The Correlational Analysis Dyad-Level Data in The Distinguishable Case
The Correlational Analysis Dyad-Level Data in The Distinguishable Case
Abstract
Many theories of interpersonal relationships distinguish between individual-levelprocesses and dyadic or
group-level processes. This suggests that two-person relationships should be studied at the level of the dyad as
well as at the level of the individual. We discuss correlational methods for dyads when each dyad contains two
different types of individuals (e.g., a husband and wife, a mother and child, or an expert and a novice). In such
dyadic interaction designs,the dyad members are said to be distinguishable. We present a method for
computing the overall correlation for distinguishable dyads, and we discuss a model for separating the
dyad-level and individual-level components of such a correlation. The computational techniques and their
interpretation are described using data from 98 heterosexual couples.
449
450 R. Gonzalez and D. Griffin
(distinguishable dyad members). In the ex- 1985) that decomposes the observed over-
changeable case, each partner is drawn from all within-partner correlation into latent dy-
the same category or type (e.g., adult males), adic and individual-level components, and
and thus both scores are treated as though the generalized actor-partner model (Gon-
they are sampled from the same statistical zalez & Griffin, 1997; Kenny, 1996) that de-
distribution. In the distinguishable case, composes the observed relations into par-
each partner belongs to a different category tial path coefficients (beta weights). The
or type (e.g., fathers vs. sons), and their latter model was used by Murray et al.
scores on each variable are drawn from dis- (1996). This article will focus on the dyadic
tributions that may differ, yielding the possi- effects model and discuss the meaning and
bility that these two samples may differ in computation of couple-level and individ-
their means, their variances, or their covari- ual-level correlations. However, it must be
ances. emphasized that the choice of an underly-
The decision to treat dyadic partners as ing model is a theoretical rather than a sta-
distinguishable or exchangeable should be tistical question (Kenny, 1996).
guided by theoretical assumptions rather We now turn to a more detailed discus-
than empirical tests. Thus, even if the two sion of these overall correlations. We will
types of partners do not differ in mean, review the pairwise coding, show how to
variance, or covariance terms, they should estimate these two overall correlations, pro-
be treated as distinguishable rather than ex- vide tests of significance,compare the SEM
changeable if they are distinguished in the (structural equation modeling) approach to
relevant theory. When other variables are the pairwise approach, and provide a con-
tested, the two types of partners may well crete example. We will then review the dy-
differ. Furthermore, no power is lost by adic model that decomposes the observed
treating the partners as distinguishable as overall within-partner correlation into la-
long as the relevant parameter estimates tent dyadic and individual-level compo-
are pooled across both types of partners. nents. This multilevel model will not only
We now turn to a specific research exam- provide new insight for the overall correla-
ple that we will use throughout this article. tions, but will also be a useful model in
Murray, Holmes, and Griffin (1996) col- many research settings.
lected trust and conflict ratings from both
members of 98 heterosexual couples. One
Estimating and Testing the Overall
question of interest addressed by this study
Correlations in the Distinguishable Case
is: Across the entire sample of men and
women, do ratings of trust relate to reports Suppose a researcher collected two vari-
of conflict? In other words, is there a signifi- ables, X and I: from each individual in a
cant overall within-partner correlation be- sample of heterosexual couples. For in-
tween trust and conflict? A second question stance, as in Murray et al. (1996), a re-
of interest involves a different overall cor- searcher could collect trust ratings (variable
relation: Across the entire sample, do rat- X ) from both the husband and the wife, and
ings of an individual’s trust relate to the reports of conflict (variable Y) from both
partner’s report of conflict? In other words, the husband and the wife. This results in
is there a significant overall cross-partner four variables: x h , X,, Yh, and Y,, where
correlation between trust and conflict? the subscript h represents husband and the
The meaning of these “overall” correla- subscript w represents wife. There are a to-
tions could then be examined in light of a tal of six correlations that can be computed
model of the underlying process that gave in this situation; these correlations are
rise to it. Although the data could be mod- shown in Figure 1.
eled in a number of ways (Kenny, 1996), The correlation between x h and Yh, de-
two specific underlying models seem of noted cor(Xh, Y h ) in Figure 1, is the within-
greatest relevance: a dyadic version of the partner correlation between trust and con-
group effects model (Kenny & La Voie, flict for husbands, and the correlation
Correlational analysis of dyad-level data 45 1
SEM model presented in Figure 1 con- lation approach, the estimation of the over-
strains the two X variables to have equal all within-partner and overall cross-partner
variances, and also constrains the two Y correlations proceeds in four steps. First,
variables to have equal variances. Of the six the data are arranged in the pairwise man-
observed covariances, the two within-part- ner (Griffin & Gonzalez, 1995). Second, the
ner covariances between variables X and Y assumptions necessary for pooling the cor-
are constrained to be equal as are the two relations across the two categories of indi-
cross-partner cross-variable covariances. viduals (i.e., equality of variances within
The resulting SEM analysis yields a x 2 with variables and equality of covariances within
4 degrees of freedom (df)that provides an and across categories) are checked. Third,
omnibus test on all four constraints. Note the overall within-partner and cross-part-
that it is also possible to test each assump- ner correlations are computed as partial
tion separately within SEM by relaxing correlations with between-class mean dif-
each assumption and comparing the differ- ferences (e.g., sex differences) partialled
ence in x2s from the full model (df = 4) to out. Fourth, the overall within-partner and
each reduced model (df= 3). overall cross-partner correlations are tested
If the omnibus x 2 test is nonsignificant, for significance by a method that takes into
then the pooled covariance between X and account the degree of nonindependence
Y is an appropriate estimate of the “overall within dyad members.
within-partner covariance,” and the signifi- In the distinguishable case, the painvise
cance test associated with this parameter correlation model requires five columns of
tests the overall correlation against the null data (see Griffin & Gonzalez, 1995, for a
hypothesis. Similarly,the pooled covariance general discussion of the pairwise model).
between an individual’s X and the partner’s As shown in Table 1, the first column (la-
Y is an estimate of the “overall cross-part- beled C ) consists of binary codes repre-
ner covariance,” and the significance test senting the “category” variable (e.g., sex of
associated with this parameter tests the subject). If the researcher decided to code
overall cross-partner correlation against wives as “1” and husbands as “2” the first
the null hypothesis.2 In the SEM output, the column would consist of “1”in the first row
overall within-partner and cross-partner and “2” in the second row. This pattern
correlations will appear as standardized co- would be repeated for each of the Ndyads in
efficients. the sample, yielding a column containing 2N
binary codes. The second column (labeled
X)contains the scores on variable X corre-
Using the pairwise approach to estimate
sponding to the dyad member represented
the overall correlations
by the category code in column one. For ex-
The painvise approach offers an alternative ample, the first woman’s score on trust is ad-
method for estimating the overall correla- jacent to the first “1”in column one. Below
tions; it provides parameter estimates that that, the first man’s score on trust is adjacent
are identical to those given by SEM under to the first “2” in column one. This pattern
maximum likelihood. In the pairwise corre- continues, yielding a total of 2N scores.
Column three is created by the pairwise
2. One possible generalization of this model is a re-
reversal of column two. For example, adja-
laxation of the variance constraints (i.e., a model cent to each person’s score on trust in col-
that has all four variances as free parameters). On umn two is placed her or his partner’s score
the surface this generalized model may appear at- on trust in column three. This “reversed”
tractive; however, it is difficult to interpret because column of scores on X is referred to as X ‘ .
the resulting parameters are standardized accord-
ing to the particular variances involved. Thus, even
Columns four and five consist of the scores
when the covariances are set equal across classes, on variable Y (e.g., conflict), which are also
the standardized solutions for each class (e.g., the coded in the “painvise” format and labeled
correlations for men and women) will be unequal. Y and Y’, respectively. Table 1 presents
454 R. Gonzalez and D. Griffin
Table 1. Symbolic representation for the pairwise data setup for two variables in the
distinguishable case
Variable
Dyad
No. C X x Y r
1 1 XI1 XlZ YI 1 YlZ
2 x
12 Xl 1 y12 Yl 1
2 1 x
21 x
22 y2, y22
2 x22 XZl y22 Y2l
3 1 x3 1 x32 y31 y32
2 x
32 x3, y32 y31
4 1 x41 x42 y41 y42
2 x42 x41 y42 y41
Note: The first subscript represents the dyad and the second subscript represents the individual. Categorization
of individuals as 1 or 2 is based on the class variable C. Primes denote the reverse coding described in the text.
these five columns in symbolic form. The served overall within-partner correlation,
reasoning behind the pairwise coding is with the direction of the bias controlled by
that interdependence is built directly into the magnitudes and directions of the two
the data matrix. By constructing such links mean differences. The overall within-
between data points, subsequent analyses partner correlation must be corrected for
become relatively straightforward. these mean differences by partialling on the
The Appendix provides statistical tests category variable.Thus, the appropriate esti-
for each of the assumptions discussed in the mate of the overall within-partner correla-
previous section. Note that the painvise ap- tion is
proach tests assumptions separately,
whereas above we described an omnibus
test for the set of assumptions in SEM.
Those concerned about inflated Type 1 er-
ror rates for the separate tests of the as-
sumptions in the painvise approach may This equation is simply the partial correla-
wish to use a Bonferroni corrected a-level. tion between variables X and X holding C
Separate tests may be desirable because the constant, denoted by r,,., (where variables
omnibus test could mask violations (e.g., an to the right of the dot have been partialled
assumption may be violated even when the out). In our ongoing example, the corrected
omnibus test is not statistically significant). estimate of the overall within-partner cor-
Given that the assumptions are met, the relation would be the partial correlation
overall within-partner correlation is com- between trust and conflict with sex of sub-
puted using all 2N individual scores in col- ject partialled out. Most statistical packages
umns two (X)and four (Y) of Table 1-or have procedures for computing partial cor-
equivalently, columns three ( X ’ ) and five relations.
( Y ) . However, the simple correlation be- The computation of the overall cross-
tween column X and column Y does not, in partner correlation proceeds in a similar
general, estimate the “true” overall within- manner. The “partial” overall cross-partner
partner correlation. Instead, if there are correlation is given by
between-category mean differences on one
or both variables (e.g., men have a higher
score on trust than do women or vice versa),
these mean differences will bias the ob-
Correlational analysis of dyad-level data 455
Again, this is simply the partial correlation Rempel, 1989) and conflict (a 5-item index
between an individual’s X (column 2) and adapted from Braiker & Kelley, 1979, in-
his or her partner’s Y (column 5, or Y’) dexing the self-rating of the frequency of
holding C constant. overt behavioral conflict). The relevant
Significancetests for the two overall cor- means, variances, covariances, and correla-
relations are presented in the Appendix. tions are presented in Table 2. The relevant
The reader may examine the symbolic form partial pairwise correlations needed for
of the significance test to develop intuition testing the overall within-partner correla-
for how interdependence influences these tion rxy and the overall cross-partner cor-
tests. Even though significance tests for the relation rXy’are presented in Table 3. Each
painvise approach are asymptotically entry in Table 3 is a partial correlation in
equivalent to the Z-tests in SEM under the that the category variable C has been par-
null hypothesis, in a single sample the two tialled out. The information needed to
tests will not be identical. A Monte Carlo compute all statistics and tests presented
simulation that assesses the performance of in this article is contained in those two
the painvise and SEM tests for rxyis given tables.
in the Appendix. The simulation suggests The SEM analysis estimating the overall
that the pairwise approach performs at within-partner and cross-partner correla-
least as well as the SEM approach with re- tions tests all assumptions simultaneously;
spect to the effective Type I error rate, with the omnibus test with 4 degrees of freedom
the SEM approach tending to be slightly was nonsignificant ( x 2 = 7.98, p = .09), in-
liberal. dicating that it was appropriate to interpret
the pooled overall correlations as summa-
ries of the relation between trust and con-
Illustrating the overall within-partner and
flict across the entire sample.
cross-partner correlations for the
In the pairwise approach, the assump-
distinguishable case
tions are tested individually. An examina-
In the following section we use data col- tion of the diagonal elements in Table 2
lected by Murray et al. (1996) to illustrate reveals that men and women had approxi-
the pairwise analysis of the overall within- mately equal variances on each variable.
partner correlation when dyadic partners The t values corresponding to the depend-
are distinguishable. Murray and colleagues ent variances test presented in the Appen-
collected data from both members of 98 dix were 1.31 for trust and 1.04 for conflict,
heterosexual couples. We chose two vari- neither of which is statistically significant at
ables from their data: trust (Holmes & a = .05 with 96 degrees of freedom. The
Note: Correlations appear below the diagonal, variances on the diagonal, and covariances above the diagonal.
The means on each variable (e.g., the mean female trust score) are presented in the last row. From Murray et
al.. 1996.
456 R. Gonzalez and D. Griffin
Table 3. Partial pairwise correlation p < .001. The SEM Z was -4.27. The over-
matrices for trust (T) and conflict (C)from all cross-partner correlation was -.218,
Murray et al. (1996) with an effective sample size of 175.0 and
a pairwise Z of -2.88. The SEM Z was
Trust and Conflict -2.67 for the identical standardized’ esti-
T T’ mate.
T‘ .284
C - .350 - .218 Separating Dyad-Level and
C’ - .218 - .350 .463 Individual-Level Effects
In contrast to the overall correlations dis-
Note: The prime denotes the “reverse” coding of the
variable as described in the text. Boldfaced values cussed above, the dyadic effects model fo-
are the partial intraclass correlations. cuses on three different questions. First,
are dyadic members similar to each other
on each variable? That is, is there signifi-
assumption that covxy for women equals cant dyad-level variance in each variable?
covxyfor men is tested next3 (again, see the Second, if there is dyad-level variance in
Appendix for details on tests of dependent each variable, are the two variables corre-
covariances). For trust and conflict, covxy lated at the dyadic level? That is, is there
equaled -.857 for women and -.438 for a significant dyad-level correlation be-
men. This difference was not statistically tween the trust ratings and the reports of
significant by the test on dependent covari- conflict? Third, within each dyad, does the
ances given in the Appendix, Z = -1.64. relative standing of the two individuals on
The final assumption tested is that the two one variable relate to their relative stand-
cross-partner covariances are equal. The as- ing on the second variable? That is, is there
sumption that cov,,’ for one partner (e.g., a significant individual-level correlation
the population covariance between between trust and conflict? These ques-
women’s trust and partner’s report of con- tions assess whether the overall within-
flict) equals covxy‘for the other partner partner correlation reflects dyad-level
(e.g., the population covariance between processes, individual-level processes, or
men’s trust and partner’s report of conflict) both.
appears to fit. The difference between the The overall within-partner correlation
trust-conflict covariances across partners serves as a summary of the relation between
was not statistically significant by the co- variables X and Y across all individuals,but
variance test presented in the Appendix, Z it does not reveal whether the relation be-
= -.33. tween variables X and Y exists at the level
Estimates of the two overall correlations of the individual, at the level of the dyad, or
are identical for the SEM and pairwise ap- both. Figure 2 presents one model of the
proaches, although the tests of significance sources of the linear relation between Xand
differ slightly. The overall within-partner Y that allows the separation of individual-
correlation (partialled on sex) between level and dyad-level effects (Kenny & La
trust and conflict was -.350. Thus, a nega- Voie, 1985). Once again, we emphasize that
tive correlation existed between trust and the appropriateness of this model is not
reports of conflict when pooling both sexes provable by statistical means; alternate
and controlling for mean differences. In the structural models (e.g., Kenny, 1996) may be
pairwise analysis, the effective sample size theoretically justified. In the dyadic effects
N*1 = 166.24, and the Z value was -4.51, model for the distinguishable case, the vari-
ance of a given observed variable is as-
3. Note that when the variances are equal across
sumed to result from three different
classes, the equality of covariance assumption can sources: variation due to category member-
be tested using either covariances or correlations. ship (which is partialled out from the model
Correlational analysis of dyad-level data 457
'i
'i /
,
--
J J 1
variance variance
Ixl El lyrJ
2
&
"\ \m A
m Shared
variance
t 'd
shown here), variation due to dyads, and scores in a distinguishable dyadic design is
variation due to individuals within dyads. modeled as a weighted sum of the dyad-
The dyadic component of a given variable is level correlation (rd) and the individual-
the portion of that variable that is shared within-dyad correlation (ri).The dyad level
between dyadic partners; the individual correlation contributes more to the overall
component is the portion of a variable that within-partner partial correlation when the
is unshared between dyadic partners. shared variance between partners is large
As Figure 2 illustrates, in this model the (as indexed by the partial intraclass correla-
covariation of two variables is also assumed tions rxx'.cand ryy,.c),whereas the individ-
to result from the same three sources. First, ual-level correlation contributes more to
mean category differences on X and Y the overall within-partner partial correla-
(which are held constant in the pairwise tion when the shared dyadic variance is
model) give rise to one portion of the ob- small.
served overall correlation. Second, the dy-
adic (shared) portions of X and Y are re-
lated through the dyadic correlation r d . Assumptions of the dyadic model
Finally, the individual (unshared or unique)
portions of X and Y are related through the The assumptions for the distinguishable
individual-level correlation ri. After the dyadic model are identical to the assump-
class differences are partialled out, the cor- tions for the overall correlations (equal
rected overall within-partner correlation variances on X, equal variances on X
can be decomposed into the remaining two equal covariances between an individual's
parts: X and the partner's I: and equal covari-
ances between X and Y within each class).
As with the overall case, these assumptions
'xy.c = C G r d -k
can be tested either separately with the
J I - ) ; x ' c J G5 (3) tests provided in the or simultaneously
with SEM. The omnibus test in SEM is the
4-degrees-of-freedom x* test given by the
In words, the overall within-partner partial model in Figure 2 with the observed co-
correlation between all X scores and all Y variance matrix as input. Note that this is
458 R. Gonzalez and D.Grifin
the identical x 2 from the omnibus test of structural model. Instead, this model is
the assumptions for the overall correla- based on the six covariances4 presented in
tions. Table 2. Third, when using SEM the model
The dyadic effects model has one addi- in Figure 2 could be generalized to allow for
tional assumption that must be examined as separate individual-level correlations ri for
well: The intraclass correlations must both each class (i.e., separate individual-level
be significant (Kenny & La Voie, 1985).The correlations for men and women), whereas
partial intraclass correlation yields the pro- the pairwise model can only estimate a
portion of shared or dyad-level variance in pooled individual-level correlation.
each variable. Stated another way, the The output from an SEM program will
square root of the intraclass correlations provide the estimates of rd and ri, as well as
define the “reliability coefficients” for the tests of significance for these parameters.
underlying dyadic latent variables. If the in- We identified the model by setting the vari-
traclass correlations are zero, then there is ances of all latent variables to 1 (there are
no dyad-level variance, and Td is meaning- two latent dyadic variables and four latent
less. A simple asymptotic test for the partial individual variables) and requiring the two
intraclass correlation is provided in the Ap- individual-level loadings on each variable
pendix. to be equal (e.g., the individual loading on
The SEM framework offers more flexi- trust is set equal for men and women) and
bility than does the pairwise approach be- the two dyadic loadings on each variable to
cause it permits a relaxed model with dif- be equal (e.g., the dyadic loading on trust is
ferent individual-level correlations for each set equal for men and women). This para-
member. If the x2 is significant, then the meterization facilitates generalization to
pooled dyadic structural model does not fit more variables and groups of larger size, as
the data. One option available in that case discussed later in this article. Under the
is to refit the model with separate individ- present identification scheme, the unstan-
ual-level correlations, and examine the dardized coefficients are the correlations.
change in x 2 between the full and reduced For different identification schemes, one
model (see the Appendix for the necessary would need to use the standardized coeffi-
EQS syntax). cients rather than the unstandardized coef-
ficients. See Bentler (1995) for an illustra-
Using SEM to estimate the dyadic tion of different methods of identifying
model such a model; Gonzalez and Griffin (1998)
Figure 2 also illustrates the structural
model used to estimate the two underlying 4. For some simple SEM models, a correlation matrix
correlations rd and ri in the SEM approach. or a covariance matrix as input leads to identical
Sample EQS syntax and guidelines for solutions (see Bollen, 1989, or Long, 1983, for a
nontechnical discussion of this issue). However, a
AMOS are presented in the Appendix. The covariance matrix should be used as input when-
SEM approach involves three important ever there are equality constraints in the model. In
changes from the pairwise structural model. the present models, the two dyadic loadings from
First, instead of the pairwise data setup each latent variable leading to the two individual
(e.g., columns X and X ’ each with the entire scores are constrained to be equal and the two
individual loadings leading to a given variable are
2N set of observations differing only in or- constrained to be equal. Thus, the models pre-
der), the observed variables are coded sented here must be evaluated by using unstan-
separately by categories (e.g.,Xwrepresents dardized input. Note also that the individual-level
the wife’s score on trust and x h represents correlations are identical to correlated errors pro-
the husband’s score on trust, each column vided by standard SEM models; however, the para-
meterization provided here is slightly different
with N observations). Second, and follow- from usual to make the multilevel nature of the
ing from the first point, the category effect data clearer and to allow easy generalization to
is not directly partialled out from the SEM more than two variables.
Correlational analysis of dyad-level data 459
is driven by the denominators of Equations tion of variance in conflict that was shared
4 and 5. The numerator of rd is the overall between the partners. The same informa-
cross-partner correlation r,,‘.,, which in- tion is obtained from the standardized path
dexes the strength of the dyadic relation in coefficients of the SEM model after they
raw score terms. When rxY’.,is 0, then rd are squared. When the two partial intraclass
must be 0. correlations are tested by the asymptotic 2
Significance tests for the distinguishable tests, both are significant: 2 = 3 . 0 6 , ~< .01
painvise case are, with one slight exception, for trust, and 2 = 5.83,~< .001,for conflict.
identical to those presented in Griffin and The relevant tests from the SEM output are
Gonzalez (1995) for the exchangeable case. 5.39 and 8.28, again statistically significant.
The asymptotic tests include those for the Recall that the two tests will not coincide
partial intraclass correlations rXx,.,and r,,‘.,, except asymptotically under the null hy-
which index the amount of dyadic or shared pothesis.
variance in each variable, the partial raw- Turning to the multilevel correlations,
score dyadic correlation rxY’.,, which in- the pairwise analysis yields a dyad-level
dexes the dyadic relation uncorrected for correlation between trust and conflict of
the degree of shared variance, and the la- -.601. This value is obtained by disattenu-
tent dyadic correlation rd. The individual- ating the overall cross-partner correlation
level correlation ri is tested using a standard rxy’ = -.218 by the amount of dyad-level
t test for Pearson correlations, and this test variance in each variable, rxx‘= .284 and ryyl
of variable C . Details of the tests can be -.601. Note that exactly the same estimate
found in the Appendix. is obtained from SEM. Under special cir-
Although the significance tests from the cumstances the estimated latent correlation
SEM and pairwise approaches are equiva- can obtain “out of bounds” values greater
lent under the null hypothesis, in practice than 1.0.Some programs such as EQS auto-
the null hypothesis will not be exactly true matically restrict the possible values of cor-
and the significance tests from the two relations to fall between - 1and 1, and then
methods will differ. In the Appendix we the pairwise and SEM values will diverge.
present simulation data comparing the tests When tested by the pairwise significance
of significance provided by the two models.
test, rd is statistically significant,2 = -2.88,
Note that the significance test from the
p < .01 (the SEM 2 value is -3.49).
pairwise approach is slightly more well-be-
Finally, the individual-level correlation is
haved than the SEM version (see Gonzalez
obtained by correcting the difference be-
& Griffin, 1998,for a discussion of the limi-
tween the combined individual and dyadic
tations in SEM significance tests).
correlation rxyand the pure dyadic correla-
tion rxy’for the amount of unique individ-
An illustration of the dyadic effects model ual-level variance in each variable, l-rxxl
and l-ryy*.In this case, the individual-level
We use the Murray et al. (1996) data to
correlation between trust and conflict is
illustrate the distinguishable dyadic effects
-.212, yielding a t(96) = 2 . 1 2 , ~< .05 (the
model. For trust, the observed partial in-
SEM the corresponding estimate of ri is
traclass correlation of .284 indicates that
identical and the 2 value is 2.19).
about 28% of the variance in trust was
shared between dyadic partners.5 Similarly,
for conflict, the observed partial intraclass
Interpreting these results
correlation of .463 represents the propor-
How should these results be interpreted?
5. Note that for the intraclass, the percentage of vari- First, the relations across men and women
ance explained is given by r not rz. between trust and conflict were symmetric.
Correlational analysis of dyad-level data 461
That is, the relation between men’s trust members, or (b) because of the selection
and women’s report of conflict was roughly principles that led couples to end up to-
equal to the relation between women’s gether, or (c) because of some third vari-
trust and men’s report of conflict. This is an able operating on both partners. Only when
important conclusion in itself, but is also a dyad members are randomly assigned can
necessary assumption for the dyadic effects we conclude that the similarities on each
model to be appropriate. Second, the mem- variable, and the dyad-level correlation be-
bers of heterosexual romantic couples were tween each variable, reflect actual dyadic
moderately similar (at a correlational, but processes (or shared environments that
not necessarily at a mean level, because have dyad-level effects).
mean sex differences were partialled out of Once again, we stress that multilevel
this analysis) on levels of both trust and conclusions cannot be reached by the com-
conflict. The significant intraclass correla- mon practice of (a) correlating dyad means
tions indicated that men and women within on X and Y or (b) correlating individual
a couple tended to resemble each other, scores on X and E: As noted earlier, both
although the proportions of variance were the mean-level correlation r, and the over-
only moderate (28% and 46% dyad-level all within-partner correlation are weighted
variance for trust and conflict, respec- combinations of the dyad-level and the in-
tively). dividual-level correlation.
Finally, and most important, trust and
conflict were related at both the level of the
dyad and the level of the individual. This
Extensions to Larger Groups and More
situation is by no means preordained. Sub-
Variables
stantial correlations may be observed at
only one level of analysis, and in special Clearly, the two-person, two-variable prob-
cases, each of the two levels can have oppo- lem we have demonstrated here is the sim-
site signs. Recall that the dyad-level correla- plest possible setup. In the distinguishable
tion represents dyadic processes because it case, because of the multiple equality con-
indexes the extent to which the similarity straints, the pairwise approach becomes
between men and women on X (trust) re- cumbersome when larger groups or multi-
lates to the similarity between men and variate analyses are desired. The SEM ap-
women on Y (conflict). Conceptually, this proach, however, is well-suited to such ex-
suggests that the dyadic processes that give tensions. (Note that the standard SEM
rise to shared positive trust are related to approach is not easy to implement in the
those dyadic processes that reduce shared exchangeable case.) For example, imagine
perceptions of conflict. In this case, an indi- that one collected trust and conflict scores
vidual who was relatively more trusting on members of family units where each
tended to report relatively less conflict, so family consisted of a father, mother, daugh-
that both levels moved in the same direc- ter, and son, and where the four categories
tion. But it is easy to imagine situations were treated as distinguishable. An SEM
(theoretically justified) for an opposite re- model generalized from Figure 2 could ad-
sult. If the individual-level relation primar- dress such questions as: Are trust and con-
ily reflected neuroticism, for example, one flict related overall, at the family level, and
might find that the more neurotic and en- at the individual level? (See Cook, 1994, for
meshed partner was both more trusting and a similar model of family functioning using
more likely to report conflict. a latent variable approach.) With four dis-
Note the limitations provided by an ex- tinguishable family members, a step-down
isting couples design. We do not know approach to the equality constraints should
whether the significant dyad-level correla- be used: For example, after testing for a
tions came about (a) because of dyadic common overall within-individual correla-
processes that occurred between the dyad tion, the constraints might be relaxed to al-
462 R. Gonzalez and D.Griffin
low the children to have a different overall ther benefits come when multiple distin-
correlation than the adults. guishable members and/or multiple vari-
Or, as another example, one might wish ables are analyzed. However, we believe
to study whether the multilevel relations that contrasting the merits of the two ap-
between trust and conflict in couples re- proaches is not as important as illustrating
main when a third variable, such as rela- their common abilities to separate dyadic
tionship satisfaction, is held constant. This and individual-level relations.
could be accomplished by extending the We hope that analytic methods that sepa-
model presented in Figure 2 so that the cor- rate the relative contribution of dyad or
related latent dyadic variables underlying group-level processes from individual-level
trust and satisfaction were used to predict processes may encourage the development
the latent dyadic variable underlying con- of theory in interpersonal behavior. Every
flict, and the correlated individual-level step of the model-testing procedure pre-
variables underlying trust and satisfaction sented here may help to guide theory about
were used to predict the individual-level the multilevel nature of interpersonal be-
variable underlying conflict. Examples of havior. For example, the explicit testing of
this extension (and others) are given in the equality of the two cross-partner corre-
Gonzalez and Griffin (1997). lations should encourage theory-building
about when relations between variables
across classes of individuals will be equal
Summary and Conclusion
and when they are likely to be unequal.
We have presented a technique for analyz- We believe that the most important as-
ing correlational data from distinguishable pect of this model is the explicit separation
dyads. The pairwise correlation model has of dyad-level and individual-level relations.
the advantage of simplicity,both conceptu- At the moment, little theory in social psy-
ally and in the computer software required. chology has been developed about how
Simulation results presented in the Appen- dyad-level or group-level processes differ
dix show that the pairwise approach is at from individual-level processes. However,
least as accurate as SEM in protecting in the present examples we found that in
against Type I error when the null hypothe- romantic couples the relations between
sis is true. A benefit of the structural equa- trust and conflict appear at both levels. Per-
tions modeling approach is that it allows a haps empirical findings such as these will
test of whether separate individual-level promote more precise and complex multi-
correlations are needed for each class. Fur- level theories of interpersonal behavior.
References
Bakeman, R., & Beck, S. (1974). The size of informal iterative approach to the analysis of family data.
groups in public. Environment & Behavior, 6, Annals of Human Genetics, 55, 77-90.
378-390. Gonzalez, R., & Griffin, D. (1997). On the statistics of
Becker, R. A., Chambers, J. M., & Wilks, A. R. (1988). interdependence: Treating dyadic data with re-
The new S language. Pacific Grove, CA: spect. In S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal re-
Wadsworth. lationships (2nd ed., pp. 271-302). New York: Wiley.
Bentler, P. M. (1995). EQS Structural Equations Pro- Gonzalez, R., & Griffin, D. (1998). Correct tests of
gram Manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software. significance f o r parameters in SEM. Unpublished
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent manuscript, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor,
variables. New York: Wiley. and University of Sussex, Brighton, UK.
Braiker, H. B., & Kelley, H. H. (1979). Conflict in the Griffin, D., & Gonzalez, R. (1995). The correlational
development of close relationships. In R. L. Bur- analysis of dyad-level data: Models for the
gess & T. L. Huston (Eds.), Social exchange in de- exchangeable case. Psychological Bulletin, 118,
veloping relationships. New York Academic Press. 430439.
Cook, W. (1994). A structural equation model of dy- Holmes, J. G., & Rempel, J. K. (1989). Trust in close
adic relationships within the family system.Journal relationships. In C. Hendrick (Ed.),Review ofper-
of Consulting & Clinical Psycho6ogy, 62, 500-510. sonality and social psychology (pp. 187-220). New-
Eliasziw, M., & Donner, A. (1991). A generalized non- bury Park, CA: Sage.
Correlational analysis of dyad-level data 463
Kendall, M., & Stuart, A. (1966). The advanced theory Long, J. S. (1983). Confirmatory factor ana1ysis:A pref-
of statistics (2nd ed.). London: Griffin. ace to L l S R E L . Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Kenny, D. A. (1979). Correlation and causality. New Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (1996). The
York: Wiley. benefits of positive illusion: Idealization and the
Kenny, D. A. (1995). Design issues in dyadic research. construction of satisfaction in close relationships.
Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70,
164-184. 79-98.
Kenny, D. A. (1996). Models of non-independence in Robinson, W. S.(1950). Ecological correlations and the
dyadic research.Journal of Social and Personal Re- behavior of individuals.American Sociological Re-
lationships, 13, 279-294. view, 15, 351-357.
Kenny, D. A., & La Voie, L. (1985). Separating individ-
ual and group effects. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 48, 339-348.
Appendix
This appendix summarizes several statisti- A computationally simpler method to
cal tests that are mentioned in the text. test two dependent variances was given by
Kenny (1979). One creates two new vari-
ables: a sum within dyads and a difference
Testing the Difference Between Two within dyads. That is, a new variable equal
Dependent Variances to the sum of the two scores within a dyad
To test the difference between the variance (denoted S ) and a new variable equal to the
of each individual in the dyad, select only difference of the two scores within the dyad
those cases coded with a “1”in column one (denoted D).The test for the correlation
and compare the variance of X (column between S and D (i.e., the usual t-test for
two) with that of X ’ (column three), and the Pearson correlation with N - 2 degrees
compare the variance of Y (column four) of freedom as in Equation A4 below) is
with that of Y’ (column five). We illustrate equivalent to the test for two correlated
with an example from Murray et al. (1996). variances presented in Equation A l .
The variance for the women on trust was
1.35 and the variance for the men on trust Testing the Difference Between ’Itro
was 1.74. These two samples are not inde- Dependent Covariances
pendent because the women and men are
paired as a function of their dyad. To test We present a simple asymptotic Z test that
the null hypothesis that the population vari- directly compares two dependent covari-
ance for the women equals the population ances (rather than correlations). Using the
variance for the men, one can use the t-test asymptotic property that
the woman’s conflict as 2, the man’s trust as indexing the dependence of an individual’s
3, and the man’s conflict as 4. The necessary X on his or her partner’s Y).
variances and covariances are denoted us- Given the four elements of the partial
ing subscripts (e.g., C12 denotes the covari- pairwise correlation matrix in the distin-
ance between the woman’s trust and her guishable case (rXy,,,rxx’.crryy‘.c,and rxy?.&
conflict; VI denotes the variance of the significance testing of the overall within-
woman’s trust). The test for two dependent partner correlation proceeds in the same
covariances (e.g., C12 and C34) is given by manner as for the exchangeable case (Grif-
fin & Gonzalez, 1995).6 First, we consider
the test for the overall within-partner corre-
lation. Under the null hypothesis that pXy.,
= 0, the approximate large-sample variance
of r,,, is -1 ,where
where COrepresents the average of the two
sample covariances being tested (i.e. (C12 + N;
C34)/2). This 2 can be compared to the criti-
cal value of 1.96 for a two-tailed test at a =
.05.
Using the values from Table 2, we can
test whether the covariance between the Thus, the overall (partial) correlation rXy.,
woman’s trust and her conflict ( - 3 5 7 ) dif-
can be tested using the critical ratio 2 =
fers from the covariance between the
male’s trust and his conflict (- .438). The rxy., , or more simply z = rxy.,JN7. The
resulting test statistic is
tial) Pearson correlation using one individ- adjusted for dependent observations and
ual from each dyad because the two indi- disattenuation. When rxx’.cand/or ryyf.care
viduals are essentially identical. When there small, rd will tend to be large and may even
is complete independence within dyads on exceed 1.0. Under these circumstances, D*
both variables, the correlation and its sig- will be small and so the “inflated” value of
nificance test essentially reduce to the (par- rd will generally not be significant. Note
tial) Pearson estimates obtained using both that the p values for testing rxy’..and rd
members of each dyad. against their respective null hypotheses will
Similarly, the asymptotic variance of the always be identical in the painvise ap-
overall cross-partner correlation rxy’.cunder proach.
1 The individual-level correlation rj can be
the null hypothesis is - where N ; = computed directly from the pairwise setup
N2*
as per Equation 5, or equivalently by corre-
2N . Nr can be thought of lating the difference scores within a dyad
+ rx2yy‘.c
1+ rxx‘.cryy’.c (e.g., female minus male scores on trust cor-
as the “effective sample size” for the overall related with female minus male scores on
cross-partner correlation rxy’.cadjusted for conflict). The significance of ri can be tested
dependent observations, and under the null using the usual Pearson correlation table
hypothesis can range between N, the (or the associated t test for a Pearson corre-
number of dyads, and 2N, the number of lation
‘xy’.c
individuals. The critical ratio - -
-
Ir rJN-2
Table 4. Bias and effective Type I error rates for the latent variable model for the pairwise
and SEM approaches
Null hypothesis that Pd =0 NO.
of Samples
Average Pairwise SEM Consistent
Pxx. = Pyy' Pi Estimate rd Type I Error 'Qpe I Error with Model
N = 30
.so -.5 .078 .039 .047 487
SO 0 -.010 .040 .058 497
SO .5 - .055 .043 .070 486
.75 -.5 .016 .052 .080 500
.75 0 .006 .040 .080 500
.75 .5 .002 .050 .072 500
N = 200
SO - .5 .001 .060 .050 500
SO 0 - .002 .030 .036 500
SO .5 - .004 .048 .060 500
.75 - .5 .001 .058 .064 500
.75 0 .001 .054 .060 500
.75 .5 - .001 .068 .074 500
Note: Number of dyads ( N ) was either 30 or 200. Details of the simulation are provided in the text.
Examination of Table 4 suggests that, for from the pairwise approach (see Gonzalez
the population values examined in this & Griffin, 1998, for a discussion of the ef-
simulation, the pairwise approach per- fects of parameterization on tests of signifi-
formed at least as well as the SEM ap- cance).
proach (as implemented in EQS) with re- We also performed an analogous simula-
spect to Type I error rates, even with a small tion on the overall within-partner correla-
number of dyads ( N = 30).* In all but one tion rxyto examine the Trpe I error rate of
case the SEM approach had a greater effec- the asymptotic test presented here for the
tive Type I error than did the painvise ap- pairwise distinguishable case. Again, a
proach; in 8 out of the 12 combinations of population mean difference of 5, with unit
population values, the pairwise approach variance, was introduced between the two
had an effective Type I error rate that was classes. Table 5, based on N = 30 dyads,
closer (in an absolute difference sense) to presents the average estimate over the 500
0.05 than was the SEM approach. We note runs and the effective Type I error rate. As
that a different parameterization of the shown in Table 5, the asymptotic test for the
equivalent SEM model (i.e., variances of pairwise approach performed well even
the latent variables as free parameters and with samples of 30 dyads. Again, the SEM
indicator paths fixed to 1) leads to effective approach produced effective Type I error
Type I errors that are quite close to rate rates that tended to be liberal.
Table 5. Bias and effective Type I error rates f o r the overall within-partner correlation rXy
Pairwise SEM
Pxxq = Pyy Pxy, Average Estimate Type I Error Type I Error
-.5 - .5 - .006 .046 .068
- .5 0 .006 .060 .088
- .5 .5 - .003 .046 .064
0 - .5 - .006 .040 .064
0 0 .Ooo .050 .064
0 .5 .ooo .040 .046
.5 -.5 -.002 ,058 .082
.5 0 .007 .054 .072
.5 .5 - .001 .042 .058
Note: Number of dyads in the simulation was 30. Details of the simulation are provided in the text.
the section of the program output giving directly into AMOS, assigns names to each
“standardized” estimates. Note that the two of the six paths (making sure that paths that
models test identical constraints (e.g., yield are assumed equal receive identical names),
identical x* values), and the parameters in and gives names to the variances of each of
one model can be transformed into the pa- the four observed variables so that they can
rameters in the other model using the equa- be estimated (again, making sure to assign
tions presented in the text. the same name for variances that are as-
The first EQS syntax provides estimates sumed to be equal).
of rxyand rxy’.As displayed in Figure 1, this The second EQS syntax provides esti-
model does not contain latent variables, so mates of ri and rd with the restriction that the
we need to create latent variables that are two ri’s are equal to each other. This pro-
identical to the observed variables. This al- vides parameter estimates that are identical
lows us to place the necessary equality re- to those from the pairwise model. As men-
strictions required by the model. For exam- tioned in the text, the SEM framework has
ple, by equating a latent variable to the the advantage that it can estimate a model
wife’s trust score and a different latent vari- that permits different ri’s for each individual.
able to the husband’s trust score, one can To perform this more relaxed test, simply
set the variance of those two latent vari- delete the line “(E3,El) = (E4,E2);”. The
ables equal to each other, thus providing a model in Figure 2 can also be implemented
way to test whether the observed variance in AMOS. One simply draws the model, as-
for the wife’s trust is equal to the observed signs names to each path, making sure that
variance of the husband’s trust. paths that are assumed to be equal are as-
In the SEM package AMOS, the model signed identical names, and fixes the vari-
in Figure 1 is a little more straightforward ances of the latent variables to 1.
to implement. One simply draws Figure 1
/TITLE
MODEL I N F I G U R E 1: ESTIMATING r x y and r x y ‘
L i n e s b e g i n n i n g w i t h ! a r e comments
/SPECIFICATIONS
VARIABLES = 4; CASES = 98;
METHODS = ML;
MATRIX = COV;
/LABELS
V 1 = W i f e T r u s t ; V2 = Husband T r u s t ;
V3 = W i f e C o n f l i c t ; V4 = Husband C o n f l i c t ;
468 R. Gonzalez and D. Griffin
/EQUATIONS
V 1 = 1 F1 ; ! l a t e n t v a r i a b l e s w i t h o u t
V2 = 1 F2 ; ! e r r o r a r e d e f i n e d ,
V3 = 1 F3 ; ! i n d i c a t o r s a r e f i x e d t o 1
V4 = 1 F4 ;
/VARIANCES
F 1 = l*; ! l a t e n t variances are f r e e
F2 = l*;
F3 = l*;
F4 = l*;
/ C O V A R I ANCES
F2. F1 = .5*; ! a l l s i x p o s s i b l e c o v a r i a n c e s
F3, F 1 = .5*; ! a r e f r e e
F4, F 1 = .5*;
F3, F2 = .5*;
F4. F2 = .5*;
F4. F3 = .5*;
/CONSTRAINTS
( F l , F 1 ) = ( F 2 . F 2 ) ; ! l a t e n t X v a r i a n c e s equal
(F3, F3) = (F4, F 4 ) ; ! l a t e n t Y v a r i a n c e s equal
(F3, F 1 ) = ( F 4 . F 2 ) ; ! p o o l e d cov(X,Y)
(F4, F1) = (F3. F2); ! p o o l e d cov(X,Yprime)
/MATRIX
1.351
0.440 1.743
-0.857 - 0.365 2.421
-0.443 - 0.438 1.025 2.006
/PRINT
C O V A R I A N C E = YES;
/END
/TITLE
MODEL I N FIGURE 2 WITH INDIVIDUAL COVARIANCES SET EQUAL
L i n e s b e g i n n i n g w i t h ! a r e comments
/SPECIFICATIONS
VARIABLES = 4; CASES = 98;
METHODS = ML;
MATRIX = C O V ;
/LABELS
V 1 = W i f e T r u s t ; V2 = Husband T r u s t ;
V3 = W i f e C o n f l i c t ; V4 = Husband C o n f l i c t ;
F 1 = L a t e n t Dyad T r u s t : F2 = L a t e n t Dyad C o n f l i c t ;
E l = Err W i f e T r u s t ; E2 = E r r Hus T r u s t ;
E3 = Err W i f e Con; E4 = Err Hus Con;
/EQUATIONS
V 1 = 1* F 1 + 1* E l ;
V2 = I*F1 + 1* E2 ;
V3 = 1* F2 + 1* E3 ;
V4 = 1* F2 -k 1* E4 ;
Correlational analysis of dyad-level data 469
/VARIANCES
F1 = 1; ! l a t e n t and e r r o r v a r i a n c e s a l l f i x e d t o 1
F2 = 1;
E l = 1;
E2 = 1:
E3 = 1;
E4 = 1;
/COVARIANCES
F2. F 1 = .5*; ! e s t i m a t e dyad l e v e l cov(X.Y)
E3, E l = .5*; ! e s t i m a t e i n d i v i d u a l l e v e l cov
E4, E2 = .5*; ! e s t i m a t e i n d i v i d u a l l e v e l cov
/CONSTRAINTS
( E 3 , E l ) = (E4,E2): ! s e t two c o v ( X . Y ) ’ s equal
( V 1 , E l ) = (V2,E2); ! c o n s t r a i n e r r o r v a r i a n c e s w i t h i n
(V3,E3) = (V4,E4); ! v a r i a b l e t o be equal
(V1,Fl) = (V2,Fl); !constrain i n d i c a t o r s w i t h i n
(V3.F2) = ( V 4 , F 2 ) : ! v a r i a b l e t o be equal
/MATRIX
1.351
.440 1.743
-.857 - .365 2 . 4 2 1
-.443 - .438 1.025 2.006
/PRINT
C O V A R I A N C E = YES:
/END