Parncutt (2007) - Can Researchers Help Artists
Parncutt (2007) - Can Researchers Help Artists
Parncutt (2007) - Can Researchers Help Artists
Introduction
Recent years have seen a steady stream of new books that present the results of music
performance research and show how it can be applied to the everyday teaching and practice of music performance. Parncutt and McPherson (2002) covered general aspects of music psychology, the psychology of specific musial skills, and the acoustics of different musical instruments, and attempted to develop specific strategies for applying that research
in music education at all levels. Rink (2002) combined relevant humanities scholarship (including music history and analysis) and scientific research (e.g., the psychology of memory) with the academically grounded views and experience of eminent performers and
Article
teachers. Williamon (2004) presented a broad coverage of current performance research
that is relevant for the best performers and music students, as well as physical and psychological techniques that can help music students achieve excellence. Odam and Bannan
(2005) addressed such diverse topics as creativity, musical communication, improvisation,
physiology of performance, and questions of artistic and ethnic interculturality. As the title The child as musician suggests, McPherson (2006) considered early musical development, but also presented a wealth of information that is relevant for post-secondary music
institutions.1 Altenmller, Wiesendanger and Kesselring (2006) focused on the physiological basis of the virtuoso technique and demonstrated that modern brain research is becoming increasingly relevant for musicians. Lehmann, Sloboda and Woody (2007) aimed to
help musicians without scientific or research training to interpret and apply the results of
psychological research in music performance.
Fuelling these new books is a steady expansion of interest in music performance research, including not only conferences and postgraduate courses, but also a general acknowledgment from scholars and practitioners alike that collaborative, interdisciplinary
investigations can contribute both to a greater understanding of performance and to the
application of that understanding to practical music making. The theoretical knowledge
gained in this research does not necessarily have practical applications for teachers and
performers (nor should it), but clearly a lot of it can be applied in useful ways, many of
which are only beginning to be put into practice.
In parallel with this grassroots expansion, government agencies, research councils,
grant awarding bodies, and international post-secondary music institutions are encouraging performance teachers to engage in and document their practice-related or practice-based research. This is driven in part by a desire to quantify how performance
teachers spend their time working within higher education, which can have both positive
and negative consequences for the performers themselves. On the positive side, greater
transparency may highlight the quality, difficulty and value of their everyday work, which
may help them to organise their time more effectively or to attract funding. On the negative side, every investigation of performance teachers daily work may be perceived by
them as a restriction of their creative freedom.
The above books represent a sizeable and relatively accessible reservoir of information
that can be useful for teaching and curriculum development. The question I wish to address in this paper is how, exactly, such materials might best be used. For mostly they do
not directly answer fundamental questions such as the following:
How can music educators both full- and part-time - best inform themselves about
relevant music performance research given their busy schedules and limited time?
To what extent should they apply, or be able to apply, such research in their teaching?
How much time should music students spend studying and applying that research?
I use the term post-secondary music institution as a blanket term for all institutions whose primary aim is to train music performance skills. The term is intended to include all colleges, academies, conservatories, Hochschulen and universities of music (music universities). It is beyond
the present scope to distinguish among different kinds of post-secondary music institution. The
term post-secondary is equivalent to higher or tertiary. In this context, the term music
usually refers (ethnocentrically) to the notated music of Western cultural elites.
Article
To what extent should the curricula2 offered by post-secondary music institutions include units based on music performance research that are presented by performerresearchers?
Which units of study ought to be compulsory, which elective, and which entirely voluntary?
Post-secondary music institutions are increasingly coming under pressure to improve or
enlarge the academic content of their activities (teaching and research) so as to emulate
the status of universities. At the same time, demands on individual music educators are
changing: as the volume of empirical research in music education and music performance
grows, they are increasingly expected to be acquainted with aspects of that research that
apply directly to their speciality, and to be able to apply it.
It is natural for well-established post-secondary music institutions with a long tradition
of excellence to resist change, especially when that change involves research with which
the administration may not be familiar and whose implications and benefits may be unclear in advance. Moreover, communication between performance teachers and performance researchers is often not well developed.
This raises fundamental questions about the possible benefits and dangers of teaching
performance research to students. Can the efficiency of post-secondary music education
be enhanced by presenting and applying research on music performance? By efficiency I
mean the ratio of output to input, where input may be regarded as the time and effort
that students devote to their training as well as the financial cost of their training, and
output includes the musical quality of students performances after their training, the
quality of the general education that music educators pass onto their students, and the
general contribution that music students later make, either directly or indirectly, to the
quality of life at different levels through their musical activities. Although the concept of
efficiency has a disagreeable neo-liberal feel about it, no-one will deny that it is important to manage public money responsibly and to get the most out of limited resources; and
students will be grateful for useful tips on how to get the most out of their practice time.
My paper is primarily practically oriented and draws on my experience both as a music
student and a music performance researcher. Some details about my experience may be
appropriate here, since performers and students reading this article may evaluate the authority of the text on the basis of my musical credentials. I studied music at the Faculty of
Music, University of Melbourne from 1976 to 1980. The program had three nominally equal
strands that covered the history, theory and performance of Western music. My piano
teacher, Diana Weekes, has always been interested in performance research; she is now
affiliated with the Elder Conservatorium of Music, Adelaide, and commented in some detail on a draft of this paper. I concurrently studied physics, which allowed me to compare
three different ways of academic thinking (epistemologies): humanities, sciences and musical practice. My undergraduate studies also gave me the opportunity to experience a
mixture of intensive performance practice and a full-time load of academic subjects
considerably more than I expect from the music students to whom this paper is addressed.
For several years after finishing my undergraduate studies, I appeared regularly as soloist
or accompanist in amateur performances. As a music performance researcher, I have carried out research on the psychology of piano fingering that combined empirical observa-
I avoid the ambiguous term course and instead refer to units and curricula. A curriculum
(or program) typically lasts 3-4 years; a unit within a curriculum typically lasts one semester.
Article
tion and data analysis with computer modelling (e.g., Parncutt et al., 1997). I have also
co-edited a book that covers the main areas of performance research (Parncutt & McPherson, 2002).
In the following, I will focus on aspects of music performance research that could realistically enrich the training of musicians and music educators. This research addresses issues as diverse as improvisation, expression, practice, performance anxiety, music medicine, and the physics, physiology and psychology of performance on specific instruments.
After considering each of these topics in turn, I will proceed to some practical and political issues. Why are many such matters currently not taught, or not adequately taught?
What might be the effect of introducing such materials to music and music education curricula? What political strategies might promote their introduction?
My contribution has an exploratory character. The envisaged pedagogical applications
of music performance research are still in an early stage of development. I offer a range
of issues for discussion but avoid giving premature answers to specific questions.
Academic units in post-secondary music curricula
Music students are generally exposed to academic materials such as music history and music theory/analysis, as well as conducting. Staff meetings about music curricula may feature lively discussions about the advantages and disadvantages of academic work for practising musicians. Some may argue that academic work should be reduced to an absolute
minimum, claiming that music students need to spend as much time as possible on practice.
This argument is supported by psychological research on expertise. According to Ericsson et al. (1993), it takes about 10 years and, during that time, about 10 000 hours of
practice to become a recognized expert in just about any competitive discipline, of which
music performance is a good example. Most research on the acquisition of expertise - in
areas as diverse as sport, chess or music is consistent with the idea that you have to
clock up at least that amount of hard work if you want to make it in your chosen field.
Of course there are individual differences. In the case of music, it takes longer for
some instruments (pianists?) than others (flautists?) to acquire basic technical skills. Some
instruments (violin?) may be fundamentally harder to play well (relative to the skills of the
best current performers) than others (cello?). And instrumentalists may begin learning
their instrument at a very early age (four?) while singers may wait until their voices mature. Individual students entering any tertiary institution vary widely in the level of their
relevant cognitive, physiological skills or gifts as well as other less quantifiable parameters such as motivation, creativity, and imagination; those with more of the latter may
eventually overtake those who initially have more of the former. Ericsson did not ignore
such variations, but went beyond them. His point, which is supported by a wide range of
empirical data, was that the amount of practice time is centrally important in spite of obvious individual differences.
A music student who averages three hours of practice per day every day of the year
(which may mean practising six hours on some days to balance the holidays, days off, days
of illness, days during which practice was impossible due to travel, etc.) can accumulate
about 1000 hours in a year. If the 10000-hour-hypothesis of Ericsson and colleagues is correct and it has stood its ground since 1993 and if a student enters a post-secondary mu-
Article
sic institution with some 5000 hours of accumulated practice behind him or her, keeps up
an average of three hours per day every day throughout four years of study and continues
to practise and perform at about this rate after finishing study, Ericssons hypothesis predicts that s/he has a good chance of becoming a recognised professional musician.
An obvious implication is that the academic units that I am proposing in this paper
should take up a relatively small proportion of music students time perhaps between 10
% and 30% - so that students have plenty of time for practice. But students should not
practice too much, either. Too much practice can be counterproductive, for two reasons:
Workaholism. Moderate amounts of workaholism can be viewed as positive and even necessary. Both the research on expertise cited above and everyday experience suggest that
one needs to be at least a little obsessed with ones work in order to succeed in competition with other, similarly slightly obsessed people in the same field. But obsession can also
lead to psychological and social problems. Workaholics tend to be perfectionist, unable to
delegate responsibility, stressed and unhealthy (Spence & Robbins, 1992). Like other addictions, excessive work is associated with identity issues, rigid thinking, withdrawal,
progressive involvement, and denial as well as problems involving decision making
goals effectiveness interpersonal relations (Porter, 1996, abstract). High achievers
know not only how to work long and intensively, but also how to plan and fill their recreational time appropriately.
The risk of injury. The amount of practice that a musician can do in one day can be limited by the risk of performance injuries and other medical problems, for which the most
common causes are stress and overuse (Brandfonbrener & Kjelland, 2002). There are presumably large individual differences in the ability to cope with stress. The risk of injury
can be reduced by appropriate playing techniques and by practising more efficiently so as
to reduce the total duration of daily practice. The latter may be achieved without slowing
progress by regular silent (mental) practice, which may be regarded as part of practice in
the broader sense of any preparation for performance. But the degree to which mental
practice can substitute for real physical interaction with instrument and sound is limited.
It follows that academic work may at the very least be a relevant distraction that reduces the chance that music students become obsessed with practice or develop medical
problems assuming, of course, that the students already have the time and facilities that
are necessary to practise effectively. But academic work can also broaden students horizons in ways that make them more interesting, self-efficacious (self-reflective, creative,
independent) people and also, directly or indirectly, improve their musicianship. In this
sense, academic training may be considered as a central and integral part of the training
of all-round, intelligent musicians.
How much time, exactly, should music students spend on academic work by comparison
to performance and practice? This question is not easy to answer:
The ratio depends on the optimal amount and distribution of practice time for a given
student on a given instrument.
The ratio depends on the kind of academic materials that are likely to be musically
useful to individual students. What will help them to acquire the technical abilities
that they need to reach their specific musical goals? To develop effective, original interpretations? To make appropriate career choices? What are their academic interests
- regardless of any implications for their performance?
The ratio depends on each individual students personality and approach to learning
(cognitive style). Curricula need to be flexible enough to allow for considerable varia-
Article
tions in these factors and if possible within the usual financial restrictions - provide
for monitoring and discussion in which students can verbalise and develop their own
specific needs and aspirations.
The ratio depends on the institution its history, its general orientation, and its internal culture. Changes to the curricular offerings of an institution should be big enough
to maintain the institutions dynamism and small enough that valuable traditions are
maintained.
To understand these issues properly, it may be interesting to research the history of the
specific institution (e.g., Wright, 2005) and to compare the history of similar institutions
in different countries, for example the Royal College of Music in London, the Paris Conservatoire, the Juillard School in New York, the Indiana University Jacobs School of Music in
Bloomington, the Sydney Conservatorium, and equivalent institutions in Asia, Africa and
South America. It may also be useful to compare the basic statistics of such institutions:
student and staff numbers, budgets, internal structure, links to other institutions or universities, courses offered, balance of academic and practical work, assessment practices
and extracurricular support services. This may be done on the basis of existing data without subjecting performance teachers to (additional) evaluation procedures.
Specific academic units
Music students may be offered, required to take, or required to choose among academic
units of differing degrees of specificity. They are traditionally offered courses in music
history, music theory and aural skills. To these may be added a mixture of introductory
and advanced or specific courses.
Introductory courses (aka foundation materials) may include general introductions to
areas such as music psychology and music performance research. They should be placed
relatively early in the program (in the first or second year) and be prerequisites for later,
more specific units. It may be appropriate to make them compulsory for all students, as
without them they are not in a good position to evaluate the advantages of later units. At
the very least, they may represent an interesting alternative for those who are already
proficient in aural skills.
More advanced units should generally be elective or voluntary, and may be classified
into broad areas such as theory/analysis/composition, performance, teaching, and personal development. The present paper focuses on performance and personal development,
but the other two categories are no less important. Students of theory, analysis and composition should be given the opportunity to learn about and apply relevant recent research
in music psychology (e.g., Bigand et al., 2003; Ferguson & Parncutt, 2004; Krumhansl,
1990; Larson, 2004; Parncutt, 1989, 2004; Temperley, 2001; Tillmann, et al., 2003). Regarding teaching, all music students should be encouraged or required to take advanced
units, because so many will teach in their later careers; such units may focus for example
on the application of appropriate pedagogical theories to specific teaching contexts and
situations, or the educational implications of research in developmental music psychology
(e.g., McPherson, 2006).
Advanced academic units on performance may apply either to most (all?) instruments or
to specific instruments.3 Units that apply to all instruments may address questions such as
3
Here, I consider the singing voice as an instrument and different voices (from bass to soprano) as
Article
how to practise efficiently or improve ones ability to memorise, improvise, sight-read, or
communicate structure and emotion (expression). They may also address aspects of personal development including performance anxiety and medical problems. Any topic that
supposedly applies to all instruments will be approached differently by performers of different instruments, but presenting such materials in a general fashion will also allow students to benefit from exposure to issues faced by other instrumentalists. Units that apply
to specific instruments or instrument families may address the physics, physiology and
psychology of performance on that instrument in detail.
Post-secondary performance institutions have never completely neglected these aspects of a rounded music education, but from the point of view of modern performance
research, their approach has been problematic. Performance teachers have been expected
to address such issues on the basis of their personal experience and intuition. The recent
growth of research in these areas means that this material can increasingly be taught in
separate units by experts who perform the research themselves. In the following, I give a
quick overview of such units just enough to whet the readers appetite. More details can
be found in the books listed above.
Physics, physiology and psychology of performance on specific instruments
Instrument families differ considerably in their underlying physics as well as the physiology
and psychology of performance. To cover more than one instrument family would exceed
the scope of this article. Therefore, I will focus on piano, which also happens to be the
instrument that I know the best.
Once musicians acquire a substantial technique, they tend, both as performers and
teachers, to focus on expressive communication and interpretation. The consequences for
student pianists were documented by Parncutt and Holming (2000). We observed that student pianists know remarkably little about the physics, physiology and psychology of piano
performance not because of any lack of interest, but because of a lack of exposure to
the material or to pianists who understand it well and regard it as important. Piano students know relatively little about the mechanics and acoustics of the piano, the relevant
physiology of the fingers, hands and arms, and relevant aspects of the psychology of motor
control and rhythmic coordination. They are unclear about what can and cannot affect the
timbre of a single piano tone or a whole passage of music. They do not know that the timbre of an isolated tone depends only on key velocity, noises (hammer-string, key-keybed,
finger-key) and pedals. Nor can they explain how the piano nevertheless produces remarkable timbral richness and variation. They are unable to describe their own strategies for
determining fingerings and the contrasting roles of physical, anatomic, motor, and cognitive constraints, and they are often unclear about how fingering may depend on expertise
and interpretation. They often do not realise that if they play one note in a chord in one
hand louder than the other tones, and all keys in the chord are struck (or begin to move)
at the same time, the faster key will reach the keybed before the others, so the louder
tone will physically begin earlier; nor can they explain why this effect is often inaudible,
how it can be controlled, or whether it should be controlled or deliberately encouraged
(cf. Goebl, 2001).
We may have been expecting too much. After all, the same students may not have been
members of an instrumental family.
Article
able to demonstrate much knowledge about music history or theory, either. But they may
nevertheless have been excellent pianists. One does not need to understand how a car
works in order to be able to drive it excellently. Independently of any discussion about
academic study, music students must be constantly aware of the sound that they are producing: the goal is more important than the road that leads to it. Moreover, the process of
acquiring analytical knowledge about the mechanics of performance may affect student
musicians instinct for aural exploration or their sensitivity for those mystical qualities of
music that cannot be reduced to physics or rationality.4
One may nevertheless confidently assert that piano students at a music university
should have at least some familiarity with the above topics and questions. Because pianists spend long hours every day practising, it is important that this time be spent efficiently. Improved knowledge of relevant physics, physiology, and psychology - which can
be acquired in a fraction of the time necessary to acquire advanced pianistic skills - may
help pianists to achieve technical and interpretative goals more quickly. These arguments
may be valid even if the technical and musical benefits are unclear or are not enjoyed by
all pianists.
Practice
Since musicians and music students spend very large amounts of time practising, it is important for them to systematically consider and compare different approaches to practising. The ultimate aim of such an exercise might be to make their practice more efficient,
so that they can reach specific technical goals with a minimum of time and effort, leaving
more time for interpretational exploration.
Relevant psychological and pedagogical research in this area was summarized by Barry
and Hallam (2002). A diversity of approaches is generally better than a limited number;
possible approaches include the study and analysis of scores, mental versus physical practice, and listening to recordings of works being studied, as well as live performances.
Practice is more efficient when musicians can think and talk about the advantages and
disadvantages of their practice routine or habits, and plan the structure of their practice
sessions according to the specific goals of that session. In the psychological literature, this
is referred to as metacognition, or thinking about thinking.
An important prerequisite for any long-term practice program is intrinsic motivation.
Practice is most effective if the musician is motivated not only from without (extrinsic
motivation, such as the pressure of a coming concert) but also from within (intrinsic motivation, such as the pleasure or reward of practising for its own sake). Intrinsic motivation
is related to the experience of flow (Csikszentmihalyi & Rich, 1997; B. J. Kenny & Gellrich, 2002), and can be enhanced by subdividing a practice goal (such as learning a piece
for performance on a certain date) into individual subgoals. To encourage flow states,
these subgoals should be pitched relative to the musicians level of skill: they should be
difficult enough to represent interesting challenges, but not so difficult that they might
lead to frustration.
Music students could benefit from a study unit or similar that not only presents this
4
It would be interesting to study this effect empirically does it exist at all, and if so what is its
duration?
Article
kind of research but also gives them a forum to talk about and investigate their own practice routine and to find out how other students and teachers practise. Such a unit might
aim to balance theory based on published psychological studies against systematic, constructive interaction with peers who have similar or different problems. The instructor
should also spend some time supervising the practice of individual students. Students
would need a method of evaluating the success of the new approaches to practise to
which they are exposed.
Improvisation
The ability to improvise may be unnecessary but still useful for students with a classical
focus. Training in improvisation can improve their practical knowledge and practical command of relevant musical styles and help them to recover from memory lapses in performance. This point together with the growing literature on improvisation in various disciplines suggests that the time has come for performance teachers in all musical styles, including classical, to address this issue. Many undergraduate curricula already include a
selection of theoretical and practical approaches to improvisation in various Western
(jazz, organ, early music) and non-Western musical styles and genres, and in music education. The teachers of such courses may benefit from collaboration with performance researchers, who can provide theories, strategies, and evaluations of pedagogical approaches.
The topic of improvisation is interesting for all musicians and music educators from
early childhood education to the training of the most advanced performers. Why has it has
been neglected? Within the European musical tradition, the art of improvisation all but
died out during the 19th Century, as printed music became freely available. Previously, it
had been common for both teachers and students to improvise exercises and thereby to
simultaneously train technical, improvisational, auditory, harmonic and expressive skills
(Gellrich, 1992). During the 20th Century, many mainstream Western music teachers felt
unable to improvise or to teach improvisation, because they themselves had no training or
experience. Today, it is still difficult to convince teachers, parents or administrators of
the virtues of developing improvisational skills, because many of the best musicians still
lack those skills - a vicious circle.
It is not hard to play an ostinato accompaniment while a student improvises a simple
melody over the top (or vice-versa), or to create call-answer improvisations together with
a student. The hardest challenge may be to overcome deeply entrenched inhibitions. But
once the ice is broken, both teacher and student can make steady progress. Can music
performance research develop new and effective approaches?
Lassnig (2004) developed psychologically inspired strategies for teaching improvisation.
He first recommended having students improvise within strictly defined limits. For example, a student might improvise rhythmically and expressively on a single tone, then on two
tones, then three. Or analogous dynamic, rhythmic or articulatory limits may be established in advance of an improvisation. It is revealing and motivating for students even
advanced jazz players - to (re-) discover the extent to which one can make music within
such strict limits. Such an approach can also clarify why many musicians find improvisation
difficult: improvisers must invent two things at once, notes and expression. Having recognized that, a pedagogically effective strategy may be to focus on the expressive aspect
Article
and to encourage flow states in which musical and technical challenges are matched to
existing skills. This may indirectly accelerate the learning of patterns such as scales and
chords. In this context, Lassnig developed the idea that instead of practising improvisation
on specific structural elements such as a chord or mode, specific structural elements can
be combined with specific skills, so that only one element and one skill are practised at a
time. Examples of skills are listening, seeing, moving (technique), feeling and knowing.
Separating these from each produces a large number of combinations to practise individually.
Expression
Music students seldom receive explicit instruction on specific means of musical expression
(possible exception: affect in early music). Instead, they imitate the expressive styles of
their teachers and other performers that they experience in concerts and on records. In
that way they gradually develop their own expressive style. This process happens largely
intuitively, without students or teachers analysing the detailed, note-for-note relationship
between expressive parameters such as timing and dynamics and the structure or emotion
that is being expressed.
Popular ideas about musical performance and performers are largely based on accounts
of musical talent and genius, for example in biographies of eminent musicians in programme notes and magazines. Such sources emphasize that musicians strong, personal
experiences of the emotions that they express is the basis of their ability to move an audience. But while authentic emotional involvement is clearly important, performers are also
aware of the pitfalls of getting too involved. In order to remain technically in control, it is
often necessary to maintain an appropriate distance from the emotions that one is communicating. Seen from this viewpoint, the art of musical performance is one of manipulating the audiences motions.
How is this done? Psychological research on expression has clarified the issue in a number of ways. First, a distinction has been made between structural and emotional communication. Expressive performance parameters that make it clear, for example, when a
phrase ends and a new phrase begins, are structural. The way in which this is done (the
shape of tempo/dynamic functions or gestures) can determine the specific emotions that
are communicated. Thus, it is possible to separate the goals of structural and emotional
expression but not the means, which overlap.
Friberg and Battel (2002) analysed some of the means with which performers communicate musical structure to their audiences. Their approach is well suited to computer simulations of musical expression, in which a computer converts a score into a sound file that
sounds considerably more musical (natural and expressive) than a direct conversion of
score into sound. Their approach may also be applied in music education, if the complex
relationship between specific structures and associated expressive devices can be concisely explained and illustrated by a human performer at a real instrument. In Parncutt
(2003), I presented a simplified and generalised version of their theory based on an extended concept of accent - any musical event to which attention may be drawn, or any
salient (perceptually important) event. An event can be salient for reasons that are clear
from the score (immanent accent) or for reasons associated with the manner of performance (performed accent). Examples of immanent accents are melodic accents (the peak of
10
Article
a melodic contour) or grouping accents (the start of a phrase). Examples of performed accents are temporary decreases of tempo (the delaying and lengthening of individual tones)
and temporary increases (or sometimes decreases) in dynamic level. Performers often use
performed accents to attract attention to important events (immanent accents), which
can make the structure of a piece of music clear to listeners. In other words, performed
accents often have the function of reinforcing immanent accents. This apparently trivial
observation can form the basis for a systematic approach to understanding expression and
to raising awareness of ones own intuitive expressive strategies. The fact that performed
accents can be introduced into a performance in many different ways and combinations is
consistent with the wide variety of possible effective interpretations of a piece of music.
The way in which performed accents are deployed is a matter of interpretation and involves a number of intuitive, artistic choices. These include the temporal positions at
which the accents fall (i.e., which immanent accents should be reinforced and which not),
the strength of the accents (how clearly they are perceived), and the physical way in
which they are realized. In piano performance, for example, performed accents may be
realized by changing loudness, tempo, or both; and the shape of the function of loudness
and/or tempo against time is variable. Since the performer is not necessarily aware of the
way in which s/he is using performed accents, and one of the aims of applying this theory
of expression in music education is to make this process more conscious and deliberate at least during practice and training.
Psychological theories of structural communication can form the basis for a new approach to music analysis that is directly relevant to music performance. A unit on this subject might begin with the analysis of passages of music that students are currently practising, marking different kinds of immanent accents with different symbols. Next, students
might analyse their own playing of the passages - perhaps by listening to recordings of
themselves. What are they intuitively doing in the vicinity of specific immanent accents?
What kinds of accents are they choosing for expressive treatment? What kind of treatment? Which accents are being ignored? How does this pattern change when they play different kinds of music? In this way, students can develop performance-oriented analyses of
their repertoire while at the same time becoming more aware of their own expressive
strategies.
A similarly analytic approach has been developed by Juslin and Persson (2002) in the
domain of emotional communication. On the basis of a large body of empirical data, Juslin
summarized the expressive techniques associated with specific emotions such as anger or
tenderness. For example, anger is characterized by high sound level, sharp timbre, spectral noise, fast tempo, staccato articulation, abrupt tone attacks, increased durational
contrasts between long and short notes, no ritardando, sudden accents, accents on tonally
unstable notes, crescendo, phrase accelerando, large vibrato extent (p. 223). This seemingly trivial observation becomes interesting when students try to apply it to specific musical contexts. Which of the points in Juslins list can appropriately be applied to a passage in a given style? How successfully can students communicate a given emotion using
this method? To what extent are they capable of discriminating between different emotions in their performance? This is surely an important skill that deserves to be studied and
practised separately from other skills. As such it could usefully be included in every musicians technical toolkit. Like other skills, it can first be acquired analytically and later
combined with other skills - integrated into the students holistic personal interpretive
style.
11
Article
Empirical data (e.g., Woody, 1999) confirm that an analytical approach of this kind can
help musicians to communicate emotion. But it is unclear to what extent such training can
transfer from the confines of a controlled experimental situation to the concert platform
and to a range of different musical styles. Besides, an overtly analytical approach may be
more suitable for some personalities and cognitive styles than others. Beyond that, it is
the everyday experience of performance teachers that some students have something to
say musically and others do not. The extent to which the desire and ability to communicate musically can be trained, and if so how, is unclear.
Personal development
The personal and musical development of individual students may be supported through
guided or coached self-reflection, including such personal factors as motivation, behavioural patterns, stress management, goal orientation, self efficacy, self regulation, and
attributions (Dweck, 1999; Painsi, 2007). Here, I will not consider the personal development of musicians in detail, but instead address two central aspects: anxiety and injuries.
These two issues are closely related to each other (Williamon & Thompson, 2006). Both
are important for most musicians, because most musicians suffer or have suffered from
both. And in spite of recent developments in research and improved dissemination of research findings, the general level of awareness among musicians about symptoms, causes,
preventive measures and treatments in both areas remains low. In music curricula, it may
therefore be appropriate to address these two topics within the same unit that also considers broader issues of the three-way interaction among the individual musician, the music being performed, and the society that consumes the product. Such a unit should
balance classroom work with individual coaching.
Performance anxiety
Research on performance anxiety has been summarized by various authors including T. D.
Kenny and Ackermann (in press), Mornell (2002), and Wilson & Roland (2002). In the following I aim to whet the readers appetite with selected ideas from this burgeoning field
of research.
It is common knowledge among musicians that a small or moderate amount of performance anxiety can benefit a performance. The performer becomes more alert and more responsive to the audience, the situation, and unexpected musical events as they occur
spontaneously during a performance. Performance anxiety only becomes a problem when
it is so strong that the performance is negatively affected by errors and slips of memory,
loss of control over interpretation, and so on.
The extent and nature of performance anxiety depends on three main factors: the personality of the musician, their mastery of the task in hand, and (their perception of) the
situation in which they perform (Wilson, 2002). The knowledge that these three aspects
can be separately analysed may help musicians to develop realistic strategies to counter
performance anxiety in specific situations:
A musician may be generally shy, anxious or afraid of people, regardless of whether
music is involved or not (trait anxiety). Such students may benefit from psychotherapy
12
Article
that is tailored to deal with this problem. Of course, this is a sensitive issue that
should be approached very carefully by teachers.
Performance anxiety depends on the extent to which a student is technically and musically prepared for a performance. If this turns out to be the main problem, the student has no choice but to practise more, or more effectively.
Performance anxiety may be treated or managed in a variety of ways including meditation, physical relaxation, breathing exercises, yoga, aerobic exercise, hypnotherapy, Feldenkrais, the Alexander technique, psychotherapy, guided imagery, religious faith, coping
skills development and assertiveness training. The empirical literature is only beginning to
systematically investigate the effectiveness of such therapies and interventions.
Music medicine
In a broad definition, the term music medicine involves all medical problems that may
be caused or exacerbated by musical performance, ranging from counterproductive everyday levels of stress and tension to chronic pain and disabilities.
Music students and musicians often suffer from chronic tension or reduced muscle elasticity in the pelvis, lower spine or the back of the neck (Erlitz-Lanegger, 1997). Problems
like this can arise when similar motor actions are repeated countless times while the rest
of the body is held in a relatively inflexible posture. Since motor actions and postures differ from one instrument to the next, each instrument or family has its own characteristic
medical problems. Medical problems also depend on the kind of repertoire being performed, the personality of the musician, and the interaction between the two. For this
reason, it is important that medical practitioners specializing in music medicine have a
complete picture of the musical situation in which the problems develop (Brandfonbrener
& Kjelland, 2002).
A lot of useful material could be packed into a unit on music medicine. The unit could
begin with a presentation of relevant background information in anatomy and physiology,
and proceed to develop exercises that students can perform at and away from their instrument. Students may be given advice on organising their practice time, dealing with
the stress of approaching performances, and sport and nutrition. Teachers who work with
children may need specific guidance. All these points can also be topics for group discussions.
Those who are not convinced of the relevance of such materials for music or music
education curricula should consider the implications of not providing this information. Prevention is better than cure: serious problems can presumably be prevented if one is aware
of the problem in advance and has a repertoire of strategies to avoid it. But if a musician
waits for a problem to become chronic, it can affect her or his whole career and necessitate repeated medical consultation over a long period. Regardless of whether the costs
13
Article
are carried by the individual or the state, medical treatment is expensive. Of course, an
increased awareness of the causes and symptoms of music medical problems may not necessarily prevent them. But even if increasing music students awareness can only sometimes prevent the emergence of medical problems, it is worthwhile investing in awareness-raising.
Analytic versus holistic thinking
How should the above materials be presented to music students? An optimal pedagogical
approach depends not only on the students interests, motivation and relevant background
knowledge, but also on fundamental issues such as how they think (Painsi, 2004).
In their everyday work, musicians rely on a wide range of technical and cognitive skills.
They need to be able to think both analytically and holistically. Analytical or logical thinking is necessary to understand the mechanics of musical instruments, the conventions of
music notation, ones own performance technique (motor control), and the physical and
perceptual complexities of musical sound production. Holistic or intuitive thinking is associated with skills of musical interpretation and empathy with other people, including conductors and ensemble musicians, audience members, and composers, both living and
dead. Evidence for the constant interaction between analytic and holistic thinking in music performance is the increased size of the corpus callosum in musicians (Schlaug et al.,
1995); the corpus callosum links the two brain hemispheres.
Everyday experience suggests that, on the whole, musicians and non-musicians think in
different ways they have different cognitive styles. Since musicians spend such a lot of
time expressing themselves in sound, one might expect them to excel in ways of thinking
consistent with that activity. Since they spend less time doing other things, one might expect that they are less skilled in mental abilities that are inconsistent with musical practice and performance. Musicians also differ considerably from each other in this regard
(Kemp, 1996).
Brandler and Rammsayer (2003) investigated the cognitive styles and abilities of musicians and non-musicians by asking them to perform standard psychological tests. The musicians scored better on verbal memory tasks, while the non-musicians were better at
logical problems such as completing a series of pictures, spotting which two pictures out
of a set of five violate an implied rule, completing a matrix, and topological reasoning. It
was unclear whether these differences were innate or learned; the non-musician participants were studying psychology, law and physics and were therefore regularly practising
their logical thinking skills, but may also have self-selected to these units on the basis of
pre-existing skills. The authors of the study regarded the enhanced verbal memory skills of
the musicians as part of a global cognitive style "that deals with simultaneous relationships
and more global properties of patterns" (p. 132) consistent with the holistic, intuitive
nature of audiation (the imagination of sound structures and associated meanings).
If these findings are true of most music students and their professors, what are the implications for the content of music and music education curricula? One could argue that
since the best musicians are best at global, intuitive thinking, the contents of the curriculum should be matched to that skill. The trouble with that approach is that musicians and
music educators need the ability to think analytically in their everyday work. Like the
14
Article
members of any other profession, the degree to which they possess that ability depends
on the degree to which they use it. If young musicians are denied training in analytic
thinking, they may always be deficient in it, which may negatively impact on their work as
musicians or music educators, or their flexibility when it comes to changing professions or
working across academic and practical disciplines.
The best way to enhance the existing analytical, logical thinking skills of music students
is to encourage them to develop those skills in areas that are of direct relevance to them.
In this paper, I have given several examples of such areas. Developing this ability will also
help students to apply the findings of performance research to their own musical practice.
A certain amount of analytic and logical thinking is necessary to understand the relationship between the methods and the findings of research projects.
Recommendations
The above considerations suggest that major post-secondary music institutions should, as
far as possible within existing human and financial constraints, include relevant performance research in their curricula. Beyond compulsory introductions to music psychology and
music performance research, curricula should include research-based electives (lectures,
seminars, workshops, group teaching and so on) in areas such as:
Technical aspects of performance:
physics, physiology, and psychology of performance on own instrument
improvisation
expression
sight-reading
memory
intonation
Students should be required to take some of these units, but have the freedom to choose
among them. Consider a European Bachelors curriculum comprising altogether 180 ECTS
points (European Credit Transfer System) over three years of full-time study. If the listed
units were worth 2 points each and students were required to take 5 of them, the set
would be worth 10 points or 6% of the curriculum. If this proposal reduces the level of
other, traditional subjects, it should only be realised if the shift of emphasis is likely to
improve the overall efficiency of the curriculum. By efficiency I mean the ratio of desired output to input: the output is what students, teachers, administrators, politicians
and the public want to achieve, and the input is the amount of time, effort and money
that they invest.
For these goals to be achieved, it would be important to clearly define the objectives
of each unit. It may not be enough simply to give a summary of relevant research and to
try to apply this material to the needs of the individual students in practical exercises. For
15
Article
example, the objectives of a unit on physics, physiology and psychology of piano performance might be to reduce the cognitive and physical load on the pianist, to free resources
that can be used for expression and interpretation, and to develop the technical skills to
achieve specific interpretive goals. An evaluation of the unit would investigate the extent
to which such objectives were achieved.
Frequent objections
Because much of the relevant scientific (including psychological) research on music performance is quite recent, much of it has not yet found its way into the curricula of postsecondary music institutions. Eminent performance teachers are rightfully cautious of new
developments, so a time lag between research developments and their implementation is
normal and appropriate. The duration of the lag depends on how individual researchers
who wish to teach these materials respond to critical resistance from performers. If researchers are to develop realistic, mutually acceptable strategies for curricular change,
they must take the practitioners objections seriously and adjust their pedagogical strategies accordingly. That may mean systematically documenting these objections and analysing accompanying arguments and related issues.
Objections based on academic content. Performance teachers may object that foreign
ideas and teachers might interfere with the content and quality of their teaching and the
teacher-student relationship. While this is a real danger, the counterarguments are
equally strong:
Great performers of the past and present generally had several different teachers,
suggesting that this is a good strategy for all students.
Students have rights and freedoms, including the freedom to seek out information
from a range of sources.
The research upon which the proposed units is based does not comprise facts or
truths, but rather ideas and hypotheses for which arguments generally exist on both
sides. The proposed units will not indoctrinate students, if new ideas are presented
together with arguments and evidence that both support and undermine them. An
anti-positivistic approach trains students to evaluate arguments and decide for themselves what is likely to be true or useful for them.
Performance teachers may feel that the analytical approach of this kind of teaching inhibits musical spontaneity. They are right that analytical thinking is not always conducive to a
good performance. Ideally, analytical thinking should be confined to practise; the main
technical difficulties should have been solved before the performance. In reality, this is
seldom the case and musicians on stage must often switch between relatively analytic
thinking (in technically challenging passages) and relatively intuitive thinking (where they
have mastered the technical problems and can focus on expression and interpretation).
Seen from this point of view, training in analytic thinking should help musicians to consciously make that switch, both during practice and on stage.
Performance teachers may feel disempowered by their lack of familiarity with the content of the proposed units. Rather than admitting ignorance or any other lack of ability,
which an artist on the free market is naturally reluctant to do, performance teachers may
point out that they did not need this pedagogical material to rise to the top of their profession. Logically, their students should not need it, either. But the context of music and
16
Article
music performance is constantly changing, so what was best for a teacher is not necessarily the same as what is best for her or his student; and no specialist in any area can possibly keep track of developments in all relevant areas - a problem that is becoming increasingly salient as the amount of research literature increases in all academic disciplines.
One should not forget that students may end up being better than their teachers, and one
could even argue that that should be the aim of every good teacher - if only because their
students, if successful as performers, will be exposed to challenges to which they themselves were not.
Objections based on pedagogical tradition. Performance teachers may consider it unwise
to change a successful pedagogical tradition: if it aint broke, dont fix it. However, just
because a pedagogical tradition is good, does not mean that it cannot be made even better, and a tradition that promoted excellence in the past may lead to mediocrity if it does
not keep pace with changes in musical practice, pedagogy and culture. Every student generation is exposed to new influences and expectations to which post-secondary music institutions should be sensitive. One should therefore strive to be proactive to anticipate
future changes in artistic practice, social context and student expectations. Curriculum
planners should regard the past and the future as equally important however difficult it
may be to respond adequately to the challenges and uncertainties of the future.
A common argument refers to the importance of a strong teacher-student relationship.
For centuries, music education has resembled an apprenticeship in which the master
shows the student the details of her or his craft. For this approach to work, the student
must regard the teacher as an authority. The exaggerated esteem in which music students
often hold their performance teachers may not be such a bad thing, since it motivates
them to practise, which increases the amount of time they devote to practice an important ingredient for success. To a large extent, this is true regardless of the content of the
teaching. However, there is no reason why the incorporation of performance research into
the curriculum should affect student-teacher relationships. Teachers can become actively
involved in the units and discuss the content with their students both collectively and individually. Moreover, students will respect and value teachers who are open to outside
influences.
A final important argument against the introduction of new units is that it is difficult to
foresee the benefits in advance. Like every other objection, it is important to take this
one seriously. Every new unit should be introduced first on a trial basis. It should be
evaluated by both students and other experts and it should be clear that repetitions of the
unit profit from the comments received on previous units.
Politics: Changing the System
It is not easy to introduce innovations of this kind in post-secondary music institutions with
a long, strong tradition of musical excellence, for it is precisely these institutions that
tend to be the most conservative. It is difficult to argue for change in a system that is already working well. How can your colleagues be sure that the innovation you are proposing is likely to lead to further improvement and will not endanger the high standard already achieved?
An important initial point is that change of some kind is inevitable, because the context
in which post-secondary music institutions work is constantly changing. The academic con-
17
Article
text is changing: post-secondary music institutions are under pressure to offer research
degrees and support research projects, and performance research is constantly producing
new findings that are adapted as the research is received by musicians. The political context is changing: educational institutions are increasingly expected to make their goals
transparent and develop strategies to achieve them as cost-efficiently as possible. The
sociocultural context is changing: children grow up in a world that is saturated with music
of all kinds, which can devalue individual musical experiences; and musicians and educators are expected to be more flexible, changing musical styles and roles depending on cultural developments or the demands of the market.
Build on existing strengths. The quality of an educational institution depends on the balance between focus and breadth in its curricula. An excellent institution will specialise in
one or more areas in which it excels by comparison to its competitors, and promote diversity (breadth) in other areas. New units should enhance either an existing focus or the
general breadth of the curriculum.
Present evidence. When attempting to convince colleagues of the advantages of music
performance research for post-secondary music institutions, it is important to focus on
solid arguments and avoid unfounded opinions. There is plenty of research on the topics
described in this paper that could be presented to colleagues (see reference list).
Understand colleagues agendas. The success of any strategy for change will depend on
the aims, needs, plans and aspirations of all involved people. How might they benefit from
the proposed changes? What problems are they already trying to solve?
Performance researchers may be divided into two categories. Some regard their research as pure and not necessarily of any practical use for musicians. Others believe
that their research can make a positive contribution to music education. Researchers
in the second category may be motivated by the possibility of realising that potential
provided that their contribution is indeed a positive one. They may as I do wish to
strengthen their partnership with such institutions and to make a constructive contribution to their curricula and other activities on the basis of research findings.
Students vary in their personal agenda, from those who are absolutely focused on
their instrument and passionately want to succeed as performers to those who are curious about almost everything. Students are the main benefactors of the developments
that I am advocating, but they often have little political power within their institution
- even if they are represented on committees.
While all performance teachers want the students in their class to succeed as performers, some may be sceptical that (additional) academic units may help them to
achieve that aim unless the benefits of a specific unit are demonstrated during some
kind of trial followed by an appropriate form of evaluation.
Administrators are interested in concrete success indicators that apply to the whole
institution, enhance its reputation, and increase its chances of attracting funding from
public and private sources. They may be amenable to arguments that additional units
of the kind described here may, in the long term, contribute to improving success indicators. The main visible success indicators of a post-secondary music institution are
its (inter-) nationally known graduate performers. But the vast majority of students do
not make it to that level, suggesting that the main contribution of a post-secondary
music institution to public life is invisible. Graduates of post-secondary music institutions (as well as many of the dropouts) organize, promote and coordinate all kinds of
18
Article
musical activities at all kinds of levels, using the expertise - both practical and academic - that they acquired during their student years.
The general public benefits from a rich cultural life across all social groups and strata
- age, sex, income. It benefits from a stable, bright future illuminated by excellent,
forward-looking institutions and active, capable, caring people. In a democracy, politicians should want much the same as the public that support them, insofar as the
goals can be clearly stated and understood.
Understand democracy. A specific political problem that one encounters when trying to
introduce new academic materials to a post-secondary music institution is the tension between two different democratic principles: majority rules and minority rights. Decisions
made by high-level committees in post-secondary music institutions tend (rightly) to go in
the direction of performers' wishes, because performers tend to represent the majority
and performance is the main aim of the institution. The situation of the performers is
strengthened further by the prevailing belief that the ability to perform is the most important prerequisite for good music tuition at the highest level. While that is certainly true, it
is also true that good teaching involves not only high-level personal experience as a performer and sound teaching methods, but also academic knowledge that may involve quite
different ways of thinking. Moreover, no one person can reasonably be expected to do
both at an internationally competitive level. Thus, the minority rights of academics, theorists and composers should be respected in the interest of both artistic and academic quality without losing sight of the main aim of training musicians and promoting music.
Revise the mission statement. A mission statement makes the goals of an institution
transparent. The mission statement of a post-secondary music institution might begin by
making it clear that music performance is the main goal of the institution, and that everything else that the institution does should be related to that goal. Thus, the institution
might strive to promote the quality, quantity and diversity both of performed music and of
musical training.
While such a statement might be perceived by performance teachers as promoting and
defending their professional interests, other passages in a typical mission statement may
not be viewed so positively. Performance teachers may feel that their artistic freedom is
being limited by a statement that has been formulated by colleagues and administrators
who have different goals and aspirations. In that case, they should have the opportunity to
participate in a democratic process to reformulate the text.
One way of promoting the teaching of performance research in a music institution is to
refer to it in the institutions mission statement. The inclusion of a specific goal in a mission statement is not a guarantee that the goal will be achieved, but it does improve
chances considerably.
A mission statement can also refer to interdisciplinarity. The application of music performance research involves crossing a difficult boundary: that between research and practice. But there is another difficult boundary within music research, namely that between
the humanities and the sciences, which should also be addressed. A post-secondary music
institution that recognizes the difficulty of crossing such boundaries, and is prepared to
support those that strive to do, is more likely to be able to apply academic research to its
own benefit.
Optimise content and pedagogy. Academic units for music students should as far as possible be designed especially for music students, differing from units offered at regular universities in the following respects:
19
Article
Theories should be presented in conjunction with specific examples to which the students can relate directly, such as a piece that one or more of them is currently playing. It is often best to begin with such an example and describe a specific problem associated with it before proceeding to develop a relevant theory; later, one can return
to such examples, apply the theory to them, and develop specific applications, implications or strategies that demonstrate both the theorys effectiveness and its limits.
Teachers should be researcher-performers. They should not only contribute to research in the topic area of the unit, but should also perform music themselves at a
level that will convince music students that they know what they are talking about
from a practical point of view. They should be able to apply research results not only
to the students specific practical issues but also to their own. Today, this criterion is
becoming easier to meet due to the steadily rising number of musicians doing music
performance research: their names can be found on the programs of recent conferences on music performance research and in the contents pages of this journal.
Inform and involve performance teachers. Units in the area of music performance research require considerable preparation. New content must be integrated, and new didactic methods developed. Moreover, the number of students interested in a given unit may
depend primarily on the interest that can be generated amongst performance teachers
before the unit begins. A new unit should be advertised well in advance, for example with
an email that includes a link to an attractive, informative webpage; posters; an introductory event; and individual meetings with performance teachers. Meetings should take
place early, because they invariably generate issues that individual performance teachers
would like to see addressed. Such discussions can help the researcher-teacher to bridge
the gap between the terminologies and epistemologies of research and practice, and can
also inspire future research projects. Performance teachers are more likely to take part in
research projects and associated teaching if they feel a sense of ownership. Thus, both
projects and teaching should address issues that are of direct concern to performance
teachers and their students, and the opinions and suggestions of both should influence the
course of the research and the content of the teaching. But the participation of performers should not affect the amount of time that they have available for private practice and
public performance.
Vary teaching format. Performance research, like any other academic content, may be
presented to students in many different formats: isolated guest lectures or workshops,
voluntary units for which the students can or cannot receive credit towards their degree,
electives, or compulsory units. The research can also be presented to performance teachers, who may then be invited or inspired to incorporate aspects of it into their teaching.
The wisest strategy would appear to be to aim for a diversity of presentation formats, as
this gives students the widest choice and also provides an avenue for new topics to be accepted gradually into the curriculum. New materials can be introduced stepwise, starting
with guest lectures, gradually proceeding to higher levels of institutionalisation, and pos-
20
Article
sibly culminating in a compulsory unit.
Involve and empower students. Students are most likely to understand and be able to
apply performance research if they are involved in it, either as participants or as members
of research teams. However, students may not be ready for research until they reach honours or postgraduate level; and even then only those with academic ambitions tend to be
seriously interested in research. Meanwhile, students can contribute to the acceptance of
new units into the curriculum by evaluating them. It is already common for students to
evaluate individual units, but they can also be asked to evaluate the whole curriculum. A
mentor system can also be useful. Each student can be assigned to a specific member of
staff (other than their main performance teacher) and encouraged to talk about issues relating to their curriculum and the choices that they have made within it. Students can talk
to their mentors about the kinds of units they would like to see offered, and which units
they consider too long, too difficult, or superfluous, and so on. Students can also make
their preferences known through an elective system where they must complete, say, four
out of ten offered units as part of the curriculum, and are also free to attend other units
without receiving credit for them. Mentoring should allow students to express their opinions freely in the context of a constructive, non-hierarchical interaction with teachers and
administrators. When it does, it gives the institution valuable information about how to
improve its programs to suit the needs of the students. When students feel valued and
identify with their institution, they are more likely to maintain contact after their courses
are complete. And when they stay in contact, they can also be asked to evaluate the
course retrospectively after they have entered professional life. What aspects of the
course made a positive contribution to their careers?
Expand and diversify teaching staff. The permanent staff of post-secondary music institutions are often not in a position to teach materials of the kind presented here. Nor are
they necessarily in a good position to have new positions created that cover these materials. New positions are expensive and must be supported by strong arguments. One way
forward may be first to include some new subjects in the curriculum and have them taught
by temporary staff. Once the success of the units can be demonstrated, it is time to make
preliminary enquiries about the possibility of a new position, or a re-organisation of existing positions, such as a new relationship or affiliation with an external department such as
psychology.
Musical interdisciplinarity
The ideas presented in this paper were inspired by a vision of musical and musicological
interdisciplinarity (Parncutt, 2006). Every discipline, if left to itself, tends for practical
reasons to specialise and avoid interaction with other disciplines. Crossing major interdisciplinary borders can be risky because one may not feel confident about one's abilities on
the other side of the dividing line and because ones colleagues may not be in a position to
appreciate interdisciplinary contributions. Music (-ology) is no exception: pianists tend to
remain within musical practice, music historians within the humanities, and music psychologists within the sciences.
The research on expertise cited above confirms that is takes a lot of hard work to become an expert in a given discipline, which in turn suggests that no one person is capable
of becoming an expert in more than one discipline. Instead, interdisciplinarity happens
21
Article
when an expert in one discipline say violin performance works together with an expert
in a constrasting discipline say psychology. A prerequisite for such an interaction is constructive openness: one should respect not only the representatives of the other discipline
as individuals, but also their ideas, conclusions and specific proposals even when one
does not fully understand the methods, traditions and intuitions behind them. One should
neither believe nor reject everything, but instead aim for a middle path: an appropriate
compromise between critical distance and unquestioning acceptance.
No member of a team of experts representing different disciplines should expect an individual to be an expert in two or more areas. Instead, all members should be genuinely
curious about the approach and content of all disciplines relevant to their research question, and especially about other team members corresponding knowledge. They should
welcome and offer constructive criticism, and respect each other's disciplinary and personal positions. Thus, for example, music performance researchers must acquaint themselves thoroughly with the issues and values of the performers themselves both teachers
and students if they are to succeed in making a positive contribution to their craft.
Post-secondary music institutions that develop a positive spirit of constructive interdisciplinary cooperation are, or will be, in a strong position to take advantage of recent research in music performance. But interdisciplinarity should not be regarded as an end in
itself. It is only one of many strategies for training performers and promoting music, and
as such should only be pursued insofar as it helps music institutions to achieve their practical musical goals.
Acknowledgments
I am grateful to Jane Ginsborg, Antonia Ivaldi and Gunter Kreutz for the invitation to
speak at the conference Teaching, Learning and Performing Music at the Royal Northern
College of Music, Manchester in July 2006, and for the subsequent invitation to submit this
article. For much of my knowledge about the educational and practical aspects of this
work, I am indebted to Gary McPherson, co-editor of the book Science and psychology of
music performance (2002); Michele Biasutti, who invited me to speak on this topic at the
International Symposium on Psychology and Music Education, Universit di Padova, in November 2004, and to write up the contents for his book Applicazioni educative della
ricerca sull'esecuzione musicale (2006); Aaron Williamon, my co-author for a paper on
this topic at the conference Performance Matters in Porto, Portugal, in 2005; and Margit
Painsi, my student assistant who made interesting comments on a draft of this paper and
whose PhD project was a source of inspiration. For detailed, insightful comments I am
grateful to Stacey Bartsch, Gunter Kreutz and Diana Weekes. Finally, the article was inspired by my graduate students in various areas of performance research: Charlotte
Baumgartner, Elena Gasenzer, Werner Goebl, Patrick Holming, Karin Jost and Johann Lassnig.
References
Altenmller, E., Wiesendanger, M., & Kesselring, J. (Eds.) (2006). Music, motor control
and the brain. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Barry, N. H., & Hallam, S. (2002). Practice. In R. Parncutt & G. E. McPherson (Eds.), Science and psychology of music performance (pp. 151-166). New York: Oxford University
Press.
22
Article
Bigand, E., Poulain, B., Tillmann, B., & DAdamo, D. (2003) Cognitive versus sensory components in harmonic priming effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 159-171
Brandfonbrener, A. G., & Kjelland, J. M. (2002). Music medicine. In R. Parncutt & G. E.
McPherson (Eds.), Science and psychology of music performance (pp. 83-96). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Brandler, S., & Rammsayer, T. H. (2003). Differences in mental abilities between musicians and non-musicians. Psychology of Music, 31, 123-138.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., and Rich, G. J. (1997). Musical improvisation: A systems approach. In
K. R. Sawyer (Ed.), Creativity in performance (pp. 43-66). London: Ablex.
Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Rmer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice
in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363-406.
Erlitz-Lanegger, E. (1997). Funktionsgerechtes Krpertraining: Ein Handbuch fr Lehrer,
Kursleiter, Trainer. Graz: Scientia.
Ferguson, S., & Parncutt, R. (2004). Composing 'In the Flesh': Perceptually-informed harmonic syntax. Proceedings of Sound and Music Computing (Paris/France, 20-22 Oktober
'00).
Friberg, A., & Battel, G. U. (2002). Structural communication. In R. Parncutt & G. E.
McPherson (Eds.), Science and psychology of music performance (pp. 199-218). New
York: Oxford University Press.
Gellrich, M. (1992). ben mit Lis(z)tWiederentdeckte Geheimnisse aus der Werkstatt der
Klaviervirtuosen. Frauenfeld: Im Waldgut.
Gellrich, M., & Parncutt, R. (1998). Piano technique and fingering in the 18th and 19th
centuries: Bringing a forgotten method back to life. British Journal of Music Education,
15 (1), 5-23.
Goebl, W. (2001). Melody lead in piano performance: Expressive device or artifact? Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 110, 563-572.
Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J. A. (1998) Innate talents: Reality or myth?
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 399-407.
Jost, K. (2004). Untersttzende Methoden beim Erlernen der traditionellen Notation im
Instrumentalunterricht mit Kindern. Diplomarbeit, University of Graz.
Juslin, P. N., & Persson, R. S. (2002). Emotional communication. In R. Parncutt & G. E.
McPherson (Eds.), Science and psychology of music performance (pp. 219-236). New
York: Oxford University Press.
Kemp, A. E. (1996). The musical temperament: The psychology and personality of musicians. New York: Oxford University Press.
Kenny, B. J., & Gellrich, M. (2002). Improvisation. In R. Parncutt & G. E. McPherson
(Eds.), Science and psychology of music performance (pp. 117-134). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Kenny, D.T., & Ackermann, B. (in press). Anxiety in public performance, stress and health
issues for musicians. In S. Hallam, I. Cross, & M. Thaut (Eds.). Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press
Krumhansl, C. L. (1990). Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. Oxford, England: Oxford
University Press.
Larson, S. (2004). Musical forces and melodic expectations: Comparing computer models
23
Article
with experimental results. Music Perception, 21, 457-498.
Lassnig, J. (2004). Theorie und Praxis der Kreativitt als Grundlage einer ImprovisationsDidaktik im Jazz-Unterricht. Diplomarbeit, Kunstuniversitt Graz.
Lehmann, A. C., Sloboda, J. A., & Woody, R. H. (2007). Psychology for musicians: Understanding and acquiring the skills. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
McPherson, G. E. (Ed.) (2006). The child as musician: A handbook of musical development.
Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
McPherson, G. E., & Gabrielsson, A. (2002). From sound to sign. In R. Parncutt & G. E.
McPherson (Eds.), Science and psychology of music performance (pp. 99-116). New
York: Oxford University Press.
Mornell, A. (2002). Lampenfieber und Angst bei ausbenden Musikern. Frankfurt: Lang.
Odam, G., & Bannan, N. (Eds.) (2005). The reflective conservatoire: Studies in music education. London : Guildhall School of Music & Drama.
O'Neill, S. A., & McPherson, G. E. (2002). Motivation. In R. Parncutt & G. E. McPherson
(Eds.), Science and psychology of music performance (pp. 31-46). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Painsi, M. (2004). Wie erklren Schler ihre musikalischen Erfolge und Misserfolge?
Musikerziehung, 5, 235-242.
Painsi, M. (2007). Motivations- und Stressmanagementtraining fr Schler an Musikschulen
mit Lehrerbeteiligung. Dissertation, University of Graz, Austria.
Parncutt, R. (1989). Harmony: A psychoacoustical approach. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Parncutt, R. (2004). Enrichment of music theory pedagogy by computer-based repertoire
analysis and perceptual-cognitive theory. In J. W. Davidson & H. Eiholzer (Eds.), The
music practitioner: Research for the music performer, teacher and listener (pp. 101116). London, England: Ashgate.
Parncutt, R. (2003). Accents and expression in piano performance. In K. W. Niemller
(Ed.), Perspektiven und Methoden einer Systemischen Musikwissenschaft (pp. 163-185).
Frankfurt/Main, Germany: Peter Lang.
Parncutt, R. (2006). Interdisciplinary musicology. Foreword to Musicae Scientiae, special
issue 2005-2006 "Interdisciplinary musicology", 7-11.
Parncutt, R., & Holming, P. G. S. (2000). Is scientific research on piano performance useful for pianists? Poster and abstract at International Conference on Music Perception &
Cognition (Keele, England, July).
Parncutt, R., & McPherson, G. E. (Eds., 2002). The science and psychology of music performance: Creative strategies for teaching and learning. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Parncutt, R., Sloboda, J. A., Clarke, E. F., Raekallio, M., & Desain, P. (1997). An ergonomic model of keyboard fingering for melodic fragments. Music Perception, 14, 341382.
Porter, G. (1996). Organizational impact of workaholism: Suggestions for researching the
negative outcomes of excessive work. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 1,
70-84.
Rink, J. (Ed.) (2002). Musical performance, A guide to understanding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Schlaug, G., Jncke, L., Huang, Y., Staiger, J. F., & Steinmetz, H. (1995). Increased corpus callosum size in musicians. Neuropsychologia, 33/8, 1047-55.
Spence, J. T., & Robbins, A. S. (1992). Workaholism: Definition, measurement and pre-
24
Article
liminary results. Journal of Personality Assessment, 58, 160-178.
Temperley, D. (2001). The cognition of basic musical structures. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Tillmann, B., Janata, P., Birk, J. & Bharucha, J. J. (2003). The costs and benefits of tonal
centers for chord processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
and Performance, 29 (2), 470-482.
Williamon, A. (2004). Musical excellence: Strategies and techniques to enhance performance. London, England: Oxford University Press
Williamon, A., & Thompson, S. (2006). Awareness and incidence of health problems among
conservatoire students. Psychology of Music, 34, 411-430.
Wilson, G. D. (2002). Psychology for performing artists (2nd ed.). London: Whurr.
Wilson, G. D., & Roland, D. (2002). Performance anxiety. In R. Parncutt & G. E. McPherson
(Eds.), Science and psychology of music performance (pp. 47-62). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Woody, R. H. (1999). The relationship between explicit planning and expressive performance of dynamic variations in an aural modeling task. Journal of Research in Music Education, 47, 331-342.
Wright, D. (2005). The South Kensington music schools and the development of the British
conservatoire in the late Nineteenth Century. Journal of the Royal Musical Association,
130(2), 236-282.
Richard Parncutt is Professor of Systematic Musicology at the University of Graz, Austria. His research addresses musical structure (pitch, consonance, harmony, tonality, melody, counterpoint,
tension, rhythm, meter, accent), performance (psychology, piano, applications), the origins of tonality and of music, and musicological interdisciplinarity. He holds qualifications in music and physics from the University of Melbourne and a PhD from the University of New England. He was guest
researcher with Ernst Terhardt in Munich, Johan Sundberg in Stockholm, Annabel Cohen in Halifax,
Al Bregman in Montreal, and John Sloboda in Keele, England. Until 1998 he was lecturer at the University of Keele.
25