Copia - Leadership
Copia - Leadership
Copia - Leadership
LEADERSHIP
Julian Barling, Amy Christie, and Colette Hoption
Writing of this chapter was supported by grants from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada.
183
in and critical to the understanding of the dominant leadership theories of the present day. In this
preliminary section we provide a brief history of
the progression of leadership theories that form the
foundation of current thinking and research in the
field of leadership today. To complement this section, a visual depiction of leadership trends over the
past quarter of a century is provided in Table 7.1,
which we will reflect on in greater detail at the end
of this section.
Leadership theory
4
5
3
0
3
0
2
2
0
3
1
2
2
0
1
0
0
1
0
0
0
1983
1982
1981
1980
0
0
1
4
0
2
2
2
1
8
1985
1984
0
0
0
0
0
2
3
1
4
5
10
1987
1986
0
0
2
1
1
3
4
1
2
6
1989
1988
0
0
1
3
0
7
4
2
2
11
1991
1990
TABLE 7.1
0
0
3
0
1
8
3
0
3
10
1993
1992
0
0
1
1
2
8
0
2
4
12
1995
1994
1
2
2
2
7
8
3
2
8
23
1997
1996
11
22
1
1
1
1
2
7
0
1
27
1999
1998
30
15
1
6
4
4
4
1
0
4
24
2001
2000
52
12
1
2
0
2
5
0
0
3
24
2003
2002
75
28
3
6
4
2
7
8
0
0
7
35
2005
2004
105
18
8
7
5
5
19
7
3
4
1
57
2007
2006
284
135
14
26
16
27
49
15
37
19
40
246
58
Total
Leadership
185
Leadership
theory identifies four categories of leadership behaviors that motivate followers to achieve their goals
(House & Mitchell, 1974). Participative leadership
behavior involves including followers in decision
making and soliciting follower feedback. Consistent
with the focus on participation in decision making
that was already prevalent in the 1970s (e.g., Alutto
& Belasco, 1972), this leadership behavior was
hypothesized to enhance motivation by fostering
overlap between follower and organizational goals and
by providing followers with more appreciation
and understanding of the pathway between effort
and goal achievement. Directive pathgoal-clarifying
leadership behavior mimics Initiating Structure,
motivating followers by providing task structure,
feedback, and procedures that reduce role ambiguity, linking follower effort to performance and goal
attainment, and communicating the rewards contingent on performance. By contrast, supportive leadership behavior is similar to Consideration, whereby
leaders demonstrate their concern for the needs and
best interests of followers and, by doing so, remove
some of the potential obstacles that may prevent
followers from obtaining their goals. The final
leadership behavior identified in the theory is
achievement-oriented leadership behavior, which
involves creating challenging and high-standard
performance goals and expressing confidence in
followers abilities to meet such challenges. Followers
should then respond with greater self-efficacy and
effort toward goal attainment.
Pathgoal leadership theorys second focus is on
situational factors that render leadership behaviors
more or less effective. These factors relate to the
organizational environment, job design, and follower
characteristics. However, empirical tests have not
yielded conclusive support for the situational factors. For example, in a meta-analysis of 120 studies,
Wofford and Liska (1993) found support for only
6 of 16 moderation hypotheses predicted by path
goal leadership theory (zr ranged from .31 to .51
corrected for unreliability of measures and sampling
error). Schriesheim and Neiders (1996) qualitative
summary of the empirical literature suggested that
the most consistent results for the situational focus
of the theory have been for the relationship between
directive clarifying behavior (most often measured
188
using the LBDQ Initiating Structure scale) and follower satisfaction when task characteristics (e.g.,
autonomy, task variety, and feedback) promote
intrinsic motivation. We found the relationships
between the remaining categories of leadership
behaviors and the performance outcomes far less
conclusive. It has been suggested that improper
measurement and incomplete or inappropriately
specified testing may account for the theorys
empirical shortcomings, as a result of which future
research should fairly test the tenets of pathgoal
leadership theory (Schriesheim, Castro, Zhou, &
DeChurch, 2006). Nonetheless, the complexity of
the model may inhibit the possibility of subjecting
the theory to an omnibus test.
Substitutes for leadership. Kerr and Jermiers
(1978) substitutes for leadership theory further
extends the situational perspective of leadership, in
particular path-goal leadership theory, by illuminating additional situational contingencies of leadership behaviors (House, 1996). Specifically, Kerr and
Jermier identified numerous situational variables
that influence the relationship between leadership
and its outcomes. These variables fit within one of
two primary categories: neutralizers and substitutes.
Neutralizers of leadership are situational factors that
block the effects of leadership, rendering leadership
behaviors inconsequential. One example of a neutralizer of leadership is spatial distance between the
leader and the follower. By contrast, substitutes for
leadership both neutralize leadership and positively
influence attitudinal and performance outcomes.
Examples of leadership substitutes include the followers intrinsic interest in the task, ability, training,
and experience. These are substitutes because they
relate positively to follower satisfaction, morale, and
performance and therefore eliminate the need for
leadership.
Because the substitutes for leadership theory has
intuitive appeal, the inadequate substantiating evidence for the theory across multiple rigorous study
designs and samples remains a surprise (e.g., Dionne,
Yammarino, Atwater, & James, 2002). Most past
research suggests that substitutes for leadership have
an additive effect; that is, they influence important
outcomes irrespective of leadership behaviors without diminishing or negating the influence of leader-
Leadership
Leadership
Leadership
Leadership Emergence
Some people are born to move and shake the world.
Their blessings: high energy, exceptional intelligence,
extreme persistence, self-confidence and a yearning
to influence others (Avolio, 1999a, p.18). Although
much of the discussion in this chapter focuses on the
issue of leadership behaviors and effectiveness, precisely which individuals attain positions of leadership
in the first instance is of equal intrigue. As suggested by Avolio (1999a), one possibility is that
some individuals are naturally predisposed to
become leaders. Consistent with recent advances in
the burgeoning field known as social neuroscience
(Cacioppo et al., 2007), the notion that genetic and
biological factors play an important role in the development of leadership should not be unexpected.
Nevertheless, well-controlled studies on possible
genetic and/or biological effects on leadership
development remain scant.
Empirical research on the role of genetic factors in
behavior was initially stimulated by the classic studies
familial adversity (either loss of a parent or separation from parents). As a result of such adversity, they
learned to take responsibility for themselves from an
early age. Certainly the Cox and Cooper study justifies further research on early family influences on
subsequent leadership emergence and behaviors.
There has been interest in the specific nature of
the early environment that might influence subsequent leadership. Generally, studies have shown that
parents warmth and acceptance and their achievement demands predict predispositions to leadership
behaviors in 10th-grade adolescents and in boys aged
16.5 years and girls aged 15.6 years (Bronfenbrenner,
1961, and Klonsky, 1983, respectively). Similarly,
Towler (2005) showed that young adults (18
25 years of age) with fathers who exercised high levels of psychological control were less likely to exhibit
charismatic leadership. Hartman and Harris (1992)
also showed that college students who subsequently
held management positions modeled the leadership
of individuals whom they admired early in their
lives; most of these individuals were their parents.
A more recent study of 196 pairs of twins who
were part of the ongoing Minnesota Twin Family
Study (Avolio, Rotundo, & Walumbwa, 2009)
refines our understanding of the role of early experiences on later leadership role occupancy in several
important ways. First, Avolio et al. (2009) focused
on Baumrinds (1971) notion of authoritative parenting, which is a combination of psychological autonomy, acceptance, and supervision, reflects positive
parenting, and is not to be mistaken for authoritarian
parenting. Avolio et al. (2009) showed that authoritative parenting was associated with lower levels of
prehigh school childrens modest (delinquency and
family/school offenses) and serious (serious crime,
drug use) rule-breaking behavior. Second, modest
and serious rule-breaking behaviors predicted subsequent leadership role occupancy differently:
Specifically, modest rule-breaking behavior was positively, and serious rule-breaking behavior negatively,
associated with leadership role occupancy. Avolio
et al. (2009) suggested that early experiences with
rule breaking (a) enable parents to guide their children to learn from these experiences and (b) reflect
the same qualities necessary for leadership role occupancy. In contrast, just as early diagnoses of conduct
Leadership
managers who had received transformational leadership training sold significantly more personal loans
and credit cardstwo indices of special relevance to
the banksin comparison to the banks run by managers in the control group. The generalizability of
this phenomenon is supported: Kelloway, Barling,
and Helleur (2000) showed that transformational
leadership could be taught to a sample of managers
of a health care facility.
Dvir, Eden, Avolio, and Shamir (2002) also
focused on the development of transformational
leadership in their study of infantry soldiers. Seven
individuals were randomly assigned to the transformational leadership condition, which included
5 days of training, consisting of role playing exercises,
simulations, video presentations, and group, peer,
and trainer feedback. Akin to the booster sessions
described in Barling et al.s (1996) study, the leaders
participated in a 3-hour session prior to a leadership
assignment to reinforce the lessons of leadership
training. Although the specific effects of transformational leadership in this context are discussed later
in this chapter, it is important to note here that the
training was effective: Both knowledge of transformational leadership theory and transformational
leadership behaviors rated by subordinates were
significantly enhanced by the training, whereas
there were no such changes in the control group.
Methodologically, it is also worth noting that
although the Barling et al. (1996), Dvir et al. (2002),
Kelloway et al. (2000), and Mullen and Kelloway
(2009) studies focused on fewer than 30 leaders, all
their evaluations were based on samples of more
than 30 individuals (employees) per group, thereby
fulfilling one of Terpstras (1981) criteria for a rigorous evaluation.
Most recently, following the earlier studies showing that transformational leadership behaviors can be
developed through training and that safety-specific
transformational leadership predicted employee
safety behaviors (Barling, Loughlin, & Kelloway,
2002), Mullen and Kelloway (2009) conducted an
experiment and showed that safety-specific transformational leadership could be developed in leaders.
Leaders attitudes toward safety were influenced by
the training, as were employees safety behaviors.
More intriguingly, Mullen and Kelloway also
196
included a group that received general transformational leadership training, and no significant effects
on safety emerged for this group, raising the question of how specific leadership training needs to be
in order to influence desired subordinate outcomes.
It also remains to be seen whether safety training by
itself would yield the same results as those from the
safety-specific transformational leadership training;
if not, then stronger support for the effectiveness of
transformational leadership would result.
Skarlicki and Latham (1996, 1997) directed their
training toward meeting the specific demands that
leaders in their sample faced, such as enhancing
citizenship behavior within a union. In these studies,
leaders (union shop stewards) in the experimental
group were provided with four 3-hour sessions
focused on training behaviors that influence follower
perceptions of procedural and interactional justice.
The results strongly supported the effectiveness of
the leadership training. Perceptions of union fairness
were higher among union members whose shop
stewards had attended the training. Evidence for a
downstream effect was also found in both studies:
Changes in the shop stewards behaviors resulted in
increases in rank-and-file members citizenship
behaviors on behalf of the union.
A possible opportunity lost in executive and leadership development deserves mention. Training in
organizations has long been studied, with robust
lessons learned from decades of research (Aguinis &
Kraiger, 2009; Salas & Cannon-Bowers, 2001). To
date, however, executive and leadership development
initiatives have largely failed to benefit from the literature on training. One consequence of this is that, as
is apparent from the prior discussion, the research on
executive and leadership development interventions
has not considered the types of issues considered central within training, such as needs assessment, different delivery modes, and the transfer of training.
Similarly, any potential indirect benefits of leadership
development (e.g., leader self-efficacy, commitment;
Tannenbaum, Mathieu, Salas, & Cannon-Bowers,
1991) have also been ignored. Parenthetically, the
training literature has perhaps not devoted sufficient
attention to leadership development. It is likely that
our understanding of executive and leadership development and the effectiveness of intervention initia-
Leadership
shipwho emerges as a leader and who is an effective leaderand research has focused on whether and
how personality influences both leadership emergence
and effectiveness. For example, individuals are more
likely to occupy leadership roles or be perceived as
leaders when they are achievement-oriented and
socially potent, meaning that they are hard-working
and thrive when they are in charge of others (Arvey
et al., 2006). Self-monitoring behaviors also predict
emergent leadership, particularly because high selfmonitors are more likely to exhibit task-oriented
behaviors in groups (Eby, Cader, & Noble, 2003).
Furthermore, results from two meta-analyses reviewing the accumulated empirical studies link personality
traits to leadership. First, Lord, de Vader, and Alliger
(1986) showed that leader intelligence (r = .52), masculinity-femininity (r = .34), and dominance (r = .17)
predicted perceptions of leadership (correlations corrected for unreliability of measures, sampling error,
and range restriction). Second, researchers Judge,
Bono, Ilies, and Gerhardt (2002) showed that the
Big Five model of personality (i.e., extraversion,
agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and
openness to experience) explained 28% and 15% of
the variance in leader emergence and leader effectiveness, respectively. These reviews suggest that personality trait perspectives of leadership emergence and
effectiveness offer considerable credibility.
In a refined analysis of the intersection between
personality and leadership, Judge et al.s (2002) quantitative review showed that of the Big Five personality
traits, openness to experience and extraversion were
significantly related (all correlations corrected for
average reliability of measures and measurement
error) to both leadership emergence (r = .24 and
r = .33, respectively) and effectiveness (r = .24 and
r = .24, respectively), while conscientiousness was
significantly related to leadership emergence (r = .33)
and neuroticism significantly related to leadership
effectiveness (r = .22). Openness to experience is
defined by creativity and risk-taking, which the
authors argue are relevant to both leader selection and
performance, and extraverts are social, energetic, and
dominant and therefore may more easily hold others
attention and have social influence (Judge et al.,
2002). Similarly, conscientious individuals characteristically demonstrate discipline in and diligence
197
toward their work (McCrae & John, 1992); it follows that conscientious personalities are likely to
be selected for leadership roles. Likewise, the relative insecurity and emotional instability of individuals high on neuroticism can limit their leadership
effectiveness.
Despite knowledge about average tendencies, little
is known about the processes through which leader
personality traits influence organizational outcomes.
Peterson, Smith, Martorana, and Owens (2003)
offered one explanation, showing that CEO personality affects team dynamics at the senior-management
level. For example, dynamics such as corrupt behaviors, risk-taking propensity, flexibility, and cohesiveness are affected by CEO personality and that each of
these dynamics relates to organizational performance.
Personality and transformational leadership behaviors. As the popularity of the transformational
leadership framework persists (see Table 7.1), extensive research has been invested into understanding
the connections between personality and transformational leadership behaviors. A meta-analysis analyzing 384 correlations from 26 samples (Bono & Judge,
2004) showed consistent relationships (correlations
corrected for measure reliability and measurement
error) between transformational leadership and
extraversion (r = .22) and neuroticism (r = .17).
Individuals who were outgoing and optimistic (i.e.,
extraverted personality) tended to display more idealized influence and inspirational motivation behaviors.
Conversely, those who were distressed, anxious, and
prone to insecurities (i.e., neurotic personality) were
unlikely to have the confidence needed to take on
transformational roles. There were, however, inconsistent findings between agreeableness and openness
to experience and the transformational leadership
behaviors, and the authors also reported a relatively
weak relationship between personality and transactional leadership, which was unexpected. The authors
did not predict a link between conscientiousness and
transformational leadership because, as they noted,
There is no particular reason to expect that conscientious individuals will exhibit vision, enthusiasm, or
creativity (Bono & Judge, 2004, p. 903).
Overall, the associations established in Bono
and Judges (2004) study were relatively modest
198
specifically, the Big Five explain twice as much variance in leadership emergence as in leader charisma,
and almost five times more in emergence than in
intellectual stimulation and individualized consideration. The leader development section of this chapter
highlights the malleability of leadership behaviors,
and thus leadership training may partially explain
this discrepancy (Bono & Judge, 2004); leadership
behaviors may be less stable than personality characteristics. Consequently, personality traits can tell us
more about who is likely to attain leadership positions than how individuals might lead once they
must fulfill those roles.
The Big Five are not the only characteristics associated with transformational behaviors. Proactive
personality (Crant & Bateman, 2000), histrionic personality (Khoo & Burch, 2008), secure attachment
style (Popper, Mayseless, & Castelnovo, 2000), and
positive affectivity (Rubin, Munz, & Bommer, 2005)
are also positive correlates of transformational leadership. Of particular interest in recent years is the
relationship between narcissism (defined as immense
self-love; Judge, LePine, & Rich, 2006) and transformational leadership. Judge et al.s study confirmed
that narcissistic personalities possess inflated selfevaluations as evidenced by their high self-ratings of
transformational leadership; importantly, Judge et al.s
study also demonstrated that followers rated narcissistic leaders lower on transformational leadership
than did the leaders themselves. These results highlight the role of perceptions of leadershipa topic
of growing interest. Accordingly, our discussion
now shifts to the topic of followers personalities and
how those personalities might determine followers
perceptions of leadership.
Follower personality and leadership. Any discussion of leadership and personality would be incomplete without accounting for the personality of
followers. A growing research interest suggests that
follower personality is integral to the leadership
process. For example, Bernerth, Armenakis, Feild,
Giles, and Walker (2007) related perceptions of LMX
to both follower and leader dimensions of the Big
Fiveleaders conscientiousness and agreeableness,
and followers conscientiousness, extraversion, openness, and neuroticism were associated with follower
Leadership
The effect size statistics reported in this section on gender are positive when in favor of males and negative when in favor of females.
199
Leadership
Leadership
A second problem concerns the ability to generalize from cross-cultural studies over time. Thus,
although Hofstedes (2001) work has been fruitful
in guiding many cross-cultural studies of leadership,
the characteristics inherent in the national cultures
studied may change over time as a result, for example, of economic recessions and globalization (e.g.,
Ralston, Holt, Terpstra, & Kai-Cheng, 1997). Indeed,
Fukushige and Spicers (2007) qualitative research
suggested that Japanese work culture was in the
process of becoming a meritocracy. To account for
these and other changes, contemporary research
should take the time to reevaluate Hofstedes dimensions and ensure that the cultures studied are accurately depicted in their present-day state.
Finally, a third challenge to cross-culture leadership studies is complying with the various national
ethical standards for research. Aguinis and Henle
(2002) reviewed some of the studies which described
the differing ethical standards across countries and,
in particular, drew attention to Leach and Harbins
(1997) research. Although there are some universal
ethical standards (e.g., privacy, avoiding harm, remuneration for participations), there are also noticeable
differences, such as Chinas ethics codes being
most divergent from the American Psychological
Associations (APA) guidelines, as well as the United
States standards for sharing and duplicating data.
OUTCOMES OF LEADERSHIP
Jack Welch is a figurehead in contemporary business leadership (Amernic, Craig, & Tourish, 2007),
and this is largely due to performance outcomes
attributed to him. Byrne (1998) suggests that
if leadership is an art, then surely
Welch has proved himself a master
painter. Few have personified corporate
leadership more dramatically. Fewer
still have so consistently delivered on
the results of that leadership. For
17 years, while big companies and their
chieftains tumbled like dominoes in an
unforgiving global economy, Welch has
led GE to one revenue and earnings
record after another. (p. 90)
203
Undoubtedly, the long-standing interest in leadership in organizations derives from the widespread
belief that leaders like Jack Welch have the potential to affect important organizational outcomes.
Numerous studies have demonstrated the beneficial
consequences of positive leadership, and transformational leadership in particular, on follower attitudes and states and performance in organizations.
More recently, there has also been more interest into
leaderships effect on follower health, well-being,
and safety. In this section of the chapter, we review
these leadership outcomes, explore the mechanisms
through which leadership exerts its effects, and discuss the conditions that moderate these effects (see
Figure 7.1 for an overview). First, however, several
observations flowing from the tendency to attribute
so much success to one leader (e.g., Jack Welch)
require elaboration. First, the widespread myths
linking single leaders like Welch to the fate of organizational behemoths like GE, with its operations in
Prelaunch
Phase
IndividualLevel
Variables
GroupLevel
Variables
Direct Outcomes
Numerous quantitative reviews have now established that transformational leadership relates to
various metrics of organizational effectiveness, such
as satisfaction with the leader, job satisfaction, motivation, follower perceptions of effective leadership,
leader performance, and group-organizational performance. Judge and Piccolo (2004) showed that across
87 studies, transformational leadership (r .44) was
positively related to these leadership effectiveness
Postlaunch
Phase
Launch
Phase
SocietalLevel
Variables
Sample Activities
Identification of opportunities
Initial opportunity evaluation
Assembly of required
resources
Gathering pertinent information
IndividualLevel
variables
GroupLevel
Variables
SocietalLevel
Variables
Sample Activities
Choosing legal form of new
venture
Obtaining intellectual property
protection
Developing initial business
model and strategies
Dependent Measures
Dependent Measures
IndividualLevel
Variables
GroupLevel
Variables
Sample Activities
Building customer base
Hiring key employees
Improving product design
Conducting negotiations
Influencing, motivating others
Dependent Measures
Financial Measures (growth in
sales, earnings, number of
employees; value of initial
public offering)
Success in Obtaining Required
Resources
Attitudinal Measures (e.g.,
personal and life satisfaction)
Measures of Entrepreneurs
Personal Health and WellBeing
FIGURE 7.1. Outcomes, moderators, and mediators of leadership. LMX = leadermember exchange;
OCBs = organizational citizenship behaviors.
204
SocietalLevel
Variables
Leadership
Leadership
commitment, which were directly related to organizational citizenship behaviors and task performance.
Similar empirical evidence suggests that followers
self-concordance, or the extent to which activities
such as job-related tasks or goals express individuals authentic interests or values, partially mediates
the relationship between transformational leadership and follower attitudes (Bono & Judge, 2003,
p. 556). Future research would benefit from further
explaining the ways through which leadership can
influence follower perceptions of meaningful work
and, correspondingly, their attitudes, performance,
and well-being.
Leadership
Leadership
Leadership
Prototypicality
In the predominant leader-centric tradition, heroic
images of leaders overshadow the role of the group
that the leader belongs to and leads, yet many leader
behaviors are targeted toward mobilizing followers,
emphasizing group goals, and uplifting group morale
(Chemers, 2001). Accordingly, group characteristics
are critical for leader effectiveness. The social identity
analysis of leadership postulates that the congruence
between group characteristics and leader characteristics is critical to understanding evaluations of leader
effectiveness and leader endorsement. The term
leader group prototypicality is used throughout this literature to describe the extent to which leaders represent group norms, values, and standards, also known
as group prototypes. Group prototypes are fuzzy sets
of characteristics that in a given context define the
group and they describe and prescribe group membership appropriate attributes and behavior in a specific context (Giessner & van Knippenberg, 2008,
p. 15); this effect is heightened as the salience of the
group increases (Hogg, 2001). The social identity
analysis of leadership that underlies group prototypicality describes the mechanisms through which leaders emerge and gain follower endorsement.
Unlike LMX and transformational leadership theory, which focus on the nature of leadership, the
social identity analysis of leadership is concerned
with identifying the features of leaders and followers
that critically define a leaders emergence and development (van Knippenberg & van Knippenberg,
2005). In addition, unlike implicit leadership theories, which provide a within-person understanding
214
Leadership
surrounding a prototypical leader (e.g., who is presumed to have the collectives interests at heart)
could afford the leader leeway in his or her actions
(including actions that lead to failure) that should
extend to that individuals unconventional or risky
behaviors (van Knippenberg et al., 2000; van
Knippenberg & van Knippenberg, 2005).
The situation of low group prototypicality has
also been studied. Van Knippenberg and van
Knippenberg (2005) found a significant relationship
between leader self-sacrifice and evaluations of
leader effectiveness, and this was most pronounced
for leaders who were low in group prototypicality.
This finding suggests that leaders self-sacrificing
behaviors may compensate for the lack of group prototypicality: Self-sacrificing behavior, not unlike group
prototypicality, signals that a leader is genuinely interested in the collective good. Future directions for
research may take this a step further and posit situations in which low leader group prototypicality is
actually beneficial for followers, such as during
times of change or when the groups culture is
counterproductive.
Relatively new to leadership studies, the social
identity analysis of leadership is predominantly
studied in laboratory experiments. It follows that the
challenge is to apply these concepts to real-world situations or to experiments that maximize ecological
validity, where group prototypes and leader prototypicality may not be as salient as they appear in
experiment designs. In this vein, Hogg and colleagues (2006) conducted an experiment in which
the group prototype was not explicit. To create an
ambiguous group prototype, they manipulated the
salience of either a stereotypically feminine (e.g., creative) or masculine (e.g., rational) group norm rather
than explicitly stating that the group prototype was
male or female. They also manipulated leader
gender and measured participants sex-role orientation (i.e., beliefs about female and male behavioral
norms), and they found that even when the group
prototype was ambiguous, male leaders were judged
as more effective when the group prototype was
stereotypically masculine. Similarly, female leaders
were judged as more effective when the group prototype was stereotypically feminine. It follows that even
under conditions of inexplicitness the significance
215
Leadership
supervisors, followers, and followers family members, Hoobler and Brass (2006) found evidence
consistent with the notion that supervisors displaced
their anger on followers, as evidenced by abusive
supervisory behaviors, and that followers displaced
their aggression onto their family members.
Specifically, when supervisors perceived a breach in
their psychological contract with their organizations,
then followers were more likely to report abusive
supervisory behaviors; this was particularly the case
for leaders with a hostile attribution bias or the tendency to overly blame others. Moreover, family
members of abused followers reported higher levels
of family undermining committed by the abused followers. A second empirical study also lends some
support to the displaced aggression theory of abusive
supervision. In this study, leaders who experienced
interactional injustice from their immediate supervisors were more likely to be perceived by their followers as abusive. However, this relationship only
held for supervisors with an authoritarian leadership
style (Aryee, Chen, Sun, & Debrah, 2007).
Although concern about the predominant negative focus in much of psychology is not new, such
negativity may be less prevalent in the field of leadership, with its strong emphasis on the benefits of positive leadership. In fact, further research is required to
more fully understand the antecedents of abusive
supervision and how follower attributes and behaviors moderate these relationships (Tepper, 2007).
For example, leader depression is one empirically
tested predictor of abusive supervision (Tepper,
Duffy, Henle, & Lambert, 2006), while other proposed antecedents, such as leader personality and
organizational culture, remain unexplored (Tepper,
2007). Perhaps one explanation for the overall lack
of studies on negative leadership is that leaders
behaviors are not always consistent; for example,
they could be charismatic, yet also display bouts of
hostile behavior (Pfeffer, 2007). Steve Jobs of Apple
Computer might reflect this: He has been credited
with reviving Apple and painted as charismatic in the
process (e.g., Harvey, 2001), but he also has a reputation for sadistic perfectionism, often without discernible provocation (Berglas, 1999, p. 29). These
complexities and inconsistencies are difficult to
capture in single studies but must be reflected in
217
Unethical Leaders
Despite considerable concern devoted to this issue
by the lay public, the ethics of leadership has all too
often escaped systematic study by organizational
scholars (Brown, Trevio, & Harrison, 2005),
although this critical omission is now being reversed.
Greater attention is being accorded to ethical issues
in leadership from an array of different approaches,
such as personality (e.g., House & Howell, 1992;
Judge et al., 2006), values (e.g., Bass & Steidlmeier,
1999), moral reasoning (Turner, Barling, Epitropaki,
Butcher, & Milner, 2002), moral orientation (Simola,
Barling & Turner, in press), generalized ethical
leadership (Brown et al.), follower attributions
(Dasborough & Ashkanasy, 2002), and behavioral
integrity (Dineen, Lewicki, & Tomlinson, 2006).
Brown et al. (2005) defined ethical leadership as
the demonstration of normatively appropriate conduct through personal actions and interpersonal
relationships, and the promotion of such conduct to
followers through two-way communication, reinforcement, and decision-making (p. 120). The
authors conceptually distinguished this concept of
ethical leadership from other similar constructs,
such as transformational leadership and leader honesty, and showed that ethical leadership was positively associated with follower satisfaction with the
leader, job dedication, willingness to speak up about
problems, and leader effectiveness.
More frequently, scholarly attention has been
drawn to depictions of leaders as immoral or unethical, which should describe leaders who fail to uphold
the behaviors described by Brown et al. (2005). One
of the most prominent distinctions is between socialized charismatic and personalized charismatic leaders (e.g., House & Howell, 1992; Howell & Avolio,
1992) or, in parallel, transformational, and pseudotransformational leaders (e.g., Bass & Steidlmeier,
1999). Personalized charismatic, or pseudotransformational, leaders offer the illusion of transformational leadership through their strong inspirational
218
Leadership
LMX-7
Supervisory Behavior Description
Questionnaire (SBDQ)
Ohio State Leader Behavior Description
Questionnaire (LBDQ)
Need for Supervision Scale
Romance of Leadership Scale (RLS)
Leadermember exchange
Leadership style
Romance of leadership
Self-leadership
Situational leadership/leader
adaptability
Supervisory control
Transformational leadership
Stodgill, 1963
Implicit leadership
Tepper, 2000
Conger and Kanungo, 1994
Abusive Supervision
Conger-Kanungo (C-K) scale of charismatic
leadership
Ethical Leadership Scale
Followership Questionnaire
Abusive supervision
Charismatic leadership
Ethical leadership
Follower style
Source
Measure
Construct
TABLE 7.2
Leadership
221
Education
The educational or school context also applies to the
study of leadership. Intriguingly, effective leadership
in this context may have long-term consequences
for student attitudes and performance. Whether the
focus is on the influence of principals on teachers or
on the influence of teachers on students, many of
the behaviors under consideration (e.g., student performance) are largely discretionary, increasing the
potential influence of leadership.
Like other contexts we have showcased thus far,
studies in the education milieu have focused on
transformational leadership, showing, for example,
that teachers transformational leadership is related
to students perceptions of teacher performance
and students involvement in their own studies
(Harvey, Royal, & Stout, 2003). Nguni, Sleegers,
and Denessens (2006) study also highlighted the
effectiveness of transformational leadership but reiterated that any leadership effects are often indirect.
In addition, their results supported the augmentation hypothesis (Bass, 1998) demonstrating that
teachers transformational leadership provided
unique variance after accounting for the effects of
Leadership
Followership
Perhaps Napoleon Bonaparte said it best: Soldiers
generally win battles; generals get credit for them.
The leader-centric studies that dominate research
on leadership result in an incomplete knowledge
about leadership; however, advocating an exclusive
follower-centric research agenda would result in
similarly unbalanced knowledge. Thus, we advocate
a relational view of leadership, one in which leaders
and followers together produce leadership. Inserting
followers into the leadership equation is not novel,
but, as reflected in the opening quote to this section,
the contribution of followers is underappreciated
and, moreover, often restricted to obeying orders
and taking direction (Baker, 2008).
We join the call for research to combat such a
passive stereotype of followers; it diminishes the
role of followers in organizational success and
leader effectiveness and followers ability to motivate leadership change (e.g., Deluga, 1987), and by
default it exaggerates the role and importance of
leaders. Suggestive support for followers as active
contributors to leadership emerged in Dvir and
Shamirs (2003) longitudinal study in which leaders were rated as less transformational as followers
developed their own leadership skills. One possible
explanation is that with follower development, a
transformational relationship emerged, meaning that
the responsibility to motivate, inspire, stimulate, and
nurture are shared between leader and follower.
Although the possibility for bidirectional socialization
remains to be investigated directly in a leadership
context, evidence of such effects exists in other hierarchical relationships, including parentchild relationships (e.g., Glass, Bengtson, & Dunham, 1986).
Developing implicit followership theories (i.e.,
expectations and beliefs about followers) might be
especially useful in understanding followership.
With expectations on leaders and followers, dyadic
data become essential. Each member of the
leaderfollower dyad gains meaning from and
through the other.
Shared Leadership
Moving beyond leaderfollower distinctions, what
about the situation in which many individuals in
the same group demonstrate leadership behaviors?
223
Authentic Leadership
Consistent with the attention given to some major
ethical lapses by leaders, the notion of authentic
leadership has attracted much interest since the turn
of the century. Following early debate, Avolio and
his colleagues included the following components in
their definition of authentic leadership (see Avolio
& Luthans, 2006; Walumbwa, Avolio, Gardner,
Wernsing, & Peterson, 2008): self-awareness, relational transparency, balanced processing of all relevant data in an objective manner before decisions
are made, and finally, an internalized moral perspective. Although the relative newness of this perspective means that few empirical studies have been
reported, Walumbwa et al. have provided a reliable
and valid measure, and their data supported a
higher-order multidimensional model of the construct. One benefit of these early data is that they
were collected not just in the United States but also
in Kenya and China (Walumbwa et al., 2008), allaying concerns that the concept of authentic leadership may be culturally bound.
Important questions remain for this nascent theory. On a conceptual level, there are differing views as
Leadership
Supporting this notion is the aforementioned finding that leaders apologies following transgressions
are related to transformational leadership ratings
(Tucker et al., 2006); Tucker et al. argued that apologies are critical moments in the leaderfollower
relationship and defined such moments as distinct interactions that, while occurring relatively
infrequently, serve to punctuate or reinforce the
status quo (p. 197).
Similarly, whether or not charismatic leaders are
more likely to emerge in times of crisis has been
debated (for a review. see Shamir & Howell, 1999).
Although some studies suggest that charismatic leaders emerge in times of crisis (e.g., Roberts & Bradley,
1988), evidence to the contrary is not uncommon
(e.g., Pillai & Meindl, 1998). Alternatively, it is possible for leaders to frame situations in a way that is perceived as critical or extraordinary, suggesting reverse
causality (Shamir & Howell, 1999). Nevertheless,
recent research suggests that transformational leaders
may be more effective under critical performance
conditions than in more ordinary situations (Lim &
Ployhart, 2004). Clarifying these issues is required if
researchers are to elucidate the role of leadership in
critical moments.
Preliminary evidence points to potential differences in leadership behaviors and leadership perceptions in ordinary versus extraordinary situations.
However, many questions remain. Is leadership best
conceptualized as an average frequency of behaviors
over time, or do some situations enhance the salience
of certain leadership behaviors? Do any benefits that
accrue to leaders because of their behaviors during a
crisis carry over to an everyday basis? Researchers
have the opportunity to provide new knowledge by
focusing on questions such as these.
Humility
One way to understand leadership is to through the
core role of humility. Morris et al. (2005) defined
humility as a personal orientation founded on a
willingness to see the self accurately and a propensity to put oneself in perspective (p. 1331). While
acknowledging that other leadership theories also
accord a critical role to humility (e.g., level 5 leadership; Collins, 2001), the behaviors involved in transformational leadership make it as relevant to humility.
225
Humility has been identified as important to understanding transformational leadership (Bass & Riggio,
2006) in that humility would restrain leaders from
becoming entranced with public adulation and
would influence leaders to be other-focused (Morris
et al, 2005). Despite this recognition of the importance of humility in transformational leadership,
there has not yet been a focus on how the specific
humility-related behaviors engaged in by leaders
underlie transformational leadership.
bases of behavior and the research methods underlying both. With the majority of leadership researchers
being trained in the social sciences, collaboration
across fields traditionally seen as unrelated will be
a necessity.
Humor
Although there is some research on humor in the
workplace (e.g., Fleming, 2005; Francis, 1994;
Yovetich, Dale, & Hudak, 1990), its prevalence in
organizations is widespread and diverse in nature.
Leaders have certainly been exhorted to provide a
so-called fun workplace culture (Pfeffer, 1998),
including the use of humor (Fleming, 2005). Such
appeals are based on the notion that fun workplace
cultures are associated with many benefits, including increased worker motivation (Crawford, 1994),
commitment, and performance (Avolio, Howell, &
Sosik, 1999). In this sense, humor is instrumental
and strategic; as explained by Fleming, humor is
ultimately a serious business. It is unsurprisingly
driven by very sober corporate motives (p. 288).
Although humor can sometimes be beneficial,
positive outcomes do not always ensue. Avolio and
colleagues (1999) found that the frequent use of
humor does not always lead to better performance;
they speculated that some issues (e.g., setting target
objectives) were not amenable to humor because of
their seriousness. Additionally, positive outcomes
depend on the type of humor used. Decker and
Rotondo (2001) distinguished between negative
(e.g., sexual and insult humor) and positive (e.g.,
nonoffensive humor) forms of humor and concluded
that leaders who used positive forms of humor
received more positive leader ratings than leaders
who used negative forms of humor. One question
that immediately emerges is: Why would leaders
choose to use negative forms of humor?
According to the superiority theory of humor,
negative forms of humor (such as insults) reinforce
the hierarchy between leaders and followers
(Westwood, 2004). Leaders who want to maintain
power distance may be especially prone to using
humor in this manner. In contrast, positive forms of
humor should minimize the distance between leaders
and followers (Barsoux, 1996). In essence, humor can
be used to communicate a leaders values, especially
Leadership
Leadership Selection
While genetic and early family environment influences leadership role occupancy and behaviors,
the selection of leaders is in the hands of organizations. To facilitate leadership selection,
questionnaires and different assessment tools
(e.g., structured interviews; Krajewski, Goffin,
McCarthy, Rothstein, & Johnston, 2006) are frequently used. Nonetheless, the basic conceptual
assumption, that performance on a questionnaire
can predict performance on the battlefield, is
debatable (Gladwell, 2004, 2008).
Use of personality tests to aid in selecting leaders
rests on the premise that certain personality traits are
empirically associated with effective (and noneffective; Hogan & Hogan, 2001) leadership behaviors.
Perhaps the most widely used personality inventory
227
References
Agle, B. R., Nagarajan, N. J., Sonnenfeld, J. A., &
Srinivasan, D. (2006). Does CEO charisma matter? An
empirical analysis of the relationships among organizational performance, environmental uncertainty, and
top management team perceptions of CEO charisma.
Academy of Management Journal, 49, 161174.
Leadership
breaking-news-alessandra-facchinetti-at-valentino.
aspx
Barry, D. (1991). Managing the bossless team: Lessons in
distributed leadership. Organizational Dynamics, 20,
3147.
Barsoux, J. (1996). Why organizations need humor.
European Management Journal, 14, 500508.
Bass, B. M. (1960). Leadership, psychology, and organizational behavior. New York: Harper & Brothers.
Bass, B. M. (1983). Issues involved in relations between
methodological rigor and reported outcomes in evaluations in evaluations of organizational development. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 197199.
Bass, B. M., & Avolio, B. J. (1990). Manual for the Multifactor Leadership Questionnaire. Palo Alto, CA:
Consulting Psychologists Press.
Bass, B. M., Avolio, B. J., Jung, D. I., & Berson, Y. (2003).
Predicting unit performance by assessing transformational and transactional leadership. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 88, 207218.
Bass, B. M., & Riggio, R. E. (2006). Transformational
leadership (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bass, B. M., & Steidlmeier, P. (1999). Ethics, character,
and authentic transformational leadership behavior.
Leadership Quarterly, 10, 181217.
Batstone, E., Boraston, I., & Frenkel, S. (1977). Shop
stewards in action: The organization of workplace conflict and accommodation. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology Monographs, 4, 1102.
Berglas, S. (1999). What you can learn from Steve Jobs.
Inc, 21, 2931.
Bernerth, J. B., Armenakis, A. A., Feild, H. S., Giles, W. F.,
& Walker, H. J. (2007). Is personality associated with
perceptions of LMX? An empirical study. Leadership
& Organization Development Journal, 28, 613631.
Bird, R., Hall, A., Moment, F., & Reggiani, F. (2007).
What corporate responsibility activities are valued by
the market? Journal of Business Ethics, 76, 189206.
Bligh, M. C., Kohles, J. C., & Meindl, J. R. (2004).
Charisma under crisis: Presidential leadership,
rhetoric, and media responses before and after the
September 11 terrorist attacks. Leadership Quarterly,
15, 211239.
230
Leadership
Dupr, K. E., & Barling, J. (2006). Predicting and preventing supervisory workplace aggression. Journal of
Occupational Health Psychology, 11, 1326.
Dutton, J. E., Frost, P. J., Worline, M. C., Lilius, J. M., &
Kanov, J. M. (2002). Leading in times of trauma.
Harvard Business Review, 80, 5461.
Dvir, T., Eden, D., Avolio, B. J., Shamir, B. (2002). Impact
of transformational leadership on follower development and performance: A field experiment. Academy
of Management Journal, 45, 735744.
Dvir, T., & Shamir, B. (2003). Follower developmental
characteristics as predicting transformational leadership: A longitudinal field study. Leadership Quarterly,
14, 327344.
Eagly, A. H., Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C., & van Engen,
M. L. (2003). Transformational, transactional, and
laissez-faire leadership styles: A meta-analysis comparing women and men. Psychological Bulletin, 129,
569591.
Eagly, A. H., & Johnson, B. T. (1990). Gender and leadership style: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,
108, 233256.
Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (1991). Gender and the emergence of leaders: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 685710.
Dirks, K. T. (2000). Trust in leadership and team performance: Evidence from NCAA basketball. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 85, 10041012.
232
Leadership
233
Hains, S. C., Hogg, M. A., & Duck, J. M. (1997). Selfcategorization and leadership: Effects of group prototypicality and leader stereotypicality. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 10871099.
Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the childs environment? A
group socialization theory of development. Psychological Review, 102, 458489.
Hartman, S. J., & Harris, O. J. (1992). The role of parental
influences on leadership. The Journal of Social Psychology, 132, 153167.
Harvey, A. (2001). A dramaturgical analysis of charismatic
leader discourse. Journal of Organizational Change
Management, 14, 253265.
Harvey, S., Royal, M., Stout, D. (2003). Instructors transformational leadership: University student attitudes
and ratings. Psychological Reports, 92, 395402.
Heinitz, K., Liepmann, D., & Felfe, J. (2005). Examining
the factor structure of the MLQ: Recommendation
for a reduced set of factors. European Journal of
Psychological Assessment, 21, 182190.
Hersey, P., & Blanchard, K. (1988). Management of organization behavior: Utilizing human resources, 5th ed.,
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Herold, D. M., Fedor, D. B., Caldwell, S., & Liu, Y. (2008).
The effects of transformational and change leadership
on employees commitment to a change: A multilevel
study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 346357.
Hinkin, T. R. & Schriesheim, C. A. (2008). An examination of nonleadership: From laissez-faire leadership
to leader reward omission and punishment omission.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 12341248.
Hofmann, D. A., & Jones, L. M. (2005). Leadership, collective personality, and performance. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 90, 509522.
Hofmann, D. A., & Morgeson, F. P. (1999). Safety-related
behavior as a social exchange: The role of perceived
organizational support and leader-member exchange.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 286296.
Hofstede, G. (2001). Cultures consequences: Comparing
values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across
nations (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Hogan, R., & Hogan, J. (2001). Assessing leadership: A
view from the dark side. International Journal of
Selection and Assessment, 9, 4051.
Hogg, M. A. (2001). A social identity theory of leadership.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 184200.
Hogg, M. A., Fielding, K. S., Johnson, D., Masser, B.,
Russell, E., & Svensson, A. (2006). Demographic category membership and leadership in small groups: A
social identity analysis. Leadership Quarterly, 17,
335350.
Hogg, M. A., Hains, S. C., & Mason, I. (1998).
Identification and leadership in small groups:
234
Salience, frame of reference, and leader stereotypicality effects on leader evaluations. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 75, 12481263.
Hoobler, J. M., & Brass, D. J. (2006). Abusive supervision
and family undermining as displaced aggression.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 11251133.
Hooijberg, R., & Choi, J (2000). From selling peanuts
and beer in Yankee Stadium to creating a theory of
transformational leadership: An interview with
Bernie Bass. Leadership Quarterly, 11, 291306.
Hooker, C., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2003). Flow, creativity, and shared leadership: Rethinking the motivation
and structuring of knowledge work. In C. L. Pearce &
J. A. Conger (Eds.), Shared leadership: Reframing the
hows and whys of leadership. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Hoption, C., Phelan, J., & Barling, J. (2007). Transformational leadership in sport. In M. R. Beauchamp &
M. A. Eys (Eds.), Group dynamics advances in sport and
exercise psychology: Contemporary themes (pp. 4560).
Abingdon, England: Routledge.
Houghton, J. D., & Neck, C. P. (2002). The revised selfleadership questionnaire: Testing a hierarchical factor
structure for self-leadership. Journal of Managerial
Psychology, 17, 672691.
Houghton, J. D., Neck, C. P., & Manz, C. C. (2003). Selfleadership and superleadership: The heart and art of
creating shared leadership in teams. In C. L. Pearce
& J. A. Conger (Eds.), Shared leadership: Reframing
the hows and whys of leadership (pp. 123140).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
House, R., & Howell, J. M. (1992). Personality and charismatic leadership. Leadership Quarterly, 3, 81108.
House, R., Javidan, M., Hanges, P., & Dorfman, P. (2002).
Understanding cultures and implicit leadership theories across the globe: An introduction to project
GLOBE. Journal of World Business, 37, 310.
House, R. J. (1971). A path goal theory of leader effectiveness. Administrative Science Quarterly, 16, 321339.
House, R. J. (1977). A 1976 theory of charismatic leadership. In J. G. Hunt & L. L. Larsen (Eds.), Leadership:
The cutting edge (pp. 189207). Carbondale, IL:
Southern Illinois University Press.
House, R. J. (1996). Pathgoal theory of leadership:
Lessons, legacy and a reformulated theory. Leadership
Quarterly, 7, 323352.
House, R. J. (2004). Illustrative examples of GLOBE
findings. In R. J. House, P. J. Hanges, M. Javidan,
P. W. Dorfman, & V. Gupta (Eds.), Culture, leadership and organizations: The GLOBE study of 62 societies (pp. 38). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
House, R. J., & Aditya, R. N. (1997). The social scientific
study of leadership: Quo vadis? Journal of Management, 23, 409473.
Leadership
House, R. J., & Podsakoff, P. M. (1994). Leadership effectiveness: Past perspectives and future directions for
research. In J. Greenberg (Ed.), Organizational behavior: The state of the science (pp. 4582). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Howell, J. M., & Avolio, B. J. (1992). The ethics of charismatic leadership: Submission or liberation? The
Executive, 6, 43.
Ilies, R., Gerhardt, M. W., & Le, H. (2004). Individual differences in leadership emergence: Integrating metaanalytic findings and behavioral genetics estimates.
International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 12,
207219.
Kark, R., Shamir, B., & Chen, G. (2003). The two faces
of transformational leadership: Empowerment and
dependency. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88,
246255.
Karnes, F. A., & DIlio, V. R. (1989). Student leaders and
their parents perceptions of the home environment.
Gifted Child Quarterly, 33, 165168.
Keller, R. T. (2006). Transformational leadership, initiating structure, and substitutes for leadership: A longitudinal study of research and development project
team performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91,
202210.
Keller, T. (1999). Images of the familiar: Individual differences and implicit leadership theories. Leadership
Quarterly, 10, 589607.
Kellett, P. (1999). Organisational leadership: Lessons
from professional coaches. Sport Management Review,
2, 150171.
Kelley, R. E. (1992). The power of followership: How to
create leaders people want to follow and followers who
lead themselves. New York: Doubleday.
Kelloway, E. K., & Barling, J. (1993). Members participation in local union activities: Measurement, prediction and replication. Journal of Applied Psychology,
78, 262279.
Judge, T. A., Piccolo, R. F., & Ilies, R. (2004). The forgotten ones? The validity of consideration and initiating
Leadership
McGue, M., & Bouchard, T. J. (1998). Genetic and environmental influences on human behavioral differences. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 21, 124.
Meindl, J. R. (1995). The romance of leadership as a
follower-centric theory: A social constructionist
approach. Leadership Quarterly, 6, 329341.
Meindl, J. R. (1998). Appendix: Measure and assessments for the romance of leadership approach. In
F. Dansereau & F. J. Yammarino (Eds.), Leadership:
The multiple-level approachesPart B: Contemporary
and alternative (p. 199302). Stamford, CT: JAI.
Meindl, J. R., & Ehrlich, S. B. (1987). The romance of
leadership and the evaluation of organizational performance. Academy of Management Journal, 30, 91109.
Meindl, J. R., Ehrlich, S. B., & Dukerich, J. M. (1985).
The romance of leadership. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 30, 78102.
Mitchell, M. S., & Ambrose, M. L. (2007). Abusive supervision and workplace deviance and the moderating
effects of negative reciprocity beliefs. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 92, 11591168.
Morris, J. A., Brotheridge, C. M., & Urbanski, J. C.
(2005). Bringing humility to leadership: Antecedents
and consequences of leader humility. Human
Relations, 58, 13231350.
Morgan, B. B., Jr., Salas, E., & Glickman, A. (1993). An
analysis of team evolution and maturation. Journal of
General Psychology, 120, 277291.
Mullen, J., & Kelloway, E. K. (2009). Safety leadership: A
longitudinal study of the effects of transformational
leadership on safety outcomes. Journal of Occupational
and Organizational Psychology, 2, 253272.
Nathan, B. R., & Lord, R. G. (1983). Cognitive categorization and dimensional schemata: A process
approach to the study of halo in performance ratings.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 102114.
Neubert, M. J. (1999). Too much of a good thing or the
more the merrier? Exploring the dispersion and gender composition of informal leadership in manufacturing teams. Small Group Research, 30, 635646.
Nguni, S., Sleegers, P., & Denessen, E. (2006).
Transformational and transactional leadership effects
on teachers job satisfaction, organizational commitment, and organizational citizenship behavior in
primary schools: The Tanzanian case. School
Effectiveness and School Improvement, 17, 145177.
Nye, J. S. (2008). The powers to lead. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Offermann, L. R., Kennedy, J. K., & Wirtz, P. W. (1994).
Implicit leadership theories: Content, structure, and
generalizability. Leadership Quarterly, 5, 4358.
Paunonen, S. V., Lnnqvist, J., Verkasalo, M., Leikas, S.,
& Nissinen, V. (2006). Narcissism and emergent
Sahin, S. (2004). The relationship between transformational and transactional leadership styles of school
principals and school culture. Kuram ve Uygulamada
Egitim Bilimleri, 42, 387396.
Schyns, B., & Felfe, J. (2006). The personality of followers and its effect on the perception of leadership: An
overview, a study, and a research agenda. Small
Group Research, 37, 522539.
Leadership
240