Resilience, Helplessness, Control Orientations and Set

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

RESILIENCE, HELPLESSNESS, CONTROL

ORIENTATIONS AND SET


Lance King
16.08.06

Resilience and Helplessness


In her book Self-Theories: Their Role in Motivation, Personality and
Development (1999), Carol Dweck describes the characteristics of resilience as
being an orientation towards setting learning goals, adopting mastery behaviour
and believing in the flexibility of intelligence and the primacy of effort; and the
characteristics of helplessness as being an orientation towards setting
performance goals, adopting challenge avoidance behaviour and a belief in the
fixedness of intelligence and the primacy of ability. These personal orientations
are most clearly demonstrated in reactions to failure situations where resilient
individuals tend to attribute failure to a lack of effort and are prepared to take
effective remedial action while more helpless individuals attribute failure to a lack
of ability and tend to give up (Dweck, 1999).

This model is supported by many studies: Koestner and Zuckerman (1994)


investigated the goal orientations of 60 college students and found that those
who were performance oriented often exhibited classic helpless behaviours,
including making self-defeating performance attributions and negative selfevaluations. Conversely those who were learning oriented tended to exhibit more
adaptive behaviours and were more mastery oriented. In Australia support has
come from a study of 893 college students where the learning oriented students
showed a much more positive attitude towards their studies and were more likely
to choose a difficult task to complete than their performance oriented colleagues
who opted for more easy tasks (Archer, 1994). Burley, Turner & Vitulli (1999)
studying US college students (ages 17-59) found that learning orientated
students showed much more adaptive achievement-oriented behaviours than
their performance oriented colleagues and found that the age of the student was

a good predictor of their orientation with the older students being much more
likely to be learning oriented in comparison with the more performance oriented
younger students.

To summarise the existing research with respect to student behaviour:

RESILIENT
STUDENTS
set learning goals
learn in order to
understand

HELPLESS
STUDENTS
set performance goals
learn in order to get
excellence or an A
pass

tasks

to test themselves, to
work towards mastery

to gain approval or
avoid disapproval

challenge

seek out new


challenges

avoid new challenges

to achieve success

believe effort is more


important than ability

believe ability is more


important than effort

reaction to failure

focus on the process,


find the problem,
change the process,
learn from their
mistakes, put in more
effort

blame themselves,
repeat the same
process or do even
less, give up

view of intelligence

believe intelligence is
flexible and can be
developed and
improved the more I
learn, the smarter I get

believe intelligence is
fixed, unalterable with
a definite limit I can
learn new things but
my intelligence stays
the same

goals

Students who feel confident, have a sense of agency and perceive meaning in
their academic work will pursue learning goals (Seifert, 2004, p.145).

This dichotomy of behaviour and belief between the two extremes of personal
orientations is very clear but the questions remain:
-

can students learn to be resilient, and if so how?

are there structures or systems that can be put in place in educational

And

institutions to incline more students towards a resilient frame of mind?

Helplessness and Control


In looking at the research it is clear that parallels can be drawn between the
helplessness/resiliency model and ideas about control:

The learned helplessness paradigm (Seligman, 1975) suggests that when


people believe they are powerless to control what happens to them, they
become passive and restrictive in coping abilities. On the other hand,
when individuals believe that events and outcomes are controllable,
learned helplessness is avoided, and, instead, active attempts are made
to overcome adversive situations (Luthar, 1991, p. 600).

The control that a person actually has or perceives that they have is cited often in
the literature as the most significant determinant of helpless or resilient
behaviour.

Gernigon, Fleurance & Reine showed that with junior high students learning a
perceptual motor task only a controllable situation ending in success contributes
to the development of learned competence, and only an uncontrollable situation
ending in failure induces learned helplessness (2000, p.53).
Experiences with uncontrollable events may lead to the expectation that future
events will elude control, resulting in disruptions in motivation, emotion and
learning termed learned helplessness (Peterson, 1995, p. 12).

The expectation of non-contingency (between acts and outcomes) is the crucial


determinant of the symptoms of learned helplessness(Valas, 2001, p. 72)

Generalisation across these studies support the idea that the control the
individual can exert or believes s/he can exert over any given situation is the
critical pre-disposing factor for an orientation towards helplessness or resilience.

Expectation, Attribution and Control


Firmin, Hwang, Copella and Clark (2004) found that 1st year psychology students
(from a private mid-western US university) who started an examination by
attempting difficult questions first performed significantly poorer on the
subsequent easy questions than their fellow students who started with the easy
questions first; even though the results showed that both groups had achieved as
well as each other on the difficult questions. This study highlights the point that it
was the expectation of failure, not failure itself, that produced helplessness and
the deterioration of academic performance.
Expectations of success or failure are related directly to attributions the
messages we give ourselves about the causes of events that we are involved in.
Attributions generally have three dimensions locus (does the cause originate
within the individual?), stability (is the cause stable or changeable?), and
controllability (can the individual influence the cause?). Students who attribute
success and failure to internal, controllable causes are more likely to take action
to produce positive outcomes and develop an expectation of success whereas
students who attribute both success and failure to causes outside themselves
over which they have no control are likely to feel helpless and to develop
expectations of failure (Seifert, 2004).

In a study of 1430 high school dropouts in the USA, Suh & Suh (2006) analysed
the characteristics of those who went on to gain university degrees and found

that the three most prominent factors associated with degree attainment were
academic aspiration, organisational skill and (internal) locus of control.

In research into distance education Morris and Wu (2005) found that the
combined presence of the two factors of available financial aid and an internal
locus of control enabled them to predict completion likelihood (and consequently
the likelihood of dropping out) for individuals with a 74.5% accuracy.

In the educational context, locus of control is revealed through the attributions we


make for our successes and failures at school tasks. If someone believes they
have some control over their task outcomes they are more likely to persevere,
put in effort, learn from mistakes and take action to produce the result they want.
But what are the factors that produce an internal locus of control?

Success, Failure and Control


Interestingly enough there is some research to suggest that (American students
at least) believe that their lives are more and more controlled by outside forces.
Twenge, Zhang & Im (2004) report that the average college student in 2002 had
a more external locus of control than 80% of college students in the early 1960s
(p. 308). And given the events of 11/9 2001, it is, I guess, not surprising that
there have been generalisations in attributions made across the (USA)
community which have resulted in increased belief that events are out of the
control of the average person. Unfortunately, as Tweng et al. report, the
implications are uniformly negative, as externality is correlated with poor school
achievement, helplessness, ineffective stress management, decreased selfcontrol, and depression (p. 309).

From the research presented here it would seem that the necessary conditions
for increasing a students internality would be for them to 1) have some
experience of taking control of their own learning, 2) gain some success from
doing so and 3) notice the connection. This would theoretically then lead to the

student building up more personal attributions of successful control, more


expectation of academic success and would lead to more successful, and more
effective learning.
A study of Chinese and Korean students bears this out students with higher
academic grades scored higher on internality and lower on externality (Park &
Kim, 1998, p. 191) and also honour students were found to be more likely to
attribute their success to effort and were less likely to attribute any failure to a
lack of ability than were the students on academic probation (p.191).

This idea is also supported by an USA study of first year university students
which reported that those students who entered university with lower scores on
the locus of control scale (internals) obtained significantly higher GPAs than
those who scored higher (externals) on the same scale (Gifford, Briceno-Perriot
& Mianzo, 2006, p. 19). [GPA = students grade point averages across all
subjects at the end of their first year of university study]

Also conversely if students are immersed in learning situations in which they


have little or no control over their own learning one might expect to increase
externality and decrease effectiveness as shown by Chaput De Saintongue &
Dunn (1998) - Learning environments where adverse events are perceived as
being pervasive and inalterable will prevent the development of the autonomous
learner and impair student achievement (p. 583).

These differences in academic success may be attributable to the different


reactions to stress between internally and externally oriented students - Wolk and
Bloom (1978) reported that more internal students found high stress and time
constraints facilitated their task performance but the same pressures were
debilitating effects for the more external students, and a 1991 study of 144 high
risk adolescents showed that in comparison to children with an internal locus of

control, those with an external orientation showed greater declines in functioning


with increasing stress levels (Luther, 1991).

Teaching Strategies and Control


Helping students to become more internally control oriented would appear from
the evidence here to be a high effect strategy for improving academic success.

In order to help students to gain a more internal locus of control the first
requirement for the teacher/tutor/lecturer is to give more of the control of the
teaching/learning interface to the students. One way to facilitate this process is
for the teacher to adopt more student centred rather than traditional teacher
centred approaches to learning. The keys to student centred learning according
to Biggs (1999) are:
-

reliance upon active rather than passive learning

increased responsibility, accountability and autonomy of the learner

interdependence between teacher and learner (as opposed to


complete dependence or independence)

mutual respect and a reflexive approach to teaching and learning

and a commitment by both parties to consult about all aspects of the


teaching learning process.

Such approaches which focus on supporting the autonomy of the student have
long been shown to increase involvement and intrinsic motivation of the student.
As Reeve, Jang, Carrell, Jeon & Barch (2004), found in a study of 20 teachers
from two mid-western high school in the USA where who were trained in
autonomy supportive behaviours, the teachers were able to teach and
motivate their students in more autonomy supportive ways. We also found that
the more teachers used autonomy supportive instructional behaviours, the more
engagement their students showed (p.165). Also from the USA, Filak and
Sheldon (2003) showed that the autonomy supportiveness of teachers has been

shown to very important for maximal learning, growth and creativity of students
(p. 236).

Student autonomy can only occur if students gain some control over their own
learning process.

According to Martin and Marsh of the University of Western Sydney:


Students also develop a sense of control when they see that they are able
to make choices and decisions in class that affect the way work is done.
One way to do this is to provide students with choices over class
objectives, assessment tasks, criteria for assessment, and due dates for
work assigned (2003, p. 36).

SET and Control


Another way to give students more control in the classroom is to engage them
with the process of SET the student evaluation of teaching.

SET is an instrument designed to assess quality - the quality of teaching. As long


as two preconditions are met:
-

anonymity - the teacher cannot identify any individual student and

confidentiality - the results are confidential to the teacher

then SET creates the perfect learning loop for both teachers and students.
Students become empowered with the ability to have some influence over the
teaching process and teachers become empowered with the ability to continually
improve the effectiveness of their teaching which improves the effectiveness of
the students learning and so on.

The process of SET is in itself a process of providing for student influence and
autonomy with the students motivation to participateimpacted significantly by
their expectation that they will be able to provide meaningful feedback - Chen &
Hoshower, (2003, p. 84). As long as students have evidence that their feedback

is being used to improve the course then the SET process itself will help students
to become more internal in their control orientations by showing them that they
can have influence over their own teaching/learning interface. One way cited in
Chen & Hoshower to achieve this is to require every instructor to cite on the
course syllabus one recent example of how student evaluations have helped
improve this particular course or have helped the instructor to improve his or her
teaching (p. 84). Following on from this it would come as no surprise that
students who believe that their feedback on evaluations will improve teaching, or
the course, or both, should be highly motivated to provide such feedback (p.84).

But is that motivation, and/or the grades or scores given to individual teachers
dependent on the control orientation of the student? One would expect the more
internally oriented students to be more willing to take part in SET and to be more
thorough in their answers and maybe more constructive in their criticism of tutors
but as most SETs do not offer an element of choice this is hard to gauge. In
looking at the grades awarded by students to tutors, Rich and Bush (1978)
looked for congruence between high and low faculty control style in teaching and
internal and external student control style in orientation. They found that
students identified as having an internal locus of control who experience low
faculty control style will yield more favourable student evaluations of instruction,
similarly, a high controlling instructional style will yield more favourable ratings
from externally controlled students (p. 196-7).

In a similar more recent study Grimes, Millea & Woodruff (2004) propose that
the degree to which students do not accept personal responsibility for their
performance and grades significantly affects their overall evaluation of teaching
effectiveness and course satisfaction (p.130). In other words they are
suggesting that more internally oriented students are more likely to use
successful study strategies, cope better with stress and achieve higher grades
and so award higher evaluations of teachers than externally oriented students
who are more likely to study badly, cope poorly, receive low grades and blame

the whole problem on the teacher! This view was borne out by their study which
showed that more internally oriented students had a greater probability of
assigning above average evaluation marks with respect to instructor performance
whereas more externally oriented students had a greater probability of assigning
average and below average instructor evaluation marks (p. 129).

So is this anything new? Internally oriented students take responsibility for their
own learning, earn better grades and give good SETs to teachers. But are SETs
themselves a good mechanism for increasing the internal control orientation of
students? Filak and Sheldon (2003) in a study of 1,269 undergraduates
concluded that students feelings of competence and autonomy were significant
predictors of both teacher and course evaluations (p. 244) which would seem to
suggest that the teachers who allowed more for the development of control by
students scored consistently higher than other teachers, independent of the
control orientation of the student.

Conclusion
Establishing some control over ones own learning would appear to be a critical
factor in both avoiding helplessness in an academically challenging situation and
achieving consistent success. The development of an internal locus of control
with respect to learning is predicated upon the student experiencing control or
autonomy with respect to some of the parameters of learning. One area that
teachers/tutors/lecturers can allow students to have influence over is in delivery
methods in the classroom. One mechanism to achieve this is through the Student
Evaluation of Teaching. By using SET on a regular basis, and being willing to
change teaching method to suit the learning of the student, teachers can
demonstrate to students the efficacy of good quality feedback in improving
teaching and learning and on the success of that learning for the student. This
mechanism will also demonstrate to the student the advantages of using
influence and taking control of some of the parameters of learning. This should in
turn increase the internality of the students locus of control which will lead to

more successful learning. The consequences for the teacher will be more
efficient delivery methods, more academic success for their students and better
evaluations.
A win-win situation.

Future Directions
The author was not able to locate any papers which seek to explore the influence
of the use of SET on the development of control orientations in students. This
indicates a good area for future study.

Lance King

REFERENCES
Archer, J. (1994). Achievement goals as a measure of motivation in university
students. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19, 430-446.
Burley, R. C., Turner, L. A., & Vitulli, W. F. (1999). The relationship between goal
orientation and age among adolescents and adults. The Journal of Genetic
Psychology, 160, 84-89.
Biggs, J. B. (1999). Teaching for quality learning at university: What the student
does. Philadelphia: Open University Press.
Chaput De Saintongue, D. M., & Dunn, D. M. (1998). The helpless learner: A
pilot study in clinical students. Medical Teacher, 20(6), 583-587.
Chen, Y., & Hoshower, L. B. (2003). Student evaluation of teaching
effectiveness: an assessment of student perception and motivation. Assessment
& Evaluation in Higher Education, 28(1), 71-86.
Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self Theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and
development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
Filak, V. F., & Sheldon, K. M. (2003). Student psychological need satisfaction
and college teacher-course evaluations. Educational Psychology, 23(3), 235-247.
Firmin, M., Hwang, C., Copella, M., & Clark, S. (2004). Learned helplessness:
the effect of failure on test-taking. Education, 124(4), 688-674.

Gernigon, C., Fleurance, P., & Reine, B. (2000). Effects of uncontrollability and
failure on the development of learned helplessness in perceptual-motor tasks.
Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71(1), 44-55.
Gifford, D. D., Briceno-Perriot, J., & Mizano, F. (2006). Locus of control:
Academic achievement and retention in a sample of university first-year students.
Journal of College Admission 191, 18-25.
Grimes, P. W., Millea, M. J., & Woodruff, T. W. (2004). Grades whos to blame?
Student evaluation of teaching and locus of control. Journal of Economic
Education, 35(2), 129-147.
Koestner, R., & Zuckerman, M. (1994). Causality orientations, failure and
achievement. Journal of Personality, 62, 321-345.
Lea, S. J., Stephenson, D., & Troy, J. (2003). Higher education students
attitudes to student-centred learning: beyond educational bulimia? Studies in
Higher Education, 28(3), 321-334.
Luthar, S. S. (1991). Vulnerability and resilience: A study of high risk
adolescents. Child Development, 62, 600-616.
Martin, A. J., & Marsh, H. W. (2003). Fear of failure: Friend or foe?. Australian
Psychologist, 38(1), 31-38.
Morris, L. V., Wu, S., & Finnegan, C. L. (2005). Predicting retention in online
general education courses. The American Journal of Distance Education, 19(1).
23-36.
Park, Y-S., & Kim, U. (1998). Locus of control, attributional style, and academic
achievement: Comparative analysis of Korean, Korean-Chinese, and Chinese
students. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 191-208.
Peterson, C. (1995). Learned helplessness: A theory for the age of personal
control. New York: Oxford University Press.
Reeve, J., Jang, H., Carrell, D., Jeon, S., & Barch, J. (2004). Enhancing students
engagement by increasing teachers autonomy support. Motivation and Emotion,
28(2), 147-169
Rich, H. L., & Bush, A. J. (1978). An investigation of the joint influence of faculty
control and student locus of reinforcement on instructional evaluation. Journal of
Educational Research 71(4), 194-198.
Seifert, T. L. (2004). Understanding student motivation. Educational Research,
46(2), 137-149.

Suh, S., & Suh, J. (2006). Educational engagement and degree attainment
among high school dropouts. Educational Research Quarterly, 29(3), 11-20.
Turner, J. C., Meyer, D. K., Midgley, C., & Patrick, H. (2003). Teacher discourse
and sixth graders reported affect and achievement behaviours in two highmastery/high-performance mathematics classrooms. The Elementary School
Journal, 103(4), 357-383.
Twenge, J. M., Zhang, L., & Im, C. (2004). Its beyond my control: A crosstemporal meta-analysis of increasing externality in locus of control, 1960-2002.
Personality & Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 308-319.
Wolk, S., & Bloom, D. (1978). The interactive effects of locus of control and
situational stress upon performance accuracy and time. Journal of Personality,
46(2), 279-299.

You might also like