The Impact of School On EFL Learning Motivation: An Indonesian Case Study
The Impact of School On EFL Learning Motivation: An Indonesian Case Study
The Impact of School On EFL Learning Motivation: An Indonesian Case Study
RESEARCH METHODOLOGY
1
One learner moved to the capital with her family between the second and third field visit,
and her data have been excluded from this report.
FINDINGS
Mean standard
deviation Cohen’s d
Difference for effect
N Aug 02 Mar 04 t-value in means Significance size
Satisfaction with progress in English so far 192 2.58 2.31 −4.734 −0.27 .000* −0.46
(1 = not satisfied; 2 = satisfied; 3 = very satisfied) 0.56 0.62
Expectation of ultimate success in English 190 2.39 2.37 −0.346 −0.02 .730 −0.04
(1 = not confident; 2 = reasonably confident; 3 = confident) 0.52 0.56
Attitude toward experience of learning English 195 2.83 2.47 −7.766 −0.36 .000* −0.74
(1 = not happy; 2 = OK; 3 = happy) 0.38 0.59
Perceived importance of English (1 = not important; 188 2.61 2.71 2.248 +0.10 .026 0.20
2 = important; 3 = very important) 0.52 0.47
Reasons for importance (1 = not important; 2 = important; 3 = very important)
Because I need English for my career in the future 189 2.69 2.84 3.610 +0.15 .000* 0.33
(instrumental) 0.52 0.39
Because I enjoy learning English (intrinsic) 186 2.27 2.12 −3.307 −0.16 .001* −0.32
0.50 0.50
Because I want to meet foreigners & learn about 190 2.49 2.35 −2.270 −0.14 .024 −0.21
other countries (integrative) 0.62 0.69
Because my parents encourage me to learn English 186 2.46 2.54 1.735 +0.09 .084 0.15
(extrinsic—parental) 0.60 0.58
Because English is an assessed school subject 186 2.25 2.53 4.745 +0.28 .000* 0.43
(extrinsic—academic) 0.69 0.62
Perceived importance of English compared with 188 2.26 2.29 0.842 +0.04 .401 0.09
other school subjects (1, less important; 2, same; 0.44 0.47
3, more important)
TESOL QUARTERLY
(+0.15), whereas its intrinsic interest and value for getting to know for-
eigners and other countries are less prioritized now (−0.16 and −0.14,
respectively).
A finding from the first phase of data collection and analysis in August
2002 (reported in Lamb, 2004b) was that pupils’ learning and use of
English were not confined to formal school. At the end of the research
period, pupils were again asked to report how often they used English in
out-of-school activities. Results indicated that all forms of this activity had
increased over this period, with the largest increases appearing in com-
puter usage involving English (+0.41 on a scale of 0–3) and watching
English language TV programmes or videos (+0.38), though listening to
English language songs remained the single most popular activity, en-
gaged in daily by over a third of all pupils. Speaking English was still
quite a rare event, with few doing it more than once a month, and
according to interview comments, their conversants were almost always
other Indonesians (e.g., older siblings or parents) rather than native
speakers or other foreigners, of whom there are very few in the city. In
addition to these informal activities, 54% of pupils had taken a private
English course over the last 20 months, usually at a local language school
(25 different institutions were mentioned) but sometimes at home with
a tutor.
2
Quotations are translated except where stated.
clearly”). Among those who said they were happy, the most frequent
comments also related to the teacher’s style or methods and praised
different aspects of lessons (e.g., “Here the teacher explains carefully,
not just using English but with Indonesian too”; “the lessons are enjoy-
able and not too tense”; “The teachers here don’t just stick to the ma-
terial but also give practice, like speaking, listening and the rest”). A
similar shift in learners’ thinking is evident in responses to the final item
on both questionnaires, which invited pupils to make any further com-
ments or put questions to the researcher. At the beginning of the re-
search period, the most frequent comments (34%) were statements
about the importance of English; at the end, the most frequent com-
ments (30%) were questions about how to learn it well (e.g., “I want to
ask, is there an easier way to learn to become fluent in English besides
learning by heart and looking words up in the dictionary?”).
During their second year of study, pupils were placed in eight differ-
ent classes, with three different teachers. An analysis of variance (Tukey
post hoc test) was carried out to find out if there were significant differ-
ences in the responses of classes taught by different teachers. The only
item where a clear difference was found (significant at the 0.05 level) was
on attitudes toward the experience of learning English, where one
teacher was found to generate much higher ratings than the other two.
Mirroring the survey results, all the focal learners, whether apparently
active learners or not, appeared to recognise the potential importance of
English for their futures. As one less motivated learner (J) put it in his
first interview, when asked what his ambitions were: “To be good at
English, because in the future, according to my parents, globalization is
going to happen . . . . Western people are going to come to Indonesia,
In the last two interview phases there was evidence of learners becom-
ing more aware of their own motivation and trying to regulate it. Learner
D had portrayed herself in the first two interviews as a highly motivated
learner who believed she was making progress in her English. In her last
interview, however, she made the critical comments above about the
teacher and when asked whether English was more or less important to
her, she replied “sure, more important, but now I feel so-so . . .” (original
English). She seemed able to make a distinction between the objective
importance of English to her future and her feelings, which she knew
were temporary and related to her class teacher. One of the more mo-
tivated boys (K) acknowledged that, whereas his motivation had not
changed, he hadn’t been making the necessary efforts to learn outside of
class: “I think I’m becoming lazy . . . . because my friends always calling
me to bermain [play]” (original English), and he went on to say he must
study harder for a state exam in 2 months time. Another learner (F)
explained at the start of her last interview that she did not like her
current teacher because she got angry quickly. Asked whether her peers
thought the same, she said “Maybe just me, because they don’t like
English . . . . they think English is very difficult.” In other words, it was
because she cared about English so much that she felt so negatively about
the teacher. For now, she put her energies into her private English
DISCUSSION
Over this 20-month period of junior high school, aspects of the learn-
ers’ motivation to learn English seem to have been relatively constant,
whereas others changed. Throughout the interviews with the focal learn-
ers and the comments in the questionnaires, there was a consistent
strong recognition of the long-term value of English for their own and
indeed for their country’s future. There was a small rise, in fact, in their
instrumental orientation, whereas their integrative orientation fell
slightly. However, as the learners spent more time in classrooms, the
process of learning assumed greater weight in their motivational think-
ing. There was a significant increase in the number who felt that they
were not making enough progress and a significant drop in the numbers
of learners who were happy with their school English lessons. At the same
time, though, the general expectation of ultimate success in English
remained constant, perhaps reflecting a sense that school was only one
of many possible venues for learning the language.
Although caution must be shown in interpreting results from single-
item instruments, it appears that general variables such as instrumental
and integrative orientations are less susceptible to change than class-
room-related variables, a finding in line with results obtained by Gardner
et al. (2004). These results were confirmed by interview data, in which
the more motivated learners could be surprisingly forthright in their
criticism of English lessons while continuing to assert their belief in the
importance of the language and their personal desire to learn it.
CONCLUSION
The study has found that during their first 20 months in junior high
school, pupils’ attitudes toward English, particularly their view of its
personal and societal relevance, were relatively stable, whereas attitudes
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I would like to thank Lynne Cameron, Gary Chambers, Mike Baynham, and four
anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments on earlier versions of this article,
and Hywel Coleman for many stimulating discussions of the context.
THE AUTHOR
REFERENCES
Anderman, E. M., & Maehr, M. L. (1994). Motivation and schooling in the middle
grades. Review of Educational Research, 64, 287–309.
Bempechat, J., & Boulay, B. A. (2001). Beyond dichotomous characterizations of
student learning. In D. McInerney & S. Van Etten (Eds.), Research on sociocultural
influences on motivation and learning. Volume 1 (pp. 15–36). Greenwich, CT: Infor-
mation Age.
Bourdieu, P. (1991). Language and symbolic power (G. Raymond & M. Adamson,
Trans.). Cambridge: Polity Press.
Chambers, G. (1999). Motivating language learners. Clevedon, England: Multilingual
Matters.
Chen, J. F., Warden, C. A., & Chang, H.-T. (2005). Motivators that do not motivate:
The case of Chinese EFL learners and the influence of culture on motivation.
TESOL Quarterly, 39, 609–633.
Note: In this context, father’s job is still a better index of socioeconomic status than mother’s job, though this is beginning to change. The Elite II class
was formed midway through the first academic year; Learners A and D were originally in regular classes. Family English is as reported in the initial
questionnaire—the degree of competence was not specified.
779
APPENDIX B
Interview Guide
• Choice of language to use in interview