Salvadoran Spanish - S - Aspiration & Deletion
Salvadoran Spanish - S - Aspiration & Deletion
Salvadoran Spanish - S - Aspiration & Deletion
in a Bilingual Context1
Michol F. Hoffman
1 Introduction
One of the best known features of Peninsular and Latin American Spanish
is the aspiration and deletion of syllable- and word-final /-s/. This variable
has been documented as early as the sixteenth century (Lipski 1984) and
has been investigated in many varieties of Spanish (Cedergen, Poplack,
Lipski). This variable is of interest for its social as well as linguistic conditioning. It appears in monomorphemes, e.g. menos 'less', as well as an
inflectional marker indicating person for verbs, and serves as the plural
marker, e.g., casas 'houses'. It is thus subject to a variety of phonological,
morphological, and syntactic constraints. The aspiration and deletion of
1-s/ is a feature of the Spanish ofEl Salvador, a lesser-studied variety.
This paper provides a preliminary investigation of this variable in the
speech of Spanish/English bilingual youth from El Salvador residing in
Toronto, Canada. The paper begins with a discussion of the frequency of
aspiration and deletion in three speech styles. It then offers a varbrul
analysis of social factors predicting aspiration and deletion and continues
with a Varbrul analysis of linguistic factors predicting aspiration and deletion.
1.1 The Spanish-speaking Population in Toronto
Numerous linguistic studies have examined United States Spanish as well
as issues of language contact and maintenance within Spanish-speaking
communities. That Canada, and Toronto in particular, are home to large
and vibrant Spanish speaking communities, is less well-known. Toronto's
Spanish speaking population has many countries of origin. The 1996 Census of Canada reports approximately 116,500 residents of Latin American
ethnicity in Toronto, although members of the community say that this is
a low estimate and that the number is closer to 200,000.
Though Spanish speaking Latin American immigration to Canada can
be traced from the mid-twentieth century, it expanded exponentially dur1
Many thanks to Greg Guy for his advice and support in the preparation of this paper.
U. Penn Working Papers in Linguistics, Volume 7.3 (2001)
116
MICHOL HOFFMAN
ing the early 1970s. Thousands of Chileans fleeing the military coup obtained landed immigrant status and are still a great presence within the
Spanish speaking community. Beginning in the 1970's, improved Canadian immigration policies facilitated the arrival of many immigrants from
Colombia and Ecuador. The early 1980's brought large-scale immigration
from Central America, due to the adverse socio-political conditions in El
Salvador, Nicaragua, and Guatemala. The escalation of civil war and its
consequences in El Salvador forced thousands to flee to Canada, mostly
as political and economic refugees. An estimated 34,000 Salvadorans
came to Toronto as immigrants from the eighties through 1991. Since then
the number has continued to grow. The more recent immigrants are motivated more by economic hardship and the resulting adverse social conditions than by political persecution.
117
Latino. They also report code switching, or speaking what many of them
call "Spanglish" with friends who speak both Spanish and English.
1.3 Salvadoran Spanish
118
All Speakers
MICHOL HOFFMAN
Conversation
42%
52%
34%
10%
4%
4%
1%
51%
40%
28%
10%
5%
4%
1%
1%
6%
6%
8%
2%
2%
3%
Men
Women
Age of Arrival
11-15
6-10
<5
Duration of StllX
33%
12-18
44%
9-10
48%
3-6 years
3 Social Factors
The social factors I considered for the analysis of /-s/ weakening were sex,
age of arrival in Canada, duration of stay in Canada, and parents' level of
education in El Salvador. As indicated in Table 1, men weaken /-s/ more
than women in all three styles, as do speakers with the later ages of arrival
and shorter durations of stay. This points to the hypothesis that speakers
who have lived in Canada the longest are most likely to retain word and
syllable final/-s/.
For the most part, the Varbrul analysis of these social factors reflects
the raw statistics. For this analysis, I combined the two shortest duration
factors (3-6 years and 9-10 years}, leaving two factors: speakers who have
been in Canada for 3-10 years, and for 12-18 years.
Results in Table 2 reveal that men are more likely to weaken /-s/ than
women, with factor weights of .57 for men and .42 for women. This is
consistent with other studies of 1-s/ deletion and aspiration and suggests
that women are less likely to use the more stigmatized variants. What
could account for this result? This gender difference could be reflective of
the expectations of women's behavior within the Salvadoran community:
speakers mentioned that women should be seen to be "proper" and "modest". This discrepancy could also reflect the women's desire to distance
themselves from the negative stereotype associated with young Salvadoran men. These youth, often associated with gangs, have a bad reputation within the community and in Toronto in general.
119
Sg
Men
.58
2267
.42
Women
1635
Age of arrival
11-15
.56
2097
7-10
.52
1221
.42
1-5 years
584
Duration of stay
3-10
.52
3052
12-18
.48
850
Parents' level Qf educatiQn in El Salvador
(significant for deletion only)
Elementary
.52
374
High school
.50
474
Post-secondary .48
708
52
34
51
40
28
46
33
19
17
16
120
MICHOL HOFFMAN
121
4 Linguistic Factors
Investigations of 1-sl aspiration and deletion have considered variety of
phonological, morphological, and syntactic constraints. With a view to
providing an exhaustive examination of linguistic factors affecting this
variable, the preliminary analysis coded for approximately ten constraints.
Many were found to be not significant and were discarded. These included
following syllable stress and plural 1-sl type, as well as the number of
syllables in the word and a factor group designed to measure functional
effect of disambiguating information in the discourse.
This paper will discuss four significant linguistic constraints governing word final 1-sl deletion: the phonological factors 'following segment'
and 'current syllable stress', for which I shall present calculation results
both for all 1-sl weakening and for deletion; the factors of morphological
category and position of plural within the noun phrase, for which I shall
discuss results for deletion only. 2
4.1 Phonological Constraints
Previous studies have exhibited differing results with regard to the effect
of following segment on aspiration and deletion. In most varieties of
Spanish, aspiration and deletion are most frequent in pre-consonantal position as compared to following vowels and pause (Mason 1994: 153).
Lipski's (1985) Salvadoran data show high rates of aspiration and deletion
before consonants (68%) and before vowels (58%), but a much lower
weakening rate before pause (15%) (144). He provides no break down of
consonant features.
The following segment factor group initially consisted of individual
consonants. After testing for significance, I combined like-featured factors
into the following groups: obstruents except for It/, sonorants, vowels,
pause, and It/, which was kept separate as it appeared to have a distinct
effect on 1-sl on weakening. Varbrul calculations shown in Table 3 reveal
that for 1-sl weakening or combined aspiration and deletion, obstruents
(both voiced and voiceless) other than It/, and sonorants promote aspiration and deletion. In contrast, following It/ inhibits strongly. The effect of
following It/ on aspiration, promoting 1-sl retention, is expected as It/ and
lsi are homorganic and maximally similar. The aspiration qualities associ2
This paper discusses 1-s/ aspiration and deletion in word- final position only.
Phonological constraints on word internal aspiration and deletion in this corpus
were similar.
122
MICHOL HOFFMAN
ated with /tl are acoustically or phonetically associated with /s/. In articulatory terms, the tongue has to pass through an /sf-like position on its way
to articulating /tl. This is not the case for other consonants. Following
vowels promote aspiration and deletion, albeit very weakly. Following
pause exhibits a strong effect discouraging all weakening.
Probability
Following Segment
.71
obstruents
sonorants
.74
vowels
.51
.28
pause
Cun:ent S~llable Stre~s
.45
stressed
.55
unstressed
1346
724
819
297
63
67
44
23
908
2348
42
51
Table 3: Phonological Factors Predicting Word Final/-s/ Weakening (Aspiration and Deletion Combined)
The calculations for deletion only (Table 4), reveal a definite voicing
effect, with voiced obstruents (.72) and sonorants (.65) encouraging deletion, and all voiceless obstruents discouraging with a factor weight of .33.
Guy (1996) found the same result for following consonant voicing on 1-sl
deletion in Buenos Aires Spanish. This difference in obstruent voicing
between alli-s/ weakening and /-s/ deletion only is due to the general effect of voiceless obstruents on aspiration: the non-coronal voiceless obstruents promote aspiration and this effect is absent when we consider
deletion alone. Following vowels and following pause discourage deletion
as they did for combined aspiration and deletion.
The effect of following vowel for deletion only is consistent with
other studies of deletion in other varieties of Spanish (e.g., Poplack 1980,
Guy 1996), but the results for following pause are generally inconsistent
(cf. Poplack 1980, Terrell 1975). Lipski's Salvadoran data show fewer
instances of weakening before pause, but more weakening before vowels
than in my sample.
With regard to syllable stress, For both aspiration and deletion combined, and for deletion alone, current stressed syllables discourage deletion and unstressed syllables encourage deletion. This is consistent with
the general findings that stressed syllables are more resistant to reduction
than unstressed syllables. Following syllable stress, a significant con-
123
straint in the study of 1-s/ deletion for other varieties of Spanish, was
found not significant in this sample. 3
Probability
%
N
Following Segment
voiceless obstruents .33
216
13
voiced obstruents
.72
306
42
sonorants
.65
389
36
vowels
.40
319
17
pause
.39
206
16
Current Syllable Stress
stressed
.40
318
15
unstressed
.60
1118
24
Table 4: Phonological Factors predicting Word Final /-s/ Deletion
Although not significant, following stressed syllable and unstressed syllable discouraged aspiration and deletion weakly in contrast to other studies where following unstressed syllable favored deletion.
4
Nosotros did not produce an effect that lent itself to combination with other
factors.
5
This factor consisted mainly of the oblique pronoun les.
124
MICHOL HOFFMAN
%
Probability
N
Verbs
.44
333
23
Nominals6
.32
551
18
Predicate adjectives .50
43
31
Las!les as pronouns .61
64
45
.61
99
45
Quantifiers
Nosotros
.50
35
36
Monomorphemes
.32
173
13
Entonces, pues
.71
138
51
Table 5: Morphological Factors predicting Word Final /-s/ Deletion
4.3 Syntactic Constraints
With regard to the effect of position in the NP on deletion, or absence of
plural marker, one can discern a clear pattern: generally, deletion is more
likely farther to the right in the noun phrase. As reported in Table 6, positions 1 and 2 inhibit deletion with factor weights of .39 and .40, and positions 3 and 4 encourage it at .57 and .64. When we consider preceding
plural marker with position, words in second position preceded by a plural
maker are least likely to delete. This is true of words preceded by both
plural marked nominals as well as by numbers. In contrast, words in second position preceded by no plural marker favor deletion. Words in third
125
.39
.40
.57
482
173
41
9
22
18
29
32
482
121
52
12
21
17
22
14
40
20
28
53
Position in NP
1
2
3
4
.64
Position Preceded by Plural Marker
I
2 prec. by pl.
2 prec. by 0
3+4 prec. by pl.
3+4 prec. by mixed
3+4 prec.by 0
.35
.31
.61
.44
.50
.76
5 Discussion
These results raise several questions with regard to linguistic and social
constraints on 1-sl aspiration and deletion in Salvadoran and other varieties of Spanish. Lipski ( 1985) suggests that since the aspiration and deletion of /-sf is not as advanced as in other varieties, Salvadoran Spanish offers an opportunity to examine the variation in earlier stages and look at
the "spread of the weakening process" (148). With regard to phonological
constraints, several findings, such as the universal conservative effect of
current syllable stress in this study, confirm previously documented con-
I combined 3ro and 4'h positions because there were so few tokens for 4th position.
126
MICHOL HOFFMAN
straints on the variable. The import of others, e.g., the effect following
pause, has yet to be established.
Morphological category is another area that begs for further investigation. Guy (2000) provides evidence for multiple underlying representations for lexical exceptions. The data for lexical exceptions in this corpus
are a good fit for that model. There is also the question of functional motivation for the retention of the plural marker. Both inflectional categories,
nominals and verbs, discourage deletion. Nonetheless monomorphemes,
where discourse ambiguity is not at issue, show higher rates of deletion
and similar factor weights to the inflectional categories.
This paper provided evidence of the state of 1-sl aspiration and deletion in the Spanish of young Salvadoran immigrants to Toronto, Canada.
Of course, more research is needed to shore up these preliminary observations. More specifically, it would be revealing to expand this study to include young speakers of other /-s/ weakening varieties, as well as more
Salvadoran youth, more closely associated with groups perceived as socially marginal. The continued effect of duration of stay should also be
monitored. I predict that changes to this variable are largely dependent on
social factors and on the situation and status of Spanish spoken in Toronto. Only time and more research will tell.
References
Canfield, D. Lincoln. (1981). Spanish Pronunciation in the Americas. Chicago:
The University of Chicago
Cedergren, Henrietta. (1973 ). The Interplay of social and linguistic factors in Panama. Unpublished Ph.d dissertation, Cornell University.
Guy, Gregory R. (1996). The general and the particular: Constraints on /s/ deletion
in Argentine Spanish. Paper presented at NW AVE 25, Las Vegas.
Guy, Gregory R. (2000). Modelling Lexical Exceptions to Phonological Processes.
Paper presented at 4"' Bilingual Workshop on Theoretical Linguistics, Toronto.
Labov, William.(l994). Principles of Linguistic Change. Vol. 1: Internal Factors.
Oxford: Blackwell.
Lipski, John M.(l985). "lsi in Central American Spanish". Hispania 68. 143-49.
Mason, Keith W. (1994). Comerse las eses: a selective bibliographic survey of Is/
aspiration and deletion in dialects of Spanish. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation,
University of Michigan.
Poplack, Shana. ( 1980). "The notation of the plural in Puerto Rican Spanish: Competing restraints on Is/ deletion". in Locating language in time and space.
William Labov, ed 55-67. New York: Academic Press.
127
Terrell, Tracey. (1975) La interacci6n de Ia aspiraci6n y Ia elisi6n sobre Ia /s/ implosiva y fial en el espaiiol de Puerto Rico. Manuscript. Irvine: University of
California.
Terrell, Tracey. (1980). "Diachronic reconstruction by dialect comparison of variable constraints: s-. aspiration and deletion in Spanish". in Variation Omnibus.
David Sankoff and Henrietta Cedergren, eds. 115-125. Edmonton: Linguistic
Research.