Intelligence, Democracy, and International Environmental Commitment
Intelligence, Democracy, and International Environmental Commitment
Intelligence, Democracy, and International Environmental Commitment
Environmental Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/envres
Davis Center for Russian and Eurasian Studies, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, United States
Center for Institutional Studies of National Research University Higher School of Economics, Moscow
Department of Economics, Westminster International University in Tashkent; Global Intelligence for Development Research and Analytics (GIDRA), Colibri
Law Firm, Uzbekistan
b
c
art ic l e i nf o
a b s t r a c t
Article history:
Received 19 December 2015
Received in revised form
25 January 2016
Accepted 28 January 2016
This paper investigates the determinants of nations' commitment to environmental protection at the
international level by focusing on the role of national intelligence and the level of democracy. The national intelligence is measured by nations IQ scores. The ndings based on a sample of 152 nations
provide strong evidence that intelligence has statistically signicant impact on ratication of international environmental agreements, and the countries with IQ 10-points above global average are 23%
more likely to sign multilateral environmental agreements than others. The ndings also demonstrate
that it is the combination of high-level of intelligence of nations and democracy, that likely result in
international environmental commitments.
& 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords:
IQ
International environmental commitment
Greenhouse gas emission
Intelligence
International agreements
Democracy
1. Introduction
Over the past 250 years, the global temperature has risen by
0.85 C and the period from 1983 to 2012 was likely the warmest
30-year period of the last 1400 years (Ahrens and Henson, 2015,
p. 507). This had crucial impact on biodiversity and ecosystem
processes. For example, the ice thickness in the central Arctic has
fallen by nearly 50% (Kwok and Rothrock, 2009). The average
mean ocean level has increased by 20 cm over the past century
and this increase has been greater than the average rate in the
course of past two thousand years. As a result, this has intensied
extreme weather events, such as tornadoes, oods and wildres.
The economic cost of changing weather with respect to agriculture
is incomputable, and consequently, the food security in many
countries is at risk.
Scholars are in general consensus that the driving force of the
global climate change has been the rapid greenhouse gas (GHG)
emissions over the past century. Indeed, over the past ve decades
GHG emissions per capita in developed nations have been four times
higher than discharges per capita in low-income nations. Moreover,
the forecasts suggest that these emissions will rise up to 90% by
2030 and the GHG discharges from developing nations are projected
n
Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: [email protected] (A. Obydenkova),
[email protected] (R. Salahodjaev).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2016.01.042
0013-9351/& 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
83
Obydenkova, 2014).
Still, while the past research suggests that the ruling elite tends
to under-evaluate environmental goods to achieve rapid economic
growth and benet from foreign direct investments (FDI), there is
robust evidence that more intelligent [bureaucrats] demonstrate
less of a preference for smaller, immediate rewards versus larger,
delayed rewards (Shamosh and Gray, 2008; p. 296). Indeed, if
environmental quality is a luxury good, we may then conjecture
that its demand rises more than proportionally with regard to
intelligence. Indeed, the environmental protection has also longterm benet and might have lower visibility for electorate (even
more though within the context of the Great Recession).
Recent studies demonstrated close interaction between international politics and governmental actions and national support in
the regions with various levels of democracy (Obydenkova, 2008,
2012). Based on the nexus of international level and national
support, we conjecture that the factor that has high visibility is the
commitment to environmental protection at the international level. Therefore, signing international treaties might increase the
popularity of the national government (democracy search for
popular support) as long as the population of this state has higher
level of intelligence and therefore can evaluate positively the
governmental environmental commitments at the international
level. In other words, the intelligent population will understand
the importance of long-term benet commitments even if they
result in short-term economic pitfalls. Thus, a democratic government might wish to increase its' popularity for intelligent population and to gain their support and credibility. This conjunction is
even more important within the context of Great Recession, where
the choices of the national democratic governments are very important and risk-prone in terms of gaining popularity. Based on
this discussion, we conjecture the main hypothesis of this study:
The national government of a democratic state with high level
of intelligence of population, will be more supportive to demonstrate to this population its commitment to international agreements on environmental protection. Consequently, high-IQ societies would be inclined to pledge more resources to ecological
conservation (Salahodjaev, 2016) and to follow consumption patterns less destructive to the ecosystem vitality as intelligent individuals have longer time horizons (Potrafke, 2012). Similarly as
suggested by (Squalli, 2014, p. 34), more intelligent people are
less wasteful because they predict the long-term nancial (and by
extension potentially environmental) benets of current investments on environmentally friendly technologies, thus potentially
contributing to lower environmental emissions.
On the other hand, existence of market failures leads to inefcient use of natural resources that promotes rent seeking,
hinders welfare and reduces quality of life. And collective intelligence may effectively direct the actions of self-centered economic agents and may prevent bureaucrats from ignoring environmental concerns of the population (Minowitz, 2004). On the
other hand, past research suggests that cognitive able individuals
aim to achieve the efcient, competitive markets (Lynn and Vanhanen, 2012) and more likely to see the invisible hand, supporting policies that create prosperity (Jones, 2011, p. 53). For example, on the macroeconomic level, intelligence is negatively
correlated with corruption, crime and shadow economy factors
that are positively associated with greenhouse gas emissions
(Salahodjaev, 2015a).
Finally, Kanazawa's (2010, 2012) Savanna-IQ Interaction Hypothesis (otherwise known as the intelligence paradox) posits
that more intelligent individuals are more likely to acquire and
espouse evolutionarily novel preferences and values that our ancestors did not possess. Concerns for the environment are distinctly evolutionarily novel. Our ancestors during the Pleistocene
Epoch were not concerned about the environment at all, because
84
3. Method
Following conventional practice in cross-country studies, Eq.
(1) is specied to explore the effect of intelligence on environmental commitment.
DAi = 1 + 2 IQ i + 3 Xi + i
2. Data and methodology
2.1. Dependent variable
The dependent variable in this study is a binary variable that
takes value 1 if country has deposited its instrument of acceptance or ratied the Doha Amendment. The data is from United
Nations Framework Convention on Climate Change. The Doha
Amendment (adopted: 8 December 2012) establishes a second
commitment period (201320) for the Kyoto Protocol, an international agreement to reduce the emissions of greenhouse gases.
The second commitment period (8 years) aims to reduce GHG
emissions by 18% below 1990 levels and is crucial to preserve
impetus of the ongoing efforts to address the negative consequences of climate change.
2.2. Intelligence
Turning to our main right hand side variable, as concerns the
intelligence measure, we rely on nation IQ scores from Lynn and
Vanhanen (2012). In their seminal work, Lynn and Vanhanen
(2002) have collected the single country studies in which IQ tests
have been carried out. The authors were able to compile a dataset
for intelligence scores for 81 countries. They have estimate national IQs by setting the IQ in UK at 100 (standard deviation 15)
and the scores for remaining nations were rescaled to this metric.
In their 2012 study, they estimated IQ scores for additional 111
countries all the geopolitical territories with population size
above 40,000. In this dataset, national IQ scores range from 60.1
(Malawi) to 107.1 (Singapore).
2.3. Other determinants of environmental commitment
The selection of control variables is mainly driven by arguments posited in the section above. First, past studies lend support
for the positive link between democracy and environmental
commitments (Neumayer, 2002b). We use average of civil rights
and political liberties as a proxy for democratic institutions. We
also control for the effect of English common law as past studies
show that countries with English legal system are associated with
better institutions (Treisman, 2000; Serra, 2006).
(1)
4. Main results
Table 3 provides the main econometric results of logistic regression estimates. Model (1) provides the coefcients for a simple
univariate regression where acceptance of Doha Amendments is
regressed only on intelligence levels. It is obvious that intelligence
has a very strong positive link with environmental commitment.
The coefcient for IQ is 0.094 and statistically signicant at the 1%
level. This coefcient indicates that nations with nation IQ 10-points
above global average are 23% more likely to sign MEA.2 The pseudoR2 statistics also shows that intelligence alone denes 14% of the
cross-national differences in acceptance of Doha Amendment.
In Model (2), we include democracy index and a dummy variable for nations with British civil law. In line with the ndings of
Neumayer (2002b), that democracy is positively associated with
ratication of multilateral environmental agreements. However,
institutional commitment is not associated with historical adoption of British civil law. While controlling for political regime and
past institutional origins slightly reduces estimate for IQ, it retains
its positive effect and signicance level.
Model (3) incorporates economic development (logged GDP
per person) and air pollution (logged per capita CO2 emissions).
Except for GDP per capita, air pollution is statistically signicant
and suggests that countries with higher levels of GHG emissions
are less likely to ratify international environmental agreements.
1
See http://data.worldbank.org/indicator/all.
we use the margins command to estimate the predicted probability of ratifying Doha Amendment at each level of intelligence
2
85
Table 1
Descriptive statistics.
Variable
Description
Source
Mean
Std. Dev.
Environmental commitment
Intelligence
Democracy
English civil law
Economic development
Emissions per capita (log)
Catholic
Protestant
Other religion
Tropics
Absolute latitude (log)
0.396
83.944
4.607
0.332
9.109
0.662
31.424
21.938
31.466
0.368
2.905
0.490
10.794
1.991
0.472
1.222
1.585
35.666
35.164
31.792
0.434
0.959
See http://data.worldbank.org/indicator/all.
Table 2
Correlation matrix.
II
III
IV
VI
VII
VIII
IX
XI
1.00
0.45
0.37
0.02
0.35
0.28
0.14
0.26
0.00
0.18
0.14
1.00
0.52
0.25
0.76
0.73
0.07
0.27
0.17
0.50
0.58
1.00
0.04
0.44
0.37
0.33
0.53
0.03
0.19
0.35
1.00
0.11
0.08
0.22
0.04
0.20
0.24
0.19
1.00
0.93
0.12
0.11
0.10
0.47
0.47
1.00
0.00
0.02
0.01
0.50
0.52
1.00
0.50
0.46
0.26
0.20
1.00
0.36
0.26
0.06
1.00
0.02
0.11
1.00
0.73
1.00
Table 3
Intelligence and environmental commitment: main results.
Intelligence (IQ)
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
0.0940nnn
(0.0172)
0.0885nnn
(0.0192)
0.2022nn
(0.0939)
0.1028nnn
(0.0260)
0.1932n
(0.1019)
0.0902nnn
(0.0179)
0.1289nnn
(0.0256)
0.1139nnn
(0.0334)
0.2070n
(0.1237)
0.1262nnn
(0.0346)
0.2260n
(0.1251)
0.3424nnn
(0.1151)
0.4900
(0.3818)
0.5669
(0.3942)
0.5919
(0.4016)
0.5996nn
(0.2982)
0.5024
(0.4740)
0.6540
(0.4649)
0.5946n
(0.3449)
0.8655n
(0.5027)
0.7005
(0.4762)
0.6320n
(0.3529)
0.0560
(0.4269)
0.9083nn
(0.4320)
0.4000
(0.3231)
0.8372
(0.6916)
0.6783nn
(0.3161)
1.0036
(0.7125)
0.7149nn
(0.3245)
0.7641
(0.6658)
0.3798
(0.3351)
(0.2992)
(1.4836)
15.0417nnn
14.3852nnn
(1.6193)
152
0.14
181
0.25
Democracy
(8)
IQnDemocracy
English civil law
GDP per capita (log)
Emissions per capita (log)
Muslim
Other religion
0.6646
(0.6419)
0.7568nn
Tropics
Absolute latitude (log)
0.6342nnn
(0.2438)
Constant
7.0897nnn
15.7841nnn
(3.7756)
8.5991nn
(1.6556)
(1.9834)
(4.5719)
(4.7433)
(3.9119)
8.8813nnn
8.8866nnn
10.3100
(2.0206)
N
Pseudo-R2
190
0.14
185
0.18
178
0.20
186
0.16
159
0.18
152
0.21
8.3029nnn
nn
nnn
0.1230nnn
(0.0291)
0.2501nn
(0.0988)
0.0153n
(0.0093)
0.6871
(0.4542)
0.0060
(0.0088)
0.0130
(0.0083)
0.0126
(0.0091)
Catholic
(9)
150
0.25
86
5. Robustness tests
To test validity of our ndings we run a number of robustness
tests. First, we re-estimate the effect of IQ on environmental
commitment using an alternative set of geographical control
variables: average temperature and a binary variable for Small
Island developing states (SIDS) (Table 4). For example, Fors (2014)
explores the link between island status and quality of institutions.
The author documents that SIDS are associated with more democratic institutions and stronger rule of law. To assess the tradeenvironment nexus we also include trade as a share of GDP as a
proxy for the trade liberalization policy (see e.g. Neumayer, 2000).
Further, rapid industrialization is associated with air pollution and
increase in acute respiratory infections (Chauhan and Johnston,
2003).3 Therefore, we may anticipate that ratication of MEA is a
function of manufacturing industry lobbying. Therefore, we add
employment in industry as a share of total employment to proxy
for industry lobbying. The results reported in model (1) suggest
3
Consequently, air pollution has negative implications for the society as it has
been related to childhood iron deciency (Rahman et al., 2012) and infant mortality
(Chay and Greenstone, 2003).
Table 4
Intelligence and environmental commitment: robustness test.
Intelligence (IQ)
Democracy
Trade as % of GDP
Employment in industry (log)
0.5199
Temperature
1 if SIDS
(1)
(2)
(3)
0.0932nnn
(0.0309)
0.2732nn
(0.1222)
0.0135nn
(0.0058)
0.9144n
0.1791n
(0.0951)
0.0500
(0.2737)
0.0227
(0.0140)
0.1448nn
(0.0675)
0.4419nnn
(0.1637)
0.0155nn
(0.0075)
2.1306nn
(0.5542)
0.0181
(0.0327)
0.6106
(0.7172)
(1.0478)
0.0529
(0.0689)
0.6344
(1.3749)
(0.8346)
0.0466
(0.0459)
0.0070
(0.9593)
8.1152nnn
(2.7331)
(6.8398)
43
0.14
88
0.27
Constant
11.3451n
18.9274nnn
(6.9493)
N
Pseudo-R2
131
0.21
p o 0.1.
p o 0.05.
po 0.01.
nn
nnn
Table 5
Intelligence and environmental commitment: robustness test.
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
IQ
0.1654nnn
(0.0571)
0.1322nn
(0.052)
0.1021nn
(0.0399)
0.1456nnn
(0.0396)
Excluded
region
N
Pseudo-R2
Africa
Asia
Europe
Latin America
102
0.32
108
0.35
112
0.13
137
0.28
Notes: The vector of control variables as in Table 3 included but not reported here;
Standard errors in parentheses.
n
p o 0.1.
nn
nnn
p o 0.05.
p o 0.01.
6. Conclusion
The analysis presented in this study contributes to better understanding of the nexus of national intelligence, consequences of
democracy, and commitment of national government to international environmental treaties. Our results suggest that developing
nations may benet from investing in cognitive abilities as the effect of democracy on environment depends on the level of national
IQ. Recent advances in empirical research also provide potential
policy suggestions to improve human capital (intelligence) in developing countries (e.g., Kaufman et. al, 2014; Eppig et. al., 2011).
The ndings presented in this paper also provide for further
arguments on the debated issue of impact of political regime on
environmental politics. The research on the consequences of political regimes in general and democracy in particular is highly
inconclusive with the regard to management of public resources.
The study contributes to answering the question on the channels
and mechanisms of the effects of political regime on the quality of
4
87
References
Almond, G., Verba, S., 1989. The Civic Culture: Political Attitudes and Democracy in
Five Nations. Sage Publications, London (1st ed. 1963).
Ahrens, C.D., Henson, R., 2015. Meteorology Today, 11th ed. Brooks Cole, United
States.
Ashraf, Q., Galor, O., 2013. The Out of Africa hypothesis, human genetic diversity,
and comparative economic development. Am. Econ. Rev. 103 (1), 146.
Blomquist, G.C., Whitehead, J.C., 1998. Resource quality information and validity of
willingness to pay in contingent valuation. Resour. Energy Econ. 20 (2),
179196.
Brunekreef, B., 1997. Air pollution and life expectancy: is there a relation? Occup.
Environ. Med. 54 (11), 781.
Chauhan, A.J., Johnston, S.L., 2003. Air pollution and infection in respiratory illness.
Br. Med. Bull. 68 (1), 95112.
Chay, K.Y., Greenstone, M., 2003. The impact of air pollution on infant mortality:
evidence from geographic variation in pollution shocks induced by a recession.
Q. J. Econ. 118 (3), 11211167.
Coyle, D., Stieb, D., Burnett, R., DeCivita, P., Krewski, D., Chen, Y., Thun, M., 2003.
Impact of particulate air pollution on quality-adjusted life expectancy in Canada. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health Part A 66 (1619), 18471864.
Deary, I.J., Batty, G.D., Gale, C.R., 2008. Childhood intelligence predicts voter turnout, voting preferences, and political involvement in adulthood: the 1970
British cohort study. Intelligence 36 (6), 548555.
Eppig, C., Fincher, C.L., Thornhill, R., 2011. Parasite prevalence and the distribution
of intelligence among the states of the USA. Intelligence 39 (2), 155160.
Ferreira, S., Akay, A., Brereton, F., Cuado, J., Martinsson, P., Moro, M., Ningal, T.F.,
2013. Life satisfaction and air quality in Europe. Ecol. Econ. 88, 110.
Fors, H.C., 2014. Do island states have better institutions? J. Comp. Econ. 42 (1),
3460.
Fredriksson, P.G., Neumayer, E., 2013. Democracy and climate change policies: is
history important? Ecol. Econ. 95, 1119.
Fredriksson, P.G., Wollscheid, J.R., 2014. Political institutions, political careers and
environmental policy. Kyklos 67 (1), 5473.
Frankel, J.A., 2009. Environmental effects of international trade. Expert Report No.
31 to Sweden's Globalisation Council. ISSN: 1654-6245, pp. 188.
Hope, A.L., Jones, C.R., 2014. The impact of religious faith on attitudes to environmental issues and Carbon Capture and Storage (CCS) technologies: a mixed
methods study. Technol. Soc. 38, 4859.
Jones, G., 2011. National IQ and national productivity: the hive mind across Asia.
Asian Dev. Rev. 28 (1), 5171.
Kanazawa, S., 2010. Evolutionary psychology and intelligence research. Am. Psychol. 65, 279289.
Kanazawa, S., 2012. The Intelligence Paradox: Why the Intelligent Choice Isn't Always the Smart One. Wiley, New York.
Kaufman, A.S., Zhou, X., Reynolds, M.R., Kaufman, N.L., Green, G.P., Weiss, L.G., 2014.
The possible societal impact of the decrease in US blood lead levels on adult IQ.
Environ. Res. 132, 413420.
Kodila-Tedika, O., Asongu, S., 2015. Genetic distance and cognitive human capital. A
cross-national investigation. J. Bioecon. http://link.springer.com/article/10.
1007%2Fs10818-015-9210-7
Kwok, R., Rothrock, D.A., 2009. Decline in Arctic sea ice thickness from submarine
and ICESat records: 19582008. Geophys. Res. Lett. 36, 15.
La Porta, R., Lopez-de-Silanes, F., Shleifer, A., Vishny, R., 1999. The quality of government. J. Law Econ. Organ. 15 (1), 222279.
Libman, A., Obydenkova, O., 2014. The governance of commons in a large nondemocratic state: the case of forestry in the Russian Federation. Publius: J. Fed.
44 (2), 298323.
Lynn, R., Vanhanen, T., 2002. IQ and the Wealth of Nations. Praeger, Westport, CT.
Lynn, R., Vanhanen, T., 2012. National IQs: a review of their educational, cognitive,
economic, political, demographic, sociological, epidemiological, geographic and
88