Exposure To Violent Media: The Effects of Songs With Violent Lyrics On Aggressive Thoughts and Feelings

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

2003, Vol. 84, No. 5, 960 971

Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.


0022-3514/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.960

Exposure to Violent Media: The Effects of Songs With Violent Lyrics on


Aggressive Thoughts and Feelings
Craig A. Anderson and Nicholas L. Carnagey

Janie Eubanks

Iowa State University

Texas Department of Human Services

Five experiments examined effects of songs with violent lyrics on aggressive thoughts and hostile
feelings. Experiments 1, 3, 4 and 5 demonstrated that college students who heard a violent song felt more
hostile than those who heard a similar but nonviolent song. Experiments 25 demonstrated a similar
increase in aggressive thoughts. These effects replicated across songs and song types (e.g., rock,
humorous, nonhumorous). Experiments 35 also demonstrated that trait hostility was positively related
to state hostility but did not moderate the song lyric effects. Discussion centers on the potential role of
lyric content on aggression in short-term settings, relation to catharsis and other media violence domains,
development of aggressive personality, differences between long-term and short-term effects, and
possible mitigating factors.

as by behaving in symbolically aggressive ways. Though the


aggression catharsis hypothesis has been thoroughly explored and
debunked in several entertainment media domains, there has been
relatively little work on the effects of songs with violent lyrics on
aggression-related variables such as aggressive thoughts and
feelings.

Someone told me once that theres a right and wrong. Punishment was
cure for those who dare cross the line. But it must not be true for
jerk-offs just like you. And maybe its just bullshit. I should play god
and shoot you myself.
Tool, Jerk-Off (1992)

As evidenced by the creation of the Parents Music Resource


Center and the policy of labeling music products containing violent lyrics, many people are concerned with potential deleterious
effects of listening to songs with violent lyrics. An accumulating
body of scientific research spanning 4 decades supports the hypothesis that exposure to violent media is causally related to
subsequent expression of aggression in both short- and long-term
time frames (e.g., Anderson & Bushman, 2002a; Berkowitz, 1993;
Bushman & Anderson, 2001). The vast majority of this research
has focused on violent television and movies (Huesmann & Miller,
1994). Recently, a small but relatively consistent research literature has shown that short-term exposure to violent video games
causes increases in aggression and aggression-related variables
(Anderson & Bushman, 2001).
Nonetheless, there remains among the general population and
many practitioners a very strong belief in the age-old catharsis
hypothesisthe belief that experiencing and expressing aggressive
emotions and thoughts will decrease subsequent aggressive
thoughts, feelings, and emotions (Bushman, 2002; Bushman,
Baumeister, & Stack, 1999). This ancient Greek idea, later popularized by Breuer and Freud (18931895/1955) and now usually
labeled venting, states that aggressive impulses can be reduced by
watching, reading, or singing about anger and aggression as well

Media Differences
There are numerous differences between watching violent television, playing violent video games, and listening to popular
music. One is the lack of a video component to audio-only music.
Another is that aggressive lyrical content of popular music is often
discernible only to the most attentive of listeners, whereas videobased media (including music videos) make their violent content
abundantly and graphically clear. Some rock music songs have
such garbled lyrics that they have given rise to debates about what
the lyrics are (e.g., Louie, Louie; Inna-Godda-Da-Vida; see
Marsh, 1993). A third difference concerns attention. A large proportion of time spent listening to music involves paying attention
to the music (not the lyrics) or to other tasks. Thus, effects of
violent lyrics may generally be attenuated (relative to video-based
media violence) simply because the lyrics are not processed by the
listeners.
Nonetheless, there are valid reasons to worry about potentially
harmful effects of violent music lyrics. Numerous studies have
shown that aggressive words can prime aggressive thoughts, perceptions, and behavior (e.g., Anderson, Benjamin, & Bartholow,
1998; Bargh, Chen, & Barrows, 1996; Bargh & Pietromonaco,
1982). Indeed, such effects can occur even when the stimulus has
not been consciously recognized (e.g., Bargh et al., 1996, Experiment 3). Furthermore, listeners are capable of recognizing themes
of music (i.e., violence, sex, suicide, and Satanism) even when it
is difficult to comprehend specific lyric content (Hansen & Hansen, 1991). Additionally, music stimuli are played repeatedly, both
by radio stations as well as by listeners themselves.

Craig A. Anderson and Nicholas L. Carnagey, Department of Psychology, Iowa State University; Janie Eubanks, Texas Department of Human
Services, Austin, Texas.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Craig A.
Anderson, Department of Psychology, W112 Lagomarcino Hall, Iowa
State University, Ames, Iowa 50011-3180. E-mail: caa@iastate.edu
960

EXPOSURE TO SONGS WITH VIOLENT LYRICS

Another difference between video-based entertainment and music concerns the amount of imagination needed or allowed to fill in
details of the story being told. The lack of visual images in music
both allows and requires listeners to imagine details. Concrete
images probably play a major role in transfer of ideas from the
video world to ones own real-world situations. When ones video
antagonists are similar to ones real antagonists, violent solutions
modeled in the video world are more likely to be attempted in the
real world than when the video antagonists are dissimilar (Bandura, 1986; Berkowitz, 1993; Geen, 1990). The lack of concrete
images in violent music may well allow listeners to imagine audio
antagonists similar to real-world antagonists.
Thus, there are reasons to expect violent-lyric songs to be either
more or less influential than violent video materials. The present
article reports five experiments testing the hypothesis that brief
exposure to songs with violent lyrics can increase two variables
that are key mediators of situational influences on aggression:
aggressive cognitions and aggressive affect. In the next section, we
briefly review the existing research literature. Then we show why
the general aggression model (GAM; Anderson & Bushman, 2001;
Anderson & Huesmann, in press) suggests a focus on aggressive
cognitions and affect.

961

than students who preferred other genres of music, such as alternative, adult contemporary, dancesoul, or country. Listeners to
heavy metal music held more negative attitudes toward women.
Rap music fans were more distrustful. Similarly, Took and Weiss
(1994) found a correlation between preference for rap and heavy
metal music and below-average academic performance, school
behavior problems, drug use, arrests, and sexual activity. Still
other studies have found correlations between music type preferences and a variety of maladaptive behaviors but have not specifically linked lyric preferences to those behaviors. (For a recent
review, see Roberts, Christenson, & Gentile, in press.)

Experimental Studies of Music Without Video


Music Without Lyrics
McFarland (1984) looked at the effects of exposure to tense,
calm, or no-background music (without lyrics) on the emotional
content of stories written for the Thematic Apperception Test.
Participants who heard tense music wrote the most unpleasant
stories. Like the music video studies, these results tell us little
about lyric effects. However, they indicate that research on lyrics
must control for effects of type of music.

Past Music Research


Music With Lyrics
Experimental Studies of Music Videos
Several field and laboratory experiments have examined effects
of aggressive music videos. Waite, Hillbrand, and Foster (1992)
observed a significant decrease in aggressive behavior on a forensic inpatient ward after removal of Music Television (MTV).
Peterson and Pfost (1989) found that exposing males to nonerotic
violent music videos led to a significant increase in adversarial
sexual beliefs and negative affect. Johnson, Jackson, and Gatto
(1995) found that males who had been randomly assigned to view
violent rap music videos became more accepting of the use of
violence in dealing with interpersonal problems. Related research
found that males and females exposed to violent rap music videos
became more accepting of teen dating violence (Johnson, Adams,
Ashburn, & Reed, 1995). College students exposed to rock music
videos with antisocial themes produced a greater acceptance of
antisocial behavior (Hansen & Hansen, 1990). Students were also
more likely to accept stereotypic sex-role behavior after being
exposed to music videos that displayed similar behavior (Hansen,
1989; Hansen & Hansen, 1988).
Music video studies are valuable in their own right, but they do
not provide information about the effects of exposure to violent
lyrics without video. Music videos are much more like other video
media (TV, movies) in that they can tell a story with graphically
violent images; the finding that they produce similar effects is not
surprising.

Correlational Studies of Music Preference and Behavior


Correlational studies have suggested a connection between the
kind of music youth listen to and various maladaptive behaviors
and attitudes, though the direction of causality is not clear. Rubin,
West, and Mitchell (2001) found that college students who preferred rap and heavy metal music reported more hostile attitudes

Only a few studies have specifically examined the influence of


violent songs on aggression-related variables. Interestingly, most
have found no effects of lyrical content (e.g., Ballard & Coates,
1995; St. Lawrence & Joyner, 1991; Wanamaker & Reznikoff,
1989). For example, participants in Ballard and Coatess (1995)
study heard one of six songs varying in genre (rap vs. heavy metal)
and lyric (homicidal, suicidal, neutral). Lyric content had no impact on mood measures, including anger. In other studies showing
no effect, the genre of the songs (heavy metal) made the lyrics
nearly incomprehensible, a problem noted by the researchers
themselves.
Barongan and Hall (1995) reported a study suggesting that
antisocial lyrics can affect behavior, but the target behavior was
not clearly aggressive; thus, its relevance to our work is unclear.
Male college students listened to misogynous or neutral rap music,
viewed three vignettes (neutral, sexualviolent, assaultive), and
then chose one of the three vignettes to be shown to a female
confederate. Those who had listened to the misogynous music
were significantly more likely to choose the assaultive vignette.
Wester, Crown, Quatman, and Heesacker (1997) reported mixed
results. Male undergraduates were exposed to either (a) sexually
violent music and lyrics, (b) the same music without lyrics, (c)
sexually violent lyrics without music, or (d) no music or lyrics.
Results yielded no differences in negative attitudes toward women
among the four groups. However, participants exposed to violent
lyrics viewed their relationships with women as more adversarial.
Overall, the few published studies on the effects of exposure to
songs with violent lyrics have produced mixed results, perhaps
because of methodological problems involving confounds with
arousal or lyrics that were indecipherable. We build on prior work
by using a social cognitive theoretical perspective that has
emerged from aggression research in several different domains,
including media violence.

ANDERSON, CARNAGEY, AND EUBANKS

962
Theoretical Perspective

The theoretical basis for the present experiments comes from


our earlier work on GAM (Anderson, 1997; Anderson, Anderson,
& Deuser, 1996; Anderson & Bushman, 2002b; Anderson, Deuser,
& DeNeve, 1995; Anderson & Dill, 2000; Anderson & Huesmann,
in press). This model draws on empirical and theoretical contributions from several research groups, most notably the social
cognitive work of Bandura (1986), Berkowitz (1993), Crick and
Dodge (1994), Geen (1990), Huesmann (1988), and Mischel
(1973). Figure 1 presents the single-episode portion of this model.
Effects of situational (e.g., violent media) and personality (e.g.,
trait hostility) input variables combine (sometimes interactively) to
influence aggressive behavior by influencing the present internal
state (cognition, affect, and arousal) and subsequent appraisal and
decision processes. These main pathways are linked in Figure 1 by
the bold lines with arrows. The dashed lines within the Present
Internal State box indicate that these components affect each other.
Because of potential arousal effects on other variables, it is important to control induced arousal when examining effects on
cognition and affect. The focus of this article is lyric effects on
current cognition and affect, so we do not discuss appraisal and
action aspects of GAM.
According to GAM, long-term effects accrue via the development of highly accessible knowledge structures and emotional
desensitization to violence by well-established social cognitive
learning and systematic desensitization processes. In brief, each
media violence episode constitutes a learning trial in which one
rehearses aggressive thoughts and primes aggression-related affects, creating and making chronically accessible hostile attitudes,
beliefs, expectations, and scripts (Anderson & Bushman, 2002b;
Anderson & Huesmann, in press.)

Overview

theoretical and empirical reasons. GAM explicitly incorporates


individual differences as factors important in each individual life
episode and as something that develops from life experiences. Past
research has demonstrated the importance of trait hostility in a
variety of aggression contexts. For instance, the effects of exposure to violent movies sometimes (but not always) differ for people
who score low versus high on measures of trait aggressiveness
(Anderson, 1997; Bushman, 1995; Bushman & Geen, 1990). Similarly, aggressive personality has been linked to two aggressionrelated biases. Dill, Anderson, Anderson, and Deuser (1997) found
that aggressive people are more likely to expect others to solve
problems by use of aggressive behaviors (hostile expectation bias)
and are more likely to see interpersonal interactions as aggressive
encounters (hostile perception bias).
Humorous content was included both to test the generalizability
of violent song effects and because past research suggests that
humor might mitigate effects of aggression-stimulating variables
(Baron, 1978; Berkowitz, 1970; Mueller & Donnerstein, 1977).
Humorous (vs. nonhumorous) content may combine additively or
interactively with violent (vs. nonviolent) content in their effects
on subsequent aggressive thoughts and feelings. If they combine
additively, then violent humorous songs should yield higher levels
of aggressive affect and thought than nonviolent humorous songs
but should be fairly comparable with no-song control conditions. If
they combine interactively, then humorous songs should yield
relatively low levels of aggressive thought and affect regardless of
whether they are also violent or nonviolent.
Experiments 1 and 2 assessed effects of violent lyrics on state
hostility and aggressive cognitions, respectively. Experiment 3
assessed effects of violent lyrics and trait hostility on state hostility
and aggressive cognitions using a broader set of songs and a
different measure of aggressive cognition. Experiments 4 and 5
examined the combined effects of violent humorous song lyrics on
aggressive thought and affect and included trait hostility.

The present studies investigated effects of violent songs on


aggressive thought and affect, controlling for arousal effects by
song selection and by measuring perceived arousal. We also investigated potential moderating effects of two variables: trait hostility and humorous content. Trait hostility was included for both

Experiment 1
Method
Participants
Twenty-nine female and 30 male students from a large Midwestern
university participated. About half were recruited by members of a senior
psychology class as part of a class project. The rest were from the
psychology participant pool in a later semester, and participated for extra
credit. This experiment used a 2 (song) 2 (sex) 2 (participant pool)
factorial design. The participant pool factor was included in the statistical
analysis but had no reliable effects.

Songs

Figure 1. The general aggression model. Main pathways are linked by


bold lines with arrows. Dashed lines within the Present Internal State box
indicate that these components affect each other. From Human Aggression, by C. A. Anderson & B. J. Bushman, 2002, Annual Review of
Psychology, 53, p. 34. Copyright 2002 by Annual Reviews. Reprinted with
permission.

We solicited suggestions from students from the same undergraduate


population for pairs of contemporary rock songs that had the following
characteristics. One song had to have clearly violent content, whereas the
other had to have no (or minimal) violent content. Both had to be understandable, the same type (e.g., both hard rock or both soft rock), and about
the same length. Finally, we wanted both songs to be by the same group.
We used two songs, each about 5 min long, by the group Tool: Jerk-Off
(violent; Tool, 1992, from the album Opiate) and Four Degrees (nonviolent; Tool, 1993, from the album Undertow).

EXPOSURE TO SONGS WITH VIOLENT LYRICS

Procedure
After reading and signing a consent form, participants learned that the
experiment involved how different songs affect performance on various
tasks. They were to listen to a contemporary song, complete a couple of
psychological tasks, and then answer a few questions about the song.
Participants then listened to the assigned song, completed the State Hostility Scale (SHS; Anderson et al., 1995), completed a longer unrelated
task, and were debriefed.
The SHS contains 35 sentences describing current feelings (either hostile
or friendly). For example, two hostile items read, I feel like yelling at
somebody and I feel furious. Respondents rate each sentence on a
5-point Likert-type scale (1 strongly disagree, 3 neither agree nor
disagree, 5 strongly agree). The friendly items are reverse scored. The
scale typically produces internal reliability estimates in the .90 .95 range,
but three items (I feel willful, I feel tender, I feel vexed) often show
poor itemtotal correlations. Willful displayed a low itemtotal correlation in the present study, so we dropped it. Coefficient alpha was .96.

Results and Discussion


Sex was included in the analyses as a covariate rather than as
another two-level factor.1 The 2 (song: violent vs. nonviolent) 2
(participant pool: volunteer vs. psychology) ANCOVA yielded
two statistically reliable effects. As predicted, the violent song
produced higher levels of state hostility than did the nonviolent
song (Ms 2.60 and 2.19, respectively), F(1, 54) 5.97,
MSE 0.426, p .02. In addition, females reported higher levels
of state hostility than males (Ms 2.62 and 2.17, respectively),
F(1, 54) 6.71, MSE 0.426, p .02.2 This somewhat unusual
finding is probably due to the fact that in our participant pool,
females typically do not like hard rock music to the same extent as
males.
These results indicate that the violent content of rock songs can
increase feelings of hostility when compared with similar but
nonviolent rock music. It is important to note that this violentlyrics effect occurred in the absence of any provocation. The low
absolute level of the SHS means reflect this lack of provocation.

Experiment 2
Method
Experiment 2 was identical to Experiment 1 in all respects except that
the dependent variable was a measure of aggressive cognition. Sixty-one
undergraduates (30 females, 31 males) participated either as volunteers or
as a part of their introductory psychology class.
The dependent variable was based on participants ratings of a large
number of word pairs from Bushman (1996). Bushman identified 10
words as clearly aggressive in meaning (blood, butcher, choke, fight,
gun, hatchet, hurt, kill, knife, and wound) and 10 words as ambiguous
in meaning, having both aggressive and nonaggressive meanings (alley,
animal, bottle, drugs, movie, night, police, red, rock, and stick). Bushman showed that people who score high on trait hostility tended to
perceive relatively greater similarity of meaning between pairs of
aggressive and ambiguous words (from these two lists) than do people
who score low on trait hostility.
We adapted Bushmans (1996) task in the following way. All possible pairs of these 20 words were presented to participants with
instructions to rate each pair on how similar, associated, or related the
paired words seemed to be. Ratings were made on 7-point scales
anchored at 1 (not at all similar, associated, or related) and 7 (extreme-

963

ly similar, associated, or related). We calculated three average similarity scores for each participant: aggressiveaggressive word pairs
(45), aggressiveambiguous word pairs (100), and ambiguous
ambiguous word pairs (45). Our reasoning and predictions were quite
simple. If listening to violent lyrics increases the accessibility of
aggressive thoughts in semantic memory, then ambiguous words will
tend to be interpreted in a relatively more aggressive way, leading to
relatively higher similarity ratings of aggressiveambiguous pairs. This
same semantic priming process might also increase the perceived similarity of aggressiveaggressive pairs and of ambiguousambiguous
pairs, but these increases should be small relative to the violent song
effect on aggressiveambiguous pairs. We used the other two word-pair
types as within-subject controls. Thus, we predicted violent (vs. nonviolent) song participants to give larger similarity ratings of aggressive
ambiguous word pairs relative to their ratings of ambiguousambiguous
and aggressiveaggressive word pairs.

Results and Discussion


We computed a contrast score reflecting the main prediction.
We averaged each participants aggressiveaggressive and
ambiguousambiguous scores. From this control rating we then
subtracted each persons aggressiveambiguous score. On this
contrast score, smaller scores indicate that the aggressive
ambiguous pairs were seen as relatively more similar (or less
dissimilar) than the control-word pairs. We predicted that participants who heard the violent song would have significantly smaller
contrast scores. An ANCOVA was conducted on this contrast.3
The 2 (song: violent vs. nonviolent) 2 (subject pool: volunteer
vs. introductory psychology) ANCOVA yielded only one reliable
effect, the predicted main effect of music lyric content, F(1,
56) 4.24, MSE 0.113, p .05. Table 1 presents the mean
similarity ratings as a function of song and word-pair type.
As can be seen in Table 1, the violent song led to higher
similarity ratings for aggressiveambiguous word pairs than did
the nonviolent song; the mean similarity score increased by almost
half of a scale point (0.45) on a 7-point scale. The corresponding
song effect on the control pairs (aggressiveaggressive and
ambiguousambiguous) was much smaller; the mean similarity
increase was just slightly more than a quarter of a scale point
(0.27). As predicted, violent song participants had significantly
smaller contrast scores. Figure 2 presents these results in a different way, clearly illustrating the violent-lyrics effect on aggressive
cognition. In sum, hearing a violent rock song led participants to
interpret the meaning of ambiguous words such as rock and stick
in an aggressive way.

1
Nonetheless, for all studies, preliminary analyses explored the possibility of sex interactions. There were none, thereby satisfying the homogeneity of slopes assumption of analysis of covariance (ANCOVA).
2
Unless otherwise indicated, reported means for all experiments are the
appropriate least squares adjusted means, adjusted for other factors in the
model such as sex and trait hostility. Significance levels are based on
two-tailed tests.
3
The astute reader will realize that each effect in this between-subjects
analysis of variance is identical to the interaction of that effect with the
repeated-measures factor control versus aggressiveambiguous.

ANDERSON, CARNAGEY, AND EUBANKS

964
Experiment 3
Overview and Design
Broadening Aspects

Experiment 3 was designed to broaden our tests in three ways.


First, on the basis of a pilot study, we used a larger set of 4 violent
and 4 nonviolent songs. Twenty-six female and 24 male college
students listened to 10 rock songs and rated how violent each was
on a unidimensional scale anchored at 0 (not at all) and 10
(extremely). Two songs (with means of about 5.00), were dropped.
The 4 selected nonviolent song means ranged from 2.19 to 4.11.
The 4 selected violent song means ranged from 7.45 to 8.25. Each
of the violent songs was rated as significantly more violent than
each of the nonviolent songs, all ps .0001. Participants in
Experiment 3 listened to 1 of these 8 songs before completing the
SHS and an aggressive thoughts accessibility measure.
Second, we examined potential moderating effects of trait hostility by including a modified version of the Caprara Irritability
Scale (Anderson, 1997; Dill et al., 1997; Caprara et al., 1985).
Third, we used a different procedure to measure accessibility of
aggressive thoughts: Participants were timed as they read aggressive and nonaggressive words. An advantage of this reaction time
task is that suspiciousness about the true purpose of the study is
unlikely to influence responses (Anderson, 1997). The clear demand to read all words as quickly as possible effectively overrides
any suspicion or hypothesis-related demands. One cannot intentionally speed up responses that are already given as quickly as
possible. Attempts to selectively delay responses to one class of
stimuli are easily detected by examining response time
distributions.

Figure 2. Perceived similarity differences between violent and nonviolent song conditions as a function of word-pair type. Aggr. aggressive;
Amb. ambiguous.

Finally, we counterbalanced the order in which the two dependent variables were assessed on the basis of other findings that
measuring one aggression-related variable frequently changes the
outcome of subsequently measured variables, a sort of psychological uncertainty principle (Lindsay & Anderson, 2000). Thus, the
experiment used a 3 (song type) 2 (order) 2 (sex) factorial
design with trait hostility as a continuous independent variable.

Method
Participants

Additional Controls
Two other changes controlled for possible interpretational difficulties. We measured perceived arousal to check on our assumption that the violent and nonviolent songs did not differ in arousal
properties. We also added a no-song control condition to assess
whether the violent songs, the nonviolent songs, or both influenced
state hostility and aggressive cognition. We expected this control
condition to most closely resemble the nonviolent song condition.4

Eighty-three female and 79 male undergraduates at a large Midwestern


university participated for class credit. Seven participants (4 females, 3
males) were dropped because they could not understand the lyrics. One
additional female and 4 additional males were dropped because of procedural difficulties with the word pronunciation (WP) task. Three females
and 2 males were dropped because of suspicion. There was no apparent sex
or song pattern to these difficulties. The final sample had 75 female and 70
male participants.

Procedure
Table 1
Effects of Violent Versus Nonviolent Song on Perceived
Similarity of AggressiveAggressive (AgAg), Aggressive
Ambiguous (AgAm), and AmbiguousAmbiguous (AmAm) Word
Pairs
Word-pair type

Song
V
NV
VNV diff.

Control average:
AgAg and
Contrast:
AgAg AmAm
AmAm
AgAm ControlAgAm
4.86
4.60
0.26

2.94
2.66
0.28

3.90
3.63
0.27

3.37
2.92
0.45

Note. V violent; NV nonviolent; diff. difference.

0.53
0.71
0.28

Preselection. Because it was known that the sample size would be


limited and that the power to detect a noncrossover interaction (between
song condition and trait hostility) would be low, participants were preselected on trait hostility (cf. McClelland & Judd, 1993). Participants completed the Caprara Irritability Scale as part of a larger mass-testing questionnaire. Those who scored in the upper and lower quartiles were then
called and signed up for participation. These pretest scores were used only
to generate the list of names and phone numbers to be called and were
discarded after the calling lists were generated. Thus, the experimenter was
unaware of the participants scores on this scale while calling and running
experimental sessions.

Participants in the no-song control group actually heard a nonviolent


song after they had completed all dependent variables except for the Music
Questionnaire. This was done so that all groups would have the same cover
story and the same debriefing.

EXPOSURE TO SONGS WITH VIOLENT LYRICS


General instructions. After reading and signing consent forms, participants learned that they would be filling out several questionnaires, listening to a song, and doing some computer tasks. They were also told that
toward the end of the experiment you will fill out a questionnaire that will
ask questions mainly about the song you listened to previously. This
instruction was included to ensure that participants listened to the lyrics.
Trait hostility. Next, participants completed an adaptation of the Caprara Irritability Scale (Caprara et al., 1985). The adaptation (Anderson,
1997; Dill et al., 1997) involved reverse scoring and using the 10 friendly
filler items, along with the original 20 irritable items.
Songs. After completing the trait hostility scale (THS), participants in
the violent and nonviolent song conditions listened to one of eight songs.
The four violent songs were Shoot Em Up by Cypress Hill (1991); I
Wouldnt Mind by Suicidal Tendencies (1994); Hit Em Hard by Run
DMC (Simmons, McDaniels, Coward, Criss, & Gist, 1993); and JerkOff by Tool (1992). The four nonviolent songs were Live at PJs by the
Beastie Boys (1992); Love vs. Loneliness by Suicidal Tendencies
(1994); In the House by Run DMC (Simmons, McDaniels, & Phillips,
1993); and Finger Lickin Good by the Beastie Boys (1992). Participants
then completed the dependent variables. Participants in the no-song condition went directly from the THS to the dependent variables.
State hostility. The SHS was the same one described earlier. Preliminary analyses revealed that two items did not correlate well with the
remaining 33 (willful, tender); they were dropped from subsequent analyses. Coefficient alpha was .96.
Word pronunciation. This task involves reading words out loud as they
appear on a computer screen, one per trial. A sound-activated voice key
measures the latency between the word presentation and the point in time
that participants name the word. The task was created in HyperCard 2.1
(Apple Computer, 19871999); the timer was triggered by a MacRecorder
on a Macintosh computer. Ninety-six words were presented twice, a total
of 192 trials. A different random order was used for each participant. The
word list consisted of 24 aggressive words (e.g., assault, choke), 24 escape
words (e.g., abandon, desert), 24 anxiety words (e.g., insecure, foolish),
and 24 control words (e.g., behold, listen). The anxiety and escape words
were also considered control words on the basis of previous studies (e.g.,
Anderson, 1997).
Other questionnaires. After completing the second dependent variable,
participants in the song conditions filled out two questionnaires designed to
measure perceptions of how aroused or energized they were and their
reactions to the song. Participants in the control condition, who had not yet
heard any song, listened to a randomly selected nonviolent song before
completing these two questionnaires.
The Perceived Arousal Scale (PAS: Anderson et al., 1995, 1996) contains 31 adjectives describing feelings of arousal (e.g., energetic) or lack of
arousal (e.g., listless). Participants ratings were based on how they felt at
the present moment using 5-point Likert-type scales (1 very slightly or
not at all, 3 moderately, 5 extremely). The lack-of-arousal items are
reverse scored. One item (agitated) displayed a low itemtotal correlation
and was dropped. Coefficient alpha was .95.
On the Music Questionnaire participants rated how understandable and
familiar the lyrics were using 11-point scales (0 not at all, 5
moderately, 10 perfectly). They also indicated how much they thought
about the purpose of the study as opposed to the tasks themselves (0
thought mostly about the purpose, 3 thought equally about the purpose
and the tasks, 5 thought mostly about the tasks themselves). The Music
Questionnaire concluded with items designed to assess suspicion, focus of
attention, and perceptions regarding effects of the song on later responses.

Results and Discussion


As in Experiments 1 and 2, sex was included as a covariate in
all analyses. Task order was included as a factor because prior
work has suggested that the priming effects tend to wear off fairly

965

quickly, especially on state hostility. None of the task order effects


were significant, but the state hostility results appeared somewhat
different depending on task order, as is shown in a subsequent
section.

Preliminary Analyses
We first examined results from the PAS to see if the selected
violent and nonviolent rock songs had similar arousal properties.
The main effect of type of song did not approach significance, F(1,
140) 1.31, p .25. Thus, violent-lyric effects on other variables
cannot be attributed to arousal differences. The only reliable effect
on PAS was a negative relation to trait hostility, F(1,
140) 16.65, MSE 0.512, p .001.
We next examined the Music Questionnaire ratings (understandability, familiarity, purpose, accuracy of suspicions, and estimated
song influence on PAS, SHS, and WP). The two song conditions
(violent vs. nonviolent) did not differ reliably on any of these
variables ( ps .20).
Interestingly, there was one reliable sex effect and three reliable
trait hostility effects on these Music Questionnaire items. Males
(M 5.80) rated the lyrics as more understandable than did
females (M 4.90), F(1, 140) 6.77, MSE 4.900, p .02.
Trait hostility was positively related to beliefs that the song influenced performance on PAS and on SHS, Fs(1, 140) 4.36
and 4.34, MSEs 0.900 and 0.987, ps .05. Also, trait hostility
was positively related to familiarity, F(1, 140) 4.78, p .04.
Trait hostility did not interact with the song manipulation in any
of the analyses, so this interaction term was dropped from the
statistical model. We then compared the control (no-song) and the
nonviolent song conditions on WP and SHS. As expected, there
were no reliable differences between these two conditions ( ps
.25), so these conditions were combined into one larger nonviolent
condition for all subsequent analyses.5

Main Analyses
State hostility. Analyses ignoring task order yielded only a
significant effect of trait hostility (b .21), F(1, 141) 11.15,
MSE 0.411, p .005. Individuals who scored high on trait
hostility tended to score high on state hostility. The violent song
effect was in the predicted direction, with those who heard a
violent song reporting higher levels of SHS (Ms 2.14 and 1.98),
but was not significant, F(1, 141) 2.02, p .16.
The lack of a reliable effect of song content on state hostility
might appear to conflict with the results of Experiment 1. However, in Experiment 1 the SHS was administered immediately after
the song. We therefore reanalyzed Experiment 3 SHS scores using
only the 71 participants who completed the SHS first, even though
5
For SHS, means for the control, nonviolent, and violent song conditions were 1.98, 1.98, and 2.14, respectively. For word pronunciation,
corresponding means were 1.35, 5.93, and 11.08. Reported analyses simply
treated all participants who heard a nonviolent song and those who completed the main dependent variables prior to hearing a song as if they were
in one larger group. An alternative procedure for combining different
conditions is to use equal contrast weights. Results were stronger with the
contrast procedure for both dependent variables. Nonetheless, we report the
simpler (and somewhat more conservative) procedure in the text.

ANDERSON, CARNAGEY, AND EUBANKS

966

the Task Order Song Content interaction was not statistically


reliable, F(1, 139) 2.20, p .15. Those who heard a violent
song had significantly higher SHS scores (M 2.23) than those
who did not hear a violent song (M 1.92), F(1, 67) 4.49,
MSE 0.378, p .04. Thus, it appears that the effect of listening
to violent songs on state hostility is reliable, but it can be disrupted
fairly easily by other intervening activities. SHS scores of participants who did the WP task first did not differ as a function of song
condition, F(1, 70) 0, ns (Ms 2.01 and 2.06), in the violent
song and nonviolent conditions.
Word pronunciation. Preliminary analyses compared average
reaction times to the three nonaggressive word typesanxiety,
escape, and control words. As anticipated, there were no effects of
sex, song, or trait hostility on these three word types (all ps .10).
Thus, they were averaged to form a nonaggressive word type. For
the main analyses we subtracted average reaction time to aggressive words from the average reaction time to nonaggressive words.
This WP measure is positive when aggressive words are more
accessible than nonaggressive words; negative scores indicate the
opposite.
As predicted, violent songs increased the relative accessibility of
aggressive words. Those who listened to a violent song had larger
WP scores (M 11.1 ms) than those who had not listened to a
violent song (M 4.3 ms), F(1, 141) 3.91, MSE 0.399, p
.05. In addition, males produced higher WP scores than females
(Ms 13.6 and 1.7 ms, respectively), F(1, 141) 12.91,
MSE 0.399, p .001. Trait hostility was not systematically
related to WP (F 1).

Method
Participants
Sixty-five female and 74 male students from a large Midwestern university successfully completed all portions of the experiment.

Dependent Variables
The SHS, PAS, and Music Questionnaire were the same as in Experiment 3. Accessibility of aggressive thoughts was based on a WC task
consisting of a list of 98 word fragments (Anderson, Flanagan, Carnagey,
Benjamin, Eubanks, & Valentine, 2002). The respondents task is to fill in
the missing letters in order to form a word. Half of the fragments can be
completed to form either aggressive or nonaggressive words (e.g., h_t
can become hit or hat). Tasks similar to this have often been used as
measures of implicit memory (e.g., Roediger, Weldon, Stadler, & Reigler,
1992). Accessibility of aggressive thoughts was the proportion of word
completions that were aggressive.

Procedure
After reading and signing a consent form, participants learned that the
experiment was designed to see how songs affect thoughts and emotions.
They then completed the measure of trait hostility. Next, participants either
listened to one of the humorous songs (violent or nonviolent) or they
continued directly to the dependent measures (no-song control). The songs
were A Boy Named Sue (violent) by Johnny Cash (Silverstein, 1994) and
Hello Mudduh, Hello Fadduh! (nonviolent) by Allan Sherman (1991).
Next, participants completed the SHS and the WC task. Participants in the
two song conditions then completed the Music Questionnaire. Those in the
control condition listened to the nonviolent song and then completed the
Music Questionnaire.

Experiment 4
Results and Discussion
Overview and Design
Experiment 4 further tested the generality of the violent song
effect on aggressive thoughts and feelings by using humorous
songs and including trait hostility. A humorous violent song is
likely to have two competing processes at work. The violent
content may well prime aggressive thoughts and negative feelings.
At the same time, the humorous content may produce positive
feelings. If so, participants who hear the violent humorous song
may well respond very much like no-song control participants,
because these competing processes cancel each other out, especially for affective measures. A humorous nonviolent song does
not have this competition problem. Only the positive aspects of the
humorous content are at work. In other words, if humor and violent
content combine additively, nonviolent humorous songs will yield
levels of aggressive affect and thought that are significantly lower
than levels obtained from violent humorous and no-song control
conditions.
The experiment used a 3 (song) 2 (order) 2 (sex) factorial
design, with trait hostility as a continuous variable. Participants
were randomly assigned to hear either the violent humorous song,
the nonviolent humorous song, or no song.6 Half of the participants completed the SHS first, whereas the other half completed it
after a cognitive task. A different measure of aggressive thought
accessibility was used in this study, a word completion (WC) task.
Finally, participants completed the PAS and the Music Questionnaire, as in Experiment 3.

Preliminary Analyses
Once again, sex and trait hostility were treated as covariates.
One item was dropped from the PAS because of a low itemtotal
correlation, leaving 30 items for the final scale. Coefficient alpha
was .94. Final analyses on perceived arousal yielded only a main
effect of sex, F(1, 134) 6.48, MSE 0.452 p .02. Males
scored higher than females on this measure (Ms 3.35 and 3.05,
respectively). The main effects of song, F(2, 134) 1.0, and trait
hostility, F(1, 134) 1.92, p .15, were both nonsignificant.
Thus, any song or trait hostility effects on SHS or WC cannot be
attributed to differences in arousal.

State Hostility
One item was dropped from SHS because of low itemtotal
correlations. Coefficient alpha was .94. The final analysis yielded
significant main effects of trait hostility, (b .27), F(1,
134) 34.24, MSE 0.209, p .0001, and song, F(2,
6
Because it was known that sample size would be limited, participants
were preselected on trait hostility using a 10-item version of Capraras
Irritability Scale. Students who scored in the upper and lower quartiles
were called and signed up for participation. This was done to ensure that
there would be an adequate number of participants scoring either high or
low on trait hostility to allow for adequate analysis to be done on this
independent variable.

EXPOSURE TO SONGS WITH VIOLENT LYRICS

134) 3.58, p .05. As predicted by an additive model of humor


and violent content effects, the no-song control group (M 1.97)
and the humorous violent song group (M 1.90), had essentially
the same levels of SHS, F(1, 134) 0.55. The violent (M 1.90)
and nonviolent (M 1.72) song conditions were marginally different, F(1, 134) 3.52, p .07. Most important, as predicted by
the additive model, the humorous nonviolent song group had
significantly lower SHS scores than the average of humorous
violent song and control groups, F(1, 134) 6.55, p .02.
The Song Task Order interaction was not significant (F 1).
Nonetheless, as before, we separately examined the SHS scores of
the 70 participants who completed that scale first. The results were
essentially the same as with the whole sample, except that the
control mean was slightly smaller (instead of larger) than the
humorous violent mean (Ms 1.96 and 1.98, respectively). Once
again there was a significant effect of trait hostility (b .26), F(1,
65) 20.85, MSE 0.205, p .0001. The violent (M 1.98)
and nonviolent (M 1.75) song conditions were marginally different, F(1, 65) 3.44, p .07. The humorous nonviolent song
condition produced a lower level of SHS than the average of the
other two conditions, F(1, 65) 3.86, p .06.

Word Completions
The final analysis yielded main effects of trait hostility and song
condition. Trait hostility was positively related to WC (b .011),
F(1, 134) 4.21, MSE 0.0029, p .05. The highest mean for
proportions of aggressive WCs was in the humorous violent song
condition (M .229) and the lowest in the humorous nonviolent
song condition (M .200); the control group mean fell in between
(M .218), F(2, 134) 3.26, p .05. The humorous violent song
yielded significantly higher WC scores than the humorous nonviolent song, F(1, 134) 6.46, p .02.
In summary, these results suggest that humorous and violent
content essentially canceled each other out in terms of effects on
state hostility but did not entirely do so for aggressive cognitions.
Another way of summarizing these results is that the effects of
violent lyrics on aggressive affect and cognition appear to apply to
humorous songs as well as to more standard forms of popular
music.

Experiment 5
Overview and Design
Experiment 5 further replicated Experiments 1 4. It tested the
effects of violent lyrics and trait hostility on state hostility and
aggressive cognition with both humorous and nonhumorous songs.
The design was a 2 (violent or nonviolent music lyrics) 2
(humorous or nonhumorous lyrics) factorial, with a no-song condition and trait hostility as a continuous independent variable.
Different songs were used in Experiment 5 to further test
generality.

Method
Participants
Eighty-two female and 69 male students at a large Midwestern university participated for partial course credit. One participant was dropped from

967

all analyses because he failed to comply with experimental instructions.


Aggressive cognition data from 1 male and 1 female were dropped because
of procedural problems.

Procedure
After reading and signing consent forms, participants learned that the
experiment was investigating effects of music on mood. They were told
that they would respond to a questionnaire (trait hostility), listen to a song,
and then respond to two more questionnaires. To ensure that participants
listened to the lyrics of the song, they were told that they would be asked
questions about the song later in the experiment.
The trait hostility and SHS measures from Experiments 1 4 were used
again. Coefficient alpha for trait hostility was .85. One SHS item, I feel
willful, had a low correlation with the other items and was dropped.
Coefficient alpha was .96. The WC task from Experiment 4 assessed
accessibility of aggressive thoughts.
The two humorous songs were The Night Santa Went Crazy (violent;
Yankovic, 1996), and Gump (nonviolent; Yankovic, 1996) by Weird Al
Yankovic. The two nonhumorous songs were Country Death Song
(violent; Violent Femmes, 2000) and I Held Her in My Arms (nonviolent; Violent Femmes, 1986) by the Violent Femmes.
Participants in the no-song control condition went directly from the THS
to the dependent variables. Half of the participants completed SHS first,
whereas the other half first performed the WC task.

Results and Discussion


Analysis Strategy
Sex and trait hostility were again treated as covariates. Preliminary analyses revealed no reliable effects of task order.

State Hostility
For the total sample, there were significant main effects on SHS
of music condition, F (4, 143) 2.96, MSE 0.361, p .03, and
trait hostility (b .279), F (1, 143) 15.45, p .0001. The sex
effect did not approach significance (F 1.0). The two violent
song conditions yielded significantly higher SHS scores than did
the two nonviolent song conditions (Ms 2.95 and 2.72 respectively), F(1, 143) 4.88, p .03.
However, as in Experiment 3, the SHS effects were clearer for
participants who completed that scale first. Figure 3 displays the
means for the five conditions. For these 83 participants, contrasts
examining the 2 (violent vs. nonviolent lyrics) 2 (humorous vs.
nonhumorous lyrics) conditions yielded a significant main effect
of violent content, F(1, 76) 4.28, MSE 0.387, p .05, and a
marginally significant main effect of humorous content, F(1,
76) 2.97, p .09. Participants in the violent song conditions had
higher SHS scores than those in the nonviolent song conditions
(Ms 3.00 and 2.68, respectively). Those in the humorous song
conditions had slightly lower state hostility scores than those in the
nonhumorous conditions (Ms 2.71 and 2.98, respectively). The
Humor Violence interaction did not approach significance
(F 1.0).
As in Experiment 4, the least hostility was expressed by those
who had just heard a song with nonviolent humorous lyrics
(M 2.53). Somewhat different from Experiment 4 was the
finding that the no-song control mean (M 2.62) was closer to the
humorous nonviolent song than to the humorous violent song
mean (M 2.89).

ANDERSON, CARNAGEY, AND EUBANKS

968

5) similarly yielded a significant effect overall (d .44, K 5,


N 450) as well as within nonhumorous (d .43, K 3, N
283) and humorous (d .46, K 2, N 167) conditions.

General Discussion
Empirical Contributions

Figure 3. State hostility scores as a function of humorous and violent


lyric content, Experiment 5.

The two conditions expected to yield the largest and smallest


meansviolent nonhumorous versus nonviolent humorous did
exactly that, and differed significantly (Ms 3.11 and 2.53,
respectively), F(1, 76) 6.42, p .02. Trait hostility was also
significantly related to SHS (b .288), F(1, 76) 8.86, p .01.
The sex effect was not significant, F(1, 76) 2.36, p .12.

Word Completions
We computed the same proportion of aggressive WC scores as
in Experiment 4. Males generated higher WC scores than females
(Ms .291 and .220, respectively), F(1, 141) 15.00,
MSE 0.0117, p .001. Contrasts examining the 2 (violent vs.
nonviolent lyrics) 2 (humorous vs. nonhumorous lyrics) conditions yielded a significant main effect of violent content, F(1,
141) 6.16, p .02. Those who had listened to a violent song had
higher WC scores than those who had heard a nonviolent song
(Ms .279 and .228, respectively), mirroring the violent lyrics
effect of Experiment 4. The no-song control condition yielded WC
scores in between the violent and nonviolent conditions (M
.268), somewhat closer to the violent condition. This also mirrors
the results of Experiment 4. Neither the humor main effect nor
the Violence Humor interaction approached significance (Fs
1.0).

Two potentially problematic issues in music lyric research both


revolve around the possibility that the comparison violent and
nonviolent songs differ in ways that artifactually produce (or
mask) differences in aggression-related dependent variables. In the
present case, the two issues concern whether the obtained violent
versus nonviolent song results were actually due to (a) systematic
differences in the music itself rather than the lyrics or (b) differences in arousal properties. Across the five experiments, music
characteristics of comparison songs were controlled in several
different ways. In Experiments 1, 2, and 5, the violent and nonviolent songs were matched by the artists and were in much the same
style. Furthermore, multiple violent and nonviolent songs were
used in Experiments 3 and 5. Arousal was assessed and ruled out
as a factor in Experiments 3 and 4. Across studies there were seven
violent songs by seven artists and eight nonviolent songs by seven
artists. The consistent results from these five experiments provide
strong evidence that songs with violent lyrics increase aggressionrelated cognition and affect and that this effect is the result of the
violence in the lyrics. It is not an artifact of confounded musical
style, specific performing artist, or arousal.
The increase in aggressive thoughts was shown in three different
ways. Violent songs led to more aggressive interpretations of
ambiguously aggressive words (Experiment 2), increased the relative speed with which people read aggressive (vs. nonaggressive)
words (Experiment 3), and increased the proportion of aggressive
word completions (Experiments 4 and 5).

Pooled Results
To ensure an accurate summary of these five experiments, we
pooled the violent (vs. nonviolent) song effects on aggressive
affect and thought using meta-analytic techniques. We calculated
the average d by dividing the difference between the relevant
adjusted means by the pooled standard deviation.
Four experiments provided five independent tests of violent
versus nonviolent lyrics on state hostility (one each from Experiments 1, 3, and 4; two from Experiment 5). As can be seen in the
first three bars in Figure 4, violent lyrics increased state hostility
overall (d .55, K 5, N 242) as well as within nonhumorous (d .55, K 3, N 161) and humorous (d .57, K 2,
N 81) conditions. The five independent effects on aggressive
thoughts (one each from Experiments 2 4; two from Experiment

Figure 4. Pooled violent versus nonviolent lyric effect sizes (d) and
95% confidence intervals on state hostility and aggressive thoughts: All
samples versus three nonhumorous samples versus two humorous samples.
For state hostility, only the results from participants who completed the
SHS first are included; ns 242, 161, and 81. For aggressive thoughts,
ns 450, 283, and 167. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.

EXPOSURE TO SONGS WITH VIOLENT LYRICS

We used the same measure of aggressive feelings across studies


because it and scales like it have been widely used and validated
by numerous researchers. In Experiments 1, 3, 4, and 5, violent
songs increased feelings of hostility, relative to appropriate nonviolent songs, in the absence of provocation or threat. These
effects occurred across a variety of both humorous and nonhumorous songs and were not attributable to differences in arousal.
Furthermore, supplemental analyses revealed that the violent
song effects did not interact with trait hostility in the three experiments that assessed this factor or with gender in any of the
experiments. In sum, the consistency of results across songs,
musical genre, artist, research method, trait hostility, and gender
attests to the validity and generality of the obtained effects, as do
the pooled results in Figure 4.

Implications for Violence


Our focus was on precursors to aggression, as outlined by GAM,
rather than on aggressive behavior itself. The main reason for this
focus was to allow in-depth testing and analysis of these critical
precursors to aggression. These results support GAM-based predictions of how exposure to media violence influences present
internal affective and cognitive states. Research on potential violent song effects on aggressive behavior becomes even more
important now that we have clearly demonstrated that such songs
increase aggressive thoughts and feelings.
GAM illustrates how exposure to media violence can produce
both short- and long-term increases in aggressive behavior. In both
contexts, violent lyrics most likely operate through both the affect
and cognition routes, influencing appraisals of the situation and
emotional state and (eventually) the behavioral decision.

Short-Term Effects: Priming Versus Venting


In the immediate situation, exposure to violent lyrics increases
the accessibility of aggressive thoughts and affect. These results
confirm GAM and the social cognitive theories on which it is
based in demonstrating priminglike effects and therefore add to the
research literature contradicting popular notions of positive catharsis or venting effects on aggressive thoughts and feelings (Bushman, 2002; Bushman et al., 1999).
The violent-song-inspired increases in aggressive thoughts and
feelings can influence perceptions of ongoing social interactions,
coloring them with an aggressive tint. Such aggression-biased
interpretations can, in turn, instigate a more aggressive response
(verbal or physical) than would have been emitted in a nonbiased
state, thus provoking an aggressive escalatory spiral of antisocial
exchanges. In sum, listening to angry, violent music does not
appear to provide the kind of cathartic release that the general
public and some professional and pop psychologists believe.

Long-Term Effects
Direct effects. Long-term effects operate in much the same
manner, except that the proximate source of the high accessibility
of aggressive cognitions and affects is the chronic state of the
individual rather than a very recently heard violent song. Repeated
exposure to violent lyrics may contribute to the development of an
aggressive personality (Anderson & Bushman, 2002b; Anderson &

969

Huesmann, in press), as is true for long-term TV violence effects


(e.g., Eron, 1982; Huesmann, 1988; Huesmann & Guerra, 1997;
Huesmann & Miller, 1994).
Indirect effects. There may also be an indirect contribution,
just as has been found in the TV literature. Specifically, short-term
increases in aggression due to violent lyrics affect the persons
social environment as well as the person. Close others (friends,
family, peers, teachers) are influenced by these initially temporary
increases in aggression and naturally respond to them in a negative
way. Over time, these relationships deteriorate, and acquaintances
begin to expect aggressive and conflictual interactions with the
person and will therefore elicit further aggressive behaviors via
well-established expectancy effects. In other words, repeated
short-term media violence effects (lyrics, TV, movies, video
games) can indirectly create a more hostile social environment,
which further promotes the development of chronic hostility biases
in the persons internal makeuptheir perceptual and social
scripts and schemata and related knowledge structuresin short,
their personality.

Balance
Of course, there is reason to believe that this type of hostile
thought and affect priming by violent songs may last only a fairly
short time. Presumably, if the next song heard is nonviolent or if
some other nonviolent event occurs, the short-term priming effects
of violent lyrics will dissipate. This would seem especially likely
if some positive event occurs. Furthermore, as noted earlier, lyrics
are often secondary to the music itself, so much so that some
violent songs have lyrics that are undecipherable.
At present, research on the effects of violent lyrics is in its
infancy. There are now good theoretical and empirical reasons to
expect effects of music lyrics on aggressive behavior to be similar
to the well-studied effects of exposure to TV and movie violence
and the more recent research efforts on violent video games.
Additional research on music lyric effects is clearly needed, both
short-term and longitudinal studies. GAM provides a useful framework for understanding the current studies and for guiding such
future research.

References
Anderson, C. A. (1997). Effects of violent movies and trait irritability on
hostile feelings and aggressive thoughts. Aggressive Behavior, 23, 161
178.
Anderson, C. A., Anderson, K. B., & Deuser, W. E. (1996). Examining an
affective aggression framework: Weapon and temperature effects on
aggressive thoughts, affect, and attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 366 376.
Anderson, C. A., Benjamin, A. J., & Bartholow, B. D. (1998). Does the gun
pull the trigger? Automatic priming effects of weapon pictures and
weapon names. Psychological Science, 9, 308 314.
Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2001). Effects of violent video games
on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: A meta-analytic review of the
scientific literature. Psychological Science, 12, 353359.
Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002a, March 29). The effects of
media violence on society. Science, 295, 23772378.
Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002b). Human aggression. Annual
Review of Psychology, 53, 2751.
Anderson, C. A., Deuser, W. E., & DeNeve, K. M. (1995). Hot tempera-

970

ANDERSON, CARNAGEY, AND EUBANKS

tures, hostile affect, hostile cognition, and arousal: Tests of a general


model of affective aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 434 448.
Anderson, C. A., & Dill, K. E. (2000). Video games and aggressive
thoughts, feelings, and behavior in the laboratory and in life. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 772790.
Anderson, C. A., Flanagan, M., Carnagey, N. L., Benjamin, A. J., Eubanks,
J., & Valentine, J. C. (2002). Specific effects of violent video game
content on aggressive thoughts and behavior. Manuscript submitted for
publication.
Anderson, C. A., & Huesmann, L. R. (in press). Human aggression: A
social cognitive view. In M. A. Hogg & J. Cooper (Eds.), Handbook of
social psychology.
Apple Computer. (19871999). HyperCard 2.1. Cupertino, CA: Author.
Ballard, M. E., & Coates, S. (1995). The immediate effects of homicidal,
suicidal, and nonviolent heavy metal and rap songs on the moods of
college students. Youth and Society, 27, 148 168.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social
cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social
behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on
action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230 244.
Bargh, J. A., & Pietromonaco, P. (1982). Automatic information processing and social perception: The influence of trait information presented
outside of conscious awareness on impression formation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 437 449.
Baron, R. A. (1978). Aggression-inhibiting influence of sexual humor.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 189 197.
Barongan, C., & Hall, G. C. (1995). The influence of misogynous rap
music on sexual aggression against women. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 19, 195207.
Beastie Boys. (1992). Finger lickin good. On In your head [CD]. Nashville, TN: Capitol.
Beastie Boys. (1992). Live at PJs. On In your head [CD]. Nashville, TN:
Capitol.
Berkowitz, L. (1970). Aggressive humor as a stimulus to aggressive
responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 710 717.
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control.
New York: McGraw-Hill.
Breuer, J., & Freud, S. (1955). Studies in hysteria (Vol. 2, A. A. Brill,
Trans.). London: Hogarth. (Original work published 18931895)
Bushman, B. J. (1995). Moderating role of trait aggressiveness in the
effects of violent media on aggression. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 69, 950 960.
Bushman, B. J. (1996). Individual differences in the extent and development of aggressive cognitiveassociative networks. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 811 819.
Bushman, B. J. (2002). Does venting anger feed or extinguish the flame?
Catharsis, rumination, distraction, anger, and aggressive responding.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 724 731.
Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2001). Media violence and the
American public: Scientific facts versus media misinformation. American Psychologist, 56, 477 489.
Bushman, B. J., Baumeister, R. F., & Stack, A. D. (1999). Catharsis,
aggression, and persuasive influence: Self-fulfilling or self-defeating
prophecies? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 367
376.
Bushman, B. J., & Geen, R. G. (1990). Role of cognitive emotional
mediators and individual differences in the effects of media violence on
aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 156 163.
Caprara, G. V., Cinanni, V., DImperio, G., Passerini, S., Renzi, P., &
Travablia, G. (1985). Indicators of impulsive aggression: Present status
of research on irritability and emotional susceptibility scales. Personality
and Individual Differences, 6, 665 674.

Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social


information processing mechanisms in childrens adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74 101.
Cypress Hill. (1991). Shoot em up. On Juice (Original Soundtrack) [CD].
New York: MCA.
Dill, K. E., Anderson, C. A., Anderson, K. B., & Deuser, W. E. (1997).
Effects of aggressive personality on social expectations and social perceptions. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 272292.
Eron, L. D. (1982): Parent child interaction, television violence, and
aggression of children. American Psychologist, 37, 197211.
Geen, R. G. (1990). Human aggression. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Hansen, C. H. (1989). Priming sex-role stereotypic event schemas with
rock music videos: Effects on impression favorability, trait inferences,
and recall of subsequent malefemale interaction. Basic and Applied
Social Psychology, 10, 371391.
Hansen, C. H., & Hansen, R. D. (1988). How rock music videos can
change what is seen when boy meets girl: Priming stereotypic appraisal
of social interactions. Sex Roles, 19, 287316.
Hansen, C. H., & Hansen, R. D. (1990). Rock music videos and antisocial
behavior. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 11, 357369.
Hansen, C. H., & Hansen, R. D. (1991). Schematic information processing
of heavy metal lyrics. Communication Research, 18, 373 411.
Huesmann, L. R. (1988). An information-processing model for the development of aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 14, 1324.
Huesmann, L. R., & Guerra, N. G. (1997). Childrens normative beliefs
about aggression and aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 72, 408 419.
Huesmann, L. R., & Miller, L. S. (1994). Long-term effects of repeated
exposure to media violence in childhood. In L. R. Huesmann (Ed.),
Aggressive behavior: Current perspectives (pp. 153186). New York:
Plenum Press.
Johnson, J. D., Adams, M. S., Ashburn, L., & Reed, W. (1995). Differential
gender effects on African American adolescents acceptance of teen
dating violence. Sex Roles, 33, 597 605.
Johnson, J. D., Jackson, L. A., & Gatto, L. (1995). Violent attitudes and
deferred academic aspirations: Deleterious effects of exposure to rap
music. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 16, 27 41.
Lindsay, J. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2000). From antecedent conditions to
violent actions: A general affective aggression model. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 533547.
Marsh, D. (1993). Louie Louie. New York: Hyperion.
McClelland, G. H., & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical difficulties of detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychological Bulletin, 114,
376 390.
McFarland, R. A. (1984). Effects of music upon emotional content of TAT
stories. Journal of Psychology, 116, 227234.
Mischel, W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality. Psychological Review, 80, 252283.
Mueller, C., & Donnerstein, E. (1977). The effects of humor-induced
arousal upon aggressive behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 11, 73 82.
Peterson, D. L., & Pfost, K. S. (1989). Influence of rock videos on attitudes
of violence against women. Psychological Reports, 64, 319 322.
Roberts, D. F., Christenson, P. G., & Gentile, D. A. (in press). In D. Gentile
(Ed.), Media violence and children. Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing.
Roediger, H. L., Weldon, M. S., Stadler, M. L., & Riegler, G. L. (1992).
Direct comparison of two implicit memory tests: Word fragment and
word stem completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 18, 12511269.
Rubin, A. M., West, D. V., & Mitchell, W. S. (2001). Differences in
aggression, attitudes towards women, and distrust as reflected in popular
music preferences. Media Psychology, 3, 25 42.
Sherman, A. (1991). Hello mudduh, hello fadduh! (A letter from camp). On

EXPOSURE TO SONGS WITH VIOLENT LYRICS


Dr. Demento 20th anniversary collection, disc one [CD]. Santa Monica,
CA: Rhino Records.
Simmons, J., McDaniels, D., Coward, R., Criss, A., & Gist, K. (1993). Hit
em hard [Recorded by Run DMC]. On down with the king [CD]. New
York: Profile Records.
Simmons, J., McDaniels, D., & Phillips, P. (1993). In the house [Recorded
by Run DMC]. On down with the king [CD]. New York: Profile Records.
Silverstein, S. (1994). A boy named Sue [Recorded by Johnny Cash]. On
Johnny Cash super hits [CD]. New York: Columbia Records.
St. Lawrence, J. S., & Joyner, D. J. (1991). The effects of sexually violent
rock music on males acceptance of violence against women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 15, 49 63.
Suicidal Tendencies. (1994). I wouldnt mind. On Suicidal for life [CD].
New York: Sony.
Suicidal Tendencies. (1994). Love vs. loneliness. On Suicidal for life [CD].
New York: Sony.
Took, K. J., & Weiss, D. S. (1994). The relationship between heavy metal
and rap music and adolescent turmoil: Real or artifact? Adolescence, 29,
613 621.
Tool. (1992). Jerk-off. On Opiate [CD]. New York: BMG Music.
Tool. (1993). Four degrees. On Undertow [CD]. New York: Volcano
Recordings/BMG.

971

Violent Femmes. (1990). I held her in my arms. On The blind leading the
naked [CD]. New York: Warner Brothers.
Violent Femmes. (2000). Country death song. On Hallowed ground [CD].
Santa Monica, CA: Rhino Records.
Waite, B. M., Hillbrand, M., & Foster, H. G. (1992). Reduction of aggressive behavior after removal of Music Television. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 43, 173175.
Wanamaker, C. E., & Reznikoff, M. (1989). The effects of aggressive and
nonaggressive rock songs on projective and structured tests. The Journal
of Psychology, 123, 561570.
Wester, S. R., Crown, C. L., Quatman, G. L., & Heesacker, M. (1997). The
influence of sexually violent rap music on attitudes of men with little
prior exposure. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 497508.
Yankovic, A. (1996). Gump. On Bad hair day [CD]. Santa Monica, CA:
All American Music Group.
Yankovic, A. (1996). The night Santa went crazy. On Bad hair day [CD].
Santa Monica, CA: All American Music Group.

Received July 11, 2002


Revision received December 5, 2002
Accepted December 15, 2002

You might also like