Stigma and Intellectual Disability A Review of Related Measures and

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Research in Developmental Disabilities

Review article

Stigma and intellectual disability: A review of related measures and


future directions
Shirli Werner a,*, Patrick Corrigan b,1, Nicole Ditchman b,2, Kristin Sokol b,3
a
b

The Paul Baerwald School of Social Work and Social Welfare Hebrew University of Jerusalem Mount Scopus, Jerusalem 91905, Israel
College of Psychology, Illinois Institute of Technology Chicago, IL 60616, USA

A R T I C L E I N F O

A B S T R A C T

Article history:
Received 29 September 2011
Received in revised form 7 October 2011
Accepted 7 October 2011
Available online 23 November 2011

The theoretical construct of stigma has received much attention in psychiatric disability
research, leading to the development of widely used measures. Such measures have had
real world impact in that they allow for the assessment of stigma change efforts. The study
of stigma has not received the same level of attention for persons with intellectual
disabilities. In this manuscript we evaluate existing measures of intellectual disability
stigma through a systematic review of the literature. Twenty-four scales were reviewed
and evaluated. Findings indicate a paucity of stigma measures based on theoretical
conceptualizations pointing to a need for further development of measures to pursue the
study of public, self, and family stigma as related to intellectual disability.
2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords:
Intellectual disability
Stigma
Scale
Systematic review
Measurement

Contents
1.

2.

3.

Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Stigma . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
1.1.
Impact of stigma on individuals with intellectual disabilities . . .
1.2.
Impact of stigma on family members of individuals with ID. . . .
1.3.
1.4.
Stigma scales . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Material and methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Search strategy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
2.1.
2.2.
Inclusion criteria . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Scale evaluation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
2.3.
2.3.1.
Reliability and validity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Multidimensionality . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
2.3.2.
2.3.3.
Feasibility and appropriateness of the scale. . . . . . . . . .
Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
3.1.
Public attitude scales . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Reliability and validity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
3.1.1.
Multidimensionality . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
3.1.2.
Feasibility and appropriateness of public stigma scales
3.1.3.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

* Corresponding author. Tel.: +972 2 5881377; fax: +972 2 5883927.


E-mail addresses: [email protected] (S. Werner), [email protected] (P. Corrigan), [email protected] (N. Ditchman), [email protected] (K. Sokol).
1
Tel.: +1 312 567 6751; fax: +1 312 567 6753.
2
Tel: +1 312 567 3511; fax: +1 312 567 3493.
3
Tel.: +1 312 567 5246; fax: +1 312 567 6753.
0891-4222/$ see front matter 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2011.10.009

749
749
750
750
751
751
751
751
751
752
752
752
752
753
753
753
759

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

3.2.

4.

5.

Self-stigma scales . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Reliability and validity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
3.2.1.
3.2.2.
Multidimensionality . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Feasibility and appropriateness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
3.2.3.
Family stigma scales . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
3.3.
Reliability and validity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
3.3.1.
Multidimensionality . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
3.3.2.
Feasibility and appropriateness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
3.3.3.
Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Theoretical limitations of existing scales . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
4.1.
Methodological limitations of existing scales . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
4.2.
Why has stigma conceptualization been rarely used in the ID eld?
4.3.
Applying stigma conceptualization to the ID eld . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
4.4.
Limitations and future directions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
4.5.
Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

749

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.

759
759
759
759
760
760
760
760
760
760
761
761
761
762
762
762
762

1. Introduction
The World Health Organizations (WHO) International Classication of Functioning, Disability, and Health (ICF) has
recognized that societal attitudes toward individuals with disabilities can negatively impact full participation in community
life (WHO, 2001). While stigma has been widely studied in the eld of psychiatric disability, leading to fairly well-developed
theoretical and methodological frameworks, the same level of attention and rigorous investigation has not been found in the
intellectual disabilities (ID) eld. Although people with ID differ from those with mental illness (MI), it is clear that
individuals with ID also constitute a stigmatized group who face many barriers to employment, housing, and community
participation. In an effort to advance understanding of stigma and ID, we aimed to carefully review and summarize existing
knowledge and identify key characteristics of research, which will guide future directions and efforts. Specically, this
review aims to systematically obtain and evaluate existing measures of ID stigma.
1.1. Stigma
One of the earliest denitions of stigma was offered by Goffman (1963) who described it as an attribute that is deeply
discrediting reducing the bearer from a whole and usual person to a tainted, discounted one (p. 3). Several theorists and
researchers have further conceptualized stigma and its dimensions. Social psychologists have dened stigma as comprised of
cognitive, emotional, and behavioral aspects known as stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination. The negative attitudes and
stereotypes about a discredited subgroup lead to prejudice and discrimination toward such subgroups (Corrigan, 2000;
Corrigan & Watson, 2002). One way to approach understanding stigma is to see it as negative attitudes. Attitudes are
evaluative statements toward a person, object, or event. Attitudes are composed of affective, cognitive, and behavioral
components (Chan, Livneh, Pruett, Wang, & Zheng, 2009). Attitudes are a combination of seemingly factual statements and
an emotional reaction or value component. Attitudes can be either negative or positive; however, stigma is comprised
explicitly of negative attitudes (Corrigan & Lundin, 2001). Stereotypes are knowledge structures, or collective opinions,
known to most members of a social group (Hilton & von Hippel, 1996; Krueger, 1996). They are attitudes about a larger group
of people (i.e., people with MI), and like attitudes, can be either positive or negative (Corrigan & Lundin, 2001). Stereotypes
are helpful because they allow individuals to generate more efcient impressions and expectations of others that belong to
the stereotyped group (Hamilton & Sherman, 1994). Prejudice involves an evaluative component and includes a negative
emotional response toward the stereotyped group (Eagly & Chaiken, 1993). The cognitive and affective response of prejudice
leads to the behavioral reaction of discrimination (Crocker, Major, & Steele, 1998), including avoidant behavior, increased
social distance (Corrigan, 2002), hostile behavior (Weiner, 1995) and withholding help (Corrigan, 2000). Thus, stigma is seen
as a behavioral chain that starts from the stigmatizing mark, progresses through the attitude structures and results in
discrimination (Lam, Tsang, Chan, & Corrigan, 2006). Although important, attitudes fall short of addressing all of the
components that are encompassed by the stigma construct. Attitudes might best be conceptualized as a piece of the pie when
it comes to understanding how individuals with ID experience discrimination.
Some of the common stereotypes about persons with MI include beliefs that they are dangerous and responsible for
causing their illness (Link, Monahan, Stueve, & Cullen, 1999; Pescosolido, Monhan, Link, Stueve, & Kikuzawa, 1999).
Prejudice occurs when individuals endorse these negative attitudes (e.g., thats right; all persons with MI are violent) and
consequently generate negative emotional reactions (e.g., they all scare me). Finally, discrimination related to MI may
include coercion, segregation, hostile behaviors, and withholding help (Corrigan, Markowitz, Watson, Rowan, & Kubiak,
2003), resulting in limited work and housing opportunities (Rusch, Angermeyer, & Corrigan, 2005).
The eld of psychiatric disability has also differentiated between several types of stigma. First, public stigma is the most
well known type of stigma and focuses on the attitudes of the general population toward stigmatized persons (Link,

750

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

Struening, Rahav, Phelan, & Nuttbrock, 1997; Phelan, Bromet, & Link, 1998). A second type of stigma, self-stigma, focuses on
the experiences of stigmatized individuals themselves and the potential for internalization of societys negative views.
Targeted individuals may develop poor self-esteem, experience heightened negative emotions, and behaviorally withdraw
and conceal their stigmatized status from others (Corrigan & Watson, 2002).
Corrigan (2008) has dened self-stigma as consisting of four constructs: (1) awareness relates to people knowing common
stereotypes about others with a MI; (2) agreement occurs when a an individual supports the stereotypes as factual and accurate;
(3) application comes about when individuals internalize the stereotypes, applying them to themselves; and nally, (4) hurts
relate to the decreased self-esteem or self-efcacy that may result from internalizing stereotypes. It is important to note that
while harmful effects of stigma have been well documented, individual reactions to stigma vary, ranging from signicant loss in
self-esteem to righteous anger, and there are even some individuals who experience neither (Corrigan & Watson, 2002).
Finally, the third type of stigma is that experienced by persons surrounding the individual, including family members and
professionals. Family stigma is a specic case of stigma by association and is dened as the stigma experienced by individuals
as a consequence of being associated with a relative with a stigmatic mark (Larson & Corrigan, 2008). While the broader topic
of family burden has received attention in both the MI and ID literature, family stigma is just beginning to receive attention in
the ID eld.
Although substantial differences exist between individuals with MI and individuals with ID, some key similarities suggest
we may approach examining stigma for both groups in a similar fashion. First, both disability groups experience
discrimination, which limits their ability to achieve life goals. There are differences in the extent to which specic disabilities
are stigmatized, referred to as the hierarchy of preference toward disability. Within this hierarchy people with mental
disabilities (including MI and ID) are consistently the most highly stigmatized groups (Hernandez, Keys, & Balcazar, 2000;
Thomas, 2000; Yuker, 1988).
Some of the commonly held stereotypes about individuals with ID are highlighted in various studies. For example, in
qualitative interviews with 31 nurses, Slevin and Sines (1996) reported people with ID being perceived as aggressive. Within
qualitative interviews, individuals with ID reported that others perceive them as lacking the potential for change (Jahoda &
Markova, 2004). Further, in a multinational study, individuals in the general public have reported low expectations of the
abilities of individuals with ID (Siperstein, Norins, Corbin, & Shriver, 2003). Similarly, school children have perceived children
with ID as more severely impaired and less academically and socially able than they actually are (Siperstein & Bak, 1980).
Further, research has shown that the social identities of individuals with ID can be devalued and depersonalized into
stereotypic caricatures, which may negatively impact individuals with ID and their families (Crocker et al., 1998).
1.2. Impact of stigma on individuals with intellectual disabilities
Public stigma toward individuals with ID can take several forms. Individuals with ID may be exposed to teasing and stares,
and may be avoided by others (Pratt, 2010). Discrimination toward individuals with ID is evidenced through the lack of
decent services and discriminatory treatment within hospitals, such as the humiliation of public bathing, lack of privacy, and
an overly restrictive environment (Jahoda & Markova, 2004). Also illustrating the discrimination faced by individuals with ID
is the use of segregated day services and workshops instead of community-based services (Siperstein, Parker, Noris, &
Widaman, 2011), and the limited number of employment and choice making opportunities made available to individuals
with ID (Wehmeyer & Bolding, 1999). Stigma has been cited as one of the potential barriers to the delivery of adequate
services to this population (Gill, Kroese, & Rose, 2002), resulting in poorer treatment, rejection, and devalued roles within
society (Corrigan et al., 2003). Moreover, discriminatory behaviors lead to the lack of inclusion and social acceptance of
persons with ID within local communities as well as broader society (Jahoda & Markova, 2004).
Many individuals with moderate to mild ID are indeed aware of the stigma associated with their disability (Jahoda,
Wilson, Stalker, & Cairney, 2010). These individuals experience the stigma of their social identity through their interactions
with others, and this experience is often an emotionally painful one (Beart, Hardy, & Buchan, 2005). Some individuals may
even attempt to hide their disability as a way of avoiding stigmatization (Jahoda & Markova, 2004). Very few studies (beyond
Jahoda & Markova, 2004; Jahoda et al., 2010) have been conducted to examine self-stigma among individuals with ID and
most instruments are not suitable to examine stigma among individuals with more severe ID or with limited verbal skills
(Ali, Strydom, Hassiotis, Williams, & King, 2008). Further, regardless of level of awareness, individuals may nevertheless still
be prone to negative stigmatic treatment and its consequences (Cooney, Jahoda, Gumley, & Knott, 2006). We aim to identify
the instruments used in this eld to assess self-stigma in order to assess the need for further development in this area.
1.3. Impact of stigma on family members of individuals with ID
Most studies conducted looking at family stigma and ID have been qualitative in nature. Based on focus groups and indepth interviews, family members and caregivers reported their experiences of stigma based on their association with the
stigmatized individual (Cooney et al., 2006; Dagnan & Waring, 2004; Edwardraj, Mumtaj, Prasad, Kuruvilla, & Jacob, 2010;
Jahoda & Markova, 2004; Perkins, Holburn, Deaux, Flory, & Vietze, 2002). Other studies have found an association between
experiences of stigma and experiences of distress among parents of individuals with ID (Baxter, 1989; Mak & Cheung, 2008;
Shin & Nhan, 2009). A recent study looked at stigmatization of aging female family caregivers of adults with ID and adults
with MI and found similar levels of stigma reported by both groups (Chou, Pu, Lee, Lin, & Kroger, 2009).

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

751

Given the impact of stigma on the lives of individuals with ID and their families it is clear that there is a need to advance
this body of knowledge and to develop focused interventions to reduce stigmas negative impact. The development of valid,
reliable and theoretically driven scales is a rst step in understanding how the public reacts to individuals with disabilities as
well as how people with disabilities and their families react to stigma in their environment. Such measures must be
appropriate and suitable for the specic research population. These measures also provide a means to assess the impact and
effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions.
1.4. Stigma scales
Stigma conceptualization in the MI eld has set the foundation for the development of an array of measurement tools
intended to specically address public, self, and family stigma. Some examples of these scales include the Emotional Reaction
to Mental Illness Scale (Angermeyer & Matschinger, 1996), Internalized Stigma of Mental Illness Scale (ISMI; Ritsher, Otilingam,
& Grajales, 2003), the Attribution Questionnaire-27 (AQ-27; Corrigan et al., 2001, 2002); the Devaluation-Discrimination Scale
(Link & Phelan, 2001) and the Self-Stigma of Mental Illness Scale (SSMIS; Corrigan, 2008). Unfortunately, such an abundance of
scales addressing stigma is lacking in the ID eld.
In the ID literature, very few scales have been developed based on a stigma framework; however, several scales attempt to
gauge public attitudes toward disability. In addition, some studies have employed scales intended to measure attitudes
toward general disability groups and not necessarily specic to ID. These include: the Semantic Differential Scale (Osgood,
Suci, & Tannenbaum, 1957), the Scale of Attitudes toward Disabled Persons (Antonak, 1982, 1988), the Attitudes Toward Persons
with Disabilities Scale (Yuker, Block, & Campbell, 1960), the Interaction with Disabled Persons Scale (Gething, 1994), and the
Multidimensional Attitudes Scale (MAS, Findler, Vilchinsky, & Werner, 2007). Further, some scales have examined attitudes
toward specic disability-related issues or situations, such as attitudes toward school inclusion (Multinational Youth
Attitudes Questionnaire; Siperstein, Parker, Noris, & Widaman, 2007), attitudes toward community living (Community Living
Attitudes Scale; Henry, Keys, Jopp, & Balcazar, 1996) or attitudes toward the reproductive rights of people with ID (The Scale of
Attitudes Toward Mental Retardation and Eugenics; Antonak, Fielder, & Mulick, 1993) rather than general stigma toward
individuals with ID. Thus, the current study aims to review the available stigma instruments in the ID eld in order to assess
the need for the development of measures designed explicitly to measure the stigma of ID.

2. Material and methods


2.1. Search strategy
Bibliographic databases including: Pubmed, ERIC, PsycNET, Web of Knowledge, Proquest, Social Work Abstracts, and
Social Science Research Network, were used to search for manuscripts that either developed or employed stigma or attitude
measures in the ID eld. The search was conducted within the title or abstract of the manuscript using the following search
terms: intellectual disability, developmental disabilities, learning disabilities (the term used in the UK), cognitive disability,
mental retardation, attitudes, stereotypes, stigma, questionnaire, and scale. Manuscripts were limited to those published in
English. No limits were placed on the year of publication.
In the search, we aimed to nd manuscripts describing scale development as in most of these manuscripts the scales
themselves were available. When original papers for an instrument could not be located, we sought out studies that had used
these scales to obtain scale information. We also attempted to nd scales by searching for them online, and if scales were not
too old we contacted the authors and asked for the original scale.
2.2. Inclusion criteria
Because scales using the stigma framework were rare, we included manuscripts including scales measuring either stigma
or attitudes toward individuals with ID. We did not include scales that focused on general disability groups or solely on
specic issues (such as school inclusion). We included all scales that were relevant for the measurement of public, self or
family stigma. Development or validation studies of the identied scales were sought out as well as articles using the scales.
A ow chart depicting the systematic review process that was utilized is presented in Fig. 1. The search strategy yielded a
total of 710 potentially relevant manuscripts. After removing duplicate manuscripts, those that appeared to meet the
inclusion criteria were read in full. Thus, 81 manuscripts were examined for more detailed evaluation, of these 31 were
retained for the nal review and 25 additional manuscripts were added through hand-searching, yielding a total of 56
manuscripts.
2.3. Scale evaluation
Given the above search strategy, the full items for 17 of the scales were available. For the additional scales, the description
of the scale items found in the manuscripts was used for scale evaluation. Scales were evaluated based on the following
factors recommended by Kodaka, Postuvan, Ingaki, and Yamada (2010).

752

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

Fig. 1. Flow chart for systematic review.

2.3.1. Reliability and validity


Reports about internal consistency (Cronbachs a); stability (e.g., testretest or split-half reliability tests) and/or
reproducibility (e.g., replication of factor structure among different populations) were examined. Scales were evaluated for
their use of extensive literature reviews, expert consensus and/or focus group interviews during the process of scale
development.
2.3.2. Multidimensionality
This factor included the comprehensive adoption of the theoretical concepts of stigma or attitudinal dimensions based on
extensive theoretical backgrounds of the concept of stigma or attitudes toward individuals with ID. In this factor scales were
examined for their focus on components of attitudes (affective, cognitive, and emotional) or stigma (stereotypes, prejudice,
and discrimination) constructs. Further, we assessed reports of scale factor structure.
2.3.3. Feasibility and appropriateness of the scale
This factor was based on the assessment of the number of items in the scale and the complexity of the rating method so
that it can include all members of general public. Further, we evaluated the appropriateness or suitability of the measure to a
wide range of participants (e.g., use among various age groups, appropriateness for individuals with ID, use in various
countries and t to a range of languages and cultural backgrounds) as well as suitability to the current year (i.e., when the
scale was last employed).
3. Results
The systematic review retrieved 56 manuscripts describing 24 scales that had been developed and employed to study
attitudes, stereotypes or stigma within the ID eld. Of these, 17 focused on public attitudes or stigma, ve focused on selfstigma (one focused on both public and self-stigma) and three focused on family stigma. Nine of the public stigma scales
were developed in the United States, three in Australia, two in Ireland, and one each in the United Kingdom, Sweden, Canada,
Japan, and Israel; all ve self-stigma scales were developed in the United Kingdom and the family stigma scales were

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

753

developed in China, Taiwan, and Australia. The following sections report the evaluation of the scales, while more detailed
information about the specic scales can be found in Tables 13.
3.1. Public attitude scales
3.1.1. Reliability and validity
Table 1 presents reliability coefcient information for the scales reviewed. As seen, reliability was not reported for several
public attitude scales. Most scales focused solely on internal consistency, which was found to be moderate for three of the
scales and good for ve others; while split half coefcient and testretest coefcient were rarely examined (e.g., Clunies-ross
& Thomas, 1986; Hagan, 1980).
In terms of validity, only a few of the scales reported conducting wide literature reviews as a basis for their development,
while others based their development on previously existing scales. It was unclear how the questionnaire items were
derived for most of the scales reviewed. Six public attitude scales (out of 17) did not clearly report evidence of validity, while
several of the scales reported evidence primarily for face validity.
Other types of validity evidence were only reported for a few of the scales. For example, researchers utilizing the Peer
Attitudes toward the Handicapped Scale (PATHS) have reported it to have evidence for construct and factorial validity (Cluniesross & Thomas, 1986). According to the manual, items were derived based on structured interviews with students, parents,
teachers, and counselors and standardized on the responses of over 700 students in the United States (Bagley & Green, 1981).
As another example, the Attitude Behavior Scale (Jordan, 1969) was found to have good content validity as items were
developed along with school psychologists. Construct validity was ascertained as the postulated semantic structure and the
obtained statistical structure were similar. Concurrent validity was inferred from the fact that older, more experienced, and
more knowledgeable samples scored more positively on the scale (Jordan, 1969).
More rigorous validity examinations can be seen in a few of the scales. For example, the construct validity of the Mental
Retardation Attitude Inventory Revised (Antonak & Harth, 1994) was established by conducting factor analyses, which
supported the original four factors (social distance, integrationsegregation, private rights, and subtle derogatory beliefs).
Several examinations of validity were performed for the Classical and Modern Scales of Attitudes Toward People with
Intellectual Disability (Akrami, Ekehammar, Claesson, & Sonnander, 2006), which was found to have construct validity and
partial discriminant validity.
Finally, the most comprehensive scale found in our review was the Attitudes to Disability Scale (ADS; Power, Green, & the
WHOQOL-DIS Group, 2010). This recent scale was developed as part of an international study following the World Health
Organizations WHOQOL methodology (WHO, 1998a, 1998b) and examined the possibility of constructing a single crosscultural attitude scale. Development of the scale was based on an extensive literature search as well as on consultation with a
wide range of people and professionals including people with ID and their families. The researchers used a focus group
design across 12 centers from around the world in order to develop questionnaire items that they subsequently pilot and
eld tested. Exploratory and conrmatory factor analyses yielded four dimensions. Although individuals with ID were
included in this scales development, only general disability was the target of the items rather than a specic focus on
intellectual disability.
3.1.2. Multidimensionality
Most of the scales reviewed were based on public attitudes (as reected in their titles) rather than specically
addressing stigma. The scales reviewed did not include the constructs of stereotypes, prejudice and discrimination. Attitudes
are generally conceptualized as comprising affective, behavioral, and cognitive aspects (Findler et al., 2007). However, most
of the scales reviewed did not fully include all three of these aspects. For example, the SCATE (Miller & Loukellis, 1982) and
the Behavioral Intent Scale (Roberts & Lindsell, 1997) only address behavioral aspects of attitudes, while only emotional
reactions are examined in the Semi-projective Scale (Weller & Aminadav, 1992) and the Pictographic Scale (Nowicki, 2005).
Although the Attitude Toward Mental Retardation Scale (Gash, 1993) included items focusing on cognitive, behavioral, and
emotional aspects of attitudes, the items were dispersed across the scale factors (see Table 1). Further, the authors do not
report their intention to measure these aspects, but rather this is a byproduct of the way the items are worded.
Some measures utilized factor analyses to examine scale dimensions. For example, the Attitude Toward Mental Retardation
scale (Gash, 1993) provided psychometrics supporting a four-factor structure; however, these factors were not found in
subsequent analyses in other cultures (Tang, Davis, Wu, & Oliver, 2000). Further, these factors were solely psychometric and
not based on theory. As another example, factor analysis was performed on the Behavioral Intention Scale (Roberts & Lindsell,
1997); however, the results revealed only one factor and no multi-dimensions.
The Mental Retardation Attitude Inventory (MRAI) has been one of the most widely employed attitude measures in the ID
eld to date (Antonak & Harth, 1994). The MRAI was developed to measure cognitive, affective, and behavioral aspects of
attitude and based on a preexisting scale of attitudes toward racial minority groups (Harth, 1974). As a result, the subscales
in the MRAI (social distance, integrationsegregation, private rights, and subtle derogatory beliefs) are more comprehensive
than those used in previous scales as items relate to different contexts in which stigma might occuri.e., the school context,
employment, and living arrangements. The items on the ADS (Power et al., 2010) were also developed based on the ABC
model of attitudes. However, it should be noted that the domains on this scale (i.e., inclusionexclusion, discrimination,
gains, and hope) are not strictly the domains of cognition, emotion, and behavior.

754

Table 1
Summary of scales that measure public stigma in ID.
Authors

Country

Description

Validity and reliability

Items and scoring

Published studies

Siperstein (1990)

USA

Adjectives depict affective


feelings, physical appearance and
academic and social behavior.
Three factor structure: popular,
rejected and withdrawn child

Developed by asking
students to lists words to
describe someone they
like or do not like

34 Adjectives

Attitude Behavior Scale-Mental


Retardation

Jordan (1969)

USA

Literature. Content,
construct and concurrent
validity.Inter-item
correlations above 0.60

240 Items

Attitudes To Disability
Scale (ADS)

Power et al. (2010)

UK

Literature. exploratory
and conrmatory factor
analyses.Cronbach
a = 0.760.80

16 Items scored on
5-point Likert scale;
ID form uses smiley
faces response format

Power et al. (2010)

Attitude Toward Mental


Retardation

Gash (1993)

Ireland

Validity not clear.


Reliability not reported.
The 4 factors structure was
not repeated in other
studies

20 Yes or no items

Gash (1993), Gash and


Coffey (1995), Tang
et al. (2000)

Attitudes Toward Persons with


an Intellectual Disability
Questionnaire

Rillotta and
Nettelbeck (2007)

Australia

Constructed via Guttmans facet


analysis. Consists of six levels
(societal stereotype, societal
norm, personal moral evaluation,
personal hypothetical action,
personal feeling and personal
action)
Multidimensional scale
composed of 4 subscales
(inclusion-exclusion,
discrimination, gains and hopes)
measuring attitudes toward
people with disabilities. General
form and personal form
Four-factor questionnaire: social
concern with children with MR;
willingness to form a close
relationship or identify with a
hypothetical child with MR
coming to their class; sociability
toward children with MR;
concern with schooling
arrangements
Unidimensional scale measuring
students attitudes toward
people with ID

Bak and Siperstein (1986),


Castago (2001), Elam and
Sigelman (1983), Hemphill
and Siperstein (1990), Manetti,
Schneider, and Siperstein (2001),
Sigelman and McGrail (1985),
Siperstein and Bak (1985),
Siperstein, Bak, and OKeefe
(1988), Wisely and Morgan (1981)
Kenneth (1970), Zwiebel (1987)

Validity not clear.


Cronbach a = 0.89

Rillotta and
Nettelbeck (2007)

Behavioral Intent Scale

Roberts and
Lindsell (1997)

Australia

31 Items with
maximum score of 124.
Frequency of contact
rated on 5-point scale;
other items use a
4-point Likert scale
10 Items scored on a
4-point scale from
denitely no to
denitely yes

Unidimensional measure of
childrens behavioral intentions
toward a hypothetical classmate.
Items range from very little
involvement to more intimate
behavioral intent. Scale originally
developed for physical
disabilities

Based on previous
measure of friendship.
Face validity.
a = 0.840.93

Nowicki (2005),
Laws and Kelly (2005)

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

Scale
Adjective Checklist to Measure
Childrens Attitudes

McConkey (1990)

Ireland

Mental Retardation Attitude


Inventory-Revised (MRAI-R)

Antonak and
Harth (1994)

USA

Modern and Classical Attitudes


Scales Toward People with ID

Akrami et al. (2006)

Sweden

Peer Attitudes Toward the


Handicapped Scale (PATHS)

Bagley and
Green (1981)

USA

Pictographic Scale

Nowicki (2005)

Canada

Scale of Childrens Attitudes


Toward Handicaps (SCATE)

Hagan (1980)

USA

Semantic Differential Scale

Fenrick and
Petersen (1984)

USA

Semi projective emotional


attitudes toward mild and
severe mental retardation

Weller and
Aminadav (1989)

Israel

Social Distance

Fenrick and
Petersen (1984)

USA

Ratings of condence in meeting


and working with people with ID
in three areas: contact, reactions
to contact and willingness to
have contact.
Multidimensional scale
composed of 4 subscales (social
distance, integrationsegregation, private rights and
subtle derogatory beliefs)
measuring attitudes toward
mental retardation

Classical items (8) tap direct or


open prejudice; Modern items
(11) tap covert and subtle
prejudice
Children rate where they think a
ctional child described should
work (with them or somewhere
else). Items measure 3 subscales:
physical, learning (describe
behaviors common in children
with learning disabilities, and
behavioral)
Measures affective aspects of
attitudes by tapping into
childrens feelings about a target
child
Three behavioral intentions are
examined: friendship, socialdistance and subordinatesuperordination for different
disabilities. Participants are
presented with a drawing of two
gures and a brief narrative
description and asked to indicate
the way they think the nondisabled gure might react to the
gure with a disability
Rate pairs of opposing
descriptions (e.g., helpful-not
helpful; friendly-not friendly)
Multidimensional scale
consisting of 9 categories
measuring attitudes toward
persons with mental disabilities

Measure of the degree to which


participants would accept a child
in various social activities

Reliability and validity not


reported in the
manuscript

5 Items rated on a
5-point scale from
denitely yes to
denitely no

McConkey and
Truesdale (2000)

Literature and existing


scale. Construct validity.
Factor analyses. Cronbach
a = 0.91 for the overall
scale; lower internal
consistencies for subscales
(Krajewski & Flaherty,
2000)

29 Items scored on
4-point Likert scale

Partial discriminant
validity (not for all items
on classic scale). Construct
validity. Cronbach a = 0.63
Construct and factorial
validity. Split half
coefcient = 0.89;
testretest
coefcient = 0.75.
Conrmatory factor
analysis supported three
dimensions
Validity not clear.
a = 0.840.93

19 items

Al-Kandari and Salih (2010),


Antonak and Harth (1994),
Hampton and Xiao (2007,
2008), Horner-Johnson
et al. (2002), Krajewski and
Flaherty (2000), Ouellette-Kuntz,
Burge, Brown, and Arseault
(2010), Yazbeck, McVilly, and
Parmenter (2004)
Akrami et al. (2006)

30 Items rated on
5-point scale from
work with me to
stay at home

Clunies-ross and Thomas (1986),


Laws and Kelly (2005)

Nowicki (2005)

Face validity. Split-half


reliability = 0.83;
Cronbach a = 86;
testretest = 0.67

5 Items rated using


drawings ranging from
very negative to
very positive affect
6 Items for each type of
disability. Includes
drawing for each
disability

Validity not clear.Internal


consistency = 0.86

27 Items rated on a
6-point scale

Townsend, Wilton,
and Vakilirad (1993)

Validity not clear.


Reliability not reported

54 Statements are
selected to represent
one of three categories:
severe MR, mild MR or
physically
handicapped
6 Items

Weller and Aminadav (1992)

Internal consistency = 0.67

Miller and Loukellis (1982),


Young (1997)

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

Condence at Meeting Items

Townsend et al. (1993)

755

756

Table 1 (Continued )
Scale

Authors

Country

Description

Validity and reliability

Items and scoring

Published studies

Tachibana and Watanabes


scale

Tachibana and
Watanabe (2004a,
2004b)

Japan

Validity not clear.


Reliability not reported

16 Items rated on
5-point Likert scale,
uncertain response
was assigned 0

The Test of Knowledge About


Mental Retardation

Antonak (1994)

USA

Score condensed into 4 groups:


social services for people with
ID; anti-social norm; prosocial norm and independent
life
An error-choice questionnaire,
introduced as knowledge about
mental retardation
questionnaire, measuring
attitudes toward mental
retardation

Literature. Face validity.


Reliability reported as
acceptable

40 Items with
four-option multiple
choice format and
dichotomous questions

Tachibana and
Watanabe (2004a, 2004b),
Tachibana (2005), Tachinaba
(2006) (all studies based
on same sample)
Antonak (1994)

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

Note. Literature = construction of scale based on literature review. MR = mental retardation.

Table 2
Summary of scales that measure self stigma in ID.
Authors

Country

Description

Power et al. (2010)

UK

See description provided in Table 1.

Cooney et al. (2006)

UK

The Social Comparison


Scale

Dagnan and
Sandhu (1999)

UK

Stigma Scale

Szivos (1991)

UK

Items assess the frequency


participants report experiencing
stigmatized treatment from key
gures in their lives (8) and on
nonthreatening experiences (5).
Adapted from Gilbert and Allen
(1994) to assess individuals
comparison of themselves to others.
Statements begin with when I am
with other people, I generally
feel. . .. Two factors were found:
social attractiveness and
achievement and group belonging.
Assesses participants perceptions of
their own stigmatization on 3 factors
(Feeling Different, Anxiety and Poor
In-Group Concept).

Tool to Measure
Self-Perceived
Stigma

Ali et al. (2008)

UK

Two dimensional scale of perceived


stigma (perceived discrimination
and reaction to discrimination).

Validity and reliability

Items and scoring

Published studies
Power et al. (2010)

No consultation with individuals


with ID. Validity not
reported.a = .48 - .63

13 Items scored on 5-point


Likert scales

Cooney et al. (2006)

Good total item correlation


(0.28)a = 0.56

6 Bipolar items rated on a


12.5 cm visual analogue scale

Cooney et al. (2006),


Dagnan and Sandhu (1999),
Dagnan and Waring (2004)

Items derived from individuals


with ID and parents perspectives
and from a previous self-image
questionnaire. Factor analyses.
a= .75- .81. Good item total
correlations (.34 - .62)
Literature. Consultation with
individuals with ID. Face and
content validity. Internal
reliability a=.84; test-retest
Kappa .72 and .69

10 Items scored using a 5-pint


Likert scale and blocks of
increasing size

Abraham, Gregory, Wolf,


& Pemberton (2002),
Dagnan and Waring, 2004,
Szivos (1991),
Szivos-Bach (1993)

10 Items with photo images;


Yes-No response with
drawing of thumbs up and
thumbs down.

Ali et al. (2008)

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

Scale
Attitudes To Disability
Scale (ADS)
Experience of Stigma
Checklist

Note. Literature = construction of scale based on literature review.

757

758

Scale

Authors

Country

Description

Validity and reliability

Items and scoring

Published studies

Afliate Stigma Scale

Mak and
Cheung (2008)

China

Literature. Concurrent and


construct validity. a = 0.940.95

22 Items scored on a 4-point


Likert scale (strongly disagree to
strongly agree)

Mak and
Cheung (2008)

Caregivers Burden Scale,


Stigma Domain

Chou et al. (2009)

Taiwan

Good construct validity


(.45 inter-domain
correlation).a = .88- .95

2 items scored on a 5-point Likert


scale (never to always)

Chou et al. (2009)

Parent-Perceived
Reactions of Others

Baxter (1989)

Australia

Multidimensional scale
composed of 3 subscales
(cognitive, affective and
behavioral) measuring
internalization of stigma in
caregivers of people with
disabilities
The domain of stigma measures
the degree of stigma that carers
perceived because of having a
family member with ID.
Parents are asked if people react
to their child in any of the listed
ways and if this reaction
distresses them.

Validity by correlation with


stress. No information on
reliability.

8 items rated on 3-point rating


scales.

Baxter (1989)

Note. Literature = construction of scale based on literature review.

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

Table 3
Summary of scales that measure family stigma in ID.

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

759

Finally, we also found that several of the public attitude scales reviewed did not focus on cognition, emotional and
behavioral aspects at all. For example, McConkey and Truesdales (2000) measure focused on attitudes related to condence
when meeting a person with ID. A closer review of the Attitudes Toward Persons with an Intellectual Disability Questionnaire
(Rillotta & Nettelbeck, 2007) found that despite its general title, the items are generally limited to only school inclusion. The
Adjective Checklist to Measure Childrens Attitudes (Siperstein, 1980) was found to have three factors (popular, rejected, and
withdrawn child) and not based on a specied theory of attitude formation. Finally, the items on the relatively recent Modern
and Classical Attitudes Scale were structured to measure two forms of stigma a classical form and a modern form, which
arise from peoples tendency to present themselves as socially or politically correct. The classical form is dened as overt
and blatant and taps direct prejudice. The modern form is more subtle and covert and is characterized by denial of continued
discrimination, antagonism, and resentment (Akrami et al., 2006).
3.1.3. Feasibility and appropriateness of public stigma scales
Regarding the length of the scales, ve of the public stigma scales were relatively short (less than 10 items) and nine scales
seem fairly feasible although they include a larger number of items (between 16 and 31 items). A few other scales seem too
lengthy for completion (e.g., the Semi-projective Emotional Attitude and the Attitude Behavior Scale). Feasibility in some of the
scales is attained by use of drawings (SCATE, Hagan, 1980), smiley faces (Power et al., 2010) or yes or no answer format
(Gash, 1993). Items in the Test of Knowledge about Mental Retardation (Antonak, 1994) are aimed at testing knowledge;
however, some individuals might not like the idea of their knowledge being examined.
In terms of appropriateness of the scales, it is important to acknowledge that some of the scales had their last
reported usages (according to our review) more than a decade ago (e.g., The Test of Knowledge about Mental Retardation,
Semi-projective Emotional Attitudes, Attitude Behavior Scale, SCATE), calling into question the relevance of some of the
items in todays society. The public attitude scales we reviewed had been constructed and used primarily in western
countriesnamely, the United States, Australia, Ireland, and Canada. Some instruments were used only by the research
group that developed them. Only a few of the scales were found to have been used in various countries (i.e., ADS, MRAI).
Six of the scales were limited to school aged children and two scales were appropriate only for adult populations. The
ADS scale was utilized only on a sample of individuals with ID and was not utilized on individuals without disabilities
(Power et al., 2010).
3.2. Self-stigma scales
3.2.1. Reliability and validity
Three of the self-stigma scales were found to have moderate internal consistency estimates, and two report low internal
consistency. Reliability of the Tool to Measure Self Perceived Stigma was examined using internal consistency and testretest.
The Stigma Scale was found to have good item total correlations and internal consistency (Szivos, 1991).
The current review elicited fewer self-stigma scales than public stigma. The self-stigma scales reviewed are summarized
in Table 2. Of the self-stigma scales reviewed, only two reported an extensive literature review as the basis for their
construction. Further, some scales (e.g., Experience of Stigma Checklist, the Social Comparison Scale) were developed without
consulting individuals with ID or caregivers. For other scales, like the Tool to Measure Self-Perceived Stigma, support for face
and content validity was demonstrated by consulting with professionals, caregivers, and individuals through focus groups
and pilot studies with individuals with ID (Ali et al., 2008). Input from parents and individuals with ID were also used in the
development of the Stigma Scale (Dagnan & Waring, 2004). The Social Comparison Scale (Dagnan & Sandhu, 1999) was
reported to have a good total item correlation. Finally, no clear validity information about the Experience of Stigma Checklist
(Cooney et al., 2006) was found in our review.
3.2.2. Multidimensionality
Three of the scales reviewed specically addressed stigma in their titles. However, similar to the public attitudes scales,
most self-stigma scales focused only on some of the stigma dimensions (i.e., aspects of awareness, agreement, application,
and hurts as proposed by Corrigan, 2008). For example, most items in the Tool to Measure Self Perceived Stigma (Ali et al.,
2008) focused on the awareness of the individual of stigma. Specic subscales included experience of discrimination and
emotional reaction to discrimination. Items on the Experience of Stigma Checklist (Cooney et al., 2006) were developed
specically for the school context. Items on this checklist represented primarily behavioral aspects of stigmai.e., receiving
stigmatized treatment rather than aspects of internalized self-stigma.
Two self-stigma scales did not directly address stigma conceptualizations (Dagnan & Sandhu, 1999; Power et al., 2010).
The Social Comparison Scale (Dagnan & Sandhu, 1999) was based on cognitive perspectives of social comparison, specically,
people with ID who have fewer opportunities for valued roles may be less able to buffer against the effects of negative social
comparisons (Gilbert & Allen, 1994). Finally, the ADS factors of inclusion, discrimination, gains, and prospects, do not include
items to assess the introversion aspects of self-stigma (Power et al., 2010).
3.2.3. Feasibility and appropriateness
Most of the self-stigma scales were relatively brief in length, ranging between 6 and 13 items. Further, most scales
attempted to accommodate for cognitive decits of the study population by using simplied answer formats, such as yes or

760

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

no response options accompanied by a thumbs up/down drawing and illustrations (Ali et al., 2008), drawings of blocks of
increasing size or a visual analogue (Dagnan & Sandhu, 1999).
Self-stigma scales have been developed as early as 1991 (the Stigma Scale) and as late as 2010 (ADS). All self-stigma scales
were constructed in the UK and all but one (ADS) were used solely in the UK. Further, most of the scales were used with
populations of adults or adolescents and only one was also used with children with ID.
3.3. Family stigma scales
3.3.1. Reliability and validity
A summary of the family stigma scales reviewed is presented in Table 3. Of the three scales presented, two showed very
good internal consistency scores while the third presented no data on reliability (Baxter, 1989). In terms of their validity,
construction of the Afliate Stigma Scale (ASS) was based on an extensive literature review on associate stigma as well as on
several focus groups. Further, authors used a factor analysis in order to determine the factor structure of the scale and found a
one-factor solution. Both concurrent and construct validity were reported for this scale (Mak & Cheung, 2008). Good
construct validity was also reported for the two stigma items employed within the study by Chou et al. (2009). In the Parent
Perceived Reactions of Others (Baxter, 1989) scale the only validity ascertained was that parental stigma was found to be
correlated with parental stress.
3.3.2. Multidimensionality
The ASS was constructed based on the conceptualizations of family stigma whereby afliate stigma was proposed to
consist of three interlocking components of cognitions, affect, and behavioral responses. However, factor analyses suggested
a one-factor solution (Mak & Cheung, 2008). Within the Chou et al. (2009) study no specic stigma scale was used, but rather
two items out of a caregiver burden interview were given to examine the degree to which the caregivers felt ashamed of
having a family member with ID; thus focusing only on emotional aspects. Finally, the scale developed by Baxter (1989) was
based on a stress process framework thought to be part of negative life experience such as stigma. This scale was based on
families perceptions of stigma from others rather than on the familys internalized stigma and the scale items measure only
a single dimension.
3.3.3. Feasibility and appropriateness
All three family stigma scales seem to be of feasible length. All items are clearly worded and rated on a Likert-type scale. It
should be noted that most items on the Afliate Stigma Scale are worded negatively. Two of the family stigma scales have
been very recently developed, while the Parent-Perceived Reactions of Others is somewhat older (Baxter, 1989). In terms of
cross-cultural appropriateness, two scales were developed and employed solely in Asian countries and the third in Australia.
As for population, Mak and Cheungs (2008) study focused solely on parents of children with ID. Baxter (1989) examined
parents of children ages three to 19, and the scale was not used, to the best of our knowledge, in any additional studies.
4. Discussion
The current systematic review summarizes the existing scales used to measure stigma and attitudes in the eld of ID.
Most scales target the measure of public attitudes, fewer measure self-stigma, and even fewer measure family stigma.
Several explanations can be provided for this nding. First, similar to the eld of MI, the concept of public stigma appears to
be much further developed conceptually than the concepts of self and family stigma, affording more clarity and ease for the
development of public measures. Second, reports of self-stigma and family stigma might be more affected by social
desirability and other methodological biases, which render the use of qualitative methodology easier to employ than the use
of quantitative scales. Third, recruitment of participants to complete public stigma scales is usually easier as these can be
completed by any members of the general public (e.g., college students) rather than the need to recruit individuals with ID or
their family members. This review highlights the theoretical and methodological limitations of available scales.
4.1. Theoretical limitations of existing scales
We found that most of the public scales reviewed are based on general attitude conceptualization, while self and family
scales employ a stigma conceptualization. This might be the result of family and self-stigma measures being relatively more
recent than the public attitudes measures. However, although most attitude denitions conceptualize attitudes as composed
of three elements, (affect, behavior, and cognition) almost all scales did not reect all these elements in their measurement,
but rather focused on only one or two components a problem which also exists in some of the MI stigma instruments.
Regarding self-stigma, scales have mainly focused on measuring perception or experiences of stigma rather than
internalization of stigma.
An additional interesting nding is that there appears to be a difference in interest in stigma among countries. While
many western countries (e.g., the USA, the UK, and Australia) have shown an interest in public attitudes, scales focusing on
self-stigma were developed solely in the UK, while scales focusing on family stigma were mainly from Asian countries. This
nding suggests that while many countries are aware of the existence of public stigma toward persons with ID, only a few

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

761

countries examine the ramications on the individual and their family. The relative lack of focus on the individual may be a
result of the contemplation that individuals with ID are not prone to negative effects of stigma (Jahoda et al., 2010), or might
be associated with cultural variations in the social representations of ID. For example, Chinese culture is characterized by
collectivism, or having close linkages between individuals and a greater sense of obligation to the group than to the
individual. Further, Chinese people tend to emphasize the biological roots of ID (Kung, 2001) and may be more prone to
considering children with disabilities as bad seeds and disgraces to their families (Sue & Sue, 1987). As such, they may keep
their children a family secret. Given these beliefs, these families may be exposed to the experience of family stigma (Mak &
Cheung, 2008) while placing only a limited amount of focus on self-stigma. This nding strengthens the need to expand this
line of research and develop culturally sensitive scales.
4.2. Methodological limitations of existing scales
Our review has brought to light several methodological limitations of the existing scales. First, some of the scales are quite
outdated and a number of items may no longer be relevant in present day. Second, family- and self-stigma scales have been
developed more recently, suggesting there is emerging interest in this area, but that scales are still in need of further
development and renement. Third, some self-stigma scales were developed without consulting individuals with ID and
other key stakeholders, limiting their content validity. We encourage additional qualitative research with ID stakeholders,
including both individuals with ID and their families, as a rst stage in scale construction in order to identify the stereotypes
specic to ID. In turn, these stereotypes may inform application of the four stage of self-stigma model as has been proposed in
the MI eld (see Corrigan, 2008).
Fourth, given that most self-stigma and family-stigma scales have focused mainly on the experience of stigma, we argue
that future scales should include measures of actual experiences of stigma (by individuals with ID and/or their families),
perceived public stigma (that may or may not be accurate), stigmatizing attitudes that individuals with ID and their family
members hold toward other individuals with ID, and, nally, self-endorsement of (self-stigma about) oneself or ones family
member. Measuring these different aspects will allow researchers to parse out the relationship between these factors.
Finally, wording on some of the scales (for example, Afliate Stigma Scale) are quite negative. In future scale development
we would advise for the construction of a more balanced scale with both positively and negatively worded items. Given these
limitations and in order to advance our knowledge and research in the area of stigma and ID, we should rst ask ourselves
why this area lags behind the stigma work being done for MI, and how can this gap be narrowed in the future.
4.3. Why has stigma conceptualization been rarely used in the ID eld?
A few hypotheses might be offered to explain why the focus on stigma in the ID eld falls short of the degree of richness
that exists in the MI eld. First, some of the ID scales focus more on social inclusion aspects rather than on stigma. Some
researchers have argued for the need to throw away the term stigma suggesting that it has not provided a rallying point for
collective strategies to improve access or challenge prejudice (Sayce, 1998). It has also been pointed out that there is a danger
that a word which originally meant a mark of disgrace on an individual will emphasize a focus on the psychological
experience of shame and away from the meanings added by later academics and activists, namely social and economic
patterns of exclusion. Thus, it has been suggested that discrimination or exclusion may be better terms (Sayce, 1998).
This might mean that stigma and inclusion are viewed as two separate or even opposing constructs, with the goal of
promoting inclusion of individuals with ID. However, we would like to argue that stigma and inclusion are not opposite but
rather complementary constructs on the same continuum. Indeed, anti-stigma initiatives may be one useful tool to promote
social-inclusion of individuals with ID (Cobigo & Stuart, 2010). For true inclusion to occur, research must be able to map the
stigma within the community (Corrigan, 2002).
Another reason for limited focus on stigma within the ID eld might be the belief held by some that individuals with ID
are spared any negative impact from stigma (Todd, 2000). However, recent literature clearly shows that some individuals
with ID are indeed aware of stereotypes about them and are prone to their negative consequences (Jahoda et al., 2010). Thus,
there is a need for continuing research and scale development focusing on adapting the self-stigma framework to the ID eld.
In this adaptation process, researchers need to be aware of the limitations of employing quantitative research methods
among individuals with ID. Likert scales can be appropriate for individuals with borderline or mild cognitive impairment
(Hartley & MacLean, 2006); however, individuals with more severe ID may not be able to respond in a reliable and valid manner
that is accessible or understood by the researcher (Cummins, 2002). Scales could incorporate a pre-testing protocol to determine
whether, and to what level of complexity, respondents are able to use the scale (Cummins & Lau, 2005), use simple and concrete
wording, and a reduced choice format and illustrations to enhance comprehension (Hartley & MacLean, 2006). Possibly one of
the more challenging tasks for researchers would be in constructing a measure that could be used with individuals who have
lower cognitive abilities and most likely would require a an instrument which is accessible to their limited verbal skills.
4.4. Applying stigma conceptualization to the ID eld
One of the criticisms regarding stigma has been that stigma directs the attention to an attribute within the person rather
than on the individuals who produce the stigma (Sayce, 1998). Thus, this leads to a different understanding of the

762

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

responsibility for the problem, which, consequently leads to a different plan of action (Sayce, 1998) with more focus on the
individual rather than on the larger society and environment. One of the ways of advancing the area of stigma and ID is
applying a clear conceptualization of stigma during scale development.
Currently, most existing scales have focused on the construct of attitude; which, although important, does not
successfully capture all of the steps involved from identifying differences in others and forming attitudes about those
differences to discrimination. Additionally, general attitude research is less focused and specic than the stigma concept. The
construct of stigma seems to be a broader and more dynamic construct through which one can start the investigation of the
impact on individuals with disabilities. Moreover, stigma offers a framework for conceptualizing the formation and
allowance of discriminatory practices based on differential power that exists in social relationships and allows for more
targeted points of intervention.
The current dearth of psychometrically adequate scales based on stigma conceptualization in the area of ID makes it
difcult to examine and consolidate ndings and form empirically based recommendations to alleviate its negative effects.
Thus, it is important to examine what researchers can learn from MI models and how they might be applied to the study of
stigma in the ID world. Applying Corrigan and Watsons (2002), conceptualization of stigma as composed of stereotypes,
prejudice, and discrimination provides us with a possible framework for examining the different elements of stigmatizing
attitudes and their behavioral consequences, in effort to gauge stigma change initiatives. Using this model will make it easier
to examine the phenomena underlying stigma and make them accessible to empirical research.
At the rst stage of research, researchers should attempt to identify the common stereotypes related to ID stigma. This can
be done by employing qualitative research methods including focus groups or face-to-face interviews. We propose that such
qualitative methods should include various stakeholder groups including individuals with ID, family caregivers, and care
providers.
Given the literature provided, stigma may be considered as a process moving from public to the individual levels of self
and family (Larson & Corrigan, 2008; Phelan et al., 1998). Thus, it is important to develop scales to measure each of these
types of stigma. This will allow researchers to examine the associations between different types of stigma and learn about
the impact of stigma on individuals and families, something that the available scales permit only partially and incompletely.
Further, family stigma scales can be employed among various members of the family as it is possible that different
stereotypes are attached to different family members depending on their roles (e.g., parents, siblings, and children).
Through the development of adequate measurement tools one can examine the extent and severity of stigma in a given
society, and with regard to the specic disability group of ID, as it may differ from other disability groups. This will bring
about public awareness interventions in order to promote positive change. Developing measures of stigma based on this
model may prove useful for program evaluation in the eld of ID services, and for assessing anti-stigma interventions.
4.5. Limitations and future directions
The primary limitation of the current review is the possibility that the search was not exhaustive of all relevant literature.
The review was limited to English published studies. Further, for several of the scales detailed manuscripts explaining
questionnaire development were not found.
5. Conclusion
In this article we have summarized the existing literature on ID stigma with special emphasis on the scales available in
the literature. Although a large number of public attitude scales exist, most do not directly address stigma. Further, given the
scarcity of scales in self-stigma and family-stigma, we argue there is a need for additional scale development. We suggest
that three separate scales should be developed; one each for measuring public, family and self-stigma. However, we advise
that the scales should examine similar stereotypes and issues in order to be able to examine the development (or association)
from public stigma to self-stigma and family-stigma.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Nev Jones and Russel Houser for their helpful suggestions on a previous draft of this manuscript.
References
Abraham, C., Gregory, N., Wolf, L., & Pemberton, R. (2002). Self-esteem, stigma and community participation amongst people with learning difculties living in the
community. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 12, 430443.
Akrami, N., Ekehammar, B., Claesson, M., & Sonnander, K. (2006). Classical and modern prejudice: Attitudes toward people with intellectual disabilities. Research
in Developmental Disabilities, 27, 605617.
Al-Kandari, H. Y., & Salih, F. A. (2010). Adapting Antonak and Harths mental retardation attitude inventory for Kuwaits culture. Digest of Middle East Studies, 17, 1
31.
Ali, A., Strydom, A., Hassiotis, A., Williams, R., & King, M. (2008). A measure of perceived stigma in people with intellectual disability. The British Journal of
Psychiatry, 193, 410415.
Angermeyer, M. C., & Matschinger, H. (1996). The effect of personal experience with mental illness on the attitude towards individuals suffering from mental
disorders. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 31, 321326.

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

763

Antonak, R. F. (1982). Development and psychometric analysis of the scale of attitudes toward disabled persons. Journal of Applied Rehabilitation Counseling, 13,
2229.
Antonak, R. F. (1988). Methods to measure attitudes toward people who are disabled. In H. E. Yuker (Ed.), Attitudes toward persons with disabilities (pp. 109126).
New York: Springer.
Antonak, R. F. (1994). Development and psychometric analysis of an indirect measure of attitudes toward persons with mental retardation using the error-choice
method. Mental Retardation, 32, 347355.
Antonak, R. F., Fielder, C. R., & Mulick, J. A. (1993). A scale of attitudes toward the application of eugenics to the treatment of people with mental retardation.
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 37, 7583.
Antonak, R. F., & Harth, R. (1994). Psychometric analysis and revision of the mental retardation attitude inventory. Mental Retardation, 32, 272280.
Bagley, M. T., & Green, J. F. (1981). Peer attitudes toward the handicapped scale (PATHS). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Bak, J., & Siperstein, G. N. (1986). Protective effects of the label mentally retarded on childrens attitudes toward mentally retarded peers. American Journal of
Mental Deciency, 91(1), 9597.
Baxter, C. (1989). Investigating stigma as stress in social interactions of parents. Journal of Mental Deciency Research, 33, 455466.
Beart, S., Hardy, G., & Buchan, L. (2005). How people with intellectual disabilities view their social identity: A review of the literature. Journal of Applied Research in
Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 4756.
Castago, K. S. (2001). Special Olympics unied sports: Changes in male athletes during basketball season. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 18(2), 193206.
Chan, F., Livneh, H., Pruett, S., Wang, C. C., & Zheng, L. X. (2009). Societal attitudes toward disability: Concepts, measurements, and interventions. In F. Chan, E. Da
Silva Cardoso, & J. A. Chronister (Eds.), Understanding psychosocial adjustment to chronic illness and disability (pp. 333370). New York: Springer.
Chou, Y. C., Pu, C. Y., Lee, Y. C., Lin, L. C., & Kroger, T. (2009). Effect of perceived stigmatisation on the quality of life among ageing female family carers: A comparison
of carers of adults with intellectual disability and carers of adults with MI. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 53, 654664.
Clunies-ross, G., & Thomas, M. (1986). Use of the peer attitudes toward the handicapped scale in Australia: Grade differences in the scores of primary school
students. Australia and New Zealand Journal of Development Disabilities, 12, 257261.
Cobigo, V., & Stuart, H. (2010). Social inclusion and mental health. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 23, 453457.
Cooney, G., Jahoda, A., Gumley, A., & Knott, F. (2006). Young people with intellectual disabilities attending mainstream and segregated schooling: Perceived
stigma, social comparison and future aspirations. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 50, 432444.
Corrigan, P. W. (2000). Mental health stigma as social attribution: Implications for research methods and attitude change. Clinical Psychology Science Practice, 7,
4867.
Corrigan, P. W. (2002). Empowerment and serious mental illness: Treatment partnerships and community opportunities. Psychiatric Quarterly, 73, 217228.
Corrigan, P. (2008). A toolkit for evaluating programs meant to erase the stigma of mental illness. IL: Illinois Institute of Technology.
Corrigan, P., & Lundin, R. (2001). Dont call me nuts!: Coping with stigma of mental illness. Champaign, IL: Recovery Press.
Corrigan, P., Markowitz, F. E., Watson, A., Rowan, D., & Kubiak, M. A. (2003). An attribution model of public discrimination towards persons with mental illness.
Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 44, 162179.
Corrigan, P. W., River, L. P., Lundin, R. K., Penn, D. L., Uphoff-Wasowski, K., Campion, J., Mathisen, J., Gagnon, C., Bergman, M., Goldstein, H., & Kubiak, M. A. (2001).
Three strategies for changing attributions about severe mental illness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27, 187195.
Corrigan, P. W., Rowan, D., Green, A., Lundin, R., River, P., Uphoff-Wasowski, K., White, K., & Kubiak, M. A. (2002). Challenging two mental illness stigmas: Personal
responsibility and dangerousness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 28, 293309.
Corrigan, P., & Watson, A. M. (2002). Understanding the impact of stigma on people with mental illness. World Psychiatry, 1, 1620.
Crocker, J., Major, B., & Steele, C. (1998). Social stigma. In Gilbert, D., Fiske, S. T., & Lindzey, G. (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (vol. 2, pp. 504553). New
York: McGraw-Hill
Cummins, R. A. (2002). Proxy responding for subjective well-being: A review. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 25, 183207.
Cummins, R. A., & Lau, A. L. D. (2005). Personal wellbeing index-intellectual disability (English) (3rd ed.). Geelong, Australia: School of Psychology, Deakin University.
Dagnan, D., & Sandhu, S. (1999). Social comparison, self-esteem and depression in people with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 43,
372379.
Dagnan, D., & Waring, M. (2004). Linking stigma to psychological distress: Testing a social-cognitive model of the experience of people with intellectual disabilitie.
Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 11, 247254.
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The social psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Edwardraj, S., Mumtaj, K., Prasad, J. H., Kuruvilla, A., & Jacob, K. S. (2010). Perceptions about intellectual disability: A qualitative study from Vellore, South India.
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54, 736748.
Elam, J. J., & Sigelman, C. K. (1983). Developmental differences in reactions to children labeled mentally retarded. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 4(3),
303315.
Findler, L., Vilchinsky, N., & Werner, S. (2007). The Multidimensional Attitudes Scale toward persons with disabilities (MAS): Construction and validation.
Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 50, 166176.
Fenrick, N. J., & Petersen, T. K. (1984). Developing positive changes in attitudes towards moderatelyseverely handicapped students through a peer tutoring
programme. Education and Training of the Mentally Retarded, 19, 8390.
Gash, H. (1993). A constructivist attempt to promote positive attitudes towards children with special needs. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 8, 106
125.
Gash, H., & Coffey, D. (1995). Inuences on attitudes towards children with mental handicap. European Journal of Special Needs Education., 10, 116.
Gething, L. (1994). The interaction with disabled persons scale. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 9, 2342.
Gilbert, P., & Allen, S. (1994). Assertiveness, submissive behavior and social comparison. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 295306.
Gill, F., Kroese, B. S., & Rose, J. (2002). General practitioners attitudes to patients who have learning disabilities. Psychological Medicine, 32, 14451455.
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Hagan, M. (1980). An examination of the validity and reliability of the scale of childrens attitudes towards exceptionality. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Indiana State University at Terre Haut.
Hamilton, D. L., & Sherman, J. W. (1994). Stereotypes. In R. S., Wyer, Jr. & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed., pp. 168). Hillsdale: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Hampton, N. Z., & Xiao, F. (2007). Attitudes toward people with developmental disabilities in Chinese and American students: The role of cultural values, contact,
and knowledge. Journal of Rehabilitation, 73, 2332.
Hampton, N. Z., & Xiao, F. (2008). Psychometric properties of the mental retardation attitude inventory-revised in Chinese college students. Journal of Intellectual
Disability, 52, 299308.
Harth, R. (1974). Attitudes toward minority groups as a construct in assessing attitudes toward the mentally retarded. Education and Training of the Mentally
Retarded, 6, 142147.
Hartley, S. L., & MacLean, W. E., Jr. (2006). A review of the reliability and validity of likert-type scales for people with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual
Disability Research, 50, 813827.
Hemphill, L., & Siperstein, G. N. (1990). Conversational competence and peer response to mildly retarded children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 128
134.
Hernandez, B., Keys, C., & Balcazar, F. (2000). Employer attitudes toward workers with disabilities and their ADA employment rights: A literature review. Journal of
Rehabilitation, 66, 416.
Henry, D. B., Keys, C. B., Jopp, D., & Balcazar, F. (1996). The community living attitudes scales, mental retardation form: Development and psychometric properties.
Mental Retardation, 34, 149158.
Hilton, J., & von Hippel, W. (1996). Stereotypes. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 237271.

764

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

Horner-Johnson, W., Keys, C., Henry, D., Yamaki, K., Oi, F., Watanabe, K., Shimada, H., & Fugjimura, I. (2002). Attitudes of Japanese students toward people with
intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 46, 365378.
Jahoda, A., & Markova, I. (2004). Coping with social stigma: People with intellectual disabilities moving from institutions and family home. Journal of Intellectual
Disabilities Research, 48, 719729.
Jahoda, A., Wilson, A., Stalker, K., & Cairney, A. (2010). Living with stigma and self-perceptions of people with mild intellectual disabilities. Journal of Social Issues,
66, 521534.
Jordan, J. E. (1969). Guttman facet design and development of a cross-cultural attitudes toward mentally retarded persons scale. East Lansing, MI: Michigan State
University.
Kenneth, M. N. (1970). Attitudes of Texas Mexican-Americans toward mental retardation: A Guttman Facet analysis.
Kodaka, M., Postuvan, V., Ingaki, M., & Yamada, M. (2010). A systematic review of scales that measure attitudes toward suicide. International Journal of Social
Psychiatry published online doi:10.1177/0020764009357399.
Krajewski, J., & Flaherty, T. (2000). Attitudes of high school students toward individuals with mental retardation. Mental Retardation, 38, 154162.
Krueger, J. (1996). Personal beliefs and cultural stereotypes about racial characteristics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 536548.
Kung, W. W. (2001). Consideration of cultural factors in working with Chinese American families with a mentally ill patient. Families in Society, 82, 97107.
Lam, C. S., Tsang, H., Chan, F., & Corrigan, P. W. (2006). Chinese and American perspectives on stigma. Rehabilitation Education, 20, 269279.
Larson, J. E., & Corrigan, P. (2008). The stigma of families with mental illness. Academic Psychiatry, 32, 8791.
Laws, G., & Kelly, E. (2005). The attitudes and friendship intentions of children in United Kingdom mainstream schools towards peers with physical or intellectual
disabilities. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 52, 7999.
Link, B. G., Monahan, J., Stueve, A., & Cullen, F. T. (1999). Real in their consequences: A sociological approach to understanding the association between psychotic
symptoms and violence. American Sociological Review, 64, 316332.
Link, B. G., & Phelan, J. C. (2001). Conceptualizing stigma. Annual Reviews in Sociology, 27, 363385.
Link, B., Struening, E. L., Rahav, M., Phelan, J. C., & Nuttbrock, L. (1997). On stigma and its consequences: Evidence from a longitudinal study of men with dual
diagnoses of mental illness and substance abuse. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 38, 177190.
Mak, W. W. S., & Cheung, R. Y. M. (2008). Afliate stigma among caregivers of people with intellectual disability or mental illness. Journal of Applied Research in
Intellectual Disabilities, 21, 532545.
Manetti, M., Schneider, B., & Siperstein, G. (2001). Social acceptance of children with mental retardation: Testing the contact hypothesis with an Italian sample.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 279286.
McConkey, R. (1990). Community reactions to group homes: Contrasts between people in areas with and without a group home. In W. I. Fraser (Ed.), Key issues in
mental retardation research (pp. 415425). London: Routledge.
McConkey, R., & Truesdale, M. (2000). Reactions of nurses and therapists in mainstream health services to contact with people who have learning disabilities.
Journal of Advanced Nursing, 32, 158163.
Miller, M., & Loukellis, I. (1982). Assessment of rural elementary students attitudes toward the handicapped.
Nowicki, E. A. (2005). Understanding childrens perceptions of intellectual and physical disabilities: Attitudes, knowledge, and social cognition. Exceptionality
Education Canada, 15(3), 2139.
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
Ouellette-Kuntz, H., Burge, P., Brown, H. K., & Arseault, E. (2010). Public attitudes toward individuals with intellectual disabilities as measured by the concept of
social distance. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 23, 132142.
Perkins, T. S., Holburn, S., Deaux, K., Flory, M. J., & Vietze, P. M. (2002). Children of mothers with intellectual disability: Stigma, mother-child relationship and selfesteem. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disability, 15, 297313.
Pescosolido, B. A., Monhan, J., Link, B. G., Stueve, A., & Kikuzawa, S. (1999). The publics view of the competence, dangerousness, and need for legal coercion of
persons with mental health problems. American Journal of Public Health, 89, 13391345.
Phelan, J., Bromet, E. J., & Link, B. G. (1998). Psychiatric illness and family stigma. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 24, 115126.
Power, M. J., & Green, A. M. the WHOQOL-DIS Group. (2010). The attitudes to disability scale (ADS): Development and psychometric properties. Journal of
Intellectual Disability Research, 54, 860874.
Pratt, H. D. (2010). Psychological issues in chronically ill adolescents. In H. Omar, D. E. Greydanus, D. R. Patel, & J. Merrick (Eds.), Adolescence and chronic illness: A
public health concern (pp. 151164). New York: Nova Science Publishers.
Rillotta, F., & Nettelbeck, T. (2007). Effects of an awareness program on attitudes of students without an intellectual disability towards persons with an intellectual
disability. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 32, 1927.
Ritsher, J. B., Otilingam, P. G., & Grajales, M. (2003). Internalized stigma of mental illness: Psychometric properties of a new measure. Psychiatry Research, 121, 31
49.
Roberts, C. M., & Lindsell, J. S. (1997). Childrens attitudes and behavioural intentions towards peers with disabilities. International Journal of Disability,
Development and Education, 44, 133145.
Rusch, N., Angermeyer, M. C., & Corrigan, P. W. (2005). Mental illness stigma: Concepts, consequences, and initiatives to reduce stigma. European Psychiatry, 20,
529539.
Sayce, L. (1998). Stigma, discrimination and social exclusion: Whats in a word. Journal of Mental Health, 7, 331343.
Shin, J. Y., & Nhan, N. V. (2009). Predictors of parenting stress among Vietnamese mothers of young children with and without cognitive delay. Journal of
Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 34, 110.
Sigelman, C. K., & McGrail, L. E. (1985). Developmental differences in evaluative reactions to physically and mentally handicapped children. Journal of Social &
Clinical Psychology, 3, 353366.
Siperstein, G. N. (1980). Adjective checklist (ACL) and friendship activity scale (FAS): Instruments for measuring childrens attitudes. Boston: Center for Social
Development and Education, University of Massachusetts.
Siperstein, G. N., & Bak, J. (1980). Students and teachers perceptions of the mentally retarded child. In J. Gottlieb (Ed.), Educating mentally retarded persons in the
mainstream (pp. 207230). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.
Siperstein, G. N., & Bak, J. (1985). Effects of social behavior on childrens attitudes towards their mildly and moderately mentally retarded peers. American Journal
of Mental Deciency, 90(3), 319327.
Siperstein, G. N., Bak, J. J., & OKeefe, P. (1988). Relationship between childrens attitudes toward and their social acceptance of mentally retarded peers. American
Journal on Mental Retardation, 93(1), 2427.
Siperstein, G. N., Norins, J., Corbin, S., & Shriver, T. (2003). Multinational study of attitudes toward individuals with intellectual disabilities. Washington, DC: Special
Olympic Inc.
Siperstein, G. N., Parker, R. C., Noris, B. J., & Widaman, K. (2007). A national study of youth attitudes toward the inclusion of students with intellectual disabilities.
Exceptional Children, 73, 435455.
Siperstein, G. N., Parker, R. C., Noris, J., & Widaman, K. F. (2011). A national study of Chinese youths attitudes toward students with intellectual disabilities. Journal
of Intellectual Disability Research published online, doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2011.01382.x.
Slevin, E., & Sines, D. (1996). Attitudes of nurses in general hospital towards people with learning disabilities: Inuence of contact, and graduate-non-graduate
status, a comparative study. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 24, 11161126.
Sue, S., & Sue, D. (1987). Cultural factors in the clinical assessment of Asian Americans. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 479487.
Szivos, S. E. (1991). Social comparison with siblings made by adolescents with a learning disability. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 1, 201212.
Szivos-Bach, S. (1993). Social comparisons, stigma and mainstreaming: The self-esteem of young adults with a mild mental handicap. Mental Handicap Research, 6,
217236.

S. Werner et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 33 (2012) 748765

765

Tachibana, T. (2005). Attitudes of Japanese adults toward persons with intellectual disability: An exploratory analysis of respondents experiences and opinions.
Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 40, 352359.
Tachinaba, T. (2006). Attitudes of Japanese adults toward persons with intellectual disability: Effect of perception concerning intellectual disability. Education and
Training in Developmental Disabilities, 41, 5869.
Tachibana, T., & Watanabe, K. (2004a). Attitudes of Japanese adults toward persons with intellectual disability: Comparison over time and across countries.
Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 39, 227239.
Tachibana, T., & Watanabe, K. (2004b). Attitudes of Japanese adults toward persons with intellectual disability: Relationship between attitudes and demographic
variables. Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 39, 109126.
Tang, C. S. K., Davis, C., Wu, A., & Oliver, C. (2000). Chinese childrens attitudes toward mental retardation. Journal of Development and Physical Disabilities, 12, 73
87.
The WHOQOL Group. (1998a). World Health Organization quality of life assessment (WHOQOL): Development and general psychometric properties. Social Science
& Medicine, 46, 15691585.
The WHOQOL Group. (1998b). Development of the World Health Organization WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment. Psychological Medicine, 28, 551558.
Thomas, A. (2000). Stability of Tringos hierarchy of preference toward disability groups: 30 years later. Psychological Reports, 86(3,Pt2), 11551156.
Todd, S. (2000). Working in the public and private domains: Staff management of community activities and the identities of people with intellectual disability.
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 44, 600620.
Townsend, M. A. R., Wilton, K. M., & Vakilirad, T. (1993). Childrens attitudes toward peers with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 37,
405411.
Weiner, B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility: A foundation for a theory of social conduct. New York: Guilford Press.
Weller, L., & Aminadav, C. (1989). Attitudes toward mild and severe mental handicap in Israel. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 62, 273280.
Weller, L., & Aminadav, C. (1992). Measuring semi-projective emotional attitudes toward mild and severe mental retardation. The British Journal of Mental
Subnormality, 74, 3948.
Wehmeyer, M. L., & Bolding, N. (1999). Self-determination across living and working environments: A matched-samples study of adults with mental retardation.
Mental Retardation, 37, 353363.
World Health Organization. (2001). International classication of functioning, disability and health. Geneva: World Health Organization.
Wisely, D. W., & Morgan, S. B. (1981). Childrens ratings of peers presented as mentally retarded and physically handicapped. American Journal of Mental Deciency,
86(3), 281286.
Yazbeck, M., McVilly, K., & Parmenter, T. R. (2004). Attitudes toward people with intellectual disabilities-an Australian perspective. Journal of Disability Policy
Studies, 15(2), 97111.
Young, H. (1997). Student attitudes toward individuals with disabilities: Inclusive versus traditional classrooms in elementary school. Masters of Science
dissertation, Rochester Institute of Technology.
Yuker, H. E., Block, J. R., & Campbell, W. J. (1960). A scale to measure attitudes toward disabled persons. Human Resources Studies, 5, 14.
Yuker, H. E. (Ed.). (1988). Attitudes toward persons with disabilities. New York: Springer Publishing.
Zwiebel, A. (1987). Changing educational counsellors attitudes toward mental retardation: Comparison of two measurement techniques. International Journal of
Rehabilitation Research, 10, 383389.

You might also like