Lamb Psychophysiology of Yoga Review 2006
Lamb Psychophysiology of Yoga Review 2006
Lamb Psychophysiology of Yoga Review 2006
NOTE: Some basic psychological research is included in this bibliography, but see the extensive
Psychology and Psychotherapy bibliography for a complete list of citations.
Abhang, R. Y. Updating yoga for healthy living. Souvenir, Conference on Yoga Update,
held at Kaivalyadhama, Bombay, India, 1996, pp. 92-96.
Abrams, A. I. Paired-associate learning and recall: A pilot study of the Transcendental
Meditation program. In D. W. Orme-Johnson and J. T. Farrow, eds., Scientific Research
on the Transcendental Meditation Program: Collected Papers, Vol. 1. New York:
M.E.R.U. Press, 1977, pp. 377-381.
___________. The effects of meditation on elementary school students. Dissertation
Abstracts International, 1977, 37(9-A):5689.
A comparison of the autonomic changes during three relaxation procedures. Swami
Vivekananda Yoga Research Foundation, http://www.vkyogas.org.in.
A comparison of the physiological effects of savasana with supine guided relaxation.
Swami Vivekananda Yoga Research Foundation, http://www.vkyogas.org.in.
A comparison of the psychophysiological effects of three relaxation procedures.
Swami Vivekananda Yoga Research Foundation, http://www.vkyogas.org.in.
Adhyatmananda, Swami. Yoga: Advantages and suggestions. Article available online:
http://www.divyajivan.org/yoga&health/.
Aftanas, L., and S. Golocheikine. Human anterior and frontal midline theta and lower
alpha reflect emotionally positive state and internalized attention: High-resolution EEG
investigation of meditation. Neuroscience Letters, 7 Sep 2001, 310(1):57-60. PMID:
11524157.
Abstract: EEG spectral power and coherence estimates in the individually defined delta,
theta, alpha-1, alpha-2, and alpha-3 bands were used to identify and characterize brain
regions involved in meditative states, in which focused internalized attention gives rise to
emotionally positive "blissful" experience. Blissful state was accompanied by increased
anterior frontal and midline theta synchronization as well as enhanced theta long-distant
connectivity between prefrontal and posterior association cortex with distinct "center of
gravity" in the left prefrontal region (AF3 site). Subjective scores of emotio nal
experience significantly correlated with theta, whereas scores of internalized attention
with both theta and alpha lower synchronization. Our results propose selective
associations of theta and alpha oscillating networks activity with states of interna lized
attention and positive emotional experience.
___________. Non-linear dynamic complexity of the human EEG during meditation.
Neuroscience Letters, 20 Sep 2002, 330(2):143. PMID: 12231432
Abstract: We used non- linear analysis to investigate the dynamical properties underlying
the EEG in the model of Sahaja Yoga meditation. Non- linear dimensional complexity
(DCx) estimates, indicating complexity of neuronal computations, were analyzed in 20
experienced meditators during rest and meditation using 62-channel EEG. When
compared to rest, the meditation was accompanied by a focused decrease of DCx
estimates over midline frontal and central regions. By contrast, additionally computed
linear measures exhibited the opposite direction of changes: power in the theta-1 (4-6
Hz), theta-2 (6-8 Hz) and alpha-1 (8-10 Hz) frequency bands was increased over these
regions. The DCx estimates negatively correlated with theta-2 and alpha-1 and positively
with beta-3 (22-30 Hz) band power. It is suggested that meditative experience,
characterized by less complex dynamics of the EEG, involves 'switching off' irrelevant
networks for the maintenance of focused internalized attention and inhibition of
inappropriate information. Overall, the results point to the idea that dynamically changing
inner experience during meditation is better indexed by a combination of non- linear and
linear EEG variables.
Agrawal, R. C., N. Dikshit, R. Maheshwari, S. Bose, and B. N. Bisariya. Effect of
Shavasana on vascular response to cold pressure test, serum cholesterol level and platelet
stickiness in hyper-reactors. Indian Heart Journal, 1977, 29(4):182-185.
Agte, Vaishali, and Shashi Chiplonkar. Thermic responses to vegetarian meals and
yogic exercise. Annals of Nutrition and Metabolism, 1992. 36(3):141-147.
Abstract: The thermic effect (TEF) of vegetarian meals was easured for breakfast and
lunch in 6 lean healthy men (18-25 years) during normal feeding (NF) and with 20%
overfeeding (OF) on 28 successive days. The energy contents of breakfast were 223 +- 10
and 330 +- 48 kcal, and those of lunch were 1,033 +- 22- and 1,247 +- 222 kcal in NF
and OF, respectively. In NF, the TEF per 180 min was 32.7 +- 8.6 and 54.8 +- 6.3 kcal
for breakfast and lunch, respectively. In oF, the TEF was 38.3 +- 8.3 kcal for breakfast
and 57.2 +- 5.4 kcal for lunch. The increase in total TEF due to OF was nonsignificant (p
< 0.2). In response to 20% OF, adaptive thermogenesis was manifested mainly through
an increase in the resting metabolic rate of 4.9% (p < 0.001). In both feeding regimes, the
percent TEF was higher for breakfast than for lunch (p < 0.05). Regression analysis of
TEF versus calorie load indicated a stable component of 42 kcal with a 2% rate of
increase. Yoga exercises were performed from 16.00 to 17.00 daily. The thermic effect of
yoga exercises observed from 17.10 to 18.30 was 21 kcal and persisted beyond 90 min,
indicating the role of yoga in energy metabolism.
Ahuja, M. M. S., M. G. Karmarkar, and S. Reddy. TSH, LH, cortisol response to
TRH and LH-RH and insulin hypoglycemia in subjects practicing Transcendental
Meditation. Indian Journal of Medical Research, 1981, 74:715-720.
Akers, T. K., D. M. Tucker, R. S. Roth, et al. Personality correlates of EEG change
during meditation. Psychological Reports, 1977, 40(2):439-442.
observed. This alpha activity could not be blocked by various sensory stimuli when the
Yogi was in samadhi, although it could easily be blocked when he was not meditating.
Even during deep meditation when the Yogis appeared quite relaxed and in a sleep- like
condition, the EEG record showed only prominent alpha activity. Only in one Yogi was
occasional hump activity observed, the alpha rhythm persisting in the rest of the period.
Bagchi and Wenger (1957) also found the normal alha pattern, sometimes with good
amplitude modulation, in the EEG records of some Yogis during meditation. Okuma et
al. (1958) observed in Zen practitioners that the alpha waves of these subjects increased
remarkably with the progress of their performance, even if their eyes were kept open. Das
and Gastauts (1955) observations on high amplitude fast waves in the EEG records of
Yogis during meditation have not been confirmed.
The significance of prominent alpha activity observed during meditatin is not yet clear.
Yogis generally claim that during samadhi they are oblivious to their external and
internal environments, and in the present experiments their alpha rhythm could not be
blocked by external stimuli. They also did not pass into delta activity.
___________, G. S. Chhina, and B. Singh. Studies on Shri Ramanand Yogi during his
stay in an air tight box. Indian Journal of Medical Research, 1961, 49:82-89.
___________, et al. Some electrographic observations in yogis. Indian Journal of
Physiology and Pharmacology, 1960, 42:112-113.
Anandamitra Acarya, Avadhutika. Physiological benefits of asanas. In Avadhutika
Anandamitra Acarya, Yoga for Health. 2d ed. Paco, Manila, Philippines: Ananda Marga
Publications, 1990, pp. 16-34.
Anantharaman, V. Yoga as a prophylactic health care. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1989, 28(1):5357.
___________, and Sarada Subrahmanyam. Physiological benefits in hatha yoga
training. The Yoga Review, 1983, 3(1):9-24.
Anderson, Erica. A world of yoga: Class to books, its taking over. J201, Spring 2004.
Author email: [email protected]. Article available online:
http://www.journalism.indiana.edu/gallery/j201spring04/HealthWise/Casey/Index.html.
Dr. Mark Moseman, an Indiana University health center physician believes the
attachment to yoga can be attributed in part to proprioception; or, as some like to call it,
the sixth sense.
Proprioception explains the phenomenon of how one can feel what position ones arms
and legs are in when moving. The exploration of that, he thinks, can be held partly
accountable for yogas use as a treatment to ailments.
As proprioception starts to communicate with the body, it starts to get more neurons
involved. In turn, the body is better at protecting it from damage, Moseman said . . .
Anita, Patil. Effects of tones and phonetics on the higher functions of the brain. In H. R.
Nagendra, R. Ragarathna, and S. Telles, Yoga Research & Applications: Proceedings of
the 5th International Conference on Frontiers in Yoga Research and Applications.
Bangalore, Vivekananda Kendra Yoga Research Foundation, 2000, p. 90.
Arambula, P., E. Peper, M. Kawakami, and K.H. Gibney. The physiological
correlates of Kundalini Yoga meditation: A study of a yoga master. Applied
Psychophysiol Biofeedback, Jun 2001, 26(2):147-153. PMID: 11480165.
Abstract: This study explores the physiological correlates of a highly practiced Kundalini
Yoga meditator. Thoracic and abdominal breathing patterns, heart rate (HR), occipital
parietal electroencephalograph (EEG), skin conductance level (SCL), and blood volume
pulse (BVP) were monitored during prebaseline, meditation, and postbaseline periods.
Visual analyses of the data showed a decrease in respiration rate during the meditation
from a mean of 11 breaths/min for the pre- and 13 breaths/min for the postbaseline to a
mean of 5 breaths/min during the meditation, with a predominance of
abdominal/diaphragmatic breathing. There was also more alpha EEG activity during the
meditation (M = 1.71 microV) compared to the pre- (M = .47 microV) and postbaseline
(M = .78 microV) periods, and an increase in theta EEG activity immediately following
the meditation (M = .62 microV) compared to the pre-baseline and meditative periods
(each with M = .26 microV). These findings suggest that a shift in breathing patterns may
contribute to the development of alpha EEG, and those patterns need to be investigated
further.
Arenander, A. Neurophysiological model of transcending. Paper presented at Maharishi
International Univeristy Neuroscience Conference, Fairfield, Iowa, 1980.
Aron, A., D. W. Orme -Johnson, and P. Brubaker. The TM program in the college
curriculum: A four-year longitudinal study of effects on cognitive affective functioning.
College Student Journal, 1981, 15(2):140-146.
Aroor, A. S., S. Rao, P. L. N. Rao, and K. K. Bhatt. Effect of yogic practices on
biological markers of ageing. In R. C. Sawhney, K. Sridharan, and W. Selvamurthy, eds.,
Physiology of Human Performance. Delhi, India: DIPAS, 1990, pp. 156-165.
Arpita (Joan Harrigan). Physiological and psychological effects of Hatha Yoga: A
review of the literature. Research Bulletin Himalayan International Institute, 1983, 5:2543. Reprinted in the Journal of The International Association of Yoga Therapists, 1990,
1(I & II).
Aslan, U. B., and A. Livanelioglu. Effects of hatha yoga training on aerobic power and
anaerobic power in healthy young adults. Fizyoterapi Rehabilitasyon, Apr 2002,
13(1):24-30.
Abstract: Purpose: This clinical study was carried out with the aim of investigating
whether Hatha Yoga (HY) training affects aerobic and anaerobic power in healthy young
adults. Material and method: 33 sedentary, healthy, young adult subjects, aged 18 to 26
were divided into two groups according to age, sex and activity levels. 10 female and 7
male (mean 20.06 +/- 2.41 years, range 18-26 years) young adults were trained with
Hatha Yoga (HYG). The aerobic exercise group (AEG) consisted of 9 female and 7 male
(mean 19.75 +/- 1.81 years, range 18-26 years) young adults who performed aerobic type
strength and stretch exercises of at least 60% maximal heart rate or higher. Both training
programs were given by a supervisor, one hour per day, four days per week, for six
weeks. Subjects in both groups were assessed by Coopers 12 minutes running test for
cardiovascular endurance and vertical jump test for anaerobic power before and after
training. Results: Aerobic and anaerobic power increased by 9.8%, 5.5% following HY
and by 6.6%, 2.3% following aerobic training respectively. A significant increase was
found in aerobic power and anaerobic power (p < 0.001) in HYG. There was a significant
increase in aerobic power (p < 0.01) in AEG, while anaerobic power of subjects in AEG
were consistently higher compared to that of before training, statistically the difference
was not significant (p > 0.05). Although there was no substantial differences between the
groups concerning cardiovascular endurance (p > 0.05), anaerobic power was
significantly higher (p < 0.05) in the HYG. Conclusion: The results of this study suggest
that HY training has positive effects on cardiovascular aerobic and anaerobic power.
Therefore HY could be an exercise option for enhancing aerobic and anaerobic power in
young adults.
Asrani, U. A. Brain research is more fundamental than research in consciousness,
siddhis, samadhis and sex-raptures. In U. A. Asrani, Yoga Unveiled, Part Two. Delhi,
India: Motilal Banarsidass, 1993, pp. 248-250.
Auriol, Bernard M. Approche neuro-physiologique de la mditation. Paper presented at
Entretiens de Toulouse (E.N.A.C.), consacrs l'quilibre Corps-Esprit, organiss par
lAssociation Zen Midi-Pyrnes. [In French.]
___________. Les effets psychologiques de la Mditation Transcendantale: Iinterview
par J. R. Nadal. France Culture, Sep 1977. [In French.]
___________. Effets de la technique de Mditation Transcendantale sur les nvroses et
les psychoses. Paper presented at Symposium International Science et Conscience,
MERU, Paris, Dec 1978. [In French.]
___________. Le hatha-yoga. In Bernard Auriol, Introduction aux Mthodes de
Relaxation. Privat - Toulouse,1979, 1987. Article available online:
http://auriol.free.fr/yogathera/Relaxation/yoga.htm. [In French.]
___________. La Meditation Transcendantale et las technique de Benson. In Bernard
Auriol, Introduction aux Mthodes de Relaxation. Privat - Toulouse,1979, 1987. Article
available online: http://auriol.free.fr/yogathera/Relaxation/mt.htm. [In French.]
Austin, James H. Consciousness evolves when the self dissolves. In Jensine Andresen
and Robert K. C. Forman, eds., Cognitive Models and Spiritual Maps: Interdisciplinary
Explorations of Religious Experience. Thorverton, England: Imprint Academic, 2000.
Abstract: We need to clarify at least four aspects of selfhood if we are to reach a better
understanding of consciousness in general, and of its alternate states.
First, how did we develop our self-centred psychophysiology? Second, can the four
familiar lobes of the brain alone serve, if only as preliminary landmarks of convenience,
to help understand the functions of our many self-referent networks? Third, what could
cause ones former sense of self to vanish from the mental field during an extraordinary
state of consciousness? Fourth, when a persons physical and psychic self do drop off
briefly, how has conscious experience then been transformed? In particular, what
happens to that subjects personal sense of time?
Our many-sided self arose in widely distributed brain networks. Since infancy, these
self-oriented circuits have been over-conditioned by limbic biases. Selfhood then seems
to have evolved along lines suggesting at least in shorthand the operations of a kind of I
MeMine complex.
But what happens when this egocentric triad briefly dissolves? Novel states of
consciousness emerge. Two personally-observed states are discussed: (1) insight-wisdom
(kensho-satori); (2) internal absorption. How do these two states differ
phenomenologically? The physiological processes briefly suggested here emphasize
shifts in deeper systems, and pivotal roles for thalamo-cortical interactions in the front
and back of the brain.
Zen and the Brain: Toward an Understanding of Meditation and
Consciousness. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press, 1998. Critiqued by Eleanor Rosch in
issue no. 54, Dec 2000 Jan 2001, IONS Noetic Sciences Review for its brain-centric
approach that does not allow for a nonlocal reality. See:
http://www.noetic.org/Ions/publications/r54zen.htm.
___________.
Abstract: Asymmetrical shoulder, thoracic and buttock pressure affect ipsilateral nasal
resistance, autonomic tone, and hemisphericity. This factor must be taken into
consideration when conducting psychological experimentation, and, in fact, may have
confounded much prior research.
Badawi, K., R. K. Wallace, and D. Orme -Johnson, et al. Electrophysiologic
characteristics of respiratory suspension periods occurring during the practice of the
Transcendental Meditation program. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1984, 46(3):267-276.
Bagchi, B. K., and M. A. Wenger. Electro-physiological correlates of some yogic
exercises. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, Suppl, 1957, 7:132149.
Some excerpts from the findings and discussion: an extreme slowing of respiration rate
4 to 6 per minute . . .more than 70% increase of palmar electrical resistance but without
change in basic waking EEG and EKG patterns . . . heart slowing (24 per minute) through
particular maneuvers . . . physiologically Yogic meditation represents deep relaxation of
the autonomic nervous system without drowsiness or sleep and a type of cerebral activity
without highly accelerated electro-physiological manifestation but probably with more or
less insensibility to some outside stimuli for a short or long time.
___________. Simultaneous EEG and other recordings during some yogic practices.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1958, 10:193.
Bagga, O. P. , and A. Gandhi. A comparative study of the effect of Transcendental
Meditation (T.M.) and Shavasana practice on the cardiovascular system. Indian Heart
Journal, Jan 1983, 35(1):39-45.
This study was designed to evaluate the effect of Transcendental Meditation and
Shavasana practice on blood pressure, radial and carotid pulse, ECG, EMG and skin
resistance.
___________, A. Gandhi, and S. Bagga. A study of the effect of Transcendental
Meditation and yoga on blood glucose, lactic acid, cholesterol and total lipids. Journal of
Clinical Chemistry and Clinical Biochemistry, 1981, 19(8):607-608.
Baker, M. A. The effects of hatha yoga and self-recording on trait anxie ty and locus of
control. Doctoral dissertation, United States International University, 1979. Dissertation
Abstracts International, 1980, 41:680B.
Bakker, R. Decreased respiratory rate during the Transcendental Meditation technique:
A replication. In D. W. Orme-Johnson and J. T. Farrow, eds., Scientific Research on the
Transcendental Meditation Program: Collected Papers, Vol. 1. New York: M.E.R.U.
Press, 1977, pp. 140-141.
during school lunch break. Primary CV outcome measures were changes in blood
pressure (BP), heart rate (HR), and cardiac output (CO) at rest and in response to two
laboratory stressors, a simulated car driving stressor and an interpersonal social stressor
interview. RESULTS: The TM group exhibited greater decreases in resting SBP (P<.03)
from pre- to postintervention, compared to the CTL group. The TM group exhibited
greater decreases from pre- to postintervention in SBP, HR, and CO reactivity (P's<.03)
to the simulated car driving stressor, and in SBP reactivity (P<.03) to the social stressor
interview. CONCLUSION: The TM program appears to have a beneficial impact upon
CV functioning at rest and during acute laboratory stress in adolescents at-risk for
hypertension.
___________, Frank A. Treiber, J. Rick Turner, Harry Davis, and William B.
Strong. Acute effects of Transcendental Meditation on hemodynamic functioning in
middle-aged adults. Psychosomatic Medicine, Jul/Aug 1999, 61(4):525-531. Abstract
available online: http://www.psychosomatic.org/v61n899.html#525.
Objective: Increased peripheral vasoconstriction (ie, total peripheral resistance, or TPR)
has been implicated as playing an important role in the early development of essential
hypertension. Some studies have demonstrated that Transcendental Meditation (TM)
reduces high blood pressure, but the hemodynamic adjustments behind these blood
pressure reductions have not been elucidated. The aim of this study was to provide a
preliminary investigation of the acute effects of TM on TPR.
Bauhofer, V. Physiological cardiovascular effects of the Transcendental Meditation
technique. Was scheduled for inclusion in Scientific Research on the Transcendental
Meditation Program: Collected Papers, Vol. 2. Rheinweller, Germany: Maharishi
European Research University Press, publication date unknown. Doctoral dissertation,
Julius-Maximilian University, Wurzburg, Germany, 1978.
Bauman, Alisa. Is yoga enough to keep you fit? Yoga Journal, Sep/Oct 2002, pp. 84-91,
158.
Many yogis wonder if they need to supplement their yoga practice with weight-training
or aerobic exercise like running to stay in shape. To investigate, Yoga Journal headed to
a university sports medicine lab [at the University of California at Davis] to test three
yogis for strength, endurance, flexibility, and lung capacity.
Flexibility: The yogis compare favorably with top performers who train for maximum
flexibility in fields like gymnastics and ballet.
Body composition: According to skinfold measurements taken with calipers, all three
yogis had body fat ratios and body mass indices comparable to those of elite endurance
athletes like top marathoners and bicyclists.
Muscular strength, endurance, and balance: Based on Biodex tests or elbow and knee
flexion and extension, measuring maximum force exerted, muscle endurance, and muscle
11
balanceboth between the right and left sides and between agonist and antagonist
muscles, the yogis scored mostly in or near the normal ranges.
Lung capacity: Using a spirometer, two of the yogis perfomed within a few percentage
points of the norm and one performed better than the norm.
Cardiorespiratory fitness: All three yogis produced . . . VO2max measurements in the
same range as fairly active athletes.
Beary, F., and H. Benson. A simple psychologic technique which elicits the
hypometabolic changes of the relaxation response. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1974,
36(2):115-120. (A meditation technique based on Transcendental Meditation.)
Becker, D. E., and D. Shapiro. Physiological responses to clicks during zen, yoga, and
TM meditation. Psychophysiology, 1981, 18(6):694-699.
Belaia, N. A. Effect of certain asanas used in the system of yoga on the central nervous
and cardiovascular system. Vopr Kurortol Fizioter Fiz Kult, May-Jun 1976, 0(3):13-18.
[In Russian.]
Bena, E., and J. Formanek. The influence of the headstand on EEG, pulse frequency
and blood pressure. Proceedings Physical Fitness Satellite Symposium, 25th Conference
on Physiological Science, Prague, 1971.
Benefits of meditation. Article available online:
http://www.lifepositive.com/Spirit/meditation/index.html.
Benefits of yoga. Article available online:
http://www.lifepositive.com/Body/yoga/index.html.
Bennett, J. E., and Trinder. Hemispheric laterality and cognitive-style associated with
Transcendental Meditation. Psychophysiology, 1977, 14(3):293-296. Also in D. H.
Shapiro, and R. N. Walsh, eds., Meditation: Classic and Contemporary Perspectives.
New York: Aldine Publishing, 1984, pp. 506-509.
Benson, H., T. Dryer, and L. H. Hartley. Decreased oxygen consumption during
exercise with elicitation of the relaxation response. Journal of Human Stress,1978; 4:3842.
___________, M. M. Greenwood, and H. Klemchuk. The relaxation response:
Psychophysiologic aspects and clinical applications. International Journal of Psychiatry
in Medicine, 1975; 6:87-98.
___________, J. W. Lehman, M. S. Malhotra, R. F. Goldman, J. Hopkins, and M. D.
Epstein. Body temperature cha nges during the practice of g tum- mo (heat) yoga. Nature,
1982, 295:234-236.
12
13
and two other postures (resting in a chair and resting in the supine position) as methods of
recovery from induced physiological stress (treadmill running).
Twenty-one males and six females (age range = 21-30 years) were allowed to rest in one
of the above postures after completing a treadmill workout. Recovery was assessed by
measuring resting and exercise recovery heart rate and blood pressure. These factors were
measured before and every two minutes after the treadmill running until they returned to
their initial resting levels.
The results revealed that the effects of treadmill exercise stress were reversed in
significantly (p < 0.01) shorter time with Shavasana than with either of the other two
resting postures.
Comments: The Shavasana pose (sometimes spelled Savasana or called corpse pose)
is often overlooked as an effective yoga pose. Seemingly easy, it is one of the most
challenging poses in yoga. Shavasana is practiced in a relaxed supine position, feet apart,
palms facing up to gently open the chest. The neck should be extended. (Placing a folded
towel underneath the neck is recommended.) What primarily distinguishes Shavasana
from the other two modes of relaxation used in this study is utilization of the breath.
Abdominal yogic breathing is sequenced with normal breathing throughout Shavasana.
___________, and M. V. Rajapurkar. Biomechanical analysis of padmasana. YogaMimamsa, 1989, 28(3):12-32.
___________, and M. V. Rajapurkar. Body composition, cardiovascular endurance and
anaerobic power of yogic practitioner. Indian Journal of Physiology & Pharmacology,
1993, 37(3):225-228.
___________, and M. V. Rajapurkar. Somatotype as an indicator to the performance
ability of selected yogasanas. NIS Scientific Journal, 1990, 13(3):7-19.
___________, M. V. Rajapurkar, and S. K. Ganguly. Effect of yogic training on body
density in school going boys. NIS Scientific Journal, 1990, 13(2):23ff.
___________. Yoga and body fat. Krida Tantra, 1993, pp. 17-22.
Berger, B. G., and D. R. Owen. Mood alteration with yoga and swimming: Aerobic
exercise may not be necessary. Perceptual Motor Skills, 1992, 75(3, Part 2):1331-1343.
Berker, E. Stability of skin resistance responses one week after instruction in the
Transcendental Meditation technique. In D. W. Orme-Johnson and J. T. Farrow, eds.,
Scientific Research on the Transcendental Meditation Program, Collected Papers, Vol. 1.
Germany: Maharishi European Research University Press, 1976, pp. 243-247.
Bernardi, Luciano, Peter Sleight, Gabriele Bandinelli, Simone Cencetti, Lamberto
Fattorini, Johanna Wdowczyc-Szulc, and Alfonso Lagi. Effect of rosary prayer and
14
15
resistance (GSR). After taking initial recordings all the subjects practised Nadi-Shodhana
Pranayama for a period of 4 weeks. At the end of 4 weeks same parameters were again
recorded and the results compared. Baseline heart rate and blood pressure (systolic and
diastolic) showed a rendency to decrease and both these autonomic parameters were
significantly decreased at breaking point after pranayamic breathing. Although the GSR
was recorded in all subjects the observations made were not conclusive. Thus pranayama
breathing exercises appear to alter autonomic responses to breath holding probably by
increasing vagal tone and decreasing sympathetic discharges.
Bhaskaracharyulu, C., R. Sitaram, G. Kumari, B. K. Sahay, M. K. V. Annapurna,
S. Madhavi, and K. J. R. Murthy. The effect of yoga on lipoprotein profile in diabetics.
Journal of the Diabetic Association of India, 1986, 26:120-124.
Bhatnagar, O. P. The effect of yoga training on neuromuscular excitability and muscular
relaxation. Neurology India, Dec 1977, 25(4):230-232.
___________, V. Anantharaman, S. S. Sathiamoorthy, and V. Srinivasan. A
progressive study of physiological effects of certain yoga asans. Indian Journal of
Physiology and Pharmacology, 1977, 21(3):278.
___________, A. K. Ganguly, V. Ananthraman, and K. S. Gopal. Influence of yogic
training on thermoregulation. Proceedings of the International Union of Physiology Sci
XI, 1974, 378.
Bhavanani, Ananda Balayogi, Madanmohan, and Kaviraja Udupa. Acute effect of
much bhastrika (a yogic bellows type breathing) on reaction time. Indian Journal of
Physiology and Pharmacology, 2003, 47(3):297-300.
Abstract: Reaction time (RT) is an index of the processing ability of [the] central nervous
system and a simple means of determining sensory- motor performance. It has been
reported that yoga training improves human performance including central neural
processing. Earlier studies from our laboratories have shown that yoga training produces a
significant decrease in visual reaction time (VRT) and auditory reaction time (ART). The
present work was planned to determine if mukh bhastrika (a yogic technique in which
breath is actively blasted out in whooshes following a deep inspiration) has any effect on
central neural processing by studying its effect on RT. 22 healthy schoolboys who were
practicing yoga for the past three months were recruited for the present study. VRT and
ART were recorded before and after nine rounds of much bhastrika. Mukh bhastrika
produced a significant (P<0.01) decrease in VRT as well as ART. A decrease in RT
indicates an improved sensory- motor performance and enhanced processing ability of
central nervous system. This may be due to greater arousal, faster rate of information
processing, improved concentration and/or an ability to ignore extraneous stimuli. This is
of applied value in situations requiring faster reactivity such as sports, machine operation,
race driving and specialized surgery. It may also be of value to train mentally retarded
children and older sports persons who have prolonged RT.
16
17
18
19
control group was used. A total of 287 college students (89 men and 198 women)
enrolled in the yoga training program.
Subjects were taught yoga poses, breathing techniques and relaxation in 50-minute class
meetings twice weekly for 15 weeks. Class adherence was very high (99.96%). The main
outcome measure was vital capacity over time for asthmatics, smokers and subjects with
no known lung disease. The large number of subjects287was a valid sample for a
study of this type.
The study showed a statistically significant (p < 0.001) improvement in vital capacity
across all categories over time. It is not known whether this positive improvement was
the result of yoga poses, breathing techniques, relaxation or other aspects of exercise in
the subjects life. However, these findings were consistent with those of other research
studies.
Comments: Increases in lung capacity and function are among the trademark benefits of
yoga exercise as long as it is of sufficient quality and duration and involves a distinct
yogic breathing component. Earlier studies have demonstrated yoga- induced increases in
forced expiratory volume in one second (FEV-1), the factor that is perhaps the most
functional index of lung function. This is an important benefit for those who have
diminished lung volume and function from emphysema or a sedentary lifestyle.
Bivolaru, Gregorian. The law of resonance. Article available online:
http://www.natha.dk/resonance.htm.
The fundamental secret of yoga is to create and maintain a process of resonance, in other
words a process of initiating and amplifying a vibratory response (a link) in a receiving
system that is attuned to an emitting system . . . In yoga, the process of resonance is
created and maintained mainly by permanently focused attentiveness (effortless mental
concentration) . . .
Blanchard, E. B., and L. D. Young. Self-control of cardiac functioning: A promise as
yet unfulfilled. Psychol. Bull., Mar 1973, 79(3):145-63.
Blank, S. E., K. Raman, G. Chock, and J. W. Krieger. Heart rate and oxygen cost
responses to power yoga asanas in beginning practitioners. Medicine & Science in Sports
& Exercise, May 2001, 33(5) Supplement :S107.
Blanz, Larry T. Personality changes as a function of two different meditative
techniques. Dissertation Abstracts International, May 1974, 34(11-A):7035.
Blasdell, K. S. The effects of the Transcendental Meditation technique upon a complex
perceptual motor task. In D. W. Orme-Johnson and J. T. Farrow, eds., Scientific Research
on the Transcendental Meditation Program, Collected Papers, Vol. 1. Germany:
Maharishi European Research University Press, 1976, pp. 322-325..
20
22
23
24
25
26
27
29
The work is reported by Olivia Carter and Jack Pettigrew of the University of
Queensland, Australia, and colleagues at the University of Queensland and the University
of California, Berkeley.
Perceptual rivalry arises normally when two different images are presented to each eye,
and it is manifested as a fluctuationtypically, over the course of secondsin the
dominant image that is consciously perceived. The neural events underlying perceptual
rivalry are not well understood but are thought to involve brain mechanisms that regulate
attention and conscious awareness.
Some previous work had suggested that skilled meditation can alter certain aspects of the
brains neural activity, though the significance of such changes in terms of actually
understanding brain function remains unclear.
To gain insight into how visual perception is regulated within the brain, researchers in the
new study chose to investigate the extent to which certain types of trained meditative
practice can influence the conscious experience of visual perceptual rivalry.
With the support of His Holiness the Dalai Lama, 76 Tibetan Buddhist monks
participated in the study, which was carried out at or near their mountain retreats in the
Himalaya, Zanskar, and Ladakhi Ranges of India. The monks possessed meditative
training ranging from 5 to 54 years; among the group were three retreatist meditators,
each with at least 20 years of experience in isolated retreats.
The researchers tested the experience of visual rivalry by monks during the practice of
two types of meditation: a compassion-oriented meditation, described as a
contemplation of suffering within the world combined with an emanation of loving
kindness, and one-point meditation, described as the maintained focus of attention on a
single object or thought, a focus that leads to a stability and clarity of mind.
Whereas no observable change in the rate of visual switching during rivalry was seen
in monks practicing compassion meditation, major increases in the durations of
perceptual dominance were experienced by monks practicing one-point meditation.
Within this group, three monks, including two of the retreatists, reported complete visual
stability during the entire five- minute meditation period. Increases in duration of
perceptual dominance were also seen in monks after a period of one-point meditation.
In a different test of perceptual rivalry, in this case prior to any meditation, the duration
of stable perception experienced by monks averaged 4.1 seconds, compared to 2.6
seconds for meditation-nave control subjects. Remarkably, when instructed to actively
maintain the duration, one of the retreatist monks could maintain a constant visual
perception during this test for 723 seconds.
The findings suggest that processes particularly associated with one-point meditation
perhaps involving intense attentional focus and the ability to stabilize the mind
30
contribute to the prolonged rivalry dominance experienced by the monks. The researchers
conclude from their study that individuals trained in meditation can considerably alter the
normal fluctuations in conscious state that are induced by perceptual rivalry and suggest
that, in combination with previous work, the new findings support the idea that
perceptual rivalry can be modulated by high- level, top-down neural influences.
Cassel, R. N. Fundamentals involved in the scientific process of Transcendental
Meditation. Journal of Instruc. Psychol., 1976, 3(3):2-11.
Cauthen, N., and C. Prymak. Meditation versus relaxation: An examination of the
physiological effects with Transcendental Meditation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 1977, 45:496-497.
Chakrabarti, Ghosh, Sahana. Physiological changes during meditation. In Chakrabarti,
Ghosh, and Sahana, Human Physiology. 2d ed., 1984, pp. 1236-1244.
Chandra, F. J. Medical & physiological aspects of yoga (booklet). Cambridge, England:
Cambridge Yoga Publications, n.d.
___________. Medical and physiological aspects of headstand. The Journal of The
International Association of Yoga Therapists, 1990, 1(I&II):29-34. Reprinted as a
booklet by Cambridge Yoga Publications, Cambridge, England.
___________. [On fainting and Yoga postures.] A discussion in the article Yoga and the
cardiovascular system. The Journal of The International Association of Yoga Therapists,
1991, 2(1):7.
___________. Yoga and the cardiovascular system. The Journal of The International
Association of Yoga Therapists, 1991, 2(1):29-34. Reprinted as a booklet by Cambridge
Yoga Publications, Cambridge, England.
Chaya, M. S., H. R. Nagendra, and G. L. Khanna. Metabolic changes during yoga
practice: A review. In H. R. Nagendra, R. Ragarathna, and S. Telles, Yoga Research &
Applications: Proceedings of the 5th International Conference on Frontiers in Yoga
Research and Applications. Bangalore, Vivekananda Kendra Yoga Research Foundation,
2000, pp. 190-193.
Chhina, G. S. The voluntary control of autonomic responses in yogis. Proceedings of the
International Union of Physiology Sci X, 1974, 103.
Chin, Richard M. The Energy Within: The Science Behind Every Oriental Therapy from
Acupuncture to Yoga. New York: Marlowe & Co., 1995.
Chohan, I. S., H. S. Nayar, P. Thomas, and N. S. Greetha. Influence of yoga on blood
coagulation. J Assoc Phys India, Sep 1979, 27(9); Thrombosis & Haemostasis, 30 Apr
31
1984, 51(2):196-197.
Clarke, John. The health benefits of postures. Yoga International, Jan/Feb 1993. Article
available online: http://www.himalayaninstitute.org/cgilocal//do.pl?form=viewstory&id=83.
Clay, C. C., L. K. Lloyd, J. L. Walker, K. R. Sharp, and R. B. Pankey. The metabolic
cost of hatha yoga. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, Aug 2005,
19(3):604-610.
Abstract: To determine the metabolic and heart rate (HR) responses of hatha yoga, 26
women (19-40 years old) performed a 30- minute hatha yoga routine of supine lying,
sitting, and standing asanas (i.e., postures). Subjects followed identical videotaped
sequences of hatha yoga asanas. Mean physiological responses were compared to the
physiological responses of resting in a chair and walking on a treadmill at 93.86 m.min(1) [3.5 miles per hour (mph)]. During the 30-minute hatha yoga routine, mean absolute
oxygen consumption (Vo(2)), relative Vo(2), percentage maximal oxygen consumption
(%Vo(2)R), metabolic equivalents (METs), energy expenditure, HR, and percentage
maximal heart rate (%MHR) were 0.45 L.min(-1), 7.59 ml.kg(-1).min(-1), 14.50%, 2.17
METs, 2.23 kcal.min(-1), 105.29 b.min(-1), and 56.89%, respectively. When compared to
resting in a chair, hatha yoga required 114% greater O(2) (L.min(-1)), 111% greater
O(2)(ml.kg(-1).min(-1)), 4,294% greater %Vo(2)R, 111% greater METs, 108% greater
kcal.min(-1), 24% greater HR, and 24% greater %MHR. When compared to walking at
93.86 m.min(-1), hatha yoga required 54% lower O(2)(L.min(-1)), 53% lower
O(2)(ml.kg(-1).min(-1)), 68% lower %Vo(2)R, 53% lower METs, 53% lower kcal.min(1), 21% lower HR, and 21% lower %MHR. The hatha yoga routine in this study required
14.50% Vo(2)R, which can be considered a very light intensity and significantly lighter
than 44.8% Vo(2)R for walking at 93.86 m.min(-1) (3.5 mph). The intensity of hatha
yoga may be too low to provide a training stimulus for improving cardiovascular fitness.
Although previous research suggests that hatha yoga is an acceptable form of physical
activity for enhancing muscular fitness and flexibility, these data demonstrate that hatha
yoga may have little, if any, cardiovascular benefit.
Clements, G., and S. L. Milstein. Auditory thresholds in advanced participants in the
Transcendental Meditation program. M.E.R.U. (Maharishi European Research
University) Report no. 7702.
Cole, Roger. Physiology of yoga. Iyengar Yoga Institute Review, Oct 1985.
___________. Relaxation: Physiology and Practice booklet. Del Mar, Calif.: Synchrony
Applied Health Sciences, 1994. Available from Synchrony Applied Health Sciences,
12759 Via Felino, Del Mar, CA 92014, 619-792-1527.
Contents: Fight or Flight vs. Relaxation Response; Physiology of Relaxation Asanas;
How to Promote Quiescent Relaxation; Selected Relaxation Postures, Relaxation Asana
32
Sequence; Asanas to Practice during Menstruation; Relaxation Asana Tips; EEG Theta
Activity during Yoga Resting Postures
Cooper, M. J., and M. M. Aygen. Effect of meditation on serum cholesterol and blood
pressure. Journal of the Israel Medical Association, 1978, 95(1):1-2.
Copeland, Paul. C.Y.T.A. research. Yoga Journal, May/Jun 1976, pp. 15-16.
Corby, James C., Walton T. Roth, Vincent P. Zarcone, Jr., and Bert S. Kopell.
Psychophysiological correlates of the practice of Tantric Yoga meditation. Archives of
General Psychiatry, May 1978, 35(5):571-580. Also in D. H. Shapiro, and R. N. Walsh,
eds., Meditation: Classic and Contemporary Perspectives. New York: Aldine Publishing,
1984, pp. 440-464. PMID: 365124. *
Abstract: Autonomic and electroencephalographic (EEG) correlates of Tantric Yoga
meditation were studied in three groups of subjects as they progressed from normal
consciousness into meditation. Groups differed in their level of meditation proficiency.
Measures of skin resistance, heart rate, respiration, autonomic orienting responses, resting
EEG, EEG alpha and theta frequencies, sleep-scored EEG, averaged evoked responses,
and subjective experience were employed. Unlike most previously reported meditation
studies, proficient meditators demonstrated increased autonomic activation during
meditation while unexperienced meditators demonstrated autonomic relaxation. During
meditation, proficient meditators demonstrated increased alpha and theta power, minimal
evidence of EEG-defined sleep, and decreased autonomic orienting to external
stimulation. An episode of sudden autonomic activation was observed that was
characterized by the meditator as an approach to the Yogic ecstatic state of intense
concentration. These findings challenge the current "relaxation" model of meditative
states.
Corey, P. W. Airway conductance and oxygen consumption changes associated with
practice of the Transcendental Meditation technique. In D. W. Orme-Johnson and J. T.
Farrow, eds., Scientific Research on the Transcendental Meditation Program, Collected
Papers, Vol. 1. Germany: Maharishi European Research University Press, 1976, pp. 94107.
Corrigan, G. E. Fatal air embolism after Yoga breathing exercises. Journal of the
American Medical Cowger, Ernest Leon, Jr. The effects of meditation (Zazen) upon
selected dimensions of personal development. Dissertation Abstracts International, Feb
1974, 34(8-A, pt. 1):4734.
Coulter, H. David. The physiology of bhastrika. Yoga International, no. 22. Article
available online: http://www.himalayaninstitute.org/cgilocal//do.pl?form=viewstory&id=96.
Contents: Controlling the breath; Oxygen and carbon dioxide; The chemoreceptors; High
altitude and the bellows breath; Bhastrika, the bellows exercise
33
34
In a monastery in northern India, thinly clad Tibetan monks sat quietly in a room where
the temperature was a chilly 40 degrees Fahrenheit. Using a yoga technique known as g
Tum- mo, they entered a state of deep meditation. Other monks soaked 3-by-6-foot sheets
in cold water (49 degrees) and placed them over the meditators shoulders. For untrained
people, such frigid wrappings would produce uncontrolled shivering.
If body temperatures continue to drop under these conditions, death can result. But it
was not long before steam began rising from the sheets. As a result of body heat
produced by the monks during meditation, the sheets dried in about an hour.
Attendants removed the sheets, then covered the meditators with a second chilled, wet
wrapping. Each monk was required to dry three sheets over a period of several hours . . .
During visits to remote monasteries in the 1980s, Benson and his team studied monks
living in the Himalayan Mountains who could, by g Tum- mo meditation, raise the
temperatures of their fingers and toes by as much as 17 degrees. It has yet to be
determined how the monks are able to generate such heat.
The researchers also made measurements on practitioners of other forms of advanced
meditation in Sikkim, India. They were astonished to find that these monks could lower
their metabolism by 64 percent. It was an astounding, breathtaking [no pun intended]
result, Benson exclaims.
To put that decrease in perspective, metabolism, or oxygen consumption, drops only 1015 percent in sleep and about 17 percent during simple meditation. Benson believes that
such a capability could be useful for space travel. Travelers might use meditation to ease
stress and oxygen consumption on long flights to other planets.
In 1985, the meditation team made a video of monks drying cold, wet sheets with body
heat. They also documented monks spending a winter night on a rocky ledge 15,000 feet
high in the Himalayas. The sleep-out took place in February on the night of the winter
full moon when temperatures reached zero degrees F. Wearing only woolen or cotton
shawls, the monks promptly fell asleep on the rocky ledge. They did not huddle together
and the video shows no evidence of shivering. They slept until dawn then walked back to
their monastery.
Curtis, William D., And Harold W. Wessberg. A comparison of heart rate, respiration,
and galvanic skin response among meditators, relaxers, and controls. Journal of Altered
States of Consciousness, 1975-1976, 2:319-324.
Cusumano, Jerome A., and Sharon E. Robinson. The short-term psychophysiological
effects of hatha yoga and progressive relaxation on female Japanese students. Applied
Psychology: An International Review, 1992, 42(1):77-90.
Abstract: This study explored the effects of hatha yoga and progressive relaxation on
heart rate, blood pressure, physical self-efficacy, and self-esteem. Ninety-five female
Japanese undergraduates participated in the three weekly treatment sessions. Results
showed that both treatments were effective in lowering heart rate and blood pressure and
35
36
spontaneous breathing. These results were largely determined by the breathing frequency
because the two types of meditation induce low breathing frequencies which led to a
pronounced and in-phase RSA. During the meditation the low breathing frequencies led
to a decrease in the high frequency of heart rate variability, whereas the low frequency
and the extent of RSA increased. The heart rate primarily reflected the degree of physical
effort. The high degree of cardiorespiratory synchronization during meditation in
unexperienced meditators suggests that the physiological implications of meditation does
not require prior experience in meditation.
Czamara, Joli Michele. Therapeutic benefits of yoga: A 10-week pilot study. Masters
thesis. DYouville College, Buffalo, New York, 2002. Masters Abstracts International,
Feb 2003, 41/01, p. 217. First 24 pages available online:
http://wwwlib.umi.com/dissertations/preview/1409936. UMI #1409936.
Abstract: The purpose of this study was to determine whether a 10-week yoga practice of
postures, breathing, and relaxation can increase a person's strength, balance, functional
flexibility, and mental and physical quality of life. A sample of 16 volunteers was
recruited from a community-based yoga center. A quasi-experimental, one- group within
subject control, pre-post-test design was used for this study. Data were analyzed at the
significance level of p < .05 for one group pre- and post-test of two data sets. The first set
generating physical performance data of five tests. The second set, a survey measuring
mental and physical health. The Mann-Whitney showed significance at the p < .01 for the
sit-to-stand physical test. A West showed significance at the p < .05 for the mental
component of the questionnaire. This study suggests that, even a relatively short (10weeks) program of yoga will result in improvements of lower limb strength and selfperception of mental well-being of community-dwelling adults (mean age = 46.81) who
are novice yoga practitioners.
Davidson, J. M. The physiology of meditation and mystical states of consciousness.
Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 1976, 19:345-380.
Davidson, Teresa. The neurophysiology of the control of breathing. Yoga Rahasya,
1999, 6(3):37-43.
Dalal, A. S., and T. X. Barber. Yoga, yoga feats, and hypnosis in the light of empirical
research. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1969, 11:155-166.
Damle, P. S., Amrito, M. M. Gore, and S. S. Badade. Effect of meditation on heart rate
response to acoustic shocks. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1997, 32(1&2):21-26.
Dange, V. S. Effect of yoga therapy on obesity and lipid profile. Proceedings ICYR,
1987, article no. 17.
Das, N., and H. Gastaut. Variations de lactivit electrique du cerveau, du coeur et des
muscles squelettiques au cours de la Mditation et de lextase yogique [Variations in the
electrical activity of the brain, heart, and skeletal muscles during yogic meditation and
37
trance]. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, suppl. 6, 1955, pp. 211219. [In French.]
Dash, Manoj, and Shirley Telles. Yoga training and motor speed based on a finger
tapping task. Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, Oct 1999, 43(4):458-462.
Also in H. R. Nagendra, R. Ragarathna, and S. Telles, Yoga Research & Applications:
Proceedings of the 5th International Conference on Frontiers in Yoga Research and
Applications. Bangalore, Vivekananda Kendra Yoga Research Foundation, 2000, pp.
298-302.
Abstract: A finger tapping task was used to assess motor speed (MS) of both hands in 53
adults and 152 children before and after yoga training and in 38 adults of a non- yoga
(control) group. All subjects were right hand dominant. The 30-second tapping speed
(TS) test was considered as three time intervals, i.e. 0-10 second (TS1), 10-20 seconds
(TS2) and 20-30 seconds (TS3). There was a significant (Student's t-test) increase in all
three TS values following 10 days of yoga in children and 30 days of yoga in adults.
However for both groups at baseline and final assessments, TS2 and TS3 were
significantly lower than TS1. Hence the TS was increased after yoga training during the
first 10-seconds of the test but not during the next 20 seconds. These results suggest an
increase in motor speed for repetitive finger movements following yoga training, but not
in strength or endurance, as the increase was not sustained over 30 sec.
___________. Improvement in hand grip strength in normal volunteers and rheumatoid
arthritis patients following yoga training. Indian Journal of Physiology and
Pharmacology, Jul 2001,45(3):355-360. PMID: 11881576.
Abstract: The present study aimed at assessing the effects of a set of yoga practices on
normal adults (n = 37), children (n = 86), and patients with rheumatoid arthritis (n = 20).
An equal number of normal adults, children, and patients with rheumatoid arthritis who
did not practice yoga were studied under each category, forming respective control
groups. Yoga and control group subjects were assessed at baseline and after varying
intervals, as follows, adults after 30 days, children after 10 days and patients after 15
days, based on the duration of the yoga program, which they attended, which was already
fixed. Hand grip strength of both hands, measured with a grip dynamometer, increased in
normal adults and children, and in rheumatoid arthritis patients, following yo ga, but not
in the corresponding control groups, showing no re-test effect. Adult female volunteers
and patients showed a greater percentage improvement than corresponding adult males.
This gender-based difference was not observed in children. Hence yoga practice
improves hand grip strength in normal persons and in patients with rheumatoid arthritis,
though the magnitude of improvement varies with factors such as gender and age.
Datar, S. V., and V. A. Kulkarni. Yogic practices and cardiovascular efficienc y. YogaMimamsa, 1997, 32(1&2):8-13.
Datey, K. K., and S. J. Bhagwat. Yoga and health. Golden Jubilee Year Souvenir,
Kaivalyadhama, India, 1975.
38
39
this period recordings were repeated at the seventh to eighth month and twelfth to
fourteenth month.
During each exercise session on a bicycle ergometer, blood samples were taken before
and after exhaustive exercise to assess the subjects level of oxidative stress due to
exercise by recording various markers of oxidative stress and antioxidant activity to
observe if there is any effect of Yogic practices in the modulation of oxidative stress.
Tests were conducted on 20 Yoga proficient participants (Yoga instructors ) for recording
of various physiological parameters during yogic practices. Among them 8 subjects
showed better responses as compared toYoga trainees.
We found significant reduction in perceived exertion after maximal exercise in the Yoga
group after 4-5 months of Yogic training, which demonstrates the potential of yogic
exercises in sports training and in different occupational situations both in the military
and civil sectors. The data on oxidative stress recorded before and after exercise indicate
that the subjects practicing yogic exercises could successfully cope with oxidative stress
by changes in three systems: increased metabolism of glutathione, elevated production of
antioxidant enzymes, and more efficient elimination of per-oxidation products.
Deikman, A. J. Experimental meditation. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1963,
136:329-343. Also in Charles Tart, Altered States of Consciousness. New York: John
Wiley & Sons, 1969.
___________. Implications of experimentally produced contemplative meditation.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1966, 142:101-116.
___________. Deautomatization and the mystic experience. Psychiatry, 1966, 29:324338. Also in Charles Tart, Altered States of Consciousness. New York: John Wiley &
Sons, 1969.
Delmonte, M. M. Personality characteris tics and regularity of meditation. Psychological
Reports, 1980, 46:703-712.
___________. Expectation and meditation. Psychological Reports, 1981, 49:699-709.
___________. Factors influencing the regularity of meditation practice in a clinical
population. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 1984, 57:275-278.
___________. Response to meditation in terms of physiological, behavioural and selfreport measures: A brief summary. Psychological Reports, Feb 1985, 56(1):9-10.
___________. Biochemical indices associated with meditation practice: A literature
review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, Winter 1985, 9(4):557-561.
Abstract: Research findings on biochemical responsivity to meditation are reviewed.
Although there are some contradictory and inconclusive outcomes, there is nevertheless
sufficient evidence of interest to warrant further investigation of this area. However, in
40
41
43
Dhume, R. R., and R. A. and Dhume. A comparative study of the driving effects of
dextroamphetamine and yogic meditation on muscle control for the performance of
balance on balance board. Indian Journal of Physiology & Pharmacology, 1991,
35(3):191-194.
DiCarlo, L. J., P. B. Sparling, B. T. Hinson, T. K. Snow, and L. B. Rosskopf.
Cardiovascular, metabolic, and perceptual responses to Hatha Yoga standing postures.
Medicine, Exercise, Nutrition and Health, 1995, 4:107-112.
Abstract: Cardiovascular, metabolic, and perceptual responses during a 32-minute
treadmill walk (TW) at 4 mph were compared with those measured during a vigorous,
standing-pose, hatha yoga [Iyengar-based] routine (YR). Subjects were six male and four
female yoga practitioners age 38-47 years. The 32- minute YR consisted of a series of
standing poses [Utthita Trikonasana, Virabhadrasana 2, Parivrtta Parsvakonasana,
Virabhadrasana 1, and Parivrtta Ardha Chandrasana, with Tadasana performed
between each pose] held for 40 s with 10 s for transitions. Heart rate (HR), blood pressure
(BP), oxygen uptake (VO2), and rating of perceived exertion (RPE) were measure at 8,
16, 25, and 32 minutes. All comparisons between YR and TW were significantly
different (p<0.05) except HR and RPE at 8 min. At 16, 24, and 32 minutes, both HR and
RPE were higher during YR than TW (138, 139, 144 vs 117, 118, 120 beats/min; 15.4,
15.3, 15.9 vs 12.5, 12.7, 12.9). Blood pressures were higher during YR at all four time
intervals (systolic 153, 148, 147, 147 vs 133, 131, 127, 130 mm Hg; disstolic 85, 93, 86,
89 vs 70, 70, 71, 68 mm Hg). Conversely, VO2 was higher during TW than YR at each
time interval. Across the 32- minute session, mean energy expenditure was 34% VO2max
for YR and 46% VO2max for TW. The elevated HR, BP, and RPE responses associated
with YR vs TW can be attributed in large part to the static exercise components inherent
in this type of yoga. Yet, the vigorous YR standing poses resulted in a lower metabolic
demand than brisk walking (4.1 vs 5.4 METS). These findings can be used to better
understand hatha yoga from the perspective of physiological responses and appropriate
exercise prescription.
Dietrich, A. Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness: The transient
hypofrontality hypothesis. Consciousness and Cognition, Jun 2003, 12(2):231-256.
PMID: 12763007. Author email: [email protected].
Abstract: It is the central hypothesis of this paper that the mental states commonly
referred to as altered states of consciousness are principally due to transient prefrontal
44
45
Editors of Yoga Journal. [On retinal detachment, glaucoma, and inversions]. Yoga
Journal, Nov/Dec 2000, p. 52.
Effect of 10 days of yoga (special memory development) techniques on memory in
children. Swami Vivekananda Yoga Research Foundation, http://www.vkyogas.org.in.
Effect of uninostril yoga breathing on performance in a tweezer dexterity task.
Bangalore, India: Vivekananda Yoga Kendra Prakashana, 1996-1997.
Elias, A. N., S. Guich, and A. F. Wilson. Ketosis with enhanced GABAergic tone
promotes physiological changes in Transcendental Meditation. Medical Hypotheses, Apr
2000, 54(4):660-662. PMID: 10859661.
Abstract: Transcendental meditation (TM) is a stylized form of physical and mental
relaxation which is associated with changes in the secretion and release of several
pituitary hormones. The hormonal changes induced by TM mimic the effects of the
inhibitory neurotransmitter gamma aminobutyric acid (GABA). It is hypothesized that
TM produces changes in pituitary hormone secretion by enhancing hypothalamic
GABAergic tone as a result of TM associated ketosis. Ketosis enhances the entry of
glutamate, the amino acid substrate of GABA into synaptosomes, making more glutamate
available for conversion to GABA through the glutamate decarboxylase pathway.
Elson, Barry D., Peter Hauri, and David Cunis. Physiological changes in yoga
meditation. Psychophysiology, Jan 1977, 14:52-57.
Epstein, M. D., and J. D. Lieff. Psychiatric complications of meditation practice.
Journal of Transpersonal, 1981, 13(2):137-147.
Ernst, Carl W. The psychophysiology of ecstasy in Sufism andYoga. NCMJ, May/Jun
1998, 59(3):182-184.
Ewing, Arthur H. The Hindu conception of the functions of breath: A study in early
Hindu psycho-physics. Journal of Asian and Oriental Studies, 1901, 22:249-308.
Fabbro, F., A. Muzur, R. Bellen, R. Calacione, and A. Bava. Effects of praying and a
working memory task in participants trained in meditation and controls on the occurrence
of spontaneous thoughts. Perceptual and Motor Skills, Jun 1999, 88(3 Pt 1):765-770.
MEDLINE PMID: 0010407883.
Abstract: So-called a intrusive thoughts appear independently from external stimuli and
are the cause of severe disturbances in depressed patients. Following Baddeleys 1986
discoveries regarding articulatory suppression, we investigated the influence of praying
and of a working memory task on the number of spontaneous thoughts reported by 20
subjects compared to the control (quiet) state. Two groups of subjects were tested: those
trained in meditation and controls. Significant reduction in simultaneous thought arousal
was obtained during both the working memory task and the recitation of prayer. In all
47
48
Fischer, R. A cartography of the ecstatic and meditative states. Science, 1971, 174:897904.
Fong, K. Y., R. T. Cheung, Y. L. Yu, C. W. Lai, and C. M. Chang. Basilar artery
occlusion following yoga exercise: A case report. Clin Exp Neurol (Australia), 1993,
30:104-109.
Fontana, David. Buddhism and Western neuroscience. Network, Aug 2000, 73:2-5.
Frank, N. L. Study of meditation and blood pressure (letter). The New England Journal
of Medicine, 1976, 294(14):786.
Frederick, Ambellur N., and Theodore X. Barber. Yoga, hypnosis, and self-control of
cardiovascular functions. Proceedings of the Annual Convention of the American
Psychological Association, 1972, 7(part 2):859-860.
French, A. P., A. C. Schmid, and E. Ingalls. Transcendental Meditation, altered reality
testing, and behavioral change: A case report. Journal Nerv Ment Dis, 1975, 161:55-58
Fried, Robert, and Joseph Grimaldi. The Psychology and Physiology of Breathing: In
Behavioral Medicine, Clinical Psychology, and Psychiatry. The Plenum Series in
Behavioral Psychophysics. Plenum 1993.
Friedman, E. H., and A. J. Coats. Neurobiology of exaggerated heart oscillations
during two meditative techniques (letter). International Journal of Cardiology, 28 Apr
2000, 73(2):199. PMID: 10905879. (In response to Peng et al. below.)
Friedman, Nicole Lisa. Zen breath meditation awareness improves heart rate variability
in patients with coronary artery disease. Ph.D. dissertation. Alliant International
University, San Diego, 2002. Dissertation Abstracts International, Jun 2002, B 62/12, p.
5948. . First 24 pages available online:
http://wwwlib.umi.com/dis sertations/preview/3037196. UMI #3037196.
Abstract: How patients with coronary artery disease respond to stress can effect their
cardiac health. This study examined whether patients with documented coronary artery
disease would be able to learn a self- help skill which would reduce cardiac reactivity
during mildly stressful and restful activities. Cardiac stress was determined measuring
Heart Rate Variability (HRV), an indication of autonomic arousal. HRV has been shown
to be a predictor of sudden cardiac death in patients with cardiac disease. 56 patients with
documented coronary artery disease were randomized to receive either a cardiac stress
management video or a meditation video which guided them through a standard a
standard Zen breath awareness meditation. The technique involved becoming
attentionally absorbed in the breath, but not manipulating it. Patients HRV (SDNN) was
measured during several conditions including rest, reading, paced breathing, Stroop color
word conflict stressor, post stressor rest, post stressor reading, and post stressor paced
breathing. Patients who received meditation instruction significantly increased heart rate
variability post intervention compared to patients who received a stress management
49
lecture (p </= .007). In addition, patients who engaged in meditation practice handled
stress better, as indicated by an increase in heart rate variability during the Stroop task (p
</= .042) and post-intervention pre-stressor paced breathing period (p </= .006). Results
suggest that engaging in even one brief period of Zen breath meditation awareness can be
effective for improving the hearts response to stress for patients with coronary artery
disease.
Frostell, C., J. N. Pande, and G. Hedenstierna. Effects of high frequency breathing on
pulmonary ventilation and gas exchange. Proceedings ICYR, 1984-1985, article no. 15,
pp. 125-130.
Fujimoto, Kenko. Shirarezaru Kenkoho: Yoga no Tokko, Uso no Yoni Yomigaeru
Atama, Karada, Kokoro [Unknown Health Methods : Beneficial Effects of Yoga for the
Mind, Body, and Heart]. Tokyo: Seishun Shuppansha, 1975. [In Japanese.]
Funderburk, James. Science Studies Yoga: A Review of Physiological Data. Honesdale,
Penn.: Himalayan International Institute of Yoga Science & Philosophy, 1977.
Contents: PHYS IOLOGICAL RESPONSES TO HATHA YOGA: Muscular-Articular
Responses to Hatha Yoga (EMG studies, Flexibility, Pressure changes in internal
cavities, Power of breath), Circulatory Responses to Hatha Yoga (Cardiovascular
efficiency, Blood flow alterations, Heart rate, Heart control, Blood pressure, Blood
composition), Respiratory Responses to Hatha Yoga (Nostril dominance, Respiratory
pattern: breath rate, breath holding time, respiratory amplitude, Air movement: tidal
volume, minute ventilation, vital capacity, Gaseous transfer), Endocrine and Nervous
System Responses to Hatha Yoga (Secretory products, Autonomic balance, EEG);
PHYSIOLOGICAL RESPONSES TO MEDITATION: General Physiological Responses
to Meditation (Background: description of meditation, classification of experiments on
meditation, organization of part II, Muscular system responses to meditation, Circulatory
system responses to meditation, Respiratory system responses to meditation, Endocrine
and nervous system responses to meditation), EEG during Meditation (The EEG rhythms:
alpha waves, theta waves, delta waves, beta waves, other aspects of EEG rhythms, EEG
responses to stimuli), APPENDIXES: References, Figures, Glossary of Physiology
Terms, Glossary of Yoga Terms, Biographical Notes on Yoga Researchers, INDEX
Funk, Edwin, and John Clarke. The nasal cycle: Observations over prolonged periods
of time. The Journal of The International Association of Yoga Therapists, 1994, no. 5, pp.
9-12.
Funk, Leslie. Hot Yoga: Physiological concerns while exercising in the heat. Yoga
World, Apr-Jun 2001, no. 17, pp. 6-7. Author email: [email protected].
Gaertner, H., et al. Influence of Sirshasana (headstand position). Paper given at 10th
Congress of the International Haematological Society, Stockholm, 30 Aug 4 Sep 1964.
50
Measured the effects of one-half hour of sirsasana, including blood chemicals, heartbeat,
blood pressure, and respiration.
Ganguly, S. K. Effect of short term yogic training programme on cardio-vascular
endurance. SNIPES Journal, 1981, 4(2):45-50.
___________. Immediate effect of kapalabhati on cardiovascular endurance. YogaMimamsa, 1989, 28(1):1-7.
___________, and M. V. Bhole. Influence of breathing on flexibility. Yoga-Mimamsa,
1985, 26(1):1-7.
___________, and M. L. Gharote. Cardio vascular efficiency before and after yogic
training. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1974, 17(1):89-97.
___________, and M. L. Gharote. Effect of yogic training on endurance and flexibility
level. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1989, 27(3&4):29-39.
___________, and M. L. Gharote, and S. R. Jolly. Immediate effect of kapalabhati on
the cardiovascular endurance. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1988, 27(1&2):8-9.
Gash, Arnold, and Joel S. Karliner. No effect of Transcendental Meditation on left
ventricular function. Annals of Internal Medicine, Feb 1978, 88(2):215-216.
Gaylord, C., D. Orme -Johnson, and E. Travies. The effect of Transcendental
Meditation technique and progressive relaxation on EEG coherence and stress reactivity
and mental health in black adults. International Journal of Neuroscience, 1989, 46(1):7786.
Gellhorn, Ernest, and William F. Kiely. Mystical states of consciousness:
Neurophysiological and clinical aspects. PMID: 5030966. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, Jun 1972,154(6):399-405.
Gharote, M. L. Effect of yogic exercises on the strength and endurance of the abdominal
muscles of the females. Vyayam Vidnyan, 1970, 4(1):11-13.
___________. Effect of air swallowing on the gastric acidity. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1971,
14(1&2):7-10.
___________. A psychophysiological study of the effects of short term yogic training on
the adolescent high school boys. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1971, 14(1&2):92-99.
___________. Energy expenditure during deep meditative state. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1973,
114(1&2):57-62.
51
52
53
54
___________, and S. Lakshmanan. Some observations on Hatha Yoga: The bandhas, an anatomical
study. Yoga Life, Jan 1973, 4(1):3-18.
56
57
58
CRH after running and of CRH after meditation, but no significant differences in CRH
increases between groups. CRH was correlated with positive mood changes after running
and mediation. Cortisol levels were generally high but erratic in both groups. We
conclude that positive affect is associated with plasma CRH immunoreactivity which
itself is significantly associated with circulating beta-EP supporting a role for CRH in the
release of beta- EP. Increased CRH immunoreactivity following meditation indicates,
however, that physical exercise is not an essential requirement for CRH release.
Harvey, J. R. The effect of yogic breathing exercises on mood. Journal of the American
Society of Psychosomatic Dentistry and Medicine, 1983, 30(2):39-48.
Hassanagas, Pavlos K. The physiological evaluation of the effects resulting from
application of yoga. Athens, Greece: International Association of Yoga Science Centres,
1982. Email: [email protected], URL: http://www.yoga.org.mk.
___________. Comparative values of the functional and [motor] functions of the
beginners and advanced group of yoga practitioners. Athens, Greece: International
Association of Yoga Science Centres, 1997. Email: [email protected], URL:
http://www.yoga.org.mk.
___________. The phenomenon of yoga and the search for a science of yoga. Athens,
Greece: International Association of Yoga Science Centres, 1997. Email:
[email protected], URL: http://www.yoga.org.mk.
___________. Positive changes in the psychic functions as a result of regular yoga
practice. Athens, Greece: International Association of Yoga Science Centres, 1997.
Email: [email protected], URL: http://www.yoga.org.mk.
___________. Possibilities for application of yoga techniques to normal psychophysical
development of children and youth. Athens, Greece: International Association of Yoga
Science Centres, 1997. Email: [email protected], URL: http://www.yoga.org.mk.
___________. Survey of the somatic improvements of the yoga practitioners. Athens,
Greece: International Association of Yoga Science Centres, 1997. Email:
[email protected], URL: http://www.yoga.org.mk.
___________. Yoga and science: The phenomenon of yoga and the search for a science
of yoga. Athens, Greece: International Association of Yoga Science Centres, 1997.
Email: [email protected], URL: http://www.yoga.org.mk.
___________. Possibilities for application of yoga techniques for proper psychophysical
development of children and youth. Athens, Greece: International Association of Yoga
Science Centres, 1999. Email: [email protected], URL: http://www.yoga.org.mk.
59
Hebert, R., and D. Kehmann. Theta bursts: An EEG pattern in normal subjects
practicing the Transcendental Meditaiton technique. Eleftroencephalgraphy and Clinical
Neurophysiology, 1977, 42:397-405.
Henrotte, J. G. Yoga et biologie. Atomes, May 1969, no. 265. [In French.]
Herxheimer, A. Study of meditation and blood pressure (letter). The New England
Journal of Medicine, 1976, 294(14):786.
Herzog, H., V. R. Lele, T. Kuwert, K. J. Langen, E. R. Kops, and L. E. Feinendegen.
Changed pattern of regional [cerebral] glucose metabolism during yoga meditative
relaxation. Neuropsychobiology, 1990-91, 23(4):182-187.
Abstract: Using positron emission tomography (PET), measurements of the regional
cerebral metabolic rate of glucose (rCMRGlc) are able to delineate cerebral metabolic
responses to external or mental stimulation. In order to examine possible changes of brain
metabolism due to Yoga meditation PET scans were performed in 8 members of a Yoga
meditation group during the normal control state (C) and Yoga meditative relaxation
(YMR). Whereas there were intraindividual changes of the total CMRGlc, the alterations
were not significant for intergroup comparison; specific focal changes or changes in the
interhemispheric differences in metabolism were also not seen; however the ratios of
frontal vs. occipital rCMRGlc were significantly elevated (p less than 0.05) during YMR.
These altered ratios were caused by a slight increase of frontal rCMRGlc and a more
pronounced reduction in primary and secondary visual centers. These data indicate a
holostic behavior of the brain metabolism during the time of altered state of
consciousness during YMR.
Hesman, Tina. University will wire students brains to track changes. Post-Dispatch (St.
Louis, MO), 19 Mar. 2005.
Maharishi University of Management hopes to wire all of its 750 students and issue
them brain integration report cardspurportedly a new way of determining how their
college experience changes the brain.
While this is an unconventional idea, this is an unconventional school.
Researchers at Maharishi University hope to prove that the deep body relaxation
technique of meditation can increase academic performance, improve judgment and
decision- making skills.
Others are skeptical. But the idea may not be as far-fetched as it sounds. Scientists
elsewhere also have been studying how meditation affects the brain. A recent study of
Tibetan monks showed they had more active brain waves during meditation . . .
Hirai, T. ElectrHirai, T. Electroencephalographic study on Zen meditation. Folia
Psychiatr. Neurol. (Japan), 1960, 62:76-105.
60
61
Hoffman, Kevin, and John Clarke, M.D. A comparative study of the cardiac response
to bhastrika: A yogic breathing exercise and the exercise tolerance test. Research Bulletin
of the Himalayan International Institute, 1982, 4(2): 7-16. Reprinted in The Journal of
The International Association of Yoga Therapists, 1996, no. 7, pp. 35-42.
Holmes, D. S. Meditation and somatic arousal reduction: A review of the experimental
evidence. American Psychologist, 1984, 39(1):1-10.
___________, S. Solomon, B. M. Cappo, et al. Effects of Transcendental Meditation
versus resting on physiological and subjective arousal. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1983, 44(6):1245-1252.
Honebrink, Andrea. Meditation: Hazardous to your health? Dont overlook the side
effects of this powerful transformative technique. Utne Reader, Mar/Apr 1994, p. 26.
Houshmand, Zara, Robert B. Livingstone, and B. Alan Wallace, eds. Consciousness
at the Crossroads: Conversation with the Dalai Lama on Brain Science and Buddhism.
Ithaca, N.Y.: Snow Lion Publications, 1999.
Hubert, J. Z., and T. M. Srinivasan. Thermodynamic description of pure
consciousness. The Yoga Review, 1984, 4(1&2):55-68.
Abstract: The experience of Pure Consciousness (PC) is met on the way to Samadhithe
goal of all yogic practices. Though the earlier seven steps of control of body and mind
discussed in Astanga Yogaare fairly comprehensively described and understood in
current scientific terminology, the state of Samadhi seems to defy all such simplistic and
logical approaches. There is therefore an urgent need to understand the physical and the
psychophysiological basis of PC and to relate it to the evolutionary process through
modern scientific idea. In this paper, we try to describe the state of PC using the
thermodynamic principle of minimum entropy production.
Ikemi, Y., H. Ishikawa, J. R. Goyeche, and Y. Sasaki. Positive and negative
aspects of the altered states of consciousness induced by autogenic training, Zen and
yoga. Psychother Psychosom (Switzerland), 1978, 30(3-4):170-178.
Improvement in audio and audio visual memory following yoga training of high
school children. Swami Vivekananda Yoga Research Foundation,
http://www.vkyogas.org.in.
Infante, J. R., M. Torres-Avisbal, P. Pinel, J. A. Vallejo, F. Peran, F. Gonzalez, P.
Contreras, C. Pacheco, A. Roldan, and J. M. Latre. Catecholamine levels in
practitioners of the transcendental meditation technique. Physiology and Behavior, Jan
2001, 72(1-2):141-146. MEDLINE PMID: 11239991.
Abstract: With the aim of evaluating the sympathetic-adrenal medulla system in subjects
practicing transcendental meditation (TM), their plasma catecholamine levels were
62
determined at two different times of day. The study group consisted of 19 subjects who
regularly practice either TM or Sidhi-TM technique, with a control group made up of 16
healthy subjects who had not previously used any relaxation technique. Catecholamine
plasma levels were determined by high performance liquid chromatography, at 0900 and
2000 h. Morning and evening norepinephrine (NE) levels and morning epinephrine (E)
levels were significantly lower in the TM group than in the control subjects (morning NE
levels, pg/ml, mean+/-S.E.: TM group 136.6+/-13.0, control 236.8+/-21.0, P=.0001;
evening NE levels: TM group 119.7+/-10.8, control 175.6+/-17.4, P=.009; morning E
levels, pg/ml: TM group 140.2+/-10.6, control 196.7+/-23.8, P=.019). No differences
were recorded for evening E levels and dopamine (DA) levels. No significant differences
were found for catecholamine levels measured at different times of day in the TM group,
demonstrating a lack of daily hormonal rhythm. Anxiety levels were similar in both
groups. Based on the results obtained, it can be considered that the regular practice of TM
has a significant effect on the sympathetic-adrenal medulla system. A low hormonal
response to daily stress caused by sympathetic tone regulation through regular TM could
explain our results, as well as the physiological and other effects related to the field of
health described in those who practice meditation.
Influence of 10 days of yoga training on psychophysiological indicators of stress in
children. Swami Vivekananda Yoga Research Foundation, http://www.vkyogas.org.in.
Influence of yoga practice on exercise tolerance in healthy adults and children.
Swami Vivekananda Yoga Research Foundation, http://www.vkyogas.org.in.
Influence of yoga practices on voluntary control over autonomic functions. Swami
Vivekananda Yoga Research Foundation, http://www.vkyogas.org.in.
Iyengar, B. K. S. Chakras, bandhas, and kriyas. Iyengar Yoga Institute Review, Summer
1989, 9(3):1-10. (On the psychophysiology of Yoga.)
___________. Good health through yoga. Yoga Rahasya, 1995, 2(2):14-22.
___________. Physiology and cakra. In B. K. S. Iyengar, Astadala Yogaml, Vol. 2.
New Delhi, India: Allied Publishers Limited, 2001, pp. 174-181.
Jahnke, Roger. Physiological mechanisms operating in the human system during the
practice of Qigong and Yoga/Pranayama. Article available online (footnotes not
included): http://www.healthy.net/asp/templates/article.asp?PageType=Article&id=388#.
Excerpted from Roger Jahnke, The Most Profound Medicine: The Supreme Essence of
the Asian Traditional Systems of Medicine are the Self Applied Health Maintenance
Practices: Qigong and Yoga/Pranayama. Roger Jahnke, 1990.
Javalgekar, R. R., and G. P. Kale. Efficacy of yoga in regulation of cardiac function.
Symposium Int Soc Heart Research, Indian Section, 1980.
63
64
Abstract: While for centuries a wakeful and tranquil state or experience variously called
samadhi, pure awareness, or enlightenment had been said to be a normal
experience and the goal of meditation in Vedic, Buddhist, and Taoist traditions, there was
little known about this behavior unt il recently, when the practice of transcendental
meditation (TM) became available for study in Western scientific laboratories. Derived
from the Vedic tradition, TM is unique because it requires no special circumstances or
effort for practice. Based upon a wide spectrum of physiological data on TM, we
hypothesize that meditation is an integrated response with peripheral circulatory and
metabolic changes subserving increased central nervous activity. Consistent with the
subjective description of meditation as a very relaxed but, at the same time, a very alert
state, it is likely that such findings during meditation as increased cardiac output,
probable increased cerebral blood flow, and findings reminiscent of the extraordinb
ary character of classical reports: apparent cessation of CO2 generation by muscle,
fivefold plasma AVP elevation, and EEG synchrony play critical roles in this putative
response.
___________, A. F. Wilson, and J. M. Davidson. Adrenocortical activity during
meditation. Hormones & Behavior, 1978, 10(1):54-60.
___________, A. F. Wilson, J. P. O'Halloran, and R. N. Walsh. Forearm blood flow
and metabolism during stylized and unstylized states of decreased activation. American
Journal of Physiology, Jul 1983, 245(1):R110-116. PMID: 6869572.
Abstract: We have measured forearm oxygen consumption and blood flow changes
during two wakeful rest behaviors. We have observed acute reduction of forearm
respiration (28%) during an acute stylized rest state (TM) and a nonsignificant small
decline (11%) during unstylized ordinary eyes-closed rest. These changes were not
associated with significant change of forearm blood flow or glycolytic metabolism.
Hence, forearm oxygen consumption decline was due almost solely to decreased rate of
oxygen extraction. Small variation of forearm blood flow implies that little of the
previous findings of increased nonrenal, nonhepatic circulation during TM or increased
nonrenal circulation during ordinary rest can be accounted for by altered muscle blood
flow, which therefore is consistent with possible increased cerebral blood flow. However,
reduced muscle metabolism was a likely contributor to the forearm metabolic decline.
The lack of coupling between metabolic and blood flow changes during TM indicates
limitation of obligatory coupling between cardiovascular and metabolic function in the
rest state of TM.
___________, A. F. Wilson, H. Pirkle, J. P. O'Halloran, and R. N. Walsh. Metabolic
control in a state of decreased activation: Modulation of red cell metabolism. American
Journal of Physiology, 1983, 245:C457-C461. PMID: 6416079.
Abstract: Very little is known in depth of the biochemical and physiological changes
induced at the cellular level by human behavioral states. For study of the physiology of
behavior at this level, the erythrocyte may be useful, because it is readily available and its
65
metabolism and metabolic control are comparatively well understood. In this report we
describe a marked decline of red cell glycolytic rate induced by the transcendental
meditation technique (TM). This decline was significantly correlated with decreased
plasma lactate concentration and with relaxation as indicated by electrodermal response.
The occurrence of sleep was not correlated with the metabolic changes. The observed
lack of variation of blood pH, blood gases, glucose, and hematocrit in this behavior
implies that the decrease of erythrocyte metabolism is not an epiphenomenon of
respiratory change or substrate availability. Based upon further measurements indicating
persisting alteration of the red blood cell, we suggest the possibility of attachment of a
humoral agent(s) to the cell in the mechanism of this effect. This behavioral effect is
unique, and the effector(s) responsible may increase our understanding of metabolic
control of the erythrocyte and of TM.
___________, A. F. Wilson, W. R. Smith, and M. E. Morton Redistribution of blood
flow in acute hypometabolic behavior. American Journal of Physiology, Jul 1978,
235(1):R89-92. PMID: 354414.
Abstract: Cardiac output, renal and hepatic blood flows, arterial lactate concentration, and
minute volume were measured before, during, and after 40 min of rest induced either by
the practice known as transcendental meditation (TM) or by an ordinary eyes-closed
rest-relaxation period. Two groups of normal young adults were studied: one group
consisted of regular practitioners of TM and the other of similar individuals studied prior
to learning this technique. Marked declines of renal blood flow were noted in both
groups. Decline of hepatic blood flow, increased cardiac output, decreased arterial
lactate, and minute volume were also recorded in the TM- induced rest period. These
changes imply a considerable increase of nonrenal, nonhepatic blood flow during TM
(44%) and, to a lesser extent, during rest (12%). Increased cerebral and/or skin blood
flow is hypothesized to account for part of the redistributed blood flow in the practitioner.
___________, Archie F. Wilson, and E. VanderLaan. Plasma prolactin and growth
hormone during meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1978, 40:329-333.
___________, Archie F. Wilson, E. Vanderlaan, and S. Levine. Plasma prolactin and
cortisol during Transcendental Meditation. In D. W. Orme-Johnson and J. T. Farrow,
eds., Scientific Research on the Transcendental Meditation Program, Collected Papers,
Vol. 1. Germany: Maharishi European Research University Press, 1976.
Johnson, Stephen J. Effects of yoga-therapy on conflict resolution, self-concept, and
emotional adjustment. Doctoral dissertation, University of Southern California, 1974.
Dissertation Abstracts International, Apr 1974, 34(10-A):6385.
Jones, Christopher Martin. Understanding the practice of meditation from the
perspectives of neuroscience, phenomenology, and Zen Buddhism. Masters thesis.
University of Louisville, Kentucky, 2002. Masters Abstracts International, Dec 2002,
40/06, p. 1383. First 24 pages available online:
http://wwwlib.umi.com/dissertations/preview/1409363. UMI #1409363.
66
Abstract: This thesis investigates the practice of meditation from these three distinct
perspectives. Meditation practice as defined in this analysis includes both concentration
meditation and mindfulness or awareness meditation as developed in Zen Buddhism.
Our examination of meditation begins with neuroscience. In the first chapter, we examine
how neuroscience explains the construction of self and reality through processes of the
brain. We follow the path of stimuli from the sense organs to the higher structures of the
brain as this information is processed to form both our external reality as well as our
internal sense of self. We then examine studies of the effect of the practice of meditation
on these association processes of the brain, as researched by neuroscientists.
While neuroscience provides us with an objective understanding of how the brain
functions during meditation, it is limited in providing us with an explanation of the
subjective experience of meditation. In the second chapter, in order to gain an
understanding of the subjective processes of the mind, we must turn to philosophy.
Through phenomenology, we are provided with a similar sequential explanation of the
construction of self and reality. We then examine how the practice of meditation affects
these subjective experiences.
While pheno menology provides a more in-depth analysis of our subjective experience, it
is limited to an abstract description of understanding selfand reality. In the third chapter,
we examine the experiential practice of meditation as outlined by practitioners of Zen.
This explanation of meditative practice provides an understanding of the Buddhist nondual view of reality as the ground out of whch our objective and subjective
understandings of self and reality arise.
Neuroscience, phenomenology, and Zen are three very different perspectives for
examining reality. This thesis points to similarities in how each perspective understands
the construction of self and reality, thus providing a deeper understanding of the practice
as a whole. Previously, attempts to understand meditation were limited to either objective
or subjective approaches; however this thesis combines both objective and subjective
approaches to meditation as well as a more complete understanding of the non-dual
perspective of Zen.
Joseph, C., A. Shankar Ram, D. D. Kulkarni, M. Ramachanadra, G. Narasimhalu,
and T. Desiraju. Post meditational effects of Brahmakumari (BK) and Transcendental
Meditation (TM) on computer-averaged event related evoked potential components
recorded in the P300 cognitive paradigm. Indian Journal of Physiology and
Pharmacology, 1987, 31(5).
___________, A. Shankar Ram, H. N. Murthy, and T. Desiraju. Comparison of senior
yogis with control subjects on personality traits, levels of self-actualisation and
adjustment. Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, 1987, 31(5).
67
68
69
70
Karmananda, Dr. Swami. Mysteries of the pineal. Yoga (Sivananda Math), Mar 1979,
17(3).
Kasamatsu, A., and R. Hirai. An electroencephalographic study of the Zen meditation
(Zazen). Folia Psychiatrica Neurologica Japonica, 1966, 20(4):315-336. Also in C. T.
Tart, ed., Altered States of Consciousness. Garden City, N.Y.: Doubleday, 1972, pp. 501514. Also in D. H. Shapiro, and R. N. Walsh, eds., Meditation: Classic and
Contemporary Perspectives. New York: Aldine Publishing, 1984, pp. 480-492.
Keuning, J. On the nasal cycle. Rhinology International, 1968, 6:99-136.
Khasky, A. D., and J. C. Smith. Stress, relaxation states, and creativity. Perceptual
Motor Skills, Apr 1999, 88(2):409-416. PMID: 10483629.
Abstract: 114 participants in four groups practiced 25 minutes of progressive muscle
relaxation, yoga stretching, imagery, or a control task. Before and after training,
participants took state versions of the Smith Quick Stress Test (which measures Somatic
Stress, Negative Affect, and Worry) and the Smith R-State Inventory (which measures
relaxation-related states Disengagement, Physical Relaxation, Mental Relaxation,
Strength and Awareness, Joy, Love and Thankfulness, and Prayerfulness). After training,
all took both the Verbal and Figural forms of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. At
posttest, groups' scores did not differ on Creativity; however, when compared with yoga
stretching, imagery trainees had lower posttest scores on Negative Affect. Both yoga
stretching and imagery trainees displayed higher scores on self- reported Physical
Relaxation than did controls. Progressive muscle relaxation trainees had lower scores on
Somatic Stress than controls. Paradoxically, for all relaxation trainees, Disengagement
(feeling "distant, far away, indifferent") correlated positively with both Negative Affect
and Physical Relaxation, suggesting that disengagement in relaxation may not lead to
relaxation- induced anxiety but may help one cope with such anxiety.
Katkov, A. Ju. Kde kl juc k anabiozu jogov [Where is the key to the anabiosis of
yogis?]. Chimia i zizn, 1976, no. 12, pp. 107-109. [In Russian.]
___________. Prozvolna ja gipoventiljaci ja v ritme odno dychanie v minutu kak
sredetvo snizeni ja metabolizma [Conscious hypoventillation at a one breath-a-minute
pace as a means of reducing metabolism]. In Abstrakty I. Pracovnej Konferencie of
Problematike Jgy v Rehabilitacii [Abstracts of the First Conference on the Applications
of Yoga in Rehabilitational Therapy], Koice-aca, 21-23 Jun 1978, pp. 50-53. [In
Russian.]
Kaushik, R. P. A scientific look at yoga. Yoga Today, Nov 1979, 4(7):15-18.
Kaushik, Y. P., Y. Paul, and M. Gupta. Yoga for memory development of schoolgoing children. In H. R. Nagendra, R. Ragarathna, and S. Telles, Yoga Research &
Applications: Proceedings of the 5th International Conference on Frontiers in Yoga
Research and Applications. Bangalore, Vivekananda Kendra Yoga Research Foundation,
2000, pp. 285-288.
71
72
73
74
75
Kras, D. J. Transcendental Meditation and EEG alpha activity. In David W. OrmeJohnson, and John T. Farrow, eds., Scientific Research on the Transcendental Meditation
Program: Collected Papers, Vol. I. Germany: Maharishi European Research University
Press, 1976, pp. 175-181.
Kristeller, J. L. Heart rate slowing: Biofeedback vs. meditation. Res Psychol Med, 1979,
1:486.
Kubota, Y., W. Sato, M. Toichi, T. Murai, T. Okada, A. Hayashi, and A.
Sengoku. Frontal midline theta rhythm is correlated with cardiac autonomic activities
during the performance of an attention demanding meditation procedure. Cognitive Brain
Research, Apr 2001, 11(2):281-287. PMID: 11275489.
Abstract: Frontal midline theta rhythm (Fm theta), recognized as distinct theta activity on
EEG in the frontal midline area, reflects mental concentration as well as meditative state
or relief from anxiety. Attentional network in anterior frontal lobes including anterior
cingulate cortex is suspected to be the generator of this activity, and the regulative
function of the frontal neural network over autonomic nervous system (ANS) during
cognitive process is suggested. However no studies have examined peripheral autonomic
activities during Fm theta induction, and interaction of central and peripheral mechanism
associated with Fm theta remains unclear. In the present study, a standard procedure of
Zen meditation requiring sustained attention and breath control was employed as the task
to provoke Fm theta, and simultaneous EEG and ECG recordings were performed. For
the subjects in which Fm theta activities were provoked (six men, six women, 48% of the
total subjects), peripheral autonomic activities were evaluated during the appearance of
Fm theta as well as during control periods. Successive inter-beat intervals were measured
from the ECG, and a recently developed method of analysis by Toichi et al. (J. Auton.
Nerv. Syst. 62 (1997) 79-84) based on heart rate variability was used to assess cardiac
sympathetic and parasympathetic functions separately. Both sympathetic and
parasympathetic indices were increased during the appearance of Fm theta compared
with control periods. Theta band activities in the frontal area were correlated negatively
with sympathetic activation. The results suggest a close relationship between cardiac
autonomic function and activity of medial frontal neural circuitry.
Kugler, J. Neurologische Storungen nach Yogaubungen. [Neurologic disorders
following yoga exercises]. Med Klin. (West Germany), 15 Sep 1972, 67(37):1195. [In
German.]
Kulkarni, D. D. Yoga and the problem of intra-individual variations. Proceedings,
National Conference on Biomedical Research in Yoga, University of Poona, Pune, India,
1991, pp. 101-104.
___________. Yoga and neuropsychology. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1997, 33(1):1-22.
Kulkarni, T. R. Neuro-physiological considerations. In T. R. Kulkarni, Upanishads and
Yoga. Bombay: Bharatiya Vidya Bhavan, 1972, pp. 99-125.
76
78
79
80
Lakshmikanthan, C., R. Alagesan, et al. Yoga and the heart. Indian Journal of
Physiology and Pharmacology, 1978, 22(2):240-241.
Lang, R., K. Dechof, K. A. Meurer, and W. Kaulman. Sympathetic activity and
Transcendental Meditation. Journal of Neural Transm., 1979, 44:117-135.
Laubry, C., and T. Brosse. Documents recueillis aux Indes sur les Yoguis par
lenregistrement simltane du pouls, de la respiration et de lelectrocardiogramme [Data
gathered in India on a yogi with simultaneous registration of the pulse, respiration, and
electrocardiogram]. Presse Medicale, 1936, 44:1601-1604. [In French.]
Laurie, Gina. An investigation into the changes in skin resistance during the
Transcendental Meditation technique. In David W. Orme-Johnson, and John T. Farrow,
eds., Scientific Research on the Transcendental Meditation Program: Collected Papers,
Volume I. Maharishi European Research University Press, 1976, p. 216-223.
Lazar, S. W., G. Bush, R. L. Gollub, G. L. Fricchione, G. Khalsa, and H. Benson.
Functional brain mapping of the relaxation response and meditation. Neuroreport, 15
May 2000, 11(7):1581-1585.
Meditation is a conscious mental process that induces a set of integrated physiologic
changes termed the relaxation response. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)
was used to identify and characterize the brain regions that are active during a simple
form of meditation. Significant (p<10(-7)) signal increases were observed in the groupaveraged data in the dorsolateral prefrontal and parietal cortices,
hippocampus/parahippocampus, temporal lobe, pregenual anterior cingulate cortex,
striatum, and pre- and post-central gyri during meditation. Global fMRI signal decreases
were also noted, although these were probably secondary to cardiorespiratory changes
that often accompany meditation. The results indicate that the practice of meditation
activates neural structures involved in attention and control of the autonomic nervous
system.
Lazarus, Arnold A. Psychiatric problems precipitated by Transcendental Meditation.
Psychological Reports, 1976, 39:601-602.
Lee, Sung W., Carol A. Mancuso, and Mary E. Charlson. Prospective study of new
participants in a community-based mind-body training program. Journal of General
Internal Medicine, Jul 2004, 19(7):760-765. PMID: 15209590. Author email:
[email protected].
Abstract: BACKGROUND: Mind-body practices such as yoga are widely popular, but
little is known about how such exercises impact health-related quality of life.
OBJECTIVE: To measure changes in health-related quality of life associated with 3
months of mind-body training as practiced in community-based settings. DESIGN:
Prospective cohort study. SETTING: Eight centers for practice of mind-body training.
PARTICIPANTS: One hundred ninety-four English-speaking adults who had taken no
81
more than 10 classes at the centers prior to enrollment in the study. One hundred seventyone (88%) returned the 3- month follow-up questionnaire. INTERVENTION:
Administration of the SF-36 questionnaire at the start of training and after 3 months.
MEASUREMENTS AND MAIN RESULTS: At baseline, new participants in mind-body
training reported lower scores than U.S. norms for 7 of 8 domains of the SF-36: mental
health, role emotional, social, vitality, general health, body pain, and role physical (P <
.002 for all comparisons). After 3 months of training, within-patient change scores
improved in all domains (P < .0001), including a change of +15.5 (standard deviation
21) in the mental health domain. In hierarchical regression analysis, younger age (P=
.0003), baseline level of depressive symptoms (P= .01), and reporting a history of
hypertension (P= .0054) were independent predictors of greater improvement in the SF36 mental health score. Five participants (2.9%) reported a musculoskele tal injury.
CONCLUSIONS: New participants in a community-based mind-body training program
reported poor health-related quality of life at baseline and moderate improvements after 3
months of practice. Randomized trials are needed to determine whether benefits may be
generalizable to physician-referred populations.
Lehmann, D., P. L. Faber, P. Achermann, D. Jeanmonod, L. R. Gianotti, and D.
Pizzagalli. Brain sources of EEG gamma frequency during volitionally meditationinduced, altered states of consciousness, and experience of the self. Psychiatry Research,
30 Nov 2001, 108(2):111-21. Author email: [email protected]. PMID: 11738545.
Abstract: Multichannel EEG of an advanced meditator was recorded during four
different, repeated meditations. Locations of intracerebral source gravity centers as well
as Low Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography (LORETA) functional images of the
EEG gamma (35-44 Hz) frequency band activity differed significantly between
meditations. Thus, during volitionally self- initiated, altered states of consciousness that
were associated with different subjective meditation states, different brain neuronal
populations were active. The brain areas predominantly involved during the self- induced
meditation states aiming at visualization (right posterior) and verbalization (left central)
agreed with known brain functional neuroanatomy. The brain areas involved in the selfinduced, meditational dissolution and reconstitution of the experience of the self (right
fronto-temporal) are discussed in the context of neural substrates implicated in normal
self-representation and reality testing, as well as in depersonalization disorders and
detachment from self after brain lesions.
Lehrer, Paul M. Psychophysiological effects of relaxation. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 1978, 46(3):389404.
___________, Yuji Sasaki, and Yoshihiro Saito. Zazen and cardiac variability.
Psychosomatic Medicine, Nov/Dec 1999, 61(6):812-821.
Abstract: OBJECTIVE: This study examined the effects of tanden breathing by Zen
practitioners on cardiac variability. Tanden breathing involves slow breathing into the
lower abdomen. METHODS: Eleven Zen practitioners, six Rinzai and five Soto, were
each studied during 20 minutes of tanden breathing, preceded and followed by 5- minute
periods of quiet sitting. During this time, we measured heart rate and respiration rate.
82
RESULTS: For most subjects, respiration rates fell to within the frequency range of 0.05
to 0.15 Hz during tanden breathing. Heart rate variability significantly increased within
this low- frequency range but decreased in the high- frequency range (0.140.4 Hz),
reflecting a shift of respiratory sinus arrhythmia from high- frequency to slower waves.
Rinzai practitioners breathed at a slower rate and showed a higher amplitude of lowfrequency heart rate waves than observed among Soto Zen participants. One Rinzai
master breathed approximately once per minute and showed an increase in very- lowfrequency waves (<0.05 Hz). Total amplitude of heart rate oscillations (across frequency
spectra) also increased. More experienced Zen practitioners had frequent heart rhythm
irregularities during and after the nadir of heart rate oscillations (i.e., during inhalation).
CONCLUSIONS: These data are consistent with the theory that increased oscillation
amplitude during slow breathing is caused by resonance between cardiac variability
caused by respiration and that produced by physiological processes underlying slower
rhythms. The rhythm irregularities during inhalation may be related to inhibition of vagal
modulation during the cardioacceleratory phase. It is not known whether they reflect
cardiopathology.
___________, S. Schoickett, P. Carrington, and R. L. Woolfolk. Psychophysiological
and cognitive responses to stressful stimulation in subjects practicing progressive
relaxation and clinically standardized meditation. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
1979, 18:293303.
___________, R. L. Woolfolk, A. J. Rooney, B. McCann, and P. Carrington.
Progressive relaxation and meditation: A study of psychophysiological and therapeutic
differences between two techniques. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1983, 21: 651
662.
Leininger, P M., R. M. Hakim, B. R. Wagner, and J. Spencer. Appropriate balance
tests when assessing the effects of yoga on healthy community-dwelling older adults.
Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, May 2003, 35(5) Supplement:S168 .
Abstract: Purpose: The purpose of this study was to determine if selected balance tests
are sensitive and appropriate when determining if there is a significant difference in the
balance abilities on selected balance test with a group of yoga and non-yoga trained older
adults. Diminished balance abilities in the elderly often predispose the individual to a
potentially serious fall. If yoga training can be shown to produce significant improvement
in balance with older adults it might reduce the incidence of falls and prove an effective
intervention. There are numerous balance tests incorporated with older adults, but some
prove more sensitive and valid when determining the risk for falls in the elderly.
Subjects: 42 healthy and active community-dwelling older adults, 24 (mean age, 70.50,
range 62 - 79) who have recently participated in yoga classes (Y), and 20 (mean age
72.05, range 63 - 80) who had not (NY). Methods: Three balance measurements, the Berg
Balance Test (BBT), the Multidirectional Reach Teat (MDRT), and the Timed Up and
Go Test (TUG) were administered to the two groups. Analysis: Means were calculated
for test scores from both groups. An independent t-test compared the difference in means.
Results: Only the BBT showed statistical difference (54.67 (Y) VS 53.15 (NY), p < .01,
although not clinically significant. Two of the items on the BBT, the Tandem Stand and
83
the One Leg Stand, showed significant differences, 3.54 (Y) VS 3.00 (NY), and 3.54 (Y)
VS 2.85 (NY) respectively, p < .05. Conclusions: When testing the effect of intervention
on balance ability of healthy active older adults, the BBT, MDRT and TUG tests may not
be sensitive to detect significant change and may not be the most appropriate indicators.
Further studies utilizing either more challenging balance tests (e.g. timed single- leg
stance), or tests on a less healthy population may be necessary to detect a significant
effect of yoga training on balance.
Lepicovsk, V., and D. Brta. Changes in plethysmogram and ECG during uddiyana
exercise. Jgov Cvicen, 1982, pp. 92-96. [In Czech.]
___________, and C. Dostlek. Influence of some hatha yogic exercises upon the
cardiovascular system. Int Cong Int Med Praha, August 22-27, 1982.
___________, and C. Dostlek. Effect of nauli upon cardiovascular system. YogaMimamsa, 1987-1988, 26(3&4):25-42.
___________, C. Dostlek, and M. Kovarova. Hathayogic exercise jalandharabandha in
its effect on cardiovascular response to apnoea. Act Nerv Super (Praha), Jun 1990,
32(2):99-114. PMID: 2399805.
Abstract: Jalandharabandha (JB) is the important constituent of apnoea (kumbhaka) in
hathayogic breathing exercises. It is performed by pressing the chin into the jugular notch
and creating thus the positive pressure on the neck region. The influence of JB on the
heart rate and vasomotor response was studied in relationship to different lung volumes.
The course of R-R intervals is highly significantly different according to the type of
apnoea. JB leads to the diminution of bradycardia, but does not change the position of the
maximum and minimum in comparison to the apnoea without JB. Application of JB
increases the number of vasodilatatio ns and shortens the latencies of vasodilatations,
duration and amplitude of reactions. JB during breath holding decreases the vagal reflex
changes and may thus work as a stabilizing component in yogic breathing exercises.
Lesh, Terry. Research on meditation. In Terry Lesh, Meditation for Young People. New
York: Lothrop, Lee & Shepard, 1977, pp. 47-85.
Levander, V. L., H. Benson, R. C. Wheeler, and R. C. Wallace. Increased forearm
blood flow in a wakeful hypometabolic state. Federal Proceedings, 1972, 31:403.
(Transcendental Meditation.)
Levin, Jeff, and Lea Steele. The transcendental experience: Conceptual, theoretical, and
epidemiologic perspectives. Explore, Mar 2005, 1(2):89-101.
Linden, William. Practicing of meditation by school children and their levels of field
dependence-independence, test anxiety, and reading achievement. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, Aug 1973, 41(1):139-143. Also in D. H. Shapiro, and R. N.
Walsh, eds., Meditation: Classic and Contemporary Perspectives. New York: Aldine
Publishing, 1984, pp. 89-93.
84
85
CBF distribution in the meditative state as well as during the resting state of normal
consciousness, and that characteristic patterns of neural activity support each state. These
findings enhance our understanding of the neural basis of different aspects of
consciousness.
Luthra, V. Effect of breath holding on aerobic and anaerobic capacities. Yoga-Mimamsa,
Oct 1985, 24(3):29.
Lutz, Antoine, Lawrence L. Greischar, Nancy B. Rawlings, Matthieu Ricard, and
Richard J. Davidson. Long-term meditators self- induce high-amplitude
gamma synchrony during mental practice. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 8 Nov 2004. Author email addresses: [email protected] or [email protected].
Article available online: http://www.pnas.org/cgi/reprint/0407401101v1.pdf.
Abstract: Practitioners understand meditation, or mental training, to be a process of
familiarization with ones own mental life leading to long- lasting changes in cognition
and emotion. Little is known about this process and its impact on the brain. Here we find
that long-term Buddhist practitioners self- induce sustained electroencephalographic highamplitude gamma-band oscillations and phase-synchrony during meditation. These
electroencephalogram patterns differ from those of controls, in particular over lateral
frontoparietal electrodes. In addition, the ratio of gamma-band activity (2542 Hz) to
slow oscillatory activity (413 Hz) is initially higher in the resting baseline before
meditation for the practitioners than the controls over medial frontoparietal electrodes.
This difference increases sharply during meditation over most of the scalp electrodes and
remains higher than the initial baseline in the postmeditation baseline. These data suggest
that mental training involves temporal integrative mechanisms and may induce short-term
and long-term neural changes.
MacLean, C. R., K. G. Walton, S. R. Wenneberg, D. K. Levitsky, J. V. Mandarino,
R. Waziri, and R. H. Schneider. Altered responses of cortisol, GH, TSH and
testosterone to acute stress after four months practice of Transcendental Meditation
(TM). Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 30 Nov 1994, 746:381384.
___________, K. G. Walton, S. R. Wenneberg, D. K. Levitsky, J. P. Manda rino, R.
Waziri, S. L. Hillis, and R. H. Schneider. Effects of the Transcendental Meditation
program on adaptive mechanisms: Changes in hormone levels and responses to stress
after 4 months of practice. Psychoneuroendocrinology, May 1997, 22(4):277295.
Madanmohan, D. P. Thombre, B. Balakumar, T. K. Nambinarayanan, S. Thakur,
N. Krishnamurthy, and A. Chandrabose. Effects of yogic training on reaction time,
respiratory endurance and muscle strength. Indian Journal of Physiology and
Pharmacology, Oct 1993, 37(4):350-352. PMID: 1291472.
Abstract: There is evidence that the practice of yoga improves physical and mental
performance. The present investigation was undertaken to study the effect of yoga
training on visual and auditory reaction times (RTs), maximum expiratory pressure
86
(MEP), maximum inspiratory pressure (MIP), 40 mmHg test, breath holding time after
expiration (BHTexp), breath holding time after inspiration (BHTinsp), and hand grip
strength (HGS). Twenty seven student volunteers were given yoga training for 12 weeks.
There was a significant (P < 0.001) decrease in visual RT (from 270.0 +/- 6.20 (SE) to
224.81 +/- 5.76 ms) as well as auditory RT (from 194.18 +/- 6.00 to 157.33 +/- 4.85 ms).
MEP increased from 92.61 +/- 9.04 to 126.46 +/- 10.75 mmHg, while MIP increased
from 72.23 +/- 6.45 to 90.92 +/- 6.03 mmHg, both these changes being statistically
significant (P < 0.05). 40 mmHg test and HGS increased significantly (P < 0.001) from
36.57 +/- 2.04 to 53.36 +/- 3.95 s and 13.78 +/- 0.58 to 16.67 +/- 0.49 kg respectively.
BHTexp increased from 32.15 +/- 1.41 to 44.53 +/- 3.78s (P < 0.01) and BHTinsp
increased from 63.69 +/- 5.38 to 89.07 +/- 9.61 s (P < 0.05). Our results show that yoga
practice for 12 weeks results in significant reduction in visual and auditory RTs and
significant increase in respiratory pressures, breath holding times and HGS.
___________, Ananda Balayogi Bhavanani, and Kaviraja Udupa. Effect of direction
of head on heart rate and blood pressure. Yoga-Mimamsa, Jul 2002, 34(2):116-122.
Abstract: Indian culture stresses the importance of direction during performance of daily
activities. Some yoga teachers prescribe that yogic relaxation and polarity practices must
be done while lying with head towards north in order to align oneself with the earths
electromagnetic field. There is some evidence that earths magnetic field influences
physiological functions. Hence, the present study was undertaken to see whether head
direction has any effect on heart rate (HR) and blood pressure during supine rest. 43
normal healthy school children were recruited and their recordings were taken after 5
minutes of supine rest. The subjects were randomly assigned to lie with their head
towards north, east, south and west directions on four different days. HR and blood
pressure were recorded at the end of 5 minutes of supine rest. HR was loweset in north
and highest in south, the difference being statistically significant by students paired t
test. Systolic pressure was lowest in the north and significant ly higher in the west. Lying
supine with head towards north had the lowest rate-pressure-product as compared to the
west. Our study demonstrates that lying supine with head in different directions has a
definite effect on the HR and blood pressure. Further studies in different age groups and
in hypertensive patients may help in understanding the mechanisms and implications of
this phenomenon.
___________, U. C. Rai, V. Balavittal, D. P. Thombre, and Swami Gitananda.
Cardiorespiratory change during Savitri Pranayam and Shavasan. The Yoga Review,
1983, 3(1):25-34.
Abstract: The present study was conducted in trained (n=7) and untrained (n=7)
volunteers to determine the effect of savitri pranayam and shavasan on O2 consumption,
heart rate and blood pressure. In trained subjects we found a consistent and significant
(p<0.01) reduction in O2 consumption within a few minutes of starting savitri pranayam.
During shavasan, there was significant reduction in O2 consumption (p<0.05), heart rate
(p<0.001) and diastolic blood pressure (p<0.05). In untrained subjects, the changes in
above mentioned parameters were statistically insignificant.
87
88
___________. Yoga: A scientific lifestyle. Joy: The Journal of Yoga, Oct 2003, 2(10).
Includes the psyc hophysiological concept, Yoga and physical fitness, Yoga and
biochemical change, Yoga and hormonal balance, Yoga and psychiatric disorders, Yoga
and cardiovascular response, and Yoga and the respiratory system.
Maharshi Vinod Research Foundation. Effect of training in Ashtanga Yoga on
creativity, value system, alpha rhythm and visual evoked potential (V.E.P.) in youth.
Yoga-Mimamsa, 1987.
___________. Effect of training in Ashtanga Yoga on attention, numerical memory,
concentration and reaction time in yo uth. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1986.
Majmundar, Matra. The physiology of yoga therapeutics. Integrating Yoga
Therapeutics into Rehabilitation Seminar, St. Francis Memorial Hospital, San Francisco,
California, April 1-2, 2000.
___________. Physiology of Yoga Therapeutics. Forthcoming.
Majumdar, M., P. Grossman, B. Dietz-Waschkowski, S. Kersig, and H. Walach.
Does mindfulness meditation contribute to health? Outcome evaluation of a german
sample. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, Dec 2002, 8(6):719-730.
PMID: 12614525.
Abstract: Objectives: This exploratory study is the first systematic outcome evaluation to
examine the effects of an 8-week meditation-based program in mindfulness in a German
sample. Design: Twenty-one (21) participants with chronic physical, psychologic, or
psychosomatic illnesses were examined in a longitudinal pretest and post-treatment
design with a 3- month follow- up. Outcome Measures: Both quantitative and qualitative
data were gathered. Emotional and general physical well-being, sense of coherence,
overall psychologic distress, and satisfaction with life were measured with standardized
instruments. Results: Overall, the interventions led to high levels of adherence to the
meditation practice and satisfaction with the benefits of the course, as well as effective
and lasting reductions of symptoms (especially in psychologic distress, well-being, and
quality of life). Changes were of moderate-to-large effect sizes. Positive complementary
effects with psychotherapy were also found. Conclusions: These findings warrant
controlled studies to evaluate the efficacy and cost effectiveness of mindfulness-based
stress reduction as an intervention for chronic physical and psychosomatic disorders in
Germany.
Makwana, K., N. Khirwadkar, and H. C. Gupta. Effect of short term yoga practice on
ventilatory function tests. Indian Journal of Physiology & Pharmacology, Jul 1988,
32(3):202-208. PMID: 3198241.
Abstract: Twentyfive normal male volunteers undergoing a ten weeks course in the
practice of yoga have been studied by some parameters of ventilatory functions tests. The
89
observations recorded at the end of ten weeks of the course have shown improved
ventilatory functions in the form of lowered respiratory rate, increased forced vital
capacity, FEV1, maximum breathing capacity and breath holding time, while tidal
volume and %FEV1, did not reveal any significant change. Thus, a combined practice of
yoga seems to be beneficial on respiratory efficiency.
Malathi, A., A. Damodaran, Neela Iyar, and Laxmi Patel. Effect of yogic training on
body composition and physical fitness. Physiotherapy: The Journal of the Indian
Association of Physiotherapists, 1998-1999, 1:19-23.
___________, A. Damodaran, N. Shah, G. Krishnamurthy, P. Namjoshi, and S.
Ghodke. Psychophysiological changes at the time of examination in medical students
before and after practice of yoga and relaxation. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 1998,
40(1):35-40.
___________, A. Damodaran, N. Shah, N. Patil, and S. Maratha. Effect of yogic
practices on subjective well being. Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, Apr
2000, 44(2):202-206.
___________, and V. G. Parulkar. Effect of yogasanas on the visual and auditory
reaction time. Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, 1989, 33:110-112.
___________, Neela Patil, Nilesh Shah, A. Damodaran, and Marathe. Promotive,
prophylactic benefits of yogic practices in middle-aged women. Unpublished ms.
Malec, James, and Carl N. Sipprelle. Physiological and subjective effects of Zen
meditatio n and demand characteristics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
1977, 45:339-340. Also in D. H. Shapiro, and R. N. Walsh, eds., Meditation: Classic and
Contemporary Perspectives. New York: Aldine Publishing, 1984, pp. 548-549.
Malhotra, V., S. Singh, K. P. Singh, P. Gupta, S. B. Sharma, S. V. Madhu, and O. P.
Tandon. Study of yoga asanas in assessment of pulmonary function in NIDDM patients.
Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, Jul 2002, 46(3):313-320. PMID:
12613394.
Abstract: Certain yoga asanas if practiced regularly are known to have beneficial effects
on [the] human body. These yoga practices might be interacting with various somatoneuro-endocrine mechanisms to have therapeutic effects. The present study done in
twenty four NIDDM patients . . . 30 to 60 year[s] old provides metabolic and clinical
evidence of improvement in glycemic control and pulmonary functions. These middleaged subjects were type II diabetics on antihyperglycemic and dietary regimen. Their
baseline fasting and postprandial blood glucose and glycosylated Hb were monitored
along with pulmonary function studies. The expert gave these patients training in yoga
asanas [and they were practiced] 30-40 min/day for 40 days under guidance. These
asanas consisted of 13 well-known postures, done in a sequence. After 40 days of yoga
asanas regimen, [tests of] the parameters were repeated. The results indicate that there
90
was significant decrease in fasting blood glucose levels (basal 190.08 +/- 90.8 in mg/dl to
141.5 +/- 79.8 in mg/dl). The postprandial blood glucose levels also decreased (276.54
+/- 101.0 in mg/dl to 201.75 +/- 104.1 in mg/dl), glycosylated hemoglobin showed a
decrease (9.03 +/- 1.4% to 7.83 +/- 2.6%). The FEV1, FVC, PEFR, MVV increased
significantly (1.81 +/- 0.4 lt to 2.08 +/- 0.4 lt, 2.20 +/- 0.6 lt to 2.37 +/- 0.5 lt, 3.30 +/- 1.0
lt/s to 4.43 +/- 1.4 lt/s and 64.59 +/- 25.7 lt min to 76.28 +/- 28.1 lt/min respectively).
FEV1/FVC% improved (85 +/- 0.2% to 89 +/- 0.1%). These findings suggest that better
glycemic control and pulmonary functions can be obtained in NIDDM cases with yoga
asanas and pranayama. The exact mechanism as to how these postures and controlled
breathing interact with [the] somato- neuro-endocrine mechanism affecting metabolic and
pulmonary functions remains to be worked out.
Malhotra, V., S. Singh, O. P. Tandon, S. V. Madhu, A. Prasad, and S. B. Sharma.
Effect of Yoga asanas on nerve conduction in type 2 diabetes. Indian Journal of
Physiology and Pharmacology, Jul 2002, 46(3):298-306. PMID: 12613392.
Abstract: Twenty Type 2 diabetic subjects between the age . . . of 30-60 years were
studied to see the effect of 40 days of Yoga asanas on the nerve conduction velocity. The
duration of diabetes ranged from 0-10 years. Subjects suffering from cardiac, renal and
proliferative retinal complications were excluded from the study. Yoga asanas included
Suryanamskar, Tadasan, Konasan, Padmasan Pranayam, Paschimottansan,
Ardhmatsyendrasan, Shavasan, Pavanmukthasan, Sarpasan and Shavasan. Subjects were
called to the cardio-respiratory laboratory in the morning time and were given training by
the Yoga expert. The Yoga exercises were performed for 30-40 minutes every day for 40
days in the above sequence. The subjects were prescribed certain medicines and diet. The
basal blood glucose [and] nerve conduction velocity of the median nerve was measured
and repeated after 40 days of [the] Yogic regimen. Another [control] group of 20 Type 2
diabetes subjects of comparable age and severity . . . were kept on prescribed medication
and light physical exercises like walking. Their basal & post 40 days parameters were
recorded for comparison. Right hand and left hand median nerve conduction velocity
increased from 52.81 +/- 1.1 m/sec to 53.87 +/- 1.1 m/sec and 52.46 +/- 1.0 to 54.75 +/1/1 m/sec respectively. Control group nerve function parameters deteriorated over the
period of study, indicating that diabetes is a slowly progressive disease involving the
nerves. Yoga asanas have a beneficial effect on glycemic control and improve nerve
function in mild to moderate Type 2 diabetes with sub-clinical neuropathy.
Malloy, Jim. Meditation: Can sitting with your eyes closed improve your quality of life?
Article available online: http://www.selfgrowth.com/articles/Malloy1.html. (Discusses
health benefits.)
Mandell, Arnold J. Toward a psychobiology of transcendence: God in the brain. In
Richard J. Davidson and Julian M. Davidson, The Psychobiology of Consciousness.
Plenum Publishing, 1980, pp. 379-464.
Mandlik, Vishwas V. Effect of jalandhar bandh on blood pressure. Article available
online: http://www.yogapoint.com/info/research4.htm.
91
Results: If jalandhara bandha is performed improperly, by only fixing the chin in the
jugular notch, blood pressure may rise, possibly leading to hypertension, but blood
pressure was considerably reduced if jalandhara bandha was properly performed, i.e., by
contracting all the muscles of the neck and bending the neck a little forward.
___________, Premchand Jain, and Kiran Jain. Effect of yoga nidra on EEG. Yoga
Vidya Dham. Article available online: http://www.yogapoint.com/info/research5.htm#.
___________, and Ramesh Varkhede. Effect of Omkar chanting on concentration,
memory and level of fatigue. In H. R. Nagendra, R. Ragarathna, and S. Telles, Yoga
Research & Applications: Proceedings of the 5th International Conference on Frontiers
in Yoga Research and Applications. Bangalore, Vivekananda Kendra Yoga Research
Foundation, 2000, pp. 269-272.
Manjunath, N. K., C. Satish, S. Telles, and H. R. Nagendra. Card sorting studies on
students at school level undergoing personality development training through yoga.
Swami Vivekananda Yoga Research Foundation.
___________. Maize learning tests show beneficial effects in school students undergoing
IAYT. Swami Vivekananda Yoga Research Foundation.
___________, and S. Telles. Planning ability and speed of execution improve following
yoga training. Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, 1990.
___________, and S. Telles. Factors influencing changes in tweezer dexterity scores
following yoga training. Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, Apr 1999,
43(2):225-229.
___________, and S. Telles. Effect of yoga on perception and performance. In H. R.
Nagendra, R. Ragarathna, and S. Telles, Yoga Research & Applications: Proceedings of
the 5th International Conference on Frontiers in Yoga Research and Applications.
Bangalore, Vivekananda Kendra Yoga Research Foundation, 2000, pp. 303-306.
___________, and S. Telles. Improved performance in the Tower of London test
following yoga. Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, Jul 2001, 45(3):351354. PMID: 11881575
Abstract: Twenty girls between 10 and 13 years of age, studying at a residential school
were randomly assigned to two groups. One group practiced yoga for one hour fifteen
minutes per day, 7 days a week, while the other group was given physical training for the
same time. Time for planning and for execution and the number of moves required to
complete the Tower of London task were assessed for both groups at the beginning and
end of a month. These three assessments were separately tested in increasingly complex
tasks requiring 2-moves, 4- moves and 5- moves. The pre-post data were compared using
the Wilcoxon paired signed ranks test. The yoga group showed a significant reduction in
92
planning time for both 2-moves and 4- moves tasks (53.9 and 59.1 percent respectively),
execution time in both 4-moves and 5- moves tasks (63.7 and 60.3 percent respectively),
and in the number of moves in the 4- moves tasks (20.9 percent). The physical training
group showed no change. Hence yoga training for a month reduced the planning and
execution time in simple (2- moves) as well as complex tasks (4, 5- moves) and facilitated
reaching the target with a smaller number of moves in a complex task (4-moves).
___________, and Shirley Telles. Effects of sirsasana (headstand) practice on autonomic
and respiratory variables. Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, Jan 2003,
47(1).
Abstract: The present study had two aims: (1) To assess heart rate variability (HRV)
along with non-specific autonomic measures (used in earlier studies), before and after
two minutes of the headstand. (2) To compare changes in two categories of subjects, i.e.,
those who practiced the headstand in a traditional way (without any support) and those
who used the support of the wall (a present day adaptation). The subjects were forty
male volunteers (age range 19 to 36 years), with twenty subjects under each category.
The following changes were significant after the practice, compared to values at baseline.
(i) Both categories had an increase in the power of the low frequency component (LF)
and a decrease in the high frequency component (HF) of the HRV spectrum, increased
LF/HF ratio, and decreased heart rate. (ii) Subjects who practiced the headstand with the
support of a wall showed reduced finger plethysmogram amplitude suggesting increased
sympathetic vasomotor tone. (iii) Practicing the headstand without support was
associated with an increase in the skin conductance level, suggestive of increased
sympathetic sudomotor tone. Hence, both categories showed similar changes in the HRV
components though changes in sympathetic vasomotor and sudomotor activity were
different. These changes suggest sympathetic activation, irrespective of the method of
practice.
___________, and S. Telles. Quiet natural surroundings optimize the effect of yoga in
increasing spatial memory scores. Unpublished data, Swami Vivekananda Yoga Research
Foundation.
___________, S. Telles, and H. R. Nagendra. Autonomic and respiratory measures in
children with impaired vision following yoga and physical activity programmes.
Submitted by Swami Vivekananda Yoga Research Foundation to Journal of
Psychosomatic Medicine.
Mason, L. I., C. N. Alexander, F. T. Travis, G. Marsh, D. W. Orme -Johnson, J.
Gackenbach, D. C. Mason, M. Rainforth, and K. G. Walton. Electrophysiological
correlates of higher states of consciousness during sleep in long-term practitioners of the
Transcendental Meditation program. Sleep, Feb 1997, 20(2):102-110. PMID: 9143069.
Abstract: Standard ambulatory night sleep electroencephalograph (EEG) of 11 long-term
practitioners of the Transcendental Meditation (TM) program reporting higher states of
consciousness during sleep (the experimental group) was compared to that of nine short-
93
term practitioners and 11 non-practitioners. EEG tracings during stages 3 and 4 sleep
showed the experimental group to have: 1) theta-alpha activity simultaneously with delta
activity and 2) decreased chin electromyograph (EMG) during deep sleep (p = 0.002)
compared to short-term practitioners. Spectral analysis fast Fourier transform (FFT) data
of the first three cycles showed that: 3) the experimental subjects had significantly greater
theta 2 (6-8 Hz)-alpha 1 (8-10 Hz) relative power during stages 3 and 4 than the
combined control groups [t(30) = 5.5, p = 0.0000008] with no difference in time in delta;
4) there was a graded difference across groups during stages 3 and 4 in theta 2-alpha 1
power, with experimentals having greater power than short-term practitioners, who in
turn had greater power than non-practitioners [t(30) = 5.08, p = 0.00002]; and 5)
experimentals also had increased rapid eye movement (REM) density during REM
periods compared to short-term practitioners (p = 0.04). Previous studies have found
increased theta-alpha EEG activity during reported periods of transcendental
consciousness during the TM technique. In the Vedic tradition, as described by
Maharishi Mahesh Yogi, transcendental consciousness is the first of a sequence of higher
states. The maintenance of transcendental consciousness along with deep sleep is said to
be a distinctive criterion of further, stabilized higher states of consciousness. The findings
of this study are interpreted as physiological support for this model.
Massy, Jean. Teacher education 1: Physiology and yoga. Yoga Health, Jul 1981, 6(3):1722.
Compiled from lectures given by Jean Massy to the British Wheel of Yoga Teacher
Education Tutors Conference, 1980. Based on the British Wheel of Yogas Teacher
Diploma regulations.
___________. Teacher education 2: Nerves and muscles. Yoga Today, Aug 1981,
6(4):22-25.
___________. Teacher education 3: Symmetry & change. Yoga Today, Sep 1981,
6(5):42-46.
___________. Teacher education 4: The vital systems. Yoga Today, Oct 1981, 6(6):3438.
Matsumoto, M., and J. C. Smith. Progressive muscle relaxation, breathing exercises,
and ABC relaxation theory. Journal of Clinical Psychology, Dec 2001, 57(12):15511557. PMID: 11745596.
Abstract: This study compared the psychological effects of Progressive Muscle
Relaxation (PMR) and breathing exercises. Forty-two students were divided randomly
into two groups and taught PMR or breathing exercises. Both groups practiced for five
weeks and were given the Smith Relaxation States Inventory before and after each
session. As hypothesized, PMR practitioners displayed greater increments in relaxation
states (R-States) Physical Relaxation and Disengagement, while breathing practitioners
displayed higher levels of R-State Strength and Awareness. Slight differences emerged at
94
Weeks 1 and 2; major differences emerged at Weeks 4 and 5. A delayed and potentially
reinforcing aftereffect emerged for PMR only after five weeks of trainingincreased
levels of Mental Quiet and Joy. Clinical and theoretical implications are discussed.
Copyright 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Maupin, E. An exploratory study of individual differences in response to a Zen
meditation exercise. Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, 1962.
McClure, C. Cardiac arrest through volition. Calcutta Medicine, 1959, 90:440-441.
McCuaig, L. Salivary electrolytes, protein and pH during Transcendental Meditation.
Experientia, 1974, 30:988-989.
McEvoy, T.M., L. R. Frumkin, and S. W. Harkins. Effects of meditation on brainstem
auditory evoked potentials. International Journal of Neuroscience, 1980, 10:165-170.
McLanahan, Sandra Amrita. Health, Yoga and Anatomy: A Prominent Doctor
Explains the Medical Benefits of Yoga video. Buckingham, Va.: Shakticom. 90 minutes.
In this [video], Dr. McLanahan shows how Yoga postures affect the different organs and
functions of the body. Also covered is the importance of [a] vegetarian diet to maintain
optimum health.
McNamara, Patrick. The motivational origins of religious practices. Zygon, Mar 2002,
37(1)143-160.
Abstract: I hypothesize that people engage in religious practices, in part, because such
practices activate the frontal lobes. Activation of the frontal lobes is both intrinsically
rewarding and necessary for acquisition of many of the behaviors that religions seek to
foster, including self- responsibility, impulse and emotion modulation, empathy, moral
insight, hope, and optimism. Although direct tests of the hypothesis are as yet
nonexistent, there is reasonably strong circumstantial evidence (reviewed herein) for it.
Recent brain- imaging studies indicate greater anterior activation values and increased
blood flow to frontal sites during prayer and meditation. Regular prayer is positively
correlated with better overall mental health. Religiosity is correlated with higher levels of
self- monitoring, empathy, and moral insight and other positive behaviors and negatively
correlated with depression and impulsive and risky behaviors. Independent data show that
self- monitoring, empathy, hope, and moral insight are all selectively associated with
intact frontal function, whereas depression, impulsiveness, and drug and alcohol abuse
are associated selectively with frontal dysfunction. If religious practices do indeed
preferentially activate and stimulate development of the frontal lobes, (a) religious
practices should be considered as possible adjuncts for some patients in treatment for
mental health disorders, and (b) the frontal lobes (rather than the temporal lobes) should
be considered the major brain site that supports the core components of religious
experience.
95
Meastan, J., and M. V. Bhole. Cardiac output in normal, deep and ujjayi pranayamic
breathing: A preliminary study. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1979, 19(4):11-17.
Medical, psychological, and spiritual benefits of long -term Iyengar Yoga practice.
Yoga Rahasya, 2000, 7(1).
The meditation: The source of energy. Bindu, no 4.
Using meditation over a longer period of time can have an enduring beneficial effect on
your brain activity. One who meditates regularly experiences a last relaxing effect in
daily life.
Meditation mapped in monks. BBC News, Sci/Tech, 1 Mar 2002. Article available
online: http://news.bbc.co.uk/hi/english/sci/tech/newsid_1847000/1847442.stm.
As reported by Hindu Press International, 6 Mar 2002: Scientists investigating the effect
of the meditative state on Buddhist monks brains have found that portions of the
organ previously active become quiet, while pacified areas become
stimulated. Dr Andrew Newberg, a radiologist at the University of
Pennsylvania, told BBC world Services Discovery program: I think we are
poised at a wonderful time in our history to be able to explore religion and
spirituality in a way which was never thought possible. Using a brain
imaging technique, Newberg and his team studied a group of Tibetan Buddhist
monks as they meditated for approximately one hour. When they reached a
transcendental high, they were asked to pull a kite string, releasing an
injection of a radioactive tracer. By injecting a tiny amount of radioactive
marker into the bloodstream of a deep meditator, the scientists saw how the
dye moved to active parts of the brain. Later, when the subjects finished
meditating, the regions were imaged and the meditative state compared with
the normal waking state. There was an increase in activity in the front
part of the brain, the area that is activated when anyone focuses attention
on a particular task, Dr Newberg explained. Also, a notable decrease in
activity in the back part of the brain, recognized as the area responsible
for orientation, reinforced the general suggestion that meditation leads to
a lack of spatial awareness.
Meditation on meaningful thoughts causes opposite changes in middle latency
auditory evoked potentials in different subjects. Swami Vivekananda Yoga Research
Foundation, http://www.vkyogas.org.in.
Mehta, Mira. How yoga works; Physiological efficiency. In Mira Mehta, Health through
Yoga: Simple Practice Routines and a Guide to the Ancient Teachings. London:
Thorsons, 2002, pp. 1-45; 95. (Combines Yoga and Ayurveda.)
Mehta, R. The Science of Meditation. Delhi, India: Motilal Banarsidass, 1981.
96
97
The results suggest that lower chemosensitivity to hypercapnia in yoga practitioners may
be due to an adaptation to low arterial pH and high PaCO(2) for long periods.
Mocsny, Bill. Yoga and immunity. Yoga Journal, May/Jun 1976, p. 16.
Mohan, S. Mitti. Variations in the differential air flow through nostrils in man with
change of posture from supine to prone position. Indian Journal of Physiology and
Pharmacology, Jul-Sep 1985, 29(3):165-170.
___________. Reversal of nostril dominance by posture. Journal of Indian Medical
Association, 1991, 89:88-91.
___________. Reflex reversal of nostril dominance by application of pressure to the
axilla by a crutch. Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, 1993, 37(1):147150.
___________. Nostril dominance and autonomic nervous system activity. Journal of
Indian Psychology, 1995, 13(2):28-41.
___________. Nostril dominance and ESP: A further study. Journal of Indian
Psychology, 1996, 14(1&2):9-13. (See original study below by Krishna Rao et al.)
___________. Nostril dominance (svara) and bilateral volar galvanic skin resistance.
International Journal of Yoga Therapy, 1999, no. 9, pp. 33-39. Reprinted from Indian
Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, Jan 1996, 40(1):58-64.
___________, and R. Eccles. Effect of inspiratory and expiratory air flow on congestion
and decongestion in the nasal cycle. Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology,
1989, 33:191-193.
Mohanthy, R., H. R. Nagendra, R. Nagarathna, and S. Telles. Influence of yoga
training on dexterity skills of school children. In H. R. Nagendra, R. Ragarathna, and S.
Telles, Yoga Research & Applications: Proceedings of the 5th International Conference
on Frontiers in Yoga Research and Applications. Bangalore, Vivekananda Kendra Yoga
Research Foundation, 2000, pp. 295-297.
Monjo, Pedro de Vicente. Emotion and heart rhythm as influenced with yoga. YogaMimamsa, 1984, 23(2):1-20.
___________, M. L. Gharote, and J. M. Bhagwat. Effect of kapalabhati and uddiyana
bandha on cardiac rhythms. Yoga-Mimamsa, April 1984, 23(4):41-62.
98
99
eds., Meditation: Classic and Contemporary Perspectives. New York: Aldine Publishing,
1984, pp. 637-644.
___________, G. R. Schacterle, J.V. Esposito, S. D. Chod, M. L. Furst, J.
DiPonziano, and M. Zaydenberg. Stress, meditation and saliva: A study of separate
salivary gland secretions in endodontic patients. Journal of Oral Medicine, Oct-Dec
1983, 38(4):150160.
___________, G. R. Schacterle, M. L. Furst, J. Goldberg, B. Greenspan, D.
Swiecinski, and J. Susek. The effect of stress and meditation on salivary protein and
bacteria: A review and pilot study. Journal of Human Stress, Dec 1982, 8(4):3139.
Moses, Robin. Effect of yoga on flexibility and respiratory measures of vital capacity
and breath holding time. Doctoral dissertation. University of Oregon, 1972.
There were 27 male physical education university students in the experimental and
control groups, and they were measure before and after 10 ten weeks of Hatha-Yoga or
physical education classes for changes in extension- flexion ranges of the left ankle, hip,
hip and trunk, and neck. Statistical analysis of the results showed a significant increase in
flexibility for the hip, hip and trunk, and neck for the Yoga group.
Motajova, J., and K. Vicenik. Effect of Hatha Yoga on heart activity in exercising
women. Activitas Nervosa Superior, 1980, 22(2):125-126.
Motoyama, Hiroshi. Chakra, Nadi of Yoga, and Meridian Points of Acupuncture. Tokyo:
The International Association for Religion and Parapsychology, 1972.
___________. The Ejection of Energy from the Chakra of Yoga and Meridian Points of
Acupuncture. Tokyo: The International Association for Religion and Parapsychology,
1975.
___________. A Psychophysiological Study of Yoga. Tokyo: The International
Association for Religion and Parapsychology, 1976.
___________. Western and Eastern medical studies of pranayama and heart control.
Research for Religion and Parapsychology, 1977, 3(1):1-69.
___________. An electrophysiological study of prana (ki). Research for Religion and
Parapsychology, 1979. Tokyo: The International Association for Religion and
Parapsychology. Also in Spiritual India and Kundalini, 1978, 2(4):25-38.
___________. The ChakrasInto New Dimensions. 1978.
Contents: Introduction, The practice of Tantric Yoga, The chakras and nadis as described
in the Upanishads, The chakras and nadis as described in Shat-Chakra-Nirupana, The
chakras and paranormal abilities described in Gorakshashatakam, The chakras as
100
explained by Rev. Leadbeater, The chakras and nadis as described by Swami Satyananda,
Experience and experiments of the chakras by Motoyama.
___________. An electrophysiological study of prana (ki). Research for Religion and
Parapsychology, Nov 1978, 4(1):1-27.
___________. Yoga and siddhi [psychophysiological correlates]. The International
Association for Religion and Parapsychology Newsletter, May 1980 (Part I), Jul 1980
(Part II), Sep 1980 (Part III), Nov 1980 (Part IV), Jan 1981 (Part V), Mar 1981 (Part VI).
___________. Toward a Superconsciousness: Meditational Theory and Practice. Asian
Humanities Press, 1990.
From the publisher: This book is designed to be a guidebook for the initiates as well a
seasoned practitioners of Yoga meditation, and approaches the subject from the
comprehensive standpoints of physiology, neurophysiology, psychology,
parapsychology, philosophy and religion. People who practice other forms of Eastern of
Western meditation should also benefit from this book, because it clearly demarcates the
phenomena that characterize each stage of meditational practice and hence serves as a
yardstick for their progress in spiritual growth.
___________. A Study of Yoga from Eastern & Western Medical Viewpoints. Encinitas,
Calif.: California Institute for Human Science. URL:
http://www.cihs.edu/whatsnew/motoyama_book.asp.
___________, and Gaetan Chevalier. Similarities and dissimilarities of meridian
functions between genders. Submitted for publication: Subtle Energies and Energy
Medicine, Nov 2001.
Abstract: The Single Square Voltage Pulse (SSVP) method was applied on specific
acupuncture points of about 2500 male and female subjects who live in California. Data
was gathered during a period of six years from 1994 to 2000 at the California Institute for
Human Science (CIHS). The gathered data were examined to investigate possible gender
differences regarding meridian function. From the cur rent curve generated by the
application of the SSVP method, only the BP (Before Polarization; the pre-polarization
current in the dermis) was used in this study. Analysis of the data showed that male
meridian function is more active and has more energy than female meridian function
during cold and hot climates, whereas female subjects have more energy and have more
active meridian function during mild climates. On the contrary, the distribution of the
most excessive meridians and the most deficient meridians shows the same pattern
between males and females, which indicates that males and females are almost the same
in their vital energy system.
Mukhedkar, D. Physiological monitoring of yogics by means of microcomputers. YogaMimamsa, 1988, 27(1&2):50-51.
101
102
103
104
Nicolas, Antonio T. de. Neurobiology and yoga: From the gods of the amygdala to the
gods of the heart. Article available online:
http://www.infinityfoundation.com/ECITneuroyogaframeset.htm.
Nilsson, Robert. Pictures of the brains activity during Yoga Nidra. Bindu, 1997, no. 11,
pp. 8-11. Also in Swami Satyananda, Yoga Nidra (Munger, Bihar, India: Yoga
Publications Trust,1998), pp. 169-178. Available online: http://www.scandyoga.org/english/bindu/27e_pict.html.
Using PET scans of regular practitioners of Kriya Yoga who were practicing Yoga Nidra
from a tape or CD guided by Swami Janakananda, researchers found that subjects were
in a deeply relaxed state the whole time, similar to that of sleep. The theta activity rose
significantly on all the twenty-one electrodes (11%p). The reduction of the alpha activity
(2% NS) was not significant; this shows that this meditative state is altogether different
from that of the sleeping state and comprises conscious awareness . . . the state was
constant and evenly distributed over the entire brain for the forty- five minutes the
relaxation lasted . . . There was a surprisingly significant similarity between the pictures
[of the brains] of the seven yoga teachers who were measured.
Something that may seem paradoxical to those who have no experience of meditation is
perhaps the fact that these clear results, which reveal a high degree of concentration, are
brought about entirely without effort. While you are lying in Yoga Nidra, you are not
trying to force the various things that you experience. On the contrary. The EEG shows
that you are completely relaxed from start to finish. You just listen to the instructions and
experience clearly what happens, as a child listening to a fairy taleactive and
participating, but without effort.
The results confirm the experience of the yogi: Concentration is a spontaneous state,
which comes of its own accord when a method is used that removes whatever is
hindering it.
And as the doctors said, It proves that the 1.5 kg (brain mass) with the unknown content
can control its own activity in an astonishingly precise manner. From a holistic point of
view, it indicates that the soul and body act in unity.
___________. Yoga Nidra in relation to relaxa tion based on suggestion. Bindu, no. 11, p.
7.
Researchers at a university clinic in Cologne, Germany, in the early 1980s found that
Yoga Nidra had a more thorough effect than the relaxations based on suggestion or
hypnosis. During Yoga Nidra the alpha waves covered the whole brain, whereas they
occurred only here and there during the other relaxations. Furthermore, the level of Alpha
waves was constant throughout the entire Yoga Nidra, while they came and went during
the other relaxations. The balance between the EEG in the two brain halves was better in
Yoga Nidra, which means that the two brain halves communicated better. These results
105
were confirmed in 1997 in the research described above under Pictures of the brains
activity during Yoga Nidra.
Nityabodhananda, Swami. Psycho-physiological aspects [of the ajna chakra]. In Swami
Nityabodhananda, Ajna Chakra. Monghyr (Bihar), India: The Bihar School of Yoga,
1973, pp. 8-13.
Contents: Introduction, Scriptural reference, Psycho-physiological aspects
Novak, Petr, and C. Dostalek. Effect of madhya nauli on cortical auditory evoked
potentials (a topographic study). Yoga Mimamsa, 1990, 28(4):22-31.
___________, V. Lepicovsk, C. Dostlek, and P. Hjek. Influence of rhythmic nasal
air insufflation upon EEG mapping : Modelling of kapalabhati. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1990,
29(2):13-26.
Oak, J. P., and M. V. Bhole. Pulse rate during and after bahya kumbhaka with different
conditions of abdominal wall. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1983-84, 22(3&4):71-76.
___________, and M. V. Bhole. Feelings during bahya kumbhaka with different
conditions of the abdominal wall. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1984, 22(3&4):77-86.
___________, B. R. Sharma, and M. V. Bhole. Understanding the technique and effects
of kapalabhati as found in English and other European languages. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1990,
28(4):38-72.
Okuma, T., E. Kogu, K. Ikeda, and H. Sugiyama. The EEG of Zen and Yoga
practitioners. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurohysiology, 1957, 9:51-52.
Orme-Johnson, D. W. Autonomic stability and Transcendental Meditation.
Psychosomatic Medicine, Jul 1973, 35(4):341-349. Also in D. H. Shapiro, and R. N.
Walsh, eds., Meditation: Classic and Contemporary Perspectives. New York: Aldine
Publishing, 1984, pp. 432-439.
Osis, K, and E. Bokert. ESP and changed states of consciousness induced by meditation.
Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research, 1971, 65:17-65.
___________, E. Bokert, and M. L. Carlon. Dimensions of the meditative experience.
Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 1973, 5(1):109-135.
Overbeck, K.-D. Auswirkungen der Technik der Transzendentalen Meditation (TM) auf
die psychische und psychosomatische Befindlichkeit [Effects of the transcendental
meditation technic on the psychological and psychosomatic state]. Psychother Psychosom
Med Psychol, Nov 1982, 32(6):188-192. Collected Papers v3.241. [Article in German.]
106
107
vascular resistance, improved cardiac stroke output, reduction in blood pressure, reduced
hearat rate, and improved cardiovascular endurance.
Pansare, M.S., A. N. Kulkarni, and U. B. Pendse. Effect of yogic training on serum
LDH levels. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 1989, 29:177-178.
Parada, Bijay Kumar, and Shirley Telles. Yoga reduces reaction time. In H. R.
Nagendra, R. Ragarathna, and S. Telles, Yoga Research & Applications: Proceedings of
the 5th International Conference on Frontiers in Yoga Research and Applications.
Bangalore, Vivekananda Kendra Yoga Research Foundation, 2000, pp. 276-278.
Paranjape, S. D., and M. V. Bhole. Resting neuro-muscular activity as influenced by
long term yogic traininga study based on finger ergography. Yoga-Mimamsa, 19(4):1826.
___________, R. J. Rahu, and M. V. Bhole. Shavasana as a contributing factor to
pratyaharasome preliminary findings. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1978, 19(2&3):25-33.
Parulkar, V. G., S. L. Prabhavalker, and J. V. Bhall. Observa tions on some
physiological effects of Transcendental Meditation. Indian Journal of Medical Science,
1974, 28(3):156-158.
Paske, Tadeusz. Relaxation-concentration investigations and training according to the
Yoga model in Poland. In Swami Digambarji, ed., Collected Papers on Yoga. Lonavla,
India: Kaivalyadhama, 1975, p. 37-40.
Patel, C., and M. Carruthers. Coronary risk factor reduction through biofeedback-aided
relaxation and meditation. J R Coll Gen Proceedings, 1977, 27:401-405.
Pelletier, Kenneth R. The effects of the Transcendental Meditation program on perceptual
style: Increased field independence. Paper presented at a meeting of the Western
Psychological Association, San Francisco, California, April 1974 (University of California
School of Medicine, San Francisco, California). In David W. Orme-Johnson, and John T.
Farrow, eds., Scientific Research on the Transcendental Meditation Program: Collected
Papers, Volume I. Maharishi European Research University Press, 1976, pp. 337-345.
___________. Influence of Transcendental Meditation upon autokinetic perception.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1974, 39:1031-1034. Also in D. H. Shapiro, and R. N.
Walsh, eds., Meditation: Classic and Contemporary Perspectives. New York: Aldine
Publishing, 1984, pp. 223-226.
Peng, C. K., J. E. Mietus, Y. Liu, G. Khalsa, P. S. Douglas, H. Benson, and A. L.
Goldberger. Exaggerated heart rate oscillations during two meditation techniques.
International Journal of Cardiology, 31 Jul 1999, 70(2):101-107. PMID: 10454297. (See
also Friedman et al. above, a response to Peng et al.)
108
Abstract: We report extremely prominent heart rate oscillations associated with slow
breathing during specific traditional forms of Chinese Chi and Kundalini Yoga
meditation techniques in healthy young adults. We applied both spectral analysis and a
novel analytic technique based on the Hilbert transform to quantify these heart rate
dynamics. The amplitude of these oscillations during meditation was significantly greater
than in the pre- meditation control state and also in three non- meditation control groups: i)
elite athletes during sleep, ii) healthy young adults during metronomic breathing, and iii)
healthy young adults during spontaneous nocturnal breathing. This finding, along with
the marked variability of the beat-to-beat heart rate dynamics during such profound
meditative states, challenges the notion of meditation as only an autonomically quiescent
state.
Pero, G., and G. Spoto. Study on the anatomy of yoga asana and their neurological
effect: A comparative study. Abstract, 1 st World Congres of Yoga and Ayurveda, San
Marino, Italy, June 8-11, 1985; Yoga-Mimamsa, 1985, 24(3):17-18.
Persinger, M. A. Striking EEG profiles from single episodes of glossola lia and
Transcendental Meditation. Perceptual Motor Skills, Feb 1984, 58(1):127-133. PMID:
6371700.
Abstract: Transient, focal, epileptic- like electrical changes in the temporal lobe, without
convulsions, have been hypothesized to be primary correlates of religious experiences.
Given these properties, direct measurement of these phenomena within the laboratory
should be rare. However, two illustrated instances have been recorded. The first case
involved the occurrence of a delta-wave-dominant electrical seizure for about 10 sec.
from the temporal lobe only of a Transcendental Meditation teacher during a peak
experience within a routine TM episode. The second case involved the occurrence of
spikes within the temporal lobe only during protracted intermittent episodes of glossolalia
by a member of a pentecostal sect . Neither subject had any psychiatric history. These
observations are commensurate with the hypothesis that religious experiences are natural
correlates of temporal lobe transients that can be detected by routine EEG measures.
Physiological effects of sirsasana. Submitted by Swami Vivekananda Yoga Research
Foundation to Journal of Applied Physiology.
Piggins, D., and D. Morgan. Note upon steady visual fixation and repeated auditory
stimulation in meditation and the laboratory. Perceptual Motor Skills, Apr 1977,
44(2):357-368.
Porkodi, K., S. Subramaniam, and T. S. Kanaka. Effects of yoga and meditation in
health and disease. Proceedings First National Conference on Yoga, Science and Society,
Banaras Hindu University, 1979, pp. 104-106.
Prabhu, R., S. Telles, R. Nagarathna, and H. R. Nagendra. Comparison of HRV in
DRT vs. shavasana. Swami Vivekananda Yoga Research Foundation.
109
Pratap, V. Steadiness in normals before and after yogic practices: An exploratory study.
Yoga-Mimamsa, 1968, 11:1-13.
___________. Difference in magnitude of response in yogic and non-yogic conditions.
Yoga-Mimamsa, 1969, 12(2):9-18.
___________. Diurnal pattern of nostril breathingan exploratory study. YogaMimamsa, Oct 1971 and Jan 1972, 14(3&4):1-18.
___________, W. H. Berrettini, and C. Smith. Arterial blood gases in pranayama practice.
Perceptual Motor Skills, Feb1978, 46(1):171-174. PMID: 25412.
Abstract: Pranayama is a Yogic breathing practice which is known experientially to
produce a profound calming effect on the mind. In an experiment designed to determine
whether the mental effects of this practice were accompanied by changes in the arterial
blood gases, arterial blood was drawn from 10 trained individuals prior to and
immediately after Pranayama practice. No significance changes in arterial blood gases
were noted after Pranayama. A neural mechanism for the mental effects of this practice is
proposed.
Prestie, David. Neurochemistry and ananda. In H. R. Nagendra, R. Ragarathna, and S.
Telles, Yoga Research & Applications: Proceedings of the 5th International Conference
on Frontiers in Yoga Research and Applications. Bangalore, Vivekananda Kendra Yoga
Research Foundation, 2000, pp. 61-66.
Puente, A. E. Psychophysiological investigations on transcendental meditation.
Biofeedback Self Regulation, Sep 1981, 6(3):327-342.
Raghuraj, P., R. Nagarathna, H. R. Nagendra, and S. Telles. Pranayama practice
increases grip strength without lateralized effects. Indian Journal of Physiology and
Pharmacology, 1997, 41(2):129-133. PMID: 9142556.
Abstract: The present study was conducted to determine whether breathing through a
particular nostril has a lateralized effect on hand grip strength. 130 right hand dominant,
school children between 11 and 18 yrs of age were randomly assigned to 5 groups. Each
group had a specific yoga practice in addition to the regular program for a 10 day yoga
camp. The practices were: (1) right-, (2) left-, (3) alternate- nostril breathing (4), breath
awareness and (5) practice of mudras. Hand grip strength of both hands was assessed
initially and at the end of 10 days for all 5 groups. The right-, left- and alternate- nostril
breathing groups had a significant increase in grip strength of both hands, ranging from
4.1% to 6.5%, at the end of the camp though without any lateralization effect. The breath
awareness and mudra groups showed no change. Hence the present results suggest that
yoga breathing through a particular nostril, or through alternate nostrils increases hand
grip strength of both hands without lateralization.
110
111
112
113
114
Abstract: Present day neurophysiology stops with attributing thinking processes as the
highest level of function of the brain. It has been common knowledge to oriental thinkers
for many centur ies, that there are many further states of the human mind, culminating in
the state of thoughtless awareness; the fourth state of consciousness. This state must have
a physiological basis. The complicated structure of the brain, the extravagant abundance
of neural and glial elements in the brain, the infinite possibilities of synaptic junctions
and synaptic transmission, and the multitude of neurotransmitters and neuromodulators;
all these point to the definite possibility of a much greater level of performance and
achievement for the human brain than has been apparent so far. Not only the theories but
also the experience of Eastern seers have shown that the brain can transcend the
boundaries of logic and reason, and experience states of awareness, commonly
unrecognized. In the past few decades, knowledge about the functioning of the human
brain has been growing exponentially and scientists of diverse disciplines are
concentrating on unraveling its mysteries. It is necessary for scientists to investigate this
state with all available tools and find the neurophysiological basis of this state.
___________. Yoga, mind and the brain. In Vivekananda Kendra Prakashan, Yoga: The
Science of Holistic Living. Madras, India: Vivekananda Kendra Prakashan, 1996, pp.
181-187.
___________. Yoga and the brain. The Hindu.
Raman, Krishna, M.D. Bio-mechanics of Yoga. Article available online:
http://medicineau.net.au/columns/yoga/biomech.htm.
___________. Yogasanas: A physicians perspective. Yoga Rahasya, 2(2): 30-36.
Ramaswami, Srivatsa. Yogic breathing exercises and their health benefits. In Srivatsa
Ramaswami, Yoga for the Three Stages of Life: Developing Your Practice as an Art
Form, a Physical Therapy, and a Guiding Philosophy. Rochester, Vt.: Inner Traditions
International, 2000, pp. 189-211.
Ramaswami, Sundar, and Anees A. Sheikh. Psychophysiology of the meditative state.
In Anees A. Sheikh, and Katharina S. Sheikh, eds., Eastern & Western Approaches to
Healing: Ancient wisdom & Modern Knowledge. New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1989,
pp. 449-454.
Rangan, S. C. B. An experimental study to investigate the effect of sarvangasana and
halasana. M.P.E. thesis, Lakshmibai College of Physical Education, Gwalior, India, 1969.
Rani, N. J., and P. V. K. Rao. Body awareness and yoga training. Perceptual Motor
Skills, 1994, 79:11031106.
Rao, H. V. G., N. Krishnaswamy, R. L. Narasimhaiya, J. Hoenig, and M. V.
Govindaswamy. Some experiments on a yogi in controlled states. Journal of the AllIndia Institute of Mental Health, 1958, 1:99-106.
115
Ray, Tushar K. Biochemistry of stretching yoga postures. Paper presented at the 10th
International Conference on Yoga for Positive Health, University of South Florida,
Tampa, 15-17 Dec 2000. Email: [email protected]
Ray, U. S., S. Mukhopadhyaya, S. S. Purkayastha, V. Asnani, O. S. Tomer, R.
Prasha d, L. Thakur, and W. Selvamurthy. Effect of yogic exercises on physical and
mental health of young fellowship course trainees. Indian Journal of Physiology and
Pharmacology, Jan 2001, 45(1):37-53.
Abstract: A study was undertaken to observe any beneficial effect of yogic practices
during training period on the young trainees. 54 trainees of 20-25 years age group were
divided randomly in two groups i.e. yoga and control group. Yoga group (23 males and 5
females) was administered yogic practices for the first five months of the course while
control group (21 males and 5 females) did not perform yogic exercises during this
period. From the 6th to 10th month of training both the groups performed the yogic
practices. Physiological parameters like heart rate, blood pressure, oral temperature, skin
temperature in resting condition, responses to maximal and submaximal exercise, body
flexibility were recorded. Psychological parameters like personality, learning, arithmetic
and psychomotor ability, mental well being were also recorded. Various parameters were
taken before and during the 5th and 10th month of training period. Initially there was
relatively higher sympathetic activity in both the groups due to the new work/training
environment but gradually it subsided. Later on at the 5th and 10th month, yoga group
had relatively lower sympathetic activity than the control group. There was improvement
in performance at submaximal level of exercise and in anaerobic threshold in the yoga
group. Shoulder, hip, trunk and neck flexibility improved in the yoga group. There was
improvement in various psychological parameters like reduction in anxiety and
depression and a better mental function after yogic practices.
___________, K. S. Hegde, and W. Selvamurthy. Effects of yo gic asanas and physical
exercise on body flexibility in middle aged men. The Yoga Review, Summer & Autumn
1983, 3(2&3):75-79.
Abstract: Changes in body flexibility due to the regular practice of yogic asanas and
physical exercises were studied on 40 healt hy physically active middle aged (40-48
years) men. Subjects were randmly divided into two equal groups. In group A, selected
yoga asanas were administered daily for 1 hour for 6 months, while group B had hysical
exercise training programme for the same duration. The flexibility measurements were
made using Leighton Flexometer at the level of neck, shulder, trunk and hip, prior to and
after the six month of the course in both the groups. There was imporovement in neck
rotation in both the groups, while the shoulder flexion-extension and hip flexionextension values increased only in the yoga group after training. There was no significant
change in trunk flexion-extension in either group.
___________. Improvement in muscular efficiency as related to a standard yogic exercise
in middle aged men. Indian Journal of Medical Research, 1986, 83:343ff.
117
118
119
Contents: Eastern Yoga and Western medicine; Nervous systems of the body; The brain;
The spine and its appendages; The nerves; The autonomic nervous system; The skeletal
muscles; The bones; Connective and supporting tissues; The skin; The heart and the
vascular system; Respiration; The immune system; External secretions; The hormones;
The eyes and vision; Sound, hearing, and balance; The gastrointestinal organs and
digestion; Sexual function, gynecology, and pregnancy; The emotions; Time and body
rhythms; Aging and longevity; Yogasana illustrations; Injuries incurred during improper
yogasana practice; Body symmetry; Balancing; Gravity; Glossary; References; Index
Each section contains descriptive text and illustrations, along with yogasanas effect on
the system in question.
___________. Neuromuscular transfo rmation as driven by yogsana practice. Yoga
Rahasya, 2003, 10(1):86-90. Abstracted from the authors book, A Physiological
Handbook for Teachers of Yogasana.
Rodenbeck, Joachim. Neti: Nose cleansing [see section entitled What does physiology
say?]. Available online: http://www.scand-yoga.org/english/shop/uk_neti.html.
Roggla, G., S. Kapiotis, and H. Roggla. Yoga and chemoreflex sensitivity. Lancet, 10
Mar 2001, 357(9258):807.
Roldan, E., and C. Dostalek. EEG patterns suggestive of shifted levels of excitation
effected by hathayogic exercises. Act. Nerv. Super. (Praha), 1985, 27:81-88.
___________, J. Los, and C. Dostalek. The effects of paced breathing at frequencies
between 12 and 288 cpm on heart rate in healthy subjects. Activ Nerv Sup, 1983, 25:195.
Romanowski, W., ed. Teoria i Metodyka Cwiczen Relaksowo-Koncentrujacych [Theory
and Praxis of Relaxation and Concentration]. Warsaw, 1975. [In Polish.]
Romanoski, W., et al. Studies on physiological and psychological evaluation of yoga
system of exercises. Wychowanie Fyziczne i Sport, 1969, 13(3), pp. 139-70. [In Polish.]
Romanowski, W., and T. Pasek. Current psycho-physiological aspects of the activity of
physical educators. [Title of journal unknown], 1970, pp. 48-51. [Original in Polish.]
Roney-Dougal, S. M. On a possible psychophysiology of the yogic chakra system.
Journal of Indian Psychology, Jul 1999, 17(2). Reprinted in Yoga (Sivananda Math), May
2000, 11(3):35-46 (Part 1), Jul 2000, 11(4):39-47 (Part 2), Sep 2000, 11(5):40-48 (Part
3). Articles available online: (Part 1):
http://www.yogamag.net/archives/2000/3may00/chakra1.shtml; (Part 2):
http://www.yogamag.net/archives/2000/4july00/chakra2.shtml; (Part 3):
http://www.yogamag.net/archives/2000/5sep00/chakras3.shtml.
120
Ross, Robyn. Yoga and neurological illness. In Michael Weintraub, ed., Alternative
Medicine in Neurological Illness. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders, 2000.
Routt, Thomas J. Low normal heart and respiration rates in individuals practicing the
Transcendental Meditation technique. In David W. Orme-Johnson, and John T. Farrow,
eds., Scientific Research on the Transcendental Meditation Program: Collected Papers,
Vol. I. Germany: Maharishi European Research University Press, 1976, pp. 256-260.
Rozmarynowaski, Mat. Experiments with vajroli. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1979, 19(4):36-45.
___________. Experiments with danda dhauti following vastra dhauti A personal
experience. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1991, 29(4):37-40.
___________. Experiment with kaya-kalpa. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1991, 29(4):41-52.
the three sessions. Conclusion: The results indicate that power yoga does not consistently
provide heart rate intensities that meet ACSM recommendations fo r aerobic training in
beginning power yoga participants. In addition the use of perceived exertion during
power yoga does not appear to be an accurate correlation with heart rate.
Sahai, M., and D. D. Malhotra. Yoga for health. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1969, 11(3):31-38.
Sahay, B. K., B. Sadasivudu, R. Yogi, C. Bhaskaracharyulu, P. S. Raju, S. Madhvai,
M. V. Reddy, N. Annapurna, and K. J. R. Murthy. Biochemical parameters in normal
volunteers before and after yogic practices. Indian Journal of Medical Research, 1982,
76:144-148.
Sahu, R. J., and M. V. Bhole. Effect of om recitation and physical activity on psychomotor performance. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1981, 20(1&2):33-37.
___________. Effect of 3 weeks yogic training programme on psycho- motor
performance. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1983, 22(1&2):59-62.
___________. Effect of two types of Pranava (omkar) recitation on psycho-motor
performance. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1983, 22(3&4):23-29.
___________. Experiences during listening to high and low pitched omkars. YogaMimamsa, 1983, 22(3&4):98-103.
___________. Feelings and sensations during recitation of high and low pitched omkars.
Yoga-Mimamsa, 1983, 22(3&4):104-109.
___________. Effect of short term yogic training programme on dexterity: A pilot study.
Yoga-Mimamsa, 1984, 23(2):21-27.
___________. Psycho- motor performance as influenced by techniques similar to
pranadharana and nadanusandhana in shavansana. a preliminary study. Yoga-Mimamsa,
1983, 22(3&4):36-41.
___________. Effect of yogic training programme on some aspects of psycho- motor
performance. To be published.
___________, and M. L. Gharote. Effect of sort term yogic training programme on
dexteritya pilot study. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1984, 23(2):21-27.
___________, and M. L. Gharote. Effect of short term yogic practices on the perception
of the third dimensiona pilot study. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1985, 24(2):11-20.
Sakthignanavel, D. Effect of pranayama with aerobic exercise on aerobic fitness. YogaMimamsa, 1998, 32(4):1-12.
122
123
remarkably better after six months of practice, some of the symptoms having completely
disappeared (head protrusion, asymmetry of the shoulders, mamillas and hips, shortening
of the pectoralis and back extensors), 9 children still had slight to medium relaxation of
the frontal abdominal wall, 8 children still had bent shoulders, and 1 child still had
shortened calf and thigh extensors. The adults were in a weak or very weak
psychophysical condition, they tired easily, they complained of sleep disturbances,
fluctuation of emotional state and irritability. After 3 months of practice, the vital
capacity of 8 of the adults tested (53.3%) had increased by 435 ml. The time duration of
apnoea had lengthened for all of the practicing adults, but with a truly large variation
among them (a median of 14%). The deep waist-bend length of all the practicing adults
had lengthened by an average of 9.5 cm, and the average length increase for the 3- minute
running test was 42 m. All those who practiced had experienced an alleviation of psychic
difficulties.
Sawada, Y., and A. Steptoe. The effects of brief meditation training on cardiovascual
stress responses. Journal of Psychophysiology, 1988, 24:249-257.
Schecter, H. The Transcendental Meditation program in the classroom: A psychological
evaluation. In David W. Orme-Johnson, and John T. Farrow, eds., Scientific Research on
the Transcendental Meditation Program: Collected Papers, Vol. I. Germany: Maharishi
European Research University Press, 1976, pp. 403-409.
Schell, F. J., B. Allolio, and O. W. Schonecke. Physiological and psychological effects
of Hatha-Yoga exercise in healthy women. International Journal of Psychosomatics,
1994, 41(1-4):46-52.
Abstract: Hatha-Yoga has become increasingly popular in western countries as a method
for coping with stress. However, little is known about the physiological and
psychological effects of yoga practice. We measured heart rate, blood pressure, the
hormones cortisol, prolactin and growth hormone and certain psychological parameters in
a yoga practicing group and a control group of young female volunteers reading in a
comfortable position during the experimental period. There were no substantial
differenfes between the groups concerning endocrine parameters and blood pressure. The
course of heart rate was significantly different; the yoga group had a decrease during the
yog practice. Significant differences between both groups were found in psychological
parameters. In the personality inventory the yoga group showed markedly higher scores
in life satisfaction and lower scores in excitability, aggressiveness, openness,
emotionality and somatic complaints. Significant differences could also be observed
concerning coping with stress and the mood at the end of the experiment. The yoga group
had significant[ly] higher scores in high spirits and extravertedness.
Schmidt, T., A. Wijga, A. Von Zur Muhlen, G. Brabant, and T. O. Wagner. Changes
in cardiovascula r risk factors and hormones during a comprehensive residential three
month kriya yoga training and vegetarian nutrition. Acta Physiol Scand Suppl, 1997,
640:158-162.
125
The following review of this study appears in an article by Ralph La Forge entitled
Spotlight on Yoga in the May 2001 issue of IDEA Health and Fitness Source
(http://www.findarticles.com/cf_0/m0BTW/5_19/74886169/p1/article.jhtml?term=yoga):
Study: Researchers at Hannover Medical University in Hannover, Germany, investigated
some of the effects of a comprehensive, residential three- month kriya yoga training
program on cardiovascular risk factors in 106 healthy adults (58 men and 48 women ages
18-64 years, mean age = 29.6 years). Kriya yoga is an advanced tantric meditation
process combining physical and mental techniques; sessions last four hours or longer.
The yoga program, which took place at the Scandinavian Yoga and Meditation School in
Ha, Sweden, consisted of three parts: During the first month, subjects practiced
traditional yoga (daily hatha yoga exercises, breathing techniques, deep relaxation and
meditation). In the second month, they learned kriya yoga. In the final month, they
practiced kriya yoga daily, along with other yoga techniques and advanced meditations.
Practical work in the schools kitchen, garden, fields and woods constituted an essential
part of the training. Throughout the program, participants adhered to a low- fat (23% fat)
vegetarian diet with no alcohol or caffeine. Pre- and postlaboratory assessments included
blood lipids and lipoproteins, complete blood counts, fibrinogen (a plasma protein that
contributes to the formation of blood clots), urinary and serum hormone levels, stress
reactivity and blood pressure. The subjects were compared to control groups living their
normal lives in Hannover; subjects and controls were matched for age, gender and
respective initial risk factor levels.
The subjects lost an average of 5.7 kilograms and reduced their body mass index (BMI)
by 1.88. Men experienced significant reductions in total serum cholesterol (from 181 to
166 milligrams per deciliter [mg/dl]), LDL cholesterol (from 117 to 102 mg/dl) and LDLto-HDL ratio (from 2.7 to 2.2) (p [less than] 0.001 in each category). Reductions in these
categories were not significant fo r women, whose initial levels were lower than the
mens. HDL cholesterol did not change significantly in men or women. In the 15
participants whose HDL was initially [less than] 35 mg/dl, however, HDL did increase
significantly (from 30 to 40 mg/dl, p [less than] 0.001).
Compared to the matched controls, male and female yoga participants had, after three
months, significantly reduced their fibrinogen levels (decreasing the risk of blood clots)
(p = 0.04). Blood pressure and heart rate dropped significantly during the course, with
blood pressure improvements being more pronounced (decreasing from 150/82 to123/69)
in those with initial systolic blood pressure levels [greater than or equal to] 140
millimeters of mercury (both p [lesser than] 0.000 1). Serum testosterone levels and
urinary excretions of adrenaline, noradrenaline, dopamine and aldosterone all fell
significantly in the study group compared to the control group. Schmidt and his fellow
researchers concluded that commitment to intensive yoga therapy can significantly
reduce cardiovascular risk factors, especially in individuals with increased risk factor
levels, such as elevated BMI, cholesterol, blood pressure and fibrinogen.
126
Comments: The results of this study were not entirely unexpected considering the
intensive and residential nature of the intervention. Nonetheless, reductions in total
cholesterol, LDL cholesterol and blood pressure were substantial. Such clinical results
rival those attained by many drugs prescribed for lowering lipid and blood pressure
levels. The challenge for fitness and health promotion professionals as lifestyle-change
agents is knowing how to implement effective strategies to foster lifelong behavior
change. Finally, it is noteworthy that, as was the case in this program, yoga therapy is
often far more comprehensive than mere exercise poses.
Schneider, R. H., S. I. Nidich, J. W. Salerno, H. M. Sharma, C. E. Robinson,
R. J. Nidich, and C. N. Alexander. Lower lipid peroxide levels in practitioners of
the Transcendental Meditation program. Psychosomatic Medicine, Jan-Feb 1998,
60(1):38-41. Author email: [email protected]. PMID: 9492237.
From a review by Bella Neparstek, Health Journeys: Eighteen subjects were long-term
practitioners of the TM program (average 16.5 years). Twenty-three controls were not
practicing a formal stress management technique. Venous blood samples were analyzed
for lipid peroxides by the TBARS assay. A dietary questionnaire was also used to assess
fat intake, red meat consumption, antioxidant vitamin supplementation, and smoking.
Differences between groups and subgroups were analyzed by T-test, and correlations.
The study found significantly lower serum levels of lipid peroxides in the TM
practitioners, as compared with the controls (-15%, p = .026). No significant differences
were found between groups on smoking, fat intake, or vitamin supplementation. TM
practitioners also had lower red meat consumption but matched subgroup analysis. Partial
correlations did not confirm a relationship between red meat intake and lipid peroxide
levels.
These preliminary findings suggest that lower serum lipid peroxide levels are associated
with a reduction of psychosocial stress from using the Transcendental Meditation
technique. [Oxidative stress and free radical activity are increased by psychosocial stress,
and are known to contribute to the pathophysiology of atherosclerosis and other chronic
diseases associated with aging.]
Prospective controlled trials are needed to confirm that this effect is because of TM
practice rather than other lifestyle factors, such as diet.
Schulte, H. J., and V. V. Abhyanker. Yogic breathing and psychologic states. Arizona
Medicine, Sep 1979, 36(9):681-683.
Overview: One of the authors (VVA) recently traveled to India and had the opportunity
to interview several well-known yogic masters regarding their use of yogic breating for
selected emotional disorders. The goal of this paper is to discuss the psychologic changes
associated with respiration controlled by the Yogic method. Two case examples will be
presented with discussion.
127
Scientific validation of yoga: Asanas. Akhand Jyoti: The Light Divine, Nov/Dec 2003.
Article available online: http://www.akhand-jyoti.org/novdec03/article7.html.
Sedlak, W. Joga v swietle wsplcesnej biofiziky [Yoga in the light of modern
biophysics]. Zeszyty Naukowe, KUL, 1972, 15(2):43-52. [In Polish.]
Selvamurthy, W. Yoga for everyone: A physiologists view. Journal of Oriental
Research, 1996, 63:7-32.
___________, H. S. Nayar, N. T. Joseph, and S. Joseph. Physiological effects of yogic
practices. NIMHANS (National Institute of Mental Health and Neuro Sciences of India)
Journal, Jan1983, 1(1):71-79.
Abstract: A comprehensive study was conducted on 30 healthy men [soldiers] (20-30
years of age) to evaluate the effects of six months of regular yogic practice on autonomic
balance, thermoregulatory efficiency, orthostatic tolerance, energy metabolism and
biochemical profile. The subjects were randomly divided into two groups (A & B) of 15
each. Group-A served as control, while in group-B yogic training was administered daily
in the morning hours for one hour under the supervision of qualified Yoga instructor from
Vishwaytan Yogashram for six months. Various physiological tests and biochemical
estimations were done before, and after, every month of yogic training, in both the
groups. Yogic practice for six months resulted in a trend of shift in the autonomic
equilibrium towards relative parasympathodominance, improvement in thermoregulatory
efficiency and orthostatic tolerance. It has also brought about improvement in physical
performance by minimizing the energy expenditure during submaxmal exercise. The
changes in the biochemical profile indicated a relative hypometabolic state after six
months of yogic practice. Physiological significance of these finds is discussed in this
paper.
___________, and U. S. Ray. Yoga and physiology. In H. R. Nagendra, R. Ragarathna,
and S. Telles, Yoga Research & Applications: Proceedings of the 5th International
Conference on Frontiers in Yoga Research and Applications. Bangalore, Vivekananda
Kendra Yoga Research Foundation, 2000, pp. 257-259.
___________, U. S. Ray, K. S. Hedge, and R. P. Sharma. Physiological responses to
cold (100C) in men after six months practice of yoga exercises. International Journal of
Biometeorology, 1983, 32:188-193.
Shachoy, Cator. The chakra system and human physiology: The healing potential of
integrating ancient yogic wisdom and modern medical understanding. Common Ground,
Fall 2001, pp. 14, 126-128. Author contact: 415-235-9380
Shah, C. S. Neurophysiology of meditation. Article available online:
http://www.geocities.com/neovedanta/a28.html.
128
Shankaranand, B. Yoga, brain and consciousness. The Yoga Review, Spring 1982,
2(1):1-5.
Shannahoff-Khalsa, D., M. R. Boyle, and M. E. Buebel. The effects of unilateral
forced nostril breathing on cognition. International Journal of Neuroscience, 1991,
57:239-249.
Abstract: Ultradian rhythms of alternating cerebral dominance have been demonstrated in
humans and other mammals during waking and sleep. Human studies have used the
methods of psychological testing and electroencephalography (EEG) as measurements to
identify the phase of this \natural endogenous rhythm. The periodicity of this rhythm
approximates 1.5 3 hours in awake humans. This cerebral rhythm is tightly coupled to
another ultradian rhythm known as the nasal cycle, which is regulated by the autonomic
nervous system, and is exhibited by greater airflow in one nostril, later switching to the
other side. This paper correlates uninostril airflow with varying ratios of verbal/spatial
performance in 23 right-handed males. Relatively greater cognitive ability in one
hemisphere corresponds to unilateral forced nostril breathing in the contralateral nostril.
Cognitive performance ratios can be influenced by forcibly altering the breathing pattern.
___________, and B. Kennedy. The effects of unilateral forced nostril breathing on the
heart. International Journal of Neuroscience, Nov 1993, 73(1-2):47-60. PMID: 8132418.
Abstract: Three experiments are described that employ impedance cardiography to
monitor the effects of unilateral forced nostril breathing (UFNB) on the heart.
Experiment 1 includes 7 subjects (4 males, 3 females) with a respiratory rate of 6
breaths/min (BPM). Experiment 2 includes 16 trials using one subject to examine the
intraindividual variability, at 6 BPM. Experiment 3 includes 10 trials with the same
subject in experiment 2, but with a respiratory rate of 2-3 breaths/s. This rapid rate of
respiration is a yogic breathing technique called "breath of fire" or "kapalabhatti" and
employs a very shallow but rapid breath in which the abdominal region acts like a
bellows. All 3 experiments demonstrated that right UFNB increases heart rate (HR)
compared to left. Experiment 1 gave 7 negative slopes, or lowering in HR with left nostril
breathing and 7 positive slopes, or increases in HR with right nostril breathing, p = .001.
The second and third experiments showed differences in HR means in which right UFNB
increases HR more than left, p = .013, p = .001, respectively. In experiment 2 stroke
volume was higher with left UFNB, p = .045, compensating for lower HR. Left UFNB
increased end diastolic volume as measured in both experiments 1 and 2, p = .006, p =
.001, respectively. These results demonstrate a unique unilateral effect on sympathetic
stimulation of the heart that may have therapeutic value.
Shapiro, Deane H., Jr. Meditation: Self-Regulation and Altered States of Consciousness,
Hawthorne, N.Y.: Aldine Publishing, 1980.
___________. Classic perspectives of meditation: Toward an empirical understanding of
meditation as an altered state of consciousness. In D. H. Shapiro, and R. N. Walsh, eds.,
129
130
Singh, B. S. Ventilatory response to CO2. II. Studies in neurotic psychiatric patients and
practitioners of Transcendental Meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1984, 46:347-362.
Singh, Baldev, and G. S. Chhina. Some reflections of ancient Indian physiology: The
science of medicine and physiological concepts in ancient and medieval India. Published
by Dr. S. K. Manchanda, Secretary General XXVI International Congress of
Physiological Sciences, Department of Physiology, All-India Institute of Medical
Sciences, New Delhi, 1974.
Singh, R. H. Comparative physiological and biochemical studies on different types of
yogic practices. Proceedings 61 st Indian Science Congress, part III, abstract, pp. 79-80.
___________. Studies on neurophysiological, endocrine and metabolic response to the
practice of yoga. Proceedings 60th Session Indian Science Congress, Physiology Section,
Chandigarh, 1973.
___________, and K. N. Udupa. Psychobiological studies on some hatha- yogic
practices. Quarterly Journal of Surgical Sciences, 1977, 13(3-4):290-293.
___________, R. M. Shettiwar, and K. N. Udupa. Physiological and therapeutic studies
on yoga. The Yoga Review, 1982, 2(4):185-209.
___________, et al. A comprehensive study on physiological response to the practice of
yoga. Proceedings 59th Session Indian Science Congress, Physiology Section, Calcutta,
1972.
Sinha, B., U. S. Ray, A. Pathak, and W. Selvamurthy. Energy cost and
cardiorespiratory changes during the practice of Surya Namaskar. Indian Journal of
Physiology and Pharmacology, Apr 2004, 48(2):184-190. PMID: 15521557.
Summary: The present study was undertaken to observe the energy cost and different
cardiorespiratory changes during the practice of srya-namaskara. Twenty-one male
volunteers from the Indian Army practiced selected yogic exercises six days a week for
three months. The practice schedule consisted of Hatha-Yoga sanas (28 min),
prnyma (10.5 min), and meditation (5 min). Subjects first practiced kapla-bhti
prnyma for 2 min, then yoga-mudr for 2 min; after that they rested until oxygen
consumption and heart rate (HR) came to resting value. Subjects subsequently performed
SN for 3 min 40 sec on average. After three months of training, subjects performed the
entire yogic practice schedule in the laboratory, and measurements were taken. Their
pulmonary ventilation, carbon dioxide output, oxygen cons umption, HR, and other
cardiorespiratory parameters were measured during the actual practice of SN. Oxygen
consumption was highest in the eighth posture (1.22+/-0.073 1 min(-1)) and lowest in the
first posture (0.35+/-0.02 1 min(-1)). Total energy cost throughout the practice of SN was
13.91 kcal and at an average of 3.79 kcal/min. During practice, the highest HR was
101+/-13.5 bpm. As an aerobic exercise SN seems to be ideal, as it involves both static
stretching and the slow dynamic component of exercise with optimal stress on the
cardiorespiratory system.
131
.
Sivasankaran, Satish. The effect of a six- week yoga training and meditation program on
endothelial function. American Heart Association Scientific Sessions 2004, New Orleans,
7-10 Nov, 2004. With additional input by Gerald F. Fletcher, M.D., Mayo Clinic
Jacksonville, Florida.
Reported by Peggy Peck, WedMD Medical News, in article Yoga Gets Heart Health:
Yoga and Meditation 3 Times a Week Improves Heart Disease Risk, 8 Nov 2004.
Article available online: http://my.webmd.com/content/article/96/103877.htm.
Stretching may do more than make you limber, according to new research from Yale
University School of Medicine. Findings show that people who practice yoga and
meditation at least three times a week may reduce their blood pressure, pulse andmost
importantlytheir risk of heart disease.
Moreover, yoga improves heart health in both healthy individuals and those with
diagnosed heart disease, says Satish Sivasankaran, M.D., who conducted the study while
training at Yale. He says that volunteers taking a six-week yoga-meditation program
improved blood vessel function by 17%. Blood vessel function, also called endothelial
function, is the way vessels contract and expand to aid blood flow and is a measur e of
healthy vessel function. However, study participants who had heart disease had close to a
70% improvement in endothelial function.
Endothelial function is an important indicator of atherosclerosis because as the disease
and plaque build-up progresses, the blood vessels become less supple and less able to
constrict and expand.
Stress is known to increase the risk of coronary events. Both anxiety and type A
behavior have been associated with coronary diseases, Sivasankaran, who is now a
cardiology fellow at the Lahey Clinic in Burlington, Mass., tells WebMD. Yoga and
meditation, on the other hand, are often recommended as a way to relieve stress.
The study, which was presented during the opening day of the American Heart
Association's 2004 Scientific Sessions here, is the first to look at the way blood vessels
respond to stress.
The endothelial function improved in the total cohort of patients and was most dramatic
in patients already diagnosed with heart disease, he explains.
And, it doesnt take years of lotus positions and meditation to see improvementthe
study volunteers had measurable improvement in just six weeks, he says. The yoga and
meditation program included 40 minutes of postural yoga, 20 minutes of deep relaxation,
15 minutes of yoga breathing, and 15 minutes of meditation.
132
The study enrolled 33 patients, 30% of whom had heart disease. The study required
them to practice yoga and meditation for an hour and a half at least three times a week.
More than 60% of the volunteers were men and the average age of the study participants
was 55.
The researchers monitored blood pressure, pulse, body mass index (BMI, an indirect
measure of body fat used to measure weight), and cholesterol levels at the beginning of
the study and again after six weeks.
The researchers used an ultrasound to measure the blood flow in an artery of the arm, he
explains.
Yoga Improves Blood Pressure
At the beginning of the study the average blood pressure was 130/79 mmHg. The
American Heart Association says that a normal blood pressure reading is 120/80 mmHg.
After six weeks the average blood pressure reading was 125/74 mmHg, which was a
significant decrease with yoga and meditation classes. The volunteers also had a modest
reduction in BMIfrom 29 to 28, and they had an average reduction in pulse rate of
nine beats per minute, he says.
While people with heart disease had the biggest improvement in blood vessel function,
that improvement was independent of any improvements in blood pressure, he says.
And after six weeks it was the healthy patients who posted the biggest improvements in
blood pressure, pulse rate, and BMI.
Even with a small number of patients for a short period of time there was a benefit of
yoga and meditation seen in people with heart disease, he says. He says, however, that
the researchers dont know the mechanism involved in that benefit, which means that
more study is needed.
Gerald F. Fletcher, MD, a cardiovascular disease specialist at the Mayo Clinic Florida in
Jacksonville, tells WebMD that it is probably exercise. There are several studies that
suggest that exerciseany kind of exerciseimproves oxygen consumption, which
improves endothelial function. Fletcher, who was not involved in the study, is a
spokesman for the AHA.
Im not sure that meditation has a specific benefit, but if combining meditation with
exercise will get people to exercise, then I'm all for it. But the most important message is
that exercise works, Fletcher says.
Smith, D. E., J. L. Glaser, R. H. Schneider, and M. C. Dillbeck. Erythrocyte
sedimentation rate (ESR) and the Transcendental Meditation (TM) program.
Psychosomatic Medicine 1989, 51:259. Also in AGE, 1987, 10(4):160. Collected Papers
v5.377.
133
Findings: Lower erythrocyte sedimentatio n rate levels indicating less serious illness and
slower aging.
Smith, Jonathan C. Meditation research: Three observations on the state-of-the-art. In
Deane H. Shapiro, Jr., and Roger N. Walsh, Meditation: Classic and Contemporary
Perspectives. Hawthorne, N.Y.: Aldine Publishing, 1984, pp. 677-678.
___________. Personality correlates of continuation and outcome in meditation and erect
sitting control treatments. J Consult Clin Psychol, Apr 1978, 46(2):272-279. Also in D.
H. Shapiro, and R. N. Walsh, eds., Meditation: Classic and Contemporary Perspectives.
New York: Aldine Publishing, 1984, pp. 557-564.
Somani, S. M., S. G. Bhat, T. K. Bera, and M. A. Hollinger. Pharmacological aspects
of yoga. In S. M. Somani, ed., Pharmacology in Exercise and Sports. New York: CRC
Press, 1995, pp. 329-346.
Sovik, R. The science of breathingthe yogic view. In E. A. Mayer and C. B. Saper, eds.,
Progress in Brain Research, Vol 122: The Biological Basis for Mind Body Interactions.
New York: Elsevier Science BV, 2000, pp. 491-505.
Section headings: Introduction; Yoga and breathing; Stress, arousal, and self-regulation;
The respiratory rhythm; Non- volitional influences on breathing; Voluntary control of
breathing; A yogic view of breathing; The challenge of sustained awareness; Optimal
breathing; Integration of voluntary and automatic control; Studies of voluntary
respiratory control; Research hypotheses; Conclusion
Spicuzza, L., A. Gabutti, C. Porta, N. Montano, and L. Bernardi. Yoga and
chemoreflex response to hypoxia and hypercapnia. Lancet, 28 Oct 2000,
356(9240):1495-1496.
Abstract: We tested whether chemoreflex sensitivity could be affected by the practice of
yoga, and whether this is specifically because of a slow breathing rate obtained during
yoga or as a general consequence of yoga. We found that slow breathing rate per se
substantially reduced chemoreflex sensitivity, but long-term yoga practice was
responsible for a generalised reduction in chemoreflex.
Srinivasan, T. M. Electrophysiological correlates during yogic practices. The Yoga
Review, Winter 1981, 1(4):165-173.
Srivastava, Niraj. Effect of hypoxia inducing yogic exercises on cardiovascular
parameters. Thesis for Doctor of Medicine (Physiology), BRD Medical College,
Gorakphur, India, 2003. Author email: [email protected].
Keeping in view the frequency of hypertension- induced cardiovascular incapacitation in a
wide range of the population, the present study evaluated some of the beneficial effects of
yogic practices on cardio vascular/respiratory parameters, viz., blood pressure, heart
134
rate, electrocardiogram, and various pulmonary functions. Since in some earlier studies
yogic practices have been shown to reduce not only blood pressure levels but also
mitigate electrocardiographic heterogenicity, the present study assessed the overall
influence of yogic practices on cardiovascular and respiratory parameters in
normal human subjects.
Stachenfeld, N. S., G. W. Mack, L. DiPietro, T. S. Morocco, A. C. Jozsi, and E. R.
Nadel. Regulation of blood volume during training in post- menopausal women. Med. Sci.
Sports Exerc., Jan 1998, 30(1):92-98. PMID: 9475649. (Compares aerobic exercise and
Yoga.)
Abstract: In younger people the increase in aerobic capacity following training is related,
in part, to blood volume (BV) expansion and the consequent improvements in maximal
cardiac output. This training- induced hypervolemia is associated with a decrease in
cardiopulmonary baroreflex (CPBR) control of peripheral vascular tone. PURPOSE: To
test the hypothesis that improvement in peak oxygen consumption (VO2peak) during
training in older women is associated with specific central adaptations, such as BV
expansion and a reduction in CPBR control of vascular tone. METHODS: Seventeen
healthy older women were randomized into training (N = 9, 71 +/- 2 yr) and control (N =
8, 73 +/- 3 yr) groups. The training group exercised three to four times per wk for 30 min
at 60% peak heart rate for 12 wk and then 40-50 min at 75% peak heart rate for 12 wk.
The control group participated in yoga exercises over the same time period. We measured
resting BV (Evans blue dye), VO2peak, and the forearm vascular resistance response to
unloading low pressure mechanoreceptors during low levels of lower body negative
pressure (through -20 mm Hg) before and after aerobic training. The slope of the increase
in forearm vascular resistance (response) per unit decrease in central venous pressure
(stimulus) was used to assess CPBR responsiveness. RESULTS: Aerobic training
increased VO2peak 14.2% from 24.2 mL x kg(-1) x min(-1) to 27.7 mL x kg(-1) x min(1) (P < 0.05), a smaller improvement than typically seen in younger subjects. Blood
volume (59.9 +/- 1.9 and 60.9 +/- 1.9 mL x kg[-1]) and CPBR function (-3.98 +/- 0.92
and -3.46 +/- 0.94 units x mm(-1) Hg) were similar before and after training.
CONCLUSIONS: These data indicate that the inability to induce adaptations in CPBR
function may limit BV expansion during training in older women. In addition, the
absence of these specific adaptations may contrib ute to the relatively poor improvements
in VO2peak in older women during short (10-12 wk) periods of training.
Stanck, A., Jr., M. Kuna, P. Novk, M. A. Srinivasan, C. Dostlek, and S.
Vishnudevananda. Observations on respiratory and cardiovascular rhythmicities during
yogic high- frequency respiration. Physiol Res., 1991, 40(3):345-354. PMID: 1751482.
Yogic high- frequency respirationkapalabhati (KB)was studied in 24 subjects from a
point of rhythmicity. Respiratory movements, blood pressure and R-R intervals of ECG
were recorded in parallel and evaluated by spectral analysis of time series. Respiratory
signals during KB were modulated by 0.1 Hz rhythm in 82% of experiments. This
component was also present in R-R intervals and blood pressure during KB. Frequency
(0.2-0.3 Hz) was observed in 67% of respiratory records. The presence of the component
135
136
137
duration resulted in a frequency and amplitude spectrum (0.5--28.6 c/sec), and the mean
frequency and the mean voltage of each EEG lead. The EEG frequency spectra
constituted a continuum with increasing theta and delta activity and decreasing alpha
activity as the participants tended to fall asleep. The frequency spectrum during TM
corresponded to a spectrum situated between that of wakefulness and drowsiness and
remained virtually unchanged during the 20 min of meditation. The EEG mean frequency
of the TM group was about 1 c/sec slower than that of the control group. Intra- or
interhemispheric differences between quantities of EEG activity remained stable during
TM, nor did we observe any theta bursts. There was no consistent EEG patter associated
with a successful or unsuccessful meditation, nor did the EEGs of two meditators who
stated they had felt drowsy during TM show a different pattern.
Stone, R. A., and J. De Leo. Study of meditation and blood pressure (letter). New
England Journal of Medicine, 1976, 294(14):787.
Stroebel, Charles F., and Bernard C. Glueck. Passive meditation: Subjective and
clinical comparison with biofeedback. In G. Schwartz and D. Shapiro, Consciousness and
Self-Regulation. New York: Plenum Press, 1977. Also in D. H. Shapiro, and R. N. Walsh,
eds., Meditation: Classic and Contemporary Perspectives. New York: Aldine Publishing,
1984, pp. 201-208.
A study of stress levels in remand home children, before and after yoga. Swami
Vivekananda Yoga Research Foundation, http://www.vkyogas.org.in.
Subramanian, S. Yogaits probable role in maintaining and restoring normal health.
Proceedings First National Conference on Yoga, Science and Society, Banaras Hindu
University, 1979, pp. 32-34.
___________. Comparative studies on physiological response to a course of yogic
exercises. Proceedings 59th Session of Indian Science Congress, Physiological Section
(Part III), 1972.
Subramanyam, S. Yoga for health. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1989, 28(1):60-64.
Subrahmanyam, Sarada, and K. Porkodi. Comparative studies on yoga, meditation
and muscular exercises. The Yoga Review, Summer & Autumn 1983, 3(2&3):91-100.
___________, M. Satyanarayana, and K. R. Rajeswari. Effects of yoga and meditation
training on manual Xerox operators. The Yoga Review, 1984, 4(1&2):41-53.
Sudarsan, B., N. Janakiramiah, K. T. T. Shetty, B. N. Gangadhar, Vedamuthachar,
and D. K. Subbakrishna. Effect of Sudarshan Kriya Yoga (SKY) on plasma cortisol.
Indian Journal of Psychiatry, Apr 2001, 43.
Abstract: Sudarshan Kriya Yoga has been tried as an alternative treatment in depression.
Studies on patients with dysthymia and major depression showed significant
antidepression effect. The biological basis of the therapeutic efficacy of SKY has not
138
been investigated. In the present study we examined the effect of SKY on plasma
cortisol. Plasma cortisol levels were measured before and after a SKY treatment in 20
major depressive patients who received no psychotropic medication. There was
significant reduction in plasma cortisol levels. The relevance of this finding to the
biological correlates of SKY will be presented.
Sudharani, A., and S. Telles. Effect of yoga on memory. In H. R. Nagendra, R.
Ragarathna, and S. Telles, Yoga Research & Applications: Proceedings of the 5th
International Conference on Frontiers in Yoga Research and Applications. Bangalore,
Vivekananda Kendra Yoga Research Foundation, 2000, pp. 307-310.
Sudsuang, R., V. Chentanez, and K. Veluvan. Effect of Buddhist meditation on serum
cortisol and total protein levels, blood pressure, pulse rate, lung volume and reaction
time. Physiology and Behavior, Sep 1991, 50(3):543-548. PMID: 1801007.
Abstract: Serum cortisol and total protein levels, blood pressure, heart rate, lung volume,
and reaction time were studied in 52 males 20-25 years of age practicing Dhammakaya
Buddhist meditation, and in 30 males of the same age group not practicing meditation. It
was found that after meditation, serum cortisol levels were significantly reduced, serum
total protein level significantly increased, and systolic pressure, diastolic pressure and
pulse rate significantly reduced. Vital capacity, tidal volume and maximal voluntary
ventilation were significantly lower after meditation than before. There were also
significant decreases in reaction time after meditation practice. The percentage decrease
in reaction time during meditation was 22%, while in subjects untrained in meditation,
the percentage decrease was only 7%. Results from these studies indicate that practising
Dhammakaya Buddhist meditation produces biochemical and physiological changes and
reduces the reaction time.
Sugi, Y., and K. Akutsu. Studies on respiration and energy metabolism during sitting in
zazen. Res J Phys Ed, 1968, 12(3):190-206.
Sukumaran, M., B. G. Nagaraja, B. V. Suresh, B. H. Hanumanthiah, G.
Narasimhalu, and T. Desiraju. Changes in concentrations of urinary, HVA, MHPG,
VMA, 5-HIAA, 17-keto steroid and of blood cortisol and lactate following meditation
sessions. Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, 1987, 31(5).
Sundararajan, G. S. Passive adaptation through Yoga. The Yoga Review, Summer &
Autumn 1983, 3(2&3):81-86.
Abstract: Optimal inputs fed into the system, in an organised planned schedule, over a
period of time, produce progresive permanent improvements in the functional capactities
of the bodythe process of adaptation.
In the case of hypodynamic and static work-outs as in Yoga, a new concept of passive
adaptation is evolved. Certain specialized sensory regulatory receptors have been
139
140
Talukdar, B., S. Verma, S. C. Jain, and Mazumdar. Effect of yoga training on plasma
lipid profile, lipid perodixation and Na K ATPase activity in essential hypertension.
Indian Journal of Clinical Biochemistry, 1996, 11(2):129-133.
Tart, C. T. Measuring the depth of an altered state of consciousness, with particular
reference to self- report scales of hypnotic depth. In E. Fromm and R. E. Shor, eds.,
Hypnosis: Developments in Research and New Perspectives. 2d ed. New York: Aldine,
1979.
Tebecis, A. K. A controlled study of the EEG during Transcendental Meditation:
Comparison with hypnosis. Folia Psychiatrica et Neurologica Japonica, 1975,
29(4):305-13. PMID: 783025.
Abstract: A controlled, quantitative investigation of the electroencephalogram (EEG) and
transcendental meditation (TM) revealed that EEG changes during TM were rarely as
pronounced or consistent as previous reports suggest. There was considerable variation
between subjects, some displaying no EEG changes at all during TM compared with an
equal period of non- meditation. Any changes that did occur in a particular individual
were not necessarily repeated in a subsequent session. A comparison of mean EEG
parameters of the experimental group revealed no consistent significant differences
between meditation and non- meditation, although trends towards increased theta and
decreased beta activity during meditation were apparent. The biggest differences in mean
EEG parameters were between subject groups. In particular, the group of meditators
exhibited significantly more theta activity (during both TM and non- meditation) than a
randomly selected group of individuals that had never meditated or been hypnotized. The
EEG characteristics of the group of meditators were similar to those of a group of
subjects experienced in self- hypnosis. It is concluded that the most obvious EEG changes
during meditation are long-term. In people who regularly practise TM (or self- hypnosis),
the EEG gradually (over weeks or months) tends to slow down. Such a slowed down
EEG is apparent during both normal waking conditions and altered states of
consciousness in these individuals.
Telles, S., and T. Desiraju. Changes in respiratory and autonomic activities and oxygen
consumption in meditations and pranayamas. Indian Journal of Physiology and
Pharmacology, 1987, 31(5).
___________, and T. Desiraju. Oxygen consumption during pranayamic type of very
slow-rate breathing. Indian Journal of Medical Research, Oct 1991, 94:357-363. PMID:
1794892.
Abstract: To determine whether the yogic Ujjayi pranayamic type of breathing that
involves sensory awareness and consciously controlled, extremely slow-rate breathing
including at least a period of end- inspiration breath holding in each respiratory cycle
would alter oxygen consumption or not, ten males with long standing experience in
pranayama, and volunteering to participate in the laboratory study were assessed. These
141
subjects aged 28-59 yr, had normal health appropriate to their age. Since kumbhak (timed
breath holding) is considered as an important phase of the respiratory cycle in the
pranayama, they were categorised into two groups of five each, one group practising the
short kumbhak varieties of pranayama, and the other the long kumbhak varieties of
pranayama. The duration of kumbhak phase was on an average 22.2 percent of the
respiratory cycle in the short kumbhak group, and 50.4 per cent in the long kumbhak
group. The oxygen consumption was measured in test sessions using the closed circuit
method of breathing oxygen through the Benedict-Roth spirometer. Each subject was
tested in several repeat sessions. Values of oxygen consumption of the period of
pranayamic breathing, and of post-pranayamic breathing period, were compared to
control value of oxygen consumption of the prepranayamic breathing period of each test
session. The results revealed that the short kumbhak pranayamic breathing caused a
statistically significant increase (52%) in the oxygen consumption (and metabolic rate)
compared to the pre-pranayamic base- line period of breathing. In contrast to the above,
the long kumbhak pranayamic breathing caused a statistically significant lowering (19%
of the oxygen consumption (and metabolic rate).
___________, and T. Desiraju. Heart rate and respiratory changes accompanying yogic
conditions of single thought and thoughtless states. Indian Journal of Physiology and
Pharmacology, 1992, 36(4):293-294.
___________, and T. Desiraju. Heart rate alterations in different types of pranayamas.
Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, 1992, 36(4):287-288.
___________, and T. Desiraju. Autonomic changes in Brahmakumaris Raja Yoga
meditation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 1993, 15:147-152.
___________, and T. Desiraju. Recording of auditory middle latency evoked potentials
during the practice of meditation with the syllable OM. Indian Journal of Medical
Research, 1993, 98[B]:237-239. PMID: 8119759.
Abstract: Middle latency auditory evoked potentials were examined in 7 proficient
subjects during the practice of meditation on the syllable 'OM', to determine whether
these potentials would differ significantly from those recorded during the baseline state
without practicing mediation. Similar records were also obtained in 7 'naive' subjects,
matched for age, before and during a control period which involved sitting with eyes
closed, and with no special instructions for focusing their thoughts. There was
considerable inter-subject variability in the different components. However, during
meditation there was a small but significant reduction in the peak latency of the Nb wave
(the maximum negativity occurring between 35 and 65 msec). This reduction was
observed consistently during the 3 repeat sessions of each subject, while the 'naive'
subjects did not show this change. These results suggest that the inter-subject variability
of middle latency auditory evoked potentials precludes using them as the method of
choice for assessing the effe cts of meditation. The small but consistent decrease in the Nb
wave peak latency, indicates that the middle latency auditory evoked potentials do
142
change with meditation. However, the variability of the potentials may mask subtle
changes.
___________, B. Hanumanthaiah, R. Nagarathna, and H. R. Nagendra. Improvement
in static motor performance following yogic training of school children. Perceptual &
Motor Skills, Jun 1993, 76(3 Pt 2):1264-1266. PMID: 8337075.
Abstract: Two groups of 45 children each, whose ages ranged from 9 to 13 years, were
assessed on a steadiness test, at the beginning and again at the end of a 10-day period
during which one group received training in yoga, while the other group did not. The
steadiness test required insertion of and holding for 15 sec. a metal stylus without
touching the sides of holes of decreasing sizes in a metal plate. The contacts were
counted as errors. During the 10-day period, one group (the Yoga group) received
training in special physical postures (asanas), voluntary regulation of breathing
(Pranayama), maintenance of silence, as well as visual focussing exercises (tratakas) and
games to improve the attention span and memory. The other group (control) carried out
their usual routine. After 10 days, the Yoga group showed a significant (Wilcoxon's
paired signed-ranks test) decrease in errors, whereas the control group showed no
change.
___________, B. Hanumanthaiah, R. Nagarathna, and H. R. Nagendra. Duration of
yoga practice and motivation influence static motor performance. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 1994.
___________, B. Hanumanthaiah, R. Nagarathna, and H. R. Nagendra. Plasticity of
motor control systems demonstrated by yoga training. Indian Journal of Physiology and
Pharmacology, 1994, 38(2):143-144. PMID: 8063362.
Abstract: The static motor performance was tested in two groups with 20 subjects in each
(age range 17 to 22 years, and 5 females in each group). Tests were carried out at the
beginning and end of a 10 day period. The test required being able to insert and hold a
metal stylus within holes of varying sizes for 15 sec. Accidental contacts between the
stylus and the sides of the holes, were registered on a counter as errors. During the 10
days one group (the yoga group) practised asanas (physical postures), pranayama
(voluntary regulation of breathing), meditation, devotional sessions, and tratakas (visual
focussing exercises). The control group followed their usual routine. At the end of 10
days the yoga group showed a significant reductio n in number of errors (Wilcoxon paired
signed ranks test), while the control group did not change. Our earlier study showed a
similar improvement in children (9-13 years). It was interesting to note the same degree
of plasticity in motor control systems in young adults. The implications for rehabilitation
programmes have been discussed.
___________, C. Joseph, S. Venkatesh, and T. Desiraju. Alteration of auditory middle
latency evoked potentials during yogic consciously regulated breathing and attentive state
of mind. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 1992, 14:189-198. PMID: 8340237.
143
144
age 27.2 years +/- 3.3 years, four males) were assessed before and after test sessions
conducted on two consecutive days. On one day the test session involved practicing SAV
pranayama for 45 minutes (SAV session). During the test period of the other day,
subjects were asked to breathe normally for 45 minutes (NB session). For half the
patients (randomly chosen) the SAV session was on the first day and the NB session on
the next day. For the remaining six patients, the order of the two sessions was reversed.
After the SAV session (but not after the NB) there was a significant (P < .05, paired t
test) increase in oxygen consumption (17%) and in systolic blood pressur e (mean
increase 9.4 mm Hg) and a significant decrease in digit pulse volume (45.7%). The latter
two changes are interpreted to be the result of increased cutaneous vasoconstriction. After
both SAV and NB sessions, there was a significant decrease in skin resistance (two factor
ANOVA, Tukey test). These findings show that SAV has a sympathetic stimulating
effect. This technique and other variations of unilateral forced nostril breathing deserve
further study regarding therapeutic merits in a wide range of disorders.
___________, R. Nagarathna, and H. R. Nagendra. Autonomic changes during OM
meditation. Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, 1998, 39(4):418-420.
___________, R. Nagarathna, and H. R. Nagendra. Autonomic changes while
mentally repeating two syllables, one meaningful and the other neutral. Indian Journal of
Physiology and Pharmacology, 1998, 42(1):57-63.
___________, R. Nagarathna, H. R. Nagendra, and T. Desiraju. Physiological
changes in sports teachers following 3 months of training in yoga. Indian Journal of
Medical Sciences, Oct 1993, 47(10):235-238. PMID: 8112782.
Abstract: 1. This report shows that in a group of 40 physical education teachers who
already had an average of 8.9 years physical training, 3 months of yogic training
produced significant improvement in general health (in terms of body weight and BP
reduction and improved lung functions). 2. There was also evidence of decreased
autonomic arousal and more of psychophysiological relaxation (heart rate and respiratory
rate reduction), and improved somatic steadiness (decreased errors in the steadiness test).
3. The changes at the end of 3 months in volar GSR in different directions
(increase/decrease/no change), depending on the initial values, suggests that practising
yoga may help to bring about a balance in different autonomic functions, so that
functioning is optimised.
___________, R. Nagarathna, H. R. Nagendra, and T. Desiraju. Alterations in
auditory middle latency evoked potentials during meditation on a meaningful syllabus
OM. International Journal of Neurosciences, 1994, 76:87-93. PMID: 7960473.
Abstract: Middle latency auditory evoked potentials were recorded in 18 male volunteers
with ages between 25 and 45 years, 9 of whom had more than 10 years of experience in
"Om" meditation (senior subjects), whereas the other 9 had no meditation experience
(naive subjects). Both groups were studied in two types of sessions. (1) Before, during,
and after 20 minutes of mentally repeating "one" (control session), and (2) a similar
145
session, though with 20 minutes of mentally chanting "Om" (meditation session). The
senior subjects showed a statistically significant (paired t-test) increase in the peak
amplitude of Na wave (the maximum negative peak between 14 and 18 ms) during
meditation, while the same subjects showed a statistically significant reduction in the Na
wave peak amplitude during control sessions. In contrast, the naive subjects had a
significant decrease in the Na wave peak amplitude during meditation sessions and a
nonsignificant trend of reduction during control sessions, as well. This difference
between senior and naive subjects was significant (two-way ANOVA). There were no
significant changes in short latency wave V or Pa wave (the positive peak between the Na
wave and 35 ms). The changes in the Na wave suggest that both mediation on a
meaningful symbol, and mental repetition of a neutral word cause neural changes at the
same level (possibly diencephalic). However, the change could be in opposite directions,
and this difference could be correlated with differences in the duration of experience in
meditation between senior and naive subjects.
___________, R. Nagarathna, V. Ramana, and H. R. Nagendra. A combination of
focusing and defocusing through yoga reduces optical illusion more than alone. Indian
Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, 1997, 41(2):129-133.
___________, H. R. Nagendra, and R. Nagarathna. Effect of uninostril yoga breathing
on tweezer dexterity. Submitted by Swami Vivekananda Yoga Research Foundation to
Indian Journal of Physiology and Allied Sciences, Feb 1998.
___________, S. Narendran, P. Raghuraj, R. Nagarathna, and H. R. Nagendra.
Comparison of changes in autonomic and respiratory parameters of girls after yoga and
games at a community ho me. Perceptual Motor Skills, Feb 1997, 84(1):251-257.
___________, V. Ramaprabhu, and S. K. Reddy. Effect of yoga training on maze
learning. Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, Apr 2000, 44(2):197-201.
___________, S. K. Reddy, and H. R. Nagendra. Oxygen consumption and respiration
following two yoga relaxation techniques. Applied Psychophysiology And Biofeedback,
Dec 2000, 25(4):221-227. MEDLINE PMID: 11218923.
Abstract: The present study was conducted to evaluate a statement in ancient yoga texts
that suggests that a combination of both calming and stimulating measures may be
especially helpful in reaching a state of mental equilibrium. Two yoga practices, one
combining calming and stimulating measures (cyclic meditation) and the other, a
calming technique (shavasan), were compared. The oxygen consumption, breath rate,
and breath volume of 40 male volunteers (group mean +/- SD, 27.0 +/- 5.7 years) were
assessed before and after sessions of cyclic meditation (CM) and before and after
sessions of shavasan (SH). The 2 sessions (CM, SH) were 1 day apart. Cyclic meditation
includes the practice of yoga postures interspersed with periods of supine relaxation.
During SH the subject lies in a supine position throughout the practice. There was a
significant decrease in the amount of oxygen consumed and in breath rate and an increase
in breath volume after both types of sessions (2- factor ANOVA, paired t test). However,
146
the magnitude of change on all 3 measures was greater after CM: (1) Oxyge n
consumption decreased 32.1% after CM compared with 10.1% after SH; (2) breath rate
decreased 18.0% after CM and 15.2% after SH; and (3) breath volume increased 28.8%
after CM and 15.9% after SH. These results support the idea that a combination of yoga
postures interspersed with relaxation reduces arousal more than relaxation alone does.
___________, P. R. Vani, R. Nagarathna, and H. R. Nagendra. A combination of
focusing and defocusing through yoga reduces optical illusion more than focusing alone.
Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, 1997, 41(2):71-74.
___________, R. P. Vempati, and S. K. Reddy. Effect of yoga training on maze
learning. Unpublished date, Swami Vivekananda Yoga Research Foundation.
Thomas, Andrew. Yoga and fascia. The Journal of The International Association of
Yoga Therapists, 1992, no. 3, pp. 39-42.
___________. Yoga and cardiovascular function. The Journal of The International
Association of Yoga Therapists, 1993, no. 4, pp. 39-41.
Thomas, Tommijean, Christopher D. Tori, and Benjamin A. Thomas. Assessing the
[psychological] benefits of practicing Iyengar Yoga. Yoga Rahasya, 1998, 5(2):30-33.
Contact: Dr. Tommijean Thomas, [email protected].
Abstract:
Objectives:
a. To determine the physical, physiological and psychological effects of practicing
Iyengar Yoga
b. To determine the reason and motivation for practitioners to continue yoga practice.
c. To evaluate perception of stress and coping mechanisms amongst Iyengar Yoga
practitioners
d. To compare the personality characteristics of Iyengar Yoga Practitioners with the
normal adults who did not practice Iyengar Yoga.
Participants and Methodology:
367 Iyengar Yoga practitioners, from North and South America, Asia, South Africa, New
Zealand and Australia, who attended the Iyengar Yoga Festival, Pune in 1998
participated in this study. All these participants answered three questionnaires pertaining
to their yoga practice, the perception and coping of stress and their personality traits. It
took each participant at least one hour to answer all the questions listed. Their replies
were then assessed using specific statistical tests.
Results:
Yoga Practice: 97% of the participants practiced asanas for an average of 8 hours per
week while 71% practiced pranayama for an average of 2.3 hours a week.
147
Life Style: Yoga practice was a strong motivating factor amongst all the practitioners to
improve health habits. 94% balanced work with recreation while 85% followed a
vegetarian diet. It is pertinent to note that Iyengar Yoga does not compel the
practitioner to any specific dietary or life style regiment.
Improvement in the Physical and Physiological Disorders following Yoga Practice: 65%
of the participants had a medical problem when they started their yoga practice. Most
people indicated that, on the average, they improved, with some proclaiming cures and
others remission. There was a significant improvement in the physical health of all the
participants. Significant benefits were seen with reference to the respiratory,
genitourinary, circulatory and digestive systems.
There was a very significant improvement with relation to emotional disorders especially
those pertaining to mood disorders and substance abuse.
Reasons and Motivation to Continue Yoga Practice: Although, majority of practitioners
started their yoga practice to seek physical benefit it is the psychological benefits
especially mental control and stress/ tension control which now motivates them to
continue their practices.
Perception of Stress and Coping Mechanisms: The awareness of stress among yoga
practitioners was similar to that of a normal population. Coping methods, however, were
more active and varied than for the typical person. The management of stress by those
practicing yoga was characterized by heightened self-control, using difficulties as a
means to enhance personal growth and the ability to detach oneself from tension and
anxiety. The results of the study also revealed that those who adopted a planful and
optimistic approach to problem solving significantly reduced perceived stress.
Interestingly, those primarily involved in the teaching of yoga had the lowest perceived
stress.
Personality Characteristics: A comparative study of the personality differences between
yoga practitioners and normal non-practicing individuals shows that the yoga
practitioners had a higher than average values when assessed for their sensitivity,
flexibility and self reliance. However, they were less conforming and submissive as
compared with normal non practitioners.
Conclusion:
The results of this study clearly documents that practice of Iyengar Yoga improves the
physical, physiological and psychological well being of the practitioner; a heightened self
control; an enhancement of personal growth, a low perception of stress. The personality
of the practitioners indicated that they were more sensitive, flexible and self-reliant but at
the same time exhibited a mind of their own by being non-conforming and submissive.
___________, Christopher D. Tori, Benjamin A. Thomas, and Rajvi H. Mehta.
Medical, psychological, and spiritual benefits of longterm Iyengar Yoga practice. Yoga
Rahasya, 2000, 7(1):77-86.
148
149
150
Abstract: This study tested the prediction that reading Vedic Sanskrit texts, without
knowledge of their meaning, produces a distinct physiological state. We measured EEG,
breath rate, heart rate, and skin conductance during: (1) 15-min Transcendental
Meditation (TM) practice; (2) 15-min reading verses of the Bhagavad Gita in Sanskrit;
and (3) 15- min reading the same verses translated in German, Spanish, or French. The
two reading conditions were randomly counterbalanced, and subjects filled out
experience forms between each block to reduce carryover effects. Skin conductance
levels significantly decreased during both reading Sanskrit and TM practice, and
increased slightly during reading a modern language. Alpha power and coherence were
significantly higher when reading Sanskrit and during TM practice, compared to reading
modern languages. Similar physiological patterns when reading Sanskrit and during
practice of the TM technique suggests that the state gained during TM practice may be
integrated with active mental processes by reading Sanskrit.
___________, and C. Pearson. Pure consciousness: Distinct phenomenological and
physiological correlates of consciousness itself. International Journal of Neuroscience,
Jan 1999, 100(1-4):77-89. PMID: 10938552.
Abstract: This paper explores subjective reports and physiological correlates of the
experience of consciousness itselfself awareness isolated from the processes and
objects of experience during Transcendental Meditation practice. Subjectively, this state
is characterized by the absence of the very framework (time, space, and body sense) and
content (qualities of inner and outer perception) that define waking experiences.
Physiologically, this state is distinguished by the presence of apneustic breathing,
autonomic orienting at the onset of breath changes, and increases in the frequency of
peak EEG power. A model, called the junction point model, is presented that integrates
pure consciousness with waking, dreaming, or sleeping. It could provide a structure to
generate a coherent program of research to test the full range of consciousness and so
enable us to understand what it means to be fully human.
___________, J. Tecci, A. Arenander, and R. K. Wallace. Patterns of EEG coherence,
power, and contingent ne gative variation characterize the integration of transcendental
and waking states. Biological Psychology, Nov 2002, 61(3):293-319. PMID: 12406612.
Author email: [email protected].
Abstract: Long-term meditating subjects report that transcendental experiences (TE),
which first occurred during their Transcendental Meditation (TM) practice, now
subjectively co-exist with waking and sleeping states. To investigate neurophysiological
correlates of this integrated state, we recorded EEG in these subjects and in two
comparison groups during simple and choice contingent negative variation (CNV) tasks.
In individuals reporting the integration of the transcendent with waking and sleeping,
CNV was higher in simple but lower in choice trials, and 6-12 Hz EEG amplitude and
broadband frontal EEG coherence were higher during choice trials. Increased EEG
amplitude and coherence, characteristic of TM practice, appeared to become a stable
EEG trait during CNV tasks in these subjects. These significant EEG differences may
underlie the inverse patterns in CNV amplitude seen between groups. An Integr ation
151
Scale, constructed from these cortical measures, may characterize the transformation in
brain dynamics corresponding to increasing integration of the transcendent with waking
and sleeping.
___________, J. J. Tecci, and J. Guttman. Cortical plasticity, contingent negative
variation, and transcendent experiences during practice of the Transcendental Meditation
technique. Biological Psychology, Nov 2000, 55(1):41-55. Author email:
[email protected]. PMID: 11099807.
Abstract: This study investigated effects of transcendent experiences on contingent
negative variation (CNV) amplitude, CNV rebound, and distraction effects. Three groups
of age-matched subjects with few (<1 per year), more frequent (10-20 per year), or daily
self-reported transcendent experiences received 31 simple RT trials (flash (S(1))/tone
(S(2))/button press) followed by 31 divided-attention trials - randomly intermixed trials
with or without a three- letter memory task in the S(1)-S(2) interval). Late CNV
amplitudes in the simple trials were smallest in the group with fewest, and largest in the
group with most frequent transcendent experiences. Conversely, CNV distraction effects
were largest in the group with fewest and smallest in the group with most frequent
transcendent experiences (the second groups values were in the middle in each case).
These data suggest cumulative effects of transcendent experiences on cortical preparatory
response (heightened late CNV amplitude in simple trials) and executive functioning
(diminished distraction effects in letter trials).
___________, and R. Keith Wallace. Autonomic patterns during respiratory
suspensions: Possible markers of Transcendental Consciousness. Pyschophysiology,
1997, 34:39-46.
Abstract: In two experiments, we investiaged physiological correlates of Transcendental
Consciousness during Transcendental Meditation sessions. In the first, experimenterinitiated bells, based on observed physiological patterns, marked three phases during a
Transcendental meditation session in 16 individuals. Interrater reliability between
participant and experimenter classification of experiences at each bell was quite good.
During phases including Transcendental Consciousness experiences, skin conductance
responses and heart rate deceleration occurred at the onset of respiratory suspensions or
reductions in breath volume. In the second experiment, this autonomic pattern was
compared with that during forced breath holding. Phasic autonomic activity was
significantly higher at respiratory suspension onset than at breath holding onset. These
easily measured markers could help focus research on the existence and characteristics of
Transcendental Consciousness.
___________, and R. Keith Wallace. Autonomic and EEG patterns during eyes-closed
rest and transcendental meditation (TM) practice: The basis for a neural model of TM
practice. Consciousness and Cognition, Sep 1999, 8(3):302-318. PMID: 0010487785.
Abstract: In this single-blind within-subject study, autonomic and EEG variables were
compared during 10- min, order-balanced eyes-closed rest and Transcendental Meditation
152
(TM) sessions. TM sessions were distinguished by (1) lower breath rates, (2) lower skin
conductance levels, (3) higher respiratory sinus arrhythmia levels, and (4) higher alpha
anterior-posterior and frontal EEG coherence. Alpha power was not significantly
different between conditions. These results were seen in the first minute and were
maintained throughout the 10- min sessions. TM practice appears to (1) lead to a state
fundamentally different than eyes-closed rest; (2) result in a cascade of events in the
central and autonomic nervous systems, leading to a rapid change in state (within a
minute) that was maintained throughout the TM session; and (3) be best distinguished
from other conditions through autonomic and EEG alpha coherence patterns rather than
alpha power. Two neural networks that may mediate these effects are suggested. The
rapid shift in physiological functioning within the first minute might be mediated by a
neural switch in prefrontal areas inhibiting activity in specific and nonspecific
thalamocortical circuits. The resulting restfully alert state might be sustained by a basal
ganglia-corticothalamic threshold regulation mechanism automatically maintaining lower
levels of cortical excitability. Copyright 1999 Academic Press.
Travis, T., C. Kondo, and J. Knott. Heart rate, muscle tension, and alpha production of
Transcendental Meditation and relaxation controls. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation,
1976, I(4):387-394.
Trynham, R. The effects of experimental meditation, feedback and relaxation training on
electromyograph and self-report measures of relaxation and altered states of
consciousness. Biofeedback & Self Regulation, 1978, 3(2):187ff.
Tulpule, T. H. Cardio-respiratory, metabolic and hormonal changes in middle aged men
following yogic exercise. Maharashtra Med J, 1978, 25(8):303-308.
Udupa, Kaviraja, Madanmohan, Ananda Balayogi Bhavanani, P. Vijyalakshmi, and
N. Krishnamurthy. Effect of pranayam training on cardiac function in normal young
volunteers. Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, Jan 2003, 47 (1):27-33.
Article available online: http://www.ijpp.com/vol47_no1_orgn_artcl_1.htm.
Abstract: Systolic tire intervals (STI) are non- invasive and sensitive tests for measuring
the ventricular performance. It has been reported that practice of pranayam modulates
cardiac autonomic status and improves cardio-respiratory functions. Keeping this in
view, the present study was designed to determine whether pranayam training has any
effect on ventricular performance as measured by STI and cardiac autonomic function
tests (AFT). Twenty- four school children were randomly divided into two groups of
twelve each. Group I (pranayam group) subjects were given training in nadishuddhi,
mukh-bhastrika, pranav and savitri pranayams and practised the same for 20 minutes
daily for a duration of 3 months. Group II (control group) subjects were not given any
pranayam training. STI (QS2 , LVET and PEP) and AFT (RRIV and QT/QS2 ) were
measured in both the groups at the beginning and again at the end of three months study
period. Pranayam training produced an increase in RRIV and a decrease in QT/QS2
suggesting an enhanced parasympathetic and blunted sympathetic activity respectively.
QS2 , PEP and PEP/LVET increased significantly, whereas LVET was reduced
153
significantly in pranayam group. In contrast, the changes in STI and AFT were much
less marked in the control group. Our study shows that three months of pranayam
training modulates ventricular performance by increasing parasympathetic activity and
decreasing sympathetic activity. Further studies on a larger sample size may illustrate the
underlying mechanism(s) involved in this alteration.
Udupa, K. N. Studies on physiological aspects of yoga. In K. N. Udupa, Stress and Its
Management by Yoga. 2d ed. Delhi: Motilal Banarsidass, 1985.
___________, and R. H. Singh. The scientific basis of yoga (letter). Journal of the
American Medical Association, 1972, 220:1365ff.
___________, and R. H. Singh. Biochemical studies on meditation. Quarterly Journal of
Surg. Sc., 1977, 13:294ff.
___________, and R. H. Singh, K. N. Dwivedi, H. Pandey, and V. Rai. Comparative
biochemical studies on meditation. Indian Journal of Medical Research, 1975,
63(12):1676-1679.
___________, R. H. Singh, and R. M. Shettiwar. Studies on physiological and
metabolic response to the practice of yoga in young normal volunteers. Journal of
Research in Indian Medicine, 1971, 6(3):345-353.
___________, R. H. Singh, and R. M. Shettiwar. A comparative study of the effect of
some individual yogic practices in normal persons. Indian Journal of Medical Research,
1975, 63(8):1066-1071.
___________, R. H. Singh, and R. M. Shettiwar. Neurohumoral changes following
meditation. Journal of Research in Indian Medicine, 1975, 10(2):64-66.
___________, R. H. Singh, and R. M. Shettiwar. Physiological and biochemical studies
on the effect of yoga and certain other exercises. Indian Journal of Medical Research,
1975, 63(4):620-625. PMID: 1213761.
___________, R. H. Singh, and R. M. Shettiwar. Studies on the effect of some yogic
breathing exercises (Pranayams) in normal persons. Indian Journal of Medical Research,
1975, 63:1062-1065.
___________, R. H. Singh, and R. M. Shettiwar. Studies on the combined practice of
some yogic practices. Journal of Research Indian Med. Yoga & Homeo., 1976.
___________, R. H. Singh, and R. M. Shettiwar. Studies on the physiological effects of
the yogic posture Shavasana. Journal of Research Indian Med. Yoga & Homeo., 1978,
12(1).
154
155
156
Vedantam, Shankar. Tracing the synapses of spirituality. The Washington Post, 17 Jun
2001, p. A01.
In Philadelphia, a researcher discovers areas of the brain that are activated during
meditation. At two other universities in San Diego and North Carolina, doctors study how
epilepsy and certain hallucinogenic drugs can produce religious epiphanies. And in
Canada, a neuroscientist fits people with magnetized helmets that produce spiritual
experiences for the secular.
The work is part of a broad new effort by scientists around the world to better
understand religious experiences, measure them, and even reproduce them. Using
powerful brain imaging technology, researchers are exploring what mystics call nirvana,
and what Christians describe as a state of grace. Scientists are asking whether spirituality
can be explained in terms of neural networks, neurotransmitters and brain chemistry.
What creates that transcendental feeling of being one with the universe? It could be the
decreased activity in the brains parietal lobe, which helps regulate the sense of self and
physical orientation, research suggests. How does religion prompt divine feelings of love
and compassion? Possibly because of changes in the frontal lobe, caused by heightened
concentration during meditation. Why do many people have a profound sense that
religion has changed their lives? Perhaps because spiritual practices activate the temporal
lobe, which weights experiences with personal significance . . .
Vempati, R. P., and S. Telles. Yoga based isometric relaxation (progressive relaxation)
vs supine rest: A study of oxygen consumption, breath volume and autonomic measures.
Journal of Indian Psychology, Apr 1999, 17(2):46-52. Abstract available online:
http://www.yogamedicine.com/publications.htm.
___________. Yoga based relaxation reduces sympathetic activity in subjects based on
baseline levels. Paper presented at 5th International Conference on Frontiers in Yoga
Research: Applications, 1999. Abstract available online:
http://www.yogamedic ine.com/publications.htm.
___________. Yoga-based guided relaxation reduces sympathetic activity judged from
baseline levels. Psychological Reports, Apr 2002, 90(2):487-494. PMID: 12061588.
Abstract: 35 male volunteers whose ages ranged from 20 to 46 years were studied in two
sessions of yoga-based guided relaxation and supine rest. Assessments of autonomic
variables were made for 15 subjects, before, during, and after the practices, whereas
oxygen consumption and breath volume were recorded for 25 subjects before and after
both types of relaxation. A significant decrease in oxygen consumption and increase in
breath volume were recorded after guided relaxation (paired t test). There were
comparable reductions in heart rate and skin conductance during both types of relaxation.
During guided relaxation the power of the low frequency component of the heart-rate
variability spectrum reduced, whereas the power of the high frequency component
increased, suggesting reduced sympathetic activity. Also, subjects with a baseline ratio of
LF/HF > 0.5 showed a significant decrease in the ratio after guided relaxation, while
157
subjects with a ratio < or = 0.5 at baseline showed no such change. The results suggest
that sympathetic activity decreased after guided relaxation based on yoga, depending on
the baseline levels.
___________. Increased visual perceptual accuracy following yoga training.
Vivekananda Kendra Yoga Research Foundation. Abstract available online:
http://www.yogamedicine.com/publications.htm.
Venkatesh, S., T. R. Raju, et al. A study of structure of phenomenology of
consciousness in meditative and non- meditative states. Indian Journal of Physiology and
Pharmacology, 1997, 41(2):149-153.
Vicenk, K., and J. Motajov. Continuous observing of the heart rhythm changes during
Hatha-Yoga exercises. Jgov Cvicen, 1982, pp. 35-39. [In Czechoslovakian.]
___________. Study of the heart rhythm variability during Hatha Yoga exercises. Activ
Nerv Sup, 1982, 24:175-176.
Vinekar, Shyamala., M.D. Neurophysiological basis of the Yogas. Journal of Indian
Medical Profession Annual 1967-68. (Dr. Vinekar, [email protected], has
indicated that reprints are not available.)
___________. Scientific basis of yoga. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1976-1977, 18(3&4):89-97.
Vinod, R. A review of lab, tools used in the studies of brain laterality. Yoga-Mimamsa,
1991, 30(2 &3):1-16.
Vinod, S. D., R. S. Vinod, and U. Khire. Effect of comprehensive training in ashtanga
yoga on reaction time as a measure of general intelligence in relation to age, sex and
socioeconomic status. Yoga-Mimamsa, 1988, 27(1&2):11-12.
Vogel, C. M., R. Albin, and J. W. Alberts. Lotus footdrop: Sciatic neuropathy in the
thigh. Neurology, Apr 1991, 41(4):605-606.
Votava, Jir. How yogic exercises influence [the] musculoskeletal system. Jgov
Cvicen, 1982, pp. 43-45. [In Czechoslovakian.]
Summary: Medical rehabilitation is traditionally more interested in [the]
musculoskeletal system than in the others. Yogic exercises influence the whole body. The
effect on muscles and joints can be sometimes unimportant. Nevertheless, yogic postures
influence [the] musculoskeletal system in different ways:
1) Passive stretching of muscle
2) Activation of certain muscles, particularly on [the] trunk and proximal parts of [the]
extremities
3) General relaxation after asana
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
Of Related Interest
Ades, P. A., P. D. Savage, M. E. Cress, M. Brochu, N. M. Lee, and E. T. Poehlman.
Resistance training on physical performance in disabled older female cardiac patients.
Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 23 Aug 2003, 35(8):1265-1270.
Abstract: Purpose: We evaluated the value of resistance training on measures of physical
performance in disabled older women with coronary heart disease (CHD). Methods: The
study intervention consisted of a 6- month program of resistance training in a randomized
controlled trial format. Training intensity was at 80% of the single-repetition maximal
lift. Control patients performed light yoga and breathing exercises. Study participants
included 42 women with CHD, all >or= 65 yr of age and community dwelling. Subjects
were screened by questionnaire to have low self- reported physical function. The primary
study measurements related to the performance of 16 household activities of the
Continuous Scale Physical Functional Performance test (CSPFP). These ranged from
dressing, to kitchen and cleaning activities, to carrying groceries and walking onto a bus
with luggage, and a 6- min walk. Activities were measured in time to complete a task,
weight carried during a task, or distance walked. Other measures included body
composition, measures of aerobic fitness and strength, and questionnaire-based measures
of physical function and depression score. Results: Study groups were similar at baseline
by age, aerobic capacity, strength, body composition, and in performing the CSPFP.
After conditioning, 13 of 16 measured activities were performed more rapidly, or with
increased weight carried, compared with the control group (all P < 0.05). Maximal power
for activities that involved weight-bearing over a distance, increased by 40% (P < 0.05).
Conclusions : Disabled older women with CHD who participate in an intense resistancetraining program improve physical capacity over a wide range of household physical
activities. Benefits extend beyond strength-related activities, as endurance, balance,
coordination, and flexibility all improved. Strength training should be considered an
important component in the rehabilitation of older women with CHD.
Ainsworth, B. E., W. L. Haskell, A. S. Leon, D. R. Jacobs, Jr., H. J. Montoye, J. F.
Sallis, and R. S. Paffenbarger, Jr. Compendium of physical activities: classification of
energy costs of human physical activities. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise,
Jan 1993, 25(1):71-80. PMID: 8292105.
A coding scheme is presented for classifying physical activity by rate of energy
expenditure, i.e., by intensity. Energy cost was established by a review of published and
unpublished data. This coding scheme employs five digits that classify activity by
purpose (i.e., sports, occupation, self-care), the specific type of activity, and its intensity
as the ratio of work metabolic rate to resting metabolic rate (METs). Energy expenditure
in kilocalories or kilocalories per kilogram body weight can be estimated for all activities,
167
specific activities, or activity types. General use of this coding system would enhance the
comparability of results across studies using self reports of physical activity.
Ainsworth, B. E., W. L. Haskell, M. C. Whitt, M. L. Irwin, A. M. Swartz, S. J.
Strath, W. L. O'Brien, D. R. Bassett, Jr., K. H. Schmitz, P. O. Emplaincourt, D. R.
Jacobs, Jr., and A. S. Leon. Compendium of physical activities: an update of activity
codes and MET intensities. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, Sep 2000, 32(9
Suppl):S498-504. Author email: [email protected]. PMID: 10993420.
We provide an updated version of the Compendium of Physical Activities, a coding
scheme that classifies specific physical activity (PA) by rate of energy expenditure. It was
developed to enhance the comparability of results across studies using self-reports of PA.
The Compendium coding scheme links a five-digit code that describes physical activities
by major headings (e.g., occupation, transportation, etc.) and specific activities within
each major heading with its intensity, defined as the ratio of work metabolic rate to a
standard resting metabolic rate (MET). Energy expenditure in MET- minutes, MET- hours,
kcal, or kcal per kilogram body weight can be estimated for specific activities by type or
MET intensity. Additions to the Compendium were obtained from studies describing
daily PA patterns of adults and studies measuring the energy cost of specific physical
activities in field settings. The updated version includes two new major headings of
volunteer and religious activities, extends the number of specific activities from 477 to
605, and provides updated MET intensity levels for selected activities.
Anderson, P., and S. A. Andersson. Physiological Basis of the Alpha Rhythm. New
York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1968.
Bell, Harold J., and James Duffin. The respiratory response to passive limb movement
is suppressed by a cognitive task. Journal of Applied Physiology, 2004. Author email:
[email protected].
Abtract: Feedback from muscles stimulates ventilation at the onset of passive movement.
We hypothesized that central neural activity via a cognitive task source would interact
with afferent feedback, and we tested this hypothesis by examining the fast changes in
ventilation at the transition from rest to passive leg movement, under two conditions: (A)
no task, and (B) solving a computer-based puzzle. Resting breathing was greater in
condition (B) than in condition (A); evidenced by an increase in mean SEM breathing
frequency (18.2 1.1 br min-1 versus 15.0 1.2 br min-1, p = 0.004) and ventilation
(10.93 1.16 l min-1 versus 9.11 1.17 l min-1 p < 0.001). In condition (A) the onset of
passive movement produced a fast increase in mean SEM breathing frequency ( = 2.9
0.4 br min-1, p < 0.001), tidal volume ( = 233 95 ml, p < 0.001) and ventilation ( = 6.00
1.76 l min-1, p < 0.001). However, in condition (B) the onset of passive movement
only produced a fast increase in mean SEM breathing frequency ( = 1.3 0.4 br min-1,
p = 0.045), significantly smaller than in condition (A) (p = 0.007). These findings provide
evidence for an interaction between central neural cognitive activity and the afferent
feedback mechanism, and we conclude that the performance of a cognitive task
suppresses the respiratory response to passive movement.
168
169
Cappo, B. M., and D. S. Holmes. The utility of prolonged respiratory exhalation for
reducing physiological and psychological arousal in non-threatening and threatening
situations. Journal of Psychosomatic Res., 1984, 28:265-273.
Chen, W., and W. Sun. Tai chi chuan: An alternative exercise for health promotion and
disease prevention for older adults in the community. International Quarterly of
Community Health Education, 1997, 16(4):333-339.
Cohen, J. A., D. H. Char, and D. Norman. Bilateral orbital varices associated with
habitual bending. Arch Ophthalmol, Nov 1995, 113(11):1360-1362.
Crick, F. H. C., and C. Koch. Consciousness and neuroscience. Cerebral Cortex, 1998,
8: 97107.
Cuthbert, Bruce N., Peter J. Lang, Cyd Strauss, David Drobes, Christopher Patrick
J., and Margaret M. Bradley. The psychophysiology of anxiety disorder: Fear memory
imagery. Psychophysiology, May 2003, 40(3):407-422. Peter Lang email:
[email protected].
Abstract: Psychophysiological
170
171
Engel, B. T., and R. A. Chism. Effect of increases and decreases in breathing rate on
heart rate and finger pulse volume. Psychophysiology, 1967, 4:83-89.
Frankeny, J. R., and R. G. Holly, and C. R. Ashmore. Effects of graded duration of
stretch on normal and dystrophic skeletal muscle. Muscle & Nerve, 1983, 6:269-277.
Giardino, Nicholas D., Robb W. Glenny, Soo Borson, and Leighton Chan.
Respiratory sinus arrhythmia is associated with efficiency of pulmonary gas exchange in
healthy humans. American Journal of PhysiologyHeart and Circulation Physiology,
May 2003, 284(5):H1585-H1591
Abstract: Respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) may be associated with improved
efficiency of pulmonary gas exchange by matching ventilation to perfusion within each
respiratory cycle. Respiration rate, tidal volume, minute ventilation (E), exhaled carbon
dioxide (CO2), oxygen consumption (O2), and heart rate were measured in 10 healthy
human volunteers during paced breathing to test the hypothesis that RSA contributes to
pulmonary gas exchange efficiency. Cross-spectral analysis of heart rate and respiration
was computed to calculate RSA and the coherence and phase between these variables.
Pulmonary gas exchange efficiency was measured as the average ventilatory equivalent
of CO2 (E/CO2) and O2 (E/O2). Across subjects and paced breathing periods, RSA was
significantly associated with CO2 (partial r = 0.53, P = 0.002) and O2 (partial r = 0.49, P
= 0.005) exchange efficiency after controlling for the effects of age, respiration rate, tidal
volume, and average heart rate. Phase between heart rate and respiration was significantly
associated with CO2 exchange efficiency (partial r = 0.40, P = 0.03). These results are
consistent with previous studies and further support the theory that RSA may improve the
efficiency of pulmonary gas exchange.
Green, Judith, and Robert Shellenberger. The subtle energy of love. Subtle Energies
and Energy Medicine Journal, 1993, 4(1).
Abstract: That love promotes health surprises few people and yet from the perspective of
poets, philosophers and healers who for millennia have understood the powers of love,
the scientific study of love and physical health is in its infancy. Because love has many
facets and is manifested in many ways as described here, it was banned from Western
Science that insisted upon observable and simple independent variables. In this article we
present data indicating the salutary effects of love on physical health; these data are from
several areas--psychology, sociology, medicine, epidemiology and healing--and together
form a foundation for understanding and enhancing love and its effects. Three processes
are formulated to explain the health promoting effects of love--psychophysiologic,
psychophysical and psychosocial/behavioral. Love is described as an energy by virtue of
its capacity to produce effects; it is subtle, not because its effects are subtle, but because it
has been ineffable to science.
Halmek, Josef, Tom Kra, Pavel Jurk, Miroslav Sou ek, Darrel P. Francis, L.
Ceri Davies, Win K. Shen, Andrew J. S. Coats, Miroslav Novk, Zuzana Novkov,
Roman Panovsk , Ji Toman, Josef umbera, and Virend K. Somers. Variability of
172
phase shift between blood pressure and heart rate fluctuations: A marker of short-term
circulation control. Circulation, 2003, 108:292. Author email: [email protected].
Abstract: Background: We postulated that the variability of the phase shift between blood
pressure and heart rate fluctuation near the frequency of 0.10 Hz might be useful in
assessing autonomic circulatory control. Methods and Results: We tested this hypothesis
in 4 groups of subjects: 28 young, healthy individuals; 13 elderly healthy individuals; 25
patients with coronary heart disease; and 19 patients with a planned or implanted
cardioverter-defibrillator (ICD recipients). Data from 5 minutes of free breathing and at 2
different, controlled breathing frequencies (0.10 and 0.33 Hz) were used. Clear
differences (P<0.001) in variability of phase were evident between the ICD recipients and
all other groups. Furthermore, at a breathing frequency of 0.10 Hz, differences in
baroreflex sensitivity (P<0.01) also became evident, even though these differences were
not apparent at the 0.33-Hz breathing frequency. Conclusions: The frequency of 0.10 Hz
represents a useful and potentially important one for controlled breathing, at which
differences in blood pressureRR interactions become evident. These interactions,
whether computed as a variability of phase to define stability of the blood pressureheart
rate interaction or defined as the baroreflex sensitivity to define the gain in heart rate
response to blood pressure changes, are significantly different in patients at risk for
sudden arrhythmic death. In young versus older healthy individuals, only baroreflex gain
is different, with the variability of phase being similar in both groups. These
measurements of short-term circulatory control might help in risk stratification for sudden
cardiac death.
Harman, Willis. Two liberating concepts for research on consciousness. IONS Noetic
Sciences Review, Spring 1993, no. 25. Article available online:
http://www.noetic.org/Ions/publications/review_archives/25/issue25_14.html.
How do we integrate consciousness into scientific research? And how does science
account for subjective experience?
Hong, Y., J. X. Li, and P. D. Robinson. Balance control, flexibility, and
cardiorespiratory fitness among older tai chi practitioners. British Journal of Sports
Medicine, 2000; 34(1):29-34.
Howlett, T. A. Hormonal responses to exercise training: A short review. Clinical
Endocrinology, 1987, 26:723-742.
International Society for the Advancement of Respiratory Psychophysiology. See
website: http://cscwww.cats.ohiou.edu/isarp/.
Jacobson, E. Variation of blood pressure with skeletal muscle tension and relaxation.
Annals of Internal Medicine, 1939, 12:1194-1212.
Jennen, Christiane, and Gerhard Uhlenbruck. Exercise and life-satisfactory- fitness:
Complementary strategies in the prevention and rehabilitation of illnesses. Evidence-
173
174
175
176
reported on and discussed. From these results, one sees that individuals can intentionally
affect their autonomic nervous system bala nce, and thus, their HRV.
Mills, N., and A. Heywood. The Health Benefits of Tai Chi and Chikung: A Review of
the Evidence (a resource for teachers of tai chi, provided by The Forum). Cardiff, U.K.:
Whitechurch Hospital, Department of Psychology, 2002.
Muehsam, David J., M. S. Markham, Patricia A. Muehsam, Arthur A. Pilla, Ronger
Shen, and Yi Wu. Effects of Qigong on cell- free myosin phosphorylation: Preliminary
experiments. Subtle Energies and Energy Medicine Journal, 1994, 5(1).
Abstract: This work exa mines the effect of Qigong from two experienced practitioners on
in vitro cell- free myosin phosphorylation. This system has a demonstrated sensitivity to
variations in static magnetic fields above and below ambient values. The results show
that both Qigong practitioners were able to consistently yield results similar to those
observed for variations in applied magnetic fields near the ambient level. Qigong
treatment with the myosin reaction mixture in the ambient magnetic field reduced
phosphorylation in each experiment by an average of approximately 15% (p < .05). For
trials with the myosin samples in a magnetic shield, two of four (one for each
practitioner) Qi treatments yielded significant reductions in phosphorylation (average
approximately 10%, p < .05). These effects were somewhat lower and not as consistent as
those obtained under ambient field conditions. The results obtained in this study
demonstrate that Qigong practice can consistently affect a biologically relevant enzyme
system, requiring no physical contact between the practitioner and the sample. The
mechanisms of the Qigong effects observed in this study are as yet unclear.
Nagler, W. Vertebral artery obstruction by hyperextension of the neck. Arch Phys Med
Rehabil, 1973, 54:237-240.
Paul, G. L. Physiological effects of relaxation training and hypnotic suggestion. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 1969, 74:425-437.
Pryse-Phillips, W. Infarction of the medulla and cervical cord after fitness exercises.
Stroke, 1989, 24(6):355-359.
Raman, Dr. Krishna. [Physiology of] asanas; [Physiology of] pranayama. In Dr.
Krishna Raman, A Matter of Health: Integration of Yoga & Western Medicine for
Prevention & Cure. Chennai (Madras), India: Eastwest Books, 1998, pp. 304-336; 337342.
Recordati, G., and T. G. Bellini. A definition of internal constancy and homeostasis in
the context of non-equilibrium thermodynamics. Exercise Physiology, 89(1):27-38.
Author email: [email protected].
Abstract: The constancy of the internal environment, internal homeostasis, and its
stability are necessary conditions for the survival of a biological system within its
177
environment. These have never been clearly defined. For this purpose nonequilibrium
thermodynamics is taken as a reference, and the essential principles of equilibrium,
reversibility, stationary steady state and stability (Lyapounov, asymptotic, local and
global), are briefly illustrated. On this basis, internal homeostasis describes a stationary
state of nonequilibrium, the actual state of rest, X(t), resulting from the relation X(t) =
XS+ x(t), between a time-independent steady state of reference (XS), and time-dependent
fluctuations of the state variables, x(t). In humans, two resting spontaneous homeostatic
states are: (1) the conscious state of quiet wakefulness, during which time-dependent
variables display bounded oscillations around the mean time- independent steady state
level, this conscious state being thus stable in the sense of Lyapounov, and (2) the
unconscious stable state of non-rapid eye movement sleep, in which the time-dependent
variables would approach the lowest spontaneously attainable time- independent state
asymptotically, sleep becoming a globally stable and attractive state. Exercise may be
described as a non-resting, unstable active state far away from equilibrium and
hibernation is a resting, time-independent steady state very near equilibrium. The range
between sleep and exercise is neurohumorally regulated. For spontaneously stable states
to occur, slowing of the metabolic rate, withdrawal of the sympathetic drive and
reinforcement of the vagal tone to the heart and circulation are required, thus confirming
that the parasympathetic division of the autonomic nervous system is the main controller
of homeostasis.
Roitbak, A. I. [Variation of reaction time as a function of respiration phase: Possible
causes of this phenomenon.] Studii si Certari de Neurologie, 1960, 5:549-556. [In
Russian.]
Sakakibara, M., and J. Hayano. Effect of slowed respiration on cardiac
parasympathetic response to threat. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1996, 58:32-37.
___________, S. Takeuchi, and J. Hayano. Effects of relaxation training on cardiac
parasympathetic tone. Psychophysiology, 1994, 31:223-228. PMID: 8008785.
Abstract: To examine the hypothesis that the relaxation response is associated with an
increase in cardiac parasympathetic tone, the frequency components of heart rate
variability during relaxation training were investigated in 16 college students.
Electrocardiograms and pneumograms were recorded during a 5- min baseline period
followed by three successive 5- min sessions of the autogenic training (relaxation) or by
the same periods of quiet rest (control), while subjects breathed synchronously with a
visual pacemaker (0.25 Hz). Although neither the magnitude nor the frequency of
respiration showed a significant difference between relaxation and control, the amplitude
of the high- frequency component of heart rate variability increased only during relaxation
(p = .008). There was no significant difference in the ratio of the low-frequency (0.040.15 Hz) to the high- frequency amplitudes. The increased high- frequency amplitude
without changes in the respiratory parameters indicates enhanced cardiac
parasympathetic tone. Thus, our results support the initial hypothesis of this study.
Enhanced cardiac parasympathetic tone may explain an important mechanism underlying
the beneficial effect of the relaxation response.
178
179
180
181
182
Abstract: Scalp surface field potential changes (EEG) have long been associated with
activities of neuronal processes in the brain. In spite of much knowledge, success in
identifying EEG signatures which represent specific brain activation states has been
limited. Using on line fast-fourier transforms of brainwave signals and computer displays
with one-second updates of 5, 7, 10, 12, 14, 16, 20 and 28 Hz with 1 Hz bandwidth
specific brainwave signatures were identified for attentional, cognitive, imaginal, and
somatosensory states. The data bring into question the functional utility ascribed to the
commonly used brainwave frequency bands designated as Delta, Theta, Alpha, and Beta.
The quest for the neuroanatomic substrate of conscious intention is addressed within the
context of brain based parking spots and how they pertain to the invisible and
quantifyingly problematic thing called mind.
Timmons, B., J. Salamy, J. Kamiya, and D. Girtan. Abdominal-thoracic respiratory
movements and levels of arousal. Psychonomic Science, 1972, 27(3):173-175.
Waite, P. J., S. Hawks, and J. A. Gast. The correlation between spiritual well-being and
health behaviors. American Journal of Health Promotion, 1999, 13:159-162.
Wallace, R. K., H. Benson, and A. F. Wilson. A wakeful hypometabolic physiologic
state. American Journal of Physiology, 1971, 221(3):795-799.
Wang, Chenchen, Jean Paul Collet, and Joseph Lau. The effect of Tai Chi on health
outcomes in patients with chronic conditions: A systematic review. Archives of Internal
Medicine, 8 Mar 2004, 164(5)493-501.
Abstract: Objective: To conduct a systematic review of reports on the physical and
psychological effects of Tai Chi on various chronic medical conditions. Data Sources:
Search of 11 computerized English and Chinese databases. Study Selection: Randomized
controlled trials, nonrandomized controlled studies, and observational studies published
in English or Chinese. Data Extraction: Data were extracted for the study objective,
population characteristics, study setting, type of Tai Chi intervention, study design,
outcome assessment, duration of follow-up, and key results. Data Synthesis: There were
9 randomized controlled trials, 23 nonrandomized controlled studies, and 15
observational studies in this review. Benefits were reported in balance and strength,
cardiovascular and respiratory function, flexibility, immune system, symptoms of
arthritis, muscular strength, and psychological effects. Conclusions: Tai Chi appears to
have physiological and psychosocial benefits and also appears to be safe and effective in
promoting balance control, flexibility, and cardiovascular fitness in older patients with
chronic conditions. However, limitations or biases exist in most studies, and it is difficult
to draw firm conclusions about the benefits reported. Most indications in which Tai Chi
was applied lack a theoretical foundation concerning the mechanism of benefit. Welldesigned studies are needed.
Winkelman, Michael. Physiological and therapeutic aspects of shamanistic healing.
Subtle Energies and Energy Medicine Journal, 1990, 1(2).
183
Abstract: This paper addresses the psychophysiological and therapeutic bases of some
universal aspects of shamanistic healing practices. Shamanistic healing is universal
because of the interrelated physiological, experiential and therapeutic bases in the use of
altered states of consciousness (ASC). Case studies of cross-cultural therapeutic use of
ASC illustrate cultural effectiveness. The scientific bases of therapeutic effectiveness are
addressed through an examination of the psychophysiology of ASC. Clinical and
physiological evidence for the therapeutic effectiveness of ASC in general and
hallucinogens in particular is reviewed. Possession and the temporal lobe syndrome are
briefly examined in the context of the psychophysiology of ASC.
Wirth, D. P., and J. R. Gram. The psychophysiology of nontraditional prayer.
International Journal of Psychosomatics, 1994, 41(1-4):68-75.
Examined the effect of distant prayer upon autonomic and CNS parameters.
Xu, S. H. Psychophysiological reactions associated with qigong therapy. Chinese
Medical Journal (English), Mar 1994, 107(3):230-233. PMID: 8088187.
Abstract: Qigong as a part of the traditional Chinese medicine is similar to western
meditation, Indian Yoga or Japanese Zen, which can all be included in the
category of traditional psychotherapy. A series of physiological and psychological effects
occur in the course of Qigong training, but inappropriate training can lead to physical and
mental disturbances. Physiological effects include changes in EEG, EMG, respiratory
movement, heart rate, skin potential, skin temperature and finger tip volume, sympathetic
nerve function, function in stomach and intestine, metabolism, endocrine and immunity
systems. Psychological effects are motor phenomena and perceptual changes: patients
experienced warmness, chilliness, itching sensation in the skin, numbness, soreness,
bloatedness, relaxation, tenseness, floating, dropping, enlargement or constriction of the
body image, a sensation of rising to the sky, falling off, standing upside down, playing on
the swing following respiration, circulation of the intrinsic Qi, electric shock,
formication, during Qigong exercise. Some patients experienced dreamland illusions,
unreality and pseudohallucination. These phenomena were transient and vanished as the
exercise terminated. Qigong deviation syndrome has become a diagnostic term and is
now used widely in China.
Zhang, J. Z., J. Zhao, and Q. N. He. EEG findings during special psychical state (Qi
Gong state) by means of compressed spectral array and topographic mapping. Computers
in Biology and Medicine, 1988, 18(6):455-463. PMID: 3060312
Abstract: Wallace first reported the changes in EEG during transcendental mediation [6].
Banquet [1] observed, on the basis of spectral analysis of the EEG, that the mediation
state was a unique state of consciousness, and separate from wakefulness, drowsiness or
sleep. The Qi Gong of China is not the same as either transcendental mediation or the
Yoga Gong. The EEG during Qi Gong state is clearly different from those recorded
during the resting state. The changes in the EEG during the Qi Gong have not been
reported previously. The EEG alpha activity during the Qi Gong state occurs
184
predominantly in the anterior regions. The peak frequency of EEG alpha rhythm is slower
than the resting state. The change of EEG during Qi Gong between anterior and posterior
half is negative correlation. These changes are statistically significant.
Ongoing Research
Mark Malay
San Diego, CA
[email protected]
Conducting research for his Ph.D. on: Do measurable physiological changes occur for
Hatha-Yoga asanas, and are those changes correlated to hypnotic ability? Contacted
IAYT 4/1/02.
Steve Ragsdale, Ph.D.
Professor, Department of Biochemistry
Beadle Center
University of Nebraska
[email protected]
402-472-2943
Is exploring the relationship between biochemistry, the anatomy of the nervous system,
and the cakras. Contacted IAYT in 2000.
Philip Stevens, BSc (Psych, Physiol), BSc (hons) (Physiol)
Ph.D. candidate in collaboration with the department of Complementary Medicine in the
School of Health Sciences and the School of Electrical and Computer Engineering
RMIT University
Email: [email protected]
Conducting the following research project:
Electrophysiological Correlates of Yoga Breathing Techniques
Various styles of breathing are promoted as beneficial. Inhalation and exhalation
methods often differ widely, however, with various combinations of abdominal
&/or thoracic dominance being favored. Different rhythms of breathing, from
hyperventilation to very-slow-rate, are used, often with various esoteric and/or
medical claims. Validation studies are few, particularly comparative studies of
more than one technique. While certain, specific breathing practices have been
studied so far that show differential effects on brain laterality and cardiac
autonomic variations, no study to-date has described and cross-correlated the
effects of the all the main, breathing techniques commonly promoted in yoga
classes today.
This project seeks to:
1. catalogue and describe the various breathing techniques available today using standard,
anatomical, physiological, psychological and medical-terminologies.
185
186
If you have any queries regarding this project, want more information, or wish to be
considered for the experimental trials, please contact me on 0419 806 066 or
[email protected]
187