Warning vs. Reinforced Self-Affirmation As Methods of Reduction of The Misinformation Effect
Warning vs. Reinforced Self-Affirmation As Methods of Reduction of The Misinformation Effect
Warning vs. Reinforced Self-Affirmation As Methods of Reduction of The Misinformation Effect
Malwina Szpitalak*
Romuald Polczyk*
Introduction
Misinformation effect
The misinformation effect consists in the decrement in
eyewitness report accuracy arising after exposure to misinformation concerning the original event. Technically, it
refers to including by a witness into his/her testimony details inconsistent with the original event, originating from
sources other than the event itself (Polczyk, under revision). This phenomenon has been widely explored since
early seventies (seminal work: Loftus, Miller & Burns,
1978; Pezdek, 1977).
The typical experimental procedure used to explore
the misinformation effect consists of three phases. In the
first phase, participants typically watch a video clip or a sequence of slides, which is called original material. Next,
they are exposed to the postevent material which, in the experimental group, contains some misleading information.
For example, in the original material the robber had a gun,
but it was said in the postevent material that he had a knife.
Afterwards, subjects are asked to answer questions about
original material, including questions referring to the misled details. Numerous experiments using various modifica-
* Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University Al. Mickiewicza 3 31-120, Krakw e-mail: [email protected];
romuald.polczyk@uj,edu.pl
86
87
Method
Participants
Two hundred and nine (127 female, 82 male) students
(except psychology students) took part in the experiment.
Mean age was 19.17 (SD = 1.75). No gratification for the
participation was given. The participants were recruited
mainly from the university database and participated for
credit points.
Materials
1. The original material: an audio recording (male voice)
of a duration of 1 min 57 sec, presenting the fictious
planned reform, consisting in introducing a final
exam at the end of the education. The speaker, intruduced as one of the creators of the reform, elaborated on
various advanteges of introducing such an exam; thus,
the speech was informative but also persuasive
2. The postevent material: a description of the original
material, which in the experimental group contained six
misleading details.
3. The memory test - a set of 10 open-ended questions,
including six critical ones.
4. A list of 60 nouns from various categories to remember,
e.g. forest, horse, needle, kitchen, pen
88
Results
The open-ended answers in the final test were recoded as: 1 - answer consistent with misinformation; 0
- all other answers or lack of answer. The answers 1
were summed up across all six critical questions, giving
a numeric estimate of the susceptibility to misinformation
ranging from 0 to 6. In Table 1 descriptive statistics for the
mean number of answers consistent with misinformation
are presented.
Levels of factors
Method
M
none
warning
RSA
non-misled
misled
none
non-misled
none
misled
warning non-misled
warning misled
RSA
non-misled
RSA
misled
N
67
76
66
97
112
36
31
35
41
26
40
Legend:
Method: method of reducing the susceptibility to misinformation
M misinformation
RSA reinforced self-affirmation
The results of planned comparisons comparing all combinations of levels of factors are presented in Tables 2 and 3.
As can be seen in Table 2, misled subjects performed worse
from control ones regardless of the method of reducing the
misinformation effect. Of greater importance however are
the comparisons done in the misled group among various
combinations of methods of reducing the misinformation
effect, presented in Table 3 and visualized on Figure 1.
To start with, misled warned persons were more resistant to misinformation then those from the control group,
which confirms the hypothesis two (see Table 3). The third
hypothesis was also confirmed participants engaged in
the reinforced self-affirmation procedure were less susceptible to misinformation than participants from the control
group. Finally, RSA proved more effective in reducing the
misinformation effect than did the warning (Table 3).
Table 2. Comparisons between misled and non-misled
groups in the mean number of answers consistent to
misinformation across all combinations of methods of
reducing the misinformation effect
Method
F
df
p
2p
none
98.73 1,203 < 0.001 0.49
warning 43.92 1,203 < 0.001 0.22
RSA
14.98 1,203 < 0.001 0.07
Table 3. Differences between groups in the mean number
of answers consistent to misinformation
Misinformation Comparisons of methods
F
df
p
2p
none
warning
0.48 1,203 0.488 < 0.01
Non-misled
none
RSA
0.64 1,203 0.424 < 0.01
warning
RSA
0.03 1,203 0.873 < 0.01
none
warning
9.79 1,203 0.002
0.05
Misled
none
RSA
27.43 1,203 < 0.001 0.14
warning
RSA
5.24 1,203 0.023
0.03
L
Legend:
M misinformation
RSA reinforced self-affirmation
Discussion
As was expected, misled persons were more vulnerable
to misinformation than non-misled ones. This confirmed
the first hypotheses and replicated the well-known misinformation effect. This is but another warning how vulnerable the memory can be to distortions, at least in some circumstances.
The second hypothesis was also confirmed: warning
was efficient in reducing the misinformation effect. This
result is consistent with outcomes of many other studies
(e.g. Blank, 1998; Chambers & Zaragoza, 2001; Echterhoff, Hirst & Hussy, 2005; Ecker, Lewandowsky & Tang,
2010; Greene et al., 1982; Wright, 1993). It is also worth
89
90
References
Apsler, R. & Sears, D. O. (1968). Warning, personal involvement, and
attitude change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9,
162-166.
Bekerian, D.A. & Bowers, J.N. (1983). Eyewitness testimony: Were we
misled? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
and Cognition, 1, 139-145.
Blank, H. (1998). Memory states and memory tasks: an integrative
framework for eyewitness memory and suggestibility. Memory, 6,
481-529.
Braun, K. A., Ellis, R. & Loftus, E. F. (2002). Make my memory: How
advertising can change our memories of the past. Psychology and
Marketing, 19, 1-23.
Centofanti, A. T., Reece, J. E. (2006). The cognitive interview and its
effect on misleading post-event information. Psychology, Crime and
Law, 12, 669-683.
Chambers, K. L. & Zaragoza, M. S. (2001). Intended and unintended
effects of explicit warnings on eyewitness suggestibility: Evidence
91