Antolin M. Llorente-Principles of Neuropsychological Assessment With Hispanics - 2007
Antolin M. Llorente-Principles of Neuropsychological Assessment With Hispanics - 2007
Antolin M. Llorente-Principles of Neuropsychological Assessment With Hispanics - 2007
Neuropsychological
Assessment with Hispanics
Issues of Diversity in Clinical Neuropsychology
Series Editor: Elaine Fletcher-Janzen, Ed.D., San Angelo, Texas
Forthcoming:
Neuropsychology of Poverty
by Elsa Shapiro
Principles of
Neuropsychological
Assessment with Hispanics
Theoretical Foundations and Clinical Practice
Edited by
Antolin M. Llorente
Department of Pediatrics
University of Maryland School of Medicine
and
Mount Washington Pediatric Hospital
Baltimore, Maryland
Editor
Antolin M. Llorente, Ph.D.
Department of Pediatrics
University of Maryland School of Medicine
Baltimore, MD 21201
USA
[email protected]
Series Editor
Elaine Fletcher-Janzen, Ed.D.
San Angelo, TX
USA
9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
springer.com
La educacin empieza en la cuna.
[We] live from birth to death in a world of persons and things which is in large
measure what it is because of what has been done and transmitted from previous
human activities. When this fact is ignored, experience is treated as if it were
something which goes on exclusively inside an individual, body and mind. It ought
not to be necessary to say that experience does not occur in a vacuum. There are
sources outside an individual which give rise to experience.
John Dewey
As long as our brain is a mystery, the universe, the reflection of the structure of
the brain will also be a mystery.
Santiago Ramn y Cajal
I have sworn upon the altar of God, eternal hostility against every form of tyranny
over the mind of man.
Thomas Jefferson
Bestir thyself therefore on this occasion; for though we will always lend thee
proper assistance in difficult places, as we do not, like some others, expect thee to
use the arts of divination to discover our meaning, yet we shall not indulge thy
laziness where nothing but thy own attention is required; for thou art highly
mistaken if thou dost imagine that we intended when we begun this great work to
leave thy sagacity nothing to do, or that without sometimes exercising this talent
thou wilt be able to travel through our pages with any pleasure or profit to
thyself.
Henry Fielding
Dedicado con amor agape, eterno aprecio y sincero respeto
a la
Sra. Armanda R. Fernndez
vii
Preface
ix
x Preface
with learning in general (cf. Kleim et al., 1997). Support for such a bidirectional
and nonlinear view of brain-behavior relationships also has been supported by stud-
ies in language development (Bates, Thal, and Janowski, 1992). A specific behav-
ior is capable of altering brain structure, and such transformation is associated with
enhanced performance, including new acquisition, competence, and expertise.
These examples suggest that the relationship between central nervous system struc-
tural alterations and behavioral competence or expertise appears to be exponential
and nonlinear in select circumstances. Similarly, psychoneuroimmunology most
recently has demonstrated the intimate nature of brain-behavior relationships and
their bidirectional interaction. This branch of neuroscience has shown unequivo-
cally that the level of functioning of individuals suffering from AIDS-related
dementia was enhanced as a result of interpersonal contextual variables (Kemeny
and Gruenewald, 2000). Enhancements in patients adaptation and functional level
was shown to be associated with increments in immune system response, and an
increase in T-cell response led to a reduction in viral load and associated changes
in brain functions leading to neurobehavioral benefits (e.g., affect, cognition). In
this case, the effect of an infectious disease with neurological involvement capable
of infringing upon behavior was diminished by an intervention (e.g., touch) with
significant impact on immune system response and indirectly on neural substrates
and underlying functions.
The relationship between culture and brain also is bidirectional, and in some
instances, nonlinear. With regard to our current understanding of neuropsychiatric
disorders, lest we are willing to admit intellectual bankruptcy, it is a well-known
fact that cultural context is inextricably intertwined with the expression of such
phenomena (cf. Mezzich and Lewis-Fernndez, 1997). In this regard, and despite
its simplistic approach, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-
Fourth Edition (DSM IV, Appendix) acknowledges the impact of culture and its
modulation on the manifestations of abnormal brain-driven affect, behavior, and
cognition (American Psychiatric Association, 1994, 2000). For example, the
expression of neuropsychopathology in two different Hispanic patients, or in the
same patient at distinct points in time, may vary depending on whether the patient(s)
attribute their problems to nervios a common cultural description of psychologi-
cal problems or to a known medical condition found in his or her family
(Guarnaccia, Lewis-Fernandez, and Marano, 2003). Regardless of the fact that
neuropsychopathology in both patients (or in a patient), may have the same
neurobiological etiology (e.g., endogenous clinical depression consequent to
diminished 5-HT availability in brain), their individual expressions and personal
interpretations may be different as a result of their distinct attributions as a conse-
quence of cultural contextualization. In other words, cultural context provides
patients, metaphorically speaking, a license that permits them to navigate through
the maze of attributions to reach the one that is perceived as most self-preserving,
indirectly impacting neural substrates with less negative effects on functional level.
Such a context also is critical in the rehabilitation of the Hispanic patient and plays
a major role in treatment outcome.
Preface xi
This book partly emerged out of necessity as a way of educating attorneys, graduate
students, interns, fellows, and some colleagues who over the course of the years
developed a genuine interest in cognitive processes and culture, particularly in
neuropsychology; in some instances, instruction in such issues was required by
their professions, educational curriculum, or other personal circumstances. However,
during the course of such interactions, unbeknownst to most of them, they indi-
rectly contributed to my own education and professional growth as it related to this
topic, and they thus surreptitiously contributed to this volume. Others contributed
much more directly, and even a book as small as this, and of so modest a
contribution to the rapidly evolving scholarly area of cross-cultural neuropsychol-
ogy applied to Hispanics, could not have been produced without the unselfish
assistance and encouragement of several individuals. Dedicated students and col-
leagues unselfishly provided assistance, time, and guidance. I greatly appreciated
the support and contributions of Professor Elaine Fletcher-Janzen, who convinced
me that writing this book, or as she called it, this labor of love, was meritorious
from several standpoints, including scholarly and humanitarian reasons, and who
encouraged me in the first place. Also, I want to acknowledge the contributions of
my early mentor, Professor Vicki Green, for originally introducing me to the field
of cross-cultural psychology as she supervised my first case as a clinician-in-train-
ing while assessing an unaccompanied child from Central America who, fearing for
her life, had immigrated to the U.S., seeking asylum in an American court. I addi-
tionally would like to express my sincere appreciation to Professors Paul Satz,
Louis DElia, and Wilfred van Gorp for supporting my career goals as a neuropsy-
chology fellow while at UCLA, Professors Christianne Cox, Keith Slifer, and Gina
Richman for providing excellent role models of scholar-practitioners when I was
an intern at Kennedy Krieger Institute/Johns Hopkins University School of
Medicine, and Professors Robert S. Schlottman and David Thomas for introducing
me to neuroscience and neuropsychology as a graduate student at Oklahoma State
University. I also would like acknowledge Professor Tony Wong for constructive
criticisms that have been included in this book from our cross cultural
conversations. I am additionally and particularly indebted to Professor Jossette
Harris for allowing me to use portions of our chapter which appear in different
xiii
xiv Acknowledgments
areas throughout this book, because I could not have expounded specific points
without her assistance. I also like to thank my coauthors, all committed young stu-
dents of brain-behavior relationships. I express my humble gratitude to Ms. Sara A.
Lawless (Chapter 1), Mr. Christian von Thomsen and Ms. Lori Gallup (Chapter 4),
Dr. Deborah Weber (Chapter 7), Dr. Brian Potter (Chapter 8), and Dr. Erik Lane
(Chapter 6). I also want to acknowledge the contributions of postdoctoral fellows
whom I have trained and who contributed to this volume, including Dr. Christine
French (Chapter 5) and Dr. Peter Smith (Chapter 6), individuals who carry upon
their shoulders the burden of assisting and educating future neuropsychologists
about Boas, Dewey, Luria, and Vygotsky and the impact of culture on the human
brain and indirectly on neurocognitive mechanisms.
I am also grateful to my colleagues for their assistance and for taking on respon-
sibilities beyond those specified in their job descriptions, which allowed me to have
free time to complete this volume particularly Dr. Julie Ries (Chapters 1 and 8).
My special thanks also are extended to Springer and Kluwer Publishing for their
patience during the period of time it took to produce this work. In particular, my
sincere appreciation is extended to Ms. Janice Stern for her executive editorial
assistance and for her willingness to keep foreboding forces at bay while I com-
pleted the volume. Finally, I want to express my gratitude to Ms. S. Geethalakshmi,
production editor, for her assistance in the timely delivery of this manuscript.
Although significant limitations were placed on manuscript length, precluding a
comprehensive review of the subject matter, I alone take full responsibility for the
content of this book, and my colleagues must be exonerated from literary weak-
nesses or shortcomings in style or substance encountered in the same. Mea culpa.
Finally, I extend my special gratitude to my lovely confidante and North Star,
Tina M. Llorente, a bright Abigail Adams of our times, for her continued support
and patience when I was absent while writing this book, in spite of my physical
presence.
Antolin M. Llorente
Seven Valleys, Pennsylvania
December 2006
Contents
Preface ............................................................................................................... ix
xv
xvi Contents
xvii
xviii Contributors List
Peter Smith
Mount Washington Pediatric Hospital
Baltimore, MD 21209
Christian von Thomsen
Loyola College, Maryland
Baltimore, MD 21211
Deborah Weber
Childrens National Medical Center
Department of Neuropsychology
Washington, DC 20010
List of Tables
Table 2.1 U.S. Census Estimate for the Hispanic Population According
to Country of Origin . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30
Table 2.2 Total Number of Immigrants (Absolute Migration) Across Six
Decades (1931-1990): Argentina, Cuba, and Mexico . . . . . . . . . . . 33
Table 2.3 Percentage of Total Legal Immigrants and Reported
Occupational Allegiance at Time of U.S. Entry for 1990 . . . . . . . . 34
Table 2.4 Percentage and Total Number of Legal Immigrants Reporting
Intended Metropolitan Region of Initial Residence for Three
countries for 1990: Five Selected U.S. regions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35
Table 4.1 Mean WISC-IV Scores of Hispanics and White Non-Hispanic
Children: Data from Standardization Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68
Table 4.2 Mean WISC-IV Scores of Hispanic and White Non-Hispanic
Children Equated for Age, Gender, Number of Parents Living
in the Household, Parental Education Level, and U.S. Region:
Data from Standardization Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69
Table 4.3 Matched Sample Mean WISC-IV Scores of Spanish versus
English-Speaking Hispanic and White Non-Hispanic English-
Speaking Children (Equated for Age, Gender, and Parental
Education Level) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69
Table 5.1 Early Language Developmental Expectations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82
Table 5.2 Language Disorders and Their Pseudonyms, Related
Pathology, and Neurological Disturbances . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97
Table 5.3 Neurocognitive Skills Impaired by Language Disorders . . . . . . . . 99
Table 5.4 Characteristics of Selected Language Disorders . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 100
Table 6.1 Assessment Domains, Sample Procedures Utilized
in Evaluations, and Potential Functions Screened . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124
Table 7.1 Contingency Table Used to Describe Base Rate,
Sensitivity and Specificity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 153
Table 7.2 Contingency Table: Example . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 154
Table 7.3 Contingency Table Used to Explain Base Rate, Negative
Predictive Value and Positive Predictive Value . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 155
Table 7.4 Contingency Table: Example . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 155
Table 7.5 SENAS Scales . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 162
xix
List of Figures
xx
Chapter 1
Introduction and Theoretical Foundations
1
See below for a comprehensive explanation of the use of the term Hispanic throughout this
volume.
1
2 S.A. Lawless et al.
diverse and large populations has been growing at an exponential rate as the result
of the aforementioned changes in the composition of American society. The need
to provide services to such populations encompasses all aspects of society but most
critically educational settings, public policy environments, health care, and the civil
and criminal justice systems. Moreover, the nonrandom, ever-shifting immigration
trends of the U.S., and their impact on neuropsychology, further complicate
attempts to respond to a challenge of such scope and magnitude (cf. Llorente, 1997;
Llorente et al., 1999, 2000). Countries of migrational origin, and the reasons for
immigrations, particularly large-scale migrations, have changed globally and
within the U.S. as a result of alterations in sociopolitical and economic climates,
and this shift has significant implications for neuropsychology (Llorente, 1997;
Llorente et al., 1999, 2000). With those changes, varying occupational status, edu-
cational attainment, and patterns of geographical settlement of immigrants in the
U.S., as well as other parts of the globe (e.g., Australia, China, France, Turkey),
have been observed, which likely affect observed brain-behavior relationships
(Llorente et al., 2000).
Efforts have been made to increase cultural competency among educators,
researchers and practitioners, including awareness, language skills, and stand-
ardized training and assessment procedures, methods, and instrumentation to
address the emerging scientific and applied need. For example, the Standards for
Educational and Psychological Testing (1999) established by the American
Psychological Association (APA), the American Educational Research
Association (AERA), and the National Council on Measurement in Education
(NCME) has put greater emphasis on ensuring the fairness in assessments of
individuals with diverse ethnic and racial backgrounds. The American
Psychological Association (APA, 1991, 2001, 2003) has established the
Guidelines for Providers of Psychological Services to Ethnic, Linguistic, and
Culturally Diverse Populations, urging consultations, supervision, and continu-
ing education to increase cultural competency in clinical practice. The Houston
Conference on Specialty Education and Training in Clinical Neuropsychology
(Hannay et al., 1998) included cultural and individual differences and diversity
as a recommended area of study under the Generic Psychology Core, and listed
recognition of multicultural issues under the Assessments and Treatment and
Interventions core skills sections (Hannay et al., 1998; Wong et al., 2000). Wong
et al. (2000) and others (Pontn and Len-Carrin, 2001; van Gorp, Myers and
Drake, 2000) have recommended ways in which educators and practitioners
could be more sensitive in identifying and understanding cultural, linguistic, and
ethnic differences through careful interviews, education, and the clinicians own
awareness of his/her biases.
Despite these efforts, a number of salient issues need to be addressed in
neuropsychological practice, research, and theory, and universals need to be par-
tially discarded. However, before embarking on a course to address such topics,
important fundamental, theoretical, nomenclatural, definitional, and ethical issues
require attention.
1 Introduction and Theoretical Foundations 3
because humans can use tools and symbols; as a result, they create cultures, and cultures
have a vitality, a life of their own. They grow and change and exert a very powerful influ-
ence on their members. They determine the end result of competent development the
sorts of things that its members must learn, the ways they should think, the things they are
most likely to believe. (p. 84)
Vygotsky, however, was clearly not the first person to appreciate the importance of
culture in the understanding of illness, injury, and intervention, but provided a
greater understanding of the impact of ones environment, and indirectly culture,
on development. According to Christensen and Castano (1996), Luria also made
significant contributions to these new conceptual models, which like Vygotskys,
were in essence cultural-historical models (Luria, 1979; cf. Wartofsky, 1983).
Although not related directly to neuropsychology, and unfortunately forgotten,
yet significantly influential from a theoretical standpoint, the German (and later
American) cultural anthropologist Franz Boas was an early and strong proponent
of theories underlying the forces of culture on behavior and indirectly on the brain.
According to Boas (Kuper, 1999), considered the father of modern American
anthropology, based on his studies of the Inuit in the latter part of the 19th century,
it was culture that shapes humans, not their physiology or psychology. In addition,
culture was not a linear or upward mechanism, but an emergent characteristic
acquired from art, rituals, songs, traditions, and customs.
In the U.S., from a philosophical perspective, another view leading to a meta-
physical paradigmatic shift in psychology, and indirectly on neuropsychology, was
provided by the neopragmatist philosopher, psychologist, and educator John
Dewey, the founder of the Chicago School of Pragmatism. According to Dewey,
whose moral epistemology is contextualist (see Boydston, 1981; Dewey, 1938),
contextualism discarded the idea that values and norms were void of external
influences and practices. He rejected any notion of intrinsic value as a property
that has value in itself, regardless of context. As noted in page v of this book, he
stated that:
individuals live from birth to death in a world of persons and things transmitted from
previous human activities. When this fact is ignored, experience is treated as if it were
something which goes on exclusively inside an individuals body and mind. It ought not to
be necessary to say that experience does not occur in a vacuum.2
2
It is poignant to note that the philosophical foundations of radical environmentalism, and to some
extent, contextualism, were affected, or at least perceived in the U.S., to be impacted by the ideas
and writings of Marx and Engels, and coupled with the Cold War such theoretical foundations,
were not part of the core educational expositions in American neuropsychological educational
circles. As noted by Lezak (1995), neuropsychology is a child of its time and place.
6 S.A. Lawless et al.
during and throughout neural development (Kennepohl, 1999). Although there has
been limited empirical support, Kennepohls model provides new avenues for
continuing research concentrating on cultural and ethnic factors in neuropsychology.
Kennepohls model is another example that demonstrates neuropsychologys ability
to endure a paradigmatic shift and pursue problem-solving solutions relevant to
such a shift.
Why are these fundamental, theoretical, philosophical, and historical antecedents
important? It is critical to recognize that neuropsychology as a discipline has an
underlying epistemological and natural history and that its evolution, in many
respects, is similar to that of all other scientific disciplines. It is also critical to
understand that as a discipline, neuropsychology is not free of underlying roots and
zeitgeist (cf. Lezak, 1995), which in essence has an architecture and structure of
attitudes, beliefs, methods, practices, skill sets, and values that define it and that
shape its present and future. As a discipline, neuropsychology is not impervious to
a process of alteration, evolution, and metamorphosis, as is evident for all other
sciences, natural or social. Finally, such alterations are the results of shifts in those
characteristics that define neuropsychology, leading to evolutionary phases in the
discipline and the adoption of new ideas and explanatory frameworks and changes
in theory and clinical practice, including the inclusion of cultural factors and ethni-
city, in essence a paradigmatic shift.
Although space constraints limit its exposition, this chapter must address the
relationship between the brain and culture through genetics, as such a relationship
supports a hypothesis suggesting that genes (and indirectly brains) and culture are
closely intertwined. This is particularly true from our vantage point at the onset of
the 21st century, when great advances have emerged in genetics, particularly during
the last part of the 20th century as a result of the Human Genome Project and other
investigations.
However, before we address the topic in humans, let us examine for a moment a
very important yet often forgotten experiment, conducted with animals in the 1960s
in Russia by the geneticist Belyaev. Belyaev was interested in taming wild, and
aggressive, foxes which were being used for their coats in his fur business. To do so,
Belyaev began by selecting tamer wild foxes from his breeding stock, those that were
most approachable, less shy, and less likely to flee when in his presence. As he pro-
ceeded with his breeding program, he discovered that he indeed was producing tamer
animals, and in some instances he produced foxes that would actually approach him
and his staff. However, these alterations in behavioral characteristics (tamability)
were accompanied by an unforeseen result, namely that the tamed foxes fur also had
been altered. More important, as far as neuropsychology is concerned, the tamed
foxes had the behavioral and physical characteristics of domesticated dogs, such as
tamed comportment, floppy ears, rolled tails and shorter snouts, and brains that were
8 S.A. Lawless et al.
smaller than those of wild foxes. In other words, by selecting a specific, desirable
behavior (approachability, tamability), and genetically selecting for it over many
generations (over 20 to be precise), Belyaev not only impacted the physical and
behavioral characteristics of the animals, but also their brain! [Belyaev, 1979]
Unfortunately, an animal study does not support a hypothesis suggesting that
genes, the environment, and culture are inextricably related in brain development in
humans. Therefore, let us examine human studies. One study involves the work of the
neuroscientist Rizzolatti and his colleagues (c.f., Kohler et al., 2002), who researched
brain functions using functional MRI scans of the brain. These researchers have dem-
onstrated that mirror neurons, which have been speculated to be responding to an
action, also respond to the vision of an action, most likely a mechanism of brain
function involved in imitation, a critical aspect of the transmission of cultural varia-
bles. In fact, in another study they have shown that these types of neurons may not
respond to the observation and enactment of a behavior or action, but respond in a
similar fashion to a noise associated with the action (Kohler et al., 2002). Another
recent finding involves language, a domain closely involved with culture and brain
functions. In this study, reported by Lai et al. (2001), investigators discovered a muta-
tion responsible for a severe type of speech and language disorder. This gene, known
as forkhead box P2, is a gene responsible for modulating other genes, and when
abnormal, leads to language and speech disorders, because the gene is necessary for
the normal development of speech and grammar, language closely associated with the
transmission of culture through narrative, songs, and other factors closely associated
with language.
In sum, it is clear from these examples that brains, through genetic mechanisms
and culture, and through a myriad of mechanisms, including imitation and language,
are closely intertwined. As we stated in the Preface, there appears to be a bidirec-
tional relationship between brain and behavior, and because behavior is partially
shaped by the environment, particularly in humans, and because humans are
impacted by their culture, culture affects brain.
It is also critical to note that ethnicity, culture, and race are not related to
nationality. Individuals with the same nationality or region of origin may have dif-
ferent races, ethnicity, and cultural characteristics, and this is particularly true for
Hispanics (cf. Shorris, 1992). It is also critical to understand that Hispanics, as will
be noted later, may have multiple racial and ethnic backgrounds.
Within the context of culture, ethnicity, and race, the concept of Hispanic is bet-
ter defined. The term Hispanic is used throughout this text to refer to all individuals
perceiving themselves to be Latino, Spanish, and Spanish-speaking individuals.
Puente and Ardila (2000) note that some dictionaries, such as the Merriam-
Websters Dictionary, include individuals from Portugal as Hispanics. In that sense,
individuals who speak Portuguese or dialects thereof may be considered Hispanic,
and thus the term would include individuals from Brazil and other regions of the
world. They also note that El Diccionario de la Lengua Real Espaola (Real
Academia Espaola, 1984) is more restrictive, limiting the term Hispanics to indi-
viduals from Spain or Spanish-speaking Latin America. As noted by Puente and
Ardila (2000), such a restrictive definition may be incorrect. Therefore, the term
Hispanic in this text is intended to represent individuals from Latin or Central
America and the Caribbean (i.e., Latino), as well as from Mexico and from other
Spanish-speaking (e.g., Spain) origins and the U.S., and individuals who identify
themselves as such because they perceive themselves as Hispanic. As might be
expected, and as noted by Harris and Llorente (2005), Hispanic individuals living
in the U.S. and other parts of the world share many of their institutional and
societal structures, including values, political, economic, and general educational
systems. However, groups of Hispanic individuals vary greatly with regard to
country of origin, educational attainment, religion, use of language(s), and other
important variables, and the pan-ethnic label Hispanic fails to include these
unique individual attributes.
Although an argument could be made that any ethnic group living within the
U.S. may represent a heterogeneous cohort, this is especially the case for
Hispanics living in this country. Aside from issues related to language differ-
ences to be covered in detail in Chapter 5, significant heterogeneity emerges even
if language is excluded. In order to understand the genesis of such heterogeneity, it
is critical to learn that the pan-ethnic label Hispanic fails to include unique
attributes, and race unfortunately often, but inappropriately, has been used as an
ethnic category, even by governmental entities, which creates significant confusion
and problems, particularly for neuropsychology (see Chapters 2, 3, and 6; cf.
Llorente et al., 1999). Although as noted by Harris and Llorente (2005), the iden-
tification as a separate race stems from the blending of races within the history of
some Hispanic peoples, including the result of intermarriage of the European
Spaniards with the indigenous Indians, producing the mestizo, many a Mexican-
American peoples favor the distinction of the term Chicanos. This preference rep-
resents certain political and ethnic perspectives and emerges because they often
consider themselves to be descendents of this new race that migrated northward
to the U.S. from Mexico. However, others prefer the term Mexican-American. As
noted by Harris and Llorente (2005), even within a specific nationality/ethnic
1 Introduction and Theoretical Foundations 11
grouping, such as the largest category in the U.S. (Mexican), there are addi-
tional factors to take into consideration, including the fact that the term Mexican
encompasses both U.S.-born and Mexican immigrants.
Evidently, the term Hispanic is in fact a pan-ethnic term used to identify a
number of cultural or ethnic groupings, and Hispanic individuals can claim one or
more of many racial origin(s) as well as any Spanish-speaking country of origin,
nationality, or ethnicity. Even within an intra-ethnic definition, individuals emigrat-
ing from Mexico may identify themselves with any of many ethnic groups that
reside in that country (Vzquez, 1994). In addition to traditional Spanish, many of
these individuals may use languages which exceed over 200 different indigenous
living languages, including Mayan, Nhuatl, and Tamaulipeco (Harris and Llorente,
2005). Similar considerations, for example, are applicable to Hispanics from
Guatemala and other nations. Therefore, Hispanic individuals within the U.S. may
consequently be of any race, any ethnicity or combination of ethnicities (e.g., par-
ents with Puerto Rican and Colombian nationalities), and may be monolingual
Spanish speakers, monolingual English speakers, bilingual (e.g., Spanish-Mayan),
or multilingual (e.g., Nhuatl-Spanish-English), even if English is not yet a profi-
cient language (Harris and Llorente, 2005). At one end of this continuum
Hispanics may represent recent immigrants, monolingual Spanish-speaking indi-
viduals, and at the other end of the spectrum they may represent children
whose ancestors have been living in the U.S. for multiple generations and whose
parents may not share the same ethnicity (Hispanic or other), and may not even
speak the Spanish language (Harris and Llorente, 2005).
Pontn (2001b) provides an intriguing, although simple, model of the diversity
of the Hispanic population, one that is useful when interpreting assessment
results during the course of neuropsychological evaluation. He proposes to view
the population as a cube with variation along three dimensions, namely (a) years
of exposure to education, (b) country of origin, and (c) language proficiency
(English, Spanish, or bilingual). Although such a model is a good starting point,
clearly it could easily be made more complex and comprehensive by adding more
factor values, such as proficiency in indigenous languages, acculturation (Pontn,
2001b; Roysircar, 2004), region of origin or residence (urban/rural), or migration
history (Llorente et al., 1999).
In the final analysis, it is sincerely hoped that the reader realizes that the terms
culture, ethnicity, and race encompass different constructs that are not interchange-
able. It is also hoped that the reader surmises that the term Hispanic is being used
in this text as a literary term to refer to individuals of a Spanish or Latino back-
ground who identify themselves as Hispanic, yet in no way are these individuals
being characterized as a homogenous group, since heterogeneity is the rule within
this population (cf. Harris and Llorente, 2005; Puente and Ardila, 2000; Shorris,
1992). Hispanics can be of many races and ethnocultural backgrounds, and even
within the same ethnic group, there are interethnic differences. The term Hispanic,
or that of other groups of individuals for that matter, such as Cuban or Mexican,
is being used for ease of expression, not any other reason, and the authors hope that
individuals understand the diversity encompassed by such pan-ethnic terms.
12 S.A. Lawless et al.
Acculturation occurs when foreign cultural traits and values are adopted by a
society on a large scale or when a minority group or an individual adopts, assimi-
lates, or conforms to and integrates the characteristics, norms, and values of another
culture (cf. Berry, 1997; Portes and Rumbaut, 1990). In the process, the culture of
the receiving society is altered as a result of such acculturation, as in changes in
American society associated with large-scale migrations of Hispanics and other
ethnic minority groups (e.g., Italians), yet the emerging culture is not completely
new, but rather an interrelationship of amalgamations leading to the union of the
existing traditional and foreign traits. Within this context, acculturation and assimi-
lation are being used to described the adoption of values and foreign cultural traits
by an individual, in this case, Hispanic, as result of immigration or residence within
a foreign culture (U.S.). In this context, for example, a Hispanic individual from the
Dominican Republic adopts new values and traits over the years after his arrival in
the U.S., and in this sense a certain degree of assimilation occurs in this individual.
As is the case for many of these variables, acculturation can exist and occur in vary-
ing degrees along a spectrum. For example, some Hispanic individuals, upon
arrival in the U.S., as result of desire or necessity, exhibit little if any acculturation
to American society, whereas others extensively acculturate to American society en
masse. It is critical to note that many variables influence degree of acculturation,
including age of the individual or residential area of preference (Portes and
Rumbaut, 1990). In this regard, it is easy to realize how there is no need for a
Hispanic individual who immigrates to Miami, Florida, to assimilate American
culture unless it is desired or perceived to be beneficial, whereas the opposite may
be required if a Hispanic individual, upon arrival to the U.S., were to reside in
Bismarck, North Dakota. In some instances, some of these individuals partially or
completely may discard their Hispanic identity, or may never become acculturated
as measured by bilingual status along with other acculturation traits.
From an applied standpoint, the issue of acculturation is important for cross-cultural
neuropsychology. As noted by Pontn (2001a), level of acculturation is critical because
it assists the clinician to make important pragmatic determinations related to the assess-
ment process. When necessary, acculturation should be assessed formally during the
course of a neuropsychological examination, if at all possible. Acculturation scales have
been shown to predict generational cohort, degree of acculturation, and other factors
(Marin et al., 1984). These scales depend on test items that tap into preferred language
use, language spoken in the home, language use during leisure time, or friendships.
Aside from the fact that there are scales that permit the objective assessment of accul-
turation for Hispanics (Marin et al., 1984), assessment is helpful because it helps guide
the process in terms of its components, including language use during the assessment,
the selection of assessment procedures, and test performance interpretation to name
a few factors (cf. Pontn, 2001a). If formal assessment is not possible, acculturation
also can be gauged through the use of similar information, including language spoken
in the home, the nature of friendships, and leisure activities. Aside from measuring
language dominance and literacy, readily published formal reading, reading compre-
hension, and phonemic processing tests can also be used to address the degree of accul-
turation of a client.
1 Introduction and Theoretical Foundations 13
most likely play distinct roles in neuropsychology. For example, not every literate
Hispanic individual has received formal education in excess of a few years of
schooling or any at all. Therefore, although literacy can have significant impact on
neuropsychological assessment, or cognitive development and mechanisms for that
matter, lack of education can have independent effects that are not associated with
illiteracy and that are capable of accounting for problems sometimes observed in
neuropsychological performance. Although Heaton et al. (1986) have made a
strong argument as to the importance of education in neuropsychological test per-
formance, research addressing culture and education merits attention, particularly
for Hispanics. In this regard, Ostrosky-Solis et al. (2004) examined the influence of
education and culture on neuropsychological performance in indigenous and non-
indigenous populations in Mexico. Although sample sizes were small, their results
suggested that culture and education exert independent effects on neuropsychological
performance. Culture reportedly dictates what it is important for survival and
education could be considered as a type of subculture that facilitates the develop-
ment of certain skills.
From an applied standpoint, the issue of education will be explored in more
detail in later chapters. However, it is sufficient here to note that many tests and
procedures, including those considered by many practitioners as gold standards in
instrumentation, have significant deficiencies in this regard, and many do not have
normative data for specific groups of individuals, particularly Hispanics, with lim-
ited or very advanced educational backgrounds (Ardila, 1998; Llorente, 1997;
Llorente et al., 1999, 2000; Puente and Ardila, 2000). Even more perplexing is the
fact that such data may not even be available for individuals from the mainstream
culture, yet clinicians continue to use such norms without questioning the validity
or reliability of the inferences derived from them. For example, the reader is asked
to determine how many 75- to 79-year-old Hispanics who came from educational
backgrounds with 9 to 11 years of education are found in the WAIS-III standardiza-
tion sample, or more relevantly, how many 10-year-old white children who came
from backgrounds whose average parental educational is 8 years are found in the
WISC-IVs standardization sample?3
With regard to test construction, as noted by Harris and Llorente (2005), there
are significant implications for level of education associated with the standardization
of cognitive and neuropsychological procedures when applied to Hispanics. They
note that normative studies comprised of examinees with lower mean education,
or in the case of children, lower parental education, are important to scrutinize
because Test publishers typically stratify socioeconomic status within ethnic
3
Because education plays such a pre-eminent role in neuropsychology and its clinical practice,
and because low levels of educational attainment have been implicated in poor neuropsychological
performance in some instances, it is given special attention in several chapters throughout this
text. However, the reader should not assume that all Hispanics come from impoverished back-
grounds with low levels of educational attainment, as such an assumption is far from actuality in
the U.S., the fifth largest Hispanic population on the face of the globe.
1 Introduction and Theoretical Foundations 15
(1970) model, this approach is how practice evolves in a discipline, and how
paradigmatic shifts eventually become extant models of thought and practice.
Criticizing such a posture fails to take into account the natural progression of a
discipline, particularly during the infancy of a paradigmatic shift.
Another important factor to consider is the generalization of test data, normative
data in particular, that have been collected in other countries, that were developed for
groups of individuals different from the individual undergoing assessment (see
Llorente et al., 1999), and that are used with such an individual simply because he or
she speaks the common language (i.e., Spanish) or other convenient reasons (cf.
Mitrushina et al., 1999). Many problems are inextricably related to this assessment
posture, including lack of language dominance and ethnic differences (cf. Puente and
Ardila, 2000). It also includes cultural factors. For example, norms developed with
Spanish-speaking individuals in Spain may not be applicable to individuals who
speak Spanish in the U.S., despite the fact that both may be fluent in Spanish. This
issue is encountered with greater frequency as a result of increased globalization. As
tests are produced in specific countries (e.g., Australia, United Kingdom, and Spain)
and applied in other areas of the world, where populations may be inherently different
from an ethnic and cultural standpoint yet not linguistically, and it is likely that such
instances may become more prevalent, yet not always appropriate. As a case in point,
it is not unusual to see a client, pediatric or adult, seem confused with a test stimulus
(e.g., Union Jack) from a test developed in another country, leading to failure by the
patient on that particular test item in the U.S.
An egregious posture that occurs every so often also includes the use of other
professionals acting as health professionals or in a similar capacity without having
training in the health sciences, but acting as an evaluator for purposes of assessment
because he or she may be bilingual. Such unethical and illegal conduct does not
require or deserve further elucidation. Another problem sometimes encountered in
the practice of neuropsychology is the use of a unique and live translation of a
test published in English into Spanish, a test without a published Spanish version
by a bilingual clinician. In this case, a clinician essentially creates a unique, live,
on-site, unpublished Spanish translation of a test that does not have an authorized
or published version, adding to the problem by referring to existing norms for the
English version test. It is clear that such postures should be avoided, and an expla-
nation for such a rationale is not necessary (cf. Artiola i Fortuni et al., 2005).
Another inappropriate approach frequently encountered in practice is the admin-
istration of only one scale of a comprehensive test (e.g., WAIS III, Performance
Scale). This practice involves administering only one scale of a test with many or
multiple scales and subsequently using such narrow information to generalize and
inferentially approximate an individuals overall or omnibus score, such as his or
her overall intellect. This practice is unfounded and marked by several problems,
and it relies on inappropriate assumptions about cognitive processes and psycho-
metric properties of tests. One important misconceived assumption is that cognitive
processes may be discrete phenomena. In other words, when administering a visual
reasoning scale of a test without administering its accompanying verbal reasoning
scale, it is incorrect to assume that visual processes occur independently of verbal
18 S.A. Lawless et al.
processes or that language and verbal reasoning processes do not enter the
evaluative process during the completion of a task that predominantly may require
visual processing skills (Lezak et al., 2004). Another erroneous assumption is
the belief that administering such a scale provides a more accurate interpretation
and gauge of the individuals overall skill. Although an accurate measurement may
emerge for the scale administered, such a restrictive assessment process fails to
assess other major functional areas, areas that actually may be impaired. Although
such a practice was used inappropriately for years with individuals with severe
sensory (auditory or visual) handicapping conditions, such as deaf and hard-of-
hearing persons or the legally blind, such an approach should not be used with a
Hispanic individual. Finally, the reader should surmise from the above that we are
not arguing against the use of a single test scale (WAIS-III, Performance IQ score)
to examine a specific domain, such as visual (nonverbal) reasoning, but rather we
are arguing against the misuse of such a scale to infer an individuals overall index
(WAIS-III, Full Scale IQ score) from his score on the single scale.
From a psychometric standpoint, an assessment posture whereby only one scale
is administered is not appropriate for multiple reasons. However, due to lack of
space, only major reasons will be addressed here. First, perusal of the correlations
between subscales and overall index for most tests reveals that such subscales are
incapable of accounting for the overall test variance. For some tests (e.g., WAIS-
III), such test scales (e.g., Performance IQ score) account for approximately 79%
to 90% of the total variance in overall intellect (FSIQ), depending on specific
demographics. Second, it should not be assumed that such a variance is the same
for all ages; differences exist in the amount of variance that is accounted for by
specific subscales from the total variance as a function of chronological age. Third,
as noted by Kaufman and Flanagan (2004), as the difference between an individuals
subscale scores increases, the validity of the omnibus index may not be interpretable
or its interpretation may become difficult because the subscales may not be
measuring the construct they were meant to assess, leading to a spurious omnibus
index. Finally, the base rates of such differences are important (cf. McCaffrey et al.,
2003), yet sometimes not available, or worse yet, not considered, thus biasing and
hindering interpretation. In summary, such an approach should be avoided, and an
appropriate, comprehensive measure should be administered.
Although a more comprehensive discourse related to this topic is found in
Chapter 5, a narrow summary of specific issues associated with bilingualism appli-
cable to neuropsychological assessment will be provided here. As noted by Harris
and Llorente (2005), the relationship of language proficiency and bilingualism to
cognitive performance has long been a sensitive topic but one that has direct bear-
ing on the performance discrepancies observed for some ethnic minority (e.g.,
Hispanic) versus non-minority groups. Despite their absurd conclusions, early
studies suggested that bilingualism might represent a cognitive liability, yet later
investigations revealed that such differences were the result of artifact associated
with flawed research methodologies, including failure to control for socioeconomic
and other variables, heterogeneity in the samples defining bilingual, and other
factors (cf. Paradis, 1978).
1 Introduction and Theoretical Foundations 19
The majority of factors discussed above are common knowledge and have been
well articulated in the cross-cultural neuropsychological literature by several
authors. One area, however, that is capable of hampering neuropsychological
assessments with Hispanic populations that has not been given proper attention is
psychometric issues associated with reliability and validity.
Reliability (Rx) is a measure of consistency or the degree of stability or accuracy
of test results associated with inferences in neuropsychological assessments. Using
traditional psychometric theory, an individuals score on a test (Xtest score) is com-
prised of a true score (Xtrue), representing the actual characteristic or trait under
investigation, and error (Xerror), assumed to be random in nature. Because random
error enters the measurement process, a persons true score is not observable on any
test (Kirk, 1990). In other words:
Reliability then represents the ratio of true score variance over the observed score
variance. Although a more detailed coverage of this area is beyond the scope of this
discussion, and Chapter 6 provides a comprehensive discussion of ways to reduce
such error variance, it is important to realize that the reliability of the inferences
derived from a battery of neuropsychological test is impacted by each one of its
components, and that the use of interpreters, or any other factors, such as an inap-
propriate measurement as a result of language differences, lack of acculturation,
and so on, may lead to increased bias or diminished reliability, thus hindering the
evaluative process, particularly nonrandom error.
It is critical to note that, unfortunately, the overall reliability (Rx) of an assess-
ment battery is the multiplicative product of the reliability of each of its compo-
nents (1, 2, n). In essence:
where 1, 2, and n represent each test or procedure used in a battery and overall
assessment during the course of neuropsychological evaluation. In other words, the
reliability of a neuropsychological battery (or evaluation) is, at best, as high as the
lowest reliability coefficient of any of the tests or procedures used in such a battery (or
evaluation) (cf. Anastasi and Urbina, 1997). Therefore, pretend that a clinician uses an
interpreter as part of an assessment but only for one measure of verbal reasoning. Lets
pretend that the inference derived from the administration of this test possesses the
lowest reliability in the battery during the assessment. The reliability of the entire
assessment is at best as high as the lowest reliability of any test or procedure adminis-
tered to the Hispanic patient regardless of greater reliability of any other tests. If the
clinician further uses information from the verbal reasoning test in the inferential proc-
ess, and such a test was conducted with the aid of an interpreter, possessing a lower
1 Introduction and Theoretical Foundations 23
reliability coefficient than that of any other test procedure used by the examiner during
the course of assessment, then that test will impact the overall test battery reliability
during the assessment and will limit the overall accuracy and consistency not to sur-
pass its reliability. It is evident from this exposition that bias, particularly nonrandom
effects, should be reduced during the course of neuropsychological assessment.
Introducing biased sources of error may exert damaging influences during the course
of assessment, and it is imperative to reduce such influences during the course of neu-
ropsychological evaluations with Hispanic populations.
Now that the concept of reliability has been introduced, it is also critical to note
a concept that has received limited attention in the cross-cultural literature but
deserves scrutiny here. Validity refers to a tests ability to measure a construct that
it was designed to assess or that it purports to measure (Anastasi and Urbina, 1997).
In terms of cross-cultural neuropsychology, one aspect of this issue has been
referred to as cognitive equivalency (see Puente and Ardila, 2000). For example, as
will be noted in Chapter 4, cross-cultural research has demonstrated that specific
ethnic minorities may perform better than others in specific cognitive measures
such as digit span, a test of auditory simple attention (cf. Kwak, 2003). Although it
is possible that specific cognitive strengths and weaknesses in different ethnic
minority groups may account for such findings as a result of cultural factors, it is
critical to first rule out other factors capable of accounting for such differences,
including lack of cognitive equivalency of test measures. For example, using digit
span, it is evident that numbers in various languages do not have the same number
of syllables, capable of differentially impacting memorization and later recall
(Kwak, 2003). Therefore, this factor alone may be capable of accounting for per-
formance differences between ethnic groups, unrelated to actual differences in
cognitive abilities. Although this construct has been termed or relabeled cognitive
equivalence, it is not new, and it is directly tied to the construct validity of a test,
and sound construct validity has to be at the psychometric heart of newly developed
measures for Hispanics.
Within this area of inquiry, ethnicity and test variance represent another critical
topic worth mentioning in neuropsychological assessment. By variance, reference is
being made to the statistical concept associated with the amount of variance accounted
for by any variable (e.g., age, education, ethnicity/culture, injury type, severity of
insult) or combinations of variables on overall test performance during the course of
neuropsychological evaluation. This is an intriguing yet critical issue meriting atten-
tion because bright attorneys, colleagues, lay individuals, and students, whether in a
facetious or heart-felt fashion, often inquire about it. The answer to this question is
complex, yet it has received little detailed scientific scrutiny. Figure 1.1 shows graphs
that attempt to capture the essence of a rational response to such a complex question.
On one hand, the type of trauma or injury and its severity is critical to the formulation
of any coherent response. The type of trauma is important because there are insults
that require significant repeated exposure before they have significant impact on
brain functions with ecological and clinical consequences. Although modern views
of the effects of brain injury have advanced (DeBlesser, 1988), early annals of neu-
roscience noted that severity of an insult is important because the extent of damage
24 S.A. Lawless et al.
Figure 1.1 Venn diagram-culture/ethnicity and total test variance-diagrams displaying diffferent
factors posited to account for total test performance variance. Figure 1.1 (a) shows total test per-
formance variance (white) accounted for age (slanted lines), culture/ethnicity (grey), education
(horizontal lines), and injury (cross-hatched lines) in the case of an individual who sustained
severe TBI. Figure 1.1 (b) shows the amount of total test performance variance (white) accounted
for by age (slanted lines), culture/ethnicity (grey), and education (horizontal lines) in the case of
noninjured individual. Note differences in the amount of total test performance variance accounted
for by culture/ethinicity in each case, but particularly in the case of the TBI individual
may account for much of the variance in neuropsychological test performance, leav-
ing any other variable(s) little to account for (Lashley, 1938). For example, if a
Hispanic patient sustains a severe brain injury with significant loss of consciousness
and trauma, extensive posttraumatic amnesia, and objective evidence of injury (e.g.,
diffused axonal injury, injured galea, remarkable MRI results), it is most likely that
the severity of such trauma accounts for a large proportion of the overall test perform-
ance variance, and cultural factors, or any other variable(s) for that matter, will
account for a small amount of the total performance variance4 (Figure 1.1a). In con-
trast, if healthy, uninjured Hispanic individuals are chosen for participation in a nor-
mative study, even when controlling for age and education, ethnicity, cultural, and
linguistic factors may account for a large proportion of the variance, particularly if
there are specific variables confounded with the concept of ethnicity in those individ-
uals, such as literary ability or other variables that partially define ethnic and cultural
differences in those individuals (Figure 1.1b). This issue is well exemplified by
research addressed in greater detail later (cf. Rey et al., 1999). However, it is interest-
ing to note that despite the fact that ethnicity and cultural factors may account for a
small portion of the total test performance variance in the case of the severely brain-
injured individual above, his or her rehabilitation, recovery, and attributional factors
related to the injury, and its long-term outcome, nonetheless, may be modulated
extensively by cultural factors (see Chapter 8).
Finally, while discussing psychometric issues, it is important to address one final
issue that is actually applicable to all populations, not just Hispanics. This critical
4
Other variables being held equal such as brain reserve capacity etc.
1 Introduction and Theoretical Foundations 25
Important ethical issues addressing cultural factors also must be considered in the
application and practice of cross-cultural neuropsychology when intervening with or
assessing Hispanics. In this regard, the Ethical Principles of Psychologists (APA,
2002) and the Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing (1999) clearly
note the importance of considering cultural factors and ethnicity from several aspects.
Although it is beyond the scope of this chapter to address all these issues in detail, a
few salient aspects of the Ethical Principles of Psychologists as they relate to cultural
issues, and Hispanics in particular, will be mentioned. From the standpoint of the
Ethical Principles of Psychologists (APA, 2002), its Preamble notes:
Psychologists are committed to increasing scientific and professional knowledge of behavior
and peoples understanding of themselves and others and to the use of such knowledge to
improve the condition of individuals, organizations, and society. Psychologists respect and
protect civil and human rights and the central importance of freedom of inquiry and expression
in research, teaching, and publication. They strive to help the public in developing informed
judgments and choices concerning human behavior. In doing so, they perform many roles,
such as researcher, educator, diagnostician, therapist, supervisor, consultant, administrator,
social interventionist, and expert witness. This Ethics Code provides a common set of princi-
ples and standards upon which psychologists build their professional and scientific work.
Clearly, the Preamble, although general and aspirational, addresses the inclusion of
cross-cultural factors in neuropsychology, as it requires a commitment on the part
of clinicians, educators, practitioners, and researchers to improve the condition
of individuals and society. In addition, the Preamble requires the protection of basic
civil and human rights.
Aside from its Preamble, ethical principles and regulations provide more spe-
cific guidance related to these issues that are applicable to ethnic minorities includ-
ing Hispanics. For example, the ethical principle addressing Respect for Peoples
Rights and Dignity (Principle E) notes:
Psychologists are aware of and respect cultural, individual, and role differences, including
those based on age, gender, gender identity, race, ethnicity, culture, national origin, reli-
gion, sexual orientation, disability, language, and socioeconomic status and consider these
factors when working with members of such groups. Psychologists try to eliminate the
effect on their work of biases based on those factors, and they do not knowingly participate
in or condone activities of others based upon such prejudices [italics added].
for practice have greater specificity, and because of their emphasis and considera-
tion of cultural, ethnic, and race factors are quite applicable to the work that psy-
chologists perform (teaching, research, clinical, and so on) with clients from
diverging backgrounds in general, and more specifically, Hispanics. Although sim-
ilar to the Ethical Principles, the Standards of Educational and Psychological
Testing (1999) provide specific and detailed guidelines that are applicable to those
who work with Hispanic populations. Although a detailed examination of all these
issues will not be covered, it is critical to note specific issues addressing the assessment
of multicultural groups including Hispanic populations. For example, with regard to test
application and construction, according to the Standards for Educational and
Psychological Testing (1999), such tests should be suitable for the background (e.g.,
cultural) of the test taker. The construction of tests should also include information on
validity and reliability of the inferences derived for such populations. In reference to test
interpretation, the Standards indicate that contextual information should be provided in
the interpretation of test scores, and when unavailable, cautions should be raised against
the misinterpretation of test scores.
With regard to ethical issues and their interaction with other factors already dis-
cussed in this chapter (e.g., language), in addition to professional organizations,
several clinicians and researchers have addressed important factors associated with
such considerations. Although their point may be too strong, as they essentially
propose that the assessment of monolingual Hispanics should be conducted by
someone with an advanced degree in Spanish, a practice that does not occur in the
U.S. or abroad during the course of most neuropsychological evaluations with
individuals from their respective mainstream culture, the point made by Artiola i
Fortuni and Mullaney (1998) is important, and if toned down to an ethical and logical
level in its application and interpretation, critical and timely. They essentially note
that an evaluation of a Hispanic individual should not be undertaken by a clinician
who does not have a certain degree of mastery of the language of the individual
undergoing assessment, and doing so is unethical. As noted by Artiola and Mullaney
(1997), although this point should be a logical step in the assessment process and
decision-making process of competency, it is something that sometimes fails to
occur, at a rate much greater than one might suspect, including judicial proceedings.
Because an ethical course of action has not been followed by large number of practi-
tioners, particularly in the U.S., Pedersen and Marsella (1982) have equated the cur-
rent status of the field regarding the lack of an ethical posture during the course of
cross-cultural psychological assessment and treatment as a crisis. This crisis pervades
a large number of settings including educational and legal arenas.
In sum, neuropsychological assessments of monolingual, and in some instances of
bilingual (see Chapter 5), Hispanics should be conducted in Spanish by a Spanish-
speaking expert cognizant of the clients ethnic and cultural background with language
fluency adequate to conduct a competent examination. The complexity of such evalu-
ations requires that they be conducted in the language in which the client can
dispatch his or her responsibilities most fluently. This is particularly true for edu-
cational, legal, or other types of evaluations with significant economic, personal,
and social consequences for the client and his society.
28 S.A. Lawless et al.
Antolin M. Llorente
A review of the most recent decennial U.S. Census (U.S. Census, 2001) indicates
that Hispanics account for approximately 11% of the total American population.
The conservative 11% estimate represents a total of approximately 32 million legal
individuals of Hispanic origin living in the U.S. Table 2.1 presents a brief descrip-
tion of the most recent census estimates for the U.S. Hispanic population according
to country of origin for selected nations (U.S. Census Bureau, 2001). The data col-
lected by the U.S. Census is to a large extent impacted by patterns of American
immigration, and although a comprehensive review of such patterns is beyond the
scope of this book, a brief examination of American immigration trends for
Hispanics will be reviewed.
It is first proper to examine the biased nature of American migrations as they
relate to Hispanics, in an attempt to understand with greater insight their subse-
quent impact on the acquisition and application of neuropsychological norms and
standards. Close scrutiny of migrational patterns reveals that American migration,
legal immigration to the U.S., is not the product of random mechanisms and
processes (Hamilton and Chinchilla, 1991; U.S. Immigration and Naturalization
Service, 1991; Portes and Rumbaut, 1990; Portes and Borocsz, 1989). The nonran-
dom nature of these migratory patterns is the result of selective factors associated
with both the host and sending nations (Portes and Rumbaut, 1990). With regard to
host country receiving factors, Garcia (1981), and reviews of historical records,
convincingly have noted that the U.S. government has had selective immigration
aims in the past that are arbitrary by their very nature and that significantly affect
current and past immigration patterns. In addition, revisions in American immigra-
tion laws and guidelines during the past decades led to significant alterations in
migrational patterns. The Immigration and Naturalization Service (U.S. Immigration
and Naturalization Service, 1991) notes that the predominant shift occurred due to
the elimination of country specific quotas, replacing them with quotas partially
based on humanitarian concerns and shifting American migrational patterns from
European to Asian and Latin American immigration. This change in immigration
1
This chapter is largely based on previous work by the author, most notably Llorente et al.,
1999, 2000.
29
30 A.M. Llorente
Table 2.1 U.S. Census Estimate for the Hispanic Population According to Country of Origin
Hispanic or latino by type Number Percent
Mexican 20,640,711 58.5
Puerto Rican 3,406,178 9.6
Central American 1,686,937 4.8
South American 1,353,562 3.8
Cuban 1,241,685 3.5
Dominican 764,945 2.2
Spainiard 100,135 0.3
All Other Hispanic or Latino 6,111,665 17.3
(e.g., write in Hispanic or Latino)
Total 35,305,818 100
Adapted from U.S. Census Bureau, 2001.
Note: For the purpose of Census reporting, country of origin is defined by the origin of the head
of household, the individual responsible for completing the Census.
Thousands
16,000
14,000
Italy
12,000
Mexico
Canada
0
1821- 1841- 1861- 1881- 1901- 1921- 1941- 1961- 1981-
1840 1860 1880 1900 1920 1940 1960 1980 1990
Figure 2.1 Changes in U.S. Immigration (19011990)
2 American Population Estimates, Trends in American Immigration, and Neuropsychology 31
Thousands
1600
1400
1200
1000
800
600
400
200
0
1900 1920 1940 1960 1980
Figure 2.2 Total legal U.S. immigration (19011990)
policy altered the profile that typified U.S. migrations for over 200 years. This
shift in migrational trends is best depicted by Figure 2.1.
The substantial variability observed in the number of immigrants allowed to
enter the U.S. during the past 90 to100 years is another marker capable of
elucidating the nonrandom and rapidly shifting nature of American immigration
patterns. Figure 2.2 clearly shows the toll that various socioeconomic and historical
events (e.g., the Great Depression, World War II) had on the total number of immi-
grants allowed to enter the U.S. between the early 1930s and the mid to late 1940s.
This figure additionally depicts the increasing number of legal immigrants that
have been allowed to enter the U.S. in the last three to five decades and the sudden
shifts in total migration that have taken place across time. Although reliable data
are not yet available, significant alterations will be evidenced shortly after
September 2001, and in particular after the Immigration and Naturalization Service
was absorbed by the Department of Homeland Security, which led to new immigra-
tion guidelines as a result of governmental restructuring and which, most critically,
selectively impacted specific groups of Hispanics.
Although data for level of education are not available, Figure 2.3 shows the
reported occupational allegiance of legal immigrants entering the U.S. from 1976
to 1990. These data indicate that the various occupational categories, closely asso-
ciated with the educational attainment of such legal immigrants, during those two
decades were not proportionally represented. Although great variability in immi-
grants occupational and educational attainment is observed in the literature, dis-
32 A.M. Llorente
Table 2.2 shows the number of legal immigrants entering the United States from
Argentina, Cuba, and Mexico, by decades, between 1931 and 1990. Perusal of this
table indicates that the total number of immigrants from Mexico surpassed three
million during the past 60 years, while the total number of immigrants from
Argentina during the same period only reached a total of approximately 131,000
immigrants. During the same time span, the total number of immigrants from Cuba
reached an approximate total of 732,000 individuals. A great deal of variability was
observed in timing of maximum immigration and the magnitude of maximum
immigration. Whereas immigration from Mexico peaked at approximately 1.5 mil-
lion between 1981 and 1990, migration from Cuba peaked at approximately
264,000 during the 1970s, while migration from Argentina to the U.S. reached
approximately 50,000 legal immigrants between 1961 and 1970. With regard to
absolute migration, the number of Argentinean immigrants is approximately 24
times less than the number of Mexican immigrants and approximately six times less
than the number of Cubans entering the U.S. during the same period. The total
number of immigrants from Cuba also is four times less relative to the total number
of immigrants from Mexico during the same six decades.
Although analyses could have been conducted to determine whether the
expected number of immigrants from each nation under investigation differed
statistically for these countries across the six decades, such analyses are beyond
2 American Population Estimates, Trends in American Immigration, and Neuropsychology 33
the scope of this exposition. However, it should be noted that analyses conducted
in the past for Hispanic data easily reached statistical significance (cf. Llorente
et al., 1999, 2000 indicating that the expected distribution of absolute numbers of
immigrants changed significantly over time and for each country). It is also clear
from the data presented above that the interrelationship and intrarelationship for
these nations as they relate to absolute immigration is in all likelihood statistically
significant. These data also underscore the biased nature of American immigra-
tion patterns.
An examination of the absolute number of immigrants was critical in an
attempt to understand American migratory patterns. However, the proportion of
immigrants for three separate decades (1961 to1970, 1971 to 1980, and 1981 to
1990) relative to the total population of each country at the end of those decades
is just as important to our understanding of Hispanic migrations to the U.S. This
analysis was thus conducted for each country. In 1970, Argentina had an
approximate population of 24,300,000 inhabitants and an approximate migration
to the U.S. (1961 to 1970) of 50,000 or 0.2% of its population. In 1970, Cuba and
Mexico had respective populations of 8,500,000 and 48,000,000, and approxi-
mate American immigrations of 208,000 and 454,000, between 1961 and 1970 or
3% and 1% of their respective populations. In 1980, Argentina had a total
estimated population of 27,000,000 and a U.S. immigration of approximately
30,000 or 0.1% of its population, whereas Cuba had a total estimated population
of 10,000,000 inhabitants and a U.S. migration of 265,000 individuals, a total of
2.6% of that countrys population. Mexicos U.S. immigration was 1% of its total
number of inhabitants (total U.S. immigration, 1971 to1980 = 640,294 / total
estimated population, 1980 = 72,000,000). In 1990, the relative population
percentages for Argentina, Cuba, and Mexico were 0.1%, 1.3%, and 1.8%,
respectively.
In summary, in terms of relative immigration, migrations from Argentina have
remained relatively constant and small in magnitude over the last three decades.
Cuban immigration reached its peak during the 1970s and 1980s, with decreasing
American immigration during the 1990s, while Mexicos relative immigration to
the U.S. has been steadily increasing during the same period.
34 A.M. Llorente
Table 2.4 shows the number of legal immigrants admitted to this country from
Cuba, Mexico, and Haiti (for purposes of comparison) for the year 1990 and their
intended area of initial residence for five major metropolitan regions. As noted by
Llorente et al. (1999, 2000) these data indicate that immigrants tend to have geo-
graphical predilection for certain areas within the United States. Whereas 11% and
34% of Mexican immigrants reported Los Angeles, California, as their preference
for initial residence during 1990, only 2.5% of immigrants from Cuba reported this
destination as their intended initial residence. In sharp contrast, 72% of Cuban
immigrants reported Miami, Florida, as their initial intended destination or resi-
dence for the year 1990, whereas only 0.2% of immigrants from Mexico reported
this city as their intended initial area of residence (U.S. Immigration and
Naturalization Service, 1991). With regard to New York, New York, only 3.4% and
1% of immigrants from Cuba and Mexico, respectively, selected this metropolitan
region as their intended residence. Chicago, Illinois, and Houston, Texas, had a
relatively low percentage of legal immigrants, although immigrants from Mexico,
due to its large absolute migration, had a large number of its immigrants reporting
Houston as their intended area of initial residence.
Table 2.4 Percentage and Total Number of Legal Immigrants Reporting Intended Metropolitan
Region of Initial Residence for Three Countries for 1990: Five Selected Regions
Country, percent, and (total number) of immigrants
Metropolitan region Cuba Haiti Mexico
Chicago, Illinois 0.8 (84) .98 (199) 6.2 (41,846)
Houston, Texas 0.3 (31) 0.1 (24) 5.2 (34,973)
Los Angeles, California 2.5 (274) 0.27 (55) 34 (231,267)
Miami, Florida 72 (7,7685) 17.9 (3,635) 0.2 (1,273)
New York, New York 3.4 (358) 37.7 (8,066) 0.9 (6,436)
Adapted from INS, 1991.
36 A.M. Llorente
Distinct patterns of selective immigration were not only observed for the major
metropolitan cities listed in Table 2.4, they were also evidenced for other groups
of Hispanics from other countries as well. These results suggest that certain
Hispanic immigrants have historically selected specific metropolitan areas of resi-
dence different from those chosen by other Hispanic immigrants (Portes and
Rumbaut, 1990).
The data presented above suggest that the total number of immigrants (absolute
migration) to the U.S. varies substantially among countries and fluctuates over time
for each nation as a result of variables affecting both the host and sending states.
Therefore, migrational patterns must be considered dynamic nonrandom processes
that change over time as a result of selective factors (Llorente et al., 1999, 2000). As
noted by Llorente et al. (1999, 2000), the variability of these patterns of immigration
may be the result of various social, political, and economic trends (pushpull factors;
see Hamilton & Chinchilla, 1991) affecting all countries sharing migrations (i.e., the
host and sending countries). The range of occupational status of foreign immigrants
also varies extensively between nations regardless of the total size of their migration.
This variation may be the result of selective recruitment policies or similar nonran-
dom variables adopted by the host country to satisfy certain of its unmet occupational
classification requirements as well as other factors (cf. Garcia, 1981 and Portes and
Rumbaut, 1990). For this reason, occupational allegiance from immigrants entering
the U.S. cannot be assumed to be random, as a certain degree of selectivity for spe-
cific vocational groups was observed for some nations. Instead, nonrandom shifts in
the occupational choice of immigrants from foreign countries should be expected
longitudinally, including the occupational choice of immigrants from the same
country, and associated with occupation, differential educational levels. Finally, a
great deal of affinity for certain geographical/residential areas was observed for most
immigrant groups across the time span under investigation. In sum, and as previously
argued (cf. Llorente et al., 1999; 2000; Llorente, 1997), a review of the migrational
literature reveals that American immigration is not the result of random mechanisms
or processes (Portes and Rumbaut, 1990; U.S. Immigration and Naturalization
Service, 1991, 1981, 1975) but rather the result of predominantly shifting and chang-
ing biased migrational patterns.
Despite the fact that adolescents and children account for approximately half of the
worlds immigrant population (Cole, 1998), there is a dearth of information in the
literature addressing the specific mental health needs of immigrant youth (Hicks,
Lalonde, and Pepler, 1993), particularly unaccompanied children, including
Hispanics. In spite of this limitation, there is convergence in the literature suggest-
ing greater prevalence of neuropsychopathology, particularly posttraumatic stress
disorder and depression, in unaccompanied immigrant children. In addition,
unaccompanied children may exhibit greater psychological vulnerability and risk
factors as a result of trauma than other children or adults, in conjunction with
underdeveloped adaptive, cognitive, and neuropsychological resources secondary
to their early developmental stage. Risk, resiliency, and personality characteristics
also appear to serve as moderating variables. Finally, immigration context, includ-
ing community assimilation and integration, have been shown to play a major role
(Portes and Rumbaut, 1990).
Adding to the complexity of the problem, any dialogue addressing neuropsycho-
logical problems faced by unaccompanied immigrant children and adolescents also
should take into consideration serious health problems observed with greater preva-
lence among this population. Malnutrition, developmental disabilities, medical
illnesses, injuries, and central nervous system involvement are often encountered
with greater prevalence in these children (Westermeyer, 1991). Given the psycho-
logical and physical trauma that some unaccompanied Hispanic or other children
experience prior to entering the U.S., many of them migrate with a history of
traumatic stress (Almqvist and Brandell-Forsberg, 1997; Portes and Rumbaut,
1990). More important, as it has direct bearing on this chapter, it is vital to note that
unaccompanied Hispanic immigrant children represent one of the largest groups of
youth entering the U.S.
2
This portion of the chapter substantively appeared in Llorente (2004).
42 A.M. Llorente
Depression
(cf. Rousseau, 1995; Sack et al., 1995). Other factors known to have a negative
influence on mental health outcomes of unaccompanied youth include poverty and
low SES (Howard and Hodes, 2000), school problems, and discrimination (Hyman
et al, 2000).
In conclusion, although the effects of certain demographic variables have been known to
significantly affect neuropsychological performance (cf. Adams et al., 1982; Laosa, 1984;
Heaton et al., 1986), the etiology behind differences in demographic characteristics have
not been well researched, especially among ethnic minority groups including Hispanics.
The present chapter examined a plausible candidate capable of modulating demographic
variables, namely, American immigration trends.
With regard to migrations, examination of this process revealed nonrandom,
shifting, selective, and dynamic mechanisms affecting patterns of U.S. immigra-
tion. Regardless of the causes for the nonrandom nature of immigration (e.g., eco-
nomic, immigration laws of the host country), absolute and relative migration to
this country were observed to vary drastically. Fluctuations in such migratory
patterns over time within the same country of origin were also evidenced. Similarly,
the occupational affiliation of immigrants from foreign nations to the U.S. varied
extensively. More important, occupational status, and most probably, educational
attainment differed significantly for immigrants from various nations over time
independent of absolute migration. Parenthetically, such factors can even vary for
immigrants from the same country, and therefore represent an intracountry variation
over time. Such migrations can be observed when subgroups of individuals arrive
from the same host country at distinct periods of time, and who thus have distinct
demographic characteristics. Finally, selective patterns of geographical settlement
within the U.S. were observed for most immigrant groups across time.
These results have significant implications for neuropsychology. They suggest
that migrational trends are capable of modulating demographic characteristics,
partly infringing upon inferential processes for Hispanic groups. This infringement
could possibly bias normative data and their application, including assessment or
rehabilitation. Such plausible biases could potentially invalidate or place into ques-
tion studies comparing intellectual functioning or other neuropsychological
domains in research participants or patients between sites. Similarly, such selective
factors could potentially render invalid norms or research comparing Hispanic
groups at the same or different geographical sites.
The present findings also suggest that differences in demographic characteris-
tics, modulated by immigration patterns, are capable of mimicking abnormal brain-
behavior relationships (cf. Rey et al., 1999). Therefore, care should be exercised not
to attribute neuropsychological differences to nonexistent brain-behavior relation-
ships when those differences can be accounted for by more objective variables,
such as demographic characteristics from inappropriate standardization samples
46 A.M. Llorente
Antolin M. Llorente
For many members of ethnic minority and non-minority groups living in the U.S.,
Hispanics included, the interrelationship between neuropsychological test perform-
ance and education, socioeconomic factors, and other specific variables, such as
nutrition, access to health care, and stress, is particularly complex, with significant
implications for a science of brain-behavior relationships and its practice. While
educational attainment to a large degree dictates, and certainly facilitates, income
potential, it is also the case that socioeconomic advancement facilitates educational
attainment and other specific advantages, and both of these variables have been
shown to impact neuropsychological performance. These are important factors
meriting attention, and many investigators have noted that it is these factors, rather
than cultural or ethnic factors, that require attention.
Socioeconomic Factors
47
48 A.M. Llorente
critical to note, because they are factors that impact quality of life and brain devel-
opment that have significant impact on current and later neuropsychological func-
tioning (see below).
Harris and Llorente (2005) note that parental income of Hispanic children is
often constrained by the lack of legal residency, further limiting employment and
educational opportunities. Data on the number of illegal residents who gradu-
ate from high school each year and are unable to enroll in college are not readily
available, but it is estimated that there are approximately 1.6 million children living
without legal residency status in the U.S. (Passel, Capps & Fix, 2004). Only 7.3%
of Hispanics of all ages who entered the U.S. between 1990 and 2002 had become
citizens by 2002, compared with 73.3% of those entering before 1970 (U.S. Census
Bureau, 2003). Certainly, this circumstance partially contributes to the low percent-
age of Hispanics graduating from college in the U.S. and indirectly affects their
socioeconomic status. Harris and Llorente further note that These realities con-
spire to depress the SES for Hispanic groups as a whole. As a given minority
group gains an economic foothold, educational and other opportunities tend to
increase (Harris and Llorente, 2005; cf. Portes and Rumbaut, 1990).
The interaction between SES and other factors also is important to examine
because it has significant and critical repercussions for neuropsychology. Variables
such as nutrition, access to medical care, educational attainment, and the presence
of CNS or other physiological diseases are vital, as they have a pivotal impact on
neuropsychological functioning.
Nutrition
Nutrition in Hispanic populations living in the U.S. and abroad, aside from humani-
tarian concerns, represents an important topic for neuropsychology. In this regard,
before addressing data from the scientific literature, we would like to present three
scenarios from experiences that the senior author has had that underscore the impor-
tance of nutrition and neuropsychological assessment with these populations.
The first is based on an experience encountered both in the U.S. and abroad, as
the senior author conducted seminars to train graduate students and professionals.
During such seminars, the information presented to participants from all walks of
life (medical and neuropsychology students, neurologists, pediatricians, psychia-
trists, psychologists, social workers, and others) was welcomed. However, on
several occasions, the attendees, although happy to be exposed to recent advances
in the neurosciences and neuropsychology, indicated that the greatest problem
encountered in their practice was related to a lack of appropriate nutrition in
patients undergoing medical, neuropsychological, or psychological assessment.
When asked to expound on this point, conference participants unequivocally indi-
cated that they consistently discovered a history of nutritional deprivation or
malnutrition, in many instances with childhood onset and covering a large portion
of their patients lives.
3 Hispanic Populations: Special Issues in Neuropsychology 49
1
Many factors impact costs and their subsequent affordability. This information is not included
as a criticism but as an educational fact which neuropsychologists should recognize, since many
factors conspire to create such a set of unique circumstances.
50 A.M. Llorente
obvious reason, is because they can only be obtained from daily diet, yet poor diets
are the catch of the day in the U.S. and abroad in many settings (e.g., schools,
homes). In addition, lack of appropriate nutrition, including metals and other nutri-
ents, in a diet has been associated with brain dysfunction in humans and animals, a
fact with a long historical record of support (cf. Winick and Noble, 1966; Winick
and Rosso, 1969). Because Hispanics, as noted above, have a greater probability of
coming from impoverished backgrounds where lower SES affects their ability to
rise above the poverty level, with potentially significant nutritional implications
and indirect implications for brain functions and neuropsychological skills, this is
an issue that deserves special attention when working with such populations.
Specific and unique cultural expectations and experiences also play a major role
that impacts factors such as access to health care and, indirectly, neuropsychology
and its application with Hispanic populations. For example, although a two-tier
medical system encompassing private and public providers (quality of care aside)
may exist in their native countries, the majority of Hispanics come from countries
such as Mexico or countries in the Caribbean, Central America, and South
America, where a socialized medical system is available to the majority of the
population using relatively simple access mechanisms and low out-of-pocket
costs. Therefore, many Hispanic individuals have difficulties accessing and
traveling through the complex maze associated with the third-party insurance sys-
tems encountered in the U.S. This difficulty limits their access to health care,
particularly mental health services and advanced specialty services such as neu-
ropsychology. Others, including those who are U.S. citizens or born in the U.S.,
including children, the elderly, and specific Hispanic populations such as veterans
of foreign wars, do not have any health care insurance, which clearly places con-
straints on accessibility and services. The lack of regular and scheduled medical
care has led indirectly to problems and to the overwhelming use of emergency
medical services (with devastating consequences for emergency services in
selected hospitals) in selected communities throughout the U.S. This occurs partly
because limited access to regular and preventive care often have led to the need
for emergency medical and mental health care (Baker et al., 1996). This can be
surmised from the increasing rates of emergency room admissions and utilization
by Hispanics in the U.S. in the last two decades.
Socioeconomic status, sociopolitical climates, and sheer societal economic
realities and constraints also have significant impact on access to health care and
medical services by all U.S. citizens. Unfortunately, either as a result of the issues
mentioned thus far, including poverty, or language barriers, or a combination of
both, it is well known that Hispanics, including those born in the U.S. or foreign-
born U.S. citizens, have less access or seek less dental, medical, and mental health
services (cf. Weinick, Zuvekas and Cohen, 2000). Data from the U.S. Department
3 Hispanic Populations: Special Issues in Neuropsychology 51
of Health and Human Services (USDHHS) makes clear that significant health
disparities exist in the U.S. between Hispanics and individuals from mainstream
culture, and this has significant impact on American neuropsychology. From a
data-based standpoint, the USDHHS published a report noting the presence of such
disparities (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2004).
Education
As briefly noted in Chapter 1 and above, education is a critical factor. Aside from
the fact that education has been shown to impact neuropsychological functioning
(cf. Heaton et al., 1986; Ostrosky-Solis et al., 2004), it is crucial to examine this
variable as a special factor because large numbers of Hispanics living in the U.S.
have achieved low levels of education. For example, Harris and Llorente (2005)
note that in the Caucasian non-Hispanic population, 88.7% graduate from high
school, 29.4% achieve a minimum of a Bachelors degree, and 4.0% have attained
less than 9 years of education. By contrast, in individuals from Mexico 25 years
or older, 48.6% have not received a high school diploma, 26.6% have graduated
from high school, and only 5.2% achieve a minimum of a Bachelors degree. Aside
from specific cognitive protective factors related to education in neuropsychology
as postulated by Satz (Satz, 1993), limited or lack of education also interacts with
other factors, in some instances leading to increased risk factors in Hispanic popu-
lations such as limited economic opportunity, limited knowledge of the impact of
preventive medical care or good nutrition, and so on. Therefore, education is an
important variable to keep in mind when interpreting neuropsychological test
performance and working with Hispanic populations.
For specific health problems, illnesses, and diseases, Hispanics living in the U.S. and
abroad are most likely associated with impoverished environments, as noted by the
World Health Organization (1998), and unfortunately bear a disproportionate burden
of disease, injury, morbidity, and mortality when compared with non-Hispanic
whites. In addition, the leading causes of death among Hispanics vary from those for
non-Hispanic whites in the U.S., information supported by statistical resources kept
by the United States Department of Health and Human Services (USDHHS, 2004).
For example, the report from the USDHHS indicates that Hispanics of all ethnic
backgrounds experienced more age-adjusted years of potential life lost before age
75 years per 100,000 population than non-Hispanic whites for mortality associated
with stroke (18% more), chronic liver disease and cirrhosis (62%), diabetes (41%),
and human immunodeficiency virus disease (168%), all with significant implications
for neuropsychology and its applied endeavors (cf. Llorente et al., 2000a, 2001).
52 A.M. Llorente
Although Hispanics have made significant gains in the last few years related to
ownership of real estate in the U.S., significant disparities still remain between
them and majority groups (Toms Rivera Policy Institute, 2003). Similarly,
quality of housing is often taken for granted by clinicians and researchers alike,
despite its health significance, including potentially harmful environmental con-
taminants that significantly impact brain function (e.g., lead, mercury, organo-
phosphates, and sulfur dioxide) and that are often found in neighborhoods with
large Hispanic populations throughout the U.S. Why is it essential and necessary
to examine such variables during the course of assessment with Hispanic popula-
tions? From a neuropsychological standpoint, it is critical to note that quality of
housing may also be associated with other health-related and non-health-related
variables, such as environmental deprivation, overcrowding, and added stress, all
of which affect neuropsychological functioning.
It is also interesting to note that some of the factors mentioned in Chapter 2, such
as geographical affinity of specific Hispanic groups, is not just supported by census
or other data examined by Llorente et al. (1999), but they are additionally supported
by housing data (Toms Rivera Policy Institute, 2003).
3 Hispanic Populations: Special Issues in Neuropsychology 53
Religion
Religion, especially organized religion, plays a major role in the life of many
Hispanics living in the U.S., and it represents a strong component of their cultural
heritage and identity. According to Espinosa, Elizondo, and Miranda (2005),
approximately 71% of Hispanics identify themselves as Catholic (Milwaukee
Archdiocese, 2005), and about 23% of the 41.3 million Hispanics in the U.S. in 2004
identified themselves as Protestants or other Christians. Complicating this issue, and
their cultural identity as well, is the fact that many Hispanics, in addition to practic-
ing Catholicism or other organized religion, additionally may adopt other, less
formal religious practices such as Santera. The emphasis placed by Hispanics on
religious beliefs and practices dates back to their ancestors and the colonization of
the Americas by Spaniards and other western European societies (cf. Dussell, 1992),
as well as the introduction of African religious practices into Hispanic cultures,
impacting all Hispanics, but particularly those from nations near countries where
such practices are highly prevalent (e.g., Dominican Republic and Haiti).
Although some clinicians may argue that these issues are irrelevant to neuropsy-
chological practice, they are important to evaluate because they may significantly
impact the conclusions reached about a specific Hispanic patient. For example,
from a diagnostic standpoint, a clinician may reach an erroneous diagnostic
conclusion because he or she perceives that a client is experiencing hallucinations
or other symptoms associated with a psychotic process based on the clients
answers to questions during a clinical interview or on a rating scale. In fact, the
client may practice Santera and may truly believe that he or she hear voices, but
this belief is not subsequent to neuropsychopathology.
The ever-increasing racial and ethnic diversity of the U.S. population, as noted in
Chapter 2, emphasizes the need to account for the role of culture on diagnosis.
Groce and Zola (1993) noted, An individuals culture is not a diagnostic category;
no cultural heritage will wholly explain how any given individual will think and
act, but it can help health care professionals anticipate and understand how and why
families make certain decisions (p. 1049). Groce and Zola have also emphasized
the importance of cultural awareness and its impact on expectations of a clients
physical, mental, and psychological development, given the potential for variability
in these expectations and a clients experiences within his or her own cultural envi-
ronment. Cultural awareness and understanding clearly need to be considered an
integral part of a clinicians knowledge base when they are called upon to diagnose
and provide consultation (Reeve, Groce, Persing, and Magge, 2004). Additionally,
Groce and Zola caution that cultural belief systems may be approached in an
54 A.M. Llorente
and community invest in an individual. In some cultures, even now, illness and
disability may be viewed as a form of punishment or retribution much as they
were viewed hundreds of years ago (Groce and Nola, 1993; Reeve, Groce, Persing,
and Magge, 2004). For example, the beliefs of witchcraft found in some Caribbean
societies (e.g., Dominican Republic) hold that illness or disability may be transmit-
ted through ones associations. Or, if a child has an illness with a rapid onset, some
people in Latin American societies believe that the condition is due to the evil eye
(Groce and Zola, 1993; see also Simpson, Mohr and Redman, 2000). In Manaus,
Brazil, an individual may consider cleft lip or cleft palate to result from contagion
(e.g.., contracted by sharing eating utensils), personal conduct (e.g.., a pregnant
mother looked at an animal with a split mouth or the father cut open the mouth of
a fish to remove a hook during his childs pregnancy), or Gods will (Reeve, Groce,
Persing, and Magge, 2004). In contrast, chronic illness and disability may be seen
by some cultures as a unique gift. For example, some Mexican immigrants believe
that their child has been singled out by God for the role because of their past
kindnesses to a relative or neighbor who was disabled. (Ries et al., 2007)
Consideration should be given to the acculturation of a patient and his or her
family as it may have important implications for diagnosis. Groce and Zola (1993)
advocate for the assessment of acculturation on an individual basis, particularly an
individual or a familys belief system and social structure. The authors note:
No one can simply assume on the basis of a persons cultural heritage, dress, or language
what his or her individual ideas or understandings may be. Nonetheless, it is important to
remember that traditional attitudes about disability may hang on long after other cultural
beliefs are gone, although more acculturated individuals may be sophisticated enough to
know that publicly expressing beliefs, such as the presence of witchcraft, is unacceptable.
(p. 1055).
take on greater significance with immigrants since they may possess more pronounced
characteristics capable of enhancing the expectancy effects of the examiner, particu-
larly those characteristics that are observable (e.g., anxiety associated with no prior
psychological contact [cf. Egeland, 1967]; pronounced accent in speech).
In addition it is critical to note that many negative stereotypes about immigrants
and ethnic minorities, including Hispanics, have permeated American society and
culture, including television, radio, and other mass media, and these affect lay as
well as professional individuals. As noted by Llorente et al. (2000), these stereo-
types are capable of having deleterious impact upon a neuropsychological evalua-
tion, despite their unsubstantiated nature and the fact that they have insufficient
weight to withstand the rigor of scientific scrutiny. This issue is best exemplified
within American society through mainstream perceptions of the immigrant.
Such perceptions historically have been negative, even within the mental health
professions (degaard, 1932; Sanua, 1970). As a case in point, during earlier large-
scale migrations to the U.S. at the beginning of the last century, a correlation was
initially noted between immigrants and innate marginality or psychopathology
associated with the process of immigration. Although incorrect, such perceptions
were the result of early, inaccurate, and methodologically flawed epidemiological
studies indicating the presence of higher incidence of mental illness among immi-
grants on the basis of hospital admissions (Jarvis, 1866; Rothman, 1971; Sanua,
1970); higher suicide rates among immigrants living in the U.S.; European versus
U.S. differential suicide rates in groups of individuals of the same nationality
(cf. Faris and Dunham, 1939); and differential patterns and rates of mental disorders
in metropolitan areas relative to suburban areas with large immigrant density (Faris
and Dunham, 1939). On the basis of these findings, researchers concluded that
immigration was associated with mental disorders (degaard, 1932). Unfortunately,
these investigations suffered from poor methodology marked by biased samples and
similar confounds, and sound research later demonstrated that the effects of objec-
tive variables such as age, poverty, and area of residence (Kohn, 1973; Hollingshead
and Redlich, 1958; Kessler and Cleary, 1980 ; Srole, Langner, and Mitchell, 1962)
accounted for the majority of the differences observed. Despite the significant
shortcomings of the earlier investigations, these findings found their way into main-
stream culture and became part of the perception of the immigrant, not just held by
lay people but also by mental health providers, regarding the mental abilities and
psychological make-up of the immigrant (Llorente et al., 2000). These stereotypes
are capable of biasing the outcome of a neuropsychological examination.
Llorente et al. (2000) noted that there is no reason to believe, despite our
present level of knowledge with regard to which factors affect immigrants and
large-scale migrations (e.g., context of immigration, SES of the immigrant, reasons
for immigration; cf. Portes and Rumbaut, 1990), that new generations of neuropsy-
chologists will behave differently towards these populations, including Hispanics,
relative to past generations of clinicians, unless the former become cognizant of the
biases against these populations and the factors capable of encroaching upon their
assessments. Although immigration can be one of the greatest stressors that any
individual may experience in life, immigration per se is not necessarily responsible
for mental illness or the etiology behind abnormal brain-behavior relationships.
Chapter 4
Intellectual Abilities: Theoretical
and Applied Assessment Considerations1
The assessment of intellectual abilities has held a special place in the history of
psychology. Starting with the establishment of the civil service program in China
approximately 4,200 years ago, to the Spanish physician Juan Huartes Examen de
Ingenios in 1575, the assessment of aptitude, whether valid or invalid throughout
the centuries, has attempted to capture the broad range of intellectual abilities noted
by Sir Francis Galton in 1869 when he stated that There is continuity of natural
ability reaching from one knows not what height, and descending to one can hardly
say what depth (Galton, 1869).
Although the latter part of the statement made by Galton clearly exemplifies his
profound understanding of individual differences in intellectual gifts, other state-
ments he made have been associated with controversy, as they were adopted by
the eugenics movement, and the measurement of intellectual abilities has been far
more scrutinized than the measurement of other neuropsychological constructs, in
some instances disregarding the importance of the latter. Like no other function of
the mind, intellectual skills seem to include judgment of character and potential
in life; intercultural differences in this construct were controversial from early peri-
ods (cf. Nell, 2000), most recently in the vernacular literature and in the general
publics awareness after publication of Herrnsteins book The Bell Curve
(Herrnstein and Murray, 1994; Nell, 2000).
It is interesting to note that Spearman chose g for general intelligence when
he partially laid the groundwork for his theories regarding intellectual abilities
impacting current assessment. The question whether or not research should and can
work with truly general constructs has been addressed in the emic-etic paradigm
originally defined by Pike (1967, as quoted in Berry, 1999). Pike suggested
the term etic for an approach that views behavior from an outside perspective,
while he proposed the term emic for an approach that views behavior from a
perspective inside the system in question (Berry, 1999). In cross-cultural psychology,
these labels may be used to indicate whether the emphasis of a given study is placed
on understanding meaning by utilizing concepts from within the culture in question
1
Portions of this chapter originally appeared in Harris and Llorente (2005).
57
58 C. von Thomsen et al.
other conceptual issues also should play an important role. To understand cross-
cultural factors and their implication during the course of intellectual assessment
with Hispanics, fields and concepts from sociology, genetics, and anthropology
also play important roles. When such factors are included in theoretical frame-
works, they lead to comprehensive models of cognitive processes such as the
ecocultural framework developed by Berry (2001), which is capable of account-
ing for cross-cultural mechanisms that are applicable to intellectual variables in
Hispanic populations. This perspective includes universalist views of cognition
and intellectual abilities related to specific cognitive mechanisms, as espoused by
many in the field today (cf. Echemendia and Harris, 2004), as well as the effects
of cultural factors on individuals, including Hispanics. Berry (2001) devised a
framework in which individual differences, including intellectual skills, emerge
within the greater contextual space of universalist factors as a function of sets of
variables, including ecological (Eco) and sociopolitical factors, together with
other arrays of variables, including culture, genetics, and acculturation, that lead
to the emergence of such individual differences. In essence, the model explains
individual differences in intellectual abilities as the emergence of general univer-
sal psychological mechanisms that are found in all humans (species) that are then
infinitely modified through maturation by cultural and other variables as a collec-
tion of adaptations to context or ecology. This model clearly adopts concepts
from evolutionary theory (Darwin, 1967). It is also consistent with the view noted
in Chapter 1 related to the fact that an individual constructs his society and soci-
ety constructs the individual (cf. Wartofsky, 1983).
Although borrowing from Hebb (1949) regarding what appear to be simple yet
important distinctions between intellectual abilities, intelligent behavior and
assessed intellect, Sternberg (1984, 1997) also developed a comprehensive theory
of intelligence that is capable of incorporating cultural influences in cognition.
According to Sternbergs triarchic theory of intelligence, there are three main com-
ponents that describe mental functioning and, ultimately, how people process infor-
mation (Sternberg, 1997). The first component includes a contextual piece that
considers the unique environment in which individuals reside with regard to intelli-
gence. Within this component, intelligence consists of the individuals ability to
adapt, select, and shape his or her environment and make it meaningful to the self.
Secondly, the componential subtheory addresses the individuals internal ability to
complete mental processes, which consists of encoding, transforming, and
comparing information, as well as higher level executive functions consisting of
planning, organizing, and monitoring the information (Sternberg). This second
part of the triarchic theory is primarily related to information processing. Finally,
the third component is referred to as two-facet and describes an individuals
intelligence as their ability to work with novel stimuli and their level of automatic-
ity of various cognitive processes (Sternberg).
From a theoretical standpoint, Cattell (1963), Horn (1967; 1979), and Carroll
(2005) independently devised theories of intelligence. These theories were then
combined and used as the underlying theoretical basis for the development of
the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities - Third Edition (WJ-III).
60 C. von Thomsen et al.
Cattell and Horns theory of cognitive abilities distinguishes between two types
of intelligence: fluid intelligence and crystallized intelligence. Fluid intelli-
gence consists of cognitive abilities that are typically thought of as free of cul-
tural influence (even though Berrys [2001] and Sternbergs [1997] models
point in a different direction) and includes procedures such as word list recall.
According to this theory, fluid intelligence is related to level of maturity and
continues to expand until adolescence and young adulthood. Crystallized intel-
ligence is considered to be skills that an individual acquires as a result of resid-
ing in a specific culture. Crystallized intelligence is thought to continue to
expand and develop throughout ones life as an individual continues to interact
with his or her culture and learn over time, and it is therefore much affected by
education and other contextual opportunities. Cattell-Horns theory of intelli-
gence is a hierarchical model that includes all major aspects of intelligence
(McGrew and Woodcock, 2001).
John Carrolls three-stratum theory of intelligence is also hierarchical. His the-
ory identifies 69 specific abilities that are narrow in nature and compose Stratum I.
Stratum II consists of broader categories of intelligence that group the 69 narrow
abilities. Finally, Stratum III incorporates a general intelligence which Carroll
refers to as g. The WJ-III was developed utilizing the theory of cognitive abilities
established by Cattell, Horn, and Carroll (CHC theory) as its theoretical foundation
(McGrew and Woodcock, 2001). This theory incorporates aspects from two major
sources, including Cattell and Horns work with fluid (Gf) and crystallized (Gc)
intellectual abilities and Carrolls hierarchical conceptualization of cognitive abili-
ties that stem from narrow abilities, to broader abilities, and finally to general intel-
ligence, or g (McGrew and Woodcock). The application of such a comprehensive
framework of intelligence has led to the development of an excellent measure of
cognitive abilities.
Although a strong argument has been made for the importance of cultural fac-
tors, alternative views should not be excluded. Genetic factors are frequently
considered to exert a powerful influence on intelligence (Laosa, 1996), and prob-
ably account for a portion of the inter- and intracultural variations (cf. Kwak,
2003). For example, there appears to be evidence for differential onset of certain
brain-related conditions in Hispanics (Clark et al., 2005), as well as possible dif-
ferential cognitive strengths and weaknesses associated with specific cultural and
ethnic backgrounds (e.g., Korean versus Hispanic). However, research in
the field of molecular genetics makes it seem likely that a complex combination
of different genes contributes to variations in cognitive ability and other neu-
ropsychological domains, and that environmental factors may play a crucial role
in determining which and when those genes are expressed (cf. Lai et al., 2001;
Laosa, 1996).
In summary, one chief problem related to this topic and the process of assess-
ment is that intragroup variance continues to be neglected by clinicians when plan-
ning assessments with diverse clients generally (APA, 2001) and Hispanic clients
specifically (Artiola i Fortuni et al., 2005; Pontn & Len-Corren, 2001).
4 Intellectual Abilities: Theoretical and Applied Assessment Considerations 61
While a number of available cognitive tests seems capable of producing results that
are unbiased in select applications for members of ethnic minorities, including
Hispanics, a recent review reported that about a third of such measures have been
identified in empirical studies of cultural bias to yield biased or mixed results
(Valencia, Suzuki, and Salinas, 2001).
In 2004, Echemendia and Harris published data from a 1994 survey of U.S.
neuropsychologists regarding the use of tests with Hispanic populations
(Echemendia and Harris, 2004). Even though the data were 10 years old at the
time of publication, the authors expressed confidence that not much had changed
in this time, given a lack of published Spanish tests, as well as their own per-
sonal experience. Their sample of 475 responses indicated great variability in the
use of tests and norms with Hispanics. In terms of what tests are used with what
population, both Hispanic and non-Hispanic clients are most often administered
the WAIS for assessment of cognitive capabilities (Echemendia and Harris,
2004). This finding was stable even across different levels of self-perceived
competence to work with Hispanic clients, as well as self-perceived language
competence (in Spanish).
Regarding test translation, most clinicians (58%) indicated that they used the
English WAIS with bilingual clients and the Spanish version with monolingual
Spanish speakers, respectively (52%). However, a considerable percentage (20% for
the bilingual group and 29% for the monolingual Spanish group) utilized a verbatim
translation of the English test items (Echemendia and Harris, 2004). The authors
remark that this behavior is highly problematic and can interfere with validity of the
measures used (see Chapter 1). When asked what norms are typically employed to
compare WAIS test results from bilinguals with the general population, 68% of clini-
cians stated they used English norms, 4% Spanish norms, and 28% relied on clinical
judgment. With monolingual Spanish speakers, 6% of WAIS scores are compared
with English norms, 64% with Spanish norms, and 29% by using clinical judgment.
The frequent use of clinical judgment rather than norms can be viewed as an appropri-
ate response to lack of suitable norms (Echemendia and Harris, 2004) or as clinicians
tendency to revert to intuitive decision-making processes.
Another important result from this study is data regarding the use of translators.
While more frequently reported by neuropsychologists who also endorse lower
competence in Spanish fluency and proficiency, reliance on translators is contro-
versial because standardized assessments depend on standardized instructions and
interactions (Echemendia and Harris, 2004; Ardila, 2005).
In conclusion, Echemendia and Harris (2004) stated that U.S. neuropsycholo-
gists are not prepared adequately to provide services to Latinos (Echemendia and
Harris, 2004, p. 11). According to them, this is partly due to the fact that bilingual
clients are usually administered U.S. normed version of tests, while monolingual
Spanish-speaking individuals most often receive a translated version of the same
test (Echemendia and Harris, 2004).
62 C. von Thomsen et al.
to this analysis, the reduced performance in overall intellect for Hispanic examinees
only emerged in the group of Hispanic children whose native language was
Spanish. As noted by Harris and Llorente (2005), the typical finding of reduced
performance, particularly in verbal indices, for Hispanic examinees was only evi-
denced in the sample of Hispanic children whose native language was Spanish. In
fact, although the sample is small, the native English speaking Hispanic children
surpass the non-minority examinees in their Processing Speed Index. Such findings
are a powerful illustration of the impact of socioeconomic and linguistic variables
upon performance. As noted by Harris and Llorente (2005), What initially
appeared to be a large gap between Hispanic and non-Hispanic White children,
now appears to be a minimal difference. On the basis of these results Harris and
Llorente (2005) also concluded that native language is an important moderating
variable and these studies demonstrate the challenges test developers and clini-
cians face in determining when a suitable level of language proficiency has been
reached in an individual undergoing assessment (see Table 4.3).
Altogether, these general findings led Harris and Llorente (2005) to address
another vital issue often encountered in intellectual assessment (see also the work
of Ardila et al. 1994). Harris and Llorente (2005) note that the construct equiva-
lency of a test and its underlying theoretical framework are critical issues to take
into account. They more specifically note the importance of conceptual equivalence
in the construct and measurement of intelligence and the significance of intellect
moderator variables. They note that differences exist in the conceptualization of
intelligence by various theoreticians (cf. Carroll, 2005; Cattell, 1963; Ceci, 1996;
Spearman, 1927; Sternberg, 1985, 1997; Thurstone, 1938), as noted above, yet
Each model has implications for a specific culturally-defined context or question.
For example, how is the intelligence necessary that describes successful adapta-
tion by a child in a metropolitan area in the U.S. different than the intelligence that
describes the successful adaptation of a child to his community in Nicaragua or
Zambia? It is certainly not contested that intelligence can be represented differently
within different cultural groups; however, they also note that this is not to say that
intelligence can also be represented similarly when individuals share salient
aspects of their cultural and educational backgrounds. In this regard, they note that
investigations concerning the cultural equivalence of the WISC in cross-national
studies supports the notion of selected universal cognitive processes across cultures
(cf. Georgas et al., 2003a, b). In addition, the construct structure for specific ethnic
or cultural groups that emerges in factor analytic studies within the U.S. is also
generally consistent, although there may be some differences in factor loadings in
the lower age ranges due to developmental differences in the acquisition of specific
cognitive abilities, such as working memory skills (Wechsler, 2003). Similar
score comparisons among ethnic groups within a given country also have emerged,
and socioeconomic factors have been found to partially account for variability in
performance (Georgas, 2003), and there is remarkable consistency in the factor
structure of the WISC, when translated and adapted into other languages in other
countries (Georgas et al., 2003a, b). When mean score differences have been
identified among cross-national samples, these findings have been thought to be
4 Intellectual Abilities: Theoretical and Applied Assessment Considerations 65
Pediatric Assessment
Gonzalez (2001) identifies a number of tests that are appropriate to use with
Hispanic children. He stresses that language cannot be ignored by the assessing
clinician, especially given that many Hispanic children are exposed to various lev-
els of English and Spanish. Gonzalez (2001) suggests an in-depth review of lan-
guage proficiency of each individual client in more than one domain (e.g., home
and school) and strongly recommends consultation and cooperation with, if not
referral to, a bilingual neuropsychologist.
Bateria III
For cognitive assessment, the Batera III Woodcock-Muoz (Bateria III; Munoz-
Sandoval, Woodcock, McGrew, and Mather, 2005) represents a comprehensive
resource for clinicians. This is the Spanish version of the Woodcock-Johnson III
(WJ-III; Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001), including cognitive and achieve-
ment batteries. Cognitive ability is measured with the Pruebas de Habilidades
Cognitivas (Bateria-III COG). All tests comprising the Batera have been
adapted from the WJ-III. A special characteristic of the Batera is the Language
Exposure/Use Questionnaire included as a screening in the beginning of the test.
This instrument allows the examiner to verify information about which language
is preferred or spoken more proficiently by the client. For example, questions
about percentage of time each language is spoken are provided. Likewise, the
manual contains a checklist and training exercises for the examiner to ensure
proficiency.
Otero (2006) points out that true Spanish proficiency is essential to correctly
administer and score the Batera III. She notes several instances where the list of
correct answers to given questions does not contain words that are used by residents
66 C. von Thomsen et al.
of certain countries, so that the burden to recognize the answers as correct lies with
the examiner (Otero, 2006).
The norming sample consisted of 1,413 individuals who were identified as
native Spanish speakers. Of these, 1,134 individuals came from Mexico, Costa
Rica, Panama, Argentina, Colombia, Puerto Rico, and Spain. The remaining 279
participants consisted of U.S. residents from nine states, 89 of whom were born in
the U.S. The rest were ascertained from a variety of Latin American countries.
Spanish language dominance was ensured in the U.S. residents by means of oral
language screening and consultation with bilingual experts. Note that not every
country of origin was represented by an equal number of participants. Mexico, for
example, was overrepresented in relation to Spain, thus impacting comparability of
test results (Otero, 2006). On the other hand, a comparison of Batera III data with
WJ-III is easily achieved because normative data from the Batera were equated
with U.S. norms from the WJ-III. In other words, language proficiency in English
and Spanish may be compared using a single score.
Data from the calibration sample, as provided in the manual, indicate generally
good reliability for individual subtests ranging from .80 for Word Completion to
.93 for Verbal Comprehension. The COG battery offers four clusters with reliabili-
ties ranging from .88 for Auditory Processing to .94 for Verbal Abilities (in the
Extended Battery).
The test authors have conducted a confirmatory factor analysis which indicated
good fit between the organizational structure of the Batera III and the CHC model
of cognitive abilities, and have noted that internal factor structure of the Batera III
and the WJ III appeared very similar. However, these factor analyses are the only
validity data provided in the manual (Otero, 2006).
Despite a norming sample that might at times suffer from small cell sizes and
other weaknesses (e.g., poor sampling for specific groups), the thoughtful and
proven organization of this test, together with its comparability with the widely
used WJ-III, make it a notable and in some instances a useful tool for the Spanish-
speaking clinician who is looking for a well-designed instrument to assess cogni-
tive capabilities. The test also covers many aspects of a neuropsychological battery
in a single test.
The original Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) was published
in 1949 (Wechsler). The original scale (Wechsler, 1949) has undergone three
revisions since its original publication, resulting in the publication of the
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Revised (WISC-R; Wechsler, 1974),
the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Third Edition (WISC- III;
Wechsler, 1991), and the most recent, the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Children - Fourth Edition (WISC-IV; Wechsler, 2003). These revisions have
each reflected concomitant advances in theoretical models of intelligence,
4 Intellectual Abilities: Theoretical and Applied Assessment Considerations 67
Table 4.3 Matched Sample Mean WISC-IV Scores of Spanish versus English-Speaking
Hispanic and White Non-Hispanic English Speaking Children (Equated for Age, Gender, and
Parental Education Level) Data from Standardization sample
IQ index score Hispanic-spanish Hispanic-english Non-nispanic white
FSIQ 93.00 (10.95) 96.58 (12.87) 94.12 (15.58)
VCI 92.31 (9.74) 96.19 (13.02) 94.31 (12.06)
PRI 94.50 (12.24) 97.92 (11.54) 96.69 (15.73)
WMI 93.65 (13.07) 96.27 (15.20) 97.42 (15.04)
PSI 98.31 (11.47) 98.27 (13.84) 91.58 (14.23)
N = 26.
Source: Adapted from the WISC-IV Clinical Use and Interpretation. Reprinted by permission.
measure that is cross-culturally valid (Keith et al., 1999). However, a word of cau-
tion is necessary. The issue was initially discussed in Chapter 1, and care must be
exercised in interpreting scores from tests that were originally developed in other
cultures than the environment in which the test is being administered, so the find-
ings reported by Keith et al., may not be generalized. Although the DAS was recali-
brated in the U.S., its original and predecessor scales were developed in the United
Kingdom. Therefore, a careful test interpretation approach should prevail when
using this test with American populations, including Hispanics. As a result of this
criticism, it is important to ask whether such an issue really makes a difference (see
Chapters 1 and 6). Such a query is pivotal because if such an issue has no effects
on the psychometric properties of an intellectual test or other procedure used during
the course of neuropsychological assessment or the inferences derived from them,
it has no real bearing on assessment results. Lets examine this issue with the DAS.
A close examination at the correlations coefficients between tests of achievements
and the DAS demonstrates that the correlations between the DAS (originally devel-
oped abroad) and those tests of achievement are far lower, and as noted by Anastasi
and Urbina (1997), are below levels for decision making (<.50) than are those cor-
relations between tests such as the WISC (or other tests originally developed in the
U.S). Why is that the case? In order to understand the differences, one has to exam-
ine two major factors. First, a close examination of each test protocol is required,
which easily demonstrates the significant differences between requirements for
reading in the DAS (e.g., Word Reading) versus tests developed in the U.S.,
showing that reading tests for the DAS are far more difficult than reading achieve-
ment tests developed in the U.S., predominantly for early levels of academic
achievement. The second factor is related to differences in the educational systems
of the two countries, particularly during early periods of instruction, and the reader
is independently invited to examine such issues. Nevertheless, this example under-
scores the importance of paying attention to such factors when foreign tests, includ-
ing tests of intellect developed abroad, are applied with American children, but
particularly Hispanic children living in the U.S.
The Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) was published in 1999 and
is similar to the traditional Wechsler tests in that it yields a Verbal IQ, Performance
IQ, and Full Scale IQ (The Psychological Corporation). The WASI differs from the
72 C. von Thomsen et al.
other Wechsler tests in that it consists of four subtests that provide an estimated
assessment of cognitive functioning (The Psychological Corporation, 1999). The
Verbal scale consists of two subtests; the Vocabulary and Similarities subtests;
whereas the Performance scale is comprised of Matrix Reasoning and Block Design
subtests (The Psychological Corporation). Administration of the four subtests usu-
ally takes approximately 30 minutes to complete and the shortened two-subtest
version takes approximately 15 minutes to complete (The Psychological
Corporation). The two-subtest version consists of the Vocabulary and Matrix
Analogies (The Psychological Corporation). The WASI is individually adminis-
tered to examinees from 6 through 89 years of age (The Psychological Corporation).
The WASI manual states that this assessment instruments primary use should be
for screening purposes or to evaluate cognitive functioning (The Psychological
Corporation). In addition in most instances the WASI should not be utilized for
legal or forensic purposes or when a comprehensive assessment of cognitive func-
tioning is required (The Psychological Corporation).
The WASI standardization sample consisted of 2,245 individuals between the
ages of 6 to 89 and was considered to be representative of the U.S. English-speaking
population (The Psychological Corporation, 1999). However, the normative data
appear to be more representative of race than education in the 1997 census data
(Salvia and Ysseldyke, 2001). Similar to the WAIS-III and the WISC-IV, the nor-
mative sample was not standardized for use with modifications, thus if modifica-
tions are utilized they must be noted on the protocol and in the psychological report
(The Psychological Corporation).
Some studies demonstrated that the WASI correlates highly with other Wechsler
tests, including the WAIS-III and the WISC-III; however, in one study, when the
WASI was administered prior to the WAIS-III, the WASI IQs were not predictive
of the WAIS-III IQ scores (Axelrod, 2002). Similar findings were found in a study
that assessed 72 male patients at a Veterans Affairs Medical Center (Ryan et al.,
2003). Finally, the WASI poses difficulties for clinicians, as some subtests
responses are inaccurate for adults but given credit by the test publisher because
young children provided such answers in the standardization sample (e.g., see
responses in Vocabulary [alligator]).
The Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT) is a short cognitive assessment that
takes approximately 20 to 30 minutes to administer (Kaufman and Kaufman,
1990). The K-BIT consists of three subtests, Expressive Vocabulary and Definitions,
which combine to form a Vocabulary standard score and Matrices, which forms a
different standard score (1990). The K-BIT utilizes the same normative sample
as other Kaufman cognitive instruments, including the Kaufman Adolescent and
Adult Intelligence Test (KAIT) (Kaufman and Kaufman, 1993). The K-BIT can be
4 Intellectual Abilities: Theoretical and Applied Assessment Considerations 73
The Wechsler Adult Intelligence - Third Edition (WAIS-III) was published in 1997
by the Psychological Corporation (Wechsler). The current WAIS began in
1939 with the publication of the Wechsler-Bellevue Intelligence Scale
(Wechsler, 1997). After revisions were made to this initial assessment instru-
ment, it was then renamed the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale and was
published in 1955 (1997). Revisions to the WAIS in 1981 resulted in the publi-
cation of the WAIS-R. The current WAIS-III is utilized for the assessment of
intellectual ability. The WAIS-III is individually administered to adults aged 16
through 89 years (Wechsler).
Upon administration and scoring of the WAIS-III, the traditional three
composite IQ scores are generated: Verbal, Performance, and Full Scale
(Wechsler, 1997). In addition to these three IQ scores, four index scores can be
calculated: Verbal Comprehension, Perceptual Organization, Working Memory,
74 C. von Thomsen et al.
and Processing Speed (Wechsler). The four index scores were an enhancement
that was added to the WAIS-III based on revisions of the WAIS-R (Tulskee,
Saklofske, Wilkens, and Weiss, 2001). Throughout the WAIS-III manual, equal
weight was provided to the IQ and the index scores (Tulskee et al., 2001). The
WAIS-III consists of 14 subtests, 11 of which came from the WAIS-R, and three
new subtests: Symbol Search, Matrix Reasoning, and Letter-Number Sequencing
(Wechsler, 1997).
Hispanic individuals whose primary language spoken is not English may be at a
disadvantage on the Verbal subtests of the WAIS-III, thus obtaining deflated intel-
lectual quotients (Wechsler, 1997). This low performance may be an underestimate
of the examinees actual cognitive abilities and may be attributed to differences in
language (Wechsler). According to the WAIS-III manual, some individuals may
require modifications of the test procedures; however, the WAIS-III was not stand-
ardized for use with modifications (Wechsler). When modifications are necessary,
the examiner should be informed of and follow the general principle of test use that
has been established by the Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing
(American Psychological Association, 1985):
When a test user makes a substantial change in test format, mode of administration, instruc-
tions, language, or content the user should revalidate the use of the test for changed condi-
tions or have a rationale supporting the claim that additional validation is not necessary or
possible. (Standard 6.2, p 41).
This standard applies to the assessment of the Hispanic population when their
primary mode of communication is not English or when they are deemed as not
proficient in English. When modifications are utilized, including translation of
tasks to Spanish or modified instructions, they may affect the test scores, which
may then raise questions about the validity of the scores. If modifications are uti-
lized, it is then important to note them on the record form and again in the psycho-
logical report (Wechsler 1997).
The WAIS-III normative data was collected on individuals who speak English
fluently (Wechsler, 1997). The standardization sample for the WAIS-III consisted
of 2,450 adults 16 through 89 years of age (Kaplan and Saccuzzo, 1997). These
individuals were then broken down into 13 different groups based on age: 16 to 17,
18 to 19, 20 to 24, 25 to 29, 30 to 34, 35 to 44, 45 to 54, 55 to 64, 65 to 69, 70 to
74, 75 to 79, 80 to 84, and 85 to 89 (Kaplan and Saccuzzo). All of the specific aged
groups consisted of 200 participants with the exception of two groups aged 80 to
84 and 85 to 89 that were comprised of 150 and 100 subjects, respectively (Kaplan
and Saccuzzo, 1997). The sample was selected to correspond well to the U.S. Census
in terms of gender (Kaplan and Saccuzzo, 1997). If the WAIS-III is translated to
the examinees native language, this may invalidate the normative information
(Wechsler, 1997). The WAIS-III manual states that in situations similar to that
described above use your clinical judgment to evaluate performance in circum-
stances where translation of directions is necessary for completion of a given task
(Wechsler, 1997, p 34).
4 Intellectual Abilities: Theoretical and Applied Assessment Considerations 75
Geriatric Population
Since persons of Hispanic heritage form the most rapidly growing minority group
in the U.S., the geriatric segment of the overall population is also experiencing a
significant diversification (LaRue, Romero, Ortiz, Liang, and Lindeman, 1999).
One of the main tasks in geriatric neuropsychology is to distinguish normal from
abnormal aging processes (Rosselli and Ardila, 2001), especially for differential
diagnoses or rule-outs of dementias such as Alzheimers disease (AD) with other
conditions such as mood disturbances. Compared with non-Hispanic Whites,
Hispanics who live in the U.S. have been found to show a mean onset of AD symp-
toms more than 6 years earlier, even if matched for education, gender, and location
(Clark et al., 2005). At this point, it is unclear if a similar prevalence or incidence
rate of AD can be expected in the Hispanic population (LaRue et al., 1999).
As Lopez and Taussig (1991) point out, the conflict between emic and etic
approaches to assessment becomes evident in testing of elderly populations. Older
Hispanic clients, foreign or U.S. born, are more often less fluent in English, adhere
more closely to a culturally different worldview, and frequently have not been
exposed to extensive education, which mediates differences in cognitive
development.
Clinicians face the choice of using etic instruments such as the WAIS for assess-
ment, which carries with it the risk of underestimating cognitive performance in
elderly Hispanics, and emic instruments such as the EIWA (Wechsler, 1968).
Lopez and Taussig (1991)s study demonstrated that emic measures tend to over-
estimate performance in elderly Hispanics much as the etic instruments biases
results in the other direction. Additionally, both tests did not cause over- or
underestimation across different samples and tasks but instead exhibited more
pronounced errors in certain subgroups and subtests. Thus, the authors conclude
that assessment must not rely on scores from one test but should draw upon mul-
tiple data sources, including history, behavioral observation, and a variety of
tests. Furthermore, clinicians must be aware of each instruments inherent flaws
regarding certain populations and refrain from simply administering a standard
battery to all clients another example of how aspirational ethics influence neu-
ropsychological assessment with Hispanic clients (cf. Ardila, 2005; APA, 2001;
Artiola i Fortuny and Mullaney, 1998).
Cultural standards influence the definition of what is normal and useful for
assessment of Hispanic clients. While utilization of an etic test might be useful to
gain information about placement options in specific elderly programs or legal
stipulations, classification of what is impaired cognitive functioning may be much
more dependent on emic cultural standards.
Norms for elderly Hispanic populations are still lacking (LaRue et al, 1999;
Rosselli and Ardila, 2001) but are currently being developed (e.g., Mungas
et al., 2005). LaRue and colleagues (1999) provide preliminary norms for a
neuropsychological battery suitable for assessing the cognitive decline typically
76 C. von Thomsen et al.
observed in AD in Hispanics, including the Digit Forward Task, the Fuld Object
Memory Test (Fuld, 1981), Verbal Fluency, Clock Drawing, and Color Trails 1
and 2. Rosselli and Ardila (2001) offer an overview of tests that have been used
with the elderly Hispanic population.
Cognitive Status
Instruments to determine mental status are among the most frequently used to
screen for dementia. While these instruments tend to be short and typically do not
contain complex items, they usually have been translated into Spanish without
extensive back-translation or more advanced procedures used in test adaptation.
Furthermore, clinicians tend to prefer a version they have used for a while, which
has led to some variability regarding exactly what questions are asked (Mejia,
Gutierrez, Villa, and Ostrosky-Solis, 2004).
A Spanish version of the Minimental Status Exam was examined by this group
of researchers because the original version (Folstein, Folstein, and McHugh, 1975)
is one of the most frequently used instruments for detection of dementia (Mejia
et al., 2004). When administered to a sample of Spanish-speaking elderly that had
previously been categorized as normal, mild cognitive impairment (MCI), or
dementia patients, the MMSE mean of the normal group was 20, which is below
the established impairment cutoff point of 23. Level of education was found to
function as an important moderator in this study (Mejia et al., 2004), so that clini-
cians relying on the MMSE will misdiagnose individuals with low education as
demented. As Rosselli and Ardila point out, low academic achievement is more
likely a result of economic factors when found in Hispanics, and less likely associ-
ated with failure to adapt to the academic environment, than with non-Hispanic
Whites (Rosselli and Ardila, 2001). They also note that one typical item from men-
tal status exams, What season is it?, might be of less relevance for orientation in
Hispanics from tropical or subtropical countries (Rosselli and Ardila, 2001).
One approach to dealing with the many cultural influences on neuropsychologi-
cal measures is to employ functional measures instead (Rosselli and Ardila, 2001).
However, even this is a poor solution, since many functional skills may be viewed
by Hispanic elderly as limited to (for example) one gender, such as cooking or
managing money (Rosselli and Ardila, 2001).
After providing a brief overview of the historical roots (see Sattler 2001 for a more
comprehensive review) of cognitive assessment, classic theories of intelligence
were reviewed, including those of Carroll, Cattell, Horn, Spearman, and Sternberg.
Special emphasis was placed on the role of cultural factors in these influential
4 Intellectual Abilities: Theoretical and Applied Assessment Considerations 77
theories. Emic and etic approaches were described, and recent attempts to integrate
these were introduced. The literature relevant to these questions seems to indicate
that complete generality and equivalence of a g factor cannot be assumed across
all cultures. More complex and modern models of intelligence, such as those pro-
posed by Sternberg and Berry, are more comprehensive and better account for cul-
tural factors. Also, specific models of influences on cognitive functioning in
Hispanics have been suggested by researchers, including variables such as accul-
turation, language proficiency, and immigration patterns, as noted in previous
chapters in this volume and as elucidated above.
Recent data regarding the use of tests with Hispanic populations were reported.
Even though research suggests that language proficiency and cultural status
strongly influence performance on cognitive and neuropsychological tests, many
clinicians reportedly continue to utilize tests in a way that may yield biased results.
The use of appropriate norms is an important problem in this regard, but difficult
challenges emerge as a result of the heterogeneity of the Hispanic population.
A broad range of assessment instruments was briefly reviewed, as were their
applicability to Hispanic children, adolescents, and adults and specific instruments
suitable for the elderly Hispanic population. Although significant advances have
been made in instrumentation in the last decade, a great deal of work remains
ahead. In particular, advances in instrumentation with established, appropriate
norms and psychometric properties remain to be constructed that will lead to valid
and reliable inferences with Hispanics.
In conclusion, a review of the extant literature supports a view of intellectual
assessment with Hispanics that is complex, dynamic, and challenging to clinicians
and researchers alike. Despite greater understanding of such factors, the range of
variables that influence performance on tests of cognitive abilities has continued to
grow in recent years, while the development of instruments that take these variables
into account has lagged behind despite best efforts by test developers and publish-
ers. However, it is encouraging to see the recent publications of sophisticated
assessment instruments and up-to-date norms.
Chapter 5
Language: Development, Bilingualism,
and Abnormal States
78
5 Language: Development, Bilingualism, and Abnormal States 79
of the commonly used and psychometrically sound measures that will be helpful to
clinicians evaluating specific language impairments in Hispanic populations in the
U.S. and abroad.
Language Development
After perusing many texts that expound on the development of language, one might
perceive language development to be incremental in nature, occurring in discrete
stages. However, this simplification of the language development process does not
take into consideration the fluidity and complex process of expressive and receptive
language development. Nevertheless, a discussion of language development as an
increasingly complex and cumulative process will facilitate a greater understanding
of its course and progression. Readers of this section should be reminded that lan-
guage development is divided into stages based on age only for ease of discussion
and that allowances should be made for individual variability. In fact, individual
variability of language acquisition can range over many months (Bates, Thal,
Finlay, and Clancy, 2003); therefore, the following text should not be construed as
diagnostic criteria, but rather as a simple guideline to understand general language
development. Table 5.1 should be used as a quick reference guide to general devel-
opmental milestone expectations.
Neonates are not known for their conversational proficiency; however, this is not
to say that they do not communicate with their environment. Many students of lan-
guage development agree that infants are born into the world prepared to acquire
language (e.g., Bates, Thal, Finlay, and Clancy, 2003; Chomsky, 1991; Stuart, 2002;
Tager-Flusberg and Sullivan, 1998). In fact, infants are very adept at influencing and
Table 5.1 Early Language Developmental Expectations
Approximate age of onset Language function/behavior
Birth Reflexive vocalizations tied
to diffuse feeling states
Vegetative sounds
Emotional-prosodic in quality
6 weeks Cooing and pleasure noises
Differentiated cries
Responds to voice
2 months Defined babbling with oral-motor exploration
Distinguishes different speech sounds
3 months Orients head to voice
Vocal response to speech
Vocalizes two different vowel sounds
4 months Cries reflect specific feeling states
Increased frequency of imitative babbling
Imitates tone
Varies pitch of vocalizations
6 months Babbling with consonant use
Laughs out loud
Prosodic imitations of speech
8 months Canonical babbling with temporal-sequential
properties imitative of true speech
Production of word-like sounds
Vocalizes three different vowel sounds
Inhibition of nonnative language sounds
Defined pitch, tone, and prosody
Comprehension of simple words and commands
1113 months True production of words
Vocalizes four different vowel-consonant combinations
Gestures accompany word production
Comprehends some gestures
Follows simple one-step commands
Points to objects when asked show me
1820 months Two-word phrases
Naming explosion
Increased use of different type of words
(nouns, verbs, adjectives)
24 months Increased verbal fluency
Use of more complex phrases using
nouns and other word types
Understands and responds to yes/no and wh- questions
28 months Three-word phrases
Increase in grammatically correct utterances
36 months Development of egocentric speech
Production of 3- to 4-word phrases
Uses pronouns
Understands concept of one
Understands two prepositions
Follows two-step commands
Adapted from Bates, Thal, Finlay, and Clancy (2003); Bayley (1993); Eisenson, 1984; Kolb and
Fantie (1997); Sattler (1998); and Warner and Nelson (2003).
5 Language: Development, Bilingualism, and Abnormal States 83
being influenced by their surroundings, to which new parents can readily attest
(Taylor, 1999). Nevertheless, much of their communication is reflexive. That is,
vocalizations made by very young infants (birth to approximately three months of
age) are reflexive and occur as a result of diffuse feeling states (Bates, Thal, Finlay,
and Clancy, 2003; Eisenson, 1984). Their physical being is uncomfortable in some
way, whether this lack of comfort is represented by hunger pangs or elevated tem-
perature. They reflexively verbalize their lack of homeostasis by crying or making
another similar verbalization. As stated by Joseph (1996), these random vocaliza-
tions typically are emotional-prosodic in quality and mediated by limbic and
brainstem nuclei (p. 128). Neonates have very little awareness of their surround-
ings unless they somehow infringe on their homeostasis. If a blanket covers the
eyes, a diaper is wet, or if the child is startled, the child reflexively vocalizes in
order to have the uncomfortable stimuli removed. As such, the attitudes of accept-
ing or rejecting are the only feeling states an infant under one month of age experi-
ences and reacts in an attempt to return to homeostasis by removing the
uncomfortable stimuli in favor of pleasing stimuli (Joseph, 1996). Undifferentiated
cries and other vocalizations, commonly referred to as coos (Kolb and Fantie, 1997)
characterize this prespeech period, or prelocutionary stage (Stuart, 2002), and are
precursors to an infants development of more accurate and differentiated speech
sounds (Tager-Flusberg and Sullivan, 1998).
By two to three months of age, infants continue to engage in much random
vocalization in response to their individual needs (Joseph, 1996). However, more
defined babbling begins to emerge, although it is better explained by accidental
motor activity and positively reinforcing activity rather than purposeful approxima-
tions of speech (Joseph, 1996). This early babbling could be construed as a means
of testing the waters as infants experiment with the sounds they can make with
their tongue, lips, and mouth (Bates, Thal, Finlay, and Clayson, 2003). It is an
important developmental precursor to meaningful speech (Oller, 1986), and it is
strongly influenced by biological mechanisms underlying the language-articulatory
system (Oller, Eilers, Steffens, Lynch, and Urbano, 1994). According to Sattler
(1998), infants should be able to vocalize two separate vowel sounds by three
months of age. Again, these vowel sounds, although they may sound like words to
eager parents, are little more than accidental verbalizations produced by the inter-
play between an infants random oral motor activity.
After the first several months of a childs life, a distinct change in the vocaliza-
tions of the infant occurs. Although much of an infants vocalizations continue to
be random and reflexive, they begin to reflect certain feeling states instead of a
general lack of homeostasis (Joseph, 1996). In fact, many parents report that they
can decipher the differences between their childs cries at this stage. Distinct cries
and vocalizations serve different purposes, such as relating causes of discomfort
(i.e., hunger, wetness, tiredness), requesting a goal or desired object, and interacting
with others (Tager-Flusberg and Sullivan, 1998). As these vocalizations are more
tied to specific stimuli, they begin to interact with their world in a qualitatively dif-
ferent manner. In fact, being able to influence and interact with their environment
is a significant motivating factor in language learning (Bates, Thal, Finlay, and
Clancy, 2003; Tager-Flusberg and Sullivan, 1998).
84 C. French and A.M. Llorente
1984). Although they continue to make significant gains in the quantity of words
they are able to produce, children in this stage also make significant gains in the
quality of their communication. By two and a half years of age, children typically
have about 500 words in their lexicon from which to draw, many of which are nouns
and concrete objects. An increasing percentage of other words, such as verbs,
prepositions, and the like, are used on a more frequent basis (Bates, Thal, Finlay,
and Clancy, 2003; Fenson et al., 1994; Warner and Nelson, 2000).
After children acquire the foundational skills for language by three years of age,
they begin engaging in what could be termed true language. According to Joseph
(1996), true language incorporates verbal language production of words, including
concrete nouns and more abstract relational terms, adjectives, and prepositions.
True language at this age involves thinking words out loud, which coincides with
the development of egocentric speech. This type of thinking out loud is also
known as egocentric speech and usually involves self-directed self-explanatory
monologue (Joseph, 1996). Many a caregiver has unobtrusively observed a child
playing alone, dictating his actions out loud. As the child matures, this egocentric
speech turns inward and develops into thought (Joseph, 1996).
By entrance into these early childhood years, often signified by enrollment in
preschool, children begin to use their language skills to categorize and control their
environment (Kolb and Fantie, 1997). Now that the young child is exposed to adults
and same-age peers, even further language development occurs. They begin to experi-
ence the importance of knowing and being sensitive to their listener (Bates, Thal,
Finlay, and Clancy, 2003; Tager-Flusberg and Sullivan, 1998). Three-year-old children
learn the rules of communication, which involved turn-taking and pragmatics (Tager-
Flusberg and Sullivan, 1998; Warner and Nelson, 2000). As they are exposed to more
formal educational practices and cultural experiences children of this age typically are
viewed as making rapid strides in grammar, semantics, and syntax (Bates, Thal, Finlay,
and Clancy, 2003; Tager-Flusberg and Sullivan, 1998; Warner and Nelson, 2000).
Beyond the preschool years, children continue to make significant strides in
language development. However, many of the changes made are likely to be much
less apparent to caregivers. By age six, children typically have access to approxi-
mately 14,000 words. However, the way they combine the words and the length of
their utterances may continue to grow quite significantly through the early school
years. Formal training in grammar (morphology and syntax), semantics, and prag-
matics further increase language development and conversational efficiency (Bates,
Thal, Finlay, and Clancy, 2003).
As some may assume, all developmental markers of language acquisition are not
necessarily exactly the same among different languages and cultures or ethnic
groups. Therefore, Table 5.1, indicating specific language developmental milestones
may not be applicable to all languages and many clinicians and researchers assert
5 Language: Development, Bilingualism, and Abnormal States 87
that language milestones vary in nature and timing depending on the language
being acquired (Bates, Thal, Finlay, and Clancy, 2003; Choi, 1999; Crago and
Allen, 1999). However, others have indicated that the language milestones and
developmental trajectories should be considered similar for different languages
(Bedore, 1999; Fortin and Crago, 1999; Shonkoff and Phillips, 2000) and for
achievement of language developmental milestones during simultaneous language
acquisition (Petitto and Holowka, 2002). Prelinguistic language markers may be
similar between languages, especially between English and Spanish (Bedore,
1999). However, general babbling may have wide variations depending on the
childs native language. For instance, children learning Spanish and Italian meet
certain language developmental milestones sooner because the phonological sys-
tem is less complex than the English phonological system (Holm, Dodd, Stow, and
Pert, 1999; Leonard, 1999; Rhodes, Kayser, and Hess, 2000). Different languages
also have other factors that affect language development, including morphology,
inflection, word order, grammar, syntax, honorifics, and tone (Bedore, 1999; Fortin
and Crago, 1999; Leonard, 1999). All of these factors are also influenced by a
childs exposure to a second or third language (Leonard, 1999; Rhodes, Kayser, and
Hess, 2000). (For efficiency and ease of discussion, Spanish is used to denote a
second language.)
Bilingualism is a special case of language development. Multiple languages can
be learned simultaneously or sequentially. Regardless of the inherent benefits of
being bilingual, many individuals, including those that have frequent contact with
limited English-proficient students, have erroneous beliefs and negative perceptions
of limited English-proficient and bilingual individuals. It is clear that there are
many individuals in the nations schools and workplaces who have limited profi-
ciency in English or are bilingual. In fact, in the mid-1990s, 7% of students enrolled
in schools in the United States (Taylor, 1999) and in excess of 18% of individuals
in the workplace had limited English proficiency.
Just as it is assumed that an infant is born into the world prepared for language
development (Comrie, 2000), it also must be understood that the brain is not limited
to the acquisition of only one language. The single space theory contends that
there is not enough room in the brain for more than one language. This argument
leads to further assumptions that learning more than one language will actually
compromise or crowd other abilities at the cost of multiple language acquisition
and development (Mushi, 2002).
Acquisition of a second language does not differ significantly from development
of language in general (Hamayan and Damico, 1991; Krashen, 1982); however,
some distinguish between language acquisition and language learning, indicating
that the latter occurs due to a conscious effort of learning the rules of a second lan-
guage, whether by implicit or explicit means (Krashen, 1982; Rosa and Leow,
2004). As with infants who approximate speech sounds by mimicking others, sec-
ond language learners form habits based on what they hear others say. Students in
high school Spanish classes can often be heard repeating over and over, Como
est? Bien, gracias. Y tu? (How are you? Fine, thank you. And you?). This
automatic habit formation is the foundation for language learning (Hamayan and
88 C. French and A.M. Llorente
Damico, 1991). Second language acquisition also involves conscious rule learning
(Rosa and Leow, 2004), similar to what toddlers and preschool age children learn
in their interactions with peers and adults and through formal instruction.
Individuals learning a second language learn proper grammar and phonology
through active learning (S.H. Ochoa, personal communication, 2000). Through
social interactions with peers, there also is a phase of natural acquisition of mean-
ingful language. As Krashen (1982) noted, children learn a new language by doing.
Therefore, single words will increase to phrases, even though there will be
developmental errors and the interference of verbal habits from the first language
(S.H. Ochoa, personal communication, 2000). The more an individual engages in
any skill, the more proficient he or she will become.
The definition of a bilingual individual has many different facets which are
important to understand, especially when interacting with or teaching bilingual
individuals. A person can be bilingual through sequential (coordinate) or simulta-
neous (compound) means (Mushi, 2002; Quinn, 2001). Simultaneous bilinguals
grow up learning two languages at once. Simultaneous bilinguals could have grown
up in homes where one parent spoke English and the other parent spoke Spanish to
the child (Mushi, 2002). They could have been exposed to both languages by one
parent. Either way, simultaneous language learners develop two languages
simultaneously (Mushi, 2002). Contrary to what many lay individuals might
assume, simultaneous acquisition of two languages does not differ significantly
from single language development and there is little evidence of negative effects
(Krashen, 1982). However, simultaneous bilingual development is typically four to
five months behind monolingual language development, at least until the early
school years when the child is able to catch up to their single-language-learning
peers (Hamayan and Damico, 1991). Simultaneous language learners will tend to
outperform their sequential language-learning peers (Collier, 1995). However, it is
important to note that the individual must reach a certain level of proficiency in
both languages before positive effects can be observed (Cummins, 1979; Cascallar
and Arnold, 2001).
Sequential, or successive, language learners vary greatly in the reasons and
contexts in which they learn a second language. Some second language learners
elect to learn the language for purposes of upward mobility and the need to
belong or to meet academic expectations or educational programming criteria
(cf. Mushi, 2002). Individuals in these categories may decide to enroll in an
English-as-a-second-language class as an adolescent or adult. In contrast, circumstantial
language learners have to learn the second language in order to achieve minimum
expectancies for success in a society (to survive) or to meet purposes other than
those their native language can serve (cf. Mushi, 2002). This is the case for many
groups that have been displaced from their home country for whatever reason, or
groups of immigrants as noted in Chapter 2.
The rate and means at which individuals learn a second language also vary
greatly. Although children might be exposed to English during classroom instruc-
tion, they may continue to use their native language during recess or lunch, thereby
reducing their exposure to the second language. As Quinn (2001) notes, this has a
5 Language: Development, Bilingualism, and Abnormal States 89
is the mixed bilingual individual. In this particular type of bilingualism, the individual
is dominant in a particular skill in one language (i.e., speaking), but has better skills
in other areas in another language (Hamayan and Damico, 1991). A specific type
of mixed bilingualism is receptive bilingualism, in which a person can understand
spoken and written language in the second language, but is unable to write or speak
the second language (Centeno and Obler, 2001).
As previously inferred, placing a non-English-proficient or limited English-
proficient student into a regular English-speaking classroom can have detrimental
effects on many areas of functioning, including cognitive, emotional, social, aca-
demic, and linguistic development (Petitto and Holowka, 2002). In subtractive
bilingualism, the development of a second language has detrimental effects on the
maintenance and further development of the first language (Cummins, 1981;
Hamayan and Damico, 1991). If a child has not yet gained full proficiency in the
first language and is subsequently placed in an environment where they are
expected to learn a second language, the development of the first language will
likely slow down and perhaps even stop altogether (Collier, 1995). This occurs
when early exposure to both languages is provided but with no adequate training,
resulting in semi- or a-lingualism (Hamayan and Damico, 1991; Mushi, 2002;
Petitto and Holowka, 2002; Piper, 1993). A semilinguals language skills in the two
languages are not equivalent to the skills of a monolingual speaker of either lan-
guage (Centeno and Obler, 2001). To overcome semilingualism, Hamayan and
Damico (1991) state that the first language must be valued and developed. As can
be seen, the extent to which a child learns the first language prior to exposure to a
second language is highly correlated to how well a second language can be learned
(Collier, 1995).
Before reviewing the benefits of being bilingual, it is important to review the
negative perceptions and erroneous beliefs held by lay people about bilingual indi-
viduals. First, some contend that individuals who are bilingual have a smaller
vocabulary and are therefore at a disadvantage. In fact, some may believe and pro-
pose that these individuals (with smaller vocabularies) are at risk for developing a
language delay or disorder (Mushi, 2002; Petitto and Holowka, 2002). Another
misconception about bilingual individuals is that they cannot think of the proper
word or get confused when they code switch, or when they use both languages
simultaneously, and that it reflects poor bilingual and cognitive ability (Mushi,
2002; Petitto and Holowka, 2002; Rhodes, Kayser, and Hess, 2000).
Another misperception of bilingual individuals, especially of non-English-
proficient children entering into school in the United States for the first time, is the
appearance of rapid increases in English during the first few weeks or months
(Petitto and Holowka, 2002). Many teachers perceive the child to be picking up the
language very quickly and automatically assume that the child is as fluent in English
as their monolingual counterparts. However, this linguistic faade will lead to
erroneous beliefs by the teacher. This rapid learning of English is only surface
fluency and is described by Cummins (1979, 1984; Paul, 1996; Woodcock and
Muoz-Sandoval, 1993a, 1993b) as Basic Interpersonal Communication Skills
5 Language: Development, Bilingualism, and Abnormal States 91
cognitive tasks that monolinguals do not have available to them. Even very young
bilingual children engage in code switching, although the nature of code switching
in young children is qualitatively different than code switching in older children
and adults (Rhodes, Kayser, and Hess, 2000). For example, young children tend to
insert single words to express meaning (I like perros [dogs]). By the time a child
is about three years old, Rhodes, Kayser, and Hess (2000) state that children of this
age code switch by making a complete statement in both languages, and this is
often observed in Hispanic children living in the U.S. For example, a child would
say, Quiero una manzana. I want an apple. This method is used to resolve ambi-
guities, clarify statements, and attract attention. By eight years of age, a childs
code-switching activities are used for emphasizing statements, making commands,
and elaborating upon previous statements. For example, a child might state, Quiero
un lpiz (I want a pencil). These examples are types of intrasentential code-switch-
ing activities (Hamayan and Damico, 1991). By nine or ten years old, code switch-
ing takes on a different nature, occurring at the phrase and sentence levels, and is
known as intersentential code switching (Hamayan and Damico, 1991). A child
may say one sentence in English, and say the next sentence in Spanish. As can be
seen, the practice of code switching enriches a bilinguals verbal expressions and
increases their cognitive flexibility (Hamayan and Damico, 1991).
There are additional benefits to being bilingual. According to Cummins and
Gultusan (1975) and Mushi (2002), children who are bilingual become more
knowledgeable about language and are therefore able to think about language in an
abstract way. In other words, they are better suited for objectification of language
and metacognition. Furthermore, bilingual speakers are typically more aware of
language, especially as the contexts and situations for each language vary through-
out a typical day. From this assumption arises the verbal mediation hypothesis
(Hakuta, Ferdman, and Diaz, 1987). This hypothesis purports that bilingual
individuals use language more efficiently because they are more aware of language.
Based on this review of the limitations and benefits of bilingual individuals, it is
evident that the benefits of being bilingual clearly outweigh the limitations that are
surreptitiously placed on those that are bilingual.
In summary, it is clear that bilingual individuals, especially those that are simul-
taneous language learners, have an advantage over monolinguals. It is especially
important for teachers and others who have frequent contact with sequential
language learners to understand that they do not develop the level of language pro-
ficiency that would allow them to be successful in academic environments until
many years after their exposure to the second language. Fallacies about bilingual
children being unable to succeed in academic environments are compounded by the
lack of knowledge regarding simultaneous versus sequential language learning. The
importance of understanding the many facets of bilingualism has been clearly
addressed in this section. In addition, new research (Harris and Llorente, 2005)
points to the importance of addressing multiple language issues when considering
differences in performance on measures of cognitive ability (see Chapter 4), and
this is a vital issue because when developing norms for neuropsychological proce-
dures and tests, it is common not to include individuals in the standardization
sample who are not fluent in English, although those who speak English as a second
5 Language: Development, Bilingualism, and Abnormal States 93
language are commonly included. However as noted by Harris and Llorente (2005),
Realistically, very little is known about the language abilities of these individu-
als and the degree to which they are [really] bilingual. In addition, Harris and
Llorente note that language preference is not synonymous with language
proficiency, yet it is usually self-reported preferred language that enters as a key
variable for planning assessment strategies and for other decision making, such as
inclusion in normative studies. Furthermore, as previously noted, the degree of
proficiency can vary widely among individuals from ethnic with minority status,
which is not the case for the vast majority of nonminority White examinees.
According to Harris and Llorente (2005), Within various ethnic groups, such as
Hispanic, American Indian, and Alaskan Native, not only does this imply heteroge-
neity of English receptive and expressive abilities, but the very concept of bilingual-
ism signifies more than a simple characterization of two languages for many of
these groups.
family issues, behavior, and language dominance. Some of the more common
reasons for acquired language disorders include cerebral vascular accidents,
acquired traumatic brain injury, epilepsy, tumors, and hypoxic or anoxic events that
affect the language centers and circuits of the brain, specifically the perisylvian arc
(Kaufman, 2001). However, some individuals have congenital language disorders
as a result of genetic or other etiology and struggle with language expression and
reception from birth. Whether a language disorder is acquired or congenital, an
individuals functioning is clearly affected. When one thinks about the daily inter-
actions with individuals at school, work, or home, one can begin to realize how
dependent society is on communication. With language delays or disorders, indi-
viduals have greater difficulty facilitating connections with those around them. In
fact, many children with congenital language disorders have a tendency to with-
draw from social interactions because of their reduced or limited ability to compre-
hend information or express their thoughts and needs. It becomes clear that
language is important not only for the exchange of information between individu-
als, but for social connectedness as well. From a clinical standpoint, in Hispanics
as well as other groups, the course of language disorders may also provide impor-
tant diagnostic information because it is related to the natural history of a specific
condition affecting language skills. Therefore, the course observed in children with
autism in which proper, initial language development is followed by rapid declines
in these skills (APA, 2000) may provide important diagnostic information when
working with Hispanic children.
Language disorders are commonly believed to affect the expression of language.
However, language disorders may also impact an individuals capacity to understand.
Language disorders are also different from speech disorders. Speech disorders affect
the oral-motor output of language and do not impact the actual language code that is
being verbalized. Children who stutter or have poor articulation are thought to have a
speech disorder and not a language disorder, unless there are other impairments.
Speech and linguistic disorders, although different, can occur simultaneously. This
section will focus mainly on language disorders with regard to understanding the
differences between the many different forms of aphasia.
Before entering into a discussion of specific linguistic disorders, it is important
to lay the foundation for understanding how a language disorder is defined. According
to the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (1982) a language disorder is:
the impairment or deviant development of comprehension and/or use of a spoken, written,
and/or other symbol system. The disorder may involve (1) the form of language (phonologic,
morphologic, and syntactic systems), (2) the content of language (semantic system),
and/or (3) the function of language in communication (pragmatic system) in any combina-
tion (p. 949).
Table 5.2 Language Disorders and Their Pseudonyms, Related Pathology, and Subsequent
Neurologic Disturbances
Neurologic
Pseudonym Related pathology disturbances
Brocas aphasia Nonfluent Left frontal lobe adjacent to Contralateral hemi-
aphasia, primary motor cortex paresis, sensory
Motor aphasia, loss, or visual-
Expressive field cut
aphasia
Conduction Arcuate fasciculus and/or Possible depending
aphasia supramarginal gyrus of the on location and
left hemisphere, disconnec- extent of lesion
tion between Brocas area,
inferior parietal lobule, and
Wernickes area
Expressive Left frontal cortex Contralateral hemi-
aphasia paresis of upper
extremity
Global aphasia Global left hemisphere Contralateral hemi-
involvement of the language paresis, sensory
axis, middle cerebral artery loss, or visual-
field cut
Wernickes Fluent aphasia, Left superior temporal cortex, Contralateral upper
aphasia Receptive extending from the auditory extremity hemi-
aphasia, association cortex toward paresis
Sensory apha- the inferior parietal lobule
sia,
Jargon aphasia,
(Non-pure)
word
deafness
Word blindness Left temporal superior lobe and Ideomotor apraxia in
angular gyrus of parietal buccofacial and
cortex, white matter beneath extremity
the supramarginal gyrus activities
Word deafness Cortical Primary auditory area in the Possible depending
deafness superior temporal lobe on location and
(Heschls gyrus), discon- extent of lesion
nection between Wernickes
area and the pathways
from the medial geniculate
nucleus, bilateral involve-
ment of the superior tempo-
ral gyrus
Anomic aphasia Lesion or abnormality in any Possible depending
part of the language cortex on location and
extent of lesion
(continued)
98 C. French and A.M. Llorente
perusal of Tables 5.2, 5.3, and 5.4 will help the reader familiarize themselves with
the primary names of the numerous language disorders, their pseudonyms, probable
related anatomical pathology, and associated neurologic deficits.
Brocas Aphasia
aphasia
Wernickes X X X X X X
aphasia
Word X X
blindness
Word X X
deafness
Note: X = major dysfunction; x = minor dysfunction; *write to dictation; **read out loud.
Adapted from Benson (1993); Joseph (1996); Kalat (1998); and Lezak (1995).
99
100
known as Brocas area (cf. Kalat, 1998), which is a small area in the left, lateral
frontal cortex adjacent to the primary motor cortex areas 44, 45, and 46 in
Broadmanns map. After following up on this discovery during autopsy of several
more language-impaired patients, Broca discovered strong similarities in the
presenting problems (i.e., nonfluent verbal output, reduction in grammatical com-
plexity, intact language comprehension) and the affected physiological structure of
the brain (Bates, Thal, Finlay, and Clancy, 2003; Kalat, 1998). Although there
likely was damage to other cortical and subcortical structures to cause such chronic
and severe language impairments with these patients, as damage only to Brocas
area would only cause limited language impairment, Broca had made an important
discovery in finding the anatomical structure related to language functioning
(cf. Benson, 1993; Kalat, 1998).
Limited or impaired language production is the main characteristic of Brocas
aphasia. Even individuals with Brocas aphasia who use American Sign Language
to communicate have impaired expressive language, even though they can use their
upper extremities and hands to perform other non-language-related tasks. Although
impaired expressive language ability appears to be easily described at the outset,
there are many components that make up language production and output. An indi-
viduals language impairment may be so severe that they can only produce noises
instead of words. This significantly limits an individuals ability to communicate.
As previously noted, individuals with Brocas aphasia may also be known as
nonfluent aphasics; that is, the ease with which individuals with Brocas aphasia
produce expressive language is hindered (Benson, 1993). Brocas aphasia also
affects an individuals ability to name objects, although his or her performance can
typically be improved with phonetic or contextual cues (Benson, 1993). Another
difficulty that individuals with Brocas aphasia encounter is the impaired ability to
repeat words and phrases (Benson, 1993). Furthermore, these individuals experience
significant difficulty using words that are not nouns or verbs. They may omit word
endings, prepositional phrases, conjunctions, and modifiers in their expressive lan-
guage. When the meaning of their communication is dependent upon these types of
words, their output is very much impaired.
Individuals with Brocas aphasia have difficulty with the pronunciation and
meaning of language, while their comprehension of language remains fairly intact
(Benson, 1993), and these problems are discernable during bedside visits. It is criti-
cal to note that this is quite applicable to Hispanic patients, and these individuals
are able to comprehend most of what is said or written in Spanish, if monolingual,
and English and Spanish if bilingual. However, they also have difficulty under-
standing the connector words in sentences; that is, they understand nouns and verbs
much better than they comprehend pragmatic language, prepositional phrases, and
conjunctions (Bates, Thal, Finlay, and Clancy, 2003; Kalat, 1998). Basically, the
comprehension of individuals with Brocas aphasia resembles that of normal peo-
ple who are greatly distracted (p. 390). Just as they have difficulty using these
words, they have difficulty comprehending them.
With regard to the identifiable anatomical correlates of Brocas aphasia, the spe-
cific area that is responsible for the fluency expression of language is in the left
102 C. French and A.M. Llorente
frontal cortex directly adjacent to the primary motor area as noted above. According
to Joseph (1996), information from the posterior language axis converges via the
arcuate fasciculus in Brocas area and receives the final sequential (syntactical,
grammatical) imprint so as to become organized and expressed as temporally
ordered motoric linguistic articulations (p. 134). This area also is responsible for
the musculature of the mouth and face and the right hand (Joseph, 1996). Because
of this close relationship with the primary motor cortex, individuals with Brocas
aphasia may also suffer from hemiparesis of their right upper extremities, in
addition to sensory loss or visual-field disturbance (cf. Benson, 1993).
According to Professor Benson (1993), there are two subtypes of Brocas
aphasia: Big Brocas aphasia and Little Brocas aphasia. Big Brocas aphasia
results after injury to the brain, leaving the individual with severe total aphasia and
contralateral hemiparesis. As healing and restorative processes occur in the brain,
the symptoms ameliorate, leaving the individual with some of the aforementioned
characteristics (i.e., nonfluent output, dysnomia, impaired repetition, impaired use
of modifiers and connectors). Reportedly, this pattern of impairment is indicative
of injury to the dominant, typically left, hemispheres frontal opercular region,
with an associated lesion in the basal ganglia. On the other hand, Little Brocas
aphasia begins with the basic aphasic symptoms, which improve to feature hesitant
output and mild agrammatism. This pattern of impairment is indicative of damage
to the left hemispheres frontal opercular region without extending lesions into
subcortical structures.
As can be seen, there are variations in the presentation of Brocas aphasia (Bates,
Thal, Finlay, and Clancy, 2003; Benson, 1993). The characteristic symptoms differ
not only as a result of injury, but also as a result of gender differences. Joseph
(1996) contends that Brocas area is not as well developed in men compared to
women. He indicates that because the anterior regions of the female brain are more
responsible for expressive and emotional speech, women are likely to become more
severely aphasic with left frontal injuries compared to their male counterparts.
Conversely, the left parietal region is argued to house more of a mans expressive
and emotional speech capacity, therefore putting men at risk for Brocas aphasia
with left parietal injuries.
Wernickes Aphasia
Karl Wernicke was a contemporary of Paul Broca and had a career as a German
neurologist and psychiatrist. Through his work, Wernicke discovered an area of the
brain that was responsible for language output in the left superior temporal cortex,
extending from the auditory association area of the first temporal gyrus toward the
inferior parietal lobe (Benson, 1993; Joseph, 1996; Kalat, 1998) posterior area 22
in Broadmanns map. It is closely tied to Brocas area and is connected by the arcu-
ate fasciculus. As a result of this close connection, the distinction between Brocas
5 Language: Development, Bilingualism, and Abnormal States 103
aphasia and Wernickes aphasia is often muddy, and the two language impairments
share many of the same deficits. Nevertheless, damage to Wernickes area is known
to affect comprehension of semantics (Bates, Thal, Finlay, and Clancy, 2003).
Joseph (1996) indicated that the area known as Wernickes area is responsible for
decoding and encoding auditory-linguistic information to extract or impart
temporal-sequential order and related linguistic features (p. 139) from language
expressed verbally or via written means. Joseph also related that Wernickes area is
responsible for providing meaning and labels for information expressed by others.
Because of the physiological structures of the brain affected in individuals with
Wernickes aphasia, they might also experienced hemiparesis of their contralateral
upper extremity.
Although it is best known as Wernickes aphasia, this acquired language disorder
has many other pseudonyms. Some of these alternate names include fluent aphasia,
receptive aphasia, sensory aphasia, jargon aphasia, and non-pure-word deafness.
These additional names will become clear when the specific symptoms of
Wernickes aphasia are discussed.
The basic characteristics of Wernickes aphasia or fluent aphasia include difficulty
understanding spoken and written language (Joseph, 1996; Lezak, 1995). Expressive
language (speaking and writing) is typically intact unless there are concomitant inju-
ries in language pathways or other language centers. Also, the motor production and
fluency of speech is typically undamaged (Bates, Thal, Finlay, and Clancy, 2003;
Benson, 1993; Joseph, 1996; Kalat, 1998; Lezak, 1995). Interestingly, Kalat (1998)
indicated that deaf individuals are still able to understand sign language even if they
have Wernickes aphasia. However, those that communicate through verbal means
have difficulty comprehending what is being said to them. Joseph (1996) contended
that individuals suffering from Wernickes aphasia cannot distinguish between the
separate units of speech and their temporal order. They will typically be able to deci-
pher and understand more familiar and commonly used words, such as family mem-
ber names. However, their comprehension of less commonly used words, in addition
to prepositional phrases, possessives, or verb tense changes, is very limited. As a
result, what an individual with Wernickes aphasia likely hears is a blur of verbal out-
put that carries absolutely no meaning. Nevertheless, it has been shown that their
comprehension can be improved if the sounds are separated by long intervals so they
can decipher the individual sounds (Joseph, 1996).
In his lectures, Benson (1993) made a clear differentiation between severity level
of comprehension between the two modalities of receptive language spoken and
written language. Individuals with relatively weaker comprehension of spoken lan-
guage compared to comprehension of written language are known as word deaf. When
this pattern emerges, Benson argues that there is likely pathology deep in the connec-
tions of the first temporal and Heschls gyrus. On the other hand, when individuals
experience more greatly impaired comprehension of written language, they are known
to have word blindness, which is associated with involvement of the angular gyrus.
Besides difficulty in receptive language and comprehension, individuals with
Wernickes aphasia often have some difficulties with expressive language, includ-
104 C. French and A.M. Llorente
ing reading and writing. In fact, Joseph (1996) purports that the writing of an
individual with Wernickes aphasia will likely be unintelligible. Because they have
difficulty initially understanding language, these individuals have select difficulties
repeating words and phrases (Bates, Thal, Finlay, and Clancy, 2003; Joseph, 1996).
Wernickes sufferers also have significant difficulty with anomia and often omit
nouns and verbs in their expressive language (Joseph, 1996; Kalat, 1998). Their
naming ability is not typically aided by cueing (Benson, 1993). Although their
speech is usually fluent, they may experience nonfluency and articulation problems
when they pause to think of the name of something. As a result, these individuals
use circumlocution to express their ideas. Also, speech is often empty because of
the omission of nouns and verbs (Joseph, 1996). Instead, the language of Wernickes
patients often contains paraphasic errors and neologistic distortions (Benson, 1993;
Joseph, 1996) to replace the lack of content. As a result, these individuals may first
appear to a mental health professional as evidencing psychotic thought processes
because of these errors. These individuals also typically omit pauses and sentence
endings, so that their verbal output may sound like a foreign language (Joseph,
1996). This pattern of expressive language difficulties in Wernickes sufferers is a
direct result of damage to the respective language area, in that Wernickes area
also acts to code linguistic stimuli for expression prior to its transmission to Brocas
area (Joseph, 1996, p. 140). Furthermore, these individuals, including Hispanic
patients, may perpetuate others belief that they are indeed psychotic because they
do not have any awareness that what they are saying is meaningless, and anosog-
nosia is quite common in Wernickes aphasia (Joseph, 1996).
Anomic Aphasia
Conduction Aphasia
One of the most difficult types of aphasia to observe at the patients bedside, par-
tially because it may only be observed for a period of time during the acute phase
of rehabilitation, conduction aphasia results from lesions within the arcuate fascicu-
lus and the white matter of the supramarginal gyrus, as well as lesions in the poste-
rior perisylvian region of the dominant hemisphere (Bates, Thal, Finlay, and
Clancy, 2003; Benson, 1993). With such lesions, the three main areas of the lan-
guage center, namely Brocas area, Wernickes area, and the inferior parietal lobe,
become disconnected (Joseph, 1996). Based on the location and extent of the
lesion, neurological disturbances vary; however, Benson argued that individuals
with conduction aphasia commonly experience ideomotor apraxia that involves
buccofacial and limb activities (p. 26).
Individuals with conduction aphasia are similar to those with Wernickes aphasia
in many respects. They typically have good articulation and fluent output, but expe-
rience different levels of anomia circumlocution and are unable to repeat what
others say (Benson, 1993; Kalat, 1998). They also have difficulty maintaining
conversations and make paraphasic errors and word substitutions.
Although individuals with conduction aphasia have several characteristics that
resemble the language deficits of individuals with Wernickes aphasia, they typi-
cally have better comprehension of written and verbal language (Benson, 1993;
Joseph, 1996; Kalat, 1998). Also, individuals with conduction aphasia are not as
106 C. French and A.M. Llorente
likely to experience anosognosia; that is, they are aware of their language deficits
(Joseph, 1996). As a result, they engage in much circumlocutional speech in
attempts to come up with the proper words through successive approximations.
Regardless, Joseph (1996) indicated that these individuals have shorter, unrelated
utterances than those with Wernickes aphasia. Those suffering from conduction
aphasia also have notable difficulty in reading out loud or writing to dictation
(Joseph, 1996).
Expressive Aphasia
Expressive aphasia is another language impairment that results from damage to the
language center of the brain, typically the left frontal convexity (Joseph, 1996).
Because of the localization of pathology in individuals with expressive aphasia,
they may experience hemiparesis of the contralateral upper extremity. Expressive
aphasia typically limits an individuals capacity to speak, which impacts their
ability to repeat what others say, articulate words, or provide more than one- or
two-word utterances. Comprehension of language is usually spared in these
individuals, as is the ability to copy words by writing, silent reading comprehension,
and verbalization of semantically meaningful words or statements (Joseph, 1996).
In many cases, these individuals may be able make emotional statements or sing
words that they are unable to say (Joseph, 1996). Although reading is intact, the
ability to write, even to dictation, is significantly impacted. According to Joseph
(1996), individuals with expressive aphasia have major deficits in grammar, pros-
ody, naming, fluency, and syntax, and often evidence paraphasias, substitutions,
and omissions of relational words.
Global Aphasia
One of the most devastating events in a Hispanic patients life, as the name suggests,
global aphasia results in dysfunction in all the aspects of language (Benson, 1993).
It typically results from damage to the left middle cerebral artery, secondary to cer-
ebrovascular disease and related cerebrovascular accidents (Benson, 1993; Joseph,
1996). As a result, there are typically several neurological disturbances associated
with global aphasia, including contralateral hemiparesis of the upper and lower
extremities, sensory loss, and even visual-field cuts. Not only is comprehension of
language affected in spoken and written modalities, expression of language is also
significantly impacted. Individuals who have suffered from cerebrovascular acci-
dents have language skills that are compromised in areas of speaking, writing, and
repeating (Benson, 1993; Joseph, 1996). In addition, their speech, if present at all,
5 Language: Development, Bilingualism, and Abnormal States 107
is severely nonfluent (Benson, 1993; Kaufman, 2001). They may have difficulty
maintaining conversation and have brief utterances.
Sensory Aphasia
Mixed Aphasia
Pathology in the anterior and posterior cortical areas may result in mixed transcorti-
cal (extrasylvian) aphasia (Benson, 1993). It shares the characteristics of motor and
sensory aphasia, in addition to global aphasia; however, an individuals ability to
repeat words or phrases is typically spared in mixed aphasia. Mixed aphasia can be
caused by several different events, including occlusions within the internal carotid
artery or the residual effects of hypoxic events or edema (Benson, 1993). Although
neurological disturbances are likely, there are no consistent findings, which reflect
the true mixed features of this type of aphasia. Although repletion is intact, indi-
viduals with mixed aphasia experience many other difficulties, including poor
comprehension of spoken and written language, reading, naming, fluency, and use
of paraphasias (Benson, 1993).
There are several other types of extrasylvian, or transcortical aphasias. Though they
are not as common as the perisylvian aphasias (e.g., Brocas aphasia, Wernickes
aphasia, global aphasia), a brief review of them is in order. The ability to repeat
words and phrases is typically spared in individuals with any of the extrasylvian
aphasias. Besides extrasylvian motor and mixed aphasias, the other three transcorti-
cal aphasias include motor, supplementary motor, and subcortical aphasia.
5 Language: Development, Bilingualism, and Abnormal States 109
Extrasylvian motor aphasia occurs when there is injury anterior and superior to
Brocas area, in the frontal area of the language cortex, resulting in a disconnection
between Brocas area and the supplementary motor area (Benson, 1993). Neurological
disturbances vary depending on the injury. For the most part, individuals with
motor aphasia experience nonfluent speech and echolalia, even though their ability
to repeat is intact. Furthermore, these individuals ability to comprehend spoken
and written language remains largely intact.
Individuals with extrasylvian supplementary motor aphasia often present with
mutism immediately following injury. According to Benson (1993), these individuals
experience hypophonic expressive language that improves with practice. To create
this pattern of symptoms, there is likely injury to the cingulated gyrus and supple-
mentary motor area, resulting in neurological impairment of the contralateral side.
These disturnces may include weakness in the lower extremity, in addition to
sensory impairments (Benson, 1993).
Extrasylvian subcortical aphasia may result from injury to the language cortex;
however, most occurrences of subcortical aphasia result from injury to subcortical
structures, namely the basal ganglia and thalamus, although other subcortical
structures may be involved as well (Benson, 1993). Depending on the location and
extent of the injury, there may be neurological disturbances on the ipsalateral or
contralateral side. It is important to note that if the injury involves subcortical struc-
tures as well as the language areas, the level of impairment is likely to be greater
than if the injury were confined to small subcortical structures; otherwise, the
characteristics of subcortical aphasia are likely to quickly ameliorate (Benson,
1993). Similar to supplementary motor aphasia, individuals with subcortical aphasia
initially present with mutism, which develops into a hypophonic, slow, and poorly
articulated outputcontaminated with paraphasias which disappear when asked to
repeat spoken language (Benson, 1993, p. 31).
Single-Modality Disturbances
learning disability, simply because there has to be a loss of ability, rather than sim-
ply the incomplete or lack of development of reading. Its only characteristic is the
loss of ability to read, even in the presence of normal comprehension of spoken
language. It is similar to pure word blindness as Benson (1993) described it; how-
ever, pure word blindness is the lack of comprehension of written language. Even
though the individual with pure word blindness is not able to comprehend any writ-
ten language, they have the ability to read. Individuals with alexia cannot even read
written language.
Optic aphasia is another type of single-modality disturbance. Like alexia, it
affects the ability to read. However, optic aphasia limits the individuals ability to
read fluently because he or she can only read one letter at a time (Kalat, 1998).
Therefore, the ability to integrate the letters to form a word with meaning will also
be impacted, which in turn impacts comprehension.
Aphemia is the third single-modality disturbance. It is also known as pure
word dumbness, or anarthria (Benson, 1993). Injury directly to or inferior to
Brocas area may result in aphemia. As with subcortical aphasia and supplemen-
tary motor aphasia, individuals with aphemia initially present with mutism,
gradually developing into hypophonic, dsyprosodic, and slow speech. Although
it also shares some similarities with Little Brocas aphasia, aphemia does not
impact grammar in either speech or writing (Benson, 1993). Also, the ability to
understand spoken language and express language in verbal and written modali-
ties remain intact (Benson, 1993).
This section is concluded by addressing aspects of bilingualism affected by
specific pathologies sometimes observed in Hispanic populations. Case studies,
and more recently, neuroimaging and neurosurgical studies, have elucidated
important data regarding bilingualism in individuals who have sustained neu-
rovascular trauma. For example, the research literature suggests that there are
usually different levels of recovery in each language (e.g., Catalan v. Spanish)
after strokes affecting substrates in the brain subserving language functions. In
these cases, is not unusual for an individual to exhibit almost complete recovery
in the language learned first, with greater loss in the language most recently
learned (Junque, Vendrell, and Vendrell, 1995; Paradis, 1977). Although open
to alternate interpretation (Hines, 1996), such findings, coupled with recent
neuroimaging studies, have suggested the presence of a clear neuroanatomical
dissociative representation in the brains of bilinguals for each language (cf.
Gomez-Tortosa et al., 1995).
There are several different aspects of language that are evaluated during assessment.
A thorough language evaluation involves an assessment of an individuals ability to
understand what is said to them (i.e., receptive language) and their ability to express
their thoughts (i.e., expressive language). Beyond these two main areas of language
5 Language: Development, Bilingualism, and Abnormal States 111
Vocabulary
The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Third Edition (PPVT-III; Dunn and
Dunn, 1997) is a commonly used measure to assess receptive vocabulary for
individuals aged 2 years, 6 months, through 90 years of age. According to
Anastasi and Urbina (1997), vocabulary measures such as the Peabody Picture
Vocabulary Test Third Edition (PPVT-III; Dunn and Dunn, 1997) are useful
to assess use vocabulary. This test consists of 204 test plates, each consisting
of four numbered pictures. As each test plate is shown to the individual, the
examiner provides the stimulus word orally. According to the test directions
(Dunn and Dunn, 1997), the examinee is required to indicate the correct picture
by pointing to or stating the number of the picture that best represents the
meaning of the word. For each age group, certain blocks of items are adminis-
tered until the individual obtains eight errors in a block, at which time the test is
discontinued.
Other measures of expressive and receptive vocabulary include the Expressive
One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test 2000 Edition (EOWPVT-2000; Gardner,
2000), the Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test 2000 Edition (ROWPVT-
2000; Gardner, 2000), and the Beery Picture Vocabulary Test (BPVT; Beery and
Taheri, 1992). The Test of Word Finding (TWF; German, 1986) contains a group
of tasks that assess an individuals verbal abilities similar to the Evaluation of
Language Fundamentals Preschool Second Edition (CELF-II Preschool; Wiig,
Secord, and Semel, 2004) both of have which Expressive Vocabulary subtests.
Rapid/Speeded Naming
Rapid or speeded naming techniques can be traced back to Norman Geschwind and
his identification of a disconnection syndrome (Baron, 2004). These tasks draw
upon language and executive functioning, as a person has to quickly call to mind
the verbal labels for visual stimuli. Rapid naming tasks have been created using a
variety of stimuli, including blocks of color, color words, numbers, letters, and
simple objects.
There are several examples of rapid naming measures. The Clinical Evaluation
of Language Fundamentals Fourth Edition Rapid Automatic Naming subtest
(CELF-IV; Semel, Wiig, and Secord, 2003) is a criterion-referenced measure used
to assess an individuals ability to rapidly name shapes, colors, and color-shape
combinations. The NEPSY Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment has
the Speeded Naming subtest (NEPSY; Korkman, Kirk, and Kemp, 1998), which
requires the child to rapidly identify the size, color, and shape of objects presented
to them (e.g., small, blue triangle). The Comprehensive Test of Phonological
Processing Rapid Naming subtests (CTOPP; Wagner, Torgesen, and Rashotte,
1999) incorporate some of the aforementioned tasks, and include the rapid naming
of colors, numbers, letters, and objects. The Delis-Kaplan Tests of Executive
Function (D-KEFS; Delis, Kaplan, and Kramer, 2001) have provided for a new and
unique way to measure speeded naming ability. The Delis-Kaplan Tests of
5 Language: Development, Bilingualism, and Abnormal States 113
Verbal Fluency
Verbal fluency has long been evaluated by use of the FAS or other means of Controlled
Oral Word Association tasks (Baron, 2004; Johnstone, Holland, and Larimore, 2000).
FAS, as it is commonly known, requires individuals to provide the examiner with as
many words as they can that begin with those letters, and allows 60 seconds for each
letter. The individual is instructed not to use names of people, places, or numbers.
Similar to FAS, category verbal fluency is often used. Common category prompts
include animals, food, fruits, and names (Spreen and Strauss, 1998). Other evaluation
measures also include a verbal fluency subtest within their battery. Examples of these
include the NEPSY Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment Verbal Fluency
subtest (Korkman, Kirk, and Kemp, 1998) and the D-KEFS Verbal Fluency Test
(Delis, Kaplan, and Kramer, 2001). The CELF-IV Word Associations subtest
(CELF-IV; Semel, Wiig, and Secord, 2003) is a criterion-referenced verbal fluency
measure. Pontn and his colleagues (1996) provide normative data on a verbal fluency
task that have been stratified by age and educational levels for Hispanics ascertained
from the Los Angeles, California, metropolitan areas with their specific cultural and
ethnic factors as noted in Chapter 2.
Phonological Processing
However, relying only on these tools may leave out qualitative information about
an individuals language abilities. Simply allowing an individual to tell a story may
allow the practitioner exceptional insight into the individuals ability to maintain
conversation appropriately, express the information in a grammatically correct
fashion, and pronounce words in articulately.
Lezak (1995) noted several aspects of speech that are important to assess during
language evaluations. These aspects include prosody, fluency, and articulation,
which are not always amenable to formal language evaluation. Rhodes and col-
leagues (Rhodes, Kayser, and Hess, 2000) also noted that linguistic complexity is
another important aspect to evaluate, especially in individuals who are
multilingual.
Lezak (1995) and others (Damico, 1985, 1991; Johnston, 1982; Warner and
Nelson, 2000) detail informal techniques used to identify discourse abilities by
means of language sampling techniques. Johnston (1982) and Damico (1985, 1991)
modified this type of descriptive assessment of language abilities for use with
limited English-proficient students; however, the technique also can be used to gain
additional insights into the specific language abilities of an individual. The informal
language assessment is meant to be descriptive and not based on norm-based stand-
ards of evaluation. The informal language assessment consists of two parts: the oral
monologic assessment and the oral dialogic assessment.
The Oral Monologic Assessment is an assessment of language abilities based on
communication that is preplanned based on visual or verbal cues (p. 183, Damico,
1991). Oral Monologic assessment evaluates skills in three main areas: static tasks
(object description, giving directions), dynamic tasks (story reformulation, narrative
analysis), and abstract tasks (opinion-expressing) (Damico, 1991). On the contrary,
the Oral Dialogic Assessment is spontaneous and unplanned communication that is
typically embedded within conversation. The individual must be able to modify his
or her dialogue based on the thoughts and ideas expressed by another person. Based
on an analysis of the information conveyed by the speaker, information is gained
regarding fluency, clarity of expression, and comprehension. Damico (1991) details
four main categories in which language can be analyzed, including quantity, qual-
ity, relation, and manner.
As can be seen, clinicians may spend much time in formal and information evalua-
tion of individuals with language impairments. However, with the ever-changing
needs of the population of the United States, clinicians will likely encounter indi-
viduals with language impairments that are bilingual, multilingual, or with limited
English proficiency. As a result, it is important to understand the intricacies of
evaluation with this special group of individuals. Even two decades ago, research
was touting the importance of second-language proficiency in school psychologists
in order to effectively evaluate children who were linguistically different (Figueroa,
5 Language: Development, Bilingualism, and Abnormal States 117
Sandoval, and Merino, 1984; Kamphaus, 1993). At the same time, psychologists
are encouraged to use instruments appropriate to evaluate a childs language
abilities in their first and second (or third) language (Figueroa, Sandoval, and
Merino, 1984; Ochoa, Rivera, and Ford, 1997). In order to gain an accurate picture
of a multilingual individuals language level of impairment (if any), there must be
a valid assessment of skills in both languages, using information from a variety of
sources, including formal and informal evaluation, as well as observational data.
Centeno and Obler (2001) detail two main concerns when working with multi-
lingual individuals. They indicate that the clinicians initial responsibility is to
evaluate the level of language balance or dominance in order to determine the most
appropriate evaluation measures. Second, Centeno and Obler strongly assert that
neuropsychological and other deficits may be falsely exaggerated by impaired
language proficiency. This pattern of impaired language proficiency, if not fully
examined, may indeed mimic a language impairment. For instance, semilingualism,
when two languages are not equivalent to the skills of a monolingual speaker of
either language (Centeno and Obler, 2001), may be the result of a language impair-
ment or the mode of instruction (S.H. Ochoa, personal communication, 2000). If an
individuals semilingualism is falsely attributed to a language impairment, inappro-
priate goals and intervention strategies may be employed to remediate the
impairment, rather than supplement the lack of proficiency in either language.
Some of the more commonly used measures designed to assess language profi-
ciency in English and Spanish include the Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery
Revised English and Spanish Form (WLPB-R; Woodcock, 1991), the Woodcock
Language Proficiency Battery Revised Spanish Form (WLPB-R; Woodcock and
Muoz-Sandoval, 1995), the Woodcock-Muoz Language Survey English Form
(WMLS; Woodcock and Muoz-Sandoval, 1993a), and the Woodcock-Muoz
Language Survey Spanish Form (WMLS; Woodcock and Muoz-Sandoval, 1993b),
which can be used with individuals ages 2 through 90. The Woodcock Language
Proficiency Battery Revised English and Spanish Form (WLPB-R; Woodcock,
1991) and the Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery Revised Spanish Form
(WLPB-R; Woodcock and Muoz-Sandoval, 1995) provide the following scores:
Oral Language, Broad Reading, Basic Reading Skills, Reading Comprehension,
Broad Written Language, Basic Writing Skills, Written Expression, and Broad
Ability. In addition to these broad ability areas, the Woodcock Language Proficiency
Battery Revised Spanish Form (WLPB-R; Woodcock and Muoz-Sandoval,
1995), the Woodcock-Muoz Language Survey English Form (WMLS; Woodcock
and Muoz-Sandoval, 1993a), and the Woodcock-Muoz Language Survey Spanish
Form (WMLS; Woodcock and Muoz-Sandoval, 1993b) also provide a score
detailing the individuals level of Cognitive Academic Language Proficiency
(CALP; Cummins, 1984).
The Bilingual Verbal Abilities Test (BVAT; Muoz-Sandoval, Cummins,
Alvarado, and Ruef, 1998) is designed to assess the level of language proficiency in
English and another language. It is comprised of three tests from the Woodcock-
Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability Revised (WJ-R Cognitive; Woodcock
and Johnson, 1989), namely the Picture Vocabulary, Oral Vocabulary, and Verbal
118 C. French and A.M. Llorente
achievement for individuals aged two to adulthood. Namely, these measures are the
Batera Woodcock-Muoz Pruebas de habilidad cognitiva III (Tests of Cognitive
Ability; Woodcock, Muoz-Sandoval, McGrew, Mather, and Schrank, 2004a) and
the Batera Woodcock-Muoz Pruebas de aprovechamiento III (Tests of Achievement;
Woodcock, Muoz-Sandoval, McGrew, Mather, and Schrank, 2004b).
Summary
The senior author remembers eagerly reading the book The Art of Loving (Fromm,
1956) for the first time as a teenager. In its Foreword, Dr. Fromm notes in its first
sentence that reading of this book would be a disappointing experience for anyone
who expects easy instruction in the art of loving. Unfortunately, the same disap-
pointment is applicable to this chapter, in that the reader will quickly surmise the
complexity involved in the neuropsychological assessment of the Hispanic client.
This chapter, or this entire volume for that matter, is incapable of providing easy
instruction in such an art.
Such complexity is the result of the potential, synergistic impact of all the
factors so far discussed. For example, level of education, specific cultural back-
ground, and other demographic variables (e.g., age and geographical region within
the U.S.), in conjunction with language fluency and proficiency (English, Spanish,
both, neither for valid assessment purposes), coupled with the limited availability
of tests and norms from which valid and reliable inferences can be generated
interact to create the aforementioned complexity.1 Therefore, detailed examples
will be provided below in an attempt to show potential strategies or elucidate plau-
sible courses of action to address such issues. However, before providing applied
examples, there are a few theoretical issues regarding neuropsychological assessment
not covered thus far that should be addressed.
Traditional psychological evaluation approaches often focus on identifying the
diagnosis (e.g., depression, mental deficiency, learning disability) used to determine
1
The authors firmly believe that such complexity is also commonly encountered daily in unique
individuals in American society from other ethnic groups that would be qualified by the U.S.
Census Bureau as part of a majority group if due attention and weight are given to important
demographic characteristics. In this regard, and for ridiculously obvious reasons, the reader is
asked to consider the following patients: A White individual from an Amish background from
Lancaster, Pennsylvania, who received a fifth grade education and predominantly speaks
Pennsylvania Dutch; a White child from a Cajun background from Lafayette, Louisiana,
who only speaks standard English at school; and a highly educated White adult from an
Orthodox Jewish background from inner city New York who predominantly speaks Hebrew.
Clearly, even ethnic minority is a relative term, and more important, race should never be con-
founded with culture or ethnicity.
121
122 A.M. Llorente and D. Weber
comportment. To achieve such a lofty goal, the astute and ethical neuropsychologist
depends on a variety of collateral sources of information, including the patients
verbal report, past personal and family history, school, vocational and other records
(e.g., military), and cross-informant reports (e.g., caretaker, partner, teacher). In
addition, the neuropsychologist depends on information related to current and past
over-the-counter and prescribed medication use, as well as current and past use of
complementary therapy and controlled substances, cross-diagnostic data including
laboratory reports (e.g., blood work, metabolic block), results from past audiological,
medical, neuropsychological, ophthalmologic, and psychological evaluations and
data from structural and functional neuroimaging, in addition to an armamentarium
of psychometric instruments, clinical interviewing, and behavioral observations.
A comprehensive and integrated neuropsychological approach to the assessment of
the Hispanic client also is concerned with the functional and/or practical
conceptualization of the condition afflicting the individual, if any, at various levels
within different contexts. In the forensic arena, such evaluations also require the use
of assessment techniques to examine response bias and feigned symptoms (cf.
Rogers, 1997). It is also involved with the determination of disruptive mechanisms
and their rehabilitation (cf. Lezak et al., 2004; Rourke, Fisk, and Strong, 1986).
Given the various and numerous issues discussed thus far, in conjunction with
the factors presented in Chapters 15, it is clear that the major obstacle during the
course of assessment of Hispanic patients becomes the ability to obtain reliable
and valid results. Nevertheless, state of the art, comprehensive, and integrated
neuropsychological evaluations examine brain-behavior relationships by assess-
ing more specific domains than general intellect and psychological assessment in
the Hispanic patient. The evaluations are often supplemented by the assessment
of other domains in order to address the specific referral question. Assessment
typically includes multiple domains of functioning (e.g., intelligence, academic
achievement, attention, executive functions, information processing, perception
and perceptual organization, learning and memory, language, motor skills, and
personality factors to name a few), and in some instances, when valid and pru-
dent, includes assessment of premorbid functioning through the use of methods
or tests that permit such estimations. Although only provided as guideline, Table
6.1 shows some of these domains with tests that could potentially be used with
Hispanics.
damage, thus supporting the premise that behavior is controlled by the brain. Based
on the premise that there is an organic basis to behavior, performances on behavio-
ral measures are used to assess brain functioning. This approach is designed to draw
on a broad range of abilities and functions of Hispanic patients, regardless of the
referral question. Obvious limitations include cost-effectiveness, limited flexibility
in assessment, and emphasis on differential diagnosis instead of interventions.
A strength of the method is the large number of normative and standardized bases
of many of these tests, not to mention the ability to develop batteries of tests with
very high internal consistencies and so on.
Similarly, the Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery (Christensen, 1975;
Golden, 1986) has a strong focus on process and is based on the application of the
theories and procedures developed by A.R. Luria. Lurias theory of higher cortical
function viewed information processing as involving simultaneous and
successive mental processes. This model involved the transitional interaction of
various regions and zones in the brain, which thus produces complex behavior. This
approach matches corresponding neurological strengths with methods of acquiring
and presenting information that capitalize on an individuals strengths.
Another approach, the Boston Process Approach, also involves the integration
of qualitative and quantitative methods to analysis and interpretation of test results.
According to Kaplan (1996), evaluations to assess cognitive function are often
scored as right or wrong en route to a total score designed to identify global
achievement. However, this type of assessment approach does not assess strategies
that may be employed when an individual scores a right or wrong answer. This
achievement-oriented approach fails to take into consideration the multitude of
diverse processes (and systems) that an individual may use or engage to arrive at a
final solution. Employing a process-oriented approach enables analysis of a per-
sons unique problem-solving behavior and compensatory strategies. Final
assessment of clinical limits is possible with alternative formats of various available
traditional tests, and formal assessment of the clinical limits provides relevant
diagnostic information regarding strategies an individual employs to compensate
for cognitive difficulties or subtle cognitive dysfunction. Although the Boston
Process Approach allows great flexibility in addressing specific referral questions,
research addressing the validity of the approach is limited, and it is possible for the
clinician to overdiagnose and overinterpret pathology (cf. Reynolds and
Mayfield, 1999).
Another major approach is the integrative flexible battery approach (combination
of traditional, educational, psychological, and developmental tests). This approach
focuses on the individual and is designed to address specific referral questions. The
flexible battery approach lends itself to the needs of the individual in addition to
being sensitive to a wide range of patient variables, including gender, language,
familial history of handedness, handedness, age, educational background, family
structure, individual and family medical, psychiatric, and neurological history,
etiology of dysfunction, and premorbid functioning. This approach provides an
analysis of an individuals strengths and weaknesses instead of focusing on a
specific localization of impairment. It employs both an ideographic and nomothetic
128 A.M. Llorente and D. Weber
approach to assessment (cf. Fennell and Bauer, 1989). As with other approaches,
this method suffers from inherent weaknesses, including the fact that the test bat-
tery created has not been validated for the purpose used in most instances.
Despite the emphasis of neuropsychology on brain functioning, and the resulting
interpretation of assessment results within this context, other environmental factors
and their interactions and potential influences on the outcomes of neuropsychologi-
cal assessment should not be ignored. These variables include immigration patterns,
culture, socioeconomic status, educational attainment, acculturation, and a clients
primary language.
2
It should be clear from the previous footnote that from the authors vantage point, such patients
are few, particularly in the U.S.
6 The Neuropsychological Assessment of the Hispanic Client 129
some of the decisions regarding how best to evaluate an adult Hispanic client, a
55-year-old, right-handed, married, Hispanic female from Illinois with an under-
graduate degree in accounting from the University of Chicago (she recently
resigned from her position as a result of memory difficulties), who reports to be
bilingual (English- and Spanish-speaker), referred for neuropsychological assess-
ment to rule-out the onset of Alzheimers disease (Alzheimer-type dementia,
AD). In this case, as we refer to the initial discussion in this chapter, the patient
described is a Hispanic woman whose education took place in the U.S. in a quality
institution and is equal to or greater than 16 years (i.e., 16 years). Because the
panethnic term Hispanic is meaningless, upon further scrutiny of her cultural and
ethnic identity, it was determined that her specific cultural background was that of
a second-generation Latina who identified herself as Mexican-American from
a family background whose ancestors immigrated to Mexico from Spain at the turn
of the 19th century. In addition, both of her parents came from similar backgrounds
and spoke only Spanish and were without formal exposure to any aboriginal lan-
guage, although they were exposed informally in social situations while growing up
in Mexico. Her parents had immigrated to Illinois during the late 1920s, both with
advanced educational backgrounds (undergraduate and graduate degrees). The cli-
ent grew up in the Chicago metropolitan area (North Central U.S. region) and
resided there all her life. Concerns related to poor nutrition, probable exposure to
environmental toxins, or similar factors did not emerge, and she has experienced a
benign medical history thus far. When asked by intake staff, she reported she was
bilingual, but she predominantly uses English, except at home when visiting her
parents, where she constantly speaks Spanish. This information was supported
by the intake coordinator, as she sometimes spoke in Spanish during the intake
interview.
How should a comprehensive, integrated, and culturally competent neuropsy-
chological assessment be conducted with this client? How would it assess this
clients skills in an effort to rule out AD? At first glance, an examination of the
information collected and presented above would not appear important, particularly
because the client appears to be fully fluent in English. However, a closer examina-
tion of her demographic characteristics, history, and language fluencies, coupled
with the availability of specific tests available and, more important, normative data,
reveal a more complex picture.
Before getting into details addressing one of neuropsychologists favorite
subjects, namely test selection, let us examine other vital assessment components.
During the course of evaluation, the collection and review of collateral sources of
information, including the patients verbal report, past personal and family history,
and cross-informant reports (e.g., husband, former supervisor, sibling) are critical.
In this particular case, the information emphasizes declines in global or overall
functioning and recent or acute cognitive changes (e.g., memory difficulties
noted); changes or alterations in her psychosocial and recent (as well as immediate
past) vocational histories (with specific emphasis on her recent resignation as a
result of memory problems); significant changes in self-care and emotional func-
tioning, beyond what would be expected from normal aging and her personal and
130 A.M. Llorente and D. Weber
3
Although the senior author is fully fluent in Spanish, and is cognizant of the clients background having
visited, trained students, and collaborated with colleagues in Guatemala, the chief reason behind this
referral to his service, he subsequently referred this client to a colleague with greater competency in
assessing clients from such an indigenous background. In some instances, the senior author has assisted
individuals and families obtain assistance from their respective embassies so that they can receive financial
support to travel to be assessed by a clinician that is versed in the assessment of specific clients from
unique backgrounds or other ethnic minority groups (e.g., Middle Eastern).
6 The Neuropsychological Assessment of the Hispanic Client 133
along with other tests (e.g., SENAS), provide appropriate subtests to examine such
issues, whether the patient speaks English or Spanish. Finally, her level of adapta-
tion and behavioral and emotional functioning should also be examined, not simply
to be used for diagnostic purposes or to establish a rule-out, as requested, of AD if
possible, but additionally to determine her relative strengths and weaknesses in
order to establish appropriate interventions and treatments if required, with specific
emphasis on quality of life, safety, social, legal, and career implications, should the
assessment results fail to permit a rule-out of AD.
Although the present examples have been brief and limited by space considera-
tions, it is sincerely hoped that the reader begins to develop a sense of the awesome
responsibilities faced by the ethical and thoughtful clinician when evaluating
Hispanics. A sample report is included in the Appendix to this volume that under-
scores many of the issues addressed here and in previous chapters.
This chapter briefly covered theoretical and pragmatic factors associated with the
practice of neuropsychology with Hispanics. Factors that influence assessment in
this population include acculturation, immigration trends, language, educational
levels, socioeconomic status, and examiner knowledge and characteristics of such
populations. The pressure to decrease the disparities in special education placement
of children, the health status of Americans, judicial outcomes across ethnic groups,
in addition to regulations and mandates addressing fairness in psychological assess-
ment, and in particular in neuropsychology, and the economic incentives of test
publishers, are likely to shape the future of assessments with Hispanic populations
in the U.S. and abroad.
To accommodate the increasing diversity within the context of neuropsychologi-
cal practice and theory, neuropsychologists should develop theoretical models and
instruments that are capable of accounting for the amount of variance in total per-
formance associated with culture. From an applied standpoint, tests that are
developed for Hispanics that emphasize fluid reasoning skills and that are normed
for such populations, should be of paramount importance to cross-cultural neu-
ropsychology. In addition, a global approach should be taken during the course of
assessment of Hispanic populations that emphasize a framework that permits the
examination of unique strengths and weaknesses. Some of the aforementioned test
batteries, including the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability, the SENAS,
and others, accomplish some of these goals, but more research and productivity in
test development and standardizations are required. In addition further research that
examines the utility of these tools is necessary. In addition, cross-cultural neuropsy-
chology, applied to Hispanics, needs to consider within-group differences and the
impact of geographical regions and immigration status on assessment outcomes. In
fact, within-group differences can be even more pronounced than differences across
ethnic groups in some instances. Results from cross-cultural studies also emphasize
6 The Neuropsychological Assessment of the Hispanic Client 135
The cultural and ethnic landscapes of the United States (U.S.) are becoming
increasingly more diverse, as noted in Chapter 2. Although large-scale migrations
historically have exhibited geographical predilection, diversity is no longer mani-
fested only in selected border towns or large metropolitan areas, since small cities
and suburbs across the country are reflecting an increasingly diverse population,
particularly large-scale increases in the Hispanic population. The effects of
multiculturalism are acutely felt in the decision-making processes associated with
educational placement, diagnostic formulation, legal proceedings, and treatment
planning. Clearly, the important role that neuropsychological evaluations play in
the aforementioned decision-making processes will only continue to be effective if
the impact of bias in neuropsychological practice, and in particular, test bias, is
adequately addressed. For decades neuropsychology has grappled with the effects
of such potential biases.
Cognitive ability testing (CAT) has made an effort to address the effects of
multiculturalism and test bias. Helms (1992) wrote that while many CAT develop-
ers have attempted to reduce cultural influences on CATs through construction of
culture-fair tests, these devices represent attempts to control the influences of
different cultures rather than to measure them (p. 1091). In fact, the position
adopted here suggests that it has been such inadequate attempts to control for
culture and its various manifestations that have partially led to bias. In particular,
the attempts to define and operationalize culture, race, and ethnicity have proven
difficult even within the multicultural literature. As such, It seems reasonable to
ask whether it is possible to control something that one has not conceptualized
adequately (Helms, 1992, p. 1091).
These poor definitions and inappropriate attempts to control or eradicate such
factors, including the impact of language and ethnicity, led to the early development
of procedures believed to be culture-fair, yet later proven to be just as biased as
136
7 Hispanics and Cultural Bias: Test Development and Applications 137
existing measures (Sattler, 2001). For example, the notion that the development of
the System of Multicultural Pluralistic Assessment (SOMPA; Mercer and Lewis,
1978) was a culture-free procedure was later dispelled (cf. Sattler, 2001), partially
as a result of lack of established validity for minority populations and poor stand-
ardization based on a small sample of children from California. It is our opinion
that part of the problem at this early stage in the development of tests for use with
ethnic minorities, including Hispanics, was due to a poor understanding of brain-
behavior relationships, leading to the development of procedures that allegedly
reduced the effects of cultural factors, for example linguistic skills. This was based
on the assumption that tests assessing a specific set of skills or a specific skill (e.g.,
visual reasoning) would not involve or would be partially devoid of the concurrent
use of other skills (e.g., language). As noted in the Preface and Chapter 1, brain-
behavior relationships are not discrete or culture-free, and therefore the develop-
ment of a measure on the basis of such an erroneous assumption led to the
emergence of procedures that failed to accomplish their intended purpose.
While the impact of cultural variables on neuropsychological measures in relation
to standardized administration, psychometric properties, and other factors will be
addressed later, empirical evidence has not been presented thus far in this chapter to
support an argument buttressing the need to address such variables. However, extant
neuropsychological research has investigated a broad range of cultural factors that
unequivocally impact brain-behavior relationships. Although space limitations prevent
a comprehensive discussion of this research, relatively recent data-based investiga-
tions include the relationship between culture and lateralization (Ardila et al., 1989a;
Mandal, Ida, Harizuka, Upadhaya, 1999); hemispheric specialization and culture
(Best and Avery, 1999; Moss, Davidson, and Saron, 1985); and various investigations
showing that differences in cultural context probably affect performance (Len-
Carrin, 1989). Studies have additionally demonstrated that culture may be related to
self-reports of emotional and behavioral functioning (e.g., DuPaul et. al., 2001;
Carlson, Uppal, and Prosser, 2000). Although the research did not focus on Hispanics,
empirical investigations also have shown the impact of cultural variables on the accu-
racy of self-perception of neurological impairment post head injury (Prigatano,
Ogano, and Amakusa 1997).
Although the relationship of culture to neuropsychology, in particular
neuropsychological assessment, cannot be denied, even when empirical evidence is
taken into consideration, it is difficult to disentangle the specific mechanisms of
how culture, however defined, affects the assessment of people from cultural and
racial minorities. In effect, the absence of clearly articulated, theoretically based
models for examining the influence of race-related cultural factors on cognitive
ability is reflected in the ambiguous language used to discuss racial factors and
CATs (Helms, 1992, p. 1089). Part of the difficulty has been the use of cultural
bias and cultural equivalence without a clear contrast of these terms. As noted in
Chapter 1, any measurement on a test (Xtest score) is comprised of a true score (Xtrue),
representing the actual characteristic or trait under investigation, and error (Xerror).
The error noted in this equation, and assumed to enter the measurement process, is
138 P. Smith et al.
Another hypothesized source of cultural test bias stems from inequitable social
consequences. This argument points out that minority group members have unjustly
suffered from extensive past discrimination, labeling, and inequality of educational
opportunities. Therefore, poor test performance is a reflection of a lack of exposure
to the content being examined, leading to inappropriate inferences being made from
low scores. This process actually continues the cycle of inequitable social conse-
quences. These children are labeled as having cognitive impairments, which then
leads to special education services and decreased academic expectations. When
children graduate or are discharged from the educational system after being pro-
moted as a result of their chronological age, they are ill prepared to break the cycle
of inequitable social consequences by obtaining employment that has lower educa-
tional requirements and less opportunity for advancement. Again, the culmination
of these factors renders the minority test taker at a distinct disadvantage on these
tests (Reynolds, 2000). In summary, the labeling of minority children as
intellectually or academically deficient in early elementary school may result in an
academic trajectory that is lower than the trajectory of a child from the majority
culture. The children from the minority culture are set up, early on, by factors out
of their control, to have restricted or limited future academic experiences, likely
reducing their performance on future standardized measures.
It is also believed that when a test is extended from the majority culture and
given to a minority test taker, attributes such as intelligence, neuropsychological
functions, and personality that are allegedly being assessed may not be tapped
equivalently in the minority test taker (Reynolds, 2000; Sattler, 2001). Thus a meas-
ure of intelligence may measure discrepant constructs of intelligence or other
abilities among children of two different cultures. In essence, such constructs are
partly culturally defined and tests developed by and for the majority culture likely
do not capture the nuances, both subtle and otherwise, of intelligence or other cog-
nitive ability tests as it manifests itself within the minority culture.
Other clinicians and researchers argue that culturally biased measures do not
hold the same predictive validity between cultures. Since issues related to the valid-
ity of the underlying constructs purported to be measured have been raised, it
follows that the predictive value of the purported skills tapped may be lower when
applied to minority test takers. Thus, the utilization of various measures to predict
behavior does not extend across lines of culture, which raises the question as to
what purpose the administration and scoring of the measure with questionable pre-
dictive validity serves. This is especially pertinent because neuropsychology is
being increasingly used to establish treatment planning, to predict behavior, and to
provide consultation with significant economic and social implications. In fact,
many Hispanic researchers and educators question the validity of the inferences
derived from current standardized measures as they apply to Hispanic populations
(Reynolds, 2000).
An additional and significant source of cultural bias likely stems from the
underrepresentation of minorities in the measures standardization sample
(cf. Llorente et al., 1999, 2000; Reynolds, 2000). As noted in Chapter 2, when a
normative sample is stratified according to the U.S. census, a significantly smaller
7 Hispanics and Cultural Bias: Test Development and Applications 141
sample of people from a minority culture is obtained. In fact, many cells in such
stratifications, particularly for Hispanics and other ethnic minorities, remain
empty without individuals stratified into them. This limitation greatly affects
clinical judgment by the inappropriate comparison of a minority patients perform-
ance to an inadequate sample, simply because the proportion of minority test takers
matches the latest census proportions. Although such a comparison is inappropriate
from a psychometric standpoint, from a conceptual standpoint, as noted in Chapter
2, more egregious is the fact that scrutiny of the U.S. Census reveals that Hispanics,
regardless of their actual ethnic background, are classified under the same racial
category as Hispanics, erroneously confusing ethnicity with racial background.
Such a posture is problematic for neuropsychology because it is the cultural and
ethnic background of the individual that influence the factors that are being
assessed by the psychological procedure such as his or her cognitive abilities, not
his racial background. Unfortunately, this practice has both historical and practical
foundations. One need only look at the standardization samples of many early ver-
sions of the neurocognitive tests to see the low sample sizes and reliance upon
White samples.
Reynolds (2000) additionally notes that the majority of psychologists in the
United States are of European ancestry. Thus, lower test scores for minority ethnic
groups may be the result of differences in language content and use between the
neuropsychologist and patient. This discrepancy may also amplify the already pro-
found power differential between the two parties, thus increasing the chances that
decreased performance is not reflective of actual impairment.
Finally, some researchers have hypothesized that different cultures are just that,
different, and require separate measures of psychological and neuropsychological
constructs (Helms. 1992; Reynolds, 2000). Helms (1992) argued that the intelli-
gence quotient for African Americans is distinct from that of Caucasian Americans,
thus requiring the development and implementation of separate measures, and this
issue may be applicable to Hispanics as well. In fact, these differences may likely
extend beyond aptitude into such constructs as personality. This emphasizes the
need for the development of mechanisms to address test bias with different popula-
tions across multiple domains. Wong, Strickland, Fletcher-Janzen, Ardila, and
Reynolds (2000) offer a number of practical suggestions for mediating the effects
of cultural bias on ones clinical practice. The foundation of these suggestions
involves recognizing the need for multicultural competence and seeking out train-
ing regarding cross-cultural diversity and sensitivity. Additionally, clinicians are
advised to consider cultural nuances and the importance of providing a culturally
sensitive environment for interviewing and assessment. As always, the importance
of a sound and through clinical interview is underscored. Sufficient time and
research are essential to a competent clinical interview for a culturally dissimilar
patient. The fourth suggestion involves making a concerted effort to find a
competent neuropsychologist in the region who speaks the patients dominant lan-
guage. Recognize that other cultural factors, especially gender, aside from language
barriers, may require one to refer the patient to a more culturally appropriate
clinician. Every effort to avoid the utilization of interpreters is advised, as noted in
142 P. Smith et al.
Chapter 1. Translated tests should be avoided unless score interpretations have been
validated for that version of the test and for the individual under consideration.
Additionally, the test battery should be the result of a thoughtful process that keeps
culturally salient issues at the forefront. Finally, cross-cultural issues should be
clearly and effectively communicated in the final report (Wong et. al., 2000).
Although these recommendations provide good guidelines, before specific
recommendations are provided in this chapter addressing methods and tactics
to reduce bias in test development to be applied with Hispanic populations, it is
critical to examine in a detailed fashion a factor that has accounted for a great deal
of methodological problems in the development of assessment measures. Such
a factor is at the core of problems related to limited validity and reliability.
The U.S. Census has a long and distinguished history. Originally mandated by
Article One of the U.S. Constitution in 1787, the charter census was conducted in
1790 and terminated approximately two years later. The census has been conducted
every decade thereafter, with its original goal in mind, governmental applications.
The original Census only consisted of six questions (see Figure 7.1). However,
it is critical to examine those questions. Several of those queries have significant
historical importance as a result of their demographic content, predominantly
addressing race, not ethnicity or culture, which bears significantly on the develop-
ment of subsequent and modern censuses and, indirectly and most unfortunately,
the current standardization of neuropsychological tests. One question in the original
census inquired how many Free White males of 16 years of age or upward
lived in the household, men able to participate in the U.S. military or available for
work. The original census also queried the number of Free White males under the
age of 16 years that lived in the household (e.g., collected to estimate the future
military potential). Two other items inquired about the number of White females
and Slaves of other members in the household.
Unfortunately, although the U.S. census has since its inception served our nation
well, particularly when its intended use is taken into consideration (e.g., appropria-
tion of resources, taxation, military potential), the census was not devised for
4. Race
Fill one circle for the race that person considers
himself/herself to be.
If Indian (Amer.), print the name of the of enrolled
or principal tribe
White
Black or Negro
Indian (Amer.)
Eskimo
Aleut
Asian or Pasific Islander (API)
Chinese Japanese
Filipino Asian Indian
Hawiian Samoan
Korean Guamanian
Vietnamese Other API
Other race
7. Is this person of Spanish/Hispanic Origin?
No (not Spanish/Hispanic)
Yes, Mexican, Mexican, Am., Chicano
Yes, Puerto Rican
Yes, Other Spanish/Hispanic
(Print one group, for example:
Argentinian, Colombian, Nicaraguan,
Salvadorean, Spaniard, and so on)
What is most unfortunate is the fact that Eurocentric and other test publishers
have depended for decades on the U.S. Census to stratify samples of the U.S.
population, using census data as the standard for identifying individuals of all ethnic
backgrounds, and in particular, Hispanics living in the U.S. Clinicians unfortunately
have subsequently used those tests, with such poor samplings, to generalize to all
Hispanics living in the U.S., and in some instances living abroad, or to those that
have recently immigrated to the U.S. What might be wrong with such a process?
There are so many theoretical and methodological problems with such an
approach that it is difficult to find a good explanatory starting point. For example,
the basic idea behind sampling is that of studying the attributes of a larger group of
people (population) on the basis of studying a smaller group of individuals (sam-
ple). However, not all individuals in the population under investigation are alike,
and samples must then choose people from the population that best reflect all the
different characteristics and attributes of the larger group or population. Because
the first step in sampling is definition of the population, in this case Hispanics,
7 Hispanics and Cultural Bias: Test Development and Applications 145
White
Black, African-Am., or Negro
American Indian or Alaska Native Print name
of enrolled or principal tribe
(even though Hispanics have been ill-defined because race was interchanged with
ethnicity), the first major problem encountered is in the definition of the population
itself. Because the population was initially ill defined, the inferences derived from
the sample about the population will have poor validity, and therefore generaliza-
tions will be of equally poor value.
Clearly, the more a sample represents or reflects attributes of a larger population,
the more confidence can be placed on the inferences derived from such a sample
about the larger population, and this is a basic tenet of sampling procedures (Yamane,
1967). In order to attain such a level of confidence, several sampling procedures can
be used, including random, nonprobability, cluster, and stratified random sampling.
Standardization sampling procedures commonly use the last-named procedure to
sample the population of the U.S., as noted earlier, to create a standardization sam-
ple that is representative of the U.S. population. Unfortunately, although such a
procedure is appropriate for age and race, because of the confusion between race
and ethnicity whereby the latter has been inappropriately interchanged for the latter,
the representation of ethnicity is inaccurate, and a sample based on such a definition
146 P. Smith et al.
will not be representative of the U.S.s Hispanic population. Furthermore, let us prepos-
terously assume for a moment that a better definition of Hispanic than the one
used by the U.S. Census was available. It is possible that such a definition, as cap-
tured by the U.S. Census, may only be applicable to selected groups of Hispanics from
the U.S. but not from other parts of the U.S. or other parts of the world, and in that
sense the sample would still not be representative of the universe of Hispanics,
but only of those living in the U.S., who may have varying degrees of accul-
turation to American society, bilingualism, or other characteristics, as noted in
previous chapters.
Another critical issue is sample size. Most test publishers attempt to standardize
tests using stratified random samples that represent the U.S. population, using the
U.S. Census to mimic the population of the U.S., including Hispanics. The sample
size depends on many factors, including the level of confidence, desired precision
and, most important, as far as Hispanic culture and ethnic identity are concerned,
the variation in the population. Therefore,
Although the data provided by the U.S. Census are appropriate when they are used
for specific demographic variables such as percentage of individuals living in the
West with 2034 years of age with greater than 8 years of education, they are
not appropriate when used to develop psychological or neuropsychological tests
through the use of a stratified variable such as ethnicity. They are not appropriate
because the variation in the population of these two variables is quite distinct.
Therefore, although such a method is appropriate for U.S. government purposes, it
is not appropriate for neuropsychologists or test publishers to establish sample sizes
for Hispanics on the basis of data published by the U.S. Census.
Another major problem is the fact that although standardization samples might
be randomized stratification samples, they still represent the convenience sampling
method associated with data collection specifics such as data collection centers and
other variables that are not captured by the U.S. Census. For example, for many
childrens tests, standardization data are collected in school districts that are able
and willing to participate in such studies. Similar situations arise with adults, which
in the final analysis lead to a convenience sample. In fact, standardization testing
sites frequently are not available from specific metropolitan or rural areas, and in
some instances data are not available from every state within the U.S. or representa-
tive regions in the U.S. where specific populations with unique ethnicities are
encountered (e.g., Utah, Louisiana). The U.S. Census and, indirectly, standardiza-
tions based on such stratification data may thus be biased. Finally, how about the
racial classification of children and data reported to the U.S. Census by their par-
ents or other caretakers?
Other factors could be addressed related to this topic, namely the misuse of the
U.S. Census and its inability to serve as a tool to establish appropriate representa-
tive samples that appropriately represent the U.S. population, including complex
variables and interactions between other factors described above, such as levels of
7 Hispanics and Cultural Bias: Test Development and Applications 147
With regard to test development and utilization, several techniques may lead to
decrements in potential biases, including those identified above. These techniques
and practices vary in complexity, content, method, scope, and their effects on
potential sources of nonrandom error. Such techniques may include alterations in
test content, statistical methods including item analysis and oversampling of non-
dominant groups, and other methods presented below.
Guiding factors behind the development of tests for Hispanics should be under-
girded with respect to cultural differences not out of necessity, mandate, or eco-
nomic gain but out of genuine effort to develop instruments that are cross-cultural
and fair. Tests and procedures should enhance assessment in cross-cultural contexts
in general, as well as neuropsychological procedures and screening batteries
designed to assess the effects of conditions affecting neurological functioning,
including brain injury, demanding instruments that are as sensitive and as culturally
148 P. Smith et al.
relevant as possible. For example, the use of color or other alterations in test content
that may be more universally employed across cultures may reduce bias during the
course of test development and utilization. Although the cognitive neuropsychol-
ogy literature guards against the broad assumption that color perception is a com-
pletely culture-free phenomenon (Bornstein, 1973), hues may be used in some
circumstances as stimulus because they transcend cultural distinctions. The use of
color (e.g., yellow, red) in neuropsychological test procedures has substantially
increased during the last three decades. Although unpublished, a manuscript by
Llorente and colleagues (Llorente et al., unpublished manuscript) noted that the
Wechsler Scales for Children increasingly used color in its content throughout its
different subtests and restandardizations. For example, in 1974 (WISC-R; Wechsler,
1974), all of the plates in Picture Completion were in black and white (0% in color).
By 1991, or 17 years later (WISC-III, Wechsler, 1991), 23/30, or 76% of all the
plates in Picture Completion, were in rich colors. By 2004, or 30 years later (WISC-
IV; Wechsler, 2004), 37/38 plates, or 97% of all the plates in Picture Completion
were produced in color. Similar, increases in color are evident in other subtests.
However, this is not the case for other tests. For example, tests frequently used in
neuropsychological assessment continue to use black and white line work, which
poorly depicts stimuli. This issue is important to note, because it is not only appli-
cable to Hispanics, but to all examinees: Such poor depiction of stimuli leads to
inaccurate test results and poor inferential processes, partially because the test does
not represent accurately the stimuli as it appears in the environment (real world)
or the examinees phenomenological reality. As a good exemplar, the plate for
Asparagus, in one of the most commonly used tests of confrontational naming in
neuropsychology is submitted for the readers consideration. No wonder large
numbers of clients, adults or children, fail such item.
In Chapter 1, we addressed the importance of increased reliability during the
course of assessment with Hispanics. Albeit illegal and unethical, neuropsycho-
logical tests and procedures are sometimes photocopied (Mitrushina et al., 1999).
Such a practice has often resulted in distorted target stimuli, introducing
uncontrolled error variance, particularly when used with Hispanic populations
groups of individuals for which studies addressing validity and reliability may not
be available. Such problems also significantly impact the comparability of
research and clinical findings from studies employing altered procedures.
Therefore the creation of tests that use color not reproducible by readily available
commercial photocopying equipment eliminates these alterations by using profes-
sionally printed color protocols with tints and tones that ensure stable test per-
formance and therefore comparability with normative studies and previous
research investigations.
The development of multiple forms of the same test when possible provides for
repeated administrations of a test, permitting repeated administration of an equiva-
lent test to the same Hispanic patient, thus permitting the comparison of his or her
performance over time with his or her own baseline (cf. Llorente et al., 1999),
without dependence on normative samples. Such a method is especially helpful in
neuropsychological rehabilitation in which the patients performance is tracked
7 Hispanics and Cultural Bias: Test Development and Applications 149
longitudinally subsequent to insult over short periods of time in the acute setting.
Such alternate forms may be developed by using the standard test form as the basis
for the development of other forms through mirror imaging or rotation by 180
degrees of the original form, or similar methods, whenever possible (see Llorente
et al., 2003). Such methods of developing alternate retest forms of the test assure
that stimulus placement and distance traveled between stimuli will remain gener-
ally equivalent for all forms of the protocol.
Although space limitations in this volume preclude an expansive description,
item analysis is another procedure that may be used to control for cultural bias. Item
analysis may constitute either qualitative or quantitative means of addressing cul-
tural test bias. It is an analytical procedure used to evaluate the appropriateness of
individual test items to identified traits (cf. Camilli and Shepard, 1994). When
applied in a general sense, item analysis can be conceptualized as ensuring that
individual items do not differentially discriminate one persons performance or that
of a group from other groups. How individuals or groups are organized is of less
interest than whether an individual or group performs differently on that individual
item. This allows for the development of test items that are appropriate for groups.
This procedure can be carried out in a variety of settings from market research
surveys, classroom evaluation of the fairness of test questions on a final, to develop-
ment of unbiased neuropsychological tests for Hispanics. Qualitative item analysis
is concerned with content and face validity and item construction. In addition,
qualitative item analysis may focus on test content and language use. Experts from
different cultures can be utilized to help reduce and eliminate offensive or overtly
monoculturally relevant items from tests. A quantitative approach to item analysis
is concerned with the statistical properties of the items and the overall test com-
prised by such items. Individual item characteristics such as item difficulty, item
discrimination, guessing, and differential functioning are typically considered
within the realm of this approach. As such, Item Response Theory attempts to inte-
grate the first three aspects (difficulty, discrimination, and guessing) into a mathe-
matical formula that can guide test construction (Camilli and Shepard, 1994; Sattler
2001). Item difficulty is framed in terms of the percentage of people who answered
a specific item correctly. The range for item difficulty varies between 0.0 (everyone
obtains an incorrect answer) to 1.0 (everyone obtains a correct answer). Many tests
and procedures currently in use (e.g., Wechsler scales, Stanford-Binet, Differential
Abilities Scale, and Woodcock-Johnson) arrange test items in terms of increasing
difficulty. As it applies to cross-cultural neuropsychology, item analysis can be
used to ensure that any item on a particular test exhibits the same degree of diffi-
culty for all test takers, including Hispanics. A secondary gain associated with this
approach to item development and item placement within a test is that it allows for
the test taker to have a sense of efficacy as he or she approaches ever increasingly
difficult items.
Item discrimination can be understood as how an item discriminates between
examinees who do well on the test as a whole and those who do poorly (Sattler,
2001, p. 113). Scores range from 1.0 to +1.0. Similar to correlations coefficients,
scores at the ends of the spectrum reflect strong relationships while scores in the
150 P. Smith et al.
Another issue that requires discourse in this volume is the concept of base rates.
Base rates are understood as the frequency of a trait (such as abnormal test finding
or presence of symptom) in a population. Since many signs, symptoms, and test
score discrepancies are not pathonogmonic to a particular mental health disorder,
understanding how often these aforementioned entities are present in a given popu-
lation aids in determining whether or not there is a pathological state or normal
variation within a population, including Hispanics (Lezak et al., 2004; McCaffrey
et al., 2003). This is especially important since a large portion of the nosology and
nomenclature of clinical syndromes and disorders is actually delineated by a
description of the associated cluster of symptoms. Therefore, the examiner should
have an understanding of the base rates of the neurobehavioral symptoms associ-
ated with the patients they are evaluating (Lezak et al., 2004, p. 129).
Understanding how a collection of symptoms is distributed within a certain
population therefore aids in the attempt to make a valid diagnosis. This process can
have direct relevance in obtaining and conducting culturally sensitive assessments.
While many tests use base rates to examine the frequency of discrepancy scores
based on nondemographic variables, such as discrepancies in IQ scores or index
scores, this process could also be applied to rates of score distributions of nonma-
jority populations. If conducted correctly, this would allow clinicians to examine a
patients profile and compare his or her performance to a group defined by their
shared cultural, ethnic, or socioeconomic status to determine clinically relevant dif-
ferences from age-determined norms.
In order to understand how the use of base rates may reduce bias during the course
of neuropsychological assessment, a brief explanation of how base rates of particular
traits or discrepancies affect neuropsychological assessment and clinical decision
making is required. The usefulness of a particular trait in clinical decision making is
directly related to how frequently that entity occurs in the clinical population being
studied. Stated differently, Because the diagnostic utility of a test is relative to the
base rate of the diagnosis in the population of interest, the extensive use of tests in
neuropsychological assessment makes knowledge of the use of base rates in this con-
text highly relevant (McCaffrey, Palav, Bryant, and Labarge, 2003, p. 3).
For these reasons, an examination of the psychometric properties of a tests
sensitivity, specificity, positive and negative predictive value as they are applied to
different cultural groups instead of clinical groups is required. A brief introduction
to the terminology associated with base rates is presented to facilitate this under-
standing. One aspect of test construction that will be examined first is related to a
tests ability to accurately diagnose or differentiate between groups of people with
or without a specified disorder based on the presence or absence of a sign. There
are six terms related to this ability. The initial four terms include True Positive (TP),
False Positive (FP), True Negative (TN), and False Negative (FN) (see Table 7.1).
The table attempts to indicate the appropriate location of each term when the
presence of a condition is known with a group of people and their result on the test
is used to identify that condition.
As can be observed from Table 7.1, a True Positive refers to the ability of an
instrument or test to accurately identify or detect an entity when the entity or
7 Hispanics and Cultural Bias: Test Development and Applications 153
Table 7.1 Contingency Table Used to Describe Base Rate, Sensitivity, and Specificity
Condition
Present Absent
Test (Diagnostic) Positive Result True Positive False Positive
Negative Result False Negative True Negative
Sensitivity (SSy) and specificity (SPy) are inversely related or SSy = 1/SPy
disorder within a cultural context or presence (and or) absence of other conditions
or disorders or signs. It is especially useful when many people identified as
Hispanic that have different levels of English proficiency as it has direct implica-
tions for CATs (cf. Harris and Llorente, 2005). Such is the case because clinicians
who perform neuropsychological evaluations need to understand how environmental
considerations may adversely affect patients performance on demanding, context-
reduced CATs (Harris and Llorente, 2005, p. 394). Their performance on these
types of tests has a direct impact on the types of services and/or what types of
educational programs they may receive. In effect, the validity of the measures
needs to be questioned when the interpretation of the tests is looking for impair-
ments, when they may be more effective at determining the absence of a disorder
through the absence of statistically significant and clinically relevant discrepancies
in their performance.
The previous information allows test developers to examine how well an instru-
ment can differentiate between the groups of people with or without a particular
disorder. However, it does not address how much value the sign has for prediction
when the clinical diagnosis is unknown. The predictive value of a sign is reliant
upon the integration of a tests sensitivity, specificity, and the base rate of the trait
in the specified population. Positive predictive value (PPV) is the likelihood that
an individual from a specific population has a disorder given a positive sign.
Conversely, negative predictive value (NPV) is the likelihood that an individual
does not have a disorder given a negative sign. (McCaffrey et al., 2003, p. 4) (see
Table 7.3).
While many consider PPV and NPV to be synonymous with sensitivity and spe-
cificity, there are distinct differences that require explanation. The primary differ-
ence between these terms is the use of actuarial data or base rates to determine the
probability of the presence or absence of the disorder from a specified sample. PPV
and NPV have been characterized as being inversely impacted by changes in the
base rates. As the base rate increases, so does the PPV, while NPV decreases, with
all other factors being held equal. (McCaffrey et al., 2003)
The effectiveness of using signs to aid in diagnostic accuracy relies upon under-
standing the prevalence of disorders within certain populations, including Hispanics.
For conditions that are common, and for a test to be useful with Hispanic popula-
tions, the use of a sign needs to provide a success rate that must exceed the base
rate of that disorder. This becomes especially difficult for signs as the base rates
decline, which reflects disorders that can be characterized as rare (McCaffre et al.,
2003). An example of this can be observed with Table 7.4.
7 Hispanics and Cultural Bias: Test Development and Applications 155
Table 7.3 Contingency Table Used to Explain Base Rate, Negative Predictive Value
and Positive Predictive Value
Condition
Condition with 10% base rate (TP+FN)/N Present Absent
Test (Diagnostic) Positive Result True Positive False Positive PPV
Negative Result False Negative True Negative NPV
Sensitivity Specificity
Table 7.4 illustrates the necessity to incorporate base rate information into common
practice. Even with a test that has adequate sensitivity and specificity, the PPV has
dropped substantially when the condition being detected occurs in only 10% of the
population. In such a circumstance, the positive result on the test does not
adequately determine the presence of the disorder. However, the negative result on
the test ends up having greater significance for determining the absence of the
identified condition.
Through the use of base rate information, a discrepancy between the perform-
ance of a patient from a minority culture and a normed sample, although statisti-
cally significant, may not be clinically relevant, as the discrepancy may occur with
great frequency, thereby diminishing the impact of the statistical significant differ-
ence and, indirectly, the value of the test. Neuropsychologists can demonstrate cul-
turally sensitive case conceptualization and test interpretation when they can
explain, through the use of base rates, that a minority patients performance does
deviate from the norm (and classified as abnormal), but occurs so frequently in a
given population (minority status) that the result is not clinically relevant.
When it comes to determining the appropriate frequency for determining clinical
relevance there is not one accepted standard. According to Sattler (2001), whether
an occurrence is unusual or rare depends on how one defines unusual. What is
the appropriate delineation between unusual or rare? A difference that occurs in
15% or 20% of the population may be considered unusual by some, whereas others
may consider a difference unusual only if it occurs in 5% or 10% of the popula-
tion. (p. 447). Part of what this dilemma addresses is the confidence that clinicians
can have in their interpretation of the data that they are presented with, their knowl-
edge of cultural factors for a specific patient, and so on. The more infrequent the
156 P. Smith et al.
occurrence, the greater confidence a clinician can have in their interpretation that
an observed difference is meaningful. Sattler (2001) suggests that in order to be
considered unusual or rare, the difference should occur in 15% or less (in one direc-
tion) of the standardization sample (p. 447). As such, these discrepancies can have
a direct impact upon the diagnostic formulations when they are not understood with
the use of base rates. For example, if an intelligence test is utilized as part of an
educational or neuropsychological assessment with a client from a minority culture,
there may be an increased rate of reported borderline intellectual functioning when
this test is utilized as a sole marker of cognitive functioning. This increased rate of
a diagnostic label within a minority population would be an artifact of blind
interpretation of test scores rather than a demonstration of how a different cultural
background may affect test performance, which would be minimized if the examin-
ers had an understanding of how to use base rates to influence interpretation, case
conceptualization, and diagnostic accuracy. Because many assessment measures
are normed using figures based on the U.S. Census to create the normative sample,
there is an underlying assumption that the distributions of scores within the two
groups are equivalent. In fact, different patterns of performance across ethnic groups
or between the sexes may be due to test artifacts and, therefore, may not represent
true differences in performance between these groups (cf. Llorente, 1999; Reynolds,
2000). This assumption may lead to erroneous inferences based on an individual
patients performance when he or she is from a nondominant group. By using base
rates, the clinical relevance of differing patterns of performance on neuropsycho-
logical measures can be addressed in a culturally sensitive manner.
However, what if multiple groups do differ on a particular test and therefore
have different distributions of scores? The creation of independent norms per cul-
ture may be extravagant, but the use of the base rate of discrepancies between
majority and nonmajority cultures may be an appropriate alternative to allow for
the appropriate interpretation of a patients performance. This type of data analysis
could be cultivated from the norms that have already been developed for a large
number of tests. In this manner, updated norm samples would not have to be ascer-
tained. Instead, an analysis of the distribution of scores obtained via different cul-
tures could be analyzed and discrepancy tables developed as the basis of majority
versus nonmajority cultures.
There are additional methods that can be utilized to address the aforementioned
concerns related to neuropsychological assessment and cultural sensitivity, includ-
ing the use of epidemiological research, modified data collection and analysis, and
enhanced future test development. Epidemiological research is primarily concerned
with the description and study of proximal and distal factors that affect public
health. Utilizing primarily descriptive and analytical approaches to the study of
factors affecting public health, data can be collected in ways that are broader in
perspective than many neuropsychological studies allowing for broader generaliza-
tions of societal trends within particular populations. Incorporating this type of
research into neuropsychological practice would provide neurophysiologists with
information about a multitude of risk factors that may be related to health concerns,
such as lower cognitive scores. It may also reveal relationships between different
7 Hispanics and Cultural Bias: Test Development and Applications 157
factors, such as those mentioned by Reynolds (2000), that have not been implicated
in previous research studies across society. Examples of research can include exam-
ining the base rates of different neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders based
on identified culture to see if there are different base rates of certain disorders.
As noted above, the use of Eurocentric constructs in test development may be
seen in many procedures in the types of questions asked or tasks required on tests
of cognitive abilities, including neuropsychological tests that rely on right or wrong
answers reflecting an underlying emphasis on a dualistic approach to problem solv-
ing. However, this approach has diminished with recent revisions and updates of the
many existing tests (e.g., Wechsler Intelligence Tests), whereby a process approach
has been incorporated into the test allowing for reduction in the reliance upon a
dualistic evaluation of responses, as well as qualitative components of such
responses. The increased range and acceptance of alternative responses also reflects
the recognition that there may be different cognitive strategies that are utilized dur-
ing problem solving, partially the result of different cultural backgrounds. This has
direct implications for people from minority groups who may utilize alternative
strategies that differ in process not content, from the majority culture or vice versa.
For example, when a Hispanic male is required to respond in an appropriate form
to a social dilemma, his cultural background may dictate that he incorporate cultur-
ally appropriate responses that focus on family before personal gain. While the
content of his answer may have been regarded as incorrect according to majority
culture standards in prior evaluative settings, the increased focus on the presence of
different strategies allows for more valid assessment of his underlying ability and
cultural factors impacting his response.
In addition, future data collection and research should begin to address the con-
cerns raised in the past related to cultural insensitivity during the course of neu-
ropsychological assessment and test development. Data collection strategies that
include a qualitative approach to describe cohorts of subjects and their related or
shared experiences could begin to address such questions by providing additional
factors that can later be operationalized in studies examining different perform-
ances across neuropsychological tests. This would include examining how certain
diagnostic signs or symptoms may be affected by culture, leading to the develop-
ment of classification systems based on neuropsychological and/or neuropsychiat-
ric functions that are influenced by cultural factors.
The present state of affairs in the discipline also argues in favor of comparing an
ethnic individuals performance with various normative data sets when possible
and available (see Mitrushina et al., 1999), and with the standardization sample
most similar in terms of demographic characteristics of the individual undergoing
evaluation. Although this issue may seem elementary, it is not unusual in clinical
practice to discover that an inappropriate set of norms has been used simply
because that individual belongs to the same minority group (e.g., Hispanics). Of
course, one major problem is the lack of extant samples, a situation that supports
an assessment posture favoring the use of longitudinal examinations for these popu-
lations. As noted by Llorente et al. (2000), the present findings also have impli-
cations for private and governmental bodies responsible for the development of
158 P. Smith et al.
The WISC-III (Wechsler, 1991) was for years frequently employed to assess and
evaluate intellectual functions in children, including Hispanics in the U.S. It was
normed employing youths ages 6 to16 years. The normative sample was comprised
of 200 children (100 boys, 100 girls) at each of the aforementioned age levels for a
total sample of 2,200 youths. The normative sample for the WISC-III (Wechsler,
1991) was stratified using updated data from the 1980 U.S. Census according to the
following characteristics: age (6 years, 0 months to 16 years, 11 months), gender
(male, female); Race, not ethnicity (White, Black, Hispanic, and Other); parental
education (average number of years of school completed by the parents or parents
living with the child, or the highest level of education of a single parent, using 8,
911, 12, 1315, and 16 years of education as stratification cutoffs); and geo-
graphical region (North East, North Central, South, West) as well as Community
Size (Metropolitan Statistical Area or MSA, greater than 100,000 inhabitants, etc.).
Tables 2.2 to 2.6 in the tests manual show these data. Table 2.2 in the manual dis-
plays demographic characteristics by Age, Race/Ethnicity and mean years of
7 Hispanics and Cultural Bias: Test Development and Applications 159
Parental Education. Close scrutiny of this table reveals that 200 children at age 6
years comprised the overall 60 to 611 sample, nominally characterized as 6-year-
olds. When the Hispanic group is further examined at this age level, 3.5, 3.0, 3.0,
and 1.5% of the 200 children or 7 ([3.5 200]/100), 6, 6, and 3 youths (a total of
22 children), respectively, comprise the subsample. Immediately, it becomes criti-
cal to note that no Hispanic child with parents with average education equal or
greater than 16 years is found at this age level (see Table 2.2.). Further scrutiny of
this group, using Table 2.4 (percentages by Age, Gender and Race) indicates that
of the 22 6-year-olds, 5.5% are boys and 5.5% are girls of Hispanic origin for a total
of 11 ([5.5 200]/100) boys and 11 girls. Clearly, of these children, none of them
come from parents with 16 or more years of education. Further perusal of Table 2.5
reveals that 0.5% of 6-year-old Hispanic children come from the North Central
region of the U.S. (including Chicago) comprising a total of one child ([200
0.5]/100)! Is this child 1 of the 11 boys or 1 of the 11 girls? Clearly, it is not a child
whose parents averaged 16 or more years of education. Is the child then from the
group of parents who averaged 8 or less, 9 to 11, 12, or 13 to 15 years of education?
With regard to the application of these norms, are there no 6-year-old Hispanic
youths requiring placement in schools or necessitating neuropsychological assess-
ment who come from backgrounds where both parents have a Bachelors degree
residing in Chicago? Most unlikely! What if the child had been a 14-year-old
Hispanic from the Northeast, or a 9-year-old with highly educated parents (>16
years of education)? Again, no Hispanic child is represented in such groups.
Although it may appear that such weaknesses in the test standardization sample
have little relevance, such an assumption would be erroneous. In fact, critical deci-
sions are constantly made about Hispanic patients, including adults, children, and
the elderly in the U.S. and abroad, related to school placement and judicial and
vocational issues on the bases of these norms, in some instances life-and-death
decisions in the case of individuals undergoing criminal proceedings and facing the
possibility of capital punishment!
Unfortunately, it is only when tests are examined with this detailed degree of
scrutiny that a proper examination of the issues so far discussed can be best eluci-
dated, underscoring the importance of all these factors during the course of neu-
ropsychological assessments with Hispanics. It is also critical to note that these
issues are not unique to the Wechsler Scales and that they impact a large number of
tests and procedures. It is not surprising, then, yet interesting, because it displays in
a clear fashion his acumen, that Professor Wechsler himself argued against the use
of normative data for his scales (see Kaufman, 1994; Preface). Again, the issues
discussed above should not be perceived as an attack on the test. As test authors
ourselves, we understand the painstaking intricacies of developing neuropsycho-
logical procedures and acquiring good normative data for tests in the U.S. and
abroad. Rather, the example and criticism are provided to underscore the impor-
tance and potential consequences of the misuse of such data and tests by poorly
trained, biased, or ignorant individuals. It is also provided for readers so they can
understand the limitations of such tests and standardization samples as a result of
problems in their acquisition, with significant negative repercussions if used
inappropriately when using a nomothetic approach with Hispanics.
160 P. Smith et al.
In the final section of this chapter, an attempt will be made to describe examples of
modern tests that attempted to address the potential cultural biases discussed.
Examples of cognitive, behavioral-emotional, and neuropsychological procedures
will be presented. Although several issues referenced above related to cultural bias
have not been addressed in these measures, they represent some of the best exam-
ples available to date of instrumentation that has been developed using a concerted
effort to address cultural bias in test development.
Although during the development of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Children - Fourth Edition (WISC-IV, Wechsler, 2004) an effort was made to
address cultural issues by closely following the U.S. Census, including a large
stratification of Hispanics with oversampling in some instances, the use of the U.S.
Census to stratify a tests standardization sample left much to be desired for obvi-
ous reasons, particularly as noted for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children -
Third Edition (WISC-III; Wechsler, 1991). To complicate matters further, the WISC
IV was developed in English, and its application, therefore, was limited to individu-
als who were fluent in English and whose families identified themselves as
Hispanic. In addition, the use of the test was limited for a large number of Hispanic
children who possess different levels of English proficiency. Therefore, the pub-
lisher of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales developed the WISC-IV in Spanish
(Wechsler, 2004). In addition to the stratification described above, additional sam-
ples of Hispanic children were collected whose families had originated from Cuba,
Central America, the Dominican Republic, Puerto Rico, and South America. In
addition, items were designed to minimize the impact of cultural bias as a result of
cultural differences between countries, allowing for a statistical examination of
item bias using IRT methods of analysis (Wechsler et al., 2004, p. 21).
Supplemental demographic data provided in the test manual allows for a compari-
son with subgroups of the Hispanic population comprising the standardization
sample, enhancing the inferential process. It is clear that such a process of test
development is far superior to old translations and simple adaptations of tests, a
significant leap forward in the development of tests that attempt to address cultural
factors, and worthy of commendation, an ethical approach to test development for
Hispanics.
The Behavior Assessment System for Children - Second Edition (BASC-II;
Reynolds and Kamphaus, 2004) provides another good example. For this test, its
developers and publisher created a Spanish version of the BASC-II concurrent with
the English version. This process allows for the scales to be used with Spanish-
speaking individuals residing in the United States (Reynolds and Kamphaus,
2004, p. 247), and the Spanish version includes all of the items in the same order
as the English version.
To accomplish such a goal, a professional group was used to translate test items
from the original BASC. Bilingual psychologists subsequently were used to express
7 Hispanics and Cultural Bias: Test Development and Applications 161
each item in a manner that would be comprehensible across dialects and would be
culturally appropriate for use among with multiple U.S. Spanish-speaking
populations (Reynolds and Kamphaus, 2004, p. 247). The items these individuals
reviewed were then distributed to a separate group of psychologists throughout the
United States with instructions to compare the English and Spanish versions and to
make suggestions to clarify or improve item wording while maintaining the psy-
chological content and appropriateness of the translation (Reynolds and
Kamphaus, 2004, p. 247). The suggestions were submitted to the translation service
through repeated review rounds until final decisions on the items were reached.
Once the Spanish-language forms were created, they were subjected to a
number of statistical comparisons to ensure that both sets of forms could ade-
quately measure their intended constructs prior to completion of the final
Covariance Structure Analysis (CSA), which is also known as a Confirmatory
Factor Analysis, and reliability studies (Reynolds and Kamphaus, 2004, p. 247).
During the standardization process, parents and children were allowed to complete
the appropriate forms in either Spanish or English. Approximately 400 parents and
150 children completed the Spanish versions, allowing for a comparison of the
effects of culture and language. The protocols were divided into three groups
depending on the identified ethnicity of the child and the language of the form that
was used. The groups were non-Hispanic and English form, Hispanic and English
form, and Hispanic and Spanish form.
Separate CSAs were conducted for each group resulting in a standardized factor
loading for each item on its scale. The magnitude and rank order were compared
across groups. Those with an acceptable loading (>.30) in the three groups were
retained. If the results were not similar across groups the item was excluded from
further analysis. The Spanish language forms were used within the norm sample,
as it was more representative of the general population (Reynolds and Kamphaus,
2004, p. 248). However, these forms were not included in the internal-consistency
and scale correlational studies of the norm sample.
An assumption of similar psychometric properties between the Spanish and
English forms was not assumed just because the Spanish forms consist[ed] of
translated versions of items from the English form (Reynolds and Kamphaus,
2004, p. 248). With regard to the Parent Report Scales (PRS) the median reliabili-
ties are lower than those obtained in the English-form samples, and they were
considered adequate. (Reynolds and Kamphaus, 2004, p. 247) A similar pattern
was obtained with regard to the Self Report Scales in that the median reliability
values are slightly lower than those obtained on the English (SRP) forms
(Reynolds and Kamphaus, 2004, p. 249). This is an admirable example of the
development of tests in Spanish that are not separate from the English version, but
include and have an appreciation of how culture affects a tests development and its
psychometric properties.
Similar advances were used in the development of the Spanish and English
Neuropsychological Assessment (SENAS; Mungas, Reed, Haan, and Gonzalez,
2005). They developed an instrument to identify cognitive abilities relevant for the
162 P. Smith et al.
It should be patently clear that the historical record provided in most rehabilitation
textbooks is Eurocentric in its perspective. For example, although Eurocentric history
tends to note the emergence of trephination in Egypt, it fails to note that in the
New World, the advanced Chinchoros culture of Northern Chile, precursors to
the complex, eminent, and sophisticated Inca State, perfected artificial mummifica-
tion as a means to protect the body and soul in the afterlife. However, unbeknownst
to large number of students in the field of neuropsychology, psychology, and
rehabilitation, is the fact that the Chinchoros perfected this process in the third
millennium BC, most likely before or temporally concurrent with the Egyptians
(Mosely, 1993). This example is provided because it underscores the importance of
attending to our cultural biases, even when addressing a disciplines history, an
issue that should be taken into consideration when rehabilitating individuals from
ethnic minority backgrounds, including Hispanics.
It is also important to examine the precursory roots and historical foundations of
neuropsychological rehabilitation in detail to foster greater appreciation of the current
state of the field and its future, providing a better context for cross-cultural perspec-
tives and their specific applications to Hispanics. Such a rich history actually spans
across cultures, particularly views and positions addressing rehabilitation.
As note by Len-Carrin (1997), some of the oldest discoveries of treatment and
interventions for brain damage can be dated as early as the Mesolithic Age. Skulls
were discovered with holes on their left side that appear to be a form of surgical
intervention, trephination. Physicians practicing during the Hippocratic period
noted brain-behavior connections such as the brains intellectual capacities, regu-
lating a majority of body functions, allowing it to make judgments. In keeping with
tradition, Hippocratic physicians used trephinations for the treatment of selected
mild brain injuries.
According to Len-Carren (1997), quoted in Ries, Potter, and Llorente (2007),
The first hospitals and convalescence homes for the treatment of physical lesions
were founded in 499 to 429 BC Greece, which indicates an important shift in the
treatment of illness in Western societies. Previously, children born with deformities
were displayed in public places for a period of time or were thrown off of Mount
Taigeto. In Imperial Rome, it was acceptable to take the life of children who were
164
8 Rehabilitation 165
born with any type of physical lesion, abandon them, or release them into the Tiber
River in flower baskets.
In the Second Century B.C., Galen was among the first to require a detailed
clinical examination of all patients, noting all symptoms in order to provide
information for diagnosis and treatment. Through anatomical dissection of monkeys,
he provided detailed findings regarding the anatomy of the nervous system and of
brain trauma (Ries et al., 2007).
The 18th century marked a period of intellectual and scientific advances that led
to new theories and hypotheses about functional anatomy, specifically neuroanat-
omy. The French Revolution (1789) prompted the emergence of equal treatment for
the ill, whereas advances in science and medicine prompted the use of electricity
for rehabilitation of hemiplegias (Len-Carrin, 1997), as well as a greater under-
standing of the nervous system. The 19th century was marked by a scientific
continuation of the discovery of localization of functions as well as systems within
the brain. According to many neuroscientists, most influential were the discoveries
by Paul Broca and Carl Wernicke regarding the language system subsequent to
neuropathological investigations and, as noted in Chapter 1, the proposition by the
Spanish histologist Ramn y Cajal (1889) describing the neuron system and the
discovery of chemical neuronal transmission (cf. Len-Carrin, 1997). Advances
also were possible as a result of the contributions made by the English neurologist
Hughlings Jackson, which addressed the hierarchical nature of the central nervous
system, an often forgotten contribution in the neurosciences.
In the 20th century, the Russian scholar Vygotsky provided a framework noting
the importance of cultural factors in human development (cf. LeFrancois, 1995),
which had significant applications, particularly from a theoretical perspective for
neuropsychology. One of Vygotskys colleagues, Alexander Luria, would use this
framework to make significant theoretical and applied contributions to the field of
rehabilitation. Luria also reportedly had direct impact on the field through the
establishment of rehabilitative centers in the Soviet Union after World War II.
Despite concurrence with previous writings that brain injury was well recognized
and and its rehabilitation perhaps attempted by ancient civilizations as noted above
(cf. Courville, 1967), it is an accepted fact by most serious students in the field of
rehabilitation that the modern, evidence-based history of modern neurorehabilitation
had its humble yet most influential beginnings in association with the emergence of
the Industrial Revolution and World War I, as well as with advances in technology
and Western medicine (cf. Ries et al., 2007). This evolution and renaissance were
partially the result of pragmatic factors impacting rehabilitative medicine, including
low survival rates of victims who had sustained central nervous system (CNS) trauma
before that time and, indirectly, the investigation of rehabilitative methods and their
impact on recovery after injury (cf. Gurdjian, 1973). As noted by Ries et al. (2007),
Technological advances in the treatment of infections additionally led to increased
survival, and advances in technology itself played a major role (e.g., angiography)
leading to the study of previously untreatable injuries or poorly understood CNS
diseases and their treatments (e.g., strokes) (DeJong, 1982).
166 J.K. Ries et al.
Figure 8.1 Premorbid Occupation and rehabilitation outcomes after brain injury. Study showing
percentages of return-to-work WWI German veterans (brain-injured patients) as a function of
premorbid occupational allegiance (see text for explanation). From Goldstein and Reichmann
(1920), with permission from the publisher
the CDC (CDC, 1998) noted that cerebrovascular disease is twice as high among
African-American men (53.1 per 100,000) as among white men (26.3 per 100,000)
and twice as high among African American women (40.6 per 100,000) as among
white women (22.6 per 100,000). Similar health disparities are applicable to
Hispanics. In addition, although the contrasts between ethnic minority groups, including
Hispanics, and the dominant cultural group have revealed meaningful epidemiologi-
cal differences, modern investigations also have shown the emergence of interethnic
differences in the epidemiology of TBI. For example, in an elucidating study, Kraus
et al. (1986) found differential epidemiology rates of TBI in Hispanics compared
to African-Americans and Asians/Native Americans related to median income in
these groups.
Although many other examples could be provided for other injuries and
disorders, it should be sufficient to note that racial, and more important and
critical, ethnic differences, in conjunction with other variables such as socio-
economic status, health care access, and other variables, have historically led
to differential rates of incidence and prevalence of brain injuries or acquired
diseases of the brain requiring rehabilitation. Therefore, the close relationship
between the epidemiology of acquired brain injury and ethnic minority status,
namely Hispanic, buttresses the importance of attending to such factors capability
to impact neuropsychological interventions and rehabilitation in the U.S.
Although culture and ethnicity were discussed in detail in previous chapters, they
are briefly addressed here, in some instances from different perspectives, to note
their impact on the rehabilitation process. The term culture has been described by
the American Psychological Association (APA, 2003) as the embodiment of a
worldview through learned and transmitted beliefs, values, and practices, including
religious and spiritual traditions. It also encompasses a way of living informed by
the historical, economic, ecological, and political forces on a group (p. 380). As
noted by Ries et al. (in press), culture embodies and influences all facets of the
individual including cognition and emotions. Differences in cultural background
include not just language differences, but also differences in group identity, beliefs,
and values (Dana, 1993), all of which influence the use of services including reha-
bilitation, the presentation of neuropsychological symptoms, the assessment and
interventional techniques employed by the field, and all or most aspects of treat-
ment including neuropsychological rehabilitation.
Although as noted by Ardila (2003), culture is in the brain, and as noted in
Chapter 1, there may be an intricate interaction between culture and brains
mediated by genetic mechanisms. In addition, race unfortunately has been used
interchangeably with the term ethnicity. As noted by Llorente et al. (2000) and
Harris and Llorente (2005), such a definitional interchange unfortunately has been
a source of significant confusion. This is partially the result of the fact that race and
8 Rehabilitation 169
ethnicity are distinct constructs. In this regard, Jalali (1988) defined ethnicity as
the culture of [a] people [that] is thus critical for values, attitudes, perceptions,
needs, and modes of expression, behavior, and identity (p. 10). Therefore, ones
culture and ethnicity are similar as they are learned and passed down from genera-
tions, whereas race typically refers to cultural groups with permanent attributes that are
not learned nor can be changed because they are biologically-based (cf. Carter and
Qureshi, 1995). The subtle but significant definitional differences noted above
are important.
For example as it relates to Hispanics, the term has been used to describe a
heterogeneous group of individuals, and as stated by Llorente et al. (2000), has
been misused because it has been used as a racial category rather than an ethnic label.
Harris and Llorente also note that this panethnic label fails to capture the unique
attributes of an individual including, oddly enough, his or her racial background in
some instances. Even when referring to a specific Hispanic group, such as Mexican
or Puerto Rican, there are several unique attributes to consider. For example, whether
they are Mexican-American of Mexican descent born and raised in the U.S. or
Mexican immigrants born and raised in Mexico from dozens of possible ancestral
backgrounds, including one or more of 50 aboriginal origins, should be included
when considering ethnicity. Similarly, level of acculturation to American society and
dominant language should be included (cf. Harris and Llorente, 2005). These distinc-
tions are critical if we are to understand specific triggers that lead to variations in rates
of interethnic outcomes during TBI after rehabilitation. Such distinctions are required
because it is possible that specific demographics (e.g., level of education, literacy,
specific occupational predilections or opportunities), interacting with specific ethnic
subgroup, may be associated, or may modulate or moderate, interethnic and intraeth-
nic TBI rates. Most important, as noted by Ries et al. (2007), culture can have signifi-
cant impact on brain injury rehabilitation and prospective outcomes whether
addressing pediatric or adult populations (cf. Yeats et al., 2002; Uomoto and Wong,
2000).
Ries and her colleagues (in press) also note that general treatment adherence in
multicultural clients [is] more likely to end treatment prematurely, due to frustration,
misunderstanding, and role ambiguities of treatment, and the role of such factors in
the rehabilitation process of ethnic minority individuals has found support in the lit-
erature (cf. Sue and Zane, 1987). Sohlberg and Mateer (2001) indicate that identity
and self-concept are influenced and impacted by culturally mediated norms about
assertiveness, aggression, emotional expression, and individual goal attainment ver-
sus sacrificing for the greater good. In fact, as noted in previous chapters, cultural
identity and ethnicity also may underlie beliefs about illness and disability by the
patient and clinician, as well as its meaning and interpretation. Whereas personal
independence is highly valued in Western culture, it is not necessarily a goal in other
cultures, which have different beliefs about (1) persons degree of responsibility and
control over health and (2) the role of family in dealing with illness, according to Ries
et al. (in press; cf. Watanabe et al., 2001; Uomoto and Wong, 2000).
Finally, it is critical to note that population predictions indicate marked
increases in all minority groups; most notably, the Hispanic population grows
170 J.K. Ries et al.
60.0
Percent of Population
50.0
40.0
30.0
20.0
10.0
0.0
2000 2010 2020 2030 2040 2050
Year
Figure 8.2 Expected percent of Hispanic relative to non-Hispanic White population across
decades (U.S. Census, 2004)
Blonder (1991) examines the concept of culture within the context of neuropsychological
theories and research. Blonder noted that the effects of cultural variations on lin-
guistics impact the cerebral organization of language and reveal both organizing
principles as well as the impact of neuroplasticity on the brain and language
functions. In spite of variation in syntactic markers in languages, aphasic speakers
show similar patterns of problems following lesions to Brocas area. The grammatical
endings are lost or used incorrectly, and words are produced in their uninflected
172 J.K. Ries et al.
These are questions that should guide a practitioners interactions with patients
from different cultures.
Fadiman (1997) exemplifies what can occur if there is a lack of understanding of
different cultures in the medical setting, including the rehabilitation milieu. This book
examines the potential cultural conflict between Western beliefs and an individual
culture when a seizure disorder is perceived as a gift within a specific culture, but as
a disorder in Western medicine. The author provides some questions to ask to
promote greater understanding of a patients culture such as What do you call the
problem? What do you think has caused the problem? How severe is the sickness?
What are the most important results you hope to receive from this treatment?
Additional studies have supported the use of similar questions in understanding a
patients perspective of illness, disease, and disability within their culture (Garca
Coll and Magnuson, 2002; Reeve, Groce, Persing, and Magge, 2004).
Li (2003) proposes a unique model that incorporates the dynamic interplay of
culture and biology across the lifespan. This framework highlights how interactive
processes and developmental plasticity at different levels are closely connected to
each other and unfold across different time scales. Consequently, together they
channel reciprocal cultural experiential influences on behavioral, cognitive, and
brain development throughout the life span (p. 173). Li describes the reciprocal
and dynamic process by which genetics and neuronal mechanisms interface with
environment and culture to determine development.
174 J.K. Ries et al.
Attempts have been made to incorporate culture into specific models of rehabilita-
tion as a moderating variable rather than as an independent variable. Such a mode-
ling posture has assisted investigators to better understand the role of culture in
rehabilitation. For example, ethnicity has been hypothesized to be intricately
related to SES, accounting for a portion of the findings that implicate ethnicity in
TBI. In this regard, culture interacts, as a moderator variable, with SES to create a
pattern of performance that is unique to a specific individual. Although related to
race, not ethnicity, this is depicted in individuals who come from specific African-
American populations in which the rates of TBI as a result of violence are greater
than those for European-Americans, most likely as a result of lower levels of SES
combined with other factors. In this case, TBI severity related to violence, for
example, could be associated with SES yet moderated by a specific ethnic back-
ground within a race construct (e.g., African-American, Hispanic), or other
variable(s). Such methods of examining culture and other factors, as moderator
variables, have been postulated by various investigators (e.g., Baron and Kenney,
1986; Holmbeck, 1997) and have received limited yet sound empirical validation.
In the case of children, for example, an investigation has shown that African-
Americans compared to Whites (race), independent of SES, were able to moderate
differences in injured groups (TBI v. Orthopedic) in parental and family outcomes
(Yeats et al., 2002). In contrast, Handwerker (2002) proposes methods of analyses
of transposed matrices to take advantage of the unique point of view that ethnogra-
phy offers its focus on similarities and differences among informants rather than
among variables. According to Handwerker:
Without explicit construct validation of the cultures, one cannot know which groups to
compare. If one divides ones data by identity (ethnicity, class, gender, age, etc), counts
the responses for each, and compared the results, one imposes cultural differences by
assumption, not evidence. (p. 2095)
1
Portions of this chapter one from Ries et al. (2007)
8 Rehabilitation 175
Hune, MacGregor and deVeber (2003) found that the combination of Eastern and
Western medicine occur frequently without knowledge. They studied the prevalence
of use of Eastern medicine practices in first-generation Chinese-Canadian children
with stroke and cerebrovascular disease. They found that over 53% of the children
received some sort of alternative (Eastern) medicine (e.g. acupuncture, herbal
medications) in addition to the Western medicine they received. Interestingly,
Western doctors were unaware of most of the alternative medicine use until a
Cantonese-speaking nurse practitioner interviewed them. This stresses the impor-
tance of the impact of culture and the role language has in the treatment of pediatric
and adult populations. In contrast, integration of Western and Chinese traditional
medicine has been proposed as the treatment for primary nephrotic syndrome in
China (Zhimin, 2003).
Although strides have been made in this area, a great deal of work remains. For
example, in the Yeats et al. (2002) and other studies (cf. CDC, 1998), it is unclear
who constituted the group labeled as African-American. Is it possible that specific
subgroups of African-American families, from specific backgrounds (e.g., where
violence is explicitly accepted or exhibited) or other demographic variables, are
mostly responsible for such effects? In fact, to date, although we are aware that
African-Americans have a higher rate of TBI as a result of violence, for example,
a limited understanding of the specific variables that moderate this relationship is
known. Clearly, factors such as acculturation-assimilation, education, geographical
areas, climate, and substance abuse may play a role on how violence is exhibited,
and such moderators must be examined within the African-American context, and
other cultures, including Hisporics in such investigations and future studies to deter-
mine whether more precise intraethnic factors can lead to specific prediction. It is
also critical to mention that such distinctions are important because they may
reduce stereotypes that paint all African-American families (or other ethnic
groups) with the same brush. It is also critical to note that sometimes such investi-
gations cannot be conducted as a result of the small number of participants from
different backgrounds within a specific ethnic group, and this issue plagues many
studies, including our own investigations. Nevertheless, it is an issue that merits
significant attention and resources. Acknowledgement of the importance of more
specifically defining the conceptual frameworks results in the call to provide accu-
rate and thorough information about study sample characteristics by various jour-
nals and federal funding agencies (Garca Coll and Magnuson, 2002).
diffuse symptoms and to make the patient feel understood (p. 515). This tech-
nique involves active listening and informing the patient about the symptoms
commonly experienced in brain injury rehabilitation. Journaling involves recording
symptoms and subjective experience (How does the symptom make you feel?)
with reasonable frequency in order to define the symptoms, provide a baseline of
symptoms, foster coping style, and to provide a concrete measure of progress.
Structuring involves establishing or returning an individual to a routine to increase
his or her productivity and adjustment. It is defined as, a predictable, purposeful
set of activities that allows patients to channel their energy productively. It gives
them a sense of control over their immediate environment, provides them with
positive feedback on their progress, and helps them achieve short-term realistic
goals (p. 522).
Finally, the purpose of reframing is defined as, shifting the perspective of the
process from tragedy to challenge, from future to present, from unmanageable
issues to manageable issues (p. 524). This technique involves paradoxical/
cognitive-behavioral interventions (e.g., guided imagery), as well as reframing of
spiritual issues (spiritualizing). Pontn, Gonzalez, and Mares describe three case
studies in which these techniques were used and found to be effective. They also
discuss the limitations to these specific techniques, and readers should aware of the
studys limitations as a result of its small sample size.
Unfortunately, the majority of the empirically validated treatments has been
based on White Americans and does not reflect the cultural diversity of the United
States. Christophersen and Mortweet (2001) note that the lack of empirical data
(e.g., base rates, treatment efficacy) on cultural diversity makes it difficult to estimate
when and where cultural differences are important. Guidelines to improve cultural
awareness and understanding were created by the American Psychological
Association (2003) due to:
the continuing evolution of the study of psychology, changes in society at large, and
emerging data about the different needs of particularly individuals and groups historically
marginalized or disenfranchised within and by psychology based on their ethnic/racial
heritage and social group identity or membership. (p. 377)
skills in clinical and other applied psychological practices, (6) sychologists are
encouraged to use organizational change processes to support culturally informed
organizational development and practices.
With regard to interventional outcomes, the literature supports the fact that
racial-ethnic differences exist in outcomes. For example, in a recent study pub-
lished by Hanks et al. (2003), data supported the fact previously reported that ethnic
and racial minorities showed greater frequency of TBI associated with violence
than Whites. In fact, they showed rates of violent TBI to be approximately twice as
high in ethnic minorities (74%) compared to Whites (46%). Most important, these
results suggested the presence of poorer outcomes in minorities as a result of vio-
lent TBI. It should be noted that this disparity is similar, for example, to that for
strokes in African-Americans populations and other diseases and conditions
impacting Hispanics as well. One of the most comprehensive studies assessing TBI
outcome to date was sponsored by the National Institute for Disability and
Rehabilitation Research (NIDRR), Traumatic Brain Injury Model Systems (cf.
Harrison et al., 1996), established in 1987 to demonstrate the benefits of a coordi-
nated system of neurotrauma and rehabilitation care and conduct innovative
research on all aspects of care for those who sustain traumatic brain injuries and
establish a national TBI database. In this study, a comparison was made again
between the ethnic minority group (e.g., African-American, Asian/Pacific
Islander) and Whites. This investigation revealed the presence of poorer outcome
as measured by the Community Integration Questionnaire assessing role perform-
ance in the community, with the minority group scoring lower than Whites after
significant analyses were made to covariate for various potential demographic and
injury confounders, including age, gender, and trauma cause and severity. While
there may be many moderating variables implicated in outcome studies, recognition
of health and mental health care needs and access to them may negatively impact
minority populations. In this regard, a study by Slomine et al. (2006) examined
White and non-White children and adolescents with moderate to severe TBI
from four trauma centers. Approximately 12 months post injury, non-White youth
were significantly more likely to have unrecognized needs and lack of appropriate
service utilization despite need when compared to the White youth, and according
to the authors these findings may provide an explanation related to poorer outcome
in youth from poorer family functioning. The most common unmet need in their
study was appropriate treatment and targeting of cognitive dysfunction impact-
ing performance in the classroom and neurobehavioral sequelae.
In another study recently published using existing Veterans Administrations
records (Stansbury et al., 2004), they note the importance of appropriately docu-
menting ethnicity, as it has high significance in adult stroke rehabilitation. More
important, they note the pitfalls associated with methodological issues when using
race (ethnicity) as a predictor with dichotomous response variables. Similar find-
ings have been obtained for other outcome measures in other studies for various
types of brain injury and its rehabilitation, but their detailed examinations are
beyond the scope of this chapter (cf. Burnett et al. 2003). Finally, a U.S. volunteer
surgical team performed approximately 100 craniofacial surgeries to repair cleft
178 J.K. Ries et al.
In summary, despite the great advances attained by Goldstein and his colleagues
during the early part of the last century, particularly related to the outcome of
specific vocational groups subsequent to habilitation, neuropsychological rehabili-
tation remains in a state of immature development as it relates to the understanding
of the impact of culture on treatment outcomes, particularly ethnic minority popula-
tions including Hispanics living in the U.S. Nevertheless, it is critical for clinicians
to be cognizant of epidemiological and demographic issues that impact their daily
duties with Hispanic clients, which in the U.S. encompass a large number of indi-
viduals. It is also important for multicultural models to guide empirically validated
rehabilitative methods, and Groce and Zola (1993) offer useful advice to overcome
the challenge of providing culturally sensitive neuropsychological evaluations:
They note that No one individual can anticipate all the problems that might arise
in an attempt to understand chronic illness and disability in a multicultural society,
but we can all have enough sensitivity to realize that there might be significant
difference, and enough respect for others to ask questions and listen carefully to the
replies (p. 1055). This level of sensitivity, or the aspirations of the work under-
scored in this chapter relative to cultural sensitivity, should not be exclusively
applicable to clinical settings but should permeate research studies examining TBI,
an issue that has not received due attention despite governmental efforts to include
Hispanics in national research protocols. It is also important for the reader to recognize
that many of the issues raised here are applicable when providing feedback to
Hispanic families.
8 Rehabilitation 179
Antolin M. Llorente
180
Summary and Concluding Remarks 181
adaptation of tests (Muniz and Hambleton, 1996). The guidelines not only address
issues associated with translations, but also discuss the selection of appropriate
tests, consideration of potential biases, appropriate scoring methods, appropriate
communication of findings, revision of tests, etc. Even when using validated trans-
lated instruments, the test results should be interpreted with caution, and within an
appropriate cultural context. Individuals assessing these patients should be compe-
tent to conduct such evaluations. It is also important to be aware of the unique needs
of Spanish-English bilinguals living in the United States. For many, using either
Spanish or English testing materials and norms can underestimate their cognitive
abilities (Puente and Ardila, 2000). To reduce bilingualism effects in testing, it
would be ideal to have special norms for Spanish-English bilinguals (until test are
developed that are not biased), and a bilingual examiner who can provide instruc-
tions and understand answers in either language or any mixture of both languages.
If this is not feasible, their results could perhaps be interpreted using both norms
(English and Spanish versions).
As noted in Chapter 7, modern neuropsychological procedures have used more
complex methods of tests development using operating-receiver curves or item
analytic procedures creating standards for test development far more advanced and
sophisticated that a simple adaptation, or worst, a translation of a test. If a patients
primary language is unfamiliar, it is best to refer the patient to a neuropsychologist
who can competently perform an evaluation in that language. Only as a last resort
and if necessary, an interpreter should be used for clinical interviews and consulta-
tions. In such cases, clinicians must select interpreters carefully, and rapport should
be established between a neuropsychologist and an interpreter prior to meetings
with patients. It is important that interpreters are familiar with neuropsychological
principles and terms as well as different Hispanic cultures and regional languages.
Still, it is crucial that clinicians are aware of the increased possibility of miscom-
munications and misunderstandings. Having an interpreter in a room may change
the comfort level and dynamics during meetings, and the subtleties in communica-
tions, such as nonverbal cues and complex language responses can be lost easily
through interpretations. It is also pivotal to note the use of interpreters during the
course of neuropsychological assessment was not advocated or recommended in
this volume, and such a practice should be avoided at all costs, particularly in foren-
sic proceedings, and the use of family members as interpreters completely
discarded.
In conclusion, significant attention towards ethical considerations should be
given to scientific models and applied methods in neuropsychology addressing
cross-cultural issues related to Hispanics, using irreproachable standards of practice
in clinical, educative, and research endeavors.
References
Adams, R.L., Boake, C., and Crain, C. (1982). Bias in neuropsychological test classification related
to education, age and ethnicity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 143145.
Adrenal, A. (2002). Spanish-English bilingualism in the United States of America. In F. Fibber
(ed.) Advances in the neurolinguistics of bilingualism. Essays in honor of Michael Paradis.
(pp. 4967). Udine (Italy).
Ahmad, A., Mohamed, T., and Ameen, N.M. (1997). A 26-month follow-up of posttraumatic
stress symptoms in children after the mass-escape tragedy in Iraqi Kurdistan. Nordic Journal
of Psychiatry, 52, 357366.
Ajdukovic, M. (1995). Mothers perception of their relationship with their children during
displacement: A six month follow up. Child Abuse Review, 5, 3439.
Ajdukovic, M., and Ajdukovic, D. (1993). Psychological well being of refugee children. Child
Abuse and Neglect, 17, 843854.
Ajdukovic, M., and Ajdukovic, D. (1998). Impact of displacement on the psychological well being
of refugee children. International Review of Psychiatry, 10, 186195.
Alden, K., Poole, C., Chantavanich, S., Ohmar, K., Aung, N.N., and Mollica, R.F. (1996).
Burmese political dissidents in Thailand: Trauma and survival among young adults in exile.
American Journal of Public Health, 86, 15611569.
Alegria, M., Vila, D., Woo, M., Canino, G., Takeuchi, D., Vera, M., Febo, V., Guarnaccia, P.,
Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., and Shrout, P. (2004). Cultural relevance and equivalence in the NLAAS
instrument: Integrating etic and emic measures in the development of cross-cultural measures
for a psychiatric epidemiology and services study of Latinos. International Journal of Methods
in Psychiatric Research, 13, 270288.
Almqvist, K., and Brandell-Forsberg, M. (1997). Refugee children in Sweden: Post-traumatic
stress disorder in Iranian preschool children exposed to organized violence. Child Abuse and
Neglect, 21, 351366.
Almqvist, K., and Broberg, A.G. (1999). Mental health and social adjustment in young refugee
children 3 1/2 years after their arrival in Sweden. Journal of the American Academy of Child
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 723730.
American Educational Research Association. (1999). Standards for educational and psychologi-
cal Testing. Washington, DC: AERA Publications.
American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, and National
Council on Measurement in Education. (1999). The standards for educational and psychologi-
cal testing. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(3rd ed.) revised. Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
185
186 References
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(4th ed.) text revision. Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychological Association. (1991). Guidelines for providers of services to ethnic, linguistic,
and culturally diverse populations. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
American Psychological Association. (1992). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of
conduct. American Psychologist, 47, 15971611.
American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of
conduct. Retrieved Jan. 5, 2006, from http://www.apa.org/ethics/code2002.html.
American Psychological Association. (2001). Guidelines on multicultural education, training,
research, practice, and organizational change for psychologists. Retrieved Nov. 3, 2005, from
http://www.apa.org/divisions/div45/resources.html.
American Psychological Association. (2003). Guidelines on multicultural education, training, research,
practice, and organizational change for psychologists. American Psychologist, 58, 377402.
American Psychological Association. (1985, 1999). The Standards for educational and psycho-
logical testing. Washington, DC: Author.
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1982). Committee on language, speech and
hearing services in the schools. Definitions: Communicative disorders and variations.
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, 24, 949950.
Anastasi, A., and Urbina, S. (1997). Psychological testing (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River,
NJ: Prentice Hall.
Angel, B., Hjern, A., and Ingleby, D. (2001). Effects of war and organized violence on children:
A study of Bosnian refugees in Sweden. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 71, 415.
Anger, W.K., Cassitto, M.G., Liang, Y.X. et al. (1993). Comparison of performance from three
continents on the WHO-recommended Neurobehavior Core Test Battery. Environmental
Research, 62, 125147.
Applebaum, H. (1987). Perspectives in cultural anthropology. Albany, NY: State University of
New York Press.
Ardila, A. (1993). Future directions in the research and practice of cross-cultural neuropsychology.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 19 (abstract).
Ardila, A. (1998). A note of caution: Normative neuropsychological test performance: Effects of
age, education, gender and ethnicity: A comment on Saykin et al. (1995). Applied
Neuropsychology, 5, 5153.
Ardila, A. (2003). Culture in Our Brains: Cross-Cultural Differences in the Brain-Behavior
Relationships. In A. Toomela (ed.), Cultural Guidance in the Development of the Human
Mind. London: Greenwood Publishing.
Ardila, A. (2005). Cultural values underlying psychometric cognitive testing. Neuropsychological
Review, 15, 185195.
Ardila, A., and Moreno., S. (2001). Neuropsychological test performance in Aruaco Indians: An
exploratory study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, 510515.
Ardila. A., Ardila. O., Bryden M.P., Ostrosky F, Rosselli. M., and Steenhuis. R. (1989a). Effects
of cultural background and education on handedness. Neuropsychologia, 27, 893897.
Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., and Rosas, P. (1989b). Neuropsychological assessment of illiterates:
Visuo-spatial and memory abilities. Brain and Cognition, 11, 147166.
Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., and Puente, A. (1994). Neuropsychological assessment of the Spanish
speaker. New York: Plenum Press.
Arroyo, W., and Eth, S. (1996). Post-traumatic stress disorder and other stress reactions. In R.J.
Apfel and S. Bennett (eds.), Minefields in their hearts: The mental health of children in war
and communal violence (pp. 5274). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Artioli i Fortuny, L., and Mullaney, H.A. (1998). Assessing patients whose language you do not
know: Can the absurd be ethical? The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 113126.
Artioli i Fortuny, L., Garolera, M., Hermosillo, R.D., Feldman, E., Fernandez Barillas, H., Keefe, R.,
et al. (2005). Research with Spanish-speaking populations in the United States: lost in the
translation. A commentary and a plea. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,
27, 555564.
References 187
Athey, J.L., and Ahearn, F.L. (1991). The mental health of refugee children: An overview. In F.L.
Ahearn and J.L. Athey (eds.), Refugee children: Theory, research, and services. The Johns
Hopkins series in contemporary medicine and public health (pp. 319). Baltimore, MD: The
Johns Hopkins University Press.
Auffrey, J., and Robertson, M. (1972). Case history information and examiner experience as
determinants of scoring validity on the Wechsler intelligence tests. Proceedings of the 80th
Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, 7, 553554.
Axelrod, B.N. (2002). Validity of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence and other very
short forms of estimating intellectual functioning. Assessment, 9, 1723.
Badcock, K.A., and Ross, M.W. (1982). Neuropsychological testing with Australian aborigines.
Australian Psychologist, 17, 297299.
Baker, D., Stevens, C., Brook, R. et al. (1996). Determinants of emergency department use:
Are race and ethnicity important? Annals of Emergency Medicine, 28, 677682.
Ballard, W.S., Tighe, P.L., and Dalton, E.F. (1989). IDEA Oral Language Proficiency Test
Pre- IPT Spanish. Brea, CA: Ballard and Tighe Publishers.
Ballard, W.S., Tighe, P.L., and Dalton, E.F. (1996a). IDEA Oral Language Proficiency Test IPT 1
Spanish. Brea, CA: Ballard and Tighe Publishers.
Ballard, W.S., Tighe, P.L., and Dalton, E.F. (1996b). IDEA Oral Language Proficiency Test IPT II
Spanish. Brea, CA: Ballard and Tighe Publishers.
Baron, I.S. (2004). Neuropsychological evaluation of the child. Oxford: University Press.
Baron, R.M., and Kenny, D.A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in socialpsy-
chological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182.
Bates, E., Dale, P.S., and Thal, D. (1995). Individual differences and their implications for theories
of language development. In P. Fletcher and B. MacWhinney (eds.), Handbook of child
language. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Bates, E., Thal, D., and Janowsky, J. (1992). Early language development and its neural correlates.
In S. Segalowitz and I. Rapin (eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology: Vol. 7. Child
Neuropsychology (pp. 69110). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Bates, E., Thal, D., Finlay, B., and Clancy, B. (2003). Early language development and its neural
correlates. In I. Rapin and S. Segalowitz (eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology: Child
neuropsychology (Vol. 8, 2nd ed.). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psych
Corp.
Becker, D.F., Weine, S.M., Vojvoda, D., and McGlashan, T.H. (1999). Case series: PTSD
symptoms in adolescent survivors of ethnic cleansing. Results from a 1-year follow up
study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38,
775781.
Bedore, L.M. (1999). The acquisition of Spanish. In O.L. Taylor and L.B. Leonard (eds.),
Language acquisition across North America: Cross-cultural and cross-linguistic perspectives
(pp. 157207). San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group, Inc.
Beery, K.E., and Taheri, C.M. (1992). Beery Picture Vocabulary Test. Odessa, FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources, Inc.
Beiser, M., Dion, R., Gotowiec, A., Hyman, I., and Vu, N. (1995). Immigrant and refugee children
in Canada. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 40, 6772.
Belyaev, D.K. (1979). Destabalizing selection as a factor in domestication. Journal of Hereditz,
70, 301308.
Bemak, F., and Timm, J. (1994). Case study of an adolescent Cambodian refugee: A clinical,
developmental and cultural perspective. International Journal for the Advancement of
Counselling, 17, 4758.
Benson, D.F. (1993). Aphasia. In K.M. Heilman and E. Valenstein (eds.), Clinical neuropsychology
(3rd ed., pp. 1736). New York: Oxford University Press.
Benton, A.L., Hamsher, K. de S., Rey, G.J., and Sivian, A.B. (1994). Multilingual Aphasia
Examination (3rd ed.). Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates.
188 References
Berman, H. (2001). Children and war: Current understandings and future directions. Public
Health Nursing, 18, 243252.
Berry, J.W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation and adaptation. Applied Psychology, 46, 568.
Berry, J.W. (2001). Contextual studies of cognitive adaptation. In J.M. Collis and S. Messick (eds.),
Intelligence and personality: Bridging the gap in theory and measurement (pp. 319333).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.
Berry, J. W. (1999). Emics and etics: a symbiotic conception. Culture and Psychology, 5, 165171.
Berthold, S.M. (1999). The effects of exposure to community violence on Khmer refugee
adolescents. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 12, 455471.
Best, C.T., and Avery, R.A. (1999). Left-hemisphere advantage for click consonants is determined
by linguistic significance and experience. Psychological Science, 10, 6570.
Bird, H.R. (1996). Epidemiology of childhood disorders in a cross-cultural context. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37, 3549.
Bland, J.M., and Altman, D.G. (1986). Statistical methods for assessing agreement between two
methods of clinical measurement. Lancet, i, 307310.
Blau, T. (1998). The psychologist as expert witness (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
Blonder, L.X. (1991). Human neuropsychology and the concept of culture. Human Nature,
2, 83116.
Bond, L. (1987). The golden rule settlement: A minority perspective. Educational Measurment:
Issues and Practice, 6, 2325.
Bornstein, M.H. (1973). Color vision and color naming: A psychological hypothesis of cultural
difference. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 257285.
Bos, C.S., and VanReusen, A. K. (1991). Academic interventions with learning disabled students:
A cognitive/metacognitive approach. In J.E. Obrzut and G.W. Hynd (eds.), Neuropsychological
foundations of learning disabilities (pp. 659684). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Botwinick, J. (1967). Cognitive Processes in Maturity and Old Age. New York: Springer.
Boydston, J.A. (ed.). (1981). Dewey, J. The Later Works, 19251953. Carbondale: Southern
Illinois University Press.
Braden, J.P., and Kratochwill, T.R. (1997). Treatment utility of assessment: Myths and realities.
School Psychology Review, 26, 475485.
Brandt, J. (February, 2005). Neuropsychological Crimes and Misdemeanors, Presidential Address,
33rd Annual Meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, St. Louis, Missouri.
Brislin, R.W. (1983). Translation and content analysis of oral and written material. In
H.C. Tridndis and J.W. Berry (Eds.), Handbook of Cross-Cultural Methodology. Boston: Allyn
& Bacon.
Broca, P. (1861). Perte de la parole, ramollissement chronique et destruction partielle du lob
anterieur gauche de cerveau. Bulletins de la Societe dAnthropologie, 62, pp. 235238.
Brownell, R. (2001). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test Spanish Bilingual Edition.
Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.
Bruininks, R.H., Woodcock, R.W., Weatherman, R.F., and Hill, B.K. (1996). Scale of Independent
Behavior (rev. ed.). Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing Company.
Burnett, D.M., Kolakowsky-Hayner, S.A., Slater, D., Stringer, A., Bushnik, T., Zafonte, R., and
Cifu, D.X. (2003). Ethnographic analysis of traumatic brain injury patients in the National
Model Systems database. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 84,263267.
Byrd, D.A., Sanchez, D., & Manly, J.J. (2005). Neuropsychological test performance among
Caribbean-born and U.S.-born African-American elderly: The role of age, education, and
reading level. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 10561069.
Camilli, G., & Shepord, L.A. (1994). Methods for identifying biased test items. Thousand Oaks,
CA. Sage.
Campbell, T., Dollaghan, C., Needleman, H., and Janosky, J. (1997). Reducing bias in language
assessment: Processing dependent measures. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,
40, 519525.
Camus, J. (1917/1918). Physical and occupational re-education of the maimed. London: Bailliere,
Tindal and Cox.
References 189
Canadian Psychological Association. (1991). Canadian Code of Ethics for Psychologists. Ottawa,
Ontario: Canadian Psychological Association.
Carlson, C.I., Uppal, S., and Prosser, E. (2000). Ethnic differences in processes contributing to the
self-esteem of early adolescent girls. Journal of Early Adolescence, 20, 4467.
Carroll, J.B. (2005). The three-stratum theory of cognitive abilities. In D.P. Flanagan and P.L.
Harrison (eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues. New York:
Guilford Press.
Carroll, J.B., Davies, P., and Richman, B. (1971). The American heritage word frequency book.
Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Carter, R.T., and Qureshi, A. (1995). A typology of philosophical assumptions in multicultural
counseling and training. In J.G. Ponterotto, J.M. Casas, L.A. Suzuki, and C.M. Alexander
(eds.), Handbook of Multicultural Counseling (pp. 239262). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE
Publications, Inc.
Cascallar, E.C., and Arnold, J. (2001). Second language acquisition. In M.O. Pontn and J. Len-
Carrin (eds.), Neuropsychology and the Hispanic patient: A clinical handbook (pp. 5974).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cattell, R.B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence; A critical experiment. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 54, 122.
Ceci, S.J. (1996). On intellegence (expanded ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Centeno, J.G., and Obler, L.K. (2001). Principles of bilingualism. In M.O. Pontn and J. Len-
Carrin (eds.), Neuropsychology and the Hispanic patient: A clinical handbook (pp. 7586).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1998). Tobacco use among U.S. racial/ethnic minor-
ity groups - African Americans, American Indians and Alaska Natives, Asian Americans and
Pacific Islanders, and Hispanics: A report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta: Author.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1999). Traumatic brain injury in the United States:
A report to congress. Atlanta: Author.
Cervantes, R.C., and Acosta, F.X. (1992). Psychological testing for Hispanic-Americans. Applied
and Preventive Psychology, 1, 209219.
Choi, S. (1999). Acquisition of Korean. In O.L. Taylor and L.B. Leonard (eds.), Language acquisition
across North America: Cross-cultural and cross-linguistic perspectives (pp. 281334). San
Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group, Inc.
Chomsky, N. (1991). Linguistics and cognitive science: Problems and mysteries. In A. Kasher
(ed.), The Chomskyan turn. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
Christensen, A.L. (1975). Lurias neuropsychological investigation. New York: Spectrum.
Christensen, A.L., and Castano, C. (1996). Alexander Romanovitch Luria (19021977): Contributions
to neuropsychological rehabilitation. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 6, 279303.
Christophersen, E.R., and Mortweet, S.L. (2001). Treatments that work with children: Empirically
supported strategies for managing childhood problems (pp. 310). Washington DC: American
Psychological Association.
Clark, C.M., DeCarli, C., Mungas, D., Chui, H.I., Higdon, R., Nunez, J., et al. (2005). Earlier
onset of Alzheimer disease symptoms in Latino individuals compared with Anglo individuals.
Archives of Neurology, 62, 774778.
Clarke, G.N., Sack, W.H., and Goff, B. (1993). Three forms of stress in Cambodian adolescent
refugees. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 21, 6577.
Cole, E. (1998). Immigrant and refugee children: Challenges and opportunities for education.
Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 14, 3650.
Collier, V.P. (1992). A synthesis of studies examining long-term language minority student data
on academic achievement. Bilingual Education Research Journal, 16, 187221.
Collier, V.P. (1995). Acquiring a second language for school. Directions in Language and
Education, 1, 110.
Committee on Minority Representation in Special Education (2002). Representation of minority
students in special and gifted education. In M.S. Donovan and C.T. Cross (eds.), Minority
students in special and gifted education. (pp. 3590).
190 References
Comrie, B. (2000). From potential to realization: An episode in the origin of language. Linguistics,
38, 9891004.
Condeluci, A. (1997). Community inclusion: The ultimate goal of rehabilitation. In J. Len-
Carrin (ed.), Neuropsychological rehabilitation: Fundamentals, innovations, and directions
(pp. 483495). Delray Beach, FL: GR/St. Lucie Press.
Connelly, J., and Schweiger, M. (2000). The health risks of the UKs new asylum act. British
Medical Journal, 321, 56.
Courville, C.B. (1967). Injuries of the skull and brain as described in the myths, legends and folk-
tales of the various peoples of the world. New York: Vantage Press.
Coutinho, M.J., and Oswald, D.P (2000). Disproportionate representation in special education:
A synthesis and recommendations. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 9, 13156.
Crago, M.B., and Allen, S.E.M. (1999). Acquiring Inuktitut. In O.L. Taylor and L.B. Leonard
(eds.), Language acquisition across North America: Cross-cultural and cross-linguistic
perspectives (pp. 245278). San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group, Inc.
Crowley, M. (1992). Behavioural difficulties and their relationship to language impairment. In
J. Law (ed.), The early identification of language impairment in children: Therapy in practice
(pp. 6383). London: Chapman and Hall.
Cruz, B., Teck, B. (1998). The official spanglish dictionary: Un users guide to more than 300
words and phrases that arent exactly Espanol or Ingles. New York: Fireside Books.
Crystal, D. (1987). Cambridge encyclopedia of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cummins, J. (1979). Linguistic interdependence and the educational development of bilingual
children. Review of Educational Research, 49, 222251.
Cummins, J. (1981). The role of primary language development in promoting educational success
for language minority students (pp. 349). In Schooling and language minority students.
Sacramento, CA: California Department of Education.
Cummins, J. (1984). Bilingualism and special education: Issues in assessment and pedagogy. San
Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.
Cummins, J. (1989). Empowering language minority students. Sacramento, CA: California
Association for Bilingual Education.
Cummins, J., and Gulutsan, M. (1975). Set, objectification and second language learning.
International Journal of Psychology, 10, 91100.
Cunningham, M., and Cunningham, J.D. (1997). Patterns of symptomatology and patterns of tor-
ture and trauma experiences in resettled refugees. Australian and New Zealand Journal of
Psychiatry, 31, 555565.
Damasio, A.R., and Damasio, H. (2000). Aphasia and the neural basis of language. In M.M.
Mesulam (Ed.), Principles of behavioral and cognitive neurology (2nd ed., pp. 294315).
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Damico, J.S. (1985). Clinical Discourse Analysis: A functional language assessment technique. In
C.S. Simon (ed.), Communication skills and classroom success: Assessment of language-
learning disabled students (pp. 165204). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Damico, J.S. (1991). Descriptive assessment of communicative ability in limited English profi-
cient students. In E.V. Hayaman and J.S. Damico (eds.), Limiting bias in the assessment of
bilingual students (pp. 157217). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Dana, R. (1993). Multicultural assessment perspectives for professional psychology. Needham
Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Darwin, C. (1967). On the origin of species. A facsimile of the first edition. New York, Atheneum.
Das, J.P, Naglieri, J., and Kirby, J.R. (1994). Assessment of cognitive processes: The PASS theory
of intelligence. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Davies, M., and Webb, E. (2000). Promoting the psychological well-being of refugee children.
Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 5, 541554.
DeBlesser, R. (1988). Localisation of aphasia: Science or fiction? In G. Denes, C. Semenza, and
P. Bisiacchi (eds.), Perspectives on cognitive neuropsychology. East Sussex, U K: Lawrence
Earlbaum Associates.
References 191
Elbedour, S., ten-Bensel., and Bastien, D.T. (1993). Ecological integrated model of children in
war: Individual and social psychology. Child Abuse and Neglect, 17, 805819.
Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, P., and Taub, E. (1995). Increased cortical rep-
resentation of fingers of the left hand in string players. Science, 270, 305307.
Elliot, C.D. (1990). Differential Ability Scales. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Elliott, S.N. (1990). The nature and structure of the DAS: Questioning the tests organizing model
and use. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 8, 406411.
Espino, C.M. (1991). Trauma and adaptation: The case of Central American children. In F.L.
Ahearn and J.L. Athey (eds.), Refugee children: Theory, research and services. The Johns
Hopkins series in contemporary medicine and public health (pp. 106124). Baltimore, MD,
US: The Johns Hopkins Press.
Espinosa, G., Elizondo, V., and Miranda, J. (eds.). (2005). Latino religions and civic activism in
the United States. New York: Oxford University Press.
Evans, J.L. (2001). An emergent account of language impairments in children with SLI: Implications
for assessment and intervention. Journal of Communication Disorders, 34, 3954.
Ezkenazi, B., and Maizlish, N.A. (1988). Effects of occupational exposure to chemicals on
neurobehavioral functioning. In R.E. Tarter, D.H. van Thiel, and K.L. Edwards (eds.),
Medical neuropsychology: The impact of disease on behavior (pp. 223264). New York:
Plenum.
Fadiman, A. (1997). The spirit catches you and you fall down: A Hmong child, her American
doctors, and a collision of two cultures. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux.
Fantino, A.M., Colak, A. (2001). Refugee children in Canada: Searching for identity. Child
Welfare, 80, 587596.
Farris, R.E.L., and Dunham, H.W. (1939). Mental Disorders in Urban Areas. Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Felsman, J.K., Leong, F.T., Johnson, M.C., and Felsman, I.C. (1990). Estimates of psychological
distress among Vietnamese refugees: Adolescents, unaccompanied minors and young adults.
Social Science and Medicine, 31, 12511256.
Fennell, E.B., and Bauer, R.M. (1989). Models of inference in evaluating brain-behavior relation-
ships in children. In C. Reynolds and E. Fletcher-Janzen (eds.), Handbook of Clinical
Neuropsychology (2nd ed., pp. 204215). New York: Plenum Press.
Fenson, L., Dale, P.S., Reznick, S., Bates, E., Thal, D., and Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early
communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
59 (Serial No. 242).
Figueroa, R.A., Sandoval, J., and Merino, B. (1984). School psychology and limited-English
proficient (LEP) children: New competencies. Journal of School Psychology, 22, 131143.
Flynn, J.R. (1984). The mean IQ of Americans: Massive gains 19321978. Psychological Bulletin,
95, 2951.
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., and McHugh, P.R. (1975). Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading
the cognitive status of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.
Fortin, J., and Crago, M.B. (1999). French language acquisition in North America. In O.L. Taylor
and L.B. Leonard (eds.), Language acquisition across North America: Cross-cultural and
cross-linguistic perspectives (pp. 209242). San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group, Inc.
Fox, P.G., Cowell, J.M., and Montgomery, A.C. (1994). The effects of violence on health and
adjustment of Southeast Asian refugee children: An integrative review. Public Health Nursing,
11, 195201.
Franco, J.N. (1983). An acculturation scale for Mexican-American children. Journal of General
Psychology, 108, 175181.
Fromm, E. (1956). The Art of Loving. New York: Harper & Raw.
Fuld, P.A. (1981). The Fuld Object Memory Evaluation. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting Instrument
Company.
Galton, F. (1869/1892/1962). Hereditary genius: An Inquiry into its laws and consequences.
London: Macmillan/Fontana.
References 193
Gannotti, M.E., and Handwerker, W.P. (2002). Puerto Rican understandings of child disability:
Methods for the cultural validation of standardized measures of child health. Social Science
and Medicine, 55, 20932105.
Garbarino, J., and Kostelny, K. (1996). The effects of political violence on Palestinian childrens
behavior problems: A risk accumulation model. Child Development, 67, 3345.
Garbarino, J., and Kostelny, K. (1996). What do we need to know to understand children in war
and community violence? In R.J. Apfel and S. Bennett (eds.), Minefields in their hearts: The
mental health of children in war and communal violence (pp. 3351). New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Garbarino, J., Kostelny, K., and Dubrow, N. (1991). What children can tell us about living in
danger? American Psychologist, 46, 376383.
Garca Coll, C., and Magnuson, K. (2002). Cultural differences as sources of developmental
vulnerabilities and resources. In J.P. Shonkoff and S.J. Meisels (eds.) Handbook of early
childhood intervention (2nd ed.) (pp. 94114). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Garcia, M. (1981). Desert Immigrants: The Mexicans of El Paso, 18801920. New Haven, CT:
Yale University Press.
Gardner, M.F. (1990a). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test Revised. Novato, CA:
Academic Therapy Publications.
Gardner, M.F. (1990b). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test Revised, Spanish Edition.
Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.
Gardner, M.F. (2000). Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (2000 ed.). Novato, CA:
Academic Therapy Publications.
Garmezy, N., and Rutter, M. (eds.) (1983). Stress, Coping, and Development in Children.
Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press.
Geltman, P.L., Augustyn, M., Barnett, E.D., Klass, P.E., and McAlister Groves, B. (2000). War
trauma experience and behavioral screening of Bosnian refugee children resettled in
Massachusetts. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 21, 255261.
Georgas, J. (2003). Cross-cultural psychology, intelligence, and cognitive processes. In Georgas,
J., Weiss, L.G., Van de Vijver, F.J.R., and Saklofske, D.H. (eds.). Culture and Childrens
Intelligence: Cross-cultural analysis of the WISC-III. (pp. 2337). San Diego CA: Academic
Press.
Georgas, J., Weiss, L.G., Van de Vijver, F.J.R., and Saklofske, D.H. (2003a). A cross-cultural
analysis of the WISC-III. In Georgas, J., Weiss, L.G., Van de Vijver, F.J.R., and Saklofske,
D.H. (eds.). Culture and Childrens Intelligence: cross-cultural analysis of the WISC-III. (pp.
277 313). San Diego CA: Academic Press.
Georgas, J., Weiss, L.G., Van de Vijver, F.J.R., and Saklofske, D.H. (eds.). (2003b). Culture and
Childrens Intelligence: cross-cultural analysis of the WISC-III. San Diego CA: Academic Press.
German, D.J. (1986). Test of Word Finding. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.
Golden, C.J. (1981). The Luria-Nebraska Childrens Battery: Theory and formulation. In G.W.
Hynd and J. E. Obrzut (eds.), Neuropsychological assessment and the school-aged child:
Issues and procedures (pp. 277302). Orlando, FL: Grune and Stratton.
Golden, C.J. (1986). Manual for the Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery: Childrens revi-
sion. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
Goldstein, K. (1919). Die behandlund, frsorge und begutachtung der hirnverletzen. Zugleich ein
beitrag zur verwendung psychologischer methoden in der klinik. Leipzig: F.C.W. Vogel.
Goldstein, K. (1942). After effects of brain injuries in war: Their evaluation and treatment; the
application of psychologic methods in the clinic. New York: Grune and Straton.
Goldstein, K., and Reichmann, F. (1920). ber praktische und theoretische ergebnisse aus
den erfahrungen an hirnschu verletzten. Ergebnisse der inneren Medizin und Kinderheilkunde, 18,
405530.
Golomb, M. R., Hune, S., MacGregor, D.L., and deVeber, G. (2003). Alternative therapy use by
Chinese-Canadian children with stroke and cerebrovascular disease. Journal of Child
Neurology, 18, 714717.
194 References
Gomez-Tortosa, E., Martin, E., Gaviria, M., Charbel, F., and Ausman, J. (1995). Selective deficit
of one language in a bilingual patient following surgery in the left perysylvian area. Brain and
Language, 48, 320325.
Gonzalez, J.J. (2001). Pediatric assessment. In: Pontn, M.O., and Len-Carrin, J. (eds.)
Neuropsychology and the Hispanic patient (pp. 105136). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Goodglass, H., and Kaplan, E. (1983a). Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination. Philadelphia:
Lea & Febiger.
Goodglass, H., and Kaplan, E. (1983b). The assessment of aphasia and related disorders (2nd ed.).
Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.
Green, B., Korol, M., Grace, M., Vary, M., Leonard, A., Gleser, G., and Smitson-Cohen, S. (1991).
Children and disaster: Age, gender and parental effects on PTSD symptoms. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 945951.
Greenbaum, C.W., Erlich, C., and Toubiana, Y.H. (1993). Settler children and the Gulf War. In
L.A. Leavitt and N.A. Fox (eds.), The psychological effects of war and violence on children
(pp. 109130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Groce, N.E., and Zola, I.K. (1993). Multiculturalism, Chronic Illness, and Disability. Pediatrics,
91, 10481055.
Grossman, F.D. (1978). The effect of an examinees reported academic achievement and/or
physical condition on examiners scoring and of the WISC-R Verbal IQ. Dissertation Abstracts
International, 38, 4091A. (University Microfilms No. 7728,462).
Guarnaccia, P.J., and Lopez, S. (1998). The mental health adjustment of immigrant and refugee
children. The Child Psychiatrist in the Community, 7, 537553.
Guarnaccia, P.J., and Rogler, L. H. (1999). Research on culture-bound syndromes: New directions.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 13221327.
Guarnaccia, P.J., Lewis-Fernandez, R., and Marano, M.R. (2003). Toward a Puerto Rican popular
nosology: Nervios and Ataque de Nervios. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 27, 339366.
Guilford, J.P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Gurdjian, E.S. (1973). Head injuries from antiquities to the present with special reference to pen-
etrating head wounds. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas Publisher.
Hakuta, K., Ferdman, B.M., and Diaz, R.M. (1987). Bilingualism and cognitive development:
Three perspectives. In S. Rosenberg (ed.), Advances in applied psycholinguistics: Vol. 2.
Reading, writing, and language learning (pp. 284319). New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Hale, J. B., and Fiorello, C. (2002). Beyond the academic rhetoric of g: Intelligence testing guide-
lines for practitioners, part I. National Association of School Psychologists.
Retrieved March 25, 2003, from http://www.nasponline.org/ publications/cq312beyondg.html.
Hale, J.B., Fiorello, C.A., Kavanaugh, J.A., Hoeppner, J.B., and Gaither, R.A. (2001). WISC-III
predictors of academic achievement for children with learning disabilities: Are global and
factor scores comparable? School Psychology Quarterly, 16, 3155.
Haley, S., Coster, W., Ludlow, L., Haltiwanger, J. and Andrellos, P. (1992). Pediatric evaluation
of disability inventory (PEDI). Boston: New England Medical Center Hospital and PEDI
Research Group.
Hall, C.C.I. (1997). Cultural malpractice: The growing obsolescence of psychology with the
changing U.S. population. American Psychologist, 52, 642651.
Halstead, W.C., and Wepman, J.M. (1959). The Halstead-Wepman Aphasia Screening Test.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 14, 915.
Hamayan, E.V., and Damico, J.S. (1991). Developing and using a second language. In E.V.
Hamayan and J.S. Damico (eds.), Limiting bias in the assessment of bilingual students (pp.
3975). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Hamers, J.F., and Blanc M.H.A. (1989). Bilinguality and Bilingualism. Cambridge, England:
Cambridge University Press.
Hamilton, N., and Chinchilla, N. (1991). Central American migration: A framework for analysis.
Latin American Research Review, 26, 75110.
References 195
Handwerker, W.P. (2002). The construct validity of cultures: Cultural diversity, culture theory, and
a method for ethnography. American Anthropologist, 104, 106122.
Hanks, R., Wood, D., Millis, S., Harrison-Felix, C., Pierce, C., Rosenthal, M., Bushnik, T., High
Jr., W., and Kreutzer, J.S. (2003). Violent traumatic brain injury: Occurrence, patient charac-
teristics, and risk factors from the traumatic brain injury model systems project. Archives of
Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 84, 249254.
Hannay, H.J., Bieliauskas, L.A., Crosson, B.A., Hammake, T.A., Hamsher, K. deS, and Koffler,
S.P. (1998). Proceedings of the Houston Conference on Specialty Training in Clinical
Neuropsychology: Policy Statement. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13, 160166.
Harris, G. (1919). The redemption of the disabled: A study of the programmes of rehabilitation for
the disabled of war and of industry. London: D. Appleton Publisher.
Harris, J.G., and Llorente, A.M. (2005). Cultural considerations in the use of the Wechsler
Intelligence Scale for Children - Fourth Edition (WISC-IV). In A. Prifitera, D.H. Saklofske,
and L.G. Weiss (eds.), WISC-IV Clinical Use and Interpretation Scientist-Practioner
Perspectives (pp. 382413). Burlington, MA: Elsevier Academic Press.
Harris, J.G., Echemenda, R., Ardila, A., and Rosselli, M. (2001). Cross-cultural cognitive and
neuropsychological assessment. In J.W. Andrews, H. Janzen, and D. Saklofske (eds.), Ability,
achievement, and behavioral assessment. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Harris, J.G., Tulsky D.S., and Schultheis, M.T. (2003). Assessment of the non-native English
speaker: assimilating history and research findings to guide clinical practice. In D.S. Tusky,
D.H. Saklofske, G.J. Chelune, R.J. Heaton, R.J. Ivnik, R. Bornstein et al. (eds.), Clinical
Interpretation of the WAIS-III and WMS-III. (pp. 343390). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Harrison, C., Newton, C.N., Hall, K., & Kreutzer, J.S. (1996). Deseriptive findings from the
Traumatic Injury Model Systems National Data Base. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation,
11, 114.
Harrison, P.L., and Oakland, T. (2003). Adaptive Behavior Assessment System (2nd ed.). San
Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Hart, A.C. (ed.), and Hopkins, C.A. (2002). International Classification of Diseases Ninth
Revision Clinical Modification (9th ed.). Reston, VA: St. Anthony Publishing.
Heaton, R.K., Grant, I., and Matthews, C. (1986). Differences in neuropsychological test performance
associated with age, education, and sex. In I. Grant and K.M. Adams (eds.), Neuropsychological
assessment of neuropsychiatric disorders (pp.100120). New York: Oxford University Press.
Hebb, D.O. (1949). Organization of behavior. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.
Hehir, T. (2002). Eliminating ableism in education. Harvard Educational Review, 72, 131.
Boston: Harvard Education Publishing Group.
Helms, J.E. (1992). Why is there no study of cultural equivalence in standardized cognitive ability
testing? American Psychologist, 47, 10831101.
Heptinstall, E., Sethna, V., and Taylor, E. (2004). PTSD and depression in refugee children:
Association with pre-immigration trauma and post-migration stress. European Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, 373380.
Hernandez, A.E., Dapretto, M., Mazziotta, J., and Bookheimer, S. (2001). Language switching
and language representation in Spanish-English bilinguals: An fMRI study. Neuroimage,
14, 510520.
Herrnstein, R.J., and Murray, C. (1996). The bell curve: Intelligence and class structure in
American life. Simon & Schuster Adult Publishing Group.
Hicks, R., Lalonde, R.N., and Pepler, D. (1993). Psychosocial considerations in the mental health of
immigrant and refugee children. Canadian Journal of Community Mental Health, 12, 7187.
Higgs, T.V. (1985). Language acquisition and language learning: A plea for syncretism. Modern
Language Journal, 69, 814.
Hines, T.M. (1996). Failure to demonstrate selective deficit in the native language following sur-
gery in the left perisylvian area. Brain and Language, 54, 168169.
Hjern, A., Angel, B., and Hoejer, B. (1991). Persecution and behavior: A report of refugee chil-
dren from Chile. Child Abuse and Neglect, 15, 239248.
196 References
Hjern, A., Angel, B., and Jeppson, O. (1998). Political violence, family stress and mental health
of refugee children in exile. Scandinavian Journal of Social Medicine, 26, 1825.
Hodes, M. (1998). Refugee children may need a lot of help (Editorial). British Medical Journal,
316, 793725.
Hodes, M. (2000). Psychologically distressed refugee children in the United Kingdom. Child
Psychology and Psychiatry Review, 5, 5768.
Holden, C. (1996). Small refugees suffer the effects of early neglect. Science, 274, 10761077.
Hollingshead, A.B. (1957). Two factor index of social position. Unpublished manuscript,
Department of Sociology, Yale University. New Haven, CT.
Hollingshead, A.B., and Redlich, F.C. (1958). Social class and mental illness: A community study.
New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.
Holm, A., Dodd, B., Stow, C., and Pert, S. (1999). Identification and differential diagnosis of
phonological disorder in bilingual children. Language Testing, 16, 271292.
Holmbeck, G.N. (1997). Toward terminological, conceptual, and statistical clarity in the study of
mediators and moderators: Examples from the child-clinical and pediatric psychology litera-
tures. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 599610.
Horn, J.L. (1967). Intelligence Why it grows, why it declines. Trans-action, 5, 2331.
Horn, J.L. (1979). Trends in the measurement of intelligence. Intelligence, 3, 229239.
Horn, J.L. (1989). Cognitive diversity: A framework of learning. In P.L. Ackerman, R.J. Sternberg,
and R. Glaser (eds.), Learning and individual differences (pp. 61116). New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Howard, M., and Hodes, M. (2000). Psychopathology, adversity and service utilization of young
refugees. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 368377.
Hubbard, J. Realmuto, G.M., Northwood, A.K., and Masten, A.S. (1995). Comorbidity of psychi-
atric diagnoses with posttraumatic stress disorder in survivors of childhood trauma. Journal of
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 11671173.
Human Rights and Equal Opportunity Commission. (2001). Face the facts: Some questions and
answers about immigration, refugees and indigenous affairs. Sydney: Human Rights and
Equal Opportunity Commission, Australia.
Hyman, I., Vu, N., and Beiser, M. (2000). Post-migration stresses among Southeast Asian refugees
youth in Canada: A research note. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 31, 281293.
International Dyslexia Association (IDA). (May 2000). Dyslexia basics. Fact Sheet #962.
Irvine, S.H., and Berry, J.W. (eds.) (1988). Human Abilities in Cultural Context. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Jalali, B. (1988). Ethnicity, cultural adjustment, and behavior: Implications for family therapy. In
L. Comas-Diaz and E.E.H. Griffith (eds.), Clinical guidelines in crosscultural mental health
(pp. 932). New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.
Jarvis, E. (1866). Influence of distance from and nearness to an insane hospital on its use by the
people. American Journal of Insanity, 22, 361406.
Jensen, A. (1980). Bias in mental testing. New York: Free Press.
Jensen, A.R., (1979). Cumulative deficit in IQ of blacks in the rural south. In L. Willerman,
and R.G. Turner (Eds.), Readings about individual and group differences (pp. 8391).
San Francisco; W.H. Freeman and Company.
Johnston, J.R. (1982). Narratives: A new look at communication problems in older language dis-
ordered children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 13, 144155.
Johnstone, B., Holland, D., and Larimore, C. (2000). Language and academic abilities. In G.
Groth-Marnat (Ed.), Neuropsychological assessment in clinical practice: A guide to test inter-
pretation and integration (pp. 335354). New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.
Jones, R.L. (1996) (ed.). Handbook of Tests and Measurements for Black Populations. Hampton,
VA: Cobb and Henry.
Joseph, R. (1996). Neuropsychiatry, neuropsychology, and clinical neuroscience: Emotion, evolu-
tion, cognition, language, memory, brain damage, and abnormal behavior (2nd ed.).
Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.
References 197
Joshhi, M.R., Dahlgren, M., and Boulware-Gooden, R. (2002). Teaching reading in an inner city
school through a multisensory teaching approach. Annals of Dyslexia, 55, 229242.
Junque, C., Vendrell, P., and Vendrell, J. (1995). Differential impairments and specific phenomena
in 50 Catalan-Spanish bilingual aphasic patients. In M. Paradis (ed.), Aspects of Bilingual
Aphasia. Oxford: Pergamon Pres.
Jupp, J.J., and Luckey, J. (1990). Educational experiences in Australia of Indo-Chinese adolescent
refugees. International Journal of Mental Health, 18(4), 7891.
Kalat, J.W. (1998). Biological psychology (6th ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing
Company.
Kamphaus, R.W. (1993). Clinical assessment of childrens intelligence: A handbook for profes-
sional practice. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Kamphaus, R.W., Petoskey, M.D., and Rowe, E.W. (2000). Current trends in psychological testing
of children. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 31, 155164.
Kaplan, E. (1996). A process approach to neuropsychological assessment. In M. Dennis, E.
Kaplan, M. Posner, D. Stein, and R. Thompson (eds.), Clinical Neuropsychology and Brain
Function: Research, Measurement, and Practice. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Kaplan, E., Fein, D., Kramer, J., Delis, D., and Morris, R. (1999). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Children Third Edition, PI. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., and Weintraub, S. (1983). Boston Naming Test (Revised 60-item
version). Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.
Kaplan, R.M., and Saccuzzo, D.P. (1997). Psychological testing: Principles, application, and
issues. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company.
Kaufman, A.S., and Flanagan, D.P. (2004). Essentials of WISC IV assessment. New York:
Wiley.
Kaufman, A.S., and Kaufman, N.L. (1990). Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT). Circle Pines,
MN: American Guidance Service.
Kaufman, A.S., and Kaufman, N.L. (1993). Kaufman Adolescent and Adult Intelligence Test
(KAIT). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Kaufman, A.S. (1994). Intelligent testing with the WISC-III. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Kaufman, D.M. (2001). Clinical neurology for psychiatrists (5th ed.).Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders
Company.
Kay, J., and Ellis, A. (1987). A cognitive neuropsychological case study of anomia. Brain,
110, 613629.
Kayser, H. (1993). Hispanic cultures. In D. Battle (ed.), Communication disorders in multicultural
populations (pp. 114157). Boston: Andover Medical Publishers.
Keith, T.Z., Quirk, K.J., Schartzer, C., and Elliott, C.D. (1999). Construct bias in the Differential
Ability Scales? Confirmatory and hierarchical factor structure across three ethnic groups.
Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 17, 249268.
Kemeny, M.E., and Gruenewald, T.L. (2000). Affect, cognition, the immune system and health. In
E.A. Mayer and C. Saper, (eds.), The Biological Basis for Mind Body Interactions. Progress
in Brain Research Series (pp. 291308). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science B.V.
Kennepohl, S. (1999). Toward a cultural neuropsychology: An alternative view and a preliminary
model. Brain and Cognition, 41, 365380.
Kessler, R.C., and Cleary, P.D. (1980). Social class and psychological distress. American
Sociological Review, 45, 463478.
Kinzie, J.D. (1988). The psychiatric effects of massive trauma on Cambodian refugees. In: Wilson
J.P., Harel Z., Kahana B., eds. Human adaptation of extreme stress: From the Holocaust to
Vietnam (pp. 305317). New York: Plenum Press.
Kinzie, J.D., and Sack, W. (1991). Severely traumatized Cambodian children: Research findings
and clinical implications. In F.L. Ahearn and J.L. Athey (eds.), Refugee children: Theory,
research and services. The Johns Hopkins series in contemporary medicine and public health
(pp. 92105). Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press.
198 References
Kinzie, J.D., Boehnlein, J.K., Leung, P.K., and Moore, L.J., Riley, C., and Smith, D. (1990). The
prevalence of posttraumatic stress disorder and its clinical significance among Southeast Asian
refugees. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 913917.
Kinzie, J.D., Sack, W., Angell, R., and Clarke, G. (1989). A three-year follow-up of Cambodian
young people traumatized as children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 501504.
Kinzie, J.D., Sack, W.H., Angell, R.H., Manson, S.M and Ben, R. (1986). The psychiatric effects
of massive trauma on Cambodian children: I. The children. Journal of the American Academy
of Child Psychiatry, 25, 370376.
Kirk, R.E. (1990). Experimental design: Procedures for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.).
Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing.
Kleim, J.A., Swain, R.A., Czerlanis, C.M., Kelly, J.L., Pipitone, M.A., and Greenough, W.T.
(1997). Learning-dependent dendritic hypertrophy of cerebellarstellate neurons: Plasticity of
local circuit neurons. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 67, 2933.
Klingman, A. (1994). Childrens response to the Gulf War: Assessment via ordinal and nominal
quantification of compositions. School Psychology International, 15, 235246.
Kocijan-Hercigonja, D., Rijavec, M., and Hercigonja, V. (1998). Mental health condition
and adjustment of refugee and displaced children in a war area. Psychiatria Danubina,
10, 2329.
Kocijan-Hercigonja, D., Rijavec, M., Marusic, A., and Hercigonja, V. (1997). Coping strategies of refu-
gee, displaced, and non displaced children in a war area. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 52, 4550.
Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, M.A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds,
understanding actions: Action representation in auditory mirror neurons. Science, 297, 846848.
Kohn, M.L. (1973). Social class and schizophrenia: A critical review and reformulation.
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 7, 6079.
Kolb, B., and Fantie, B. (1997). Development of the childs brain and behavior. In C.R. Reynolds
and E. Fletcher-Janzen (eds.), Handbook of clinical child neuropsychology (pp. 1741).
New York: Plenum Press.
Korkman, M., Kirk, U., and Kemp, S. (1998). NEPSY: A Developmental Neuropsychological
Assessment. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and practice in second language acquisition. New York: Prentice Hall.
Kraus, J.F., Fife, D., Ramstein, K., Conroy, C., and Cox, P. (1986). The relationship of family
income to the incidence, external causes, and outcomes of serious brain injury, San Diego
County, California. American Journal of Public Health, 76, 13451347.
Kress, G. (2003). Literacy in the new media age. London: Routledge.
Krupinski, J., and Burrows, G. (eds.). (1986). The Price of Freedom: Young Indochinese Refugees
in Australia. Sydney: Pergamon Press.
Kuhn, T.S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions (1st ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Kuhn, T.S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd. ed.). Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Kuhn, T.S. (1996). The structure of scientific revolutions (3rd ed.). Chicago: The University of
Chicago Press.
Kuper, A. (1999). Culture: The anthropologists account. Harvard University Press.
Kwak, K. (2003). South Korea. In J. Georgas, L. Weiss, F.J.R van de Vijver and D.H. Saklofske
(eds.), Culture and childrens intelligence: Cross cultural analysis of the WISC-III (pp. 227
240). London: Academic Press.
LaCalle, J. (1987). Forensic psychological evaluations through an interpreter: Legal and ethical
issues. American Journal of Forensic Psychology, 5, 2943.
Lacelle-Peterson, M.W., and Rivera, C. (1994). Is it for all kids? A framework for equitable
assessment policies for English language learners. Harvard Educational Review, 64, 5575.
Lai, C.S.L. et al. (2001). A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language dis-
order. Nature, 413, 519523.
References 199
Lambert, N., Nihira, K., and Leland, H. (1993). AAMR Adaptive Behavior Scale School (2nd
ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Lankshear, C., and Knobel, M. (2003). New literacies: Changing knowledge and classroom learn-
ing. Buckingham: Open University Press.
Laor, N., Wolmer, L., Mayes, L.C., and Gershon, A. (1997). Israeli preschool children under
scuds: A 30-month follow-up. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 36, 349356.
Laor, N., Wolmer, L., Mayes, L.C., and Golomb, A., Silverberg, D.S., Weizman, R., and Cohen,
D.J. (1996). Israeli preschoolers under Scud missile attacks: A developmental perspective on
risk-modifying factors. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 416423.
Laosa, L.M. (1984). Ethnic, socioeconomic and home language influences upon early perform-
ance on measures of abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 11781198.
Laosa, L.M. (1996). Intelligence testing and social policy. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology, 17, 155173.
LaRue, A., Romano, L.J., Ortiz, I.E., Liang, H.C., and Lindeman, R.D. (1999). Neuropsychological
performance of Hispanic and non-Hispanic older adults: An epidemiologic survey. Clinical
Neuropsychologist, 13, 474486.
Lashley, K.S. (1938). Factors limiting recovery after central nervous system lesions. Journal of
Nervous and Mental Disease, 88, 733755.
Laurel, F., and Zimmerman, M. (2001). Posttraumatic stress disorder and major depressive disor-
der: Investigating the role of overlapping symptoms in diagnostic comorbidity. Journal of
Nervous and Mental Diseases, 189, 548551.
Law, J. (1992). Factors associated with language impairment. In J. Law (ed.), The early identifica-
tion of language impairment in children: Therapy in practice (pp. 4162). London: Chapman
and Hall.
LeFranois, G.R. (1995). Of children: An introduction to child development (8th ed.). Belmont,
CA: Wadsworth Publishing Company.
Leonard, L.B. (1999). The study of language acquisition across languages. In O.L. Taylor and
L.B. Leonard (eds.), Language acquisition across North America: Cross-cultural and cross-
linguistic perspectives (pp. 318). San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group, Inc.
Len-Carrin, J. (1989). Trail Making Test scores for normal children: Normative data from
Spain. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 68, 627630.
Len-Carrin, J. (1997). A historical view of neuropsychological rehabilitation: The search for
human dignity. In J. Len-Carrin (ed.), Neuropsychological rehabilitation: Fundamentals,
innovations, and directions (pp. 339). Delray Beach, FL: GR/St. Lucie Press.
Lezak, M.D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Lezak, M.D., Howieson, D.B., and Loring, D.W. (2004). Neuorpsychological Assessment
(4th Ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Li, S. (2003). Biolcultural orchestration of developmental plasticity across levels: The interplay of
biology and culture shaping the mind and behavior across the life span. Psychology Bulletin,
129 (2), 171194.
Llorente, A.M. (August, 1997). Neuropsychologic assessment of Hispanic populations: The influ-
ence of immigration on assessment. Paper presented at the 105th Annual Convention of the
American Psychological Association, Chicago, IL.
Llorente, A.M. (2000). Evaluation of developmental neurocognitive and neurobehavioral changes
associated with pesticide exposure: Recommendations for the U.S. Environmental Protection
Agency on the assessment of health effects of pesticide exposure in infants and young children.
In D. Otto, R. Calderon, P. Mendola, and E. Hilborn (eds.), Assessment of Health Effects of
Pesticide Exposure in Young Children (pp. 2232). Research Triangle Park, NC: Environmental
Protection Agency (EPA/600/R-99/086).
Llorente, A.M. (June, 2004). Psychological issues affecting unaccompanied immigrant
children. Invited presentation at the 11th National Conference on children and and the law.
Washington, DC.
200 References
Llorente, A.M., Cassatta, A., Perez, L., Sines, M. (unpublished manuscript). Not the same old
thing: Alterations in test stimuli across five decades on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Children and its probable impact on performance with Hispanic children in three case
studies.
Llorente, A.M., LoPresti, C.M., Guzzard. C., Satz, P., and Evans, G. (2000a). HIV-1 infection
spectrum disease: Neuropsychological manifestations and cross-cultural considerations in
adulthood, adolescence, and childhood. In E. Fletcher-Janzen, T. Strickland, and C.R.
Reynolds (eds.), Handbook of Cross-Cultural Neuropsychology (pp. 215246). New York:
Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
Llorente, A.M., Amado, A., Voigt, R.G., Berretta, M.C., Fraley, K.A., Jensen, C.L., & Heird, W.L.
(2001a). Internal consistency, temporal stability, and reproductivity of individual index scores
of the Test of Variables of Attention (T.O.V.A) in children with attention-deficit, hyperactivity
disorder (AD/HD). Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, 535546.
Llorente, A.M., LoPresti, C.E., Levy, J.K., and Fernandez, F. (2001). Neuropsychological and
neurobehavioral correlates associated with HIV infection: Assessment considerations with
Hispanic populations. In M. Pontn and J. Len-Carrin (eds.), Neuropsychology and the
Hispanic Patient (pp. 209242). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.
Llorente, A.M., Pontn, M.O., Taussig, I.M., and Satz, P. (1999). Patterns of American immigra-
tion and their influence on the acquisition of neuropsychological norms for Hispanics.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 603614.
Llorente, A.M., Taussig, I.M., Perez, L., and Satz, P. (2000). Trends in American immigration:
Influences on neuropsychological assessment and inferences with ethnic-minority popula-
tions. In E. Fletcher-Janzen, T. Strickland, and C.R. Reynolds (eds.), Handbook of Cross-
Cultural Neuropsychology (pp. 345359). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
Llorente, A.M., Williams, J., DElia, L., Satz, P. (2003). Childrens Color Trails Test 1 and 2
Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc (PAR).
Lopez Cardozo, B., Vergara, A., Agani, F., and Gotway, C.A. (2000). Mental health, social func-
tioning and attitudes of Kosovar Albanians following the war in Kosovo. Journal of the
American Medical Association, 284, 569.
Lopez, S.R., and Taussig, I.M. (1991). Cognitive-intellectual functioning of Spanish-speaking
impaired and nonimpaired Elderly: Implications for culturally sensitive assessment.
Psychological Assessment, 3, 448454.
Losen, D.J., and Orfield, G. (2002). Introduction: Racial inequity in special education. In D. Losen
and G. Orfield (Eds.), Racial inequity in special education (p. 124). Boston: Harvard
Education Publishing.
Loughry, M., and Flouri, E. (2001). The behavioral and emotional problems of former unaccompanied
refugee children 34 years after their return to Vietnam. Child Abuse and Neglect, 25, 249263.
Lu, F.G., Lim, R.F., and Mezzich, J.E. (1995). Issues in the assessment and diagnosis of culturally
diverse individuals. In J. Oldham and M. Riba (eds.), Review of Psychiatry, 14, 477510.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Lukman, B., and Bach-Mortensen, N. (1995). Symptoms in children of torture victims: Post
traumatic stress disorders? World Pediatrics and Child Care, 5, 3242.
Luria, A.R. (1976). Cultural Development: Its Cultural and Social Foundations Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Luria, A.R. (1979). The making of mind: A personal account of Soviet Psychology. In M. Cole and
S. Cole (eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Luria, A.R. (1966). Higher Cortical Functions in Man. London: Tavistock.
Macksoud, M.S., and Aber, J.L. (1996). The war experiences and psychosocial development of
children in Lebanon. Child Development, 67, 7088.
MacMillan, D.L., and Speece, D.L. (1999). Utility of current diagnostic categories for research
and practice. In R. Galllimore, L.P. Bernheimer, D.L. MacMillan, D.L. Speece, and S. Vaughn
(eds.), Developmental perspectives on children with high incidence disabilities (pp. 111133).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.
References 201
Malgady, R.G., Rogler, L.H., and Tryon, W.W. (1992). Issues of validity in the Diagnostic
Interview Schedule. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 26, 5967.
Malzberg, B., and Lee, E.S. (1956). Migration and mental disease: A study of first admissions to hos-
pitals for mental disease, New York, 19391941. New York: Social Science Research Council.
Mandal, M.K, Ida, Y., Harizuka, S., and Upadhaya, N. (1999). Cultural difference in hand prefer-
ence: Evidence from India and Japan. International Journal of Psychology, 34, 5966.
Marin G., and Marin, B.V. (1991). Research with Hispanic populations. Newbury Park, CA:
Sage.
Marin, G., Sabogal, F., Marin, B., and Otero-Sabogal, R. (1984). Development of a short accul-
turation scale for Hispanics. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 9, 183205.
Markowitz, F. (1996). Living in limbo: Bosnian Muslim refugees in Israel. Human Organization,
55, 127132.
Marlowe, W.B. (2000). Multicultural perspectives on the neuropsychological assessment of chil-
dren and adolescents. In E. Fletcher-Janzen, T.L. Strickland, and C.R. Reynolds (eds.),
Handbook of cross-cultural neuropsychology (pp. 145165). New York: Kluwer Academic/
Plenum Publishers.
Masser, D.S. (1992). Psychosocial functioning of Central American refugee children. Child
Welfare, 71(5), 439456.
Masterman, M. (1974). The nature of a paradigm. In I. Lakatos and A. Musgrave (eds.), Criticism
and the growth of knowledge (pp. 5889). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Mather, N., and Woodcock, R.W. (2001). Application of the Woodcock-Johnson Tests
of Cognitive Ability-Revised to the diagnosis of learning disabilities. In A.S. Kaufman and
N.L. Kaufman (eds.), Specific learning disabilities and difficulties in children and Adolescents
(pp. 5596). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Matthey, S., Silove, D., Barnett, B., Fitzgerald, M.H., and Mitchell, P. (1999). Correlates of
depression and PTSD in Cambodian women with young children: A pilot study. Stress
Medicine, 15, 103107.
McCaffrey, R.J., Palav, A.A., OBryant, S.E., and Labarge, A.S. (2003). A brief overview of base
rates. In A.E. Puente and C.R. Reynolds (eds.), Critical issues in neuropsychology (pp. 19).
New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
McCallin, M. (1992). Living in detention: A review of the Psychological wellbeing of Vietnamese
children in the Hong Kong detention centers. Geneva: International Catholic Child Bureau.
McCloskey, L.A., and Southwick, K. (1996). Psychosocial problems in refugee children exposed
to war. Pediatrics, 97, 39439.
McCloskey, L.A., Southwick, K., Fernandez-Esquer, M.E., and Locke, C. (1996). The psychologi-
cal effects of political and domestic violence on Central American and Mexican immigrant
mothers and children. Journal of Community Psychology, 23, 95116.
McGrew, K.S., and Woodcock, R.W. (2001). Technical Manual. Woodcock-Johnson III. Itasca, IL:
Riverside Publishing.
McNamara, K.M., and Hollinger, C.L. (1997). Intervention-based assessment: Rates of evaluation
and eligibility for specific learning disability classification. Psychological Reports, 81, 620622.
McKelvey, R.S., and Webb, J.A. (1997). A prospective study of psychological stress related to
refugee camp experience. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 31, 549554.
McKenzie, K.J., and Crowcroft, N.S. (1994). Race, ethnicity, culture, and science. British Medical
Journal, 39, 286287.
Mejia, S., Gutierrez, L.M., Villa, A.R., and Ostrosky-Solis (2004). Cognition, functional status,
education, and the diagnosis of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in Spanish-speaking
elderly. Applied Neuropsychology, 11, 194201.
Melendez, F. (2001). Forensic assessment of Hispanics. In M.O. Pontn & J. Len-Carrin (Eds.),
Neuropsychology and the Hispanic Patient: A Clinical Handbook (pp. 321340). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.
Melville, M.B., and Lykes, M.B. (1992). Guatemalan Indian children and the sociocultural effects
of government-sponsored terrorism. Social Science and Medicine, 34, 533548.
202 References
Mercer, J., and Lewis, J. F. (1978). System of Multi-Pluralistic Assessment (SOMPA). San
Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Mezzich, J. E., and Lewis-Fernandez, R. (1997). Cultural considerations in psychopathology. In A.
Tasman, J. Kay, and J.A. Lieberman (eds.), Psychiatry (pp. 563571). Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders.
Mghir, R., Freed, W., Raskin, A., and Katon, W. (1995). Depression and posttraumatic stress
disorder among a community sample of adolescent and young adult Afghan refugees. Journal
of Nervous and Mental Disease, 183, 2430.
Miller, K.E. (1996). The effects of state terrorism and exile on indigenous Guatemalan refugee
children: A mental health assessment and an analysis of childrens narratives. Child
Development, 67, 89106.
Minicucci, C., and Olsen, L. (Spring, 1992). Programs for secondary limited English proficient
students: A California study. (Occasinal Papers in Bilingual Education, No. 5). Washington,
DC: National Clearinghouse for Bilingual Education.
Mitrushina, M.N., Boone, K.B., DElia, K.F., and DElia, L. (1999). Handbook of normative data
for neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.
Mollica, R.F., Poole, C., Son, L., Murray, C.C., and Tor, S. (1997). Effects of war trauma on
Cambodian refugee adolescents functional health and mental health status. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent-Psychiatry, 36, 10981106.
Montgomery, E. (1998). Refugee children from the Middle East. Scandinavian Journal of Social
Medicine, Supplement 4.
Montgomery, G.T., and Orozlo, S. (1984). Validation of a measure of acculturation for Mexican
Americans. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 6, 5363.
Morrow, R.D. (1994). Immigration, refugee and generation status as related to behavioral disor-
ders. In R.L Peterson and S. Ishii-Jordan (eds.), Multicultural issues in the education of stu-
dents with behavioral disorders (pp. 196207). Cambridge, MA, US: Brookline Books.
Mosely, M.E. (1993). The Incas and their ancestors: The archeology of Peru. New York: Thames
and Hudson.
Moss, E., Davidson, R.J. and Saron, C. (1985). Cross-cultural differences in hemisphericity: EEG
asymmetry discriminates between Japanese and Westerners. Neuropsychologia, 23, 131135.
Mullen, E.M. (1995). Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
Service.
Mungas, D. (1996). The process of development of valid and reliable neuropsychological
assessment measures for English and Spanish-speaking elderly persons. I.G. Yeo and
D. Gallagher-Thompson (eds.), Ethnicity and the dementias (pp. 3346). Washington, DC:
Taylor and Francis.
Mungas, D., Reed, B.R., Crane, P.K., Haan, M.N., and Gonzalez, H. (2004). Spanish and English
neuropsychological assessment scales (SENAS): Further development and psychometric
characteristics. Psychological Assessment, 16, 347359.
Mungas, D., Reed, B.R., Haan, M.N., and Gonzalez, H. (2005) Spanish and English
Neuropsychological Assessment Scales: Relationship to demographics, language, cognition,
and independent functioning. Neuropsychology, 19, 466475.
Muiz, J., and Hambleton, R.K. (1996). Directrices para la tradicin y adapcin de los tests.
Papeles del Psicologo, 66, 6370.
Muoz-Sandoval, A.F., Cummins, J., Alvarado, C.G., and Ruef, M.L. (1998). Bilingual Verbal
Abilities Test. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Muoz-Sandoval, A., Woodcock, R.W., McGrew, K.S., and Mather, N. (2005) Bateria III
Woodcock-Munoz. Itasca, IL: Riverside.
Mushi, S.L.P. (2002). Simultaneous and successive second language learning: Integral ingredients
of the human development process. Early Child Development and Care, 172, 349358.
Nagel, J. (1994). Constructing ethnicity: Creating and recreating ethnic identity and culture.
Social Problems, 41, 152176.
Naglieri, J.A. (1997). Naglieri Nonverbal Ability Test. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
Corporation.
References 203
Naglieri, J.A., and Ronning, M.E. (2000). Comparison of White, African American, Hispanic, and
Asian children on the Naglieri Nonverbal Ability Test. Journal of Psychoeducational
Assessment, 18, 230239.
Naglieri, J.A., Booth, A.L., and Winsler, A. (2004). Comparison of Hispanic children with and
without limited English proficiency on the Naglieri nonverbal ability test. Psychological
Assessment, 16, 8184.
National Center for Education Statistics (February 2000). Racial and ethnic distribution of ele-
mentary and secondary students [on-line]. Available: http://www.nces.ed.gov/edstats/
Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T.J., Boykin, A.W., Brody, N., Ceci, S.J., Halpern, D.F.,
Loehlin, J.C., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R.J., and Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and
unknowns. American Psychologist, 55, 77101.
Nell, V. (2000). Cross-cultural neuropsychological assessment: Theory and Practice. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Oakes, J. (1990). Multiplying inequalities: The effects of race social class, and tracking on oppor-
tunities to learn mathematics and science. Santa Monica, CA: RAND.
Obradovic, B., Kanazir, V., Zalisevskij, G., Popadic, K., and Simic, I. (1993). A threat to mental
health of children and young people in exile. Psihijat dan, 25, 9198.
Ochoa, S.H., Rivera, B., and Ford, L. (1997). An investigation of school psychology training per-
taining to bilingual psych-educational assessment of primarily Hispanic students: Twenty-five
years after Diana v. California. Journal of School Psychology, 35, 329349.
degaard, . (1932). Emigration and insanity: A study of mental disease among the Norwegian-
born population of Minnesota. Acta Psychiatrica et Neurologica, 4, 1206, Supplement.
Office of Special Education Programs (2001). Twenty-third annual report to congress on the
implementation of the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act. Section II, Sudent
Characteristics (pp. 2230).
Olazaran, J., Jacobs, D.M., and Stern, Y. (1996). Comparative study of visual and verbal short-
term memory in English and Spanish speakers: testing a linguistic hypothesis. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 105110.
Oller, D.K. (1986). Metaphonology and infant vocalizations. In B. Lindblom and R. Zetterstrom,
(eds.), Precursors of early speech (pp. 2135). New York: Stockton Press.
Oller, K., Eilers, R., Steffens, M., Lynch, M., and Urbano, R. (1994). Speechlike vocalizations in
infancy: An evaluation of potential risk factors. Journal of Child Language, 21, 3358.
Orton, S.T. (1937). Reading, writing, and speech problems in children. New York: W.W. Norton.
Ostrosky-Solis, F., Ramirez, M., Lozano, A., Velez, A. (2004). Culture or education?
Neuropsychological test performance of a Maya indigenous population. International Journal
of Psychology, 39, 3646
Otero, M. (2006). Bateria III Woodcock-Munoz (Bateria III). School Psychologist, 60, 8689.
Paardekooper, B., de Jong, J.T.V.M., and Hermanns, J.M.A. (1999). The psychological impact of war
and the refugee situation on South Sudanese children in refugee camps in Northern Uganda: An
exploratory study. Journal of Child Psychology, Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40, 529536.
Padilla, E.R., Roll, S., Gomez Palacio, M. (1982). The performance of Mexican children and
adolescents on the WISC-R. Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 16, 122128.
Palacio, M., G., Padilla, E.R., and Roll, S. (1984). Escala de Intelligencia Revisada para el Nivel
Escolar (WISC-RM). Mexico, D.F.: Editorial Manual Moderno.
Papageorgiou, V., Frangou-Garunovic, A., Iordanidou, R., Yule, W., Smith, P., and Vostanis, P.
(2000). War trauma and psychopathology in Bosnian refugee children. European Journal of
Adolescent Psychiatry, 9, 8490.
Paradis, M. (ed.). (1978). Aspects of bilingualism. Columbia, SC: Hornbeam Press.
Paradis, M. (1977). Bilingualism and aphasia. In H. Whitaker and H.A. Whitaker (eds.), Studies
in Neurolinguistics, Volume 3 (pp. 65121). New York: Academic Press.
Paradis, Michel (ed.). (1995). Aspects of Bilingual Aphasia. London: Pergamon Press.
Passel, J.S., Capps, R., and Fix, M. (2004). Undocumented immigrants: Facts and figures,
http://www.urban.org/uploadedpdf/1000587_undoc_immigrants_facts.pdf
204 References
Paul, P.V. (1996). First- and second-language English literacy. Volta Review, 98, 516.
Peal, E., and Lambert, W.E. (1962). The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. Psychological
Monographs, 76, 23.
Pedersen, P., and Marsella, A.J. (1982). Ethical crisis for cross cultural counseling and therapy.
Professional Psychology, 13, 492496.
Perez-Foster, R.M. (2001). When immigration is trauma: Guidelines for the individual and family
clinician. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 71, 153170.
Petitto, L.A., and Holowka, S. (2002). Evaluating attributions of delay and confusion in young
bilinguals: Special insights from infants acquiring a signed and a spoken language. Sign
Language Studies, 3, 433.
Pigatano, G.P., Ogano, M., and Amakusa B. (1997). A cross-cultural study on impaired self-
awareness in Japanese patients with brain dysfunction. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology,
Neuropsychiatry and Behavioral Neurology, 10, 135143.
Piper, T. (1993). And then there were two: Children and second language learning. Ontario:
Pippin Publishing Ltd.
Plank, G.A. (2001). Application of the cross battery approach in the assessment of American
Indian children: A viable alternative. American Indian and Alaskan Native. Mental Health
Research, 10, 2133.
Pontn, M.O., and Len-Carren, J. (Eds.) (2001). Neuropsychology and the Hispanic patient.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.
Pontn, M.O. (2001a). Hispanic culture in the United States. In: Pontn, M.O., and Len-Carrin,
J. (eds.) Neuropsychology and the Hispanic patient (pp. 1538). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum and Associates.
Pontn, M.O. (2001b). Research and assessment issues. In: Pontn, M.O., and Len-Carrin, J.
(eds.) Neuropsychology and the Hispanic patient (pp. 3958). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum and Associates.
Pontn, M.O., Gonzalez, J., and Mares, M. (1997). Rehabilitating brain damage in Hispanics. In
J. Len-Carrin (ed.), Neuropsychological rehabilitation: Fundamentals, innovations, and
directions (pp.513529). Delray Beach, FL: GR/St. Lucie Press.
Pontn, M.O., Satz, P., Herrera, L., Ortiz, F., Urrutia, C.P., Young, R., DElia, L.F., and Namerow,
N. (1996). Normative data stratified by age and education for the neuropsychological
Screening Battery for Hispanics (NeSBHIS): Initial report. Journal of the International
Neuropsychological Society, 2, 96104.
Portes, A., and Bach, R.L. (1985). Latin Journey: Cuban and Mexican Immigrants in the United
States. Berkley: University of California Press.
Portes, A., and Borocsz, J. (1989). Contemporary immigration: Theoretical perspectives on deter-
minants and modes of incorporation. International Migration Review, 23, 606630.
Portes, A., and Rumbaut, R.G. (1990). Immigrant America: A portrait. Los Angeles: University
of California Press.
Poser, U., Kohler, J.A., and Schnle, P.W. (1996). A historical review of neuropsychological reha-
bilitation in Germany. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 6, 257278.
Potocky, M. (1996). Refugee children: How are they faring economically as adults? Social Work,
41, 364373.
Prifitera, A., Saklofske, D.H., and Weiss, L.G. (2005). WISC-IV clinical use and interpretation.
San Diego: Elsevier Academic.
Prifitera, A., Weiss, L.G., and Saklofske, D.H. (1998). The WISC-III in context. In A. Prifitera,
and D.H. Saklofske (eds.), WISC-III Clinical use and interpretation: Scientist-practitioner
perspectives (pp. 138.) San Diego CA: Academic Press.
Prigratano, G.P., Ogano, M., and Amakusa, B. (1997). A cross-cultural study on impaired self-
awareness in Japanese patients with brain dysfunction. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology,
and Behavioral Neurology, 10, 135143.
Pryor, C.B. (2001). New immigrants and refugees in American schools: Multiple voices.
Childhood Education, 77(5), 275283.
References 205
Robertson, L.C., Lamb, M.R., and Knight, R.T. (1988). Effects of lesions of temporal-parietal
junction on perceptual and attentional processing in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 8,
37573769.
Rogers, R. (1997). Clinical assessment of malingering and deception (2nd ed.). New York:
Guilford Press.
Rogler, L.H. (1996). Framing research on culture in psychiatric diagnosis: The case of the DSM-
IV, Psychiatry, 59, 145155.
Rogler, L.H. (1994). International migrations: A framework for directing research. American
Psychologist, 49, 701708.
Roid, G.H. (2003). Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale (5th ed). Itasca, IL: Riverside.
Romaine, S. (1995). Bilingualism. (2nd ed.). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Rorer, L.G., and Dawes, R.M. (1982). A base-rate bootstrap. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 50, 419425.
Rosa, E.M., and Leow, R.P. (2004). Awareness, different learning conditions, and second language
development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 269292.
Rosselli, M., and Ardila, A. (2001). Normal and abnormal aging. In: Pontn, M.O., and Len-
Carrin, J. (eds.) Neuropsychology and the Hispanic patient (pp. 341360). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., and Rosas, P. (1990). Neuropsychological assessment of illiterates: II.
Language and praxic abilities. Brain and Cognition, 12, 281296.
Rothman, D.J. (1971). The discovery of the asylum. Boston: Little and Brown.
Rourke, B.P., Fisk, J.L., and Strong, J.D. (1986). Neuropsychological assessment of children:
A treatment oriented approach. New York: Guilford Press.
Rousseau, C., Drapeau, A., and Corin, E. (1996). School performance and emotional problems in
refugee children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 66, 239251.
Rousseau, C., Drapeau, A., and Corin, E. (1997). The influence of culture and context on the pre-
and post-migration experience of school aged refugees from Central America and Southeast
Asia in Canada. Social Science and Medicine, 44, 11151127.
Rousseau, C. (1993). The place of the unexpressed: Ethics and methodology for research with ref-
ugee children. Canadas Mental Health, 41, 1216.
Rousseau, C. (1995). The mental health of refugee children. Transcultural Psychiatric Research
Review, 32, 299331.
Rousseau, C., and Drapeau, A. (1998). Parent-child agreement on refugee childrens psychiatric
symptoms: A transcultural perspective. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 37, 629636.
Rousseau, C., Said, T.M., Gagne, M.J., and Bibeau, G. (1998). Resilience in unaccompanied
minors from the north of Somalia. Psychoanalytic Review, 85(4), 615637.
Roysircar, G. (2004). Acculturation and ethnic identity concerns with immigrant and international
student clients. In T.B. Smith (ed.), Practicing multiculturalism: Affirming diversity in coun-
seling and psychology (pp. 256275). New York: Allyn and Bacon.
Rudic, N., Rakic., V., Ispanovic-Radojkovic, V., Bojanin, S., and Lazic, D. (1993). Refugee
children and young people in collective accommodation. In P. Kalicanin and J. Bukelic (eds.),
The stresses of war (pp.8589). Belgrade: Institute for Mental Health.
Rumbaut, R.G. (1991). The agony of exile: A study of the migration and adaptation of Indochinese
refugee adults and children. In F.L. Ahearn and J.L. Athey (eds.), Refugee children: theory,
research and services. The Johns Hopkins series in contemporary medicine and public health
(pp. 5391). Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press.
Russell, W.R., and Witty, C.W.M. (1952). Studies in traumatic epilepsy: Factors influencing
incidence of epilepsy after brain wounds. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry,
15, 9398.
Ryan, J.J., Carruthers, C.A., Miller, L.J., Souheaver, G.T., Gontkousky, S.T., and Zehr, M.D.
(2003). Exploratory factor analysis of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence
(WASI) in adult standardization and clinical samples. Applied Neuropsychology,
10, 252256.
References 207
Sack, W.H., Angell, R.H., Kinzie, D., and Rath, B. (1986). The psychiatric effects of massive
trauma on Cambodian children: II. The family, the home, and the school. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 25, 377383.
Sack, W.H., Clarke, G., Him, C., Dickason, D., Goff, B., Lanham, K., and Kinzie, J.D. (1993).
A 6-year follow up study of Cambodian refuges adolescents traumatized as children. Journal
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 431437.
Sack, W.H., Clarke, G.N., and Seeley, J. (1995). Posttraumatic stress disorder across two genera-
tions of Cambodian refugees. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 34, 11601166.
Sack, W.H., Clarke, G.N., and Seeley, J. (1996). Multiple forms of stress in Cambodian adolescent
refugees. Child Development, 67, 107116.
Sack, W.H., Him, C., and Dickason, D. (1999). Twelve-year follow-up study of Khmer youths
who suffered massive war trauma as children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 11731179.
Sack, W.H., McSharry, S., Clarke, G.N., Kinney, R., Seeley, J., and Lewinsohn, P. (1994). The
Khmer Adolescent Project: I. Epidemiologic findings in two generations of Cambodian refu-
gees. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 182, 387395.
Sack, W.H., Seeley, J.R., Clarke, G.N. (1997). Does PTSD transcend cultural barriers? A study
from the Khmer adolescent refugee project. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 4954.
Saigh, P.A. (1991). The development of posttraumatic stress disorder following four different
types of traumatization. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29, 213216.
Salvia, J.M., and Ysseldyke, J.E. (2001). Assessment (8th ed). Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin
Company.
Sanua, V.D. (1970). Immigration, migration, and mental illness: In E.B. Brody (ed.)., Behavior in
New Environments: Adaptation of Migrant Populations (pp. 291322). Beverly Hills, CA:
Sage Publications.
Sastry, P.S. (1985). Lipids of nervous tissue: Composition and metabolism. Progress in Lipid
Research, 24, 69176.
Sattler, J.M. (2001). Assessment of children. Revised and updated fourth edition. San Diego, CA:
Jeromre M. Sattler, Publisher, Inc.
Sattler, J.M., and Winget, B.M. (1970). Intelligence testing procedures as affected by expectancy
and IQ. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 26, 446448.
Sattler, J.M. (1998). Clinical and forensic interviewing of children and families: Guidelines for the
mental health, education, pediatric, and child maltreatment fields. San Diego. CA: Jerome M.
Sattler, Publisher, Inc.
Satz, P. (1993). Brain reserve capacity on symptom onset after brain injury: A formulation and
review of the evidence for threshold theory. Neuropsychology, 7, 273295.
Savin, D., Sack, W.H., Clarke, G.N., Meas, N., and Richart, I. (1996). The Khmer adolescent
project: III. A study of trauma from Thailands Site II refugee camp. Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 384391.
Scar, S., and Weinberg, R.A. (1978). The influence of family background on intellectual attain-
ment. American Sociological Review, 43, 674692.
Schwarz, E.D., and Kowalski, J.M. (1991). Malignant memories: PTSD in children and adults
after a school shooting. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
30, 936944.
Sellers, A.H., Burns, W.J., and Guyrke, J. (2002). Differences in young childrens IQs on the
Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence Revised as a function of stratification
variables. Applied Neuropsychology, 9, 6573.
Semel, E., Wiig, E.H., and Secord, W.A. (2003). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals
(4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: PsychCorp.
Servan-Schreiber, D., Le Lin, B., and Birmaher, B. (1998). Prevalence of posttraumatic stress dis-
order and major depressive disorder in Tibetan refugee children. Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 37, 874879.
208 References
Seymour, H.N., and Roeper, T. (1999). Grammatical acquisition of African American English. In
O.L. Taylor and L.B. Leonard (eds.), Language acquisition across North America: Cross cul-
tural and cross-linguistic perspectives (pp. 109152). San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing
Group, Inc.
Shepherd, M.J. (2001). History lessons. In A.S. Kaufman and N.L. Kaufman (eds.), Specific
learning disabilities and difficulties in children and adolescents. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Shepherd, I., and Leathem, J. (1999). Factors affecting performance in cross-cultural neuropsy-
chology: From a New Zealand biocultural perspective. Journal of the International
Neuropsychological Society, 5, 8384.
Sheridan, S.M., and Gutkin, T.B. (2000). The ecology of school psychology: Examining
and changing our paradigm for the 21st century. School Psychology Review, 29,
485501.
Shonkoff, J.P., and Phillips, D.A. (eds.). (2000). Communicating and learning. In From neurons
to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development (pp. 124162). Washington,
DC: National Academy Press.
Shorris, E. (1992). Latinos. New York: Norton.
Silove, D., Sinnerbrink, I., Field., Manicavasagar, V., and Steel, Z. (1997). Anxiety, depression
and PTSD in asylum seekers: Associations with pre-migration trauma and post-migration
stressors. British Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 351357.
Simpson, G., Mohr, R., and Redman, A. (2000). Cultural variations in the understanding of trau-
matic brain injury and brain injury rehabilitation. Brain Injury, 14, 12540.
Sinnerbrink, I., Silvone, D., Field, A., Steel, Z., and Manicavasagar, V. (1997). Compounding of
pre-migration trauma and post-migration stress in asylum seekers. The Journal of Psychology,
131, 463470.
Slomine, B.S., McCarthy, M.L., Ding, R. et al. (2006). Health care utilization and needs after
pediatric traumatic brain injury. Pediatrics, 117, 663674.
Smedley, A. (1993). Race in North America: Origin and evolution of a world view. Boulder, CO:
Westview.
Sohlberg, M.M., and Mateer, C.A. (2001). Variables contributing to neurological and neurobehav-
ioral recovery. In M.M. Sohlberg and C.A. Mateer (eds.). Cognitive rehabilitation: An integra-
tive neuropsychological approach (pp. 5988). New York: Guilford Press.
Sourander, A. (1998). Behavior problems and traumatic events of unaccompanied refugee minors.
Child Abuse and Neglect, 22, 719727.
Sparrow, S.S., Balla, D.A., and Cicchetti, D.V. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales
Interview Edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Speorman, C.E. (1927). The abilities of man. New York: McMillan.
Spreen, O., and Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration,
norms, and commentary (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Srole, L., Langner, T.S., and Mitchell, S.T. (1962). Mental health in the metropolis: The Midtown
Manhattan Study. New York: New York University Press.
Stansbury, J.P., Reid, K.J., Reker, D.M., Duncan, P.W., Marshall, C.R., and Rittman, M. (2004).
Why ethnic designation matters for stroke rehabilitation: Comparing VA administrative data
and clinical records. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Development, 41, 269278.
Stein, B., Comer, D., Gardner, W., and Kelleher, K. (1999). Prospective study of displaced chil-
drens symptoms in wartime Bosnia. Social Psychiatric Epidemiology, 34, 464469.
Sternberg, R.J. (1984). Toward a triarchic theory of human intelligence. The Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 7, 269315.
Sternberg, R.J. (1997). A Triarchic view of giftedness: Theory and practice. In N. Coleongelo
and G.A. Danis (Eds.), Handbook of gifted education (pp. 4353). Boston: Allyn and
Bacon.
Stuart, S. (2002). Communication: Speech and language. In M.L. Batshaw (ed.), Children with
disabilities (5th ed.; pp. 229241). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Books Publishing Co.
References 209
Sue, S., and Zane, N. (1987). The role of culture and cultural techniques in psychotherapy: A cri-
tique and reformulation. American Psychologist, 42, 3745.
Suinn, R.M., Richard-Figueroa, K., Len, S., and Vigil, P. (1987). The Suinn-Law Asian Self-
Identity Acculturation Scale: An initial report. Educational and Psychological Measurement,
6, 103112.
Summerfield, D. (2000). Childhood, war, refugeedom and trauma: Three core questions for men-
tal health professionals. Transcultural Psychiatry, 37, 417433.
Suzuki, L.A., and Kugler, J.F., (1995). Intelligence and personality assessment: Multicultural
perspectives. In J.G. Ponterotto, J.M. Casas, L.A. Suzuki, and C.M. Alexander (eds.),
Handbook of multicultural counseling (pp. 493515). Thousand Oaks, CA; Sage
Publications.
Tager-Flusberg, H., and Sullivan, K. (1998). Early language development in children with mental
retardation. In J.A. Burack, R.M. Hodapp, and E. Zigler (eds.), Handbook of mental retarda-
tion and development (pp. 208239). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Tannen, D. (1980). Spoken/written language and the oral/literate continuum. In Proceedings of the
6th annual meeting, Berkely Linguistic Society (pp. 207218).
Taylor, O.L. (1999). Cultural issues and language acquisition. In O.L. Taylor and L.B. Leonard
(eds.), Language acquisition across North America: Cross-cultural and cross-linguistic per-
spectives (pp. 2137). San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group, Inc.
Teeter, P.A., and Semrud-Clikeman, M. (1997). Child Neuropsychology. Assessment and interven-
tions for neurodevelopmental disorders. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Thabet, A.A., Abed, Y., and Vostanis, P. (2004). Comorbidity of PTSD and depression among
refugee children during war conflict. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
45, 533542.
Thabet, A.A., and Vostanis, P. (2000). Post traumatic stress disorder reactions in children of war:
A longitudinal study. Child Abuse and Neglect, 24, 291298.
Thurstone, L.L. (1938). Primary mental abilities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Toms Rivera Policy Institute. (2003). El sueo de su casa: The home ownership potential of
Mexican-Heritage Families. Los Angeles: Author.
Tousignant, M., Habimana, E., Biron, C., Malo, C., Sidoli-LeBlanc, E., and Bendris, N. (1999).
The Quebec adolescent refugee project: psychopathology and family variables in a sample
from 35 nations. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
38, 14261432.
Tulsky, D.S., Saklofske, D.H., Wilkins, C., and Weiss, L.G. (2001). Development of a general
ability index for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale-Third Edition. Psychological Assessment,
13, 566571.
Unger, J.B., Gallaher, P., Shakib, S., Ritt-Olson, A., Palmer, P.H., and Johnson, A.C. (2002). The
AHIMSA Acculturation Scale: A new measure of acculturation for adolescents in multicul-
tural society. Journal of Early Adolescence, 22, 225251.
United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. (1992). Handbook on procedures and cri-
teria for determining refugee status under the 1951 convention and the 1967 protocol
relating to the status of refugees. Geneva: United Nations High Commissioner for
Refugees.
United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. (2001). Refugees by numbers. New York:
United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees.
U. S. Census Bureau. (1998). Statistical Abstract of the United States: 1998. http://www.Census.
gov.
U.S. Census Bureau. (March, 2004). U.S. Interim Projections by Age, Sex, Race and Hispanic
Origin. http://www.census.gov/ipc/www/usinterimproj/ Washington DC: Author.
United States Census Bureau (2003). Poverty status of the population in 1999 by sex, age, Hispanic origin.
and race. March 2000. See http:www.census.gov/populations/socdemo/hispanic/pp1171/Tab141.
pdf.
U.S. Census Bureau. (2001). Census 2000 Brief. Washington, DC: U.S. Bureau of the Census.
210 References
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2004). Health disparities experienced by
Hispanics. Washington, DC: Author.
U.S. Immigration and Naturalization Service. (1975). 1974 statistical yearbook of the Immigration
and Naturalization Service. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
U.S. Immigration and Naturalization Service. (1981). 1980 statistical yearbook of the Immigration
and Naturalization Service. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
U.S. Immigration and Naturalization Service. (1991). 1990 statistical yearbook of the Immigration
and Naturalization Service. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Uomoto, J.M., and Wong, T.M. (2000). Multicultural perspectives on the neuropsychology of
brain injury assessment and rehabilitation. In E. Fletcher-Janzen, T. Strickland, and C.R.
Reynolds (eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural neuropsychology (pp. 169184). New York:
Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
Valencia, R.R., Suzuki, L.A., and Salinas, M.F. (2001). Test bias. In: R.R. Valencia and L.A.
Suzuki (eds.), Intelligence testing and minority students: Foundations, performance factors,
and assessment issues. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
van Gorp, W.G., Myers, H.F., and Drake, E.B. (2000). Neuropsychology training: Ethnocultural
considerations in the context of general competency training. In E. Fletcher-Jaugen, T.L.
Strickland, and C.R. Reynolds (Eds), Handbook of Cross-cultural Neuropsychology (pp. 19
27). New York: Kluwer/Plenum Publishers.
Vzquez, J.Z. (1994). Una historia de Mexico [A history of Mexico]. Mexico City: Editorial
Patria.
Verney, S.P., Granholm, E., Marshall, S.P., Malcarne, V.L., and Saccuzzo, D.P. (2005). Culture-
fair cognitive assessment. Information processing and psychophysiological approaches.
Assessment, 12, 303319.
Vygotsky, L., and Luria, A. (1930/1993). Studies on the history of behavior: Ape, primitive, and
child. Ed. and trans. Victor I. Golod and Jane E. Knox. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and
Associates.
Wagner, R.K., Torgesen, J.K., and Rashotte, C.A. (1999). The Comprehensive Test of Phonological
Processing (CTOPP). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Waniganayake, M. (2001). From playing with guns to playing with rice: The challenges of
working with refugee children. An Australian perspective. Childhood Education, 77,
289294.
Warner, C., and Nelson, N.W. (2000). Assessment of communication, language, and speech:
Questions of What to do next? In B.A. Bracken (ed.), The psychoeducational assessment of
preschool children (pp. 145185). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Wartofsky, M. (1983). The childs construction of the world and the world construction of the
child: From historical epistemology to historical psychology. In F.S. Kessel and A.W. Sigel
(eds.), The child and other cultural inventions (pp. 188215). New York: Praeger.
Watanabe, Y., Shiel, A., McLellan, D.L., Kurihara, M., and Hayashi, K. (2001). The impact of
traumatic brain injury on family members living with patients: A preliminary study in Japan
and the U. K. Disability and Rehabilitation, 23, 370378.
Wechsler, D. (1949). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. New York: The
Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1950). Cognitive, Conative, and non-intellective intelligence. American Psychologist,
5, 7883.
Wechsler, D. (1974). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised. San
Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1991). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition. San
Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1968) Escala de Inteligencia Wechsler para Adultos. New York: Psychological
Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition Administration and Scoring
Manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.
References 211
Wechsler, D. (1999). Manual for the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio,
TX: Harcourt Assessment.
Wechsler, D. (2003). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition. San
Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (2004). Manual for the WISC-IV Spanish: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children
- Fourth Edition - Spanish (2004). San Antonio, Texas: Harcourt Assessment.
Wechsler, D., Kaplan, E., Fein, D., Kramer, J., Morris, R., Delis, D., et al. (2004). WISC-IV
Integrated Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children Fourth Edition - Integrated Technical and
Interpretative Manual. San Antonio Texas: Harcourth Assessment, Inc.
Weine, S. M., Becker, D.F., McGlashan, T.H., and Laub, D. (1995). Psychiatric consequences of
ethnic cleansing: Clinical assessments and trauma testimonies of newly resettled Bosnian
refugees. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152(4), 536542.
Weine, S., Becker, D.F., McGlashan, T.H., and Vojvoda, D. (1995). Adolescent survivors of eth-
nic cleansing: Observations on the first year in America. Journal of the American Academy
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 11531159.
Weinick, R.M., Zuvekas, S.H., Cohen, J.W. (2000). Racial and ethnic differences in access to and
use of health care services, 1977 to 1996. Medical Care Research and Review, 57, 3654.
Wernicke, C. (1874). Der Aphasische Symptomencomplex. Breslau: Cohn and Weigert.
Westermeyer, J. (1991). Psychiatric services for refugee children: An overview. In F.L. Ahearn
and J.L. Athey (eds.), Refugee children: theory, research and services. The Johns Hopkins
series in contemporary medicine and public health (pp. 127162). Baltimore, MD: The Johns
Hopkins University Press.
Whelan, T.B. (1999). Integrative developmental neuropsychology: A general systems and social-
ecological approach to the neuropsychology of children with neurogenetic disorders. In S.
Goldstein and C.R. Reynolds (eds.), Handbook of neurodevelopmental and genetic disorders
in children (pp. 8498). New York: Guilford Press.
Wiig, E.H., Secord, W.A., and Semel, E. (2004). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals
Preschool (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: PsychCorp.
Winick, M., and Noble, A. Cellular response in rats during malnutrition at various age. Journal of
nutrition, 89, 3003006.
Winick, M., and Rosso, P. (1969). Head circumference and cellular growth of the brain in normal
and marasmic children, Journal of Pediatrics, 74, 774778.
Wolff, P.H., Bereket, T., Egasso, H., and Tesfay, A. (1995). The orphans of Eritrea: A comparison
study. Journal of Child Psychology, Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 36, 633644.
Wong, T.M., Strickland, T., Fletcher-Janzen, E., Ardila, A., and Reynolds, C.R. (2000).
Theoretical and practical issues in neuropsychological assessment and treatment of cul-
turally dissimilar patients. In E. Fletcher-Janzen, T. Strickland, and C.R. Reynolds
(eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural neuropsychology (pp. 318). New York: Kluwer
Academic/Plenum Publishers.
Woodcock, R.W. (1981). Bateria Woodcock de Proficiencia en el Idioma. Allen, Texas: DLM
Teaching Resources.
Woodcock, R.W., and Muoz-Sandoval, A.F. (2001). Woodcock-Muoz Language Survey
Normative Update. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Woodcock, R., McGrew, K., and Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III. Itasca, IL: Riverside
Publishing.
Woodcock, R.W. (1991). Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery Revised English and Spanish
Form. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Woodcock, R.W., and Johnson, M.B. (1989). Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability
Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Woodcock, R.W., and Muoz-Sandoval, A.F. (1993a). Woodcock-Muoz Language Survey,
English Form. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Woodcock, R.W., and Muoz-Sandoval, A.F. (1993b). Woodcock-Muoz Language Survey,
Spanish Form. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
212 References
Woodcock, R.W., and Muoz-Sandoval, A.F. (1995). Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery
RevisedSpanish. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Woodcock, R.W., Muoz-Sandoval, A.F., McGrew, K., Mather, N., and Schrank, F. (2004a). Batera
Woodcock-Muoz Pruebas de habilidad cognitiva (3rd ed.). Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Woodcock, R.W., Muoz-Sandoval, A.F., McGrew, K., Mather, N., and Schrank, F. (2004b).
Batera Woodcock-Muoz Pruebas de aprovechamiento (3rd ed.). Itasca, IL: Riverside
Publishing.
World Health Organization. (1998). The World Health Report. Geneva: Author.
Wu, T. (2004). A culturally sensitive health care practice model Theory construction and its
testing. The American Journal of Chinese Medicine, 32 (3), 467485.
Yamane, T. (1967). Elementary sampling theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Yeates, K.O., Taylor, H.G., Woodrome, S.E., Wade, S.L., Stancin, T., and Drotar, D. (2002). Race
as a moderator of parent and family outcomes following pediatric traumatic brain injury.
Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 27, 393403.
Zhimin, L. (2003). Self-care in Chinese school-age children with nephrotic syndrome. American
Journal of Maternal and Child Nursing, 28, 8185.
Zimmerman, I.L., Steiner, V.G., and Pond, R.E. (2002). Preschool Language Scales (4th ed.).
San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Zindi, F. (1994). Differences in psychometric performance. The Psychologist: Bulletin of the
British Psychological Society, 7, 549552.
Appendix
Sample Report
1
All demographic and other identifying information have been modified to protect his identity.
213
214 Sample Report
camouflaging. During the clinical interview, he admitted that such events occurred,
but he reportedly was not able to recall details about the incidents. A review
of police reports and records, interviews with news media sources involved in the
case and review of videotapes provided, verbal reports obtained from eyewitnesses,
and verbal reports from defense counsel supported the occurrence of the alleged
events. Furthermore, Mr. P reportedly confessed and was subsequently charged with
the aforementioned capital offenses. He admitted to a history of previous involve-
ment with the law, particularly in 1999, when he was arrested and charged with the
assault of a Texas State Trooper and an Oklahoma Police Officer (verified through
court records). He subsequently served approximately 12 months in jail for those
offenses. Review of records failed to reveal the presence of other criminal behavior,
except petty theft. When asked, he indicated behaving appropriately while incarcer-
ated at the present center, information consistent with data provided by the detention
centers warden and review of records.
Mr. P was able to correctly recall portions of his history, but some informa-
tion had to be obtained from records provided by a detective working for defense
counsel and data obtained from affidavits ascertained during a trip to his native
country by this clinician from individuals familiar with Mr. Ps past. Family
members were interviewed but not his biological mother, as she perished in an
automobile accident 12 years ago. These individuals did not note developmental
delays in early motor functions but did note difficulties in speech development,
although the reports were sketchy. For example, one of his sisters noted that he
exhibited appropriate vocabulary and speech and language development, but that
his articulation was not within normal limits, thus making him difficult to under-
stand by individuals outside his home (dysarthria). No other information was
remarkable or available in this regard. In addition, it appears as if Mr. P exhib-
ited mild adaptive delays in self-help skills as he was growing up, and his sisters
reportedly helped Mr. P to dress and bathe past the age of expectation (report-
edly until about the age of 8 years, delayed even within a cultural context). With
regard to past family medical history, several members of his family noted the
presence of alcoholism and psychiatric disorders, the latter predominantly
associated with severe mood disturbances, including anxiety, recurrent unipolar
depression and possible bipolar disorder, and postpartum depression, all requir-
ing treatment, and in some instances inpatient hospitalization. The interview
with these family members also revealed that Mr. P had an impoverished back-
ground that included physical and sexual abuse, malnutrition, and neglect. He
also experienced poor living conditions in his native country, and quality of
housing was poor and without basic necessities such as running water and other
services as he was growing up in rural southern or suburban northern Mexico.
With regard to the reported past abuse, according to his sisters, Mr. P was report-
edly physically abused as a child (statutory rape) by a woman who had sexual
contact with him at the age of 12 years for an extended period of time (a
neighbor of his family that resided close to them). His siblings additionally
reported that he had been exposed to a significant amount of violence as he grew
Sample Report 215
up in his home, the result of marital violence between his parents (his mother
was regularly beaten and raped by his father while he was intoxicated, and
Mr. P was exposed to these events), and physical violence perpetuated upon him
by his father, including severe and odd punishments (hit with tree branches
and power extension cords, made to kneel on the blades of a saw or corn kernels
until his knees bled, etc.).
During the clinical interview, Mr. P reported selected aspects of his medical
history, most important and relevant to this case, his history of multiple brain
injuries as he fell from a moving vehicle and a severe TBI that occurred in a motor-
cycle-automobile collision in which he lost consciousness for an extended period
of time (GCS = 3 upon arrival at the hospital; 12 unconscious days; extended PTA;
all corroborated by existing hospital records). Mr. Ps family also reported several
incidents during his childhood that might have resulted in neurological involve-
ment, including an incident resulting in a concussion in which he was hit in the
head as a small child by a beast of burden. However, except for the motorcycle
accident, the family did not seek medical care for those incidents due to economic
factors and the nonavailability of formal medical care in Mexico; only a verbal
report from a local healer (curandera) was available. However, a record docu-
menting the car accident was recently located by defense counsel, an event in
which one individual may have perished, supporting the report provided by the cli-
ent and his family related to the severity of the incident. He also reported occasional
headaches, events treated with over-the-counter analgesics. Imaging was conducted
in the past, but reports from those procedures were not available. However, hospital
records (daily medical notes) from his extended hospitalization noted above indi-
cated the presence of a remarkable CT scan indicative of frontal bilateral diffuse
involvement. A repeat scan was not conducted.
With regard to past psychiatric history, Mr. P reported the presence of auditory
hallucinations while incarcerated and prior to incarceration, and it is possible that
he may have received psychiatric and/or psychological care prior to his incarcera-
tion, but records of such treatment(s) are not available. These hallucinations pre-
dated his current psychopharmacological treatment. However, it appears as if he
has experienced bouts of depression which may have gone untreated in the past
bouts of depression with psychotic features, probably exacerbated by his past drug
use. Such events were differentially diagnosed from cultural issues that may
explain such events such as his participation in brujera, which he admitted dur-
ing the clinical interview. He has received psychiatric care while incarcerated and
is currently receiving medication as noted above but has never received protracted
psychotherapy throughout his life.
Review of academic records obtained during a visit by this clinician and his
mitigation specialist to his past school districts in Mexico revealed the presence of
significant variability in school performance. Records provided suggest that Mr. P
attended first through sixth grades at a primary rural school in Mexico (grades and
similar academic information is not available). He attended secondary school
(grades seven through nine) at a secondary suburban school in northern Mexico.
216 Sample Report
According to his report cards, Mr. P received the following grades in his first year
of secondary school, (translating grades from the Mexican educational system to
the U.S. educational system, a posture that may have diminished validity): B in
Spanish, B in math, C in additional Spanish instruction, B in biology, B in philoso-
phy, B in chemistry, B in history, B in geography, A in civics, B in PE, A in Art,
and a B in a metal works course. His grades from his second year of secondary
school indicate that he received the following grades: C in Spanish, C in math, B in
additional Spanish instruction, C in biology, C in physics, C in chemistry, C in his-
tory, B in geography, C in civics, B in PE, B in art, and a C in a technology course.
The next years grades from secondary school indicate that he received the follow-
ing grades: C in Spanish, C in math, C in additional Spanish instruction, C in biol-
ogy, B in physics, C in chemistry, C in history, C in geography, C in civics, B in
PE, A in art, and a B in a technology course. School records appear to indicate that
Mr. Ps GPA from secondary school was B-C (7.6/10). His record from the equiva-
lent of high school in the U.S. (grades 10 through 12) while living in Mexico, indi-
cate that he received the following grades while in the 10th grade: C in reading and
editing, C in languages, C in philosophy, C in sociology, and A in Art History.
However, there are reports that either his family or colleagues may have assisted
him throughout his academic career while in Mexico. Subsequent to arrival in the
U.S., Mr. P attended high school as an ESL. student. He was not placed in special
education; however, according to school officials, there was no Special
Education available for Spanish-speaking students at that time in TX, and psy-
choeducational testing was only conducted upon parental request, a request
never entertained by his family. Therefore, no psychoeducational assessments are
available. At that time he received the following grades for the first semester of
11th grade: C in Reading II, B in ESL, F in American History, B in Algebra I,
and an A (PE). Mr. P received an N for his classes for second semester 11th
grade. The N may indicate that no grade was issued due to his increased
absences (as high as 23 absences in two of his classes). The same records indicate
that he was not in school the next year (12th grade) and that he dropped out at
that time. No efforts were made by his school district or his family to have him
enroll in school again.
The interview with Mr. P failed to reveal any type of military service in the U.S.,
information later confirmed by this clinicians personnel through a contact with the
U.S. Military Clearing House. The interview with Mr. P revealed a history of
employment in unskilled positions. He reportedly was employed in the U.S. in the
construction industry building streets and houses. He essentially was a carpenter,
as described by one of his former supervisors who was willing to assist in the case.
He was also employed as a cook in restaurants. However, he did well in these
positions and rose to cook manager in one position, and his performance in all of
the above was reportedly very good according to his former supervisors, all of
whom missed his performance after he left them. He was unable to remember
any other specific employment experiences, yet noted having worked in other
places. Records were not available.
Sample Report 217
2
It should also be noted that none of the aforementioned evaluations, despite their forensic nature,
ever addressed the issue of response bias, a posture inconsistent with current minimum standards
of practice.
3
Just as egregious and unethical, an interpreter was employed and/or the use of English was
employed as a way of communicating with an individual who is predominantly fluent in Spanish,
and this information used as the basis for many of the expert opinions expressed, despite signifi-
cant literature indicating that such a course of action should not be taken, particularly in this case in
which capital punishment was under consideration.
218 Sample Report
Behavioral Observations
Mr. P was evaluated in an attorney-client contact room within the confines of a state
prison. He was evaluated without upper extremity restraints. The entire examination
was conducted in Spanish, as he is predominantly fluent using Spanish (see below).
Although the court had explained the purpose of this examination, similar to the
introduction provided prior to the start of his evaluation by defense counsel, ethical
(consent) and legal (limits of confidentiality) and other information was initially
addressed with the client by this clinicion. During the initial interview and the evalu-
ation, his speech was slow but coherent. The content of his speech was impoverished
for someone his age (e.g., rudimentary vocabulary), and he exhibited moderate dys-
arthria. He was well oriented to person, time, and place.
Although Mr. P initially appeared to be somewhat apprehensive initially, he
slowly acclimated to the assessment situation and appeared to put forth appropriate
effort on all tasks. He initiated conversations about his present condition (incarcera-
tion, where he is reportedly being treated well).
In response to confrontational questions surrounding his present condition, he
exhibited significant remorse but he indicated that he was unable to remember spe-
cifics related to the events that took place. He noted having seen the videos, and
agreed that it was him, but reported not being able to remember being there. A
discussion of his childhood did not elicit blunt affect. Overall, affect appeared to be
within normal limits. Mood appeared to be within normal limits (he is being medi-
cated). The assessment environment was quiet throughout most of the examination,
thus reducing threats to the validity of this evaluation. Mr. P was asked to show the
examiner scars found in his knees from the reported abuse, and he reluctantly
agreed, displaying significant scarring from such incidents. He refused to talk about
his parents marital difficulties and his own sexual abuse as he was growing up but
did not deny them.
Mr. Ps demeanor was observed outside of the one-on-one testing situation when
he took small breaks during the course of the two-day evaluation to use the restroom
(he had lunch in the test room). During these observational periods, he was at all
times appropriate from a behavioral standpoint consistent with his imprisonment
records of unremarkable behavior. Although Mr. P did not wear upper extremity
cuffs, at no time did this examiner feel unsafe in the exam room while assessing
this individual.
Assessment Results
Assessment Validity
Due to the legal nature of this case, coupled with the fact that Mr. P may have
been motivated to present himself in a good light (or ill) as a result of his
impending legal proceedings, several procedures believed to be sensitive to the
presence of feigned symptom exaggeration and/or response bias were adminis-
tered. The results of these procedures revealed that Mr. P was being straightfor-
ward and honest in his responses to test items. He performed within the range
of expectation on the Rey 15-Item Memory Test (Score = 15/15). Similarly, his
scores on the DCT fell within normal limits (50th to 75th percentile). On a
more complex probabilistic procedure (SVT), his performance fell within
expectation, as he obtained a perfect score on all three trials. Other tests indices
obtained throughout the assessment also supported a hypothesis suggesting the
presence of appropriate effort, including better recognition than recall during
memory tasks, lower levels of performance on increasingly more difficult test
items consistent with expectation, as well as expected performance and other
220 Sample Report
test indices (validity indices). The level of rapport established with this indi-
vidual also was appropriate.
However, it should be noted that limitations are associated with this component of
his examination, as there are no norms for individuals from his ethnic (not racial)
background on the SVT and the DCT.
4
Consultation was sought by this clinician with a colleague familiar with the clients cultural
background in an attempt to better understand Mr. Ps background, including his familys abo-
riginal background, consistent with APAs Principles of Psychologists.
Sample Report 221
Mr. P was administered the WMPHC-R, an aptitude test (this procedure was
selected as a result of its extensive normative sample, including oversampling of
individuals from Mexican backgrounds, its standardized Spanish version, and the
lack of more appropriate tests available in the U.S. with Hispanic samples from
Mexican backgrounds at the time this assessment was conducted). On this intel-
lectual instrument, he obtained an overall score of 78 (71 to 85), placing his overall
performance (Broad Cognitive Ability) within the 6th percentile and within the
Borderline range of intellectual abilities when compared to his same-age peers.
However, when controlling for his educational level, his overall score fell in the
Dull Normal range (82; 12th percentile). He obtained the following Standard
Scores (SS) (mean = 100; standard deviation = 15):
populations; the reader is asked to think of other reasons). On this test, a measure
of non-verbal intellect, he obtained a score of 88 3 (95% confidence interval;
Below Average). It should be noted that his WMPHC-III subtest score on a meas-
ure of fluid reasoning (Analysis-Synthesis) is qualitatively and quantitatively
consistent with his TONI-2 score.
Although Mr. P spoke fluently, and his speech was coherent and normal in pros-
ody, he exhibited significant dysarthria. His score (22/30) on a task requiring con-
frontational naming (BNT) fell in the range of expectation (35th percentile,
average) when compared to norms for Hispanic individuals with 11 to 15 years of
education. Functional and phonemic cues did not aid his performance on this test.
He scored in the average range (50th percentile) on the Spanish Token Test, a meas-
ure of receptive language.
Verbal Memory
Mr. Ps verbal memory was assessed using a rote verbal memory as he performed
a list learning task (WHO-UCLA AVLT-SV). On this measure his score fell
within the impaired range (<1st percentile) on the fifth recall of the original list
(Trial V), a measure of overall learning. Similarly, his score fell in the impaired
range (<1st percentile) on indices assessing short and long free delayed recall. All
these indices were corrected for individuals with his same low educational level
(11 to 15 years to use the most appropriate approach). While learning the word
list, he predominantly used a primacy and recency approach as expected, provid-
ing further support for the validity of this assessment. He exhibited no retroactive
or proactive interference.
Visual Memory
Motor/Sensory Skills
Informal lateral preference assessment (use of hand to write, throw a ball [pan-
tomimed], etc.) suggests that Mr. P is right-hand dominant. Using this informa-
tion, his motor/sensory functioning was assessed. On the Grooved Pegboard
Test, a test requiring fine motor coordination and dexterity, his scores fell
within the impaired range with his right (dominant) and left (nondominant)
hands (preferred hand 43rd percentile; nondominant hand = 56th percentile)
when compared to 20- to 39-year-old males with less than a high school educa-
tion. There was a nonsignificant raw score lateralizing discrepancy of 10%
favoring his preferred hand. His performance on the Sensory Perceptual Exam
revealed no errors.
224 Sample Report
On Color Trails Test 2, a test requiring inhibition and shifting of set, his score
using appropriate demographics (compared to 18- to 29-year-olds with 9 to 11
years of education) fell in the impaired range (231 sec; <1st percentile). His
interference score on this measure fell within normal limits (1.24 >16th percen-
tile), suggesting that many of his difficulties may be due to complex attentional
difficulties. In addition, he exhibited significant difficulty in organization and
planning while performing the copy portion of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex
Figure. He scored in the impaired range when compared to same age peers on
the three components (color naming [1st percentile], word reading [2nd percen-
tile], and color-word reading [<1st percentile]) of another procedure assessing
the ability to inhibit a common response for a more complex response (Stroop
Interference Test). His impaired score on this measure fell in the impaired range
despite the fact that he exhibited reading-decoding at an 11th-grade level.
Behavioral/Emotional/Personality Functioning
During this assessment, Mr. P was asked to complete the Draw-A-Person Test a
measure administered to investigate his cognitive level. His drawing of a person
was unremarkable from a developmental standpoint. Using the Goodehough-Harris
system, his performance is similar to that produced by other adults. He also com-
pleted the Beck Anxiety and Depression Inventories. On these measures, he
obtained scores of 5 and 6, respectively. Although not diagnostic, these scores sug-
gest that he is not experiencing anxious or depressive symptoms while medicated.
5
See Garcia, A. (1988). Investigacin del test no verbal inteligencia (TONI) en la ciudad de
Chihuahua. Unpublished thesis, UAC, Chihuahua, Mexico.
6
See Siriany, A. (1989). Developing a slide projection group administration procedure for the
TONI. Unpublished manuscript, Universidad Austral de Chile, Valdivia, Chile.
226 Sample Report
7
According to the AAMR and its definition, used herein, mental retardation is considered when
significant and concurrent limitations, both in intellectual functioning and in adaptive skills origi-
nates before the age of 18. By impairments, scores two standard deviations or below are consid-
ered deficient.
8
Aside from cross-cultural issues already mentioned (e.g., translation of a test), it should also be
noted that no mention was made of the possibility that he may have been suffering from a pro-
found depressive episode, or other condition(s), including dementia, PTSD, etc.
Sample Report 227
self-report and reports from family members could be considered biased, review of
records indicated the presence of a lack of delays in adaptation in most areas. His
records revealed an appropriate academic history, and his level of academic attain-
ment is inconsistent with that usually attained by individuals with MR, including
Mild MR. Similarly, although his vocational history is marked by employment in
unskilled jobs, his performance in those jobs was appropriate and such a finding
also supports a postulate indicative of a lack of vocational adaptive delays. In this
regard, he was employed throughout his life prior to being incarcerated with sig-
nificant occupational stability, and he did quite well in some those positions.
Similar conclusions can be reached regarding his history of interpersonal relation-
ships (e.g., friends in school, etc.). Therefore, altogether, although he currently
suffers from below-average intellect, and there is a significant history of adaptive
delays in two reported domains, he does not appear to have suffered from impaired
intellect as a child, clearly before the age of 18 years. Hence, with a reasonable
degree of certainty, Mr. Ps profile does not meet the diagnostic criteria for mental
retardation as set forth by the AAMR because of my inability to concretely note
that his intellectual impediments began before the age of 18 years (the third AAMR
prong) and because his intellectual impediments do not rise to an impaired level
even during the course of his current intellectual assessment, coupled with the fact
that he may only have exhibited adaptive delays in limited areas. Finally, there are
other phenomenological issues to consider that support such a diagnostic posture,
including the fact that Mr. P was able to elude police for a period of approximately
48 hours as a result of camouflaging he used during the two days in which the
reported shootings and robberies took place, as well as the fact that he was able to
immigrate to the U.S. by himself without any assistance.
With regard to the organicity rule-out, the pattern of neuropsychological per-
formance observed during the course of this evaluation suggests that Mr. P suffers
from organic brain damage (encephalopathy). Such a hypothesis is supported by the
preponderance of his scores on several domains and his history, particularly the
protracted and severe use of use of controlled substances and his head injuries.
Therefore, it is impossible to rule-out the presence of static encephalopathy (brain
damage) at this time.9
Although he is said to have participated in an event that led to the tragic deaths
of several individuals, he apparently committed these acts without previous
planning under the influence of controlled substances in the course of two days.
Specific information obtained during the course of this evaluation indicates that
these individuals came in contact with Mr. P at random and that he did not choose
9
Although not requested by the referral source or required and therefore not provided, using
DSM-IV, a dementia could have been considered as a diagnostic alternative to MR, or as a
stand-alone diagnosis. For example, a dementia due to multiple etiologies (e.g., head injuries,
drug use) could have been considered, particularly within the context of his deficits in executive
skills and impaired visual and auditory memory. Diagnostic consideration should also be given
to PTSD and other diagnoses.
228 Sample Report
229
230 Index
O S
Occupational allegiance, 52 Scale of Independent Behavior Revised
Occupational group allegiance, 51 (SIB-R), 115
and immigration, 3132, 3435 Second language acquisition, 88
Optic aphasia, 110 Semilingualism, 117
234 Index