BeyondSTS PDF
BeyondSTS PDF
BeyondSTS PDF
DANA L. ZEIDLER
Department of Secondary Education, College of Education, University of South Florida,
Tampa, FL 33620-5650, USA
TROY D. SADLER
School of Teaching & Learning, College of Education, University of Florida, Gainesville,
FL 32611-7048, USA
DOI 10.1002/sce.20048
Published online 23 March 2005 in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com).
ABSTRACT: An important distinction can be made between the science, technology, and
society (STS) movement of past years and the domain of socioscientific issues (SSI). STS
education as typically practiced does not seem embedded in a coherent developmental
or sociological framework that explicitly considers the psychological and epistemological
growth of the child, nor the development of character or virtue. In contrast, the SSI movement
focuses on empowering students to consider how science-based issues reflect, in part, moral
principles and elements of virtue that encompass their own lives, as well as the physical
and social world around them. The focus of this paper is to describe a research-based
framework of current research and practice that identifies factors associated with reasoning
about socioscientific issues and provide a working model that illustrates theoretical and
conceptual links among key psychological, sociological, and developmental factors central
to SSI and science education. C 2005 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Sci Ed 89:357 – 377, 2005
INTRODUCTION
As the 21st century unfolds, professional associations (e.g., American Association for
the Advancement of Science, 1989, 1993; National Science Education Standards, 1996;
CMEC’s Pan-Canadian Science Project, 1997; Queensland School Curriculum Council,
2001) in science recognize the importance of broadly conceptualizing scientific literacy
C 2005 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
358 ZEIDLER ET AL.
to include informed decision making; the ability to analyze, synthesize, and evaluate in-
formation; dealing sensibly with moral reasoning and ethical issues; and understanding
connections inherent among socioscientific issues (SSI) (Zeidler, 2001). Achieving a prac-
tical degree of scientific literacy necessarily entails practice and experience in developing
habits of mind (i.e., acquiring skepticism, maintaining open-mindedness, evoking critical
thinking, recognizing multiple forms of inquiry, accepting ambiguity, searching for data-
driven knowledge). Habits of mind may suffice when arriving at individual decisions based
on an informed analysis of available information; however, they may not be sufficient in a
world where collective decision making is evoked through the joint construction of social
knowledge. In the real world of dirty sinks and messy reasoning, arriving at ideal personal
decisions through objective evaluation of neutral evidence is a phantom image.
It is clear from recent international research in science education that current reform ini-
tiatives in our field demand increased emphasis on the nature of science (NOS) and scientific
inquiry, as well as development of broad conceptual frameworks encompassing progressive
visions of scientific literacy that entail a commitment to the moral and ethical dimensions
of science education—including the social and character development of children (Zeidler
& Keefer, 2003). In particular, students are expected to develop an understanding of the
epistemology of scientific knowledge as well as the processes/methods used to develop such
knowledge. In addition to other considerations, it is believed that students’ understanding of
science as a “way of knowing” is absolutely necessary if informed decisions are to be made
regarding the scientifically based personal and societal issues that increasingly confront
our students. Such decisions necessarily involve careful evaluation of scientific claims by
discerning connections among evidence, inferences, and conclusions. Students capable of
such decisions display a functional degree of scientific literacy.
The focus of this paper is to provide a synopsis of current research and practice that
identifies factors associated with reasoning about SSI and distinguishes it from science –
technology– society (STS) education. While the study of SSI is conceptually related to
past research on STS education, it is important to point out that they represent unique
approaches. STS education, as typically envisioned and practiced, does not seem to be
embedded in a coherent developmental or sociological framework that explicitly considers
the psychological and epistemological growth of the child, nor the development of character
or virtue. The lack of a theoretical framework with respect to STS materials has been noted
by others (Hodson, 2003; Jenkins, 2002; Shamos, 1995), suggesting that STS may be an
underdeveloped idea in search of a theory. Because we suggest substantial reconsideration
of the use of the STS approach, it is important to consider limitations identified with STS
education; therefore, we do so in the following section.
STS science teaching conveys the image of socially constructed knowledge. Its student-
oriented approach . . . emphasizes the basic facts, skills, and concepts of traditional sci-
ence . . . but does so by integrating that science content into social and technological contexts
meaningful to students. (p. 59)
MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 359
In 1982, the National Science Teachers Association (NSTA) published a position paper
describing characteristics of a scientifically literate person as one who would understand
and be knowledgeable of the connections and interdependency of science, technology, and
society (NSTA, 1982). This shift in emphasis was solidified with the publication of the
NSTA 1985 Yearbook, which was devoted to STS teaching (Bybee, 1985). Unfortunately,
STS education has become relatively diffuse over the course of its tenure, consisting of
approaches as disparate as isolated courses addressing STS issues or ancillary text boxes
in science textbooks (Pedretti & Hodson, 1995). Shamos (1995) noted, quite correctly,
that the problem with STS curriculum is that many of the issues under study (e.g., nuclear
power, global warming) are not particularly exciting or relevant to students because they are
removed from their everyday personal experiences. While STS education typically stresses
the impact of decisions in science and technology on society, it does not mandate explicit
attention to the ethical issues contained within choices about means and ends, nor does it
consider the moral or character development of students.
Recently, some science educators have advocated a more issues-driven STS curricu-
lum in the form of science – technology– society – environment education (STSE) (Hodson,
1994, 2003; Pedretti, 1997). It has become clear, however, that while STSE represents an
improvement over STS strategies, it does not directly address the personal and individual
moral and ethical development of students and it is fair to say that most science educators
do not see the subtle distinctions between them. Traditional STS(E) education (or perhaps
STS(E) education as currently practiced by and large) only “points out” ethical dilemmas or
controversies, but does not necessarily exploit the inherent pedagogical power of discourse,
reasoned argumentation, explicit NOS considerations, emotive, developmental, cultural or
epistemological connections within the issues themselves. Hence, STS(E) approaches have
become somewhat marginalized in the curriculum and in practice. What was once described
as a megatrend in science education (Roy, 1984) has been relegated to brief mentions in
current school science textbooks as well as in science teacher preparation texts (see, e.g.,
Chiapetta & Koballa, 2002; Trowbridge, Bybee, & Powell, 2000). This decline in emphasis
in STS(E) education is part of a striking paradox, for the commitment to the inextricable
connections between science, technology, society and the environment remains a major
theme in current science reform documents such as the United States National Science
Education Standards and Project 2061’s Benchmarks for Science Literacy.
One likely reason for the recent decline in interest in STS(E) may be a lack of focus or
well-developed unifying theoretical basis. Even staunch supporters of STS have acknowl-
edged the absence of a coherent and cohesive framework for STS. Consider the following
statements regarding STS:
In this essay I have suggested that science, technology, society (STS) consists of several
seemingly competing, if not conflicting perspectives because they relate to different notions
of power, policy, and method. Nevertheless, the perspectives can be combined. Combining
the perspectives does not mean however that we create a unitary approach of STS. What
I intend is rather a pluralistic and open approach. To open the doors among the different
perspectives is a major challenge for STS which may also require a thorough deliberation
of the different related policy interests. (Fuglsang, 2001, p. 46)
Ziman, another ardent proponent of STS education, writes, “The fundamental purposes of
STS education are genuinely and properly diverse and incoherent” (1994, p. 22). Although
these candid admissions of the considerable difficulties in both describing the purposes
of STS and melding the myriad STS factions could be interpreted as assertions merely ac-
knowledging the complexity of STS, we suggest that the reported pluralism and incoherence
360 ZEIDLER ET AL.
. . . when socioscience is the icing on the cake, not an essential basic ingredient, part of a
good-quality product but not fundamental to teaching science, dominant discourses of sci-
ence as an abstract body of knowledge are not destabilized and implicit gender hierarchical
binaries are readily reinforced. (Hughes, 2000, p. 347)
Similarly, Bingle and Gaskell (1994) have further noted that much of STS education, as
practiced, is most closely aligned with Latour’s (1987) notion of “ready-made science” that
carries with it the connotation of positivist knowledge claims at the expense of constitutive
values that stress “science-in-the-making” and suggests a social constructivist view of con-
textual values for evaluating scientific knowledge claims. These authors stress that where
SSI arise, it is legitimate for individual citizens to acknowledge and evaluate contextual
factors deemed meaningful with respect to the scientific claims under consideration: “A so-
cial constructivist view of science . . . challenges the scientists’ position of privilege because
individual citizens have just as much access to the standards of evaluating the impact of the
social context as do scientists themselves, a prospect that would probably be unsettling to
most scientists” (Bingle & Gaskell, 1994, p. 198).
Whereas the overarching purpose of the STS approach is to increase student interest
in science by placing science content learning in a societal context, SSI education aims
to stimulate and promote individual intellectual development in morality and ethics as
well as awareness of the interdependence between science and society. SSI therefore does
not simply serve as a context for learning science, but rather as a pedagogical strategy
with clearly defined goals. Certainly, knowledge and understanding of the interconnec-
tions among science, technology, society, and the environment are major components of
developing scientific literacy; however, these interconnections do not exist independently
of students’ personal beliefs. It is our stance that STS(E) approaches can be remodeled and
substantially improved by adding an essential missing component—consideration of each
student’s own moral and ethical development.
the ethical dimensions of science, the moral reasoning of the child, and the emotional
development of the student” (p. 344). Further, recent research (Zeidler & Keefer, 2003) in
the area of SSI has provided theoretical and conceptual links among key psychological,
sociological, and developmental factors associated with SSI education. We envision SSI
in a manner that considers how controversial scientific issues and dilemmas affect the
intellectual growth of individuals in both personal and societal domains.
In order to advance the claim that science educators should attend to SSI related to cul-
tivating the morality of our students to achieve a “functional” view of scientific literacy,
a coherent conceptual framework must be developed that is flexible enough to allow for
multiple perspectives while enabling educators and curriculum specialists to better under-
stand the moral growth of the child. One framework recently proposed because of its utility
in addressing socioscientific discourse in terms of the psychological, social, and emotive
growth of the child is derived from a cognitive-moral reasoning perspective (Zeidler &
Keefer, 2003). This initial model served as an impetus for our article by identifying poten-
tial lines of research that might prove promising in the development of an SSI framework.
We have now further extended and refined that model with new research from within the
science education community and related research external to science education. It consists
of themes that collectively attend to many of the factors inherently limited by or missing
from STS education. This framework should be viewed as a tentative conceptual model that
identifies four areas of pedagogical importance central to the teaching of SSI: (1) nature of
science issues, (2) classroom discourse issues, (3) cultural issues, and (4) case-based issues.
These issues can be thought of as entry points in the science curriculum that can contribute
to a student’s personal intellectual development and in turn, help to inform pedagogy in
science education to promote functional scientific literacy (see Figure 1).
It should be noted that while our view of cognitive and moral development is concep-
tualized, in part, from a neo-Kohlbergian perspective, it does not preclude (and in fact,
invites) the use of post-modern perspectives of developmental reasoning (e.g., see Hughes,
2000). While these issues certainly cannot comprise all the sufficient conditions of scientific
literacy (functional or otherwise), we suggest that these are critical and necessary issues
for science educators to grapple with in order to promote functional scientific literacy. For
example, NOS issues become important because they reveal how varied epistemological
views influence the way in which students select and evaluate evidence, and are considered
to have bearing on their pre-instructional views of SSI. Discourse issues direct our attention
to how students construct arguments and utilize fallacious reasoning, and compel us to
consider how prior belief convictions help frame emotional responses, principled commit-
ments, or stances on moral issues. Cultural issues remind us that discourse is futile without
mutual respect and tolerance of dissenting views, while underscoring that the decisions stu-
dents make are the result of realizing that as moral agents, they are impacted by normative
values as well as cultural beliefs about the natural world. Case-based issues enable science
educators to move beyond STS curriculum and cultivate habits of mind that promote ethical
awareness and commitment to issue resolution and the moral sensitivity to hear dissenting
voices by examining how power and authority are embedded in scientific enterprises.
Other authors use the term “functional” when speaking of scientific literacy (Jenkins,
1990, 1997; Ryder, 2001; Shamos, 1995); however, it is important to note that they tend
to do so from what can be described as a technocratic perspective. For example, Ryder
(2001) presents an analysis of some 31 studies ranging in degree of technocratic decision
making on controversial topics (in that there is a lack of clear consensus within the scientific
community about data related to these topics). These topics range from risk assessment in
genetic counseling situations to managing methane from a waste disposal site. The authors
do acknowledge that the studies selected for review are not necessarily representative of
the many contexts of science, and the examples tend to focus on arguments based on utility
in a democratic and technologically sophisticated society. While it cannot be denied that
these aspects of science are important because of their inherent connections to NOS (e.g.,
understanding the role of models in science, assessing the validity and quality of data, and
uncertainty in science), we believe that this view is too narrow with regard to promoting
functional scientific literacy in that it pays scant attention to the role of personal epistemo-
logical and intellectual development in the context of varied cultural settings. For Shamos
(1995), functional scientific literacy seems to be relegated to those who are the science
elite having the expert content knowledge to fully appreciate the scientific and technologi-
cal intricacies of issues (thereby achieving “true scientific literacy”) and have more than a
general sense of Hirsch’s (1987) notion of “cultural literacy” (i.e., general scientific terms
familiar to citizens in western society). An individual who possesses functional scientific
literacy for Shamos, therefore, is one who has “command of a science lexicon, [and] also
be able to converse, read, and write coherently, using such science terms in perhaps a non-
technical but nevertheless meaningful context” (1995, p. 88). In a comprehensive review of
the literature, Laugksch (2000) points out that such conceptualizations of functional scien-
tific literacy are embedded in a meaning of literate that “require[s] the scientifically literate
individual to use science in performing a function (italics added) in society” (p. 84). Again,
our view of functional scientific literacy affirms what the views above do not; that any view
of functional scientific literacy falls short of the mark if it ignores the fundamental factors
aimed at promoting the personal cognitive and moral development of students.
Although Kohlberg (1986) provided educators and researchers interested in the area of
moral reasoning and development with a rich conceptual basis to raise important questions
about the nature of moral education, new questions have emerged about the adequacy of
MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 363
the assumption that all one has to do to bring about changes in moral behavior is to induce
changes in moral stages or structures. Furthermore, new questions have also emerged about
the distinction between reasoning about formal societal constructs (e.g., laws, duty, social
institutions) and engaging in the resolution of differences among individuals via argumen-
tation and discussion during face-to face interactions. The former type of reasoning deals
with what Rest et al. (1999) term “macromorality,” while the latter deals with issues of
“micromorality.” The difference can be likened to examining societal conventions from a
theoretical perspective (e.g., principles of justice, meta-ethics) and understanding a partic-
ular praxis of social constructs (e.g., face-to-face negotiations, normative ethics). Once this
distinction is made, it exposes a more robust conceptualization of the complex relationship
that exists between moral reasoning and action and has implications for decisions related to
pedagogy. For example, researchers point out that the role of affect and emotions in moral
functioning had been overlooked in past research, and that the particular realm of one’s
life being considered (e.g., family, school, peers, workplace, intimate relationships) plays a
normative role in moral decision making and character formation (Berkowitz, 1997, 1998;
Nucci, 1989, 2001; Sadler & Zeidler, 2004; Turiel, 1998; Zeidler & Schafer, 1984, Zeidler
et al., 2002).
(1) Nature of Science Issues reveal the emphasis placed on students’ epistemological
beliefs as they pertain to decisions regarding SSI (e.g., Bell, 2004; Bell, Lederman, & Abd-
El-Khalick, 2000). Epistemological orientations regarding the nature of science influence
how students attend to evidence in support of, or in conflict with, their pre-instructional
belief systems regarding social issues. In this context, moral reasoning proper is understood
to be the result of the opportunity for learners to make meaning using empirical and social
criteria in both formal and informal educational contexts through rational discourse. Abd-El-
Khalick’s (2001) and Bell’s (2003) research has suggested that students’ decisions regarding
SSI are analogous to decisions engaged by scientists regarding the justification of scientific
knowledge in that both processes require the use of rational discourse and invoke value
judgments and common sense. These findings highlight the importance of tapping students’
epistemological orientations (including NOS views) in the process of evaluating scientific
data regarding social issues.
Likewise, Zeidler et al. (2002) have shown that students who harbor naı̈ve and relativistic
conceptions of science will likely dismiss scientific knowledge as irrelevant to decision
making when reasoning about SSI because they tend to distort whatever data, evidence,
or knowledge claims are available to them for the purpose of supporting a predetermined
viewpoint with respect to the issue under consideration. Related research informing the
issues of socioscientific reasoning and NOS confirms student reliance on personal relevance
over evaluative decisions based on contemplation of presented evidence (Sadler, Chambers,
& Zeidler, 2004). In this study, students rated articles according to which had more “scientific
merit,” but in determining which articles they found to be most convincing, many (40%)
364 ZEIDLER ET AL.
selected articles that most complemented their own personal beliefs independently of their
scientific merit. This pattern of responses suggested that for some students, scientific merit
(e.g., evidence, data) and persuasiveness were not synonymous. In order to fully appreciate
the empirical nature of science, students must understand what constitutes data and how
it can be utilized in the process of decision making. However, if that student is confused
by what data is, then assertions or arguments evoked hold little meaning. In analyzing
students’ comments about how data were used to support positions on global warming,
Sadler, Chambers, and Zeidler (2004) also revealed that nearly half (47%) of the students
lacked adequate conceptions of scientific data. Some of the students comprising this group
were able to recognize data without the ability to describe its use or significance, whereas
others could not even distinguish among data, unfounded opinions, and predictions.
Bell and Lederman (2003) have examined the reasoning patterns of university profes-
sors representing various fields (including science educators, science philosophers, and
research scientists) on SSI and found similar reasoning patterns among these groups, in-
cluding emphases on personal philosophy and commitments over reasoning based on sci-
entific evidence. Although the participants of this study held varied views of the NOS, their
decision-making strategies and actual decisions on science and technology-based issues
yielded no discernable patterns unique to particular NOS views. While all of these individ-
uals showed some degree of “superficial” evidence-based reasoning, the primary influence
guiding their decisions were personal values, factors related to morality or ethics, and social
considerations. The authors suggested in very clear terms that moral development is a factor
of interest when assessing decision-making strategies on SSI. Their research findings also
provided supporting evidence for earlier work that revealed explicit links between college
students’ levels of moral reasoning and decision making on SSI irrespective of their science
content knowledge level of sophistication (Zeidler & Schafer, 1984).
Similarly, Walker and Zeidler (2003) found that students’ reference to empirical evidence
supporting various positions during a debate activity in high school was limited following
online exploration of SSI with previous instruction regarding NOS and exposure to multiple
articles of evidence used to gather support and frame arguments for their debate position.
Yet when other members of the class voted as to which group presented the best argument,
the majority (75%) of the students chose the group as being the most convincing that utilized
a wide base of background information, had the input of different people, quoted statistics
and generally were convinced by their arguments even when presented with a position
contrary to their own. The findings underscore the need for explicit instruction in NOS, so
that careful evaluations of evidence regarding SSI and subsequent decisions can be utilized.
This line of research has also revealed more direct transfer of NOS considerations when
reasoning about SSI during informal debate or discourse particularly when the SSI issues
centered around genetically modified foods and global warming (Sadler, Chambers, &
Zeidler, 2004; Walker and Zeidler, 2003; Zeidler et al. 2002), suggesting that the degree of
personal relevance of the issue is associated with increased validation of knowledge claims.
It reasonably follows that the degree to which students perceive personal relevance related
to scientific topics will determine, in part, the seriousness of the issues at-hand and the
merit of conflicting or competing claims. If a goal of teaching NOS in science classrooms
is to develop students’ abilities to critically evaluate competing scientific claims, then we
should be guiding them in the process of synthesizing and applying their understanding of
the nature of science as they evaluate and make decisions regarding socioscientific issues.
The significance of this lies not so much in that future generations of students may be
able to articulate the meaning of the nature of science and describe its relevant attributes
(although that would be a pedagogically notable benchmark), but rather that NOS under-
standing can benefit them in evaluating the efficacy many kinds of claims—scientific or
MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 365
otherwise—based upon the merit of supporting evidence in everyday life. This goal of de-
veloping transferable reasoning skills is one that is central to promoting the use of SSI in
science curricula.
(2) Classroom Discourse Issues stress the crucial role discourse plays in peer interac-
tions and its impact on reasoning. This research underscores the importance of developing
students’ views about science through argumentation in the constructions of shared social
knowledge via discourse about SSI (e.g., Zeidler et al., 2003). While many science educators
acknowledge the importance of rich and diverse classroom discussions in the promotion
of scientific literacy (Aikenhead, 1985, 2000; Driver, Newton, & Osborne, 2000; Vellom,
1999; Zeidler, 1984, 1997; Zeidler, Lederman, & Taylor, 1992), those who seek to study
it have difficulty locating substantive argumentation or classroom discussions in school
(Newton, Driver, & Osborne,1999), or find the quantity and quality of discussion with ex-
plicit focus on science content very low (Levinson, 2003). Perhaps this is because teachers
find it difficult to implement sustained student discourse with confidence because of the
complex nature of argumentation. This is precisely what Levinson (2003) found in his in-
tensive case study of one “Science for Public Understanding” college class whose focus
was on exploring controversial science issues (i.e., SSI). Despite the fact that the intent of
the course was to allow students to develop and express an informed personal viewpoint
on SSI, the teachers who cotaught the class dominated much of the classroom discourse.
Levinson further suggested that because of their inherent complexity, attending to moral
and ethical issues may be an unrealistic expectation for science teachers without some type
of support from other teachers representing interdisciplinary studies and/or professional
development to aid in facilitating the dynamics of argumentation and discourse.
Settelmaier (2003) focused on high school science students’ exchanges using a “dilemma”
approach. While the results revealed that dilemma-based stories were a viable tool for
introducing SSI which challenged students’ rational, social, and emotional skills, as well as
grounding the practice of critical self-reflection concerning their personal value and beliefs
systems, several problematic factors in using SSI in the classroom were identified including
logistical and planning problems of integrating coverage of content with moral dilemmas
and matching the appropriateness of the dilemmas with student interests. Students can
easily go off-task if the topic is not well focused, extends too long, or the outcomes are not
clear. It may be the case that students simply do not have occasion to practice tolerating
ambiguity—whether it be in the form of anomalous data or conflicting points of view.
Additional considerations entailing fallacious argumentation have been identified and
explored (i.e., validity concerns, naı̈ve conceptions of argument structure, effects of core
beliefs on argumentation, inadequate sampling of evidence altering representation of argu-
ment and evidence) and serve as a reminder of the complex nature of discourse involving
SSI (Zeidler, 1997; Zeidler et al., 2003). However, the value of argument in the development
of moral reasoning has been amply demonstrated in the research literature (Berkowitz &
Oser, 1985; Berkowitz, Oser, & Althof, 1987; Keefer, 2002; Keefer, Zeitz, & Resnick, 2000)
in terms of creating dissonance thereby allowing opportunity for re-examining one’s beliefs
and thought-processes. Being exposed to and challenged by the arguments of others pro-
vides the opportunity to attend to the quality of claims, warrants, evidence, and assumptions
among competing positions. The idea of creating dissonance through the use of anomalous
data has broad meaning in the context of SSI which not only supports the use of conflicting
empirical data, but further taps into potentially conflicting counter positions and arguments
that arise through dialogic interaction between or among discussants in which logically
supported position statements may conflict with a person’s pre-existing beliefs regarding a
given scientific concept or issue.
366 ZEIDLER ET AL.
Driver et al. (1996) have demonstrated how students can handle conflicting evidence
related to SSI. They indicated that through carefully planned science instruction, students
draw on past experiences and combine them with new ideas to explain decisions in scien-
tific contexts. Dialogic interaction, also referred to as transactive discussions in the moral
education research literature, has been shown to improve science learning and lead to more
robust ways to conceptualize the physical world: “Whereas transaction can foster students’
logical development by focusing on scientific problems and issues, teachers can foster
the development of social and moral reasoning by focusing on ethical and social issues”
(Berkowitz & Simmons, 2003, p. 133). It is important to note that transactional discourse
occurs when one student can clearly internalize and articulate the thoughts, arguments, or
position of another student. Their reasoning then becomes transformational in the sense
that one individual’s reasoning becomes integrated with that of another. Berkowitz and
Simmons reported that students who use more transactive discussions during classroom
discourse also learn to solve scientific and mathematical problems more effectively, conse-
quently enhancing the development of scientific reasoning and problem-solving strategies.
In a comprehensive review of the literature, Sadler (2004) summarizes trends related to
argumentation as a means of expressing informal reasoning and reiterates that the personal
experiences of decision-makers emerged as a consistent normative influence on informal
reasoning related to SSI. More specifically, informal reasoning was found to either mediate
scientific knowledge or prevent the consideration of scientific knowledge. Sadler cites re-
search that suggests conceptual understanding of content improved informal reasoning on
SSI, but more work in this area is needed. Kuhn (1993) further points out that SSI will nec-
essarily entail the use of informal reasoning inasmuch as they are complex, open ended, and
often consist of contentious problems without predetermined solutions. Informal reasoning
is compatible with the kinds of dilemmas that face students confronted with real-world is-
sues in that the issues at-hand are more times than not, dynamic (i.e., premises may change
as new information and perspectives arise).
It would seem, then, that opportunity to engage in informal reasoning through argu-
mentation allows for the evaluation of evidence as well as thought, but finding appropriate
pedagogical strategies to seamlessly integrate such dynamic social interaction in the science
classroom remains a high priority. Teaching science in this context includes attention and
sensitivity to students’ moral commitments, emotions, and moral behavior. The develop-
ment of character in children (as seen via the development of moral reasoning) becomes an
additional important pedagogical outcome arising from the intrinsic nature of argumenta-
tion as pedagogy.
(3) Cultural Issues highlight pluralistic and sociological (subsuming gender) aspects of
science classrooms. By explicitly attending to cultural issues, science educators recog-
nize, acknowledge, and maximize opportunities afforded by diverse assemblies of learn-
ers. The diversity of modern science classrooms includes learners from various cultures
(e.g., Cobern & Loving, 2000; Lemke, 2001), developmental abilities (e.g., Mastropieri &
Scruggs, 1992; McGinnis, 2000), and genders (e.g., Brickhouse, 2001; Tsai, 2000). This
cultural/sociological perspective toward education underscores the necessity to appreciate
students as moral agents intimately involved with their own cultural, natural, and techno-
logical environments. Regarding individual students as moral agents interacting with their
classroom contexts and experiences emphasizes the moral nature of education in general
and teaching in particular. The essence of good teaching, in this view, must include the
ethical and moral development of young people (Clark, 2003; Loving, Lowry, & Martin,
2003). In fact, ethical and cognitive growth appear to be tightly linked in the development
of intercultural understanding (Endicott, Bock, & Narvaez, 2002).
MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 367
Cultural issues are particularly significant for educational experiences related to SSI.
Whereas moral theorists adopting cognitive-developmental perspectives on morality (e.g.,
Kohlberg, 1986; Rest et al., 1999) have assumed the primacy of moral principles, critical the-
orists who adopt cultural perspectives on morality (e.g., Belenky et al., 1986; Gilligan, 1987;
Noddings, 1984; Partington, 1997; Snarey, 1985) have expanded the scope and challenged
the construction of moral discourse in order to include care, emotion, and contextual fac-
tors. The contributions of multicultural (Boyes & Walker, 1988; Snarey, 1985) and feminist
(Donenberg & Hoffman, 1988; Hekman; 1995; Gilligan, 1987; Noddings, 1984) research
on morality inspired some science education researchers to look beyond isolated cognitive
factors (viz. situated cognition) contributing to socioscientific decision making in order to
perceive the significance of affect.
Zeidler and Schafer (1984) produced some of the first empirical evidence suggest-
ing the importance of emotions in the resolution of SSI. Although a priori notions of
morality drove their research design consistent with a Kohlbergian perspective, qualita-
tive analyses of dyadic interviews led the authors to postulate the significance of emo-
tional reactions to controversial environmental issues. Sadler and Zeidler (2004; in press)
extended the investigation on the role of emotions in informal reasoning regarding SSI.
Both studies focused on student reactions to, perceptions of, argumentation toward, and
resolutions of genetic engineering issues. These findings confirmed the significance of
emotion for the resolution of SSI. More specifically, participants actively displayed a
sense of empathy toward fictitious characters described in scenarios to which they were
responding as well as to friends, family members, and acquaintances experiencing sit-
uations reminiscent of these scenarios. For example, one participant’s decision making
regarding cloning issues was driven primarily by her feelings toward a very close fam-
ily member who desperately wanted to have children but remained infertile even after
conventional fertility treatments. This individual and many others like her in the study ex-
hibited a genuine sense of care that ultimately guided her negotiation and resolution of
SSI.
Howes (2002) also noted the appearance and use of empathy and other relationally based
concerns in her teacher-research studies with 10th-grade biology students. Three main
aspects of a semester-long human genetics course were examined: (1) a unit focusing on
prenatal testing for genetic conditions, (2) a set of imaginary cases posed to individual
students during interviews, and (3) students’ writing and classroom talk concerning the role
of science in society. During the instructional unit on the subject of prenatal tests, the girls
in this study considered the comfort and safety of the fetus, and occasionally of the woman
carrying the fetus, as primary aspects in making decisions concerning prenatal testing. This
may have been an artifact of disincentives to discuss abortion in a high-school classroom,
especially on the teacher’s part—deflecting the more deeply emotional and controversial
decisions concerning abortion to those concerning prenatal tests alone. These girls explicitly
utilized the aspects of comfort and safety when arguing for their decisions as to whether
tests should be utilized in particular fictional cases, and used stories from direct personal
experience, or related those they had heard from female adults, to illustrate their points.
Boys were not studied in the prenatal testing instructional context. In the second research
context, boys and girls were presented with similar fictional cases in which they were asked
to put themselves in the place of a scientist in charge of ameliorating an urgent situation
(e.g., the imminent extinction of an endangered animal, or a town suffering from a cholera
outbreak). The small sample from this study indicated that girls put the safety and comfort
of both the suffering parties and the scientist her/himself in the forefront of their decision
making more than boys. This finding is complicated in the third portion of the study. In
writing and discussion throughout the semester, both girls and boys strongly stated that
368 ZEIDLER ET AL.
the role of science in society was to provide cures for human diseases, to avoid disturbing
ecosystems, and, in general, to do good for the Earth and for humanity. Their passion in
this regard is also evident in preservice elementary teachers, as indicated by Cobern and
Loving’s (2002) survey study.
The empathy and care demonstrated in these studies is consistent with notions of emotion-
based morality and care originally conceptualized in research with women (Belenky et al.,
1986; Gilligan, 1987; Noddings, 1984). In addition, feminist critiques of science have
led to a notion that leaving emotional, political, and social influences out of scientific
decision making is not only impossible, but dangerous. Importantly, however, feminist
researchers insist that “care” and “emotion” not be assigned solely to women, as this supports
a dichotomy that equates men with rationality and women with emotion (Harding, 1998;
Hughes, 2000). Helping students to practice both of these segments of their personalities
and knowledge-making skills may be where much of the promise of feminist approaches
lies (Gilligan, 1987; Tong, 1996).
Several researchers have corroborated the importance of this perspective in the context
of SSI (Korpan et al., 1997; Sadler & Zeidler, 2004, 2005; Zeidler & Schafer, 1984). For ex-
ample, Sadler and Zeidler (2005) not only concluded that individuals may rely on emotions
for the resolution of SSI, but that informal reasoning based on emotion was often equivalent
to strictly cognitive approaches to decision making in terms of logical constructs such as in-
ternal consistency and coherence. Whereas the Kohlbergian paradigm suggests that emotive
decision making represents inherently underdeveloped moral reasoning, Sadler and Zeidler
used an evaluative framework derived from argumentation and informal reasoning theory
and research (Kuhn, 1991; Toulmin, 1958) that did not discount any modes of decision mak-
ing a priori. Rather, the quality of decision making was assessed based on logical criteria
of informal reasoning. Using this framework, it was concluded that evaluative distinctions
among emotive and other forms of reasoning in terms of their decision-making adequacy
were unfounded.
These findings draw attention to the importance of cultural issues and individual students’
beliefs when addressing SSI in science classrooms. Particular situations are, by definition,
embedded in particular cultures and thus intertwined with particular relationships (Gruen,
1994; Plumwood, 1993). Recommendations that science education be grounded in local
community issues (Aikenhead, 1997) and in students’ interests (Calabrese Barton, 1998a,
1998b; Seiler, 2001) draw on the conclusion that individual students create identities through
experiences and perspectives shaped by a culturally diverse society. Here is where we find
a link between feminist and cultural perspectives, as both sets of theorists recognize per-
sonal identity as “embedded in individual and social circumstances” (Kozoll & Osborne,
2004). That identity shaped by culture is made clear through the “worldview” perspective
as developed by Cobern (1993), who states that “a people’s world view provides a special
plausibility structure of ideas, activities, and values that allows one to gauge the plausibil-
ity of any assertion” [emphasis in original] (p. 57). While students’ cultural experiences,
therefore, will certainly influence their decisions, it is also clear that personal identities are
not fixed, but vary with setting and are fashioned through students’ social, intellectual, and
moral growth (Hughes, 2000; Kozoll & Osborne, 2004). Kozol and Osborne (2004) offer
the intriguing possibility that science education can support students’ development when it
“opens up the horizons from within which science and self are understood and contributes
toward the evolution of both” (p. 170).
Explorations of identity, culture, and their relationships to science, while describing
personal identity as influenced by context (e.g., classroom, family, ethnic group), recognize
identity as a vehicle through which individuals interact with and explain their world. Given
this, it follows that we need research in this area to see how we might create culturally
MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 369
relevant or culturally responsive pedagogy (see, e.g., Foster, 1995; Gay, 2000; Hollins &
Oliver, 1999; Ladson-Billings, 1994; Quintanar-Sarellana, 1997; Smith, 2004; Tate, 1995)
employing SSI contexts. Central to this research should be the results of SSI in teaching
science with all students, as well as further learning concerning students’ identities and
worldviews. In their complexity, SSI may offer multiple connections to varied interests and
belief systems, thus assisting students in creating pathways to science learning. However,
we cannot say this with certainty, as the uses of socioscientific issues per se have not been
widely examined in science classrooms with an eye to cultural and feminist perspectives.
Research in this area may provide a richer picture of moral codes and ethical perspectives,
thus supporting teachers in engaging their students in socioscientific issues.
Students possess diverse arrays of cultural experiences that necessarily contribute to the
manners in which they approach and resolve controversial issues including SSI. World-
views and identities engendering moral choices are brought into any classroom. Therefore,
it is imperative for science educators to foster classroom environments that encourage the
expression of diverse perspectives even when those perspectives are not consistent with
traditional notions of science. In recognizing the moral nature of teaching and the fact
that students are active moral agents, it is essential for teachers to understand that their
classrooms cannot be value-free, but they certainly should be value-fair (Loving, Lowy, &
Martin, 2003).
(4) Case-Based Issues reinforce the stance that in order to develop scientifically literate
citizens, the science education community must reach beyond past STS practices which
usually do not pay explicit attention to the moral growth of the child and instead involve
students with the kinds of issues and problems to ponder that embrace both their intellect
and their sense of character. Recent studies involving example cases of genetically modi-
fied foods (Walker & Zeidler, 2003), human genetic engineering (Sadler & Zeidler, 2004;
Zohar & Nemet, 2002), animal experimentation (Simonneaux, 2001; Zeidler et al., 2002),
and environmental dilemmas (Hogan, 2002; Kolstø, 2001b) provide strong support for the
efficacy of using controversial socioscientific case studies to foster critical thinking skills
and moral and ethical development. These studies strongly suggest that curricula using such
issues provide an environment where students become engaged in discourse and reflection
that affect cognitive and moral development. The essence of the strategy of using SSI has
been described as follows:
If we hope to stimulate and develop students’ moral reasoning abilities, then we must
provide students with rich and varied opportunities to gain and hone such skills. The present
argument rests on the assumption that using controversial SSI as a foundation for individual
consideration and group interaction provides an environment where students can and will
increase their science knowledge while simultaneously developing their critical thinking
and moral reasoning skills (Simmons & Zeidler, 2003, p. 83).
Similarly, other researchers have developed (or modified existing) protocols that have ad-
dressed the implementation of case-based issues in science classrooms. Pedretti (2003) has
had successful experiences with preservice teachers who embraced the idea of incorporating
SSI via STSE in the curriculum. Using Ratcliffe’s (1997) model as an organizational frame-
work has allowed Pedretti’s (2003, p. 231) students to develop their own decision-making
models for pedagogy outcomes and includes
Keefer (2003) has reported the results of using case studies with undergraduate and
graduate science students and has noted that the values of the domain of concern for the
issue, rather than gender or a general disposition for a particular moral orientation (contra
Gilligan), accounted for differences in students’ reasoning. His work examined ethical
care responses in professional contexts and led to an empirically derived model for deci-
sion making in practical contexts using moral case issues. Keefer’s (2003, p. 253) model
entails the following:
This moral heuristic has been used successfully in the analysis of engineering ethics case
studies for professionals and is strikingly similar to the six-component model described
above. Keefer makes it clear in his analysis that ethical instruction is most successful
when it is integrated into those authentic contexts that will subsequently be practiced by
students.
Another complementary approach to case-based SSI provides a more explicit critical
examination of students’ personal interests and values as they provide arguments that eval-
uate scientific knowledge claims. Kolstø (2000) reports on a consensus project model used
mainly with upper secondary science students which emphasizes that scientific knowledge
is formulated by consensus building via critical discourse among (competent) peers. The
major premise of the consensus approach is that a general knowledge of the human na-
ture and limitations of scientific claims is needed to place scientific statements in adequate
terms so consensus decisions regarding SSI can be achieved. Necessarily broad in nature,
consensus projects tend to have four key attributes (Kolstø, 2000, p. 652):
Such an approach no doubt places great demands on the teacher whose role is that of a
counselor, consultant and critic, as well as an expeditor. Students realize from the onset that
their positions will be challenged and met with critical appraisal in the process of consensus
building.
MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 371
wish to cultivate future citizens and leaders who care, serve the community, and provide
leadership for new generations, then we have a moral imperative to delve into the realm of
virtue, character, and moral development.
REFERENCES
Abd-El-Khalick, F. (2001). Embedding nature of science instruction in preservice elementary science courses:
Abandoning scientism, but. . . . Journal of Science Teacher Education, 12(3), 215 – 233.
Aikenhead, G. (1994). What is STS in science teaching? In J. Solomon & G. Aikenhead (Eds.), STS education:
International perspectives on reform. New York: Teachers College Press.
Aikenhead, G. (1997). Toward a first nations cross-cultural science and technology curriculum. Science Education,
81, 217 – 238.
Aikenhead, G. S. (1985). Collective decision making in the social context of science. Science Education, 69,
453 – 475.
Aikenhead, G. S. (2000). Renegotiating the culture of school science. In R. Millar, J. Leach, & J. Osborne (Eds.),
Improving science education: The contribution of research. Buckingham: Open University Press.
American Association for the Advancement of Science. (1989). Science for all Americans. Washington, DC:
American Association for the Advancement of Science.
American Association for the Advancement of Science. (1993). Benchmarks for science literacy. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Belenky, M. F., Clinchy, B. M., Goldberger, N. R., & Tarule, J. M. (1986). Women’s ways of knowing: The
development of self, voice, and mind. New York: Basic Books.
Bell, R. L. (2003). Exploring the role of nature of science understanding in decision-making: Pipe dream or
possibility? In D. L. Zeidler (Ed.), The role of moral reasoning on socioscientific issues and discourse in
science education. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Press.
Bell, R. L. (2004). Perusing Pandora’s box: Exploring the what, when, and how of nature of science instruction,
In L. Flick & N. Lederman (Eds.), Scientific inquiry and nature of science: Implications for teaching, learning,
and teacher education. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Bell, R. L., & Lederman, N. G. (2003). Understandings of the nature of science and decision making on science
and technology based issues. Science Education, 87(3), 352 – 377.
Bell, R. L., Lederman, N. G., & Abd-El-Khalick, F. (2000). Developing and acting upon one’s conception of the
nature of science: A follow-up study. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37, 563 – 581.
Berkowitz, M. W. (1997). The complete moral person: Anatomy and formation. In James M. DuBois, (Ed.),
Moral issues in psychology: Personalist contributions to selected problems. New York: University Press of
America.
Berkowitz, M. W. (1998). Finding common ground to study and implement character education: Integrating
structure and content in moral education. Journal of Research in Education, 8(1), 3 – 8.
Berkowitz, M., & Oser, F. (1985). Moral education: Theory and applications. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
and Associates.
Berkowitz, M. W., Oser, F., & Althof, W. (1987). The development of sociomoral discourse. In W. M. Kurtines &
J. L. Gewitz (Eds.), Moral development through social interaction (pp. 337 – 345). New York: Wiley.
Berkowitz, M. W., & Simmons, P. (2003). Integrating science education and character education: The role of peer
discussion. In D. L. Zeidler (Ed.), The role of moral reasoning on socioscientific issues and discourse in science
education. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Press.
Bingle, W. H., & Gaskell, P. J. (1994). Scientific literacy for decisionmaking and the social construction of scientific
knowledge. Science Education, 78(2), 185 – 201.
Boyes, M. C., & Walker, L. J. (1988). Implications of cultural diversity for the universality claims of Kohlberg’s
theory of moral reasoning. Human Development, 31, 44 – 59.
Brickhouse, N. W. (2001). Embodying Science: A feminist perspective on learning. Journal of Research in Science
Teaching, 38, 282 – 295.
Bybee, Rodger (Ed.). (1985). Science – Technology – Society. In 1985 NSTA yearbook. Washington: National
Science Teachers Association.
Calabrese Barton, A. (1998a). Feminist science education. New York: Teachers College Press.
Calabrese Barton, A. (1998b). Teaching science with homeless children: Pedagogy, representation, and identity.
Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 35(4), 379 – 394.
Carpendale, J. I., & Krebs, D. L. (1992). Situational variation in moral judgment: In a stage or on a stage? Journal
of Youth and Adolescence, 21, 203 – 224.
Chiappetta, E. L., & Koballa, T. R. (2002). Science instruction in the middle and secondary schools (5th ed.).
Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.
374 ZEIDLER ET AL.
Clark, C. M. (2003). Care of the soul in teacher education. Paper presented at the American Association of Colleges
of Teacher Education, New Orleans, LA.
Cobern, W. W. (1993). Contextual constructivism: The impact of culture on the learning and teaching of science.
In K. G. Tobin (Ed.), The practice of constructivism in science education (pp. 51 – 69). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Cobern, W. W., & Loving, C. C. (2000). Defining “science” in a multicultural world: Implications for science
education. Science Education, 85, 50 – 67.
Cobern, W. W., & Loving, C. C. (2002). Investigation of preservice elementary teachers’ thinking about science.
Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39(10), 1016 – 1031.
Colby, A., & Kohlberg, L. (1987). The measurement of moral judgment. Vol. 2: Standard issue scoring manual.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Council of Ministers of Education Canada (CMEC) Pan Canadian Science Project. (1997). Common framework
of science learning outcomes: K-12. Available at URL: http://www.qscc.qld.edu.au/kla.sose.publicatons.html.
Donenberg, G., & Hoffman, L. (1988). Gender differences in moral development. Sex Roles, 18, 701 – 717.
Driver, R., Leach, J., Millar, R., & Scott P. (1996). Young people’s images of science. Bristol, PA: Open University
Press.
Driver, R., Newton, P., & Osborne, J. (2000). Establishing the norms of scientific argumentation in classrooms.
Science Education, 84(3), 287 – 312.
Endicott, L., Bock, T., & Narvaez, D. (2002). Learning processes at the intersection of ethical and intercultural
education. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New
Orleans, LA.
Foster, M. (1995). African American teachers and culturally relevant pedagogy. In J. A. Banks & C. A. McGee
Banks (Eds.), Handbook of research on multicultural education (pp. 570 – 581). New York: Macmillan.
Fuglsang, L. (2001). Three perspective in STS in the policy context. In S. H. Cutcliffe & C. Mitcham (Eds.),
Visions of STS: Counterpoints in science, technology, and science studies. Albany, NY: State University of
New York Press.
Gay, G. (2000). Culturally responsive teaching: Theory, research, and practice. New York: Teachers College Press.
Gilligan, C. (1987). Moral orientation and moral development. In E. Kittay & D. Meyers (Eds.), Women and moral
theory (pp. 19 – 33). Totowa, NJ: Rowman & Littlefield.
Goodlad, J. (2003). Teaching what we hold sacred. Educational Leadership, 61(4), 18 – 21.
Gruen, L. (1994). Toward an ecofeminist moral epistemology. In K. J. Warren (Ed.), Ecological feminism,
(pp. 120 – 138). New York: Routledge.
Harding, S. (1998). Is science multicultural?: Postcolonialisms, feminisms, and epistemologies. Bloomington, IN:
Indiana University Press.
Hekman, S. J. (1995). Moral voices, moral selves: Carol Gilligan and feminist moral theory. University Park, PA:
The Pennsylvania State University Press.
Hirsch, E. D., Jr. (1987). Cultural literacy: What every American needs to know. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Hodson, D. (1994). Seeking directions for change: The personalization and politicization of science education.
Curriculum Studies, 2, 71 – 98.
Hodson, D. (2003). Time for action: Science education for an alternative future. International Journal of Science
Education, 25(6), 645 – 670.
Hogan, K. (2002). Small groups’ ecological reasoning while making an environmental management decision.
Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39, 341 – 368.
Hollins, E. R., & Oliver, E. I. (1999). Pathways to school success: Culturally responsive teaching. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Howes, E. V. (2002). Connecting and science: Constructivism, feminism and science education reform. New York:
Teachers College Press.
Huges, G. (2000). Marginalization of socioscientific material in science – technology – society science curricula:
Some implication for gender inclusivity and curriculum reform. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37(5),
426 – 440.
Jenkins, E. W. (1990). Scientific literacy and school science education. School Science Review, 71(256),
43 – 51.
Jenkins, E. W. (1997). Towards a functional public understanding of science. In R. Levinson & J. Thomas (Eds.),
Science today (pp. 137 – 150). London: Routledge.
Jenkins, E. W. (2002). Linking school science education with action. In W. M. Toth & J. Desautels (Eds.), Science
education as/for sociopolitical action. New York: Peter Lang.
Keefer, M. W. (2002). Designing reflections on practice: Helping teachers apply cognitive learning princi-
ples in an SFT—inquiry-based learning program. Interchange: A Quarterly Review of Education, 33(4),
395 – 417.
MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 375
Keefer, M. W. (2003). Moral reasoning and case-based approaches to ethical instruction in science. In D. L. Zeidler
(Ed.), The role of moral reasoning on socioscientific issues and discourse in science education. Dordrecht:
Kluwer Academic Press.
Keefer, M. W., Zeitz, C. M., & Resnick, L. B. (2000). Judging the quality of peer-led student dialogues. Cognition
and Instruction, 18(1), 55 – 83.
Kohlberg, L. (1986). A current statement on some theoretical issues. In S. Modgil & C. Modgil (Eds.), Lawrence
Kohlberg: Consensus and controversy (pp. 485 – 546). Philadelphia: The Falmer Press.
Kolstø, S. D. (2000). Consensus projects: Teaching science for citizenship. International Journal of Science
Education, 22(6), 645 – 664.
Kolstø, S. D. (2001a). Scientific literacy for citizenship: Tools for dealing with the science dimension of contro-
versial socioscientific issues. Science Education, 85(3), 291 – 310.
Kolstø, S. D. (2001b). ‘To trust or not to trust, . . . ’—pupils’ ways of judging information encountered in a socio-
scientific issue. International Journal of Science Education, 23, 877 – 901.
Korpan, C. A., Bisanz, G. L., Bisanz, J., & Henderson, J. M. (1997). Assessing literacy in science: Evaluation of
scientific news briefs. Science Education, 81, 515 – 532.
Kozoll, R. H., & Osborne, M. D. (2004). Finding meaning in science: Lifeworld, identity, and self. Science
Education, 88: 157 – 181.
Kuhn, D. (1991). The skills of argument. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kuhn, D. (1993). Science as argument: Implications for teaching and learning scientific thinking. Science Educa-
tion, 77, 319 – 337.
Ladson-Billings, G. (1994). The dreamkeepers: Successful teachers of African American children. San Francisco,
CA: Jossey-Bass.
Latour, B. (1987). Science in action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Laugksch, R. C. (2000). Scientific literacy: A conceptual overview. Science Education, 84, 71 – 94.
Lemke, J. L. (2001). Articulating communities: Sociocultural perspectives on science education. Journal of Re-
search in Science Teaching, 38, 296 – 316.
Levinson, R. (2003, March). Teaching bioethics in science: Crossing a bridge too far? Paper presented
at the Annual Meeting of the National Association of Research in Science Teaching, Philadelphia,
PA.
Loving, C. C., Lowy, S. W., & Martin, C. (2003). Recognizing and solving ethical dilemmas in diverse science
classrooms. In D. L. Zeidler (Ed.), The role of moral reasoning on socioscientific issues and discourse in science
education. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Press.
Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. E. (1992). Science for student with disabilities. Review of Educational Research,
62, 377 – 412.
McGinnis, J. R. (2000). Teaching science as inquiry for students with disabilities. In J. Minstrell & E. van Zee
(Eds.), Inquiring into inquiry learning and teaching in science (pp. 425 – 433). Washington, DC: American
Association for the Advancement of Science.
National Research Council. (1996). National science education standards. Washington, DC: National Academy
Press.
National Science Teachers Association. (1982). Science – technology – society: Science education for the 1980’s.
Washington, DC: National Science Teachers Association.
Newton, P., Driver, R., & Osborne, J. (1999). The place of argumentation in the pedagogy of school science.
International Journal of Science Education, 21(5), 553 – 576.
Noddings, N. (1984). Caring: A feminine approach to ethics and moral education. Berkeley, CA: University of
California Press.
Nucci, L. P. (1989). Challenging conventional wisdom about morality: The domain approach to values education.
In L. P. Nucci (Ed.), Moral development and character education (pp. 183 – 203). Berkeley, CA: McCutchan
Publishing.
Nucci, L. P. (2001) Education in the moral domain. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Partington, G. (1997). Cultural invariance and the denial of moral regression: A critique of Piaget and Kohlberg.
International Journal of Social Education, 11, 106 – 119.
Pedretti, E. (1997). Septic tank crisis: A case study of science, technology and society education in an elementary
school International Journal of Science Education, 19(10), 1211 – 1230.
Pedretti, E. (2003). Teaching science, technology, society and environment (STSE) education: Preser-
vice teachers’ philosophical and pedagogical landscapes. In D. L. Zeidler (Ed.), The role of moral
reasoning on socioscientific issues and discourse in science education. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic
Press.
Pedretti, E., & Hodson, D. (1995). From rhetoric to action: Implementing STS education through action research.
Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 32, 463 – 485.
376 ZEIDLER ET AL.
Plumwood, V. (1993). Ethics and the instrumentalising self. In Feminism and the mastery of nature (pp. 141 –
164). New York: Routledge. Queensland School Curriculum Council (QSCC) (2001). Studies of society and
environment. Available at URL: http://www.cmec.ca/science/framework/index.htm.
Quintanar-Sarellana, R. (1997). Culturally relevant teacher preparation and teachers’ perceptions of
the language and culture of linguistic minority students. In J. E. King, E. R. Hollins, &
W. C. Hayman (Eds.), Preparing teachers for cultural diversity. New York: Teachers College
Press.
Ratcliffe, M. (1997). Pupil decision-making about socioscientific issues within the science curriculum. Interna-
tional Journal of Science Education, 19(2), 167 – 182.
Rest, J., Narvaez, Bebeau, M. J., & Thoma, S. J. (1999). Postconventional moral thinking: A neo-Kohlbergian
approach. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Roy, R. (1984). S-S/T/S Project: Teaching science via science, technology, and society material in the pre-college
years. University Park, PA: The Pennsylvania State University.
Ryder, J. (2001). Identifying science understanding for functional scientific literacy. Studies in Science Education,
36, 1 – 46.
Sadler, T. D. (2004). Informal reasoning regarding socioscientific issues: A critical review of research. Journal of
Research in Science Teaching, 41, 513 – 536.
Sadler, T. D., Chambers, F. W., & Zeidler, D. L. (2004). Student conceptualizations of the nature of science in
response to a socioscientific issue. International Journal of Science Education, 26, 387 – 409.
Sadler, T. D., & Zeidler, D. L. (2004). The morality of socioscientific issues construal and resolution of genetic
engineering dilemmas. Science Education, 88(1), 4 – 27.
Sadler, T. D., & Zeidler, D. L. (2005). The significance of content knowledge for informal reasoning regarding
socioscientific issues: Applying genetics knowledge to genetic engineering issues. Science Education, 89,
71 – 93.
Sadler, T. D., & Zeidler, D. L. (in press). Patterns of informal reasoning in the context of socioscientific decision-
making. Journal of Research in Science Teaching.
Seiler, G. (2001). Reversing the “standard” direction: Science emerging from the lives of African American
students. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38, 1000 – 1014.
Settelmaier, E. (2003, March). Dilemmas with dilemmas: Exploring the suitability of dilemma stories as a way
of addressing ethical issues in science education. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National
Association of Research in Science Teaching, Philadelphia, PA.
Shamos, M. H. (1995). The myth of scientific literacy. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Simmons, M. L., & Zeidler, D. L. (2003). Beliefs in the nature of science and responses to socioscientific issues.
In D. L. Zeidler (Ed.), The role of moral reasoning on socioscientific issues and discourse in science education.
Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Simonneaux, L. (2001). Role-play or debate to promote students’ argumentation and justification on an issue in
animal transgenesis. International Journal of Science Education, 23, 903 – 927.
Smith, M. (2004 June). Culturally responsive teaching strategies for Native students. Paper presented at the
Integrating Mathematics and Science Education Research into Teaching Conference, University of Maine,
Orono, ME.
Snarey, J. R. (1985). Cross-cultural universality of social-moral development: A critical review of Kohlbergian
research. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 202 – 232.
Tate, William F. (1995). Returning to the root: A culturally relevant approach to mathematics pedagogy. Theory
into Practice, 34(3), 166 – 173.
Tong, R. (1996). Feminist approaches to bioethics. In S. M. Wolf (Ed.), Feminism and bioethics: Beyond repro-
duction. New York: Oxford University Press.
Toulmin, S. (1958). The uses of argument. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Trowbridge, L. W., Bybee, R. W., & Powell, J. C. (2000). Teaching secondary school science: Strategies for
developing scientific literacy (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Tsai, C. (2000). The effects of STS-oriented instruction on female tenth graders’ cognitive structure outcomes
and the role of student scientific epistemological beliefs. International Journal of Science Education, 22,
1099 – 1115.
Turiel, E. (1998). The development of morality. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child
psychology (pp. 863 – 932). New York: Wiley.
Vellom, P. (1999). Reasoning about data in middle school science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 36(2),
179 – 199.
Walker, K. A., & Zeidler, D. L. (2003, March). Students’ understanding of the nature of science and their reasoning
on socioscientific issues: A Web-based learning inquiry. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National
Association of Research in Science Teaching, Philadelphia, PA.
MORALITY AND SOCIOSCIENTIFIC ISSUES IN SCIENCE EDUCATION 377
Wark, G., & Krebs, D. L. (1996). Gender and dilemma differences in real-life moral judgment. Developmental
Psychology, 32, 220 – 230.
Yager, R. E. (1996). History of science/technology/society as reform in the United States. In R. E. Yager (Ed.),
Science/technology/society as reform in science education. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Zeidler, D. L. (1984). Moral issues and social policy in science education: Closing the literacy gap. Science
Education, 68, 411 – 419.
Zeidler, D. L. (1997). The central role of fallacious thinking in science education. Science Education, 81, 483 – 496.
Zeidler, D. L. (2001). Participating in program development: Standard F. In D. Siebert & W. McIntosh (Eds.),
College pathways to the science education standards (pp. 18 – 22). Arlington, VA: National Science Teachers
Press.
Zeidler, D. L., & Keefer, M. (2003). The role of moral reasoning and the status of socioscientific issues in science
education: Philosophical, psychological and pedagogical considerations. In D. L. Zeidler (Ed.), The role of
moral reasoning on socioscientific issues and discourse in science education. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic
Publishers.
Zeidler, D. L., Lederman, N. G., & Taylor, S. C. (1992). Fallacies and student discourse: Conceptualizing the role
of critical thinking in science education. Science Education, 76, 437 – 450.
Zeidler, D. L., Osborne, J., Erduran, S., Simon, S., & Monk, M. (2003). The role of argument and fallacies during
discourse about socioscientific issues. In D. L. Zeidler (Ed.), The role of moral reasoning on socioscientific
issues and discourse in science education. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Press.
Zeidler, D. L., & Schafer, L. E. (1984). Identifying mediating factors of moral reasoning in science education.
Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 21(1), 1 – 15.
Zeidler, D. L., Walker, K. A., Ackett, W. A., & Simmons, M. L. (2002). Tangled up in views: Beliefs in the nature
of science and responses to socioscientific dilemmas. Science Education, 86(3), 343 – 367.
Ziman, J. (1994). The rationale of STS education is in the approach. In J. Solomon & G. Aikenhead (Eds.), STS
education: International perspectives on reform. New York: Teachers College Press.
Zohar, A., & Nemet, F. (2002). Fostering students’ knowledge and argumentation skills through dilemmas in
human genetics. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39, 35 – 62.