Color Combination PDF
Color Combination PDF
Color Combination PDF
DOI 10.1007/978-3-642-27851-8_226-2
# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
Color Combination
Jose Luis Caivano*
Secretaria de Investigaciones FADU-UBA, University of Buenos Aires and Conicet, Buenos Aires, Argentina
Synonyms
Color coordination; Color harmony; Color mixture; Color syntax; Color union
Definition
Color combination is mainly an aspect of color syntax. To combine means to put one thing in relation to
another, or several things together, so that the individuals lose significance and the meaning of the whole
predominates. To combine also means to organize an ordered sequence. In some cases it acquires the
sense of mixing or merging. However, mixing color pigments or lights normally yields just one color as
the result of the mixture, and in this sense it cannot be termed a color combination, for which two or more
colors in some relation must be perceived. Combination certainly is very closely connected to harmoni-
zation and coordination. Color combination, thus, is meant whenever there is more than one color
associated, related, or harmonized with another: two colors already determine a certain kind of
combination.
Overview
Color combination is, in principle, an aspect of color syntax. All perceptible colors can be organized in the
so-called ▶ color order systems or models. This is usually made by means of three color variables or
dimensions, for instance, hue, saturation, and lightness (HSL), or hue, blackness, and chromaticness
(according to the Natural Color System), or hue, value, and chroma (according to the Munsell color
system), or some other similar triad of variables. These color order systems allow for the precise
identification and notation of colors and their arrangement in a logical way. Some of the systems even
allow to predict the results of color mixtures. It is possible to compare color order systems to dictionaries,
which assemble all the words available in a language in alphabetical order. So, color order systems arrange
and organize all the colors that humans can see, produce, and use, according to certain sequences
determined by the mentioned color variables.
This possibility of having the repertoire of all perceptible colors orderly arranged facilitates the
selection of colors, by following certain criteria, in order to use them in artistic compositions, architectural
works, or pieces of design. Only in few rare occasions (for instance, in experimental situations) colors are
seen isolated; in the great majority of cases (both in nature and in human productions), colors appear in a
context where there are also other colors. That is to say, every color is combined in a certain way with
other colors. Such as words (which in a dictionary appear isolated) are combined with other words in order
to make sentences and phrases with some sense and give origin to poetry, narrative, essay, etc., and also in
the same way as sounds are combined according to the criteria and invention of a music composer to give
origin to musical pieces, so colors are grouped in larger compositional units. And it is the context, the
*Email: [email protected]
Page 1 of 8
Encyclopedia of Color Science and Technology
DOI 10.1007/978-3-642-27851-8_226-2
# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
particular combination, the way in which colors are grouped together and related to one another, what
gives a sense, a certain kind of signification or meaning, some utility to the whole composition and to each
of the involved colors.
Painters dispose and mix colors in their ▶ palettes with the final aim of combining them in a canvas.
Architects combine materials with different natural colors in a building or either use paints to endorse
different parts of their work with color. Filmmakers and directors of photography decide about the color
sequences that appear in successive scenes of the film. Clothing and fashion designers think about the
chromatic combinations of the apparels they produce. Landscape designers choose and arrange the
botanical species and other materials taking into account also color combinations. And it is possible to
continue providing this kind of examples almost indefinitely, because there is practically no profession,
discipline, or human activity in which color does not play a role.
If according to different authors and experiments, the number of perceptible colors may range,
depending on various factors, from 2,000,000 to 7,000,000, the combinatorial possibilities rise to billions,
even restricting them to the minimal expression of just two- or three-color combinations. Now, how are
colors combined, with which kind of criteria, and in which type of contexts? At first, it is possible to talk
about spatial and temporal contexts, depending on the colors being arranged simultaneously in an object
or successively in a certain temporal sequence.
The possibility of both colors being some distance apart is not considered here because in this case the
color filling the separation, the background, appears as a third color. Also, there is no sense in considering
a total superposition of both colors (both occupying exactly the same space), because in this case the result
is just one color, and hence, this cannot be termed a color combination.
These three possibilities produce different results or have different consequences for color light and for
color-pigment combination and also differ if there is a mixture or blending of the involved colors or if
opaque color surfaces that do not mix together are combined. Combining colors imply that in some cases
the colors are mixed and give origin to new colors. However, if the result of the mixture is just one color,
this will not be a color combination.
For instance, considering transparent color filters:
1. If over an area of a transparent color filter A another piece of color filter B is set in relation of interiority,
the outcome is two colors: color A and a new color, C, which is the subtractive mixture of A and B,
while color B is missed.
2. If the colors overlap, the result is three colors: A, B, and C.
3. In the case of juxtaposition, there is no color mixture, so that the result is color A just besides color B.
Page 2 of 8
Encyclopedia of Color Science and Technology
DOI 10.1007/978-3-642-27851-8_226-2
# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
Page 3 of 8
Encyclopedia of Color Science and Technology
DOI 10.1007/978-3-642-27851-8_226-2
# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
Fig. 2 One example of Jannello’s logical scheme providing rules for selecting harmonious colors to be used in a color
combination
César Jannello had a logical way to face the issue of color harmony. He used to pose the aesthetic
problem in design in terms of constancy or variation of perceptual variables: too much constancy
produces boredom, too much variation generates visual chaos. Thus, it is in-between these two extremes
that a fruitful field of harmonies in design can be found. Starting from the three color variables or
dimensions – for instance, hue, saturation, and lightness – there are just eight possibilities for the selection
of colors, whether these variables are kept constant or change. In Fig. 2, the sign plus (+) means constancy,
and the sign minus ( ) represents variation of the considered dimension. The first formula, the one in
which everything is constant, is not of much use because it gives as a result the selection of just one and the
same color (even when it may be boring, a color combination where the same color is repeated is possible,
however). In the remaining formulas, where some type of variation appears, the interval of variation may
be kept constant or may change according to some criterion, for instance, by modifying hue, lightness, or
saturation in regular steps or intervals; by increasing intervals; by choosing opposite poles; etc. This
model provides a logical basis for the selection of colors to be applied in a combination.
Page 4 of 8
Encyclopedia of Color Science and Technology
DOI 10.1007/978-3-642-27851-8_226-2
# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
Page 5 of 8
Encyclopedia of Color Science and Technology
DOI 10.1007/978-3-642-27851-8_226-2
# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
Color combinations have been studied from the semantic point of view by various authors. Elda Cerrato
points out the basic concepts behind the idea of color combination, discusses some color order systems
and color harmony theories related to this (mainly Ostwald, Munsell, and Arnheim), and addresses the
issue of how culture conditions harmonies, preferences, and aesthetical principles of color
combinations [5].
Shigenobu Kobayashi, working at the Nippon Color and Design Research Institute, has devised a
method to classify single colors or three-color combinations by their associated images [6, 7]. Through the
analysis using the axes warm/cool, soft/hard, and clear/grayish as coordinates, this method, which was
also extended and developed with some collaborators [8, 9], can plot climatic and cultural differences in
color semantics. In 1997, Kobayashi and Iwamatsu extended the color combination research to be able to
include five-color combinations [10], even when from the countless number of possible five-color
combinations they chose to make the survey by selecting 20 pairs of contrasting combinations of five
colors (40 color combinations in total). To justify that particular research, they point out that “a five-color
combination makes it easier to gain a psychological understanding of scenic conditions and convey the
sense of ambience than two- and three-color combinations.”
The book by Hideaki Chijiiwa, from the Musashino College of Art, at Musashino Art University,
intends to be a manual for choosing color combinations for different purposes, taking into account
meanings and mood [11]. For practical applications in art, design, industry, or everyday life, it provides
a guide for selecting two-, three-, and four-color combinations associated to adjectives such as striking,
tranquil, exciting, natural, warm, cold, young, feminine, and surprising. As for the quantity of colors to be
employed in a combination, the author advises to limit them to two or three. A warning is made when
using four-color combinations that should be selected very carefully, while five-color combinations are
directly discouraged. The book by Bride M. Whelan continues in the same venue [12].
Lars Sivik (working sometimes in collaboration with Anders Hård and Charles Taft) carried out
research on the meanings of color combinations [13–15]. The descriptive model uses the Natural Color
System as a basis, and the methods are aimed at studying the stability and variability of color-meaning
associations across time and cultures. These studies “literally mapped the world of color with respect to
how associations to various words systematically vary across different parts of the color world.” In the
research published in 1989, Sivik selected 130 words by a semantic differential scaling method, and the
subjects judged color images as “to how well the different word went with the color composition in
question” [15]. The main purpose was “to obtain a small number of variables that would be reasonably
representative of all color describing variables.”
Other authors have also used the semantic differential method to study the meanings of color
combinations, and more specifically their affective values, by applying this tool to two- and three-color
harmonies [16, 17].
Colors, in general, and color combinations, in particular, can be considered as a system of signs; they
certainly have syntactic aspects (which include both the elements and the combinatory), semantic aspects,
and also pragmatic aspects that imply the use of these signs by the interpreters. In a previous publication,
the author of this entry describes and illustrates various semiotic concepts with examples taken from color
theory and provides an account of some of the advances of color theory within the framework of semiotic
categories [18].
Some color theorists go even beyond these considerations, proposing that colors and color combina-
tions can be taken as a language, for instance, Luckiesh in 1918 [19], Sanz in 1985 and 2009 [20, 21],
Oberascher in 1993 [22], as well as Hård in the already mentioned article published in 1997 [2]. However,
this should be taken perhaps with certain caution. Human languages (for instance, verbal languages,
English, Spanish, French, German, etc.) serve not only for communicational purposes but also for
cognitive and modeling purposes; they allow to build categories, models, and theories about the world,
Page 6 of 8
Encyclopedia of Color Science and Technology
DOI 10.1007/978-3-642-27851-8_226-2
# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
in order to understand it, explain it, and make it meaningful for the human species. If it is possible to
demonstrate that color combinations can have a similar status, then the color-language idea will be more
than just a metaphor.
Cross-References
▶ Color Contrast
▶ Color Harmony
▶ Color Order Systems
▶ Complementary Colors
▶ Palette
References
1. Hård, A., Sivik, L.: A theory of colors in combination – A descriptive model related to the NCS color-
order system. Color Res. Appl. 26(1), 4–28 (2001)
2. Hård, A.: Colour as a language. Thoughts about communicating characteristics of colours. In: Sivik,
L. (ed.) Colour and Psychology. From AIC Interim Meeting 96 in Gothenburg, pp. 6–12. Scandina-
vian Colour Institute, Stockholm (1997)
3. Hård, A.: A colour combination theory for a human environment. In: Mondial Couleur 85, Pro-
ceedings of the 5th Congress of the AIC. Centre Français de la Couleur, Paris (1985)
4. Sivik, L., Hård, A.: Some reflections on studying colour combinations. Color Res. Appl. 19(4),
286–295 (1994)
5. Cerrato, E.: Cultura y combinatorias de color: cómo la cultura condiciona armonías, preferencias,
recomendaciones, leyes estéticas en las combinatorias del color. In: Caivano, J., Amuchástegui, R.,
López, M. (comp.) Argen Color 2002, Actas del 6 Congreso Argentino del Color, pp. 27–34. Grupo
Argentino del Color, Buenos Aires (2004)
6. Kobayashi, S. (comp.): A Book of Colors. Kodansha International, Tokyo (1987)
7. Kobayashi, S.: Color Image Scale. Kodansha International, Tokyo (1991)
8. Kobayashi, S., Sato, K.: The theory of the color image scale and its application. In: Billmeyer, F.,
Wyszecki, G. (eds.) AIC Color 77, Proceedings of the 3rd Congress, pp. 382–383. Adam Hilger,
Bristol (1978)
9. Kobayashi, S., Suzuki, H., Horiguchi, S., Iwamatchu, K.: Classifying 3-color combinations by their
associated images on the warm/cool and clear/grayish axes. In: Nemcsics, A., Schanda, J. (eds.) AIC
Colour 93, Proceedings of the 7th Congress, vol. C, pp. 32–36. Hungarian National Color Commit-
tee, Budapest (1993)
10. Kobayashi, S., Iwamatsu, K.: Development of six methods of color psychological study. In: AIC
Color 97, Proceedings of the 8th Congress, pp. 727–730. The Color Science Association of Japan,
Kyoto (1997)
11. Chijiiwa, H.: Color Harmony. A Guide to Creative Color Combinations. Rockport Publishers,
Rockport (1987)
12. Whelan, B.M.: Color Harmony 2. A Guide to Creative Color Combinations. Rockport Publishers,
Rockport (1994)
13. Sivik, L.: Evaluation of colour combinations. In: Mondial Couleur 85, Proceedings of the 5th
Congress of the AIC. Centre Français de la Couleur, Paris (1985)
Page 7 of 8
Encyclopedia of Color Science and Technology
DOI 10.1007/978-3-642-27851-8_226-2
# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
14. Sivik, L.: Dimensions of meaning associated to color combination. In: AIC Symposium 1988, Colour
in Environmental Design, pp. 10.1–10.5. Winterthur Polytechnic, Winterthur (1988)
15. Sivik, L.: Research on the meanings of color combinations. In: AIC Color 89, Proceedings of the 6th
Congress, vol. II, pp. 130–133. Grupo Argentino del Color, Buenos Aires (1989)
16. Kansaku, J.: The analytic study of affective values of color combinations: a study of color pairs. Jap.
J. Psychol. 34, 11–12 (1963)
17. Nayatani, Y.: An analysis of affective values on three-color harmony by the semantic differential
method. In: Richter, M. (ed.) AIC Color 69, Proceedings of the 1st Congress, pp. 1073–1081. Muster-
Schmidt, Göttingen (1970)
18. Caivano, J.: Color and semiotics: a two-way street. Color Res. Appl. 23(6), 390–401 (1998)
19. Luckiesh, M.: The Language of Color. Dodd, Mead and Company, New York (1918)
20. Sanz, J.C.: El lenguaje del color. Hermann Blume, Madrid (1985)
21. Sanz, J.C.: Lenguaje del color: sinestesia cromática en poesía y arte visual. H. Blume, Madrid (2009)
22. Oberascher, L.: The language of colour. In: Nemcsics, A., Schanda, J. (eds.) AIC Colour 93, Pro-
ceedings of the 7th Congress, vol. A, pp. 137–140. Hungarian National Color Committee, Budapest
(1993)
Page 8 of 8