Review Autism Spectrum Disorders: 2000 by Cell Press
Review Autism Spectrum Disorders: 2000 by Cell Press
Review Autism Spectrum Disorders: 2000 by Cell Press
Autism is a neurodevelopmental syndrome that is defined by deficits in social reciprocity and communication, and by unusual restricted, repetitive behaviors
(American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Autism is a
disorder that usually begins in infancy, at the latest, in
the first three years of life. Parents often first become
concerned because their child is not using words to
communicate, even though he or she recites passages
from videotapes or says the alphabet. Though social
deficits may not be immediately obvious in early years,
they become gradually more evident as a child becomes
more mobile and as other children become more socially
sophisticated. Young children with autism often do not
seek out others when they are happy, show or point to
objects of interest, or call their parents by name. In
preschool years, repetitive behaviors, such as using peripheral vision to look at lines or wheels, or specific hand
and finger movements, begin to develop.
Autism is a heterogeneous condition; no two children
or adults with autism have exactly the same profile,
but difficulties fall into core domains that are reliably
measured and usually consistent across time, even
though specific behaviors may change with development. A child who may spend a great deal of time spinning objects and watching them out of the corner of her
eye at age four, may not show this behavior at all as a
teenager, but may be fascinated by sunroofs or bald
heads or World War I. A child whose primary method
of communicating requests at age two is to lead his
mother to whatever he wants, place her hand on it, or
use her hand to gesture toward it, may learn to ask for
what he wants quite clearly and persistently by the time
he is three or four, but continues to show difficulty carrying out back and forth conversations and coordinating
eye contact and gestures.
Because of its links to genetics and neural development and the severe abnormalities in social interaction
by which it is defined, autism offers the opportunity for
scientists to study the neurobiological origins of social
communication skills basic to human behavior. For example, even as infants, typically developing children
are more adept at catching the eye of other people,
To whom correspondence should be addressed (e-mail: cathy@
yoda.bsd.uchicago.edu).
Review
coordinating vocalizations with their intentions, and communicating all but the most extreme emotions with facial
expressions than are much older children or adults with
autism. Autism is a disorder of contrasts between
spared abilities and deficits in areas of social-communicative development that we take for granted: a child
with autism who can recite the alphabet and recognize
numbers may not turn to his name or follow a pointing
gesture. As adults, individuals with autism have a range
of outcomes from complete dependence to rare examples of successful employment. However, they almost
never marry and only rarely form ordinary, reciprocal
friendships. The possibility of identifying links between
the acquisition (or failure of acquisition) of basic social
behaviors and neurobiology has, in part, fostered the
recent surge of interest in the neuroscience of autism.
Issues in Diagnosis and Defining the Phenotype
Over the last 20 years, the conceptualization of a spectrum of autism-related disorders, including Childhood
Disintegrative Disorder, Aspergers Disorder, and Pervasive Developmental Disorder-Not Otherwise Specified
(PDD-NOS), which all include qualitative deficits in social
behavior and communication, has been supported by
longitudinal, epidemiological, and family studies (see
Filipek et al., 2000 for a general review). However, these
disorders vary in pervasiveness, severity, and onset. The
umbrella category is termed Pervasive Developmental
Disorder in the most common diagnostic systems
(American Psychiatric Association, 2000; World Health
Organization, 1992). Retts syndrome has been included
in the category of autism spectrum disorders within psychiatric systems because of the overlap in symptoms
in toddlers and preschool children but, because of its
different course (e.g., loss of purposeful hand use) and
neurological characteristics (e.g., deceleration in head
circumference), is a more differentiable group than the
other syndromes and so generally studied as a distinct
disorder. Clear distinctions among the other disorders
within the autism spectrum, as listed on Table 1, are
possible according to the degree of accompanying language deficit or general cognitive delay (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) or according to the severity
of social or behavioral symptoms (Lord et al., 2000).
Aspergers disorder involves the presence of autistic
social deficits and repetitive, circumscribed interests in
individuals who are verbally fluent. It has been of interest
to scientists because of the contrast, as discussed earlier, between relatively intact and often verbose qualities
of language and limited behaviors. Its prevalence is in
dispute because, in controlled studies, it has been difficult to reliably differentiate Aspergers syndrome from
autism without mental retardation or language delay,
except by neuropsychological profile (which may be tautological).
On the other hand, the appeal of the different categorizations is in part because there is such a range of abilities and patterns of deficits within the autism spectrum
that the possibility of finding subcategories related to
Social
Interaction
Exclusions
Behavior
Communication
Age of Onset
Autistic Disorder
Retts Disorder
Aspergers Disorder
This category is to be
used in cases of
pervasive impairment
in social interaction
and communication
with presence of
stereotyped behaviors
of interests when
criteria are not met for
a specific disorder.
PDD-NOS
Neuron
356
Review
357
and language but also many other aspects of functioning, including sensory responsiveness, play, and motor
activity. Approximately 30% of autistic patients also
have an associated seizure disorder.
As shown in Figure 1, adapted from Fombonne (1999),
epidemiological studies indicate an increasing prevalence for autism spectrum disorders in the last 15 years.
Much of the higher rates may be accounted for by more
complete ascertainment and the expansion of diagnostic frameworks to include milder forms of the disorder
(Fombonne, 1999). However, it is interesting to note that
the highest estimates of prevalence have all occurred
in the last 5 years, though in small samples in studies
where diagnoses were not always based on standard
clinical procedures. These findings and the increases in
the numbers of children with autism spectrum disorder
referred to school systems (see Baird et al., 2000) and
state programs have sparked the interest of many advocacy and scientific groups. The question of rising prevalence is particularly difficult to study because measures
and diagnostic systems to establish base rates have
changed greatly in the last 10 years, and, though available now, are expensive and time consuming (e.g., require a substantial parent interview and structured observation) if the milder forms of autism spectrum
disorder are to be identified.
Many, but not all, individuals with autism, have mental
retardation and almost all individuals with autism have
a history of language delay. However, there are autism
spectrum disorders (e.g., Aspergers disorder) in which
neither language delay nor mental retardation is present.
Families with children with moderate or mild mental retardation or normal intelligence with autism are no more
likely to have children with mental retardation without
autism than other families. Thus, the mental retardation
appears to be a part, but not a necessary part, of autistic
Neuron
358
Review
359
Neuron
360
(1995) observed changes in the hippocampus. Consistent with the neuropathological findings of Kemper and
Bauman, Haznedar et al. (1997) observed decreased
volume and decreased PET activity in the anterior cingulate gyrus of autistic subjects. Finally, there have been
no neuropathological findings in the basal ganglia of
autistic patients. However, Sears et al. (1999), using
structural MRI, observed an increased volume of the
caudate nuclei in autistic subjects that was proportional
to compulsions and rituals.
Neurobiology of Social Behavior
Regardless of whether an individual with an autism
spectrum disorder is mentally retarded or not and
whether the individual has language impairment or obvious stereotypical behaviors, all persons with autism
spectrum disorders have some disturbance of normal
social behavior. These deficits range from subtle abnormalities in social reciprocity, particularly with peers, to
much more obvious difficulties in the use of eye contact,
facial expression, and social motivation. A growing body
of literature indicates that important aspects of social
cognition, such as the salience or interpretation of facial
expressions or the appreciation of angle of gaze, are
markedly impaired in autistic subjects (Baron-Cohen et
al., 2000). Thus, an understanding of the brain systems
responsible for social cognition and for interpretation of
the social aspects of facial expression might be predictive of regions of brain dysfunction in autism.
Evidence from both human and animal studies indicates that certain brain regions are preferentially involved in social behavior. In the macaque monkey, these
would include the amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex, superior temporal gyrus, and temporal polar cortex. Damage
to any of these regions in macaque monkeys produces
a profound alteration of social behavior. Cortical areas,
such as the anterior cingulate gyrus, have also been
implicated in the production of species-specific vocalizations. Yet, other cortical regions, such as the inferior
temporal gyrus in monkeys and the fusiform gyrus in
humans, appear to be preferentially involved in the perception of faces. Neurons (face cells) that are highly
and selectively responsive to the image of faces have
been found in the inferior temporal gyrus of macaque
monkeys (Perrett et al., 1982). Neurons sensitive to the
angle of gaze have been found in the cortex of the
superior temporal sulcus (Perrett et al., 1985). Neurons
that are attuned to particular facial expressions have
also been found in the inferior and superior temporal
lobes of macaque monkeys (Hasselmo et al., 1989). Interestingly, cortical areas responsive to faces, facial expressions, and to angle of gaze send direct projections
to the primate amygdala (Stefanacci and Amaral, 2000).
These functional and neuroanatomical facts have raised
the prospect that the amygdala may be important for
components of social cognition such as the attention
to and interpretation of facial expressions. If this is the
case, the amygdala is a prime candidate for dysfunction
in autism.
A rare human disorder called Urbach-Wiethe syndrome
produces cystic calcifications of the medial temporal
lobe. In patient SM, these space-occupying lesions
mainly involve the amygdala bilaterally and she is mark-
Review
361
Neuron
362
References
volume and glucose metabolism in autistic disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 154, 10471050.
Abell, F., Krams, M., Ashburner, J., Passingham, R., Friston, K.,
Frackowiak, R., Happe, F., Frith, C., and Frith, U. (1999). The neuroanatomy of autism: a voxel-based whole brain analysis of structural
scans. Neuroreport 10, 16471651.
Holttum, J.R., Minshew, N.J., Sanders, R.S., and Phillips, N.E. (1992).
Magnetic resonance imaging of the posterior fossa in autism. Biol.
Psychiatry 32, 10911101.
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., and Damasio, A.R. (1995). Fear
and the human amygdala. J. Neurosci. 15, 58795891.
Afzal, M.A., Minor, P.D., and Schild, G.C. (2000). Clinical safety issues of measles, mumps and rubella vaccines. Bull. World Health
Organ. 78, 199204.
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders DSM-IV-TR (Text Revision) (Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association).
Amir, R.E., Van den Veyver, I.B., Wan, M., Tran, C.Q., Francke, U.,
and Zoghbi, H.Y. (1999). Rett sydnrome is caused by mutations
in X-linked MECP2, encoding methyl-CpG-binding protein 2. Nat.
Genet. 23, 185188.
Aylward, E.H., Minshew, N.J., Goldstein, G., Honeycutt, N.A., Augustine, A.M., Yates, K.O., Barta, P.E., and Pearlson, G.D. (1999).
MRI volumes of amygdala and hippocampus in non-mentally retarded autistic adolescents and adults. Neurology 53, 21452150.
Bachevalier, J. (1996). Brief report: medial temporal lobe and autism:
a putative animal model in primates. J. Aut. Dev. Disord. 26, 217220.
Bailey, A., Luthert, P., Dean, A., Harding, B., Janota, I., Montgomery,
M., Rutter, M., and Lantos, P. (1998). A full genome screen for autism
with evidence for linkage to a region on chromosome 7q. Brain 121,
889905.
Baird, G., Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Swettenham, J.,
Wheelwright, S., and Drew, A. (2000). A screening instrument for
autism at 18 months of age: a 6-year follow-up study. J. Am. Acad.
Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39, 694702.
Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., Whellwright, S., Short, L., Mead, G.,
Smith, A., and Scahill, V. (1995). Autism occurs more often in families
of physicists, engineers, and mathematicians. Autism 2, 296301.
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H.A., Wheelwright, S., Bullmore, E.T., Brammer, M.J., Simmons, A., and Williams, S.C. (1999). Social intelligence
in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11,
18911898.
Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D. (2000). Understanding other minds: perspectives from developmental cognitive
neuroscience (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Cook, E. (1998). Genetics of autism. Mental Retard. Dev. Disabil.
Res. Rev. 4, 113120.
Cook, E.H. (2000). Genetics of autism. Child Adolesc. Psychiatric
Clin. N. Am., in press.
Courchesne, E., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Press, G.A., Hesselink, J.R.,
and Jernigan, T.L. (1988). Hypoplasia of cerebellar vermal lobules
VI and VII in autism. N. Engl. J. Med. 318, 13491354.
Dam, M. (1992). Quantitative neuropathology in epilepsy. Acta Neurol. Scand. 137 (suppl.), 5154.
Filipek, P.A., Accardo, P.J., Ashwal, S., Baranek, G.T., Cook, E.H.,
Jr., Dawson, G., Gordon, B., Gravel, J.S., Johnson, C.P., Kallen,
R.J., et al. (2000). Practice parameters: screening and diagnosis of
autism: report of the Quality Standards Subcommitte of the American Academy of Neurology and the Child Neurology Society. Neurology 55, 468479.
Folstein, S.E., Santangelo, S.L., Gilman, G.E., Piven, J., Landa, R.,
Lainhart, J., Hein, J., and Wzorek, M. (1999). Predictors of cognitive
test patterns in autism families. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 40,
117128.
Horvath, K., Stefanatos, G., Sokolski, K.N., Wachtel, R., Nabors, L.,
and Tildon, J.T. (1998). Improved social and language skills after
secretin administration in patients with autisitc spectrum disorders.
J. Assoc. Acad. Minor. Phys. 9, 915.
Kawahima, H., Mori, T., Kashiwagi, Y., Takekuma, K., Hoshika, A.,
and Wakefield, A. (2000). Detection and sequencing of measles virus
from peripheral mononuclear cells from patients with inflammatory
bowel disease and autism. Dig. Dis. Sci. 45, 723729.
Kemper, T.L., and Bauman, M. (1998). Neuropathology of infantile
autism. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 57, 645652.
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E.H., Leventhal, B.L., Pickles,
A., and Rutter, M.J. (2000). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-General (ADOS-G). J. Aut. Dev. Disord. 30, 205223.
Lucarelli, S., Frediani, T., Zingoni, A.M., Ferruzzi, F., Giardini, O.,
Quintieri, F., Barbato, M., DEufemia, P., and Cardi, E. (1995). Food
allergy and infantile autism. Panminerva Med. 37, 137141.
McDougle, C.J., Scahill, L., McCracken, J.T., Aman, M.G., Tierney,
E., Arnold, L.E., Freeman, B.J., Martin, A., McGough, J.J., Cronin,
P., et al. (2000). Research Units on Pediatric Psyhopharmacology
(RUPP) Autism Network. Background and rationale for an initial
controlled study of risperidone. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N.
Am. 9, 201224.
Mundy, P., and Neal, R. (2000). Neural plasticity, joint attention and
autistic developmental pathology. International Rev. Res. Ment. Ret.
20, 139168.
Osterling, J., and Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children
with autism: a study of first birthday home videotapes. J. Aut. Dev.
Disord. 24, 247257.
Perrett, D.I., Rolls, E.T., and Caan, W. (1982). Visual neurones responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex. Exp. Brain Res.
47, 329342.
Perrett, D.I., Smith, P.A., Potter, D.D., Mistlin, A.J., Head, A.S., Milner,
A.D., and Jeeves, M.A. (1985). Visual cells in temporal cortex sensitive to face view and gaze direction. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci.
223, 293317.
Piven, J., Bailey, J., Ranson, B.J., and Arndt, S. (1998). No differences in hippocampus volume detected on magnetic resonance
imaging in autistic individuals. J. Aut. Dev. Disord. 28, 105110.
Prior, M., and Ozonoff, S. (1998). Psychological factors in autism.
In Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, F.R. Volkmar,
ed. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), pp. 64108.
Ritvo, E.R., Freeman, B.J., Scheibel, A.B., Duong, T., Robinson, H.,
Guthrie, D., and Ritvo, A. (1986). Lower Purkinje cell counts in the
cerebella of four autistic subjects: initial findings of the UCLA-NSAC
Autopsy Research Report. Am. J. Psychiatry 143, 862866.
Rodier, P.M. (2000). The early origins of autism. Sci. Am. 282, 5663.
Rodier, P.M., Ingram, J.L., Tisdale, B., Nelson, S., and Romano, J.
(1996). Embryological origin for autism: developmental anomalies
of the cranial nerve motor nuclei. J. Comp. Neurol. 370, 247261.
Rumsey, J.M., and Ernst, E.M. (2000). Functional neuroimaging of
autistic disorders. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 6, 171179.
Saitoh, O., and Courchesne, E. (1998). Magnetic resonance imaging
study of the brain in autism. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 52 (suppl.),
S219S222.
Saitoh, O., Courchesne, E., Egaas, B., Lincoln, A.J., and Schreibman,
L. (1995). Cross sectional area of posterior hippocampus in autistic
patients with cerebellar and corpus-callosum abnormalities. Neurology 45, 317324.
Hasselmo, M.E., Rolls, E.T., and Baylis, G.C. (1989). The role of
expression and identity in the face-selective responses of neurons
in the temporal visual cortex of the monkey. Behav. Brain Res. 32,
203218.
Schultz, R.T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, R.K., Anderson, A.W.,
Volkmar, F.R., Skudlarski, P., Lacadie, C., Cohen, D.J., and Gore,
Review
363