Selective Prefrontal Cortex Responses To Joint Attention in Early Infancy
Selective Prefrontal Cortex Responses To Joint Attention in Early Infancy
Selective Prefrontal Cortex Responses To Joint Attention in Early Infancy
Neurobiology
1. INTRODUCTION
Attending and responding to eye gaze is crucial for
human social interactions. Specifically, eye gaze plays
an important role in directing and coordinating attention during triadic interactions between self, other and
the environment. During a typical triadic interaction, a
person may establish eye contact with another person
and then direct that persons gaze to an object or
event. The psychological process by which two
people share attention towards the same object or
event is called joint attention. Joint attention and the
underlying
triadic
representations
(self other
person object) are thought to be unique to our
species, supporting teaching, cooperation and
language learning (Tomasello et al. 2005; Saxe 2006;
Csibra & Gergely 2009). Impairments in joint
attention are one of the earliest signs of autism
(Charman 2003).
More than 30 years ago, Scaife & Bruner (1975)
first observed that infants follow a persons gaze and
540
right hemisphere
22
19
17
14
left hemisphere
24
23
21
20
12
11
10
9
7
18
16
541
15
4
2
13
FZ
24 F4 23
12 F3 11
8
21
9
20 10
19 18
6
7
17 16
3
4
15 5
1
14
13
2
22
0.1
0
0.1
Figure 1. Prefrontal brain responses measured in five-month-old infants. This graph depicts mean oxygenated haemoglobin
concentration changes (+s.e.m.) during the joint attention, no referent and no eye contact conditions measured from 24
NIRS channels within dorsal, ventral and lateral ROIs. Black shaded box, joint attention; light grey shaded box, no referent;
white box, no eye contact; red, dorsal; yellow, lateral; blue, ventral.
too few usable trials for analysis (minimum number of four trials
per condition). Please note that an attrition rate at this level is
within the normal range for an infant NIRS study (Lloyd-Fox et al.
2010). All infants were born full-term (3742 weeks gestation)
and with normal birthweight (.2500 g). All parents gave informed
consent before the study.
(b) Stimuli and procedure
Animated photo-realistic face stimuli were generated using POSER 6.0
software (Curious Lab Inc., Santa Cruz, CA). Infants sat on their
parents lap while watching the stimuli on a computer monitor
within an acoustically shielded, dimly lit room. The faces presented
subtended a visual angle of 388 258, and each eye subtended 38
58). The experimental sessions consisted of 9 s long trials. The three
experimental conditions were randomly distributed over the session
with no more than two trials of the same condition occurring in a
row. The inter-trial interval randomly varied between 8 and 12 s.
Non-social moving visual stimuli were presented during the
inter-trial interval to keep infants attention.
(c) Data acquisition and analysis
Cortical activations were measured using a Hitachi ETG-4000 NIRS
system. The multi-channel system uses two wavelengths at 695 and
830 nm. Two custom-built arrays consisting of nine optodes (five
sources, four detectors) in a 12-channel (sourcedetector pairs)
arrangement with an inter-optode separation of 25 mm were
placed over the frontal lobe on each hemisphere using an Easycap
(Falk Minow). The NIRS method relies on the optical determination
of changes in haemoglobin concentrations in cerebral cortex which
result from increased regional cerebral blood flow. NIRS data were
continuously sampled at 10 Hz. For analysis, after calculation of
the oxyHb concentration changes, pulse-related signal changes and
overall trends were eliminated by low-pass filtering (Butterworth,
fifth-order, lower cut-off 0.5 Hz). Movement artefacts were corrected
by an established procedure (Koch et al. 2006; Wartenburger et al.
2007), which allows marking of artefacts and then padding the contaminated data segments by linear interpolation. Cortical responses
were assessed by comparing average concentration changes
(oxyHb) within trials (15 s after stimulus onset) between the experimental conditions. For statistical analyses, three regions of interest
(ROIs: dorsal, ventral and lateral prefrontal cortex) were selected
on the basis of prefrontal cortex anatomy and a meta-analysis of
the adult functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) work
Biol. Lett. (2010)
(Amodio & Frith 2006; see figure 1 for ROIs). An omnibus repeated
measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted with
within-subjects factors: ROI (3) hemisphere (2) condition (3).
Subsequently, planned comparisons were performed using repeated
measures ANOVAs for each ROI individually with within-subjects
factors: channel location (4) hemisphere (2) condition (3).
3. RESULTS
The omnibus ANOVA revealed a significant three-way
interaction between ROI, hemisphere and condition,
F4,56 3.201, p 0.019, partial h2 0.186. Planned
comparisons revealed a significant interaction between
condition and hemisphere (F2,28 6.944, p 0.004,
partial h2 0.332) for the dorsal prefrontal region.
The left dorsal prefrontal region was specifically sensitive to joint attention interactions. This region showed
the predicted pattern of an increased brain response
when the joint attention condition was compared
with the no referent condition (t14 1.795, p
0.047 one-tailed), and when the joint attention condition was compared with the no eye contact
condition (t14 2.125, p 0.026 one-tailed), whereas
in the right dorsal prefrontal region, there was no
difference between the joint attention and no referent
conditions (t1,14 0.002, p 0.494 one-tailed) and
the response was significantly decreased in the joint
attention when compared with the no eye contact condition (t1,14 21.969, p 0.034 one-tailed). As
shown in figure 1, within the left dorsal prefontal
region, comprising channels 9, 10, 11 and 12, all channels showed increased responses to joint attention
when compared with the other conditions. For channel
12 alone, this effect attained statistical significance
(F2,28 3.962, p 0.031, partial h2 0.281) (see
table S1 in the electronic supplementary material for
4. DISCUSSION
These results show that, even by the age of five
months, infants are sensitive to triadic interactions
and, like adults, they recruit a specific prefrontal
region localized in left dorsal prefrontal cortex when
engaged in joint attention with another person
(Schilbach et al. in press). It is interesting to note that
the infant brain response was lateralized to the left
hemisphere, which is not only in line with the adult
fMRI work (Schilbach et al. in press) but also concurs
with findings showing that higher rates of resting glucose metabolism in left prefrontal regions measured
with positron emission tomography, and synchronized
brain activity measured from left frontal electrodes
during resting EEG, are positively correlated with childrens tendency to initiate joint attention bids in the
second year of life (Caplan et al. 1993; Mundy Card &
Fox 2000). The finding is also interesting in light of
work showing that adults with autism have atypical
brain structure (increased grey matter) in the same
left prefrontal regions that are functionally implicated
in joint attention in healthy adults (Waiter et al.
2004; Williams et al. 2005).
A critical distinction has been drawn between
responding to joint attention (RJA) and the actual
initiation of joint attention (IJA; Mundy & Newell
2007). In the current study, however, by focusing on
how infants respond to joint attention initiated by an
adult, we were not able to examine this potentially
important difference. Schilbach et al. (in press)
assessed the neurobiological basis of the distinction
between RJA and IJA in adults, and found that while
there are regions commonly involved in both kinds of
joint attention, such as the left medial dorsal prefontal
cortex, the ventral striatum appears to be specifically
engaged only when an adult initiates joint attention.
This specific involvement of the ventral striatum has
been argued to be the basis of the positive affective
experience associated with directing someone elses
gaze. While future neuroimaging work with infants
should address this important distinction between
RJA and IJA, it is unknown whether NIRS can detect
responses from brain structures located as deep as
the ventral striatum (Grossmann 2008).
In conclusion, using a novel paradigm and a
modern optical imaging technique well suited to studying freely behaving infants, we were able to
demonstrate that parts of the left dorsal prefrontal
cortex are selectively activated already very early in
the development of joint attention. This brings into
question claims that joint attention is an ability that
only emerges towards the end of the first year of life
(also termed the nine-month-revolution; Tomasello
1999). The current findings suggest that the human
infant is neurobiologically prepared for sharing attention with other humans, and this may provide the
basis of a wide variety of uniquely human social and
cultural learning processes.
Biol. Lett. (2010)
543