Archea - 1977 - The Place of Architectural Factors in Behavioral Theories of Privacy
Archea - 1977 - The Place of Architectural Factors in Behavioral Theories of Privacy
Archea - 1977 - The Place of Architectural Factors in Behavioral Theories of Privacy
V O L U M E 33~NUMBER 3 . 1977
116
ARCHITECTURAL FACTORS
117
118
JOHN ARCHEA
ARCHITECTURAL FACTORS
119
120
JOHNARCHEA
Information Fields
T h e task of explicating the behavior-related attributes of the
physical environment necessarily falls to those who presume that
ARCHITECTURAL FACTORS
121
122
JOHN ARCHEA
experiential attributes of individuals and by the normative attributes of situations. T h e main point is that, despite an expectation
of considerable individual and circumstantial variability, the extent
to which the arrangement of their immediate physical surroundings permits people to see or be seen is regarded as the pivotal
link between environment and behavior.
Within this framework, the major behavior-related characteristic of the man-made or the natural environment is the manner
in which it concentrates, diffuses, segregates, or otherwise localizes
information. A closely related quality is the manner in which
physical surroundings facilitate the manifestation of the information that is present.
From this analysis it follows that the environmental attributes
relevant to understanding interpersonal situations are: (a) the
means by which the environment channels, obstructs, or otherwise
regulates the distribution of perceptible energies; and (b) the
means by which the environment transduces, amplifies, contrasts,
or otherwise mediates the appearance of available information.
Among the underlying physical properties that constrain the ways
in which visibility and other environmental attributes can be
conceptualized and measured are the position, extent, fixity,
density, color, radiance, and transparency or opacity of the
architectural components of settings.
Whereas the environmental or architectural attributes of a
situation affect the flow and appearance of information, the
interrelated behavioral attributes affect the process of decoding
and encoding that information. They include: the respective
locations of the participants in a situation, their head and body
orientations, the acuity of their various sensory modalities, their
psychomotor responsiveness, their familiarity with the setting, the
routinization of certain activities, and the normative or symbolic
associations shared among the participants. The underlying
human properties which limit the ways in which people can
exchange information with each other are: unidirectional vision
of high resolution but limited lateral extent, omnidirectional
audition of somewhat lower resolution, the rates at which information is processed, the categorical structure of short- and long-term
memory, and the mechanics of verbal and nonverbal expression.
Some of these characteristics are similar to the strictly behavioral constructs that were criticized earlier in this paper. The
main difference is that the framework proposed here requires
simultaneous consideration of the behavioral and the environmental attributes of situations. In contradistinction to an analysis
ARCHITECTURAL FACTORS
123
A MODEL
O F S P A T I A BEHAVIOR
L
In the framework proposed here, visual access and visual
exposure are the most fundamental attributes that subsume both
the environmental and the behavioral aspects of interpersonal
situations. In this section a model of spatial behavior is outlined,
based upon the interaction of visual access and exposure as both
are constrained by the properties of physical and human systems.
Visual Access
Visual access is the ability to monitor ones immediate spatial
surroundings by sight. A persons visual access within an informal
social setting establishes the range of opportunities available for
synchronizing that persons behavior with the behavior of those
who share the setting. The amount of information available from
ones immediate social surroundings determines both the number
of potential interpersonal relationships from which one can choose
and the number of cues available for anticipating changes in
those relationships. T h e extent to which individuals can maintain
active surveillance of their physical surroundings provides the
means for identifying the range of behaviors which are acceptable
or appropriate within the prevailing social context. Visual access
is a function of the positions of walls, doors, mirrors, and other
opaque or reflective surfaces relative to ones own position (position referring to the combined effect of location and orientation).
With regard to visual access, ones location limits the amount
of information about people or events that one can acquire directly,
over time. Opportunities for coordinating or integrating behavior
with the activities of other individuals are directly related to the
manner in which ones location in space enables him or her to
monitor the sociospatial dynamics of the situation in which he
or she is involved. The time available to adjust to changing
124
JOHN ARCHEA
ARCHITECTURAL FACTORS
125
Gradients
I n formulating visual access and visual exposure in this
manner, particular consideration has been given to the ways in
which the barriers and channels formed by the physical environment interact with the limited lateral range of the human visual
field to mediate the acquisition and presentation of information.
Further consideration of the rates at which people can process
incoming information suggests another element of the model.
Visual access and exposure gradients are abrupt changes in
the amount of ambient information available in the immediate
vicinity of openings and edges in surrounding visual barriers.
A change either in ones own position or in the positions of
others relative to access o r exposure gradients between adjacent
social settings establishes the internal stability of the situation
in which one is involved. Abrupt changes in the quality of a
persons visual access o r exposure will determine both the rate
at which information about new social opportunities becomes
available to that person and the rate at which his o r her own
behavior is revealed to new observers. If the amount or source
of ambient information changes at a rate that exceeds a persons
ability to accommodate it, then certain types of momentary
behavioral disorganization may be triggered. T h e rate of change
(the slope of an information gradient) is a function of the proximity
to edges o r openings in fixed visual barriers, momentary shifts
in the position of doors o r other semifixed barriers, and the
sizes of the spaces which are suddenly revealed o r concealed.
With regard to gradients, location-specific inflections in the
course of ones behavior can be expected to occur whenever one
changes ones own position relative to an edge o r opening in
a nearby visual barrier, thereby rendering oneself party to potential
oppol tunities and sanctions operating in an adjacent setting. T h e
rate at which a person must adjust his or her behavior to
accommodate altered social circumstances is directly related to
movement speed and proximity to an edge o r opening. Someone
who intends to minimize the social impact of sudden changes
126
JOHN ARCHEA
ARCHITECTURAL FACTORS
127
128
JOHN AKCHEA
A R C H I T E C T U R A L FACTORS
129
are the lateral range of the human visual field and the relative
positions of the walls, doors, corners, and other visual regulators
for a specific setting. Thus, the various potentials for access and
exposure can be calculated for each location within a bounded
space, and for each orientation at each location. Gradients are
identified by the differences in access and exposure values calculated for adjacent locations. T h e access and exposure surfaces
produced by this method are analogous to the trend surfaces
recently reported in analyzing the distributions of certain social
indicators in urban areas (Lewis, 1977).
IMPLICA
T I O N S F O R PRIV A c Y
According to the model, the physical environment is a relatively stable assembly of walls, doors, corners, and other regulators
of the flow of information. T h e physical arrangement of a given
setting remains substantially unchanged from one situation to
the next. T h e dynamic influence of the environment on interpersonal behavior stems from the ways in which it is used. By selecting
or changing ones location o r orientation, one establishes a potential
for obtaining and conveying behaviorally relevant information.
In Yffect, we change our environment by changing our position
within it.
Consider a large lamp placed on a desk so that it lies just
off the line of sight between the desk chair and the point where
colleagues o r visitors would enter the room. When someone enters,
the person sitting at the desk has the option of casually shifting
his o r her position so that the lamp blocks the visitors view.
As a consequence, the guest is denied access to the cues needed
to determine the hosts receptiveness to intrusion. This awkward
situation can be relieved if the host simply moves his o r her
head a few inches so that the lamp no longer obstructs the mutual
gaze paths between the potential interactants. This subtle manipulation of visual access and exposure can make a major difference
in the respective abilities of the guest and host to read each
others intentions and to synchronize their behavior. In this case,
the physical environment is an instrument which is used selectively
to inhibit o r facilitate the flow of interpersonal information.
Through the judicious selection of ones position in space,
numerous behavioral advantages can be obtained. Opportunities
for identifying the appropriate points to enter o r withdraw from
an activity and for fostering o r deterring the notice of ones
activities taken by others are both mediated by the manner in
130
JOHN ARCHEA
ARCHITECTURAL FACTORS
131
132
JOHN ARCHEA
Selective Conspicuousness
T h e management of behavior and position to further ones
own interpersonal objectives hinges on the notion that the most
accurate and vivid information about oneself is conveyed during
moments of high visual exposure. By continually realigning their
own behavior to meet the social obligations inherent in their spatial
circumstance, people regulate the availability of information about
themselves. Yet the act of determining the type of behavior which
is appropriate at a given time and place is itself a function of
a persons awareness of the interpersonal opportunities open to
them. This, in turn, depends upon visual access. Thus, to a large
extent, the process of controlling the manner in which information
about oneself is made available to others is a function of visual
access and visual exposure-as
both are constrained by the
arrangement of ones physical surroundings.
T h e concept which ties ones presentation of oneself to visual
access and exposure, and to privacy, is conspicuousness: the degree
to which a persons sociospatial display is discernible amid the
collective sociospatial displays of the surrounding persons and
events. T h e process of deliberately regulating behavior or position
to attain a desired degree of privacy is selective conspicuousness.
As the control of information about the self, privacy has
been characterized as a process of selective concealment and
disclosure (Margulis, Note 1). As the chief means by which ones
own privacy is regulated, selective conspicuousness establishes a
trade off between the spatial and the behavioral options available
in interpersonal situations. Conspicuousness is related first to the
likelihood that ones behavior will be noticed by others and second
to the appropriateness of that behavior. The mechanics of identifying the prevailing range of appropriate behaviors and of
attracting notice to oneself are initially constrained spatially,
through visual access and exposure, respectively. Ones skill at
selecting among such possibilities to initiate a contingent course
of action which satisfies ones own social objectives is the essence
of privacy regulation. What one chooses to do and where one
chooses to do it constitute the information for which others will
hold one personally accountable.
From this perspective, privacy regulation involves the simulta-
ARCHITECTURAL FACTORS
133
Loss of Privacy
I n this analysis, the loss of privacy is related to either too
much o r too little conspicuousness. T h e condition of being overly
conspicuous is partly a function of too much exposure or too
little access. Being spatially overexposed simply means that more
information about oneself is available to others than one desires.
Having poor visual access means that ones ability to gauge the
appropriateness of behavior vis-A-vis others is hampered, thereby
increasing the likelihood of pursuing a course of behavior for
which one would rather not be held accountable.
Although becoming more conspicuous than one intends is
the primary problem in the regulation of privacy, being too
inconspicuous can also present difficulties. Inconspicuousness is
related to either too little exposure o r too much access. Insufficient
exposure simply means that ones behavior cannot be accurately
acknowledged by others. This could be a serious problem in
preparing o r correcting the context in which ones future or past
behavior is assessed. O n the other hand, a person with too much
access picks u p information about others which that person would
prefer not to have. This has two potential consequences for the
unwilling recipient. T h e first is the possibility of being drawn
into events that will compromise ones abilities to control the
presentation of certain information about oneself. T h e second
is the possibility of becoming encumbered with inappropriate or
undesirable information about others which may be difficult to
conceal on later occasions. Being vulnerable to revealing trace
information that one has inadvertently acquired about another
if that
person infringes on the privacy of both-particularly
information o r its possession conflicts with a long standing relationship.
To illustrate the architectural dynamics of privacy, consider
a solitary person surrounded by thick stone walls which allow
virtually no sound transmission. With a single unlocked door,
134
JOHN ARCHEA
Co NCL USION
No matter how we conceptualize privacy, we cannot escape
the fact that the behavior required to attain or maintain it occurs
in an environment for which physical properties can be specified.
T h e existence of such environments generally precedes and
survives the situations to which privacy is attributed. Places do
not acquire their reality status because they are used, but because
their intrinsic physical properties constrain the manner in which
they can be used. T h e physical environment channels and obstructs
the distribution of virtually all information upon which the
regulation of privacy depends. People cannot act upon behavioral
cues which are obscured by physical barriers nor can they prevent
others from acting upon cues about themselves which are not
likewise obscured. T h e physical environment presents everyone
with a set of initial conditions upon which all behavior is largely
contingent. As such, it confronts the behavioral scientist with an
independent variable, the salience of which has been too long
denied.
T o date, instead of theories which link environmental variance
to specific physical properties, the field of environmental psychology has produced only psychological representations of the environment. T h e model of visual access and exposure proposed
here is a step toward the development of a capability for defining
and measuring the physical environment independently of the
ARCHITECTURAL FACTORS
135
REFERENCES
Altman, I. Privacy: A conceptual analysis. I n D. H . Carson (Ed.), Man-environment interactions: Evaluations and applications; (Part 11, Vol. 6: S.
T. Margulis, Vol. Ed.). Washington, D.C.: Environmental Design Re-
136
JOHN ARCHEA
ARCHITECTURAL FACTORS
137