCP, PIP, and DIP joints (Figure 45.6). The A2 and
A4 pulleys are most important for normal function and a stable joint, with the A3 and other pulleys
coming into play when the A2 and A4 have been damaged (Manske and Lesker, 1977; Idler, 1985; Lin
et al., 1990). Such damage to the pulleys can occur in extreme activities, in which much of the body
Source: Adapted from Spence, A.P. (1990). Basic Human Anatomy, 3rd ed. Redwood City, CA: Benjamin/Cummings and
Tubiana, R. (ed.) (1981). The Hand, Vol. 1. Philadelphia, PA: Saunders. With permission.
45-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
(a)
Abductor
pollicis Flexor
brevis retinaculum
Opponens
pollicis
Palmaris brevis
Flexor Opponens
pollicis digiti minimi
brevis
Abductor
digiti minimi
Flexor digiti
minimi brevis
Abductor
pollicis
Lumbricales
Flexor
digitorum profundus
tendon
(b)
Dorsal
interossei
Palmar
interossei
FIGURE 45.4 Intrinsic muscles of the hand: (a) palmar view, (b) dorsal view. (Adapted from Spence, A.P. (1990).
Basic Human Anatomy, 3rd ed. Redwood City, CA: Benjamin/Cummings. With permission.)
Source: Adapted from Spence, A.P. (1990). Basic Human Anatomy, 3rd ed. Redwood City, CA: Benjamin/Cummings and
Tubiana, R. (ed.) (1981). The Hand, Vol. 1. Philadelphia, PA: Saunders. With permission.
Upper Extremity Analysis of the Wrist 45-7
weight is supported by the fingers, such as rock climbing. Although, the A3 pulley is relatively weaker and
closer to the PIP joint, it is more flexible and stretches, transferring the load to the A2 and A4 pulleys,
which then fail first (Marco et al., 1998).
A1
A2 A3 C
C1 2 A
4 C
3 A
5
FIGURE 45.6 Pulley structure of the finger. (Adapted from Nordin, M. and Frankel, V.H. (2001). Basic Mechanics of
the Musculoskeletal System, 3rd ed. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. With permission.)
flexion –extension plane and a 508 range of motion in the radial –ulnar deviation plane in forearm
neutral position.
Table 45.4 reveals that the FCR, FCU, and ECRB provide larger tendon excursion during flexion and
extension movement than ECRL and ECU, while ECRL and ECU have greater tendon excursion during
radial and ulnar deviation movement. The results also demonstrate that the FCR and FCU are prime
muscles for flexion, ECRB for extension, ECRL for radial deviation, and ECU for ulnar deviation. In
spite of the three-dimensional orientation of the wrist tendons to the rotation axes and the complexity
of carpal bone motion, Table 45.4 indicates that the moment arms of wrist motion are maintained fairly
consistently and correspond well with the anatomical location of the tendons. According to An et al.
(1991), these findings are related to the anatomical considerations; the extensor retinaculum ensures a
consistent relationship of the wrist extensors (ECRB, ECBL, and ECU) to the rotation axes, while the
FCR is firmly fixed in the fibro-osseous groove, and the FCU infixed on the pisiform.
TABLE 45.4 Physiological and Mechanical Properties of Wrist Joint Muscles and Tendons
Physiological Size Tendon Excursion (mm) Moment Arm (mm)a
Muscle and Tendon Length (cm) PCSA (cm2) F/E plane R/U plane F/E plane R/U plane
FCR 10.9–12.4 2.0 25 + 4 7+1 þ15 + 3 þ8 + 2
FCU 15.2–15.4 3.2–3.4 28 + 4 12 + 3 þ16 + 3 214 + 3
ECRB 13.8–15.8 2.7–2.9 20 + 3 11 + 1 212 + 2 þ13 + 2
ECRL 11.8–18.3 1.5–2.4 12 + 3 17 + 1 27 + 2 þ19 + 2
ECU 13.6–14.9 2.6–3.4 10 + 2 16 + 2 26 + 1 217 + 3
a
þ Denotes flexion and radial deviation, and 2 denotes extension and ulnar deviation.
Sources: Adapted from, An, K.N., Hui, F.C., Morrey, B.F., Linscheid, R.L., and Chao, E.Y.S. (1981). Journal of Biomechanics,
14:659–669; Lieber, R.L., Fazeli, B.M. and Botte, M.J. (1990). Journal of Hand Surgery, 15:244–250; An, K.N., Horii, E., and
Ryu, J. (1991). Biomechanics of the Wrist Joint, New York: Springer Verlag, pp. 157–169. With permission.
Upper Extremity Analysis of the Wrist 45-9
TABLE 45.5 Phalange Lengths as Percentage of Hand Length for Males and Females
Phalanx Proximal Medial Distal
Thumb 17.1 — 12.1
Index 21.8 14.1 8.6
Middle 24.5 15.8 9.8
Ring 22.2 15.3 9.7
Little 17.7 10.8 8.6
Sources: From Davidoff, N.A. MS Thesis, Pennsylvania State University and Davidoff, N.A. and
Freivalds, A. (1993). International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 12: 255– 264. With permission.
data on the hand can be found in Garrett (1971) and detailed data on muscle moment arms and tendon
excursions for the index finger can be found in An et al. (1983).
x ¼ Ru (45:1)
where x is the tendon displacement, R is the distance from the joint center to the tendon, and u is the
joint rotation angle.
However, if the tendon is not held securely, it may be displaced from the joint when the joint is flexed
and will settle in a position along the bisection of the joint angle (see Figure 45.8b). Model II is useful for
describing tendon displacement in intrinsic muscles as:
TABLE 45.6 Interphalangeal Joint Dimensions — Mean and Standard Deviations (in parenth-
eses) in mm
Breadth Thickness
Joint Male Female Male Female
IP (I) 22.9 (3.8) 19.1 (1.3) 20.1 (1.5) 16.8 (1.0)
PIP (II) 21.3 (1.3) 18.3 (1.0) 19.6 (1.3) 16.3 (1.0)
DIP (II) 18.3 (1.3) 15.5 (1.0) 15.5 (1.3) 13.0 (1.0)
PIP (III) 21.8 (1.3) 18.3 (1.0) 20.1 (1.5) 16.8 (1.0)
DIP (III) 18.3 (1.3) 15.2 (1.0) 16.0 (1.3) 13.2 (1.0)
PIP (IV) 20.1 (1.3) 18.3 (1.0) 18.8 (1.3) 15.8 (1.0)
DIP (IV) 17.3 (1.0) 14.5 (0.8) 15.2 (1.3) 12.5 (0.8)
PIP (V) 17.8 (1.5) 14.5 (0.8) 16.8 (1.3) 14.0 (1.0)
DIP (V) 15.8 (1.3) 13.2 (0.8) 13.7 (1.3) 11.4 (0.8)
TABLE 45.7 Location of Finger Joint Centers from Distal End of Phalanx. DIP Distances as percen-
tage of Medial Phalanx Length, PIP and MCP Distances as Percentage of Proximal Phalanx Length
Finger DIP PIP MCP
Index 18 13 20
Middle 15 12 20
Ring 13 12 19
Little 17 14 24
Source: Adapted from An, K.N., Chao, E.Y., Cooney, W.P., and Limscheid, R.L. (1979). Journal of
Biomechanics, 12:775– 788. With permission.
Landsmeer’s (1960) Model III depicts a tendon running through a tendon sheath held securely against
the bone, which allows the tendon to curve smoothly around the joint (Figure 45.8c). The tendon
displacement is described by:
1 1
x ¼ 2 y þ u d y= tan u (45:3)
2 2
where y is the tendon length to joint axis measured along the long axis of the bone and d is the distance of
tendon to the long axis of the bone.
For small angles of flexion (u , 208), tan u is almost equal to u, and Equation (45.3) simplifies to:
x ¼ du (45:4)
Armstrong and Chaffin (1979) proposed a static model for the wrist based on Landsmeer’s (1962)
tendon Model I and LeVeau’s (1977) pulley-friction concepts (Figure 45.9). Armstrong and Chaffin
(1978) found that, when the wrist is flexed, the flexor tendons are supported by flexor retinaculum
on the volar side of the carpal tunnel. When the wrist is extended, the flexor tendons are supported
by the carpal bones. Thus, deviation of the wrist from neutral position causes the tendons to be
displaced against and past the adjacent walls of the carpal tunnel. They assumed that a tendon sliding
over a curved surface is analogous to a belt incurring friction forces while wrapped around a pulley.
The radial reaction force on the ligament or the carpal bones, FR, can be characterized as follows:
TABLE 45.8 Representative Tendon Insertion Distances for Each Finger with Respect to that Joint Center Coordi-
nate System (Figure 45.7). DIP distances as percentage of Medial Phalanx Length, PIP and MCP Distances as
percentage of Proximal Phalanx Length
Index Finger Middle Finger Ring Finger Little Finger
Distal Proximal Distal Proximal Distal Proximal Distal Proximal
Joint Muscle x y x y x y x y x y x y x y x y
DIP FDP 218 213 25 222 219 212 26 222 219 213 26 222 218 215 2 220
PIP FDP 222 214 18 217 227 215 22 214 228 216 23 216 224 215 20 218
FDS 222 211 18 213 227 211 22 212 228 213 23 213 224 213 20 215
MCP FDP 224 217 13 227 230 218 16 226 231 216 17 226 227 220 15 228
FDS 224 222 13 231 230 218 16 231 231 219 17 229 227 224 15 232
Source: Adapted from An, K.W., Chao, E.Y., Cooney, W.P., and Limscheid, R.L. (1979). Journal of Biomechanics, 12:
775–788. With permission.
Upper Extremity Analysis of the Wrist 45-11
FIGURE 45.7 Finger coordinate system. (Adapted from An, K.W., Chao, E.Y., Cooney, W.P., and Linscheid, R.L.
(1979). Journal of Biomechanics, 12:775– 788. With permission.)
where FR is the radial reaction force, FT is the tendon force or belt tension, m is the coefficient of friction
between tendon and supporting tissues, and u is the wrist deviation angle (in radians).
The resulting normal forces on the tendon exerted by the pulley surface can be expressed per unit arc
length as:
where FN is the normal forces exerted on tendon and R is the radius of curvature around supporting
tissues.
For small coefficient of frictions, comparable to what is found in joints (m , 0.04) and for small angles
of u, Equation (45.6) reduces to the simple expression:
FN ¼ FT =R (45:7)
Thus, FN is a function of only the tendon force and the radius of curvature. As the tendon force increases
or the radius of curvature decreases (e.g., small wrists), the normal supporting force exerted on tendon
increases. FR, on the other hand, is independent of radius of curvature but is dependent on the wrist devi-
ation angle.
This tendon-pulley model provides a relatively simple mechanism for calculating the normal support-
ing force exerted on tendons that are a major factor in work-related musculoskeletal disorders
(WRMSDs). However, this model does not include the dynamic components of wrist movements
such as angular velocity and acceleration, which might be risk factors in WRMSDs.
rθ y
h
R θ θ
θ
1/2x R
d
FIGURE 45.8 Landsmeer’s tendon models. (a) Model I, (b) Model II, (c) Model III, (Adapted from Landsmeer,
J.M.F. (1962). Annals of Rheumatoid Diseases, 21:164 – 170. With permission.)
45-12 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Ft-min
R FR
R
θ
FL
Ft
FIGURE 45.9 Wrist tendon pulley model. (From Chaffin, D.B., Andersson, G.B.J., and Martin, B.J. (1999).
Occupational Biomechanics, 3rd ed., New York: John Wiley & Sons. With permission.)
CM D CM
Wy
M × Ac
M × At
θ
Wx
R
Flexor tendons
Mw
FIGURE 45.10 Dynamic wrist tendon pulley model. (Adapted from Schoenmarklin, R.W. and Marras, W.P. (1990).
Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 34th Annual Meeting. Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society, 809 – 809.
With permission.)
Upper Extremity Analysis of the Wrist 45-13
Key forces and movements in the model include the reaction force at the center of the wrist (Wx and
Wy), the couple or moment (Mw) required to flex and extend the wrist, and the inertial force (M Ac
and M At) and inertial moment I Q € acting around the hand’s center of mass. For equilibrium, the
magnitude of moment around the wrist in the free body diagram must equal the magnitude of moment
acting around the hand’s center of mass in the moment acceleration diagram:
€
Ft R ¼ (M At þ M Ac ) D þ I Q (45:8)
where M is the mass, At is the tangential acceleration, Ac is the centripetal acceleration, FT is the tendon
force, I is the moment of inertia of the hand in flexion and extension, and Q € is the angular acceleration.
Thus, the hand is assumed to accelerate from a stationary posture, so, the angular velocity is theoreti-
cally zero, resulted in zero centripetal force (Ac ¼ V 2/R ¼ 0). Then,
€
Ft R ¼ (M At ) D þ I Q (45:9)
€ DþIQ
Ft R ¼ (M D Q) € (45:10)
(M D þ I) Q
2 €
Ft ¼ (45:11)
R
" #
€
(M D2 þ I) Q u
FR ¼ 2 sin (45:12)
R 2
where R is the radius of curvature of the tendon, D is the distance between the center of mass of hand and
wrist, M is the weight of hand, and u is the wrist deviation angle.
Equation (45.9) –Equation (49.12) indicate that the resultant reaction force, FR, is a function of
angular acceleration, radius of curvature, and wrist deviation. Thus, exertion of wrist and hand with
greatly angular acceleration and deviated wrist angle would result in greater total resultant reaction
forces on the tendons and supporting tissues than exertions with small angular acceleration and
neutral wrist position. According to Armstrong and Chaffin (1979), increases in resultant reaction
force would increase the supporting force that the carpal bones and ligaments exert on the flexor
tendons, therefore increasing the chance of inflammation and risk of carpal tunnel syndrome (CTS).
Therefore, these results might provide theoretical support to why angular acceleration variable can be
considered a risk factor of WRMSDs.
The advantage of Schoenmarklin and Marras (1990) model is that it does include the dynamic variable
of angular acceleration into assessment of resultant reaction force on the tendons. But the model is two-
dimensional, and it does not consider the coactivation of antagonistic muscles in wrist joint motions.
This points to the need for further model development to account for additional physiological factors.
tip pinch. To reduce the number of unknown muscle forces, the following assumptions were applied: (1)
the sum of the interosseous (I) forces is treated as a single force 2I; (2) half of the interosseous forces of I
act at the PIP joint and the other half act at the DIP joint; (3) the lumbrical (L) is much smaller than the
interosseous (I), as much as 1/3I. They solved the three moment equations using these assumptions and
anthropometrical data of the index finger obtained from a cadaver hand in a tip pinch position. They
reported the tendon forces normalized to the external force F, as 3.8F, 2.5F, 0.9F, and 0.3F for the
FDP, FDS, I, and L, respectively. They also found a value of 7.5F for the MCP joint force. The results
indicate that the flexor tendons are dominant and the forces are many times larger than the intrinsic
muscle forces during tip pinch.
Chao et al. (1976) presented a comprehensive analysis of the three-dimensional tendon and joint
forces of the fingers in pinch and power grip functions. Kirschner wires were drilled through the
phalanges to fix the finger configuration in the desired position and different surgical wires were inserted
into the tendon and muscles of hand specimens of the cadaver to identify different tendons on x-ray film.
The exact orientations of finger digits and the locations of the tendons were defined by biplanar x-ray
analysis. Through a free-body analysis, 19 independent equations were obtained for 23 unknown joint
and tendon forces. Using the permutation –combination principle of setting any four of the nine
tendon forces equal to zero solved the indeterminate problem. The selection of these tendons was
based primarily on electromyographic (EMG) responses and physiological assessment. They found
that high constraint forces and moments at the DIP and PIP joints were found during pinches,
whereas large magnitudes of constraint forces at the MCP joint were found during power grips. The
total of the intrinsic muscles (RI, UI, and LI) produced a greater force than the total of the flexor
tendons (FDP and FDS) in both pinch and power grip actions.
An alternate method using a typical optimization technique was suggested by Seireg and Arvikar
(1973) and Penrod et al. (1974). In this approach, force equilibrium equations and anatomical constraint
relationships were used for the equality constraints and the physiological limits on the tendon, muscle,
and joint forces were applied as the inequality constraints. In addition, the most important factor in this
method is optimal criteria that correspond to the objective function of the formulation. The possible sol-
utions can vary based on the optimal criteria selected.
Chao and An (1978a) studied the middle finger during tip pinch and power grip actions, with an aid of
three-dimensional analysis. They analyzed the same problem using the optimization and linear program-
ming (LP) technique of Chao et al. (1976) instead of the previously described EMG and permutation-
combination method. The predicted middle finger muscle and joint forces were very similar to those
of the previous study (Penrod et al., 1974), except for the intrinsic muscle forces whose predicted
values were considerably lower. They found that the highest joint contact forces for all three joints
occurred for pinch grip rather than power grip. They also found that the main flexors (FDP and FDS)
were most active in both pinch and power grip functions, whereas the intrinsic muscles were less
active in power grip than in pinch.
An et al. (1985) also applied LP optimization techniques to solve the indeterminate problem of a three-
dimensional analytic hand model. The ranges of muscle forces of the index finger under isometric hand
functions, such as tip pinch, lateral key pinch, power grip and other functional activities were analyzed.
FDP and FDS carried high tendon forces compared with other muscles in most hand functions, although
the predicted FDS force was zero in a pinch grip. The long extensors (LE) and two intrinsic muscles con-
tributed large forces in the key pinch. The large force of these intrinsic muscles in pinch action can be
explained by the role of these muscles maintaining balance and stabilization of the MCP joint. The
joint constraint forces for each finger were also studied. The Chao et al. (1976) study showed a trend
for joint constraint forces in which the DIP joint had the lowest force and the force progressively
increased for the PIP joint and was largest at the MCP joint. An et al. (1985) showed the same trend
in lateral pinch functions.
Chao and An (1978b) used a graphical presentation with a combined permutation and optimization
technique to solve the statically indeterminate tendon force problem. They analyzed the maximum tip
pinch force of the index finger as a function of external force directions (08, 308, and 458) and the
Upper Extremity Analysis of the Wrist 45-15
DIP joint flexion angles (108 –508). The results showed that the pinch strength relied on the direction of
applied external force as well as on the finger joint configuration. The tendon forces of the index finger
were also studied with the same finger posture as that in Chao et al. (1976) study, but only one angle (458)
of the external force was assumed. Also, the predicted extrinsic extensor tendon force was considerably
larger than in their previous studies.
Weightman and Amis (1982) presented a good critical review for the previously published studies and
applied their two-dimensional finger model to the analysis of resultant joint forces and muscle tensions
in various pinch actions. To create a statically determinate problem, all joints were assumed to be pin
joints with a fixed center of rotation during flexion. The relationships of the intrinsic muscle forces
were assumed identical to those of Chao and An (1978a), except that the long extensor muscles forces
dropped to zero. They also used the physiological cross-sectional area (PCSA) of the muscles to
define the force distributions in the intrinsic muscles. Their results compared to other previously pub-
lished studies with a good correlation of both muscle and joint force predictions. Based on these com-
parisons, they verified that a two-dimensional finger model could be valid for analyzing two-dimensional
finger actions, even though realistically any finger motion is still three dimensional.
where: PRik is the FDP moment arm for the ith finger and kth joint, SRik is the FDS moment arm
for the ith finger and kth joint, X1 ¼ 1 for PIP and 0 for all others, X2 ¼ 1 for DIP and 0 for all
others, X3 is the joint thickness (mm) from Table 45.5.
Consequently, the pertinent equations are as follows:
Four Cartesian coordinate systems are established to define the locations and orientations of the
tendons and to describe the joint configuration (Figure 45.11). There are two coordinate systems for
both the middle and proximal phalanges and only one system for the distal and metacarpal phalanges.
The y-axis is defined along the long axis of the each phalanx, from the proximal end to the distal end. The
x-axis is defined as perpendicular to the long axis of each phalanx and in the palmar –dorsal plane, from
the palmar side to the dorsal side of the finger bone. Both x- and y-axes have their origins at the center
of the proximal end of phalanx. Note that these definitions are different from that used by An et al. (1979)
in Figure 45.7, Table 45.7 and Table 45.8.
In terms of notation, subscript i refers to fingers, with 1 –4 for the index, middle, ring, and little finger,
respectively, subscript j refers to joints, with 1 –4 for the DIP, PIP, MP, and wrist joints, respectively, while
subscript k refers to phalanges, with 1 –4 for the distal, middle, proximal phalanges, and the metacarpal
bone, respectively.
In terms of model input values, the external force on each phalange of each finger is indicated by F(i,k).
The finger joint flexion angles, measured with reference to straight fingers as the hand is lying flat, are
indicated by (i,uj). The length of each phalanx for each finger is indicated by L(i,k).
For output variables, the FDP tendon force for each phalanx of each finger is indicated by TP(i,k). The
FDS tendon force for each phalanx of each finger is indicated by TS(i,k). Finally, joint constraint forces
along the Xk- and Yk- axes are indicated by Rxk(i,j), and Ryk(i,j), respectively.
To solve for the aforementioned unknown model output variables, a static equilibrium analyses of each
phalanx must be performed. Specifically, the summation of forces acting on each phalanx in the X- and
Rx3(i,3)
Ry2́(i,2)
Rx2(i,2)
Ry3́(i,3) Rx4(i,4)
(i, θ 2) (i, θ 3)
Ry3(i,3)
Ry1́(i,1)
Rx2́(i,2) Rx3́(i,3)
(i, θ 1) Ry2(i,2) Ry4(i,4)
Rx1(i,1)
TP(i,2) F(i,3) TP(i,3)
TS(i,2) TS(i,3) FDP
TP(i,1) F(i,2)
Rx1́(i,1)
Ry1(i,1) FDS
F(i,1)
FIGURE 45.11 Simple two-dimensional hand model. For symbols and notation, see text. (Reproduced from
Kong, Y.K. Ph.D. Dissertation, Pennsylvania State University. With permission.)
Upper Extremity Analysis of the Wrist 45-17
Y-axes must be zero. Similarly, the summation of all moments acting on each phalanx must also be equal
to zero. The resulting equations for the distal phalanx are:
½0:5 F(i,2) Rx1 (i,1) cos (i,u1 ) þ Ry1 (i,1) sin (i,u1 )
TP(i,2) ¼ L(i,2) (45:23)
aSR(i,2) þ PR(i,2)
TS(i,2) ¼ aTP(i,2) (45:24)
Ry2 (i,2) ¼ Rx1 (i,1) sin (i,u1 ) þ Ry1 (i,1) cos (i,u1 ) þ (a þ 1) TP(i,2) cos (i,u2 ) (45:25)
Rx2 (i,2) ¼ Rx1 (i,1) cos (i,u1 ) Ry1 (i,1) sin (i,u1 ) þ (a þ 1) TP(i,2) sin (i,u2 ) F(i,2) (45:26)
½0:5 F(i,3) Rx2 (i,2) cos (i,u2 ) þ Ry2 (i,2) sin (i,u2 )
TP(i,3) ¼ L(i,3) (45:27)
aSR(i,3) þ PR(i,3)
TS(i,3) ¼ aTP(i,3) (45:28)
Ry3 (i,3) ¼ Rx2 (i,2) sin (i,u2 ) þ Ry2 (i,2) cos (i,u2 ) þ (a þ 1) TP(i,3) cos (i,u3 ) (45:29)
Rx3 (i,3) ¼ Rx2 (i,2) cos (i,u2 ) Ry2 (i,2) sin (i,u2 ) þ (a þ 1) TP(i,3) sin (i,u3 ) F(i,3) (45:30)
One interesting application of such a biomechanical hand model is to identify the optimum handle
size for gripping so as to minimize tendon forces. However, in a typical power grip, there are two alter-
nate ways in which the geometry of a cylindrical handle surface and phalange contacts can be defined. In
Grip I (Figure 45.12), the point of contact between the distal phalange (L1) and a handle is assumed to be
at the middle point of the distal phalange, that is, the distal phalange is divided into two equal lengths
(L1/2). The bisector of the DIP angle establishes a right triangle with the distal phalanx as the base and
the altitude passing through the contact point to the center of handle. Through trigonometry, the DIP
joint angle then becomes:
where: u 10 is the DIP joint angle, R is the radius of the cylindrical handle, and D1 is the thickness of the
distal phalanx.
The second contact point (between the middle phalange (L2) and the handle) divides the middle pha-
lange into two unequal lengths, one being L1/2 (due to the DIP bisector) and the other being L2 – L1/2.
The bisector of the PIP angle establishes a right triangle with the medial phalanx as the base and the alti-
tude again passing through the contact point to the center of handle. Through trigonometry, the PIP
joint angle then becomes:
2(R þ D2 )
u20 ¼ 2 tan1 (45:32)
2L2 L1
where u 20 is the PIP joint angle and D2 is the thickness of the medial phalanx.
The third contact point with the handle also divides the proximal phalange (L3) into two parts, one
being the same length as the proximal part of middle phalange (L2 2 L1/2) and the other being
45-18 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
PIP joint
L2 L2 − (L1/2) L3
L2 − (L1/2)
q2́
L3 − L2+(L1/2)
L1/2
D2 D3
DIP joint
q´
L1/2 q3́
MP joint
L1
R
L1/2 D1
FIGURE 45.12 Schematic diagram of Grip I. (Reproduced from Kong, Y. K. (2001) Ph.D Dissertation.
Pennsylvania State University. With permission.).
(L3 2 L2 þ L1/2). The bisector of the MP angle creates another right triangle:
2(R þ D3)
u30 ¼ 2 tan1 (45:33)
2L3 2L2 þ L1
The third contact point divides the proximal phalange into two unequal lengths. One is L2/2 while the
other is L3 2 L2/2. The MP joint angle (u30 ) can then be estimated as:
2(R þ D3 )
u30 ¼ 2 tan1 (45:35)
2L3 L2
Based on this biomechanical hand model, tendon forces for each finger and in total were calculated for
both types of grip and for 11 cylindrical handles with diameters ranging from 10 to 60 mm. For Grip I,
tendon force were minimized at 30–35, 38–43, 40– 45, and 25–30 mm for index, middle, ring, and little
fingers, respectively. The total of tendon force for all fingers was minimized for an approximately 40 mm
cylindrical handle (see Figure 45.14). As the size of the handle deviated above or below 40 mm, the total
tendon forces increased.
For Grip II, tendon force were minimized at 23 –28, 28 –33, 28–33, and 20 –25 mm for index, middle,
ring, and little fingers, respectively. The total of tendon force for all fingers was minimized for an approxi-
mately 28 mm cylindrical handle (see Figure 45.15). The combined results for each type of grip are
Upper Extremity Analysis of the Wrist 45-19
PIP joint
L2 L2/2 L3
L2/2
θ 2́
L3−(L2/2)
L2/2 D2
DIP joint D3
θ1́
L2/2 θ 3́
MP joint
L1
D1 R
L1−(L2/2)
FIGURE 45.13 Schematic diagram of Grip II. (Reproduced from Kong, Y.K. (2001). Ph.D. Dissertation.
Pennsylvania State University. With permission.)
summarized in Table 45.9. As noted previously, as the size of the handle deviates from the optimum size,
tendon forces increase. This is an important principle that should be utilized in the design of hand tools.
Also, in either type of grip, the optimal handle sizes depends greatly on which finger is considered. There-
fore, a purely traditional cylindrical handle cannot provide optimality for all fingers simultaneously and
alternate handle shapes, such as the double frustum, need to be considered. Further details can be found
in Kong (2001) and Kong, et al. (2004), with applications to meat hook handles in Kong and Freivalds
(2003).
12 TOTAL 40
Tendon Force
10
30
8
6 20
4
10
2
0 0
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70
Grip Span (mm)
FIGURE 45.14 Finger tendon forces for Grip I. (Reproduced from Kong, Y.K. (2001). Ph.D. Dissertation.
Pennsylvania State University. With permission.)
45-20 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
16 INDEX
MIDDLE 50
14 RING
LITTLE
10
30
8
6 20
4
10
2
0 0
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70
Grip Span (mm)
FIGURE 45.15 Finger tendon forces for Grip II (Reproduced from Kong, Y.K. (2001). From Ph.D. Dissertation. The
Pennsylvania State University. With permission.)
In another in vivo tendon force measurement study, Dennerlein et al. (1998) measured only the FDS
tendon forces of the middle finger at three finger postures, which ranged from extended to flexed pinch
postures, using a gas-sterilized tendon force transducer (Dennerlein et al., 1997) and a single axis load
cell. The investigation was centered upon the average ratio of the FDS tendon tension to the externally
applied force. The average ratio ranged from 1.7F to 5.8F, with a mean of 3.3F, in the study. Tip pinches
with the DIP joint flexed were also studied with the tendon-to-tip force ratio being 2.4F. These ratios
were compared with the results of their own three-finger models as well as other contemporarily pub-
lished isometric tendon force models. These ratios were larger than those of other studies. The
average values were also slightly higher than that (1.73F) of Schuind et al.’s (1992) in vivo tendon
force measurement study. It was found that the tendon force ratios and muscle strength varied substan-
tially from individual to individual, although the ratio of force from tendon to tendon was relatively con-
stant within the same limb for all studies (Ketchum et al., 1978; Brand et al., 1981; Dennerlein et al.,
1998).
Although the intrinsic muscles are more active in pinch action than in power grip action, the relative
magnitudes of the main flexor tendon forces (such as FDP and FDS) are usually high in both actions.
These in vivo tendon forces of the flexors are presented in Table 45.10 based on the previous studies.
In general, these averages and ranges of tendon forces are very similar with a few exceptions. Schuind
et al. (1992) showed lower FDS tendon forces in power grip action than those of other types of grips.
The tendon force ranges of Brand et al. (1981) show similar magnitudes with Bright and Urbaniak
(1976) power grips. Tip pinch action studies by Ketchum et al. (1978) and Bright and Urbaniak
(1976) also show the similar ranges of tendon forces. Schuind et al. (1992) represented the significant
differences between FDP and FDS tendon forces in both pinch and power grip, whereas the others
showed that the force of FDP tendon was only slightly larger than that of the FDS tendon. These discre-
pancies can be explained by the different finger postures utilized in each study, since each finger could
have various functional muscle capacities depending upon its joint configuration (Chao and An, 1978b).
from the data of Greenberg and Chaffin (1976) and Petrofsky et al. (1980) to yield average grip span
strength decrements for spans ranging from 0 to 11 cm as shown in Table 45.12. Inputting the appropri-
ate parameters yields the variable “Grip Span Force.”
Repeated exertions will result in muscle fatigue and reduced capacity for further exertions. This effect
can be quantified from the data of Schutz (1972) as the maximum “%MVC Allowed” for a particular
wrist motion based on the exertion time for the motion and the time between exertions or motions.
If the “%MVC Required” performing the motion is greater than that allowed as calculated below
(%MVC Allowed), then a penalty is assessed to that motion.
where: TW is the work time of grip or motion (min), TR is the rest time between grips or motions (min),
and MW is the time worked during one shift (min).
For each motion a “Force Capacity” is calculated as the product of “Force Capacity Wrist” and “Grip
Span Force”:
%MVC RequiredAdj is calculated based on %MVC Required performing the motion and the previously
calculated Force Capacity. %MVC Required is typically found by dividing the subjective job force
requirement to the operator’s MVC.
The %MVC RequiredAdj is then compared to the %MVC Allowed for Equation (45.36). If the %MVC
Allowed is greater than the %MVC RequiredAdj, then no penalty is assessed to that hand motion and that
motion will have a “Force Frequency Score” (FFS) of 1. If the %MVC Allowed is less than the %MVC
RequiredAdj, then the FFSi for that individual motion is calculated as follows:
where TW is the work time posture held (min), TR is the rest time between postures (min), MW is the
time worked during one shift (min), and FMVC is the force/load held normalized by 51 lbs, from the
NIOSH Lifting Equation of Waters et al. (1993).
Five postures are considered: neck and back flexion, elbow and shoulder flexion, and shoulder abduc-
tion. For simplicity sake (and difficulty interpreting angles from job videotapes), Points are assigned to
various angles as follows: for back flexion 08 –108 ¼ 0, 108 –208 ¼ 1, 208 –458 ¼ 2, and .458 ¼ 3 points;
for neck flexion 08 –308 ¼ 0, 308 –458 ¼ 1, 458 – 608 ¼ 2, and .608 ¼ 3 points; for elbow flexion/exten-
sion (with a 908 bent elbow being considered the neutral posture) 108 flexion to 308extension ¼ 0, every-
thing else is 1 point; for shoulder flexion 08 –208 ¼ 0, 208 –458 ¼ 1, 458 –908 ¼ 2 and .908 ¼ 3 points;
and for shoulder abduction 08 –308 ¼ 0, 308 – 608 ¼ 1, 608 –908 ¼ 2, .908 ¼ 3 points.
The “Posture Score” for each body part is obtained by multiplying the point value by the %Endurance
Capacity (Equation 45.41) and dividing the product into 50, which is considered as the limit for accep-
table fatigue or endurance:
The “Overall Posture Score” (OPS) is the maximum of individual Posture Scores for each joint.
The final risk score is a weighted average (coefficients obtained from regression of a subset of jobs) of
the FFS (Equation 45.40) and the OPS in the form of a predicted incidence rate (IR) normalized to
200,000 exposure hours:
Regression of predicted incidence rates against the actual incidence rates experienced on 24 industrial
jobs in the garment and the printing industry (involving a total of 288 workers) yielded a significant
(p , 0.001) linear regression with r 2 ¼ 0.52. As a comparison, Moore and Garg’s (1995) Strain Index
yielded a nonsignificant (p ¼ 0.2) regression with r 2 ¼ 0.17. The only limitations found were for
very short cycle jobs (typically under 4-sec cycle times), in which any error in miscounting motions
could be amplified into a large error for the final predicted incidence rate. Note that the roughly 48%
45-24 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
of unaccounted variance was thought to be due individual differences such as gender, age, physical
fitness, etc. and the psychosocial risk factors.
Novice ergonomists (university graduate students) required at least several trials in becoming profi-
cient with the risk assessment model. However, by the fifth trial, average time required for job analysis
had decreased to 12 min and test –retest reliability was up to r 2 ¼ 0.99. A simplified version of this risk
index has been developed into a paper-and-pencil checklist for use in industry (see Figure 45.16), with
FIGURE 45.16 A simple carpal tunnel disorder risk index (Reproduced from Niebel, B. and Freivalds, A. (2003).
Methods, Standards, and Work Design. New York: McGraw-Hill. With permission.)
Upper Extremity Analysis of the Wrist 45-25
values greater than 1.0 indicating risk for injury (Niebel and Freivalds, 2003). However, it has not been
validated or checked for reliability.
45.5 Conclusions
A variety of models and analysis tools have been presented for the hand and wrist. For all these, the main
risk factors for WRMSDs are high external forces and awkward wrist postures that produce high tendon
forces. In addition, high velocities and accelerations, resulting from frequent rapid movements, can
further increase the risk for injury.
References
An, K.W., Chao, E.Y., Cooney, W.P., and Linscheid, R.L. (1979). Normative model of human hand for
biomechanical analysis. Journal of Biomechanics, 12:775–788.
An, K.N., Hui, F.C., Morrey, B.F., Linscheid, R.L., and Chao, E.Y.S. (1981). Muscle across the elbow joint:
a biomechanical analysis. Journal of Biomechanics, 14:659– 669.
An, K.N., Ueba, Y., Chao, E.Y., Cooney, W.P., and Linscheid, R.L. (1983). Tendon excursion and moment
arm of index finger muscles. Journal of Biomechanics, 16:419–425.
An K.N., Chao E.Y., Cooney, W.P., and Linscheid R.L. (1985). Forces in the normal and abnormal hand.
Journal of Orthopedic Research, 3:202–211.
An, K.N., Horii, E., and Ryu, J. (1991). Muscle function. In An, K.N., Berger, R.A. and Cooney, W.P.
(Eds.), Biomechanics of the Wrist Joint, New York: Springer Verlag, pp. 157 –169.
Armstrong, T.J. (1976). Circulatory and Local Muscle Responses to Static Manual Work. Ph.D. Disser-
tation, Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan.
Armstrong, T.J. and Chaffin, D.B. (1978). An investigation of the relationship between displacements of
the finger and wrist joints and the extrinsic finger flexor tendons. Journal of Biomechanics,
11:119–128.
Armstrong, T.J. and Chaffin, D.B. (1979). Some biomechanical aspects of the carpal tunnel. Journal of
Biomechanics, 12:567–570.
Batmanabane, M. and Malathi, S. (1985). Movements at the carpometacarpal and metacarpophalangeal
joints of the hand and their effect on the dimensions of the articular ends of the metacarpal bones.
Anatomical Record, 213:102 –110.
Brand, P.W. and Hollister, A. (1993). Clinical Mechanics of the Hand, 2nd ed. St. Louis, MO: Mosby Year
Book, Inc.
Brand, P.W., Beach, R.B., and Thompson, D.E. (1981). Relative tension and potential excursion of
muscles in the forearm and hand. Journal of Hand Surgery, 6:209 –218.
Bright, D.S. and Urbaniak, J.S. (1976). Direct measurements of flexor tendon tension during active and
passive digit motion and its application to flexor tendon surgery. Transactions of the 22nd Annual
Orthopedic Research Society, 240.
Cailliet, R. (1994). Hand Pain and Impairment. 3rd ed. Philadelphia, PA: Davis.
Chaffin, D.B., Andersson, G.B.J., and Martin, B.J. (1999). Occupational Biomechanics, 3rd ed., New York:
John Wiley & Sons.
Chao, E.Y.S. and An, K.N. (1978a). Determination of internal forces in human hand. Journal of Engin-
eering Mechanics Division ASCE, 104:255–272.
Chao, E.Y.S. and An, K.N. (1978b). Graphical interpretation of the solution to the redundant problem in
biomechanics. Journal of Biomechanics, 11:159– 167.
Chao, E.Y.S., Opgrande, J.D., and Axmear, F.E. (1976). Three-dimensional force analysis of finger joints
in selected isometric hand functions. Journal of Biomechanics, 9:387 –396.
Davidoff, N.A. (1990). The Development of a Graphic Model of a Human Hand, MS Thesis, University
Park, PA: Pennsylvania State University.
45-26 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Davidoff, N.A. and Freivalds, A. (1993). A graphic model of the human hand using CATIA. International
Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 12:255 –264.
Dennerlein, J.T., Miller, J., Mote, C.D. Jr., and Rempel, D.M. (1997). A low profile human tendon force
transducer: the influence of tendon thickness on calibration. Journal of Biomechanics, 30:395–397.
Dennerlein, J.T., Diao, E., Mote, C.D. Jr., and Rempel, D.M. (1998). Tensions of the flexor digitorum
superficialis are higher than a current model predicts. Journal of Biomechanics, 31:295– 301.
Fahrer, M. (1971). Considerations on functional anatomy of the flexor digitorum profundus. Annales de
Chirurgie, 25:945.
Garcia-Elias, M., Horii, E., and Berger, R.A. (1991). Individual carpal bone motion. In: An, K-N., Berger,
R.A. and Cooney, W.P. (Eds.). Biomechanics of the Wrist Joint. New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 61–75.
Garrett, J.W. (1970a). Anthropometry of the Air Force Female Hand, AMRL-TR-69–26, Wright Patterson
AFB, OH: Aerospace Medical Research Laboratory.
Garrett, J.W. (1970b). Anthropometry of the Hands of Male Air Force Flight Personnel, AMRL-TR-69–42,
Wright Patterson AFB, OH: Aerospace Medical Research Laboratory.
Garrett, J.W. (1971). The adult human hand: Some anthropometric and biomechanical considerations.
Human Factors, 13:117 –131.
Gigis, P.I. and Kuczynski, K. (1982). The distal interphalangeal joints of the human fingers. Journal of
Hand Surgery, 7:176–182.
Greenberg, L. and Chaffin, D.B. (1976). Workers and Their Tools: A Guide to the Ergonomics of Hand Tools
and Small Presses. Midland, MI: Pendell Pub.
Hales, T., Habes, D., Fine, L., Hornung, R., and Boiano, J. (1989). Health Hazard Evaluation Report,
Bennett Industries, Peotone, IL, HETA 89-146-2049, Cincinnati, OH: National Institute of Occu-
pational Safety and Health.
Hallbeck, M.S., Kamal, M.S., and Harmon, P.E. (1992). The effects of forearm posture, wrist posture and
hand on three peak pinch force types. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society
36th Annual Conference, Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, pp. 801–805.
Idler, R.S. (1985). Anatomy and biomechanics of the digital flexor tendons. Hand Clinic, 1:3–12.
Imrahn, S.N. (1991). The influence of wrist posture on different types of pinch strength. Applied Ergo-
nomics, 22:379–384.
Ketchum, L.D., Thompson, D., Pocock, G., La, C., and Wallingford, D. (1978). A clinical study of forces
generated by the intrinsic muscles of the index finger and the extrinsic flexor and extensor muscles
of the hand. The Journal of Hand Surgery, 3:571–578.
Kong, Y.K. (2001). Optimum Design of Handle Shape Through Biomechanical Modeling of Hand
Tendon Forces. Ph.D. Dissertation, University Park, PA: Pennsylvania State University.
Kong, Y.K. and Freivalds, A. (2003). Evaluation of meat hook handle shapes. International Journal of
Industrial Ergonomics, 32:13–23.
Kong, Y., Freivalds, A., and Kim, S.E. (2004). Evaluation of handles in a maximum gripping task.
Ergonomics, 47.
Landsmeer, J.M.F. (1960). Studies in the anatomy of articulation. Acta Morphologica Nederlands,
3 –4:287–303.
Landsmeer, J.M.F. (1962). Power grip and precision handling. Annals of Rheumatoid Diseases, 21:164 –
170.
LeVeau, B. (1977). William and Lissner Biomechanics of Human Motion. Philadelphia, PA: W.B. Saunders.
Lieber, R.L., Fazeli, B.M., and Botte, M.J. (1990). Architecture of selected wrist flexor and extensor
muscles. Journal of Hand Surgery, 15:244 –250.
Lin, G.T., Cooney, W.P., Amadio, P.C., and An, K.N. (1990). Mechanical properties of human pulleys.
Journal of Hand Surgery, 15B:429 –434.
Long, C., Conrad, P.W., Hall, E.A., and Furler, S.L. (1970). Intrinsic –extrinsic muscle control of the hand
in power grip and precision handling: an electromyographic study. Journal of Bone and Joint
Surgery, 52A:853–867.
Manske, P.R. and Lesker, P.A. (1977). Strength of human pulleys. Hand, 9:147 –152.
Upper Extremity Analysis of the Wrist 45-27
Marco, R.A.W., Sharkey, N.A., and Smith, T.S. (1998). Pathomechanics of closed rupture of the flexor
tendon pulleys in rock climbers. The Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery, 80A:1012–1019.
Moore, J. S. and Garg, A. (1995). The strain index: a proposed method to analyze jobs for risk of distal
upper extremity disorders. American Industrial Hygiene Association Journal, 56:443 –458.
Niebel, B. and Freivalds, A. (2003). Methods, Standards, and Work Design. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Nordin, M. and Frankel, V.H. (2001). Basic Biomechanics of the Musculoskeletal System, 3rd ed.
Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Norkin, C.C. and Levangie, P.K. (1992). Joint Structure and Function: A Comprehensive Analysis,
Philadelphia, PA: Davis.
Penrod, D.D., Davy, D.T., and Singh, D.P. (1974). An optimization approach to tendon force analysis.
Journal of Biomechanics, 7:123 –129.
Petrofsky, J.S., Williams, C., Kamen, G., and Lind, A.R. (1980). The effect of handgrip span on isometric
exercise performance. Ergonomics, 23:1129 –1135.
Schoenmarklin, R.W. and Marras, W.P. (1990). A dynamic biomechanical model of the wrist joint.
Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 34th Annual Meeting, Santa Monica, CA: Human
Factors Society, pp. 805 –809.
Schuind, F., Garcia-Elias, M., Cooney W.P. III, and An, K.N. (1992). Flexor tendon forces: In vivo
measurements. The Journal of Hand Surgery, 17A:291– 298.
Schultz, R.B., Stroave, A., and Krishnamurthy, S. (1987). Metacarpophalangeal joint motion and the role
of the collateral ligaments. International Orthopaedics, 11:149 –155.
Schutz, R.K. (1972). Cyclic Work – Rest Exercise’s Effect on Continuous Hold Endurance Capability,
Ph.D. Dissertation, Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan.
Seireg, A. and Arvikar, R.B. (1973). A mathematical model for evaluation of forces in lower extremities of
musculoskeletal system. Journal of Biomechanics, 6:313–326.
Seth,V., Weston, R.L., and Freivalds, A. (1999). Development of a cumulative trauma disorder risk asses-
sment model for the upper extremities. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 23:281–291.
Smith, E.M., Juvenile, R.B., Bender, L.F., and Pearson, J.R. (1964). Role of the finger flexors in rheuma-
toid deformities of the metacarpophalangeal joints. Arthritis and Rheumatism, 7:467–480.
Spence, A.P. (1990). Basic Human Anatomy, 3rd ed. Redwood City, CA: Benjamin/Cummings Pub. Co.
Steinberg, D.R. (1992). Acute flexor tendon injuries. Orthopaedics Clinics of North America, 23:125 –140.
Steindler, A. (1955). Kinesiology of the Human Body under Normal and Pathological Conditions. Spring-
field, IL: Thomas.
Terrell, R. and Purswell, J.L. (1976). The influence of forearm and wrist orientation on static grip strength
as a design criterion for hand tools. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 36th
Annual Conference, Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, pp. 28–32.
Tubiana, R. (Ed.) (1981). The Hand, Vol. 1, Philadelphia, PA: Saunders.
Waters, T.R., Putz-Anderson, V., Garg, A., and Fine, L. (1993). Revised NIOSH equation for the design
and evaluation of manual lifting tasks. Ergonomics, 36:749–776.
Weightman, B. and Amis, A.A. (1982). Finger joint force predictions related to design of joint replace-
ments. Journal of Biomedical Engineering, 4:197–205.
Youm, Y., McMurty, R.Y., Flatt, A.E., and Gillespie, T.E. (1978). Kinematics of the wrist. Journal of Bone
and Joint Surgery, 60A:424–431.
Yun, M.H., Cannon, D., Freivalds, A., and Thomas, G. (1997). An instrumented glove for grasp specifi-
cation in virtual-reality based point-and-direct telerobotics. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man,
and Cybernetics, Part B: Cybernetics, 27:835–846.
46
Revised NIOSH Lifting
Equation
46.1 Introduction
This chapter provides information about a revised equation developed by the National Institute for
Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH) for assessing the physical demands of certain two-handed
manual lifting tasks, which was described by Waters et al. (1993). The chapter contains sections describ-
ing what factors need to be measured, how they should be measured, what procedures should be used,
and how the results can be used to ergonomically design new jobs or make decisions about redesigning
existing jobs that may be hazardous. The chapter defines all pertinent terms and presents the mathematical
formulae and procedures needed to properly apply the NIOSH lifting equation. Several example problems
are also provided to demonstrate how the equations should be used. An expanded, more detailed
version of this chapter is contained in a NIOSH report entitled Applications Manual for the Revised
NIOSH Lifting Equation (Waters et al., 1994).
Historically, the NIOSH has recognized the problem of work-related back injuries, and published the
Work Practices Guide for Manual Lifting (WPG) in 1981 (NIOSH, 1981). The NIOSH WPG contained a
summary of the lifting-related literature before 1981; analytical procedures and a lifting equation for
46-1
46-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
calculating a recommended weight for specified two-handed, symmetrical lifting tasks; and an approach
for controlling the hazards of low back injury from manual lifting. The approach to hazard control was
coupled to the action limit (AL), a resultant term that denoted the recommended weight derived from
the lifting equation.
In 1985, the NIOSH convened an ad hoc committee of experts who reviewed the current literature on
lifting, including the NIOSH WPG.1 The literature review was summarized in a document containing
updated information on the physiological, biomechanical, psychophysical, and epidemiological
aspects of manual lifting. Based on the results of the literature review, the ad hoc committee rec-
ommended criteria for defining the lifting capacity of healthy workers. The committee used the criteria
to formulate the revised lifting equation.2 Subsequently, NIOSH staff developed the documentation for
the equation and played a prominent role in recommending methods for interpreting the results of the
lifting equation.
The rationale and criterion for the development of the revised NIOSH lifting equation are provided
in a journal article entitled: “Revised NIOSH equation for the design and evaluation of manual lifting
tasks” (Waters et al., 1993). We suggest that those users who wish to achieve a better understanding
of the data and decisions that were made in formulating the revised equation consult the article
by Waters et al., 1993. The 1993 article provides an explanation of the selection of the biomechanical,
physiological, and psychophysical criterion, as well as a description of the derivation of the individual
components of the revised lifting equation. For those individuals, however, who are primarily
concerned with the use and application of the revised lifting equation, provides a more complete descrip-
tion of the method and limitations for using the revised equation. The applications manual for the
revised NIOSH Lifting Equation (Waters et al., 1994).
Although there are limited data examining the validity or effectiveness of the revised lifting equation to
identify lifting jobs with increased risk of low back disorders, the recommended weight limits derived
from the revised equation are consistent with, or lower than, those generally reported in the literature
as being safe for workers (Waters et al., 1993, Table 2, Table 4, and Table 5). Moreover, the proper appli-
cation of the revised equation is more likely to protect healthy workers for a wider variety of lifting tasks
than methods that rely on only a single task factor or single criterion. A later section of this chapter pro-
vides a summary of studies examining the effectiveness of the NIOSH equation to identify manual lifting
jobs with increased risk of lifting-related low back pain (LBP).
Finally, it should be stressed that the NIOSH lifting equation is only one tool in a comprehensive
effort to prevent work-related LBP and disability. Some examples of other approaches are described
elsewhere (ASPH/NIOSH, 1986). Moreover, lifting is only one of the causes of work-related LBP
and disability. Other causes that have been hypothesized or established as risk factors include whole
body vibration, static postures, prolonged sitting, and direct trauma to the back. Psychosocial
factors, appropriate medical treatment, and job demands also may be particularly important in
influencing the transition of acute low back pain to chronic disabling pain (see chapter entitled
“Manual Materials Handling”).
1
The ad hoc 1991 NIOSH Lifting Committee members included: M.M. Ayoub, Donald B. Chaffin, Colin
G. Drury, Arun Garg, and Suzanne Rodgers. NIOSH representatives included Vern Putz-Anderson and Thomas
R. Waters.
2
For this document, the revised 1991 NIOSH lifting equation will be identified simply as “the revised lifting
equation.” The abbreviation WPG will continue to be used as the reference to the earlier NIOSH lifting equation,
which was documented in a publication entitled Work Practices Guide for Manual Lifting.
Revised NIOSH Lifting Equation 46-3
RWL ¼ LC HM VM DM AM FM CM
where
The term “task variables” refers to the measurable task-related measurements that are used as input
data for the formula (i.e., H, V, D, A, F, and C), whereas the term “multipliers” refers to the reduction
coefficients in the equation (i.e., HM, VM, DM, AM, FM, and CM).
Load weight L
LI ¼ ¼
Recommended weight limit RWL
where load weight (L) is equal to the weight of the object lifted (lb or kg).
VERTICAL
POINT OF
TOP VIEW PROJECTION
HORIZONTAL
HORIZONTAL H
LOCATION
LATERAL
MID-POINT BETWEEN
INNER ANKLE BONES
VERTICAL
V
LOCATION
HORIZONTAL
MID-POINT BETWEEN H
POINT OF PROJECTION
INNER ANKLE BONES HORIZONTAL
LOCATION
SAGITTAL
PLANE
TOP VIEW
SAGITTAL
MID-POINT
BETWEEN
INNER ANKLE
BONES
FRONTAL
H
POINT OF
PROJECTION
A
FRONTAL
PLANE
A
POINT OF ASYMMETRIC SAGITTAL
ASYMMETRY LINE
PROJECTION LINE ANGLE
defined by the large middle knuckle of the hand (Figure 46.1). Typically, the worker’s feet are not
aligned with the midsagittal plane, as shown in Figure 46.1, but may be rotated inward or outward.
If this is the case, then the midsagittal plane is defined by the worker’s neutral body posture as
defined above. If significant control is required at the destination (i.e., precision placement), then H
should be measured at both the origin and destination of the lift. Also, if the worker leans over on
one foot during lifting, concentrating nearly all of their support on one foot, while using the other
leg and foot as a counterbalance so that they can reach out further to pick up the load, the H
variable is measured from a point directly below the weight bearing foot, rather than the midpoint
between the ankles. In cases where it is not clear that the weight is concentrated primarily on one
foot, the point between the ankles should still be used as the reference point for measurement of
the horizontal location (H). It also important to note that it has also come to our attention that
users sometimes overestimate the magnitude of the horizontal location (H) and the asymmetric
angle (A) for some types of lifts because they mistakenly measure the task variables at the incorrect
location for the origin of the lift. This may occur when the lifters stand with the side of their body
next to a table or shelf and reach over to slide the object horizontally toward the front of the body
as they begin the lift. When the lift is performed this way, the load actually moves horizontally
toward the front of the body before it actually begins to move vertically. When this type of lift is
analyzed, the task variables should be measured at the actual location where the object first begins
to move upward (liftoff point), rather than at the point where the object first begins to move hori-
zontally. This change will generally result in smaller H values than would have been determined if
the measurements had been taken at the point where the object first began to move horizontally
rather than vertically.
Horizontal distance (H) should be measured. In those situations where the H value cannot be
measured, then H may be approximated from the following equations:
H ¼ 20 þ W/2 H ¼ 8 þ W/2
for V 25 cm for V 10 in.
H ¼ 25 þ W/2 H ¼ 10 þ W/2
for V , 25 cm for V , 10 in.
where W is the width of the container in the sagittal plane and V is the vertical location of the hands from
the floor.
3
It may not always be clear if asymmetry is an intrinsic element of the task or just a personal characteristic of the
worker’s lifting style. Regardless of the reason for the asymmetry, any observed asymmetric lifting should be con-
sidered an intrinsic element of the job design and should be considered in the assessment and subsequent redesign.
Moreover, the design of the task should not rely on worker compliance, but rather the design should discourage or
eliminate the need for asymmetric lifting.
Revised NIOSH Lifting Equation 46-11
Since the total work time (75 min) exceeds 1 h, the job is classified as moderate duration. On the
other hand, if the recovery period between lifting sessions was increased to 30 min, then the short-
duration category would apply, which would result in a larger FM value.
A special procedure has been developed for determining the appropriate lifting frequency (F) for
certain repetitive lifting tasks in which workers do not lift continuously during the 15-min
sampling period. This occurs when the work pattern is such that the worker lifts repetitively
for a short time and then performs light work for a short time before starting another cycle.
For work patterns such as this, the lifting frequency (F) may be determined as follows, as long
as the actual lifting frequency does not exceed 15 lifts per minute:
(i) Compute the total number of lifts performed for the 15-min period (i.e., lift rate times work
time)
(ii) Divide the total number of lifts by 15
(iii) Use the resulting value as the frequency (F) to determine the FM from Table 46.5
For example, if the work pattern for a job consists of a series of cyclic sessions requiring 8 min of
lifting followed by 7 min of light work, and the lifting rate during the work sessions is ten lifts
per minute, then the frequency rate (F) that is used to determine the FM for this job is equal
to (10 8)/15 or 5.33 lifts/min. If the worker lifted continuously for more than 15 min,
however, then the actual lifting frequency (10 lifts/min) would be used.
When using this special procedure, the duration category is based on the magnitude of the recovery
periods between work sessions, not within work sessions. In other words, if the work pattern
is intermittent and the special procedure applies, then the intermittent recovery periods that
occur during the 15-min sampling period are not considered as recovery periods for purposes
of determining the duration category. For example, if the work pattern for a manual lifting job
was composed of repetitive cycles consisting of 1 min of continuous lifting at a rate of 10 lifts/
min, followed by 2 min of recovery, the correct procedure would be to adjust the frequency accord-
ing to the special procedure (i.e., F ¼ [10 lifts/min 5 min]/15 min ¼ 50/15 ¼ 3.4 lifts/min).
The 2-min recovery periods would not count toward the RT/WT ratio, however, and additional
recovery periods would have to be provided as described earlier.
2. Moderate duration defines lifting tasks that have a duration of more than 1 h, but not more than
2 h, followed by a recovery period of at least 0.3 times the work time (i.e., at least a 0.3 recovery
time to work time ratio [RT/WT]).
For example, if a worker continuously lifts for 2 h, then a recovery period of at least 36 min would
be required before initiating a subsequent lifting session. If the recovery time requirement is not
met, and a subsequent lifting session is required, then the total work time must be added together.
If the total work time exceeds 2 h, then the job must be classified as a long-duration lifting task.
3. Long duration defines lifting tasks that have a duration of between 2 and 8 h, with standard indus-
trial rest allowances (e.g., morning, lunch, and afternoon rest breaks).
Note: no weight limits are provided for more than 8 h of work.
The difference in the required RT/WT ratio for the short-duration (less than 1 h) category, which is
1.0, and the moderate duration category (1 to 2 h), which is 0.3, is due to the difference in the magnitudes
of the frequency multiplier values associated with each of the duration categories. Since the moderate
category results in larger reductions in the RWL than the short category, there is less need for a recovery
period between sessions than for the short-duration category. In other words, the short-duration cat-
egory would result in higher weight limits than the moderate duration category, so larger recovery
periods would be needed.
Object Lifted
YES NO
POOR
Optimal Optimal
Handles? Grip?
YES NO NO NO YES
Fingers
Flexed
90 degrees?
YES
FAIR
GOOD
46.5 Procedures
Prior to data collection, the analyst must decide (1) if the job should be analyzed as a single-task or
multi-task manual lifting job and (2) if significant control is required at the destination of the lift.
This is necessary because the procedures differ, depending on the type of analysis required.
A manual lifting job may be analyzed as a single-task job if the task variables do not differ from task to
task, or if only one task is of interest (e.g., single most stressful task). This may be the case if one of the
tasks clearly has a dominant effect on strength demands, localized muscle fatigue, or whole body fatigue.
On the other hand, if the task variables differ significantly between tasks, it may be more appropriate to
analyze a job as a multi-task manual lifting job. A multi-task analysis is more difficult to perform than a
single-task analysis because additional data and computations are required. The multi-task approach,
however, will provide more detailed information about specific strength and physiological demands.
For many lifting jobs, it may be acceptable to use either the single- or multi-task approach. The single-
task analysis should be used when possible, but when a job consists of more than one task and detailed
information is needed to specify engineering modifications, then the multi-task approach provides a
reasonable method of assessing the overall physical demands. The multi-task procedure is more compli-
cated than the single-task procedure, and requires a greater understanding of assessment terminology
and mathematical concepts. Therefore, the decision to use the single- or multi-task approach should
be based on: (1) the need for detailed information about all facets of the multi-tasked lifting job; (2)
the need for accuracy and completeness of data regarding assessment of the physiological demands of
the task; and (3) the analyst’s level of understanding of the assessment procedures.
The decision about control at the destination is important because the physical demands on the worker
may be greater at the destination of the lift than at the origin, especially when significant control is
required. When significant control is required at the destination, for example, the physical stress is
increased because the load will have to be accelerated upward to slow down the descent of the load.
This acceleration may be as great as the acceleration at the origin of the lift and may create high loads
on the spine. Therefore, if significant control is required, then the RWL and LI should be determined
at both locations and the lower of the two values will specify the overall level of physical demand.
To perform a lifting analysis using the revised lifting equation, two steps are undertaken: (1) data is
collected at the worksite as described in Step 1 (described next); and (2) the RWL and LI values are
computed using the single- or multi-task analysis procedures described in Step 2 (described later).
may be possible to have a job in which all of the individual tasks have an STLI less than 1.0 and still be
physically demanding due to the combined demands of the tasks. In cases where the FILI exceeds the
STLI for any task, the maximum weights may represent a significant problem and careful evaluation
is necessary.
where
P 1 1
DLI ¼ FILI2
FM1,2 FM1
1 1
þ FILI3
FM1,2,3 FM1,2
1 1
þ FILI4
FM1,2,3,4 FM1,2,3
..
.
1 1
þ FILIn
FM1,2,3,4,,n FM1,2,3,,(n1)
Note that (1) the numbers in 46.5 subscript refer to the new task numbers and (2) the FM values are
determined from Table 46.5, based on the sum of the frequencies for tasks listed in subscript.
The following example is provided to demonstrate this step of the multi-task procedure. Assume that
an analysis of a typical three-task job provided the results shown in Table 46.8.
To compute the CLI for this job, the tasks are renumbered in order of decreasing physical stress, begin-
ning with the task with the greatest STLI down to the task with the smallest STLI. In this case, as shown in
Table 46.8, the task numbers do not change. Next, the CLI is computed according to the formula given
earlier. The task with the greatest CLI is Task 1 (STLI ¼ 1.6). The sum of the frequencies for Tasks 1 and 2
is 1þ2 or 3, and the sum of the frequencies for Tasks 1, 2, and 3 is 1 þ 2 þ 4 or 7. Then, from Table 46.5,
FM1 is 0.94, FM1,2 is 0.88, and FM1,2,3 is 0.70. Finally, the CLI ¼ 1.6 þ 1.0(1/0.88 2 1/
0.94) þ 0.67(1/0.70 2 1/0.88) ¼ 1.6 þ 0.07 þ 0.20 ¼ 1.9. Note that the FM values were based on the
sum of the frequencies of the subscripts, the vertical height, and the duration of lifting.
above a lifting index of 1.0, at least in theory, without substantially increasing their risk of low back
injuries above the baseline rate of injury.
36
LBS.
27
INCHES
15
INCHES
20
INCHES
1. Bring the load closer to the worker by making the roll smaller so that the roll can be lifted from
between the worker’s legs. This will decrease the H value, which in turn will increase the HM value.
2. Raise the height of the destination to increase the VM.
3. Improve the coupling to increase the CM.
If the size of the roll cannot be reduced, then the vertical height (V) of the destination should be
increased. Figure 46.8 shows that if V was increased to about 30 in., then VM would be increased
from 0.85 to 1.0; the H value would be decreased from 20 in. to 15 in., which would increase HM
from 0.50 to 0.67; the DM would be increased from 0.93 to 1.0. As shown in Figure 46.8, the final
RWL would be increased from 18.1 to 30.8 lb, and the LI at the destination would decrease from 1.9
to 1.1.
In some cases, redesign may not be feasible. In these cases, use of a mechanical lift may be more suit-
able. As an interim control strategy, two or more workers may be assigned to lift the supply roll.
46.7.1.5 Comments
The horizontal distance (H) is a significant factor that may be difficult to reduce because the size of the
paper rolls may be fixed. Moreover, redesign of the machine may not be practical. Therefore, elimination
of the manual lifting component of the job may be more appropriate than job redesign.
ES
20 INCH
44
INCHES
20 INCHES
ORIGIN
30°
SAGITTAL LINE
DESTINATION 30°
7 INCHES
Since the horizontal distance (H) is dependent on the width of the tray in the sagittal plane, this variable
can only be reduced by using smaller trays. Both the DM and VM, however, can be increased by lowering
the height of the origin and increasing the height of the destination. For example, if the height at both the
origin and destination is 30 in., then VM and DM are 1.0, as shown in the modified worksheet
(Figure 46.11). Moreover, if the cart is moved so that the twist is eliminated, the AM can be increased
from 0.90 to 1.00. As shown in Figure 46.11, with these redesign suggestions the RWL can be increased
from 13.3 to 20.4 lb, and the LI values are reduced to 1.0.
46.7.2.5 Comments
This analysis was based on a 1-h work session. If a subsequent work session begins before the appropriate
recovery period has elapsed (i.e., 1.0 h), then the 8-h category would be used to compute the FM value.
Revised NIOSH Lifting Equation 46-25
LBP and to study the validity and feasibility of using the NIOSH lifting equation in China. The NIOSH
equation was used to evaluate lifting risk for 69 workers mainly involved in manual materials handling
(MMH) (job A) and 51 machinery workers who worked in less demanding MMH tasks (job B). The
prevalence of LBP lasting for more than a week due to lifting were 26.09 and 5.88% for jobs A and B,
respectively. The NIOSH LI was estimated to be 2.4 for job A, and 0 , LI , 1 for job B. The authors
concluded that the NIOSH equation is an important tool in assessing characteristics and risk factors
of LBP for MMH tasks.
In another study, Marras et al. (1999) examined the relationship between low-back disorders and
various risk factors for LBP due to manual lifting. One of the objectives of the study was to evaluate
the validity and effectiveness of the revised NIOSH lifting equation to correctly identify jobs with
varying levels of risk of low back disorder, where job risk was defined according to historical records
of low back disorder injuries. High-risk jobs were defined as jobs in which more than 12 low back dis-
orders were recorded per 100 exposed workers (mean of 22 injuries/100 exposed workers), medium-risk
jobs were defined as jobs in which between 1 and 12 low back disorders were recorded per 100 exposed
workers, and low-risk jobs were defined as jobs in which no low back disorder was recorded per 100
exposed workers. The results indicated that the when the average horizontal distance was used, the
NIOSH equation was predictive of risk of low back disorder, resulting in an odds ratios of 3.1 (95%
CI, 2.6 to 3.8) for high-risk jobs compared to low-risk jobs. When the maximum horizontal distance
was used, the odds ratio was increased to 4.3 for high-risk jobs compared to low-risk jobs.
In another study, conducted by Lee et al. (1996), researchers examined whether the NIOSH lifting
equation was applicable for an Asian population. The application of the NIOSH equation for establishing
weight limits for Korean workers was examined using the psychophysical method. The study population
consisted of 53 male college students and 16 male field workers. The subjects were required to perform six
different lifting tasks in the sagittal plane, at various lifting frequencies and heights, for 8 h. The RWLs for
each lift were calculated using the NIOSH equation. The psychophysical method, in which subjects were
allowed to adjust the weight of lift during a 20-min period, was also used to estimate the maximum
acceptable weight of lift (MAWL) for each lifting task. While students generally had larger body sizes
than the worker population, workers were generally stronger than the students. Within each group,
neither the frequency nor the vertical height of lift was significantly related to the differences between
the NIOSH Lifting Equation (NLE) and psychophysically-based weight limits. The MAWLs of the
workers were significantly higher than those of the students. While this difference increased with increas-
ing lift frequency, it was not sensitive to lift height. When the data were adjusted to represent the entire
Korean young male population, no significant differences were observed between the NIOSH rec-
ommended weights of load and the adjusted MAWLs. Although the load constant of the NIOSH
equation was 23 kg, that of the students was 20.24 kg and that of the workers was 25.05 kg. The
authors conclude that the NIOSH weight limit equation is well suited for young, healthy Korean males.
Recently, Hidalgo et al. (1995, 1997) conducted a study designed to evaluate the validity of the psy-
chophysical, biomechanical, and physiological criteria used in establishing the NIOSH lifting equation
(Waters et al., 1991). The criteria used to develop the equation were cross-validated against the data
published by different researchers in the scientific literature. Assessment of the 1991 NIOSH lifting
equation indicated that there are differences between the NIOSH equation values and the psychophysical
limits for some types of lifts and that the RWL likely would protect about 85% of the female population
and 95% of the male population. The authors, however, noted that the 3.4 kN limit for compression
on the lumbosacral joint may be too high to protect all workers and that the energy expenditure
limits used in development of the RWL index can be sustained by 57 to 99% of worker population
when compared to the physiological limits based on previous fatigue studies. The authors concluded
that the results of the cross-validation for psychophysical criterion confirmed the validity of assump-
tions made in the 1991 NIOSH revised lifting equation, but that the results of cross-validation for
the biomechanical and physiological criteria were not in total agreement with the 1991 NIOSH
model. They did not, however, actually evaluate whether the equation would protect workers or not
in the study.
Revised NIOSH Lifting Equation 46-27
Sesek et al. (2003) conducted a study designed to investigate the ability of the revised NIOSH lifting
equation to measure the risk of low back injury using employee health outcomes to identify high-risk
manual lifting jobs. In addition to the revised NIOSH lifting equation, a slightly modified version of the
equation was evaluated, in which some factors were removed from the equation for simplification. The
authors found that, without the modifications, the revised NIOSH lifting equation was able to predict
back injuries with odds ratios of 2.1 (95% CI, 1.0 to 4.43) and 4.0 (95% CI, 1.5 to 10.3) for lifting
indices of 1.0 and 3.0, respectively. They reported that simplifying the lifting equation by removing
several variables did not significantly reduce the predictive performance of the equation. When the
authors modified the equation, they found that the modified NIOSH lifting equation was able to
predict back injuries with odds ratios of 2.2 (95% CI, 1.0 to 4.6) and 5.3 (95% CI, 1.5 to 19.1) for
lifting indices of 1.0 and 3.0, respectively. The authors concluded that these modifications to the
NIOSH lifting equation show promise for increasing both the usability and utility of the lifting
equation.
An epidemiological study, conducted by Wang et al. (1998), evaluated the relation between low back
discomfort ratings and use of the revised NIOSH lifting equation to assess the risk of MMH tasks. In the
study, the authors surveyed 97 MMH workers on site in 15 factories and designed a questionnaire to sys-
tematically collect job-related information. Approximately 90% of the workers had suffered various
degrees of lower back discomfort, and 80% had sought medical treatment. The survey showed that 42
of the 97 jobs analyzed had a RWL of 0, which was attributed to either a horizontal distance or a
lifting frequency that exceeded the bounds of the NIOSH LI. Based on the results of the study, the
authors suggested that the limits for horizontal distance and maximum allowable frequency may be
too stringent to accommodate many existing MMH jobs. The authors also reported that for the remain-
ing 55 jobs the significant positive correlation obtained between the LI and the severity of low back dis-
comfort suggests that the LI is reliable in assessing the potential risk of low back injury in MMH. The
authors concluded that their results provide useful information on the application of the NIOSH
lifting guide to the assessment of LBP.
In a study examining the effectiveness of a training course to provide instruction on the proper use of
the NIOSH lifting equation, Waters et al. (1998), trained a group of nonergonomists to use the revised
NIOSH lifting equation and then tested them 8 weeks post-training to evaluate their knowledge in
making the measurements needed to use the equation. Twenty-seven individuals from NIOSH partici-
pated in a 1-day training session on the use of the NIOSH lifting equation. The participants were sub-
sequently tested on a simulated lifting task 8 weeks later to determine their accuracy in measuring the
variables. Analysis of the results indicated that (1) interobserver variability was small, especially for
the most important factor (i.e., horizontal distance); (2) individuals can be trained to make measure-
ments with sufficient accuracy to provide consistent recommended weight limit and lifting index
values; and (3) measurement of the coupling and asymmetric variables were the least accurate and
additional training should be provided to clarify these factors.
References
ASPH/NIOSH. 1986. Proposed National Strategies for the Prevention of Leading Work-Related Diseases
and Injuries: Part 1 (Published by the Association of Schools of Public Health under a cooperative
agreement with the National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health).
Eastman Kodak, 2004. Ergonomic Design for People at Work, 2nd Ed. (John Wiley & Sons, NJ).
Hidalgo, J., Genaidy, A., Karwowski, W., Christensen, D., Huston, R., and Stambough, J. 1995. A cross-
validation of the NIOSH limits for manual lifting. Ergonomics 38(12): 2455–2464.
Hidalgo, J., Genaidy, A., Karwowski, W., Christensen, D., Huston, R., and Stambough J. 1997. A compre-
hensive lifting model: beyond the NIOSH lifting equation. Ergonomics, 40(9): 926 –927.
Lavender, S.A. and Marras, W.S. 1994. The use of turnover rate as a passive surveillance indicator for
potential low back disorders. Ergonomics 37(6): 971–978.
46-28 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Lee, K.S., Park, H.S., and Chun, Y.H. 1996. The validity of the revised NIOSH weight limit in a Korean
young male population: a psychophysical approach. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics
18(2/3): 181 –186.
Marras, W., Fine, L., Ferguson, S., and Waters, T. 1999. The effectiveness of commonly used lifting
assessment methods to identify industrial jobs associated with elevated risk of low-back disorders.
Ergonomics, 42(1): 229– 245.
NIOSH. 1981. Work Practices Guide for Manual Lifting. NIOSH Technical Report No. 81-122 (U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services, National Institute for Occupational Safety and
Health, Cincinnati, OH).
NIOSH. 1991. Scientific Support Documentation for the Revised 1991 NIOSH Lifting Equation: Technical
Contract Reports, May 8, 1991 (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, National Institute
for Occupational Safety and Health, Cincinnati, OH). Available from the National Technical Infor-
mation Service (NTIS No. PB-91-226-274).
Sesek, R., Gilkey, D., Drinkaus, P., Bloswick, D.S., and Herron, R. 2003. Evaluation and quantification of
manual materials handling risk factors. International Journal of Occupational Safety and Ergonomics.
9(3): 271–287.
Wang, M.J., Garg, A., Chang, Y.C., Shih, Y.C., Yeh, W.Y., and Lee, C.L. 1998. The relationship between low
back discomfort ratings and the NIOSH lifting index. Human Factors and Ergonomics 40(3): 509–515.
Waters, T.R., Barron, S.L., Piacitelli, L.A., Anderson, V.P., Skov, T., Haring-Sweeney, M., Wall, D.K., and
Fine, L.J. 1999. Evaluation of the revised NIOSH Lifting Equation. Spine 24(4): 386 –394.
Waters, T.R., Putz-Anderson, V., Garg, A., and Fine, L.J. 1993. Revised NIOSH equation for the design
and evaluation of manual lifting tasks. Ergonomics 36(7): 749 –776.
Waters, T.R., Putz-Anderson, V., and Garg, A. 1994. Applications Manual for the Revised NIOSH Lifting
Equation. National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health, Technical Report.
DHHS(NIOSH) Pub. No. 94-110. Available from the National Technical Information Service
(NTIS). NTIS document number PB94-176930 (1-800-553-NTIS).
Waters, T.R., Baron, S.L., and Kemmlert, K. 1998. Accuracy of measurements for the revised NIOSH lifting
equation. National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health. Applied Ergonomics. 29(6): 433–438.
Xiao, G.B., Lei, L., Dempsey, P., Ma, Z.H., and Liang, Y.X. 2004. Zhonghua Lao Dong Wei Sheng Zhi Ye
Bing Za Zhi. Study on lifting-related musculoskeletal disorders among workers in metal proces-
sing. 22(2): 81– 85.
47
Psychophysical
Approach to Task
Analysis
47.1 Introduction
One approach to the prevention of work-related musculoskeletal disorders (WRMSDs) is the psycho-
physical approach. This approach seeks to provide limits and guidelines for manual work that represent
“maximum acceptable” workloads that minimize the injury potential of the work. Most often, these data
reflect maximum acceptable weights or forces acceptable to different population percentages given other
constant task parameters such as frequency. Limited data have been collected that examine maximum
acceptable frequencies when the force or load parameter is held constant. It is often quite difficult to
assess whether forces exerted or loads handled in the workplace exceed what is thought to pose excess
risk, and the psychophysical approach is one approach to this problem.
Like all assessment methods, there are advantages and disadvantages to consider when selecting an
analysis tool. These will be discussed, as will sources of data for workplace application. No single
approach is applicable to all workplace analyses, and other tools such as biomechanical and physiologic
models for analyzing materials handling tasks, can be used as alternate or supplemental analyses.
47-1
47-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
the data, and the limitations of the data. The primary focus will be on the application of psychophysical
techniques rather than on empirical methodologies or a literature review of the theoretical under-
pinnings of the psychophysical approach. Where necessary, theoretical and empirical results will be
used to justify specific application techniques or to explain caveats of psychophysical data. Manual
materials handling (MMH) and upper extremity intensive (UEI) tasks have been the focus of psycho-
physical research, and each will be considered separately due to the disparity in application
methodologies.
Readers interested in the empirical and theoretical aspects of the psychophysical approach to MMH
task design are referred to Snook1 – 3 and Ayoub and Mital4 for further reading. For specific
information on the comparisons of the psychophysical approach to the biomechanical and physiological
approaches to MMH task design, the reader should consult Ayoub5, Mital et al.6 and Nicholson7. Less
thorough information on the empirical and theoretical aspects of the psychophysical approach to UEI
task design is available. However, there are useful discussions in Kim et al.8 and Fernandez et al.9
c ¼ kfn
The value of the constant k depends on the units of measure, while the exponent n has a value that varies
for different sensations. The value of n may be lower than 1 for stimuli such as smell and brightness, or as
high as 3.5 for electric shock.10 Ljungberg et al.11 found a value of n ¼ 1.86 for a simulated brewery lifting
task whereas Gamberale et al.12 found a value of 2.43 for a similar task. Gamberale et al. attributed the
larger value in the latter study to more demanding lifting cycles.
In MMH experiments, the subject adjusts the magnitude of the stimulus (weight, force, or frequency)
to correspond to a sensation which is “dictated” by the instructions given by the experimenter, that is,
“without straining yourself, or becoming unusually tired, weakened, overheated, or out of breath”.13
Adaptations of these instructions have been used to study UIE tasks. For UEI tasks, the instructions
are directed at having subjects select workloads that do not result in “unusual discomfort in the
hands, wrists or forearms”.14 Subjects are also monitored during experimentation for signs of soreness,
stiffness, and numbness.
Start
Record Relevant
Task/Workplace
Parameters
If Necessary,
Is Task Redesign Task or
Acceptable to Workplace to
At Least 75% of Stop
Yes Accommodate
Population? 90% of Population
if Possible
No
Can Workplace Be
Redesigned? Utilize
Can Weight, Force or Preplacement
Frequency be Reduced? No Testing
Yes
Redesign
Task or
Workplace
FIGURE 47.1 General model of the psychophysical approach to analyzing manual tasks.
47-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
To use a database, the user records the relevant task parameters, then finds the value in the database
applicable to the specific task. Since it is impossible to collect data for all combinations of tasks, one can
either use interpolation to find the appropriate value, or one can use the closest value. In the latter case,
the user should use the lower of the values in between which the task parameters fall. Next, the analyst
determines if a task is acceptable. At a minimum, an MMH task should be designed to accommodate
75% of the population;1 however, one should strive to accommodate at least 90% of the population
whenever possible. If females perform the task, then the design should be based upon accommodating
the female workers.
When a task does not accommodate at least 75% of the population, or if the task does accommodate
75% of the population and minor changes can be made to increase the acceptability of the task at little
or no cost, then the task and workplace should be redesigned to accommodate at least 90% of the popu-
lation. Options to increase the acceptability of a task will be described in more detail later. If a task cannot
be redesigned to accommodate at least 75% of the population, then preplacement testing should be
considered.
47-5
(Table continued)
47-6
TABLE 47.1 Continued
Floor level to knuckle height One lift every Knuckle height to shoulder height One lift every Shoulder height to arm reach One lift every
sec min h sec min h sec min h
Widtha Distanceb Percentc 5 9 14 1 2 5 30 8 5 9 14 1 2 5 30 8 5 9 14 1 2 5 30 8
47-7
(Table continued)
47-8
TABLE 47.2 Continued
Floor level to knuckle height One lift every Knuckle height to shoulder height One lift every Shoulder height to arm reach One lift every
sec min h sec min h sec min h
Widtha Distanceb Percentc 5 9 14 1 2 5 30 8 5 9 14 1 2 5 30 8 5 9 14 1 2 5 30 8
47-9
(Table continued)
47-10
TABLE 47.3 Continued
Shoulder height to knuckle height One lowering Arm reach to shoulder height One
Knuckle height to floor level One lowering every every lowering every
sec min h sec min h sec min h
Widtha Distanceb Percentc 5 9 14 1 2 5 30 8 5 9 14 1 2 5 30 8 5 9 14 1 2 5 30 8
47-11
10 12 15 16 18 20 21 23 30 13 15 15 19 21 23 23 29 10 12 12 15 16 18 18 23
(Table continued)
47-12
TABLE 47.4 Continued
Knuckle height to floor level Shoulder height to knuckle height Arm reach to shoulder height
One lowering every One lowering every One lowering every
sec min h sec min h sec min h
Widtha Distanceb Percentc 5 9 14 1 2 5 30 8 5 9 14 1 2 5 30 8 5 9 14 1 2 5 30 8
47-13
(Table continued)
47-14
TABLE 47.5 Continued
2.1 m push 7.6 m push 15.2 m push 30.5 m push 45.7 m push 61.0 m push
One push every One push every One push every One push every One push every One push every
95 50 18 23 28 29 33 34 40 14 17 22 23 26 27 32 14 17 19 20 23 23 28 15 17 20 23 27 12 14 17 19 23 12 14 16 19
25 22 28 34 35 40 41 49 17 21 27 29 32 33 39 18 21 24 25 28 29 34 18 21 25 28 33 15 18 21 24 28 15 17 20 23
10 26 33 40 41 46 48 57 20 24 32 33 37 38 45 20 25 28 29 32 33 40 21 25 29 33 39 17 20 24 27 32 17 20 23 27
90 10 13 16 16 18 19 23 8 10 12 13 14 15 18 8 10 11 11 12 13 15 8 9 11 13 15 7 8 9 11 13 7 8 9 10
75 14 18 21 22 25 26 31 11 13 17 17 19 20 24 11 13 14 15 17 17 21 11 13 15 17 20 9 11 12 14 17 9 10 12 14
64 50 18 23 28 29 32 33 39 14 17 21 22 25 26 31 14 17 19 19 22 22 27 14 16 19 22 26 12 14 16 18 22 12 14 15 18
25 22 28 34 35 39 41 48 17 21 26 27 31 32 37 18 21 23 24 27 28 33 17 20 24 27 32 14 17 20 23 27 14 17 19 22
10 26 32 39 41 46 48 56 20 25 30 32 36 37 44 21 25 27 28 31 32 38 20 24 28 32 37 17 20 23 26 31 16 19 22 26
a
Vertical distance from floor to hands (cm).
b
Percentage of industrial population.
c
The force required to get an object in motion.
d
The force required to keep an object in motion.
Note: Italicized values exceed 8 h physiological criteria.
Source: Reprinted from Snook, S. H. and Ciriello, V.M. Ergonomics, 34, 1197–1213, 1991. With permission.
Psychophysical Approach to Task Analysis
TABLE 47.6 Maximum Acceptable Forces of Push for Females (kg)
2.1 m push 7.6 m push 15.2 m push 30.5 m push 45.7 m push 61.0 m push
One push every One push every One push every One push every One push every One push every
sec min h sec min h sec min h min h min h min h
Heighta Percentb 6 12 1 2 5 30 8 15 22 1 2 5 30 8 25 35 1 2 5 30 8 1 2 5 30 8 1 2 5 30 8 2 5 30 8
Initial forcesc
90 14 15 17 18 20 21 22 15 16 16 16 18 19 20 12 14 14 14 15 16 17 12 13 14 15 17 12 13 14 15 17 12 12 14 15
75 17 18 21 22 24 25 27 18 19 19 20 22 23 24 15 17 17 17 19 20 21 15 16 17 19 21 15 16 17 19 21 14 15 17 19
135 50 20 22 25 26 29 30 32 21 23 23 24 26 27 29 18 20 20 20 22 23 25 18 19 21 22 25 18 19 21 22 25 17 18 20 22
25 24 25 29 30 33 35 37 25 26 27 28 31 32 34 20 23 23 24 26 27 29 20 22 24 26 29 20 22 24 26 29 20 21 23 26
10 26 28 33 34 38 39 41 28 30 30 31 34 36 38 23 26 26 26 29 31 32 23 25 27 29 33 23 25 27 29 33 22 24 26 29
90 14 15 17 18 20 21 22 14 15 16 17 19 19 21 11 13 14 14 16 16 17 12 14 15 16 18 12 14 15 16 18 12 13 14 16
75 17 18 21 22 24 25 27 17 18 20 20 22 23 25 14 16 17 17 19 20 21 15 16 18 19 21 15 16 18 19 21 15 16 17 19
89 50 20 22 25 26 29 30 32 20 21 23 24 27 28 30 16 19 20 21 23 24 25 18 20 21 23 26 18 20 21 23 26 18 19 20 23
25 24 25 29 30 33 35 37 23 25 27 28 31 33 34 19 22 23 24 27 28 29 21 23 24 26 30 21 23 24 26 30 20 22 24 27
10 26 28 33 34 38 39 41 29 28 31 32 35 37 39 22 24 26 27 30 31 33 24 26 28 30 33 24 26 28 30 33 23 25 26 30
90 11 12 14 14 16 17 18 11 12 14 14 16 16 17 9 11 12 12 13 14 15 11 12 12 13 15 11 12 12 13 15 10 11 12 13
75 14 15 17 17 19 20 21 14 15 17 17 19 20 21 11 13 14 15 16 17 18 13 14 15 16 18 13 14 15 16 18 12 103 14 16
57 50 16 17 20 21 23 24 25 16 18 20 21 23 24 25 14 15 17 18 19 20 21 15 17 18 19 22 15 17 18 19 22 15 16 17 19
25 19 20 23 24 27 28 30 19 21 23 24 27 28 29 16 18 20 20 23 24 25 18 19 21 22 25 18 19 21 22 25 17 19 20 23
10 21 23 26 27 30 31 33 22 23 26 27 30 31 33 18 20 22 23 25 26 28 20 22 23 25 28 20 22 23 25 28 19 21 23 25
Sustained forcesd
90 6 8 10 10 11 12 14 6 7 7 7 8 9 11 5 6 6 6 7 7 9 5 6 6 6 8 5 5 5 6 8 4 4 4 6
75 9 12 14 14 16 17 21 9 10 11 11 12 13 16 7 8 9 9 10 11 13 7 8 9 9 12 7 8 8 8 11 6 6 6 9
135 50 12 16 19 20 21 23 28 12 14 14 15 16 17 21 10 11 12 12 14 14 18 10 11 12 12 16 9 10 11 11 15 8 8 9 12
25 16 20 24 25 27 29 36 15 17 18 18 20 22 27 12 14 15 16 17 18 22 13 14 15 15 21 11 13 13 14 19 10 10 11 15
10 18 23 28 29 32 34 42 18 20 21 22 24 26 32 14 17 18 18 20 22 27 15 17 17 18 25 14 15 16 17 22 12 12 13 17
90 6 7 9 9 10 11 13 6 7 8 8 9 9 11 5 6 6 7 7 8 10 5 6 6 7 9 5 6 6 6 8 4 4 5 6
75 8 11 13 13 15 16 19 9 10 11 11 13 13 17 7 8 9 10 11 11 14 8 9 9 10 13 7 8 8 9 12 6 6 7 9
47-15
(Table continued)
47-16
TABLE 47.6 Continued
2.1 m push 7.6 m push 15.2 m push 30.5 m push 45.7 m push 61.0 m push
One push every One push every One push every One push every One push every One push every
89 50 11 15 18 18 20 21 26 12 13 15 15 17 18 22 9 11 13 13 14 15 19 10 12 12 13 17 10 11 11 12 16 8 9 9 12
25 14 18 22 23 25 27 33 15 17 19 19 21 23 28 12 14 16 16 18 19 24 13 15 15 16 22 12 14 14 15 20 11 11 12 15
10 17 22 26 27 30 32 39 17 20 22 23 25 27 33 14 17 19 19 21 23 28 16 18 18 19 26 14 16 17 18 24 13 13 14 18
90 5 6 8 8 9 9 12 6 7 7 7 8 9 11 5 6 6 6 7 7 9 5 6 6 6 8 5 5 5 6 7 4 4 4 6
75 7 9 11 12 13 14 17 8 10 10 11 12 12 15 7 8 9 9 10 10 13 7 8 8 9 12 7 7 8 8 11 6 6 6 8
57 50 10 13 15 16 17 18 23 11 13 14 14 16 17 21 9 11 12 12 13 14 17 10 11 11 12 16 9 10 10 11 15 8 8 8 11
25 12 16 19 20 22 23 29 14 17 18 18 20 21 26 12 14 15 15 17 18 22 12 14 14 15 20 11 13 13 14 18 10 10 11 14
10 15 19 23 23 26 28 34 17 20 21 21 23 25 31 14 16 17 18 20 21 26 15 16 17 18 24 13 15 16 16 22 12 12 13 17
a
Vertical distance from floor to hands (cm).
b
Percentage of industrial population.
c
The force required to get an object in motion.
d
The force required to keep an object in motion.
Note: Italicized values exceed 8 h physiological criteria.
Source: Reprinted from Snook, S. M. and Ciriello, V.M. Ergonomics, 34, 1197– 1213, 1991. With permission.
Psychophysical Approach to Task Analysis
TABLE 47.7 Maximum Acceptable Forces of Pull for Males (kg)
2.1 m push 7.6 m push 15.2 m push 30.5 m push 45.7 m push 61.0 m push
One pull every One pull every One pull every One pull every One pull every One pull every
sec min h sec min h sec min h min h min h min h
Heighta Percentb 6 12 1 2 5 30 8 15 22 1 2 5 30 8 25 35 1 2 5 30 8 1 2 5 30 8 1 2 5 30 8 2 5 30 8
Initial forcesc
90 14 16 18 18 19 19 23 11 13 16 16 17 18 21 13 15 15 15 16 17 20 12 13 15 15 19 10 11 13 13 16 10 11 11 14
75 17 19 22 22 23 24 28 14 15 20 20 21 21 26 16 18 19 19 20 20 24 14 16 19 19 23 12 14 16 16 20 12 14 14 17
144 50 20 23 26 26 28 28 33 16 18 24 24 25 26 31 19 21 22 22 24 24 29 17 19 22 22 27 15 16 19 19 24 14 16 16 20
25 24 27 31 31 32 33 39 19 21 28 28 29 30 36 22 25 26 26 28 28 33 20 22 26 26 32 17 19 22 22 28 16 19 19 24
10 26 30 34 34 36 37 44 21 24 31 31 33 33 40 24 28 29 29 31 31 38 22 25 29 29 37 20 22 25 25 31 18 21 21 27
90 19 22 25 25 27 27 32 15 18 23 23 24 24 29 18 20 21 21 23 23 28 16 18 21 21 26 14 16 18 18 23 13 16 16 19
75 23 27 31 31 32 33 39 19 21 28 28 29 30 36 22 25 26 26 28 28 33 20 22 26 26 32 17 19 22 22 28 16 19 19 24
95 50 28 32 36 36 39 39 47 23 26 33 33 35 35 42 26 29 31 31 33 33 40 24 27 31 31 38 20 23 27 27 33 20 23 23 28
25 33 37 42 42 45 45 54 26 30 39 39 41 41 49 30 34 36 36 38 39 46 27 31 36 36 45 24 27 31 31 38 23 26 26 33
10 37 42 48 48 51 51 61 30 33 43 43 46 47 56 33 38 41 41 43 44 52 31 35 40 40 50 27 30 35 35 43 26 30 30 37
90 22 25 28 28 30 30 36 18 20 26 26 27 28 33 20 23 24 24 26 26 31 18 21 24 24 30 16 18 21 21 26 15 18 18 22
75 27 30 34 34 37 37 44 21 24 31 31 33 34 40 24 28 29 29 31 32 38 22 25 29 29 36 19 22 25 25 31 19 21 21 27
64 50 32 36 41 41 44 44 53 25 29 37 37 40 40 48 29 33 35 35 37 38 45 27 30 35 35 43 23 26 30 30 37 22 26 26 32
25 37 42 48 48 51 51 61 30 34 44 44 46 47 56 34 39 41 41 43 44 52 31 35 41 41 50 27 30 35 35 43 26 30 30 37
10 42 48 54 54 57 58 69 33 38 49 49 52 53 63 38 43 46 46 49 49 59 35 39 46 46 57 30 34 39 39 49 29 34 34 42
Sustained forcesd
90 8 10 12 13 15 15 18 6 8 10 11 12 12 15 7 8 9 9 10 11 13 7 8 9 11 13 6 7 8 9 10 6 6 7 9
75 10 13 16 17 19 20 23 8 10 13 14 16 16 19 9 10 12 12 14 14 17 9 10 12 14 16 7 9 10 11 14 7 8 10 11
144 50 13 16 20 21 23 24 28 10 13 16 17 19 20 23 11 13 14 15 17 17 20 11 13 15 17 20 9 11 12 14 17 9 10 12 14
25 15 20 24 25 28 29 34 12 15 20 20 23 24 28 13 15 17 18 20 21 24 13 15 18 20 24 11 13 15 17 20 11 12 14 17
10 17 22 27 28 32 33 39 14 17 22 23 26 27 32 14 17 19 20 23 24 28 15 17 20 23 27 12 14 17 19 23 12 14 16 19
90 10 13 16 17 19 20 24 8 10 13 14 16 16 19 9 10 12 12 14 14 17 9 10 12 14 17 7 9 10 12 14 7 9 10 12
75 13 17 21 22 25 26 30 11 13 17 18 20 21 25 11 14 15 15 18 18 22 12 13 16 18 21 10 11 13 15 18 9 11 13 15
47-17
(Table continued)
47-18
TABLE 47.7 Continued
2.1 m push 7.6 m push 15.2 m push 30.5 m push 45.7 m push 61.0 m push
95 50 16 21 26 27 31 32 37 13 17 21 22 25 26 31 14 17 19 19 22 23 27 14 17 19 22 26 12 14 16 19 22 12 14 16 18
25 19 26 31 33 37 38 45 16 20 26 27 30 31 37 17 20 22 23 26 27 32 17 20 23 27 32 14 17 19 22 26 14 16 19 22
10 22 29 36 37 42 43 51 18 23 29 31 34 36 42 19 23 26 27 30 31 37 19 23 27 31 36 16 19 22 25 30 16 19 21 25
90 11 14 17 18 20 21 25 9 11 14 15 17 17 20 9 11 12 13 15 15 18 9 11 13 15 18 8 9 11 12 15 8 9 10 12
75 14 19 23 23 26 27 32 11 14 19 19 22 22 26 12 14 16 17 19 19 23 12 14 17 19 23 10 12 14 16 19 10 12 13 16
64 50 17 23 28 29 32 34 40 14 18 23 24 27 28 33 15 18 20 21 23 24 28 15 18 21 24 27 13 15 17 20 23 12 14 16 20
25 20 27 33 35 39 40 48 17 21 27 28 32 33 39 18 21 24 25 28 29 34 18 21 25 28 33 15 18 21 24 28 15 17 20 23
10 23 31 38 40 45 46 54 19 24 31 32 37 38 45 20 24 27 28 32 33 39 21 24 28 32 38 17 20 24 27 32 17 20 23 27
a
Vertical distance from floor to hands (cm).
b
Percentage of industrial population.
c
The force required to get an object in motion.
d
The force required to keep an object in motion.
Note: Italicized values exceed 8 h physiological criteria.
Source: Reprinted from Snook, S. H. and Ciriello, V. M. Ergonomics, 34, 1197–1213, 1991. With permission.
Psychophysical Approach to Task Analysis
TABLE 47.8 Maximum Acceptable Forces of Pull for Females (kg)
2.1 m push 7.6 m push 15.2 m push 30.5 m push 45.7 m push 61.0 m push
One push every One push every One push every One push every One push every One push every
sec min h sec min h sec min h min h min h min h
Heighta Percentb 6 12 1 2 5 30 8 15 22 1 2 5 30 8 25 35 1 2 5 30 8 1 2 5 30 8 1 2 5 30 8 2 5 30 8
Initial forcesc
90 13 16 17 18 20 21 22 13 14 16 16 18 19 20 10 12 13 14 15 16 17 12 13 14 15 17 12 13 14 15 17 12 13 14 15
75 16 19 20 21 24 25 26 16 17 19 19 21 22 24 12 14 16 16 18 19 20 14 16 17 18 20 14 16 17 18 20 14 15 16 18
135 50 19 22 24 25 28 29 31 19 20 22 23 25 26 28 14 16 19 19 21 22 24 17 18 20 21 24 17 18 20 21 24 16 18 19 21
25 21 25 28 29 32 33 35 21 23 25 26 29 30 32 16 19 21 22 25 26 27 19 21 23 24 27 19 21 23 24 27 19 20 22 25
10 24 28 31 32 36 37 39 24 26 28 29 32 34 36 18 21 24 25 27 29 30 22 24 25 27 31 22 24 25 27 31 21 23 24 27
90 14 16 18 19 21 22 23 14 15 16 17 19 20 21 10 12 14 14 16 17 18 13 14 15 16 18 13 14 15 16 18 12 13 14 16
75 16 19 21 22 25 26 27 17 18 19 20 22 23 25 12 15 17 17 19 20 21 15 16 18 19 21 15 16 18 19 21 15 16 17 19
89 50 19 23 25 26 29 30 32 19 21 23 24 26 27 29 14 17 19 20 22 23 25 18 19 21 22 25 18 19 21 22 25 17 18 20 22
25 22 26 29 30 33 35 37 22 24 26 27 30 31 33 16 20 22 23 26 27 28 20 22 24 25 29 20 22 24 25 29 20 21 23 26
10 25 29 32 33 37 39 41 25 27 29 30 33 35 37 18 22 25 26 29 30 32 23 25 26 28 32 23 25 26 28 32 22 24 25 29
90 15 17 19 20 22 23 24 15 16 17 18 20 21 22 11 13 15 15 17 18 19 13 14 15 17 19 13 14 15 17 19 13 14 15 17
75 17 20 22 23 26 27 28 17 19 20 21 23 24 26 13 15 17 18 20 21 22 16 17 18 20 22 16 17 18 20 22 15 16 18 20
57 50 20 24 26 27 30 32 33 20 22 24 25 28 29 30 15 18 20 21 23 24 26 18 20 22 23 26 18 20 22 23 26 18 19 21 23
25 23 27 30 31 35 36 38 23 25 27 29 32 33 35 17 21 23 24 27 28 30 21 23 25 27 30 21 23 25 27 30 21 22 24 27
10 26 31 34 35 39 40 43 26 28 31 32 35 37 39 19 23 26 27 30 31 33 24 26 28 30 34 24 26 28 30 34 23 25 27 30
Sustained forcesd
90 6 9 10 10 11 12 15 7 8 9 9 10 11 13 6 7 7 8 8 9 11 6 7 7 8 10 6 6 7 7 9 5 5 5 7
75 8 12 13 14 15 16 20 9 11 12 12 13 14 18 7 9 10 10 11 12 15 8 9 10 10 14 8 9 9 9 12 7 7 7 10
135 50 10 16 17 18 19 21 25 12 13 15 16 18 18 22 9 11 13 13 14 15 19 11 12 12 13 17 10 11 11 12 16 8 9 9 12
25 13 19 21 21 23 25 31 14 16 18 19 21 22 27 11 14 15 16 17 19 23 13 15 15 16 21 12 13 14 14 19 10 11 11 15
10 15 22 24 25 27 29 36 16 19 21 22 24 26 32 13 16 18 18 20 22 27 15 17 17 18 25 14 15 16 17 23 12 12 13 17
90 6 9 10 10 11 12 14 7 8 9 9 10 10 13 5 6 7 7 8 9 11 6 7 7 7 10 5 6 6 7 9 5 5 5 7
75 8 12 13 13 15 16 19 9 10 11 12 13 14 17 7 8 10 10 11 12 14 8 9 9 10 13 7 8 9 9 12 6 7 7 9
89 50 10 15 16 17 19 20 25 11 13 15 15 16 18 22 9 11 12 13 14 15 18 10 12 12 13 17 9 11 11 12 15 8 8 9 12
47-19
(Table continued)
47-20
TABLE 47.8 Continued
2.1 m push 7.6 m push 15.2 m push 30.5 m push 45.7 m push 61.0 m push
25 12 18 20 21 23 24 30 14 16 18 18 20 22 27 11 13 15 15 17 18 22 12 14 15 15 21 11 13 13 14 19 10 10 11 15
10 14 21 23 24 26 28 35 16 18 21 21 23 25 31 13 15 17 18 20 21 26 15 16 17 18 24 13 15 16 16 22 12 12 13 17
90 5 8 9 9 1 11 13 6 7 8 8 9 10 12 5 6 7 7 7 8 10 6 6 6 7 9 5 6 6 6 8 4 5 5 6
75 7 11 12 12 13 14 18 8 9 11 11 12 13 16 7 8 9 9 10 11 13 7 8 9 9 12 7 8 8 8 11 6 6 6 9
57 50 9 14 15 16 17 18 23 10 12 13 14 15 16 20 8 10 11 12 13 14 17 9 11 11 12 16 9 10 10 11 14 8 8 8 11
25 11 17 18 19 21 22 27 13 15 16 17 19 20 24 10 12 14 14 16 17 21 11 13 13 14 19 11 12 12 13 17 9 10 10 13
10 13 20 21 22 24 26 32 15 17 19 20 22 23 28 12 14 16 16 18 19 24 13 15 16 16 22 12 14 14 15 20 11 11 12 16
a
Vertical distance from floor to hands (cm).
b
Percentage of industrial population.
c
The force required to get an object in motion.
d
The force required to keep an object in motion.
Note: Italicized values exceed 8 h physiological criteria.
Source: Reprinted from Snook, S. H. and Ciriello, V. M. Ergonomics, 34, 1197–1213, 1991. With permission.
Psychophysical Approach to Task Analysis
TABLE 47.9 Maximum Acceptable Weight of Carry (kg)
2.1 m carry One carry every 4.3 m carry One carry every 8.5 m carry One carry every
sec min h sec min h sec min h
Heighta Percentb 6 12 1 2 5 30 8 10 16 1 2 5 30 8 18 24 1 2 5 30 8
Males
90 10 14 17 17 19 21 25 9 11 15 15 17 19 22 10 11 13 13 15 17 20
75 14 19 23 23 26 29 34 13 16 21 21 23 26 30 13 15 18 18 20 23 27
111 50 19 25 30 30 33 38 44 17 20 27 27 30 34 39 17 19 23 24 26 29 35
25 23 30 37 37 41 46 54 20 25 33 33 37 41 48 21 24 29 29 32 36 43
10 27 35 43 43 48 54 63 24 29 38 39 43 48 57 24 28 34 34 38 42 50
90 13 17 21 21 23 26 31 11 14 18 19 21 23 27 13 18 17 18 20 22 26
75 18 23 28 29 32 36 42 16 19 25 25 28 32 37 17 20 24 24 27 30 35
79 50 23 30 37 37 41 46 54 20 25 32 33 36 41 48 22 26 31 31 35 39 46
25 28 37 45 46 51 57 67 25 30 40 40 45 50 59 27 32 38 38 42 48 56
10 33 43 53 53 59 66 78 29 35 47 47 52 59 69 32 38 44 45 50 56 65
Females
90 11 12 13 13 13 13 18 9 10 13 13 13 13 18 10 11 12 12 12 12 16
75 13 14 15 15 16 16 21 11 12 15 15 16 16 21 12 13 14 14 14 14 19
105 50 15 16 18 18 18 18 25 12 13 18 18 18 18 24 14 15 16 16 16 16 22
25 17 18 20 20 21 21 28 14 15 20 20 21 21 28 15 17 18 18 19 19 25
10 19 20 22 22 23 23 31 16 17 22 22 23 23 31 17 19 20 20 21 21 28
90 13 14 16 16 16 16 22 10 11 14 14 14 14 20 12 12 14 14 14 14 19
75 15 17 18 18 19 19 25 11 13 16 16 17 17 23 14 15 16 16 17 17 23
72 50 17 19 21 21 22 22 29 13 15 19 19 20 20 26 16 17 19 19 20 20 26
25 20 22 24 24 25 25 33 15 22 22 22 22 22 30 18 19 21 22 22 22 30
10 22 24 27 27 28 28 37 17 19 24 24 25 25 33 20 21 24 24 25 25 33
a
Vertical distance from floor to hands (cm).
b
Percentage of industrial population.
Note: Italicized values exceed 8 h physiological criteria (see text).
Source: Reprinted from Snook, S. H. and Ciriello, V. M. Ergonomics, 34, 1197–1213, 1991. With permission.
47-21
47-22 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
limited headroom, asymmetrical lifting, load asymmetry, couplings, load placement clearance, and heat
stress.
Mital28 found that psychophysical data collected in short periods (i.e., 20 –25 min) assuming a longer
work period (8–12 h) should be reduced. Subsequently, Mital22 presented psychophysical data for males
and females performing lifting tasks for eight-hour work shifts based on the adjustments determined in
the earlier study. The data were collected from 37 males and 37 females experienced in manual lifting.
Mital22 also presented a modified database representing the combined data from his study, Snook’s1
data, and data collected by Ayoub et al.21 Although the modified database only accommodates lifting
tasks, the combined sample size is considerable. Similarly, Mital23 presented a psychophysical database
for lifting tasks for males and females working 12-h shifts. The database represents values valid for 12 h
based on adjustments of 8-h data.
assess multiple component tasks as “whole entities.” In general, the design and evaluation of multiple
component MMH jobs is one of the more underdeveloped areas of MMH research and practice.
The only other method for multitask assessment that incorporates MMH tasks in addition to lifting
that the author was able to find was the method presented by Mital.33 The data used with this method-
ology is from Mital et al.,6 which is modified psychophysical data as described earlier. The method
is similar to that developed by Jiang and Mital,34 except that capacity is predicted using more
contemporary data.
In general, this method requires that each MMH task is analyzed, and data regarding work duration,
etc. are also needed. The analyst then determines the percentage of the population that the design should
accommodate, which should be 75% or 90%. The next step involves calculating the recommended work
rate (kg m/min) for the percentage of the population being analyzed using the Mital et al.6 data. The
actual work rate is divided by the recommended work rate, which yields the risk potential. Any risk
potential values greater than 1 signal the need for task redesign. This method focuses on the individual
components that are unacceptable, as with the Snook and Ciriello20 method.
47.2.1.3 Example
An example of Snook and Ciriello’s20 multicomponent task assessment will be used to illustrate how psy-
chophysical data are used to analyze MMH tasks. The analysis was performed with the CompuTaskTM
computer program. The set of tasks is fairly simple and includes a worker bending over and lifting a
box, carrying the box 5 ft, and lowering the box to the floor. The set of tasks is performed three times
per minute for 8 h. The relevant data that need to be collected as well as the analysis are shown in
Figure 47.2. Aside from psychophysical results, physiological analyses and National Institute of
Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH) lifting equation computations (STRWL ¼ single task
recommended weight limit, FIRWL ¼ frequency independent recommended weight limit, STLI ¼ single
task lifting index, FILI ¼ frequency independent lifting index) are provided by the software.
The task with the lowest percentage of the population accommodated is the lifting task. This task
accommodates 75% of the male population and ,10% of the female population. Thus, the set of
tasks is marginally acceptable for males and unacceptable for females. Also, the overall physiological
evaluation shows that the task is not acceptable for 8 h. As was discussed earlier, the method of analysis
being used may result in violation of energy expenditure criterion. Redesign efforts would be focused on
eliminating the tasks through materials handling devices or a conveyor, or eliminating the need to lift and
lower the boxes by increasing the vertical origin and destination of the lifting and lowering tasks,
respectively.
Evaluation Results
“POPULATION PERCENTAGES” are the percentages of the male and female population that can be
expected to perform the task without excessive stress or excessive fatigue.
“SUGGESTED MAXIMUM DURATION” is the recommended continuous time the job can be performed during
an 8-h workday before exceeding the Energy Expenditure (kcal/min) guidelines for males and females.
FIGURE 47.2 Example of psychophysical analysis of a multiple-component MMH job using CompuTaskTM.
Psychophysical Approach to Task Analysis 47-25
3. Decrease the vertical distance that loads must be lifted/lowered and the distance which loads must
be pushed, pulled or carried
4. Decrease the frequency of the task or increase the number or workers performing the task
5. For pushing and pulling tasks, provide equipment that provides the least resistance so that initial
forces required to overcome inertia are as low as possible. Maintenance of mechanical assists is
very important with regards to this principle
6. For all MMH tasks, provide good hand-to-object coupling when possible, that is, tote boxes with
handles, carts with handle bars, etc
7. Decrease the duration over which the task is performed
8. Change pulling tasks to pushing tasks
TABLE 47.10 Maximum Acceptable Forces for Female Wrist Flexion (Power Grip) (N)
Repetition rate
Percent of population 2/min 5/min 10/min 15/min 20/min
90 14.9 14.9 13.5 12.0 10.2
75 23.2 23.2 20.9 18.6 15.8
50 32.3 32.3 29.0 26.0 22.1
25 41.5 41.5 37.2 33.5 28.4
10 49.8 49.8 44.6 40.1 34.0
Source: Reprinted from Snook, S. H. and Ciriello, V. M. Journal of Occupational Medicine, 16, 527–534, 1974. With
permission.
duration would need to be reduced, or factors such as wrist deviation would need to be modified to
increase the acceptability of a task.
While Fernandez and his colleagues chose frequency as the variable that subjects manipulate, Snook
et al.14 chose force as the manipulated variable. Also, Snook et al.14 used a 7-h adjustment period
which was much longer than the shorter (20–25 min) period used in the studies cited earlier.
Snook et al.14 studied tasks requiring wrist flexion with a power grasp, wrist flexion with a pinch
grip, and extension with a power grasp. Frequencies between 2 and 20 repetitive motions per minute
were studied. Subjects adjusted wrist torque during the experiment by manipulating the resistance
offered by a magnetic particle brake. Aside from reporting the torques, forces were also reported
which were computed by dividing the torques by the moment arms. The forces are reported in
Table 47.10 through Table 47.12.
Ciriello et al.46 reported the results of a psychophysical study of six hand movements. In addition to
the motions studied by Snook et al.,14 wrist extension with a pinch grip, ulnar deviation with a power
grip, and a handgrip task (power grip) were studied. Ciriello et al.47 investigated ulnar deviation with
a power grip and the handgrip task, but included clockwise screwdriver motions using 31 and 40 mm
handles and a 39 mm yoke handle. Counterclockwise screwdriver motions with a 31 mm handle
completed the experimental conditions. Subjects selected 14– 24% of maximum isometric torque for
the different motions, at different frequencies.
The data reported by Snook et al.14 and Ciriello et al.46,47 would be applied in a manner similar to that
described earlier in this section. However, these data are more generic than some of the data collected by
Fernandez and his colleagues. The Ciriello et al.47 data contain a mix of generic and task-specific data.
For example, the data collected by Snook et al.14 do not apply only to specific tasks such as drilling,
as do some of the data from other studies mentioned.37 – 40
Krawczyk et al.48 presented preferred weights for manual transfer tasks for transfer distances of 0.5 and
1.0 m and frequencies between 10 and 30 transfers per minute for an 8 h work duration. Thus, depending
on the situation, one could adjust frequency or transfer distance for a particular weight of object being
transferred.
TABLE 47.11 Maximum Acceptable Forces for Female Wrist Flexion (Pinch Grip) (N)
Repetition rate
Percent of population 2/min 5/min 10/min 15/min 20/min
90 9.2 8.5 7.4 7.4 6.0
75 14.2 13.2 11.5 11.5 9.3
75 19.8 18.4 16.0 16.0 12.9
75 25.4 23.6 20.6 20.6 16.6
10 30.5 28.2 24.6 24.6 19.8
Source: Reprinted from Snook, S. H. and Ciriello, V. M. Journal of Occupational Medicine, 16, 527–534, 1974. With
permission.
Psychophysical Approach to Task Analysis 47-27
TABLE 47.12 Maximum Acceptable Forces for Female Wrist Extension (Power Grip) (N)
Repetition rate
Percent of population 2/min 5/min 10/min 15/min 20/min
90 8.8 8.8 7.8 6.9 5.4
75 13.6 13.6 12.1 10.9 8.5
75 18.9 18.9 16.8 15.1 11.9
75 24.2 24.2 21.5 19.3 15.2
10 29.0 29.0 25.8 23.2 18.3
Source: Reprinted from Snook, S. H. and Ciriello, V. M. Journal of Occupational Medicine, 16, 527–534, 1974. With
permission.
samples of university students. Likewise, the representativeness of cadaver data used to set certain
biomechanical criteria such as lumbosacral compression limits is questionable
. Psychophysical results are consistent with the industrial engineering concept of a “fair day’s work
for a fair day’s pay”2
. Psychophysics can be used to study intermittent MMH tasks which are common in industry.2 Such
tasks are not amenable to physiological analyses
. Psychophysical results are very reproducible2
. For MMH tasks, psychophysical judgments take into account the whole job, and integrate
biomechanical and physiological factors52,53
. Psychophysical results for MMH tasks appear to be related to low-back pain1,2,27
. For MMH tasks, psychophysical data apply to a wider array of tasks than either the biomechanical
or physiological approach54
. For MMH tasks that must necessarily be performed under postural restrictions (i.e., maintenance
work and mining), psychophysics is one technique that can be used to develop handling limits
specific to the tasks being examined54
. The psychophysical approach is less costly and less time consuming to apply in industry than many
of the biomechanical and physiological techniques54
. Currently, psychophysical data represent one of the only quantitative guides for the design of
force limits for UEI work. In the absence of objective biomechanical or physiological criteria,
psychophysics may be used to elicit acceptable task parameters for UEI work8
The disadvantages and limitations of the psychophysical approach include:
. Psychophysics is a subjective method2
. The assumption that the subjective workloads selected by subjects are below the threshold for
injury has not been validated.55 There is not extensive epidemiological support for psychophysical
data for MMH tasks and no epidemiological support for using psychophysical data for the design
of UEI data. However, the same is true of most of the other criteria currently in use for designing
manual work
. Psychophysical results for high-frequency MMH tasks exceed energy expenditure criteria2
. Some psychophysical values for MMH tasks may violate the biomechanical spinal compression
criterion of 3400 N.56 However, this assumes that the spinal compression criterion of 3400 N is
correct, for which there is not much support
. Psychophysics does not appear to be sensitive to bending and twisting while performing MMH
tasks, both of which have been related to compensable low-back pain cases2
. The range of data for designing UEI tasks is somewhat limited at this time
47.5 Conclusions
Psychophysical data are one option available to the ergonomist for designing manual tasks and assessing
whether or not a task or set of tasks needs to be redesigned. These data have been applied in the workplace
for many years with considerable success. As with any assessment tool, there are advantages and
limitations associated with psychophysics as discussed in the previous section. When used properly,
psychophysical data provide the analyst with a tool applicable to a diverse set of tasks involving
manual work.
References
1. Snook, S. H., The design of manual handling tasks, Ergonomics, 21, 963– 985, 1978.
2. Snook, S. H., Psychophysical considerations in permissible loads, Ergonomics, 28, 327–330, 1985.
3. Snook, S. H., Future directions of psychophysical studies, Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environ-
ment & Health, 25, 13 –18, 1999.
Psychophysical Approach to Task Analysis 47-29
4. Ayoub, M. M. and Mital, A., Manual Materials Handling, Taylor & Francis, London, 1989.
5. Ayoub, M. M., Problems and solutions in manual materials handling: the state of the art, Ergonomics,
35, 713–728, 1992.
6. Mital, A., Nicholson, A. S, and Ayoub, M.M., A Guide to Manual Materials Handling, Taylor &
Francis, London, 1993.
7. Nicholson, L. M., A comparative study of methods for establishing load handling capabilities,
Ergonomics, 32, 1125–1144, 1989.
8. Kim, C. H., Marley, R. J., Fernandez, J. E., and Klein, M. G., Acceptable work limits for the upper
extremities with the psychophysical approach, in Proceedings of the 3rd Pan Pacific Conference on
Occupational Ergonomics, 1994, 312–316.
9. Fernandez, J. E., Fredericks, T. K., and Marley, R. J., The psychophysical approach in upper extremi-
ties work, in Contemporary Ergonomics 1995, Robertson, S. A., Ed., Taylor & Francis, London, 1995,
456–461.
10. Stevens, S. S., The psychophysics of sensory function, American Scientist, 48, 226 –253, 1960.
11. Ljungberg, A. S., Gamberale, F., and Kilbom, Å., Horizontal lifting — physiological and psychologi-
cal responses, Ergonomics, 25, 741– 757, 1982.
12. Gamberale, F., Ljungberg, A. S., Annwall, G., and Kilbom, Å., An experimental evaluation of psycho-
physical criteria for repetitive lifting work, Applied Ergonomics, 18, 311 –321, 1987.
13. Snook, S. H. and Ciriello, V. M., Maximum weights and work loads acceptable to female workers,
Journal of Occupational Medicine, 16, 527 –534, 1974.
14. Snook, S. H., Vaillancourt, D. R., Ciriello, V. M., and Webster, B. S., Psychophysical studies of repeti-
tive wrist flexion and extension, Ergonomics, 38, 1488–1507, 1995.
15. Emanuel, I., Chaffee, J. W., and Wing, J., A study of human weight lifting capabilities for loading
ammunition into the F-86H aircraft, WADC Technical Report 56 –367, Wright Air Development
Center, Wright-Patterson Air Force Base, Ohio, 1956.
16. Switzer, S. A., Weight-lifting capabilities of a selected sample of human subjects, Technical Document
Report No. MRL-TDR-62-57, Aerospace Medical Research Laboratories, Wright-Patterson Air Force
Base, Ohio, 1962.
17. Snook, S. H. and Irvine, C. H., The evaluation of physical tasks in industry, American Industrial
Hygiene Association Journal, 27, 228 –233, 1966.
18. Ciriello, V. M., Snook, S. H., and Hughes, G. J. Further studies of psychophysically determined
maximum acceptable weights and forces, Human Factors, 35, 175–186, 1993.
19. Ciriello, V. M., Snook, S. H., Blick, A. C., and Wilkinson, P. L., The effects of task duration on
psychophysically-determined maximum acceptable weights and forces, Ergonomics, 33, 187–200, 1990.
20. Snook, S. H. and Ciriello, V. M., The design of manual handling tasks: revised tables of maximum
acceptable weights and forces, Ergonomics, 34, 1197–1213, 1991.
21. Ayoub, M. M., Bethea, N., Deivanayagam, S., Asfour, S., Bakken, G., Liles, D., Selan, J., and Sherif, M.,
Determination and modeling of lifting capacity, Final Report, HEW (NIOSH) Grant No.
5R010H00545-02, 1978.
22. Mital, A., Comprehensive maximum acceptable weight of lift database for regular 8-hour work shifts,
Ergonomics, 27, 1127 –1138, 1984.
23. Mital, A., Maximum weights of lift acceptable to male and female industrial workers for extended
work shifts, Ergonomics, 27, 1115–1126, 1984.
24. Fox, R. R., A psychophysical study of high-frequency lifting, Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Texas Tech University, Lubbock, Texas, 1993.
25. Nicholson, L. M. and Legg, S. J., A psychophysical study of the effects of load and frequency upon
selection of workload in repetitive lifting, Ergonomics, 29, 903– 911, 1986.
26. Snook, S. H. and Irvine, C. H., Maximum frequency of lift acceptable to male industrial workers,
American Industrial Hygiene Association Journal, 29, 531 –536, 1968.
27. Liles, D. H., Deivanayagam, S., Ayoub, M. M., and Mahajan, P., A job severity index for the evalu-
ation and control of lifting injury, Human Factors, 26, 683–693 1984.
47-30 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
28. Mital, A., The psychophysical approach in manual lifting — a verification study, Human Factors, 25,
485 –491, 1983.
29. Smith, J. L., Ayoub, M. M., and McDaniel, J. W., Manual materials handling capabilities in non-
standard postures, Ergonomics, 35, 807 –831, 1992.
30. Gallagher, S., Acceptable weights and physiological costs of performing combined manual handling
tasks in restricted postures, Ergonomics, 34, 939–952, 1991.
31. Gallagher, S. and Hamrick, C.A., Acceptable workloads for three common mining materials, Ergo-
nomics, 35, 1013 –1031, 1992.
32. Straker, L. M., Stevenson, M. G., and Twomey, L. T., A comparison of risk assessment of single and
combination manual handling tasks: 1. Maximum acceptable weight measures, Ergonomics, 39,
128 –140, 1996.
33. Mital, A., Using “A Guide to Manual Materials Handling” for designing/evaluating multiple activity
manual materials handling tasks, in Proceedings of the IEA World Conference on Ergonomic Design,
Interfaces, Products, and Information, 1995, 550.
34. Jiang, B.C. and Mital, A., A procedure for designing/evaluating manual materials handling tasks,
International Journal of Production Research, 24, 913–925, 1986.
35. Putz-Anderson, V. (Ed.), Cumulative Trauma Disorders: A Manual for Musculoskeletal Disorders of the
Upper Limb, Taylor and Francis, London, 1988.
36. Potvin, J. R., Chiang, J., McKean, C., and Stephens, A., A psychophysical study to determine accep-
table limits for repetitive hand impact severity during automotive trim installation, International
Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 26, 625–637, 2000.
37. Davis, P. J. and Fernandez, J. E., Maximum acceptable frequencies for females performing a drilling
task in different wrist postures, Journal of Human Ergology, 23, 81 –92, 1994.
38. Fernandez, J. E., Dahalan, J. B., and Klein, M. G., Using the psychophysical approach in hand-wrist
work, in Proceedings of the M.M. Ayoub Occupational Ergonomics Symposium, Institute for Ergo-
nomics Research, Lubbock, Texas, 1993, 63.
39. Kim, C. H. and Fernandez, J. E., Psychophysical frequency for a drilling task, International Journal of
Industrial Ergonomics, 12, 209 –218, 1993.
40. Vaidyanathan, V. and Fernandez, J. E., MAF for males performing drilling tasks, in Proceedings of the
Human Factors Society 36th Annual Meeting, Human Factors Society, Santa Monica, California, 1992,
692 –696.
41. Fredericks, T. K., The effect of vibration on maximum acceptable frequency for a riveting task,
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, 1995.
42. Fredericks, T. K. and Fernandez, J. E., The effect of vibration on maximum acceptable frequency for a
riveting task, in Proceedings of the Konz/Purswell Occupational Ergonomics Symposium, Institute for
Ergonomics Research, Lubbock, Texas, 1995, 27.
43. Dahalan, J. B. and Fernandez, J. E., Psychophysical frequency for a gripping task, International
Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 12, 219–230, 1993.
44. Klein, M. G. and Fernandez, J. E., The effects of posture, duration, and force on pinching frequency,
International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 20, 267 –275, 1997.
45. Abu-Ali, M., Purswell, J. L., and Schlegel, R. E., Psychophysically determined work-cycle par-
ameters for repetitive hand gripping, International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 17, 35 – 42,
1996.
46. Ciriello, V. M., Snook, S. H., Webster, B. S., and Dempsey, P. G., A psychophysical study of six hand
movements. Ergonomics, 44, 922–936, 2001.
47. Ciriello, V. M., Webster, B. S., and Dempsey, P. G., Maximum acceptable torques of highly repetitive
screw driving, ulnar deviation, and handgrip tasks for seven hour workdays. American Industrial
Hygiene Association Journal, 63, 594 –604, 2002.
48. Krawczyk, S., Armstrong, T. J., and Snook, S. H., Preferred weights for hand transfer tasks for an
eight hour day, in Proceedings of International Scientific Conference on Prevention of Work-related
Musculoskeletal Disorders, Hagberg, M., and Kilbom, Å., Eds., 1992, 157.
Psychophysical Approach to Task Analysis 47-31
49. Ulin, S. S., Armstrong, T. J., Snook, S. H., and Franzblau, A., Effect of tool shape and work location
on perceived exertion for work on horizontal surfaces, American Industrial Hygiene Association
Journal, 54, 383–391, 1993.
50. Ulin, S. S., Armstrong, T. J., Snook, S. H., and Keyserling, W. M., Examination of the effect of tool
mass and work postures on perceived exertion for a screw driving task, International Journal of Indus-
trial Ergonomics, 12, 105 –115, 1993.
51. Ulin, S. S., Snook, S. H., Armstrong, T. J., and Herrin, G. D., Preferred tool shapes for various hori-
zontal and vertical work locations, Applied Occupational and Environmental Hygiene, 7, 327 –337,
1992.
52. Haslegrave, C. M. and Corlett, E. N., Evaluating work conditions for risk of injury–techniques for
field surveys, in Evaluation of Human Work, 2nd. ed., Wilson, J. R., and Corlett, E. N., Eds., Taylor &
Francis, London, 1995, 892–920.
53. Karwowski, W. and Ayoub, M. M., Fuzzy modelling of stresses in manual lifting tasks, Ergonomics,
27, 641 –649, 1984.
54. Ayoub, M. M. and Dempsey, P. G., The psychophysical approach to manual materials handling task
design, Ergonomics, 42, 17–31, 1999.
55. Gamberale, F. and Kilbom, Å., An experimental evaluation of psychophysically determined
maximum acceptable workload for repetitive lifting work, in Ergonomics International 88, Adams,
A. S., Hall, R. R., McPhee, B. J., and Oxenburgh, M. S., Eds., Ergonomics Society of Australia,
Sydney, 1988, 233– 235.
56. Chaffin, D. B. and Page, G. B., Postural effects on biomechanical and psychophysical weight-lifting
limits, Ergonomics, 37, 663– 676, 1994.
48
Static Biomechanical
Modeling in Manual
Lifting
48.1 Introduction
Though most manual tasks in industry involve significant body motions, it continues to be very helpful
to evaluate specific exertions within a manual task by performing a static biomechanical analysis. Such
analyses are normally performed by combining the postural information (body angles) obtained from a
stopped frame video image (or photograph) of a worker, and measured forces exerted at the hands. The
latter is often obtained with a simple handheld force gauge.
What follows is a description of a computerized static biomechanical model that has been developed
and used over the last 30 yr to predict:
1. The percentage of men and women who would be capable of exerting specified hand forces in
various work postures
2. The forces acting on various spinal motion segments
Since these two different output predictions have specific criterion values referenced in the NIOSH
Work Practices Guide (NIOSH, 1981), they are often used by professional ergonomists to determine
the relative risk of injury associated with the performance of a manual exertion of interest (Chaffin,
1988a). It also should be noted that the prediction of the percent of the population capable of performing
a specific exertion required on a job is often crucial to the determination of a job specific strength test
score for pre-employment and return to work purposes (Chaffin, 1996). Finally, because the biomechanical
population strengths and low back stresses are predicted by a computerized model which runs on
common personal computer platforms, this has meant that job and product designers and engineers
have been able to simulate various expected high exertion tasks during the early part of the design process,
and thus avoid costly prototype evaluations and retrofits when the products and processes become
operational (Chaffin, 2001). It is this latter application of the biomechanical static strength prediction
48-1
48-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
programs that provides perhaps the greatest potential benefit over other common job evaluation
methods. Many other methods require a person to be observed and measured, sometimes with expensive
instrumentation. This precludes the use of these empirical methods for use in prescriptive job design;
where in the job exists only on paper or in a computer rendered drawing of the workspace. By interfacing
a computerized biomechanical model of a person (as described in the following) into a computerized
rendering of the workspace, the designer can quickly perform a large number of simulated exertions
to determine the human consequences of altering a proposed job design, much like that being done
to accommodate various sized individuals using computerized anthropometric manikins.
What follows is a brief description of the development of a static biomechanical strength modeling
technology, including a couple illustrations of how it has been used to evaluate various manual lifting
situations.
ADJUST POSTURES TO BE
ROM NORMS
WITHIN ROM NORMS
POPULATION
INPUT DATA:
FIGURE 48.1 Biomechanical logic used to predict whole-body static exertion capabilities for given postures, hand
force directions, and anthropometric groups.
Static Biomechanical Modeling in Manual Lifting 48-3
R2
x
y R4
FLHA
V3
ID
R3
R1 M2
V6
M4
V2cg
V4cg V3cg
V2
R5 V7
M1
V4
M3
V1cg W3
W2 V5cg W1
R6 Rg
V1 M5
V6 W2
Vg
M6
M9
W1
V100g V12cg
W4 R10
V12
R7
FRHAND W5
W5 M10
M7
V10
V130g
V11cg
R8
R11
W6
W6
M8 V11 V13
W7 W7
VRFOOT
VLFOOT
FIGURE 48.2 Three-dimensional distance, force, and moment vectors used in 12 link biomechanical model for
strength prediction. (From Chaffin, D.B. and Erig, M. 1991. IIE Trans. 23(3):216 –227. With permission.)
The static moment equilibrium equations for the elbow and shoulder in this linkage can be defined as:
where F R HAND is the right hand force with X, Y, Z unit vector (i, j, k) components for each, and FRX, FRY,
and FRZ are, respectively, the X, Y, and Z components of the right hand force.
where V 1 is the forearm length with link unit vectors, V1X, V1Y, V1Z are, respectively, the X, Y, Z com-
ponents of the forearm unit vector, and LeLink is the magnitude of forearm length from anthropometric
data.
V1cg ¼ (V 1X i þ V 1Y j þ V 1Z k) (cglink)
where V1cg is the cg distance from elbow to forearm center of gravity vector expressed in unit vector form,
multiplied by the cglink, which is the magnitude of proximal distance to cg of forearm from anthropo-
metric data.
where M1 is the elbow resultant moment with X, Y, Z unit vector components, and M1X, M1Y, M1Z, are,
respectively, the elbow moment about X, Y, Z axes and
M1 ¼ (V 1 FR HAND þ V 1cg W 1 )
where R1 is the elbow joint reaction force vector with X, Y, Z unit vector components, and
M2 ¼ M 1 þ V2cg W 2 þ V 2 (R1 )
where M2 is the right shoulder resultant moment with V2cg the upperarm center of gravity vector, V 2 the
upperarm link vector, and W 2 the upperarm weight vector.
M2 ¼ M 2X i þ M 2Y j þ M 2Z k
where M2X is the right shoulder movement with X, Y, Z, unit vector components, M2X , M2Y , and M2Z ,
are respectively, the shoulder moment about the X, Y, and Z axes.
A recursive computational procedure is used to continue the analysis to compute external load
moments and forces at the elbow and shoulder of the arm or arms doing the exertion, the lumbosacral
joint, hip joints, and knee and ankle joints.
The size and mass of the person (linkage size) is most often specified as a select stratum of the popu-
lation (i.e., a percentile of specific anthropometric dimensions is selected from population surveys).
Thus, a small, medium, or large man or woman can be specified, or specific link anthropometry can
be used if available. Link length-to-stature ratios from Drillis and Contini (1966) and link mass-to-
bodyweight ratios from Dempster (1955) and Clauser et al. (1969) are used to simplify this procedure,
if specific anthropometry is not available on a subject. Most often an average male or female anthropo-
metry is chosen for assessing the strength requirements of a given task in industry.
The strength moment values used as population limit values in the program were measured by Stobbe
(1982) for 25 men and 22 women employed in manual jobs in three different industries. These values
have been combined with the earlier values from Chaffin and Baker (1970) and Schanne (1972) to
form the statistical data for the population joint moment limits.
Once the size of the person has been specified or selected from a known anthropometric data source,
the posture is entered with reference to either photographs or videos (or by manipulating a computer
generated hominoid) and then the hand forces of interest are entered. The program then computes
the load moments at each joint of the linkage, and compares each to the corresponding strength
Static Biomechanical Modeling in Manual Lifting 48-5
moment capability obtained from the previously measured populations. This provides a prediction of the
percent of the population that is capable of producing the necessary strength moments at each joint.
The logic for computing the lumbar motion segment compression force is shown in Figure 48.3. Once
the lumbar moment is computed (as described in the preceding), torso muscle contraction, which stabil-
izes the column, is estimated. Two models of torso muscle contraction will be described. In the sagittal
plane low-back model by, Chaffin (1975), a single equivalent torso muscle contraction force is
implemented. When the necessary reactive torso muscle force is added to body segment weights and
hand forces (with a minor adjustment for abdominal pressure effects) a prediction of the compression
force on the L5/S1 disc results, as shown in Figure 48.4.
When an asymmetric exertion (e.g., one-handed force, or twisted or laterally bent torso) is being ana-
lyzed, many different torso muscle actions and passive supporting tissue reactions need to be considered
(Chaffin 1988b). The first step in such a procedure requires that the position, orientation, cross-sectional
size, and length of the various connective tissues be modeled at the lumbar spinal level. A geometric
torso model proposed by Nussbaum and Chaffin (1996) for this purpose is shown in Figure 48.5.
This model includes estimates of specific tissue geometry acquired from various computed tomography
(CT) scans (Tracy et al., 1989; Chaffin et al., 1990; Moga, et al., 1993), along with passive tissue reaction
forces estimated by McCully and Faulkner (1983), Nachemson et al. (1979), Miller et al. (1986), and
others.
The most important predictors of spinal column stress, however, are the muscle reaction forces
required to stabilize the spine to external load moments. In three-dimensional (3D) torso models
various approaches have been used to predict these required reactive muscle forces. Perhaps the most
commonly cited torso biomechanical model for 3D static analysis is that developed by Schultz and
FIGURE 48.3 Logic for computing L5/S1 compression forces in 2D/3D Static Strength Prediction Program.
48-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
LOW BACK
MUSCLE FORCE
L5/S1
DISC
MOMENT
DISC
COMPRESSION BW
FORCE
L
H
FIGURE 48.4 Simple low-back model of lifting for static coplanar lifting analyses. The load on the hands LH and
torso and arm weights BW act to create moments at the L5/S1 disc of the spine. The moments are resisted primarily
by the back muscles. The high muscle forces required in such a task cause high disc compression forces.
Andersson (1981). It is depicted in Figure 48.6. Bean et al. (1988) developed a revised version of this
model that provides a more efficient computational method for solving the linear programs used to sim-
ultaneously minimize the torso muscle contraction intensities and motion segment compression forces.
The present 3D low-back model included in the computerized version described in the following text
predicts the minimum muscle force contractile intensities required to meet the moment equilibrium
requirements about the three orthogonal axes of rotation of the motion segment. Given a set of
optimal forces so computed, the model further seeks to minimize the disc compression force. Because
such an approach attempts to minimize both muscle intensity requirements and disc compression
forces simultaneously, it is referred to as a “double linear optimization” approach.
Hughes and Chaffin (1995) proposed that a nonlinear objective function be used as the basis for select-
ing the various muscle reaction forces during a given exertion. They referred to this as the sum of the
cubed muscle intensity objective. Nussbaum et al. (1996) also have proposed a neural network model
to predict torso muscle actions. Raschke and Chaffin (1996) have proposed that the external moment
is normally distributed about the torso, and activates several muscles simultaneously depending on
the direction and magnitude of the external moment.
Erector Spinae
Rectus Abdom
Int. Oblique
Ext. Oblique
Lat. Dorsi
Trans. Abdom.
Psods
Quad. Lumborum
15 cm
FIGURE 48.5 Muscle geometry illustrated for a 50th percentile male. Muscles are treated as point-wise connections
from origin to insertion (see text). An imaginary cutting plane, which bisects the L3/L4 motion segment, is also
shown. (From Nussbaum, M.A. and Chaffin, D.B. 1996. Clin. Biomech. 11(1):25– 34. With permission.)
These have been referred to as the University of Michigan’s Two-Dimensional and Three-Dimensional
Static Strength Prediction ProgramsTM (i.e., 2DSSPPTM and 3DSSPPTM ). Currently only one version is
available, 3DSSPP, which includes both the sagittal plane and 3D low-back models as well as the strength
model. The University of Michigan’s Office of Technology Transfer has granted over 2500 individual
licenses for use of these programs since 1984.
The 3DSSPP program requires more input data than the previous 2D version. 3D exertions often
involve two hand forces, which can act in any direction. Also, a model of the human body in 3D has
12 body links (some with three postural angles). The main window of the 3DSSPP program is depicted
in Figure 48.7. The input values (posture, hand forces, and anthropometry) are entered in dialogs avail-
able from the pull-down menus. Postures can be entered manually by specifying body link angles, by spe-
cifying hand locations and using the posture prediction feature, or graphically by selecting and dragging
joints using a mouse entry device. The posture prediction feature uses an inverse kinematic model with
preferred postural prediction capability and is included to allow the user to easily manipulate the figure.
Orthogonal stick figures depicting the body posture, the hand location, and the hand force directions are
provided across the top of the window. An oblique-view enfleshed hominoid, which along with the stick
figures assist with the entry and adjustment of the posture, is provided in the lower-left area of the
window and can be manipulated to appear in the same orientation as a photo, video, or digital
image. The use of the 3D hominoid was found by Beck and Chaffin (1992) to allow postures to be accu-
rately entered and represented in a computer.
Summary analysis results, including the predicted percent of the population having sufficient strength
to perform the designated exertion, back compression, and balance status, are shown in tabular form in
the lower right quadrant of the main window. From inspection of the percent capable predictions shown
in Figure 48.7 for the analysis of a 50th percentile male lifting a 44-lb stock reel, it is obvious that hip
48-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
x
Xr
Z lr
Rr
P
Lr
R
C Er
Sr
y
Sa
E
I L
X
FIGURE 48.6 Schematic of 10-muscle model. (developed by Schultz and Andersson 1981. Analysis of loads on the
lumbar spine. Spine 6(1): 76 –82. With permission.)
strength is the most limiting muscle group strength (only 86% of men have sufficient hip strength to lift
the reel). This value is below that recommended by NIOSH, which proposes that jobs should accommo-
date 99% of men’s and 75% of women’s strength (or 90% of a mixed gender population). The estimated
3D low-back compression force of 969 lbs also is above the 760 lbs recommended by NIOSH. Two
balance analyses are performed: possible static equilibrium of moments at the ankles and an estimated
center of pressure within the basis of support on the floor. The balance status indicates that the task
as depicted provides acceptable balance.
Analysis output windows for these and other calculated results are available, and two are shown in
Figure 48.8. These depict percent capable strength predictions (for 34 muscle functions) and lumbar
compression forces at the L4L5 disc; the latter includes 3D predictions of a large number of individual
muscle and spinal forces.
FIGURE 48.7 Main screen from the University of Michigan 3D Static Strength Prediction Program for personal
computers shown lifting a 44-lb stock reel. (With permission, Regents of the University of Michigan.)
performed each one. Comparison of the strength prediction program with the group strength data revealed
a very high correlation (r ¼ 0.92) for sagittal plane symmetric exertions. This same study also included
asymmetric simulations with the strength prediction program of 72 different one-arm exertions performed
by five male Army personnel. The correlations ranged from r ¼ 0.71 to r ¼ 0.83. Unfortunately, in this
latter comparison, exact postural and bracing conditions were not available to use in the simulations.
This may have contributed to the lower correlations.
The last validation involved simulations of 56 one- and two-handed, whole-body exertions in 14
different symmetric, bent, and twisted-torso postures (Chaffin and Erig, 1991). The simulation results
were compared with the group strengths of 29 young males. Photographs from several views were avail-
able to assist in replicating the postures used by these subjects. The results indicated that if care is taken to
assure that the posture used in the model simulation is the same as that chosen by people performing the
exertions, the prediction error standard deviation will be less than 6%.
In conclusion, it appears that the strength prediction models and population norms used in the
present models are accurate in predicting the percent of the population capable of performing a large
variety of different types of maximal static exertions. One caution should be noted, however. At
present the strength norms used as limits in the models are based on male and female populations
who are relatively young (i.e., 18–49 yr). To improve the models further, strength values are currently
being gathered on older populations by these investigators. In this regard, one comparison involving
98 men and women with a mean age of 73 yr, showed a major decrease in strength performance in
certain muscle functions. When these decreases were included in the 3DSSPP population data base, it
was found that some exertions that could easily be performed by younger people were predicted to be
impossible to perform by most older people (Chaffin et al., 1994).
48-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
FIGURE 48.8 Analysis result widows from 3DSSPPTM for strength prediction capability and for 3D low-back
compression. (With permission, Regents of the University of Michigan.)
Validation of the low back biomechanical model has been largely dependent on EMG estimates of
muscle reactions in subjects performing controlled torso exertions. Hughes et al. (1994) discusses this
procedure, and the results of comparisons with four different optimization procedures used to predict
torso muscle responses to different external torso moment loads. Generally speaking, relatively high cor-
relation (r . 0.8) are achieved when loading the torso approximately in the sagittal plane. With greater
asymmetric or sudden loading, more complex muscle patterns result, sometimes with a 10 –30%
Static Biomechanical Modeling in Manual Lifting 48-11
antagonistic type of muscle response. These complex responses are often not well predicted (r , 0.6) by
existing models. Thus, it is expected that existing models may underpredict the muscle-induced com-
pression and shear forces on the spinal motion segments by as much as 30% during sudden (i.e.,
jerking) motions or lateral, asymmetric exertions. The newer neural network and geometric moment dis-
tribution models are yet to be thoroughly tested under complex loading conditions. They may be less
sensitive to this cocontraction phenomenon than existing optimization models.
Back Comp. (lbs) 621 538 Back Comp. (lbs) 646 557
Disc Shear (lbs) 96 83 Disc Shear (lbs) 52 44
% Pop. Strength 94% 88% % Pop. Strength 82% 74%
Limiting Muscles Hip Ext. Hip Ext. Limiting Muscles Kn. Ext. Kn. Ext.
FIGURE 48.9 Comparison of two different postures used to lift 25- and 50-lb objects from the floor close to the feet
using the Michigan 3DSSPPTM.
48-12 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Acknowledgments
We wish to acknowledge NIH Grant AR-39599 for partial support of some of the work described in this
presentation.
References
Bean, J.C., Chaffin, D.B., and Schultz A.B. 1988. Biomechanical model calculation muscle contraction
forces: a double linear programming method. J. Biomech. 21(1): 59–66.
Beck, D.J. and Chaffin, D.B. 1992. An evaluation of inverse kinematics models for posture prediction.
Proceedings of CAES 1992, Tampere, Finland. Center for Ergonomics, University of Michigan,
Ann Arbor, MI.
Chaffin, D.B., 1975. On the validity of biomechanical models of the low back for weight lifting analysis.
ASME Proceedings. 75-WA-Bio-1, New York: Amer. Soc. of Mech. Eng.
Chaffin, D.B., 1988a. A biomechanical strength model for use in industry. Appl. Ind. Hyg. 3(3): 79–86.
Chaffin, D.B., 1988b. Biomechanical modeling of the low back during load lifting. Ergonomics 31(5):
685– 697.
Chaffin, D.B., 1996. Ergonomic basis for job-related strength testing. In Disability Evaluations, eds.
S.L. Demeter, G.B.J. Andersson, and G.M. Smith, pp. 159 –167. St. Louis, MO: American
Medical Association, Mosby.
Chaffin, D.B. (ed.) 2001. Digital Human Modeling for Vehicle and Workplace Design. Warrendale,
PA: Society of Automotive Engineers.
Chaffin, D.B. and Baker, W.H. 1970. A biomechanical model for analysis of symmetric sagittal plane
lifting. AIIE Trans. 2(1): 16 –27.
Chaffin, D.B. and Erig, M. 1991. Three-dimensional biomechanical static strength prediction model sen-
sitivity to postural and anthropometric inaccuracies. IIE Trans. 23(3): 216 –227.
Chaffin, D.B., Freivalds, A., and Evans, S.M. 1987. On the validity of an isometric biomechanical model of
worker strengths. IIE Trans. 19(3): 280–288.
Chaffin, D.B., Andersson, G.B.J., and Martin, B.J. 1999. Occupational Biomechanics, 3rd ed. New York:
John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Chaffin, D.B., Redfern, M.S., Erig, M., and Goldstein, S.A. 1990. Lumbar muscle size and location
measurements from CT scans of 96 older women. Clin. Biomech. 5(1): 9 –16.
Chaffin, D.B., Woolley, C.B., Buhr, T., and Verbrugge, L. 1994. Age effects in biomechanical modeling of
static lifting strengths. Proceedings of Human Factors Society, Nashville, TN.
Clauser, C.E., McConville, J.T., and Young, J.W. 1969. Weight, volume and center of mass of segments of
the human body. AMRL-TR-69-70, Aerospace Med. Res. Lab., OH.
Dempster, W.T. 1955. Space requirements of the seated operator. WADC-TR-55-159, Aerospace Med.
Res. Lab. Wright-Patterson AFB, OH.
Drillis, R. and Contini, R. 1966. Body Segment Parameters. BP-174-945, Tech. Rep. No. 1166.03, S. of Eng.
and Sci., NYU, NY.
Garg, A.D. and Chaffin, D.B. 1975. A biomechanic computerized simulation of human strength. AIIE
Trans. 14: 272– 280.
Hughes, R.E. and Chaffin, D.B. 1995. The effect of strict muscle stress limits on abdominal muscle force
predictions for combined torsion and extension loadings. J. Biomech. 28(5): 527 –533.
Hughes, R.E., Chaffin, D.B., Lavender, S.A., and Andersson, G.B.J. 1994. Evaluation of muscle force pre-
diction models of the lumbar trunk using surface electromyography. J. Orthop. Res. 12: 689–698.
McCully, K.K. and Faulkner, J.A. 1983. Length– tension relationship of mamallian diaphragm muscles.
J. Appl. Physiol. 54: 1681 –1686.
Moga, P.J., Erig, M., Chaffin, D.B., and Nussbaum, M.A. 1993. Torso muscle moment arms at interver-
tebral levels T10 through L5 from CT scans on eleven male and eight female subjects. Spine 18(15):
2305– 2309.
Static Biomechanical Modeling in Manual Lifting 48-13
Miller, J.A.A., Schultz, A.B., Warwick, D.N., and Spencer, D.L. 1986. Mechanical properties of lumbar
spine motion segments under large loads. J. Biomech. 19: 79 –84.
Nachemson, A.L., Schultz, A.B., and Berkson, M.H. 1979. Mechanical properties of human lumbar spine
motion segments: influences of age, sex, disc level, and degeneration. Spine 4: 1 –8.
National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health 1981. Work Practices Guide for Manual Lifting,
Technical Report No. 81-122, U.S. Dept. of Health and Human Services (NIOSH), Cincinnati, OH.
Nussbaum, M.A. and Chaffin, D.B. 1996. Development and evaluation of a scalable and deformable geo-
metric model of the human torso. Clin. Biomech. 11(1): 25 –34.
Nussbaum, M.A., Chaffin, D.B., and Martin, B.J. 1996. A back-propagation neural network model of
lumbar muscle recruitment during moderate static exertions. J. Biomech. 28(9): 1015– 1024.
Raschke, U. and Chaffin, D.B. 1996. Trunk and hip muscle recruitment in response to external anterior
lumbosacral shear and moment loads. Clin. Biomech. 11(3): 145– 152.
Schanne, F.T. 1972. Three dimensional hand force capability model for a seated person. An unpublished
Ph.D. dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI.
Schultz, A.B. and Andersson, B.J.G. 1981. Analysis of loads on the lumbar spine. Spine 6(1): 76 –82.
Stobbe, T.J. 1982. The development of a practical strength testing program in industry. An unpublished
Ph.D. dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI.
Tracy, M.F., Gibson, M.J., Szypryt, E.P., et al. 1989. The geometry of the muscles of the lumbar spine
determined by magnetic resonance imaging. Spine 14: 186 –193.
49
Industrial Lumbar
Motion Monitor
49.1 Introduction
49-1
49-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
settings, Andersson (1997) reported that LBDs affect an estimated 80% of the population during their
working career, and The National Center for Health Statistics (1977) has documented that LBDs are
the prime reason for activity limitation in those 45 yr of age or younger. Guo et al. (1999) estimated
that back pain accounts for 149 million lost workdays in the U.S. annually; 68% of these are associated
specifically with work-related back pain. Cats-Baril (1996) has shown that LBDs cost society up to 100
billion dollars annually. Despite the prevalence and cost of these injuries, there are relatively few accurate
methods available to predict the risk of occupationally related LBDs.
49.3.1 Approach
The initial study (Marras et al., 1993) involved an industrial surveillance of the trunk motions and work-
place factors associated with repetitive MMH jobs having either high or low LBD rates. The approach
used in this project was to: (1) identify industries having repetitive MMH production work;
(2) examine the medical and health and safety records for these companies, to identify those repetitive
MMH jobs that had high or a low LBD rates; (3) quantitatively monitor the trunk motions and
49-4 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
workplace factors associated with each of these jobs; and (4) evaluate the data, to determine which com-
bination of gathered factors best distinguished between the high- and low-rate groups.
reported occupationally related LBD. Occurrences of reported LBDs were considered regardless of the
amount of restricted or lost time associated with the incident.
The dependent variable in this study consisted of two levels of job-related LBD rates. Low-rate group
jobs were defined at those with at least 3 yr of records showing zero low-back injuries and zero turnover.
Turnover was defined as the average number of employees leaving a job per year. High-rate group jobs
were those associated with at least 12 injuries per 200,000 h of work exposure. (The average rate for this
group was 26.0 injuries per 200,000 h.) The incidence rate for the high LBD group category corresponds
to the 75th percentile value of risk for the 403 jobs examined. Of these jobs, 124 were categorized in the
low injury rate group, and 111 were in the high-rate group. The remaining jobs (totaling 168) were in the
medium injury rate group and were not used in this particular analysis.
The independent variables in this study consisted of workplace, individual, and trunk motion charac-
teristics that were indicative of each job. The workplace and individual characteristics consisted of vari-
ables typically considered in current workplace guidelines for MMH (NIOSH, 1981; Putz-Anderson and
Waters, 1991). Specifically, these variables were: (1) the maximum external horizontal distance of the
load from the spine; (2) the weight of the object lifted; (3) the vertical height of the load at both
the origin and destination of the lift; (4) the frequency of lifting (e.g., lift rate); (5) the asymmetric
angle of the lift (as defined by NIOSH, 1981); (6) 12 measures of employee anthropometry; (7) employee
injury history; (8) employee satisfaction; and (9) trunk motion. Trunk motions were obtained using the
iLMM. These variables consisted of the trunk angular position, velocity, and acceleration characteristics
(i.e., means, ranges, maximums, minimums, etc.) in each of the cardinal planes of movement. Selected
trunk motion factors, along with selected workplace factors, were used to develop a quantitative model of
occupational LBD risk.
49.3.4 Analysis
The data were examined initially to determine whether the trunk motions gathered were repeatable. This
analysis indicated that task-to-task variation was much larger than the variability due either to multiple
cycles performed within a task or to different employees performing the same task (Allread et al., 2000).
Hence, trunk motions were dictated largely by the design of the task, not by the individual, and repetitive
task cycles resulted in motions that were fairly similar.
The various personal, environmental, and workplace factors from the database were analyzed using
logistic regression techniques, to determine if any single factor could distinguish jobs associated with
high LBD rates from those with low rates. The most powerful single variable was maximum external
moment, which yielded an odds ratio of 5.17. Overall, however, the odds ratios were low, indicating
that few of the individual variables discriminated well between the two injury rate groups. Of the
trunk motion factors, the velocity variables generally produced greater odds ratios than maximum or
49-6 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
minimum position, range of motion, or acceleration. Table 49.2 shows the descriptive statistics of the
workplace and trunk motion factors for the high and low LBD rate groups.
Next, multiple logistic regression was used to predict the probability of high-risk group membership
as a function of the values for several workplace and trunk motion factors. A five-variable model
incorporating these factors was developed and further refined after examining a series of stepwise logistic
regression models (containing different variables, such as velocity, acceleration) fitted to several
intermediate data sets. A combination of five variables (external moment, lift rate, sagittal flexion,
twisting velocity, and lateral velocity) was found to have the greatest odds of predicting high-risk
TABLE 49.2 Descriptive Statistics of the Workplace and Trunk Motion Factors in Each of the Risk Groups
High Injury Rate Jobs (n ¼ 111) Low Injury Rate Jobs (n ¼ 124)
Factors Mean SD Min Max Mean SD Min Max
Workplace factors
Lift rate (lifts/h) 175.89 8.65 15.30 900.00 118.83 169.09 5.40 1500.00
Vertical location at origin (m) 1.00 0.21 0.38 1.80 1.05 0.27 0.18 2.18
Vertical load location at 1.04 0.22 0.55 1.79 1.15 0.26 0.25 1.88
destination (m)
Vertical distance traveled by 0.23 0.17 0.00 0.76 0.25 0.22 0.00 1.04
load (m)
Average weight handled (N) 84.74 79.39 0.45 423.61 29.30 48.87 0.45 280.92
Maximum weight handled (N) 104.36 88.81 0.45 423.61 37.15 60.83 0.45 325.51
Average horizontal distance 0.66 0.12 0.30 0.99 0.61 0.14 0.33 1.12
between load and L5S1 joint (N)
Maximum horizontal distance 0.76 0.17 0.38 1.24 0.67 0.19 0.33 1.17
between load and L5S1joint (N)
Average moment (N m) 55.26 51.41 0.16 258.23 17.70 29.18 0.17 150.72
Maximum moment (N m) 73.65 60.65 0.19 275.90 23.64 38.62 0.17 198.21
Job satisfaction 5.96 2.26 1.00 10.00 7.28 1.95 1.00 10.00
Trunk motion factors
Sagittal plane
Maximum extension 28.30 9.10 230.82 18.96 210.19 10.58 230.00 33.12
position (8)
Maximum flexion position (8) 17.85 16.63 213.96 45.00 10.37 16.02 225.23 45.00
Range of motion (8) 31.50 15.67 7.50 75.00 23.82 14.22 399.00 67.74
Average velocity (8/sec) 11.74 8.14 3.27 48.88 6.55 4.28 1.40 35.73
Maximum velocity (8/sec) 55.00 38.23 14.20 207.55 38.69 26.52 9.02 193.29
Maximum acceleration (8/sec2) 316.73 224.57 80.61 1341.92 226.04 173.88 59.10 1120.10
Maximum deceleration (8/sec2) 292.45 63.55 2514.08 218.45 283.32 47.71 2227.12 24.57
Lateral plane
Maximum left bend (8) 21.47 6.02 216.80 24.49 22.54 5.46 223.80 13.96
Maximum right bend (8) 15.60 7.61 3.65 43.11 13.24 6.32 0.34 34.14
Range of motion (8) 24.44 9.77 7.10 47.54 21.59 10.34 5.42 62.41
Average velocity (8/sec) 10.28 4.54 3.12 33.11 7.15 3.16 2.13 18.86
Maximum velocity (8/sec) 46.36 19.12 13.51 119.94 35.45 12.88 11.97 76.25
Maximum acceleration (8/sec2) 301.41 166.69 82.64 1030.29 229.29 90.90 66.72 495.88
Maximum deceleration (8/sec2) 2103.65 60.31 2376.75 0.00 2106.20 58.27 2294.83 0.00
Twisting plane
Maximum CCW twist (8) 1.21 9.08 227.56 29.54 21.92 5.36 230.00 11.44
Maximum CW twist (8) 13.95 8.69 213.45 30.00 10.83 6.08 211.20 30.00
Range of motion (8) 20.71 10.61 3.28 53.30 17.08 8.13 1.74 38.59
Average velocity (8/sec) 8.71 6.61 1.02 34.77 5.44 3.19 0.66 17.44
Maximum velocity (8/sec) 46.36 25.61 8.06 136.72 38.04 17.51 5.93 91.97
Maximum acceleration (8/sec2) 304.55 175.31 54.48 853.93 269.49 146.65 44.17 940.27
Maximum deceleration (8/sec2) 288.52 70.30 2428.94 25.84 2100.32 72.40 2325.93 22.74
Industrial Lumbar Motion Monitor 49-7
group membership. This combination of workplace and trunk motion factors formed the basis of the
LBD risk model. The model was selected for its statistical importance of the predictors and for biome-
chanical plausibility. The model variables remained consistent when tested with the various intermediate
data sets. The empirical stability of the model was checked by predicting the classification of 100 jobs,
based on the preliminary model. This model resulted in an odds ratio of 10.6.
By averaging individual probability values for moment, lift rate, sagittal flexion, twisting velocity, and
lateral velocity, the LBD risk model is able to predict the probability of high-risk group membership
(LBD risk) for any repetitive job. A chart depicting this information is shown in Figure 49.3. It is import-
ant to understand that the predictive power of this model is a result of the interaction of these five vari-
ables. Individually, each of these five factors is unable to reliably distinguish between the injury rate
groups, but when they are considered in combination, the predictive power increased tenfold.
FIGURE 49.3 The LBD risk model, showing the five factors scaled relative to risk. The vertical line indicates the
overall probability of high-risk group membership, or LBD risk, for a particular job.
49-8 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
8 Change
60
6 50 High–High
40 High–Med.
4
30 High–Low
20
2
10
0 0
Before After Before After
FIGURE 49.4 The impact of job interventions on LBD risk assessments and LBD injury rates, using the iLMM, for
three groups. The “high– high” group indicates those jobs where LBD risk remained high following a job change. The
group of “high– medium” jobs were those whose interventions produced a moderate drop in LBD risk. The “high–
low” category included those jobs where the intervention resulted in a large reduction in LBD risk. These charts show
that, on average, changes in a job’s resulting risk assessment level following an intervention were positively correlated
to the job’s LBD injury rate.
groups of jobs whose interventions produced an LBD risk assessed as “low” resulted in an average injury
rate drop of approximately 85%.
A concern in the assessment of jobs having a high LBD risk is the possibility that employees having had
a back injury, or currently experiencing low back pain, may move differently than those with healthy
spines. This could influence their trunk kinematics and the resulting risk assessment for a job using
the iLMM. However, Ferguson et al. (in review) found this concern to be unfounded. They studied
200 employees who had returned to their jobs following a low back injury, as well as 200 controls
having no reported low back pain who were experienced performing the same jobs. These individuals
all were monitored at their worksites while wearing the iLMM. The data showed no statistically signifi-
cant differences between those two groups of employees on any trunk motion measures. Further, the LBD
risk assessments using the iLMM also did not differ between these two groups.
These results support the use of the iLMM and the LBD risk model as an accurate method for deter-
mining a job’s potential to produce low-back injuries in employees performing the work. It also has been
found to be predictive, in that risk level changes arising from job interventions were positively correlated
with actual changes in LBD injury rates.
A second advantage of the iLMM system is that materials handling job risk is assessed relative to a large
database of jobs from diverse manufacturing environments. These jobs encompass a wide range of LBD
risk levels and actual low back injury rates. This enables the investigator to determine “how much is too
much.” That is, the model can assess how similar a job is to others known to have high rates of low back
injury associated with them. It also allows one to rank several jobs, based on their LBD risk values, and to
study solutions for those having the greatest likelihood of producing injury.
Third, the model demonstrates that it is the combination of several factors that determines a job’s level
of LBD risk. In other words, there is no one factor that is responsible for risk; rather, one evaluates risk
based on a mix of five inputs related to a job’s work requirements. An example of this model’s benefit
would be a job where a heavy load must be handled, resulting in a large maximum moment value on
the risk chart. If this load weight cannot be reduced using mechanical means, the job’s overall risk
level can be greatly reduced if an intervention produces significant reductions in the magnitude of the
model’s remaining four factors.
Fourth, the risk model enables the investigator to quantitatively assess and compare each task within a
job. Specific factors that contribute to a task’s risk can be identified, as are the tasks that most contribute
to the job’s overall LBD risk. This information pinpoints the specific tasks and the factors therein that
must be addressed during job redesign to reduce the job’s injury risk potential.
A fifth benefit of this LBD risk model is the assistance it can provide to the ergonomics intervention
process. Modified jobs can be remonitored using the iLMM, and the effects of those changes can be quan-
tified and compared with those values determined prior to the intervention. Traditionally, the effects of
job changes on the numbers of related musculoskeletal strains (the job’s incident rate) may take several
years to appear. The iLMM can produce more timely feedback to the investigator regarding anticipated
returns on the redesign investment (i.e., just the time needed to analyze the data). Thus, for jobs that
produce only minimal reductions in LBD risk due to planned redesign efforts, further (and perhaps
different) improvements can be attempted.
49.5 Applications: How to Use the iLMM and LBD Risk Model
consistent with those used to develop the risk model database, and the job’s LBD risk can be easily
assessed with the iLMM’s software.
Some jobs are less repetitive or have more job variation than assembly or palletizing tasks. However,
the LBD risk model still can be used to make relative comparisons between the tasks that comprise the
job. For jobs that require a large number of tasks, the risk model is helpful in comparing the factors that
make up the model. This will allow for the ergonomist to assess trade-offs between such factors as lifting
frequency, object weights handled, and the trunk motions required for the different tasks. It should be
noted that there is inherent variability in the way an individual may perform the same task repeatedly.
Because of this reality, one study (Allread et al., 2000) suggests that at least three repetitions of a task cycle
be collected for each of three individuals familiar with the job. This will best reduce this inherent varia-
bility found in MMH work.
Another issue to be considered is job rotation. Employers often use a variety of rotation schemes for
job processes. If the job to be monitored requires no rotation (employees perform the same job every
day/week/month), then the risk assessment can be directly related to the tasks observed. Jobs in
which individuals rotate regularly between a few work areas also may be used in assessing LBD risk, if
this rotation schedule is fixed. When the job rotation requires employees to do completely different
jobs on a hourly or weekly basis, it becomes difficult to relate a task’s risk values to the overall job
risk, since many tasks could contribute to the risk assessment. This issue is key to determining a job’s
suitability for LBD risk assessment. That is, does the job’s work structure enable one to define the job
in terms of a few repeatable, consistently performed tasks?
There may be some jobs that fit within the LBD risk model profile but still should not be monitored.
Seated jobs may require repetitive activities, but they usually are not ones that require significant material
handling. In any event, the iLMM may rub against a chair’s back or the waist belt will shift from its pos-
ition on the hips during seated work, and erroneous iLMM output will result. Also, jobs that require close
contact of the iLMM with a finished product could produce scratches on the product, and the employer
may not want to risk product damage. Finally, exposure to water or other liquids may damage the iLMM
or its components.
FIGURE 49.5 The three tasks of the frozen food packaging job: (a) place two packages in box; (b) record date/time
of the packaging and (c) load full box onto pallet.
interpreted as a function of the defined job tasks, as well as in terms of workplace factors such as a box’s
location on the pallet.
TABLE 49.3 Task Analysis of One Cycle of the Frozen Food Packaging Job
Task Length of Task Notes
Place two food 42 sec Task time is 7 sec per two boxes
packages in box packaged; 12 packages fit
into each box
Record date/time of 10 sec No manual materials handling
packaging required
Load full box onto 8 sec Full pallet contains seven layers
pallet of boxes
Total time of job tasks 60 sec
Industrial Lumbar Motion Monitor 49-13
49.8.2.1 Weight
Each object that is handled on a job must be weighed and recorded in the software or noted manually and
input during data analysis. If objects of varying weights are handled, then each must be weighed individu-
ally and recorded. This often occurs, for example, in mail and freight delivery operations. In the aforemen-
tioned food processing example, the weights are constant for each task. The combined weight of the two
food packages lifted together needs to be recorded, as does the weight of a fully packed box that is palletized.
(a) (b)
Incorrect
Measurement
Correct
Measurement
FIGURE 49.6 Correct (a) and incorrect (b) methods of measuring the distance a load is held from the lumbosacral
joint.
49-14 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
The measurement of these horizontal distances should not interfere with the work being done by the
employee. The ergonomist who measures these distances should stand close enough to the individual to
get accurate readings, but far enough away to not disturb the work. It is important that the individual be
able to move naturally at the job site. A monitored individual being crowded by an investigator will move
differently, change his or her trunk motions, and give erroneous information via the iLMM concerning
the required activities of the task.
XS, S, M, or L
FIGURE 49.8 Proper placement of the iLMM on the torso. The base of the iLMM is located slightly below the top of
the hips, and the iLMM T-sections are aligned vertically.
49-16 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
to always use these leg straps. They prevent the iLMM from moving about the hips, which otherwise
would move the base of the iLMM from its proper position and result in erroneous data.
49-17
49-18 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
. Lift rate. The lifting frequency for the entire job was 420 lifts/h. Because this variable is composed
of the total number of lifts from both tasks, this value is shown to be the same on all charts in
Figure 49.9. As indicated by the length of the lift rate bar on the charts, this rate is very rapid
and is comparable to some of the highest frequency material handling jobs found in industry.
. Average twisting velocity. The amount of twisting velocity required for the package loading task was
fairly low, but it was moderately high for the palletizing task, as indicated by the length of the bars
on these charts in Figure 49.9. The greater value of the two is used in the job summary chart in
Figure 49.9(c), which was taken from the palletizing task.
. Maximum moment. As shown on the charts in Figure 49.9, the external moment values were low
for both tasks comprising this job. The low weight of the individual packages (each at one pound)
and the fully packed box being palletized (12 lb) generated low maximum external moment values.
The greater moment value from the palletizing task was used in the job summary chart.
. Maximum sagittal flexion. The package loading task was performed while employees were in rela-
tively upright postures. This is reflected in Figure 49.9(a) by a short sagittal flexion bar on the chart.
However, during box palletizing, those boxes placed on the lower layers of the stack required much
forward trunk flexion. Figure 49.9(b) depicts these higher angles by the long bar for this factor.
Subsequently, this higher value of the two tasks resulted in it being used in the job summary
chart in Figure 49.9(c).
. Maximum lateral velocity. The iLMM determined that lateral velocities generated during the
package loading task were higher than those found during box palletizing. The values for the pre-
vious four factors all were larger during handing of the full box and, thus, were used in the job
summary chart. However, it is the lateral velocity value from the package loading task that must
be used in the job summary, since it is the greater of the two tasks analyzed.
It is the job summary value of 66% (taken from the chart in Figure 49.9c) that represents the prob-
ability of LBD risk for this example food processing job. The value indicates that, on the continuum
of low-risk jobs (0%) to high-risk jobs (100%), this particular job has a 66% likelihood of being con-
sidered “high risk.” As stated earlier in this chapter, a high-risk job was defined as one having 12 or
more (with an average of 26.4) low back strains per 200,000 h (or 100 workers/yr) of employee exposure.
Results here could be interpreted as indicating that this particular job has a relatively high chance of pro-
ducing a large number of low back strain injuries among individuals who do this job.
Industrial Lumbar Motion Monitor 49-19
FIGURE 49.9 Risk value charts for the frozen food packaging job: (a) “place two food packages in box” task; (b)
“load full box onto pallet” task; and (c) summary of all job tasks.
49-20 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
The primary goal of the iLMM analysis could be simply to determine whether or not a job presents a
risk of low back strain to employees doing its work. This can be determined by calculating the LBD risk
value from a job summary. As Figure 49.10 shows, there is some overlap in risk values among jobs defined
as low and high risk. However, our analyses (Marras et al., 1993) found that those jobs with fairly low
probability values (below 30%) were much more likely to be low-risk jobs. Similarly, very few jobs
having a risk value over 60% were defined as “low risk,” and no low-risk jobs had risk values over
70%. That is, jobs with probability values above 60% are virtually assured to have some low back
injury risk associated with them.
A second goal of the analysis may be to compare one job task with another, to determine which ones
require more trunk motions and external moments about the spine. This exercise can assist in learning
which tasks should be the focus of redesign efforts. Introducing ergonomics modifications to tasks
already found to have low-risk values probably will have little real impact on improving the job
overall. However, making changes to tasks whose individual factors contribute to the job’s summary
risk probability will reduce the probability for the entire job.
A third goal of the analysis may be to determine, for specific tasks, which individual components are
most responsible for its composite probability value. This type of analysis provides direct information
regarding how the job’s requirements may affect those factors used in this probability model. Examples
of ergonomics improvements that can be made on a job to affect each component are listed next. These
examples should not be considered an exhaustive list of possible changes that can be implemented to
improve working conditions.
70–80%
“High Risk”
60–70% Benchmark
50–60%
40–50%
“Low Risk”
30–40% Benchmark
20–30%
10–20%
0–10%
0 5 10 15 20 25 30
Percent of Jobs
FIGURE 49.10 Distribution of LBD risk values for jobs having high and low rates of low back disorders.
Industrial Lumbar Motion Monitor 49-21
which employees are rotated must also be considered, however. It should be cautioned that this
and the previous approach are most beneficial if the jobs that are rearranged or included in the
rotation allow employees to use different muscle groups to perform the job. Rotating individuals
into jobs that require the same muscles be used likely will have either no benefit or could produce
greater musculoskeletal stress.
3. Add employees. Dividing the job so that added personnel are available to perform the job will dis-
tribute the work across more people and lower the job’s required lift rate. Of course, the cost of the
additional employees must be compared with the benefits of reduced low back strains and their
related costs.
4. Automate. It may be possible that some job tasks can be automated through new equipment or the
use of robotics. This method of assisting the material handler will undoubtedly reduce the lifting
frequency of the job and the overall job requirements.
2. Install material handing aids. For goods that are of uniform shape or size, several types of lifting
aids are commercially available to provide handling assistance. These can be adapted to a wide
range of work environments. Handling aids, such as lifting hoists, when incorporated successfully,
greatly reduce the loading forces on the spine and result in much lower moment values. The device
should be considered carefully, since a handling aid that is difficult to use or greatly slows the job
process likely will be abandoned by employees. In addition, some material handling aids can
reduce the distance at which objects are handled. For example, lift/tilt tables are available com-
mercially and are able to be adapted to specific needs. These devices can raise or angle objects
or bins of goods so that they can be more easily accessed. This can result in reduced horizontal
reach distances.
3. Evaluate the transfer locations. The distance from the body at which individuals handle goods often
is greatest during the initial or final contact with the product. This is often true during palletizing
operations, in which cases need to be placed properly on a skid to ensure the load’s stability.
During carrying, for example, people tend to bring the product closer to their bodies. An evalu-
ation of these locations may detect workplace arrangements that cause individuals to reach further
than is necessary to handle objects.
the new system and provided feedback about how the workplace needed to be further changed to
produce actual risk reduction.
From the example job modifications just discussed, it is important to understand that these five work
place factors are interrelated. None of these factors responds independently from the others. For instance,
adding a lift table to palletizing work may reduce sagittal flexion, because the load is being raised. However,
it also can lower the external moment and twisting velocity values, because the load may be held closer to
the body during handling and be more easily accessed. If the work is self-paced, the lifting rate actually
could increase since the work may be less physically demanding, and those affected may be capable and
willing to handle more material. This example illustrates the trade-offs that must be considered when eval-
uating the probability of risk for a job and implementing ergonomic interventions.
Appendix A1
Example job profile selection screen for the BalletTM 2.0 software.
49-24 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Appendix A2
References
Allread WG, Marras WS, and Burr DL, Measuring trunk motions in industry: variability due to task
factors, individual differences, and the amount of data collected, Ergonomics, 43(6): 691 –701,
2000.
Andersson GBJ, The Adult Spine. Principles and Practice, 2nd ed. Raven, New York, 1997.
Bigos SJ, Spengler DM, Martin NA, Zeh J, Fisher L, Nachemson A, and Wang MH, Back injuries in indus-
try: a retrospective study. II. Injury factors, Spine, 11(3): 246 –251, 1986.
Cats-Baril W, Cost of low back pain prevention. Paper presented at the “Low Back Pain Prevention,
Control, and Treatment Symposium,” St. Louis, MO, March 11 –13, 1996.
Ferguson SA, Marras WS, and Burr D, The influence of individual low back health status on workplace
trunk kinematics and risk of low back disorder, Ergonomics, 47(11): 1226–1237, 2004.
Gill KP and Callaghan MJ, Intratester and intertester reproducibility of the lumbar motion monitor as a
measure of range, velocity and acceleration of the thoracolumbar spine, Clinical Biomechanics,
11(7): 418– 421, 1996.
Guo HR, Tanaka S, Halperin WE, and Cameron LL, Back pain prevalence in U.S. industry and estimates
of lost workdays, American Journal of Public Health, 89(7): 109 –1035, 1999.
Magora A, Investigation of the relation between low back pain and occupation — IV, physical require-
ments: bending, rotation, reaching and sudden maximal effort, Scandinavian Journal of Rehabili-
tation Medicine, 5: 191–196, 1973.
Marras WS, Fathallah FA, Miller RJ, Davis SW, and Mirka GA, Accuracy of a three-dimensional lumbar
motion monitor for recording dynamic trunk motion characteristics, International Journal of
Industrial Ergonomics, 9(1): 75– 87, 1992.
Industrial Lumbar Motion Monitor 49-25
Marras WS, Lavender SA, Leurgans S, Rajulu S, Allread WG, Fathallah F, and Ferguson SA, The role of
dynamic three dimensional trunk motion in occupationally-related low back disorders: the
effects of workplace factors, trunk position and trunk motion characteristics on injury, Spine,
18(5): 617–628, 1993.
Marras WS, Allread WG, Burr DL, and Fathallah FA, Prospective validation of a low-back disorder risk
model and assessment of ergonomic interventions associated with manual materials handling
tasks, Ergonomics, 43(11): 1866–1886, 2000.
National Center for Health Statistics, Prevalence of Selected Impairments. United States Government
Printing Office, Series 10, No. 134, 1977.
National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH), Work Practices Guide for Manual Lifting.
Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS), National Institute for Occupational Safety
and Health, Publication No. 81 –12, 1981.
Punnet L, Fine LJ, Keyserling WM, Herrin GD, and Chaffin DB, Back disorders and nonneutral trunk
postures of automobile assembly workers. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment, and
Health, 17: 337– 346, 1991.
Putz-Anderson V and Waters TR, Revisions in NIOSH guide to manual lifting. Paper presented at a
national conference entitled, “A National Strategy for Occupational Musculoskeletal Injury
Prevention—Implementation Issues and Research Needs,” Ann Arbor, MI, University of
Michigan, 1991.
Reisbord LS and Greenland S, Factors associated with self-reported back pain prevalence: a population-
based study, Journal of Chronic Diseases, 38: 691–702, 1985.
Stuart-Buttle C, A case study of factors influencing the effectiveness of scissor lifts for box palletizing,
American Industrial Hygiene Association Journal, 56(11): 1127– 1132, 1995.
50
The ACGIH TLVw for
Low Back Risk
50.1 Overview
The Lifting TLV (threshold limit value) was the product of a team of scientists whose goal was to develop
a lifting guideline that was accurate, used the latest scientific information, and easy to use. This group of
scientists consisted of Lawrence J. Fine, Christopher Hamrick, W. Monroe Keyserling, William S. Marras,
Robert Norman, Barbara Silverstein, and Thomas Waters with correspondence members consisting of
Peter Buckle and John W. Frank. The product of this group’s work has been presented as an ACGIH
2005 TLV. This chapter is based upon the work of the committee establishing this NIE (notice of
intent to establish) of which the two authors of this chapter were members.
The scientific rationale for this TLV is based on the most recent biomechanical, psychophysical, and
epidemiological studies, which together demonstrated a causal association between lifting activities
and increase risk of low back disorders characterized by pain and the temporary or prolonged inability
to perform normal occupational and nonoccupational activities. The model for the structure of this TLV
was based upon the structure of the Finnish lifting guidelines and the 1995 Occupational Safety and
Health Administration (OSHA) draft standard. These approaches were latter incorporated in the State
of Washington Ergonomic Rule. The approach for these efforts was to define the vertical and horizontal
space relative to the base of the spine (L5/S1) of the material handler. Surveillance studies have shown
that the single strongest indicator of risk for a lifting task was the load moment (weight of the object lifted
times the distance from the spine) relative to L5/S1 (Marras et al., 1993). In addition, biomechanical
studies have indicated that the vertical location of a load relative spine has profound biomechanical
implications for spine loading and tolerance (Marras, Granata, Davis, Allread & Jorgensen, 1999). There-
fore, a tool that identifies the origin location of the load to be lifted from a horizontal and vertical
location perspective was a reasonable approach.
Lift origins were divided into 12 horizontal and vertical zones relative within the sagittal plane of the
body (Table 50.1). The lift height zones consisted of four regions: (1) the region from 30 cm above to
8 cm below shoulder height (reach limit), (2) knuckle height to below shoulder height, (3) middle shin
height to knuckle height, and (4) floor to middle shin height. Horizontal location origins were divided
50-1
50-2 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
into three zones and were defined relative to the horizontal distance of the object lifted from L5/S1. These
horizontal zones were defined as: (1) close lifts — less than 30 cm from the spine, (2) intermediate lifts —
30–60 cm from the spine, and (3) extended lifts whose origins were between 60 and 80 cm from the spine.
Once the lift origin was defined, the maximum weight of lift was determined when lifting within each
of these zones. In order to arrive at an acceptable weight of lift from each of these 12 zones information
based upon biomechanical, psychophysical, epidemiological, and historical surveillance data were com-
pared so that patterns of risk could be established. Although previous National Institute of Occupational
Safety and Health (NIOSH) efforts have attempted to look at risk consistency between the different
approaches, none have assessed the patterns of risk using modern approaches.
The logic behind this effort assumes that several workplace factors influence the risk of low back dis-
order (LBD). These factors include: (1) exposure to high moments, (2) lifting from extreme postures,
and (3) repetitive lifting. Biomechanical reasoning dictates that risk occurs when an imposed load on
a structure exceeds the tolerance level of the structure (McGill, 1997). The workplace factors acknowl-
edged by the TLV, such as moments, posture, and repetition impose loads on the spine. These loads
are compared to biomechanical, physiological, and psychophysical tolerances. A recent review of the lit-
erature has indicated that the better defined the conditions of a lifting task, the stronger the association
with risk (NRC, 2001). The TLV assessment incorporates the most powerful of these defining factors.
output from many muscles to directly assess which muscles are active in response to an external load.
Current methods use the real time recording of electromyographic (EMG) activity from trunk
muscles and three dimensional geometric model of the trunk to predict the three-dimensional
loading of the spine under dynamic lifting conditions over time (Granata & Marras, 1993, 1995;
Marras & Granata, 1995, 1997a, b; Marras & Sommerich, 1991a, b; McGill & Norman, 1985, 1986;
van Dieen, Hoozemans, van der Beek & Mullender, 2002; van Dieen, JJ, Groen, Toussaint & Meijer,
2001). Applications of these models have demonstrated that spine loading varies as a function of rep-
etition (Granata, Marras & Davis, 1999), forward bending (Granata & Marras, 1995; Marras & Sommer-
ich, 1991b), and trunk moment (Granata & Marras, 1993, 1995; Marras & Sommerich, 1991b). Loading
can occur in compression, shear, or torsion. To date, these models are the most accurate models available
for the assessment of realistic work conditions. When the results of these studies are combined with
the epidemiological studies that lifting below knuckle height, or at a greater horizontal distance from
the trunk are more hazardous than lifting done between knuckle height and mid-chest height close to
the body, a strong rationale is present for assessing LBD risk as a function of load location during lifting.
epidemiological studies the highest risks are seen in highly repetitive lifting tasks (Marras et al., 1993) or
when the spine is flexed [(Punnett, Fine, Keyserling, Herrin & Chaffin, 1991; Vingard et al., 2000).]
Lifting tasks not only generate compressive forces on the spine, but also generate three-dimensional
loading of the spine. As a result damage can occur not only from compressive forces but also from
shear loading. Recently tolerances have been estimated for shear loading of the spine. These are expected
to occur between 750 and 1000 N (McGill, 1997). Quantitative workplace measures by Marras et al.
(1993, 1995) and Norman et al. (1998) have evaluated the biomechanical factors associated with jobs
that put the worker at a high risk of LBD. Both of these studies have evaluated the three-dimensional
factors that are associated with risk. The results of these studies agree well with the load-tolerance
model as well as the increase risk with substantial spinal flexion.
TABLE 50.2 Summary of the Percentage of Data within the Benchmark Zones for Spine Compression
Case Weight
Region 40 lbs (18.2 kg) 50 lbs (22.7 kg) 60 lbs (27.3 kg)
on the Pallet Benchmarks Handles No Handles Handles No Handles Handles No Handles
A ,3400 N 100.0 100.0 100.0 99.2 99.2 100.0
3400–6400 N 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.8 0.8 0.0
.6400 N 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0
B ,3400 N 98.2 89.1 84.5 76.4 83.6 67.3
3400–6400 N 1.8 10.9 15.5 23.6 16.4 32.7
.6400 N 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0
C ,3400 N 98.7 91.3 94.7 82.7 92.6 76.0
3400–6400 N 1.3 8.7 5.3 17.3 7.4 23.3
.6400 N 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.7
D ,3400 N 88.7 82.0 80.7 75.3 76.7 64.7
3400–6400 N 11.3 18.0 19.3 24.7 23.3 34.6
.6400 N 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.7
E ,3400 N 45.3 30.0 29.3 14.0 16.0 3.3
3400–6400 N 52.0 62.0 62.7 65.3 72.0 66.0
.6400 N 2.7 8.0 8.0 20.7 12.0 30.7
more than 208 of trunk flexion for more than 1.6 h was associated with an increased risk of LBDs (odds
ratio, OR ¼ 1.4). With more than 508 of peak flexion the risk increased even further (OR ¼ 2.4). Punnett
et al. (1991) reported an odds ratio of 4.2 for work with more than 208 of trunk flexion for less than 10%
of the cycle or shift (48 min) and a higher risk of 6.1 for similar work for more than 10% of the cycle.
With trunk flexion of more than 458 for more than 10% of the cycle the risk increased even further
(OR ¼ 8.9). Four other studies reported related findings. Vingard et al. (2000) reported an odds ratio
of 1.8 for trunk flexion more than one hour a day for men, but not for women. Holmstrom, Lindell,
and Moritz (1992) reported an odds ratio of 1.3 for stooping more than four hours per day. The
Marras study (Marras et al., 1993, 1995) quantitatively monitored 114 different workplace variables in
over 500 jobs that were classified according to historical risk of LBD. These analyses were able to
show that many, biomechanical workplace factors (such as trunk velocities) were associated with risk.
However, when a multivariate logistic model of risk was considered, five factors in combination (lift fre-
quency, sagittal torso bending angle, lateral velocity, twisting velocity, and external load moment),
described the relationship with risk of reporting a LBD incidence (OR ¼ 10.7) and LBD lost or restricted
time (OR ¼ 10.6) very well. Two of these factors (sagittal torso bending angle, and external load
moment) are most important in lifts that occur at some distance from the body and at lower heights.
Further analysis of this database strongly provides two important pieces of information relative to
load origin location. First, this epidemiologic information indicates that it is highly unusual for industry
to design jobs requiring routine lifting from floor level. Second, there is a dramatic change in the pro-
portion of high-risk jobs vs. low-risk jobs associated with this database as a function of region. Using
this historical risk (surveillance) database, very few low-risk jobs have been observed where lifting orig-
inates from low vertical heights compared to high-risk jobs.
Analysis of The Ohio State University (OSU) database indicated that six of the highly repetitive lifting
tasks performed in cell B (Table 50.6) involved lifting of weights less than 15 lbs (6.8 kg) while five of the
six high-risk jobs involved lifting more than 15 lbs (6.8 kg). Additional supportive evidence for the con-
sequences of load during lifting at shoulder height and above the shoulders has been published in the
biomechanical literature by Lindbeck et al. (1997) and Sporrong, Sandsjo, Kadefors, and Herberts
(1999). Sporrong and associates found that the supraspinatus muscle is heavily loaded in arm positions
with substantial abduction, whereas Lindbeck et al. found that work in overhead positions results in high
pressure in the supraspinatus muscle, thus increasing the shoulder load. There is substantial evidence
from epidemiological studies that overhead work is associated with increase of shoulder musculoskeletal
disorders.
This evidence, when considered collectively, demonstrated increasing risk with increasing exposure for
forward flexion of the back. In addition to biomechanical and epidemiological studies, the psychophysi-
cal studies indicate that for a considerable fraction of the working population TLV weights would be
unacceptable. Eventually, even lower weight recommendations may be needed for a strongly protective
TLV for repetitive lifting tasks.
There are several epidemiological studies which have focused on highly repetitive lifting and comp-
lement the findings of biomechanical studies (Chaffin & Park, 1973; Kelsey et al., 1984; Kerr et al.,
2001; Liira, Shannon, Chambers & Haines, 1996; Liles, Deivanayagam, Ayoub & Mahajan, 1984;
Marras et al., 1995; Waters et al., 1999). In a case referent study of hospitalization for herniated
lumbar discs, Kelsey et al. (1984) found increased risks associated with lifting more than 25 lbs 25
times per day (RR ¼ 3.5); lifting more than 25 lbs while twisting more than five times per day
(RR ¼ 3.1); and for lifting more than 25 lbs per day while twisting and having the knee straight
(RR ¼ 6.1). Similarly, Punnett et al. (1991) found an increased risk of low back disorders with frequent
lifts (more than 10 lbs more than once per minute). Liira et al. (1996) found increased odds of long-term
low back problems for those bending/lifting more than 50 times per day (OR ¼ 1.7), for frequent lifts of
less than 50 lbs (OR ¼ 1.5), awkward trunk postures (OR ¼ 2.3) and whole body vibration (OR ¼ 1.8).
As the number of risk factors increased, so did the risk. The Marras studies (1993, 1995) quantitatively
monitored 114 different workplace variables in over 500 jobs that were classified according to historical
risk of LBD. These analyses were able to show that many, biomechanical workplace factors (such as trunk
velocities) were associated with risk. However, when a multivariate logistic model of risk was considered,
five factors in combination (lift frequency, sagittal torso bending angle, lateral velocity, twisting velocity,
and external load moment), described the relationship with risk of reporting a LBD incidence
(OR ¼ 10.7) and LBD lost or restricted time (OR ¼ 10.6) very well. The high-risk jobs (OR ¼ 3.3) aver-
aged 3.77 lifts per minute and 20-lb loads. All studies provide some evidence that repetitive lifting is
hazardous, and most provide substantial evidence. The Marras et al. (1993, 1995) studies provided
the most detailed information on the most highly repetitive lifts and was used to select the weights
for the various cells in the TLV tables. The weights for cells B, D, and G primarily determined by identi-
fying weights below which there were a large proportion of low-risk jobs (i.e., with incidence rates of zero
cases) and a small proportion of the high-risk jobs (i.e., with incidence rates of 12 cases per 100 FTEs (full
time employees)). For example, the weight in cell D is 20 lbs (9.1 kg). In the OSU database on low- and
high-risk jobs, number of low jobs below 20 lbs (9.1 kg) and above 20 lbs (9.1 kg) was 49/6 for low-risk
jobs and corresponding number was 15/30 for high-risk jobs. The weight for lifts in cell G is 15 lbs
(6.8 kg). In the OSU database on low- and high-risk jobs, number of low jobs below 15 lbs (6.8 kg) and
above 15 lbs (6.8 kg) was 20/4 for low-risk jobs and 2/21 for high-risk jobs. The data from the OSU
studies are based on experienced workers performing repetitive lifting tasks and thus is more predictive
of observed elevated risk for LBD.
hand couplings, or unstable footing. Professional judgment should be used to determine weight limits
under these conditions.
If any of the conditions in the following list are present, then professional judgment should be used to
reduce weight limits below those recommended in the TLVs:
. Lifting at a frequency higher than 360 lifts per hour
. Extended work shifts: lifting performed for longer than 8 h per day
. High asymmetry: lifting more than 308 away from the sagittal plane
. One-handed lifting
. Lifting while seated or kneeling
. High heat and humidity
. Lifting unstable objects (e.g., liquids with shifting center of mass)
. Poor hand coupling: lack of handles, cut-outs, or other grasping points
. Unstable footing (e.g., inability to support the body with both feet while standing)
The lifting TLV does incorporate relatively complex data into a format that is quick, easy to use, and easy
to interpret; hence, it is a very useful tool to quickly assess many lifting tasks. The results can also direct
the user to job redesign strategies. For example, if the lifting conditions exceed the TLV, the user can then
find cells in the table that would not exceed the TLV, and then redesign the job accordingly. Since the
results are presented in a straightforward, intuitive format, the TLV also can be useful when requesting
support from management for resources to institute ergonomic interventions.
VERTICAL
POINT OF
TOP VIEW PROJECTION
HORIZONTAL
HORIZONTAL H
LOCATION
LATERAL
MID-POINT BETWEEN
INNER ANKLE BONES
V VERTICAL
LOCATION
HORIZONTAL
MID-POINT BETWEEN H
POINT OF PROJECTION
INNER ANKLE BONES HORIZONTAL
LOCATION
economics will dictate the appropriate task redesign strategy. The following hierarchy of controls is
suggested when redesigning manual material handling tasks.
1. Eliminate unnecessary lifting. Whenever possible, eliminate manual materials handling by combin-
ing operations or shortening the distances that material must be moved. Look at material flow
through the facility, and eliminate any unnecessary lifts. By doing so, we eliminate worker
exposure to the musculoskeletal disorder risk factors. In addition, the overall efficiency of a facility
is generally improved as time previously required to manually handle materials can be used for
other productive tasks.
2. Automate or mechanize lifting. If it is not possible to eliminate the lift, consider automating the
lifting task or using a mechanical lifting device. Devices such as hoists, cranes and manipulators
can eliminate the forces on the spine associated with manual materials handling. Therefore, the
likelihood of back injuries is also reduced.
3. Modify the job to fit within worker capabilities. If material must still be handled manually (or until
one of the above approaches can be implemented), design the task to reduce the stress on the body
as much as possible, with emphasis on ensuring that the weight lifted is below the Lifting TLV.
Some strategies for job design include:
. Allow for lifting loads as close to the body as possible. Some techniques to reduce reaching dis-
tances are: (a) eliminate any barriers such as the sides of bins or boxes, (b) use a turn table for
loads on pallets, and (c) use a tilt table to allow for better access into bins.
50-10 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
. Place the load as close to waist height as possible. This may be accomplished by using adjustable
lift tables or inclined conveyors to locate the object to be handled at waist height.
. Reduce the need to twist the trunk by reorienting the lifting origins and destinations.
. Reduce the weight of the load being lifted so that the weights are within the Lifting TLV.
50.3.4 Example 1
As an example of how the TLV can identify lifting
tasks which put workers at risk of injury, consider
the following scenario, as seen in a manufacturing
facility.
A producer of refractory products for use in
metal melting and high-temperature industrial
applications performs a mixing task which requires
the operator to make the mix for the forming of
parts (Figure 50.3). The mix is made in batches
and each batch requires loading a bag of filler
weighing 18 kg. The operator mixes 80 batches
per 8-h shift, or 10 per hour. The hopper
opening is between the operator and the staging
area for the bags, and a dust-control ventilation FIGURE 50.3 Lifting filler bag.
duct is also between the operator and the bag to
be lifted. Therefore, the operator is required to extend the hands away from the trunk to lift the bags.
At the point where the bag is lifted, the hands are 75 cm from the midpoint between the ankles, and
the bag is between knuckle height to below shoulder height.
Because the task is performed for more than 2 h per day (actual task duration is 8 h per day), and the
frequency of lift is 12 lifts per hour (actual lifting frequency is ten lifts per hour), the appropriate TLV
table for this task is Table 50.4. Using the cell which corresponds to a lifting height zone between
“Knuckle height to below shoulder” and a horizontal location of lift of “Extended Lifts,” the TLV for
this task is 9 kg. The actual weight being lifted is 18 kg, which exceeds the TLV.
Given that the weight lifted in this task is twice the Lifting TLV, the company realized that the task must
be redesigned. By considering the hierarchy of controls, they determined that the best method of control
was to eliminate the lift. The company installed a “super sack” system whereby the filler is purchased in
bulk, and screw feeders controlled by the operator are used to deliver the appropriate amount of filler to
the mix. The operator could then make the mix with minimal manual handling of materials, and the
relatively high risk of injury posed by the lifting task was eliminated.
50.3.5 Example 2
Another example of using the TLV to identify risk of injury is seen in the following situation from a
machine and welding shop.
One of the parts produced by the shop is a “shell” used in air compressors. It must be lifted from floor
level to a press, located at about chest level (Figure 50.4). The shell weighs 23 kg. At the current pro-
duction rate, the machine operator must perform a lift approximately every 3.5 mins, or about 17
lifts per hour. There are no obstacles, and the load is relatively compact (51 cm long and 33 cm in diam-
eter), so the operator can get close to the load.
Because this task is performed for more than 2 h per day (actual task duration is 8 h per day), and the
frequency of lift is .12 lifts per hour but 30 lifts per hour (actual lifting frequency is 17 lifts per hour),
Table 50.5 is the appropriate table to determine the TLV for this task. Using the cell which corresponds to
a lifting height zone of “Floor to middle shin height” and a horizontal location of lift of “Close Lifts,” the
TLV for this task is 9 kg. The actual weight being lifted (23 kg) is clearly greater than the TLV.
The ACGIH TLVw for Low Back Risk 50-11
TABLE 50.4 TLVs for Lifting Tasks 2 h per day with 60 Lifts per hour or .2 h per day with 12 Lifts per hour
Horizontal Location of Lift
Close Lifts: Origin Intermediate Lifts: Extended Lifts: Origin
,30 cm from Origin 30–60 cm .60 to 80 cm from
Midpoint between from Midpoint Midpoint between
Inner Ankle between Inner Inner Ankle
Lifting Height Zone Bones Ankle Bones Bonesa
Reach limitb from 30 cm above 16 kg 7 kg No known safe limit for
to 8 cm below shoulder height repetitive lifingc
Knuckle heightd to below shoulder 32 kg 16 kg 9 kg
Middle shin height to knuckle heightd 18 kg 14 kg 7 kg
Floor to middle shin height 14 kg No known safe limit No known safe limit
for repetitive lifingc for repetitive lifingc
a
Lifting tasks should not be started at a horizontal reach distance more than 80 cm from the midpoint between the inner
angle bones (Figure 50.2)
b
Routine lifting tasks should not be conducted from starting heights greater than 30 cm above the shoulder or more than
180 cm above floor level (Figure 50.2)
c
Routine lifting tasks should not be performed for shaded table entries marked “No known safe limit for repetitive lifting.”
While the available evidence does not permit identification of safe weight limits in the shaded regions, professional judgment
may be used to determine if infrequent lifts of lifht weights may be safe.
d
Anatomical landmark for knuckle height assumes the worker is standing erect with arms hanging at the side.
Source: Reprinted from ACGIH, 2004 Threshold Limit Values for Chemical Substance and Physical Agents & Biological
Exposure Indices, 2004. Cincinnati, OH: ACGIH. With permission.
The company redesigned the lifting task by installing a manipulator attached to an overhead
crane so that the shell could be lifted mechanically, thereby nearly eliminating the muscular
effort required by the machine operator (Figure 50.5). By reducing the muscular effort, the mechanical
stress on the spine is also reduced, thereby substantially reducing the risk of injury to the machine
operator.
In the year prior to installing the manipulator, the company reported nine injuries associated
with this task: two back sprains, six neck sprains, and one wrist sprain. In the year after the change,
no injuries have been reported to the population performing this task. The company also indicated
that productivity has increased by nearly 70%, and scrap rate has decreased from 2 to 0.8%.
As these examples illustrate, the Lifting TLV is a practical, easy to use tool that can be used in the
workplace to assess risk. The results can also be used to determine jobs which are candidates for redesign
and as an assessment tool after making job modifications to determine if the risk of injury has been
reduced to acceptable levels.
TABLE 50.5 TLVs for Lifting Tasks .2 h per day with .12 and 30 Lifts per hour or 2 h per day with .60 and
360 Lifts per hour
Horizontal Location of Lift
Close Lifts: Intermediate Lifts: Extended Lifts:
Origin ,30 cm Origin 30 –60 cm Origin .60 to 80 cm
from Midpoint from Midpoint from Midpoint
between Inner between Inner between Inner
Lifting Height Zone Ankle Bones Ankle Bones Ankle Bonesa
Reach limitb from 14 kg 5 kg No known safe limit for
30 cm above to repetitive lifingc
8 cm below shoulder height
Knuckle heightd to 27 kg 14 kg 7 kg
below shoulder
Middle shin height to 16 kg 11 kg 5 kg
knuckle heightd
Floor to middle shin height 9 kg No known No known safe limit
safe limit for for repetitive lifingc
repetitive lifingc
a
Lifting tasks should not be started at a horizontal reach distance more than 80 cm from the midpoint between the inner
angle bones (Figure 50.2)
b
Routine lifting tasks should not be conducted from starting heights greater than 30 cm above the shoulder or more than
180 cm above floor level (Figure 50.2.)
c
Routine lifting tasks should not be performed for shaded table entries marked “No known safe limit for repetitive lifting.”
While the available evidence does not permit identification of safe weight limits in the shaded regions, professional judgment
may be used to determine if infrequent lifts of lifht weights may be safe.
d
Anatomical landmark for knuckle height assumes the worker is standing erect with arms hanging at the side.
Source: Reprinted from ACGIH, 2004 Threshold Limit Values for Chemical Substance and Physical Agents & Biological
Exposure Indices, 2004. Cincinnati, OH: ACGIH. With permission.
TABLE 50.6 TLVs for Lifting Tasks .2 h per day with .30 and 360 Lifts per hour.
Horizontal Location of Lift
Close Lifts: Intermediate Lifts: Extended Lifts:
Origin , 30 cm from Origin 30 –60 cm from Origin . 60 to 80 cm
Midpoint between Midpoint between Inner from Midpoint between
Lifting Height Zone Inner Ankle Bones Ankle Bones Inner Ankle Bonesa
Reach limitb from 30 cm 11 kg No known safe limit for No known safe limit for
above to 8 cm below repetitive lifingc repetitive lifingc
shoulder height
Knuckle heightd to 14 kg 9 kg 5 kg
below shoulder
Middle shin height to 9 kg 7 kg 2 kg
knuckle heightd
Floor to middle shin height No known safe limit for No known safe limit No known safe limit
repetitive lifingc for repetitive lifingc for repetitive lifingc
a
Lifting tasks should not be started at a horizontal reach distance more than 80 cm from the midpoint between the inner
angle bones (Figure 50.2)
b
Routine lifting tasks should not be conducted from starting heights greater than 30 cm above the shoulder or more than
180 cm above floor level (Figure 50.2)
c
Routine lifting tasks should not be performed for shaded table entries marked “No known safe limit for repetitive lifting.”
While the available evidence does not permit identification of safe weight limits in the shaded regions, professional judgment
may be used to determine if infrequent lifts of lifht weights may be safe.
d
Anatomical landmark for knuckle height assumes the worker is standing erect with arms hanging at the side.
Source: Reprinted from ACGIH, 2004 Threshold Limit Values for Chemical Substance and Physical Agents & Biological
Exposure Indices, 2004. Cincinnati, OH: ACGIH. With permission.
The ACGIH TLVw for Low Back Risk 50-13
References
American Conference of Governmental Industrial Hygienists (ACGIH) (2004). 2004 Threshold Limit Values
for Chemical Substances and Physical Agents & Biological Exposure Indices. Cincinnati, OH: ACGIH.
Andersson, G. B., Svensson, H. O., & Oden, A. (1983). The intensity of work recovery in low back pain.
Spine, 8: 880 –884.
Brinkmann, P., Biggermann, M., & Hilweg, D. (1988). Fatigue fracture of human lumbar vertebrae. Clin
Biomech (Bristol, Avon) 3: S1– S23.
Chaffin, D. B. (1975). A computerized biomechanical model: development of and use in studying gross
body actions. J. Biomech., 2: 429– 441.
Chaffin, D. B. & Park, K. S. (1973). A longitudinal study of low-back pain as associated with occu-
pational weight lifting factors. Am. Ind. Hyg. Assoc. J., 34: 513 –525.
Garg, A., Chaffin, D. B., & Frievalds, A. (1982). Biomechanical stresses from manual load lifting: a static
vs dynamic evaluation. IIE Trans, 14: 272– 281.
Granata, K. P. & Marras, W. S. (1993). An EMG-assisted model of loads on the lumbar spine during
asymmetric trunk extensions. J. Biomech., 26: 1429–1438.
Granata, K. P. & Marras, W. S. (1995). An EMG-assisted model of trunk loading during free-dynamic
lifting. J. Biomech., 28: 1309–1317.
Granata, K. P., Marras, W. S., & Davis, K. G. (1999). Variation in spinal load and trunk dynamics during
repeated lifting exertions. Clin. Biomech. (Bristol, Avon), 14: 367 –375.
Gunning, J. L., Callaghan, J. P., & McGill, S. M. (2001). Spinal posture and prior loading history modu-
late compressive strength and type of failure in the spine: a biomechanical study using a porcine
cervical spine model. Clin. Biomech. (Bristol, Avon), 16: 471 –480.
Herrin, G. D., Jaraiedi, M., & Anderson, C. K. (1986). Prediction of overexertion injuries using biome-
chanical and psychophysical models. Am. Ind. Hyg. Assoc. J., 47: 322 –330.
Holmstrom, E. B., Lindell, J., & Moritz, U. (1992). Low back and neck/shoulder pain in construction
workers: occupational workload and psychosocial risk factors. Part 2. Relationship to neck and
shoulder pain. Spine, 17: 672 –677.
Jager, M., & Luttmann, A. (1999). Critical survey on the biomechanical criterion in the NIOSH method
for the design and evaluation of manual lifting tasks. Int. J. Indu. Ergon., 23: 331 –337.
Jager, M., Luttmann, A., & Laurig, W. (1991). Lumbar load during one-hand bricklaying. Int. J. Ind.
Ergon., 8: 261–277.
Kelsey, J. L., Githens, P. B., White, A. A. I., Holford, T. R., Walters, S. D., O’Conner, T., Ostfeld, A. M.,
Weil, U., Southwick, W. O., & Calogero, J. A. (1984). An epidemiologic study of lifting and twist-
ing on the job and risk for acute prolapsed lumbar intervetebral disc. J. Ortho. Res., 2: 61 –66.
Kerr, M. S., Frank, J. W., Shannon, H. S., Norman, R. W., Wells, R. P., Neumann, W. P., & Bombardier,
C. (2001). Biomechanical and psychosocial risk factors for low back pain at work. Am. J. Public
Health, 91: 1069– 1075.
50-14 Fundamentals and Assessment Tools for Occupational Ergonomics
Liira, J. P., Shannon, H. S., Chambers, L. W., & Haines, T. A. (1996). Long –term back problems and
physical work exposures in the 1990 Ontario Health Survey. Am. J. Public Health, 86: 382–387.
Liles, D. H., Deivanayagam, S., Ayoub, M. M., & Mahajan, P. (1984). A job severity index for the evalu-
ation and control of lifting injury. Hum. Factors, 26: 683–693.
Lindbeck, L., Karlsson, D., Kihlberg, S., Kjellberg, K., Rabenius, K., Stenlund, B., & Tollqvist, J. (1997). A
method to determine joint moments and force distributions in the shoulders during ceiling work
— a study on house painters. Clin. Biomech (Bristol, Avon), 12: 452–460.
Lotz, J. C. & Chin, J. R. (2000). Intervertebral disc cell death is dependent on the magnitude and dur-
ation of spinal loading. Spine, 25: 1477–1483.
Lotz, J. C., Colliou, O. K., Chin, J. R., Duncan, N. A., & Liebenberg, E. (1998). Compression-induced
degeneration of the intervertebral disc: an in vivo mouse model and finite-element study. Spine, 23:
2493– 2506.
Marras, W., Granata, K., Davis, K., Allread, W., & Jorgensen, M. (1997). Spine loading and probability of low
back disorder risk as a function of box location on a pallet. Int. J. Hum. Factors Manuf., 7: 323–336.
Marras, W. S. & Davis, K. G. (1998). Spine loading during asymmetric lifting using one versus two
hands. Ergonomics, 41: 817–834.
Marras, W. S., Ferguson, S. A., Lavender, S. A., Burr, D., Chany, A. M., Parakkat, J., & Yang, G. (2004).
Impact of experience and workday length on spine loading. Paper presented at the International
Society for the Study of the Lumbar Spine, Porto, Portugal.
Marras, W. S. & Granata, K. P. (1995). A biomechanical assessment and model of axial twisting in the
thoracolumbar spine. Spine, 20: 1440–1451.
Marras, W. S. & Granata, K. P. (1997a). The development of an EMG-assisted model to assess spine
loading during whole-body free-dynamic lifting. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol., 7: 259 –268.
Marras, W. S. & Granata, K. P. (1997b). Spine loading during trunk lateral bending motions. J. Biomech.,
30: 697– 703.
Marras, W. S., Granata, K. P., Davis, K. G., Allread, W. G., & Jorgensen, M. J. (1999). Effects of box fea-
tures on spine loading during warehouse order selecting. Ergonomics, 42: 980 –996.
Marras, W. S., Lavender, S. A., Leurgans, S. E., Fathallah, F. A., Ferguson, S. A., Allread, W. G., & Rajulu,
S. L. (1995). Biomechanical risk factors for occupationally related low back disorders. Ergonomics,
38: 377– 410.
Marras, W. S., Lavender, S. A., Leurgans, S. E., Rajulu, S. L., Allread, W. G., Fathallah, F. A., & Ferguson,
S. A. (1993). The role of dynamic three-dimensional trunk motion in occupationally-related low
back disorders. The effects of workplace factors, trunk position, and trunk motion characteristics
on risk of injury. Spine, 18: 617 –628.
Marras, W. S. & Sommerich, C. M. (1991a). A three-dimensional motion model of loads on the lumbar
spine: I. Model structure. Hum. Factors, 33: 123–137.
Marras, W. S. & Sommerich, C. M. (1991b). A three-dimensional motion model of loads on the lumbar
spine: II. Model validation. Hum. Factors, 33: 139 –149.
McGill, S. (2002). Low Back Disorders: Evidence-Based Prevention and Rehabilitation. Champaign, IL:
Human Kinetics.
McGill, S. M. (1997). The biomechanics of low back injury: implications on current practice in industry
and the clinic. J. Biomech., 30: 465 –475.
McGill, S. M. & Norman, R. W. (1985). Dynamically and statically determined low back moments
during lifting. J. Biomech., 18: 877–885.
McGill, S. M. & Norman, R. W. (1986). Partitioning of the L4 –L5 dynamic moment into disc, ligamen-
tous, and muscular components during lifting [see comments]. Spine, 11: 666 –678.
National Institute of Occupational Safety and Health (NIOSH), (1981). Work Practices Guide For Manual
Lifting. Cincinnati, OH: Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS), NIOSH.
Norman, R., Wells, R., Neumann, P., Frank, J., Shannon, H., & Kerr, M. (1998). A comparison of peak vs.
cumulative physical work exposure risk factors for the reporting of low back pain in the automo-
tive industry. Clin. Biomech. (Bristol, Avon), 13: 561 –573.
The ACGIH TLVw for Low Back Risk 50-15
National Research Council (NRC) (2001). Musculoskeletal Disorders and the Workplace: Low Back and
Upper Extremity. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Punnett, L., Fine, L. J., Keyserling, W. M., Herrin, G. D., & Chaffin, D. B. (1991). Back disorders and
nonneutral trunk postures of automobile assembly workers. Scand. J. Work. Environ. Health.,
17: 337–346.
Schultz, A., Andersson, G., Ortengren, R., Haderspeck, K., & Nachemson, A. (1982a). Loads on the
lumbar spine. Validation of a biomechanical analysis by measurements of intradiscal pressures
and myoelectric signals. J. Bone. Joint. Surg. [Am]., 64: 713–720.
Schultz, A., Andersson, G. B., Ortengren, R., Bjork, R., & Nordin, M. (1982b). Analysis and quantitative
myoelectric measurements of loads on the lumbar spine when holding weights in standing pos-
tures. Spine, 7: 390 –397.
Schultz, A., Haderspeck, K., Warwick, D., & Portillo, D. (1983). Use of lumbar trunk muscles in iso-
metric performance of mechanically complex standing tasks. J. Orthop. Res., 1: 77–91.
Schultz, A. B. & Andersson, G. B. (1981). Analysis of loads on the lumbar spine. Spine, 6: 76–82.
Solomonow, M., Zhou, B. H., Baratta, R. V., Lu, Y., & Harris, M. (1999). Biomechanics of increased
exposure to lumbar injury caused by cyclic loading: Part 1. Loss of reflexive muscular stabilization.
Spine, 24: 2426– 2434.
Sporrong, H., Sandsjo, L., Kadefors, R., & Herberts, P. (1999). Assessment of workload and arm position
during different work sequences: a study with portable devices on construction workers. Appl.
Ergon., 30: 495–503.
van Dieen, J. H., Hoozemans, M. J., van der Beek, A. J., & Mullender, M. (2002). Precision of estimates of
mean and peak spinal loads in lifting. J. Biomech., 35: 979 –982.
van Dieen, J. H., JJ, M. D., Groen, V., Toussaint, H. M., & Meijer, O. G. (2001). Within-subject variability
in low back load in a repetitively performed, mildly constrained lifting task. Spine, 26: 1799 –1804.
Vingard, E., Alfredsson, L., Hagberg, M., Kilbom, A., Theorell, T., Waldenstrom, M., Hjelm, E. W., Wik-
torin, C., & Hogstedt, C. (2000). To what extent do current and past physical and psychosocial
occupational factors explain care-seeking for low back pain in a working population? Results
from the Musculoskeletal Intervention Center-Norrtalje Study. Spine, 25: 493– 500.
Waters, T. R., Baron, S. L., Piacitelli, L. A., Anderson, V. P., Skov, T., Haring-Sweeney, M., Wall, D. K.,
& Fine, L. J. (1999). Evaluation of the revised NIOSH lifting equation. A cross-sectional epidemio-
logic study. Spine, 24: 386 –394; discussion 395.
Waters, T. R., Putz-Anderson, V., Garg, A., & Fine, L. J. (1993). Revised NIOSH equation for the design
and evaluation of manual lifting tasks. Ergonomics, 36: 749– 776.
Yoganandan, N. (1986). Biomechanical identification of injury to an intervetebral joint. Clin.
Biomech., 1: 149.
Index
I-1
I-2 Index
Defense Research Agency Workload scale (DRAWS), Electromyographic data, static exertions, 14-2
mental workload measurements, EMG assisted multiple muscle system models,
37-11 to 37-12 11-33 to 11-34
Delay rest allowances, 38-2 EMG muscle activity, wrist position, 39-3 to 39-4
Deltoid lateral, shoulder, 27-2 to 27-6 Empirical rules, verbal adjustment, 12-15 to 12-16
Depth perception, binocular vision, 18-12 to 18-13 Endurance, muscle, 10-4 to 10-5
Design Engineering anthropometry, 9-1 to 9-29
butterfly ballot, 7-1 Engineering lifecycle, usability, 7-5
contextual, 7-14 to 7-15 Engineering noise control, 31-29 to 31-30
eight golden rules of, 7-25 Engineering science, system approaches, 5-7 to 5-8
inclusive, 7-35 Enterprise-based cost-benefit analysis, benefits of, 4-7
as predictive task 24-1 to 24-16 Entirety method exercise, 5-33
seven principles of, 7-25 Environment, work, 5-17 to 5-18
UCD approach to, 7-8 Environmental factors, in productivity maximization,
user centered, 7-4 17-13 to 17-14
Design drivers, defining requirements and Environmental fatigue allowances, ILO fatigue allowances,
specifications, 7-20 38-5 to 38-6
Design engineering methodology, 5-7 Environmental influences, 5-17 to 5-18
Design evaluation process, 24-3 to 24-15 Equalization adjustment and selection, verbal adjustment
analytical, 24-3 to 24-15 rules, 12-16 to 12-17
assistive users, 7-27 Equipment, working, 5-16 to 5-17
cognitive walkthrough, 24-3 to 24-12 Equipment factor matching, for productivity
controlled users, 7-28 maximization, 17-12 to 17-13
heuristic analyses, 24-12 to 24-15 Ergonomic applications
participative users, 7-27 foot, 30-1 to 30-10
problem definition, 24-2 to 24-3 legs, 29-1 to 29-8
Design process, 24-3 to 24-15 low back, 28-1 to 28-7
Design requirements, properties of, 7-12 trunk moment assessment, 28-2 to 28-3
Design solutions, testing and evaluation of, trunk posture assessment, 28-3 to 28-5
7-26 to 7-34 weight lifted assessment, 28-2
Determination, risk level, 8-5 psychosocial work and response assessment, 28-6 to
Determined decisions, 5-3 28-7
Digit force, mean and standard deviation, 10-14 trunk motion assessment, 28-5
Digital readout, sound level meter, 31-15 whole-body assessment, 28-6
Dimensions, physical, of U.S. adult civilians, 30-3 Ergonomics
Disc compression failures, 11-12 aging and, 17-1 to 17-15
Disc pressures, with different backrest inclinations, 11-21 assessment methods, 34-1 to 34-15
Disc/endplate tolerance, 11-11 assessment tools, practitioners use of, 34-2
Discipline, description of the, 3-5 biomechanical analyses, 11-1
Disclosure, full and timely, 3-9 biomechanical basis of, 11-1 to 11-39
Diseases, occupational, 4-7 carpal tunnel syndrome, wrist posture, 39-1
Disencumbered consciousness, in repetitive work, 5-33 cost justification for, 4-1 to 4-12
Disposition, characteristics of, 5-13 economics of, 4-2 to 4-3
Dose exposure quantification, cumulative spine loading, hazard identification, PLIBEL, 40-1 to 40-7
13-16 to 13-17 health and medical indicators, 34-2 to 34-4
Dosimeter, 31-15 man in, 5-11
Dynamic motion, biomechanics of, low back disorder and, material handling assessment tools, 34-4 to 34-11
11-35 to 11-36 personality, role of, 25-1 to 25-11
Dynamic tendon–pulley models, hand, wrist, models, personality traits, and physical outcomes,
45-12 to 45-13 25-9 to 25-10
Dynamic work, fatigue, musculoskeletal system, 38-10 psychosocial work factors, 26-1 to 26-8
rationalization and humanization goals, 5-12
E rehabilitation, 22-1 to 22-15
aging worker, 22-12
Eccentric muscle actions, acute myofiber injury, case studies, 22-9 to 22-12
15-14 to 15-15 components of, 22-3 to 22-7
Economics definition, 22-1
ergonomics and, 4-2 to 4-3 early intervention, 22-6 to 22-7
value of work in, 4-4 functional capacity testing, 22-4 to 22-5
Economy, as proportion of inputs, 5-11 future of, 22-13 to 22-15
Elbow angle and torque, relation of, 10-16 injury prevention, 22-13
I-6 Index
Human being, as individual working person, 5-26 low back disorders, 49-1 to 49-24
Human body maximum lateral velocity, 49-22 to 49-23
assessment tools, 34-10 to 34-11 maximum moment, 49-21 to 49-22
bottlenecks in, 5-29 maximum sagittal flexion, 49-22
design to fit, 9-2 to 9-7 physical description, 49-2 to 49-3
job safety assessment tools, 34-10 to 34-11 risk model development
rapid upper limb assessment, 34-10 to 34-11 predictive ability, 49-7 to 49-8
Human capital, 4-4 validation, 49-7 to 49-8
Human factors issues task analysis, 49-11 to 49-12
in ergonomics, 5-12 workplace statistics, 49-6
typical testimony, 3-3 to 3-4 Industrial occupations, biomechanics in, 11-2
Human fault, definition of, 3-2 Industry, noise, 31-1 to 31-31
Human limitation, optimal control models and, Information processing
12-20 to 12-21 multimodal, 23-1 to 23-21
Human models, of manual control touch, 23-1 to 23-21
COM, 12-11 vision in, 23-2 to 23-12
cross-over, 12-11 Information technology (IT), ergonomic approach, 7-3
motivation, 12-10 Injury, costs to the enterprise, 4-6
OCM, 12-11 Injury, costs to the person, 4-6
quasi-linear function theory, 12-1 to 12-11 Injury, soft-tissue, pathomechanics of, 15-9
Human muscle, length–tension relationship, 11-7 Injury biomarkers, cellular biology, 15-26 to 15-27
Human performance, individual determinants of, 5-13 Injury biomechanics, musculoskeletal disorders,
Human reaction time, 3-3 16-8 to 16-9
Human work capacity, dimensions of, 5-12 Injury mechanisms
Humans in acute muscle injury factors, 15-15 to 15-22
physical subsystems of, 5-29 acute muscle injury, 15-13 to 15-14
social system theory, 5-10 acute myofiber injury, 15-14 to 15-15
work system environment, 5-1 to 5-47 musculoskeletal disorders, 16-1 to 16-13
Humeral head position, shoulder, 27-2 to 27-6 soft-tissue pathomechanics, 15-13 to 15-22
Hybrid assessment tool, low back injury risk, Injury prevention
35-6 to 35-7 floor mats, 29-5 to 29-6
Hyperacusis, concomitant auditory maladies, 31-11 kneeling aids, 29-6 to 29-7
lower extremity injuries, 29-5 to 29-9
I shoe insoles, 29-6 to 29-7
Injury process, low back disorders, 20-2 to 20-5
IEA endorsements, criteria for, 1-2 Injury susceptibility, muscle loading history, acute muscle
ILO fatigue allowances injury factors, 15-17
environmental, 38-5 to 38-6 Injury theories, cumulative, 13-8 to 13-9
mental, 38-5 In-line tool, 11-26
overview, 38-6 to 38-7 International Ergonomics Association (IEA), 1-1
physical, 38-3 to 38-5 International Organization for Standardization (ISO), and
Impairment levels classification, visual acuity, 18-5 UCD, 7-8
Inductive reasoning, testimony, 3-13 Isomorphy, of laws, 5-3
Industrial lumbar motion monitoring (iLMM) Internal loads, biomechanics, 11-6
accelerations and velocities, 49-4 Integrated work system design, 5-23
analytic tools, 49-2 Interference, signal-to-noise ratio, 31-7
average twisting velocity, 49-21 International Labor Organization (ILO) fatigue
benefits of, for lower back disorder, 49-8 to 49-9 allowances, 38-2 to 38-7
calibration and measurement accuracy, 49-3 Intelligence, effects of aging, 17-5 to 17-6
data analysis, 49-5 to 49-7 Intrinsic muscles, hand and wrist anatomy, 45-6
data collection, 49-5, 49-16 Intracellular CA2þ, cellular biology, 15-23 to 15-24
lift rate, 49-12 Intramodal interference, multimodal information
lower back disorder, 49-12 to 49-14 processing, 23-21
maximum moment, lower back disorder, 49-12 Inverse optimal allocation of attention, 12-23
data interpretation of, 49-16
data trunk kinematic information, 49-16 to 49-18
development of device, 49-2 to 49-3
J
harness set-up, 49-14 to 49-15 Job analysis
high-risk probability, 49-18 to 49-23 SHARP exposure assessment, 44-11 to 44-13
job selection, 49-10 to 49-11 significant force analysis, 44-11 to 44-13
lift rate, 49-20 to 49-21 Job description, 5-28
Index I-9
Job order Lift location studies, threshold limit values, 50-3 to 50-6
definition of, 5-14 Lift rate, lower back disorder
work system approach, 5-10 industrial lumbar motion monitor, 49-12
Job performance, effects of refractive error, 18-4 to 18-15 risk probability, 49-20 to 49-21
Job safety analysis (JSA), assessment tools, 34-10 to 34-11 Lifting, threshold limit values, 50-8 to 50-13
Job sampling, SHARP exposure assessment, 44-8 to 44-10 Lifting belts, as back support, 11-27 to 11-28
Job system, design of, 5-28 Lifting equation, NIOSH, 34-5, 35-1 to 35-2, 46-2 to 46-4
Joint motions, relative discomfort scores, 43-2 to 43-7 Lifting guidelines
Judge NIOSH, 11-28
power of, 3-15 OSU/BWC, 34-7 to 34-8
role of gatekeeper, 3-5 Lifting, threshold limit values, 50-7 to 50-12
Judgment of the expert, 3-4 determination of, 50-8
Junk science, forensic issues, 3-6 use of, 50-7 to 50-8
Jurors, behavior of, 3-14 Ligament capacity, load tolerance, 11-10, 11-11
Linear, time-invariant systems, 12-2 to 12-3
K Literature search criteria, measurement of physical and
cognitive problems, 2-2
K factors, percentile factors and, 9-5 Living wage, responsibility for, 4-6 to 4-7
Kepner-Tregoe methodology, 3-7 Load tolerance, biomechanics, 11-10 to 11-13
Keyboarding Load tolerance, bone capacity, 11-10
alternative designs, for wrist posture, 39-7 to 39-10 Load tolerance, ligament capacity, 11-10
carpal tunnel syndrome, 39-6 Load tolerance, muscle tolerance, 11-10
QWERTY, 39-5 Load tolerance, tendon capacity, 11-10
repetition, and wrist posture, 39-6 to 39-7 Load tolerance, threshold limit values, development of,
training, effect on wrist posture, 39-10 to 39-11 50-2 to 50-3
wrist posture, 39-5 to 39-11 Loading of the upper body (LUBA), assessment technique,
Knee disorders, lower extremity injuries, 29-2 to 29-4 43-1 to 43-7
Kneeling aids, lower extremity injuries, injury prevention, Loads
29-6 to 29-7 external, in biomechanics, 11-6
Knowledge, skills, experience and other facts. See KSAOs. internal, biomechanics, 11-6
Known bias, 3-15 internal, factors affecting, 11-6 to 11-10
KSAOs (knowledge, skills, experience and other facts), task Load — tolerance concept, in biomechanical concepts,
matching, 17-10 11-2 to 11-3
Loudness scales, noisiness units, 31-6
L Loudness scales
phons, 31-4 to 31-6
Language production/comprehension, effects of aging, sones, modification of, 31-6
17-6 to 17-7 sound, noise, 31-4 to 31-6
Lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), vision, 23-8 Zwicker’s method, 31-6
Law Low back disorders (LBD)
objectives of, 3-1 analytic tools, 49-2
in occupational ergonomics, 3-1 to 3-18 biomechanics of, 11-35 to 11-36
professional involvement with, 3-2 industrial lumbar motion monitor, 49-1 to 49-24
and regulations, in rehabilitation ergonomics, development of, 49-2 to 49-3
22-2 to 22-3 injury process/tissue damage, 20-2 to 20-5
Learning aptitude, in cybernetics, 5-3 low back exercise, philosophy of, 20-9 to 20-11
Legislation, compliance costs, 4-6 low back instability, 20-5 to 20-6
Leg, ergonomics applications for, 29-1 to 29-8 low back stability, 20-6 to 20-9
Length–tension relationship, in human muscle, 11-7 motor changes, 20-5
Lengthening velocity, in acute muscle injury factors, 15-16 occupational back injuries, 49-1 to 49-2
Levers rehabilitation of, 20-1 to 20-16
biomechanics, 11-5 risk model development, 49-3 to 49-8
first-class, 11-5 study design, 49-4 to 49-5
second-class, 11-5 Low back ergonomics application, 28-1 to 28-7
third-class, 11-5 manual material handling assessments, 28-5 to 28-6
Lexical design, 5-22 psychosocial work and response assessment,
Liability, prevention of, 3-2 28-6 to 28-7
Licensure, definition of, 1-1 trunk moment assessment, 28-2 to 28-3, 28-5
“Lifecycles” design, 7-7 trunk posture assessment, 28-3 to 28-5
Lift location studies, spine compression benchmark weight lifted assessment, 28-2
zones, 50-4 whole-body vibration, 28-6
I-10 Index
OSHA noise exposure limits, 31-23 work related factors, 2-10 to 2-13
OSU/BWC lifting guidelines, 34-7 to 34-8 back disorders, 2-10 to 2-11
Overall workload (OW) scale, mental workload upper extremity disorders, 2-12 to 2-13
measurements, 37-11 Physical subsystems
of humans, 5-29
P influences on, 5-29
Pistol grip tool, 11-26
Pain mechanisms, injury biomechanics, 16-8 to 16-9 Pittsburgh perceived exertion scale, 37-6
Pain, neurophysiologic mechanisms of PLIBEL
biochemical mediators, 16-6 to 16-8 ergonomics hazard identification, 40-1 to 40-7
central nervous system sensitization, 16-4 to 16-6 procedure of, 40-4 to 40-5
musculoskeletal disorders, 16-2 to 16-8 reliability, validity, 40-5 to 40-6
neural anatomy, 16-2 to 16-3 Postural assessment scales, 37-6 to 37-7
Pain outcome, in musculoskeletal disorders, 16-9 to 16-11 body part discomfort scale, 37-6 to 37-7
Pain tolerance, 11-13 Nordic questionnaire, 37-7
Pathomechanics, soft-tissue, 15-1 to 15-29 Posture
Peak finger force, ACGIH TLV, application of, assessment, RULA, 42-10
41-9 to 41-10 body, 9-7
Pedals evaluation procedures, LUBA, 43-4 to 43-7
foot controls, 30-10 predefined angular categories, 44-14
push force, effects on, 10-20 to10-21 REBA, 42-3 to 42-7
Peer approval, 3-15 scoring of, RULA, 42-10 to 42-11
Perceived physical effort SHARP, exposure assessment, 44-13 to 44-15
Borg’s scales for perceived exertion, 37-3 to 37-5 Predictive cognitive task analysis, walkthrough, 24-3
Fleishman index, 37-5 to 37-6 Presbyopia
Pittsburgh perceived exertion scale, 37-6 optical correction, 18-8 to 18-10
subjective scales for postural assessment, short-working distances, 18-8
37-6 to 37-7 Preventive work design, 5-22
workload assessment techniques, 37-3 to 37-5 Probability statistics, estimation by, 9-21 to 9-22
Percentiles, using, 9-3 to 9-6 Process-orientated, work systems, 5-12
Peripheral vision Product sales changes, measurement of, 4-5
short-working distances, 18-11 to 18-12 Production costs, measurement of, 4-5
visual fields, 18-11 to 18-12 Productivity assessment tool, 4-8
Permissible exposure limit (PEL), noise, 31-18 Productivity, effects of aging, 17-10 to 17-15
Personal opinion testimony, 3-12 Productivity maximization
Personality assessment aging, 17-10
Eysenck Personality Questionnaire, 25-5 to 25-6 complex tasks, in multiple task environments,
Five Factor Model, 25-8 to 25-9 17-11
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory, decision-making, 17-12
25-4 to 25-5 environmental factors, 17-13 to 17-14
Myers–Briggs Type Indicator, 25-5 equipment factor matching, 17-12 to 17-13
occupational settings, 25-3 to 25-9 experience, 17-12
type A personality behavior, 25-6, 25-7 memory load reduction, 17-11 to 17-12
Personality role safety, 17-14
ergonomics, 25-1 to 25-11 speed-accuracy trade-off, 17-11
low back pain, 25-10 task matching, 17-10
upper extremities loading, 25-10 training, 17-14 to 17-15
Personality traits, physical outcomes, ergonomics, worker experience, 17-10
25-9 to 25-10 Productivity measurement, 4-9
Phalange lengths, hand, 45-9 Prospective work design, 5-22
Phons, loudness scales, 31-4 to 31-6 Prototyping methods, classification of, 7-22
Phoria, binocular alignment, 18-13 Psychophysical approach
Physical exposure, at the workplace, 2-5 to 2-8 advantages and disadvantages, 47-27 to 47-28
Physical fatigue allowances, ILO, 38-3 to 38-5 manual materials handling design, 47-3,
Physical problems 47-22 to 47-25
measurements of workplace exposures, 2-5 to 2-8 multiple component tasks, 47-22 to 47-23
occupational related task and workplace design, 47-23 to 47-25
literature search criteria, 2-2 task analysis, 47-1 to 47-28
measurement of, 2-1 to 2-10 upper extremity tasks design, 47-25 to 47-27
psychosocial factors, 2-14 to 2-19 force and frequency limits, 47-25 to 47-27
study of, 2-1 to 2-20 tool and workplace design, 47-27
I-14 Index
International Labor Organization, 38-2 to 38-7 Shoe insoles, in injury prevention, 29-6 to 29-7
machine allowances, 38-2 Shoes, friction coefficients, foot, 30-6
personal allowances, 38-2 Short-working distances
temporal aspects of fatigue, 38-7 to 38-9 binocular vision, 18-12 to 18-14
time standard concept, 38-1 to 38-2 color vision, 18-14 to 18-15
Rest time, ability to exert force, 11-9 eye movements, 18-10 to 18-11
Risk analysis, flow diagram for, 8-9 peripheral vision, 18-11 to 18-12
Risk evaluation presbyopia, 18-8
of environmental influences, 8-6 refractive error, 18-8
of human factor, 8-6 vision and work, 18-15
procedure for, 8-6 to 8-7 Shoulder abduction strength and fatigue time, 11-14
RCM method for, 8-10 Shoulder and arm muscle fatigue, expected time
via technical device, 8-6 for, 11-17
Risk index, wrist, carpal tunnel disorder, 45-24 Shoulder muscle fatigue time, with varied arm flexion
Risk theory in, man– machine environment systems, postures, 11-15
8-1 to 8-11 Shoulder, biomechanical modeling of, 27-1 to 27-12
Rods, and vision, 23-8 data collection, 27-7 to 27-8
Root cause analysis, 3-7 geometrical considerations, 27-6 to 27-7
Root cause team, role of, 3-7 methods, 27-2 to 27-7
Root cause, forensic issues, 3-7 theoretical considerations, 27-2 to 27-6
Root mean square, sound level meter, 31-14 Shoulder, biomechanics of, 11-14 to 11-15
RULA (rapid upper limb assessment) posture Shoulder, geometrical considerations
assessment, 42-10 glenoid fossa concavity, 27-6 to 27-7
REBA, differences, 42-2 test results, 27-8 to 27-11
recording of, 42-10 to 42-11 Signal detection, communications, noise, effect of, 31-7
scores, 42-11 Signal-to-noise ratio, effect of, 31-7
use of, 42-9 to 42-12 Single distinct percentile point, determination of, 9-6
Running, fatigue and comfort, 30-7 to 30-8 Single-task lifting index, lifting equation,
46-17 to 46-18
S Single-task recommended weight limit, lifting
equation, 46-17
Safety Skeletal muscle physiology
productivity maximization, 17-14 anatomy, 15-2
technical risk and, 8-2 force generation and transmission, 15-4 to 15-7
Scientific certainty, 3-15 muscle contractions, 15-3 to 15-4
Scientific testimony, definition of, 3-6 musculotendon actuator, 15-8
Second-class levers, 11-5 soft-tissue pathomechanics, 15-2 to 15-8
Self-regularization, in cybernetics, 5-3 Sleep
Semantic design, 5-21 biological clock, 38-11
Sensitivity analysis, cost benefits, 4-9 circadian rhythm, and fatigue, 38-11
Sequential work system design, 5-23 countermeasures for fatigue, 38-11
Service sales changes, measurement of, 4-5 shiftwork, 32-4 to 32-6
Services, recipient of, 5-15 to 5-16 Slips, causes of, 30-6
SHARP exposure assessment Social system, 5-9
direct measurement methods, 44-6 evolution–theoretical approach, 5-7
job analysis, 44-11 to 44-13 humans, 5-10
job sampling, 44-8 to 44-10 Socio-political contexts of work, 5-26
measurements, 44-1 to 44-7 Socio-technical system approach, 5-4 to 5-6
methods, 44-8 to 44-19 animadversion, 5-5
observational methods, 44-4 to 44-6 elements of, 5-5
on-site data collection, 44-11 problem analysis in, 5-5
posture analysis, 44-13 to 44-15 Soft-tissue pathomechanics, 15-1 to 15-29
repetitive exertion analysis, 44-15 to 44-16 animal models, muscle performance and injury, 15-9
repetitive movement analysis, 44-16 cellular biology, 15-23
self-report questionnaires, 44-3 to 44-4 contraction-induced injury models, 15-8 to 15-9
work organization measurement, 44-16 to 44-19 injury mechanisms, 15-13 to 15-22
Shiftwork, 32-1 to 32-7 recommendations, 15-27 to 15-28
circadian factors, 32-2 to 32-4 skeletal muscle physiology, 15-2 to 15-8
domestic factors, 32-6 Sones
sleep factors, 32-4 to 32-6 loudness scales, 31-4 to 31-6
solutions, 32-6 to 32-7 modification of, 31-6
I-16 Index