Albena Yaneva
Albena Yaneva
Albena Yaneva
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms
Sage Publications, Ltd. is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Social Studies of Science
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
sss
ABSTRACT How do architects imagine, see and define a distant object that
to become a building? How does it become knowable, real? To answer th
questions, I follow architects as they fabricate models and scale them up a
different rates of speed. Instead of being a logical, linear procedure for g
new object that becomes progressively more knowable, ascending from th
to the concrete, scaling is a versatile rhythm, relying on surges, 'jumps' an
By focusing on the most frequently repeated moves such as 'scaling up', '
scale', 'scaling down', and describing their cognitive implications, I depict
architects involve themselves in a comprehensive dialogue with materials
Their material dialogue takes into account dispositions, resistance, stabilit
properties that change proportionally with scale. In the scaling venture, t
alternative states of the building are simultaneously achieved and maintai
of being 'less-known', abstract and comprehensive; and a state of being
known', concrete and detailed. After multiple up and down transitions be
small- and large-scale models, the building emerges, becomes visible, mate
real. These scaling trials bring the building into existence.
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
868 Social Studies of Science 35/6
FIGURE 1
The Table of Models'; Office for Metropolitan Architecture, Rotterdam. (Photograph:
Albena Yaneva)
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Yanev . Scaling Up and Down 869
mission.2 In such experiments with scaling, the tenuous and minute moves
with various tools and models, the intelligibility of materials and the
actions of the architects are all made observable. As I shall show, the
rhythm of scaling relies on procedures for partial seeing: scoping, rescaling,
extending and reducing the material features of scale models. Architects
attempt to resolve problems by using tricks of the trade such as stepping up
the scale, disguising and revealing various aspects, and inspecting and
overseeing those aspects. Scaling requires special equipment, instruments
and embodied routines for manipulating models, as well as meticulous
work with foam and paper for seeing and defining details. Through such
practices, a building can be conceived in thought and brought into exist
ence. It is generated through numerous techniques of projection (Blau &
Kaufman, 1989) and translation.3 The models are scaled and re-scaled, not
according to the architect's mind's eye (Akin & Weinel, 1982), but accord
ing to numerous material formations, practices and relations among archi
tects, consultants, models, cameras and images in a complex visual field.
Over the past 20 years, science and technology studies (STS) have
closely followed scientists, engineers and physicians in and out of their
workplaces; but architects have not been followed as their practices move
from the model shop to the panel presentation for the client, and even
tually to the construction site.4 Some recent STS research on design
practices has analysed visualization and emphasized its social and complex
dynamics. However, such research focuses mainly on engineering design
(Ferguson, 1992; Bucciarelli, 1994; Henderson, 1999; Vinck, 2003). A few
studies have dealt with design in architectural firms, but explored their
activities from a more traditional sociological perspective.5 In this paper,
the architectural office will be studied in the same way that STS has
approached the laboratory (Latour & Woolgar, 1979; Lynch, 1985; Knorr
Cetina, 1999). By following particular scaling moves as they run up and
down, I aim to expose the materialization of successive operations, and to
trace the developing appearance of the building. I will compose a story
about gradations (nuances of size and degrees of presence) in numerous
architectural objects; gradations through which architectural practices play
out on a battlefield full of unknown internal streams, orders and disorders,
flows and synchronization moves, polemics among architects, visual puz
zles, and attempts to resolve disputed states of affairs through visual
instruments and convincing images.
My sources about scaling are conversations among architects engaged
in building models for a new exhibition hall at the Whitney Museum of
American Art in New York (at the time, the design of the museum
extension was commissioned to Rem Koolhaas), interviews with architects
and a rather dilettante personal participation in model fabrications. The
present paper does not invite us to imagine an architectural office. Instead,
by meticulously reconstructing participants' discussions and actions, and
by depicting concrete manipulations with materials and scaling instru
ments, it attempts to bring us into the office and to follow the work of the
Whitney team as it conceives and designs a new exhibition hall.
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
870 Social Studies of Science 35/6
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Yaneva: Scaling Up and Down 871
large scale models, with both abstraction and precision. The final building
is never present in any single state or model, but in what all of them
together project. That is why the building is a multiple object: a composi
tion of many elements; a 'multiverse' instead of a 'universe'.11
Some studies on engineering design treat the designed object to be the
result of a social process involving lengthy negotiations and contradictory
debates among participants, and argue that its final shape depends on
various modes of consensus (Bucciarelli, 1994). Accordingly, what comes
first in the process is a subjective agreement among participants about the
meaning of the artefact designed, and its realization is triggered only after a
shared vision is gained. Like Henderson (1999) and Law (2002), I
consider models as objects over which negotiations and conflicts take place
and treat architects as being implicated in a dialogue with concrete
materials, spatial figures, proportions, dispositions and shapes. That is, a
'reflexive conversation with the materials of the situation' (Sch?n, 1985),
rather than a question of inter-subjective agreement. In this conversation
designers make numerous moves that have unintended effects, with unex
pected problems and potentials. In their design meetings, architects dis
cuss concerns about scoping and rescaling the models; they 'lend' their
bodies to many visual instruments, which enables them to see and experi
ence the internal space, 'guided' by the inner logic of the foam construc
tions, and 'influenced' by many previous choices. They are also 'con
strained'12 by numerous requirements (client demand, city politics, site
specificity, users' expectations) and 'led' to solutions. Materials, scoping
instruments and new knowledge 'talk back' to the architects, and they are
prepared to listen, thus triggering reinterpretations of interim results.
These idioms ('talk back', and so forth) are commonplace in architects'
stories about different projects at OMA.13 Following architects' commu
nication with such objective materials allows us to gain access to forms of
cognition they deploy in the course of design work. Here, more than in any
other context, architects need to make clear to one another what it is they
do - what emerges from their hands - when they engage in design work.
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
872 Social Studies of Science 35/6
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Yaneva: Scaling Up and Down 873
FIGURE 2
Kunl? and Sho are discussing the shape of some small scale 'study models'. (Photo
graph: Albena Yaneva)
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
874 Social Studies of Science 35/6
sketchy version of the building. Why do architects spend hours and hours
looking at this small piece of foam, turning it in their hands, meticulously
examining its corners and openings, positioning it in relation to different
objects at hand, passing it to each other, and inspecting it during disagree
ments and disputes? What are they able to see with its barely visible
features? What is this piece of foam telling them? How are they able to see
it? What is it that guides them to the building?
To answer such questions, I suggest that we follow the team as they
fabricate a large-scale model of an exhibition hall in the extension of
Whitney Museum of American Art; starting from that small model of the
building, modifying it, and subjecting it to numerous visual puzzles.
Architects use two parallel working tables. Various small models and
specific details are scattered on the first one, while a huge model under
construction is installed on the adjacent table. 'Crowds' of architects,
paper cuts-outs, drawings, foam pieces and instruments are gathered
around these scale models.
Architects use a particular instrument, called a modelscope,22 to look
inside the small model in order to see things that cannot be observed
directly from outside. To understand scaling and its cognitive implications,
it is important to consider how this instrument works, and what forms of
thinking are associated with it. When this miniature periscope is inserted
into the small model, it can function as design tool by providing visual
access to selective and realistic eye-level images. By doing so, it can
generate new information about the building and can enable architects to
conceptualize it with more detail, clarity and precision.
In this instance, the design task is to determine the position of a huge
escalator in the interior space of the model. Kunl? changes the place of
the escalator in the large-scale model, then asks the other members of the
team, 'Do you like it?' Nobody answers. He takes the modelscope and
the others keep encouraging him, 'scope it, scope it!' Kunl? moves to the
adjacent table, and then switches on the light source and adjusts the level
of illumination, as required for a comfortable viewing position. Then, he
carefully inserts the modelscope to inspect the small-scale model, and
checks the function of the focusing control (from 5 mm to infinity
according to a field of view of 60? for the small model 040) and adjusts the
orbital scan to achieve the required view. Kunl? now looks inside the small
model, and a deep silence follows (Figure 3).
As Kunl?'s eye inspects the interior space of the model, the eyes of the
others are looking in the direction of the scattered things around the
model, without fixing their glances. They are waiting for their turn. While
anticipating Kunl?'s reactions, they encourage him, 'ouyaou, ouyaou', as if
they were able to see inside the model along with him; as if they collectively
shared the result of his inspection. Kunl? adjusts the light guide connector,
situated inside the instrument's handle, in order to regulate the light. Then
he sets the orbital scan again and starts rotating the viewing direction with
regard to the handle, through a total arc of 360?. An orientation mark in
the image indicates the direction of view. He sees something. He says:
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Yaneva: Scaling Up and Down 875
FIGURE 3
Kunl? is using a modelscope to inspect a small model. (Photograph: Albena Yaneva)
'Here is the northern part of the hall. Ouyaou, I see it, where are you,
stairs? ... I see a staircase, the two pieces of Hopper around? Mmm, here
it is then (a space for the elevator).' While Kunl? communicates his
impressions of what he sees with small gestures, the others also begin to
express reactions. As time goes on, their silent impatience is reinstated.
While the architects from the Whitney team gather around Kunl?, they
talk with tiny particles in the situation, and with the data obtained in the
scoping venture. Rather than coming to agreement, often discussed in
studies of design as preceding the artefact fabrication, the scaling team
engages in a dialogue with a dynamic assemblage of objective materials:
dispositions, objects they see inside the model, spatial transitions, material
properties of the foam, proportions and shapes. Scaling together means
scoping the models, entering into conversation with their barely visible
figures, and discussing with the team what is seen. It turns out to be as
important to design as the drawings and scale models themselves
(Bucciarelli, 1994). The experience of scoping the small models is very
mysterious. The scoping architect remains with a sense of confusion since
he has to see something on his own, being unclear just what he is supposed
to see. Only after numerous adjustments of the instrument are Kunl?'s and
the modelscope's eyes connected, able to see at that particular moment the
shady interior of the small-scale model of the museum exhibition hall. The
others can 'see' only partially by sharing collectively Kunl?'s experience.23
They find themselves in a state of impatience, just as Kunl? was a few
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
876 Social Studies of Science 35/6
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Yaneva: Scaling Up and Down 877
Since there is no visible trace from the act of scoping in the small
model, architects rely only upon the visual experience of the one user of
the instrument. His experience is not absolutely lonely, but actively shared
by the team. A scoping ou?6 movement follows: the knowledge about the
escalator gained by Kunl?, expressed in spatial dispositions, is immediately
transposed onto the huge physical model and discussed by the team. That
is how the use of the modelscope triggers numerous material alternations.
The escalator is placed in the middle of the exhibition hall (not in the
northern part of the building) in a way that enables museum-goers to enter
the gallery immediately; thus the space usually used for circulation is now
designated for art display. The decision to move the escalator is triggered
also by the museum requirement to have 'more space for the permanent
collection', as well as by the users' expectation of a larger building, the
architect's ambition to maintain historical continuity by providing similar
principle of circulation with the one of the old Whitney building, and by
the museum decision to accommodate art in support spaces.
Gathered around the huge scale model, architects discuss the new
escalator position and repeatedly rearrange its interior. Every new disposi
tion is checked out again with the modelscope. A member of the team
takes it, sets the integral light guide, adjusts the working length and
direction and the field of view, and looks inside, inspecting the spaces.
Then, a team member moves to the next table and suggests a new physical
adjustment in the large-scale model of the exhibition hall. After each new
arrangement on the large scale, architects go back to see it in the small
scale model; they trust the monocular image of the internal space obtained
with the tiny eye of the modelscope. Then, they return to the large-scale
model to make it binocular and to perform some adjustments.
Scoping in and scoping out the tiny model, the exhibition hall of the
Whitney is made bigger and bigger, allowing architects to see its 'inside
qualities'.
The bigger it gets, the more details and more interior you see, and you
start really looking at the way a surface meets a floor, or the way you detail
something around the window, and it gets much more refined as it gets
bigger. (Cl 102)
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
878 Social Studies of Science 35/6
FIGURE 4
The interior of the exhibition hall as seen in the large-scale model. (Photograph:
Albena Yaneva)
paintings from the museum collection are placed on the walls of the model
and white plastic figures - future exhibition visitors - are painted in red;
that is, they are rendered more visible and real. These operations enable
architects to accurately arrange the interior architecture, by numerous
hand movements, to shape and produce its space (Figure 4).
The scale shift makes the model larger and more accessible for the
viewer's body; that is, it becomes large enough to simulate an interior eye
level view. It provides more visibility to concrete interior details. Architects'
corporal operations require less effort to see into the building; compared
with the scoping operation, their postures are less stooped, tense and
uncomfortable when assuming the appropriate viewing position. This
physical space manufactured with foam and paper becomes an object of
collective experience, which is visible for many actors at the same time
(Figure 5).
Only by following the material re-arrangements of the big model can
we become aware of what each architect has seen while inspecting the tiny
model with the scoping instrument. What a single architect sees is shared
with the others and changes the cognitive properties of the team.28 The way
he imagines the building, now, is made visible for the other architects by
the tentative movements of his hands that repeatedly change the escalator's
placement. Only when we follow architects' hands as they point to, and
work with, the huge model to transform its composition - taking the same
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Yaneva: Scaling Up and Down 879
FIGURE 5
The Whitney team working on the spatial arrangement of a large-scale model.
(Photograph: Albena Yaneva)
paper figures, manipulating the space - are we able to see how architects
think together.
However, scoping in and scoping out describe two different settings
where other real agencies than humans (with their intentions and isolated
individual minds) take part and shape complex metaphysical imbroglios:
stairs, escalators, foam materials, foam cutters and recalcitrant models. In
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
880 Social Studies of Science 35f 6
the two settings of partial visualizing into the model and through the
models, cognition is a complex social phenomenon, distributed among
individuals, model scope, team, visual puzzles, and materially shape-able
model. The passage from the setting 1 in which a single architect scopes,
many others react and subsequently lend their bodies to puzzling scoping
procedures, to the setting 2 in which many architects stand together at the
side of the model, inspect it with naked eyes and collectively transform its
material body, is not a transition from individually to a socially structured
experience, from a self-contained disembodied technology of cognition to
a collectively and publicly shared one. These settings differ only by the
distinctive way of distributing the action of scaling.
The task of positioning the escalator is organized in such a way that
architects use their visual language more often than their verbal expres
sions; therefore the main resource for communicatively mediating the
performance is also predominantly visual. No single architect occupying a
central position in the process supplies verbal directives. Instead, the
architects communicate in a centre-less heterogeneous network that in
cludes the materials at hand. When viewed as the externalization of an
individual cognitive process, the gestures of the impatient architects be
come entangled with those of the person inspecting the model, and provide
an additional network for mutual actions among members of the scaling
team. That is what makes the inspection of the model always a collectively
shared experience.
By following the scoping in and out procedures that architects deploy
on a daily basis to see a building, we can find two main actors simultane
ously present in the architectural work: the small-scale model and the
large-scale model.
We did small models with different cuts to see how it looks. But since the
small model is too tiny to think about the possibilities of the internal
space, we have to build the big one. People think that it's a lot of effort to
build the huge models but I am glad it works ... And also, we weren't able
to resolve the circulation problem in the small model. I did a box, a small
jewel box, and Rem liked it, but it wasn't sufficient to resolve the
circulation problem. So, we shifted the scale. (SI 102)
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Yaneva: Scaling Up and Down 881
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
882 Social Studies of Science 35/6
table to another, they pass from the small model to the large one, from the
tiny detail to a larger spatial arrangement of the exhibition hall, from the
disposition of the escalator to the overall circulation principle, mechanical
engineering and the philosophy of artistic display. That is, they move from
a small, stabilized composition of things towards a composition of a larger
scope, with greater cognitive and representational power.
Speeds of Scaling Up
Architects use the expression 'jumping up the scale' to describe the move
of passing suddenly to a much larger scale and a larger gathering of
things.
We started from the small ones, and I jumped up the scale. It's dangerous
to shift to the big scale. If we start from the big one, we will lose our
concept. If we start from the huge model, it's dangerous because we will
be lost in details. (SI 102)
The 'jump' is rapid - an almost impulsive and radical shift in scale, not a
slow and gradual one, and so it can become risky. The 'jump' can be
dangerous, according to Shiro, because as architects go into a more refined
version of the building, they risk 'losing' the coherence of the small model
- the main features of the building (the so-called 'concept'31). The 'jump'
also can mean that suddenly knowing more about the building can make it
impossible to maintain a 'knowing-less' state. Its logic can be dispersed in
numerous practical details; 'disperse' meaning that when the particular
elements are more visible and articulated on the large-scale model, the
main idea - those less well-defined, but key, features of the building that
make it function - can become lost. What has to be retained when passing
suddenly from the small to the large scale, is the consistency of the whole
assemblage, representing a state of the building when only 'few things are
known'. In addition, what is retained is the possibility to return to a smaller
scope of gathering.
'Jumping' the scale can be a risky move, because it interrupts the
gradual process of slow-scale doubling.
Of course there are some times when you 'jump'. In the Whitney project
we did such a thing. So, we do something and then, we go to a really big,
big scale to show some details that are really important. For example, for
the scheme 'A' of the Whitney project, it was the windows' shape, and we
wanted to show how it looks with the colour graphs, and we did a really
big model - 1:25. These are really big models. Like a person, like that...
(he shows a human size model with his hand). And you can see how the
detail of the glass is, and then we went back. Sometimes it's important to
do these jumps. But usually we are going up slowly. (El 102)
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Yaneva: Scaling Up and Down 883
Scaling Down
Reaching finer details through scaling up does not naturally guide archi
tects, slowly and linearly, to an ever-larger version of the building, where
more and more becomes known about it. Scaling up is immediately and
reversibly followed by scaling down.
When all the escalators of the new exhibition hall of Whitney are
decided upon, constructed in paper and painted red, they are definitively
placed inside the large-scale model of the exhibition hall. Then, the
architects return to the adjacent table to translate the changes that have
been made in the large model onto the small one: tiny red paper escalators
are carefully manufactured by Torsten and Shiro and placed in the small
scale model so that it is revised with new details and becomes a jewel-like
replica of the huge model. However, the small scale of the model doesn't
allow complete translation of all the details from the large model. Nor is a
complete reproduction needed. Some figures are simply sketched onto the
small one: the escalators are indicated as red endings, and the elevator is
indicated by a transparent Plexiglas box in the middle of the hall. In this
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
884 Social Studies of Science 35/6
way, the small and large models are mutually revised and enriched with
new details from latest developments in the project. The small-scale model
is then closed and placed back on the table, shaped proportionally and
adjusted to other visual representations. It is made compact and easily
movable from one place to another in the office, and from the architect's
office to the client.
All these briefly drawn operations are aimed at scaling down the
model. Scaling up scatters the original unity of the small-scale model,
while scaling down incorporates a patchwork of the complex scattered
details back into that unity. Scaling down is performed as a skilful arrange
ment of these disparate pieces. To achieve this patch-up, a specific tiny
movement of 'taking down the change' is realized. We can see this move
ment in another design task, which consists of determining the shape and
the position of the windows in the new Whitney extension.
We had this idea of the windows' shape and particular position, and we
wanted to see whether it works well, whether it's really good; because in
the small model we liked it, but we weren't sure if it will look good in the
big one. It wasn't only that we weren't sure that it will look good, but we
weren't sure that it would look convincing. So, we wanted to convince
other people that it was looking good. So, we built the big model, and
then we took this detail and we brought it back to the small model, and we
said: 'Okay, that's how we are going to determine it.' (El 102)
With the large model, architects can see whether the windows of the new
Whitney building 'work well'. Every part of the building has to be inte
grated with numerous other interior features, such as lighting, air
conditioning, circulation and infrastructure, mechanical structure, mate
rial properties, as well as with architects' worries, clients' concerns and
users' anticipations. As the windows' position is being defined on the large
scale model, a new adjustment, a new 'good fit'32 among the various
elements is obtained, and then translated to the small model. Once
brought back to the small model, it is then pushed again towards the large
one.
The larger and more differentiated model does not differ in
evolutionary fashion from the small one; rather, it is a tool fo
better, gaining new knowledge, enrolling more actors and refin
small-scale model. Although it is a mediator in the scaling proces
final goal, it is not an ephemeral visual device. It is kept on the
models' along with numerous small-scale models, drawings and c
and foam and paper try-outs. Although stabilized in a given shap
them is completely defined, and any of them can be materially
thus triggering a chain of modifications.
As the design process develops, the scales are shifted and new
the building are gained:
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Yaneva: Scaling Up and Down 885
something different and you have to change the drawing. And I think it's
the same for the large scale and the small scale. If we get further on in the
process, in design development, we know the shape, we know where the
floor levels are, we know where the windows are, but then you start to look
at more interior spaces and you might do a much larger model which is
proportional to the space. But that may affect the smaller one. And you
might say this window has to be like this to get this kind of light and that
means changes, and we have to take it back down and to see how it looks.
So, it's back and forth between the scales. (Cl 102)
As architects shift scales they enter into dialogue with objective materials,
far from any mental models (Gorman, 1997), that compel them and 'tell
them more' about the building, offer resistance, opposition and set up
tensions within. By doing so, they acquire more knowledge about shapes,
dispositions, locations; again, this is not knowledge of facts, but rather
knowledge about spatial transitions, not 'knowing that', but 'knowing
where'. In the translation from the small to the big, a special connection is
maintained between the two models that makes it possible for changes in
the large model to 'affect the smaller one'. Architects 'take the changes
back down' to the small model and update it. That is, more data are being
transmitted to the small model, but always schematically, so it can account
for an abstract and broad-spectrum method for presenting the state of the
building. Moving up and down in scale lets us discover two hologram-like
faces of the building: one small, vague and data-poor, the other large,
detailed and data-rich; being maintained as such, they make it possible for
the building to emerge in the architectural office.
Models are considered as small and large, respectively abstract and
concrete, as they treat compositions of things in a rather different way.
While the large model closely deals with the things - recalcitrant material
properties and adjustments - the small one stands apart from them. No
effort of translation is needed to understand the position of a window, an
escalator or a plug in the large model of the exhibition hall. However, the
meaning of the small model can be grasped only by calling to mind a few
evocative features of the building, and tracing out connections between
them. While the small model, as a first approximation of the building, has
the purpose of facilitating knowledge, inquiry and speculation, the large
model is associated with practical concerns. Therefore, since the small
model is employed simply as a means to encourage more thinking, it is
considered abstract; as the big model is used as a means to define figures of
the building beyond itself, it is a concrete presentation of the building.
However, the development of the practical cognitive power of the large
model does not weaken the abstract properties of the small one. Made for
the sake of knowing more of the building, the abstract model is pushed
towards the large one, in order to facilitate concrete achievements; regard
less of what these achievements turn out to be, the small model remains
abstract, as a tool for defining and perfecting the building.
Detached from the site parameters, the small- and the large-scale
models mutually inform each other and are simultaneously modified.
Jointly replicating and referring to each other in their redundancy, together
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
886 Social Studies of Science 35/6
Conclusion
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Yaneva: Scaling Up and Down 887
move in a random and chaotic way, nor is it a matter of pure routine; it has
definite cumulative effects. Something new emerges out of the circuit: a
projected reality - the new building. This reality becomes visible in the
redundancy between 'knowing-less' and 'knowing-more': abstraction and
concretization, idea and multiple pragmatic details. It emerges as scales are
shifted between small and large models; one pushes to the other in a long
lasting game, and all of them make the building happen. Thus, scaling up
and scaling down are not successive moves, but parallel states, each
containing the other and referring to it. Instead of emerging in a propor
tional relationship to site parameters with a definite referent or 'content',
the building is defined in scaling trials; as it passes through these trials, it
becomes more and more visible, more present, more material, real. 'Scal
ing' is not a way to fit into reality; rather, it is a conduit for its extraction.
Scaling implies seeing in different scales, through a variety of presenta
tional states. The building is simultaneously present in all of these states: it
appears as less-defined and well-defined at the same time. Architectural
design develops, at a given moment and for a certain span of time, through
a circular generative regime instead of a linear process of varying possible
designs and selecting solutions, which subsequently generates the building
- a process of 'punctuated evolution'38 or a process in which successive
artefacts follow one another 'along trajectories'.39
At a given moment in the process, a few models are detached from the
scaling circulation network. They are stabilized in a certain profile and start
working on their own, taking new, independent, and straightforward linear
paths of development. Thus, the scaling process ends up with 'stabiliza
tion'.40 Contrary to all expectations, the scaling venture fails to have as its
upshot a huge detailed 'realistic model of the whole'. The final product of
architectural design is neither the building nor a mock-up sample of the
building in scale 1:1. It is instead that particular assembly of a few 'one
shape models' detached from the scaling continuum and its circular
network. This is what I found on the 'table of models' in the early
afternoon of November 2001 (Figure 1). It was not a bunch of successors
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
888 Social Studies of Science 35/6
Notes
I am indebted to the people from the Office for Metropolitan Architecture in Rotterdam,
and especially to Ole Scheeren, Erez Ella, Carol Patterson, Torsten Schr?der, Sarah
Gibson, Kunl? Adeyemi, Shiro Agata, Shohei Shigematsu, Olga Aleksakova and Rem
Koolhaas for welcoming me in the office and letting me follow them at work; they devoted
a lot of time and patience to me and my questions. For their comments on earlier versions
of this project, I acknowledge Bruno Latour, Peter Galison, Lorraine Daston and Joel
Snyder. Albena Yaneva is a grantee of the Graham Foundation for Advanced Studies in the
Fine Arts, grant # 03069
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Yaneva: Scaling Up and Down 889
7. Compare this with studies that consider design as a work of the brain when, in a
mysterious moment of inspiration, delirium and concealment, an image of the building
appears in a flash (Boyd, 1965; Akin & Weinel, 1982).
8. A fascinating study on the cognitive dimension of engineering, conducted by Vincenti
(1990), noted that engineering design knowledge is acquired in a day-to-day enterprise
according to a systematic experimental methodology. That is how engineers acquire
empirical data needed to carry out design, since the theoretical methods cannot supply
the requisite data.
9. Architectural design is not a gradual step-by-step transfer from one scale to another,
developing towards a ratio of 1:1 (Boudon, 1972); rather, discontinuity and versatility
are its main figures (Schatz & Fiszer, 1999). It relies on surges, breaks, sudden 'jumps'
and meticulous inspections, repetitions and returns; it sets into play simultaneously
different sized actors and several scales, many of which persist throughout all the stages
of the project, regardless of their precision. This story of discontinuity to some extent
follows recent studies on engineering design that treat it as a messy non-linear process,
full of unforeseen pitfalls and unpredicted actions (Henderson, 1999) - a maze, or
complex multidimensional web of interconnections, moving toward a final well
designed product (Bucciarelli, 1994).
10. This view differs from that in studies in the philosophy of technology, which describe
the genesis of technical objects as concretization, that is, as ascending from the abstract
to the concrete; from an unpredictable abstract object, open to its environment,
towards a closed, predictable, differentiated and concrete object (Simondon, 1989).
11. On the building as a 'multiverse', not socially constructed but a stabilized gathering of
few models, tentatively adjusted together and composed in a whole, see Yaneva (2005).
12. Design studies considered constraints as a 'primary generator' triggering a process of
architectural exploration that led to a conjectural solution (Darke, 1979). Although
explicitly articulated, constraints and boundaries, which are so critical for engineering
design, are not considered inflexible; rather, they are subject to change and negotiation
(Bucciarelli, 1994). Science studies also have argued that scientific experimentation
runs according to 'multiple constraints', considered as material obstacles that shape
and delimit action in experimentation (Galison, 1995, 1998).
13. See Koolhaas & Mau (1995) and the Office of Metropolitan Architecture & Koolhaas
(2004). Koolhaas analyses different scale-projects at OMA, investigating how they are
proportionally applied to different-sized cities and urban spaces, as well as how they
generate multiple contents.
14. However, there are few accounts of scale models in architecture, as compared with the
enormous amount of writing on architectural drawing (Porter, 1979; Blau & Kaufman,
1989; Robbins, 1985, 1994; Evans, 1997).
15. Models often travel outside the architectural office to gain powerful allies among
clients, sponsors and future users, community groups and city planning commissions.
They are supposed to express concerns, expertise, opinions and expectations, which are
furthermore taken into account in design. Thus models incorporate not only a variety
of technical concerns, but also a range of other viewpoints.
16. On the dual existence of molecular models in chemistry as quasi-inscriptions and anti
inscriptions at the same time, see Francoeur (1997, 2000). like models of molecules,
architectural models are submitted to various manipulations, assembled, probed and
measured, and are used to gain knowledge about spatial arrangements. However, they
do not reveal properties of structures to ascribe them to hidden phenomena (such as
molecules in chemistry stand); they all work together in a common visual space to
'obtain' a building.
17. They tend to develop different characteristics as the result of a change in the initial
composition; like mutants, models are physically distinct variants of the same species,
the building. None of them is identical to another; each of them is a distinct
composition of things. All together they shape and contain the building.
18. 'Scription' terms include in-scription (Latour & Woolgar, 1979), and con-scription
(Henderson, 1999) and pre-scription.
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
890 Social Studies of Science 35/6
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Yaneva: Scaling Up and Down 891
31. Architects call a 'concept' the main idea of the building, taken in its relationships with
the client demand, the city, the urban fabric, and the broader social, political and
cultural context.
32. Following Alexander's definition of successful design as a 'good fit' (Alexander, 1964),
we can consider scaling up as an attempt to make a myriad of small elements and
micro-equipment fit together and 'work well'. While the 'good fit' at large scale provides
incentive for scaling down - when successful it is taken down and 'determined' on the
small scale - a 'misfit' provides an incentive for making radical changes to the small
model. Thus, scaling up and down aims at neutralizing the incongruities that cause a
'misfit'. The follow-up of scaling operations gives us the possibility to experience
cognitively the sensations about what architects call a 'good test'.
33. Architects call a 'programme' the content of the building - the internal distribution of
spaces according to functional needs, general scope and insertion in reality.
34. To solve a 'problem' means in architectural terms: 'isolating' the problematic feature,
transporting it between scales, zooming and reducing, pushing and defining, thus 'dis
solving' it through travel. By 'dis-solved' I mean 'solved' in a particular way. I use the
twofold meaning of 'solution' - as the resolution of a difficulty and as two substances
mixed together and uniformly dispersed. As scales are shifted the problematic feature is
solved in the same way one substance is dissolved in another and the building becomes
knowable.
35. Images are regularly produced at every scale-up and scale-down move; the images serve
as protocols for carefully maintaining the traces of the scaling experiences, investing the
trials with materials and shapes; documenting new data about the building. On the flat
surface of multiple collages, montages and drawings one can find imprinted the 'faces'
of the three-dimensional models, the traces of their movements and transformations.
Thus, the pure and formal redundancy of models cannot occur without the expressive
images, which capture their meaning and transmit information. The building can be
interpreted through the material body of these images that make it presentable for
client and publics.
36. The 'ability to anticipate' errors in size and relationship is considered important in the
scaling venture (Licklider, 1966). Examples from the history of technology highlight
that building successful full-sized machines was one of the challenging intellectual
problems with which early modern engineering confronted pre-classical mechanics in
the 17th century. All attempts to scale-up models of machines and to enlarge them in
correct proportion used to fail because scaling was only approached mathematically,
without taking into account the properties of the materials; namely, the robustness and
resistance of materials, qualities that diminish proportionally when a device is enlarged
(Popplow, 2003). Likewise, the difficulty of scaling-up models of vessels in naval
architecture in the 18th century was defined as the incapacity of rational mechanics to
describe or predict ship behaviour. Only the skilful model-maker was able to foresee
the displacement, stability, weathering qualities and other essentials of a large ship,
instead of using pure calculation (Schaffer, 2004).
37. According to evolutionary theories (Forty, 1986; Petrovski, 1992, 1994, 1996; Basalla,
1988; Pye, 1988) a new design product follows from earlier products through
successive functional changes. To elucidate the multiplicity of technical tools and the
drive for their improvement, these theories argue that novelty appears among
continuously evolving artefacts. They explain how the new design object comes into
being in relation to an external factor (social context, cultural atmosphere, economic or
political factors, society), always being the starting point of a new process of series of
transformations - that is, a linear and temporal succession of finished and limited
events.
38. According to which the development of an artefact - understood in relation to its
ancestors and successors - follows a staircase-like evolution of sequences of change and
continuity (Bijker, 1995: 88).
39. See what Latour (1989: 322-52) has called 'diffusion model of technology' as
compared with the 'translations model'.
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
892 Social Studies of Science 35/6
40. I am following here the actors' definition of stabilization, although the term was used in
laboratory studies (Latour & Woolgar, 1979). Architects define stabilization as a
momentary pause in the scaling up and down process; a clarification of the building
profile slowing down the versatile scaling course.
References
Akin, ?. & E.F. Weinel (eds) (1982) Representation and Architecture (Silver Spring, MD:
Information Dynamics).
Alexander, Christopher (1964) Notes on the Synthesis of Form (Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press).
Allen, Steve (2000) Practice: Architecture, Technique and Representation (Amsterdam: Overseas
Publishers Association).
Basalla, George (1988) The Evolution of Technology (Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press).
Bijker, Wiebe (1995) Of Bicycles, Bakelites, and Bulbs. Toward a Theory of Sociotechnical
Change (Cambridge, MA & London: MIT Press).
Blau, Eva & Edward Kaufman (eds) (1989) Architecture and its Image (Montreal: Canadian
Centre for Architecture).
Blau, Judith (1984) A Sociological Perspective on Architectural Practice (Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press).
Boudon, Philippe (1971) Sur l'Espace Architectural. Essai d'?pist?mologie et d'Architecture
(Paris: Dunod).
Boudon, Philippe (1972) LaVille de Richelieu (Paris: AREA).
Boudon, Philippe (1992) Introduction ? l'Architecturologie (Paris: Dunod).
Boudon, Philippe (1999) 'The Point of View of Measurement in Architectural Conception:
From the Question of Scale to Scale as Question', Nordic Journal of Architectural
Research 12(1): 7-18.
Boyd, Robin (1965) The Puzzle of Architecture (Carlton, Australia: Melbourne University
Press).
Bucciarelli, Louis L. (1994) Designing Engineers (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Call?n, Michel (1996) 'Le Travail de la Conception en Architecture', Situations, Les Cahiers
de la Recherche Architecturale 37(1): 25-35.
Cuff, Dana (1991) Architecture: The Story of Practice (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Darke, Jane (1979) 'The Primary Generator and the Design Process', Design Studies 1:
36-44.
Dupire, Alain, Bernard Hamburger, Jean-Claude Paul, Jean-Michel Savignat & Alain
Thiebaut (1981) Deux Essais sur la Construction (Bruxelles: Mardaga).
Evans, Robin (1997) Translations from Drawing to Building and Other Essays (London:
Architectural Association).
Ferguson, Eugene (1992) Engineering and the Mind's Eye (Cambridge, MA & London: MIT
Press).
Forty, Adrian (1986) Objects of Desire (New York: Pantheon).
Francoeur, Eric (1997) 'The Forgotten Tool: Design and Use of Molecular Models', Social
Studies of Science 27(1): 7-40.
Francoeur, Eric (2000) 'Beyond Dematerialization and Inscription: Does the Materiality of
Molecular Models Really Matter?', Hyle - International Journal for Philosophy of
Chemistry 6(1): 63-84.
Galison, Peter (1995) 'Context and Constraints', in J.Z. Buchwald (ed.), Scientific Practice.
Theories and Stories of Doing Physics (Chicago, IL & London: University of Chicago
Press): 13-41.
Galison, Peter (1998) Image and Logic. A Material Culture of Microphysics (Chicago, IL &
London: University of Chicago Press).
Gorman, Michael (1997) 'Mind in the World: Cognition and Practice in the Invention of
the Telephone', Social Studies of Science 27: 583-624.
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Yaneva: Scaling Up and Down 893
Greene, Herb (1976) Mind and Image: An Essay on Art & Architecture (Lexington, KY:
University Press of Kentucky).
Henderson, Kathryn (1999) On Line and On Paper: Visual Representations, Visual Culture, and
Computer Graphics in Design Engineering (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Hutchins, Edwin (1991) 'The Social Organization of Distributed Cognition', in L. Resnick
& J. Levine (eds), Perspectives on Socially Shared Cognition (Washington, DC: APA
Press).
Hutchins, Edwin (1995) Cognition in the Wild (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Itten, Johannes (1975) Gestaltung^- und Formenlehre: Mein Vorkurs am Bauhaus und Sp?ter
(New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold).
Knorr-Cetina, Karin (1999) Ep?temic Cultures: How the Sciences Make Knowledge
(Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).
Koolhaas, Rem & Bruce Mau (1995) Small, Medium, Large, Extra-Large (Rotterdam: Office
for Metropolitan Architecture).
Latour, Bruno (1984) Pasteur: Guerre et Paix des Microbes, Suivi de Irr?ductions (Paris: La
D?couverte).
Latour, Bruno (1989) La Science en Action, Introduction ? la Sociologie des Sciences (Paris: La
D?couverte).
Latour, Bruno (1990) 'Drawing Things Together', in M. Lynch & S. Woolgar (eds),
Representation in Scientific Practice (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press): 19-68.
Latour, Bruno (2001) UEspoir de Pandore: Pour une Version R?aliste de VActivit? Scientifique
(Paris: La D?couverte).
Latour, Bruno (2003) 'The Promises of Constructivism', in D. Hide & E. Selinger (eds),
Chasing Technoscience. Matrix for Materiality (Bloomington, IN: Indiana University
Press): 27^7.
Latour, Bruno & Steve Woolgar (1979) Laboratory Life (London: SAGE Publications).
Lave, Jean (1988) Cognition in Practice: Mind, Mathematics, and Culture in Everyday Life
(Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Law, John (2002) Aircraft Stories: Decentering the Object in Technoscience (Durham, NC: Duke
University Press).
Licklider, Heath (1966) Architectural Scale (New York: Brazillier).
Lynch, Michael (1985) 'Discipline and The Material Form of Image: An Analysis of
Scientific Visibility', Social Studies of Science 15: 37-66.
Office for Metropolitan Architecture & Rem Koolhaas (2004) Content (K?ln: Taschen).
Orr, Frank (1985) Scale in Architecture (New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold Company).
Petrovski, Henry (1992) The Evolution of Useful Things (New York: Vintage Books).
Petrovski, Henry (1994) Design Paradigms. Case Histories of Error and Judgment in
Engineering (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Petrovski, Henry (1996) Inventing by Design, How Engineers Get from Thought to Things?
(Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).
Popplow, Markus (2003) 'Models of Machines: A "Missing link" Between Early Modern
Engineering and Mechanics?', Preprint no. 225 of the Max-Planck Institute of the
History of Science in Berlin.
Porter, Tom (1979) How Architects Visualize (London: Studio Vista).
Porter, Tom & John Neale (2000) Architectural Supermodels. Physical Design Simulation
(Oxford & Boston, MA: Architectural Press).
Pye, David (1988) The Nature and Aesthetics of Design (London: The Herbert Press).
Raynaud, Dominique (2001) 'Comp?tences et Expertise Professionnelle de l'Architecte
dans le Travail de Conception', Sociologie du Travail 43: 451-69.
Robbins, Edward (1985) 'Drawing and the Social Production of Architecture', in PL. Knox
(ed.), The Design Professions and the Built Environment (London & Sydney: Croom
Helm/New York: Nichols Publishing Company): 42-61.
Robbins, Edward (1994) Why Architects Draw? (Cambridge, MA & London: MIT Press).
Schaffer, Simon (2004) 'Fish and Ships: Models in the Age of Reason', in S. de
Chadarevian & N. Hopwood (eds), Models: The Third Dimension of Science (Stanford,
CA: Stanford University Press).
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
894 Social Studies of Science 35/6
Schatz, Fran?oise & Stanislas Fiszer (1999) 'Dealing with Space: Tales and Scales in
Architectural Design', InnehM 1: 43-59.
Sch?n, Donald (1985) The Design Studio: An Exploration of its Traditions and Potentials
(London: RIBA Publications).
Sibum, Otto (1992) Shifting Scales: Microstudies in Early Victorian Britain. Unpublished
manuscript, Max-Planck Institute for the History of Science.
Simondon, Gilbert (1989) Du Mode d'Existence des Objets Techniques (r??dition avec postface
et pr?face) (Paris: Aubier).
Vincenti, Walter G. (1990) What Engineers Know and How They Know It: Analytical Studies
from Aeronautical History (Baltimore, MD & London: The Johns Hopkins University
Press).
Vinck, Dominque (ed.) (2003) Everyday Engineering: An Ethnography of Design and
Innovation (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Yaneva, Albena (2005) 'A Building is a Multiverse', in B. Latour & P. Weibel (eds), Making
Things Public (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
This content downloaded from 137.108.145.45 on Mon, 08 Jan 2018 21:10:58 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms