Personality and Individual Differences
Personality and Individual Differences
Personality and Individual Differences
Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, 1355 Oxford Street, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada B3H4R2
Department of Psychiatry, Dalhousie University, 5909 Veterans Memorial Lane, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada B3H2E2
Department of Psychology, Ryerson University, 350 Victoria Street, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5B2K3
d
Queen Elizabeth II Health Sciences Centre, 1276 South Park Street, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada B3H2Y9
b
c
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 23 May 2012
Received in revised form 17 November 2012
Accepted 21 November 2012
Available online 20 December 2012
Keywords:
Perfectionism
Discrepancies
Social maladjustment
Depression
a b s t r a c t
Perfectionistic concerns (i.e., negative reactions to failures, exaggerated concerns over others criticism
and expectations, and nagging self-doubts) are linked to social disconnection and depressive symptoms.
According to the perfectionism social disconnection model, perfectionistic concerns contribute to social
disconnection (i.e., feeling rejected, excluded, and unwanted by others) which, subsequently, contributes
to depressive symptoms. The social world is replete with chances for interpretations. In interpreting their
social worlds, people high in perfectionistic concerns tend to perceive interpersonal discrepancies, a distressing form of social disconnection that involves perceptions of others as dissatised with them and as
disapproving of them. These interpretations are also conceptualized as having depressing consequences
for people high in perfectionistic concerns. This study tested whether perceived interpersonal discrepancies mediate the relation between perfectionistic concerns and depressive symptoms; 240 participants
were recruited and this mediational model was tested with a four-wave, 4-week longitudinal design.
Structural equation modeling with bootstrapped tests of mediation indicated the perfectionistic concerns-depressive symptoms relationship was mediated by interpersonal discrepancies (even after controlling for perfectionistic strivings). People high in perfectionistic concerns perceive others as
dissatised with them and as disapproving of them. Feeling rejected, excluded, and unwanted by others,
people high in perfectionistic concerns are vulnerable to depression.
2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Perfectionism is a risk factor for and maintenance factor in emotional distress (Stoeber & Yang, 2010). In particular, perfectionism
is linked with depressive symptomseven after controlling for
well-established contributors to depressive symptoms such as
neuroticism (Sherry & Hall, 2009). Although perfectionism plays
an important role in depressive symptoms, the mechanisms
through which perfectionism inuences depressive symptoms
need explication.
1.1. Improving research on perfectionism and depressive symptoms
Notable limitations exist in research on perfectionism and
depressive symptoms. Most research in this area uses cross-sectional or two-wave longitudinal designs. Cross-sectional designs
Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University,
1355 Oxford Street, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada B3H4R2. Tel.: +1 902 494 8070;
fax: +1 902 494 6585.
E-mail address: [email protected] (S.B. Sherry).
0191-8869/$ - see front matter 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.017
depressive symptoms (Hewitt, Flett, Sherry, & Caelian, 2006). However, research seldom integrates these ndings (for exceptions, see
Cox, Clara, & Enns, 2009; Dunkley, Sanislow, Grilo, & McGlashan,
2006). In contrast, our study aimed to provide an integrative model
explaining how perfectionistic concerns (i.e., negative reactions to
failures, exaggerated concerns over others criticism and expectations, and nagging self-doubts) contribute to depressive symptoms
via social problems.
Whereas past perfectionism research often relied on single indicators of study constructs (Sherry, Law, Hewitt, Flett, & Besser,
2008), our study measured constructs with latent variables each
composed of multiple indicators. For example, we operationalized
perfectionism using a perfectionistic concerns latent variable. This
latent variable represents a theoretical and empirical synthesis of
several well-researched dimensions of perfectionism (Dunkley,
Blankstein, Halsall, Williams, & Winkworth, 2000; Stoeber & Otto,
2006). Latent variables also provide a more encompassing assessment of constructs that is not reliant on the properties of any
one single scale. Moreover, latent variables result in more reliable
statistical estimates, take measurement error into account, and allow tests of models with multiple predictors, mediators, and outcomes (Kline, 2005).
We address the above limitations by testing an integrative theoretical model using latent variables and a short-term, four-wave
longitudinal design. Our study thus represents a needed contribution to research on perfectionism and depressive symptoms.
1.2. Perfectionism social disconnection model (PSDM)
The social world is nuanced and replete with opportunities for
interpretations. For example, a supervisor asking a supervisee a
seemingly straight forward questionHow is your thesis
going?may be interpreted in many ways, including a benign
question from a supportive supervisor or a pressure-lled criticism
from a dissatised supervisor. Considered from this perspective,
our social worlds are heavily inuenced by interpretationsand
the interpretations we make can profoundly inuence our mood.
According to the PSDM (Hewitt et al., 2006; see Cox et al., 2009),
people high in perfectionistic concerns are vulnerable to depressive symptoms because they experience social disconnection (i.e.,
feeling rejected, excluded, and unwanted by others). Whether a
person feels a sense of connectedness (e.g., care, nurturance, and
support) or disconnection (e.g., judgment, criticism, and rejection)
depends heavily upon interpretations. People high in perfectionistic concerns tend to perceive themselves as not accepted or not
belonging and to see themselves as consistently falling short of
others expectations (Hewitt et al., 2006; Stoeber, 2012). Feeling
others are dissatised with them and disapproving of them also
appears to have depressing consequences for people high in perfectionistic concerns (Sherry et al., 2008). The sense of close connection to others that is essential to well-being is elusive for
people high in perfectionistic concerns.
Given a social world lled with ambiguities, personality traits
(e.g., perfectionistic concerns) may guide social cognition (i.e.,
how people attend to and interpret social information in their efforts to understand their social worlds). Perfectionistic concerns involve a chronic, dispositional tendency to view others as
hypercritical, demanding, and intolerant of mistakes (Dunkley
et al., 2006; Hewitt et al., 2006). This disposition may inuence
how people high in perfectionistic concerns interpret and make
meaning of their social worlds.
It seems people high in perfectionistic concerns are often
defective detectives when interpreting their social worlds
prone to experiencing interpersonal discrepancies, a form of social
cognition where they conclude they are letting others down.
Consistent with this assertion, perfectionistic concerns and
693
694
.93
Perfectionistic
concerns
(Wave 1)
.79
.72
Interpersonal
discrepancies
(Wave 1)
.67
.96
.68
.17
.10
Depressive
symptoms
(Wave 1)
.76
Perfectionistic
concerns
(Wave 2)
.73
Interpersonal
discrepancies
(Wave 2)
.69
Depressive
symptoms
(Wave 2)
.96
.98
.77
.17
.11
.79
Perfectionistic
concerns
(Wave 3)
.84
Interpersonal
discrepancies
(Wave 3)
.72
Depressive
symptoms
(Wave 3)
.92
.96
.76
.15
.11
.77
Perfectionistic
concerns
(Wave 4)
.82
Interpersonal
discrepancies
(Wave 4)
.69
Depressive
symptoms
(Wave 4)
Fig. 1. Structural model for the perfectionism social disconnection model. Ovals represent latent variables. Double-headed black arrows represent signicant latent
correlations (p < .05). Single-headed black arrows represent signicant paths (p < .05). Standardized path coefcients appear in bold. Italicized numbers (e.g., .69) appearing in
the upper right hand of endogenous variables (e.g., Depressive symptoms [Wave 4]) represent the proportion of variance accounted for by associated exogenous variables.
The COM was changed into a 5-item scale by Cox et al. (2002) by
selecting the ve highest factor loadings from Frost et al.s original,
9-item COM. Cox et al. found the psychometric properties of the
COM improved after it was reduced from 9 to 5 items. The COM
is comprised of items 9, 13, 14, 23, and 34 from Frost et al.s original, 9-item COM. The DAA subscale was not modied. The IP is a
modied (but not shortened in terms of scale length) version of
the parental perceptions subscale of the FMPS.1 For example, As
a child, I was punished for doing things less than perfect on the
parental perceptions subscale was modied to Others punish me
for doing things less than perfect on the IP. Modifying the IP in this
way enabled us to assess perfectionistic concerns apart from childhood history. The COM, DAA, and IP are rated on a 5-point scale from
1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Evidence supports the
reliability and validity of these subscales (Cox et al., 2002; Frost,
Heimberg, Holt, & Mattia, 1993; Sherry & Hall, 2009). Sherry
(2012) found the COM and IP have high alpha reliabilities (.86 and
.81 respectively) and strong correlations (rs = .91 and .68 respectively) with their original subscales. The alpha reliability for the
DAA was .99 in Sherry.
2.2.2. Perfectionistic strivings
Perfectionistic strivings were measured using a 4-item short
form of the personal standards subscale (PS; e.g., I set higher goals
than others) of the FMPS. The PS is rated on a 5-point scale from 1
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The reliability and validity
of the PS is supported by research (Cox et al., 2002). In Sherry
(2012), the PS had high alpha reliability (.87) and a strong correlation with the original subscale (r = .98).
2.2.3. Interpersonal discrepancies
The latent interpersonal discrepancies variable was measured
using three observed indicators and 14 items: A 5-item short form
of the interpersonal discrepancies subscale (e.g., I felt I had disappointed others.) of the Reconstructed Depressive Experiences Questionnaire (RDEQ-ID; Bagby, Parker, Joffe, & Buis, 1994), the 5-item
interpersonal discrepancies subscale (e.g., Were you unable to
reach goals others have imposed on you?) of the Multidimensional
Discrepancies Inventory (MDI-ID; Flett & Hewitt, 2012), and the 4item interpersonal discrepancies subscale (e.g., My best was not
good enough for others.) of the Almost Perfect Scale-Revised
(APS-R-ID; Slaney, Rice, Mobley, Trippi, & Ashby, 2001; see Sherry
1
A cross-sectional study was conducted to assess the psychometric properties of
modied scales we used. This study involved 119 undergraduates (79.0% women).
Participants averaged 20.58 years of age (SD = 4.33). This study is referenced as Sherry
(2012).
& Hall, 2009). We selected these three manifest indicators for our
latent interpersonal discrepancies variable as research suggests
they adequately measure central features of the interpersonal discrepancies construct and cohere together in a valid way (e.g.,
Mushquash & Sherry, 2012). Consistent with our study design,
measures of interpersonal discrepancies were modied to have a
short-term timeframe (i.e., during the past 7 days). The RDEQ-ID
and APS-R-ID are rated on a 7-point scale from 1 (strongly disagree)
to 7 (strongly agree). The MDI-ID is rated on a 4-point scale from 1
(not at all) to 4 (very much). Studies support the reliability and
validity of these subscales (Mushquash & Sherry, 2012). Sherry
(2012) found the RDEQ-ID, MDI-ID, and APS-R-ID have high alpha
reliabilities (.85, .85, .91 respectively) and correlate strongly with
their original subscales (rs = .68, .63, .58 respectively). Discriminant validity analyses indicate interpersonal discrepancies and
perfectionistic concerns are best represented as distinct constructs
(Mushquash & Sherry, 2012). Such results suggest that feeling one
has not achieved others standards (interpersonal discrepancies) is
different from negative reactions to failures, concerns over others
criticism, and nagging self-doubts (perfectionistic concerns).
695
3.3. SEM
SEM analyses used AMOS 7.0. A v2/df ratio around 2, a comparative t index (CFI) and an incremental t index (IFI) around .95,
and a root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) around
.06 indicate good model t (Kline, 2005). RMSEA values are reported with 90% condence intervals (CI). The Akaike information
criterion (AIC) was used in model comparisons. Smaller AIC values
indicate greater parsimony and better t.
3. Results
2.3. Procedure
Table 1
Means and standard deviations for manifest variables.
M
SD
Perfectionistic concerns
FMPS concern over mistakes
FMPS doubts about actions
FMPS interpersonal perceptions
10.01
8.39
4.27
9.57
4.81
3.57
Perfectionistic strivings
FMPS personal standards
12.93
4.27
Interpersonal discrepancies
RDEQ interpersonal discrepancies
MDI interpersonal discrepancies
APS-R interpersonal discrepancies
12.68
8.63
3.43
8.07
6.72
5.64
3.17
6.11
9.81
11.33
3.08
7.50
Depressive symptoms
POMS-D depressive symptoms
SCL-90-D depressive symptoms
DASS-D depressive symptoms
696
Table 2
Bivariate correlations.
Manifest variables
Wave 1
1. Perfectionistic concerns
2. Perfectionistic strivings
3. Inter. discrepancies
4. Depressive symptoms
Wave 2
5. Perfectionistic concerns
6. Perfectionistic strivings
7. Inter. discrepancies
8. Depressive symptoms
Wave 1
Wave 2
Wave 3
Wave 4
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
0.36
0.6
0.18
0.56
0.18
0.62
0.86
0.35
0.62
0.55
0.38
0.85
0.21
0.23
0.63
0.18
0.68
0.52
0.56
0.22
0.57
0.73
0.83
0.35
0.57
0.5
0.4
0.84
0.19
0.24
0.65
0.2
0.66
0.51
0.47
0.06
0.46
0.7
0.82
0.35
0.59
0.53
0.36
0.81
0.2
0.25
0.62
0.18
0.66
0.5
0.48
0.17
0.54
0.66
0.46
0.72
0.23
0.6
0.28
0.65
0.91
0.45
0.67
0.58
0.46
0.9
0.26
0.31
0.68
0.23
0.76
0.57
0.47
0.13
0.48
0.78
0.89
0.43
0.69
0.6
0.44
0.89
0.26
0.32
0.69
0.23
0.72
0.56
0.48
0.23
0.5
0.72
0.49
0.71
0.27
0.5
0.16
0.63
0.92
0.44
0.72
0.51
0.46
0.92
0.28
0.18
0.69
0.25
0.8
0.52
0.48
0.23
0.58
0.78
0.47
0.71
0.28
0.53
0.22
0.63
Wave 3
9. Perfectionistic concerns
10. Perfectionistic strivings
11. Inter. discrepancies
12. Depressive symptoms
Wave 4
13. Perfectionistic concerns
14. Perfectionistic strivings
15. Inter. discrepancies
16. Depressive symptoms
Note: Inter = interpersonal. Testretest correlations are bolded. Bivariate correlations P .13 are signicant (p < .05).
4. Discussion
The perfectionism social disconnection model (PSDM) relates
perfectionistic concerns to depressive symptoms through interpersonal discrepancies. Analyses indicated the measurement model
for the PSDM t the data well. The structural model for the PSDM
t the data well and all hypothesized paths were signicant. Interpersonal discrepancies also mediated the link between perfectionistic concerns and depressive symptoms in the hypothesized
manner. All paths in the PSDM were virtually unaltered after controlling for perfectionistic strivings, thus supporting our
hypotheses.
697
Dunkley, D., Sanislow, C., Grilo, C., & McGlashan, T. (2006). Perfectionism and
depressive symptoms 3 years later. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 47, 106115.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2005.06.003.
Flett, G., Hewitt, P. (2012). The multidimensional discrepancy inventory. Unpublished
manuscript.
Frost, R., Heimberg, R., Holt, C., & Mattia, J. (1993). A comparison of two measures of
perfectionism. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 119126. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(93)90181-2.
Frost, R., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of
perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 449468. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1007/BF01172967.
Hewitt, P., & Flett, G. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 456470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/00223514.60.3.456.
Hewitt, P., Flett, G., Sherry, S., & Caelian, C. (2006). Trait perfectionism dimensions
and suicide behavior. In T. Ellis (Ed.), Cognition and suicide (pp. 215235).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Kline, R. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York, NY:
Guilford.
Lovibond, P., & Lovibond, S. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 335343. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/00057967(94)00075-U.
McNair, D., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. (1992). EdiTS manual for the prole of mood
States. San Diego, CA: Educational Testing Service.
Mushquash, A., & Sherry, S. (2012). Understanding the socially prescribed
perfectionists cycle of self-defeat. Journal of Research in Psychology, 46,
700709. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp. 2012.08.006.
Shafran, R., Cooper, Z., & Fairburn, C. (2002). Clinical perfectionism: A cognitivebehavioural analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 773791. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00059-6.
Sherry, S. (2012). Psychometric data. Unpublished raw data.
Sherry, S., & Hall, P. (2009). The perfectionism model of binge eating. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 690709. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0014528.
Sherry, S., Hewitt, P., Stewart, S., Mackinnon, A., Mushquash, A., Flett, G., et al.
(2012). Social disconnection and hazardous drinking mediate the link between
perfectionistic attitudes and depressive symptoms. Journal of Psychopathology
and Behavioral Assessment, 34, 370381. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10862-0129291-8.
Sherry, S., Law, A., Hewitt, P., Flett, G., & Besser, A. (2008). Social support as a
mediator of the relationship between perfectionism and depression. Personality
and
Individual
Differences,
45,
339344.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.paid.2008.05.001.
Slaney, R., Rice, K., Mobley, M., Trippi, J., & Ashby, J. (2001). The revised almost
perfect scale. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34,
130145.
Stoeber, J. (2012). Dyadic perfectionism in romantic relationships. Personality and
Individual
Differences,
53,
300305.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
j.paid.2012.04.002.
Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism. Personality and
Social
Psychology
Review,
10,
295319.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/
s15327957pspr1004_2.
Stoeber, J., & Yang, H. (2010). Perfectionism and emotional reactions to perfect and
awed achievements. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 246251. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.044.