An Overview of Physics Education Research On Problem Solving
An Overview of Physics Education Research On Problem Solving
David P. Maloney
Abstract:
1. Introduction
Investigating humans engaged in problem solving is a very diverse and
complex endeavor, and narrowing the focus to investigating students
engaged in problem solving in introductory physics only reduces the scope
a little. Early general problem-solving research established several
aspects/facets of problem solving that apply to all domains. However, how
these general aspects play out within a particular domain depends on the
domain, and problem solving within a specific domain has features that
are particular to that domain. Because there are many interactions between
the problem-solving processes people use and their domain knowledge,
presenting research on problem solving cannot be done in a nice, clean,
sequential manner. Consequently, deciding how to structure this
introductory overview presented the author with a rather challenging
problem.
The result, after much thought and struggle, is the following organization:
(A) General background on research on problem solving; (B) Aspects of
how individuals solve physics problems, which consists of three parts—
problem-solving steps, problem-solving strategies and problem-solving
states of mind (this framework is borrowed from the ThinkFun® Game
Club website: www.thinkfungameclub.com.); (C) Aspects of “teaching
problem solving”; and (D) Miscellaneous aspects. Finally, we take stock
of where we are in understanding problem solving in physics and identify
some possible future research questions.
It is important to point out two things here at the beginning. First, even
though the focus of this presentation is problem solving in physics, a
number of the studies that are described do not involve physics.
Nonetheless these studies, because problem solving is a concern for many
fields and shares characteristics in those fields, do tell us useful things that
can be applied to problem solving in physics.
2. General Background
2.1 The Difficulty with Defining “Problem”
As stated above, problem solving involves a very diverse and complex set
of processes, and one of the difficulties with interpreting the problem-
solving literature is comparing and contrasting the activities associated
with people working the spectrum of tasks that are called problems. As
Adams points out, “The study of problem solving is almost impossible if
you try to look at it as one thing that a person does. It has many facets and
to study these it’s useful to isolate and identify the individual facets.”6
There are many definitions for problem in the literature and while they
share a number of elements, they are not identical. In many studies the
authors never provide a definition of what they are calling a problem,
rather it is taken for granted that the reader understands and agrees with
the researchers that the tasks presented to the subjects were problems for
the subjects. This approach is “problematic.”
The wide range of tasks that qualify as problems for novices, who by
definition have little domain specific knowledge for a field of study, is
part of what makes investigating problem solving a complex domain of
study. Adding to the complexity is the fact that problems can be classified
in a variety of ways, such as conceptual or quantitative, or well-defined
versus ill-defined. For the latter contrast, there is actually a spectrum along
which problems can be placed.11
Using the definition of problem given above, there are three components
of a problem: the initial state, the goal state, and procedures to eliminate
the gap between them. A very well-defined problem would have all three
aspects explicitly identified. For example, end-of-chapter numerical
exercises fit this description of well-defined since the initial state (the
given values), the final state (the quantity to be found) and the procedures
to be used are all roughly specified. The procedures to be used are not
exactly specified, but these tasks are often labeled with the section to
which they relate and doing that specifies the procedures indirectly.
(Actually one might argue that such tasks do not qualify as problems
under the Hayes definition.) Problems that have one, or more, of these
three features not explicitly identified then fall further along the spectrum
toward ill-defined. The ill-defined end of the spectrum has situations
where the problem solver may not even be sure there is a problem; such
tasks require explicit identification and definition of the problem, i.e., the
initial state, goal state, and nature of the gap, before an attempt can be
made to solve them. The vast majority of academic problems are well-
defined while real-world problems tend to be ill-defined to various
extents.
Since the nature of the tasks/situations that can be problems for people is
so broad, it is useful, at least at times, to have ways to classify and
distinguish among those that share characteristics. Not surprisingly,
considering the previously mentioned broad range of tasks that can be
problems, there are various ways to classify problems. Perhaps the most
obvious way is on the basis of the knowledge domain involved, i.e.,
physics or chemistry problems. However, there are other ways to classify
problems that can be useful if we are interested in gaining insight into
students’ solving approaches and abilities.
Looking at this scheme it is pretty clear that the vast majority of tasks that
students encounter in academic situations are types 1 to 3, but real world
problems are more commonly types 4 to 8. How do we help students
develop the skills they need to tackle those types of problems?
The difficulties described above about defining problems carries over into
discussions about “problem-solving skills”. Teaching “problem-solving
skills” is fairly commonly cited as a major goal of physics, or mathematics
or chemistry, instruction. However, determining what this means, and
whether it can actually be done, is another matter. Even a rather quick
exploration of the problem-solving literature brings to light the fact that
there is a difficulty with even identifying what qualifies as a problem-
solving skill. While many researchers, not to mention teachers, strongly
believe that there are general problem-solving skills, those skills are
always applied to, and with, knowledge from some specific domain.
Consequently, identifying skills that operate in the same way in all
domains is very difficult.
On one hand few people would argue with identifying such skills as being
able to identify given information, being able to specify what the goal
state is (stating the unknown), determining what concepts, principles, and
relations are needed, and reviewing one’s work as general problem-
solving skills. However, these processes are so general that explicitly
stating, and trying to teach, them is of limited value, unless we are talking
about working with very novice subjects who lack both knowledge and
general reasoning skills. In contrast, skills such as drawing appropriate
representations (free-body diagrams, equipotential diagrams, or ray
diagrams), translating qualitative situations into quantitative relations, or
making assumptions in order to constrain a situation to one for which
quantitative relations can actually be developed are more specific to
context.
2.5 Methodology
An issue with those early expert-novice studies is the nature of the tasks
the subjects were working on. For example, Larkin, et al. used end-of-the-
chapter tasks from an introductory calculus-based physics text as their
problems. These tasks could legitimately be called problems for the
novices in their study. However, for the experts those tasks hardly
qualified as problems because of the experts’ experience and knowledge
base. Consequently, what was actually being investigated was how large
Another approach that has been used, especially with computer tutors, is
to attempt to teach subjects how to solve problems and carefully monitor
what they do. One of the early investigations in this area was the study by
Heller and Reif. 22 They taught a “prescriptive model” of problem solving,
which focused on formulating a theoretical description, i.e., a physical
representation, to a group of subjects. The subjects using the prescriptive
model performed significantly better than a control group, and also better
than a group using a modified version of the model. The prescription these
researchers developed was very specific and restricted to problems
involving the application of Newton’s second law.
carry out, and look back.24 The elements of the Minnesota strategy are 1)
focus the problem, 2) describe the physics, 3) plan the solution, 4) execute
the solution, and 5) evaluate the answer. The strategy is explicitly used by
instructors when they solve example problems for the students and the
students are required to use it when they submit work. The third element
in this program was the use of collaborative grouping, with assigned roles
for the students, for working on the problems in discussion sessions.
problem, his/her State of Mind when working through the problem, then
we believe s/he has learned the fundamentals of problem solving.” The
author believes this applies to our students also and provides a useful
guide for research on individual differences in problem solving and for
what we need to help students learn in order to be good problem solvers.
In the ThinkFun® framework these are identified as: (1) understand, (2)
choose, (3) do, and (4) inspect. Obviously these are quite similar to the
four step Polya framework (see above). One can think of these aspects as
the overall model of the task and the general strategy one adopts toward
the problem. Several features of these steps, such as the fact that students
tend to jump right into trying to solve a problem, have been investigated in
physics.18 The common failure of people to inspect or review their
solutions is also known. 26 However, the focus will only be on one
aspect—the development and use of representations to understand the
problem—in this section. The reason for focusing on only this one aspect
is to keep this review from getting any longer and to highlight the critical
importance of representations.
With regard to the first step mentioned above, understand, a critical part of
understanding a problem is the representation the solver constructs, i.e.,
the internal mental model of the problem situation that the solver forms.
There are at least two types of representations associated with a problem
and problem solving. One is the internal mental model of the situation
which can lead to the type of stumbling block described in the next
paragraph. However, there is also the representation of what the process of
solving a problem involves, e.g., is the solver only trying to get an answer,
or is there also some learning and understanding supposed to come from
the process? More will be said about this later in the states of mind
section.
impossible, so someone trying the task cannot make progress until they re-
represent the task.
The term representation is actually used two ways when talking about
problems. As mentioned above the issue of the internal model of the
problem situation that a solver forms, i.e., the representation, is an
important aspect of problem solving. However, the term “representation”
can also be used to describe the format of the task, e.g., verbal, graphical,
diagrammatic, etc. This aspect of physics problems has been the focus of
some recent research. Meltzer investigated how students in an algebra-
based physics class performed on quizzes which had essentially
isomorphic problems in different formats (verbal, equation, graph, or
diagram). He found that student performance varied with format and
topic.28
during problem solving, and (4) beliefs, which are one’s world view. The
beliefs component of this framework obviously relates to students’
epistemologies. As an example of how students’ beliefs affect their
problem-solving efforts, Schoenfeld found subjects took an empirical
approach to a mathematical construction problem that is better addressed
using deductive geometry, even though the students were subsequently
found to be capable of making deductive geometric arguments.34
Tuminaro and Redish looked into student attempts to solve problems from
the perspective of what they call epistemic games. They define an
epistemic game as: “a coherent activity that uses particular kinds of
knowledge and process associated with that knowledge to create
knowledge or solve a problem”. They found that students use a limited set
of these games which “…appear to provide the students with guidance as
to what knowledge and procedures to access and what to ignore.”35
As Docktor and Heller point out: “Problem solving is one of the primary
goals, teaching tools, and evaluation techniques of physics courses.”36 In
this section we will look at some additional efforts to teach problem
solving beside those described above. All such efforts involve some form
of what can be called a global heuristic, having such steps as define the
problem or draw a diagram.
There has been little research on the use of computer tutors to teach
problem solving within PER until recently. Hsu and Heller have reported
on preliminary work to develop computer tutors as “personal assistants for
learning”40 which is based on the idea of cognitive apprenticeship. The
researchers are developing three types of tutors—(1) computer as coach
for the student, (2) student as tutor for the computer, and (3) computer as
assistant for the student who works more independently—and are
currently in the process of evaluating the usefulness of the tutors.41
More work on this approach has been done within the computer
science/AI community. VanLehn et al. report on using computer tutors in
a “minimally intrusive” way to help students learn to solve physics
problems.42 The ANDES tutor developed by these researchers is a very
different entity than the tutor described in the previous paragraph. ANDES
incorporates an artificial intelligence system designed to try to ascertain
the student’s mental and skill level. As such this system is not something
an instructor can modify to use with his/her course and students.
Putting this topic here rather than in section B may seem strange, but this
goes back to the intertwined nature of exploring problem solving. The
reason for putting it here is that this aspect doesn’t deal directly with
students solving specific problems, for the most part. There are two classic
studies that reported important differences in how strong and weak
students studied and used worked examples.44-45 Both groups of
researchers found that the stronger students would spend time when
studying worked examples figuring out aspects of the example that they
did not initially understand. In contrast, the weaker students tended to take
essentially everything for granted and did very little deep processing, i.e.,
trying to make sense of what was presented in the example. Not
surprisingly when the weaker students were engaged in solving problems
for themselves they would go back and look for a worked example that
they could use essentially as a “template” for the current problem. In
contrast, the stronger students would only go back to worked examples
when stuck on a specific step in their solution of a new problem, and they
would go in search of a specific idea or process, not a complete map.
what is missing. The more important of these is the self-repair where the
student has a defect in his/her mental model that produces a conflict with
the scientific model conveyed in the worked example. Students who do
not recognize and address the conflict will obviously be limited in what
they learn from the worked example.46
5. Miscellaneous Aspects
5.1 Assessing students’ problem-solving skills.
Bing and Redish investigated how students use mathematics when solving
physics problems. They contend students use a process they call
epistemological framing in which students decide the kind of knowledge,
(e.g., do they need to do a calculation, or do they need to map the
mathematics to the physical situation, or do they invoke authority, etc.), to
employ at various points in the problem-solving process. They argue that
students can get stuck when solving problems because they employ a
particular epistemological framing rather than a more appropriate one
which they also have as a resource. Bing and Redish contend that students
have identifiable warrants (reasons) that guide the framing decisions for
how to use mathematics when solving physics problems.52
main findings: (1) for all subjects the most difficult aspect of solving the
ill-defined problem was the process of making constraining assumptions
to convert the problem to a well-defined one, and (2) only subjects who
had prior experience making such assumptions had much success.55
One of the common ideas among physics instructors (and mathematics and
chemistry instructors) is that students “learn the physics” by solving
problems. However, the research has shown that if the “problems”
involved are the traditional end-of-chapter tasks this contention is not
valid. Kim and Pak found that even students who had solved more than
1000 traditional tasks in preparation for college entrance exams still had
most of the common alternate conceptions about how physical systems
behaved.56 Sherin explored the role of “intuitive knowledge” in physics
problem solving. He found that subjects did not always follow formal
physics rules, instead appealing to common sense as a guide for how to
proceed.57
Similar work has been done in chemistry. Nurrenbern & Pickering found
that students could solve numerical tasks, but struggle with essentially the
same task when presented in a qualitative manner.58 Sawrey and
Cracolice, Deming & Ehlert followed up with further investigations of the
relations and differences between effective problem solving and
conceptual understanding.59-60
There have also been studies investigating the relation between knowledge
organization and problem solving. Eylon and Reif explored how two
different knowledge organizations—hierarchical versus single-level—
affected recall and ability to use the knowledge in problem solving.
Subjects working with the hierarchical knowledge organization were
significantly better at using the knowledge on complex tasks.61
These studies, and others, make it clear that the relations between problem
solving and conceptual understanding or knowledge organization are
complex ones that we are only starting to understand.
6. Taking Stock
So where are we at this point in time? We have learned a good bit about
what students do with ‘traditional’ introductory physics tasks and some
things about other aspects of students’ efforts to solve the tasks presented
to them. We are also starting to learn things about teaching problem
solving and instructors’ approaches and values for this task. In presenting
the rough summary below it is important to acknowledge that there are
other aspects of problem solving that this review has not discussed, but
which are worthy of research. Having said that, what can we say in
summary at this time?
It is clear from the problem-solving work done to date, in all domains, that
problem-solving expertise is domain-specific. That is, someone who is an
expert at solving chess problems will not automatically also be expert at
solving physics problems. Nonetheless there is evidence for general
problem-solving skills, perhaps at several “levels.” We can envision at
least three levels, the most general level being reasoning processes at the
level of metacognition, which is domain independent. The next more
specific level is that of employing heuristics, such as guess and test or
identifying and applying constraints, which can be applied in all domains,
but which application requires some domain knowledge. And finally there
Real progress has been made in understanding how students deal with
various aspects of solving physics problems. However, as mentioned at the
outset, the domain is so complex and diverse that we have actually just
scratched the surface. The author would argue that there are a couple of
things that should be made explicit in all investigations of problem
solving. First, the specific nature of the tasks which are qualifying as
problems in the investigation needs to be specified carefully. It should be
clear whether the tasks are well-defined, or ill-defined, and if the latter,
how. In a similar way, whether the tasks are primarily quantitative or
qualitative needs to be explicitly identified. Second, there is a definite
need when talking about teaching problem-solving skills to explicitly
identify what those skills are rather than taking, as most physics
instructors do, students’ ability to solve typical numerical tasks as the
operational definition of problem-solving skill.
Acknowledgements
References
1
D. P. Maloney, “Research on Problem Solving: Physics” in Handbook of
Research on Science Teaching and Learning, D. L. Gabel, ed.
(MacMillan, New York, NY, 1994).
2
L. Hsu, E. Brewe, T. M. Foster, and K. A. Harper, “Resource Letter on
Problem Solving Research in Physics Education,” Am. J. Phys. 72,
1147-1156 (2004).
3
R. E. Mayer and M. C. Wittrock, “Problem Solving Transfer” in
Handbook of Education Psychology, D. C. Berliner and R. C. Calfee,
eds. (Macmillan, New York, NY, 1996).
4
D. H. Jonassen, “Toward a Design Theory of Problem Solving,” Ed.
Tech. Res. Devel. 48 (4), 63-85 (2000).
5
J. E. Davidson and R. J. Sternberg, eds., The Psychology of Problem
Solving (Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, 2003).
6
W. K. Adams, “Development of a Problem Solving Evaluation
Instrument: Untangling of Specific Problem Solving Skills,”
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Colorado (2007).
7
J. R. Hayes, The Complete Problem Solver (Franklin Institute Press,
Philadelphia, PA, 1981).
8
P. Heller and M. Hollabaugh, “Teaching Problem Solving Through
Cooperative Grouping. Part 2: Designing Problems and Structuring
Groups,” Am. J. Phys. 60 (7), 637-645 (1992).
9
A. Van Heuvelen and D. P. Maloney, “Playing Physics Jeopardy,” Am. J.
Phys. 67, 252-256 (1999).
10
D. P. Maloney, “Ranking Tasks: A New Type of Test Item,” J. Coll. Sci.
Teach. 25, 205-210 (1987).
11
D H. Jonassen, “What Makes Scientific Problems Difficult?” in
Learning to Solve Complex Scientific Problems, D. H. Jonassen, ed.
(Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New York, N Y, 2007).
12
Y. Chu and J. N. MacGregor, “Human Performance on Insight Problem
Solving: A Review,” J. Prob. Solv. 3, 119-150 (2011).
13
D. Perkins, Archimedes’ Bathtub (W. W. Norton and Company, New
York, NY, 2000).
14
A. H. Johnstone, “Introduction” in Creative Problem Solving in
Chemistry, C. Wood and R. Sleet, eds. (The Royal Society of Chemistry,
London, England, 1993).
15
D. Hammer, “Two Approaches to Learning Physics,” Phys. Teach. 27,
664-670 (1989).
16
C. Bereiter and M. Scardamalia, Surpassing Ourselves: An Inquiry into
the Nature and Implications of Expertise (Open Court Publishing,
Chicago, IL, 1993).
17
M. T. H. Chi, “Quantifying Qualitative Analyses of Verbal Data: A
Practical Guide,” J. Learn. Sci. 6, 271-315 (1997).
18
J. H. Larkin, J. McDermott, D. P. Simon, and H. A. Simon, “Expert and
Novice Performance in Solving Physics Problems,” Science 208, 1335-
1342 (1980).
19
C. Singh, “When Physical Intuition Fails,” Am. J. Phys. 70, 1103-1109
(2002).
20
M. T. H. Chi, P. S. Feltovich, and R. Glaser, “Categorization and
Representation of Physics Problems by Experts and Novices,” Cog. Sci.
5, 121-152 (1981).
21
C. Singh, “Assessing Student Expertise in Introductory Physics with
Isomorphic Problems II: Effect of Some Potential Factors on Problem
Solving and Transfer,” Phys Rev. ST-PER 4, 010105 (2008).
22
J. I. Heller and F. Reif, “Describing Effective Human Problem Solving
Processes: Problem Description in Physics,” Cog. Instr. 1, 177-216
(1984).
23
P. Heller, R. Keith, and S. Anderson, “Teaching Problem Solving
Through Cooperative Grouping. Part 1: Group Versus Individual
Problem Solving,” Am. J. Phys. 60 (7), 627-636 (1992).
24
G. Polya, How to Solve It (Princeton University Press, Princeton, N J,
1945).
25
W. J. Leonard, R. J. Dufresne, and J. P. Mestre, “Using Qualitative
Problem-Solving Strategies to Highlight the Role of Conceptual
Knowledge in Solving Problems,” Am. J. Phys. 64, 1495-1503 (1996).
26
J. Mason, L. Burton, & K. Stacey, Thinking Mathematically (Addison-
Wesley, NY, 2010).
27
J. H. Larkin, “The Role of Problem Representation in Physics” in Mental
Models, D. Gentner and A. L. Stevens, eds. (Lawrence Erlbaum,
Hillsdale, NJ, 1983).
28
D. E. Meltzer, “Relation Between Students’ Problem-solving
Performance and Representational Format,” Am. J. Phys. 73, 463-478
(2005).
29
P. B. Kohl and N. D. Finkelstein, “Patterns of Multiple Representation
Use by Experts and Novices During Physics Problem Solving,” Phys
Rev. ST-PER 4, 010111 (2008).
30
K. Kotovsky and H. Simon, “What Makes Some Problems Really Hard:
Explorations in the Problem Space of Difficulty,” Cog. Psych. 22, 143-
183 (1990).
31
J. Sweller, “Cognitive Load During Problem Solving: Effects on
Learning,” Cog. Sci. 21, 257-285 (1988).
32
J. Sweller and M. Levine, “Effects of Goal Specificity on Means-ends
Analysis and Learning,” J. of Exp. Psych: Learn., Mem. Cog. 8 (5), 463-
474 (1982).
33
J. Sweller, R. Mawer, and M. Ward, “Consequences of History: Cued
and Means-end Strategies in Problem Solving,” Am. J. Psych. 95 (3),
435-483 (1982).
34
A. H. Schoenfeld, Mathematical Problem Solving (Academic Press, New
York, NY, 1985).
35
J. Tuminaro and E. F. Redish, “Elements of a Cognitive Model of
Physics Problem Solving: Epistemic Games,” Phys Rev. ST-PER 3,
020101 (2007).
36
J. Docktor and K. Heller, “Robust Assessment Instrument for Student
Problem Solving,” Proc. NARST 2009 Ann. Meet. (2009).
37
E. Gaigher, J. M. Rogan, and W. W. H. Braun, “Exploring the
Development of Conceptual Understanding Through Structured
Problem-solving in Physics,” Int. J. Sci. Ed. 29, (9), 1089-1110 (2007).
38
M. M. Cooper, C. T. Cox, M. Nammouz, and E. Case, “An Assessment
of the Effect of Collaborative Groups on Students’ Problem-solving
Strategies and Abilities,” J. Chem. Ed. 85, 866-872 (2008).
39
C. A. Ogilvie, “Change in Students’ Problem-solving Strategies in a
Course That Includes Context-rich Multifaceted Problems,” Phys Rev.
ST-PER 5, 020102 (2009).
40
F. Reif and L. A. Scott, “Teaching Scientific Thinking Skills: Students
and Computers Coaching Each Other,” Am. J. Phys. 67, 819-831 (1999).
41
L. Hsu and K. Heller, “Computer Problem-solving Coaches,” Proc.
NARST 2009 Ann. Meet. (2009).
42
K. VanLehn, C. Lynch, L. Taylor, A. Weinstein, R. Shelby, K. Schulze,
D. Treacy, and M. Wintersgill, “Minimally Invasive Tutoring of
Complex Physics Problem Solving” in Intelligent Tutoring Systems,
Cerri, Gouarderes, & Paraguacu, eds. (Springer, New York, NY, 2002).
43
Y-J.Lee, D. J. Palazzo, R. Warnakulasooriya, and D. E. Pritichard,
(2008) “Measuring Student Learning with Item Response Theory,” Phys
Rev. ST-PER 4, 010102 (2008).
44
M. T. H. Chi, M. Bassock, M. W. Lewis, P. Reimann, and R. Glaser,
(1989) “Self-explanations: How Students Study and Use Examples in
Learning to Solve Problems,” Cog. Sci. 5, 121-152 (1989).
45
M. G. M. Ferguson-Hessler and T. deJong, “Studying Physics Texts:
Differences in Study Processes Between Good and Poor Performers,”
Cog. Instr. 7, 41-54 (1990).
46
M. T. H. Chi, “Self-explaining Expository Tests: The Dual Processes of
Generating Inferences and Repairing Mental Models” in Advances in
Instructional Psychology, R. Glaser, ed. (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
Mahwah, NJ, 2000).
47
E. Cohen, A. Mason, C. Singh, and E. Yerushalmi, “Identifying
Differences in Diagnostic Skills of Physics Students: Students Self-
diagnostic Performance Given Alternative Scaffolding,” Proc. 2008
Phys. Ed. Res. Conf., AIP Conference Proceedings 1064, 99-102 (2008).
48
E. Yerushalmi, E. Cohen, A. Mason, and C. Singh, “From Diagnostic
Skills to Success in the Physics Classroom,” Proc. 2008 Phys. Ed. Res.
Conf., AIP Conference Proceedings 1064, 147-150 (2008).
49
R. Safadi and E. Yerushalmi, “Students’ Perceptions of a Self-diagnostic
Task” Proc. 2009 Phys. Ed. Res. Conf., AIP Conference Proceedings
1179, 257-260 (2009).
50
C. Henderson, E. Yerushalmi, V. Kuo, P. Heller, and K. Heller,
“Grading Student Problem Solutions: The Challenge of Sending a
Consistent Message” Am. J. Phys. 72 (2), 164-169 (2004).
51
E. Yerushalmi, E. Cohen, K. Heller, P. Heller, and C. Henderson,
“Instructors’ Reasons for Choosing Problem Features in a Calculus-
based Introductory Physics Course,” Phys Rev. ST-PER 6, 020108
(2010).
52
T. J. Bing and E. F. Redish, “Analyzing Problem Solving Using Math in
Physics: Epistemological Framing via Warrants,” Phys Rev. ST-PER 5,
020108 (2009).
53
B. Sherin, (2001) “How Students Understand Physics Equations,” Cog.
Instr. 19 (4), 479-541 (2001).
54
N. Shin, D. H. Jonassen, and S. McGee, (2003) “Predictors of Well-
structured and Ill-structured Problem Solving in an Astronomy
Simulation,” J. Res. Sci. Teach. 40, 6-33 (2003).
55
D. Fortus, (2009) “The Importance of Learning to Make Assumptions,”
Sci. Ed. 93 (1), 86-108 (2009).
56
E. Kim and S-J. Pak, (2002) “Students Do Not Overcome Conceptual
Difficulties After Solving 1000 Traditional Problems,” Am. J. Phys. 70,
759-765 (2002).
57
B. Sherin, “Common Sense Clarified: The Role of Intuitive Knowledge
in Physics Problem Solving,” J. Res. Sci. Teach. 43, 535-555 (2006).
58
S. C. Nurrenbern and M. Pickering, “Concept Learning versus Problem
Solving: Is There a Difference,” J. Chem. Ed. 64, 508-510 (1987).
59
B.A. Sawrey, “Concept Learning versus Problem Solving: Revisited,” J.
Chem. Ed. 67, 253-254 (1990).
60
M. S. Cracolice, J. C. Deming, and B. Ehlert, “Concept Learning versus
Problem Solving: A Cognitive Difference,” J. Chem. Ed. 85, 873-878
(2008).
61
B. Eylon and F. Reif, “Effects of Knowledge Organization on Task
Performance” Cog. Instr. 1, 5-44 (1984).
62
A number of other sources were useful in constructing this paper,
including, but not limited to, the following: E. Bagno and B. Eylon,
“From Problem Solving to a Knowledge Structure: An Example from
the Domain of Electromagnetism,” Am. J. Phys. 65, 726-736 (1997);
Duncker, “On Problem Solving,” Psych. Monog. 58, 5 (1945); D. H.
Jonassen, ed. Learning to Solve Complex Scientific Problems (Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, New York, N Y, 2007); P. B. Kohl and N. D.
Finkelstein, “The Effect of Instructional Environment on Physics
Students Representational Skills,” Phys Rev. ST-PER 2, 010102 (2006);
A. E. Lawson, “The Development and Validation of a Classroom Test of
Formal Reasoning,” J. Res. Sci. Teach. 15, 11-24 (1978); H. A. Simon
and W. G. Chase, “Skill in Chess,” Am. Sci. 61, 394-403 (1973); R.
Taconis, M. G. M. Ferguson-Hessler, and H. Broekkamp, (2001)
“Teaching Science Problem Solving: An Overview of Experimental
Work,” J. Res. Sci. Teach. 38, 442-468 (2001); M. Ward and J. Sweller,
“Structuring Effective Worked Examples,” Cog. Instr. 7, 1-39 (1990).