Vedic Recitation and Effects On Brain

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

YNIMG-12418; No.

of pages: 12; 4C: 4, 5, 6, 7, 8


NeuroImage xxx (2015) xxxxxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

NeuroImage
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg

Brains of verbal memory specialists show anatomical differences in


language, memory and visual systems
James F. Hartzell a,, Ben Davis a, David Melcher a, Gabriele Miceli a, Jorge Jovicich a, Tanmay Nath b,
Nandini Chatterjee Singh b, Uri Hasson a
a
b

Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), University of Trento, 38060, Italy


National Brain Research Centre, Manesar, Gurgaon Dist., Haryana 122 050, India

a r t i c l e

i n f o

Article history:
Accepted 8 July 2015
Available online xxxx
Keywords:
Cortical thickness
Gray matter density
Diffusion tensor imaging
Language
Memory
Plasticity
Hippocampus

a b s t r a c t
We studied a group of verbal memory specialists to determine whether intensive oral text memory is
associated with structural features of hippocampal and lateral-temporal regions implicated in language
processing. Professional Vedic Sanskrit Pandits in India train from childhood for around 10 years in an ancient, formalized tradition of oral Sanskrit text memorization and recitation, mastering the exact pronunciation and invariant content of multiple 40,000100,000 word oral texts. We conducted structural analysis
of gray matter density, cortical thickness, local gyrication, and white matter structure, relative to matched
controls. We found massive gray matter density and cortical thickness increases in Pandit brains in
language, memory and visual systems, including i) bilateral lateral temporal cortices and ii) the anterior
cingulate cortex and the hippocampus, regions associated with long and short-term memory. Differences
in hippocampal morphometry matched those previously documented for expert spatial navigators and individuals with good verbal working memory. The ndings provide unique insight into the brain organization implementing formalized oral knowledge systems.
2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

Introduction
A large body of research has established that acquisition of certain
long-term skill sets or knowledge is linked to plasticity in both grey
matter (GM) and white matter (WM) in multiple cortical and subcortical regions (May, 2011; Zatorre et al., 2004). As reviewed by May (2011,
see references within), various expert groups such as sportsmen,
mathematicians, ballet dancers, and professional board-game players
all show particular morphological features that may be related to
learning and plasticity.
Our goal in the current work was to examine the potential impact of
extensive memorization and verbal recital practice on brain plasticity,
as identifying brain regions implicated in these functions can elucidate
the functional capacities of both lateral and medial temporal regions,
as detailed below. To investigate the potential impact of extensive
memorization and verbal recital practice on brain plasticity we recruited a sample group of traditional Sanskrit learnersYajurveda Sanskrit
Panditswho memorize and recite one set of the most ancient Sanskrit
Corresponding author at: Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), Via delle Regole
101, Mattarello, TN, Italy.
E-mail address: [email protected] (J.F. Hartzell).

texts, the Vedas and their subsidiary texts (Vedgas). The Sanskrit
Vedas are late bronze/early iron-age oral texts passed down for over
3000 years in an unbroken tradition in India. They form the core of
the ancient Sanskrit knowledge system, which developed extensive
oral and later written literature in a wide range of traditional subjects
still taught in India's Sanskrit institutions using traditional oral memorization and recitation methods (Rashtriya Sanskrit Sansthan, 2014). Professional Vedic Pandits undergo rigorous training in exact
pronunciation and invariant content of these oral texts for 7 or more
years, with 810 h of daily practice (totaling ~10,080 h over the course
of the initial training), starting in their childhood, and mastering multiple 40,000 to 100,000 word oral texts (compared to ~38,000 in the book
of Genesis). The training methods strongly emphasize traditional faceto-face oral learning, and the Yajurveda recitation practice includes
right hand and arm gestures to mark prosodic elements. After graduation from training, professional Yajurveda Pandits work as teachers or
Vedic priests, with daily recitation reduced to ~3 h.
We note that while the ability of Yajurveda Pandits to perform
large-scale, precise oral memorization and recitation of Vedic
Sanskrit texts may, prima facie, appear extraordinary or bordering
on impossible, textual memorization and recitation are in fact standard
practice in traditional Sanskrit education in India (Rashtriya Sanskrit

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027
1053-8119/ 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

Please cite this article as: Hartzell, J.F., et al., Brains of verbal memory specialists show anatomical differences in language, memory and visual
systems, NeuroImage (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027

J.F. Hartzell et al. / NeuroImage xxx (2015) xxxxxx

Sansthan, 2014).1 Thus, while the Pandit's memorization capacity may


appear unique to graduates of a Western educational system, it is one
of several memorization-related study traditions current in the Indian
subcontinent.
We had two predictions regarding brain systems possibly affected
by the intense memorization and recitation routine practiced by the
Pandits. First, we expected to see differences in cortical thickness or
gray matter density of lateral temporal regions. These form the core system for speech processing at the phonemic and syllabic level (Zhuang
et al., 2014), with left hemisphere regions of the superior temporal
plane (STP) likely sampling information at a higher rate matching that
of phonemic processing, and the right hemisphere STP sampling at a
lower rate matching syllable-level processing (Giraud and Poeppel,
2012; Kotz and Schwartze, 2010; Morillon et al., 2012; Poeppel, 2003).
Apart from their role in sublexical combinatorial processes, these regions also play a role in encoding sentential content to memory. Activity
in these regions predicts whether sentential content will be subsequently remembered (a subsequent-memory effect, Hasson et al.,
2007), and they show reduced activation for comprehension of repeated auditory sentences (repetition suppression (RS); Dehaene-Lambertz
et al., 2006; Devauchelle et al., 2009). Particularly, sentential RS effects
in these regions scale negatively with the temporal interval between
sentence repetitions (Hasson et al., 2006). Thus, extensive memorization of language content, coupled with memory for sentential content
could affect the structure of these regions.
In addition, plasticity effects linked to memory practice have been
documented in the human hippocampus, which is involved in both
the consolidation of prior experiences (e.g., Eichenbaum et al., 2007;
Milner and Peneld, 1955; Scoville and Milner, 1957) and spatial navigation (e.g., Bird and Burgess, 2008; see also Eichenbaum and Cohen,
2014). Hippocampal plasticity has been linked to spatial navigation expertise, with greater posterior hippocampal volume and smaller anterior volume shown for expert urban navigators (Maguire et al., 2000). The
hippocampus also mediates verbal memory (e.g., Fernandez et al., 1998;
Grunwald et al., 1999), and is larger for individuals who perform better
on declarative memory tasks for verbal materials (e.g., Ashtari et al.,
2011; Pohlack et al., 2014). Poppenk and Moscovitch (2011) showed
that better verbal memory for proverbs is related to greater posterior
and smaller anterior hippocampal volume, a pattern similar to that
seen for expert navigators. On the basis of this prior work we hypothesized that the intensive memorization demands of Pandit practice might
be associated with changes to hippocampal volume or density.
To examine these issues, we studied a group of Pandits (N = 21) together with closely matched controls. We examined cortical-level data
via voxel-based morphometry (VBM), cortical thickness (CT) and local
gyrication index (LGI) analyses, and subcortical data via VBM and anatomically dened regional measurements. We also evaluated white
matter data with diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) fractional anisotropy
(FA) analysis, at a whole-brain level. The main purpose of the FA analysis was to evaluate whether WM changes would be found in the vicinity
of areas linked to GM or CT differences. In particular, the frontal aslant
tract (Catani et al., 2013) has been implicated in uency and stuttering
(Kronfeld-Duenias et al., 2014), as has the forceps minor in the anterior
corpus callosum (Civier et al., 2015).

1
There are today in India around 150,000 students engaged in traditional Sanskrit studies at approximately 5000 government and private institutions (Mishra, 1997; Rashtriya
Sanskrit Sansthan, 2010-2011; Rashtriya Sanskrit Sansthan, 2014; Pathashala, 2014).
The topics (and texts memorized) at these institutions include Sanskrit literature, grammar, law, history, philosophy, astronomy, yoga, logic, and Vedas, subsidiary Vedic disciplines, and Vedic commentary (Rashtriya Sanskrit Sansthan, 2014). There are in addition
some 246 registered Ayurvedic traditional medical colleges in India where some 50,000
students memorize portions of Sanskrit core ("root") medical texts and subsidiary texts
as part of their training (Central Council of Indian Medicine, 2014; Hartzell and Zysk,
1995). Specically for Vedic studies, there are currently an estimated 34,000 Vedic Pandits
in training in both government and non-government traditional Vedic schools (Shastri,
2014; Pathashala, 2014; Rashtriya Sanskrit Sansthan, 2014; Mishra, 1997).

Methods
Participants
Forty-two male volunteers participated in the study conducted at
the National Brain Research Center in India. Twenty-one professionally
qualied Pandits were recruited from government-supported Vedic
Pandit schools in the greater New Delhi (India) area. They underwent
an extensive semi-structured interview prior to scanning to evaluate
their extent of training, family history, current practice routines, multilingualism, handedness and eye dominance. Professional qualication
constituted demonstrable mastery, i.e. complete memorization and
full recitation ability, of at least the ~ 40,000 word Yajurveda Sahit
text. All Pandits memorized part or all of one or more additional canonical texts (the length of these texts ranged from 1013 to 165,156 words
but we could not quantify precisely how much of these additional texts
was memorized by each Pandit). All began their training at an early age
(M = 12.33, SD = 1.59, range 916), trained full-time for 7 years, for a
total of approximately 10,080 h, and continued training and reciting at
reduced daily hours for additional years (M = 2.38, SD = 2.29, range
08). From the interview reports, we estimated the total practice
hours after competing the training (M = 11,141 h, SD = 27,196,
range 2365129,295). Note that Pandits enter training without any entrance exams, so there is no pre-selection for memory or recital abilities,
and the dropout rate from the study program is only around 5% (Shastri,
2014). Thus, graduating the studies is not indicative of self-selection either prior to or during the studies themselves. Pandits had all either
graduated from or were in the nal year of professional Vedic Pandit
training, and all were self-rated as uent in speaking, reading and
writing Sanskrit. None of the Pandits in our participant group came
from traditional family lineages of reciters (see SI Methods). See
Supplementary Information (SI Methods and SI Table 1) for additional
Pandit demographics and practice specics.
Twenty-one control volunteers were recruited to match the Pandit
population in gender, age (Mpandits = 21.7; SD = 2.8 vs. Mcontrols =
22.8; SD = 3.6, T-test, P = .3) and number of languages spoken
(Mpandits = 3.1; SD = 0.8 vs. Mcontrols = 3.1; SD = 1.3, T-test, P = .9). Participants in the control group were members of India's National Brain
Research Centre community or students from a nearby technical college. All volunteers were right-handed, right-eye dominant, with no
left-handed parent or sibling (Knecht et al., 2000). Multilingualism
and handedness/eye-dominance were assessed by culturally-adapted
Hindi versions of the Penn State Language History Questionnaire (v.2;
Li et al., 2006), and Edinburgh Handedness questionnaire (Oldeld,
1971). (Adaptations and translations by N.C.S., T.N., J.H, and a fourth
native Hindi/English speaker). The protocol was approved by India's
National Brain Research Centre Ethics Committee and all participants
provided written informed consent.
Image acquisition
Two T1-weighted 3D-MPRAGE sequences were acquired for each
participant on a Philips Achieva 3 T scanner with an 8-channel head receive coil (FOV 256 256 176 mm, voxel size 1x1x1mm), TE 3.2 ms,
TR 934 ms, ip angle 9, 176 sagittal-oriented slices, acceleration 2
(sense), total acquisition time 06:49.8. Image quality was evaluated
immediately after each structural acquisition to control for motion effects or other artifacts. The two structural images of each participant
were aligned using FSL's 4.1.8 FLIRT (Jenkinson et al., 2002; Jenkinson
and Smith, 2001), and averaged to increase signal-to-noise ratio.
Image intensity non-uniformities were corrected in AFNI (Cox, 1996).
The resulting mean structural image was used for all subsequent
analyses. Diffusion data were acquired for a subset of 15 Pandits and
15 controls using single-shot EPI during the same MRI session (FOV
256 256 128 mm3, voxel size 2 2 2 mm3), TE 75 ms, TR
8000 ms, ip angle 90, 64 transverse slices, slice thickness 2 mm, fat

Please cite this article as: Hartzell, J.F., et al., Brains of verbal memory specialists show anatomical differences in language, memory and visual
systems, NeuroImage (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027

J.F. Hartzell et al. / NeuroImage xxx (2015) xxxxxx

suppression, matrix 218 126, 60 diffusion encoding directions


(bvecs), b-value = 700 mm2/s, 10 b0 volumes (saved as a single averaged volume), parallel imaging with acceleration factor 2 (sense),
total acquisition time 10:59.6. Diffusion data was evaluated immediately upon acquisition to control for motion effects or other artifacts, and
re-acquired if necessary (4 scans were reacquired). The b-value of 700
was chosen to be within the range of values considered optimal for
human brain matter DTI analysis while favoring high SNR to facilitate
the detection and correction of artifacts in the diffusion weighted
images (Alexander and Barker, 2005; Ben-Amitay et al., 2012). Mean
FA of the corpus callosum body (~ 0.52) matched values reported in
the literature (Jovicich et al., 2014; see their Fig. 6).
Voxel-based morphometry (VBM)
Structural images were analyzed using the FSL's voxel-based
morphometry (VBM) analysis pipeline (Ashburner and Friston,
2000; Good et al., 2001) with FSL-VBM tools (Douaud et al., 2007).
Data consisted of the 21 aligned and averaged structural images acquired from Pandits and 21 from the control group. Brains were extracted using FSL's brain extraction tool (BET; Smith, 2002), with
manual edits to control for extraction errors, and processed using
FSL's VBM default pipeline. Note that in the FSL VBM pipeline, the
single-participant data prior to alignment to common space reects
a voxel's probability of being gray matter (calculated by a combination of Hidden Markov Random Field and Expectation Maximization
framework; see Zhang et al., 2001), and the nal data, in common
space, reect an adjustment of that value by the Jacobian of the deformation applied to the participant's data when aligning to common space. Thus, this VBM implementation most closely reects
local volume differences. We spatially smoothed the nal images
by an isotropic Gaussian kernel (FWHM = 9.42 mm). Group-level
statistical inference was achieved via nonparametric permutation
using the FSL tool randomise. Family-wise error was controlled for
at an alpha level of P b .05 by Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement
(TFCE; Smith and Nichols, 2009), in which cluster extent is
constrained by cluster-like local spatial support. Age and whole
brain Volume were included as covariates. References to anatomically dened regions within MNI space were established by intersecting
the group's MNI gray matter template mask with FSL's pre-dened
atlases. (See SI Methods for additional information.) To evaluate
the impact of smoothing kernel, we also implemented Gaussian kernels of 2.35 mm, 4.71 mm, and 7.06 mm (sigma of 1, 2 and 3, respectively) and repeated the main analysis.
Cortical thickness analysis
Cortical thickness (CT) analysis was implemented in FreeSurfer
(Dale et al., 1999), using the default processing pipeline, except for
manually bypassing FreeSurfer's automatic skull stripping routines
and using instead the skull-stripped brains created in the initial step
of the VBM analysis described above. FreeSurfer's GM segmentation
was veried manually for each participant, and no manual corrections
were needed (for example participant's segmentation, see Inline Supplementary Figure S1). The CT estimates derived for each participant
were imported into AFNI's surface-based analysis module, SUMA
(Saad et al., 2004) for further analyses. CT values were spatially
smoothed with a conservative (Pardoe et al., 2013) 10 mm smoothing
kernel on the two dimension cortical surface using an iterative Heat
Kernel method (Chung, 2004). The resulting CT values on the cortical
surface were interpolated to a surface mesh that maintained the same
number of vertices for all participants, in similar locations (using
SUMA's MapIcosahedron procedure). The resulting meshes contained
156,252 vertices per hemisphere. Statistical analysis of CT values on
the group level was performed using cluster-based thresholding that

was determined via a permutation procedure (following Nichols and


Holmes, 2002; see SI Methods for details).
Inline Supplementary Fig. S1 can be found online at http://dx.doi.
org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027.
Local gyrication index analysis
To examine potential gyrication differences between the two populations we used a method based on calculating an outer surface (tangential to the folding points of the gyri), and then parcellating it into
numerous circular patches covering the entire 2D cortical surface
(Schaer et al., 2008, 2012). For each patch, the local gyrication index
(LGI) computes the ratio of cortex within sulcal folds to the amount of
visible cortex (tangent to the patch). Higher values indicate that a greater proportion of the pial matter under the patch is in sulci. The 2D surface maps generated by this method have, by denition, a strong
degree of spatial smoothness (FWHM of ~30 mm) that is determined
by the number of surface patches used. (Each patch has a radius of
20 mm, and the computed LGI value for each patch is propagated to
all surface vertices overlapping with it, necessarily yielding less localized results than those seen for VBM or CT analyses.) Between-group
statistical tests of LGI patterns were based on permutation tests as for
the CT analysis. Permutation tests maintain the spatial autocorrelation
of each participant's data and permit sensitivity to non-stationary
changes in LGI across cortical regions.
Diffusion tensor imaging: fractional anisotropy
The 60 diffusion-encoding direction b-vectors were corrected individually for head motion using FSL's rot_bvecs, followed by eddy current
and subject motion correction with afne registration to the averaged b0
image. Fractional Anisotropy (FA) images were created using FSL's Diffusion Toolbox (FDT) after brain-extraction using BET and manual edits to
remove artifacts, then processed using FSL's Tract Based Spatial Statistics
(TBSS; Smith et al., 2006) default settings. FSL's TBSS rst erodes each
participant's FA image. For registration to common space, we used an option that selects the best target image from among the subjects, performs
a nonlinear alignment of all participants to that target, and then afne
registers the resulting aligned les to MNI152 1 mm common space.
Using the mean FA calculated from the participants' les in common
space, TBSS creates a skeletonized representation of FA-derived tracts
common to all subjects, by estimating the local surface perpendicular direction along the tracts and performing non-maximum-suppression
along the perpendicular to the voxel with the highest FA value, which
marks the center of the tract. The distance of each participant's FA
voxel to this common skeleton is then calculated, with the distance calculation constrained to the nearest voxels, and the participant's maximum FA value in the already-calculated perpendicular to each skeleton
voxel is projected into the skeleton. The aim of this method is to reduce
variance from residual misalignments of each subject's FA to common
space (Smith et al., 2006). Voxelwise cross-participant group-level statistics are then performed within a thresholded mean FA skeleton mask
(we used a threshold of 0.3). The threshold reduces the effects of high
inter-subject variability at the outer edges of the brain. We tested between group differences using 2D TFCE, controlling for family-wise
error at an alpha level of P b .05 based on cluster extent constraints,
with Age included as a covariate. References to anatomically dened
white-matter regions within MNI space were established by intersecting
the group's MNI template FA mask with FSL's predened WM atlases
(see SI Methods).
Hippocampal region-of-interest analysis
Hippocampus-optimized VBM
We also conducted a customized VBM analysis that was aimed directly at evaluating changes in the HF. This analysis consisted of the

Please cite this article as: Hartzell, J.F., et al., Brains of verbal memory specialists show anatomical differences in language, memory and visual
systems, NeuroImage (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027

J.F. Hartzell et al. / NeuroImage xxx (2015) xxxxxx

following steps. First, the initial automatic segmentations of the HF as


derived by FSL's FIRST subcortical alignment and segmentation procedures (Patenaude et al., 2011) were anonymized and then further manually evaluated and modied by one of the authors (J.H.). Segmentation
was performed in original space, using advanced FIRST options to optimize the segmentation by algorithm-determined vertex numbers
(modes) and internal reference to the thalamus for normalization. In
the second step, we performed a nonlinear registration of these edited
HF segmentations to MNI space (FSL's MNI152 T1 1 mm template)
using high-resolution (6 mm3) nonlinear warping (FNIRT) initialized
with the afne matrix generated for each participant by FSL FIRST's subcortical alignment routine. After registration we multiplied the hippocampi by their Jacobians to modulate the GM, as in the standard VBM
pipeline. Note that in contrast to the whole-brain analysis (which
works with GM probabilities), the values multiplied by the Jacobian
were the original T1 intensity values within the manually veried hippocampal segmentations. Steps 1 and 2 therefore provided a more precise inter-participant alignment of the HF specically. Third, to evaluate
the impact of various smoothing kernels (2.35 mm, 4.71 mm, 7.06 mm
and 9.42 mm, sigma 1, 2, 3 and 4 respectively), we smoothed only within
these MNI-registered right and left HF. Steps 1, 2 and 3 ensured that our
between group tests focused only on the HF, thus obviating the chance
of impacting the results from nearby regions. Then we performed voxelwise tests inside the right and left hippocampal intersection masks (i.e.
the hippocampal masks used in the randomize routine included only
voxels to which all 42 subjects contributed values). We used TFCE testing, and included Age and whole-brain Volume as covariates at all four
smoothing kernels. To evaluate the impact of using manually annotated
hippocampi, we compared the results to those obtained when applying
the same registration and analysis pipeline but using as inputs the FIRST
automatic hippocampi segmentations produced in Step 1 above, as well
as automatic hippocampi segmentations obtained from FreeSurfer for
these participants.
Hippocampal local-volume analysis
We conducted an additional analysis to identify whether there were
areas of the hippocampus whose local volume differed between groups.
The method was based on FSL FIRST's vertex analysis (Patenaude et al.,
2011), but modied to allow incorporation of manual edits on the hippocampal structure (following suggestions by Jenkinson, 2014). This
analysis was not based on comparison of mesh-based segmentations
of the hippocampus but rather on comparisons of the outer envelope
of participants' hippocampi in common space. First, using the manually

annotated hippocampal segmentations from FIRST, we constructed a


common core hippocampal shape from the entire group of participants
in common space (Pandits and controls). To this end, the individual hippocampal shapes from original space were projected to common space
(MNI152 T1 1 mm) using a rigid body alignment to maintain size and
shape differences. From the group average of these MNI-registered hippocampal shapes we then constructed a thresholded (0.9) group average boundary mask (this mask marks the outer edge of the common
HF volume, in 3D space). For each voxel in this group-level boundary
mask we then calculated its distance to the nearest boundary voxel of
each participant's binarized hippocampal mask, whether inside or outside of the common boundary mask. This returned, for each grouplevel boundary-mask voxel, a vector reecting the positive or negative
distance to each participant's boundary voxel. Group-level tests were
conducted on this signed distance data. The result of this procedure,
when applied to all participants, was a group-level statistical map showing those parts of the group-hippocampal boundary shape where
(local) distances to the shape differed between the two groups. Note
that as opposed to VBM this procedure implemented a strictly local
shape analysis that (similarly to FSL's new vertex analysis) identies
geometric changes, is independent of any tissue-classication step,
and does not involve any smoothing of the data.
Results
Evaluation of covariates
The VBM, CT, LGI and FA analyses included whole brain analyses for
the Pandit group examining correlations of two covariates. These included Starting age of recitation training, and Overall Practice Hours
since Completion of Training (OPHCT). OPHCT was included since, although all Pandits completed the common training, there was considerable variance in their subsequent practice routines, and it has been
shown that even short-term cognitive and motor practice impacts
neuroplasticity (e.g., Draganski et al., 2006; Driemeyer et al., 2008).
None of the pair-wise correlations between Age, Start Age, and OPHCT
approached signicance (Correlation tests: Start Age and Age: R =
0.23, P = .39; OPHCT and Age: R = 0.06, P = .7; Start Age and
OPHCT: R = 0.22, P = .32). Because age and whole brain volume are
also known to correlate with changes in GM, we included Age and Volume as additional covariates in all analyses, including the betweengroup tests, with the exception of the CT, LGI, and FA analyses, where
we used only Age as a covariate.

Fig. 1. Surface projection of areas where Pandits showed greater gray matter density/volume (GM) than controls as indicated by a whole brain Voxel Based Morphometry analysis. Analysis
here and all other analysis corrected for family-wise error at P b .05, using FSL's TFCE cluster-extent correction (see SI Tables 2 and 3 for cluster details).

Please cite this article as: Hartzell, J.F., et al., Brains of verbal memory specialists show anatomical differences in language, memory and visual
systems, NeuroImage (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027

J.F. Hartzell et al. / NeuroImage xxx (2015) xxxxxx

Voxel-based morphometry: whole brain analysis


The whole-brain VBM analysis revealed extensive GM differences in
cortical, cerebellar and subcortical regions. In cortical regions Pandits
demonstrated greater GM than controls in large portions of both left
and right hemispheres (10.4% left and 12.5% right of total GM template
cortical volume). To facilitate presentation, differences found in cortical
regions were projected to an inated cortical surface representation of a
brain in MNI space (Fig. 1; see SI Tables 2 and 3 for complete cluster descriptives and local maxima). Differences were found bilaterally in both
auditory and visual-stream regions, including lateral temporal cortices,
ventral occipital cortices, angular gyri, pre- and post-central gyri, posterior cingulate, lingual gyri and precuneus. Greater Pandit GM was also
found in large bilateral areas of the anterior cingulate (ACC) and ventromedial prefrontal cortices (vmPFC). We repeated the VBM analysis with
spatial smoothing kernels of 2.35 mm, 4.71 mm, and 7.06 mm. The
resulting statistical maps were almost identical, apart from an additional single cluster in the base of left STG, MTG that was only found for the
4.71 mm smoothing kernel (see SI Table 6 for additional smoothing kernel cluster specics).
Within right lateral temporal cortex a large GM cluster was found
that reached along the superior temporal sulcus (STS) into the STP,
encompassing both the lateral transverse temporal gyrus and association cortices and extending deep into the ventral anterior temporal region. Pandits' GM was also larger in the right posteromedial insula and
central operculum, the anterior and posterior parahippocampal gyrus

and the right perirhinal cortex (PRC). As shown in Fig. 1 (and see SI
Tables 2 and 3), in the left hemisphere GM differences in the lateral temporal cortex were found in posterior STG, MTG, and ITG, while in the
STP, GM differences were found in the planum temporale (PT), extending into the angular gyrus and supramarginal gyrus in the parietal lobe.
In the cerebellum Pandits showed greater GM than controls in multiple bilateral structures (Fig. 2A), encompassing 34% of the total GM in
the cerebellar template. The cerebellar subregions included both left
and right Crus I, Crus II, V, VI, VIIb, VIIIa, VIIIb, IX and X, as well as several
midline Vermis regions. Greater GM for Pandits was most dominant in
Crus 1 and Crus II, VIIb and VIIIa (the cerebellar cluster regions and relative volume in each cerebellar sub-region for which GM was higher for
Pandits is reported in SI Tables 3 and 4). In subcortical regions, we found
a more heterogeneous result pattern, with Pandits showing greater GM
than controls in a small cluster of the posteromedial right hippocampus
(Fig. 2B), whereas they showed less GM than controls in a large cluster
(62% of subcortical template GM) encompassing the more anterior
portions of the hippocampus bilaterally and bilateral regions of the
amygdala, caudate, nucleus accumbens, putamen and, thalamus (see
Figs. 2C and D, and SI Table 3).
To directly compare our hippocampus results with prior literature
that documented hippocampus-related volume changes in expert spatial navigators (London taxi drivers; Maguire et al., 2000), we isolated
the Pandit N control cluster within the right hippocampus, and also
established its left hemisphere mirror image. In each region we then calculated the mean GM change for Pandits and controls. Following

Fig. 2. Whole-brain VBM results in subcortical and cerebellar regions, overlaid onto the group GM template (left hemisphere on left). (A) Cerebellar subregions where Pandits showed
greater GM than controls include, bilaterally, Crus I, Crus II. V, VI, VIIb, VIIIa, VIIIb, IX and X, as well as several midline Vermis regions (see SI Tables 3 and 4 for details). (B) Pandits showed
greater GM than controls in the right medial posterior hippocampus; blue indicates anatomically-dened hippocampus mask. (C) Bilateral subcortical regions where Pandits showed less
GM than controls: amygdala, caudate, anterior hippocampi, nucleus accumbens, putamen and thalamus. (D) Mean GM values for Pandits and controls for subcortical structures. All results
P b .05, FWE-corrected.

Please cite this article as: Hartzell, J.F., et al., Brains of verbal memory specialists show anatomical differences in language, memory and visual
systems, NeuroImage (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027

J.F. Hartzell et al. / NeuroImage xxx (2015) xxxxxx

Maguire et al (2000), mean GM was also calculated for the anterior aspects of the hippocampus that fell within the large cluster where Pandits showed lower GM than controls. Fig. 3 bears out the greater
density for controls in the anterior hippocampus, which is markedly absent, and even reversed, in the right mid-posterior hippocampus. (Note
that diverging from our analysis, Maguire et al. did not include Age as
covariate in the between-group tests, and doing the same in the current
study revealed even stronger similarities to their ndings; see SI Discussion and SI Fig. 2 for visual comparisons). We then evaluated whether
these conclusions about the hippocampus would hold up if the whole
brain VBM analysis was repeated at different smoothing kernels. The
anterior hippocampal results (Pandits b controls) survived tests at additional FWHM Gaussian smoothing kernels of 2.35 mm, 4.71 mm, and
7.06 mm (sigma of 1, 2 and 3, respectively), while the right posterior
hippocampus result (Pandits N controls) survived at the additional
Gaussian kernel of 7.06 mm (sigma 3). We also conducted a whole
brain analysis within the Pandit group to test whether GM density correlated with Start Age or with total post-training hours of Pandit recitation practice (OPHTC), both with Age and total brain Volume as
covariates. We found no signicant correlations.

better study hippocampal differences between the groups. Both analyses considered the hippocampus as a region of interest, and examined
VBM and local-volume changes in a more circumscribed manner. The
implementation details of these analyses are described in the Methods.
In brief, in both analyses we used accurate hippocampal segmentations
in original space, obtained from FSL's automatic subcortical segmentation (FIRST; Patenaude et al., 2011), which were then further manually
annotated. For the VBM analysis we implemented a high-resolution
alignment to common space, optimized for subcortical structures. We
used the Jacobians of the deformation to common space in order to
modulate intensity values within each person's hippocampus. For the
local-volume analysis we implemented a procedure similar to FSL
FIRST's vertex-based subcortical shape analysis. This analysis was
based on 3 main steps: i) aligning participants' hippocampi to common
space, ii) producing a consensus shape of hippocampal areas where
participants overlapped, and iii) quantifying, for each point on the consensus shape's boundary, its distance to the nearest boundary of each
person's hippocampus. (This analysis is identical to FSL FIRST's vertexwise local distance calculations, but uses boundaries in voxel space rather than derived 2D meshes). Using this procedure we could determine,
for each point on the consensus shape boundary, whether the two
groups differed in local volume. In contrast to VBM, this analysis is immune to any spatial smoothing effects, and reects strictly local volume
differences.
The hippocampal-optimized VBM procedure indicated a large portion of the posterior-middle right HF where Pandits had greater GM
than controls (see Fig. 4, and see Supplementary Table 7 for cluster specics). The volume of this region formed between 73 and 98% of the hippocampal mask (depending on smoothing kernel; FWHM 2.35 mm =
73%, FWHM 4.71 mm = 80%, FWHM 7.06 mm = 92%, FWHM
9.42 mm = 98%; note that smoothing was implemented only within
the hippocampal mask, thus obviating effects of nearby regions). At
larger smoothing kernels (7.06 mm and 9.42 mm), we also found a cluster in the left posterior hippocampus where Pandits had greater GM
than controls.
The hippocampal shape analysis revealed a portion of the right midanterior hippocampus with greater volume for the control group (see
Inline Supplementary Figure S2). We then tested, within the Pandit
group, whether hippocampal GM density or shape correlated with Pandit Start Age or with total post-training hours of recitation practice
(OPHTC), both adjusted for Age and total brain Volume as covariates.
We found no signicant correlations.
Inline Supplementary Fig. S2 can be found online at http://dx.doi.
org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027.

Hippocampus-focused analyses

Cortical thickness analysis

Given that the hippocampal data in the whole-brain analysis may reect the impact of imperfect alignment or smoothing of data from outside the hippocampus, we implemented two additional analyses to

Several brain regions differed in CT between the Pandit and control


group, and in all cases the Pandit group was associated with greater
CT. Differences were found in right STS, right anterior temporal pole,

Fig. 3. Gray matter differences in hippocampi as indicated by a Whole Brain Voxel Based
Morphometry analysis. Pandits showed less gray matter than controls in bilateral anterior
hippocampal formation (left) and more gray matter than controls in a right middle-posterior
hippocampal formation cluster (right). * = P b .05, *** = P b .001.

Fig. 4. Hippocampal region-of-interest analysis: areas within the right and left hippocampi where Pandits showed greater GM than controls. In addition to the whole brain analysis, we
conducted a region-of-interest analysis using subcortex-optimized nonlinear alignment to MNI152 T1 1 mm common space. Greater GM density for Pandits was found in the mid-posterior
right hippocampus, independent of spatial smoothing kernel. Greater GM density for Pandits was found in the posterior left hippocampus, but only when using 7.06 and 9.42 mm smoothing
kernels. Statistical maps for both right and left hippocampus shown at 7.06 mm smoothing kernel, overlaid on MNI152-T1 1 mm template.

Please cite this article as: Hartzell, J.F., et al., Brains of verbal memory specialists show anatomical differences in language, memory and visual
systems, NeuroImage (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027

J.F. Hartzell et al. / NeuroImage xxx (2015) xxxxxx

Fig. 6. Areas where Pandits showed less gyrication than controls.

Start Age or Practice (OPHTC) variables adjusted for Age as covariate.


There were no signicant correlations.
Differences in fractional anisotropy
Two adjacent clusters showed greater FA in Pandits compared to
controls (see Fig. 7). No area showed the reverse pattern. The clusters
were found in close proximity to the CT and GM differences we report
for the left vmPFC/ACC (see Fig. 7), at the intersection of the left anterior
thalamic radiation, the forceps minor, the left inferior fronto-occipital
fasciculus (IFOF), the left anterior corona radiata (ACR), the genu of
the corpus callosum, the left cingulum bundle, and the left uncinate fasciculus (UF). (See SI Methods, SI Table 5 for cluster details, and SI Fig. 1
for a brain map showing the location of these clusters overlaid on mean
group FA map.) We also tested, within the Pandit group, whether either
FA or skeletonized FA correlated with Pandit Start Age or with total
post-training hours of recitation practice (OPHTC), both adjusted for
Age as covariate. We found no signicant correlations.
Discussion
Fig. 5. Areas where Pandits demonstrated greater cortical thickness than controls. (A) Single
vertex signicance value set at uncorrected threshold of P b .05, corrected for family-wise
error using cluster-extent thresholding. (B) Single vertex signicance value set at uncorrected threshold of P b .005, corrected for family-wise error using cluster-extent thresholding
(see Methods). LH = left hemisphere; RH = right hemisphere.

right occipito-temporal gyrus (OTG) and in the left rostral ACC extending into dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. Fig. 5 presents these regions as
identied by two analyses, using two single voxel thresholds to identify
both less localized clusters where all voxels passed the P b .05 threshold,
and more highly localized clusters where all voxels passed a threshold
of P b .005. We conducted a whole brain analysis to test, within the
Pandit group, whether CT correlated with Start Age or total posttraining hours of recitation practice (OPHTC), adjusted for Age as covariate. We found no signicant correlations.

Overall, we found considerable differences in the organization of the


brains of professional Vedic Sanskrit Pandits. Specically, they showed
extensive cortical and cerebellar GM increase and subcortical GM decrease. The hippocampal GM differences followed a differential anterior/posterior pattern that has been linked to expert spatial navigation
(Maguire et al., 2000), and to improved memory for verbal materials
(Poppenk and Moscovitch, 2011). Cortical CT increases were extensive,
and overlapped closely with GM differences in right temporal regions,
left medial prefrontal, and left fusiform areas. Pandits also showed signicantly less gyrication in bilateral occipital regions, and signicantly
larger FA in left inferior frontal WM clusters. Our ndings are consistent
with the possibility that the changes to medial-temporal and medial
prefrontal regions, accompanied by changes to lateral temporal regions
and cerebellum, reect the impact of the Pandits' extensive verbal
practices.

Differences in local gyrication

Hippocampus and ACC/mPFC

Two areas showed differences in local gyrication between the two


groups. These were found in the inferior and middle occipital gyri on the
left and middle occipital gyrus on the right. In both cases these cortical
regions showed reduced gyrication for the Pandit group (see Fig. 6).
We also examined the relationship between the LGI and CT ndings.
Using the regions identied by the LGI analysis as masks, we quantied
the mean CT within those regions per participant, and then evaluated
these on the group level. There was absolutely no between-group difference in mean CT within those regions. In the right hemisphere LGI cluster, the mean CT for Pandits and controls was 2.65 mm (SD = 0.18) vs.
2.66 mm (SD = 0.21). In the left hemisphere cluster, the values were
2.08 mm (SD = 0.13) vs. 2.11 mm (SD = 0.13). In short, CT values
were almost identical across groups in areas showing LGI differences.
We also tested, within the Pandit group, for correlation of LGI with the

The Pandits' pattern of hippocampal differences as evident in a


whole-brain VBM analysis were similar to those reported in the study
of London taxi drivers (Maguire et al., 2000), showing a relative decrease in bilateral anterior hippocampi, and an increase in right (but
not left) medial-posterior hippocampus. Our region-of-interest analyses
identied a local reduction in volume in the right anterior HF for
Pandits, accompanied by a VBM signature of increased GM in the
medial-posterior right HF for this group, and an increased GM cluster
in the posterior left hippocampus. Maguire et al (2000, p. 4398), who
used whole brain VBM and HF pixel counting, suggested that the increases in the posterior hippocampus may indicate that this region
stores a spatial representation for the environment and expands to accommodate this elaborated representation. A large body of subsequent
research has shown, however, that the anterior and posterior

Please cite this article as: Hartzell, J.F., et al., Brains of verbal memory specialists show anatomical differences in language, memory and visual
systems, NeuroImage (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027

J.F. Hartzell et al. / NeuroImage xxx (2015) xxxxxx

Fig. 7. Axial slices showing clusters (in green) where Pandits showed greater fractional anisotropy than controls, The statistical fractional anisotropy map (green) is overlaid on an MNI
template, and shown alongside areas of the mPFC/ACC where Pandits showed greater GM than controls in the whole-brain analysis (red-orange), P b 0.05, FWE-corrected using Threshold
Free Cluster Enhancement. Left hemisphere shown on left.

hippocampi play differential roles in a large range of cognitive processes


including, but not limited to novelty processing (Daselaar et al., 2006;
Kohler et al., 2005; Takashima et al., 2006), encoding of ongoing and recent experiences (Hartzell et al., 2014), and simulation of future events
(van Mulukom et al., 2013; see Fanselow and Dong, 2010; Poppenk
et al., 2013, and Strange et al., 2014 for reviews). Better memory for verbal materials has been associated with larger posterior and smaller anterior hippocampal segments (Poppenk and Moscovitch, 2011). One
study found that the volume of the anterior hippocampus correlates
positively with verbal memory (Hackert et al., 2002), but this was
found for an age group (6090 y.o.a.) for which the relation may reect
variations in the normal thinning patterns that the HF undergoes with
increasing age. Our results, taken together with these prior studies, support the developing evidence that hippocampal regional changes may
occur in various situations, beyond those necessitating memory for
complex spatial scenes. We note that the training of London Taxi Drivers
does in fact involve rote memorization of a large volume of preset verbal
sequences: they are required to memorize street names and place
names (30,000 landmarks) in 320 set route sequences totaling
~ 120,000 words, with part-time training over ~ 35 years
(Transport.for.London, 2014). Their oral examinations necessitate precise rote verbal recall of route details between the landmarks.
Greater Pandit GM/CT in anterior cingulate cortex and medial temporal structures is also consistent with accommodating increased memory demands. Animal studies show long-term memory encoding in the
mPFC/ACC (Weible et al., 2012; Teixeira et al., 2006), with short-term
encoding in the hippocampus (Takehara-Nishiuchi and McNaughton,
2008), mediated by connections between perirhinal/parahippocampus
and ACC (Insel and Takehara-Nishiuchi, 2013). In humans, patients
with exclusive MTL lesions perform normally on remote autobiographical memory but poorly on recent memory tests (Bayley et al., 2005),
while mPFC/ACC lesions conversely disrupt long-term memory, but
not short-term memory for recent experiential learning (Squire and
Bayley, 2007). Neuroimaging data from healthy human participants
also suggest that recall for recent vs. remote experiences differentially
relies, respectively, on hippocampal vs. medial frontal cortices
(Takashima et al., 2006). Taken together with these animal and
human studies, our ndings suggest that Vedic Sanskrit oral text information may be initially encoded via the hippocampus, then stored in
the mPFC/ACC regions, but a detailed longitudinal study is necessary
to examine this issue.
Lateral-temporal and parietal cortices: potential indicators of language
system differences
Our left and right temporal region cortical differences showed different topographies. The left postero-medial superior, middle, and inferior
temporal gyri GM patterns were largely conned to gyral surfaces,
reaching into the antero-medial PT. Many of these regions overlap

with presurgical speech interference sites (Roux et al., 2012), suggesting


the observed differences may be related, at least in part, to recitation vocalization. These left posterior lateral temporal regions are also implicated in both lexical-phonological processing and semantic-syntactic
integration in current cortical speech processing models (see
e.g., Hickok and Poeppel, 2007; Friederici, 2012), while the PT/pSTG/
SMG changes reach into areas linked to speech production (DeWitt
and Rauschecker, 2012; Fedorenko and Thompson-Schill, 2014). On
the right, greater GM/CT for Pandits reached into deep STS, and into lateral Heschl's gyrus/planum polare (HG/PP), dorsal posteromedial
insula, OP2/OP3 of posteromedial operculum, and right ventral anterior
lobe (vATL). Right HG/PP have been shown to sample acoustic information at a rate optimized for syllable-length acoustics (Kotz and
Schwartze, 2010; Morillon et al., 2012; Altmann et al., 2007) and
sound patterns (Altmann et al., 2007), with right STS linked to processing of human voices (Belin, 2006) and vocal identity (Petkov et al.,
2009). The human vATL/anterior fusiform bilaterally is considered a
hub for multi-modal/amodal semantic knowledge (Chan et al., 2011),
linked with PRC for verbal memory construction (Bozeat et al., 2000).
Greater Pandit GM in right posteromedial insula and operculum may reect speech-sound processing (Cloutman et al., 2012), vocalization
tuning (Remedios et al., 2009), and/or prosody detection (van Rijn
et al., 2005).
The increased GM for Pandits in parietal regions suggests the possible involvement of cortical resources subserving Vedic recitation gestures, articulation, and multilingualism. Differences in the left superior
and medial postcentral gyrus covered portions of the primary somatosensory cortex (Ruben et al., 2001) for the right arm, wrist, hand and
ngers, face, mouth and tongue regions (Kaas et al., 1979; Nakamura
et al., 1998), including areas known to be active during right hand and
arm movement (Sereno and Huang, 2014). We also considered that
while the Pandits and controls were matched for number of languages,
the Pandits are highly competent in Sanskrit due to their training, and
several of the areas where they demonstrate greater GM have been
linked to multilingual abilities. The differences we documented in inferior parietal and superior lateral temporal cortices match well with
greater GM found for bilinguals compared to monolinguals (Mechelli
et al., 2004), and increased vocabulary is associated with increased
GM in left posterior SMG (Richardson et al., 2010).
Notably absent were morphological differences in grey matter or
cortical thickness in bilateral inferior frontal regions that have been
linked to higher-level language functions. The left inferior frontal region
has been linked repeatedly to semantic and syntactic processing
(e.g., Bookheimer, 2002) or control processes during language
(e.g., Fedorenko et al., 2012; Fedorenko and Thompson-Schill, 2014),
whereas the right has been linked to discourse related functions
(e.g., Menenti et al., 2009). We also found no WM changes in these regions of the sort previously associated with better grammar learning
(Flel et al., 2009). The absence of differences in inferior frontal cortices

Please cite this article as: Hartzell, J.F., et al., Brains of verbal memory specialists show anatomical differences in language, memory and visual
systems, NeuroImage (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027

J.F. Hartzell et al. / NeuroImage xxx (2015) xxxxxx

could reect the fact that the Pandits' memorization, recall and production of oral language content does not require putting ideas into words
de novo, and so does not engage this particular use of these frontal regions that have been implicated in higher level language processing
through studies typically not involving recited speech. Follow-up functional studies will be useful for clarifying the functional contribution of
these temporal-parietal structural differences to the Pandits' verbal recitation practices.

Cerebellum
Pandit GM cerebellar differences were found in regions involved in
cortico-cerebellar networks subserving language and memory (Marien
et al., 2014), and executive function (Stoodley, 2012), and in which
GM increases have been correlated with factors relevant to Vedic recitation: e.g. skilled hand movements with Vermis VI/VIIb (Di Paola et al.,
2013) and bilingual semantic and phonemic uency in left Crus II
(Grogan et al., 2009). The large volume of Sanskrit memorized and recited by the Pandits, and their mastery of Sanskrit's complex morphology
(Whitney, 1924) and semantics (Apte, 1890) may also contribute to the
large increase in Pandit cerebellar GM (1/3rd of total cerebellar GM), a
nding considerably larger than previously reported in cerebellar morphology analyses.

Visual system
Increased GM and CT in Pandits' visual/visual-association cortices
may relate to their traditional multi-year training regimen that consists
of close face-to-face oral instruction and repetition (including one-onone training) and synchronized recitation gestures. Alternatively, or additionally, it may reect the type of cross-modal plasticity and enhanced
function previously documented in the visually impaired, such as ultrafast speech comprehension and exceptional spatial acoustic cue detection in blind (Dietrich et al., 2013; Voss et al., 2004). One possibility,
which necessitates further functional neuroimaging investigations, is
that occipital regions are recruited to aid the extensive oral languagerelated computations performed by Pandits; these regions have been
shown to have the potential for rapid functional plasticity even in
healthy subjects (Merabet et al., 2008).

Subcortical and gyrication differences


To our knowledge, our study is the rst to document comprehensive
reduction of GM in subcortical structures in a population of healthy participants. While unexpected, one potential explanation of this nding is
that it indicates a speeded maturation of these regions for Pandits. A developmental study of healthy children and adolescents (Wierenga et al.,
2014) showed a linear age-related reduction of GM in caudate, putamen
and nucleus accumbens (regions where Pandits had lower GM than
controls), and inverted U-shaped curves in amygdala, thalamus, hippocampus and pallidum (the latter a region where we did not nd clear
differences between the two groups).
To our knowledge, the current work is also the rst to document
local gyrication differences between two healthy adult groups. Cortical
gyrication complexity increases up through young adulthood with the
occipital lobe showing both highest variability in preadolescents, and
lowest complexity increase in adolescence (Blanton et al., 2001; Su
et al., 2013). After adolescence, gyrication decreases steadily across
much of the brain (Hogstrom et al., 2013). The Pandits in our study
began training in late childhood or early adolescence, so their decreased
occipital gyrication may indicate a training-related impact on the normal developmental curve of brain gyrication, specically, a relatively
more limited gyrication change attained in visual cortices.

WM structural differences
The WM tracts crossing through the Pandit FA clusters have been
implicated in language processing. Increased FA in left forceps minor,
genus of the corpus callosum, anterior thalamic radiation (ATR), and anterior corona radiata has been linked to mathematical ability (NavasSanchez et al., 2014), while stutterers have decreased FA in the forceps
minor (Beal et al., 2013; Civier et al., 2015). Lesion studies have implicated left inferior frontal-occipital fasciculus (IFOF), left ATR, and left
uncinate fasciculus (UF) in semantic processing (Han et al., 2013) and
uency (Almairac et al., in press), while in healthy participants left
IFOF and UF are both prominently involved in amodal (domain general)
semantic memory (de Zubicaray et al., 2011). As shown in Fig. 7, the FA
clusters border the CT/GM Pandit increases in the mPFC/ACC, suggesting
they may also be related to those structural differences.
Convergence and divergence between morphometric measures
The different measures we used provided convergent information
regarding changes in several brain regions, but several also identied
unique change patterns. The VBM results highlighted extensive differences in bilateral temporal regions, vmPFC and lateral occipital regions,
and the CT ndings documented similar changes in vmPFC, the right lateral temporal regions and right occipito-temporal regions, though less
extensively than VBM. However, the right temporal pole areas identied by the CT analysis were not identied by VBM, and conversely, occipital and posterior midline regions identied by VBM were not
identied by CT. With respect to FA ndings, there was a good overlap
between the diffusion results and the mPFC/ACC cluster identied in
both the CT and VBM analysis. Finally, within the clusters showing LGI
changes, we did not nd any changes in CT.
While it is interesting to nd convergence in some aspects of the results, it is important to note that prior work suggests that VBM, CT and
LGI target at least partially different organizational aspects of structural
morphometry. We rst address the relation between VBM and CT.
Whereas CT, as implemented in FreeSurfer, loads strictly on the local
cortical thickness, FSL's VBM analysis, which includes modulation by
the Jacobian to account for stretching and compression, reects (based
on GM probability metrics from the GM segmentation step) a combination of thickness, surface area and differences in folding. For this reason
VBM has sometimes been interpreted as measuring overall local volume (Hutton et al., 2009). Prior studies that have used both VBM and
CT to study a single dataset show their divergent, rather than strictly
convergent nature. Blankstein et al. (2009), Voets et al. (2008), and
Bermudez et al (2009) are good examples of such work. Voets et al.,
who compared VBM and surface-based morphometry (SBM), concluded that, VBM-style approaches are sensitive to a combination of cortical
thickness, surface area and shape measures. SBM, on the other hand,
uses an explicit model of the neocortex, offering independent measures
of thickness, surface area and folding patterns. Thus, areas of signicant
difference in VBM GM density may be found without a corresponding
change in SBM-derived cortical thickness (Voets et al., p. 667).
Formal attempts at relating VBM and CT have been only moderately
successful. Voets et al. (2008) tried examining the Jacobian of the warp
eld, or dividing CT by change in metric distortion on the vertex wise
level, but these did not account well for the divergence between VBM
and CT. Palaniyappan and Liddle (2012) used a region of interest analysis
and found that between-group differences in VBM data were only moderately mediated by different surface morphometry features such as CT,
LGI and surface area: a large proportion of VBM-related variance (between 36% and 80%) was not accounted for by these surface measures.
Furthermore, depending on brain region, different surface features
accounted for the between-group VBM differences. VBM and surface
measures therefore appear to target partially different aspects of brain
morphometry; this may have to do with the fact that these measures
are related to separate genetic traits (e.g., Winkler et al., 2010).

Please cite this article as: Hartzell, J.F., et al., Brains of verbal memory specialists show anatomical differences in language, memory and visual
systems, NeuroImage (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027

10

J.F. Hartzell et al. / NeuroImage xxx (2015) xxxxxx

With respect to LGI and CT, while one might expect that the two
measures would generally be negatively correlated, this relationship appears modest, and also varies spatially. As part of their study, Hogstrom
et al. (2013) examined the relationship between LGI, and CT. While
there was a negative relation between LGI and CT in all lobes, it was relatively weak (0.17 b R b 0.08), with signicant correlations limited
to medial prefrontal cortex, superior frontal gyrus, and precuneus. In all,
prior work highlights the utility of using multiple measures for understanding changes to different facets of brain morphometry, and our current ndings are largely consistent with the import of that body of work.

Potential limitations and future directions


As in any cross-sectional study, one cannot claim with absolute certainty that structural differences are caused by experience-related
changes, rather than reecting a genetic predisposition. However, several unique features of the Pandit selection and training very strongly
argue against explanations grounded in self-selection or genetic predisposition: there are no pre-entrance selection exams to Pandit studies so
that memorization ability is not tested as a pre-condition; the attrition
rate from studies is only ~5%, arguing against self-selection during training itself; and none of our specic participants came from Vedic Pandit
families, with very few having any relatives who recite (See SI Table 1).
All these are highly consistent with an experience-related explanation
rather than one based on genetic predisposition (of the sort licensed
for musicians, athletes, piano tuners and other special populations).
A second apparent interpretive challenge is the absence of statistically signicant correlations between Pandits' practice estimate or
starting age and GM/CT/LGI/FA measures. We consider power, limited
range, and possible ceiling effects as the reasons for this null result.
First, given the sample size (N = 21), to satisfy a single-voxel criteria
of P b .005, correlations would need to exceed a level of 0.56 (Pearson's
R) in each voxel within a cluster, which is a high standard that even if
found would likely be an inaccurate documentation of the actual effect
size in the population (Yarkoni, 2009). Second, while all the Pandits
had completed the basic training course, 12 of those were within
1 year of graduation, and 5 others within 3 years of graduation, resulting
in a limited range of the post-training Practice variable (OHPTC; see SI
Table 1). Third, given the reported total hours of basic training of
10,080 h (See SI Methods), it is also possible that the lack of correlation
is due to a ceiling or plateau effect, wherein training-driven plasticity asymptotes, as is seen in motor and cognitive skill acquisition studies
(Macnamara et al., 2014, see references therein; Karni et al., 1998;
Anderson, 1981). Further elucidation of the issue will require followup longitudinal studies during the training period, and/or recruitment
of a larger subject pool of qualied Pandits with a wider ranger of
post-training practice.
We note that a recent (Kalamangalam and Ellmore, 2014) smaller
scale study (Pandit N = 11) examined cortical thickness differences between Pandits and a control group and reported different results for this
measure (the study reports 2 clusters limited to inferior temporal and
orbito-frontal cortex, regions not typically associated with speech, language or memory processing). That study could not examine hippocampal or subcortical differences due to its focus on the cortical fold, and
surprisingly, did not document differences in lateral temporal regions
implicated in speech processing (STG, STS, STP), or regions implicated
in memory for verbal materials, concluding that those regions are not
impacted by memory training (VBM analysis was not conducted). The
markedly divergent results in our work are probably the result of a
more powerful sample and control for confounding variables.2 For
2
The study by Kalamangalam and Ellmore was conducted in Houston, Texas, with local
control participants, and does not report control for eye-dominance or multilingualism in
the experimental and control groups, nor Vedic lineage and assessment of Vedic competence, and does not report control for Age in the analysis pathway.

these reasons, we cannot directly compare that particular prior work


with the current ndings.
Summary
The data demonstrate that there exist extensive morphological differences in the brains of professional Vedic Sanskrit Pandits, which are
in some cases identiable by both VBM and CT measures, and in some
cases only by one of these two metrics. These ndings are consistent
with a role for medial temporal regions and medial prefrontal cortex
in large-scale language, memory and information processing. These
data further suggest that inferior frontal and lateral temporal regions
play different roles in their ability to subserve rehearsed speech. Finally,
the results raise interesting questions about the potential of intensive,
specialized expertise training to substantially drive plasticity in healthy
adult brains, and possibly alter natural developmental curves.
Acknowledgments
We thank Prof. R.K Shastri of the Ministry of Human Resource Development, Government of India, for information regarding the current
state of Vedic training at government-supported institutions in India.
We also thank Krishna Miyapuram, India Institute of Technology, Gandhinagar, for helpful discussions and assistance with translation of the
survey forms. This research has received funding from the India-Trento
Program for Advanced Research. U.H was supported by a European
Council Starting Grant (ERC-STG #263318).
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at http://dx.
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027.
References
Alexander, D.C., Barker, G.J., 2005. Optimal imaging parameters for ber-orientation estimation in diffusion MRI. Neuroimage 27, 357367. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2005.04.008.
Almairac, F., Herbet, G., Moritz-Gasser, S., de Champeur, N.M., Duffau, H., 2014. The left
inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus subserves language semantics: a multilevel lesion
study. Brain Struct. Funct. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-014-0773-1 (in press).
Altmann, C.F., Bledowski, C., Wibral, M., Kaiser, J., 2007. Processing of location and pattern
changes of natural sounds in the human auditory cortex. NeuroImage 35, 11921200.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.007.
Anderson, J.R. (Ed.), 1981. Cognitive Skills and Their AcquisitionCarnegie Mellon Symposia on Cognition Series. Taylor & Francis, New York.
Apte, V., 1890. The Practical Sanskrit-English Dictionary, Containing Appendices on Sanskrit Prosody and Important Literary and Geographic Names of Ancient India. Revised
and Enlarged edition. Motilal Barnasidass, Delhi.
Ashburner, J., Friston, K.J., 2000. Voxel-based morphometrythe methods. NeuroImage
11, 805821.
Ashtari, M., Avants, B., Cyckowski, L., Cervellione, K.L., Roofeh, D., Cook, P., Gee, J., Sevy, S.,
Kumra, S., 2011. Medial temporal structures and memory functions in adolescents
with heavy cannabis use. J. Psychiatr. Res. 45, 10551066. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1016/j.jpsychires.2011.01.004.
Bayley, P.J., Gold, J.J., Hopkins, R.O., Squire, L.R., 2005. The neuroanatomy of remote memory. Neuron 46, 799810. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.034.
Beal, D.S., Gracco, V.L., Brettschneider, J., Kroll, R.M., De Nil, L.F., 2013. A voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis of regional grey and white matter volume abnormalities
within the speech production network of children who stutter. Cortex 49,
21512161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.013.
Belin, P., 2006. Voice processing in human and non-human primates. Philos. Trans. R. Soc.
Lond. B Biol. Sci. 361, 20912107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.1933.
Ben-Amitay, S., Jones, D.K., Assaf, Y., 2012. Motion correction and registration of high bvalue diffusion weighted images. Magn. Reson. Med. 67, 16941702. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1002/mrm.23186.
Bermudez, P., Lerch, J.P., Evans, A.C., Zatorre, R.J., 2009. Neuroanatomical correlates of musicianship as revealed by cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry. Cereb.
Cortex 19 (7), 15831596. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn196.
Bird, C.M., Burgess, N., 2008. The hippocampus and memory: insights from spatial processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 182194. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2335.
Blankstein, U., Chen, J.Y., Mincic, A.M., McGrath, P.A., Davis, K.D., 2009. The complex minds
of teenagers: neuroanatomy of personality differs between sexes. Neuropsychologia
47 (2), 599603. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.014.
Blanton, R.E., Levitt, J.G., Thompson, P.M., Narr, K.L., Capetillo-Cunliffe, L., Nobel, A.,
Singerman, J.D., McCracken, J.T., Toga, A.W., 2001. Mapping cortical asymmetry and

Please cite this article as: Hartzell, J.F., et al., Brains of verbal memory specialists show anatomical differences in language, memory and visual
systems, NeuroImage (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027

J.F. Hartzell et al. / NeuroImage xxx (2015) xxxxxx


complexity patterns in normal children. Psychiatry Res. 107, 2943. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/S0925-4927(01)00091-9.
Bookheimer, S., 2002. Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the
cortical organization of semantic processing. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 25, 151188.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142946.
Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M.A., Patterson, K., Garrard, P., Hodges, J.R., 2000. Non-verbal semantic impairment in semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia 38, 12071215. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00034-8.
Catani, M., Mesulam, M.M., Jakobsen, E., Malik, F., Martersteck, A., Wieneke, C., Rogalski, E.,
2013. A novel frontal pathway underlies verbal uency in primary progressive
aphasia. Brain 136 (Pt 8), 26192628. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt163.
Central Council of f Indian Medicine, 2014. http://ccimindia.org (accessed 1 October 2014).
Chan, A.M., Baker, J.M., Eskandar, E., Schomer, D., Ulbert, I., Marinkovic, K., Cash, S.S.,
Halgren, E., 2011. First-pass selectivity for semantic categories in human
anteroventral temporal lobe. J. Neurosci. 31, 1811918129. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1523/JNEUROSCI.3122-11.2011.
Chung, M., 2004. Heat kernel smoothing and its application to cortical manifolds. Technical Report. Department of Statistics, U. W. Madison.
Civier, O., Kronfeld-Duenias, V., Amir, O., Ezrati-Vinacour, R., Ben-Shachar, M., 2015. Reduced fractional anisotropy in the anterior corpus callosum is associated with reduced speech uency in persistent developmental stuttering. Brain Lang. 143,
2031. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2015.01.012.
Cloutman, L.L., Binney, R.J., Drakesmith, M., Parker, G.J., Lambon Ralph, M.A., 2012. The
variation of function across the human insula mirrors its patterns of structural connectivity: evidence from in vivo probabilistic tractography. NeuroImage 59,
35143521. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.016.
Cox, R.W., 1996. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162173. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/
cbmr.1996.0014.
Dale, A.M., Fischl, B., Sereno, M.I., 1999. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation
and surface reconstruction. NeuroImage 9, 179194. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/
nimg.1998.0395.
Daselaar, S.M., Fleck, M.S., Cabeza, R., 2006. Triple dissociation in the medial temporal
lobes: recollection, familiarity, and novelty. J. Neurophysiol. 96, 19021911. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.01029.2005.
de Zubicaray, G.I., Rose, S.E., McMahon, K.L., 2011. The structure and connectivity of semantic memory in the healthy older adult brain. NeuroImage 54, 14881494.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.058.
Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dehaene, S., Anton, J.L., Campagne, A., Ciuciu, P., Dehaene, G.P.,
Denghien, I., Jobert, A., LeBihan, D., Sigman, M., Pallier, C., Poline, J.B., 2006. Functional
segregation of cortical language areas by sentence repetition. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27,
360371. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20250.
Devauchelle, A.D., Oppenheim, C., Rizzi, L., Dehaene, S., Pallier, C., 2009. Sentence syntax
and content in the human temporal lobe: an fMRI adaptation study in auditory and
visual modalities. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 10001012. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.
2009.21070.
DeWitt, I., Rauschecker, J.P., 2012. Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral
stream. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 109, E505E514. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.
1113427109.
Di Paola, M., Caltagirone, C., Petrosini, L., 2013. Prolonged rock climbing activity induces
structural changes in cerebellum and parietal lobe. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34,
27072714. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22095.
Dietrich, S., Hertrich, I., Ackermann, H., 2013. Ultra-fast speech comprehension in blind
subjects engages primary visual cortex, fusiform gyrus, and pulvinara functional
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study. BMC Neurosci. 14, 74. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1186/1471-2202-14-74.
Douaud, G., Smith, S., Jenkinson, M., Behrens, T., Johansen-Berg, H., Vickers, J., James, S.,
Voets, N., Watkins, K., Matthews, P.M., James, A., 2007. Anatomically related grey
and white matter abnormalities in adolescent-onset schizophrenia. Brain 130,
23752386. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm184.
Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Kempermann, G., Kuhn, H.G., Winkler, J., Buchel, C., May, A., 2006.
Temporal and spatial dynamics of brain structure changes during extensive learning.
J. Neurosci. 26 (23), 63146317. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4628-05.2006.
Driemeyer, J., Boyke, J., Gaser, C., Buchel, C., May, A., 2008. Changes in gray matter induced
by learningrevisited. PLoS One 3 (7), e2669. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.
0002669.
Eichenbaum, H., Cohen, N.J., 2014. Can we reconcile the declarative memory and spatial
navigation views on hippocampal function? Neuron 83, 764770. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.032.
Eichenbaum, H., Yonelinas, A.P., Ranganath, C., 2007. The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 30, 123152.
Fanselow, M.S., Dong, H.W., 2010. Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron 65, 719. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.031.
Fedorenko, E., Thompson-Schill, S.L., 2014. Reworking the language network. Trends
Cogn. Sci. 18, 120126. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.006.
Fedorenko, E., Nieto-Castanon, A., Kanwisher, N., 2012. Syntactic processing in the human
brain: what we know, what we don't know, and a suggestion for how to proceed.
Brain Lang. 120, 187207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2011.01.001.
Fernandez, G., Weyerts, H., Schrader-Bolsche, M., Tendolkar, I., Smid, H.G.O.M.,
Tempelmann, C., Hinrichs, H., Scheich, H., Elger, C.E., Mangun, G.R., Heinze, H.J.,
1998. Successful verbal encoding into episodic memory engages the posterior hippocampus: a parametrically analyzed functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
J. Neurosci. 18, 18411847.
Flel, A., de Vries, M.H., Scholz, J., Breitenstein, C., Johansen-Berg, H., 2009. White matter
integrity in the vicinity of Broca's area predicts grammar learning success.
NeuroImage 47, 19741981. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.046.

11

Friederici, A.D., 2012. The cortical language circuit: from auditory perception to sentence
comprehension. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 262268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.
04.001.
Giraud, A.L., Poeppel, D., 2012. Cortical oscillations and speech processing: emerging computational principles and operations. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 511517. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1038/nn.3063.
Good, C.D., Johnsrude, I.S., Ashburner, J., Henson, R.N., Friston, K.J., Frackowiak, R.S., 2001.
A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains.
NeuroImage 14, 2136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0786.
Grogan, A., Green, D.W., Ali, N., Crinion, J.T., Price, C.J., 2009. Structural correlates of semantic and phonemic uency ability in rst and second languages. Cereb. Cortex
19, 26902698. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp023.
Grunwald, T., Beck, H., Lehnertz, K., Blumcke, I., Pezer, N., Kurthen, M., Fernandez, G., Van
Roost, D., Heinze, H.J., Kutas, M., Elger, C.E., 1999. Evidence relating human verbal
memory to hippocampal N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.
96, 1208512089. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.96.21.12085.
Hackert, V.H., den Heijer, T., Oudkerk, M., Koudstaal, P.J., Hofman, A., Breteler, M.M., 2002.
Hippocampal head size associated with verbal memory performance in
nondemented elderly. NeuroImage 17 (3), 13651372.
Han, Z., Ma, Y., Gong, G., He, Y., Caramazza, A., Bi, Y., 2013. White matter structural connectivity underlying semantic processing: evidence from brain damaged patients.
Brain 136, 29522965. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt205.
Hartzell, J.F., Zysk, K.G., 1995. Columbia University Dharam Hinduja Indie Research Center
Conference: Health, Science, and the Spirit: Veda and Ayurveda in the Western
World. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 1, 297301. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/acm.
1995.1.297.
Hartzell, J.F., Tobia, M.J., Davis, B., Cashdollar, N.M., Hasson, U., 2014. Differential lateralization of hippocampal connectivity reects features of recent context and ongoing
demands: an examination of immediate post-task activity. Hum. Brain Mapp.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22644.
Hasson, U., Nusbaum, H.C., Small, S.L., 2006. Repetition suppression for spoken sentences
and the effect of task demands. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 20132029. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1162/jocn.2006.18.12.2013.
Hasson, U., Nusbaum, H.C., Small, S.L., 2007. Brain networks subserving the extraction of
sentence information and its encoding to memory. Cereb. Cortex 17, 28992913.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm016.
Hickok, G., Poeppel, D., 2007. The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8, 393402. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2113.
Hogstrom, L.J., Westlye, L.T., Walhovd, K.B., Fjell, A.M., 2013. The structure of the cerebral
cortex across adult life: age-related patterns of surface area, thickness, and
gyrication. Cereb. Cortex 23, 25212530. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs231.
Hutton, C., Draganski, B., Ashburner, J., Weiskopf, N., 2009. A comparison between voxelbased cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry in normal aging. NeuroImage
48 (2), 371380. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.043.
Insel, N., Takehara-Nishiuchi, K., 2013. The cortical structure of consolidated memory: a
hypothesis on the role of the cingulate-entorhinal cortical connection. Neurobiol.
Learn. Mem. 106, 343350. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2013.07.019.
Jenkinson, M., 2014. https://www.jiscmail.ac.uk/cgi-bin/webadmin?A2=ind1411&L=
FSL&P=R16234&1=FSL&9=A&J=on&d=No+Match%3BMatch%3BMatches&z=4.
Jenkinson, M., Smith, S., 2001. A global optimisation method for robust afne registration
of brain images. Med. Image Anal. 5, 143156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S13618415(01)00036-6.
Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., Smith, S., 2002. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images.
NeuroImage 17, 825841. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1132.
Jovicich, J., Marizzoni, M., Bosch, B., Bartres-Faz, D., Arnold, J., Benninghoff, J., Wiltfang, J.,
Roccatagliata, L., Picco, A., Nobili, F., Blin, O., Bombois, S., Lopes, R., Bordet, R.,
Chanoine, V., Ranjeva, J.P., Didic, M., Gros-Dagnac, H., Payoux, P., Zoccatelli, G.,
Alessandrini, F., Beltramello, A., Bargallo, N., Ferretti, A., Caulo, M., Aiello, M.,
Ragucci, M., Soricelli, A., Salvadori, N., Tarducci, R., Floridi, P., Tsolaki, M.,
Constantinidis, M., Drevelegas, A., Rossini, P.M., Marra, C., Otto, J., ReissZimmermann, M., Hoffmann, K.T., Galluzzi, S., Frisoni, G.B., 2014. Multisite longitudinal reliability of tract-based spatial statistics in diffusion tensor imaging of healthy elderly subjects. NeuroImage 101, 390403. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.
2014.06.075.
Kaas, J.H., Nelson, R.J., Sur, M., Lin, C.S., Merzenich, M.M., 1979. Multiple representations of
the body within the primary somatosensory cortex of primates. Science 204,
521523. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.107591.
Kalamangalam, G.P., Ellmore, T.M., 2014. Focal cortical thickness correlates of exceptional
memory training in Vedic priests. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 833. http://dx.doi.org/10.
3389/fnhum.2014.00833.
Karni, A., Meyer, G., Rey-Hipolito, C., Jezzard, P., Adams, M.M., Turner, R., Ungerleider, L.G.,
1998. The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven
changes in primary motor cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95, 861868. http://dx.
doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.3.861.
Knecht, S., Drager, B., Deppe, M., Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Floel, A., Ringelstein, E.B.,
Henningsen, H., 2000. Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy
humans. Brain 123 (Pt 12), 25122518. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/123.12.2512
2512-2518.
Kohler, S., Danckert, S., Gati, J.S., Menon, R.S., 2005. Novelty responses to relational and
non-relational information in the hippocampus and the parahippocampal region: a
comparison based on event-related fMRI. Hippocampus 15, 763774. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1002/hipo.20098.
Kotz, S.A., Schwartze, M., 2010. Cortical speech processing unplugged: a timely
subcortico-cortical framework. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 392399. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1016/j.tics.2010.06.005.

Please cite this article as: Hartzell, J.F., et al., Brains of verbal memory specialists show anatomical differences in language, memory and visual
systems, NeuroImage (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027

12

J.F. Hartzell et al. / NeuroImage xxx (2015) xxxxxx

Kronfeld-Duenias, V., Amir, O., Ezrati-Vinacour, R., Civier, O., Ben-Shachar, M., 2014. The
frontal aslant tract underlies speech uency in persistent developmental stuttering.
Brain Struct. Funct. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-014-0912-8.
Li, P., Sepanski, S., Zhao, X., 2006. Language history questionnaire: a web-based interface
for bilingual research. Behav. Res. Methods 38, 202210.
Macnamara, B.N., Hambrick, D.Z., Oswald, F.L., 2014. Deliberate practice and performance
in music, games, sports, education, and professions: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Sci. 25,
16081618. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797614535810.
Maguire, E.A., Gadian, D.G., Johnsrude, I.S., Good, C.D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R.S., Frith,
C.D., 2000. Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 97, 43984403. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.070039597.
Marien, P., Ackermann, H., Adamaszek, M., Barwood, C.H., Beaton, A., Desmond, J., De
Witte, E., Fawcett, A.J., Hertrich, I., Kuper, M., Leggio, M., Marvel, C., Molinari, M.,
Murdoch, B.E., Nicolson, R.I., Schmahmann, J.D., Stoodley, C.J., Thurling, M.,
Timmann, D., Wouters, E., Ziegler, W., 2014. Consensus paper: language and the cerebellum: an ongoing enigma. Cerebellum 13, 386410. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s12311-013-0540-5.
May, A., 2011. Experience-dependent structural plasticity in the adult human brain.
Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 475482. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.002.
Mechelli, A., Crinion, J.T., Noppeney, U., O'Doherty, J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R.S., Price,
C.J., 2004. Neurolinguistics: structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature 431,
757. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/431757a.
Menenti, L., Petersson, K.M., Scheeringa, R., Hagoort, P., 2009. When elephants y: differential sensitivity of right and left inferior frontal gyri to discourse and world knowledge. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 23582368. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.21163.
Merabet, L.B., Hamilton, R., Schlaug, G., Swisher, J.D., Kiriakopoulos, E.T., Pitskel, N.B.,
Kauffman, T., Pascual-Leone, A., 2008. Rapid and reversible recruitment of early visual
cortex for touch. PLoS One 3, e3046. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003046.
Milner, B., Peneld, W., 1955. The effect of hippocampal lesions on recent memory. Trans.
Am. Neurol. Assoc. 4248.
Mishra, K.K. (Ed.), 1997. Sanskrit Studies in India: On the Occasion of 10th World Sanskrit
Conference, Bangalore, Jan 39, 1997. Rashtriya Sanskrit Sansthan, New Delhi, India.
Morillon, B., Liegeois-Chauvel, C., Arnal, L.H., Benar, C.G., Giraud, A.L., 2012. Asymmetric
function of theta and gamma activity in syllable processing: an intra-cortical study.
Front. Psychol. 3, 248. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00248.
Nakamura, A., Yamada, T., Goto, A., Kato, T., Ito, K., Abe, Y., Kachi, T., Kakigi, R., 1998. Somatosensory homunculus as drawn by MEG. NeuroImage 7, 377386. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0332.
Navas-Sanchez, F.J., Aleman-Gomez, Y., Sanchez-Gonzalez, J., Guzman-De-Villoria, J.A.,
Franco, C., Robles, O., Arango, C., Desco, M., 2014. White matter microstructure correlates of mathematical giftedness and intelligence quotient. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35,
26192631. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22355.
Nichols, T.E., Holmes, A.P., 2002. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 125. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1002/hbm.1058.
Oldeld, R.C., 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory.
Neuropsychologia 9, 97113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4.
Palaniyappan, L., Liddle, P.F., 2012. Differential effects of surface area, gyrication and cortical thickness on voxel based morphometric decits in schizophrenia. NeuroImage
60 (1), 693699. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.058.
Pardoe, H.R., Abbott, D.F., Jackson, G.D., 2013. Sample size estimates for well-powered
cross-sectional cortical thickness studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 30003009. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22120.
Patenaude, B., Smith, S.M., Kennedy, D.N., Jenkinson, M., 2011. A Bayesian model of shape
and appearance for subcortical brain segmentation. NeuroImage 56, 907922. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.046.
Pathashala, Veda, 2014. Traditional Vedic Schools. http://sanskritdocuments.org
(accessed October 1, 2014).
Petkov, C.I., Logothetis, N.K., Obleser, J., 2009. Where are the human speech and voice regions, and do other animals have anything like them? Neuroscientist 15, 419429.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073858408326430.
Poeppel, D., 2003. The analysis of speech in different temporal integration windows: cerebral lateralization as asymmetric sampling in time. Speech Comm. 41, 245255.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00107-3.
Pohlack, S.T., Meyer, P., Cacciaglia, R., Liebscher, C., Ridder, S., Flor, H., 2014. Bigger is better! Hippocampal volume and declarative memory performance in healthy young
men. Brain Struct. Funct. 219, 255267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-0120497-z.
Poppenk, J., Moscovitch, M., 2011. A hippocampal marker of recollection memory ability
among healthy young adults: contributions of posterior and anterior segments. Neuron 72, 931937. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.014.
Poppenk, J., Evensmoen, H.R., Moscovitch, M., Nadel, L., 2013. Long-axis specialization of
the human hippocampus. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 230240. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.tics.2013.03.005.
Rashtriya Sanskrit Sansthan, 20102011. Annual Report. Rashtriya Sanskrit Sansthan
(Deemed University), Government of India, New Delhi.
Rashtriya Sanskrit Sansthan, 2014. http://www.sanskrit.nic.in (accessed October 1, 2014).
Remedios, R., Logothetis, N.K., Kayser, C., 2009. An auditory region in the primate insular
cortex responding preferentially to vocal communication sounds. J. Neurosci. 29,
10341045. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4089-08.2009.
Richardson, F.M., Thomas, M.S., Filippi, R., Harth, H., Price, C.J., 2010. Contrasting effects of
vocabulary knowledge on temporal and parietal brain structure across lifespan.
J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 943954. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21238.
Roux, F.E., Durand, J.B., Jucla, M., Rehault, E., Reddy, M., Demonet, J.F., 2012. Segregation of
lexical and sub-lexical reading processes in the left perisylvian cortex. PLoS One 7,
e50665. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050665.

Ruben, J., Schwiemann, J., Deuchert, M., Meyer, R., Krause, T., Curio, G., Villringer, K., Kurth,
R., Villringer, A., 2001. Somatotopic organization of human secondary somatosensory
cortex. Cereb. Cortex 11, 463473. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/11.5.463.
Saad, Z.S., Reynolds, R.C., Argall, B., Japee, S., Cox, R.W., 2004. SUMA: an interface for
surface-based intra- and inter-subject analysis with AFNI. 2004 2nd IEEE International Symposium on Biomedical Imaging: Macro to Nano, pp. 15101513.
Schaer, M., Cuadra, M.B., Tamarit, L., Lazeyras, F., Eliez, S., Thiran, J.P., 2008. A surfacebased approach to quantify local cortical gyrication. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 27,
161170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/TMI.2007.903576.
Schaer, M., Cuadra, M.B., Schmansky, N., Fischl, B., Thiran, J.P., Eliez, S., 2012. How to measure cortical folding from MR images: a step-by-step tutorial to compute local
gyrication index. J. Vis. Exp. e3417 http://dx.doi.org/10.3791/3417.
Scoville, W.B., Milner, B., 1957. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions.
J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 20, 1121.
Sereno, M.I., Huang, R.S., 2014. Multisensory maps in parietal cortex. Curr. Opin.
Neurobiol. 24, 3946. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.08.014.
Shastri, R.K., 2014, personal communication, October 15.
Smith, S.M., 2002. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17,
143155.
Smith, S.M., Nichols, T.E., 2009. Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference.
NeuroImage 44, 8398. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061.
Smith, S.M., Jenkinson, M., Johansen-Berg, H., Rueckert, D., Nichols, T.E., Mackay, C.E.,
Watkins, K.E., Ciccarelli, O., Cader, M.Z., Matthews, P.M., Behrens, T.E., 2006. Tractbased spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data.
NeuroImage 31, 14871505. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024.
Squire, L.R., Bayley, P.J., 2007. The neuroscience of remote memory. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.
17, 185196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2007.02.006.
Stoodley, C.J., 2012. The cerebellum and cognition: evidence from functional imaging
studies. Cerebellum 11, 352365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12311-011-0260-7.
Strange, B.A., Witter, M.P., Lein, E.S., Moser, E.I., 2014. Functional organization of the hippocampal longitudinal axis. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 655669. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1038/nrn3785.
Su, S., White, T., Schmidt, M., Kao, C.Y., Sapiro, G., 2013. Geometric computation of human
gyrication indexes from magnetic resonance images. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34,
12301244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21510.
Takashima, A., Petersson, K.M., Rutters, F., Tendolkar, I., Jensen, O., Zwarts, M.J.,
McNaughton, B.L., Fernandez, G., 2006. Declarative memory consolidation in humans:
a prospective functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S.
A. 103, 756761. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0507774103.
Takehara-Nishiuchi, K., McNaughton, B.L., 2008. Spontaneous changes of neocortical code
for associative memory during consolidation. Science 322, 960963. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1126/science.1161299.
Teixeira, C.M., Pomedli, S.R., Maei, H.R., Kee, N., Frankland, P.W., 2006. Involvement of the
anterior cingulate cortex in the expression of remote spatial memory. J. Neurosci. 26,
75557564. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1068-06.2006.
Transport.for.London, 2014. www.t.gov.uk (accessed May 6, 2014).
van Mulukom, V., Schacter, D.L., Corballis, M.C., Addis, D.R., 2013. Re-imagining the future:
repetition decreases hippocampal involvement in future simulation. PLoS One 8.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069596.
van Rijn, S., Aleman, A., van Diessen, E., Berckmoes, C., Vingerhoets, G., Kahn, R.S., 2005.
What is said or how it is said makes a difference: role of the right fronto-parietal
operculum in emotional prosody as revealed by repetitive TMS. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21,
31953200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04130.x.
Voets, N.L., Hough, M.G., Douaud, G., Matthews, P.M., James, A., Winmill, L., Webster, P.,
Smith, S., 2008. Evidence for abnormalities of cortical development in adolescentonset schizophrenia. NeuroImage 43, 665675. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2008.08.013.
Voss, P., Lassonde, M., Gougoux, F., Fortin, M., Guillemot, J.P., Lepore, F., 2004. Early- and
late-onset blind individuals show supra-normal auditory abilities in far-space. Curr.
Biol. 14, 17341738. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.c ub. 20 04. 09.0 5 1.
Weible, A.P., Rowland, D.C., Monaghan, C.K., Wolfgang, N.T., Kentros, C.G., 2012. Neural
correlates of long-term object memory in the mouse anterior cingulate cortex.
J. Neurosci. 32, 55985608. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5265-11.2012.
Whitney, D., 1924. Sanskrit Grammar, Including Both the Classical Language and the
Older Dialects of Veda and Brahmana. 5th ed. Breitkopf & Hartel, Leipzig.
Wierenga, L., Langen, M., Ambrosino, S., van Dijk, S., Oranje, B., Durston, S., 2014. Typical
development of basal ganglia, hippocampus, amygdala and cerebellum from age 7 to
24. NeuroImage 96C, 6772. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.072.
Winkler, A.M., Kochunov, P., Blangero, J., Almasy, L., Zilles, K., Fox, P.T., Duggirala, R.,
Glahn, D.C., 2010. Cortical thickness or grey matter volume? The importance of
selecting the phenotype for imaging genetics studies. NeuroImage 53, 11351146.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.028.
Yarkoni, T., 2009. Big correlations in little studies: inated fMRI correlations reect low
statistical power-commentary on Vul et al. (2009). Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 4,
294298. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01127.x.
Zatorre, R.J., Bouffard, M., Belin, P., 2004. Sensitivity to auditory object features in human
temporal neocortex. J. Neurosci. 24, 36373642. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.3045.
Zhang, Y., Brady, M., Smith, S., 2001. Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden
Markov random eld model and the expectationmaximization algorithm. IEEE
Trans. Med. Imaging 20, 4557.
Zhuang, J., Tyler, L.K., Randall, B., Stamatakis, E.A., Marslen-Wilson, W.D., 2014. Optimally
efcient neural systems for processing spoken language. Cereb. Cortex 24, 908918.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs366.

Please cite this article as: Hartzell, J.F., et al., Brains of verbal memory specialists show anatomical differences in language, memory and visual
systems, NeuroImage (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.027

You might also like