Tmp8e81 TMP
Tmp8e81 TMP
Tmp8e81 TMP
DOI 10.1007/s00265-015-1921-1
ORIGINAL PAPER
Abstract The extended pace-of-life syndrome (POLS) hypothesis suggests that variation in boldness-like behaviors
has co-evolved with variation in life-history strategies within
populations, yet both theoretically driven experiments and
evidence for phenotypic correlations between boldness-like
behaviors and reproduction-related activities are scarce. Here
we test the prediction that more exploratory individuals should
be willing to provide more effort into current reproduction
than less exploratory ones by investigating the association
between exploration behavior and parental effort in wild great
tits (Parus major). To this end, we assessed exploration behavior following a standardized assay. Then, we estimated
individual willingness to provide parental effort into brood
provisioning as (1) individual increase in nest visit rate after
the brood had been artificially enlarged and (2) individual
latency to return to the nest after this manipulation. Fast male
explorers were quicker than slow explorers to return to the
nest after the manipulation. Males paired with a partner of
similar exploration scoreeither a fast or slow female explorer increased their nest visit rate more than males paired with
Communicated by S. Pruett-Jones
* Morgan David
[email protected]
Rianne Pinxten
[email protected]
Tine Martens
[email protected]
Marcel Eens
[email protected]
1
a partner of dissimilar exploration score. The relationship between exploration and parental effort then depended on ones
partners behavior. Our test thus provides only partial support
for the extended POLS hypothesis and highlights the potential
importance of the social environment in shaping the relationship between boldness-like behaviors and fitness-maximizing
traits.
Keywords Brood size manipulation . Pace-of-life . Parus
major . POLS . Parental care . PIT tags . Provisioning .
Partners compatibility
Introduction
The extended pace-of-life syndrome (POLS) hypothesis proposes that between-individual variation in boldness-like behaviors such as exploration, aggressiveness, or risk-taking
has coevolved with between-individual variation in lifehistory strategies (Biro and Stamps 2008; Rale et al. 2010).
Life-history/behavior correlations are thus thought of as being
linked with variation in individual productivity and lifehistory trade-offs (Biro and Stamps 2008). For instance, the
extended POLS hypothesis predicts that more exploratory,
active, or aggressive individuals should show lower survival
prospects (Nicolaus et al. 2012; Auclair et al. 2013) but a
higher metabolism (Careau et al. 2011), higher energy intake
(Carter et al. 2010; David et al. 2011a), higher growth rate
(Biro et al. 2014), and higher short-term reproductive success
(Patterson and Schulte-Hostedde 2011).
So far, the extended POLS hypothesis has received mixed
support with studies showing unexpected opposite patterns
(Adriaenssens and Johnsson 2009, 2011; Smith and
Blumstein 2010; David et al. 2011b; Le Galliard et al.
2013; Bridger et al. 2015), calling its generalizability into
Methods
Study subjects
The data were collected from a suburban great tit population
located on the Drie Eiken Campus of the University of Antwerp, Belgium (51944 N, 42415 E). Circa 140 nest boxes are provided for great tits to reproduce (Rivera-Gutierrez
et al. 2010). Great tits can be identified using metal leg rings
that they receive as nestlings (day 9/10 post-hatching) or using
a unique combination of colored leg rings for adults (RiveraGutierrez et al. 2012). For all adults, one colored ring bears a
passive integrated transponder (PIT) tag (IB Technology,
Aylesbury, UK), allowing for further bird identification using
antennas (Dorset Identification B.V., The Netherlands). Before the reproductive seasons onset, nest boxes were regularly
checked for nest building indications and then checked daily
before the anticipated start of egg-laying until the last egg
hatches.
Individual parents were caught at their nest box for a short
period of time when feeding the chicks at day 9 post-hatching.
Nestlings and unringed or untagged parents were then banded
and PIT-tagged. The number of chicks present inside each nest
(thereafter termed Bbrood size^) was determined on day 9. On
average, brood size in focal nests was 7.00.3 (SE).
Assessment of exploration behavior
Exploration behavior was assessed from the 15th to the 27th
of February 2013, during the winter preceding the reproductive season when the parental effort experiment was conducted. Great tits were taken out of the nest box within which they
were sleeping at night. Birds were immediately brought to an
experimental room following capture and placed alone in a
cage (lwh: 0.830.40.5 m) comprising a small nest box
and ad libitum access to mealworms, sunflower seeds, and
water. The room temperature was 52 C and kept under a
natural light/dark cycle. On the morning following capture,
birds were tested for their exploration behavior in a novel
environment room (lwh: 4.02.42.3 m) comprising five
artificial trees (height diameter: 1.5 0.04 m) with four
branches each (two at 5 cm and two at 25 cm below the
top). This is a standard procedure for assessing exploration
behavior in great tits (Dingemanse et al. 2002; van Overveld
and Matthysen 2010; Patrick and Browning 2011; Nicolaus
et al. 2012). A sliding door providing a direct access from the
cage to the novel environment room was opened by the experimenter while the lights were still off inside the latter. Then,
lights were turned off in the room where the cages were held
and turned on in the novel environment room, which stimulates birds to enter it. Individual behavior was then recorded
for 2 min during which the number of movements between
trees and between branches of the same tree was measured
(thereafter referred to as Bexploration score^). Birds with high
exploration scores are thereafter called Bfast explorers^,
whereas those with low scores are called Bslow explorers^.
Lights from the novel environment room were turned off
again, which makes the bird Bfreeze^ and easy to catch by
the experimenter. Birds were then immediately taken back to
and released at the place they had been caught the day before.
Data processing
We computed a similarity index indicating to what extent both
partners of a given breeding pair are similar in terms of exploration behavior following the formula:
Similarity index
jfemale exploration scoremale exploration scorej
In our sample, similarity indices range from 3 (pairs of
individuals with highly similar exploration scores) to 26 (pairs
of individuals with highly dissimilar exploration scores).
Nest visit rate was computed as the number of minutes per
hour that an individual was detected at the nest divided by the
number of minutes within an hour (i.e., 60). This method has
previously been used in studies of great tits provisioning behavior using the same antennas apparatus (Wilkin et al. 2009;
Patrick and Browning 2011) and is considered to reliably reflect chicks feeding rate (Wilkin et al. 2009). To compute the
difference in nest visit rate before versus after brood size enlargement (respectively pre- and post-enlargement period),
thus controlling for between-pair initial differences in nest
visit rate, we quantified nest visit rate during 2:15 h before
and after the manipulation. The measurement of nest visit rate
after brood size enlargement started 15 min after the manipulation to allow parents to recover from the associated disturbance (see Limbourg et al. (2013)). We chose this duration as
birds were re-detected by the antenna after a median delay of
7.5 min (interquartile range, [3,14]). Eighty-eight percent of
the birds (37 out of 42) were re-detected by the antenna within
the 15-min period following the manipulation. Analyses conducted only with these individuals that were re-detected by the
antenna within the 15-min period substantially yield the same
results (not shown). For each parent, we recorded the time
delay (in number of minutes) it took to come back to its nest
In a second GLMM, we set nest visit rate as a response variable. To this end, we used the cbind command including both the number of minutes per hour that
a given bird had been detected by the antenna and the
number of minutes per hour that the bird had not been
detected by the antenna (see Crawley 2007). Brood size
and the interaction between period (pre- and post-enlargement) and sex were added as predictors. In order
to control for the non-independence of males and females behavior within the same pair and for the repeated measures of the same individual across the two periods, individual ID, nested within pair, was set as a
random intercept factor. We also tested this model with
Bperiod^ added as a random slope factor, as recommended by Schielzeth and Forstmeier (2009). The error structure was set to binomial.
Using correlation analyses, we investigated the potential
relationship between nest visit rate before brood size enlargement on day 13 and the change in nest visit rate following
brood size enlargement following Tu and Gilthorpes (2007)
standardized procedure (David et al. 2012). This is to test
whether the increase in provisioning effort following brood
size enlargement depends on the amount of provisioning provided beforehand. We may indeed expect individuals provisioning at a high rate to be less capable of increasing their
effort following brood size enlargement.
In a third GLMM, we set the log-transformed relative
change in nest visit rate (see above) as the response variable.
Brood size and the interaction between exploration score,
partners exploration score, and sex were added as predictors.
To account for the non-independence of males and females
behavior within the same pair, we added Bpair^ as a random
intercept variable. The error structure was set to Gaussian. We
could not add ones partner relative change in nest visit rate as
a predictor in this model as, due to its very random effect
structure, it would have prevented it from converging properly. We have thus tested the correlation between relative change
in nest visit rate and ones partners aside.
In a fourth GLMM, we tested the relationship between the
relative change in nest visit rate and the similarity index (see
above). The log-transformed relative change in nest visit rate
was defined as the response variable, and similarity index was
set as a predictor. We added Bpair^ as a random intercept
variable and set the model error structure to Gaussian.
Brood size
1.98
0.047
1.46
2.39
1.13
0.77
1.96
0.15
0.050
0.26
0.44
0.73
Results
Nest visit rate significantly increased between the preenlargement and the post-enlargement period (pre-enlargement period: rateSE=0.350.02; post-enlargement
period: rate SE = 0.39 0.03; b SE = 0.18 0.04, Cliffs
= 0.23, 95%CI = (0.14,0.36); 2 = 19.85, df = 1,
P<0.0001; Fig. 2), irrespective of sex (interaction sex
period: 2 =0.77, df=1, P=0.38). However, the effect of
the period no longer remained significant when Bperiod^
was added as a random slope factor in the model (b
SE=0.150.09; 2 =2.54, df=1, P=0.11). Brood size had
a significant positive effect on nest visit rate (b SE =
0.30 0.08; 2 = 11.37, df = 1, P < 0.001; r s (84) = 0.48,
95%CI=(0.28,0.64)). The change in nest visit rate following brood size enlargement was not related to nest
visit rate before the enlargement (r(42) = 0.18,
95%CI=(0.46,0.14), P=0.27; see BMethods^) nor was
it significantly predicted by time delay to come back to
the n est after the m anipulation ( r s ( 4 2 ) = 0 .05 ,
95%CI=(0.36,0.27), P=0.78).
Fig. 3 Relationship between individual exploration score, partners exploration score, and relative change in nest visit rate in males (a) and females (b).
Regression planes represent the models predictions. The relative change in nest visit rate was log-transformed (see BMethods^)
Discussion
The extended POLS hypothesis provides clear predictions about the expected relationship between withinpopulation variation in boldness-like behaviors and lifehistory traits. In the present paper, we conducted a test of
the relationship between exploration behavior and willingness to provide parental effort in a wild great tit population. Below we discuss the scope and limitations of
Fig. 5 Positive relationship
between an individuals relative
change in nest visit rate and its
partners. Each point corresponds
to both partners of a given
breeding pair. The relative change
in nest visit rate was logtransformed (see BMethods^)
our findings and their relevance and implication in relation with theory.
The artificial enlargement of brood size was designed to
stimulate individuals to put more effort into chicks provisioning than they would have normally done. Also, the computation of the relative increase in nest visit rate controlled for
potential initial between-pair differences in provisioning rate,
providing a Bcontrol^ measure to the nest visit rate as quantified after the enlargement. Despite this control, it remains
possible that the relative increase in nest visit rate may have
been influenced by any natural dynamic daily pattern of parental provisioning, something that we were not able to control in this experiment. However, if provisioning rate has been
shown to peak at dawn and dusk in great tits (Patrick and
Browning 2011), the daily pattern of provisioning was shown
not to differ between individuals with varying exploration
behaviors (Patrick and Browning 2011). We thus think that
it is unlikely that the observed relationship between the relative increase in nest visit rate and exploration was the mere
outcome of variation in daily patterns of provisioning. We
believe that the brood size manipulation was appropriate to
identify which individuals and to what extent they were willing to put more effort into parental duties and thus to place
more weight onto current reproduction than others.
Nest visit rate was computed from the proportion of minutes per hour that a bird was detected by the antenna placed
at the nest entrance (Patrick and Browning 2011). Although
this method does not give an exact measure of how much time
parents spend at the nest or any clue about the quantity and
quality of food brought to the chicks, it has already been used
in previous studies (Patrick and Browning 2011) and shown to
reliably reflect chicks provisioning rate (Wilkin et al. 2009).
We are thus confident that our estimate of nest visit rate is a
reliable proxy for parental provisioning rate and possibly for
the amount of food brought to the offspring. The possibility
yet remains that parents visiting more also bring lower-quality
food than others. Unfortunately, this issue cannot be investigated using the present experimental procedures and would
deserve further research.
The average increase in nest visit rate between the two
periods was found to be rather low (4 %) and even nonsignificant when Bperiod^ was added as a random slope factor
in the model, providing at best only little evidence that birds
increased their provisioning rate after the brood size enlargement. Also, several individuals did not increase but decreased
or kept their nest visit rate stable across the two periods (see
Fig. 2). The possibility exists that some individuals may simply have not responded to the manipulation for several reasons: on the one hand, they may not have had sufficient time
to detect and respond to the manipulation (2 h and 15 min). On
the other hand, some individuals may not have responded to
the enlargement because they had already reached their maximum level of effort into provisioning before the manipulation. However, we did not find any significant correlation
between nest visit rate before the manipulation and the increase resulting from the manipulation, which makes the latter
possibility unlikely. It may also be that some birds did not
respond to the manipulation because their partner responded
sufficiently (the reproductive compensation hypothesis;
Gowaty et al. 2007), but we think that this also is unlikely
given the positive relationship that we found between an individuals relative change in nest visit rate and its partners
of partners with similar exploration behavior are better synchronized and (2) better synchronization leads to higher reproductive success remains to be determined. The positive correlation between both partners time delay to return to the nest
and between both partners relative change in nest visit rate,
while confirming previous studies (Hinde 2006; Westneat
et al. 2011), also suggests that such synchronization phenomenon could be at work in our study. Indeed it may be that
social facilitation leads partners to change their nest visit rate
to the same extent and that any sort of social facilitation effect
is more salient when partners are of similar exploration behavior. However, this interpretation remains speculative and more
work is needed to disentangle the complex interplay among
partners behavioral types, provisioning behavior, and reproductive success (Mutzel et al. 2013).
The interaction between individual exploration score and
partners exploration score in determining brood provisioning
effort suggests the importance of the social environment in
shaping the relationship between boldness-like behaviors
and reproduction-related activities, and fitness-maximizing
traits in general (Bergmller and Taborsky 2010; Webster
and Ward 2011). It is especially important as an individuals
reproductive success greatly depends on its partners investment into breeding in biparental care species. Testing for a
positive relationship between boldness-like behaviors and reproductive effort may thus become inconclusive because of
the social environments influence (in the present case, ones
partners behavior). In particular, the importance of behavioral
compatibility for reproductive fitness within breeding pairs
(Spoon et al. 2006) may override the positive association between exploration and provisioning effort. Boldness-like behaviors are known to be substantially affected by the social
environment (Mainwaring et al. 2011; Webster and Ward
2011). We believe that it would be elusive to ignore its influence (be it a constraint or a facilitator) when testing predictions of the extended POLS hypothesis in social species,
especially in a reproductive context where both partners fitness prospects converge. Taking the social environment into
account should involve studying pairing patterns with respect
to boldness-like behaviors and determining the functional relationships between boldness-like traits and fitness (i.e.,
questioning what the factors mediating the link between both
are) (Patrick and Browning 2011; Mutzel et al. 2013).
POLS hypothesis and the multidimensionality
of reproductive investment
Overall, our results provide, at best, partial support for the
extended POLS hypothesis with possible sex effects. Our
findings differ from a previous correlational study in wild blue
tits (Cyanistes caeruleus) that showed a positive relationship
between exploration behavior and brood provisioning rate in
females only (Mutzel et al. 2013). Our results also differ from
another study using a similar experimental design that did not
identify any significant link between exploration and brood
provisioning in great tits (Patrick and Browning 2011). The
possibility remains that various wild great tit populations exhibit different patterns of behavioral correlations depending
on the specific selective pressures or the constraints they face
or on their particular life-histories (Adriaenssens and Johnsson
2009; Patrick and Browning 2011). Finally, in western bluebirds (Sialia mexicana), male provisioning rate has been
found to be negatively related to aggressiveness (Duckworth
2006). In this latter study, the direction of the relationship
between parental effort and boldness-like traits goes against
the extended POLS hypothesis predictions. However, it is
noteworthy that aggressiveness may be related to several functional behaviors that a given male may have to trade off
against one another. In the case of western bluebirds, for example, more aggressive males spend more time defending
their nest against potential predators and competitors
(Duckworth 2006). This can reasonably be considered as
involved). According to the extended POLS hypothesis, proactive individuals should overall invest more into reproduction-related activities
than reactive individuals. In this hypothetical example of a trade-off between parenting and mating effort, proactive individuals are then on average expected to have higher residual values than reactives
Conclusion
To conclude, our findings indicate that both an individuals
behavioral type and its partners can be critical in its decision
to provide more or less effort into brood provisioning. This
interaction is supposed to have a great impact on the relationship between boldness-like behaviors and reproductionrelated activities and thus on the testing of the extended POLS
hypothesis predictions. Future studies should then carefully
consider the social environment (Bergmller and Taborsky
2010) when testing predictions of the extended POLS hypothesis. Further investigations are also needed to integrate various
reproduction-related activities together into a single test of the
extended POLS hypothesis.
Acknowledgments We thank Yannick Auclair, Vincent Careau, FX
Dechaume-Moncharmont, Niels Dingemanse, Claire Doutrelant, Geert
Eens, Wolfgang Forstmeier, Luc-Alain Giraldeau, Carsten Lucass, Karine
Monceau, Wendt Mller, Kees van Oers, Samantha Patrick, Denis Rale,
and Peter Scheys for their help or their comments at any stage of this
study. Financial support was provided by the University of Antwerp
(TOPBOF) and FWO-Flanders (Research Projects G.0130.07 and
G.0280.10) to ME and RP.
Ethical standards This study was approved by the ethical committee
of the University of Antwerp (ID number 2011-31), and it was performed
in accordance with Belgian and Flemish laws. The Belgian Royal Institute for Natural Sciences (Koninklijk Belgisch Instituut voor
Natuurwetenschappen) provided ringing licences for authors and technical personnel.
References
Adriaenssens B, Johnsson JI (2009) Personality and life-history productivity: consistent or variable association. Trends Ecol Evol 24:179
180
Adriaenssens B, Johnsson JI (2011) Shy trout grow faster: exploring the
links between personality and fitness-related traits in the wild.
Behav Ecol 22:135143
Ariyomo TO, Watt PJ (2013) Disassortative mating for boldness decreases reproductive success in the guppy. Behav Ecol 24:1320
1326
Auclair Y, Knig B, Lindholm AK (2013) A selfish genetic element
influencing longevity correlates with reactive behavioural traits in
female house mice (Mus domesticus). PLoS ONE 8:e67130
Auclair Y, Knig B, Ferrari M, Perony N, Lindholm AK (2014) Nest
attendance of lactating females in a wild house mouse population:
benefits associated with communal nesting. Anim Behav 92:143
149
Barba E, Atinzar F, Marn M, Monrs JS, Gil-Delgado JA (2009)
Patterns of nestling provisioning by a single-prey loader bird, great
tit Parus major. Bird Study 56:187197
Barnett CA, Thompson CF, Sakaluk SK (2012) Aggressiveness, boldness
and parental food provisioning in male house wrens (Troglodytes
aedon). Ethology 118:984993
Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S (2014) lme4: linear mixedeffects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1-7
Bergmller R, Taborsky M (2010) Animal personality due to social niche
specialisation. Trends Ecol Evol 25:504511
Biro PA, Stamps JA (2008) Are animal personality traits linked to lifehistory productivity? Trends Ecol Evol 23:361368
Biro PA, Adriaenssens B, Sampson P (2014) Individual and sexspecific differences in intrinsic growth rate covary with consistent individual differences in behaviour. J Anim Ecol 83:
11861195
Bridger D, Bonner SJ, Briffa M (2015) Individual quality and personality:
bolder males are less fecund in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus.
Proc R Soc Lond 282:20142492
Careau V, Thomas D, Pelletier F, Turki L, Landry F, Garant D, Rale D
(2011) Genetic correlation between resting metabolic rate and exploratory behaviour in deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus). JE Vol
biol 24:21532163
Carter AJ, Goldizen AW, Tromp SA (2010) Agamas exhibit behavioral
syndromes: bolder males bask and feed more but may suffer higher
predation. Behav Ecol 21:655661
Cliff N (1996) Ordinal methods for behavioral data analysis. Routledge
Cole EF, Quinn JL (2014) Shy birds play it safe: personality in captivity
predicts risk responsiveness during reproduction in the wild. Biol
Lett 10:20140178
Crawley, MJ (2007) Proportion data. In: The R Book. Wiley, Chichester
David M, Auclair Y, Czilly F (2011a) Personality predicts social dominance in female zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata, in a feeding
context. Anim Behav 81:219224
David M, Czilly F, Giraldeau L-A (2011b) Personality affects zebra
finch feeding success in a producerscrounger game. Anim Behav
82:6167
David M, Auclair Y, Czilly F (2012) Assessing short- and long-term
repeatability and stability of personality in captive zebra finches
using longitudinal data. Ethology 118:932942
Dingemanse NJ, Both C, Drent PJ, van Oers K, van Noordwijk AJ (2002)
Repeatability and heritability of exploratory behaviour in great tits
from the wild. Anim Behav 64:929938
Dingemanse NJ, Both C, van Noordwijk AJ, Rutten AL, Drent PJ (2003)
Natal dispersal and personalities in great tits (Parus major). Proc R
Soc Lond B 270:741747