A Short Introduction To Epidemiology Second Edition
A Short Introduction To Epidemiology Second Edition
A Short Introduction To Epidemiology Second Edition
to Epidemiology
Second Edition
Neil Pearce
2nd edition
February 2005
ISBN 0-476-01236-8
ISSN 1176-1237
To Irihapeti Ramsden
Preface
Neil Pearce
Acknowledgements
Contents
1.
Introduction
8.
Incidence studies
Incidence studies
Incidence case-control studies
3.
Prevalence studies
21
22
28
9.
Precision
Basic statistics
Study size and power
6.
Validity
Confounding
Selection bias
Information bias
7.
Effect modification
Concepts of interaction
Additive and multiplicative
models
Joint effects
109
33
41
42
47
Selection of cases
Selection of controls
Measuring exposure
Cohort studies
Measuring exposure
Follow-up
Health status
95
95
102
Prevalence studies
33
Prevalence case-control studies 38
4.
Measurement of exposure
and health status
Exposure
59
60
61
109
112
113
117
117
118
119
122
125
125
126
128
67
67
73
74
83
83
Basic principles
Basic analyses
Controlling for confounding
13. Interpretation
88
89
133
133
136
140
145
Appraisal of a single study
145
Appraisal of all of the available
evidence
148
CHAPTER 1. Introduction
(In: Pearce N. A Short Introduction to Epidemiology. 2nd ed. Wellington, CPHR, 2005)
Table 1.1
The defining features of public health: populations and prevention
Populations
Individuals
Prevention
Treatment
---------------------------------------------------------------------Public health
Health systems research
Primary health care/
Health education
Table 1.2
Deaths and death rates from cholera in London 1854 in households supplied by the
Southwark and Vauxhall Water Company and by the Lambeth Water Company
Deaths
Cholera
per 10,000
Houses
deaths
houses
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Southwark and Vauxhall
40,046
1,263
315
Lambeth Company
26,107
98
37
Rest of London
256,423
1,422
59
10
11
12
13
Appropriate Technology
A related issue is the need to use
appropriate technology to address
the most important public health
research questions. In particular, as
attention moves upstream to the
population level (McKinlay, 1993) new
methods will need to be developed
(McMichael, 1995). One example of
this, noted above, is the recent rise in
interest in multilevel modelling
(Blakely and Woodward, 2000; Pearce,
2000), although it is important to
stress that it is an increase in
multilevel thinking in the
development of epidemiologic
hypotheses and the design of studies
that is required, rather than just the
use of new statistical techniques of
data analysis. The appropriateness of
any research methodology depends on
the phenomenon under study: its
magnitude, the setting, the current
state of theory and knowledge, the
availability of valid measurement tools,
and the proposed uses of the
information to be gathered, as well as
the community resources and skills
available and the prevailing norms and
values at the national, regional or local
level (Pearce and McKinlay, 1998).
Thus, there has been increased
interest in the interface between
epidemiology and social science
(Krieger, 2000), and in the
Problem-Based Epidemiology
A second issue is that a problem-based
approach may be particularly valuable
in encouraging epidemiologists to
focus on the major public health
problems and to take the population
context into account (Pearce, 2001;
Thacker and Buffington, 2001). A
problem-based approach to teaching
clinical medicine has been increasingly
adopted in medical schools around the
world. The value of this approach is
that theories and methods are taught
in the context of solving real-life
problems. Starting with the problem
at the population level provides a
reality check on existing etiological
14
Summary
15
References
16
17
18
Part I
19
20
Incidence studies
Incidence case-control studies
Prevalence studies
Prevalence case-control studies
Table 2.1
The four basic study types in studies involving a dichotomous health outcome
Study
outcome
Incidence
Sampling on outcome
-----------------------------------------------------------No
Yes
-----------------------------------------------------------Incidence studies
Incidence case-control studies
21
22
Figure 2.1
Occurrence of disease in a hypothetical population followed from birth
Birth
End of Follow up
at risk
other death
disease symptoms
lost to follow up
severe symptoms
23
Example 2.1
Martinez et al (1995)
studied 1246 newborns
in the Tucson, Arizona
area enrolled between
May 1980 and October
1984. Parents were
contacted shortly after
the children were born,
and completed a
questionnaire about their
history or respiratory
illness, smoking habits,
and education. Further
parental questionnaires
24
25
Table 2.2
Findings from a hypothetical cohort study of 20,000 persons followed for 10 years
Exposed
Non-exposed
Ratio
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Cases
1,813 (a)
952 (b)
Non-cases
8,187 (c)
9,048 (d)
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------10,000 (N0)
Initial population size
10,000 (N1)
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------95,163 (Y0)
Person-years
90,635 (Y1)
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------0.0100 (I0)
2.00
Incidence rate
0.0200 (I1)
0.0952 (R0)
1.90
Incidence proportion
0.1813 (R1)
(average risk)
0.1052 (O0)
2.11
Incidence odds
0.2214 (O1)
26
27
Table 2.3
Findings from a hypothetical incidence case-control study based on the cohort in table 2.2
Exposed
Non-exposed Odds Ratio
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Cases
1,813 (a)
952 (b)
Controls: from survivors
(cumulative sampling)
1,313 (c)
1,452 (d)
2.11
from source population
(case-cohort sampling)
1,383 (c)
1,383 (d)
1.90
from person-years
(density sampling)
1,349 (c)
1,416 (d)
2.00
28
29
Example 2.2
Gustavsson et al (2001)
studied the risk of
myocardial infarction
from occupational
exposure to motor
exhaust, other
combustion products,
organic solvents, lead,
and dynamite. They
identified first-time,
nonfatal myocardial
infarctions among men
and women aged 45-70
years in Stockholm
County from 1992-1994.
They selected controls
from the general
population living in the
same County during the
same period (i.e. density
matching), matched for
sex, age, year, and
hospital catchment area.
The odds ratio
(estimating the rate
ratio) of myocardial
Summary
30
References
31
32
33
Example 3.1
The International Study
of Asthma and Allergies
in Childhood (ISAAC)
(Asher et al, 1995;
Pearce et al, 1993)
involved a simple Phase
I global asthma
symptom prevalence
survey and a more indepth Phase II survey.
The emphasis was on
obtaining the maximum
possible participation
across the world in order
to obtain a global
overview of childhood
asthma prevalence, and
the Phase I
questionnaire modules
were designed to be
simple and to require
minimal resources to
administer. In addition,
a video questionnaire
involving the audiovisual presentation of
clinical signs and
symptoms of asthma
was developed in order
to minimise translation
problems. The
population of interest
was schoolchildren aged
6-7 years and 13-14
34
Figure 3.1
Twelve month period prevalence of asthma symptoms in 13-14 year old children in
Phase I of the International Study of Asthma and Allergies in Childhood (ISAAC)
Source: ISAAC Steering Committee (1998b)
20%
10 to <20%
5 to <10%
<5%
35
Figure 3.2
Relationship between prevalence and incidence in a steady state population
P/(1-P) = I x D
P=prevalence
I=incidence
D=duration
N=population
N(1-p) x I
Asthma
cases
[NP]
Non-asthmatic
[N(1-P)]
NP/D
36
POR = I1/I0
i.e. under the above assumptions, the
prevalence odds ratio directly estimates
the incidence rate ratio (Pearce, 2004).
However, it should be emphasised that
prevalence depends on both incidence
and average disease duration, and a
Table 3.1
Findings from a hypothetical prevalence study of 20,000 persons
Exposed
Non-exposed
Ratio
-------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Cases
909 (a)
476 (b)
Non-cases
9,091 (c)
9,524 (d)
-------------------------------------------------------------------------------------10,000 (N0)
Total population
10,000 (N1)
-------------------------------------------------------------------------------------0.0476 (P0)
1.91
Prevalence
0.0909 (P1)
0.0500 (O0)
2.00
Prevalence odds
0.1000 (O1)
37
Table 3.2
Findings from a hypothetical prevalence case-control study based on the population
represented in table 3.1
Exposed
Non-exposed
Ratio
-------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Cases
909 (a)
476 (b)
Controls
676 (c)
709 (d)
-------------------------------------------------------------------------------------0.67 (O0)
2.00
Prevalence odds
1.34 (O1)
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------
38
Example 3.2
Studies of congenital
malformations usually
involve estimating the
prevalence of
malformations at birth
(i.e. this is a prevalence
rather than an incidence
measure). Garcia et al
(1999) conducted a
(prevalence) case-control
study of occupational
Summary
39
References
40
41
Cross-Sectional Studies
In chapters 2 and 3, the health outcome
under study was a state (e.g. having or
not having hypertension). Studies could
involve observing the incidence of the
event of acquiring the disease state (e.g.
the incidence of being diagnosed with
hypertension), or the prevalence of the
disease state (e.g. the prevalence of
hypertension). More generally, the
health state under study may have
multiple categories (e.g. nonhypertensive, mild hypertension,
moderate hypertension, severe
hypertension) or may be represented by
a continuous measurement (e.g. blood
pressure). Since these measurements
are taken at a particular point in time,
such studies are often referred to as
cross-sectional studies. Prevalence
studies (see chapter 3) are a subgroup
42
Example 4.1
Nersesyan et al (2001)
studied chromosome
aberrations in
lymphocytes of persons
exposed to an
earthquake in Armenia.
They collected blood
samples from 41 victims
of the 1988 earthquake
and from 47 reference
blood donors. Those
exposed to the
earthquake had a higher
proportion of cells with
chromosome aberrations
(3.1% (SD 2.1)) than
the referents (1.7% (SD
1.3)). The differences
persisted when the data
were adjusted for age
and gender. The authors
suggested that the
43
Longitudinal Studies
Longitudinal studies (cohort studies)
involve repeated observation of study
participants over time (Pearce et al,
1998). Incidence studies (chapter 2) are
a subgroup of longitudinal study in which
the outcome measure is dichotomous.
More generally, longitudinal studies may
involve repeated assessment of
categorical or continuous outcome
measures over time (e.g. a series of
linked cross-sectional studies in the
same population). They thus can involve
incidence data, a series of prevalence
surveys, or a series of cross-sectional
continuous outcome measures.
General longitudinal studies
A simple longitudinal study may involve
comparing the disease outcome
measure, or more usually changes in the
measure over time, between exposed
and non-exposed groups. For example,
rather than comparing the incidence of
hypertension (as in an incidence study),
Example 4.2
The Tokelau Island
Migrant Study (Wessen
et al, 1992) examined
the effects of migration
on development of
Western diseases within
a population which
initially had a low
incidence of these
conditions. Round I
surveys were conducted
in the Tokelau Islands in
1968/1971, and these
44
Time series
One special type of longitudinal study is
that of time series comparisons in
which variations in exposure levels and
symptom levels are assessed over time
with each individual serving as their own
control. Thus, the comparison of
exposed and non-exposed involves
the same persons evaluated at different
times, rather than different groups of
persons being compared (often at the
same time) as in other longitudinal
studies. The advantage of the time series
approach is that it reduces or eliminates
confounding (see chapter 6) by factors
which vary among subjects but not over
time (e.g. genetic factors), or whose day
to day variation is unrelated to the main
exposure (Pope and Schwartz, 1996). On
the other hand, time series data often
require special statistical techniques
because any two factors that show a
time trend will be correlated (Diggle et
al, 1994). For example, even a threemonth study of lung function in children
will generally show an upward trend due
to growth, as well as learning effects
(Pope and Schwartz, 1996). A further
problem is that the change in a measure
over time may depend on the baseline
value, e.g. changes in lung function over
time may depend on the baseline level
(Schouten and Tager, 1996).
Time series can involve dichotomous
(binary) data, continuous data, or
counts of events (e.g. hospital
admissions) (Pope and Schwartz, 1996),
and the changes in these values may be
45
Example 4.3
Hoek et al (2001)
studied associations
between daily variations
in air pollution and
mortality in The
Netherlands during
1986-1994. The authors
found (table 4.1) that
heart disease deaths
were increased during
periods with high levels
of ozone, black smoke,
particulate matter 10
microns in diameter
(PM10), carbon monoxide
(CO), sulfur dioxide
Table 4.1
Relative risks* (and 95% CIs) of cardiovascular disease mortality associated with air
pollution concentrations in the Netherlands
Pollutant
Total CVD mortality
Heart failure mortality
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Ozone (1 day lag)
1.055 (1.032-1.079)
1.079 (1.009-1.154)
1.029 (1.013-1.046)
1.081 (1.031-1.134)
1.012 (0.984-1.041)
1.036 (0.960-1.118)
CO (7 day mean)
1.026 (0.993-1.060)
1.109 (1.012-1.216)
1.029 (1.012-1.046)
1.098 (1.043-1.156)
1.023 (1.009-1.036)
1.064 (1.024-1.106)
46
Ecologic Studies
In ecologic studies exposure
information may be collected on a
group rather than on individuals. In
the past, ecologic studies have been
regarded as an inexpensive but
unreliable method for studying
individual-level risk factors for disease.
For example, rather than go to the
time and expense to establish a cohort
study or case-control study of fat
intake and breast cancer, one could
simply use national dietary and cancer
incidence data and, with minimal time
and expense, show a strong
correlation internationally between fat
intake and breast cancer. In this
situation, an ecologic study does not
represent a fundamentally different
study design, but merely a particular
variant of the four basic study designs
described in chapter 2 in which
information on average levels of
exposure in populations is used as a
surrogate measure of exposure in
individuals.
47
Example 4.4
The International Study
of Asthma and Allergies
in Childhood (ISAAC)
(Asher et al, 1995;
Pearce et al, 1993) was
described in example
3.1. Figure 4.1 shows
the findings for current
wheeze (i.e. wheeze in
the previous 12 months)
and its association with
tuberculosis notification
rates (von Mutius et al,
2000). It shows a
negative association
between tuberculosis
rates and asthma
prevalence. This is not
compelling evidence in
itself (because of the
major shortcoming of
ecologic analyses that
48
Figure 4.1
Association of tuberculosis notification rates for the period 1980-1982 (in countries with
valid tuberculosis notification data) and the prevalence of asthma symptoms in 13-14
year old children in the International Study of Asthma and Allergies in Childhood (ISAAC)
Wet
heeze
last
Source: von Mutius
al (2000)
40
35
30
25
20
15
10
0
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
Example 4.5
Wilkinson (1992) has
analysed measures of
income inequality and
found them to be
positively associated
with national mortality
rates in a number of
Western countries. This
is a true ecologic
exposure since the level
of income inequality is a
characteristic of a
country, and not of an
individual. If this
evidence is correct, this
is clearly of crucial
importance since it
implies that
development in itself
may not automatically
be good for health, and
that the way in which
the Gross National
Product (GNP) is 'shared'
may be as important as
its absolute level. It
should be noted,
however, that this
evidence has been
disputed by other
49
Ecologic Fallacies
While stressing the potential value of
ecologic analyses, it is also important to
recognise their limitations. In particular,
ecologic studies are a very poor means
of assessing the effects of individual
exposures (e.g. diet or tobacco
smoking) since confounding (and effect
modification) can occur at the individual
level, the country (population) level, or
both (Morgenstern, 1998). For example,
almost any disease that is associated
with affluence and westernisation has in
the past been associated at the national
level with sales of television sets, and
nowadays is probably associated at the
national level with rates of internet
Example 4.6
Table 4.2 shows the data
for a hypothetical
ecological analysis. The
numbers of cases and
population numbers (and
hence disease rates), as
well as the percentage of
the population exposed,
are known for each
country. Thus, the
numbers of people
exposed and nonexposed within each
country are known, but
it is not known how
many cases were
exposed and how many
were not; thus it is not
possible to estimate the
rates in the exposed and
non-exposed groups
within each country. The
country-level data
indicate a negative
association between
exposure and disease at
50
Table 4.2
Hypothetical example of an ecologic analysis
Country 1
(35% exposed)
Cases
Rate
Country 2
(50% exposed)
Cases
Rate
Country 3
(65% exposed)
Cases
Rate
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Exposed
?/
7000
Non-exposed
?/
10000
?/
13000
?/
13000
?/
10000
?/
7000
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Total
33/
165
20000
30/
150
20000
27/
135
20000
Table 4.3
Hypothetical example of an ecologic analysis:
No confounding by country
Country 1
Country 2
Country 3
(35% exposed)
(50% exposed)
(65% exposed)
Cases
Rate
Cases
Rate
Cases
Rate
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Exposed
7/
100
7000
Non-exposed
10/
100
10000
26/
200
13000
13/
100
13000
20/
200
10000
14/
200
7000
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Total
33/
165
20000
30/
150
20000
27/
135
20000
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Ratio
0.5
0.5
51
0.5
Table 4.4
Hypothetical example of an ecologic analysis:
Confounding by country
Country 1
Country 2
Country 3
(35% exposed)
(50% exposed)
(65% exposed)
Cases
Rate
Cases
Rate
Cases
Rate
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Exposed
12/
171
15/
150
18/
139
7000
Non-exposed
10000
21/
162
13000
13000
15/
150
10000
9/
129
7000
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Total
33/
165
20000
30/
150
20000
27/
135
20000
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Ratio
1.1
1.0
1.1
Table 4.5
Hypothetical example of an ecologic analysis:
Effect modification by country
Country 1
Country 2
Country 3
(35% exposed)
(50% exposed)
(65% exposed)
Cases
Rate
Cases
Rate
Cases
Rate
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Exposed
20/
286
20/
200
20/
154
7000
Non-exposed
10000
13/
100
13000
13000
10/
100
10000
7/
100
7000
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Total
33/
165
20000
30/
150
20000
27/
135
20000
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Ratio
2.9
2.0
52
1.5
Multilevel Studies
If individual as well as population-level
data are available, then the problems
of cross-level confounding and effect
modification (illustrated in example
4.6) are avoided by using multilevel
modelling (Greenland, 2000, 2002).
This enables the simultaneous
consideration of individual level effects
(e.g. individual income) and
population-level effects (e.g. per capita
national income, or income inequality).
This approach therefore combines the
Example 4.7
Yen and Kaplan (1999)
conducted a multi-level
analysis of
neighbourhood social
environment and risk of
death in the Alameda
County Study,
comprising 6,928 noninstitutionalised adult
residents of the County
recruited in 1965.
Mortality risks were
significantly higher in
neighbourhoods with a
low social
environment, even
after account was
taken of individual
income level,
education, ethnicity,
perceived health
status, smoking status,
body mass index, and
alcohol consumption.
The authors concluded
that the findings
demonstrate the
importance of area
characteristics as a
health risk factor.
Summary
53
References
54
55
56
Part II
57
58
CHAPTER 5. Precision
(In: Pearce N. A Short Introduction to Epidemiology. 2nd ed. Wellington, CPHR, 2005)
59
Basic Concepts
Data can be summarized in various
forms, including frequency tables,
histograms, bar charts, cross-tabulations
and pie charts. However, it is usually
also useful to give a summary measure
of central tendency. The mean (or
average) is the most commonly used
measure of central tendency, because of
its convenient statistical properties. The
next step is data smoothing which
involves the combination of the data with
a statistical model. In the simplest case,
this involves assuming a particular
statistical distribution in order to obtain a
summary measure of variability of the
data. The most common measure of
variability is the standard deviation
(Armitage et al, 2002). The standard
deviation is especially useful when the
underlying data distribution is
approximately normal (i.e. symmetric
with a special type of bell-shape). If data
is not normally distributed, then it can
often be made approximately normally
distributed by an appropriate
transformation (e.g. a log
transformation), but these
transformations may distort the scientific
meaning of the findings, and make them
difficult to interpret.
Usually it is not possible to study the
entire population in which one is
interested (theoretically, this is almost
always infinite since we usually wish to
generalise our findings not only to the
population we are studying, but also to
other populations). It is therefore
necessary to consider a random sample
and to relate its characteristics to the
total population. If repeated samples are
taken from the same population, then
the mean will vary between samples.
Even if the underlying population is not
(p(1-p)/n)0.5
60
61
Z = N00.5|P1 P0|B0.5 ZB
K0.5
where:
Z
P0
= allocation ratio of referent to study group (i.e., the relative size of the
two groups)
= BC - A (P1-P0)2
http://www.cdc.gov/epiinfo/, and
Rothmans Episheet programme
(Rothman, 2002) can be downloaded
for free from
http://www.oup-usa.org/epi/rothman/
62
Example 5.1
Consider a proposed study
of 5,000 exposed persons
and 5,000 non-exposed
persons. Suppose that on
the basis of mortality
rates in a comparable
Then:
Z
N0 =
P1
0.010 (= 50/5000)
P0
0.005 (= 25/5000)
An alternative approach
is to carry out a
standard analysis of the
hypothesized results. If
we make the
assumptions given
above, then the relative
risk would be 2.0, with a
90% confidence interval
of 1.4-3.0. This
approach only has an
indirect relationship to
the power calculations.
For example, if the
63
N00.5(1-0) Z
s(A + 1)0.5
where:
1
= estimated standard
deviation of outcome measure
64
Summary
65
References
66
CHAPTER 6. Validity
(In: Pearce N. A Short Introduction to Epidemiology. 2nd ed. Wellington, CPHR, 2005)
6.1 Confounding
67
Example 6.1
Table 6.1 presents a
hypothetical example of
confounding by tobacco
smoking in a prevalence
case-control study. Onehalf of the study
participants are
"exposed to the risk
factor of interest and
one-half are not.
However, two-thirds of
the exposed people are
smokers compared with
one-third of the non-
68
Table 6.1
Hypothetical example of confounding by tobacco smoking in a prevalence
case-control study
Smokers
NonExposed exposed
Cases
800
400
Non-cases
1,200
600
Total
2,000
1,000
Prevalence (%)
40
40
Prevalence odds ratio
1.0
Control of Confounding
Non-smokers
NonExposed exposed
200
400
800
1,600
1,000
2,000
20
20
1.0
Total
NonExposed exposed
1,000
8,00
2,000
2,200
3,000
3,000
33.3
26.7
1.38
69
Example 6.2
If the data presented in
example 6.1 (table 6.1)
is analysed separately in
smokers and nonsmokers, then the
prevalence odds ratio is
70
Example 6.3
Suppose that a cohort
study of lung cancer
involves a comparison
with national mortality
rates in a country where
50% of the population
are non-smokers, 40%
are moderate smokers
with a 10-fold risk of
lung cancer (compared
to non-smokers), and
10% are heavy smokers
with a 20-fold risk of
lung cancer. Then, it
can be calculated that
the national lung cancer
Table 6.2
Estimated crude rate ratios in relation to fraction of smokers in various hypothetical
populations
Population fraction (%)
Nonsmokers
Moderate Smokersa
Heavy Smokersa
100
80
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
----
-20
30
35
40
45
50
55
60
65
25
--
--5
10
15
20
25
30
35
75
100
Bias in
relative risk
0.15
0.43
0.57
0.78
1.00b
1.22
1.43
1.65
1.86
2.08
2.69
3.08
71
Assessment of Confounding
72
Example 6.4
Wrensch et al (2000)
conducted a case-control
study of 476 adults
newly diagnosed with
glioma in the San
Francisco Bay Area
between August 1991
and April 1994, and 462
age- gender- and
ethnicity-matched
controls. In addition,
limited information was
73
74
Example 6.5
In many cohort studies
some exposed persons
will be classified as nonexposed, and vice versa.
Table 6.3 illustrates this
situation with
hypothetical data from a
study of lung cancer
incidence in asbestos
workers. Suppose the
true incidence rates are
100 per 100,000 personyears in the high
exposure group, and 10
per 100,000 personyears in the low exposure
group, and the relative
Table 6.3
Hypothetical data from a cohort study in which 15% of highly exposed persons and
10% of low exposed persons are incorrectly classified.
Actual
Observed
--------------------------------------------------------------------------------------High
Low
High Exposure
Low Exposure
Exposure Exposure
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Deaths
100
10
85 +
1=
86
9+
15 =
24
Person-years
100,000
100,000
85,000 + 10,000 = 95,000 90,000 +15,000 = 105,000
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Incidence rate 100
10
91
23
per 100,000
person years
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Rate ratio
10.0
4.0
75
Example 6.6
Table 6.4 gives
hypothetical data from a
cohort study in which the
findings for the high and
low exposure groups are
the same as in example
6.5, but there is also a
Table 6.4
Hypothetical data from a cohort study in which 15% of highly exposed persons and 10% of
low exposed persons are incorrectly classified, but the non-exposed are correctly classified
Actual
Observed
----------------------------------------------------------------------------High
Low
Non-Exposed
High
Low
Non-Exposed
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Deaths
100
10
5
86
24
5
Person-years
100,000
100,000
100,000
95,000
105,000
100,000
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Rate
100
10
5
91
23
5
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Rate ratio
20.0
2.0
1.0
18.1
4.6
1.0
76
Example 6.7
Table 6.5 shows data from
a hypothetical casecontrol study in which 70
of the 100 cases and 50 of
the 100 controls have
actually been exposed to
Table 6.5
Hypothetical data from a case-control study in which 90% of exposed cases and 60% of
exposed controls are correctly classified
Actual
Observed
Exposed
Non-exposed
Exposed
Non-exposed
Cases
70
30
63
37
Controls
50
50
30
70
Odds ratio
2.3
4.0
77
control group selected from among nondiseased members of the cohort, the
recall of occupational exposures in
controls might be different from that of
the cases. In this situation, differential
misclassification would occur, and it
could bias the odds ratio towards or
away from the null, depending on
whether members of the cohort who did
not develop lung cancer were more or
less likely to recall such exposure than
the cases.
Example 6.8
In the case-control
study of lung cancer in
Example 6.7, the
misclassification could
be made non-differential
by selecting controls
from cohort members
with other types of
cancer, or other
diseases, in order that
their recall of exposure
would be more similar to
that of the cases. As
before, 63 (90%) of the
is in a predictable
direction, towards the
null. However, it should
be noted that making a
bias non-differential will
not always make it
smaller, and that the
direction of bias from
non-differential
misclassification is
sometimes predictable
in advance.
78
Summary
79
References
80
81
82
Example 7.1
Katsouyanni et al (1993)
studied the effects of air
pollution and high
temperature in the
causation of excess
mortality during a major
heat wave in Greece in
July 1987. They found
that the effects of the
83
Table 7.1
Lung cancer risk per 1,000 people (and RR) in relation to exposure to cigarette smoke
and asbestos
Asbestos
Yes
No
-----------------------------------------------------Smoking
Yes
35/1000 (35.0)
10/1000 (10.0)
No
5/1000 (5.0)
1/1000 (1.0)
-----------------------------------------------------Rate difference
30/1000
9/1000
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Rate ratio
7.0
10.0
84
A Lawyer
Next we consult a lawyer (I do not
advise this as a real course of action;
this is just a hypothetical consultation!).
She/he is also concerned about the
effect of smoking, but the effect they are
interested in is what is the probability
that my clients lung cancer was caused
by their smoking? If we look at the
asbestos workers, we find that if they
smoked their risk of lung cancer was 35
per 1,000 whereas it was 7 per 1,000 if
they didnt smoke. Thus, assuming there
is no confounding by other factors, then
of every 35 lung cancer occurring in the
smokers, 5 would have happened
anyway, and 30 are additional cases due
to smoking. Thus, for an individual lung
cancer case, the probability that smoking
caused the cancer is 100*30/35 which is
Table 7.2
The approaches of different consultants to interpreting the data in table 8.1
Consultant
Effect
measure
Size of effect
---------------------------- Inherent
Asbestos
Statistical
workers
Others
model
Biostatistician 1
Relative risk
7.0
10.0
Relative risk
Yes
-ve
Biostatistician 2
Risk difference
30/1000
9/1000
Risk difference
Yes
+ve
Lawyer
Probability of
causation
86%
90%
Relative risk
Yes
-ve
Clinician
Individual risk
30 per
1,000
9 per
1,000
Risk difference
Yes
+ve
Public health
worker
Deaths
prevented
30 per
1,000
9 per
1,000
Risk difference
Risk difference
Yes
Yes
+ve
+ve
Epidemiologist
Combination of
factors to cause
disease
21 cases
out of 35
(60%) are
due to the
combination
of exposures
Not
applicable
Risk difference
Yes
+ve
Is there an
Interaction? Direction?
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
85
A Clinician
Background
Asbestos
Smoking
Asbestos &
Smoking
S
U
Cases
1/35 (3%)
4/35 (11%)
86
9/35 (26%)
21/35 (60%)
An Epidemiologist
87
88
89
Summary
References
Katsouyanni K, Pantazopoulou A,
Touloumi G, et al. Evidence for
interaction between air pollution and
high temperature in the causation of
excess mortality. Arch Environ Health
1993; 48: 235-42.
90
91
92
Part III
Conducting a study
93
94
8.1 Exposure
95
Demographic Factors
Questionnaires
96
Table 8.1
Types of exposure data commonly used in occupational epidemiology studies
(Source: Adapted from Checkoway et al, 2004)
Example 8.1
Raum et al (2001)
studied the impact of
maternal socio-economic
status on intrauterine
growth in the former
west and East Germany.
Information on sociodemographic or lifestyle
factors and pregnancy
outcome was available
for 3,374 live-born
singletons from West
Germany (1987/88) and
3070 from East Germany
(1990/91). Women were
recruited during
Example 8.2
Vartia (2001) studied
the consequences of
workplace bullying in the
municipal sector in
Helsinki, Finland. Every
35th member of the
Municipal Officials Union
was selected and 1037
(65.5%) responded to a
postal questionnaire. A
definition of bullying was
provided and study
97
Example 8.3
Saracci et al (1984a)
conducted a historical
cohort study of mortality
and cancer incidence of
workers exposed to
made-made vitreous
fibres at 13 European
plants. At 12 of the
plants an environmental
survey was conducted to
measure present
concentrations of fibres
in air samples. This was
used to create a jobexposure matrix. Within
Table 8.2
Asbestos concentrations (fibers/cc) in job categories in an asbestos textile plant
(Source: Adapted from Checkoway et al, 2004)
Job category
General area
Card operators
Clean-up
Raw fiber handling
1930-35
10.8
13.3
18.1
22.8
1936-45
5.3
6.5
8.8
11.0
98
1946-65
2.4
2.9
4.0
5.0
1966-75
4.3
5.3
7.2
9.0
Table 8.3
Example of an exposure history of an individual worker
Job
Card operator
Card operator
Clean-up
Clean-up
Years
1933-35
1936-1938
1939-45
1946-48
Mean exposure
10.8
6.5
8.8
4.0
Cumulative exposure
32.4
41.9
103.5
115.5
99
Example 8.4
Wing et al (1991)
conducted a historical
cohort mortality study
among workers at Oak
Ridge National
Laboratory, Tennessee.
Individual exposures to
external penetrating
radiation, primarily
gamma rays, were
measured using pocket
ionising chambers from
1943 until June 1944,
Biomarkers
More recently, there has been increasing
emphasis on the use of molecular
markers of internal dose (Schulte,
1993). In fact, there are a number of
major limitations of currently available
biomarkers of exposure (Armstrong et
al, 1992), particularly with regard to
historical exposures (Pearce et al, 1995).
For example, serum levels of
micronutrients reflect recent rather than
historical dietary intake (Willett, 1990).
Some biomarkers are better than others
in this respect (particularly markers of
exposure to biological agents), but even
the best markers of chemical exposures
usually reflect only the last few weeks or
months of exposure. On the other hand,
with some biomarkers it may be possible
to estimate historical levels provided that
certain assumptions are met. For
example, it may be possible to estimate
historical levels of exposure to pesticides
(or contaminants) from current serum
levels provided that the exposure period
is known, and the half-life is known.
Similarly, information on recent
exposures can be used if it is reasonable
to assume that exposure levels (or at
least relative exposure levels) have
100
Example 8.5
Ross et al (1992) studied
urinary aflatoxin
biomarkers and risk of
hepatocellular carcinoma
as part of an ongoing
prospective study of
18,244 middle-aged men
in Shanghai. After
35,299 person-years of
101
102
Routine Records
Most countries maintain comprehensive
death registration systems at the
national or regional levels, and cause of
death information for identified deaths
can be obtained by requesting copies of
death certificates from national, state, or
municipal vital statistics offices. In most
instances the causes of death are coded
by a nosologist trained in the rules
specified in the International
Classification of Diseases (ICD) volumes
compiled by the World Health
Organisation. Revisions to the ICD
coding are made about every ten years,
and in some instances the ICD code for a
particular cause of death may change
(Checkoway et al, 2004).
Some countries or states also maintain
incidence registers for conditions such as
cancer, congenital malformations or
epilepsy. These have most commonly
been established for cancer registration
and the International Agency for
Research on Cancer (IARC) has been
attempting to encourage the
establishment of cancer registries and to
standardize methods of cancer
registration throughout the world
(Jensen et al, 1991). Provided that
registration is relatively complete, then
cancer registrations can provide valuable
Example 8.6
Jones et al (1998)
performed a record
linkage study of prenatal and early life risk
factors for childhood
onset diabetes mellitus.
They identified 160 boys
and 155 girls born
during 1965-1986 who
had been admitted to
hospital in Oxfordshire,
England with a diagnosis
of diabetes during 19651987. For each case, up
to eight controls were
chosen from records for
live births in the same
area, matched on sex,
year of birth and hospital
or place of birth. They
Morbidity Surveys
In some circumstances, routine records
may not be available for the health
outcome under study, or may not be
sufficiently complete or accurate or use
in epidemiological studies. Although this
103
Example 8.7
Dowse et al (1990)
studied the prevalence
of non-insulin dependent
diabetes mellitus
(NIDDM) in adults aged
25-74 years in Mauritius.
A random sample of
5,892 individuals was
chosen and 5,080
(83.4%) participated.
They used a 75g oral
glucose tolerance test
with fasting and 2-h post
load blood collection.
Glucose tolerance was
classified according to
the World Health
Organisation (WHO)
criteria (World Health
Organisation, 1985). The
prevalence of NIDDM
was similar in men
(12.1%) and women
(11.7%). Age and sexstandardized prevalence
was similar in Hindu
Indians (12.4%), Muslim
Indians (13.3%), Creoles
(10.4%) and Chinese
(11.9%). The authors
104
Summary
105
References
106
107
108
109
Comparison populations
In community-based cohorts,
comparisons are usually made internally
between study participants exposed and
those not exposed to a particular risk
factor (e.g. low dietary beta carotene
intake compared with high dietary beta
carotene intake).
In studies of specific populations, an
internal comparison may still be
possible, e.g. by comparing workers
with high benzene exposure to those
with low benzene exposure. However, in
some instances this may not be possible
because good individual exposure
information is not available (apart from
the fact that workers in the factory
received high exposure on the average)
or because there is not sufficient
variation in exposure within the
population (e.g. because everyone who
worked in the factory had high
exposure). In this situation, an external
comparison may be made, e.g. with
national death rates or cancer
registration rates. In this situation, the
source population for the study is
effectively the national population, and a
comparison is being made between the
subgroup in the source population that
worked in a particular factory (for
example) and the entire source
population. Ideally the comparison
should be made between the exposed
group and the source population minus
the exposed group (i.e. everyone else in
110
Example 9.1
The Renfrew/Paisley
study was based on two
adjacent urban burghs
considered to be typical
of the West of Scotland.
During 1972-1976, men
and women aged
between 45 and 64 and
identified by door-todoor census as living in
Renfrew and Paisley
were invited to take
part. The response rate
was 80% (7,052 men
and 8,354 women).
Participants completed a
questionnaire which
included self-reported
smoking history,
occupation, address,
age, gender, and
respiratory symptoms.
Study participants were
flagged at the National
Health Service Central
Register in Edinburgh
and followed for 20
years. Hart et al (2001)
reported that high lung
cancer mortality risks
were seen for manual
compared with nonmanual workers. The
risk reduced when
Example 9.2
Rafnsson et al (2001)
studied cancer incidence
in a cohort of 1690 flight
attendants working with
two airline companies in
Iceland. The total
number of person-years
of follow-up was 27,148.
Among the 1,532
women flight
111
Example 9.3
Prescott et al (2004)
studied vital exhaustion
(fatigue, hopelessness
and depression) as a
risk factor for
ischaemic heart disease
(IHD) in 4084 men and
5479 women in
Copenhagen. The study
was based on
participants in the
Copenhagen City Heart
Study, and the
analyses were based on
10,135 people who
attended the third
follow-up examination
in 1991-1993.
Cardiovascular risk
factors were assessed
by a self-administered
questionnaire checked
with the participant by
trained staff, and by
various laboratory
tests. Vital exhaustion
was assessed using a
17-item questionnaire.
Participants were
followed until 31
December 1997 for
fatal and non-fatal IHD,
with the information
being obtained from
the National Board of
Health and National
Hospital Discharge
Register respectively.
Subjects with selfreported and verified
IHD prior to enrolment
were excluded. During
112
follow-up, 483
experienced an IHD
event, of which 25%
were fatal, and 1559
subjects died from all
causes. All but 4 of the
17 items were
significantly associated
with IHD with
significant relative risks
ranging from 1.36 to
2.10. The RR for IHD in
those with a vital
exhaustion score of 10
or more was 2.57
(95% CI 1.65-4.00)
and this altered little
after adjustment for
biological, behavioural
and socioeconomic risk
factors.
9.3 Follow-up
113
Example 9.4
Munk Nielsen et al
(2003) studied longterm mortality after
poliomyelitis by
identifying a group of
5,977 patients diagnosed
with poliomyelitis in
Copenhagen between
1919 and 1954. This
involved a review of
more than 80,000
consecutive hospital
records for
Blegdamshospitalet
which served as the
primary centre for
diagnosing and treating
patients with acute
poliomyelitis in the area
of greater Copenhagen.
Information extracted
from the records
included name, sex, date
and place of birth, date
of admission and
discharge, and details of
the acute severity of the
case.
114
discharge (whichever
came later) until the
date of death,
emigration or 1 May
1997 (whichever came
earlier).
There were 1295 deaths
compared with an
expected number of
1141 (SMR 1.14, 95% CI
1.07-1.20). Excess
mortality was restricted
to polio patients with a
history of severe
paralysis of the
extremities (SMR =
1.69, 95% CI 1.32-2.15)
or patients who had
been treated for
respiratory failure during
the epidemics (SMR =
2.71; 95% CI 2.183.37).
Summary
References
115
116
117
Example 10.1
Bigert et al (2001)
studied myocardial
infarction (MI) among
professional drivers. The
source population
comprised all men aged
45-70 years free of
previous MI and living in
Stockholm County
during 1992-1993.
Cases of first MI
generated by this source
population and risk
period were identified
from three sources: the
medical care units at the
10 emergency hospitals
within the Stockholm
County (87% of the
cases), other hospital
118
Example 10.2
Mian et al (2001) studied homicide in
Orangi, the largest squatter settlement
in Karachi with an estimated population
of 1.2 million. They defined the cases as
individuals who lived in Orangi and were
killed in Orangi between January 1994
and January 1997, due to intentional
violence, by firearms, sharp or blunt
trauma. Cases were identified in the 15
neighbourhoods (out of 103 in total in
Orangi) which field workers identified as
the highest violence neighbourhoods.
Field workers identified households
where they knew someone had been
killed; in a few neighbourhoods they also
contacted other social organisations in
the community to identify further cases.
Controls were selected from a random
sample of households enrolled in a
related study conducted at the same
time in the same 15 neighbourhoods. For
119
Sources of controls
In a population-based case-control
study, controls are usually sampled at
random from the entire source
population (perhaps with matching on
factors such as age and gender). In
some instances, it may be necessary
to restrict the source population in
order to achieve valid control
sampling. For example, if controls are
to be selected from voter registration
rolls, and these are known to be less
than 100% complete for the
geographical area under study, then
the source population might be
restricted to persons appearing on the
voter registration roll, and cases that
were not registered to vote would be
excluded; controls would then be
sampled from this redefined source
population by taking a random sample
of the roll.
In registry-based studies, selection of
controls may not be so straightforward
because the source population may not
be so easy to define and enumerate. For
example, if there are two major hospitals
in a city, and a study is based on lung
cancer admissions in one of them during
a defined risk period, then the source
population is all those who would have
come to this hospital for treatment if
they had developed lung cancer during
this risk period. This population may be
difficult to define and enumerate,
particularly if cases may also be referred
from smaller regional hospitals. The best
solution is usually to define a more
specific source population (e.g. all people
living in the city) and to attempt to
identify all cases generated by that
source population, e.g. by including
120
Matching
In some instances it may be appropriate
to match cases and controls on potential
confounders (e.g. age and gender). This
can be done by 1:1 matching (e.g. for
each case, choose a control of the same
age and gender) or by frequency
matching (e.g. if there are 25 male cases
in the 30-34 age-group then choose the
same number of male controls for this
age-group). It is important to
emphasize, however, that this will not
remove confounding in a case-control
study, but will merely facilitate its
control in the analysis. For example, in a
case-control study of lung cancer, the
cases will generally be relatively old
whereas a random general population
control sample will be relatively young.
This may lead to inefficiencies when age
Example 10.3
Cole et al (2000)
studied time urgency
and risk of non-fatal
myocardial infarction
(MI) in a study of 340
cases and an equal
number of age, sex and
community-matched
controls. Cases were
identified from
admissions to the
coronary or intensive
care units of six
suburban Boston
hospitals between 1
January 1982 and 31
December 1983. Those
eligible for inclusion
were white men and
121
time urgency/
impatience was
ascertained using four
items from the 10-item
Framingham Type A
scale. A dose-response
relation was apparent
among subjects who
rated themselves
higher on the four-item
urgency/impatience
scale with a matched
odds ratio for non-fatal
MI of 4.45 (95% CI
2.20-8.99) comparing
those with the highest
rating to those with the
lowest.
Summary
122
References
123
124
125
Example 11.1
Wilks et al (1999)
conducted a survey of
the prevalence of
diabetes in the
population of Spanish
Town, Jamaica. A
random population
sample was recruited by
door-to-door canvassing
(n=1,303) and oral
fourfold excess of
diabetes in women
compared to men, but
obesity could not
entirely account for the
high prevalences
observed which exceed
those previously
reported among
European populations.
= P (Sn + Sp - 1) + (1 - Sp)
therefore if two populations are being
compared, and their true prevalences
(according to the gold standard) are P1
and P0 respectively, then the observed
difference in prevalence between the two
centres is:
(P1 P0)(Sn + Sp - 1)
The expression (Sn + Sp -1) is
Youden's Index. When this is equal to
1 (which only occurs when the
126
Example 11.2
Table 11.1 shows
hypothetical data from a
study of asthma
prevalence in childhood.
The true prevalence
rates were 40% in the
exposed group, and 20%
in the non-exposed
group; the true
prevalence difference
was thus 20%. If 20% of
asthmatics are
incorrectly classified as
non-asthmatics (i.e. a
sensitivity of 0.80), and
10% of non-asthmatics
are incorrectly classified
as asthmatics (i.e. a
specificity of 0.90), then
the observed
127
Table 11.1
Hypothetical data from a prevalence study in which 20% of asthmatics
and 10% of non-asthmatics are incorrectly classified
Actual
Observed
----------------------------- --------------------------------------------------NonExposed
exposed
Exposed
Non-exposed
----------------------------------------------- ----------------------- --------------------------Asthmatics
40
20
32 + 6 = 38
16 + 8 = 24
Non-asthmatics 60
80
54 + 8 = 62
72 + 4 = 76
-------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Total
100
100
100
100
-------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Prevalence
40%
20%
38%
24%
128
Example 11.3
Guha Mazumder et al
(2000) studied arsenic
in drinking water and
the prevalence of
respiratory effects in
West Bengal, India. A
cross-sectional survey
involving 7,683
participants of all ages
was conducted in an
arsenic-affected region
between April 1995 and
March 1996. The
source population was
based on two areas of
the arsenic-affected
districts south of
Calcutta. A
convenience sampling
strategy was used in
which the field team
went to the centre of
each village and
selected the most
convenient hamlet to
begin sampling; all
household members
were invited to
participate and
sampling continued
from house to house
until sufficient numbers
had been recruited.
Participants were
clinically examined and
interviewed, and the
arsenic content of their
current primary
drinking water source
was measured. There
were few smokers and
analyses were confined
to non-smokers (6,864
participants). Among
both males and
females, the
prevalence of cough,
shortness of breath,
and chest sounds
(crepitations and/or
rhonchi) in the lungs
rose with increasing
arsenic concentrations
in drinking water. In
participants with
arsenic-related skin
lesions, the ageadjusted prevalence
odds ratios for cough
were 7.8 for females
(95% CI 3.1-19.5), and
5.0 for males (95% CI
2.6-9.9); the
corresponding findings
for chest sounds were
9.6 (95% CI 4.0-22.9)
and 6.9 (95% CI 5.892.8), and those for
shortness of breath
were 23.3 (95% CI
5.8-92.8) and 3.7
(95% CI 1.3-10.6). The
authors concluded that
these results add to
evidence that longterm ingestion of
arsenic can cause
respiratory effects.
Summary
129
References
Asher I, Keil U, Anderson HR, et al
(1995). International study of asthma
and allergies in childhood (ISAAC):
rationale and methods. Eur Resp J 8:
483-91.
130
Part IV
131
132
Data Management
133
Data Analysis
The basic aim of the analysis of a single
study is to estimate the effect of
exposure on the outcome under study
while controlling for confounding and
minimizing other possible sources of
bias. In addition, when confounding and
other sources of bias cannot be
removed, then it is important to assess
their likely strength and direction. This
latter task was discussed in chapter 7.
In this chapter I focus on the control of
confounding.
Effect estimation
The basic effect measures, and methods
of controlling confounding are described
below. Usually, in epidemiology studies,
we wish to measure the difference in
disease occurrence between groups
exposed and not exposed to a particular
factor.
The analysis ideally should control
simultaneously for all confounding
factors. Control of confounding in the
analysis involves stratifying the data
according to the levels of the
confounder(s) and calculating an effect
estimate which summarizes the
information across strata of the
confounder(s). For example, controlling
for age (grouped into 5 categories) and
gender (with 2 categories) might involve
grouping the data into the 10 (= 5 x 2)
confounder strata and calculating a
134
Confidence intervals
As well as estimating the effect of an
exposure, it is also important to
estimate the statistical precision of the
effect estimate. The confidence interval
(usually the 95% confidence interval)
provides a range of values in which it is
plausible (provided that there is no
uncontrolled confounding or other bias)
that the true effect estimate may lie. If
the statistical model is correct, and
there is no bias, then the confidence
intervals derived from an infinite series
of study repetitions would contain the
true effect estimate with a frequency no
less than its confidence level (Rothman
and Greenland, 1998).
RR e + 1.96 SE
P-Values
As discussed in chapter 5, the p-value is
the probability that a test statistic as
large or larger as that observed could
have arisen by chance if there is no bias
and if the null hypothesis (of no
association between exposure and
disease) is correct. The test statistic
defines the p-value and usually has the
form:
m + 1.96 SE
where m is the observed mean of the
sample, and SE is its standard error,
estimated from the standard deviation
of the sample divided by the square root
of the sample size.
This confidence interval depends on two
quantities (m and SE) which are
estimated from the sample itself, and
different results will be obtained from
different samples. Provided that the
samples are sufficiently large, then 95%
of the time, the confidence interval
estimated from the sample would
contain the true population mean. One
should note, however, that this is no
guarantee that the interval from ones
data contains the true value.
z = D/SE
where D is the observed difference and
SE is the standard error of the
difference.
This provides a test statistic (z) which
can be used to calculate the probability
(p-value) that a difference as large as
that observed would have occurred by
135
136
I0 =
cases
-------------person-time
b
---Y0
Table 12.1
Findings from a hypothetical cohort study of 20,000 persons followed for 10 years
Cases
Non-cases
Exposed
1,813 (a)
8,187 (c)
Non-exposed
952 (b)
9,048 (d)
10,000 (N1)
10,000 (N0)
Person-years
Incidence rate
Incidence proportion (average risk)
Incidence odds
90,635
0.0200
0.1813
0.2214
95,163
0.0100
0.0952
0.1052
(Y1)
(I1)
(R1)
(O1)
(Y0)
(I0)
(R0)
(O0)
Ratio
2.00
1.90
2.11
I0 e+ 1.96 SE
SE [ln(I0)] = (1/b)0.5
R0 =
137
cases
b
---------- = -----persons
N0
cases
b
---------- = -----persons
N0
(1/b - 1/N0)0.5
a/Y1
I1
RR = ----- = -----b/Y0
I0
cases
b
----------- = ---non-cases
d
(1/b + 1/d)0.5
[Obs(a) - Exp(a)]2
[a - Y1M1/T]2
2 = ---------------------- = ---------------Var(Exp(a))
O0 e+1.96 SE
[M1Y1Y0/T2]
138
O1
OR = --- =
O0
a/c
----- =
b/d
ad
bc
+1.96 SE
RR e
OR e+1.96 SE
139
[ai - Y1iM1i/Ti]2
2 = ------------------------- = -------------------Var(Exp(a))
[M1iY1iT0i/Ti2]
Pooling
Pooling involves calculating a
summary effect estimate assuming
stratum-specific effects are equal.
There are a number of different
methods of obtaining pooled effect
estimates, but a commonly used
method which is both simple and
close to being statistically optimal
(even when there are small numbers
in all strata) is the method of Mantel
and Haenszel (1959).
aiY0i/Ti
RR = ------------- biY1i/Ti
aiN0i/Ti
RR = ------------biN1i/Ti
140
Var(Exp(a))
[M1iM0iM1iN0i/Ti2(Ti-1)]
PR
OR e+1.96 SE
Standardization
RR e+1.96 SE
The Mantel-Haenszel summary odds
ratio has the form:
aidi/Ti
OR = ---------- bici/Ti
An approximate p-value for the
hypothesis that the summary odds ratio
is 1.0 can be obtained from the one
degree-of-freedom Mantel-Haenszel
summary chi-square (Mantel and
Haenszel, 1959):
[Obs(a) -
P = (ai + di)/Ti
Q = (bi + ci)/Ti
R = aidi/Ti
S = bici/Ti
R+ = R
S+ = S
QS
[ M1iN1iN0i/Ti2 - aibi/Ti]0.5
SE = --------------------------------[(aiN0i/Ti)(biN1i/Ti)]0.5
(PS + QR)
Exp(a)]2
[ai -
N1iM1i/Ti]2
= ------------------------ = ----------------------
Var(Exp(a))
[ M1iM0iN1iN0i/Ti2(Ti-1)]
141
Standardization is an alternative
approach to obtaining a summary
effect estimate (Miettinen, 1974;
Rothman and Greenland, 1998).
Pooling involves calculating the effect
estimate under the assumption that
the measure (e.g. The rate ratio)
would be the same (uniform) across
strata if random error were absent. In
contrast, standardization involves
taking a weighted average of the
disease occurrence across strata (e.g.
the standardized rate) and then
comparing the standardized
occurrence measure between exposed
and non-exposed (e.g. the
standardized rate ratio) with no
assumptions of uniformity of effect.
Standardization is more prone than
pooling to suffer from statistical
instability due to small numbers in
[ wi2Ri(1-Ri)/Ni]
SE = ----------------------R wi
where Ni is the number of persons in
stratum i. An approximate 95%
confidence interval for the
standardized rate is thus:
R e+ 1.96 SE
wiRi
R = -------- wi
The natural log of the standardized rate
has an approximate standard error
(under the Poisson model for random
error) of:
2
0.5
[ wi Ri/Yi]
SE = ---------------R wi
where Yi is the person-time in stratum i.
An approximate 95% confidence interval
for the standardized rate is thus:
R e+ 1.96 SE
The standardized risk has the form:
Multiple Regression
wiRi
R = --------- wi
The natural log of the standardized risk
has an approximate standard error
142
Table 12.2
Segis World population
Age-group
Population
----------------------------0-4 years
12,000
5-9 years
10,000
10-14 years
9,000
15-19 years
9,000
20-24 years
8,000
25-29 years
8,000
30-34 years
6,000
35-39 years
6,000
40-44 years
6,000
45-49 years
6,000
50-54 years
5,000
55-59 years
4,000
60-64 years
4,000
65-69 years
3,000
70-74 years
2,000
75-59 years
1,000
80-84 years
500
85+ years
500
----------------------------Total
100,000
----------------------------Source: Segi (1960)
Summary
143
References
144
145
146
Selection bias
Whereas confounding generally
involves biases inherent in the source
population, selection bias involves
biases arising from the procedures by
which the study subjects are chosen
from the source population. As with
confounding, if it is not possible to
directly control for selection bias, it
still may be possible to assess its likely
strength and direction. It is
unreasonable to dismiss the findings of
a particular study because of possible
selection bias, without at least
attempting to assess which direction
the possible selection bias would have
been in, and how strong it might have
been.
Information bias
With regards to information bias, the
key issue is whether misclassification
is likely to have been differential or
non-differential. In the latter case, the
bias will usually be in a know direction,
i.e. towards the null. If
misclassification has been differential,
then it is important to attempt to
147
148
149
Combination of Epidemiological
Evidence With Evidence From Other
Sources
Epidemiological evidence should be
considered together with all other
available evidence, including animal
experiments. An association is plausible
if it is consistent with other knowledge,
whereas the epidemiological evidence is
coherent if it is not inconsistent with
other knowledge. For instance,
laboratory experiments may have
shown that a particular environmental
exposure can cause cancer in laboratory
animals, and this would make more
plausible the hypothesis that this
exposure could cause cancer in humans.
However, biological plausibility is a
relative concept; many epidemiological
associations were considered
implausible when they were first
discovered but were subsequently
confirmed by other evidence, e.g. the
relation of lice to typhus. Lack of
plausibility may simply reflect lack of
knowledge (medical, biological, or
social) which is continually changing and
evolving.
150
Summary
References
151
152