Nose Amebeltp Temporal6 Final

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Borrowing temporal expressions in New

languages: a contrastive study of loanwords

Guinea

Masahiko NOSE
Keywords: Amele, Bel, Tok Pisin, temporal expressions, loanwords
1. Introduction
This study considers the loanwords of three languages in the Madang Province, Papua New
Guinea: Amele, Bel, and Tok Pisin1. The study aims to analyze the contrasts between Amele, Bel, and
Tok Pisin with regard to temporal words. Grammatically speaking, there are different and various
means of expressing time. They are realized as tense, aspect, mood forms, and several temporal
markings (primarily temporal adverbials), such as on Sunday, before noon, after breakfast. The terms
on, before, and after are grammatical means of delimiting temporal moments and relations.
Furthermore, there are a number of lexical temporal words: morning, night, yesterday, Christmas, New
Year and so on. This study focuses on lexical temporal words, and as such, it was determined that
these lexical words are likely to have been borrowed from other languages (cf. Alverson 1994, Evans
2004, Wierzbicka 1993).
If one language lacks an appropriate word for a certain concept or thing, its speakers have several
options. One is to create a new word to express or rephrase it. Another is to borrow a word from a
foreign language. Especially, temporal words can be classified as either traditional or modern words
(cf. Sinha et al. 2011). Traditional words have been used since a long time in the language, such as
morning, night, and today. They are considered to be rather basic words (cf. Swadeshs list) that are
hard to borrow and unlikely to disappear. In contrast, modern, temporal-clock, or calendar-based
words, such as January, oclock, and minute, are easier to borrow.
In this study, section 2 presents general information about the three languages and explores several
previous studies on temporal expressions. Section 3 presents the data gathered for this study, and
section 4 presents a discussion of the data in terms of loanword typology and language contact.
Section 5 is the conclusion of the study.
2. Loanwords and temporal expressions
This section outlines several preliminary studies on loanwords and temporal expressions that are
associated with language contacts. Because of language contacts, morpheme orders or constituent
orders are borrowed and lexically, specific words are borrowedso called loanwords. Temporal words
are classified in the lexicon and they can be borrowed via language contacts. Thus, this section
introduces language contacts and outlines several previous studies on loanwords.
2.1. Language contacts
There are around 800 indigenous languages spoken in Papua New Guinea and its neighboring
areas. These languages are diverse in Austronesian, Trans-New Guinea, and other genera; and they
affect each other in the form of long and various contacts (Nose 2014). As a result of the contacts, one
language borrows grammatical structures from other language(s), while another language borrows
lexical words from other language(s). According to Thomason (2001:66-67), there are several kinds of
contacts in proportion to the duration of the contact period, as shown in (1)
(1) Borrowing scale (Thomason (2001:66-67):
1

Tok Pisin is an English-based creole and one of the official languages in Papua New Guinea. In fact, Tok
Pisin is functioning as a lingua franca in the field.

1. Casual contact: lexicon, only content words, no grammatical structure


2. Slightly more intense contact: borrowing function words and content words
3. More intense contact: basic and non-basic vocabulary
4. Intense contact: heavy lexical borrowing, structural borrowings in phonology, syntax, and
morphology
Given the contact situations of Madang suburban area (see Figure 1.), there are intense contacts among
the three languages. First is the contact between Amele and Bel, while second are the contacts between
Tok Pisin and Bel, and Tok Pisin and Amele, respectively (cf. Roberts 1987, 1992). The first is a
contact between New Guinea language and Austronesian language. They come into effect in
grammatical structures and borrow lexicons from each other (cf. Ross 2001, 2009). The second contact
occurs between Tok Pisin and each of the other native languages, with the native languages borrowing
grammar and lexicon from Tok Pisin. Tok Pisin, on the other hand, borrows lexicon mostly from
English, along with some words from other indigenous languages (cf. Muysken 2008, Nose 2014,
Ross 2009)2.
Moreover, McMahon (1994: 200-201) describes lexical borrowing and structural borrowing by
stating Bloomfield calls this cultural borrowingone group of speakers borrows an object or concept
from another, and its name tends to come along too. After all, using ready-made designations is more
economical than describing things afresh. In particular, McMahon (1994:204) emphasized that basic
vocabulary (including words for body parts, weather, universal experiences like birth and death,
natural phenomena like rivers and mountains, and small numerals) is infrequently affected.
2.2. Loanword typology project: Haspelmath and Tadmor (2009)
Haspelmath and Tadmor (2009) conducted cross-linguistic research on loanwords in 41 languages.
Their Loanword typology project attempted to clarify universals or tendencies of loanwords and
characteristics of source and target languages. Haspelmath and Tadmor (2009) classified 24 kinds of
loanwords, such as The physical world, Kinship, Animals, The body, Food and drink, Clothing and
grooming, Agriculture and vegetation, Basic actions and technology, Motion, Possession, Spatial
relations, Quantity, Time, Sense perception, Emotions and values, Cognition, Speech and language,
Social and political relations, Warfare and hunting, Law, The Modern world, Miscellaneous function
words, with each contributor examining each language3.
As for Papua New Guinea, Ross (2009) examined the loanwords of Takia, an Austronesian
language spoken on Karkar Island, Madang Province. Ross (2009:759-760) pointed out that a great
mass of words were borrowed from Tok Pisin and a few words from neighboring languages Bargam,
Bel and Waskia. For example, 98 % of modern-world words in Takia are based on Tok Pisin (there has
been a huge input from Tok Pisin since 1930 [Ross 2009: 764765]). As for time and temporal words,
borrowing rates are 23.4 %, specifically 21.3% from Tok Pisin and 2.4% from Bel. Moreover, Ross
(2009:764) claimed that the following grammatical features in Takia were borrowed from neighboring
Trans-New Guinea languages: verb-final, postpositional, and clause-chaining. These features are not
2

Some researchers pointed out that there is a mixed language as a result of language contacts. Specifically,
Media Lengua (derived from Spanish and Quichua) in the Caribbean area and Maa (derived from Cushitic
and Bantu) in Africa are famous examples (Muysken 2008). As far as the authors observation is concerned,
any clue of mixed feature has not been found in Madang suburban area.
3 Moreover, Haspelmath and Tadmors (2009) work has shown that English's dominance as a word
exporter is a sign of its importance as an international tongue, and that 42% of English words are
loanwords; nevertheless, some countries try to resist word borrowing, in an attempt to maintain the national
character of their languages. In: Database of loans shows that English is the World's top borrower and
lender of words
(http://www.popsci.com/article/science/database-loans-shows-english-worlds-top-borrower-and-lender-wor
ds); Access: 28. 07. 2014

usually observed in Austronesian languages.


2.3. Language contacts in North West New Guinea
This section describes in detail the situations of language contacts in the Northwest New Guinea
area. Language contacts exist among Austronesian and New-Guinea languages and Tok Pisin. One
important study of language contact in that area has been conducted, and Ross (2001:139, 152)
discussed metatypy phenomena in Karkar Island, explaining that lexical borrowing is independent of
syntactic borrowing, and Takia (Austronesian) and Waskia (New-Guinea) borrowed
morhpo-syntactically very much from each other. Nevertheless, their speakers avoid lexical
borrowings.
(2) Example of metatypy: (Ross 2001)
Takia
Kai sa-n ab
Kai classifier-his house
Waskia
Kai ko kawam
Kai postposition house
Kais house
In (2), word formations are the same in both Takia and Waskia; however, each language has different
grammatical morphemes and lexical words. Ross (2001) explained this kind of syntactic borrowings as
induced by language contact4.
3. Approach to temporal loanwords
This section illustrates the data derived for this study. The study focused on time loanwords (56
words) specified by Haspelmath & Tadmors (2009) loanword typology, and examined the time words
of the three languages Amele, Bel, and Tok Pisin.
First, the purpose of this study is to clarify systematic descriptions of loanwords of time in
Northwest New Guinea Island. By contrasting each time word from Amele, Bel, and Tok Pisin, this
study explores grammatical and lexical aspects of contact-induced change. In particular, this study
attempts to explain why Amele and Bel borrow or exchange temporal words, with consideration of a
systematic explanation of these. This study is based on fieldwork and on-the-spot questionnaires
conducted in Papua New Guinea. Additionally, this study utilizes the descriptive grammar of other
previous studies (cf. Davies 1989; Dempwolff n.d.; Freyberg 1977; Mager 1952; Roberts 1987, 1992;
Ross 2009; and Nose 2014)5.
3.1. A contrastive study among Amele, Bel, and Tok Pisin
This section illustrates the results of temporal words and summarizes the findings from the three
languages. In particular, time words from Haspelmath and Tadmors 2009 list were chosen for this
study. The time list consists of 56 words such as nouns: time, age, hour; adjectives: young, old, fast,
slow; verbs: to hurry, to be late; and calendars: Sunday, winter (see Appendix 1). Figure 1 below is a
map of New Guinea Island, consisting of both the western and eastern parts. Its western part belongs
to Indonesia, known as Iriyan Jaya. In contrast, the eastern part of the island and a number of
peripheral islands are part of Papua New Guinea. Madang Province is situated in the northwest of New
Guinea Island and the capital of the Province is Madang. There are several native languages spoken in
and around Madang town (see the square area in Figure 1). They are Amele, Bel, and Waskia; and
Some native speakers said that Bel and Takia are both Austronesian languages, with no grammatical
difference between them (according to the authors observation).
5
The authors field research was conducted in 20112013, Sein village, and Madang city (cf. Nose 2014).
4

Takia of Karkar Island. People in Madang communicate with each other by using Tok Pisin, one of the
official languages of Papua New Guinea.

Bel

Figure 1: Geographical map of the sample and other languages in Madang Province
First, Amele is a Trans-New Guinea language, spoken in the southern region of Madang city.
Amele speakers live in the bushland area and mainly cultivate yams and sell betel nuts. Amele
speakers are one of the biggest tribes in Madang city, together with Bel and Nobnobo (Roberts 1987,
Nose 2014). Second, Bel is an Austronesian language, spoken in and around Madang city, particularly
in the coastal area and on some small islands. Bel has several alternate names: Graged, Gedaged, Siar,
Tiara, and so on. Within the scope of this study, Bel and Bilbil are practically the same language
(Dempwolff n.d.). Third, Tok Pisin is an English-based creole, widely spoken in Papua New Guinea.
Tok Pisin has English, German, Malay, and other Austronesian languages within its lexicon and
pidginized grammar, similar in part to English grammar.
Questionnaire
This study examined 56 time words including nouns, verbs, and adjectives, based on Haspelmath
and Tadmor (2009) (see Appendix 1). I sought the corresponding words from Amele and Bel speakers,
and the Amele speaker provided the corresponding words in Tok Pisin as well. The main task was
conducted in Sein village, an Amele-speaking area, in 2012. Several Bel speakers were living there,
and Bel data was confirmed with another Bel speaker living in Madang town. Additionally, I sought
out several sentences with temporal words in each language.
Results and Findings in Appendix 1
The 56 temporal words from each language are illustrated in Appendix 1. First, modern words such
as clock, week, Monday, etc., are borrowed from (or by way of) Tok Pisin. These words are the
same in Amele and Bel, and it goes without saying that Amele and Bel borrowed from Tok Pisin and
Tok Pisin brought them from English. Nevertheless, it is remarkable that the word Sunday has its
proper words in Amele and Bel, individually. By contrast, traditional words are separately maintained,

such as morning, afternoon, slow, and immediately.


Though several words, such as seasonal words, were not discernible, each language has
corresponding words. Amele does not have a proper word meaning to be late, and the corresponding
word for the day before yesterday is absent in Amele and Tok Pisin. In contrast, Bel has the word
elen, meaning the day before yesterday. Moreover, seasonal words such as spring, summer are
absent due to the climate system in Papua New Guinea6.
Additionally, Amele and Bel borrowed some words, such as sain time and haun new from
each other, but there are not many such borrowings. Sain (saen in Bel) means time, hour originally
and then, sain is used to indicate always, often, sometimes and for a long time. The word haun (faun
in Bel) means new, while the word again is expressed as haun odoga in Amele and faun naa in
Bel, respectively. Also, it is worth noting that the words now and today are the same individually
in Amele (bila) and Bel (gaso)7.
Next, considering word formations of temporal expressions, both Amele and Bel have
Noun-Adjective order, but in contrast, Tok Pisin has Adjective-Noun order, as shown in (3). Thus,
structural borrowings are assumed in Bel and Amele, but Tok Pisin is an exception.
(3) Word formations of three languages:
Always, often: Amele, sain gaid; Bel, sain dagan; Tok Pisin, oltaim/ olgeta taim
Sometimes: Amele, sain osolosol; Bel, sain taitai; Tok Pisin, samplea taim
Year: Amele, cabi gel garden fence; Bel, umaz garden fence; Tok Pisin, yaar
In (3), each language maintains its lexicon with the exception of modern words and we can observe
slight lexical contacts between Amele and Bel. Tok Pisin has certain influence on modern words, but
the indigenous languages are resistant against traditional temporal words. Words such as young,
now/today, immediately, slowly, to hurry, to begin, beginning, to last, end, to finish, to cease, ready,
always, soon, day, night, dawn are unique within the lexicon of each language.
4. Discussion
This section discusses several points of temporal loanwords in Appendix 1. Generally speaking,
Amele and Bel have only a few temporal words in common, and as such we cannot consider that there
is a deep language contact between them. Nevertheless, several temporal words, such as time and
new, are identical, and certainly there has been a limited amount of language contact. Moreover, we
need to take the influence of Tok Pisin into consideration. Modern temporal words such as clock,
Monday, and week have been introduced by way of Tok Pisin. Previous studies have shown that
New Guinea and Austronesian lack these modern words and have borrowed them from foreign
languages or Tok Pisin. There are three discussion points. The first and the second are the discussion
of contacts between Amele and Bel. The third is the influence of Tok Pisin.
First, we discuss why Amele and Bel do not possess many loanwords. Roberts (1987, 1992), Ross
(2001, 2009), and other studies (Freyberg 1977, Nose 2014) suggest that there are certainly language
contacts and sometimes metatypy might be happening in the area around Amele and Bel. In actual fact,
this study found that there are two traditional words (sain for time and haun/faun for new) and
several modern terms (clock, week, Monday, etc.). However, Ross (2001, 2009) did not suggest
there was contact between Amele and Bel. On the other hand, Robertss (1992: 32) data suggested that
sain (saen) in Amele and Bel is the same and that this word may well have originated from Bel
(Proto-Bel). Thus, this study considers that the concept of time was introduced from Bel to Amele

There are dry and wet seasons in Papua New Guinea, and Amele has the words aa sain dry/ wa sain
wet season, and Bel has saen ifunen dry/ mou, mousen rainy season (Dempwolff n.d.: 88)
7
The words meaning today and now are also observed in Jabem, an Austronesian language spoken in
Morobe Province, Papua New Guinea. In Jabem, the word form is galoc (Nose 2014).
6

(for further discussion, see Nose 2014)8. The word haun/faun is also the same in Amele and Bel, and
Roberts (1992: 13) determined that these words refer to new, young, fresh, again in both Amele and
Bel, and are also considered to have originated from Bel. According to Roberts (1992), Amele already
possessed the proper word bag for stranger or new, and later Amele replaced it (bag) with haun.
Overall, the contact between Amele and Bel is unidirectional from Bel to Amele, and their loanwords
are limited.
Second, we discuss the possibility of structural borrowing between Amele and Bel. As previously
pointed out, several words are represented by sain, as in (4), and are similar in Amele and Bel.
(4) Temporal words using sain:
a. always (English): sain gaid (Amele), sain dogan (Bel)
b. often: sain gaid, sain dogan
c. sometimes: sain osolosol, sain taitai
d. for a long time: sain erauna, sain melain
In (4), word orders for head (noun) and dependent (adjective) are N-A, such as in Amele sain gaid
(time-usual order), and Bel sain dogan (time-usual order), although their adjective forms (gaid in
Amele and dogan in Bel) are totally different. The orders they have in common can be considered to
be metatypy, as shown in section 2 of Takia and Waskia. Here we observe Appendix 2 to see the
spatial data of the three sample languages. In Appendix 2, their grammatical behaviors are partly
similar, although there is no lexical correspondence or borrowing between the three languages. The
only point in common is the postpositional tendency of Amele and Bel: jo na (postposition) in Amele
and abu-iron-nen (suffix) in Bel. Therefore, structural borrowing (a kind of metatypy) between Amele
and Bel is probable in part, but this does not occur in-depth (cf. Nose 2014).
Third, we discuss the influence of Tok Pisin on Amele and Bel. As previously stated, modern
temporal words that have been borrowed by Amele and Bel are based on Tok Pisin, as summarized in
(5). Ross (2009) explained that language contacts of Tok Pisin started in Madang after 1900, and new
modern and technological words have spread since then.
(5) Modern temporal words of Tok Pisin
Age (yaar), Year (yaar), clock (kilok), week (week), Monday to Saturday
Thus, the influence of Tok Pisin is limited to the modern temporal words (less than 10 words in Amele,
Appendix 1), and we can ask why Amele and Bel have the indigenous word Sunday (no. 43 in
Appendix 1). The word Sunday is sande in Tok Pisin, cuha in Amele and ubou in Bel. It is assumed
that Sunday basically means rest time and thats why both languages (Amele and Bel) have the
word (rest time) individually and it was not borrowed from Tok Pisin.
Next, no. 51 in Appendix 1, the word year differs amongst the three languages. Yaar/cabi gel
(also meaning garden fence) in Amele, yaar/umaz (also meaning garden fence) in Bel and yaar
in Tok Pisin. Thus, the word year is partly derived from the loanword yaar while also maintaining the
traditional meaning, garden fence. Here lies the question of why Amele and Bel each have a word
for year, that also means garden fence. The garden fence reference may have originated from
local words; at some time in the past, either language had borrowed the meaning of this (calque).
According to Roberts (1992: 3, 18), the Amele word gel was borrowed from Bel a long time ago, in
cabi gel (literally, garden fence) and gel, meaning fence. However, the word formation of
8

Roberts (1992) and Nose (2014) pointed out that sain in Amele can be used in a temporal conjunction
when-clause, but not in Bel.
(i) Sain ija haun-na. When I was young,
Moreover, the reduplication form sainsain in Amele means always, forever, but there is no corresponding
word in Bel.

garden-fence seems to have originated from Amele and then Bel borrowed its structure (i.e. calque,
or translation borrowing).
Lastly, the summation of the findings from the study. First in the findings was the determination
that Amele and Bel still maintain their indigenous nature with regards to temporal words. By contrast,
Tok Pisin has many English-based words and a few original words, such as isi isi slow, and belo
midday. The language contact between Amele and Bel is limited in temporal words and there are no
remarkable loanwords in terms of space-place, as per Appendix 2, but we observed several lexical and
structural borrowings in time and space words. The language contact of Tok Pisin is considered to
have commenced from 1900, and as a result, it has affected modern temporal words in both Amele and
Bel (see Freybergs 1977 description of Bel missionary history).
No. 1 and no. 40 in Appendix 1, the most frequent word sain/ saen in Amele and Bel for time
and hour has originated from Bel, Austronesian. This study concludes that there was no concept of
time in Trans-New Guinea languages up to that point (Nose 2014, Davies 1989).
5. Conclusion
This study has clarified that there are limited language contacts among Amele, Bel and Tok Pisin,
and as a result, lexical borrowings have occurred between them. There were findings of temporal
words, as per Appendix 1, and several spatial words as per Appendix 2 (cf. Lakoff and Johnson 1980).
First, Amele and Bel each maintain traditional temporal words, such as morning, night, and today.
This means that basic traditional words are being kept in use within each language as these are
difficult to borrow from other languages. Historically, Amele has borrowed words from Bel, and the
structural borrowing and calque (translation borrowing) have been derived from Amele into Bel.
However, their contact is limited and it is hard to define the contact they do have as metatypy.
Second, the influence of Tok Pisin is limited only in modern temporal words, possibly after 1900.
Both Amele and Bel borrow modern temporal words from Tok Pisin, such as year, Monday, and clock.
However, other traditional words and verbs to do with time have not been borrowed. Needless to say,
more modern words will be introduced via Tok Pisin in the future.
Third, the reality and history of contacts in the Madang area have been reported by previous
studies (Dempwolff n.d.; Freyberg 1977; Roberts 1987, 1992). Nevertheless, this present study cannot
clarify the direction from which borrowings have occurred, and it is also noteworthy that frequent
lexical contacts were not observed, as far as the temporal/spatial data depicts in Appendix 1 and 2.
Acknowledgements
I would like to thank Nere Tamo, Nelau Lagia, and Tami Weslydak for their contributions to the data
and kindness. I claim sole responsibility for any errors. This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI,
Grant Numbers 23720211 and 15K02478, and CASIO Scientific Foundations (2011-36)
Appendix 1: Investigation of temporal words
English
Amele
1: time
sain
2: age
yaar
3: new
haun
4: young
etac
5: old
hirah
6: early
mahu
7: late

mahu qee

8: now
9: immediately
10: fast

bila =today
mahu
mahu odi

Bel
Tok Pisin
sain
taim
umas
yaar
haun
nupela
dabeel
young
getnen/ samai
oldpela
pasak
mok
(get, early/ hurriup
wagam=earlier)
teaa (alou, loan tap: late
later)
gaso =today
nau
pasak
hurriup tru
pasak mok
hurriup

11: slow
12: to hurry
13: to be late
14: to begin
15: beginning
16: to last
17: end
18: to finish
19: to cease
20: ready
21: always
22: often
23: sometimes
24: soon
25: for a long time
26: never
27: again
28: day
29: night
30: dawn
31: morning
32: midday
33: afternoon
34: evening
35: today
36: tomorrow
37: day after tomorrow
38: yesterday
39:
day
before
yesterday
40: hour
41: clock
42: week
43: Sunday
44: Monday
45: Tuesday
46: Wednesday
47: Thursday
48: Friday
49: Saturday
50: month
51: year

cetna
mahu odi ac
*
sanan magac
sanan mec
itboc
bemenac
bemeyac
bemudugac
saiadoya
sain gaid
sain gaid
sain osolosol
nag odi
sain erauna
qee bahic
haun odoga
deel
witic
qasil
qasil
ahinec
wes
wes
bila
uba dac
ele dac
cum
*

kinkin
pasak mokein
late
fun nau
fun nauk
funati
biniime
ibinii
wadan funati
kibimei ime
sain dagan
sain dagan
sain taitai
kitek sa
sain melain
tea wok
faun naa
sain (nal, ad)
tidon(bo)
bomong
bomong
aad(ad: sun)
yusu
yusu
gaso
buani (boyanip)
naar lusek (zabop)
noosnen (noz)
elen

isi
wokin hurriup
em bai late
statim
stat bilong em
finis bilong em
end
pinis
pinisim
ready
oltaim
olgeta taim
smapela taim
i no long taim
longpela taim
i no save
gen
dai
nait
moning
moning
belo/ belo taim
abinun
abinun
today
tumora
haptomorrow
asde
*

sain
kilok
week
cuha
mande
tunde
trinde
fonde
fride
sarare
jagel
yaar/ cabi gel

52: winter
53: spring
54: summer
55: autumn/fall
56: season

*
*
sun sain
*
*

sain
kilok
week
ubou
mande
tunde
trinde
fonde
fride
sarare
hurei(fulei)
yaar(umaz:
fence)
*
*
*
*
*

winter
*
*(saen ifunen)
*
season

aua
kilok
week
sande
mande
tunde
trinde
fonde
fride
sarare
muun
garden yaar

Appendix 2: Investigation of Spatial words


English
Amele
1: In (house)
jo na/ jo duna
2: In front of
jo-orana
3: above
jo ilona
4: Place

udun/ aben

5: To sit
6: here
7: there

biliga
ene
Auna

Bel
abu-iron-nen
abu naon-nen
rat-nen
panu
(Abe/ matalik/ aben)
matal
erak
Merak

Tok Pisin
long haus
bipo haus
antap long haus (face
blong haus)
ples
sindaun
hia
Long hap

References
Alverson, H. (1994) Semantics & experience: universal metaphors of time in English, Mandarin,
Hindi, and Seshotho. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.
Davies, J. (1989) Kobon. London: Croom Helm.
Dempwolff, O. (n.d) Grammar of Gedaged language. Mimeo. Narer, Karkar Island: Lutheran
Mission.
Evans, V. (2004) The structure of time. Amsterdam /Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Freyberg, P. G. (1977). Missionary Lingue Franche: Bel (Gedaged). New Guinea Area Languages and
Language Study 3: 855-864.
Haspelmath, M, & U. Tadmor, (eds.). (2009). Loanwords in the world's languages: a comparative
handbook. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980) Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago Press.
McMahon, A. M. S. 1994. Understanding language change. Cambridge; Cambridge University Press.
Mager, J. F. (1952) Gedaged-English dictionary. Ohio: The American Lutheran Church.
Muysken, P. (2008) Functional categories. Cambridge: CUP.
Nose, M. (2014) How Amele speakers use time (in Japanese). Proceedings of the 33th meeting of
Japan Sociolinguistics Studies: 48-51.
Roberts, J. R. (1987) Amele. London: Croom Helm.
Roberts, J.R. (1992) A comparison of possible lexical borrowings between Amele and Gedaged. Ms.
SIL.
Ross, M. (2001). Contact-induced change in Oceanic languages in North-West Melanesia. In:
Aikhenvald, Alexandra, and Robert Malcolm Ward Dixon. (eds.). Areal diffusion and genetic
inheritance: problems in comparative linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press:
134-166.
Ross, M. (2009). Loanwords in Takia, an Oceanic language of Papua New Guinea. In: Haspelmath, M.
and U. Tadmor (eds.): 747-770.
Sinha, Ch, S. Sinha, V., Zinken, J. Sa, pio, W. (2011). When Time is not Space: The social and
linguistic construction of time intervals and temporal event relations in an Amazonian culture.
Language and Cognition 3(1):137-169.
(http://www.port.ac.uk/departments/academic/psychology/staff/title,50474,en.html) 2012, April. 30.
Thomason, S. G. (2001). Language Contact: an introduction. Washington, D.C.: Georgetown
University Press.
Wierzbicka, A. (1993). Why do we say in April, on Thursday, at 10 o'clock?: In search of an

explanation. Studies in Language 17:437-454.


Masahiko Nose, Associate Professor, Faculty of Economics, Shiga University

Ross (2001) metatypy

You might also like