Critical Book Report: Bima Mustaqim
Critical Book Report: Bima Mustaqim
Critical Book Report: Bima Mustaqim
Created By:
Bima Mustaqim
5143331002
This task is structured to complete one
individual assignments Course Teaching
Evaluation
ELECTRICAL ENGINEERING
EDUCATION
FACULTY OF ENGINEERING
STATE UNIVERSITY OF MEDAN
2016
INFORMATION BIBLIOGRAPHY
The book is used as a material for Critical Book Report is:
Tittle
Author
Production Year
: 1982
Publisher
: ACADEMIC PRESS
CO
NTENT
Chapter 1: The Evaluation of Learning: Quality and Quantity in Learning
In this chapter, we have argued as follows:
1. Much school learning, as well as deliberate learning episodes outside the context of
school, are closed in nature. By this, we simply mean that learning of certain contents
facts, skills, or conceptsneeds to meet particular criteria of both quantity (amount
learned) and quality.
2. In any learning episode, both qualitative and quantitative learning outcomes are
determined by a complex interaction between teaching procedures and student
characteristics. For present purposes, we emphasize here the roles played by: the
prior knowledge the student has of the content relating to the episode, his
developmental stage, his motives and intentions about the learning, his learning
strategies. "Power" factors, such as general ability, operate across the board and have
little prescriptive value in the present context.
3. While quantitative aspects of evaluating learning are well understood and applied,
qualitative aspects have not been researched or applied to nearly the same extent. In
practice, qualitative evaluation is highly subjective and is poorly integrated with
grading procedures.
4. It is increasingly recognized that instructing with a view to matching student
performance
with
preset
standards
is
important
and
logical.
Present
four level hierarchy; Marton used the structure yielded by a content analysis of each task
(usually four levels).
Our own point of departure is different from all these. We believe that there are "
natural" stages in the growth of learning any complex material or skill and that in certain
important respects these stages are similar to, but not identical with, the developmental stages
in thinking described by Piaget and his co-workers (e.g., Ginsburg & Opper, 1979; Piaget,
1950). In the following chapter, we explore this link between learning and development, and
describe the hierarchy we obtained.
and most other school subjects can be used to provide the content for any tasks the teacher
will deem appropriate to evaluate.
In Part III, we discuss the general educational implications of the SOLO Taxonomy.
Particular attention is given to issues that arise in curriculum, instructional method, and
evaluation.
In Parts I - I I I , we have tried to avoid technical and theoretical details that may be
beyond the immediate interest of some teachers. The Taxonomy does, however, raise several
methodological, theoretical, and research issues: These are considered in Part IV.
Chapter 3: History
History is like the natural sciences in the sense that events are to be placed in an
explanatory context. It is unlike the natural sciences, however, in that the concepts explaining
those events are colligatory rather than causal. The explanation of empirical events is
achieved in history by the use of concepts that have as much personal as objective meaning:
Values, feelings, and emotions enter historical explanations, in a peculiarly involving way.
Such concepts are readily evaluated in SOLO analysis.
The general procedure is the same as in other tasks: defining the component, whether
in content or process terms, and analyzing the structure of the response in terms of the way in
which the components interrelate. Several such tasks and components may be identified in
history. We chose examples here from the following:
1. Drawing conclusions from a display of information (content)
2. Making value judgments about an event
3. Reconciling conflicting evidence
4. Constructing a plausible interpretation from incomplete evidence
5. Understanding terms and concepts
6. Inducing the meaning of a concept
It would be easy to produce other examples, including ones from different historic
contexts (ours were mainly from English and Australian history), but the examples chosen
give an idea of the kinds of items that may be used for SOLO analysis and the results that
might be expected.
Several implications for teaching were discussed. The prevalent " neutral , " objective
and quantitative treatment of history in school does not encourage students to impose their
Having selected the particular area we next proceeded to analyze the following topics
in terms of SOLO response levels:
1. Numbers and operations
2. Combination of operations
3. Closure
4. Pronumeral substitution
5. The inverse operation and elementary equation solving
6. Preference for consistency
7. Mathematical systems
8. Mathematics profile series, operations test
The first seven areas listed above show general principles that are involved and the reader is
encouraged to apply these to the specific classroom content with which he is concerned. For
example, if the reader's present program includes the teaching of common fractions, he
should examine this topic in the light of the typical SOLO responses expected under the
various headings given above; For instance, what implications do the response levels under
the heading "pronumeral substitution" have for the children's understanding of the formula,
a nx a
=
b nx b
To help the reader in this exercise the eighth area was included. In this section two specific
items from a standardized test were taken and analyzed in SOLO terms.
Several implications for teaching were discussed in the areas of aims, evaluation, and
instructional strategies. It is clear that in all three areas an analysis which keeps the SOLO
level in mind is very fruitful and should contribute to a better understanding of both the place
of mathematics in the school curriculum and the classroom techniques that will enhance
performance in the subject.
Chapter 5: English
Studies in creative writing show more clearly than studies in other subjects how
widely different students' work can be at the same year/grade level. It is therefore more
difficult here than in other subjects for teachers to have any generalized expectations for a
class.
Although extended abstract responses were rare, one of the best ones ("Signs of
Spring") came from Grade 9, not Grade 12 where students would have had 3 more years to
develop. Why?
We know there are enormous differences in the amount of time different students
spend in writing: Some avoid putting pen to paper at all costs; others like using language in
poetry, diary entries, stories, etc.
What evidence there is suggests that school does little to reduce the large differences
in time children spend in writing. Annells (1975) surveyed a representative sample of
Tasmanian high schools, collecting writing samples of all kinds. He found that Grade 7
students submitted an average of 1.7 pages per day, which rose to 2.8 pages in Grade 10. On
any one day, 4 0% of high-school students submitted no continuous writing at all. Of the
writing collected, 60% was for the purpose of doing exercises (problems, short-answer
responses), 35% was didactic, that is, passing on information, and only 4% was creative
(either prose or poetry). Surprisingly, 70% of the teachers believed that creative writing
served an important educational function and that appropriate experiences and time should be
directed to creative writing.
These data are similar to those obtained in England by Britton et al. (1975). There is
little reason to suppose that the situation is basically different in other countries. Though
everyone agrees that creative writing is important, it is not adequately treated in school.
Consequently, the development of a high level of writing skill is all too often left to
individual bent.
However, it is unlikely that writing skill is only a matter of some innate " g i f t . " As
Scardamalia (in press) points out, writing has a relatively large drain on working memory.
While composing and transcribing, the student has to think about many things, including
what he has just said, what he intends to say within that paragraph, what his immediate words
must be to bridge past and intended content, the rules of grammar, punctuation, assumptions
about the reader's knowledge, and so on. It is a frightening list and no wonder that Grade 8
students often produce an incoherent chain of words equivalent to that of 6-year-olds doing
simpler tasks. In order to give the equivalent degree of mastery over writing that the young
writer tends to acquire over other subject matter, the prerequisite skills need to be analyzed,
taught, and practiced as is done in other subjects. This approach to writing is currently being
explored by Bereiter and his team at OISE and may well bear fruit.
SOLO analysis has a place in such an enterprise. A components table, similar to Table
5.3, forces the teacher to think about the basic constituents of good writing and enables her to
point out to students when those components are missing, are inadequate, or are poorly
integrated. As in task analysis of other subjects, SOLO analysis of students' written products
including the best, worst, and most typicalprovide the teacher with an explicit idea of the
kinds of components students can manage in their writing, and the levels of performance she
might reasonably expect. Needless to say, the components table would differ for different
genrerhythm and meter are relevant to poetry writing but not to prose; plot and character
are relevant to narrative but not to nonfictionforms of didactic or transactional writing
(e.g., writing a lab report, a business letter, a job application) would each require its own
component task analysis.
Once the appropriate table has been set up for the genre currently being taught, it can
be applied to evaluate students' writing, either formatively or summatively. Theformative use
of such evaluations is obvious as it pinpoints a student's particular weaknesses (see Table 8.1
and accompanying text).
In summary, SOLO analysis of writing, as in other subjects, results in criteria for
distinguishing distinct levels of quality, ranging from the almost incoherent,through the stereo
typical and conventional, to the richly expressive. SOLO does not provide or define the
components of analysis, but its use here, as in other areas, requires the subject matter expert
to analyze what he wants from his students and how those components might be integrated.
Chapter 6: Geography
General implications for teaching are considered in detail in Part III. Here we will
simply make a few points that bear directly upon geography. We have taken examples that
illustrate systematic rather than regional geography, although the case of Grong Grong brings
together several systematic phenomena (transport, agriculture, physical features) that describe
a particular region rather minutely.
We have also been careful to include both process and content aspects, or " k n o w l e
g e " and " s k i l l " objectives as the N.S.W. Board of Senior School Studies (1976) describes
them. The N.S.W. Board's publication is typical of many such statements of curricular
objectives (which when translated into classroom terms, become identical with "teacher
intentions" as described in Chapter 1). Knowledge objectives from this report include:
a) Comprehend that natural and cultural features and areas of the earth's surface reveal
graded likenesses and infinite differences. . . .
b) Recognize that alteration of areal characteristics is constantly occurring [p. 2].
Our items concerning "Why is it dark at n i g h t ? " address (a); the item on erosion in the
Andes addresses (c).
An important skill objective is, "Compile, read, and interpret maps, photographs,
graphs, and diagrams which are customarily used to illustrate geographical phenomena [p. 2 ]
. " Our items on Grong Grong and on the picture of the countryside address this objective.
In addition, the N . S . W . curriculum objectives include a section on " attitudes . "
These tend to be very vague, and in the event are rarely assessed adequately. Such would
include: "Demonstrate an interest in, and concern for, problems of the local area . . . " and 4
'Show an appreciation of the contribution of geography to the culture of mankind [pp. 2 - 3 ] .
"
The SOLO Taxonomy has not been applied to the affective or attitudinal domain, and
in fact it is rather difficult to see how it could. As far as the other objectives are concerned,
however, it is easily possible to devise items that address either knowledge or skills and
assess students' responses in terms of their quality of learning.
It would seem desirable that when such objectives are formulated, crucial items
should be constructed to test the levels of student understanding or competence at various
grade levels. The classroom teacher then has a reasonable guide as to what level of
performance he can expect from his own students. This aspect of the
Taxonomy's use is, however, applicable to a variety of subjects, and this problem is
addressed in more detail and with general application in Chapter 8.
As far as geography specifically is concerned, we may obtain an idea of the standards
expected of various geographical tasks by referring to the material reviewed in the previous
section. Jahoda's work on class-inclusion showed that elementary-school students might be
expected to master the notion that, for example, residents of Denver are also residents of
Colorado and of the United States. A unistructural level of response does not admit that if the
student lives in Denver he can also be a resident of Colorado and of the United States.
Similarly, in the area of skills, there appear to be ages before which it is unlikely to be useful
to teach map-reading skills: We saw that the idea of relative scale was meaningless for many
children before high school.
Such limitations imposed by age are very likely caused by the student's familiarity
with the task. A scaleindeed a map itselfis a geometric abstraction from the world of
experience, and consequently needs much correlation of experience if it is to be meaningful.
All children experience darkness at night. It is then to be expected relational level responses
were obtained at 11 years (see p. 134). A comparison of responses given in this chapter with
those in other chapters, such as those on history or writing, shows higher level responses are
given to naturalistic geography items than to more " s y m b o l i c " areas.
There is an important lesson here. Geography teachers, perhaps more than others, can
capitalize upon their students' experiences. Geography, after all, deals with space and
location. This is very concrete and within the immediate experience of all students. But a
subject like history, which deals by definition with environments and situations that are prior
to and thus beyond the immediate experience of students, can be experienced at best by
hearsaythe reminiscences of an aging relative or by inference, from paintings, artifacts,
and by now very peaceful looking battlefields. He has to step outside his experience and use
his imagination to relate to the historical event. The geography student has only to open his
eyes and see.
Nevertheless, higher order understanding in geography goes beyond the concrete.
Systematic and economic geography rely on cause-and-effect explanations that go way
beyond the realm of immediate experience. This is, however, an important sequence that
perhaps should not be missed: Local and regional aspects may be used to lead in to the more
abstract and symbolic formulations of geographical knowledge and skills.
In brief, SOLO analysis may be of specific assistance to the geography teacher in both
content and process (knowledge and skills) areas. Perhaps in geography more than in other
subject areas, the teacher may capitalize upon the student's immediate experience of his
immediate spatial and areal environment and move from that to the more abstract
representations of that environment. SOLO analysis seems particularly useful for isolating
those aspects that students use in their construction of knowledge and skills, and it would
seem advantageous for teachers to isolate these and build upon them when constructing their
lesson plans.
There are other applications of SOLOin the construction of critical tasks and their
evaluationbut these apply to subjects other than geography teaching and so they are dealt
with in Part III.
agreement between independent judges was obtained, with correlations between judges
ranging from .71 to .95.
The question of validity was examined from several aspects:
1. Agreement with teacher ratings of ''quality''
2. Factor analysis, with appropriate loadings of SOLO items on a school achievement
factor
3. Process analysis, which involved relating overall SOLO ratings, and transitions fro
level-to-level, with appropriate indices of cognitive processes, student motivation, and
student learning strategies
Once the relationship between SOLO level and conventional measures of
achievement had been established, the most interesting evidence on the construct validity of
SOLO came from the process analyses. These were of two main kinds: The first related
overall ratings to ability, cognitive processes, motivation, and learning strategies. A canonical
correlation showed that SOLO level was associated with school achievement in math and
English, simultaneous synthesis, and to some extent, with successive synthesis; and
independently with intrinsic motivation, a meaning strategy, avoidance of rote learning, and
to a slight extent, an organized approach to learning.
The second elucidated these associations by analyzing across levels, particularly with
respect to what was happening at each transition from prestructural to unistructural to
multistructural to relational. The psychological processes involved included simultaneous and
successive synthesis, motivation, and learning strategies. The processes associated with each
transition were compatible with the nature of the task facing the student; whether he was
shifting from level to level within a test, or performing two different tasks (interpreting
poetry and writing an essay). The work reported above can only be regarded as preliminary,
but the results are very encouraging to date.
Finally, the question of different formats for obtaining SOLO responses was
examined. In particular, we looked at fixed space, multiple-choice, and alternative structures
as possible ways of obtaining and interpreting responses. Few conclusions were reached, as
little research has been conducted so far, but certain possibilities were suggested as
warranting further investigation; in particular the use of a multiple choice format, where the
distractors are specifically generated by lower levels of SOLO.
The work reviewed here is encouraging. In the final chapter we review SOLO in a
much broader context; that of its implications for psychological theory.
(formal) modes respectively; whereas within the concrete mode, learning proceeds on a
unistructural, then multistructural, and finally, a relational, basis.
In this extended SOLO model learning and development are clearly separated but
their interaction is outlined. If we extend the analysis beyond the concrete mode, it becomes
necessary to designate both the level of learning and the mode of functioning, such that a
response might be described more completely as ''unistructural-firstorder-formal," or
"multistructural-intuitive," which might be evidenced by a student approaching his college
studies, and by a child learning to string sentences together, respectively. This conception
greatly extends the applicability of the SOLO model at both ends, so that for example it could
be used to characterize the quality of a scholar's research on the one hand, or a baby's
developing conception of object constancy on the other.
The model also seems to fit other modalities than the cognitively oriented ones that
the use of the terms concrete, formal, etc. imply. For example, the stages in the learning of
complex skills, such as knitting or playing tennis, or perceptual discrimination tasks, such as
wine tasting, can with equal profit be analyzed in these terms.
There does, moreover, appear to be a plausible case for arguing that patterns of
cognitive abilities, such as convergent and divergent ability, cognitive style, and personality
variables such as dogmatism, anxiety, and cognitive complexity, might predispose individuals
throughout adulthood to respond at unistructural, multistructural, or relational levels within
particular modalities, or to move beyond the given modality in the case of prestructural and
extended abstract responses. If such response predilections exist, the extended SOLO model
seemed to provide a heuristic framework for interpersonal communication, decision making,
and some aspects of lifespan psychology.
If these frankly speculative suggestions are ultimately supported by research, then
what started out as a descriptive model for a circumscribed contextschool learningmight
contain within it the seeds of a theory of learning with a wide range of application. Two
features are basic: the concepts of learning cycles (commencing with the formation of a
datum, then the acquisition of parallel data, and finally their integration) and the concept of
endogenously limited modality shifts. Both features seem descriptively sound, and consistent
with a wide range of research. Whether or not this model may result in the generation of
strategies of intervention remains at this stage an intriguing possibility.
MY OPINION
In my opinion: One of the competencies to be mastered as a prospective teacher is
learning evaluation. Competence is in line with the duties and responsibilities as a teacher in
learning, that evaluate learning including conducting the assessment process and the learning
outcomes. The competency assessment instruments in line with the ability of teachers, where
one of the indicators is to evaluate learning. There are many more models that describe the
basic competencies that must be mastered. This shows that on all models necessarily reflect
the competence of teachers and requires the ability of teachers to evaluate learning, because
learning is the ability to evaluate basic capabilities that absolutely must be held by teachers.
In this book has been described in such a way how a teacher to evaluate existing
learning in class. In presenting this book the author presents each subject in detail, brief and
every subject matter and provide examples of logical and factual so that readers better
understand the contents of the subject logical and factual language so that the reader better
understand the contents of the subject in question. Each chapter of the book is presented
analytically and ideas are given quite logical and orderly. Preparation of chapters organized
so that can not be found that precede each chapter, and each chapter is given directives to the
reader to understand the basic concept of what is needed to be able to master chapter per
chapter of this book. So that the reader can prepare what is needed for mastering chapter per
chapter of this book. This book is very good to be our reference in the evaluation.