Rosete and Ciarrochi 2005 Lodj EI Leadership Effectiveness
Rosete and Ciarrochi 2005 Lodj EI Leadership Effectiveness
Rosete and Ciarrochi 2005 Lodj EI Leadership Effectiveness
EFFECTIVENESS
Word Count:
6,000
Classification:
Feature Article
Running Head:
Abstract
Does Emotional Intelligence (EI) make someone a better leader? We utilized a
cross-sectional survey to examine the relationships between leadership effectiveness and
tests of ability EI and cognitive reasoning, and self-report measures of EI and personality.
The sample consisted of 117 senior executives (Males = 56; Females = 60; 1 unreported;
mean age = 40.54). Regression analyses indicated medium to large relationships between
ability EI scales and achieving business outcomes. The results held even after controlling
for reasoning ability, self-reported EI, and self-reported personality. Self-reported EI also
was linked to business outcomes, but was no longer significant when controlling for
personality. These results may have important implications for how we select and develop
executives.
Emotional Intelligence
In 1990, Salovey and Mayer drew together much of the material available on
cognition (i.e., capacity to carry out abstract reasoning) and emotion (defined as signals
that convey regular, discernable meanings about relationships) and developed a theory of
EI. There more recent refinement of their definition see EI as the ability to recognise
the meanings of emotions and their relationships and to reason and problem-solve based
on them (Mayer, Caruso & Salovey, 1999, p. 267). EI is seen as the capacity to perceive
emotions, assimilate emotion-related feelings, understand the information of those
emotions, and manage them.
Between 1994 and 1997, EI was popularized by psychologist and journalist Daniel
Goleman (1995) in his book Emotional Intelligence. During this time, Goleman (1995)
changed the definition quite substantially with the new definition appearing to equate EI
with good social behavior. Since 1997 there has been an explosion of activity in a new
and now fuzzily-defined area. EI is now used popularly to mean various things, including
motivation, empathy, sociability, warmth, and optimism (Mayer, 2001).
Mayer and Salovey (1997) argue that there are two major EI models that drive
research: (1) ability models and (2) mixed models. Ability models conceptualise EI in a
similar way to cognitive intelligence (i.e., Intelligence Quotient - IQ). These models
suggest that EI should develop over time, be correlated with measures of IQ, and be
measurable with a test based on performance (Ciarrochi, Chan & Caputi 2000; Mayer,
Salovey, & Caruso 2004). In contrast, mixed models of EI incorporate both non-cognitive
models (e.g., BarOn 1997) and competency-based models (e.g., Goleman 1995). These
mixed models typically overlap or mix with traditional models of personality and tend to
utilise self-reports as their primary mode of assessment.
Importantly, while each approach conceptualises and measures EI from different
perspectives, the approaches themselves appear to complement rather than contradict
each other (Ciarrochi, Chan & Caputi, 2000). For example, both mixed and ability based
measures of EI have been shown to be only modestly related to each other and to relate
to important criteria such as social support, mental health, and social behavior (Ciarrochi,
Scott, Deane, and Heaven, 2003; Mayer, Salovey and Caruso, 2004).
A critical question is whether EI is distinguishable from traditional measures of
personality and IQ. The ability based measure of EI has been repeatedly shown to have
incremental value over traditional measures (Ciarrochi, Chan, & Caputi, 2000; Ciarrochi,
Dean & Anderson, 2002; Mayer in press; Mayer, Salovey, Caruso, & Sitarenios, 2003;
Mayer, Salovey & Caruso). In contrast, mixed model measures of EI correlate, sometimes
strongly, with measures of personality (Ciarrochi, Chan & Caputi, 2000; Ciarrochi, Chan,
Caputi, and Roberts, 2001; MacCann, Roberts, Mathews, and Zeidner, 2004; Rosete
2004). Some research has shown that these measures can show incremental value over
personality (Ciarrochi, Chan, & Bajgar, 2001; Ciarrochi, Scott, Deane, & Heaven, 2003;
Ciarrochi, & Scott, 2005; Schutte, Malouff, Hall, Haggerty, Cooper, Golden, & Dornhein,
1997), whereas others have shown that they add little incremental value (Davies, Stankov,
& Roberts, 1998).
Our study sought to establish the extent to which mixed and ability based measures
of EI were useful for predicting leadership effectiveness over and above traditional
personality and cognitive ability measures.
In the present study we sought to replicate and extend past research by utilising a
larger sample, and administering both self-report and ability based measures of EI. We
also administered a measure of personality and reasoning ability, to examine the
incremental value of the EI measures over measures that are commonly used in the
organizational setting.
Method
Participants
The sample consisted of 122 executives from a large Australian Public Service
organisation. Executives who did not complete the main battery of measures or whose
performance ratings were not accessible were then excluded leaving a final sample of 117
executives (56 men, 60 women, 1 unreported; mean age 40.54, SD = 8.95). The final
sample consisted of both assistant directors (n = 64) and directors (n = 52) with 1
unreported. Multivariate analyses revealed that there were no differences between these
groups in terms of gender, age, tenure and qualifications.
Participants mean tenure within the organisation was approximately 15 years. The
level of qualifications amongst the sample was quite impressive with approximately 32%
reported to have completed a masters degree or higher; 22% reported to have completed
a postgraduate diploma or certificate; 33% reported to have completed an undergraduate
degree and 4% reported to have completed some form of associate diploma or certificate.
Procedure
Participants were administered a battery of psychological tests that assessed
personality, reasoning ability, and EI. In exchange for their participation, individuals were
provided with a confidential feedback report on their results on each of the instruments.
Materials
10
1997). In addition to representing a number of central EI dimensions, the SUEIT also had
the advantage of focusing on EI in the workplace, rather than general EI.
The SUEIT consists of 65 items that ask participants to indicate the extent to which a
particular statement is true of the way they typically think, feel and act at work (on a fivepoint scale: 1 = never, 2 = seldom, 3 = sometimes, 4 = usually, 5 = always). The SUEIT
report provides an overall EI score that indicates a participants general workplace EI, and
five sub-scale scores including (1) emotional recognition and expression (in oneself) which
is the ability to identify ones own feelings and emotional states, and the ability to express
those inner feelings to others (2) emotions direct cognition, which measure the extent to
which emotions and emotional knowledge is incorporated in decision making and/or
problem solving (3) understanding of emotions external, which is the ability to identify
and understand the emotions of others and those manifest in external stimuli (4)
emotional management, which is the ability to manage positive and negative emotions
both within oneself and others and (5) emotional control, which is how effectively
emotional states experienced at work such as anger, stress, anxiety and frustration are
controlled (Palmer and Stough, 2001, p. 5).
While a relatively new EI measure, there is growing research to support the SUEITs
reliability and predictive validity with leadership factors (Palmer and Stough, 2001; Palmer,
Walls, Burgess and Stough, 2001; Palmer, Gardner and Stough, 2003; Rosete, 2004).
11
recognised personality test within the Australian Public Service with availability of
Australian norms. The total scale contains 185 items and 16 subscales each containing 10
to 15 items.
12
achieved. The What section is always expressed in terms of both deliverable and
measurable targets.
The How focus of the PDA is on how leadership behaviors were adopted to achieve
business outputs. A significant part of the How aspect is an individuals ability to
demonstrate core leadership behaviours as outlined by the Australian Public Service
Commission (APSC) Leadership Capability Framework. This framework has five core
leadership capabilities: (1) Shapes strategic thinking (i.e., someone who inspires a sense
of purpose and direction; focuses strategically; harnesses information and opportunities;
and shows judgment, intelligence and commonsense); (2) Achieves results (i.e., someone
who builds organisational capability and responsiveness; marshals professional expertise;
ensures closure and delivers on intended results; and steers and implements changes and
deals with uncertainty); (3) Cultivates productive working relationships (i.e., someone who
nurtures internal and external relationships; values individual differences and diversity;
guides, mentors and develops people; and facilitates co-operation and partnerships); (4)
Communicates with influence (i.e., someone who communicates clearly; listens,
understands and adapts to an audience; and negotiates persuasively) and (5) Exemplifies
personal drive and integrity (i.e., someone who engages with risk and shows personal
courage; commits to action; displays resilience; and demonstrates self-awareness and a
commitment to personal development).
Both the What and How are initially rated on a five point scale (1 to 5) by the
participants direct manager. The meaning of the five ratings are:- (5) Exceptional Performance well beyond expectations, breaking new ground, producing outcomes of
considerable value to the organisation, often quite unanticipated; (4) Superior -
13
14
Consistent with the validity of this rating procedure, the What and How ratings have
been shown to correlate significantly with internal peer 360 performance ratings, r = .52
(Rosete 2004). In addition, past research has supported the validity of this sort of rating
system (McEvoy and Beatty, 1989; Hogan, Curphy and Hogan, 1994; Management
Advisory Committee 2001; Corporate Leadership Council, 2002). For example, McEvoy
and Beatty (1989) examined the predictive validity of performance evaluations compared
to assessment center ratings and concluded that performance ratings were as effective as
assessment center data in forecasting performance seven years later.
Results
Descriptive Statistics and Internal Reliabilities
The SUEIT
The means, standard deviations, and internal reliabilities of the SUEIT subscales
were (1) emotional recognition and expression (in oneself) (M = 37.93; SD = 6.19; r =
.74); (2) emotions direct cognition (M = 32.42; SD = 6.33; r = .78); (3) understanding of
emotions external (M = 74.86; SD = 8.83; r = .69); (4) emotional management (M =
41.15; SD = 5.82; r = .73); and (5) emotional control (M = 32.48; SD = 4.36; r = .76).
The sample mean for the overall SUEIT EI was 218.83 (SD = 22.28). These results are
similar to the normative sample (M = 226.75; SD = 17.25) as reported for the workplace
SUEIT (Palmer and Stough, 2001).
In four of the five sub-scales, and for the overall SUEIT EI score, the mean
percentile scores fell within the average range, whereas the Emotions Direct Cognition
(EDC) subscale fell within the low range. The EDC results indicate that generally,
15
executives within this organization may tend to exhibit a very analytical or technically
oriented decision-making style where decisions at work are predominantly made on facts
and technical information (Palmer and Stough 2001).
Further coefficient alpha analysis was carried out on the intercorrelations among the
five sub-scales and total SUEIT EI score. The sizes of these correlations are generally
higher than that reported for the workplace SUEIT (Palmer and Stough, 2001). In
particular, there is a substantial correlation between the Understanding of Emotions
External and Emotional Management (r = .60) scales suggesting that the subscales share
approximately 36% of the variance in this data set. The highest inter correlation was
found between Emotional Control and Emotional Management (r = .70) suggesting that
these scales share approximately 49% of the variance in this data set.
The MSCEIT
The means, standard deviations, and reliabilities of each of the subscales is as
follows (1) perceiving emotion (M = .51; r = .76), (2) using emotion to facilitate thought
(M = .49; = .70), (3) understanding emotion (M = .55; r = .78), and (4) managing
emotions (M = .41; r = .75). Overall, the raw MSCEIT sample mean of .49 (SD = .05)
scored similarly to the general scoring normative sample (M = .51; SD = .06) as reported
for the MSCEIT users manual (Mayer, et al, 2002). In all four of the branch level scores
and for the overall EI score, the mean scores fell within the average range.
Further correlational analyses were carried out on the four branch levels and total
MSCEIT EI score. Perceiving emotion was correlated with understanding emotion r = .48,
understanding emotions r = .16 and managing emotion r = .26. Using emotion with
16
correlated with understanding emotion r = .19, and managing emotion r = .34. Finally,
understanding emotion was correlated with managing emotion r = .38. The sizes of these
correlations are generally lower than that reported in the users manual (Mayer, et al,
2002).
17
more intelligent, brighter, and have higher general mental capacity than the normal
population, operating at the 84 percentile.
Correlational Analyses
Leadership Effectiveness
Pearson correlation coefficients were used to examine any relationship that may exist
between the SUEIT EI measure and leadership effectiveness as measured through the
performance management ratings. No significant correlations were found between total
SUEIT EI scores and any of the performance management measures. As can be seen in
Table 1, only the sub-branch factors of emotional management and emotional control
correlated significantly with one aspect of the performance rating, namely the how rating.
In contrast, the total EI score of the MSCEIT correlated with both what and how
ratings (see Table 1), indicating that higher ability EI was associated with better
performance. In addition, Table 1 illustrates that all of the EI subscales correlated with
how performance ratings, and two subscales, perceiving and managing emotions
correlated with what performance ratings.
Pearson correlation coefficients were also used to examine relationships between
personality factors, reasoning and leadership effectiveness as measured through the
performance management ratings. There were no significant correlations between the
five global factors and reasoning with performance ratings. At the primary factor
subscales there were some correlations. In particular, low levels of vigilance, high levels
of abstractedness, low levels of privateness, and high levels of openness to change with
18
higher what performance ratings. Low levels of vigilance were also associate with high
how performance ratings.
19
The results of the first regression analysis with the What ratings showed that neither
reasoning ability (Step 1, r = .01, R2change = .00, FChange = .00), the five global personality
factors of the 16PF (Step 2, r = .29 (R2change = .08, FChange = 1.89), or the self-report
measure of EI (Step 3, r = .31 (R2change = .00, FChange = .01), had a significant effect. In
contrast, the ability measure of EI produced significant improvement in the model (step 3,
r .37, R2change = .05, FChange = 6.22) accounting for approximately 5% of the variance in the
What perspective of the performance management system. The ability measure of EI thus
related to variance over and above that involving reasoning and personality.
The How rating analyses produced similar results. Essentially, the self-reported EI
measure did not predict variance over and above personality and reasoning, p > .2. In
contrast, the ability measure of EI significantly improved the fit of the model (Step 3b, r =
53, R2change =.22, FChange = 31.99). Thus, the ability measure of EI was able to explain an
additional 22% of the variance above both reasoning ability and personality
characteristics.
In addition to controlling for the global dimensions of personality (i.e., the Big Five),
we conducted analyses that controlled for the individual personality subscales (see Table
1). We focused on the subscales of EI that had been identified as significant. Hierarchical
regression analysis was conducted for the What performance rating with reasoning ability
at Step 1, the significant 16PF primary factors at Step 2 and any significant EI measure at
the branch level at Step 3. No significant self-report EI effects were found at step 3,
suggesting these measures do not add incremental value over and above personality and
reasoning. In contrast, we found that even when controlling for reasoning in step 1 and
the four significant 16PF subscales in step 2 (see Table 1), the MSCEIT subscales
20
(perceiving r = .26, p < .01 and managing emotions r = .08, p > .4) significantly
improved the fit of the model (Step 3, r = .46, R2change = .08, FChange = 5.10) and
accounted for 8% of the variance.
Finally, we ran hierarchical regressions that focused on the How ratings, and again
found that the self-reported EI measures did not contribute significantly to the model at
Step 3. In contrast, the ability based model (perceiving emotion, r = .40, p < .01, using
emotion r = .06, p > .5, understanding emotion r = .20, p < .05, and managing emotion r
= .05, p > .6) contributed significantly (Step 3, r = .54, R2change = .30, FChange = 9.52),
indicating that it predicted 30% of the variance in the How variance even after controlling
for reasoning and personality.
Discussion
The findings suggest that the executives who achieve the best business outcomes
are the ones who 1) score higher on an EI ability test, 2) manifest certain personality
characteristics (e.g., high openness, low vigilance and privateness), and 3) have higher
self-reported ability to manage emotions. Importantly, the ability measure of emotional
intelligence was able to predict effective leadership over and above already well
established workplace measures such as reasoning ability and personality. In contrast,
self-reported measures of emotional intelligence had little to offer over and above wellestablished measures of personality and reasoning. These results may have important
implications on how we engage in performance management, and select and develop
executives.
21
22
Our findings do not imply, however, that self-report EI measures have no practical
use. Both self-report EI and personality explained the same level of variance in leadership
success (about 4%). Thus, it may be that in some instances one might use the self-report
EI measure instead of a personality measure. Specifically, one might use the EI measure
because it provides more useable feedback to managers than personality measures. For
example, it might be more instructive to provide managers feedback on emotion
management skill (from the self-report EI measure) than on their level of privateness
(from the personality measure).
Future research is needed to evaluate whether self-report EI does have some added
practical value over personality measures. For example, do leaders improve their
performance more when provided with EI feedback compared to when they are provided
with personality feedback? Are self-report EI measures more useful for guiding
intervention programs?
23
relation to the How of performance, the capacity to perceive emotion, use emotions,
understand emotion and manage emotion all related to how effectively an executive
achieved their business outcomes.
George (2000) suggests that leadership involves the development of a collective
sense of goals, instilling in others both knowledge and appreciation of certain work
activities, and generating a sense of excitement, confidence, and trust. One could argue
that these elements all require a leader to use their emotions to enhance how they
communicate to subordinates, which in turn would assist them to effectively carry out the
role of a leader. Not surprisingly, we find all four elements are critical in enabling a leader
to meet business outcomes. The ability to perceive emotions provides an awareness of
emotions and the ability to accurately read others emotions, especially subordinates.
Using emotions provides a means to generate ideas within a team. Understanding
emotions offers insights into what motivates people and others points of view. Finally,
managing emotions may allow a leader to stay open to their emotions, extract valuable
information, and use them constructively to enhance team and individual performance
(Caruso & Salovey, 2004). All four elements may enhance a leaders ability to
communicate.
Of particular interest was the branch of perceiving emotion which in both the What
and How rating had the highest effect sizes. A leader who is skilled in perceiving emotion
would be described as someone who knows what people feel, reads people accurately, is
good at recognising their own feelings and can express their feelings appropriately
(Caruso & Salovey, 2004). These skills are particularly important as it allows a leader to
24
accurately capture important social data around them, in particular the ability to read
between the lines.
It is important to put the effect sizes we obtained here in context. Our effect size for
ability EI was in the order of .26 for the What outcomes and .50 for the How outcomes.
This effect level is considered to be large by many reviews (Barrick & Mount, 1991;
Schmidt & Hunter, 1998). For example, research has consistently shown that personality
traits such as conscientiousness are relate to most job performance criteria and typically
have an effect size in the order of .2 (Barrick & Mount, 1991). Schmidt and Hunter (1998)
have also shown that in analyzing 85 years of research in personnel selection that General
Mental Ability (GMA) tests have one of the highest effect sizes at .51.
The present results have potential implications for how we manage performance,
selection and development of executives. In terms of performance management, it is
important for an executive to both deliver outputs (What performance), and also deal
effectively with colleagues and staff (the How of performance - MAC 2001). It may be
common for technical leaders to have What but not How skills. For example, a technical
specialist may perform complex tasks tenaciously and manage to produce business
outputs, but may be ineffective at managing his or her subordinates, leading to issues of
staff turnover and underperformance. The results of this study show that emotional
intelligence may be useful in identifying who is and is not likely to deal effectively with
colleagues and staff. Furthermore, they also show which leaders are likely to achieve
business outcomes. Organizations who wish to maximize their ability to meet business
outcomes therefore have the choice to either recruit for these abilities or further develop
these abilities in their top executives.
25
26
References
Ashkanasy, N. M., E. Trevor-Roberts, et al. (2000). Leadership attributes and cultural
values in Australia and New Zealand compared: An initial report based on globe data.
27
Jossey-Bass.
Ciarrochi, J.V., Chan, A.Y.C. and Caputi P. (2000), A critical evaluation of the emotional
intelligence construct, Personality and Individual differences 28, pp. 539-561.
Ciarrochi, J.V., Chan, A.Y.C., Caputi P. and Roberts, R. (2001), Measuring Emotional
Intelligence. In Ciarrochi, Forgas and Mayer (Eds), Emotional Intelligence in
32(2), 197-209)
Ciarrochi, J., Scott, G., Deane, F.P., and Heaven, C.L. (2003), Relations between social
and emotional competence and mental health: a construct validation study,
28
29
30
31
traditional standards for an intelligence? Some new data and conclusions. Emotion,
1, pp 196 231.
Rosete, D. (2004). A Leaders profile - What attributes make an effective leader? 4th
International Test Users' Conference, Melbourne, Australia.
Rosete, D. and J. Ciarrochi (in press). Emotional intelligence and its relationship to
workplace performance outcomes of leadership effectiveness. Leadership and
Personality, 9, 185-211.
Salovey, P., Mayer, J., Goldman, S., Turvey, C. & Palfai, T. (1995). Emotional attention,
clarity and repair: Exploring emotional intelligence using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale.
In J.W. Pennebaker, Emotion, disclosure, and health (pp. 125-154) Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Schutte, N. S., J. M. Malouff, Hall, Haggerty, Cooper, Golden and Dornhein, (1997).
Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence. Personality and
32
Table 1 Correlations Coefficients between the MSCEIT, SUEIT, Personality and Reasoning Scales
with Performance Management Ratings
Performance
Performance How
What Rating
Rating
Total EI Score
.26**
.50**
Perceiving Emotion
.27*
.48**
Using Emotion
.18
.30**
Understanding Emotion
.11
.29**
Managing Emotions
.23*
.26**
SUEIT EI
.09
.15
.06
.02
-.01
-.02
.00
.08
Emotional Management
.09
.23*
Emotional Control
.17*
.21*
Vigilance
-.21*
-.18*
Abstractness
.22*
.08
Privateness
-.18*
-.14
Openness to change
.25*
.18
Reasoning
-.00
.12
Self-reported EI
Expression
Personality (16PF)
33
Table 2
Personality
ER&E
EDC
UEE
EM
EC
EI
MSCEIT
PE
UFE
UnE
ME
EI
Extraversion
0.52**
0.57**
0.34**
0.27**
0.43**
0.21*
0.15
0.06
0.17
0.10
0.22*
Anxiety
0.35**
-0.18*
0.00
-0.16
0.45**
0.44**
-0.28**
-0.22*
-0.24
-0.06
0.28**
Tough Mindedness
0.29**
0.34**
0.36**
-0.18*
-0.05
-0.08
-0.07
0.05
-0.04
-0.17
-0.14
Independence
0.43**
0.42**
0.24**
0.29
0.32**
0.24**
0.08
-0.07
0.10
0.08
0.18
Self Control
-0.06
0.22**
-0.22*
-0.04
0.13
0.14
0.00
0.07
-0.03
-0.20*
0.00
Reasoning
0.02
0.10
-0.02
-0.07
-0.09
0.15
0.11
0.14
-0.07
0.31*
-0.03
34