2 Auditory PDF
2 Auditory PDF
2 Auditory PDF
1
Different schematic representations of speech signal: (a)
time domain; (b) three frequency components of (a); (c) the
power spectrum of (b); (d) a continuous power spectrum of
speech.
The height of the bar reflects the amplitude of the wave and
is typically plotted as the square of the amplitude, or the
power.
2
When describing the effects on hearing, the
amplitude is typically expressed as a ratio of
sound pressure, P, measured in decibels (dB).
That is,
3
In addition to amplitude (intensity) and frequency (pitch),
two other critical dimensions of the sound stimulus are its
temporal characteristics
4
THE EAR: THE SENSORY TRANSDUCER
Mechanisms of the outer and middle ear (the ear drum or tympanic
membrane, and the hammer, anvil, and stirrup bones) conduct and
amplify the sound waves into the inner ear and are potential
sources of breakdown or deafness (e.g., from a rupture of the
eardrum or buildup of wax).
The muscles of the middle ear are responsive to loud noises and
reflexively contract to attenuate the amplitude of vibration before it is
conveyed to the inner ear.
This aural reflex thus offers some protection to the inner ear.
5
6
THE AUDITORY EXPERIENCE
7
Loudness and Pitch
Psychophysical Scaling.
8
One sone is established arbitrarily as the loudness of a 40-
dB tone of 1,000 Hz.
Frequency Influence.
That is,
Th i every point
i along
l a line
li sounds
d just
j as loud
l d as any
other point along the same line.
9
Masking.
10
ALARMS
11
Criteria for Alarms.
The range of appropriate levels for warning sound components on the flight
deck of the Boeing 737 (vertical line shading). The minimum of the appropriate-
level range is approximately 15 dB above auditory threshold (broken line),
which is calculated from the spectrum of the flight deck noise (solid line). The
vertical dashed lines show the components of the intermittent warning horn,
some of which are well above the maximum of the appropriate-level range.
12
2. The alarm should not be above the danger
level for hearing, whenever this condition can be
avoided. (Obviously, if the ambient noise level is
close to the danger level, one has no choice but
to make the alarm louder by criterion 1, which is
most important.)
13
5. The alarm should be informative, signaling to the listener the
nature of the emergency and, ideally, some indication of the
appropriate action to take. The criticality of this informativeness
criterion can be seen in one alarm system that was found in an
intensive care unit of a hospital. The unit contained six patients,
each monitored by a device with 10 different possible alarms:
60 potential signals that the staff may have had to rapidly
identify.
This means that the alarm should not impose on the human's
restrictive limits of absolute judgment. Just four different alarms
may be the maximum allowable to meet this criterion if these
alarms differ from each other on only a single physical
dimension, such as pitch.
Designing Alarms.
14
Second, to guarantee informativeness and to minimize
confusability, designers should try to stay within the limits of
absolute judgments.
15
A third step involves designing the specifics of the individual
sound. Patterson (1990) recommends the procedure
outlined in the figure on the next slide, a procedure that has
several embedded rationales.
16
Finally, the bottom row of previously shown figure shows the
philosophy by which repeated presentations of the alarm
sequence can be implemented.
Th first
The fi t two
t presentations
t ti may be
b att high
hi h intensity
i t it to
t
guarantee their initial detection (first sequence) and
identification (first or second sequence).
17
False Alarms.
to paraphrase from the old fable, the system "cries wolf" too
often (Bliss & Gilson, 1998).
18
Five logical steps may be taken to avoid the
circumstances of “alarm false alarms”.
Hence, two (or more) levels can signal to the human the
system's own confidence that the alarming conditions are
present. That evidence in the fuzzy middle ground (e.g.,
the odor from a slightly burnt piece of toast), which
previously might have signaled the full fire alarm, now
triggers a signal of noticeable but reduced intensity.
19
THE SOUND TRANSMISSION PROBLEM
Meanwhile, the other jet, a Pan American airplane that had just
landed, was still on the same runway, trying to find its way off.
Th air
The i traffic
t ffi controller
t ll instructed
i t t d the
th pilot
il t off the
th KLM:
KLM
The takeoff proceeded until the aircraft collided with the Pan Am
747, which had still not steered itself clear from the runway.
20
Measuring Speech Communications
21
While the objective merits of the bottom-up approach are
clear, its limits in predicting the understandability of speech
should become apparent when one considers the
contributions of top-down processing to speech perception.
Why?
22
Speech Distortions
23
For example, a raw
speech waveform such as
that shown may contain
over 59,000 bits of
information per second.
24
The human information-processing system is
designed
g to p
prolong
g the duration of the spoken
p
word for a few seconds through what is called
echoic memory.
Hearing Loss
25
Idealized median (50th
percentile) hearing loss at
different frequencies for
males and females as a
function of age.
NOISE REVISITED
The worker in our story was concerned about the impact of noise at
her workplace on her ability to hear.
26
The second form of noise-induced hearing loss is the
temporary threshold shifi (Crocker, 1997).
27
The third form of noise-induced hearing loss, which has
the most serious implications for worker health, is the
permanent threshold shifi (PTS).
Like the TTS, the PTS is greater with both louder and
longer prior exposure to noise.
28
The Environment.
29
The Listener: Ear Protection.
30
Three other categories of sensory experience that have
some direct relevance to design: touch and feel (the
t til and
tactile d haptic
h ti sense), ) limb
li b position
iti and d motion
ti
(proprioception and kinesthesis), and whole-body
orientation and motion (the vestibular senses).
Lying just under the skin are sensory receptors that respond to
pressure on theth skin
ki and d relay
l th i information
their i f ti t the
to th brain
b i
regarding the subtle changes in force applied by the hands and
fingers (or other parts of the body) as they interact with physical
things in the environment.
31
1. A problem with the membrane keyboards sometimes
found on calculators is that they do not offer the same
"feel" (tactile feedback) when the fingers are
positioned on the button as do mechanical keys.
32