BRIDGES-TheSIS - Child Trends-2002!01!01 FR ReligionSpiritAdol
BRIDGES-TheSIS - Child Trends-2002!01!01 FR ReligionSpiritAdol
BRIDGES-TheSIS - Child Trends-2002!01!01 FR ReligionSpiritAdol
in
Lisa J. Bridges, Ph .D .
Chil
January, 2002
Religion and Spirituality in Childhood and Adolescenc e
January, 200 2
Social Capital and S oci al S uppor t . .. . . . . . . . . . . .. .. .. ... ... .. .. .... .... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. .. . . . ... ...... .... 13
Health Promotion . .. .... .. .. .. ......... ..... . . . . . . . . . . .. ............ . . .. .. .. .. .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . .. .. .. .. .. 13
Promotion of Prosocial Behavior . . . . . . . . . . . . ...... .. .. .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 4
Provision of Purpose and Meaning . . . . . . . .. .. .. .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. .. .. .. .. .. .... .... .. ...... .... .. . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 5
Conceptual Links Between Parental and Familial Religiosity or Spirituality and Child and
Adolescent Well-Being ............. .. . . . . . . . . . . .. ...... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ................................ . .. . ...... .... .. .. . . . . . 1 5
Social Support...................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ........ . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . .. ........ ........................... ........... .. .. .... . 1 6
Health Promo tion ............... .. . . . . . . . . . . .. .. ....... . . . . . . . . . . .. .. . . .. .. . . .. ....................... ..................... .... ...... .. . 1 6
Direct Effects of Religiosity on Parenting . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . .... .. . . ........................................ ........ .. .. .. ... 1 7
Indirect Effects of Religiosity on Parenting . . . . . . . . ..... .... .. .. ................. . . . .. ................... ................ 2 1
Religiosity and Parental Modeling ......... .. . . . . . . . . . . .. . . ... .. .. .. .................. . . .. . .................................. 24
Measurement of Religiosity in Childhood and Adolescence .......................................... . . ............ 25
Review of Empirical Evidence Regarding Religiosity and Well-Being in Childhood an d
Adolescence ...................... .... .. . . . . . . . . . ..... . ... .. . . . . . . . . . . . .. .... .. .. .. ...................... . . ............................... . . . . 29
Delinquency and Risk-Taking Behavior . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. .. .. .. .. .. ................... ...... .. .. .... ..... . ................. 30
Prosocial and Moral Values and Behavior . . . . . . . . . .. ...... .. .. ..................... ...... . . . . .. .. .. .. .................... 39
Personality and Mental Health . . . . .. ........ . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. .... .. .... .. ......................... .. .. .... ...... .................. 41
Other Aspects of Adolescent Well-Being . ................ .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. .. .. .. .. .. .... .. .. .. . . .. . . .. . . .. . . . . . . . . . . 45
Summary ...... .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ...... .. . . . . . . . . . . . . .............. ............ .. . . . . . . . . . . . .. .. .. .. ...... .. . . .. . . . . . . .. . . .. . . . . . . . . .. 45
Review of Empirical Evidence Regarding Parental Religiosity and Well-Being in Childhood and
Adolescence ...... .. .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. .. ...... . . . . . . . . . . . . .. ........................ .. . . . .. .. . .. . .. .. .. .. . . ...... ...... .. .... .. .. . . . . .. .. .. 4 6
Conclusions ....... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. .. .... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .......................... . . . . . . . . . ... . .. .. .. .... .......... .. .. ...... .. .. . ..... .. 49
Implications ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . .. ...... . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . .......... .... ........ . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. .. .. ................ .. .. ... . . . .. .. .. .. .. 5 1
References ........ . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. ............ ............ . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . ............ .... ...... .... .......... 53
2
Religious faith, beliefs, and activities are important aspects of the lives of America n
adolescents . Although researchers have found a general age trend for religiousness to declin e
from childhood through adolescence (Benson, Donahue, & Erickson, 1989 ; King, Elder, &
Whitbeck, 1997), the percentage of American high school seniors who self-reported church
attendance at least monthly was highly stable from 1987 through 1994, ranging from a low o f
46% in 1990 to a high of 50% in 1992 (Donahue & Benson, 1995). These percentages wer e
derived from Monitoring the Future reports published by the Institute for Social Research at th e
sample . Using the same annual survey, Youniss, McLellan, and Yates (1999) reported that ove r
60% of high school seniors reported that religion was "pretty " or "very" important to them. Thi s
percentage w as highly consistent across the decades of the 1970s, 1980s , and 1990s .
associations between religion, spirituality, and well-being in adulthood . Depending upon the
types of measures of religiosity and spirituality utilized and the outcomes assessed, results wit h
adults support low but significant positive associations between religiosity and components o f
well-being, including substance abuse, mental health, physical health, and general life
satisfaction (e .g., Ellison, 1991 ; Ellison, 1993, Fischer & Richards, 1998 ; George, Larson,
Koenig, & McCullough, 2000) . Despite the obvious importance of religion in the lives o f
children and adolescents, however, considerably less attention has been paid to associations
between religiosity and well-being prior to adulthood . In this report, we will begin with a brief
possible mechanisms for associations between child and adolescent religiosity, parental
religiosity, and well-being . The fourth section will discuss issues pertaining to the measurement
of religiosity and adolescence in childhood and adolescence . Fifth, two sections will draw
together recent empirical reports attempting to evaluate the contributions of religiosity to child
and adolescent well-being, primarily focusing on work pertaining to religiosity and well-being
prior to the college years . The final section will summarize the research, summarize issues to be
addressed in future research, and will present some possible implications of the research
A consensus report prepared by a panel convened by the National Institute for Healthcare
Research (NIHR ; see George et al ., 2000) defined spirituality as behaviors, cognitions, and
emotions that arise as part of an individual's search for connection with a divine being, a higher
power, or an ultimate truth . Religion or religiousness may also involve a search for the spiritual,
that provides guidance, validation, and support for the methods with which that search is
The NIHR panel (George et al ., 2000) also identified a second type of religiousnes s
characterized by the same collectivity described above, but with different underlying goals . This
form of religiousness involves participation in a religious community in order to reach goals that
are not inherently spiritual in nature, such as opportunities for social interaction or financial gain .
This definition is consistent with Allport and Ross' (1967) description of extrinsic religiosity .
Theoretically, the distinction between these two types of religiosity is relevant for understanding
whether positive effects of religiousness are primarily due to the benefits of membership in a
4
may contribute to well-being for reasons that are more tied to personal faith-such as an sense of
The meanings of religious participation and of religious beliefs change across the cours e
of development . Most theories of religious development have as their foundation the cognitive
developmental theory of Piaget (e .g ., Piaget, 1962 ; Piaget & Inhelder, 1958) . The focus of these
theories is on the structure of religious thought as it changes across time, rather than on the
content of religious beliefs . Notable among these theories are those of Elkind (e .g. 1964, 1970)
Goldman (1964), Fowler (e .g., Fowler, 1991 ; Osmer & Fowler, 1985), and Oser (e .g ., 1991) .
These theories have in common the proposal that religious thinking, in conjunction with other
areas of thinking, moves from concrete imagery and literal beliefs in childhood to more abstract
religious thinking in adolescence . Thus, it is only in adolescence that religious beliefs and values
are expected to begin to take on the same meanings that they have in adulthood . In the following
sections, we will briefly review the theories of religious development elaborated by Elkind,
Fowler, and Oser, as well as Kirkpatrick's attachment theoretical perspective on the development
religious denominations have different foundational beliefs, including different beliefs about the
nature of God (or gods) and humankind, and the relationship between the two as expressed
through worship, prayer, and the activities of daily life . When older adolescents and adults self
report denominational affiliations, therefore, they are not simply identifying the church that they
attend, but are rather identifying a set of religious practices to which they adhere, and the
religious beliefs that underlie these practices (at least to the extent that the individual perceives
Elkind (1964 ; 1970) found that such understanding of religious beliefs and practices is
not present in young children, but rather develops across childhood . Elkind proposed that there
are three stages of religious development in childhood and adolescence that parallel th e
described by Piaget.
children are beginning to use signs and symbols to represent objects in their real lives . They are
able to use categorical thinking, but they have little ability to understand what distinguishes
categories from each other, or that an individual or object can be classified into more than one
category at the same time . Young children cannot, for example, understand that an individual
can be Catholic and American at the same time . Children who are raised in a religion may know
the name of their denomination, and that the name of the denomination represents something
about individuals who belong to that denomination, but they have very little understanding of
understanding about religion and religious beliefs . In keeping with the concrete operational
thought that characterizes children of this age, however, thinking about religion is also based on
observable behavior, rather than on thoughts, feelings, and motivations . Children understand
that denominations differ in their religious activities, but they have much less understanding
about the differences in religious beliefs that underlie different activities . Similarly, when
children at this age are asked to describe prayer, they focus on the activity of prayer, rather than
on the inner feelings and beliefs that older individuals may explore and express in prayer .
Finally, Stage 3 religious thinking becomes possible with the advent of formal
characterized by the capacity for understanding abstract concepts and for personal reflection and
exploration of religious beliefs, values, and practices . Denominations are understood to differ
because of underlying differences in beliefs, and prayer is understood as a private and personal
experience of communion with God . Thus, it is with the advent of Stage 3 religious thinking that
young adolescents' responses to questions about religion are likely to be similar in their meaning
to those of adults.
Fowler (1991 ; Osmer & Fowler, 1985) developed a theory of faith development that, like
Elkind's theory, includes a series of stages that largely follow the cognitive developmental stage
theory of Piaget . This theory is also strongly influenced by Erik Erikson's psychosocial theory
of ego identity development (e .g. Erikson, 1950 ; 1980). As defined by Fowler (1991), faith is
" . . .the dynamic process of construal and commitment by which we focus our trust and loyalty,
our dependence and confidence, in a center or centers of value, and on images and realities of
power . In faith we fi nd coherence for our lives through allegiance to an emerging, conscious
master story or stories" (p. 33) . Fowler suggested that faith develops in the context of
interpersonal relationships, and that the capacity and need for faith are innate human
characteristics . Faith includes religious faith, but faith can also include trust and loyalty to other
Fowler referred to these as stages of faith consciousness. The first stage, primal faith, occurs
during infancy . Consistent with Erikson's description of the task of infancy as being the
development of a sense of basic trust (versus mistrust) in caregivers, Fowler suggested that
experiences of being provided with consistent and appropriate caregiving promote faith in
caregivers . Faith in caregivers thus forms the foundation for subsequent faith development,
including religious faith . Such a view is consistent with the attachment theoretical approach to
Fowler's second stage is the stage of intuitive-projective faith, which emerges during the
preoperational stage as described by Piaget . During this period, children do not have a true
capacity for logical reasoning, but they are still concerned with making meaning out of their
experiences . Piaget (1962) described how preoperational children, in their fantasy play, often ac t
out events that they have experienced, sometimes painful or confusing events, and suggested that
this repetition is their way of dealing with and understanding these events . In this stage children
use their newly-developing imagination and ability to use symbols in order to understand what is
going on around them, including what parents and other important figures tell them about God,
and religion, and of the religious behaviors that are modeled by these adult figure .
The stage of intuitive-projective faith also coincides with Erikson's second and third
stages of psychosocial development autonomy versus shame and self-doubt, and initiative
versus guilt . During these stages, self-related emotions such as shame, guilt, pride and self-
confidence are being established . Positive development through these stages leaves individuals
with a healthy balance between positive self-feelings and the capacity to experience the moral
8
emotions of shame and guilt when warranted, without being overwhelmed or incapacitated by
them . Consistent with Erikson's descriptions, Fowler suggested that the emotional bases for
The third stage of faith development is the stage of mythic-literal faith . This stage i s
made possible by the development of concrete operational thought during the elementary school
years . At this stage children can think logically about real events , but they have difficulty with
abstract concepts . School - age children are very literal, and this colors their understanding of
God and religion . Erikson describes this as the stage of industry versus inferiority, when
children are focused on learning the skills that are valued in their society . In the domain of
moral reasoning , Kohlberg (e .g., Colby & Kohlberg, 1987) similarly suggested that this is the
age period during which mor al reasoning reflects a desire on the part of the child to be seen as
"good" in the eyes of parents and other authority figures-what is moral is what will please these
figures . In the realm of faith consciousness , then, Fowler described children at this stage as
typically accepting without question the religious beliefs of their parents -particularly when
those beliefs are presented unambiguously -but with little depth of understanding.
The final pre - adult stage involves synthetic-conventional faith. This stage become s
possible in the preadolescent and early adolescent periods, with the onset of formal operational
of the advances of the synthetic-conventional stage of faith development suggests that it is at this
point when self-reports of religious values may become predictive of other aspects of emotional
well-being . The greater cognitive abilities of this stage, including greater ability to understand
abstract concepts, may lead to a perception of logical inconsistencies in the religious messages
with which the adolescent has grown up as well as greater awareness of conflicting religious
beliefs and ideologies . Adolescents may consequently exhibit an increased desire to explore
alternative ideologies, and may also lead to the rejection of religion entirely .
exploration, Erikson (1960 ; 1980) identified this adolescent stage as the stage of ego identity
versus identity diffusion, and stated that among the key accomplishments in establishing a
healthy sense of personal identity was to find persons, ideologies, and institutions in which to
have faith . Adolescents may develop deeper understanding and appreciation of the religions in
which they were raised, in part because of their active exploration of that faith as well as
alternative value systems, or they may adopt religious views and values that differ from those of
Oser's theory focuses on stages in the development of religious judgment (Oser, 1991) .
Oser defines religious judgment as " . . .reasoning that relates reality as experienced to something
beyond reality and that serves to provide meaning and direction beyond learned content . . ."
(Oser, 1991, pp . 6-7) . Oser was particularly interested in developmental changes in the
explanations that children and adults have for experiences, both personal and observed, that
appear to be contrary to religious beliefs . Religious judgments thus involve the answers that
individuals find for themselves that reconcile religious faith with reality that appears to
Oser described five stages in the development of religious judgment, three of which are
typically stages of reasoning reached in childhood and adolescence, and a fourth of which
develops in a minority of individuals in adolescence . Oser developed this stage theory based on
interviews with individuals from ages 7 through 75 . In Stage 1, children's views of God are
10
highly concrete and literal . God is seen as immediately involved in the everyday events of the
world, as causing all events and as creating all things . God must be obeyed because
disobedience brings immediate punishments, such as accidents or illness . At the same time,
individuals are seen as having minimal influence on God . This form of religious judgment
parallels the earliest stage of preconventional moral reasoning as described by Colby and
Kohlberg (1987), in which laws and rules must be obeyed primarily to avoid punishment .
In Stage 2, older children and adolescents view God in a less punitive light . God can be
and practices . God's love is seen as evident in a healthy and happy life, God's wrath in his
failure to intervene in times of strife . At the same time, God is also seen as less likely to
God is seen as more distant and apart from the world, and individuals are seen as more
responsible for their own well-being . Development from Stage 2 to Stage 3 judgment is
and of prayers not answered . In the face of increasing recognition of injustice and human
suffering, this is also a stage of increased questioning of the existence of God and increasing
atheism among adolescents . In Stage 4, which begins to appear among a small percentage of
older adolescents, individuals who maintain religious faith may turn again to God as the ultimate
creator who is the source of freedom and life, and whose existence makes life meaningful .
Finally, it should be noted that Oser's theory does not suggest that all religious judgments
exhibited by an individual will necessarily be at the same stage, nor that all individuals of the
same age will exhibit the same levels of religious judgment . Rather Oser suggested, and his
II
research has found, that the percentages of religious judgments representative of each of his
Each of the theories described thus far focus on age-related trends in the development o f
the focus is on general trends, rather than on the causes of individual differences . In contrast, the
that individuals' religious beliefs and practices are influenced by those of their parents, but the
nature of this influence is strongly affected by the quality of the parent-child attachment
parents are secure are likely to adopt the religious beliefs of their parents, while those whose
attachment relationships are insecure are likely to be less influenced by parents. Further, based
on attachment theory (e .g . Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978 ; Bowlby, 1969 ; Bretherton,
1987), Kirkpatrick suggests that an individual's relationship with God can be considered as an
As with any attachment relationship established beyond infancy, the quality of the
primary attachment relationship(s) established with caregivers in infancy and early childhood is
12
expected to strongly affect qualitative characteristics of the relationship with God . Secure
relationships with religious parents may promote high levels of religiosity, and belief in a loving
and supportive God . Insecure relationships, particularly with religious parents, may promote
agnosticism and atheism, or belief in a God who is punitive and demanding . In other cases when
primary attachment relationships have been insecure, a relationship with God may serve a
relationship with God in an effort to obtain security that was unavailable to them from their early
attachment relationships . In support of this view, Kirkpatrick and Shaver (1990) found that
sudden religious conversions were four times more frequent among adults who retrospectively
reported having insecure, avoidant attachment relationships with their mothers than among adults
self-reporting secure attachments with their mothers . Further, these researchers found that
respondents who reported avoidant attachment relationships with relatively nonreligious mothers
were the most religious, particularly the most conservatively religious, of all of the respondent s
in the study . Interestingly, when maternal religiousness was high, respondent religiousness was
Conceptual Links Between Child and Adolescent Religiosity and Other Aspects of
Well-Being
As with the literature on adult religiosity and well-being, there is considerable disconnect
between theories of religious development and research on child and adolescent religiosity and
other aspects of well-being . As will be discussed in a later section, research tends to focus on
measures of current religious beliefs and practices, such as denominational affiliation, religious
participation in religious activities . Developmental changes have seldom been addressed, and no
13
studies have been conducted linking differences in faith consciousness, religious judgments, o r
There are several mechanisms through which religiosity, as tapped by these types of measures ,
may influence child and adolescent well-being (George, et al ., 2000), some of which have been
examined in more detail than others . Among the possible mechanisms are the creation of socia l
capital and provision of social support , the promotion of healthy behavior, the promotion of
prosocial behavior and the inhibition of antisocial behavior, and the provision of a sense o f
purpose and meaning in life. Also, self- selection into religious communities must be considered
As discussed by King & Furrow (2001), children and adolescents who are part of a
religious community are likely to have an expanded network of caring and nurturing adults as
well as age-mates who know them well and who can serve as resources for emotional, physical,
and financial support . Further, as children move through adolescence and increasingly develop
an understanding of prayer as a personal communication with God (Scarlett & Perriello, 1991),
God becomes a confidant and a source of emotional support as well (Waite, 2001) .
Health Promotion
Most religions include proscriptions against drug and alcohol abuse (e.g ., McBride,
Mutch, & Chitwood, 1996), and many include dietary restrictions as well that may promot e
health. Many religions specifically teach that a healthy body and a healthy spiritual life go han d
in hand . Physical health thus takes on additional spiritual meaning as a sacred duty to God a s
well as to self. Perhaps no religion states this more explicitly than the Seventh-day Adventist
14
Church . In open-ended interviews with a sample of 1,865 students attending Adventist colleges,
McBride and colleagues found that the most frequently cited reason for abstaining from alcohol
and drugs involved concerns for health . Eighty-seven percent of their respondents cited health
reasons, while 73% mentioned a commitment to follow Christ . Interestingly, the difference was
more pronounced among white students (86 .5% vs . 70%) than among African-American (92%
reported that the two most frequent means by which teenagers got involved in service activities
were school and church groups . Nolin, Chaney, Chapman, and Chandler (1997) found that
students attending religious schools were substantially more likely to report engaging in service
activities than were students attending public schools (66% versus 42%, respectively) . Using
data from Monitoring the Future surveys conducted in the 1970s, 1980s, and 1990s, Youniss and
colleagues (1999) found that the personal importance of religion was consistently and positively
associated with engaging in service activities . In every decade, approximately 74% of students
who reported that their religion was important to them engaged in service at least occasionally,
while only 25% of students reporting that religion was not important to them did so .
prosocial behavior, and reduce the probability of antisocial behavior, in another way as well .
Acceptance of the moral tenets of a religious faith may be a primary contributor to the
development of healthy guilt responses to wrongdoing or failures to act in moral and prosocial
ways . Although little research has been conducted on the connection between guilt and religion
15
in childhood and adolescence, studies with college students and other adult samples indicate that
intrinsic religiosity (i.e., commitment to religion and participation in religious activities because
of personal faith) is associated with feelings of guilt in reaction to wrongdoing (Fischer &
Richards , 1998) .
In Erikson' s psychosocial theory of development (Erikson , 1968; 1980), the major task of
adolescence is the exploration of occupational and ideological identities that will form the
Religious dogma and tradition can prove particularly attractive to adolescents because they are
searching for institutions and individuals that provide answers to questions regarding the ultimate
meaning of life, and a sense of continuity and belonging to something that is greater than self .
Although this search can create vulnerability to the influence of charismatic leaders and cults, the
senses of history, tradition, and ties to higher beings and universal principles that religions
Particularly in the years prior to adolescence, parental religiosity and spirituality may
have a more powerful impact on child well-being than does the child's own self-reported
religiosity . Some of the mechanisms of influence are the same as those described above,
including increased social capital and social support, and promotion of healthy behavior . In
addition, parents' religiosity may affect children's well-being through both direct and indirect
influences on parenting behavior . Parental religiosity may influence their family, work and
community behaviors in ways that affect children, for example, by affecting their propensity to
16
divorce . Finally, parents' religiosity may influence the behaviors and beliefs that they model for
their children .
Social Suppor t
Parents who are connected with a religious community may experience higher levels o f
social support themselves and may afford their children greater opportunities for such support
than do parents who do not participate . A consistent empirical finding is that adults who are part
of a religious community are less socially isolated than are other adults (e .g. Garfinkel, 2001,
King, 2001 ; Sherkat & Ellison, 1999) . King (2001) found that religious grandparents were more
involved with their grandchildren than were less religious grandparents . This increased
involvement was explained in large part by greater involvement in all types of family and social
ties within the community, although there were also some direct effects of personal aspects of
Health Promotio n
Parents who adhere to religious proscriptions against unhealthy behaviors may be more
likely than less religious parents to provide their children with a healthy environment, and may
serve as stronger models of health promoting behavior . As described earlier, most religions have
proscriptions against the abuse of alcohol, tobacco, and other substances . Research with adults
has found that attendance at religious activities and affiliation with conservative denominations
are associated with low levels of the use of these substances (Sherkat & Ellison, 1999) . In turn,
Oetting (1999) suggested that the strongest influence of religion on child and adolescent
substance use and abuse may be through the effect that religion has on the social norms
may directly affect parenting. First, religions imbue childrearing with divine and mora l
significance, consistent with their views on the nature of the child, which can become a resourc e
for coping with the emotions aroused by childrearing stresses. Religions also frequently provide
concrete guidance on childrearing that may provide practical coping strategies for parents .
Second, religions and religious institutions offer opportunities for parents and children t o
religion (e .g., worship services, bar mitzvahs, first communions) and activities that are mor e
opportunities for involvement with other families with children of similar ages who can offe r
difficulties has been a particular focus of theory and research on families of children with
disabilities (e.g . Rogers -Dolan & Blacher, 1995) . This largely desc riptive and qualitative body
of research suggests that parents often use religious coping strategies to deal with the stresse s
involved in having a child with disabilities or chronic medical conditions . Religious forms of
disabled child as a gift from God or when parents believe that God never gives individuals more
than they can bear-and the use of religious rituals such as prayer, attendance at religiou s
recommendations provided by religious organizations may provide a resource for parents facing
18
uncertainties regarding proper ways in which to deal with children as they develop . That said,
however, it should also be noted that recommendations for childrearing made by religiously-
oriented "experts," do not always coincide with recommendations made by child development
One focus of considerable attention has been associations between parents' religiou s
beliefs and denominational affiliation and authoritarian parenting goals (e .g., children' s
spanking and other forms of physical discipline . Most of the literature on parenting styles an d
child outcomes suggests that authoritarian parenting has negative implications for children' s
democratic parenting practices (see Maccoby & Mart in, 1983 ) . Other work has indicated ,
however, that negative effects may be most likely to be found in white, middle class samples an d
that such effects may not generalize across age, race, sex, or socioeconomic groups (e .g . Gunnoe
authoritarian parenting styles, including strong emphasis on child obedience to parental authority
press ; Sherkat & Ellison, 1999) . In one recent study, Gershoff, Miller, & Holden (1999) found
that parents who belonged to Conservative Protestant denominations (e .g., Adventist, Baptist,
Pentecostal) were significantly more likely to spank or slap their three-year-old children than
were parents who were affiliated with Mainline Protestant denominations (e .g. Methodist,
Episcopal), Roman Catholics, or those reporting no religious affiliation . Indeed, 29% of the
Conservative Protest parents reported spanking their preschoolers three or more times per week,
19
compared with 5% or fewer of the parents in the other groups . This difference appeared to be
associated with a greater belief among the Conservative Protestant parents that corporal
punishment was an effective discipline technique that had few negative consequences .
Other researchers have reported findings similar to those of Gershoff and colleague s
(1999) . Ellison and his colleagues (Ellison, Bartkowski, & Segal, 1996) found an association
between denominational affiliation and use of corporal punishment, although the effect of
theological beliefs . Using data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth, Giles-Sims,
Straus, and Sugarman (1995) found that Catholics were significantly less likely to spank their
children than were mothers self-identifying as Protestant or Other . Mothers with no religious
affiliation were not significantly different from any other group . Mahoney and colleagues (in
press) summarized this literature on associations between religiosity and corporal punishment,
reporting that the body of research evidence supporting a link between parents' Christian
conservatism and positive attitudes toward corporal punishment is quite strong, although the link
with actual use of corporal punishment is less robust and tends to be more pronounced in studies
involving families with younger children . In contrast, Mahoney and colleagues found that
Fewer studies have examined associations between religiosity and parenting behavior s
and characteristics beyond discipline practices . Some researchers have suggested that although
theologically conservative parents may endorse corporal punishment and strict discipline, they
characteristics that have been found consistently to be associated with positive child outcomes
20
(see, for example, Maccoby & Martin, 1983) . Wilcox (1998) found support for this hypothesis
in a study of parents of preschool and school-age children interviewed as part of the 1987-1988
National Survey of Families and Households (NSFS) . In that study, parents who endorsed
conservative Christian beliefs reported more frequent hugging and praising of their preschool
Wilcox (2001) also examined data from a predominantly urban sample of parents o f
children aged 10 to 18, assessing parental denominational affiliation and aspects of family social
capital, including parental involvement with children, social control (i .e ., rule setting), and
closure (i .e., overlap in social networks, for example the extent to which parents know and talk to
the child's friends' parents) . Wilcox reported that parents affiliated with orthodox or
Catholics, Orthodox Jews) were substantially more likely to be in the top third of the sample on
involvement with their children, relative to parents who reported no religious affiliation . With
regard to social control, Fundamentalist and Evangelical Protestants were more likely to set a lot
of rules for their children, and Reform Jews were much less likely to do so, relative to non-
affiliated parents . For the closure measure, the most striking finding was an extremely high level
Mahoney and colleagues (in press) reported that few studies have been published linking
religiosity and parental warmth and positivity, but that the few existing studies were consistent in
reporting positive relations between these constructs . Gunnoe, Hetherington and Reiss (1999)
found that mothers' and fathers' self reported religiousness was positively associated with
views, by high levels of warmth and affection and lack of coercive behaviors, by demands fo r
appropriately mature child behavior, and by a willingness to exert control over the child whe n
necessary in the context of clearly-communicated, well-defined rules and regulations for chil d
behavior . Brody and colleagues (Brody, Stoneman, Flor, and McCray, 1994 ; Brody, Stoneman,
& Flor, 1996) conducted studies of family interactions in married, rural African-America n
families . In their work, greater maternal religiousness was associated with lower levels o f
negative parenting behaviors, including nattering and inconsistent parenting . Further, both
maternal and paternal religiousness were associated with greater co-parenting skills, includin g
lower interparental conflict. More positive co-parenting may, in turn, positively affect chil d
development due to decreased tension in the home and greater consistency in making an d
enforcing rules.
In addition to the direct effects of religiosity on parenting , parents' religiosity may have
indirect effects on parenting, by impacting marital relationships and parents' mental health i n
ways that promote or undermine positive parenting, and consequently child well-being .
press) found support for several links between religion and divorce . First, individuals wh o
reported membership in a religious denomination were less likely to divorce than wer e
unaffiliated individuals, although this link tended to disappear in studies that controlled for othe r
demographic factors associated with divorce . Second, church attendance was linked to lowe r
divorce rates (see also Bumpass , 2001 ; Miller, Warner, Wickramaratne, & Weissman, 1997) .
This was a stronger link than that between denomination and divorce, persisting even afte r
controlling for relevant demographic factors . Further, this link was found in longitudinal studies
22
as well as cross-sectional studies . Mahoney and colleagues put the strength of the effect of
religion on divorce in concrete terms, estimating that approximately 44% of frequent church-
goers have a history of divorce, compared with approximately 60% of infrequent church-goers .
Third, couples who had similar religious values and who had similar patterns of church
attendance were found to be less likely to divorce than were less homogeneous couples .
Mahoney and colleagues (in press) also conducted meta-analyses of studies addressing
links between religion and marital satisfaction . Overall, these researchers found that th e
hypothesis that religiosity was positively associated with marital satisfaction received strong
support . Bumpass (2001) similarly reported that married couples who hold conservative or
fundamentalist religious values and who attend church regularly were more likely to report bein g
satisfied with their marriages than were those holding less traditional values and who attended
church less frequently . Mahoney and colleagues further found that studies of the relations
between personal relevance of religion and marital satisfaction indicated stronger associations
than studies looking at denominational affiliation or attendance, but also that associations were
stronger in studies of church-goers than in general population studies (suggesting that religiosity
Some studies have also found links between religiosity and domestic conflict and
violence . With respect to verbal conflict, Mahoney and colleagues (in press) report that, across
studies, the overall effect size is zero-no significant association . With respect to physical
aggression, Mahoney and colleagues found no support for a positive link between religious
involvement and increased domestic violence, and further reported that two studies (Ellison
Bartkowski, & Anderson, 1999 ; Fergusson, Horwood, Kershaw, & Shannon, 1986) have
indicated that both men and women who reported higher levels of church attendance report less
23
violence than did people who attended less frequently . The findings of Ellison and colleagues,
however, also indicated a qualification to this general finding . These researchers found that men
who endorsed conservative religious beliefs and who were married to more theologically liberal
women were much more likely to exhibit physical aggression toward their spouses than were
Religion and mental health . Parents' religious beliefs and practices may affect their ow n
mental health and consequently their ability to deal with stresses associated with parenting .
Religion provides mechanisms for coping with adversity, and may serve as a source of self-
esteem and feelings of self efficacy . Spilka, Shaver, and Kirkpatrick (1985) noted that religions
provide believers with attributions with which to understand seemingly inexplicable events (see
also Oser, 1991) . Such religious attributions create meaning and may preserve or enhance self-
esteem by allowing the individual to perceive God's love and acceptance even under trying
circumstances .
measures of religiosity and emotional well-being in adulthood (e .g . Ellison, 1991 ; Ellison, 1993 ;
Miller et al., 1997 ; Ross, 1990 ; Sherkat & Ellison, 1999), although some studies have suggested
that these findings may be stronger among African Americans than among European Americans
(e.g., Blaine & Crocker, 1995) . For example, Miller and colleagues analyzed data from a 10-
year study of offspring at high and low risk for depression. These researchers found that mothers
who reported that religion was highly important to them were 81 % less likely to have a
concurrent clinical diagnosis of major depression than were less religious mothers . Interestingly,
a similar odds ratio was obtained indicating that Catholic mothers were less likely to experience
Parents who differ in their religious beliefs and involvement may model different kinds
of behaviors, beliefs, and values for their children. Religion has been found to be positively
associated with volunteering in adults (Sherkat & Ellison, 1999) . Thus, religious parents may be
more likely to act as models of highly prosocial behavior than are less religiously active parents .
spirituality, which may in turn affect other aspects of well-being . Parents both model and
directly teach religious behavior and beliefs, thus passing these beliefs on to their children, and
they may actively manage their children's lives so as to expose them to a social network with
shared religious beliefs and values (Cornwall, 1989 ; Sherkat & Ellison, 1999) . A number of
studies have reported significant concordance between parents' and children's religious beliefs
and denominational affiliation (e .g. Benson et al ., 1989 ; Francis & Gibson, 1993 ; King, et al .,
1997 ; Miller, et al ., 1997 ; Okagaki & Bevis, 1999) . Okagaki and Bevis found that female
college students' religious beliefs were consistent with their parents' religious beliefs . This was
particularly the case when parents talked frequently about their beliefs and mothers' and fathers'
parental beliefs), and when daughters also reported having warm relationships with their parents .
King and colleagues (1997) examined parental influences on stability and change i n
adolescents' religiosity from seventh to tenth grade, using data from 365 families participating in
the Iowa Youth and Families Project, a longitudinal study of White adolescents and their
families living in rural Iowa . Although far from a representative sample, the level of detailed
information on family functioning obtained in this study over a relatively long period of time
makes it an invaluable data source . These researchers conducted a series of multivariate logistic
25
regression analyses, controlling for sex, whether the family lived on a farm, and father's
adolescents who were highly religious in seventh and tenth grades, or who became more
religious over time, indicated strong identification with parents who were also highly religious
and whose religiousness tended to increase over the same time period . Compared to consistently
nonreligious adolescents, adolescents whose religiousness increased over time also tended to
show increased identification with their parents . In contrast, adolescents who became less
religious over time, compared to adolescents who were consistently religious, tended to have
Consistent with the more recent work of Okagaki and Bevis (1999) and King an d
(Benson et at ., 1989) indicated that religious activity in the home, in conjunction with loving and
supportive relationships with parents who provided clear messages regarding religious beliefs
and practices, promoted high levels of religiousness on the part of adolescents (see also Sherkat
& Ellison, 1999) . This body of literature is thus supportive of some components of Kirkpatrick's
attachment theoretical approach to religious development (e .g. Kirkpatrick, 1997 ; Kirkpatrick &
Shaver, 1990) .
As noted earlier, most studies of religiosity in adolescence (and the very few studies of
religiosity in childhood) use snapshot measures of religious practice and beliefs . Further, no
measures of religiosity unique to children were found in our review . In the rare studies that do
include preadolescent children, measures tend to be adaptations of the same measures used with
older adolescents and adults . Gorsuch (1984) noted that, in the literature on religiosity and well-
26
being, there is an almost exclusive reliance on questionnaire measures . Additionally, the items
used to tap religiosity are highly variable across studies . Although Gorsuch's report was
published nearly two decades ago, the situation remains essentially the same .
Among the most frequently assessed aspects of religiosity are attendance at religiou s
services, denominational affiliation, and the importance or salience of religion . Most frequently,
these measures are obtained using one or a few items . In their review of social science studies of
religion and family functioning, Mahoney and colleagues (in press) reported that 80% of studies
involving marital relations and 66% of studies involving parenting relied on single ite m
indicators of religiousness . Although Gorsuch (1984) suggested that the use of single-item
measures is justified due to high reliabilities of longer questionnaires, single-item measures have
been criticized on a number of grounds, particularly for a tendency to make religion appear to be
an "all or none" construct . Most frequently, these measures are dichotomous or are responded to
Even when single item measures are not dichotomous, response patterns frequently resul t
religiosity . For example, few American adults self-report having no religious affiliation and
most respondents in studies examining religion and family functioning self-report moderate
dichotomizing responses to reflect a "high" versus "low" distinction . Similarly, most studies
cannot include members of non-Christian religions at all, due in part to low representation but
religious practice and belief. A frequently-used theoretical and empirical distinction is between
intrinsic and extrinsic religiousness, as described by Allport and Ross (1967) and as assessed via
measures such as Allport and Ross's Religious Orientation Scale (ROS), Gorsuch and Venable's
(1983) "Age Universal" revision of the ROS (as well as further abbreviations and even a single-
item version suggested by Gorsuch and McPherson, 1989) . Intrinsic religious orientatio n
individual and the extent to which orthodox religious doctrine is accepted unquestioningly .
groups and activities for reasons that have little to do with religious faith . Although Allport and
Ross indicated a single extrinsic scale, factor analytic studies have generally indicated that there
are at least two factors (Gorsuch & McPherson, 1989 ; Kirkpatrick, 1989 ; Kirkpatrick & Hood,
1990) . One of these factors, extrinsic-social religious orientation, includes items involving the
use of religion as a means toward social gain (e .g ., establishing business contacts, achieving
social status) . The second factor, extrinsic-personal, involves the use of religion to gain comfort
and security (e .g ., friendships, sense of belonging) . This set of three scales has been used in a
Strayhorn, Weidman, and Larson (1990) . Strayhorn and colleagues included twelve items
tapping self-perceptions of religiousness and having a personal relationship with God, frequency
of private religious activities (e .g ., prayer, Bible study), and involvement with church (e .g.,
28
factor analysis indicating that the twelve items loaded onto two factors that could roughly be
described as a "private religion" factor and a "public religion" factor . The private religion factor
included seven items tapping perceptions of a personal relationship with God, of experiencing
approval and disapproval from God, experiencing a religious goal or purpose in life, and of
relying on God for strength and help. The public religion factor included five items tapping
general religiousness, Bible study, and church attendance, donations, and leadership .
assessments of religiosity was achieved for the first wave of data collection of the National
representative sample of over 9,000 adolescents ages 12 to 16 when initially surveyed . Six items
dichotomous items tapping parents' religiosity were included in the parent survey . One of the
six NLSY97 items tapped the frequency with which parents attended church over the previous 12
months (less than once a month vs . once a month or more) . Other items involved statement s
about religion . Parents were asked to indicate whether each statement was true or not true for
them . One item directly tapped religious fundamentalism-the extent to which the parent
thought that the Bible, Koran, Torah, or religious teachings should be obeyed exactly as written .
Another item tapped a belief that religion was necessary for good character or moral values . The
remaining three items appear to tap a personal relationship with, and reliance on God-seeking
God's help in making decisions, frequent prayer, and a belief in God's direct influence on the
Trends (2001), the six parent religiosity items can be combined to create a single religiosity
measure with acceptable psychometric properties (i .e ., with moderate internal consistency and
29
evidence of both construct and predictive validity) . To date, however, no reports have been
published using this scale or any of the individual religiosity items from the NLSY97 .
Although scales can tap more refined aspects of religion than can single-item measures
they have continued to be criticized for failing to adequately distinguish different aspects of
religion and spirituality that may have differential effects on physical and mental health (George
et al., 2000) . Even more problematic is the fact that although multiple scales can be constructed,
it is frequently the case that only a single scale composite is actually used in research . Strayhorn
and colleagues (1990), for example, used a single 12-item composite of religiousness to
investigate associations between parental religiousness and parent and child mental health
variables (described below), despite their factor analytic results indicating two distinct
religiousness factors .
In summary, there is much room for improvement in the measurement of religiosity . The
research literature would benefit from greater standardization of terminology and measures, and
greater attention needs to be paid to the inclusion of minority religions, particularly non-
The vast majority of studies on religion and well-being have been cross-sectional studies
with adolescents . In reviewing the literature for this report, no studies were found examining
delinquency, drug and alcohol use and abuse, adolescent sexual activity, prosocial and mora l
adolescent well-being involves associations between religion and delinquency . An early study
(Hirschi & Stark, 1969) reported finding no significant effect of religion on delinquency . Since
that time, however, researchers have disputed these findings and have generally found that hig h
levels of religiosity are associated with low levels of delinquency, although associations are
religiosity in a random sample of 600 white adolescents attending high school in one larg e
suburban district. Elifson and colleagues found that multiple indicators of adolescent religiosity ,
particularly religious salience (i .e., importance of religion in daily life), belief that God answer s
prayers , and religious orthodoxy, were negatively associated with delinquency measures,
including a general delinquency composite and measures of alcohol and marijuana use .
However, they further found, using multiple regression procedures, that the association betwee n
religious salience (the strongest individual predictor among religiosity variables) and genera l
delinquency measure was nonsignificant when controlling for other factors such as the numbe r
of friends who smoke marijuana, closeness to mother, and high school grade point average .
Donohue and Benson (1995) reported results from a sample of 34,129 adolescents draw n
from a larger sample of 250,000 public school students surveyed between 1989 and 1994 as par t
Institute . Low to moderate signi ficant correlations were found between scores on a six-item
31
Some researchers have suggested that relatively weak associations may be due to the fact
that there are numerous social and moral forces apart from religious influences that proscribe
antisocial activities such as theft and crimes against persons . From this perspective, the
expectation is that antiascetic (i .e ., religiously proscribed) activities, such as drinking, drug use,
and sexual behavior, are likely to be more strongly negatively associated with religiosity than are
other acts, such as interpersonal violence, vandalism, and theft (Evans, et al ., 1996 ; McBride, et
al., 1996) .
Evans and colleagues conducted a study to test this hypothesis by administering a surve y
to a sample of students attending one public high school . These researchers used two
delinquency measures . The first was a general crime scale including 57 items. The second was
an antiascetic subscale of the general crime scale, composed of 13 items focusing on smoking,
drinking, gambling, sex, and profanity . Religiosity was assessed with a composite measure
religious services and social activities ; 2) religious salience, concerning the extent to which th e
adolescent felt that religious beliefs impacted his or her daily behavior ; and 3) hellfire, involving
beliefs in and fear of Hell and God's punishment for wrongdoing . An additional measure of
adolescents' religious networks was a single item asking how many of the adolescent's five best
friends attended church regularly. Finally, measures of secular social bonds to parents, teachers,
friends, school, and society (in the form of conformity and conventional beliefs) were included in
multiple regression models, with the expectation that inclusion of these measures of social
attachments would diminish associations between religiosity and general delinquency, more so
32
than associations between religiosity and antiascetic delinquency . When social attachment
measures were included within models predicting delinquency, results indicated that only peer
religiosity was significantly associated with delinquency, and associations were significant only
for antiascetic delinquency . In other words, religiosity was a relatively weak correlate of
delinquency, compared to social attachments, and only for those forms typically proscribed by
religious doctrine .
Drug use . In contrast to studies involving general delinquency, and consistent with th e
work of Evans and colleagues (1996) cited above, the research evidence is quite consistent in
finding negative associations between measures of religiosity and drug and alcohol use in
adolescence (e.g., Benson et al ., 1989 ; Booth & Martin, 1998 ; Brownfield & Sorenson, 1991 ;
Donahue & Benson, 1995 ; Sang & Johnson, 2001) . Donahue and Benson included both a revie w
of the literature on religion and adolescent well-being, and new analyses examining relations
between religiosity and a variety of indicators of prosocial and risk-taking behaviors, including
drug use . The new analyses utilized data from surveys conducted with more than 29,000
adolescents across the United States (for study details, see Benson, 1993) . These researchers
found low to moderate, significant correlations between religiosity questions regarding alcohol
consumption and binge drinking, marijuana use, and cigarette smoking . Of the three religiosity
measures used by Donahue and Benson, attendance at religious services was the most strongly
associated with substance use, indicating that adolescents who attended religious services more
frequently were less likely to drink, smoke, and use marijuana than were less religiously
involved adolescents . Donahue and Benson further assessed the unique impact of religiousness
(a composite of the frequency of attendance measure and measures of hours per week spent at
church programs or services and the importance of religion in the adolescent's life) on adolescent
33
well-being . Controlling for educational ability, educational aspirations, gender, grade, and
whether the adolescent lived in a single parent family, religiousness continued to have modes t
but significant negative associations with recent alcohol use, binge drinking, marijuana use, an d
smoking .
Brownfield and Sorenson (1991) examined relations between religion and drug use in a
sample of white high school-age males , including a large percentage of adolescents who had
police or court records (two-thirds of their sample). These researchers proposed that re ligious
institutions reduce the potential for drug abuse primarily by acting as sources of social support ,
affiliation), church attendance, and religiosity (i .e., how religious the adolescent feels he is) .
All of the religiosity measures used by Brownfield and Sorenson (1991) were
significantly and negatively associated with drug use . Relations with church attendance wer e
particularly strong . Only 6 .4% of the adolescents who repo rted attending church at least once a
week reported the highest level of drug use (use of alcohol, marijuana, barbiturates o r
methedrine, psychedelics, and cocaine), while 22 .5% of those who never attended church
services reported this level of use . Almost twice as many of the adolescents who reported bein g
"not religious at all" used all five drug types than did adolescents who reported being "ver y
Brownfield and Sorenson also created a social support composite including the thre e
religion measures and a measure of communication with father . This social support composite
was found to be significantly associated with drug use, even when controlling for the effect o f
peer attachment (sharing thoughts and feelings with best friend), and the much stronger effect o f
peer delinquency (whether the respondent's best friend had ever been picked up by the police) on
34
drug use . Although support for the specific social support hypothesis is less than compelling in
this report given the minimal difference between religion measures and the social suppor t
In a recent study, Jang and Johnson (2001) examined the effects of religiosity an d
neighborhood disorder on illicit drug use in a sample of over 1,000 adolescents, drawn fro m
participants in the National Youth Survey (NYS). The NYS is a longitudinal investigation of a
in 1977. Data used in this study were taken from waves three through five, when questions
regarding religiosity were included in the survey . These researchers examined several
marijuana and of "hard drugs ." They hypothesized that the effects of neighborhood disorder o n
drug use would be partially explained by the effects of neighborhood disorder on individua l
religiosity (i .e ., that religiosity would mediate the effects of neighborhood disorder on drug use) .
They further hypothesized that effects of religiosity on drug use would be stronger in more
disordered neighborhoods (i .e., that the level of neighborhood would moderate the effects o f
religiosity on drug use) . Additional hypotheses examined in this study were that the effect o f
religiosity in reducing the risk of illicit drug use would not be entirely accounted for by social
bonding ( i.e., att achment to and involvement with family, and with school) or modeling (i .e.,
association with drug-using peers and pro-drug attitudes), and that the effect of religiosity o n
In a series of sophisticated analyses controlling for sex, race, class , living in an intact
family, and family size, Jang and Johnson (2001) found support for each of their hypotheses .
Religiosity, assessed as a composite of two items reflecting attendance at religious services and
35
the personal importance of religion, was negatively associated with both marijuana and hard drug
use. Further, inclusion of religiosity in models predicting drug use substantially lowered the
association between neighborhood disorder and drug use, while the effects of religiosity on drug
use were found to be greater for adolescents living in more disorganized neighborhoods . A
slight age effect was found indicating a general trend for religiosity to have a greater impact on
drug use in later adolescence, although this effect was quite small . Finally, when social bonding
and modeling variables were included in analyses, the effect of religiosity on drug use decreased
in size, but remained significant . This is particularly impressive given that association with drug
using peers, and having pro-drug attitudes were particularly strong predictors of drug use .
Sexual behavior. In their reviews, Benson and colleagues (1989), Donahue and Benson
(1995), and Sherkat and Ellison (1999) report that the literature on relations between adolescent
sexual behavior and religiosity indicates that religiosity has a strong negative association with
the likelihood of being sexually active (see also reports on trends in adolescent sexual behavior
and reproductive health by Manlove, Terry-Humen, Papillo, Franzetta, Williams, & Ryan, 2001,
and Moore, Miller, Glei, & Morrison, 1995) . Researchers have found associations between high
levels of religiosity (including church attendance, valuing religion, and religious beliefs) and
more conservative sexual attitudes and lower levels of sexual experience (e .g. Afxentiou &
Hawley, 1997 ; Bearman & Bruckner, 2001 ; Cooksey, Rindfuss, and Guilkey, 1996 ; Donahue &
Benson, 1995 ; Durant, Pendergrast, & Seymore, 1990 ; Thornton & Camburn, 1989) . Findings
regarding contraception and disease-prevention have been less consistent (see Moore et al .,
1995). Finally, Benson and colleagues reported that some studies suggest that the constraining
adolescents . Further, this research has been conducted predominantly with female adolescents ,
and less is known about associations between religiosity and adolescent male sexual behavior .
In one study to include both males and females, Donahue and Benson reported low t o
attendance at religious services, and having ever had sexual intercourse . Donahue and Benson
further found a significant association between their composite religiousness measure and
adolescents' reports of having ever had sexual intercourse, controlling for a series of relevant
demographic characteristics (see above, p . 31) . Curiously, despite the large sample size thes e
researchers did not include controls for race or ethnicity, and no analyses were conducte d
examining whether the constraining effects of religiousness applied similarly across genders or
Cooksey and colleagues (1996) used data from two cycles of the National Survey o f
Family Growth (NSFG), conducted in the early and late 1980s , to assess the association between
use. Each cycle included retrospective questions spanning the previous 48 months, asking
respondents to indicate timing of first intercourse and contraceptive method use at firs t
intercourse . Data from females between the ages of 10 and 19 at the start of each cycle (4 8
months prior to the survey) were included in analyses . Cooksey and colleagues found that both
Catholic and Protestant Fundamentalists were more likely to be virgins at the start of the time
period under study than were teens categorized as "Others" (other religious denominations or n o
religious denomination) . The likelihood of retaining virginity across the 4-year time span was
higher for Protestant Fundamentalists than for Others as well . There was also a smaller but stil l
significant positive effect for white adolescents of being Catholic on likelihood of remaining a
37
virgin, but not for blacks . Interestingly, Cooksey and colleagues found that the increase d
likelihood of remaining a virgin associated with being Protestant Fundamentalist was greater for
white adolescents interviewed during the later cycle (in 1988) than for those interviewed durin g
the earlier cycle. No similar trend was found for black adolescents . These researchers
suggesting that the increased emphasis on promoting abstinence that developed within
Fundamentalist Protestant churches during the 1980s did have its intended effect.
Cooksey and colleagues (1996) further found that use of contraceptives . at first
intercourse was lower among Catholic and Fundamentalist Protest adolescents than amon g
Others . The negative association between religion and likelihood of contraceptive use was
particularly strong for white Catholics and for black Protestant Fundamentalists . These finding s
were consistent with earlier reports of a negative association between religiosity and
contraceptive use at first intercourse (e .g. Kahn, Rindfuss, & Guilkey, 1990) . The results o f
these studies thus present a somewhat mixed picture-adolescent girls are likely to delay onset
of sexual behavior longer if they are affiliated with a religion that strongly discourage s
premarital sex . On the other hand, if sex does occur, risk of pregnancy and exposure to sexually
In a recent study, Bearman and Bruckner (2001) used data from the Adolescent Healt h
Interview Study (Add Health) to examine associations between religiosity , Add health is a larg e
nationally representative survey of male and female adolescents in grades 7 through 12 . Usin g
data from approximately 5,600 nonblack (white, Asian, and Hispanic) adolescents, these
church attendance, and personal importance of religion) and having taken a virginity pledge both
uniquely contributed to reducing the risk of initiation of sexual intercourse, even after controlling
38
for numerous demographic characteristics, including sex, ethnicity, family structure, and SES, a s
well as measures of connectedness to parents, peers, and romantic partners . In contrast, separate
analyses conducted with nearly 1,000 black adolescents did not indicate significant association s
between either religiosity or virginity pledges and transition to first intercourse . No significant
differences in religiosity and pledge effects for males and females were found for either blacks or
nonblacks .
Two recent studies with Latina adolescents run counter to these findings of influences o f
religiosity on early sexual behavior . Villarruel (1998) examined the relationships of a variety of
cultural influences, including religion, with sexual attitudes, beliefs, and norms in a small sampl e
of 10 to 15 year-old Latina adolescents living in urban Latino communities . Using focus group s
and qualitative analyses, Villarruel found that religion was not self-reported to be an importan t
influence on their decisions about sexual behavior . Rather, the importance of virginity was tied .
more strongly to issues of self-respect . These findings are likely to be influenced by the rathe r
homogeneous nature of the sample of Puerto Rican and Mexican-American girls . Further the
Using more traditional quantitative methods, Durant and colleagues (1990) found tha t
more religious Hispanic female adolescents had lower levels of sexual experience and mor e
conservative sexual attitudes than did less religious adolescents . Baumeister, Flores, and Marin
(1995), however, found that the importance of religion and religious denomination (i .e., being
Catholic) were not significant unique predictors of not being pregnant in their sample of Latina
adolescents , controlling for the influences of other predictors, notably information provided by
mothers about sexuality, being from an intact family, and having a positive attitude towar d
school. Thus, similar to findings summarized by Benson and colleagues (1989) for black
39
behavior among Latina and Hispanic adolescent females appear to be more mixed than ar e
Although the bulk of the literature has focused on female adolescents, a few studies hav e
attitudes. In one such study, Halpern, Udry, Campbell, Suchindran, and Mason (1994) found
effects consistent with those reported in other studies for girls . Specifically, high levels of
religious attendance was associated with a delay in the onset of -sexual activity and mor e
conservative sexual attitudes and behavior in a longitudinal sample of 13- to 16-year-olds . These
researchers also found an interesting additive effect of religious attendance and free testosterone
levels (as measured through blood plasma assays) : Boys with high levels of testosterone wh o
attended church infrequently demonstrated the most permissive sexual attitudes and behaviors ,
while boys with low levels of testosterone and more frequent church attendance were the mos t
sexually conservative .
attitudes and behavior, and research findings are consistent in supporting a positive associatio n
between these constructs (Benson et at, 1989 ; Donahue & Benson, 1995) . In one rare study with
preadolescent children, Kedem and Cohen (1987) examined the impact of religious education o n
moral judgment in a sample of Israeli Jewish second and sixth grade students attending tw o
public schools---one religious, one secular . Religious topics are taught in both types o f
programs, but the number of hours are much greater in religious schools, and educationa l
methods differ as well . Kedem and Cohen state that "The aim of the Jewish studies in the
40
religious school is internalization of the commandments (the ritual and ethical rules), while th e
aim. . . in the secular school is knowledge of the ethnic roots and identification with the past of th e
nation " (p. 9). Thus, attendance at a secular versus a religious school is essentially a parent-
with a series of vignettes in which children face moral choices involving, for example, stealing ,
cheating, and aggression. Children were then asked to choose a behavioral response to the
dilemma, and to explain why that behavior was selected . Based on their responses, childre n
were given a "moral behavior" score and a "moral judgment" score . These researches found that
sixth grade children in the religious school had significantly higher (more mature) moral
judgment and moral behavior scores than the sixth graders attending secular school . No
Donahue and Benson (1995) included two sets of questions in their survey involving
prosocial behavior . One set, combined to form an altruistic values scale, included question s
about the importance of helping others, reducing hunger and poverty, and making the world a
better place . Three other items tapped altruistic behavior, including participation in charitabl e
activities, donating money or time to charity, and spending time helping others. Donahue and
altruistic behavior . These associations remained relatively strong even after controlling fo r
behavior than between religiousness and altruistic values may be due in part to the frequent
inclusion of service activities within religious education and youth fellowship programs .
41
King and Furrow (2001) used a questionnaire format to examine associations between
religious attendance, social capital, and moral outcomes in a large, ethnically-diverse sample o f
high school students . Social capital variables included social interaction, trust, and share d
vision, each assessed within three relationship domains : parents, friends, and an important non-
familial adult. Moral outcomes included self-reported altruism, empathy, and voluntary servic e
activities . Regular church attendees were found to be higher than non-attendees on all socia l
capital measures except having a trusting relationship with parents . Students who reporte d
sometimes attending religious services had social capital levels that were between those o f
regular attendees and non-attendees and were significantly lower than regular attendees on six
measures involving social interaction and shared vision with parents, friends, and a non-familial
adult. King and Furrow also reported that religious attendance was significantly associated wit h
altruism and empathy, but not with volunteer activities, controlling for demographic differences
(e .g., age, gender, ethnicity, parental education) and self-reported participation in extracurricular
school activities. However, social capital resources were found to explain more variance i n
altruism and empathy than did religiousness. This suggests that religious participation may
personality and mental health, including self-esteem, depression and suicide. Associations with
self-esteem in adolescence are generally low and frequently nonsignificant (e .g., Bahr & Martin ,
1983; Benson et at., 1989; Donahue & Benson , 1995) . Although Donahue and Benson did find
significant simple and unique associations between religiousness and self-esteem, these effects
were quite small and were significant primarily due to the very large sample size used in their
42
analyses . Bahr and Martin used questionnaire data from approximately 450 high school student s
to examine associations between measures of church attendance and evangelicalism and self-
esteem and faith in people (i .e., belief that people are basically helpful and trustworthy, versus
self-centered and untrustworthy) . Their results indicated that church attendance had a low but
significant association with faith in people, but not with self-esteem . Evangelicalism was not
significantly associated with either outcome variable, although a low but significant negative
association was found with faith in people once high school grades, parental socioeconomi c
including frequency of attendance at religious services (once a month or more versus attendanc e
on important religious holidays or less often), participation in a Bible study group (yes versu s
no), and participation in a church youth group (yes versus no), and positive psychosocial
(n=63) 11th grade students . The psychosocial outcome measures examined by Markstro m
included ethnic identity, general and school-related self-esteem, and ego strength and eg o
identity status formation, as described by Erikson (e .g., 1968, 1980) . Eight ego strength s
identified by Erikson were assessed using modified Likert-type scales : hope, will, purpose ,
competence , fidelity, love, care, and wisdom . Additionally, a total ego strength score combining
all eight scales was included. The ego identity status measure was a continuous measure o f
domain, and subsequent commitment to a set of beliefs, values , or goals. Results indicated that
there were significant main effects of all three measures of religious involvement on school-
43
related self-esteem, with more religiously involved adolescents demonstrating higher self-estee m
than less involved youth . In addition, there was a significant main effect of church attendance o n
the ego strength of will ( i.e., awareness of free will and the ability to exert self-control) .
and ethnicity, and between involvement and gender. However, these results were not consistent
and the numbers of students in some cells were so small as to make these results somewha t
suspect . In particular, the vast majority (89%) of African-American adolescents were in the
"more frequent attendance" group, leaving only 7 (11%) in the "less frequent attendance" group .
In contrast, European-American adolescents were almost equally represented in the two religiou s
attendance groups (51 % more frequent and 49% less frequent attendees). With respect to the
involvement, Markstrom suggested that the relationship may not be causal, but rather tha t
Wright, Frost, and Wisecarver (1993) conducted a questionnaire study investigating link s
white, Hispanic, and African-American high school students . Students were asked about thei r
beliefs for their everyday lives. Analyses were conducted separately for males and female s
because females were found to both attend church more often and to have higher depressio n
scores than males. Despite these mean level differences, results for males and females were
consistent : Adolescents who attended church more frequently, and those who viewed their
44
religion as providing meaning to their lives, had lower depression scores than did less religiou s
adolescents .
There is little research that addresses associations between suicide and religiosity in
adolescence . In their study described earlier , Donohue and Benson (1995) reported that
associations between religiosity measures and questions pertaining to suicide attempts and
ideation ranged from - .05 to -.11 . Associations between religiousness and suicidal ideation an d
statistically significant due to the very large sample size, however, these correlations suggest that
the positive impact of religiosity in preventing suicidal thoughts and attempts is quite modest .
Brega and Coleman (1999) investigated associations between two measures of religiosit y
and subjective stigmatization in a sample of 50 African American high school sophomores and
juniors. Subjective stigmatization involves the extent to which an individual adopts negative
stereotypes and attitudes about his or her own stigmatized group . In this case, subjective
stigmatization refers to the adolescents' acceptance of negative racial stereotypes as being true .
Subjective stigmatization has been found to be associated with low self-worth . The measures of
religiosity used in this study were somewhat different from typical measures . Church attachment
question "What do you think about the black church?" Church attendance was coded fro m
responses to the question "Would you go to church if it were left completely up to you?" and wa s
scored on a six-point scale ranging from 1) already not attending church, to 6) already atten d
church by own choice. Thus, this measure of attendance also has a strong element of impo rtanc e
in it as well . Brega and Coleman found that their measure of church attendance was th e
strongest predictor of subjective stigmatization included within their model . Church attendees
45
were significantly less stigmatized than were non-attendees, even when controlling for severa l
measures of racial socialization . Church attachment , in contrast , was not uniquely associated
A few studies have examined associations between adolescent religiosity and educationa l
aspirations . Dai (1996 ) examined data from 13,530 high school seniors who participated in the
1989 Monitoring the Future survey. Using analyses of variance to compare students who self-
reported different levels of attendance at religious services (very involved, involved, rarel y
involved, or not involved), Dai found that more religiously involved students were less likely t o
aspire to vocational school attendance and more likely to aspire to attend four-year colleges and
graduate school than were less involved students . The conceptual rationale for expecting this
association was not clearly laid out in this study, but it is likely that religious involvement an d
educational aspirations may be associated because of underlying common causes, rather tha n
Summary
evidence appears to be particularly good for links with drug use and possibly with early sexual
activity, although the evidence is better for white female adolescents than for males or fo r
females of other ethnicities . The least compelling evidence may be for associations betwee n
religiosity and self-esteem and other aspects of mental health . While positive associations tend
to be fairly weak, however, there is clearly no indication of negative effects of religiosity on any
aspect of well-being . A number of studies have suggested that effects of religiosity on well-
46
attendance--on social capital and social support . The extent to which effects of religiosity o n
well-being are indirect versus direct, possible age-related differences in the extent to which direc t
versus indirect effects are found, and gender and ethnic group differences are research topics tha t
There are a substantial number of studies linking parents' religiosity with parenting
behavior . Several of these studies were described earlier and were summarized in a recent report
by Mahoney and colleagues ( in press). Far fewer studies have examined linkages between
parents' religiosity and child and adolescent outcomes . In one study to do so, Strayhorn et al .
(1990) examined associations between parents' religiousness and a number of parent and child
mental health variables . The sample used in this study included 201 preschool-age childre n
participating in Head Start programs . These children were from low-income families and were
child- report measures . Teachers also rated child classroom behavior, and parent-child
interactions were videotaped and coded as an assessment of positive parenting behavior . Result s
indicated that parents ' religiousness demonstrated low but signi ficant correlations with parental
hostility and with a measure of positive parenting behavior, but was uncorrelated with a
composite measure of parent- and teacher-reported child problem behaviors or with chil d
A few studies with older children have found associations between parents' religiosit y
and child well-being . Some of these studies have specifically hypothesized indirect effects of
47
parents' religiosity on child well-being, through effects of religiosity on parenting . Brody and
colleagues (1996) examined direct and indirect effects of parental religiosity on child
competence in a sample of rural, two-parent African American families with children between
the ages of 9 and 12 . Child competence was assessed via measures of self-control, academic
researchers predicted that competence would be directly predicted by parental religiosity, and
indirectly by the effects of parents' religiosity on family cohesion and interparental conflict .
church attendance with a measure of importance of religion . Family cohesion was assessed as
levels of harmony (versus conflict) and engagement of family members during videotaped triadic
interactions . Results of analyses conducted separately for mothers and fathers indicated that
parent religiosity was positively correlated with family cohesiveness and interparental conflict,
although associations were stronger for fathers than for mothers . Cohesion and conflict were
both significantly associated with child self-control, which was in turn predictive of academic
competence and both internalizing and externalizing behavior problems . These results provided
support for the researchers' hypothesis that parental religiosity affects child outcomes indirectly,
by influencing the quality of family relationships . In addition to these indirect effects, however,
some direct associations were found as well : Parents' religiosity scores were directly and
negatively associated with child externalizing behavior problems, and fathers' religiosity was
associated with interparental conflict and with child internalizing behavior problems .
Gunnoe and colleagues (1999) similarly hypothesized that parental religiosity would
affect adolescent social responsibility both directly and indirectly, through the effect that
religiosity has on parenting . The study involved two-parent families (predominantly white) with
48
children between 10 and 18 years of age . Parental religiosity was assessed via a short
questionnaire tapping the extent to which religion was part of and influenced family and personal
life. Authoritarian and authoritative parenting behavior was coded from videotapes of 10-minute
problem-solving sessions in which parent-adolescent pairs were asked to discuss topics that were
was assessed via parent- and self-report measures . Mother-report was used for analyses
involving fathers' religiosity and authoritative parenting, while father-report was used for
responsibility . Both maternal and paternal authoritative parenting were positively predicted by
religiosity, and maternal authoritarian parenting was negatively associated with religiosity . More
religious parents had adolescents who were reported to be more socially responsible, and this
effect was partially mediated by effects of religiosity on the tendency to exhibit characteristically
Summary. As with the literature on adolescent's own religiosity and well-being, there is
some empirical support for weak to moderate links between parents' religiosity and child well-
being . These effects are partially mediated by the influences of parents' religiosity on parenting
behavior . Again, hypotheses of negative effects of parent religiosity on child outcomes have
generally not received empirical support . There is only a rather limited body of research that
assesses both parents' religiosity and child outcomes, however, and substantially more work is
needed.
49
Conclusions
indicates small but consistent associations between religious upbringing and positive social and
emotional functioning, although some research has suggested that findings may be stronger for
white adolescents than for African-American, Latino or Hispanic adolescents, particularly in the
area of sexual behavior . Although gender differences have frequently been found in mean levels
of religious participation and salience (with females typically being higher on both types of
measures), consistent gender differences in the effects of religiosity on well-being have not been
reported. Unfortunately, many studies (particularly those involving delinquency and risk taking
behavior) have not included both males and females in order to allow comparisons .
Although very little research has been conducted with young children, there is som e
indication that religiosity effects may be stronger for preadolescents and adolescents than for
younger children . This would not be surprising, given the probability that self-reports of
religiosity are likely to be less meaningful for children than for older individuals . Religious
beliefs and understandings change dramatically across childhood and adolescence, only
beginning to resemble those of adults in the preadolescent and early adolescent periods (e .g.
Elkind, 1970; Fowler, 1991 ; Oser, 1991) . Further, church attendance is not necessarily a
voluntary activity for children and thus self-reports of religious attendance may not have the
Finally, most of the research on religion and well-being in childhood and adolescence
suggests that effects are partially mediated by effects of religiosity on characteristics of the
characteristics, and the social support that is provided to children and adolescents by peers and
There are number of serious limitations to this research that need to be addressed in
future work . In addition to concerns regarding the diversity of measures used across studies and
the limitations of single-item measures to tap complex constructs such as religiosity, there are
deeper issues as well . Research on non-Christian minority religions in the United States is
et al ., in press) . Part of the reason for this is the small numbers of adherents to these religions in
the United States, relative to the Christian majority . Another reason is that most measures of
religious salience have been specifically designed to examine religiosity among Christians, and
Another somewhat related concern is the lack of research on spirituality, apart from
religion. The studies cited in this report tap individuals' participation in religious organizations
and activities, and the personal importance individuals place on religion . Because of the ways in
which religiosity is assessed, few if any sltudies have the ability to distinguish between
adolescents for whom spiritual matters are unimportant and those who may have strong spiritual
beliefs but do not adhere to any particular religion . Thus we know little about the effects of
differences in the processes of religious and spiritual thought, apart from snapshots of religious
activity and beliefs . Further, the lack of longitudinal studies makes it impossible to track th e
development of religiosity and spirituality across childhood and adolescence or the predictors
and antecedents of changes in religiosity and spirituality across development . In order to gain a
better grasp of the value of religiosity and spirituality for positive well-being, it will be necessary
to conduct longitudinal studies that are also more inclusive with respect to ethnicity and religion .
51
Implications
There are a number of implications of this literature for prevention and intervention
efforts aimed at improving outcomes for children and adolescents . The consistent finding of a
restraining influence of religion on drug and alcohol abuse, in conjunction with the fact that a
large majority of adolescents report some level of ongoing involvement with a religious
institution (Lindberg, Boggess, & Williams, 2000), supports the view that religious communities
have an important contribution to make in drug and alcohol abuse prevention efforts, as well as
in treatment of drug and alcohol dependency (see McBride et al ., 1996) . However, it should
also be noted that findings indicating that religiosity effects are mediated by characteristics of the
social environment indicate that religion is one path to well-being and is not necessarily the only
The work of Garbarino (e .g ., Garbarino & Bedard, 1996) seems a particularly appropriat e
way to conclude this report, in wake of the tragic events of September 11, 2001 . Garbarino has
focused much of his work on children experiencing both acute and chronic trauma, such as war .
Consistent with Fowler's (1991) work on the innate human need for faith, and Oser's (1991)
work on the development of religious judgements that allow individuals to reconcile religious
faith with seemingly contradictory realities, Garbarino emphasizes the necessity for attending to
the spiritual needs of children who have experienced traumatic events . As stated by Garbarino &
Bedard,
What does a spiritual analysis imply for intervention? It implies that we embrace
psychological, even physiological modalities in understanding trauma (and in
intervening on behalf of the traumatized individuals and groups), but only as part
of a larger strategy for healing the wounds of trauma .. . But a perspective which is
sensitive to the spiritual essence of children and youth tells us to be prepared to go
further, to recognize the need to deal with the metaphysical wounds that arise
from the overwhelming cognitions, to deal practically with the eclipse of God (or
Buddha, or Allah, or Jehovah) itself. To do this we must embrace the spiritual
52
References
Afxentiou, D., & Hawley, C .B. (1997) . Explaining female teenagers' sexual behavior an d
Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall (1978) . Patterns of attachment : A psychological study of
Allport, G .W., & Ross, J.M (1967) . Personal religious orientation and prejudice . Journal of
Bahr, H .M., & Martin, T.K. (1983) . "And thy neighbor as thyself': Self-esteem and faith in
people as correlates of religiosity and family solidarity among Middletown high schoo l
Baumeister, L ., Flores, E., & Marin, B . (1995) . Sex information given to Latina adolescents b y
Bearman, P .S., & Bruckner, H . (2001) . Promising the future : Virginity pledges and first
Benson, P .L . (1993) . The troubled journey : A portrait of 6`h-12`4 grade youth. Minneapolis:
Search Institute .
Benson , P .L., Donahue, M .J ., & Erickson, J .A. (1989) . Adolescence and religion : A review o f
the literature from 1970 to 1986. Research in the Social Scientific Study of Religion, 1 ,
153-181 .
Blaine , B., & Crocker, J . (1995) . Religiousness , race , and psychological well-being : Explorin g
social psychological mediators . Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1031-
1041 .
54
Booth, J., & Martin, J .E. (1998) . Spiritual and religious factors in substance use, dependence ,
and recovery, In H .G. Koenig (ed.), Handbook of religion and mental health (pp . 175-
Bowlby, J . (1969) . Attachment and loss, vol. I.• Attachment. New York : Basic .
Brega, A.G., & Coleman, L .M. (1999) . Effects of religiosity and racial socialization o n
223-242 .
internal working models . In J .D. Osofsky ( ed .), Handbook of infant development (2"d
Brody, G .H ., Stoneman, Z ., & Flor, D. (1996) . Parental religiosity, family processes, and yout h
32, 696-706 .
Brody, G .H ., Stoneman, Z., Flor, D., & McCrary, C . (1994) . Religion's role in organizin g
Brownfield , D., & Sorenson, A.M. (1991) . Religion and drug use among adolescents : a social
Child Trends , Inc. (2001 ). Unpublished report ofselected measures from the NLSY97.
55
Colby, A ., & Kohlberg, L . (1987). The measurement of moral judgment . Vol. 1 : Theoretical
Cooksey, E.C., Rindfuss, R.R., & Guilkey, D .K. (1996) . The initiation of adolescent sexual an d
contraceptive behavior during changing times . Journal of Health and Social Behavior ,
37, 59-74 .
Dai, Y . (1996) . Educational plans after high school : A national survey of high school seniors .
Donahue, M .J., & Benson , P .L. (1995) . Religion and the well-being of adolescents . Journal of
Durant , R., Pendergrast, R., & Seymore, C . (1990) . Sexual behavior among Hispanic femal e
Elifson, K .W ., Petersen, D.M., & Hadaway, C .K. (1983) . Religiosity and delinquency : A
Elkind , D . (1964) . Age change and religious identity . Review of Religious Research, d, 36-40 .
Elkind, D . (1970) . The origins of religion in the child . Review of Religious Research, 12, 35-42 .
Ellison, C.G. (1993 ). Religious involvement and self-perception among Black Americans .
Ellison, C .G., Bartkowski, J .P., & Anderson, K.L. (1999) . Are there religious variations i n
Ellison, C.G., Bartkowski, J .P., & Segal, M .L. (1996) . Conservative Protestantism and the
Erikson, E . (1980 ). Identity and the Life Cycle . New York: Norton.
Evans, T .D., Cullen, F .T., Burton, V.S., Dunaway, R.G., Payne, G .L., & Kethineni, S .R. (1996) .
Fergusson , D .M., Horwood, L .J ., Kershaw, K .L., & Shannon, F .T. (1986). Factors associated
with reports of wive assault in New Zealand . Journal of Marriage and the Family, 48,
407-412.
Fischer, & Richards (1998) . Religion and guilt in childhood. In J . Bybee (Ed .), Guilt and
Fowler, J.W. (1991) . Stages in faith consciousness . In F .K. Oser & W.G. Scarlett (eds.),
Francis , & Gibson, ( 1993). Parental influence and adolescent religiosity : A study of church
Garbarino, J., & Bedard, C . (1996) . Spiritual challenges to children facing violent trauma .
Childhood, 3, 467-478.
George, L .K., Larson, D .B., Koenig, H .G., & McCullough, M .E. (2000) . Spirituality and health :
What we know, what we need to know . Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19,
102-116.
Gershoff, E .T., Miller, P .C., & Holden, G.W. (1999) . Parenting influences from the pulpit :
Giles-Sims, J., Straus , M.A., & Sugarman, D .B. (1995) . Child, maternal , and family
Gorsuch, R.L . (1984) . Measurement : The boon and bane of investigating religion . America n
single-item scales . Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion , 28, 348-354.
Gorsuch, R .L ., & Venable, G .D. (1983) . Development of an "age universal" I-E scale . Journal
Gunnoe, M .L ., Hetherington, E.M., & Reiss, D . (1999) . Parental religiosity, parenting style, and
Gunnoe, M.L., & Mariner, C .L . (1997) . Toward a developmental/contextual model of the effect s
Journal.
Halpern, C .T., Udry, J .R., Campbell, B ., Suchindran , C ., & Mason,G .A. (1994) . Testosteron e
and religiosity as predictors of sexual attitudes and activity among adolescent males : A
Hirschi, T., & Stark, R . (1969). Hellfire and delinquency . Social Problems , 17, 202-213 .
Hodgkinson, V .A., & Weitzman, M .S . (1997). Volunteering and giving among America n
Jang, S .J., & Johnson, S .R. (2001). Neighborhood disorder, individual religiosity, and
adolescent use of illicit drugs : A test of multilevel hypotheses . Criminology, 39, 109-
143 .
Kahn, J .R., Rindfuss, R .R ., & Guilkey, D .K. (1990) . Adolescent contraceptive choice .
Kauffinan, (1979) . Social correlates of spiritual maturity among North American Mennonites .
Kedem, P., & Cohen, D .W . (1987) . The effects of religious education on moral judgment .
King, P.E., & Furrow, J .L. (2001) . Development al resources, moral behaviors , and faith
Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, April .
King, V., Elder, G.H., & Whitbeck, L .B. (1997) . Religious involvement among ru ral youth : An
social scientific study of religion, vol. 1 (pp. 1-30) . Greenwich, CT : JAI Press .
59
Kirkpatrick, L .A., & Hood, R .W . (1990). Intrinsic-extrinsic religious orientation : The boon or
attachments , religious beliefs, and conversion . Journal for the Scientific Study of
Lindberg, L.D., Boggess, S ., & Williams , S . (2000) . Multiple threats : The co-occurrence of
teen health risk behaviors . Report to the Office of the Assistant Secretary for Plannin g
and Evaluation, U .S. Department of Health and Human Services, by The Urban Institute .
Maccoby, E .E., & Martin, J.A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family : parent-child
interaction . In P .H. Mussen (Series Ed .) & E.M . Hetherington (Vol. Ed .), Handbook of
Mahoney, A., Pargament, K .I ., Tarakeshwar, N ., & Swank, A .B. (in press) . Religion in the
home in the 1980s and 90s : Meta-analyses and conceptual analyses of links betwee n
Manlove, J ., Terry-Humen, E ., Papillo, A .R., Franzetta, K., Williams, S ., & Ryan, S . (2001) .
McBride, D .C., Mutch, P .B., & Chitwood, D .D. (1996) . Religious belief and the initiation and
prevention of drug use among youth. In C .B . McCoy, L .R. Metsch, & J .A. Inciard i
(eds .), Intervening with drug-involved youth, pp . 110-130. Thousand Oaks, CA : Sage .
Miller, L ., Warner, V ., Wickram.aratne , P., & Weissman , M. (1997) . Religiosity and depression :
Moore, K .A., Miller, B .C., Glei, D ., & Morrison, D .R. (1995). Adolescent sex, contraception,
and childbearing : A review ofrecent research. Report to the Office of the Assistant
Secretary for Planning and Evaluation, U .S . Department of Health and Human Services ,
Nolin, M.J., Chaney, B ., Chapman , C., & Chandler, K . (1997). Student participation in
Okagaki, L ., & Bevis, C, (1999) . Transmission of religious values : Relations between parents '
Oser, F .I . (1991) . The development of religious judgment . In F .K. Oser & W .G. Scarlett (eds.),
Osmer, R., & Fowler, J.W. (1985). Childhood and adolescence--A faith developmen t
perspective . In R .J . Wicks, R .D. Parson, & D .E. Capps (eds .), Clinical handbook of
Piaget,1. (1962) . Play, dreams and imitation in childhood . New York: Norton .
Piaget, J ., & Inhelder, B. (1958 ). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence .
Rogers-Dulan, J ., & Blather, J . (1995) . African American families, religion, and disability: A
Ross, C .E . (1990) . Religion and psychological distress . Journal for the Scientific Study of
Scarlett, & Perriello (1991) . The development of prayer in adolescence. In F.K. Oser & W.G.
Scarlett (eds .), Religious development in childhood and adolescence. New Directions fo r
Sherkat, D.E., & Ellison , C.G. (1999) . Recent developments and current controversies in the
Spilka, B ., Shaver, P ., & Kirkpatrick, L.A. (1985) . A general attribution theory for the
psychology of religion . Journal for the Scientific Study ofReligion , 24, 1-20 .
Strayhorn, J .M., Weidman, C .S., & Larson, D . (1990) . A measure of religiousness, and its
relation to parent and child mental health variables . Journal ofCommunity Psychology,
18, 34-43 .
Thornton, A ., & Camburn, D . (1989). Religious participation and adolescent sexual behavior
Villarruel, A.M. (1998 ). Cultural influences on the sexual a ttitudes, beliefs, and norms of youn g
Waite , L . (2001) . Religion and marriage : The ties that bind . Presentation at the Princeto n
Wilcox, W.B . (200 1). Religion and parenting . Presentation at the Princeton Conference on
Wright, L .S., Frost, C .J., & Wisecarver, S .J. (1993). Church attendance, meaningfulness o f
Youniss, J ., McLellan, J.A., & Yates, M . (1999) . Religion, community service, and identity i n