Kids and Credibility: An Empirical Examination of Youth, Digital Media Use, and Information Credibility
Kids and Credibility: An Empirical Examination of Youth, Digital Media Use, and Information Credibility
Kids and Credibility: An Empirical Examination of Youth, Digital Media Use, and Information Credibility
This report was made possible by a grant from the John D. and Catherine
T. MacArthur Foundation in connection with its grant making initiative
on Digital Media and Learning. For more information on the initiative
visit www.macfound.org.
The John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Reports on
Digital Media and Learning
New Digital Media and Learning as an Emerging Area and “Worked Examples”
as One Way Forward by James Paul Gee
Living and Learning with New Media: Summary of Findings from the Digital
Youth Project by Mizuko Ito, Heather Horst, Matteo Bittanti, danah
boyd, Becky Herr-Stephenson, Patricia G. Lange, C. J. Pascoe, and Laura
Robinson with Sonja Baumer, Rachel Cody, Dilan Mahendran, Katynka
Z. Martínez, Dan Perkel, Christo Sims, and Lisa Tripp
Young People, Ethics, and the New Digital Media: A Synthesis from the
GoodPlay Project by Carrie James with Katie Davis, Andrea Flores, John
M. Francis, Lindsay Pettingill, Margaret Rundle, and Howard Gardner
The Civic Potential of Video Games by Joseph Kahne, Ellen Middaugh, and
Chris Evans
Kids and Credibility
with Ethan Hartsell, Alex Markov, Ryan Medders, Rebekah Pure, and Elisia Choi
This book was set in Stone Sans and Stone Serif by the MIT Press.
Printed and bound in the United States of America.
Flanagin, Andrew J.
Kids and credibility : an empirical examination of youth, digital media
use, and information credibility / Andrew J. Flanagin and Miriam
Metzger; with Ethan Hartsell ... [et al.].
p. cm.—(The John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation
reports on digital media and learning)
Includes bibliographical references.
ISBN 978-0-262-51475-0 (pbk. : alk. paper)
1. Mass media and youth—United States. 2. Digital media—United States
—Social aspects. 3. Electronic information resources—United States.
4. Information behavior—United States. 5. Truthfulness and falsehood
—United States. I. Metzger, Miriam J. II. Hartsell, Ethan. III. Title.
HQ799.2.M35F53 2010 302.23’10835—dc22 2009054316
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Contents
Research Approach 6
Overview 6
Survey Methodology 9
Sample Characteristics 12
Research Findings 15
Internet Usage among Youth 15
Perceived Trust and Credibility of Web-Based Information 31
Factors Affecting Children’s Credibility Evaluations 57
Child/Parent Dyads and Credibility Assessments 73
Web Site Exposure and Evaluation 82
Overview
Survey Methodology
Survey Administration
The survey was conducted online by the research firm Knowl-
edge Networks and was fielded between June 17 and July 26,
2009. Knowledge Networks maintains a probability-based panel
of participants and is thus the only online survey source that
meets the standard of federal and peer review, setting the gold
standard in the industry. As mentioned earlier, 2,747 children
in the United States between the ages of 11 and 18 who use the
Internet and who live at home, as well as one parent for each
child participant, completed the survey. Statistical results were
weighted to correct known demographic discrepancies between
the U.S. population and Knowledge Networks’ online panel.
Details on the design, execution, and weighting procedures of
the survey are discussed below. Additional information about
the survey methodology and subject panel used by Knowledge
Networks can be found in Appendix B.
Sample Design
Knowledge Networks has recruited the first online research
panel that is representative of the entire U.S. population. Panel
members are randomly recruited by probability-based sampling
(telephone, mail-, and Web-based surveys), and households are
provided with access to the Internet and hardware if needed
(although this did not apply to the current survey, since our
target sample included only current Internet users). After ini-
tially accepting the invitation by Knowledge Networks to join
the panel, respondents are then profiled online by answering
demographic questions, and maintained on the panel using the
10 Research Approach
Contact Procedures
Potential participants received a notification email letting them
know there is a new survey available for them to take. This
Research Approach 11
Sample Weighting
The survey responses were weighted to provide results that are
generalizable to the U.S. population of Internet households.
Two weighting strategies were employed to compensate for
12 Research Approach
Sample Characteristics
Child Demographics
The child respondents consisted of 53 percent males and 47
percent females ranging in age from 11 to 18 years, with an
average age of 14.33 (standard deviation = 2.28). Table 2 shows
the percentage of children in the sample within each age group
surveyed. Seventy-five percent of the child respondents
reported that they were white; 9 percent were black, non-His-
panic; 12 percent were Hispanic; 0.4 percent were other, non-
Hispanic; and 4 percent reported being Mixed race,
non-Hispanic.
14 Research Approach
Table 1
Percent of participants from various U.S. geographic areas
Table 2
Number and percent of participants within each age category sampled
11 378 13.8
12 371 13.5
13 385 14
14 323 11.8
15 327 11.9
16 316 11.5
17 368 13.4
18 279 10.2
Research Findings
20
Average hours per week online
15
10
0
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 1
Weekly usage of the Internet by age
100%
90%
80%
70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 2
Percentage of parents restricting their child’s Internet access by age of
child
Very often
Frequency of use
Often
Sometimes
Rarely
Never
e on kin g o
ur n p te
es ay bl ip e
Cr o t a F od e
te ine irt s st
W B og/ ass l w re
l e ld
s
it S ph on au nal
rm om c eb n
io h o te
o e
ik ine
Bu t am /p ia
st W m wit nl e
ar rite eth h a ine
so k/s blo on r
e ri fo e
st g vi m
ia
to om os
te or n g ata
ak p nl jo d
pr Pl al/ ik ofil
o l
m to g/ lin
p n
on g in
fo s ne w io
lin ati or kin de
ea nl v ile ca
en g og ed
y sel es rofi
ed
jo o g si
or id on ifi or
at et all si
Li thin es/ ru
bl cl ua ha
M on n o ine d a
a eo
en /s de
a rs e ur
n in nl
W nl
in ell o al ct
on rm etw or vi
Ra tw o in v
ip
n W r
an info l n etw h a
in
ad d cia al n atc
so f
Re Fin so oci W
e
se si
k i
U Vi
e
at e s
Re
U pd s
Po
e
Ed
U
Figure 3
Children’s use of Web-based information resources
Very often
Often
Sometimes
Rarely
Never
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 4
Frequency of file sharing by age
Very often
Often
Sometimes
Rarely
Never
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 5
Frequency of posting original content by age
22 Research Findings
Very often
Often
Sometimes
Rarely
Never
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 6
Frequency of using social networking sites by age
Very often
Often
Sometimes
Rarely
Never
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 7
Frequency of visiting virtual worlds by age
Research Findings 23
more skilled users, girls, and those who spent more time online
per week. Children who earned lower grades in school and had
less parental control of their Internet access and use also showed
higher use of social networking sites. In addition, those relying
on group processes to discern credibility and those with “heuris-
tic” decision making styles (as opposed to more analytical styles,
covered in a later section of this report, “Factors Affecting Chil-
dren’s Credibility Evaluations”) were also heavier social network
users.
Information contribution scores were derived from such things
as the extent to which children reported creating personal Web
sites, blogs, or journals, posting information to groups or shar-
ing files with others, or rating people or things online, through
sites like Amazon, eBay, IMDb, or YouTube (Cronbach’s alpha =
0.77). Greater Internet skill, reliance on group processes for
making credibility decisions, time online per week, and age
resulted in greater information contribution. In addition,
among other factors, grades in school were negatively related to
information contribution, girls were more likely to contribute,
and high Internet social trust led to greater contribution,
although higher general social trust led to lower information
contribution.
Virtual uses consisted of representing oneself with an avatar,
visiting virtual worlds, or playing games with others who are
also online (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.71). The strongest predictors
of heavy virtual uses were hours online per week, sex (with girls
more likely to engage in virtual uses), Internet skill, and age. In
addition, virtual users tended to rely on group processes more in
making credibility assessments, had been taught at some point
24 Research Findings
Very often
Often
Sometimes
Rarely
Never
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 8
Frequency of social networking use by age
Very often
Often
Sometimes
Rarely
Never
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 9
Frequency of “virtual” Web use by age
26 Research Findings
Very often
Often
Sometimes
Rarely
Never
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 10
Frequency of commercial Web use by age
Research Findings 27
Very often
Often
Sometimes
Rarely
Never
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 11
Frequency of online information contribution by age
overall, and 23 percent said that it would not make any differ-
ence to their life overall if they could not go online again. In
general, as their age increases children report that their lives
would be progressively worse without Internet access, indicat-
ing that older children experience increased reliance on the
Internet.
Summary
Overall, findings indicate that children are actively using the
Internet for a variety of reasons. Other than the reported fre-
quency with which children’s use of the Web is virtual, it seems
that use of the Internet for myriad purposes increases with age
and, therefore, with the total amount of time spent online.
Research Findings 31
This increase in time spent online with age may be due to the
corresponding reduction of parental control. Interestingly,
although many children said there is too much information
online, the majority of children also indicated their Internet
skill was fairly high, suggesting that while children may be
overwhelmed or bombarded with information, they are confi-
dent in their ability to decipher and filter through it. This sug-
gests that children believe they are relatively capable of
successfully navigating the digital world, a premise that we
explore in much greater depth in the remainder of this mono-
graph, particularly as it relates to information credibility.
70%
60%
Percent believing
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
None of it A little of it Some of it A lot of it All of it
Figure 12
Amount of online information that children find credible
Research Findings 33
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often
Figure 13
Frequency of thinking about credibility when online
45%
40%
35%
30%
25%
20%
15%
10%
5%
0%
Not at all A little Somewhat Concerned Very
concerned concerned concerned concerned
Figure 14
How concerned others should be about credibility
34 Research Findings
70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
Most people can Most people Most people Most people can
almost always can usually usually cannot almost never
be trusted be trusted be trusted be trusted
Figure 15
General social trust
Research Findings 35
70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
Most people on Most people Most people Most people on
the Internet can on the Internet on the Internet the Internet can
almost always can usually usually cannot almost never
be trusted be trusted be trusted be trusted
Figure 16
General Internet social trust
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
Not at all A little bit Some A lot A whole lot
Figure 17
Degree that children trust strangers they meet online
Not at all
A little bit
Some
A lot
A whole lot
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 18
Degree that children trust strangers they meet online by age
Research Findings 37
50%
45%
40%
35%
30%
25%
20%
15%
10%
5%
0%
Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often
Figure 19
Frequency that parents talk with their children about online credibility
Very likely
Likelihood of believing
information online
Likely
Somewhat
likely
A little bit
likely
Not at all
likely
Health News Something Entertain- People Information
to buy ment you meet for
online schoolwork
Information type
Figure 20
Credibility of online information by information type
50%
45%
40%
35%
30%
25%
20%
15%
10%
5%
0%
Blogs are Blogs are Blogs are Blogs are Blogs are
much less somewhat equally somewhat much more
believable less believable believable more believable believable
Figure 21
Credibility of blogs compared to newspapers and television
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
Not at all A little bit Some A lot A whole lot
Figure 22
Credibility of Wikipedia
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
Health News Commercial Entertainment School
Which source would you believe most for this type of information?
Internet
Television
Books
Magazines
Newspapers
Radio
In Person
Figure 23
Most credible source by information type
Research Findings 47
A whole lot
A lot
Some
A little
Not at all
Newspapers Television Internet
Figure 24
Degree to which others should find information credible by medium
Very often
Often
Sometimes
Rarely
Never
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 25
Use of credibility evaluation methods by age
50 Research Findings
Table 3
Importance of various credibility cues
Summary
Overall, kids view information on the Web as relatively credi-
ble, and they distinguish across information type and informa-
tion source when determining the credibility of online
information. They show an awareness of the possibility of nega-
tive consequences stemming from false information online,
and admit that they should probably believe some information,
such as that on Wikipedia, less than they actually do. Moreover,
children trust information more or less depending on its type,
56 Research Findings
(see Eastin 2008), income (see van Dijk 2006), or by other types
of demographic groupings.
We also suspected that certain patterns of Internet usage, access,
and experiences could impact young people’s perceptions of
credibility online. In terms of usage, we investigated how much
time young people spend with the Internet (per week as well as
the number of years they have been using the Internet), their
level of skill (technical and search skill, as well as knowledge of
the latest online trends and features, as detailed earlier under
“Children’s Web and Internet Skill Levels” in the section on
“Internet Usage among Youth”), and their use of the Internet for
specific activities (social networking, contributing information
online, visiting virtual worlds, and using the Web for commercial
purposes—see the earlier discussion of “Children’s Online Activi-
ties” in the section on “Internet Usage among Youth”).
We also looked at the extent to which children’s parents con-
trolled or restricted their access and use of the Internet. To do
this, we created a measure that reflected how many restrictions
the parent of each child reported imposing in the home, rang-
ing from 0 (parent sets no restrictions on child’s use of the Inter-
net) to 4 (parent implements all of the control mechanisms
detailed previously under “Parental Control of Children’s Access
to and Use of the Internet” in the section on “Internet Usage
among Youth”).
Young people’s prior experiences with online information
and its evaluation were also included here in order to under-
stand their effect on credibility perceptions. Specifically, we
asked children whether they had ever had a bad experience
using some information they found online that turned out not
Research Findings 59
how much they feel other people can be trusted, either online
or in person (see “Trust and the Negative Consequences of False
Information Online” in the previous section, “Perceived Trust
and Credibility of Web-based Information”).
The last factor we considered was strategies or methods for
evaluating credibility, or how the process of evaluating informa-
tion online influences the assessments that young information
consumers make. We analyzed two sets of strategies for infor-
mation evaluation. First, we posed the question, “When you
decide what information to believe on the Internet, do you . . .
[give careful thought to the information, rely on your gut feel-
ings, ask for help from other people, etc.].” Research in cogni-
tive psychology, information processing, and, especially,
adolescent decision making (Jacobs and Klaczynski 2005), indi-
cates that adolescents primarily approach information analyti-
cally or heuristically when making decisions (Klaczynski 2001).
Analytic processing involves effortful and deliberate consider-
ation of information; heuristic decisions are made more quickly,
with less cognitive effort and scrutiny. Additional research sug-
gests another decision-making strategy that may be relevant to
evaluating credibility online, namely, relying on others to help
make decisions (Scott and Bruce 1995). Given the Internet’s vast
and increasing capacity for social interaction, and the impor-
tance of relying on social means of credibility assessment found
in previous work on adults (Metzger, Flanagin, and Medders,
forthcoming), we included heuristic, analytic, and social or
group approaches or strategies of evaluating credibility in our
analysis (see also “Methods of Determining Information and
Source Credibility,” in the previous section on “Perceived Trust
and Credibility of Web-based Information”).
Research Findings 61
Summary
As we suspected, data show that young people’s uses of the
Internet, demographic and personality traits, and specific strat-
egies for evaluating credibility all played a role in the judg-
ments that children make about whether to trust information
and people they encounter online.
Among the more consistent predictors of both credibility
concern and beliefs were the personality traits of need for cog-
nition and kids’ propensity to trust others, either offline or
online. Online experiences also mattered, as credibility beliefs
seem to be shaped by past negative experiences with finding
and using bad information. The old adage of “once bitten, twice
shy” definitely seemed to operate in this arena.
Usage was important too, and in particular using the Inter-
net to immerse oneself in virtual environments (i.e., playing
72 Research Findings
My child/parent
Who is better at figuring out good
is much better
than me
or bad information?
We are equally
good
I am much better
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 26
Child/parent differences in discerning good versus bad information on-
line
76 Research Findings
My child/parent
is much more
Who is more likely to believe
likely than me
false information online?
We are equally
likely
I am much
more likely
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 27
Child/parent differences in discerning false information online
Research Findings 77
My child/parent
Who is more likely to question the
is much more
information they find online?
likely than me
We are equally
likely
I am much
more likely
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 28
Child/parent differences in the likelihood of questioning information
online
78 Research Findings
A typical user
Who is better at figuring out good
is much better
you or a typical Internet user?
than me
or bad information online,
We are equally
good
I am much
better
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 29
Child/parent differences in discerning good versus bad information on-
line, compared to a typical Internet user
A typical user
Who is more likely to believe false
is much more
likely than me
Internet user?
We are equally
likely
I am much
more likely
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 30
Child/parent differences in the likelihood of believing false informa-
tion online, compared to a typical Internet user
Summary
Overall, compelling evidence exists for an optimistic bias in
individuals’ perceptions of their ability to evaluate the credibil-
ity of information online, especially on the part of parents.
Clear evidence for an optimistic bias exists across several mea-
sures, for parents compared to their children and for both par-
ents and children compared to a typical Internet user.
Although compared to their children parents might be realis-
tic in their estimations, it is unlikely that, as a group, the par-
ents and kids surveyed are uniformly more accurate in their
judgments of credible information online than are typical Inter-
net users. Our data are consistent with some of the preliminary
trends that have emerged from research into the optimistic bias
Research Findings 81
A typical user
is much more
Who is more likely to question
likely than me
information online,
We are equally
likely
I am much
more likely
11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Age of child
Figure 31
Child/parent differences in the likelihood of questioning information
online, compared to a typical Internet user
Figure 32
Example encyclopedia entry
A whole lot
How much do you believe
this information?
A lot
Some
A little
Not at all
One-sided entry Balanced entry
Figure 33
Credibility of one-sided versus balanced encyclopedia entries
A whole lot
How much do you believe
A lot
this entry?
Some
A little
Not at all
Wikipedia Citizendium Encyclopaedia
Britannica
Figure 34
Credibility of encyclopedia entry by perceived source
88 Research Findings
Figure 35
Example encyclopedia entry
90 Research Findings
A whole lot
How much do you believe
A lot
this entry?
Some
A little
Not at all
Wikipedia Citizendium Encyclopaedia
Britannica
Placement of encyclopedia entry
Figure 36
Credibility of encyclopedia entry by placement of entry
Research Findings 91
A whole lot
How much do you believe
A lot
this entry?
Some
A little
Not at all
Wikipedia Citizendium Encyclopaedia
Britannica
Placement of encyclopedia entry
Figure 37
Credibility of encyclopedia entry by actual encyclopedia source
92 Research Findings
Figure 38
Example product Web page
The quality is
much better
than average
How would you rate the
quality of this product?
The quality is
about average
The quality is
much worse
than average 4 ratings 16 ratings 102 ratings 1,002 ratings
received received received received
Figure 39
Product quality by number of ratings, across average product ratings
96 Research Findings
The quality is
much better
than average
How would you rate the
quality of this product?
The quality is
about average
The quality is
much worse
than average 1.6 2.32 3.0 3.68 4.4 4.84 5.0
Average product rating
Figure 40
Product quality by average product ratings, across number of ratings
Very
important
Importance of credibility cue
Important
Somewhat
important
A little
important
Not at all
important
Who sells Who makes Customer Product details Price
the product the product reviews (size, color, etc.)
or ratings
Figure 41
Importance of credibility cues for purchasing decisions
others. Moreover, even after learning that the site was a fake,
these children had difficulty indicating why this was the case,
in spite of clear cues present on the site.
To further assess the degree to which children believe ficti-
tious information online, we presented children with one of
two “hoax” Web sites6 currently present on the Internet. The
first site detailed “the first male pregnancy,” and included infor-
mation about the pregnant individual and testimonials and
links to media coverage of the pregnancy (see figure 42). The
Figure 42
Male pregnancy hoax Web site
Research Findings 99
Figure 43
Rennets hoax Web site
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
Not at all A little bit Some A lot A whole lot
How much do you believe the information on this Web site?
Figure 44
Credibility of male pregnancy hoax Web site
102 Research Findings
the site to look good. On average, they found that the informa-
tion on the site was not similar to their own beliefs. Figure 45
shows the extent to which children found the site to be
believable.
Finally, we were interested to know whether there were cer-
tain background and demographic characteristics, Internet
usage and experience patterns, personality traits, or methods of
evaluating credibility that were related to believing the hoax
sites.
We found no differences in income, race, or school grades
among children on how believable they found the information
on either of the hoax Web sites to be. However, females and
younger children were more likely to believe the hoax sites.
More specifically, 11-year-olds were significantly more likely to
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
Not at all A little bit Some A lot A whole lot
How much do you believe the information on this Web site?
Figure 45
Credibility of rennets hoax Web site
Research Findings 103
Summary
This series of quasi-experiments was designed to place children
to some degree in the kind of information environment they
might reasonably be expected to occupy during their time on
the Internet. Children were tested on their ability to detect both
good and bad information, and for the most part seemed able
to do so, across both informational and commercial contexts.
Their tendency to overlook some of the nuances of information
presented on the Amazon.com pages might be explained as the
byproduct of a general unfamiliarity with that context, as chil-
dren do not typically have the resources necessary to engage in
a large number or variety of e-commerce transactions. And,
they seemed able (at least implicitly) to pick up on the nuances
of balanced versus one-sided information in the encyclopedia
entries, as well as important information-sourcing cues. Finally,
children were (mostly) successful in seeing through the hoax
sites they encountered. Although such sites make up a minority
of the Internet, the skills children reported drawing upon to
handle them are universally applicable.
Conclusions and Implications
Summary
Conclusion
One goal of this study was to move away from the simplistic
treatments children often receive in examinations of youth and
Conclusions and Implications 111
Tables
Figures
1. Benchmark distributions for Internet access among the U.S. popula-
tion of adults are obtained from KnowledgePanel recruitment data since
this measurement is not collected as part of the Current Population
Survey.
2. Since Knowledge Networks does not collect profile data for 11- and
12-year-olds, to set up the benchmarks of those with Internet access,
they first weighted all 13- to 18-year-olds to look like the 11- to 18-year-
old general population using Current Population Survey benchmarks.
Thirteen-year-olds were treated as if they were 11 and 12 years old; thus
13-year-olds were weighted to be 36.17 percent of this population
instead of 15.53 percent within all profiled members ages 13 to 18.
Then, based on the weights for all 13- to 18-year-old KnowledgePanel
members, Knowledge Networks derived the benchmarks based on those
who have Internet access from home and weighted the child respon-
dents to these Internet benchmarks.
4. Multiple regression analysis was used to produce all results presented
in this section. Detailed statistical information is available from the
authors.
5. It is interesting that younger children said they were more likely to
believe information they find online than did older children in light of
our earlier finding that older children said they believe more of the
information on the Internet than do younger children (see the section,
Notes 129
6. The Web sites were modified slightly from their original online ver-
sions for size.
References
Alexa. 2009. “Top Sites: The Top 500 Sites on the Web.” http://www
.alexa.com/topsites (accessed on August 25, 2009).
Alexander, J. E., and M. A. Tate. 1999. Web Wisdom: How to Evaluate and
Create Information Quality on the Web. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Flanagin, A. J., and M. J. Metzger. 2007. “The Role of Site Features, User
Attributes, and Information Verification Behaviors on the Perceived
Credibility of Web-based Information.” New Media & Society 9, no. 2:
319–342.
Jenkins, H. 2006. Convergence Culture: Where Old and New Media Collide.
New York: New York University Press.
Lenhart, A., and M. Madden. 2007 (April). Teens, Privacy, and Online
Social Networks. Pew Internet & American Life report. http://www
.pewinternet.org/Reports/2007/Teens-Privacy-and-Online-Social-Net-
works. aspx?r=1 (accessed on August 25, 2009).
Lenhart, A., Madden, M., & Hitlin, P. 2005 (July). Teens and Technology:
Youth Are Leading the Transition to a Fully Wired and Mobile Nation. Pew
Internet & American Life report. http://www.pewinternet.org/report
_display.asp?r=162 (accessed on April 17, 2006).
Metzger, M. J., and A. J. Flanagin, eds. 2008. Digital Media, Youth, and
Credibility. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Rainie, L. 2006. “Life Online: Teens and Technology and the World to
Come.” Keynote address, annual conference of the Public Library Asso-
ciation, Boston, MA, March 23. http://www.pewinternet.org/ppt/
Teens%20and%20technology.pdf (accessed on November 7, 2006).
Tapscott, D. 1997. Growing Up Digital: The Rise of the Net Generation. New
York: McGraw-Hill.