Psychologically Controlling Teaching
Psychologically Controlling Teaching
Psychologically Controlling Teaching
Eline Sierens
Ghent University
Maarten Vansteenkiste
Ghent University
Psychologically controlling teaching (PCT) refers to the use of intrusive behaviors that pressure students
to act, think, and feel in particular ways. The goal of the present research was to examine the dynamics
involved in PCT. Study 1 examined self-regulated learning and achievement outcomes of PCT, whereas
Study 2 examined antecedents (i.e., individual and environmental pressures). In Study 1, we found that
PCT was related negatively to students use of self-regulation strategies, which, in turn, was positively
related to academic achievement. Students relative autonomy for studying played an intervening role in
these associations. Findings of Study 2 revealed that both pressure from above (i.e., a pressuring school
environment) and pressure from within (i.e., teachers low relative autonomy for teaching) were related
to PCT, whereas pressure from below (i.e., students low relative autonomy for studying) was not. These
associations could be accounted for by depersonalization, one component of teacher burnout. The
discussion focuses on how PCT represents one aspect of the dark side of teaching, which is
understudied in educational psychology.
Keywords: psychological control, teaching style, motivation, self-determination
Most of you scored poorly on the last test, and in the last few
days, you have been behaving like little children. I am disappointed in this class. Such a statement is indicative for teachers use of psychological control. Psychologically controlling
teaching (PCT) refers to the use of intrusive and sometimes
subtle behaviors that pressure students to act, think, and feel in
particular ways. Common to these intrusive behaviors (including guilt induction, shaming, and expression of disappointment)
is that they convey a conditionally approving attitude from
teachers toward students. In this article, we introduce PCT in
the teaching literature, drawing on Barbers work in the parenting literature (Barber, 1996; Barber & Harmon, 2002) and
self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 2000; Vansteenkiste, Niemiec, & Soenens, 2010).
Parental psychological control essentially involves a conditionally approving attitude toward children, where parents withdraw
love and appeal to feelings of guilt and shame when children fail
to succeed at parentally valued tasks (Assor, Roth, & Deci, 2004;
Barber & Xia, in press). Numerous studies have shown that psychologically controlling parenting is related to maladaptive developmental outcomes in children and adolescents and to internalizing problems in particular (Soenens, Luyckx, Vansteenkiste,
Duriez, & Goossens, 2008). In the academic domain, psycholog108
Self-Determination Theory
Basic Psychological Needs
Self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 2000) is a broad
theory on motivation and personality development that has been
applied intensively in educational psychology (Niemiec & Ryan,
2009; Reeve, 2009; Vansteenkiste, Lens & Deci, 2006). At the
heart of SDT is the formulation of three basic psychological needs,
that is, the needs for autonomy (i.e., experiencing a sense of
volitional and psychological freedom), competence (i.e., experiencing personal effectiveness), and relatedness (i.e., experiencing
closeness and mutuality in interpersonal relationships). When satisfied, these needs would provide energy and foster well-being,
adjustment, and high-quality motivation. In contrast, thwarted
needs would deplete individuals energy and vitality and would
undermine their well-being and motivation.
109
110
SOENENS ET AL.
ifications for their motivational orientation and subsequent learning strategies and achievement (Niemiec & Ryan, 2009; Vansteenkiste, Zhou, et al., 2005). In SDT, the quality of motivation is
conceptualized in terms of the degree to which motivation is
autonomous (vs. relatively more controlled) in nature. Autonomously motivated students learn in a self-endorsed or volitional
fashion because of the perceived value (i.e., identified regulation)
or because of the inherent satisfaction (i.e., intrinsic motivation) of
the learning activity (Ryan & Connell, 1989). In contrast, students
with a controlled motivation learn to meet external (e.g., rewards,
external regulation) or internal (e.g., feelings of guilt, introjected
regulation) pressures (Ryan, Connell, & Grolnick, 1992). Several
studies demonstrated that an autonomous, relative to a controlled,
study motivation is associated positively with students use of
effective and thorough strategies of self-regulated learning such as
time management and deep-level cognitive processing (Vansteenkiste, Sierens, Soenens, Luyckx, & Lens, 2009) and grades (Black
& Deci, 2000).
Whereas need-supportive teaching would foster a relatively
more autonomous motivation in students, need-frustrating teaching is thought to relate negatively to relative autonomy for studying (Niemiec & Ryan, 2009). Consistent with this hypothesis,
research has indeed shown positive associations between teacher
autonomy support and autonomous (relative to controlled) motives
for studying (Vansteenkiste, Zhou, et al., 2005). Few studies,
however, have examined how student perceptions of autonomysuppressing teacher practices relate to students quality of motivation. Herein, we propose that perceived PCT, as it represents a
feature of need-frustrating teaching, would relate negatively to
autonomous (relative to controlled) motivation for studying and to
subsequent self-regulated learning strategies and grades.
Method
Participants and procedure.
The sample comprised 533
adolescents (11th and 12th grade; 41.2% boys) from nine secondary schools from the academic track in Flanders (Belgium). Participants mean age was 16.9 years (SD 0.7 years). The questionnaires were administered during a class period of 50 min,
with the first author being present to answer questions. One or two
teachers were also present during the administration of the surveys.
Students permission to participate in the study and for researchers
to scrutinize their exam scores was obtained through a procedure
of passive informed consent. Specifically, the researcher orally
explained the purpose of the study and the necessity to use students exam scores. Confidential treatment of the data was guaranteed. All students were told they could refuse participation and
deny permission for their exam scores to be used in the study by
filling out a form. None of the students chose to do so. A total of 511
exam scores were provided by the school board 1 month later.
Measures. All questionnaires in Study 1 and Study 2 were
translated from English to Dutch, the participants mother tongue,
using the guidelines of the International Test Commission
(Hambleton, 1994). All scales used a 5-point Likert-type scale,
ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), unless
111
Table 1
Correlations Among and Means, Standard Deviations, and Gender Differences for Study Variables in Study 1 (N 503)
Variable
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
PCT
Relative autonomy
Elaboration
Organization
Critical thinking
Meta-cognitive self-regulation
Time and study environment
Academic achievement
SD
Female
M
SD
Fa
.22 .05
.08
.08
.15 .27 .14
2.28 0.71
2.50 0.73
.26
.06
.24
.30
.39
.22 1.80 3.36 2.26 3.16
.52 .53
.50
.28
.07
3.18 0.60
3.12 0.62
.16
.33
.20 .04
3.08 0.84
2.90 0.87
.35
.13
.01
2.78 0.74
2.94 0.78
.48
.23
3.19 0.49
3.14 0.49
.26
3.10 0.66
3.00 0.65
66.47 7.16 65.15 7.13
2.13
1.48
3.22
3.20
2.66
3.23
3.17
67.40
0.66
3.47
0.58
0.80
0.69
0.49
0.65
7.06
37.25
6.90
3.77
16.51
18.85
4.68
8.56
12.55
Total
M
SD
Male
M
p .05. p .01.
SOENENS ET AL.
112
Table 2
Descriptive Statistics of the Psychologically Controlling Teaching Items: Student Report
Student responses (Study 1)
Kurtosis
Item: My teachers . . .
Skewness
Skewness
Kurtosis
2.09
2.03
0.89
0.76
0.34
0.03
2.08
2.35
0.59
0.34
0.35
0.94
2.78
2.54
2.42
1.78
2.30
0.24
0.37
0.42
1.33
0.60
0.85
0.41
0.67
1.61
0.37
1.77
2.67
1.98
1.23
1.74
1.15
0.01
0.66
3.40
0.87
1.15
0.82
0.58
13.28
0.29
Note. The teacher report of the Psychologically Controlling Teaching Questionnaire is analogous to the student report. For example, one item from the
teacher report version is I always try to change my students.
nal regulation 2; introjected regulation 1; identified regulation 1; and intrinsic motivation 2we summed these
weighted scores to create an index of relative autonomy for studying ( .78; see, e.g., Niemiec et al., 2006, for this procedure).
Deep-level cognitive strategy use. Participants were administered three scales from the Motivated Strategies for Learning
Questionnaire (MSLQ; Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, & McKeachie,
1991), namely, Elaboration (six items, e.g., When reading for
classes, I try to relate the material to what I already know;
.56), Organization (four items, e.g., I make simple charts, diagrams, or tables to help me organize course material; .67),
and Critical Thinking (5 items, e.g., I treat the course material as
a starting point and try to develop my own ideas about it;
.75). Rather than tapping into participants use of deep-level cognitive learning strategies for a specific course, as is commonly
done, we assessed participants deep-level learning in general.
Meta-cognitive strategy use. We measured students use of
meta-cognitive strategies using the subscales for Meta-Cognitive
Self-Regulation (12 items; e.g., Before I study new course material thoroughly, I often skim it to see how it is organized) and
Time and Study Environment (eight items; e.g., I usually study in
a place where I can concentrate on my course work) of the
MSLQ. These items also pertained to participants use of metacognitive strategy use in general rather than with respect to a
specific course. The subscales had a Cronbachs alpha of .65
(Meta-Cognitive Self-Regulation) and .70 (Time and Study Environment).
Academic performance. Academic performance was measured by students overall grades at the end of the first semester
(December), which were obtained through the school board.
Grades ranged between 41% and 88%, with a mean of 66.47%
(SD 7.17).
Plan of analysis. We examined the proposed model in which
PCT relates to motivation, SRL, and achievement using structural
equation modeling with latent variables (LISREL 8.7; Jreskog &
Srbom, 1996). In line with Holmbecks (1997) recommendations,
we tested the following models: (a) direct-effects models, (b)
full-mediation models, and (c) partial-mediation models. Full mediation is demonstrated when the addition of a direct path in the
third model does not improve fit compared with the second model.
This three-step approach was used to test different portions of our
hypothesized model (see Figure 1). Assessment of model fit was
based on multiple criteria: the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA), the comparative fit index (CFI), and the standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR). A RMSEA of .08 or
below (Hu & Bentler, 1999), a CFI value .90 (Byrne, 1994), and
a small SRMR value (.06; Quintana & Maxwell, 1999) indicate
acceptable fit to the data.
Results
Preliminary analyses. Initial analyses focused on the psychometric characteristics of the newly developed instrument and
associations with related constructs.
Reliability and validity of the PCT ScaleStudent Report.
To ensure the adequacy of the PCT Scale as a measure of perceived teacher psychological control, we investigated (a) the factorial validity and the internal consistency and (b) the external
validity of the scale by calculating zero-order correlations with
more adaptive teaching dimensions, that is, perceived autonomy
support, structure, and involvement.
To examine the factorial validity of the PCT Scale, we subjected
items to a principal component analysis. This analysis revealed one
clear factor, accounting for 46% of the variance, with all item
loadings above 0.58 (eigenvalue of 3.20). Cronbachs alpha was
.80. PCT was significantly negatively related to autonomy support
(r .44, p .01), structure (r .38, p .01), and involvement (r .29, p .01). PCT and autonomy support were more
strongly negatively related than PCT and involvement (Fisher z
3.92, p .001) and PCT and autonomy support were somewhat
more strongly related than PCT and structure (Fisher z 1.67,
p .09).
Correlational analyses and descriptive statistics. Correlation coefficients among the study variables are presented in Table
1. PCT was significantly negatively related to students relative
autonomy for studying, to the two meta-cognitive strategy use
subscales, and to academic achievement. Unexpectedly, PCT was
unrelated to the use of deep-level cognitive strategies. Further,
students relative autonomy for studying was significantly positively related to all SRL strategies, except for elaboration, and to
academic achievement. Finally, the two meta-cognitive strategy
use subscales, but not the deep-level cognitive strategy use subscales, were significantly positively related to students academic
performance.
Elaboration
Organization
.99
Parcel 1
.84
.62
.79
Perceived
Psychologically
Controlling
Teaching
Critical
SelfThinking Regulation
.54
.35
Deep-Level
Cognitive Strategies
R = .07
Parcel 3
Parcel 2
.52
113
-.07
.25***
-.28***
.68
Parcel 1
Students Relative
Autonomy
R = .11
.93
Parcel 2
.90
Parcel 3
Academic
Performance
R = .11
.36***
.67
.45***
Parcel 4
-.26***
.36***
Meta-Cognitive
Strategies
R = .34
1.00
Grades
.47 .81
Time &
Study
SelfRegulation
Figure 1. Final model of outcomes of psychologically controlling teaching. For reasons of clarity, the effects
of gender are not shown. p .001.
114
SOENENS ET AL.
Summary of Study 1
Study 1 yielded three important findings. First, the PCT scale
proved to be a reliable and valid instrument. Principal component
analysis on the PCT items yielded one clear factor, Cronbachs
alpha of the PCT scale was high, and the scale was significantly
negatively related to dimensions of need supportive to teaching,
and in particular to autonomy support. Second, as expected, higher
PCT was associated with lower SRL and achievement outcomes.
Third, students relative autonomy was found to mediate some of
these associations. PCT was negatively related to relative autonomy for studying, which in turn was positively related to students
use of deep-level cognitive and meta-cognitive strategies. Metacognitive strategy use, in turn, was significantly positively related
to academic performance. In addition, PCT continued to be significantly and negatively related to meta-cognitive strategy use
above and beyond its indirect relation through students relative
autonomy for studying.
Method
Participants and procedure. Teachers voluntarily participated in this study during a faculty meeting or in the staff rooms
of six secondary schools offering the academic track. They were
informed that participation involved filling out a survey on their
job conditions and interactions with students. The teachers were
asked to send this questionnaire to the principal researcher of this
study by regular mail. Three weeks after the initial invitation to
participation, a reminder together with the survey were mailed to
the nonrespondents. Out of the sample consisting of 488 Dutchspeaking Belgian teachers, 317 (65%) completed the questionnaire. Their age ranged from 21 to 61 years, with a mean of 40
years (SD 10.4 years); 62.8% were women. The mean number
of years of teaching experience was 16.8 (SD 10.6 years); 40.4%
115
emotionally drained from my work; .91), whereas Depersonalization assesses the development of an impersonal attitude
towards the teaching job and students (five items; e.g., I feel I
treat some students as if they were impersonal objects; .66).
Results
Preliminary analyses. Correlations among the study variables can be found in Table 3. We included years of teaching
experience in the analyses because past research has shown differences related to teaching experience in our study variables, such
as relationships with students (Pianta, Hamre, & Stuhlman, 2003).
Constraints at work and teachers relative autonomous motivation
were significantly negatively related. No other correlations among
the hypothesized antecedent variables were significant. Constraints
at work and teachers (but not students) relative autonomous
motivation were significantly related to PCT. The three variables
reflecting hypothesized antecedents were significantly related to
both emotional exhaustion and depersonalization, while both of
them were positively interrelated and positively associated with
PCT. Years of teaching experience was negatively correlated with
constraints at work, but positively related to teachers perceived
relative autonomous motivation in students.
Because past research has shown gender differences for several
of the study variables, such as burnout (Grayson & Alvarez, 2008),
we examined mean differences. We inspected gender differences
using a MANOVA analysis treating gender as between-subjects
variable and the sources of pressure as outcome variables. An
overall significant effect was found, Wilks .96; F(3, 305)
3.97; p .01, 2 .04. Follow-up univariate analyses revealed
that female teachers scored higher on perceived constraints at work
than male teachers. A MANOVA with the burnout components as
dependent variables also yielded an overall significant effect,
Wilks .97; F(2, 314) 3.97; p .01, 2 .04. Follow-up
univariate analyses revealed that female teachers scored lower on
depersonalization than male teachers. A univariate ANOVA on
PCT indicated that female teachers scored lower than male teachers. The results are shown in Table 3. Because of the effects of
gender and teaching experience, we included them as control
variables in the primary analyses.
Primary analyses. The actual analyses checked whether the
constructs in the model were measured adequately and whether the
expected associations held among these constructs.
Table 3
Correlations, Means, Standard Deviations, and Gender Differences for Study Variables in Study 2 (N 317)
Variable
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Total
M
SD
Male
M
SD
Female
M
SD
Fa
Emotional exhaustion
.38
.28
1.92 0.81
1.91 0.86
1.93 0.79 0.09
Depersonalization
.39
1.41 0.50
1.51 0.57
1.34 0.45 8.84
Psychologically controlling teaching
1.97 0.58
2.06 0.59
1.92 0.57 4.21
a
df (1, 308) for constraints at work, relative autonomy teacher, and relative autonomy students; df (1, 316) for emotional exhaustion and
depersonalization; df (1, 314) for psychologically controlling teaching.
p .05. p .01.
SOENENS ET AL.
116
Summary of Study 2
The relations between perceived pressure from above (as indexed by constraints at work) and pressure from within (as indexed
by low relative autonomy for teaching) and PCT were mediated by
depersonalization. Pressure from below, as indexed by students
low relative autonomy for learning, was not related to PCT.
Further, pressure from within was the only source of pressure that
was significantly related to emotional exhaustion. Emotional exhaustion was not related to PCT, indicating that it did not function
as an intervening variable in the relationships between different
sources of perceived pressures and PCT.
Parcel 1
Parcel 2
.57
.40
.84
Constraints at
Work
Parcel 1
.93
.14
Parcel 3
Parcel 2
.82
.91
Parcel 3
Emotional
Exhaustion
R = .19
-.22***
Parcel 1
Parcel 2
Parcel 3
.74
.92
.88
.81
-.36***
Relative
Autonomy for
Teaching
.10
-.11
.49***
.30***
-.36***
.51***
Parcel 4
Parcel 3
.71
.89
.89
.57 .73
.81
Students Relative
Autonomy for
Studying
Parcel 1
.65
Parcel 2
Parcel 1
Parcel 2
.70
-.08
.69
Parcel 2
.69
Depersonalization
R = .33
.06
Parcel 1
Psychologically
Controlling
Teaching
R = .35
Parcel 3
Parcel 4
Figure 2. Final model of antecedents of psychologically controlling teaching. For reasons of clarity, effects of
gender and years of teaching experience are not shown. p .05. p .01. p .001.
Parcel 3
General Discussion
The present studies applied the construct of psychological control, which involves the communication of a conditionally approving attitude through intrusive and manipulative practices (including shaming and expression of disappointment) to the teaching
context. Specifically, we examined associations between PCT and
a number of hypothesized antecedents, mediators, and outcomes.
Validity and reliability analyses show that PCT can be assessed as
a reliable and unidimensional factor that correlates negatively with
adaptive teaching dimensions, that is, autonomy support, structure,
and involvement. The remaining part of the discussion is organized
around two important sets of findings, that is, (a) the hypothesized
outcomes and (b) the hypothesized antecedents of PCT. Across the
two studies, we aimed to test the SDT-based notion that controlling
teaching, and PCT in particular, relates to a controlled orientation
in the functioning of both teachers and students.
117
118
SOENENS ET AL.
Practical Implications
Given the harmful correlates of PCT, from an applied perspective, it is important for teachers to refrain from PCT. To modify
psychologically controlling teacher behavior, teachers can be provided with information about what behaviors constitute PCT and
their effects on adolescent learning and achievement. To the extent
that teachers wish to positively influence their students learning,
they can be advised to teach in an autonomy-supportive fashion,
for instance, by explaining the relevance of learning strategies
(Reeve, 2009). It is equally important that pressure on and control
of teachers is reduced, as indicated by the results of Study 2. To
avoid the development of an objectifying attitude toward students,
which seems to catalyze the use of PCT, it is desirable that the
entire educational community and the general public recognize
the complexity, responsibilities, and stresses that are inherent in
the teaching profession so that the pressure from above on teachers
is reduced. To achieve this aim, principals could create an
autonomy-supportive climate in which teachers have a say in
various decisions, a rationale is provided when staff involvement
in decisions is limited, and teachers are offered opportunities for
self-direction and self-initiative. At the intraindividual level, increasing teachers pleasure and importance of their teaching might
be important because teachers motivation relates significantly to
the use of a psychologically controlling stance. Perhaps, it is better
for teachers who primarily teach for controlled reasons to reorient
themselves in their professional career and to choose a job that lies
more in line with their values and interests.
Conclusion
By introducing psychological control as a dimension of teaching
style, our aim was to contribute to both research on parental
psychological control and SDT-based research in educational psychology. Developmental research on parental psychological control has convincingly shown its negative ramifications for childrens and adolescents general well-being and adjustment. Few
studies, however, have examined the implications of parental
psychological control for educational and learning outcomes (see
Aunola & Nurmi, 2004; Vansteenkiste, Simons, et al., 2005;
Vansteenkiste, Zhou, et al., 2005 for exceptions). This research
contributes to the literature by showing that perceived teacher
psychological control is related to a nomological network of maladaptive motivational and learning outcomes in adolescents. At the
same time, this research adds to SDT-based research on teaching
style by highlighting one important feature of need-frustrating
teaching. There is increasing recognition that experiences of need
frustration do not simply reflect a lack of need satisfaction and that
it is important to identify the specific dynamics involved in needfrustrating interpersonal styles, including psychological control
(Bartholomew, Ntoumanis, Ryan, Bosch, & Thogerson-Ntoumani,
in press).
References
Assor, A., & Kaplan, H. (2001). Mapping the domain of autonomy support:
Five important ways to undermine students experience of autonomy in
119
Deci, E. L., Spiegel, N. H., Ryan, R. M., Koestner, R., & Kauffman, M.
(1982). Effects of performance standards on teaching styles: Behavior of
controlling teachers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 852 859.
doi:10.1037/0022-0663.74.6.852
Friedman, I. A. (1995). Student behavior patterns contributing to teacher
burnout. Journal of Educational Research, 88, 281289. doi:10.1080/
00220671.1995.9941312
Grayson, J. L., & Alvarez, H. K. (2008). School climate factors relating to
teacher burnout: A mediator model. Teaching and Teacher Education,
24, 1349 1363. doi:10.1016/j.tate.2007.06.005
Grolnick, W. S. (2003). The psychology of parental control: How wellmeant parenting backfires. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Grolnick, W. S., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1997). Internalization within
the family: The self-determination theory perspective. In J. E. Grusec &
L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and childrens internalization of values:
A handbook of contemporary theory (pp. 78 99). London, England:
Wiley.
Hambleton, R. K. (1994). Guidelines for adapting educational and psychological tests: A progress report. European Journal of Psychological
Assessment, 10, 229 244.
Holmbeck, G. N. (1997). Toward terminological, conceptual, and statistical clarity in the study of mediators and moderators: Examples from the
child-clinical and pediatric psychology literatures. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 65, 599 610. doi:10.1037/0022006X.65.4.599
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance
structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 155. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118
Jackson, S. E., Schwab, R. L., & Schuler, R. S. (1986). Toward an
understanding of the burnout phenomenon. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 630 640. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.71.4.630
Jreskog, K., & Srbom, D. (1996). LISREL VIII: Users reference guide.
Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International.
Kokkinos, C. M. (2006). Factor structure and psychometric properties of
the Maslach Burnout InventoryEducators survey among elementary
and secondary school teachers in Cyprus. Stress and Health, 22, 2533.
doi:10.1002/smi.1079
Lane, K. L., Harris, K. R., Graham, S., Weisenbach, J. L., Brindle, M., &
Morphy, P. (2008). The effects of self-regulated strategy development
on the writing performance of second-grade students with behavioral
and writing difficulties. The Journal of Special Education, 41, 234 253.
doi:10.1177/0022466907310370
Leung, D. Y. P., & Lee, W. W. S. (2006). Predicting intention to quit
among Chinese teachers: Differential predictability of the components of
burnout. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 19, 129 141. doi:10.1080/
10615800600565476
Levesque, C., Zuehlke, A. N., Stanek, L. R., & Ryan, R. M. (2004).
Autonomy and competence in German and American students: A comparative study based on self-determination theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 68 84. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.68
Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F.
(2002). To parcel or not to parcel: Exploring the question, weighing
the merits. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 151173. doi:10.1207/
S15328007SEM0902_1
Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1986). Maslach Burnout Inventory manual
(2nd ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Maslach, C., Schaufeli, W., & Leiter, M. (2001). Job burnout. Annual
Review of Psychology, 52, 397 422. doi:10.1146/annurev
.psych.52.1.397
Moller, A. C., & Deci, E. L. (2010). Interpersonal control, dehumanization,
and violence: A self-determination theory perspective. Group Processes
& Intergroup Relations, 13, 4153. doi:10.1177/1368430209350318
Niemiec, C. P., Lynch, M. F., Vansteenkiste, M., Bernstein, J., Deci, E. L.,
& Ryan, R. M. (2006). The antecedents and consequences of autono-
120
SOENENS ET AL.
mous self-regulation for college: A self-determination theory perspective on socialization. Journal of Adolescence, 29, 761775. doi:10.1016/
j.adolescence.2005.11.009
Niemiec, C. P., & Ryan, R. M. (2009). Autonomy, competence, and
relatedness in the classroom: Applying self-determination theory to
educational practice. Theory and Research in Education, 7, 133144.
doi:10.1177/1477878509104318
Pelletier, L. G., Seguin-Levesque, C., & Legault, L. (2002). Pressure from above
and pressure from below as determinants of teachers motivation and teaching
behavior. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 186196.
Pianta, R. C., Hamre, B., & Stuhlman, M. (2003). Relationships between
teachers and children. In W. M. Reynolds, G. E. Miller, & I. B. Weiner
(Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Vol. 7. Educational psychology (pp.
199 234). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Pintrich, P., Smith, D., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. (1991). A manual for
the use of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (Technical Report 91-B-004). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan.
Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated
learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 82, 33 40.
Quintana, S. M., & Maxwell, S. E. (1999). Implications of recent developments in structural equation modeling for counseling psychology.
Counseling Psychologist, 27, 485527.
Reeve, J. (2009). Why teachers adopt a controlling motivating style toward
students and how they can become more autonomy supportive. Educational Psychologist, 44, 159 175.
Roth, G., Assor, A., Kanat-Maymon, Y., & Kaplan, H. (2007). Autonomous
motivation for teaching: How self-determined teaching may lead to selfdetermined learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 761774.
Roth, G., Assor, A., Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2009). The
emotional and academic consequences of parental conditional regard:
Comparing conditional positive regard, conditional negative regard, and
autonomy support as parenting practices. Developmental Psychology,
45, 1119 1142.
Rozendaal, J. S., Minnaert, A., & Boekaerts, M. (2005). The influence of teacherperceived administration of self-regulated learning on students motivation and
information processing. Learning and Instruction, 15, 141160.
Ryan, R. M. (1982). Control and information in the intrapersonal sphere:
An extension of cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 43, 450 461.
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and
internalization: Examining reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 749 761.
Ryan, R. M., Connell, J. P., & Grolnick, W. S. (1992). When achievement
is not intrinsically motivated: A theory of internalization and selfregulation in school. In A. K. Boggiano & T. S. Pittman (Eds.), Achievement and motivation (pp. 167188). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Schaefer, E. S. (1965). Childrens reports of parental behavior: An inventory. Child Development, 36, 413 424.
Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the classroom:
Reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and student engagement across
the school year. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 571581.
Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic intervals for indirect effects in structural
equations models. In S. Leinhart (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp.
290 312). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Soenens, B., Luyckx, K., Vansteenkiste, M., Duriez, B., & Goossens, L. (2008).
Clarifying the link between parental psychological control and adolescents
depressive symptoms. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 54, 411444.
Soenens, B., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2005). Antecedents and outcomes of
self-determination in three life-domains: The role of parents and teachers autonomy support. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34, 589 604.
Soenens, B., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2010). A theoretical upgrade of the
concept of parental psychological control: Proposing new insights on the
basis of self-determination theory. Developmental Review, 30, 74 99.
Stoeber, J., & Rennert, D. (2008). Perfectionism in school teachers: Relations with stress appraisals, coping styles, and burnout. Anxiety, Stress,
and Coping, 21, 3753.
Taylor, I. M., & Ntoumanis, N. (2007). Teacher motivational strategies and
student self-determination in physical education. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 99, 747760.
Taylor, I. M., Ntoumanis, N., & Standage, M. (2008). A self-determination
theory approach to understanding the antecedents of teachers motivational strategies in physical education. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 30, 7594.
Vallerand, R. J., & Bissonnette, R. (1992). Intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivational styles as predictors of behavior: A prospective study. Journal of
Personality, 60, 599 620.
Vansteenkiste, M., & Lens, W. (2006). Intrinsic versus extrinsic goal
contents in self-determination theory: Another look at the quality of
academic motivation. Educational Psychologist, 41, 19 31.
Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Intrinsic versus extrinsic goal contents in self-determination theory: Another look at the
quality of academic motivation. Educational Psychologist, 41, 19 31.
Vansteenkiste, M., Mouratidis, A., & Lens, W. (2010). Detaching reasons
from aims: Fair play and well-being in soccer as a function of pursuing
performance-approach goals for autonomous or controlling reasons.
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 32, 217232.
Vansteenkiste, M., Niemiec, C. P., & Soenens, B. (2010). The five minitheories of self-determination theory: An historical overview, emerging
trends, and future directions. In T. C. Urdan & S. A. Karabenick (Eds.),
Advances in motivation and achievement: Vol. 16A. The decade ahead:
Theoretical perspectives on motivation and achievement (1st ed., pp.
105166). Bingley, England: Emerald.
Vansteenkiste, M., Sierens, E., Soenens, B., Luyckx, K., & Lens, W.
(2009). Motivational profiles from a self-determination perspective: The
quality of motivation matters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101,
671 688.
Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Soenens, B., & Matos, L. (2005).
Examining the motivational impact of intrinsic versus extrinsic goal
framing and autonomy-supportive versus internally controlling communication style on early adolescents academic achievement. Child Development, 76, 483501.
Vansteenkiste, M., Zhou, M. M., Lens, W., & Soenens, B. (2005). Experiences of autonomy and control among Chinese learners: Vitalizing or
immobilizing? Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 468 483.
Vrugt, A., & Oort, F. J. (2008). Metacognition, achievement goals, study
strategies and academic achievement: Pathways to achievement. Metacognition and Learning, 30, 123146.
Winne, P. H. (1995). Inherent details in self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 30, 173187.
Wolters, C. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (1998). Contextual differences in student
motivation and self-regulated learning in mathematics, English, and
social studies classrooms. Instructional Science, 26, 27 47.
Zimmerman, B. J. (2008). Investigating self-regulation and motivation:
Historical background, methodological developments, and future prospects. American Educational Research Journal, 45, 166 183.