How Languages Are Learned (Oxford) - Chapter 3: Factors Affecting Second Language Learning
How Languages Are Learned (Oxford) - Chapter 3: Factors Affecting Second Language Learning
How Languages Are Learned (Oxford) - Chapter 3: Factors Affecting Second Language Learning
FACTOkS A\FECTING
SECOND LANGUAGE
LEARNING
:~~27.
In Chaprer 1, it \Vas pointed out rhar ali normal children, given a normal
upbringng, are successful in rhe acquisition of rheir firsr language. This
contrasts with our experience of second language learners, whose success
vares greatly.
Activity
Characteristics of the 'good language learner'
53
lt seems rhar sorne people have a much easier rime of learning than others.
Rare of development varies \videly among firsr !anguage learners. Sorne
children can string rogerher five-, six-, and seven-\vord senrences at an age
\vhen orher children are just beginning ro label irems in their immediare
envronn1ent. Neverrheless, ali normal children eventu:i!ly master rheir firsr
!J.ngu;ige.
o-.....
'1
In second language learning, it has been observed coundess dmes rhar in rhe
same classi:-001:1 setting, sorne srudenrs progress rapidly rhrough rhe' inicial
srages of learn1ng a new language while others struggle along making very
slow progress. Sorne learners never ::i.chieve native-like command of a second
language. Are rhere personal characterisrics rhar make one Iearner more
successful than another, and if so, whar are rhey?
1 = Very important
2 =Quite importanr
3 = lmportant
4 = Not very imponanr
5 =Not at all important.
has an above-average !Q
of others
g attends to whether his or her performance
54
1
orher studies. W'hat rhis suggesrs is rhar, \vhle inrelligence, especially as
measured by verbal IQ tests, may be asrrong factor when ir comes to learning
\vhich involves language analysis and rule learning, inre!ligence may play a
less imporranr role in classroorr1s \vhere rhe insrrucrion focuses more on
cornmunicarion and interacrion.
"
sorne studies reporr rhar learners wirh a higher leve! of morivariqn are more
successtu1 language 1earners rhan rhose wirh lower motivarion, \vhile orher
srudies reporr rhar highly morivared learne-rs do nor perform any berter on- a
proficiency test rhan Iearners ~Nith much less morivarion ro learn rhe second
language. One explanarion which has been offered for rhese conflicring
findings is rhat ilie language proficiency resrs used in differenr srudies do nor
me~sure r~e same k}nd ofknowledge. Thar is, in intOrrnal language Iearning)
serr1ngs, h1ghly monvated!earners may6e more successful \vhen rhe pnaticiency
t~ measure oral communication skill~ In orher srudies, ho\vever, high!x:
mouvated learners may nor be more successful because rhe resrs are primarily
\\
<6"'-'
<;
'
p<?C' a\\
CJ is !laf.e
Intelligence
cerra1n k1n_ds oI~rs.:Tfiese tests are ofren aS:sociated \Virh sccess in school
and a lj!lliJi.~n i_~~Jli~~~ an~~~fqnd-lancruagejJe~auo~t;tl,m.~~
been
re,t_?,?rte_d. O~e years, many_ studies using a variery of inreiligence
1
(~Q j reses and differenr merhods of aSsessing Ianuuage
learninu have found
~
e
b
thar IQ seores vere a good means ofpredicring hov successfuI a learnervould
be. ~o_me recen~_-s_tu~~-~~ have sho_vn r~a_r t~ese Irl~_a_s_ure-s of_~-~~~-~JJj_g~!!,~~!r.1-~Y
be ~ore str~rlS-~I-~~~ateiro ~ertan_ kinds of S(:_~~!'ld_h1nguagc;. ?:~il_it_i~~~'.~h~n ~o
.?_thers. Fo_r examp1e, in a srudywirh French immersion srudents in Cana da, ir
was found rhat, \vhile inre_:gi_g~n~5:__~':'-~.s-~elar~-~--~- rhe developmenr ofFrench
second lanl!U:l.O-e rf'::i.--lino- crr1mm.-..- .,..,,.l "~~~J.-.. .. L __ :~ ____ _
1
55
53
lmle res~arch has acrually explored whether having a skill such as the 'ability
r;;a series of studies, Alexander Guiora and his collegues found support for
the clairn thar inhibition is a negative force, at least for se_cond language
pronunciation performance. On~~Qh:~___a_g__an.;iJ~-~i~}?,.ftP.e$;~~ qf
smalldoses ofalcohol onpronunciation (Guioraet al. 1972). Thevfound rhat
:r.t~
Sorne 1nd1v1duals may have srrong memories but only average
ab1lmes' the other components of aptirude. Ideally, one could determine
Furthermore, they~_;iy_haye.m9'"e~ith.petq;;n:iifilcet;ha1uxi~h.le;wing.
We may also note, in passing, that when larger doses of alcohol were
adminisrered, pronunciation rapidly deteriorat~d!
Personality
'
_..cmeava-rJ;::;rg~rson
u;_:_1toten.ar:gi::~~-rhatn-e?Z-E~()Ye!t~d-oerson--i~_'W.t;Us~i-~~fl tttli!tt$-U,~!,gf:zjjrig.
Hov~ever, J~~ffi-S:-~f~h dp.~~:Rl21~3YUJ:!EQQiltl~_-;ggs_!~!9_n. i\Jrhough,some
studres ,have found rhat success--in,_-_langl!&l'--- learniug--ls~,~uela_tecl-Wirh
rneasurement. Another explanatiorr which has been offered for the mixed
findings of personality srudies is
personali~.l:2cria.hl;" ma: be,ll..ll'L'?.r
fact<_>roitlyl\!Jh~::S'loijpgi,f.;..~-i;::1~~I',i~.\!!)H J&a,<:;c.tiriji9Jl.Qf
ITteraq~!QIIS:Tfe confused picrure of the research on personality factors may
. be ruec!n parrto the fact that comparisons are made between smdies rhat
!h.'
56
55
56
One factor \vhich often affeci::s morivation is rhe social dyn2mi_c_ Q_r p_9_\VC[.
rfarionshipberWeenrhe languag~. Th_-~~-~memt.~IS-~-f q___rninoriry__group.
---~~arn_~~~ili~-l~Qg~_<!gc;_Qf a _rnajoriIT$[Q!JP ma,y ha_y_~Jiiffrrf11Latri~JJdn.and
motivarion from those of majoriry group members_ learning_ a minority
IarlC..lla.e_ EVe-1 thouah-r Is -i-ripOS.Sihie to Predict rhc exacr effecr of such
~o~eralf1ctors on secbond Ianguage learning, i::he t"'acr rhar b_ngu:iges exist in
social contexcs cannot be overlooked 1,vhen we seek to undersrand the
variables \vhich atTect success in learning. Chi!dren as \Vell as adults are
sensirive to sociai dynamics and po\ver relai:ionships,
There has been a great deal of research un rhe role ofartirudes and morivario:
in second language learni.~g'. !!:._e. ?-'.'."~r)_!Ji!_l_qi.!lg~.-~_hp~y thA~Q,~_i',ti~~ff-t&t,td~
Although lirde research has been done to investigare how pedagog interacts
wirh motivation in second language classrooms, considerable work has been
done \Virhin rhe fi.eld of educacional psychology. In a review of sorne of this
work, Graham Crookes and Richard Schmidt (1991) poinr to severa! areas
\vhere educational research has reporred increased levels of morvation for
srudenrs in relarion to pedagogical pracrices. Included among rhese are:
r_
Moti.'vating students into the lesson At rhe opening stages of Iessons (and
\Vithin rransirions), ir has been observed rhat remarks reachers make abour
forthcominab activiries can lead to hicrher
levels ofinteresr on the part of rhe
b
srudents.
Yrying the activ;ties, tasks, and 1naterials Students are reassured by rhe
existence of classroon1 rourines which they can depend on. Ho\vever, lessons
which ahvays consisr of rhe same rourines, panerns, and formats h::i.ve been
sho"vn ro lead to a decrease in arrention andan ncrease in boredorn. Varying
rheactiviries, tasks, and marerials can help ro avoid rhis and increase srudenrs'
interese levels.
57
.-
- ~ _L -
57
11
in a co-operarive taskhas an imporrant role ro play. Knowing rhat rheir teammates are counring on them can increase students' n1otivarion.
''
Clearly, cultural and age differences will dererrnine rhe mosr appropriareway
Learner preferences
Learncrs ha ve c1ear preferences tOr ho-vv- they go J.bout lea.rning ne~~aterial:
irifor11oa(j(,[]a;;;J:S_kfll~ (Reid 1995). We have all heard people say rhar rhey
cannot learn sorr1erhing unril they have seen ir. Such learners would fall inro
>alled~.i!.'1L"1.'.
learners, seem rO ne;;f~ly ro hear something once or rwice befo re they know
ir. For others, who are referred to as 'kinaesthetic' learners, rhere is a need ro
Learner beliefi
and opinions abour how their instrucrion should be dehvered. I.li.~se behefa
Second language learners are not al\vays conscious of rheir individual learn~ng
sryles, but virtually all learners, particularly older learner~, have strong bel~efs
d.
iulS IS
f;
research indicares rhat learner beliefs can be srrong me ianng ~crors in .r e1r
experence in the classroom. For example: i1: a survey of rnrernan~nal
srudents learning ESL in a highly commun1canve progra1n at an ~ngh_sh.
ry Carlos Yorio (1986) found high levels of dissansfacuon
~ ak ~=~5 1
d
enrs. The rvpe 0 f communicanve 1nstrucr1on they rece1ve
among rhe Stud
.
. . .
focused exclusively on meaning and sponraneous ~omn~un1canon 1~ g:o~p\vork interaction. In rheir responses [0 a quesnonnar:e. rhe m~JOflty of
studenrs expressed concerns abour severa! aspecrs of rhe1r u~structton. most
absence of arrention ro language form, correcnve feedback, or
nora blv
, ' [he
'
.d
d. ti
.
teacher-cenrred insrrucrion. Alrhough rhis srudy d1 not . Lrec Y:xam1ne
learners' progress in relarion ro rheir opinions a~out rhe instrucnon _rhey
received, several 0 f rhem were convinced rhat r,he1r progrc~s was n~ganvel_y
affected by an insrructional approach which \vas not cons1stenr \Vith rhelf
. .
58
.JY
.. T
60
Age ofacquisition
and rhe expression of much more ~omplcared ideas. Adulrs are ofren
embarrassed by rheir lack of masrery ot rhe language and rhey may develop a
sense of inadequacy after experiences of frusrrarion in trying ro say exacrly
whar rhey mean.
The Critical Period Hypothesis has been challenged in recenr years from
severa! different poinrs of vew. Sorne srudies of rhe second language
development of older and younger learners who are learning in similar
circumstances have shown rhat, ~!l~1st in t~-~~dy,:?,!:!ges of s~coild l_angt~e
dev~!c:i_pr:iiei!rt __o~.4~~-J~~-rp~_r_~ _ a_r~. ~_or_~ ,,~ffi._c.i~D. t __ Jba _y_q~1_rrg~r~l~es.r~~:. In
~ducational research, ir has been reporred rhar learners 'vho b~gan learn1ng a
second language ar rhe primary school level did not fare berter in rhe long run
than rhose who beQ'an in early adolescence. Furrherrnore, rhere are coundess
sp~;~ r~'Fiag~~g~:9ft[~I~=!i-~1~s!im~Wii.fil0C~i!~t!Lii~a~~-lik~JtuengJ!Mi:rti~ir
p~~r~i:el)'..ach~".:'.".Uc~)righ.levels.C>f[ll!S''!YJlf!lE.s!2Ql<;gi_langt~Tu
be su re, rhere are cases where adulr second language learners have distinguished
themselves by their exceprionaJ pertOrmance. For example, one ofren sees
reference ro Joseph Conrad, anarive speaker of Polsh who became a majar
wrirer in rhe English language. Many adulr second language learners become
capable of communicating very successfully in rhe language bur, for most,
differences of accenr, word choice, or grammarical fearures disringuish rhem
from narive speakers and from second lang:uage speakers i.vho began learnng
rhe language while rhey '\Vere very young.
anecdotes abour older learners (adolescenrs and adulrs) who have reached
hgh levels 0 f proficency n a second langmge. ~aes this mean rhar rhere rs
no critica! period for second language acqu1s1non.
In rhe follo\vingpages, wewili revie'\v sorne studies designed ro i~vestigate the
Critica! Period Hyporhesis as it relates to second language learn1ng.
One explanarion for rhis differenc:e i~_xhat. a:s in first language acquistion,
rhere_ is a crirical period fo'r secohd language acquisirion. As discussed in
Chaprer 1, rhe Critical- Period Hypothesis sugg_esrs that there_ is a time in
development have focused on learners' phonologcal (prommrnnon) ach1evemenr. In general, these sn1dies have concluded rhat older learne~ al~o~t
occurs_ afrer rhe end of-the critical period may not be based on rhe innare
biologica1 strucrures beiieved ro conrribure to firsr language acquisicion or
second languageacquisirion in early chiidhood. Rath_er, older t~~.r[ler2_~f?.c;!Ld
.!:!_~1:12."~e-~ner~ _le_arnng abil_ri_i:s - r~_e sa~_'=-..9!:!~~- rh~LI1!igh1__1,1i_e ro leyn
abilities are nor as successful for language learning as rhe more specific, innare
capaciries \vhich are available ro che young child. Ir is mosr ofren ciaimed rhar
rhe criric:al _-P_~riod ends-sof11e~here ar9_':!!!fl_12__t;>~.rty,---but sorne researchers
suggeStTr"'~OUJ~rb~eeven ~arlie~:
----
--
r-..
'
'
'
59
...
61
62
J~actors
A lengrhy interview wirh each of rhe subjects in rhe srudy was rape recorded.
someonewho had arrived ar rhe age of 18 but had only lived there for 1Oyears.
Because Patkowski wanted to remove rhe possibiliry that che resulrs \Vould be
Similarly, amount of instruction, when separared from age, did not predict
success ro rhe exrent that age ofimmigracion did.
affected by accent, he did nor ask rhe raters to judge the tape-recorded
interviews themselves. Instead, he transcribed five-minure samples from the
interviews. These sJlnplcs (from \vhichany identifyingor revealing information
about in1migration hisrory had been removed) were rated by traned narivespeaker judges. The judges \vere asked to place each speaker on a raring scale
from O, rcprcsenting no knowlcdge of rhe 1anguage, ro 5, representing a leve!
ofEnglish expecred from an educated narive speaker.
Figure 3.1: Bar d1arts showing the language levels ofpre- andpost-puberty learners
ofEng/ish (Patkowski 1980).
berween learners who began ro learn English before puberry and those who
began learning English later?' However, in the light of sorne of the issues
22
20
and experienccs which son1e people have suggested might be as good as age in
predicting or expiaining a learner's eventual success in mastering a second
language. For example, he looked ar the relationship berween eventual
mastery and rhe total amount of time a speaker had been in rhe U ni red Srares
as well as the amounr of formal ESL insrruction each speaker had had.
10
The findings were quite dramaric. Thirry-two out of 33 subjects who had
begun leaming English befo re the age of 15 seo red ar rhe 4+ or the 5 level. The
homogeneiry of the pre-puberty learners seen1ed to suggest rhat, for this
group, succes5 in learning asecond language was almosr inevitable (see Figure
3. 1). On the other hand, there was much more variery in the levels achieved
2+
3+
4+
Pre-puberty learners
20
variery made rhe performance of chis group look more like rhe sort of
performance range one would expecr if one were measuring success in
12
(}
"~ 10
began to learn English before puberry and those who began learning English
6
4
la ter?', was answered with a very resounding 'yes'. When he examined rhe
other facrors which might be rhought to affect success in second language
acquisition, the picture was much less clear. There was, naturally, sorne
relationship ben..vecn rhese other factors and Iearning success. However, it
often turned out that age was so closely relared to the other factors rhar ir was '
2+
3+
4+
Post-puberty learners
'
Experience and research have shown rhat narive-like masrery of the spo_ken
language is difficulr ro arrain by older learners. Surprisin_gly, even rhe abd1ry
years might speak better than one who had been there for only 1Oyears, ir was
often the case lhal the one \Vith longer residence had also arrived aran earlier
age. Ho\vever, a person who had arrived in rhe United States at rhe age of 18
and had lived rhere for 20 years did not score significanrly better rhan
60
b:J
64
:-
''T
lntutions ofgrammaticaliry
Jacqueline Johnson and Elissa Newporr conducred a srudy of46 Chinese and
Korean speakers \Vho had begun ro learn English ar different ages. A.11 subjecrs
were srudenrs or faculry ar an American universiry and a1l had been in rhe
United Srates far ar least three vears. The srudv also included 23 native
speakers ofEnglish (Johnson anlNewporr 1989)'.
Morphology "\VJS resred using a procedure like rhe \vug test', \vhich required
learners ro complete sen ten ces by adding rhe correcr grammJric1l markers to
\Vords\vhich were supplied by the researchers. Again, to take an example from
English, learners were asked ro complete sentences such as 'Here is one boy.
'
No\v rhere are rwo of rhem. There are rwo
When rhey seo red rhe tests, Johnson and Ne\.vparr found rhar age of arrival in
the U ni red Srares was a significant predicrorofsuccess on rhe test. \Vhen rhey
grouped the learners in rhe same ""Y as Parko\vski, comparing rhose who
began their inrensive exposure ro English benveen rhe ages of 3 and 15 wirh
rhose \vho arrived in rhe United Srares benveen the ages of 17 and 39, once
again they found char there '.vas a strong relarionship bet\veen an early starr to
language learning and better performance in the second language. Johnson
and Newporr nored rhar for rhose who began befare the age of 15, and
especiallybefore the age of 1 O, rhere were few individual differences in second
Ianguage abiliry. Those \vho began larer did not have native-like language
abiliries and were more likely ro differ gready from one another in ultimare
attainment.
This srudy, rhen, furrher supporrs rhe hyporhesis rhar rhere is a crirical period
for arrainingfull narive-Iike n1asreryof a second language. Neverrheless, rhere
is sorne research \vhich suggests thar older learners may have an advanrage. ar
least in rhe early srages of second Ianguage 1earning.
For rhe stor)' con1pi-ehcnsion task, learners heard a srory in Durch :nd \vere then
asked ro re~ell rhe srory in English or Durch (according ro rheir preference).
FinaIIy) the storytelling task required learners to tell a srory in Dutch, using a
ser ofpicrures rhey \.Vere given. Rare ofdelivery of speech marrered more rhan
rhe expression of conrent or formal accuracy.
The learners were divid~d into several age groups, bur for our discussion \Ve
\vil! divide rhem in to jusr rhree groups: children (aged 3 ro 10), adolescenrs
(12 ro 15 years), and adults (18 ro 60 years). The children and adolescenrs ali
acrended , Durch schools, Sorne of rhe J.dults \Vorked in Dutch work
environn1ents, bur mosr of rheir Durch colleagues spoke English \vell. Orher
aduhs \vere par~nrs who did nor \vork ourside i:heir homes and rhus had
some,vhat less contacr\virh Durch rhan mosr of rhe orher subjects.
The !earners \verc resred rhree times, ar four- to five-month intervals. They
\vere first rested \Vithin six months of !i:heir arriva! in HoHand and \vithin six
\veeks of rheir srarring school or \vork in a Durch-language environmenr.
61
65
Activty
Child
Adoiescent
Adult
3 Adulrs and adolescenrs can make considerable and rapid progress rowards
mastery of a second language in contexts where rhey can make use of rhe
A u di tory discrmination
XY
Morpholo0j
XY
Sentence rcpetitlon
XY
Even people who know norhing abour rhe critica! period research are certain
that, in school programs for second or foreign language reaching, 'younger is
berter'. Ho"vever, borh experience and research show rhat older learncrs can
attain high. if not <na ti ve', levels of proficiency in their second language.
Furthermore, ir is essential to rhinkcarefullyaboutthegoals ofan instructional
program and rhe contexr in which it occurs befare we jump to conclusions
about rhe necessicy- or even the desirabilicy- of the earliest possible srart.
Sen.tence uanslalion
XY
Senrence judgement
XY
XY
Scory comprehension
Storyteiling
The role of rhe critical period in second language acquisition is srill much
debated. For every researcher \vho holds that rhere are marurational
constraints on 1anguage acquisition, rhere is anorher who considers rhat rhe
age factorcannoc be separared from factors such as motivation, social idenricy,
and rhe condirions for learning. Theyargue thar older learners maywell speak
wich an accent because rhey wanr to continue being idendfied with rheir first
language cultural group, and adules rarely gec access ro the same quanricy and
qualiry oflanguage input that children receive in play settings.
In rhe Snow and Hoefoagel-Hohle srudy, the adolescents were byfarrhe most
successful learners. They were ahead of everyone on ali bur one of the rests
(pronunciation) on the firsr test session. That is, wirhin the first few months
th e a<l?le:cents h.ad already made the most progress in learning Durch. As rhe
1
taole ind1caces, 1t was the aduits who were better than the children and
adolescents on pronunciation in the first test session. Surprisingly, it\vas also
the adu!ts, not the chifdrcn, whose seores were second besr on rhe ocher resrs
ar the first cest session. In other words, ado!escenrs and adules learned fasrer
rhan children in rhe firsr few momhs of e.xposure to Dutch.
Many peo ple con dude on rhe basis of studies such as rhose by Parkowski or
Newporr and Johnson char ir is betcer to begin second language insrrucrion as
early as possible. Yer ir is very imporranr ro bear in mind the conrexr of these
srudies. Theydeal with the highest possible leve! ofsecond language skills, rhe
level ar which a second language speaker is indisringuishable from a native
speaker. Bur achieving a native-like masrery of the second language is noc a
goal for ali second language learning, in aH con[exts.
By the end of the year, the children were catching up, or had surpassed, rhe
adults on severa! rneasurcs. Nevertheless, it was rhe adolescents who rerained
the highcst levds of performance overa!!.
62
68
\X!hen rhe goal is basic communicarive abiliry for all srudenrs in a school
setring, and when it is assumed rhar rhechild's narivelanguage,vill remain rhe
primary language, ir may be more efficienr ro begin second orforei'gn language
reaching larer. When learners receive onlya fe\v hours ofinsrrucron perweek,
learners who srarr larer (for example, ar age 1O, 11, or 12} ofi:en carch up \Virh
rhose who began earlier. We have often seen second or foreign language
programs \vhich begin wirh very young learners but offer only minimal
contact with the language. Even when studenrs do make progress in rhese
early-srart programs, rhey sometimes find rhemselves placed in secondary
school classes \virh srudents\vho have had no previous insrruction. Afreryears
of classes, learners feel frustrated by rhe lack of progress, and their morivation
ro conrinue may be diminished. SchooI programs should be based on realisric
estimares ofhow long ir rakes to Iearn a second language. One or nvo hours a
week \viH nor produce very advanced second language speakers, no marrer
how young rhey were when rhey began.
!ntelligence
Genesee, F. 1976. '1~he role of inreiligence in second language learning.'
Summary
The learner's age is one of rhe characrerisrics which derermine the wav in
which an individual approaches second language learning. But , rhe
opporrunities for Iearning (borh inside and ourside rhe classroom), rhe
morivarion ro learn, and individual differences in apritude for language
learnng are also imporranr derermining facrors in borh rate oflearning and
evenrual success in learning.
In rhis chaprer, we have looked ar rhe ways in which inreiligence, aprirude,
personaliry and morivarional characrerisrics, learner preferences, and age
have been found ro influencesecond language learning. \-X:'e have le:uned rhar
the study of individual Iearner variables is not easy and rhar rhe resulrs of
r~search are not enrirely sarisfacrory. This is pardy beca use of rhe lack of clear
definirions and methods for measuring rhe individual characreristics. Ir .is also
due ro rhe facr rhar rhese learner characrerisrics are nor independent of one
anorher: leJ.rner variables inreracr in complex ways. So far, researchers know
very lrde abour rhe narure of rhese con1plex inreracrions. Thus, ir remains
difficult ro n1ake precise predicrions abour ho\v a particular individuaI's
characreristics influence his or her success as a language learner. Nonerheless,
63
_.,.,...~ ~~~~.;~
- - --- - -
69