Journal
Journal
Journal
Syndrome
Francesca Foti1,2*., Deny Menghini3., Laura Mandolesi4,2, Francesca Federico1, Stefano Vicari3,
Laura Petrosini5,2
1 Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University ‘‘Sapienza’’ of Rome, Rome, Italy, 2 IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Rome, Italy, 3 Child
Neuropsychiatry Unit, Neuroscience Department, ‘‘Children’s Hospital Bambino Gesu’’’, Rome, Rome, Italy, 4 School of Movement Sciences (DiSIST), University of Naples
‘‘Parthenope’’, Naples, Italy, 5 Department of Psychology, University ‘‘Sapienza’’ of Rome, Rome, Italy
Abstract
Observing another person performing a complex action accelerates the observer’s acquisition of the same action and limits
the time-consuming process of learning by trial and error. Observational learning makes an interesting and potentially
important topic in the developmental domain, especially when disorders are considered. The implications of studies aimed
at clarifying whether and how this form of learning is spared by pathology are manifold. We focused on a specific
population with learning and intellectual disabilities, the individuals with Williams syndrome. The performance of twenty-
eight individuals with Williams syndrome was compared with that of mental age- and gender-matched thirty-two typically
developing children on tasks of learning of a visuo-motor sequence by observation or by trial and error. Regardless of the
learning modality, acquiring the correct sequence involved three main phases: a detection phase, in which participants
discovered the correct sequence and learned how to perform the task; an exercise phase, in which they reproduced the
sequence until performance was error-free; an automatization phase, in which by repeating the error-free sequence they
became accurate and speedy. Participants with Williams syndrome beneficiated of observational training (in which they
observed an actor detecting the visuo-motor sequence) in the detection phase, while they performed worse than typically
developing children in the exercise and automatization phases. Thus, by exploiting competencies learned by observation,
individuals with Williams syndrome detected the visuo-motor sequence, putting into action the appropriate procedural
strategies. Conversely, their impaired performances in the exercise phases appeared linked to impaired spatial working
memory, while their deficits in automatization phases to deficits in processes increasing efficiency and speed of the
response. Overall, observational experience was advantageous for acquiring competencies, since it primed subjects’ interest
in the actions to be performed and functioned as a catalyst for executed action.
Citation: Foti F, Menghini D, Mandolesi L, Federico F, Vicari S, et al. (2013) Learning by Observation: Insights from Williams Syndrome. PLoS ONE 8(1): e53782.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0053782
Editor: Tricia A. Thornton-Wells, Vanderbilt University, United States of America
Received July 12, 2012; Accepted December 4, 2012; Published January 10, 2013
Copyright: ß 2013 Foti et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted
use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Funding: This work was supported by the Fondation Jérôme Lejeune to L.M. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to
publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
* E-mail: [email protected]
. These authors contributed equally to this work.
function of dorsal stream network [26,27], WS individuals appear The clinical diagnosis of WS was confirmed by fluorescence in
to be the ideal participants to investigate the cognitive processes situ hybridization (FISH) genetic investigation, which showed the
involved in the observational learning. Performances of a group of characteristic deletion on chromosome band 7q11.23. WS
WS individuals were compared with those of a mental age- and participants were part of a larger pool of individuals with learning
gender-matched group of typically developing (TD) children on a disabilities attending the Children’s Hospital Bambino Gesù of
task requiring the learning of a visuo-motor sequence. The Rome for clinical and rehabilitative follow-up. All of them lived at
participants learned the sequence either by performing the task home with their families. The parents of all individuals who
after observing an actor detect the sequence of correct items by participated in the study provided written informed consent. This
trial and error (observational training) or by actually performing study was approved by the Ethic Committee of the Children’s
the task by trial and error (Fig. 1). Hospital Bambino Gesù of Rome and conducted according to the
Helsinki declaration.
Materials and Methods WS individuals were tested in a quiet room at the Children’s
Hospital Bambino Gesù. TD children were individually tested in a
Participants quiet room at their schools.
Twenty-eight WS participants and 32 TD children (used as
controls) matching the WS individuals for mental age (MA) have Intelligence Evaluation and Neuropsychological
been examined in the present study constituted by two experi- Assessment
mental conditions: Learning by Trial and Error followed by In the present study, the brief version of the Leiter International
Observational Learning (Condition 1); Observational Learning Performance Scale–Revised [28] was employed (four out of 10
followed by Learning by Trial and Error (Condition 2) (Table 1). subtests: Figure Ground, Form Completion, Sequential Order and
Only WS individuals with mental age (MA) of at least 5 years were Repeated Patterns). The brief IQ and the corresponding mental
included in the present research because participants with inferior ages were computed. Visuo-motor integration [29], visuo-spatial
MA did not succeed in completing the task. No significant perception [30] and memory [31] were assessed (Table 3).
differences in chronological age (CA), MA and IQ (P always .0.2)
among participants performing Conditions 1 and 2 were found Experimental Procedure
(Table 2). Each participant was sat in front of a computer touch screen
(distance 60 cm). In both Conditions, the experimenter acting as
Figure 1. Schematic diagrams of the two experimental conditions. Condition 1: Learning by Trial and Error followed by Observational
Learning: participants detected a sequence by trial and error (TE1), then they observed an actor detecting a sequence different from the one they had
previously detected (observational training) and, finally, they reproduced the observed sequence (OBS2). Condition 2: Observational Learning
followed by Learning by Trial and Error: participants were submitted to an observational training, then they reproduced the observed sequence
(OBS1) and, finally, detected by trial and error a different sequence they had never observed (TE2). The incorrect positions touched by the actor
during the observational training are evidenced in grey. S: starting point; F: final point.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0053782.g001
Table 1. Description of WS groups (WS1 and WS2) and TD groups (TD1 and TD2) performing the two different experimental
conditions.
CA MA IQ
Group Number Gender Mean ± SEM Mean ± SEM Mean ± SEM
the actor (F.F.) was sat near the participant. A 868 black matrix that all participants observed the same pattern of correct and
appeared on the touch screen. The subject was asked to find a incorrect touches. Two minutes after the end of the observational
hidden sequence of ‘‘correct’’ squares prepared in advance by the training the participants were required to actually reproduce the
experimenters. The sequence was composed of 10 adjacent spatial observed correct sequence.
positions in the matrix, which formed a ‘‘snake-like’’ pattern The tasks involved three phases: the Detection Phase (DP) that
(Fig.1). To explain the task to each participant the experimenter ended once the participants found the tenth correct position; the
used the same verbal instructions: ‘‘You have to find a route Exercise Phase (EP) in which they had to repeat the 10-item
formed by ten squares. When you touch a correct square it will be sequence until their performance was error-free; the Automatiza-
turned grey and you will hear a sound; conversely, if you touch a tion Phase (AP) that ended when the correct sequence was
wrong square, it will be turned red. In this case, you have to find a repeated three consecutive times without errors.
new grey square. You have to start the route each time you find a
new correct square. After finding the whole route, you have to re- Parameters
touch it three times without making lighted red squares’’. The Error parameters: DP errors, calculated as the number of
participants started touching a grey square, which was the first incorrect items touched in detecting the ten correct positions; EP
element of the sequence and was always lit up. In the search for repetitions, calculated as the number of replications needed to reach
the second correct square, the participants had to touch one of the the error-free performance. Time parameters: AP times (in msec),
four squares bordering the grey square by moving in the matrix calculated as the time spent carrying out each of the three
vertically or horizontally, but never diagonally. Each touched repetitions of the sequence.
square (correct or incorrect) was lit up for 500 ms and then lighted
off again; thus, no trace of the performed sequence remained on
the screen. In learning the sequence by trial and error, the Analysis of Error
participants tried to find the correct sequence immediately after To assess the kind of error further parameters were taken into
the verbal instructions. Conversely, in the observational learning account considering the two phases DP and EP together: the
task after the verbal instructions the participants observed the number of sequence errors, as touching a ‘‘correct’’ square in
experimenter while she detected a 10-item sequence by trial and ‘‘wrong’’ moment (e.g. touching E7 before than F7); side-by-side
error (observational training). The experimenter performed the errors, as touching the squares bordering the ‘‘correct’’ sequence
task by always making the same errors in the same positions, so (e.g. E8); illogical errors, as touching any other square (e.g. B5);
Table 2. Comparisons of chronological age (CA), mental age (MA) and Intelligence Quotient (IQ) between WS groups (WS1 and
WS2) and TD groups (TD1 and TD2) that performed the two different experimental conditions.
CA MA IQ
Group Mean ± SEM F (freedom degrees) Mean ± SEM F (freedom degrees) Mean ± SEM F (freedom degrees)
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0053782.t002
WS TD
Mean ± SEM Mean ± SEM Effect F(1, 58) value P
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0053782.t003
(F(2,50) = 154.54, P,0.0001). The interaction was not significant Attentional Task
(F(4,50) = 0.13, P = 0.96). In the tasks of observational learning Two-way ANOVAs (group6block) on reaction times or
(OBS1–OBS2) the MANOVA also revealed a not significant sub- response accuracy of the WS and TD groups revealed no
group effect (F(2,25) = 0.47, P = 0.62) and a significant parameter attentional decay in both groups, as indicated by not significant
effect (F(2,50) = 85.46, P,0.0001). The interaction was not signif- difference in the reaction times in the seven blocks (F(6,348) = 1.55,
icant (F(4,50) = 0.47, P = 0.75). Thus, we pooled together the 28 P = 0.15). A similar result was obtained when response accuracy
differently aged WS individuals. was analyzed (F(6,348) = 1.80, P = 0.10). Notably, a significant
All statistical analyses were performed by using Statistica 8.0 for difference was found between WS and TD groups on reaction
Windows and the significance level was established at P,0.05. times (F(1,58) = 13.52, P = 0.00051), given WS participants pressed
the keys at the appearance of the stimulus more quickly than TD
Results children (Fig. 5).
Figure 2. Performances exhibited by WS and TD participants in the two experimental conditions. DP: Detection Phase; EP: Exercise
Phase; AP: Automatization Phase. Data are expressed as mean 6 SEM. The asterisks indicate the significance level of post hoc comparisons between
groups (***P,0.001).
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0053782.g002
interconnections with cortical structures [34,35,36,37]. The cause By analyzing the kind of errors, some remarks can be made.
of automatization and procedural deficits of WS individuals could First of all, both groups made a very low number of illogical errors,
be their remarkable hypoplasia of the basal ganglia [38] and the thus suggesting all participants similarly managed task fundamen-
disproportionate enlargement of the cerebellum [39,40,41,42,43]. tals (Fig. 3 and 4). As for imitative errors, no difference between
Indeed, in WS individuals skill-learning abilities are impaired, as groups was found, thus suggesting participants did imitate but did
revealed by their performance in Tower of London test [21], not hyperimitate [47]. Conversely, WS individuals made more
Serial Reaction Time task [22] and Radial or Multiple Reward sequence and side-by-side errors than TD children in TE1,
Mazes [44,45,46]. particularly when a change of direction was required. Errors in
Figure 3. Errors exhibited by WS and TD participants in the two experimental conditions. Data are expressed as mean 6 SEM. The
asterisks indicate the significance level of post hoc comparisons between groups (*P,0.05; ** P,0.005; *** P,0.001).
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0053782.g003
stopping the more easy ‘‘keep-straight’’ response and performing The prevalently frontal processes require a modulation in
the more demanding ‘‘turn-left’’ response resulted by the WS more posterior brain systems, via the attention networks. Basic
difficulty in suppressing a previously correct but then inappropri- aspects of attentional processing are selective spatial attention
ate response. Correctly responding requires executive control that allows maintaining the focus of processing between spatial
processes based on frontal lobe function, as response inhibition, locations, and the attentional processing that allows a kind of
cognitive flexibility and attentional shifting [48,49,50,51]. WS ‘‘selection for action’’ [58,59,60]. Namely, the action of
individuals are impaired in spatial planning, working memory, reaching the right square required attentional modulation to
cognitive flexibility and inhibiting well-learned responses become plan, select and initiate the appropriate behavior, to direct it
inappropriate to the situation [52]. Indeed, the executive function toward the selected goal, and to inhibit actions inappropriate for
deficits that impaired WS performance dramatically reduced after the current goal. Because many brain structures that are part of
the observational training, once more indicating the teaching the attention networks are included within the dorsal stream
power of the observation. WS participants made perseverative network [27], it is not surprising that WS participants
errors that could result from difficulties in withholding the performed more errors when behavioral inhibition and attentive
inappropriate repetition of a response despite knowing that it shifting were required but no help from observing the actor was
was not the correct one. This is an important component of top- provided [61]. The ‘‘dorsal-stream vulnerability’’ in WS [62] is
down executive control. Notably, perseverations may be symptom manifested not only in the spatial and visual processing
not only of prefrontal dysfunction but also of cerebellar and basal occurring within the occipital and parietal areas but also in
ganglia damage provoking ‘‘frontal-like’’ cognitive deficits the processing of spatial information by frontal control systems,
[34,53,54,55,56,57]. as reported in an fMRI study [63].
Figure 4. Incorrect items touched on the screen by WS and TD participants in performing the tasks. On the right, the chromatic scale
indicates the sum of incorrectly touched items (brown and blue denote maximal and minimal values, respectively). S: starting point; F: final point.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0053782.g004
At the end of testing, mental representative mapping abilities performed to detect rules and sequence. In fact, the present
were evaluated by drawing the arrangement of squares just results indicate that the observation of action has a strong
discovered. WS and TD participants were similarly able to impact on action memory. The influence of action perception
represent the shape of the ‘‘snake’’. This finding is consistent with on action production requires cross-modal information be
the observation that WS individuals exhibit no difficulty in coordinated. Action and perception share the distal reference
mentally visualizing objects without spatially manipulating them and are coded in a common representational medium [65], so
[25,64] and supports the indication that the present learning that perceiving an action activates the corresponding motor
protocols encompassed requests of visual imagery. representation within the observer automatically and without
If observative experience functions as a catalyst for executed conscious effort [66,67,68].
action, it can be advanced that observing a sequence prior to The close interplay between observation and execution of
experiencing it primes subjects’ interest in the actions to be actions found in the present study is supported by studies
providing evidence of a striking overlap in the brain systems
recruited for one’s own action, observation of others’ action and
imitation of action [69]. In particular, when imitation is aimed at
learning novel actions, the activation of the ‘‘core circuit for
imitation’’ [70] involving the inferior frontal gyrus, the inferior
parietal lobule and the superior temporal sulcus seems to be
integrated with activation of the dorso-lateral and ventro-medial
prefrontal cortex, for selection of motor acts and error prediction
[71], of the premotor areas, for motor preparation [70,72,73] as
well as of cerebellar areas, whether or not it is accompanied by
actual motor acts [74,75,76].
The existence of direct feed-forward connections from
perceptual to motor processes allows observation sculpting
motor abilities by exploiting the functional overlapping between
perception and action systems. It has been suggested that
observation of actions engages motor-related processes similar to
those of actual execution, promoting the development of an
efference copy of the descending motor commands, which in
combination with a forward model provides a prediction of
sensory consequences [77,78,79,80]. Thus, action observation,
efficiently translated into the matching motor representation,
powerfully activates the feed-forward predictive processes, so
that learning does occur. Notably, even in WS individuals the
Figure 5. Performances exhibited by WS and TD participants in beneficial effect of observation was evident although linked only
the Attentional Task. Data are expressed as mean 6 SEM. to the DP. Action observation seems to result in an amelioration
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0053782.g005 of frontal functions, as motor strategy planning, decision-making
References
1. Leggio MG, Molinari M, Neri P, Graziano A, Mandolesi L, et al. (2000) 27. Rizzolatti G, Matelli M (2003) Two different streams form the dorsal visual
Representation of actions in rats: the role of cerebellum in learning spatial system: anatomy and functions. Exp Brain Res 153: 146–157.
performances by observation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97: 2320–2325. 28. Roid GH, Miller LJ (2002) Leiter – R, Leiter International Performance Scale–
2. Petrosini L (2007) ‘‘Do what I do’’ and ‘‘do how I do’’: different components of Revised. Giunti O.S. Organizzazioni Speciali, Firenze.
imitative learning are mediated by different neural structures. Neuroscientist 13: 29. Beery KE (2000) VMI: Developmental test of visual-motor integration. Italian
335–348. Adaptation. Organizzazioni Speciali, Firenze.
3. Barr R, Dowden A, Hayne H (1996) Developmental changes in deferred 30. Hammill D, Pearson N, Voress J (1994) TPV. Test di percezione visiva e
Imitation by 6- to 24-month-old infants. Infant Behav Dev 19: 159–170. integrazione visuo-motoria. Erickson, Trento.
4. Bird G, Heyes C (2005) Effector-dependent learning by observation of a finger 31. Vicari S (2007) In O. S. Giunti (Ed.), PROMEA: Prove di memoria e
movement sequence. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31: 262–275. apprendimento. Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali.
5. Meltzoff AN, Kuhl PK, Movellan J, Sejnowski TJ (2009) Foundations for a new 32. Jarrold C, Brock J (2004) To match or not to match? Methodological issues in
science of learning. Science 325: 284–288. autism-related research. J Autism Dev Disord. 34: 81–86.
6. Meltzoff AN, Andrew N (1995) Understanding the intentions of others: re- 33. Hubert V, Beaunieux H, Chételat G, Platel H, Landeau B, et al. (2007) The
enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Dev Psychol 31: 838–850. dynamic network subserving the three phases of cognitive procedural learning.
7. Gallese V, Goldman A (1998) Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of Hum Brain Mapp 28: 1415–1429.
mind-reading. Trends Cogn Sci 2: 493–501. 34. Seidler RD, Purushotham A, Kim SG, Ugurbil K, Willingham D, et al. (2005)
8. Rizzolatti G, Luppino G (2001) The cortical motor system. Neuron 31: 889–901. Neural correlates of encoding and expression in implicit sequence learning. Exp
9. Meltzoff AN, Decety J (2003) What imitation tells us about social cognition: a Brain Res 165: 114–124.
rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. 35. Torriero S, Oliveri M, Koch G, Caltagirone C, Petrosini L (2007) The what and
Philosophical Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 358: 491–500. how of observational learning. J Cogn Neurosci 19: 1656–1663.
10. Van Der Werf YD, Van Der Helm E, Schoonheim MM, Ridderikhoff A, Van 36. Torriero S, Oliveri M, Koch G, Lo Gerfo E, Salerno S, et al. (2011) Changes in
Someren EJ (2009) Learning by observation requires an early sleep window. cerebello-motor connectivity during procedural learning by actual execution and
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106: 18926–18930. observation. J Cogn Neurosci 23: 338–348.
11. Meltzoff AN, Moore MK (1997) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by 37. Doeller CF, King JA, Burgess N (2008) Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems
human neonates. Science 198: 75–78. for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:
12. Nadel J, Butterworth G (1998) Imitation in infancy. Cambridge: Cambridge 5915–5920.
University Press.
38. Jernigan TL, Bellugi U (1990) Anomalous brain morphology on magnetic
13. Nadel J (2002) The imitative mind. In Meltzoff A Prinz W editors. Cambridge: resonance images in Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. Arch Neurol 47:
Cambridge University Press. pp. 42–62.
529–533.
14. Kokkinakki T, Kugiumutzakis G (2000) Basic aspects of vocal imitation in
39. Reiss AL, Eliez S, Schmitt JE, Straus E, Lai Z, et al. (2000) IV. Neuroanatomy of
infant–parent interaction during the first 6 months. J Reprod Infant Psychol 18:
Williams syndrome: a high-resolution MRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 12: 65–73.
173–187.
40. Jones W, Hesselink J, Courchesne E, Duncan T, Matsuda K, et al. (2002)
15. Fenstermacher SK, Saudino KJ (2007) Toddler see, toddler do? Genetic and
Cerebellar abnormalities in infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome. Dev
environmental influences on laboratory-assessed elicited imitation. Behav Genet
Med Child Neurol 44: 688–694.
37: 639–647.
41. Chiang MC, Reiss AL, Lee AD, Bellugi U, Galaburda AM, et al. (2007) 3D
16. Menghini D, Vicari S, Mandolesi L, Petrosini L (2011a) Is learning by
pattern of brain abnormalities in Williams syndrome visualized using tensor-
observation impaired in children with dyslexia? Neuropsychologia 49: 1996–
2003. based morphometry. Neuroimage 36: 1096–1109.
17. Klein B, Mervis C (1999) Cognitive strengths and weaknesses of 9- and 10-year- 42. Menghini D, Di Paola M, Federico F, Vicari S, Petrosini L, et al. (2011b)
olds with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome. Dev Neuropsycho 16: 177– Relationship between brain abnormalities and cognitive profile in Williams
196. syndrome. Behav Genet 41: 394–402.
18. Bellugi U, Lichtenberger L, Jones W, Lai Z, St George MI (2000) The 43. Schmitt JE, Eliez S, Warsofsky IS, Bellugi U, Reiss AL (2001) Corpus callosum
neurocognitive profile of Williams Syndrome: a complex pattern of strengths and morphology of Williams syndrome: relation to genetics and behavior. Dev Med
weaknesses. J Cogn Neurosci 12: 7–29. Child Neurol 43: 155–159.
19. Atkinson J, Anker S, Braddick O, Nokes L, Mason A, et al. (2001) Visual and 44. Mandolesi L, Addona F, Foti F, Menghini D, Petrosini L, et al. (2009) Spatial
visuospatial development in young children with Williams syndrome. Dev Med competences in Williams syndrome: a radial arm maze study. Int J Dev Neurosci
Child Neurol 43: 330–337. 27: 205–213.
20. Atkinson J, Braddick O, Anker S, Curran W, Andrew R, et al. (2003) 45. Foti F, Menghini D, Petrosini L, Valerio G, Crinò A, et al. (2011) Spatial
Neurobiological models of visuospatial cognition in children with Williams competences in Prader-Willi syndrome: a radial arm maze study. Behav Genet
syndrome: measures of dorsal-stream and frontal function. Dev Neuropsychol 41: 445–456.
23: 139–172. 46. Foti F, Petrosini L, Cutuli D, Menghini D, Chiarotti F, et al. (2011) Explorative
21. Vicari S, Bellucci S, Carlesimo GA (2001) Procedural learning deficit in children function in Williams syndrome analyzed through a large-scale task with multiple
with Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia 39: 665–677. rewards. Res Dev Disabil 32: 972–985.
22. Vicari S, Verucci L, Carlesimo GA (2007) Implicit memory is independent from 47. Iacoboni M, Woods RP, Brass M, Bekkering H, Mazziotta JC, et al. (1999)
IQ and age but not from etiology: evidence from Down and Williams Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science 286: 2526–2528.
syndromes. J Intellect Disabil Res 51: 932–941. 48. Wager TD, Sylvester C-YC, Lacey SC, Nee DE, Franklin M, et al. (2005)
23. Mervis CB, Robinson BF, Bertrand J, Morris CA, Klein-Tasman BP, et al. Common and unique components of response inhibition revealed by fMRI.
(2000) The Williams syndrome cognitive profile. Brain Cogn 44: 604–628. Neuroimage 27: 323–340.
24. Vicari S, Bellucci S, Carlesimo GA (2005) Visual and spatial long-term memory: 49. Chambers CD, Bellgrove MA, Gould IC, English T, Garavan H, et al. (2007)
differential pattern of impairments in Williams and Down syndromes. Dev Med Dissociable mechanisms of cognitive control in prefrontal and premotor cortex.
Child Neurol 47: 305–311. J Neurophysiol 98: 3638–3647.
25. Vicari S, Bellucci S, Carlesimo GA (2006) Evidence from two genetic syndromes 50. Swick D, Ashley V, Turken U (2011) Are the neural correlates of stopping and
for the independence of spatial and visual working memory. Dev Med Child not going identical? Quantitative meta-analysis of two response inhibition tasks.
Neurol 48: 126–131. Neuroimage 56: 1655–1665.
26. Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L, Gallese V (1997) Parietal cortex: from sight to action. 51. Avanzino L, Bassolino M, Pozzo T, Bove M (2011) Use-dependent hemispheric
Curr Opin Neurobiol 7: 562–567. balance. J Neurosci 31: 3423–3428.
52. Menghini D, Addona F, Costanzo F, Vicari S (2010) Executive functions in 66. Buccino G, Binkofski F, Fink GR, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, et al. (2001) Action
individuals with Williams syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res 54: 418–432. observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an
53. Grafman J, Litvan I, Massaquoi S, Stewart M, Sirigu A, et al. (1992) Cognitive fMRI study. Eur J Neurosci 13: 400–404.
planning deficit in patients with cerebellar atrophy. Neurology 42: 1493–1496. 67. Metzinger T, Gallese V (2003) The emergence of a shared action ontology:
54. Middleton FA, Strick PL (2000) Basal ganglia and cerebellar loops: motor and building blocks for a theory. Conscious Cogn 12: 549–571.
cognitive circuits. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 31: 236–250. 68. Wilson M, Knoblich G (2005) The case for motor involvement in perceiving
55. Schmahmann JD (2004) Disorders of the cerebellum: ataxia, dysmetria of conspecifics. Psychol Bull 131: 460–473.
thought, and the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. J Neuropsychiatry Clin 69. Rizzolatti G, Sinigaglia C (2010) The functional role of the parieto-frontal
Neurosci 16: 367–378. mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations. Nat Rev Neurosci 11:
56. Clarke HF, Robbins TW, Roberts AC (2008) Lesions of the medial striatum in 264–274.
monkeys produce perseverative impairments during reversal learning similar to 70. Iacoboni M (2005) Neural mechanisms of imitation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15:
those produced by lesions of the orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurosci 28: 10972– 632–637.
10982. 71. Burke CJ, Tobler PN, Baddeley M, Schultz W (2010) Neural mechanisms of
57. Ersche KD, Roiser JP, Abbott S, Craig KJ, Müller U, et al. (2011) Response observational learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107: 14431–14436.
perseveration in stimulant dependence is associated with striatal dysfunction and 72. Buccino G, Vogt S, Ritzl A, Fink GR, Zilles K, et al. (2004) Neural circuits
can be ameliorated by a D(2/3) receptor agonist. Biol Psychiatry 70: 754–762.
underlying imitation learning of hand actions: an event-related fMRI study.
58. Berthoz A (1996) The role of inhibition in the hierarchical gating of executed
Neuron 42: 323–334.
and imagined movements. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 3: 101–113.
73. Vogt S, Buccino G, Wohlschläger AM, Canessa N, Shah NJ, et al. (2007)
59. Brass M, Haggard P (2008) The what, when, whether model of intentional
Prefrontal involvement in imitation learning of hand actions: effects of practice
action. Neuroscientist 14: 319–325.
and expertise. Neuroimage 37: 1371–1383.
60. Hoffstaedter F, Grefkes C, Zilles K, Eickhoff SB (2012) The ‘‘What’’ and
‘‘When’’ of Self-Initiated Movements. Cereb Cortex In press. 74. Chaminade T, Meltzoff AN, Decety J (2002) Does the end justify the means? A
61. Sparaci L, Stefanini S, Marotta L, Vicari S, Rizzolatti G (2012) Understanding PET exploration of the mechanisms involved in human imitation. Neuroimage
motor acts and motor intentions in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia 50: 15: 318–328.
1639–1649. 75. Calvo-Merino B, Grèzes J, Glaser DE, Passingham RE, Haggard P (2006)
62. Atkinson J, Braddick O (2011) From genes to brain development to phenotypic Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation.
behavior: ‘‘dorsal-stream vulnerability’’ in relation to spatial cognition, attention, Curr Biol 16: 1905–1910.
and planning of actions in Williams syndrome (WS) and other developmental 76. Sokolov AA, Gharabaghi A, Tatagiba MS, Pavlova M (2010) Cerebellar
disorders. Prog Brain Res 189: 261–283. engagement in an action observation network. Cereb Cortex 20: 486–491.
63. Mobbs D, Eckert MA, Mills D, Korenberg J, Bellugi U, et al. (2007) 77. Wolpert DM, Kawato M (1998) Multiple paired forward and inverse models for
Frontostriatal dysfunction during response inhibition in Williams syndrome. motor control. Neural Netw 11: 1317–1329.
Biol Psychiatry 62: 256–261. 78. Flanagan JR, Johansson RS (2003) Action plans used in action observation.
64. Farran EK, Jarrold C, Gathercole SE (2001) Block design performance in the Nature 424: 769–771.
Williams syndrome phenotype: a problem with mental imagery? J Child Psychol 79. Shadmehr R, Smith MA, Krakauer JW (2010) Error correction, sensory
Psychiatry 42: 719–728. prediction, and adaptation in motor control. Annu Rev Neurosci 33: 89–108.
65. Schütz-Bosbach S, Prinz W (2007) Perceptual resonance: action-induced 80. Hughes G, Waszak F (2011) ERP correlates of action effect prediction and visual
modulation of perception. Trends Cogn Sci 11: 349–355. sensory attenuation in voluntary action. Neuroimage 56: 1632–1640.