CNTRICS Final Task Selection: Executive Control
CNTRICS Final Task Selection: Executive Control
CNTRICS Final Task Selection: Executive Control
115135, 2009
doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn154
Advance Access publication on November 14, 2008
The third meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia
(CNTRICS) was focused on selecting promising measures
for each of the cognitive constructs selected in the first
CNTRICS meeting. In the domain of executive control,
the 2 constructs of interest were rule generation and selection and dynamic adjustments in control. CNTRICS
received 4 task nominations for each of these constructs,
and the breakout group for executive control evaluated
the degree to which each of these tasks met prespecified criteria. For rule generation and selection, the breakout group
for executive control recommended the intradimensional/
extradimensional shift task and the switching Stroop for
translation for use in clinical trial contexts in schizophrenia
research. For dynamic adjustments in control, the breakout
group recommended conflict and error adaptation in the
Stroop and the stop signal task for translation for use in
clinical trials. This article describes the ways in which
each of these tasks met the criteria used by the breakout
group to recommend tasks for further development.
Key words: cognition/clinical trials/schizophrenia
Deficits in executive control have long been thought to be
one of the hallmark cognitive characteristics of schizophrenia. Executive control processes are those mechanisms that
allow individuals to flexibly and dynamically adjust their
performance in response to changing environmental
demands and changing internal goal states. Numerous empirical studies have shown that individuals with schizo1
The Author 2008. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Maryland Psychiatric Research Center. All rights reserved.
For permissions, please email: [email protected].
115
D. M. Barch et al.
Table 1. Description of Tasks Nominated for Rule Generation and Selection Construct Definition: The Processes Involved in Activating
Task-Related Goals or Rules Based on Endogenous or Exogenous Cues, Actively Representing Them in a Highly Accessible Form, and
Maintaining This Information Over an Interval During Which That Information Is Needed to Bias and Constrain Attention and Response
Selection7
Tasks Recommended For Translation
CANTAB ID/ED task
Construct validity
Supported by both human and animal research
Designed to decompose the WCST
Neural systems
Evidence for role of ventro- and dorslolateral prefrontal cortex in extradimensional shifting (rule selection) from human and animal
research
Evidence for role of dopamine and norepinephrine in extradimensional shifting (rule selection) from human and animal research
Animal models
Animal models available for rhesus monkey, marmoset, and rat
Performance in schizophrenia
Consistent evidence for impairments in extradimensional shifting
Mixed evidence for impairments in intradimensional shifting
Psychometric data
Low test-retest reliability
Practice effects influenced by insight and learning on task over time
Potential confounds of generalized deficits with differences in discriminating power across conditions
Future directions
Modifications needed to improve test-retest reliability and to reduce practice effects
Evaluation of discriminating power across conditions is needed. If not equal, modifications will be needed to improve ability to
detect differential deficits
Switching stroop
Construct validity
Supported by computational modeling and individual differences research
Is a good measure of rule selection, but not of rule generation
Neural systems
Evidence from functional neuroimaging on the importance of DLPFC and ACC
Evidence from lesions studies for importance of DLPFC
Pharmacological sensitivity
Evidence that dopamine augmentation can improve Stroop performance in schizophrenia, but may not be specific to rule selection
Animal models
There are rat and primate models of paradigms described as measuring similar constructs
The degree to which the animal models (especially the rat models) are homologous to the human paradigm is an open question
Performance in schizophrenia
Evidence for deficits in schizophrenia in the ability to select and apply the appropriate cue on the switching Stroop
Evidence for reduced DLPFC and ACC activity in schizophrenia during the performance of incongruent Stroop trials
Psychometric data
Little psychometric data available on test-rest reliability, practice effects, or floor/ceiling effects
Future directions
Research needed on assessing and improving the psychometric characteristics of the switching Stroop
Research needed on ways to make the use of the Switch Stroop feasible in clinical trials contexts (eg, task length, standardized
administration, enhanced sensitivity to detect deficits, etc.)
Evaluation of discriminating power across conditions is needed. If not equal, modifications will be needed to improve ability to
detect differential deficits
Evaluation of the homology of putative animal models and development of better animal models if needed
Other nominated tasks
1-2 AX-CPT
Construct validity
Task modeled in a computational model of prefrontal-basal ganglia contributions to working memory
Neural systems
No published data
Pharmacological sensitivity
117
Pharmacological sensitivity
Numerous studies in humans and animals showing that ID/ED performance is sensitive to a variety of pharmacological
manipulations
Evidence that modafinal can improve ID/ED performance in schizophrenia
D. M. Barch et al.
Table 1. Continued
Tasks Recommended For Translation
remains relevant (eg, lines or shapes), but the specific feature within the dimension that is the target changes (eg,
which colored shape). This stage is designed to reinforce
perceptual dimension learning and to provide a control
for the crucial extradimensional shift stage. For the initial
intradimensional shift (IDS stage), color-filled shapes remain the only relevant dimension, then later after intradimensional shift reversal (IDR stage), again there are
new stimuli/exemplars for the extradimensional shift
(EDS state) in which the white lines become the only relevant dimension. This is followed by the extradimensional reversal stage (EDR), in which subjects must shift
between dimensions. There are thus 9 stages in all, designated SD, SDR, C-D, CD, CDR, IDS, IDR, EDS, and
EDR. The task has 2 modes: a clinical mode for testing
only once and 7 parallel modes for repeated testing. The
task has 18 outcome measures, assessing errors, and numbers of trials and stages completed.
From extensive experience with the task, the developers
have a number of recommendations for optimal task use.
First, although it is feasible to begin the task with lines and
shift to shapes, it is recommended that the initial learning
be fixed at shape. Second, they recommend, time permitting, that the subjects are screened with a short instructional test (Big circle/little circle discrimination) which
tests the subjects ability to understand what a rule is
and to be able to reverse it on command. Third, there is
a fixed rubric for the verbal instructions for this test which
are minimal but which should be followed closely.
Construct Validity
The theoretical validity of the test was established over 30
years ago and stems from early animal and human learn118
ing theory on discrimination learning, reversal, and dimensional shifting. More recent computational
approaches have confirmed for example that reversal
learning and extradimensional shifting necessarily engage
different processes and have also considered the paradigm in the context of a theory of frontal lobe function
based on working memory.18 The CANTAB test was
designed partly to decompose the well-known Wisconsin
Card Sort test (WCST) into its constituent parts including a critical extradimensional shift stage that is probably
equivalent to a category shift on the WCST. The initial
validation study demonstrating cross-species advantage
for IDS over EDS performance in humans and marmosets was by Roberts et al.19 There is an extensive and
growing bibliography of the ID/ED task, and the reader
is referred to www.camcog.com for references and
abstracts. The following constitutes a brief overview.
Neural Systems
The neural substrates of the test (or similar variants of it,
with changes in the stimuli but not the basic principles of
test design) have been defined on the basis of human neuropsychological data in brain damaged patients, functional imaging data (both functional magnetic
resonance imaging [fMRI] and positron emission tomography),20,21 and animal (monkey, rat, mouse) lesion studies. There is converging evidence that reversal learning
implicates orbitofrontal cortex function in humans, monkeys, and rats.20,2227 There is also additional evidence for
ventromedial striatal and posterior parietal involvement.20,21,28 There is considerable evidence for a role for
the primate ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in extradimensional shift learning20,22 as well as other posterior cortical
No published data
Animal models
No published data
Performance in schizophrenia
No published data
Psychometric data
No published data
Groton maze learning task
Construct validity
Not clear that the task selectively measures rule selection or generation as performance may be strongly influenced by the ability
to learn from feedback and visual spatial processing abilities
Neural systems
Evidence that performance can elicit feedback-related error negatives in ERP studies9
Pharmacological sensitivity
Evidence that speed and efficiency of performance can be improved10
Animal models
Cincinnati Water Maze test in rodents may be an animal homologue11
Performance in schizophrenia
Evidence for excessive rule-breaking errors in schizophrenia12
Psychometric data
Preliminary evidence of good psychometric characteristics13
Performance in Schizophrenia
There have been over 20 studies so far listed in the CANTAB bibliography on the ID/ED test in schizophrenia,
beginning with Elliott et al.44,45 These studies have
been not only in chronic schizophrenia46,47 but also
in first-episode psychosis.48,49 Comparisons with other
disorders have included comparisons with bipolar disorder and depression, Alzheimers, Parkinsons, and
Huntingtons diseases, OCD and ADHD, neurosurgical
patients with damage to the frontal and temporal lobes, as
well as amygdalo-hippocampectomy (see CANTAB Web
site). An explicit comparison of chronic schizophrenia
with frontal and temporal lobe lesions was published
by Pantelis et al.46 That study showed that people
with chronic schizophrenia were severely impaired
at both the intradimensional and extradimensional
shifting stages, compared with both patients with lateral
PFC damage (who were mainly impaired at the extradimensional shifting stage) and temporal lobe lesioned
patients (who did not differ from age- and IQ-matched
controls). Other studies have shown that people with
chronic schizophrenia can also fail at the simple reversal
stage or compound discrimination stages.44,50 Jazbec
et al provided one of the first direct comparisons of ID/
ED performance and the WCST in schizophrenia, with significant correlations at the extradimensional shift stage.50
In terms of pharmacological treatment, Turner et al47
showed, in a double-blind placebo-controlled proof of
concept study, that acutely administered modafinil added
to conventional antipsychotic medication in schizophrenia improved several aspects of performance including
the ID/ED test. This proof-of-concept study demonstrated that it was indeed plausible to improve aspects
of cognitive function in schizophrenia and was subsequently back translated to show improvement in the
rat plus PCP model.43
Animal Models
There are number of different animal models of ID/ED
task performance that have been developed. For example, there is a paradigm for rhesus monkeys,42 and there
have been a number of studies of ID/ED performance in
Psychometric Data
Standardized data on 341 elderly subjects were published
in Robbins et al.51 Performance on the ID/ED test declined quiet sharply in the eldest cohort (>70) especially
119
marmosets.19,22 In addition, Birrell and Brown established a rat model based on textures and odors rather
than visual stimuli.29 This has been used to establish
the neuroanatomical substrates of the task in rats (see
above) and for pharmacological studies. It has also
been used for models of schizophrenia (eg, chronic
PCP treatment, amphetamine sensitization, isolation
rearing, MAM pretreatment). A typical study has shown
benefit of a PDE inhibitor or modafinil against deficits
produced by PCP at the extradimensional shift stage
that are not remediated by conventional antipsychotic
DA receptor antagonists.43
D. M. Barch et al.
Future Directions
More analysis of the neural and neurochemical substrates
of performance is needed in both humans and animal
subjects. Several specific issues require resolution or consolidation, as indicated above. These can be pursued using functional neuroimaging approaches in humans and
lesions or neuropharmacological manipulations in experimental animals. Longitudinal studies are also required
for schizophrenia, and other studies of pharmacological
effects, though proof of concept that ID/ED performance
can be enhanced has already been demonstrated. In psychometric terms, more data are required. The test may
have to be modified to provide good test-retest reliability.
For example, the fixed sequence of testing of the component processes ID shifting and ED shifting may need to
be replaced by a version of the test that interleaves the 2
forms of shifting. In addition, research is needed to determine whether deficits on specific stages of the task reflect differential deficits or generalized deficits that reflect
the greater discriminating power of the ED compared
with the ID, CD, or SD conditions. If ED conditions indeed have greater discriminating power, the test will need
to be modified to either match the ID and ED measures
on discriminating power or introduce another control
(eg, one in which set shifting deficits convey a performance advantage).
120
vidual differences in the ability to actively maintain representations in working memory predicts individual
differences in the ability to select and effect use the appropriate task rule during Stroop task performance.60
As with the computational modeling work, these data
have been interpreted as reflecting an important role
for prefrontally maintained representations in successful
Stroop performance. One thing to note with the switching
Stroop is that while it is a good measure of the ability to
select task rules, it is not a good measure of the ability
to generate rules as the experimenter provides the rule.
In addition, there may be some confounding of taskswitching demands with rule selection demands in the
switching Stroop, given that RTs could also reflect the
time it takes to switch from one rule to another (eg, color
vs word), as well as the ability to maintain and apply the
appropriate rule. Thus, care would need to be taken to
dissociate these effects in future work with the switching
Stroop.
Neural Systems
There have been only a handful of studies using functional imaging to examine the neural substrates of performance on the switching Stroop. However, these studies
have been particularly informative as to the specific roles
played by dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) vs anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) in task performance.
Specifically, these studies have demonstrated that cuerelated activity in DLPFC was enhanced on trials
in which the participants were cued to color name as
compared with word read, consistent with the hypothesis
that greater top-down control to support rule representation is needed on color-naming trials.61,62 In contrast,
121
Fig. 1. (A) Illustration of the Structure of the switching Stroop Task. The cue is either to color or word read and then the stimulus is
congruent, neutral, or incongruent. (B) An illustration of trial structure for computing conflict adaptation effects in the trial-by-trial Stroop.
D. M. Barch et al.
tion. However, these studies provide encouraging evidence of the ability to model Stroop-like task
performance in animals.
Performance in Schizophrenia
At least one study has specifically examined switching
Stroop performance in patients with schiophrenia.55
This work used a version of the switching Stroop in
which: (1) an auditory cue signaled whether participants
should word read or color name, (2) there were 2 different
delays between the cue and the onset of the stimulus (1 vs
5 s); and (3) the neutral stimuli for color naming were
rows of Xs. The schizophrenia patients showed disproportionate interference in errors compared with controls
for color naming as well as a disproportionate total
Stroop effect in reaction times compared with controls
for color naming (see figure 2). These effects did not interact with delays, suggesting that the magnitude of the
impairments was similar for both short and long delays
(see figure 2). Functional imaging work with the Stroop
has consistently revealed deficits in the activation of
DLPFC and ACC in schizophrenia. For example, Kerns
et al71 found reduced anterior cingulate activity during
incongruent Stroop trials in schizophrenia, Weiss
et al72 found underactivation of DLPFC and anterior cingulate in schizophrenia during performance of a Stroop
task, and Yucel et al73 found underactivation of paracingulate cortex in schizophrenia. These studies did not use
switching Stroop tasks specifically and instead used standard Stroop versions. However, standard Stroop versions do still assess the ability to select and apply the
appropriate rule, though they may just not be as sensitive
to this process as the switching Stroop. Deficits on the
Stroop task are also present in the relatives of individuals
with schizophrenia, suggesting that they are associated
with vulnerability to the disorder.74,75 In addition, recent
work has shown reduced DLPFC activity during performance of the Stroop in unaffected first-degree relatives of
patients, though intact ACC activity.76 However, to our
knowledge, the switching Stroop has not been used with
relatives of individuals with schizophrenia.
Psychometric Data
There is very little information about any psychometric
characteristics of the switching Stroop. Cohen et al did
report an internal consistently of 0.9 for incongruent
color-naming errors at a 5-s cue-probe delay and an internal consistency of 0.53 for color-naming facilitation at
a 5-s delay.
Future Directions
There are a number of different important future directions for research on the switching Stroop for use in clinical trial contexts in schizophrenia. As noted above, little
is known about the psychometrics of this task, and thus,
Accuracy
0.4
Proportion Errors
0.3
0.2
0.1
0
N
Long ISI
Short ISI
Long ISI
Short ISI
Color Naming
Word Reading
Reaction Times
1500
Millseconds
1300
1100
900
700
C
Short ISI
Long ISI
Color Naming
Patients with Schizophrenia
Short ISI
Long ISI
Word Reading
Patient Controls
Healthy Controls
Fig. 2. Stroop Task Accuracy and Reaction Times from Cohen et al.55 Reprinted with permission from the American Psychological
Association. C 5 congruent, N 5 neutral, I 5 incongruent.
D. M. Barch et al.
of control are examined by investigating 2 effects: (1) conflict adaptation77 and (2) posterror adjustments.78,79
Conflict adaptation is thought to reflect behavioral
adjustments following trials with high conflict (eg, incongruent trials) vs those with low conflict (eg, neutral or
congruent trials). In other words, participants are faster
to response to an incongruent trial that is preceded by an
incongruent trial compared with an incongruent trial preceded by a congruent trial.77,80 The interpretation of this
result has been that the conflict engendered by the first
incongruent trial leads to an upregulation in control
that reduces the conflict induced by the next incongruent
trial.80 Posterror adjustments are thought to reflect a similar process. Participants are typically slower, but more
accurate, on trials following errors vs trials following correct responses.78,79 This is again thought to reflect an
upregulation of control engendered by the awareness
(not necessarily conscious) of having made an error.
The executive control breakout group felt that all 4
tasks were reasonable measures of the construct of interest. However, the breakout group felt that the ANT, the
Simon, and the Stroop were all variations on a theme,
with a similar deep structure in terms of assessing dynamic adjustments in control. Of the 3, the breakout
group felt that the Stroop version has the largest extant
literature in terms of the cognitive and neural bases of
dynamic adjustments in control and the largest existing
literature in schizophrenia. Thus, the breakout group recommended that the Stroop version be put forth for immediate translation for use in clinical trials in
schizophrenia. In addition, the breakout group made
a similar recommendation for the stop signal task, given
that it measured a somewhat different aspect of dynamic
adjustments in control and thus might provide complimentary information. Table 2 provides a brief overview
of these 4 tasks and a very brief summary of their pros
and cons in regards to the selection criteria.
Table 2. Description of Tasks Nominated for Dynamic Adjustments in Control Construct Definition: The Processes Involved in Detecting
the Occurrence of Conflict or Errors in Ongoing Processing, Identifying the Type of Control Adjustments Needed, and Recruiting
Additional Control Processes7
Tasks Recommended For Translation
125
D. M. Barch et al.
Table 2. Continued
Tasks Recommended For Translation
patients.108 Damage to PFC does seem to reduce the likelihood that individuals will correct their errors, but does
not appear to reduce post error slowing, which is not consistent with the hypothesis that DLPFC is necessary for
conflict-related adjustments in control.109
The above discussion perhaps implies more consistency in the literature on the neural substrates of dynamics adjustments of control than would be apparent based
on a thorough review of the literature. Although there is
a large amount of data to support important roles for
DLPFC and ACC in dynamic adjustments of control,
there is also conflicting data and data that suggest other
roles for ACC in action monitoring102,110,111 (for a review,
see Carter and van Veen112). As noted above, some of the
open questions involve the degree to which ACC and/or
DLPFC function is necessary for dynamic adjustments of
control and the degree to which posterror and conflict
adaptation effects reflect the same mechanisms. This is
still a rich and active area of investigation that is generating much interesting data about the neutral substrates
of cognitive control.
Pharmacological and Behavioral Manipulation
A number of studies have examined pharmacological
influences on the ERN and both conflict adaptation
and posterror slowing, with mixed results. For example,
benzodiazepines reduce the magnitude of the ERN113,114
and show some evidence of reducing the N2 congruency
effect on the Stroop.114 Benzodiazepines do not appear to
reduce posterror slowing but do reduce the influence of
errors on conflict adaptation114 and reduce the number of
errors that are corrected.113 Yohimbine (a selective
alpha-2 adrenergic receptor antagonist) increases the
ERN but does not change posterror slowing. However,
Performance in schizophrenia
A selective impairment of ANT conflict processing in schizophrenia has been reported85
Psychometric data
Preliminary evidence of good test-retest reliablity86
Simon task
Construct validity
Behavioral studies have shown robust postconflict and posterror adaptation with this task87
Neural systems
Human fMRI studies have linked the ACC to conflict-related activity and subsequent adjustments in control and the DLPFC
to supporting those adjustments as for the Stroop88,89
Individuals with lesions to rostral ACC show impaired conflict adaptation on the Simon task 96
Pharmacological sensitivity
Unknown
Animal models
Animal models available for monkey and rat, though the degree of homology needs further research
Performance in schizophrenia
Unknown
Psychometric data
Unknown
Animal Models
A growing number of studies have examined conflict- and
error-related processing in nonhuman primate models.
These studies indicate that neurons in a number of medial
prefrontal regions are modulated in response to errors,
including anterior cingulate, supplementary motor
regions including the supplementary eye fields, and the
superior frontal gyrus.119121 However, it is less clear
in monkeys that these regions also signal nonerrorrelated conflict, with some studies suggesting conflictrelated activity122 and other not,121,123 and the temporal
dynamics of the error-related signal may differ from that
seen in humans. Some work has also examined the role of
DLPFC in conflict adaptation in nonhuman primates.
This work demonstrated that lesions to DLPFC reduced
postconflict behavioral adjustments, but that lesions to
the ACC did not.124 There is also recent work in a rat
model of posterror slowing. In this study, rats were
trained a simple reaction time task, in which they had
to hold a lever for a specific period of time. The rats
showed longer RTs on trials following an error vs those
trials following a correct response. Further, inactivation
of dorsal medial PFC reduced the amount of posterror
slowing shown by the rats.125
Performance in Schizophrenia
A growing number of studies have examined fMRI and
ERP measures of error and conflict processing in schizophrenia as wellasindicesofdynamicadjustmentsincontrol.
Several studies suggest that individuals with schizophrenia
show reduced error-related ACC responses71,126132 as well
as reduced posterror slowing71,126 on the Stroop task as well
as other tasks. However, there is also evidence that patients
with schizophrenia can show normal error correction
performance even in the context of reduced ACC responses
to errors127,133 and that the relationship between the
D. M. Barch et al.
GO TRIAL
>
[The time is takes to respond is the RT]
>
Stop Signal Delay
>
Stop Signal Delay
Stop Signal
Fig. 3. An Illustration of the Structure of the Stop Signal Task. On each trial, subjects are presented with a stimulus to which they must respond.
On some trials they receive a subsequent cue that indicates that they have to stop their response (the stop signal). If the stop signal comes
quickly after the initial stimulus, it is a short stop signal delay and it is easier for participants to halt their response. If the stop signal is presented
at a longer time following the initial stimulus, it is harder for participants to halt their response.
against one another and that the first one to finish determines whether a response is executed or inhibited. Although the strong assumption of independence is
unlikely to be correct,135 it nonetheless does a good job
of characterizing behavior in the stop signal task.136
The goal of the race model is to allow estimation of the
stop signal reaction time (SSRT), which is the amount of
time needed by the stop process to successfully inhibit
a response. There are a number of ways to estimate
SSRT;134 a commonly used method does this by determining the stop signal delay at which subjects fail to
stop on 50% of trials. This value can be estimated either
by using an adaptive staircase method to estimate it directly or by evaluating performance at a range of stop
signal delay (SSD) values and interpolating to estimate
it. Once is known, then the SSRT can be estimated as,
since failing on 50% of trials means that half of the
responses are faster than SSRT and half are slower,
which is equivalent to the median RT.
A comprehensive users guide to the stop signal task
was published by Logan and Cowan,134 Once the range
of SSD values are known, then the SSRT can be estimated, since failing on 50% of trials means that half of
the responses are faster than SSRT and half are slower,
which is equivalent to the median RT. which should be
read closely by any prospective user of the task. It is im128
Stop Signal
which eliminates confounds regarding frustration or error likelihood prediction. Psychometrically, it ensures
roughly equal numbers of successful and unsuccessful
stop trials, which provides increased power to contrast
these conditions; contrast this with the go/no-go paradigm, in which there is generally a very low rate
(;10%) of commission errors. Simulation studies136
also show that SSRT is more reliably estimated using
an adaptive tracking method than using a set of fixed
SSDs. The one caution, as noted above, is that adaptive
methods may be more likely to lead subjects to adopt
waiting strategies; careful instruction and continuous
feedback throughout testing can help avoid this problem.
The stop signal task measures the ability to reactively inhibit a response once it has been initiated. Its construct
validity as a measure of control has been demonstrated
primarily in the context of impulsivity and behavioral control. In the normal population, SSRT is correlated with
real-world impulsivity as measured using paper-and-pencil
impulsivity scales.137 A large body of research has linked
performance on the stop signal task to disorders of impulse
control, including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD) (reviewed by Lijffijt et al138), obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania,139 methamphetamine
abuse,140 and cocaine use.141,142
Neural Systems
Evidence regarding the neural systems supporting stop
signal performance in humans comes from both lesion
and imaging studies. A study of frontal lesion patients143
provided evidence that the right inferior frontal gyrus is
critical for stopping in this task; patients with lesions to
this area were showed lengthened SSRT values, and
SSRT was correlated with the amount of lesion in the region. However, another study found that the medial
superior prefrontal cortex (supplementary and presupplementary motor areas) is also important for response inhibition.144,145 Neuroimaging studies have
also implicated both of these prefrontal regions, which
show activity on stop trials in comparison to go trials
that are correlated across subjects with SSRT.146149
These studies have not generally found activity in right
inferior prefrontal or medial superior prefrontal cortex
for the comparison of successful vs unsuccessful inhibition; this is predicted by the race model because that
model proposes that successful inhibition is primarily
driven by the speed of the go process rather than differences in the effectiveness of the stop process.
There is also evidence that basal ganglia regions may be
involved in stop signal inhibition, specifically the subthalamic nucleus (STN). Stimulation of the STN in individuals
with Parkinsons disease resulted inimproved stop signal reaction time.150 Neuroimaging studies have also found that
Construct Validity
D. M. Barch et al.
Psychometric Characteristics
As noted above, the use of adaptive SSRT estimation
methods imbues the stop signal task with desirable psychometric characteristics. By adapting the SSD to each
subject to ensure 50% successful inhibition, it prevents
ceiling and floor effects (which are problematic in the
go/no-go task). In unpublished work (J. R. Cohen &
R. A. Poldrack, unpublished), there was no evidence
of practice effects in SSRT estimates obtained using
adaptive methods. Previous work has also shown that
the mean proportion of inhibition, when measured using
a fixed set of SSDs, exhibits high test-retest reliability.
Future Directions
Future work on the stop signal task should focus on at
least 4 areas. First, a better characterization of response
inhibition function across a range of neuropsychiatric
disorders is needed, in order to characterize whether there
is any specificity to the inhibitory deficits suggested by the
existing literature. These studies should also include unaffected relatives, particularly given the potential for antipsychotic drugs to interfere with inhibitory functions.
Second, research is needed on ways to develop better
adaptive methods for estimation of SSRT and better procedures to prevent strategic adaptations that result in
slowing of go response times. Third, work is needed
on ways to better delineate different aspects of the
stop signal paradigm and their potential effects on reli130
D.M.B; NIH to T.S.B; NIH; James S. McDonnell Foundation for Dr. Poldrack to R.A.P; MRC, Wellcome
Trust, McDonnell Foundation, and E.U. (FP6) to
T.W.R. He consults for Cambridge Cognition, E. Lilly,
GlaxoSmithKline and Allon Therapeutics.
Acknowledgments
References
1. Reichenberg A, Harvey PD. Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia: integration of performance-based
and brain imaging findings. Psychol Bull. 2007;133:833858.
2. Kerns JB, Berenbaum H. Cognitive impairments associated
with formal thought disorder in people with schizophrenia.
J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111:211224.
3. Delawalla Z, Barch DM, Fisher Eastep JL, et al. Factors
mediating cognitive deficits and psychopathology among
siblings of individuals with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull.
2006;32:525537.
4. Barch DM, Mitropoulou V, Harvey PD, New AS, Silverman
JM, Siever LJ. Context-processing deficits in schizotypal personality disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2004;113:556568.
5. McClure MM, Bowie CR, Patterson TL, et al. Correlations
of functional capacity and neuropsychological performance
in older patients with schizophrenia: evidence for specificity
of relationships? Schizophr Res. 2007;89:330338.
6. Kerns JG, Nuechterlein KH, Braver TS, Barch DM. Executive functioning component mechanisms and schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64:2633.
7. Carter CS, Barch DM, Buchanan RW, et al. Identifying
cognitive mechanisms targeted for treatment development
in schizophrenia: an overview of the first meeting of the
Cognitive Neuroscience Treatment Research to Improve
Cognition in Schizophrenia Initiative. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;
64:410.
8. Carter CS, Barch DM. Cognitive neuroscience-based
approaches to measuring and improving treatment effects
on cognition in schizophrenia: the CNTRICS initiative.
Schizophr Bull. 2007;33:11311137.
9. Mathewson K, Dywan J, Snyder PJ, Tays W, Segalowitz S.
Electrophysiological correlates of older and younger adults
response to error feedback using a maze learning task. Psychophysiology. in press.
10. Snyder AM, Maruff P, Pietrzak RH, Cromer JR, Snyder PJ.
Effect of treatment with stimulant medication on nonverbal
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.
19.
20.
21.
22.
23.
24.
25.
131
11.
executive function and visuomotor speed in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychol. 2008;14:211226.
Williams MT, Morford LL, McCrea AE, Wood SL, Vorhees
CV. Administration of D,L-fenfluramine to rats produces
learning deficits in the Cincinnati water maze but not the
Morris water maze: relationship to adrenal cortical output.
Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2002;24:783796.
Snyder PJ, Jackson CE, Piskulic D, Olver J, Norman TR,
Maruff P. Spatial working memory and problem solving in
schizophrenia: the effect of symptom stabilization with atypical antipsychotic medication. Psychiatry Res. in press.
Pietrzak RH, Maruff P, Mayes LC, Roman SA, Sosa JA,
Snyder PJ. An examination of the construct validity and
factor structure of the Groton Maze Learning Test, a new
measure of spatial working memory, learning efficiency,
and error monitoring. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2008;23:
433445.
Frank MJ, Loughry B, OReilly RC. Interactions between
frontal cortex and basal ganglia in working memory: a computational model. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2001;1:137160.
Pietrzak RH, Cohen H, Snyder PJ. Spatial learning efficiency and error monitoring in normal aging: an investigation using a novel hidden maze learning test. Arch Clin
Neuropsychol. 2007;22:235245.
Pietrzak RH, Maruff P, Mayes LC, Roman S, Sosa J, Snyder
PJ. Construct validity and factor structure of the groton maze
learning test: a new measure of spatial working memory,
learning efficiency, and error monitoring. under review.
Snyder AM, Maruff P, Pietrzak RH, Cromer JR, Snyder PJ.
Effect of treatment with Stimulant Medication on Nonverbal Executive Function and Visuomotor Speed in Children
with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD).
Child Neuropsychol. 2008;14(3):211226.
OReilly RC, Noelle DC, Braver TS, Cohen JD. Prefrontal
cortex and dynamic categorization tasks: representational
organization and neuromodulatory control. Cereb Cortex.
2002;12:246257.
Roberts AC, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. The effects of intradimensional and extradimensional shifts on visual discrimination learning in humans and non-human primates. Q J
Exp Psychol B. 1988;40:321341.
Hampshire A, Owen AM. Fractionating attentional control
using event-related fMRI. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16:16791689.
Rogers RD, Andrews TC, Grasby PM, Brooks DJ, Robbins
TW. Contrasting cortical and subcortical activations produced by attentional-set shifting and reversal learning in
humans. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000;12:142162.
Dias R, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts. Nature.
1996;380:6972.
Chudasama Y, Robbins TW. Dissociable contributions of
the orbitofrontal and infralimbic cortex to pavlovian
autoshaping and discrimination reversal learning: further evidence for the functional heterogeneity of the rodent frontal
cortex. J Neurosci. 2003;23:87718780.
Clarke HF, Dalley JW, Crofts HS, Robbins TW, Roberts
AC. Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion. Science. 2004;304:878880.
Clarke HF, Walker SC, Crofts HS, Dalley JW, Robbins
TW, Roberts AC. Prefrontal serotonin depletion affects reversal learning but not attentional set shifting. J Neurosci.
2005;25:532538.
D. M. Barch et al.
132
42.
43.
44.
45.
46.
47.
48.
49.
50.
51.
52.
53.
54.
55.
56.
57.
58.
26. Clarke HF, Walker SC, Dalley JW, Robbins TW, Roberts
AC. Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion is behaviorally and neurochemically specific. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:1827.
27. Chamberlain SR, Menzies L, Hampshire A, et al. Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive-compulsive
disorder and their unaffected relatives. Science. 2008;321:
421422.
28. Cools R, Clark L, Owen AM, Robbins TW. Defining the
neural mechanisms of probabilistic reversal learning using
event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci. 2002;22:45634567.
29. Birrell JM, Brown VJ. Medial frontal cortex mediates perceptual attentional set shifting in the rat. J Neurosci.
2000;20:43204324.
30. Park SB, Coull JT, McShane RH, et al. Tryptophan depletion
in normal volunteers produces selective impairments in learning and memory. Neuropharmacology. 1994;33:575588.
31. Rogers RD, Blackshaw AJ, Middleton HC, et al. Tryptophan depletion impairs stimulus-reward learning while
methylphenidate disrupts attentional control in healthy
young adults: implications for the monoaminergic basis of
impulsive behaviour. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1999;146:
482491.
32. Middleton HC, Sharma A, Agouzoul D, Sahakian BJ,
Robbins TW. Idazoxan potentiates rather than antagonizes
some of the cognitive effects of clonidine. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1999;145:401411.
33. Mehta MA, Manes FF, Magnolfi G, Sahakian BJ, Robbins
TW. Impaired set-shifting and dissociable effects on tests of
spatial working memory following the dopamine D2 receptor antagonist sulpiride in human volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2004;176:331342.
34. Lange KW, Paul GM, Robbins TW, Marsden CD. L-Dopa
and frontal cognitive function in Parkinsons disease. Adv
Neurol. 1993;60:475478.
35. Boulougouris V, Glennon JC, Robbins TW. Dissociable
effects of selective 5-HT(2A) and 5-HT(2C) receptor antagonists on serial spatial reversal learning in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;33:20072019.
36. Roberts AC, De Salvia MA, Wilkinson LS, et al. 6-Hydroxydopamine lesions of the prefrontal cortex in monkeys enhance performance on an analog of the Wisconsin Card
Sort Test: possible interactions with subcortical dopamine.
J Neurosci. 1994;14:25312544.
37. Crofts HS, Dalley JW, Collins P, et al. Differential effects of
6-OHDA lesions of the frontal cortex and caudate nucleus
on the ability to acquire an attentional set. Cereb Cortex.
2001;11:10151026.
38. Lapiz MD, Bondi CO, Morilak DA. Chronic treatment with
desipramine improves cognitive performance of rats in an attentional set-shifting test. Neuropsychopharmacology.
2007;32:10001010.
39. Tait DS, Brown VJ, Farovik A, Theobald DE, Dalley JW,
Robbins TW. Lesions of the dorsal noradrenergic bundle
impair attentional set-shifting in the rat. Eur J Neurosci.
2007;25:37193724.
40. Tunbridge EM, Bannerman DM, Sharp T, Harrison PJ.
Catechol-o-methyltransferase inhibition improves setshifting performance and elevates stimulated dopamine release in the rat prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2004;24:
53315335.
41. Liston C, Miller MM, Goldwater DS, et al. Stress-induced
alterations in prefrontal cortical dendritic morphology
77.
78.
79.
80.
81.
82.
83.
84.
85.
86.
87.
88.
89.
90.
91.
92.
93.
94.
95.
133
D. M. Barch et al.
134
135