Introduction To Psychology - The Full Noba Collection
Introduction To Psychology - The Full Noba Collection
Introduction To Psychology - The Full Noba Collection
Copyright
R. Biswas-Diener & E. Diener (Eds), Noba Textbook Series: Psychology.
Champaign, IL: DEF Publishers. DOI: nobaproject.com
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund.
The Internet addresses listed in the text were accurate at the time of publication. The
inclusion of a Website does not indicate an endorsement by the authors or the Diener
Education Fund, and the Diener Education Fund does not guarantee the accuracy of the
information presented at these sites.
Contact Information:
Noba Project
2100 SE Lake Rd., Suite 5
Milwaukie, OR 97222
nobaproject.com
[email protected]
About Noba
The Diener Education Fund (DEF) is a non-profit organization founded with the mission of
re-inventing higher education to serve the changing needs of students and professors. The
initial focus of the DEF is on making information, especially of the type found in textbooks,
widely available to people of all backgrounds. This mission is embodied in the Noba
project.
Noba is an open and free online platform that provides high-quality, flexibly structured
textbooks and educational materials. The goals of Noba are three-fold:
The Diener Education Fund is co-founded by Drs. Ed and Carol Diener. Ed is the Joseph
Smiley Distinguished Professor of Psychology (Emeritus) at the University of Illinois. Carol
Diener is the former director of the Mental Health Worker and the Juvenile Justice Programs
at the University of Illinois. Both Ed and Carol are award- winning university teachers.
TABLE
of
CONTENTS
26
Research Designs
Christie Napa Scollon
47
75
98
Statistical Thinking
Beth Chance & Allan Rossman
120
History of Psychology
David B. Baker & Heather Sperry
146
Neurons
Sharon Furtak
176
207
The Brain
Diane Beck & Evelina Tapia
TABLE
of
CONTENTS
234
267
Biochemistry of Love
Sue Carter & Stephen Porges
291
310
345
365
Vision
Simona Buetti & Alejandro Lleras
391
Hearing
Andrew J. Oxenham
414
431
467
TABLE
of
CONTENTS
507
526
544
Multi-Modal Perception
Lorin Lachs
570
Topic 4: Development
591
613
636
Adolescent Development
Jennifer Lansford
657
677
Emerging Adulthood
Jeffrey Jensen Arnett
699
TABLE
of
CONTENTS
721
Aging
Tara Queen & Jacqui Smith
749
Attention
Frances Friedrich
767
Consciousness
Ken Paller & Satoru Suzuki
793
The Unconscious
Ap Dijksterhuis
813
843
865
895
917
Theory of Mind
Bertram Malle
TABLE
of
CONTENTS
948
968
998
1030
Topic 7: Social
1051
Social Neuroscience
Tiffany A. Ito & Jennifer T. Kubota
1075
1104
1133
1153
TABLE
of
CONTENTS
1179
1216
1236
1263
1290
Positive Relationships
Nathaniel M. Lambert
1308
1338
Topic 8: Personality
1354
1380
Personality Traits
Edward Diener & Richard E. Lucas
1408
TABLE
of
CONTENTS
1438
Gender
Christia Spears Brown & Jennifer A. Jewell
1458
1488
Personality Assessment
David Watson
1518
1537
Creativity
Dean Keith Simonton
1559
Self-Efficacy
James E Maddux & Evan Kleiman
1587
1618
Affective Neuroscience
Eddie Harmon-Jones & Cindy Harmon-Jones
1641
Functions of Emotions
Hyisung Hwang & David Matsumoto
1665
TABLE
of
CONTENTS
1692
Emotional Intelligence
Marc Brackett, Sarah Delaney & Peter Salovey
1720
1753
1779
1803
Drive States
Sudeep Bhatia & George Loewenstein
1823
1843
Therapeutic Orientations
Hannah Boettcher, Stefan G. Hofmann & Q. Jade Wu
1867
1902
1934
Mood Disorders
Anda Gershon & Renee Thompson
TABLE
of
CONTENTS
1971
Personality Disorders
Cristina Crego & Thomas Widiger
1994
Dissociative Disorders
Dalena van Heugten - van der Kloet
2025
2047
Psychopathy
Chris Patrick
2081
2110
Psychopharmacology
Susan Barron
2134
2167
2193
Positive Psychology
Robert A. Emmons
2214
TABLE
of
CONTENTS
2231
Index
Topic 1
Psychology As Science
Abstract
Scientific research has been one of the great drivers of progress in human
history, and the dramatic changes we have seen during the past century are
due primarily to scientific findingsmodern medicine, electronics, automobiles
and jets, birth control, and a host of other helpful inventions. Psychologists
believe that scientific methods can be used in the behavioral domain to
understand and improve the world. Although psychology trails the biological
and physical sciences in terms of progress, we are optimistic based on
discoveries to date that scientific psychology will make many important
discoveries that can benefit humanity. This module outlines the characteristics
of the science, and the promises it holds for understanding behavior. The ethics
that guide psychological research are briefly described. It concludes with the
reasons you should learn about scientific psychology.
Learning Objectives
Discuss a few of the benefits, as well as problems that have been created
by science.
Describe several ways that psychological science has improved the world.
The usual response is: Who on earth are Jenner, Borlaug, and Haber? Most
people recall that Mother Teresa helped thousands in the slums of Calcutta.
Albert Schweitzer opened his famous hospital in Africa and saved thousands
of lives. The Catholic priest, Father Damien, helped lepers with his charitable
work in Molokai, Hawaii. However, Jenner, Borlaug, and Haber were scientists
whose findings saved millions and even billions of lives. Most of us have never
heard of them. Dr. Edward Jenner discovered the smallpox vaccine, which has
saved millions of lives and allowed that plague to be eradicated. Many other
diseases have been greatly reduced in the world because of vaccines discovered
using sciencemeasles, pertussis, diphtheria, tetanus, typhoid, cholera, polio,
hepatitis, and bubonic plague, for example. The number of lives saved because
of vaccines might easily be more than a billion.
nobaproject.com - Why Science? (Overview Chapter)
Fritz Haber and Norman Borlaug saved more than a billion human lives.
They created the Green Revolution by producing hybrid agricultural crops and
synthetic fertilizer. Because of them, the world was able to experience the
population explosion of the 20th century without widespread famine and
starvation. Humanity can produce food for the seven billion people on the
planet, and the starvation that did occur was for political and other reasons,
not because we could not grow enough food. Now another scientific invention
effective birth controlhas countered the population explosion, and led to
rapidly decreasing birthrates around the globe.
If you examine the great changes and advances that humanity has
undergone in the past century, most of them can be attributed to science. The
world in 1900 was very different than the one we see today (Easterbrook, 2003).
There were few cars and most people traveled by foot, horseback, or carriage.
There were no jets, radios, televisions, birth control pills, or antibiotics. Only a
small portion of the world had telephones or electricity, whereas we find that
now across the globe, 80% of households have television and 84% have
electricity. It is estimated that three quarters of the worlds population has
access to a mobile phone! Life expectancy was 47 years in 1900 and 79 years
in 2010. This huge increase in the lifespan has been called one of the greatest
feats in human history. The percentage of hungry and malnourished people in
the world has dropped substantially across the globe. I.Q. has risen dramatically
over the past century, due to better nutrition and schooling.
The past 100 years have brought us television, air transportation, computers,
and numerous medical marvels. Labor-saving devices such as washing
machines not only have changed our work, but have given both women and
men more freedom. The world of work also has changed at a rapid pace, from
computers to manufacturing processes. Many products that were once made
by hand are now created cheaply by machines.
nobaproject.com - Why Science? (Overview Chapter)
What Is Science?
What is this process we call science, which has so dramatically changed the
world? Science is the use of systematic observational methods in acquiring
knowledge. These empirical methods have been shown to be powerful in terms
of the physical and biological world, and thus we are optimistic that they can
substantially advance understanding of behavior and the mind as well. Science
is not magicit will not solve all human problems, and might not answer all
our questions about behavior. Nevertheless, it appears to be the most powerful
method we have for acquiring knowledge about the observable world. The
essential elements of science are as follows:
1. Systematic observation is the core of science. Scientists observe the world,
in a very organized way. We often measure the phenomenon we are
observing. We record our observations so that memory biases are less likely
to enter in to our conclusions. We are systematic in that we try to observe
nobaproject.com - Why Science? (Overview Chapter)
10
11
12
13
mathematician. Thereafter, he was known for the metal nose he sported. Hardly
the quiet and meek image of the stereotypical scientist, Brahe nevertheless
perfected and used the most accurate methods of the time for measuring the
skies. Research psychologists now are trying to make equally accurate
observations and measures of human behavior. Some of these modern
researchers are geeks, but many of them are not. All of them, however, should
aspire to be objective and accurate like Tycho Brahe.
Psychological Science
Science has made dramatic leaps in knowledge, and has vastly improved our
world. Thus, I believe that the scientific method must be used to understand
our own behavior. Psychology is a young science, having become widespread
only in the past half-century. Despite this youth, we can point to the knowledge
contained in this e-text for the advances we have already made. For instance,
we have found that stress can harm peoples physical health, and we are
beginning to understand how this occurs. We are starting to comprehend how
our brains and hormones lead to our experiences. Many early clinical practice
therapies were derived from the personal experiences of therapists, what they
thought was helping their clients. Over time, more and more research was
conducted to determine which therapies were most and least effective. For
example, it has been found in many studies that cognitive behavioral therapy
can help many people suffering from depression and anxiety disorders (Butler,
Chapman, Forman, & Beck, 2006; Hoffman & Smits, 2008). In contrast, research
reveals that some types of therapies actually might be harmful on average
(Lilienfeld, 2007).
In organizational psychology, a number of psychological interventions have
been found by researchers to produce greater productivity and worker
satisfaction in the workplace (e.g., Guzzo, Jette, & Katzell, 1985). Human factor
engineers have greatly increased the safety and utility of the products we use.
nobaproject.com - Why Science? (Overview Chapter)
14
For example, the human factors psychologist Alphonse Chapanis and other
researchers redesigned the cockpit controls of aircraft to make them less
confusing and easier to respond to, and this led to a decrease in pilot errors
and crashes.
Forensic sciences have made courtroom decisions more valid. We all know
of the famous cases of imprisoned persons who have been exonerated because
of DNA evidence. Equally dramatic cases hinge on psychological findings related
to the accuracy of eyewitness identification and memory (see the Cara Laney
and Elizabeth Loftus module in the Sensation & Perception section of NOBA).
Thus, psychological findings are having practical importance in the world
outside the laboratory. Psychological science has experienced enough success
to demonstrate that it works, but there remains a huge amount yet to be
learned.
Other approaches to understanding behaviorpersonal experience,
religious teaching, and so forthcan be helpful in some cases, but this e-text
is based on findings that have been established with the scientific method.
However, some questions lie outside of science because we have not thought
of a way to observe and measure them, at least no way on which scientists can
yet agree. For instance, the existence of angels falls outside of science because
nobody has thought of a way of observing them on which scientists can agree.
We are not saying that angels do or do not exist, but simply they are not capable
of being addressed by science. Although some scientists believe in only what
they can see and observe, other scientists believe in entities such as God and
angels. But scientists so far have thought of no way to directly study these, and
therefore knowledge about them, if such is possible, must come from other
approaches. These matters lie outside of science.
There is often discussion of a new big frontier of human explorationthe
Antarctic, outer space, or nanotechnology, for example. All of these are exciting
areas, but it could be argued that the most important and challenging frontier
is that of understanding our own behavior. There is vast underexplored
nobaproject.com - Why Science? (Overview Chapter)
15
territory, intriguing complexity, and the possibility of huge payoffs. The amount
that we do not know is so extensive that there are huge opportunities for young
researchers in this field.
16
For each of these ethical guidelines, there are exceptions and complexities,
and you will learn about these if you enroll in a course on research methods.
However, in general, these guidelines are very useful in protecting the
participants in our studies as well as society.
Researchers should also consider how their research will be used. An
extreme case, for example, would be if a researcher wanted to study more
effective means of torturing people to extract information. Even if the study
made discoveries, would this knowledge truly better the world? Most
researchers hope that at least someday their findings might play a role in
nobaproject.com - Why Science? (Overview Chapter)
17
improving the quality of life. A case in point is the work of Fritz Haber, the
inventor of synthetic fertilizer mentioned earlier. Haber also discovered a
poison gas, Zyklon B, which could be used in warfare, and was later adapted by
the Nazis for killing innocent people in concentration camps. Although Haber
argued that his poison gas was simply another weapon of warfare like any other
weapon, most researchers are reluctant to produce scientific findings that might
have such a sinister purpose. Whether research psychologists work on topics
such as how the eye works or on topics such as discrimination and aggression,
most hope that their findings eventually will be of benefit to humanity. Although
much research is theoretical and abstract, it is believed that, in the long run,
the basic understanding provided by such research will eventually produce
many benefits.
18
or a professor
6. To learn how to evaluate the research claims you hear or read about
7. Because it is interesting, challenging, and fun! People want to learn about
psychology because this is exciting in itself, regardless of other positive
outcomes it might have. Why do we see movies? Because they are fun and
exciting, and we need no other reason. Thus, one good reason to study
psychology is that it can be rewarding in itself.
Conclusions
The science of psychology is an exciting adventure. Whether you will become
a scientific psychologist, an applied psychologist, or an educated person who
knows about psychological research, this field can influence your life and
provide fun, rewards, and understanding. My hope is that you learn a lot from
the modules in this e-text, and also that you enjoy the experience! I love learning
about psychology and neuroscience, and hope you will too!
19
Outside Resources
Web: Science Heroes- A celebration of people who have made lifesaving
discoveries.
http://www.scienceheroes.com/index.php?option=com_content&view=
article&id=258&Itemid=27
Discussion Questions
1. Some claim that science has done more harm than good. What do you think?
2. Humanity is faced by many challenges and problems. Which of these are
due to human behavior, and which are external to human actions?
3. If you were a research psychologist, what phenomena or behaviors would
most interest you?
4. Will psychological scientists be able to help with the current challenges
humanity faces, such as global warming, war, inequality, and mental illness?
5. What can science study and what is outside the realm of science? What
questions are impossible for scientists to study?
6. Some claim that science will replace religion by providing sound knowledge
instead of myths to explain the world. They claim that science is a much
more reliable source of solutions to problems such as disease than is
religion. What do you think? Will science replace religion, and should it?
7. Are there human behaviors that should not be studied? Are some things
so sacred or dangerous that we should not study them?
Vocabulary
Empirical methods
Approaches to inquiry that are tied to actual measurement and observation.
Ethics
Professional guidelines that offer researchers a template for making decisions
that protect research participants from potential harm and that help steer
scientists away from conflicts of interest or other situations that might
compromise the integrity of their research.
Hypotheses
A logical idea that can be tested.
Systematic observation
The careful observation of the natural world with the aim of better
understanding it. Observations provide the basic data that allow scientists to
track, tally, or otherwise organize information about the natural world.
Theories
Groups of closely related phenomena or observations.
Reference List
Butler, A. C., Chapman, J. E., Forman, E. M., & Beck, A. T. (2006). The empirical
status of cognitive-behavioral therapy: A review of meta-analyses. Clinical
Psychology Review, 26, 1731.
Diener, E., & Crandall, R. (1978). Ethics in social and behavioral research. Chicago,
IL: University of Chicago Press.
Easterbrook, G. (2003). The progress paradox. New York, NY: Random House.
Guzzo, R. A., Jette, R. D., & Katzell, R. A. (1985). The effects of psychologically
based intervention programs on worker productivity: A meta-analysis.
Personnel Psychology, 38, 275.291.
Hoffman, S. G., & Smits, J. A. J. (2008). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adult
anxiety disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69, 62132.
Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). Psychological treatments that cause harm. Perspectives
on Psychological Science, 2, 5370.
Moore, D. (2003). Public lukewarm on animal rights. Gallup News Service, May
21. http://www.gallup.com/poll/8461/public-lukewarm-animal-rights.aspx
Sales, B. D., & Folkman, S. (Eds.). (2000). Ethics in research with human
participants. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Why Science? (Overview Chapter) by Edward
Diener is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Research Designs
Christie Napa Scollon
Singapore Management University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Psychologists test research questions using a variety of methods. Most research
relies on either correlations or experiments. With correlations, researchers
measure variables as they naturally occur in people and compute the degree
to which two variables go together. With experiments, researchers actively
make changes in one variable and watch for changes in another variable.
Experiments allow researchers to make causal inferences. Other types of
methods include longitudinal and quasi-experimental designs. Many factors,
including practical constraints, determine the type of methods researchers use.
Often researchers survey people even though it would be better, but more
expensive and time consuming, to track them longitudinally.
Learning Objectives
Research Designs
We look at dinosaur bones to make educated guesses about extinct life. We
systematically chart the heavens through observation to learn about the
relationship of the stars and planets, and we track spending to predict shifts in
the economy. All of these observations form the foundation of science.
Psychology is no different in its adherence to systematic, scientific methods. In
the movie Jerry Maguire, Cuba Gooding, Jr., became famous for the phrase,
Show me the money! In psychology, as in all sciences, the phrase might as
well be Show me the data!
One of the important steps in scientific inquiry is to test our research
questions, otherwise known as hypotheses. However, there are many ways to
test hypotheses in psychological research. Which of these methods you choose
to use will depend on the type of questions you are asking as well as what
resources are available to you. All methods have limitations, which is why the
best research uses a variety of methods.
Most psychological research can be divided into two types: experimental
and correlational research.
Experimental Research
If somebody gave you $20 that absolutely had to be spent today, how would
you choose to spend it? Would you spend it on an item youve been eyeing for
weeks or would you donate the money to charity? Which option do you think
would bring you the most happiness? If youre like most people, youd choose
to spend the money on yourself (duh, right?). Our intuition is that wed be
happier if we spent the money on ourselves.
nobaproject.com - Research Designs
28
29
30
Other considerations
In addition to using random assignment, you must also avoid introducing
confounds to your experiment. Confounds are things that could undermine
your ability to draw causal inferences. For example, if you want to test whether
a new happy pill will make people happier, you could randomly assign
participants to take the happy pill or not and compare these two groups on
their self-reported happiness. However, if some people know they are getting
the happy pill, they might develop participant expectations that may influence
their self-reported happiness. This is sometimes known as a placebo effect.
Just knowing that one is getting special treatment or something new is
sometimes enough to actually cause changes in human behavior. A related idea
is participant demand. This occurs when participants try to behave in way that
they think the experimenter wants. Placebo effects and participant demand
often occur unintentionally. Even experimenter expectations can influence the
outcome of a study. For example, if the experimenter knows who took the happy
pill and who did not, and the dependent variable is the experimenters
observations of peoples happiness, then the experimenter might see
improvements in one group that are not really there.
Correlational Designs
When scientists passively observe and measure their phenomena it is called
correlational research. Here, we do not intervene and change behavior, as we
do in experiments. In correlational research we see patterns that go together,
but we usually cannot infer what causes what. Importantly, with correlational
research you can examine only two variables at a time, no more and no less.
nobaproject.com - Research Designs
31
32
Figure 1. Scatterplot of the association between happiness and ratings of the past month, a
positive correlation (r = .81). Each dot represents an individual.
33
the top left to the bottom right. What does this mean in real world terms? It
means that people are shorter in parts of the world where there is more disease.
The r value for a negative correlation is negative, that is, it has a minus () sign
in front of it. Here it is .83.
Figure 2. Scatterplot showing the association between average male height and pathogen
prevalence, a negative correlation (r = .83). Each dot represents a country. (Chiao, 2009)
The strength of a correlation has to do with how well the two variables go
together. Recall that in Professor Dunns correlational study, spending on others
was positively correlated with happiness. The more people spent on others,
the happier they were. At this point, you may be thinking to yourself, I know a
very generous person who gave away lots of money to other people but is
miserable! Or maybe you know of a very stingy person who is happy as a clam.
Yes, there might be exceptions. If an association has a lot of exceptions, it is a
weak correlation. If an association has few or no exceptions, then it is a stronger
correlation. A strong correlation is one in which the two variables always or
almost always go together. In the case of happiness and how good the week
is, the association is strong. The stronger a correlation is, the tighter the dots
nobaproject.com - Research Designs
34
Figure 3. Scatterplot showing the association between valuing happiness and GPA, a weak
negative correlation (r = .32). Each dot represents an individual.
35
Can you guess the strength and direction of the correlation between age
and year of birth? If you said this is a strong negative correlation, you are correct!
Older people always have lower years of birth than younger people. In fact, this
is a perfect correlation because there are no exceptions to this pattern. I
challenge you to find a 10-year-old born before 2003! You cant.
Quasi-Experimental Designs
What if you want to study the effects of marriage on a variable? For example,
does marriage make people happier? Can you randomly assign some people
to get married and others to remain single? Of course not. So how can you study
these important variables? You can use a quasi-experimental design. A quasiexperimental design is one in which random assignment to conditions is not
used. Instead, we rely on existing group memberships (e.g., married vs. single),
and we treat these as independent variables even though we did not assign
people to those conditions and we did not manipulate those variables. Causal
inference is more difficult with quasi-experimental designs. For example,
people who get married might differ on a variety of characteristics from those
who do not marry. If we find that the married folks are happier than the singles,
nobaproject.com - Research Designs
36
it will be hard to say that marriage causes happiness because the people who
end up getting married might be happier to begin with than the people who
remain single.
Longitudinal Studies
Another powerful research design is the longitudinal study. Longitudinal
studies follow the same people and measure them several times. Some
longitudinal studies last a few weeks, some a few months, some a year or more.
Some studies that have contributed a lot to psychology follow the same people
over decades. For example, one study has followed more than 20,000 Germans
for the past two decades. From these longitudinal data, psychologist Rich Lucas
(2003) was able to determine that indeed people who end up getting married
in life start off a bit happier than their peers who never marry. Longitudinal
studies provide valuable evidence for testing many theories in psychology, but
they can be quite costly to conduct, especially if they follow many people for
many years.
Surveys
A survey is a way of gathering information by the use of old-fashioned
questionnaires or the Internet. Compared to a study conducted in a psychology
laboratory, surveys can reach a larger number of participants with less cost.
Although surveys are typically used for correlational research, this is not always
the case. An experiment can be carried out using surveys as well. For example,
King and Napa (1998) presented participants with different types of stimuli on
paper: either a survey completed by a happy person or a survey completed by
an unhappy person. They wanted to see whether happy people were judged
as more likely to get into heaven compared to unhappy people. Can you figure
out the independent and dependent variables in this study? Can you guess
nobaproject.com - Research Designs
37
what the results were? (Happy people were judged as more likely to go to heaven
than unhappy people!)
Likewise, correlational research can be conducted without the use of
surveys. For instance, psychologists LeeAnn Harker and Dacher Keltner (2001)
examined the smile intensity of womens college yearbook photos. Smiling in
the photos was correlated with being married 10 years later!
Tradeoffs in Research
Even though there are limitations to correlational and survey research, these
are not poor cousins to experiments and longitudinal designs. In addition to
selecting a method that is appropriate to the question, many practical concerns
may influence the decision to use one method over another. One of these
factors is simply resource availabilityhow much time and money do you have
to invest in the research? (Tip: If youre doing a senior honors thesis, do not
embark on a lengthy longitudinal study unless you are prepared to delay
graduation!) Often, we survey people even though it would be better, but so
much more difficult, to track them longitudinally. Especially in the case of
exploratory research, it may make sense to opt for a cheaper and faster method
first. If results from the initial study are promising, then the researcher can
follow up with a more intensive method.
38
39
Outside Resources
Article: Harker and Keltner study of yearbook photographs and marriage
http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/psp/80/1/112/
Article: Spending money on others promotes happiness. Elizabeth Dunns
research
https://www.sciencemag.org/content/319/5870/1687.abstract
Article: What makes a life good?
http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/psp/75/1/156/
Artilce: Rich Lucass longitudinal study on the effects of marriage on
happiness
http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/psp/84/3/527/
Discussion Questions
1. What are some key differences between experimental and correlational
research?
2. Why might researchers sometimes use methods other than experiments?
3. How do surveys related to correlational and experimental designs?
Vocabulary
Confounds
Factors that undermine the ability to draw causal inferences from an
experiment.
Correlation
Measures the association between two variables, or how they go together.
Dependent variable
The variable the researcher measures but does not manipulate in an
experiment.
Experimenter expectations
When the experimenters expectations influence the outcome of a study.
Independent variable
The variable the researcher manipulates and controls in an experiment.
Longitudinal study
A study that follows the same group of individuals over time.
Operational definitions
How researchers specifically measure a concept.
Participant demand
When participants behave in a way that they think the experimenter wants
them to behave.
Placebo effect
When receiving special treatment or something new affects human behavior.
Quasi-experimental design
An experiment that does not require random assignment to conditions.
Random assignment
Assigning participants to receive different conditions of an experiment by
chance.
Reference List
Chiao, J. (2009). Culturegene coevolution of individualism collectivism and
the serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277,
529-537. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1650
Dunn, E. W., Aknin, L. B., & Norton, M. I. (2008). Spending money on others
promotes
happiness.
Science,
319(5870),
16871688.
doi:10.1126/
science.1150952
Harker, L. A., & Keltner, D. (2001). Expressions of positive emotion in women's
college yearbook pictures and their relationship to personality and life
outcomes across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
80, 112124.
King, L. A., & Napa, C. K. (1998). What makes a life good? Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 75, 156165.
Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2003). Re-examining
adaptation and the setpoint model of happiness: Reactions to changes in
marital status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 527539.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Research Designs by Christie Napa Scollon is
licensed
under
the
Creative
Commons
Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0
Conducting Psychology
Research in the Real World
Matthias R. Mehl
University of Arizona
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Because of its power to establish causality, the laboratory experiment is
traditionally considered the method of choice for psychological science. Yet, in
its systematic isolation of conditions and effects, it can yield findings that are
out of touch with reality and have limited applicability for understanding human
real-world behavior. This chapter highlights the importance of also conducting
research outside the psychology laboratory, within participants natural,
everyday environments and reviews existing methodologies for studying daily
life.
Learning Objectives
Explain ways in which daily life research can further psychological science.
Know what methods exist for conducting psychological research in the real
world.
Introduction
The laboratory experiment is traditionally considered the method of choice in
psychology research. This is so because only laboratory experiments can
unambiguously separate cause from effect and, thereby, establish causality.
Despite this unique strength, though, it is also clear that a scientific field that is
mainly based on controlled laboratory studies ends up lopsided. It accumulates
a lot of knowledge on what can happenunder carefully isolated and controlled
circumstancesbut it has little to say about what actually does happen under
the circumstances that people actually encounter in their daily lives. With the
words of one of the founding fathers of social psychology: Experimentation in
the laboratory occurs, socially speaking, on an island quite isolated from the
life of society (Lewin, 1944, p. 286). This chapter highlights the importance of
also conducting research outside the laboratory (Reis & Gosling, 2010), directly
within participants natural environments, and reviews existing methodologies
for studying daily life.
49
50
51
52
currently doing?), and experiences (e.g., How are you feeling?). That way,
researchers get a snapshot of what is going on in participants lives at the time
at which they were asked to report.
Initially, participants wore electronic wristwatches that beeped at
preprogrammed times at which they completed one of a stack of provided paper
questionnaires. With the mobile computing revolution, both the prompting and
the questionnaire completion were gradually replaced by handheld devices.
Being able to collect the momentary questionnaires digitally and time-stamped
had major methodological and practical advantages and contributed to
experience sampling going mainstream (Conner, Tennen, Fleeson, & Barrett,
2009).
Over time, experience sampling and related momentary self-report
methods have become very popular, and, by now, they are effectively the gold
standard for studying daily life. They have helped make progress in almost all
areas of psychology (Mehl & Conner, 2012). Their complex data structure (i.e.,
many measurements from many participants) has further spurred the
development of novel statistical methods (Bolger & Laurenceau, 2013). Finally,
and maybe most important, they accomplished what they sought out to
accomplish: to bring attention to what psychology ultimately wants and needs
to know about, namely what people actually do, think, and feel in the various
contexts of their lives (Funder, 2001, p. 213).
In a classic study, Stone, Reed, and Neale (1987) tracked positive and negative
experiences surrounding a respiratory infection using daily experience
sampling and found that undesirable experiences peaked and desirable ones
dipped about four to five days prior to participants coming down with the cold.
More recently, Killingsworth & Gilbert (2010) collected momentary self-reports
from more than 2,000 participants via a smartphone app and found that they
were less happy when their mind was in an idling, mind-wandering state than
when it was in an engaged, task-focused one. These are just two examples that
illustrate how experience sampling studies have yielded findings that could not
nobaproject.com - Conducting Psychology Research in the Real World
53
and
colleagues
have
developed
naturalistic
observation
54
(Mehl, Pennebaker, Crow, Dabbs, & Price, 2001). The ambient sound recordings
can be coded for participants locations (e.g., at school, in a coffee shop),
activities (e.g., watching TV, eating), interactions (e.g., in a group, on the phone),
and emotional expressions (e.g., laughing, sighing).
In a cross-cultural study, Ramrez-Esparza and her colleagues used the EAR
method to study sociability in the United States and Mexico. Interestingly, they
found that although American participants rated themselves significantly
higher than Mexicans on the question, I see myself as a person who is talkative,
they actually spent almost 10 percent less time talking (Ramrez-Esparza, Mehl,
lvarez Bermdez, & Pennebaker, 2009). In a similar way, Mehl and his
colleagues used the EAR method to debunk the long-standing myth that women
are by a factor more talkative than men. Using data from six different studies,
they showed that both sexes use on average about 16,000 words per day. The
estimated gender difference of 546 words was trivial compared to the immense
range of more than 46,000 words between the least and most talkative
individual (695 versus 47,016 words; Mehl, Vazire, Ramrez-Esparza, Slatcher, &
Pennebaker, 2007). Together, these studies demonstrate how naturalistic
observation can be used to study objective aspects of daily behavior and how
it can yield findings quite different from what other methods yield (Mehl,
Robbins, & Deters, 2012).
A series of other methods and creative ways for assessing behavior directly
and unobtrusively in the real world are described in a seminal book on
nonreactive measures (Webb, Campbell, Schwartz, Sechrest, & Grove, 1981).
For example, researchers have used time-lapse photography to study the flow
of people and the use of space in urban public places (Whyte, 1980). They have
observed peoples personal (e.g., dorm rooms) and professional (e.g., offices)
spaces to understand how personality is expressed and perceived in everyday
environments (Gosling, Ko, Mannarelli, & Morris, 2002). They have even
systematically collected and analyzed peoples garbage to measure what people
actually consume (e.g., empty alcohol bottles or cigarette boxes) rather than
nobaproject.com - Conducting Psychology Research in the Real World
55
what they say they consume (Rathje & Murphy, 2001). Because people often
cannot and sometimes may not want to accurately report what they do, the
directand ideally nonreactiveassessment of real-world behavior is of such
high importance for psychological research (Baumeister, Vohs, & Funder, 2007).
56
57
58
59
Smartphone Psychology?
A review of research methods for studying daily life would not be complete
without a vision of whats next. Extrapolating from the ongoing mobile device
revolution, it is safe to predict that smartphones will not just remain devices for
everyday online communication but, rather, will also become devices for
scientific data collection and intervention (Kaplan & Stone, 2013; Yarkoni, 2012).
They automatically store vast amounts of real-world user interaction data, and,
in addition, they are equipped with an array of sensors to track the physical (e.
g., location, position) and social (e.g., wireless connections around the phone)
context of these interactions. Miller (2012, p. 234) states, The question is not
whether smartphones will revolutionize psychology but how, when, and where
the revolution will happen. Obviously, their immense potential for data
collection also brings with it big new challenges for researchers (e.g., privacy
protection, data analysis, and synthesis). Yet, it is clear that many of the methods
described in this chapterand many still to be developed ways of collecting
real-world datawill in the future become integrated into the devices that
people naturally and happily carry with them from the moment they get up in
the morning to the moment they go to bed.
Conclusion
This chapter sought to make a case for psychology research conducted outside
the lab. If the ultimate goal of the social and behavioral sciences is to explain
human behavior, then researchers must alsoin addition to conducting
carefully controlled lab studiesdeal with the messy real world and find ways
to capture life as it naturally happens.
nobaproject.com - Conducting Psychology Research in the Real World
60
Mortensen and Cialdini (2010) refer to the synergistic give and take between
laboratory and field research as full cycle psychology. Going full cycle, they
suggest, means that researchers use naturalistic observation to determine an
effects presence in the real world, theory to determine what processes underlie
the effect, experimentation to verify the effect and its underlying processes,
and a return to the natural environment to corroborate the experimental
findings (Mortensen & Cialdini, 2010, p. 53). To accomplish this, researchers
have access to a toolbox of research methods for studying daily life that is now
more diverse and more versatile than it has ever been before. So, all it takes is
to go ahead andliterallybring science to life.
61
Outside Resources
Website: Society for Ambulatory Assessment
http://www.ambulatory-assessment.org
Discussion Questions
1. What do you think about the tradeoff between unambiguously establishing
cause and effect (internal validity) and ensuring that research findings apply
to peoples everyday lives (external validity)? Which one of these would you
prioritize as a researcher? Why?
2. What challenges do you see that daily-life researchers may face in their
studies? How can they be overcome?
3. What ethical issues can come up in daily-life studies? How can (or should)
they be addressed?
4. How do you think smartphones and other mobile electronic devices will
change psychological research? What are their promises for the field? And
what are their pitfalls?
Vocabulary
Ambulatory assessment
An overarching term to describe methodologies that assess the behavior,
physiology, experience, and environments of humans in naturalistic settings.
Ecological validity
The degree to which a study finding has been obtained under conditions that
are typical for what happens in everyday life.
External validity
The degree to which a finding generalizes from the specific sample and context
of a study to some larger population and broader settings.
Internal validity
The degree to which a cause-effect relationship between two variables has been
unambiguously established.
Reference List
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Funder, D. C. (2007). Psychology as the science
of self-reports and finger movements: Whatever happened to actual
behavior? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 396403.
Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: Capturing life as it is
lived. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 579616.
Bolger, N., & Laurenceau, J-P. (2013). Intensive longitudinal methods: An
introduction to diary and experience sampling research. New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Bond, R. M., Jones, J. J., Kramer, A. D., Marlow, C., Settle, J. E., & Fowler, J. H. (2012).
A 61 million-person experiment in social influence and political mobilization.
Nature, 489, 295298.
Brewer, M. B. (2000). Research design and issues of validity. In H. T. Reis & C. M.
Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social psychology (pp. 316).
New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Cohn, M. A., Mehl, M. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2004). Linguistic indicators of
psychological change after September 11, 2001. Psychological Science, 15,
687693.
Conner, T. S., Tennen, H., Fleeson, W., & Barrett, L. F. (2009). Experience sampling
methods: A modern idiographic approach to personality research. Social
and Personality Psychology Compass, 3, 292313.
Craik, K. H. (2000). The lived day of an individual: A person-environment
perspective. In W. B. Walsh, K. H. Craik, & R. H. Price (Eds.), Personenvironment psychology: New directions and perspectives (pp. 233266).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Fahrenberg, J., &. Myrtek, M. (Eds.) (1996). Ambulatory assessment: Computerassisted psychological and psychophysiological methods in monitoring and
field studies. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.
Funder, D. C. (2007). The personality puzzle. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Co.
Funder, D. C. (2001). Personality. Review of Psychology, 52, 197221.
Gosling, S. D., & Johnson, J. A. (2010). Advanced methods for conducting online
behavioral research. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Gosling, S. D., Ko, S. J., Mannarelli, T., & Morris, M. E. (2002). A room with a cue:
Personality judgments based on offices and bedrooms. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 379398.
Hektner, J. M., Schmidt, J. A., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2007). Experience sampling
method: Measuring the quality of everyday life. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Kahneman, D., Krueger, A., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N., and Stone, A. (2004). A
survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The Day
Reconstruction Method. Science, 306, 1776780.
Kaplan, R. M., & Stone A. A. (2013). Bringing the laboratory and clinic to the
community: Mobile technologies for health promotion and disease
prevention. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 471-498.
activities
and
conversations.
Behavior
Research
Methods,
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Conducting Psychology Research in the Real
World by Matthias R. Mehl is licensed under the Creative Commons AttributionNonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Psychophysiological
Methods in Neuroscience
Zachary Infantolino & Gregory A. Miller
University of Delaware, University of California, Los Angeles
nobaproject.com
Abstract
As a generally noninvasive subset of neuroscience methods, psychophysiological
methods are used across a variety of disciplines in order to answer diverse
questions about psychology, both mental events and behavior. Many different
techniques are classified as psychophysiological. Each technique has its
strengths and weaknesses, and knowing them allows researchers to decide
what each offers for a particular question. Additionally, this knowledge allows
research consumers to evaluate the meaning of the results in a particular
experiment.
Learning Objectives
History
In the mid-19th century, a railroad worker named Phineas Gage was in charge
of setting explosive charges for blasting through rock in order to prepare a path
for railroad tracks. He would lay the charge in a hole drilled into the rock, place
a fuse and sand on top of the charge, and pack it all down using a tamping iron
(a solid iron rod approximately one yard long and a little over an inch in
diameter). On a September afternoon when Gage was performing this task, his
tamping iron caused a spark that set off the explosive prematurely, sending the
tamping iron flying through the air. Unfortunately for Gage, his head was above
the hole and the tamping iron entered the side of his face, passed behind his
left eye, and exited out of the top of his head, eventually landing 80 feet away.
Gage lost a portion of his left frontal lobe in the accident, but survived and lived
for another 12 years. What is most interesting from a psychological perspective
is that Gages personality changed as a result of this accident. He became more
impulsive, he had trouble carrying out plans, and, at times, he engaged in vulgar
profanity, which was out of character. This case study leads one to believe that
there are specific areas of the brain that are associated with certain
psychological phenomena. When studying psychology, the brain is indeed an
interesting source of information. Although it would be impossible to replicate
the type of damage done to Gage in the name of research, methods have
developed over the years that are able to safely measure different aspects of
nervous system activity in order to help researchers better understand
psychology as well as the relationship between psychology and biology.
77
Introduction
Psychophysiology is defined as any research in which the dependent variable
(what the researcher measures) is a physiological measure, and the
independent variable (what the researcher manipulates) is behavioral or
mental. In most cases the work is done noninvasively with awake human
participants. Physiological measures take many forms and range from blood
flow or neural activity in the brain to heart rate variability and eye movements.
These measures can provide information about processes including emotion,
cognition, and the interactions between them. In these ways, physiological
measures offer a very flexible set of tools for researchers to answer questions
about behavior, cognition, and health.
Psychophysiological methods are a subset of the very large domain of
neuroscience methods. Many neuroscience methods are invasive, such as
involving lesions of neural tissue, injection of neutrally active chemicals, or
manipulation of neural activity via electrical stimulation. The present survey
emphasizes noninvasive methods widely used with human subjects.
Crucially, in examining the relationship between physiology and overt
behavior or mental events, psychophysiology does not attempt to replace the
latter with the former. As an example, happiness is a state of pleasurable
contentment and is associated with various physiological measures, but one
would not say that those physiological measures are happiness. We can make
inferences about someones cognitive or emotional state based on his or her
self-report, physiology, or overt behavior. Sometimes our interest is primarily
in inferences about internal events and sometimes primarily in the physiology
itself. Psychophysiology addresses both kinds of goals.
78
79
80
of results from fMRI analyses overlaid on an sMRI image. The blue and orange
shapes represent areas with significant changes in the BOLD signal, thus
changes in neural activation.
Figure 1. Example of fMRI analyses overlaid on an sMRI image. The orange area indicates
an increase in the BOLD signal, and the blue area indicates a decrease in the BOLD signal. We
infer that neural activity increased in the orange region and decreased in the blue region.
81
purposes, and instead electrodes are placed on the participants scalp, resulting
in a noninvasive technique for measuring neural activity.
Given that this electrical activity must travel through the skull and scalp
before reaching the electrodes, localization of activity is less precise when
measuring from the scalp, but it can still be within several millimeters when
localizing activity that is near the scalp. One major advantage of EEG is its
temporal resolution. Data can be recorded thousands of times per second,
allowing researchers to document events that happen in less than a millisecond.
EEG analyses typically investigate the change in amplitude or frequency
components of the recorded EEG on an ongoing basis or averaged over dozens
of trials (see Figure 2).
Figure 2. Example of EEG analysis output. Panel A represents changes in the relative
strength of different frequencies in the EEG data over time. Panel B represents changes in the
amplitude in the instantaneous EEG voltage over time.
82
83
commonly areas with higher metabolic needs. Over time, this tracer molecule
emits positrons, which are detected by a sensor. The spatial location of the
tracer molecule in the brain can be determined based on the emitted positrons.
This allows researchers to construct a three-dimensional image of the areas of
the brain that have the highest metabolic needs, typically those that are most
active. Images resulting from PET usually represent neural activity that has
occurred over tens of minutes, which is very poor temporal resolution for some
purposes. PET images are often combined with computed tomography (CT)
images to improve spatial resolution, as fine as several millimeters. Tracers can
also be incorporated into molecules that bind to neurotransmitter receptors,
which allow researchers to answer some unique questions about the action of
neurotransmitters. Unfortunately, very few research centers have the
equipment required to obtain the images or the special equipment needed to
create the positron-emitting tracer molecules, which typically need to be
produced on site.
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is a noninvasive method that causes
depolarization or hyperpolarization in neurons near the scalp. This method
is not considered psychophysiological because the independent variable is
physiological, rather than the dependent. However, it does qualify as a
neuroscience method because it deals with the function of the nervous system,
and it can readily be combined with conventional psychophysiological methods.
In TMS, a coil of wire is placed just above the participants scalp. When electricity
flows through the coil, it produces a magnetic field. This magnetic field travels
through the skull and scalp and affects neurons near the surface of the brain.
When the magnetic field is rapidly turned on and off, a current is induced in the
neurons, leading to depolarization or hyperpolarization, depending on the
number of magnetic field pulses. Single- or paired-pulse TMS depolarizes sitespecific neurons in the cortex, causing them to fire. If this method is used over
primary motor cortex, it can produce or block muscle activity, such as inducing
a finger twitch or preventing someone from pressing a button. If used over
nobaproject.com - Psychophysiological Methods in Neuroscience
84
85
strength. Heart rate can easily be monitored using a minimum of two electrodes
and is measured by counting the number of heartbeats in a given time period,
such as one minute, or by assessing the time between successive heartbeats.
Psychological activity can prompt increases and decreases in heart rate, often
in less than a second, making heart rate a sensitive measure of cognition.
Measures of heart rate variability are concerned with consistency in the time
interval between heartbeats. Changes in heart rate variability are associated
with stress as well as psychiatric conditions. Figure 3 is an example of an
electrocardiogram, which is used to measure heart rate and heart rate
variability. These cardiovascular measures allow researchers to monitor SNS
and PNS reactivity to various stimuli or situations. For example, when an
arachnophobe views pictures of spiders, does their heart rate increase more
than that of a person not afraid of spiders?
86
wrong button), even if it is never visibly executed. It has also been used in
emotion research to identify activity in muscles that are used to produce smiles
and frowns. Using EMG, it is possible to detect very small facial movements that
are not observable from looking at the face. The temporal resolution of EMG is
similar to that of EEG and MEG.
Valuable information can also be gleaned from eye blinks, eye movements,
and pupil diameter. Eye blinks are most often assessed using EMG electrodes
placed just below the eyelid, but electrical activity associated directly with eye
blinks or eye movements can be measured with electrodes placed on the face
near the eyes, because there is voltage across the entire eyeball. Another option
for the measurement of eye movement is a camera used to record video of an
eye. This video method is particularly valuable when determination of absolute
direction of gaze (not just change in direction of gaze) is of interest, such as
when the eyes scan a picture. With the help of a calibration period in which a
participant looks at multiple, known targets, eye position is then extracted from
each video frame during the main task and compared with data from the
calibration phase, allowing researchers to identify the sequence, direction, and
duration of gaze fixations. For example, when viewing pleasant or unpleasant
images, people spend different amounts of time looking at the most arousing
parts. This, in turn, can vary as a function of psychopathology. Additionally, the
diameter of a participants pupil can be measured and recorded over time from
the video record. As with heart rate, pupil diameter is controlled by competing
inputs from the SNS and PNS. Pupil diameter is commonly used as an index of
mental effort when performing a task.
87
Conclusion
The outline of psychophysiological methods above provides a glimpse into the
exciting techniques that are available to researchers studying a broad range of
topics from clinical to social to cognitive psychology. Some of the most
interesting psychophysiological studies use several methods, such as in sleep
assessments or multimodal neuroimaging. Psychophysiological methods have
applications outside of mainstream psychology in areas where psychological
nobaproject.com - Psychophysiological Methods in Neuroscience
88
89
Outside Resources
Book: Luck, S. J. (2005). An introduction to the event-related potential
technique. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Discussion Questions
1. Pick
about. What
psychophysiological
the hypothesis you picked in the first question, and choose what
that people
more
actual
results are the same. Why would images of the brain and
neuroscience
with these
researchers
neuroscience
language?
vice versa?
Vocabulary
Blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD)
The signal typically measured in fMRI that results from changes in the ratio of
oxygenated hemoglobin to deoxygenated hemoglobin in the blood.
Deoxygenated hemoglobin
Hemoglobin not carrying oxygen.
Depolarization
A change in a cells membrane potential, making the inside of the cell more
positive and increasing the chance of an action potential.
Hemoglobin
The oxygen-carrying portion of a red blood cell.
Hyperpolarization
A change in a cells membrane potential, making the inside of the cell more
negative and decreasing the chance of an action potential.
Invasive Procedure
A procedure that involves the skin being broken or an instrument or chemical
being introduced into a body cavity.
Lesions
Abnormalities in the tissue of an organism usually caused by disease or trauma.
Neural plasticity
The ability of synapses and neural pathways to change over time and adapt to
changes in neural process, behavior, or environment.
Neuroscience methods
A research method that deals with the structure or function of the nervous
system and brain.
Noninvasive procedure
A procedure that does not require the insertion of an instrument or chemical
through the skin or into a body cavity.
Oxygenated hemoglobin
Hemoglobin carrying oxygen.
Parasympathetic nervous system (PNS)
One of the two major divisions of the autonomic nervous system, responsible
for stimulation of rest and digest activities.
Peripheral nervous system
The part of the nervous system that is outside the brain and spinal cord.
Positron
A particle having the same mass and numerically equal but positive charge as
an electron.
Psychophysiological methods
Any research method in which the dependent variable is a physiological
measure and the independent variable is behavioral or mental (such as
memory).
Spatial resolution
The degree to which one can separate a single object in space from another.
Sympathetic nervous system (SNS)
One of the two major divisions of the autonomic nervous system, responsible
for stimulation of fight or flight activities.
Temporal resolution
The degree to which one can separate a single point in time from another.
Voltage
The difference in electric charge between two points.
Statistical Thinking
Beth Chance & Allan Rossman
California Polytechnic State University, San Luis Obispo
nobaproject.com
Abstract
As our society increasingly calls for evidence-based decision making, it is
important to consider how and when we can draw valid inferences from data.
This chapter will use four recent research studies to highlight key elements of
a statistical investigation.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
Does drinking coffee actually increase your life expectancy? A recent study
(Freedman, Park, Abnet, Hollenbeck, & Sinha, 2012) found that men who drank
at least six cups of coffee a day had a 10% lower chance of dying (women 15%
lower) than those who drank none. Does this mean you should pick up or
increase your own coffee habit?
Modern society has become awash in studies such as this; you can read
about several such studies in the news every day. Moreover, data abound
everywhere in modern life. Conducting such a study well, and interpreting the
results of such studies well for making informed decisions or setting policies,
requires understanding basic ideas of statistics, the science of gaining insight
from data. Rather than relying on anecdote and intuition, statistics allows us to
systematically study phenomena of interest.
Key components to a statistical investigation are:
Planning the study: Start by asking a testable research question and deciding
how to collect data. For example, how long was the study period of the
coffee study? How many people were recruited for the study, how were they
recruited, and from where? How old were they? What other variables were
recorded about the individuals, such as smoking habits, on the
comprehensive lifestyle questionnaires? Were changes made to the
participants coffee habits during the course of the study?
Examining the data: What are appropriate ways to examine the data? What
graphs are relevant, and what do they reveal? What descriptive statistics
can be calculated to summarize relevant aspects of the data, and what do
they reveal? What patterns do you see in the data? Are there any individual
observations that deviate from the overall pattern, and what do they reveal?
For example, in the coffee study, did the proportions differ when we
100
Inferring from the data: What are valid statistical methods for drawing
inferences beyond the data you collected? In the coffee study, is the 10%
15% reduction in risk of death something that could have happened just by
chance?
Drawing conclusions: Based on what you learned from your data, what
conclusions can you draw? Who do you think these conclusions apply to?
(Were the people in the coffee study older? Healthy? Living in cities?) Can
you draw a cause-and-effect conclusion about your treatments? (Are
scientists now saying that the coffee drinking is the cause of the decreased
risk of death?)
Distributional Thinking
When data are collected to address a particular question, an important first
step is to think of meaningful ways to organize and examine the data. The most
fundamental principle of statistics is that data vary. The pattern of that variation
is crucial to capture and to understand. Often, careful presentation of the data
will address many of the research questions without requiring more
sophisticated analyses. It may, however, point to additional questions that need
to be examined in more detail.
nobaproject.com - Statistical Thinking
101
Table 1. Frequency tables of patient reading levels and pamphlet readability levels.
102
Figure 1 makes clear that the two distributions are not well aligned at all.
The most glaring discrepancy is that many patients (17/63, or 27%, to be precise)
have a reading level below that of the most readable pamphlet. These patients
will need help to understand the information provided in the cancer pamphlets.
Notice that this conclusion follows from considering the distributions as a
whole, not simply measures of center or variability, and that the graph contrasts
those distributions more immediately than the frequency tables.
103
Statistical Significance
Even when we find patterns in data, often there is still uncertainty in various
aspects of the data. For example, there may be potential for measurement
errors (even your own body temperature can fluctuate by almost 1 F over the
course of the day). Or we may only have a snapshot of observations from a
more long-term process or only a small subset of individuals from the
population of interest. In such cases, how can we determine whether patterns
we see in our small set of data is convincing evidence of a systematic
phenomenon in the larger process or population?
Example 2: In a study reported in the November 2007 issue of Nature,
researchers investigated whether pre-verbal infants take into account an
individuals actions toward others in evaluating that individual as appealing or
aversive (Hamlin, Wynn, & Bloom, 2007). In one component of the study, 10month-old infants were shown a climber character (a piece of wood with
googly eyes glued onto it) that could not make it up a hill in two tries. Then
the infants were shown two scenarios for the climbers next try, one where the
climber was pushed to the top of the hill by another character (helper), and
one where the climber was pushed back down the hill by another character
(hinderer). The infant was alternately shown these two scenarios several
times. Then the infant was presented with two pieces of wood (representing
the helper and the hinderer characters) and asked to pick one to play with. The
researchers found that of the 16 infants who made a clear choice, 14 chose to
play with the helper toy.
One possible explanation for this clear majority result is that the helping
behavior of the one toy increases the infants likelihood of choosing that toy.
But are there other possible explanations? What about the color of the toy?
Well, prior to collecting the data, the researchers arranged so that each color
and shape (red square and blue circle) would be seen by the same number of
infants. Or maybe the infants had right-handed tendencies and so picked
nobaproject.com - Statistical Thinking
104
whichever toy was closer to their right hand? Well, prior to collecting the data,
the researchers arranged it so half the infants saw the helper toy on the right
and half on the left. Or, maybe the shapes of these wooden characters (square,
triangle, circle) had an effect? Perhaps, but again, the researchers controlled
for this by rotating which shape was the helper toy, the hinderer toy, and the
climber. When designing experiments, it is important to control for as many
variables as might affect the responses as possible.
It is beginning to appear that the researchers accounted for all the other
plausible explanations. But there is one more important consideration that
cannot be controlledif we did the study again with these 16 infants, they might
not make the same choices. In other words, there is some randomness inherent
in their selection process. Maybe each infant had no genuine preference at all,
and it was simply random luck that led to 14 infants picking the helper toy.
Although this random component cannot be controlled, we can apply a
probability model to investigate the pattern of results that would occur in the
long run if random chance were the only factor.
If the infants were equally likely to pick between the two toys, then each
infant had a 50% chance of picking the helper toy. Its like each infant tossed a
coin, and if it landed heads, the infant picked the helper toy. So if we tossed a
coin 16 times, could it land heads 14 times? Sure, its possible, but it turns out
to be very unlikely. Getting 14 (or more) heads in 16 tosses is about as likely as
tossing a coin and getting 9 heads in a row. This probability is referred to as a
p-value. The p-value tells you how often a random process would give a result
at least as extreme as what was found in the actual study, assuming there was
nothing other than random chance at play. So, if we assume that each infant
was choosing equally, then the probability that 14 or more out of 16 infants
would choose the helper toy is found to be 0.0021. We have only two logical
possibilities: either the infants have a genuine preference for the helper toy, or
the infants have no preference (50/50) and an outcome that would occur only
2 times in 1,000 iterations happened in this study. Because this p-value of 0.0021
nobaproject.com - Statistical Thinking
105
is quite small, we conclude that the study provides very strong evidence that
these infants have a genuine preference for the helper toy. We often compare
the p-value to some cut-off value (called the level of significance, typically
around 0.05). If the p-value is smaller than that cut-off value, then we reject the
hypothesis that only random chance was at play here. In this case, these
researchers would conclude that significantly more than half of the infants in
the study chose the helper toy, giving strong evidence of a genuine preference
for the toy with the helping behavior.
Generalizability
One limitation to the previous study is that the conclusion only applies to the
16 infants in the study. We dont know much about how those 16 infants were
selected. Suppose we want to select a subset of individuals (a sample) from a
much larger group of individuals (the population) in such a way that conclusions
from the sample can be generalized to the larger population. This is the
question faced by pollsters every day.
Example 3: The General Social Survey (GSS) is a survey on societal trends
conducted every other year in the United States. Based on a sample of about
2,000 adult Americans, researchers make claims about what percentage of the
U.S. population consider themselves to be liberal, what percentage consider
themselves happy, what percentage feel rushed in their daily lives, and many
other issues. The key to making these claims about the larger population of all
American adults lies in how the sample is selected. The goal is to select a sample
that is representative of the population, and a common way to achieve this goal
is to select a random sample that gives every member of the population an
equal chance of being selected for the sample. In its simplest form, random
sampling involves numbering every member of the population and then using
a computer to randomly select the subset to be surveyed. Most polls dont
operate exactly like this, but they do use probability-based sampling methods
nobaproject.com - Statistical Thinking
106
107
In this example, the key question is whether the type of motivation affects
creativity scores. In particular, do subjects who were asked about intrinsic
motivations tend to have higher creativity scores than subjects who were asked
about extrinsic motivations?
Figure 2 reveals that both motivation groups saw considerable variability in
creativity scores, and these scores have considerable overlap between the
groups. In other words, its certainly not always the case that those with extrinsic
motivations have higher creativity than those with intrinsic motivations, but
nobaproject.com - Statistical Thinking
108
109
similar age distribution between the two groups; we should have a similar
distribution of educational background between the two groups; and so on.
Random assignment should produce groups that are as similar as possible
except for the type of motivation, which presumably eliminates all those other
variables as possible explanations for the observed tendency for higher scores
in the intrinsic group.
But does this always work? No, so by luck of the draw the groups may be
a little different prior to answering the motivation survey. So then the question
is, is it possible that an unlucky random assignment is responsible for the
observed difference in creativity scores between the groups? In other words,
suppose each individuals poem was going to get the same creativity score no
matter which group they were assigned to, that the type of motivation in no
way impacted their score. Then how often would the random-assignment
process alone lead to a difference in mean creativity scores as large (or larger)
than 19.88 15.74 = 4.14 points?
We again want to apply to a probability model to approximate a p-value, but
this time the model will be a bit different. Think of writing everyones creativity
scores on an index card, shuffling up the index cards, and then dealing out 23
to the extrinsic motivation group and 24 to the intrinsic motivation group, and
finding the difference in the group means. We (better yet, the computer) can
repeat this process over and over to see how often, when the scores dont
change, random assignment leads to a difference in means at least as large as
4.41. Figure 3 shows the results from 1,000 such hypothetical random
assignments for these scores.
110
111
even though percent reduction in risk was not extremely large (dropping
from a 12% chance to about 10%11%).
This study needs to be reviewed in the larger context of similar studies and
consistency of results across studies, with the constant caution that this was
not a randomized experiment. Whereas a statistical analysis can still adjust
for other potential confounding variables, we are not yet convinced that
researchers have identified them all or completely isolated why this decrease
in death risk is evident. Researchers can now take the findings of this study and
develop more focused studies that address new questions.
112
Outside Resources
Apps: Interactive web applets for teaching and learning statistics include the
collection at
http://www.rossmanchance.com/applets/
Web: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research
http://www.icpsr.umich.edu/index.html
Web: The Consortium for the Advancement of Undergraduate Statistics
https://www.causeweb.org/
Discussion Questions
1. Find a recent research article in your field and answer the following: What
was the primary research question? How were individuals selected to
participate in the study? Were summary results provided? How strong is the
evidence presented in favor or against the research question? Was random
assignment used? Summarize the main conclusions from the study,
addressing the issues of statistical significance, statistical confidence,
generalizability, and cause and effect. Do you agree with the conclusions
drawn from this study, based on the study design and the results presented?
2. Is it reasonable to use a random sample of 1,000 individuals to draw
conclusions about all U.S. adults? Explain why or why not.
Vocabulary
Cause-and-effect
Related to whether we say one variable is causing changes in the other variable,
versus other variables that may be related to these two variables.
Confidence interval
An interval of plausible values for a population parameter; the interval of values
within the margin of error of a statistic.
Distribution
The pattern of variation in data.
Generalizability
Related to whether the results from the sample can be generalized to a larger
population.
Margin of error
The expected amount of random variation in a statistic; often defined for 95%
confidence level.
Parameter
A numerical result summarizing a population (e.g., mean, proportion).
Population
A larger collection of individuals that we would like to generalize our results to.
P-value
The probability of observing a particular outcome in a sample, or more extreme,
under a conjecture about the larger population or process.
Random assignment
Using a probability-based method to divide a sample into treatment groups.
Random sampling
Using a probability-based method to select a subset of individuals for the
sample from the population.
Sample
The collection of individuals on which we collect data.
Statistic
A numerical result computed from a sample (e.g., mean, proportion).
Statistical significance
A result is statistically significant if it is unlikely to arise by chance alone.
Reference List
Amabile, T. (1985). Motivation and creativity: Effects of motivational orientation
on creative writers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(2), 393
399.
Freedman, N. D., Park, Y., Abnet, C. C., Hollenbeck, A. R., & Sinha, R. (2012).
Association of coffee drinking with total and cause-specific mortality. New
England Journal of Medicine, 366, 18911904.
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants.
Nature, 452(22), 557560.
Ramsey, F., & Schafer, D. (2002). The statistical sleuth: A course in methods of
data analysis. Belmont, CA: Duxbury.
Short, T., Moriarty, H., & Cooley, M. E. (1995). Readability of educational materials
for patients with cancer. Journal of Statistics Education, 3(2).
Stanovich, K. (2013). How to think straight about psychology (10th ed.). Upper
Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Statistical Thinking by Beth Chance and Allan
Rossman is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
History of Psychology
David B. Baker & Heather Sperry
University of Akron, The University of Akron
nobaproject.com
Abstract
This chapter provides an introduction and overview of the historical
development of the science and practice of psychology in America. Everincreasing specialization within the field often makes it difficult to discern the
common roots from which the field of psychology has evolved. By exploring
this shared past, students will be better able to understand how psychology
has developed into the discipline we know today.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
It is always a difficult question to ask, where to begin to tell the story of the
history of psychology. Some would start with ancient Greece; others would look
to a demarcation in the late 19th century when the science of psychology was
formally proposed and instituted. These two perspectives, and all that is in
between, are appropriate for describing a history of psychology. The interested
student will have no trouble finding an abundance of resources on all of these
time frames and perspectives (Goodwin, 2011; Leahey, 2012; Schultz & Schultz,
2007). For the purposes of this chapter, we will examine the development of
psychology in America and use the mid-19th century as our starting point. For
the sake of convenience, we refer to this as a history of modern psychology.
Psychology is an exciting field and the history of psychology offers the
opportunity to make sense of how it has grown and developed. The history of
psychology also provides perspective. Rather than a dry collection of names
and dates, the history of psychology tells us about the important intersection
of time and place that defines who we are. Consider what happens when you
meet someone for the first time. The conversation usually begins with a series
of questions such as, Where did you grow up? How long have you lived here?
Where did you go to school? The importance of history in defining who we
are cannot be understated. Whether you are seeing a physician, talking with a
counselor, or applying for a job, everything begins with a history. The same is
true for studying the history of psychology; getting a history of the field helps
to make sense of where we are and how we got here.
122
A Prehistory of Psychology
Precursors to American psychology can be found in philosophy and physiology.
Philosophers such as John Locke (16321704) and Thomas Reid (17101796)
promoted empiricism, the idea that all knowledge comes from experience. The
work of Locke, Reid, and others emphasized the role of the human observer
and the primacy of the senses in defining how the mind comes to acquire
knowledge. In American colleges and universities in the early 1800s, these
principles were taught as courses on mental and moral philosophy. Most often
these courses taught about the mind based on the faculties of intellect, will,
and the senses (Fuchs, 2000).
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
Psychology as a Profession
As the roles of psychologists and the needs of the public continued to change,
it was necessary for psychology to begin to define itself as a profession. Without
standards for training and practice, anyone could use the title psychologist and
offer services to the public. As early as 1917, applied psychologists organized
to create standards for education, training, and licensure. By the 1930s, these
efforts led to the creation of the American Association for Applied Psychology
(AAAP). While the American Psychological Association (APA) represented the
interests of academic psychologists, AAAP served those in education, industry,
consulting, and clinical work.
The advent of WWII changed everything. The psychiatric casualties of war
were staggering, and there were simply not enough mental health professionals
to meet the need. Recognizing the shortage, the federal government urged the
AAAP and APA to work together to meet the mental health needs of the nation.
The result was the merging of the AAAP and the APA and a focus on the training
of professional psychologists. Through the provisions of National Mental Health
Act of 1946, funding was made available that allowed the APA, the Veterans
Administration, and the Public Health Service to work together to develop
training programs that would produce clinical psychologists. These efforts led
to the convening of the Boulder Conference on Graduate Education in Clinical
Psychology in 1949 in Boulder, Colorado. The meeting launched doctoral
nobaproject.com - History of Psychology
131
132
Conclusion
Growth and expansion have been a constant in American psychology. In the
latter part of the 20th century, areas such as social, developmental, and
personality psychology made major contributions to our understanding of what
it means to be human. Today neuroscience is enjoying tremendous interest and
growth.
As mentioned at the beginning of the chapter, it is a challenge to cover all
the history of psychology in such a short space. Errors of omission and
commission are likely in such a selective review. The history of psychology helps
to set a stage upon which the story of psychology can be told. This brief summary
provides some glimpse into the depth and rich content offered by the history
of psychology. The chapters in this e-book are all elaborations on the foundation
nobaproject.com - History of Psychology
133
created by our shared past. It is hoped that you will be able to see these
connections and have a greater understanding and appreciation for both the
unity and diversity of the field of psychology.
Timeline
134
135
Outside Resources
Web: Advances in the History of Psychology
http://ahp.apps01.yorku.ca/
Web: Center for the History of Psychology
http://www.uakron.edu/chp
Web: Classics in the History of Psychology
http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/
Web: Psychologys Feminist Voices
http://www.feministvoices.com/
Web: This Week in the History of Psychology
http://www.yorku.ca/christo/podcasts/
Discussion Questions
1. Why was psychophysics important to the development of psychology as a
science?
2. How have psychologists participated in the advancement of social issues?
3. Name some ways in which psychology began to be applied to the general
public and everyday problems.
4. Describe functionalism and structuralism and their influences on
behaviorism and cognitive psychology.
Vocabulary
Behaviorism
The study of behavior.
Cognitive psychology
The study of mental processes.
Consciousness
Awareness of ourselves and our environment.
Empiricism
The belief that knowledge comes from experience.
Eugenics
The practice of selective breeding to promote desired traits.
Flashbulb memory
A highly detailed and vivid memory of an emotionally significant event.
Functionalism
A school of American psychology that focused on the utility of consciousness.
Gestalt psychology
An attempt to study the unity of experience.
Individual differences
Ways in which people differ in terms of their behavior, emotion, cognition, and
development.
Introspection
A method of focusing on internal processes.
Neural impulse
An electro-chemical signal that enables neurons to communicate.
Practitioner-Scholar Model
A model of training of professional psychologists that emphasizes clinical
practice.
Psychophysics
Study of the relationships between physical stimuli and the perception of those
stimuli.
Realism
A point of view that emphasizes the importance of the senses in providing
knowledge of the external world.
Scientist-practitioner model
A model of training of professional psychologists that emphasizes the
development of both research and clinical skills.
Structuralism
A school of American psychology that sought to describe the elements of
conscious experience.
Tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon
The inability to pull a word from memory even though there is the sensation
that that word is available.
Reference List
Benjamin, L. T. (2007). A brief history of modern psychology. Malden, MA:
Blackwell Publishing.
Benjamin, L. T. (2000). The psychology laboratory at the turn of the 20th century.
American Psychologist, 55, 318321.
Benjamin, L. T., & Baker, D. B. (2004). From sance to science: A history of the
profession of psychology in America. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson
Learning.
Cautin, R., & Baker, D. B. (in press). A history of education and training in
professional psychology. In B. Johnson & N. Kaslow (Eds.), Oxford handbook
of education and training in professional psychology. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Evans, R. B. (1972). E. B. Titchener and his lost system. Journal of the History of
the Behavioral Sciences, 8, 168180.
Fancher, R. E. (1987). The intelligence men: Makers of the IQ controversy. New
York, NY: W.W. Norton & Company.
Fancher, R. E., & Rutherford, A. (2011). Pioneers of psychology: A history (4th
ed.). New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Company.
Fuchs, A. H. (2000). Contributions of American mental philosophers to
psychology in the United States. History of Psychology, 3, 319.
Garnets, L., & Kimmel, D. C. (2003). What a light it shed: The life of Evelyn Hooker.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. History of Psychology by David B. Baker and
Heather Sperry is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercialShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Topic 2
Biological Basis of Behavior
Neurons
Sharon Furtak
California State University, Sacramento
nobaproject.com
Abstract
This chapter on the biological basis of behavior provides an overview of the
basic structure of neurons and their means of communication. Neurons, cells
in the central nervous system, receive information from our sensory systems
(vision, audition, olfaction, gustation, and somatosensation) about the world
around us; in turn, they plan and execute appropriate behavioral responses,
including attending to a stimulus, learning new information, speaking, eating,
mating, and evaluating potential threats. The goal of this chapter is to become
familiar with the anatomical structure of neurons and to understand how
neurons communicate by electrochemical signals to process sensory
information and produce complex behaviors through networks of neurons.
Having a basic knowledge of the fundamental structure and function of neurons
is a necessary foundation as you move forward in the field of psychology.
Learning Objectives
Differentiate the functional roles between the two main cell classes in the
brain, neurons and glia.
Introduction
Imagine trying to string words together into a meaningful sentence without
knowing the meaning of each word or its function (i.e., Is it a verb, a noun, or
an adjective?). In a similar fashion, to appreciate how groups of cells work
together in a meaningful way in the brain as a whole, we must first understand
how individual cells in the brain function. Much like words, brain cells, called
neurons, have an underlying structure that provides the foundation for their
functional purpose. Have you ever seen a neuron? Did you know that the basic
structure of a neuron is similar whether it is from the brain of a rat or a human?
How do the billions of neurons in our brain allow us to do all the fun things we
enjoy, such as texting a friend, cheering on our favorite sports team, or laughing?
nobaproject.com - Neurons
148
Figure 1. Three drawings by Santiago Ramn y Cajal, taken from "Comparative study of the
sensory areas of the human cortex", pages 314, 361, and 363. Left: Nissl-stained visual cortex
of a human adult. Middle: Nissl-stained motor cortex of a human adult. Right: Golgi-stained
cortex of a 1 1/2 month old infant. From Wikipedia public domain.
Our journey in answering these questions begins more than 100 years ago
with a scientist named Santiago Ramn y Cajal. Ramn y Cajal (1911) boldly
concluded that discrete individual neurons are the structural and functional
units of the nervous system. He based his conclusion on the numerous drawings
he made of Golgi-stained tissue, a stain named after the scientist who
discovered it, Camillo Golgi. Scientists use several types of stains to visualize
cells. Each stain works in a unique way, which causes them to look differently
when viewed under a microscope. For example, a very common Nissl stain
labels only the main part of the cell (i.e., the cell body; see left and middle panels
of Figure 1). In contrast, a Golgi stain fills the cell body and all the processes
that extend outward from it (see right panel of Figure 1). A more notable
nobaproject.com - Neurons
149
150
large groups of cells send and receive information by electrical and chemical
signals.
A note of encouragement: This chapter introduces a vast amount of technical
terminology that at times may feel overwhelming. Do not get discouraged or
bogged down in the details. Utilize the glossary at the end of the chapter as a
quick reference guide; tab the glossary page so that you can easily refer to it
while reading the chapter. The glossary contains all terms in bold typing. Terms
in italics are additional significant terms that may appear in other chapters but
are not contained within the glossary. On your first read of this chapter, I suggest
focusing on the broader concepts and functional aspects of the terms instead
of trying to commit all the terminology to memory. That is right, I said read first!
I highly suggest reading this chapter at least twice, once prior to and again
following the course lecture on this material. Repetition is the best way to gain
clarity and commit to memory the challenging concepts and detailed vocabulary
presented here.
151
The place at which the axon of one neuron comes in close contact to the dendrite
of another neuron is a synapse (see Figures 23). Typically, the axon of a neuron
is covered with an insulating substance called a myelin sheath that allows the
signal and communication of one neuron to travel rapidly to another neuron.
The axon splits many times, so that it can communicate, or synapse, with
several other neurons (see Figure 2). At the end of the axon is a terminal button,
which forms synapses with spines, or protrusions, on the dendrites of neurons.
Synapsesform between the presynaptic terminal button (neuron sending the
signal) and the postsynaptic membrane (neuron receiving the signal; see Figure
3). Here we will focus specifically on synapses between the terminal button of
an axon and a dendritic spine; however, synapses can also form between the
terminal button of an axon and the soma or the axon of another neuron.
nobaproject.com - Neurons
152
nobaproject.com - Neurons
153
In addition to neurons, there is a second type of cell in the brain called glia
cells. Glia cells have several functions, just a few of which we will discuss here.
One type of glia cell, called oligodendroglia, forms the myelin sheaths
mentioned above (Simons & Trotter, 2007; see Fig. 2). Oligodendroglia wrap
their dendritic processes around the axons of neurons many times to form the
myelin sheath. One cell will form the myelin sheath on several axons. Other
types of glia cells, such as microglia and astrocytes, digest debris of dead
neurons, carry nutritional support from blood vessels to the neurons, and help
to regulate the ionic composition of the extracellular fluid. While glial cells play
a vital role in neuronal support, they do not participate in the communication
between cells in the same fashion as neurons do.
154
nobaproject.com - Neurons
155
156
nobaproject.com - Neurons
157
3. Chloride (Cl-): The cell membrane is also very permeable to chloride at rest,
but chloride remains in high concentration outside the cell. Diffusion pushes
Cl- inside the cell because it is in high concentration outside the cell.
However, electrostatic pressure pushes Cl- outside the cell because the
negative charge of Cl- is attracted to the positive charge outside the cell.
Similar to K+, these forces oppose one another with respect to Cl-.
4. Sodium (Na+): The cell membrane is not very permeable to sodium at rest.
Diffusion pushes Na+ inside the cell because it is in high concentration
outside the cell. Electrostatic pressure also pushes Na+ inside the cell
because the positive charge of Na+ is attracted to the negative charge inside
the cell. Both of these forces push Na+ inside the cell; however, Na+ cannot
permeate the cell membrane and remains in high concentration outside
the cell. The small amounts of Na+ inside the cell are removed by a sodiumpotassium pump, which uses the neurons energy (adenosine triphosphate,
ATP) to pump 3 Na+ ions out of the cell in exchange for bringing 2 K+ ions
inside the cell.
Action Potential
Now that we have considered what occurs in a neuron at rest, let us consider
what changes occur to the resting membrane potential when a neuron receives
input, or information, from the presynaptic terminal button of another neuron.
Our understanding of the electrical signals or potentials that occurs within a
neuron results from the seminal work of Hodgkin and Huxleythat began in the
1930s at a well-known marine biology lab in Woodshole, MA. Their work, for
which they won the Nobel Prize in Medicine in 1963, has resulted in the general
model of electrochemical transduction that is described here (Hodgkin &
Huxley, 1952). Hodgkin and Huxley studied a very large axon in the squid, a
nobaproject.com - Neurons
158
common species for that region of the United States. The giant axon of the
squid is roughly 100 times larger than that of axons in the mammalian brain,
making it much easier to see. Activation of the giant axon is responsible for a
withdrawal response the squid uses when trying to escape from a predator,
such as large fish, birds, sharks, and even humans. When was the last time you
had calamari? The large axon size is no mistake in natures design; it allows for
very rapid transmission of an electrical signal, enabling a swift escape motion
in the squid from its predators.
While studying this species, Hodgkin and Huxley noticed that if they applied
an electrical stimulus to the axon, a large, transient electrical current conducted
down the axon. This transient electrical current is known as an action potential
(see Figure 5). An action potential is an all-or-nothing response that occurs when
there is a change in the charge or potential of the cell from its resting membrane
potential (-70 mV) in a more positive direction, which is a depolarization (see
Figure 5). What is meant by an all-or-nothing response? I find that this concept
is best compared to the binary code used in computers, where there are only
two possibilities, 0 or 1. There is no halfway or in-between these possible values;
for example, 0.5 does not exist in binary code. There are only two possibilities,
either the value of 0 or the value of 1. The action potential is the same in this
respect. There is no halfway; it occurs, or it does not occur. There is a specific
membrane potential that the neuron must reach to initiate an action potential.
This membrane potential, called the threshold of excitation, is typically around
-50 mV. If the threshold of excitation is reached, then an action potential is
triggered.
nobaproject.com - Neurons
159
How is an action potential initiated? At any one time, each neuron is receiving
hundreds of inputs from the cells that synapse with it. These inputs can cause
several types of fluctuations in the neurons membrane potentials (see Figure 5):
160
the same synapse then the result will be a large EPSP. If you have a small EPSP
and a small IPSP at the same time and the same synapse then they will cancel
each other out. Unlike the action potential, which is an all-or-nothing response,
IPSPs and EPSPs are smaller and graded potentials, varying in strength. The
change in voltage during an action potential is approximately 100 mV. In
comparison, EPSPs and IPSPs are changes in voltage between 0.1 to 40 mV.
They can be different strengths, or gradients, and they are measured by how
far the membrane potentials diverge from the resting membrane potential.
I know the concept of summation can be confusing. As a child, I use to play
a game in elementary school with a very large parachute where you would try
to knock balls out of the center of the parachute. This game illustrates the
properties of summation rather well. In this game, a group of children next to
one another would work in unison to produce waves in the parachute in order
to cause a wave large enough to knock the ball out of the parachute. The children
would initiate the waves at the same time and in the same direction. The additive
result was a larger wave in the parachute, and the balls would bounce out of
the parachute. However, if the waves they initiated occurred in the opposite
direction or with the wrong timing, the waves would cancel each other out, and
the balls would remain in the center of the parachute. EPSPs or IPSPs in a neuron
work in the same fashion to the properties of the waves in the parachute; they
either add or cancel each other out. If you have two EPSPs, then they sum
together and become a larger depolarization. Similarly, if two IPSPs come into
the cell at the same time, they will sum and become a larger hyperpolarization
in membrane potential. However, if two inputs were opposing one another,
moving the potential in opposite directions, such as an EPSP and an IPSP, their
sum would cancel each other out.
At any moment in time, each cell is receiving mixed messages, both EPSPs
and IPSPs. If the summation of EPSPs is strong enough to depolarize the
membrane potential to reach the threshold of excitation, then it initiates an
action potential. The action potential then travels down the axon, away from
nobaproject.com - Neurons
161
the soma, until it reaches the ends of the axon (the terminal button). In the
terminal button, the action potential triggers the release of neurotransmitters
from the presynaptic terminal button into the synaptic gap. These
neurotransmitters, in turn, cause EPSPs and IPSPs in the postsynaptic dendritic
spines of the next cell (see Figures 4 & 6). The neurotransmitter released from
the presynaptic terminal button binds with ionotropic receptors in a lock-andkey fashion on the post-synaptic dendritic spine. Ionotropic receptors are
receptors on ion channels that open, allowing some ions to enter or exit the
cell, depending upon the presence of a particular neurotransmitter. The type
of neurotransmitter and the permeability of the ion channel it activates will
determine if an EPSP or IPSP occurs in the dendrite of the post-synaptic cell.
These EPSPs and IPSPs summate in the same fashion described above and the
entire process occurs again in another cell.
162
Figure 5). The inside of the cell becomes very positively charged, +40mV. At this
point, the Na+ channels close and become refractory. This means the Na+
channels cannot reopen again until after the cell returns to the resting
membrane potential. Thus, a new action potential cannot occur during the
refractory period. The refractory period also ensures the action potential can
only move in one direction down the axon, away from the soma. As the cell
becomes more depolarized, a second type of voltage-dependent channel
opens; this channel is permeable to K+. With the cell very positive relative to
the outside of the cell (depolarized) and the high concentration of K+ within the
cell, both the force of diffusion and the force of electrostatic pressure drive K+
outside of the cell. The movement of K+ out of the cell causes the cell potential
to return back to the resting membrane potential, the falling or hyperpolarizing
phase of the action potential (see Figure 5). A short hyperpolarization occurs
partially due to the gradual closing of the K+ channels. With the Na+ closed,
electrostatic pressure continues to push K+ out of the cell. In addition, the
sodium-potassium pump is pushing Na+ out of the cell. The cell returns to the
resting membrane potential, and the excess extracellular K+ diffuses away. This
exchange of Na+ and K+ ions happens very rapidly, in less than 1 msec. The
action potential occurs in a wave-like motion down the axon until it reaches the
terminal button. Only the ion channels in very close proximity to the action
potential are affected.
nobaproject.com - Neurons
163
Earlier you learned that axons are covered in myelin. Let us consider how
myelin speeds up the process of the action potential. There are gaps in the
myelin sheaths called nodes of Ranvier. The myelin insulates the axon and does
not allow any fluid to exist between the myelin and cell membrane. Under the
myelin, when the Na+ and K+ channels open, no ions flow between the
intracellular and extracellular fluid. This saves the cell from having to expend
the energy necessary to rectify or regain the resting membrane potential.
(Remember, the pumps need ATP to run.) Under the myelin, the action potential
degrades some, but is still large enough in potential to trigger a new action
potential at the next node of Ranvier. Thus, the action potential actively jumps
from node to node; this process is known as saltatory conduction.
In the presynaptic terminal button, the action potential triggers the release
of neurotransmitters (see Figure 3). Neurotransmitters cross the synaptic gap
and open subtypes of receptors in a lock-and-key fashion (see Figure 3).
Depending on the type of neurotransmitter, an EPSP or IPSP occurs in the
dendrite of the post-synaptic cell. Neurotransmitters that open Na+ or calcium
(Ca+) channels cause an EPSP; an example is the NMDA receptors, which are
nobaproject.com - Neurons
164
which
will
be
discussed
further
in
the
chapter
on
psychopharmacology.
nobaproject.com - Neurons
165
Outside Resources
Video: An animation of an action potential
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ifD1YG07fB8
Video: An animation of neurotransmitter actions at the synapse
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=90cj4NX87Yk
Video: Another animation of an action potential
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=-SHBnExxub8&list=PL968773A54EF13D21
Video: Another animation of neurotransmitter actions at the synapse
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=LT3VKAr4roo&list=PL968773A54EF13D21
Video: For perspective on techniques in neuroscience to look inside the brain
http://www.ted.com/talks/carl_schoonover_how_to_look_inside_the_brain.html
Web: For more information on the Nobel Prize shared by Ramn y Cajal and
Golgi
http://www.nobelprize.org/nobel_prizes/medicine/laureates/1906/
Discussion Questions
1. What structures of a neuron are the main input and output of that neuron?
2. What does the statement mean that communication within and between
cells is an electrochemical process?
3. How does myelin increase speed and efficiency of the action potential?
4. How does diffusion and electrostatic pressure contribute to the resting
membrane potential and the action potential?
5. Describe the cycle of communication within and between neurons.
Vocabulary
Action potential
A transient all-or-nothing electrical current that is conducted down the axon
when the membrane potential reaches the threshold of excitation.
Axon
Part of the neuron that extends off the soma, splitting several times to connect
with other neurons; main output of the neuron.
Cell membrane
A bi-lipid layer of molecules that separates the cell from the surrounding
extracellular fluid.
Dendrite
Part of a neuron that extends away from the cell body and is the main input to
the neuron.
Diffusion
The force on molecules to move from areas of high concentration to areas of
low concentration.
Electrostatic pressure
The force on two ions with similar charge to repel each other; the force of two
ions with opposite charge to attract to one another.
Excitatory postsynaptic potentials
A depolarizing postsynaptic current that causes the membrane potential to
become more positive and move towards the threshold of excitation.
Sodium-potassium pump
An ion channel that uses the neurons energy (adenosine triphosphate, ATP) to
pump three Na+ ions outside the cell in exchange for bringing two K+ ions inside
the cell.
Soma
Cell body of a neuron that contains the nucleus and genetic information, and
directs protein synthesis.
Spines
Protrusions on the dendrite of a neuron that form synapses with terminal
buttons of the presynaptic axon.
Synapse
Junction between the presynaptic terminal button of one neuron and the
dendrite, axon, or soma of another postsynaptic neuron.
Synaptic gap
Also known as the synaptic cleft; the small space between the presynaptic
terminal button and the postsynaptic dendritic spine, axon, or soma.
Synaptic vesicles
Groups of neurotransmitters packaged together and located within the
terminal button.
Terminal button
The part of the end of the axon that form synapses with postsynaptic dendrite,
axon, or soma.
Threshold of excitation
Specific membrane potential that the neuron must reach to initiate an action
potential.
Reference List
De Carlos, J. A., & Borrell, J. (2007). A historical reflection of the contributions of
Cajal and Golgi to the foundations of neuroscience. Brain Res Rev, 55(1),
8-16. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.03.010
Furtak, S. C., Moyer, J. R., Jr., & Brown, T. H. (2007). Morphology and ontogeny
of rat perirhinal cortical neurons. J Comp Neurol, 505(5), 493-510. doi:
10.1002/cne.21516
Grant, G. (2007). How the 1906 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine was shared
between Golgi and Cajal. Brain Res Rev, 55(2), 490-498. doi: 10.1016/j.
brainresrev.2006.11.004
Hodgkin, A. L., & Huxley, A. F. (1952). A quantitative description of membrane
current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve. J Physiol,
117(4), 500-544.
Lopez-Munoz, F., Boya, J., & Alamo, C. (2006). Neuron theory, the cornerstone
of neuroscience, on the centenary of the Nobel Prize award to Santiago
Ramon y Cajal. Brain Res Bull, 70(4-6), 391-405. doi: 10.1016/j.
brainresbull.2006.07.010
Pasternak, J. F., & Woolsey, T. A. (1975). On the "selectivity" of the Golgi-Cox
method. J Comp Neurol, 160(3), 307-312. doi: 10.1002/cne.901600304
Ramn y Cajal, S. (1911). Histology of the nervous system of man and
vertebrates. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Simons, M., & Trotter, J. (2007). Wrapping it up: the cell biology of myelination.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Neurons by Sharon Furtak is licensed under the
Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view
a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
The mammalian nervous system is a complex biological organ, which enables
many animals including humans to function in a coordinated fashion. The
original design of this system is preserved across many animals through
evolution; thus, adaptive physiological and behavioral functions are similar
across many animal species. Comparative study of physiological functioning in
the nervous systems of different animals lend insights to their behavior and
their mental processing and make it easier for us to understand the human
brain and behavior. In addition, studying the development of the nervous
system in a growing human provides a wealth of information about the change
in its form and behaviors that result from this change. The nervous system is
divided into central and peripheral nervous systems, and the two heavily
interact with one another. The peripheral nervous system controls volitional
(somatic nervous system) and nonvolitional (autonomic nervous system)
behaviors using cranial and spinal nerves. The central nervous system is divided
into forebrain, midbrain, and hindbrain, and each division performs a variety
of tasks; for example, the cerebral cortex in the forebrain houses sensory,
motor, and associative areas that gather sensory information, process
information for perception and memory, and produce responses based on
incoming and inherent information. To study the nervous system, a number of
methods have evolved over time; these methods include examining brain
lesions, microscopy, electrophysiology, electroencephalography, and many
scanning technologies.
Learning Objectives
Learn and understand the two important parts of the nervous system.
Explain the two systems in the peripheral nervous system and what you
know about the different regions and areas of the central nervous system.
Figure 1
The brains of some animals, like apes, monkeys, and rodents, are structurally
similar to humans (Figure 1), while others are not (e.g., invertebrates, singlecelled organisms). Does anatomical similarity of these brains suggest that
behaviors that emerge in these species are also similar? Indeed, many animals
display behaviors that are similar to humans, e.g., apes use nonverbal
communication signals with their hands and arms that resemble nonverbal
forms of communication in humans (Gardner & Gardner, 1969; Goodall, 1986;
Knapp & Hall, 2009). If we study very simple behaviors, like physiological
responses made by individual neurons, then brain-based behaviors of
invertebrates (Kandel & Schwartz, 1982) look very similar to humans, suggesting
that from time immemorial such basic behaviors have been conserved in the
nobaproject.com - The Nervous System
178
brains of many simple animal forms and in fact are the foundation of more
complex behaviors in animals that evolved later (Bullock, 1984).
Even at the micro-anatomical level, we note that individual neurons differ
in complexity across animal species. Human neurons exhibit more intricate
complexity than other animals; for example, neuronal processes (dendrites) in
humans have many more branch points, branches, and spines.
Complexity in the structure of the nervous system, both at the macro- and
micro-levels, give rise to complex behaviors. We can observe similar movements
of the limbs, as in nonverbal communication, in apes and humans, but the
variety and intricacy of nonverbal behaviors using hands in humans surpasses
apes. Deaf and dumb individuals who use American Sign Language (ASL)
express themselves in English nonverbally; they use this language with such
fine gradation that many accents of ASL exist (Walker, 1987). Complexity of
behavior with increasing complexity of the nervous system, especially the
cerebral cortex, can be observed in the genus Homo (Figure 2). If we compare
sophistication of material culture in Homo habilis (2 million years ago; brain
volume ~650 cm3) and Homo sapiens (300,000 years to now; brain volume
~1400 cm3), the evidence shows that Homo habilis used crude stone tools
compared with modern tools used by Homo sapiens to erect cities, develop
written languages, embark on space travel, and study her own self. All of this
is due to increasing complexity of the nervous system.
179
Figure 2
What has led to the complexity of the brain and nervous system through
evolution, to its behavioral and cognitive refinement? Darwin (1859, 1871)
proposed two forces of natural and sexual selection as work engines behind
this change. He prophesied, psychology will be based on a new foundation,
that of the necessary acquirement of each mental power and capacity by
gradation that is, psychology will be based on evolution (Rosenzweig,
Breedlove, & Leiman, 2002).
180
the neural tube, which extends in a rostrocaudal (head-to-tail) plane. The tube,
which is hollow, seams itself in the rostrocaudal direction. In some disease
conditions, the neural tube does not close caudally and results in an abnormality
called spina bifida. In this pathological condition, the lumbar and sacral
segments of the spinal cord are disrupted.
As gestation progresses, the neural tube balloons up (cephalization) at the
rostral end, and forebrain, midbrain, hindbrain, and the spinal cord can be
visually delineated (day 40). About 50 days into gestation, six cephalic areas can
be anatomically discerned (also see below for a more detailed description of
these areas).
The progenitor cells (neuroblasts) that form the lining (neuroepithelium)
of the neural tube generate all the neurons and glial cells of the central nervous
system. During early stages of this development, neuroblasts rapidly divide and
specialize into many varieties of neurons and glial cells, but this proliferation
of cells is not uniform along the neural tubethat is why we see the forebrain
and hindbrain expand into larger cephalic tissues than the midbrain. The
neuroepithelium also generates a group of specialized cells that migrate outside
the neural tube to form the neural crest. This structure gives rise to sensory
and autonomic neurons in the peripheral nervous system.
181
Figure 3
The peripheral nervous system is divided into somatic and autonomic nervous
systems (Figure 3). Where the somatic nervous system consists of cranial
nerves (12 pairs) and spinal nerves (31 pairs) and is under the volitional control
of the individual in maneuvering bodily muscles, the autonomic nervous system
also running through these nerves lets the individual have little control over
muscles and glands. Main divisions of the autonomic nervous system that
control visceral structures are the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous
systems.
At an appropriate cue (say a fear-inducing object like a snake), the
sympathetic division generally energizes many muscles (e.g., heart) and glands
(e.g., adrenals), causing activity and release of hormones that lead the individual
to negotiate the fear-causing snake with fight-or-flight responses. Whether the
individual decides to fight the snake or run away from it, either action requires
energy; in short, the sympathetic nervous system says go, go, go. The
parasympathetic nervous system, on the other hand, curtails undue energy
mobilization into muscles and glands and modulates the response by saying
nobaproject.com - The Nervous System
182
Figure 4
The central nervous system is divided into six important parts (Figure 4, white
labels), including the spinal cord, each specialized to perform a set of specific
functions. Telencephalon or cerebrum is a newer development in the evolution
of the mammalian nervous system. In humans, it is about the size of a large
napkin and when crumpled into the skull, it forms furrows called sulci (singular
form, sulcus). The bulges between sulci are called gyri (singular form, gyrus).
The cortex is divided into two hemispheres, and each hemisphere is further
divided into four lobes (Figure 5a), which have specific functions. The division
of these lobes is based on two delineating sulci: the central sulcus divides the
hemisphere into frontal and parietal-occipital lobes and the lateral sulcus
marks the temporal lobe, which lies below.
183
Figure 5a
Just in front of the central sulcus lies an area called the motor strip
(precentral gyrus), which connects to the muscles of the body, and on volitional
command moves them. From mastication to movements in the genitalia, the
body map is represented on this strip (Figure 5b).
Some body parts, like fingers, thumbs, and lips, occupy a greater
representation on the strip than, say, the trunk. This disproportionate
representation of the body on the motor strip is called the magnification factor
(Rolls & Cowey, 1970) and is seen in other motor and sensory areas. At the
lower end of the central sulcus, close to the lateral sulcus, lies the Brocas area
(Figure 6b) in the left frontal lobe, which is involved with language production.
Damage to this part of the brain led Pierre Paul Broca, a French neuroscientist
in 1861, to document many different forms of aphasias, in which his patients
would lose the ability to speak or would retain partial speech impoverished in
syntax and grammar (AAAS, 1880). It is no wonder that others have found
subvocal rehearsal and central executive processes of working memory in this
frontal lobe (Smith & Jonides, 1997, 1999).
nobaproject.com - The Nervous System
184
Figure 5b
Just behind the central gyrus, in the parietal lobe, lies the primary
somatosensory strip (Figure 6a) on the postcentral gyrus, which represents the
whole body receiving inputs from the skin and muscles. The somatosensory
strip parallels, abuts, and connects heavily to the motor strip and resembles it
in terms of areas devoted to bodily representation. All spinal and some cranial
nerves (e.g., the facial nerve) send sensory signals from skin (e.g., touch) and
muscles to the somatosensory strip. Close to the lower (ventral) end of this
strip, curved inside the parietal lobe, is the taste area (secondary somatosensory
cortex), which is involved with taste experiences that originate from the tongue,
pharynx, epiglottis, and so forth.
185
Figure 6a
Just below the parietal lobe, and under the caudal end of the lateral fissure,
in the temporal lobe, lies the Wernickes area (Demonet et al., 1992). This area
is involved with language comprehension and is connected to the Brocas area
through the arcuate fasciculus, nerve fibers that connect these two regions.
Damage to the Wernickes area (Figure 6b) results in many kinds of agnosias;
agnosia is defined as an inability to know or understand language and speechrelated behaviors. So an individual may show word deafness, which is an
inability to recognize spoken language, or word blindness, which is an inability
to recognize written or printed language. Close in proximity to the Wernickes
area is the primary auditory cortex, which is involved with audition, and finally
the brain region devoted to smell (olfaction) is tucked away inside the primary
olfactory cortex (prepyriform cortex).
186
Figure 6b
At the very back of the cerebral cortex lies the occipital lobe housing the
primary visual cortex. Optic nerves travel all the way to the thalamus (lateral
geniculate nucleus, LGN) and then to visual cortex, where images that are
received on the retina are projected (Hubel, 1995).
In the past 50 to 60 years, visual sense and visual pathways have been
studied extensively, and our understanding about them has increased
manifold. We now understand that all objects that form images on the retina
are transformed (transduction) in neural language handed down to the visual
cortex for further processing. In the visual cortex, all attributes (features) of the
image, such as the color, texture, and orientation, are decomposed and
processed by different visual cortical modules (Van Essen, Anderson & Felleman,
1992) and then recombined to give rise to singular perception of the image in
question.
If we cut the cerebral hemispheres in the middle, a new set of structures
come into view. Many of these perform different functions vital to our being.
For example, the limbic system contains a number of nuclei that process
memory (hippocampus and fornix) and attention and emotions (cingulate
gyrus); the globus pallidus is involved with motor movements and their
coordination; the hypothalamus and thalamus are involved with drives,
nobaproject.com - The Nervous System
187
Figure 7
As we descend down the thalamus, the midbrain comes into view with
superior and inferior colliculi, which process visual and auditory information,
as does the substantia nigra, which is involved with notorious Parkinsons
disease, and the reticular formation regulating arousal, sleep, and temperature.
A little lower, the hindbrain with the pons processes sensory and motor
information employing the cranial nerves, works as a bridge that connects the
cerebral cortex with the medulla, and reciprocally transfers information back
and forth between the brain and the spinal cord. The medulla oblongata
processes breathing, digestion, heart and blood vessel function, swallowing,
and sneezing. The cerebellum controls motor movement coordination,
balance, equilibrium, and muscle tone.
The midbrain and the hindbrain, which make up the brain stem, culminate
in the spinal cord. Whereas inside the cerebral cortex, the gray matter (neuronal
cell bodies) lies outside and white matter (myelinated axons) inside; in the
spinal cord this arrangement reverses, as the gray matter resides inside and
nobaproject.com - The Nervous System
188
the white matter outside. Paired nerves (ganglia) exit the spinal cord, some
closer in direction towards the back (dorsal) and others towards the front
(ventral). The dorsal nerves (afferent) receive sensory information from skin
and muscles, and ventral nerves (efferent) send signals to muscles and organs
to respond.
189
190
neuroscientists.
Understanding the nervous system has been a long journey of inquiry,
spanning several hundreds of years of meticulous studies carried out by some
of the most creative and versatile investigators in the fields of philosophy,
evolution, biology, physiology, anatomy, neurology, neuroscience, cognitive
sciences, and psychology. Despite our profound understanding of this organ,
its mysteries continue to surprise us, and its intricacies make us marvel at this
complex structure unmatched in the universe.
191
Outside Resources
Video: Pt. 1 video on the anatomy of the nervous system
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=D1zkVBHPh5c
Video: Pt. 2 video on the anatomy of the nervous system
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=8hC6NGQReL4
Video: To look at functions of the brain and neurons, watch
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=9UukcdU258A
Web: To look at different kinds of brains, visit
http://brainmuseum.org/
Discussion Questions
1. Why is it important to study the nervous system in an evolutionary context?
2. How can we compare changes in the nervous system made through
evolution to changes made during development?
3. What are the similarities and differences between the somatic and
autonomic nervous systems?
4. Describe functions of the midbrain and hindbrain.
5. Describe the anatomy and functions of the forebrain.
6. Compare and contrast electroencephalograms to electrophysiological
techniques.
7. Which brain scan methodologies are important for cognitive scientists?
Why?
Vocabulary
Afferent nerves
Nerves that carry messages to the brain or spinal cord.
Agnosias
Due to damage of Wernickes area. An inability to recognize objects, words, or
faces.
Aphasia
Due to damage of the Brocas area. An inability to produce or understand words.
Arcuate fasciculus
A fiber tract that connects Wernickes and Brocas speech areas.
Autonomic nervous system
A part of the peripheral nervous system that connects to glands and smooth
muscles. Consists of sympathetic and parasympathetic divisions.
Brocas area
An area in the frontal lobe of the left hemisphere. Implicated in language
production.
Central sulcus
The major fissure that divides the frontal and the parietal lobes.
Cerebellum
A nervous system structure behind and below the cerebrum. Controls motor
movement coordination, balance, equilibrium, and muscle tone.
Cerebrum
Consists of left and right hemispheres that sit at the top of the nervous system
and engages in a variety of higher-order functions.
Cingulate gyrus
A medial cortical portion of the nervous tissue that is a part of the limbic system.
Computerized axial tomography
A noninvasive brain-scanning procedure that uses X-ray absorption around the
head.
Ectoderm
The outermost layer of a developing fetus.
Efferent nerves
Nerves that carry messages from the brain to glands and organs in the
periphery.
Electroencephalography
A technique that is used to measure gross electrical activity of the brain by
placing electrodes on the scalp.
Event-related potentials
A physiological measure of large electrical change in the brain produced by
sensory stimulation or motor responses.
Forebrain
A part of the nervous system that contains the cerebral hemispheres, thalamus,
and hypothalamus.
Fornix
(plural form, fornices) A nerve fiber tract that connects the hippocampus to
mammillary bodies.
Frontal lobe
The most forward region (close to forehead) of the cerebral hemispheres.
Functional magnetic resonance imaging
(or fMRI) A noninvasive brain-imaging technique that registers changes in blood
flow in the brain during a given task (also see magnetic resonance imaging).
Globus pallidus
A nucleus of the basal ganglia.
Gray matter
Composes the bark or the cortex of the cerebrum and consists of the cell bodies
of the neurons (see also white matter).
Gyrus
(plural form, gyri) A bulge that is raised between or among fissures of the
convoluted brain.
Hippocampus
(plural form, hippocampi) A nucleus inside (medial) the temporal lobe
implicated in learning and memory.
Homo habilis
A human ancestor, handy man, that lived two million years ago.
Homo sapiens
Modern man, the only surviving form of the genus Homo.
Hypothalamus
Part of the diencephalon. Regulates biological drives with pituitary gland.
Immunocytochemistry
A method of staining tissue including the brain, using antibodies.
Lateral sulcus
The major fissure that delineates the temporal lobe below the frontal and the
parietal lobes.
Lesion studies
A surgical method in which a part of the animal brain is removed to study its
effects on behavior or function.
Limbic system
A loosely defined network of nuclei in the brain involved with learning and
emotion.
Magnetic resonance imaging
Or MRI is a brain imaging noninvasive technique that uses magnetic energy to
generate brain images (also see fMRI).
Magnification factor
Cortical space projected by an area of sensory input (e.g., mm of cortex per
degree of visual field).
Medulla oblongata
An area just above the spinal cord that processes breathing, digestion, heart
and blood vessel function, swallowing, and sneezing.
Motor strip
A strip of cortex just in front of the central sulcus that is involved with motor
control.
Neural crest
A set of primordial neurons that migrate outside the neural tube and give rise
to sensory and autonomic neurons in the peripheral nervous system.
Neural induction
A process that causes the formation of the neural tube.
Neuroblasts
Brain progenitor cells that asymmetrically divide into other neuroblasts or nerve
cells.
Neuroepithelium)
The lining of the neural tube.
Occipital lobe
The back part of the cerebrum, which houses the visual areas.
Parietal lobe
An area of the cerebrum just behind the central sulcus that is engaged with
somatosensory and gustatory sensation.
Pons
A bridge that connects the cerebral cortex with the medulla, and reciprocally
transfers information back and forth between the brain and the spinal cord.
Rostrocaudal
A front-back plane used to identify anatomical structures in the body and the
brain.
Somatic nervous system
A part of the peripheral nervous system that uses cranial and spinal nerves in
volitional actions.
Somatosensory strip
A strip of cerebral tissue just behind the central sulcus engaged in sensory
reception of bodily sensations.
Spina bifida
A developmental disease of the spinal cord, where the neural tube does not
close caudally.
Sulcus
(plural form, sulci) The crevices or fissures formed by convolutions in the brain.
Sympathetic nervous system
A division of the autonomic nervous system, that is faster than its counterpart
that is the parasympathetic nervous system and works in opposition to it.
Generally engaged in fight or flight functions.
Temporal lobe
An area of the cerebrum that lies below the lateral sulcus; it contains auditory
and olfactory (smell) projection regions.
Thalamus
A part of the diencephalon that works as a gateway for incoming and outgoing
information.
Transduction
A process in which physical energy converts into neural energy.
Wernickes area
A language area in the temporal lobe where linguistic information is
comprehended (Also see Brocas area).
White matter
Regions of the nervous system that represent the axons of the nerve cells;
whitish in color because of myelination of the nerve cells.
Working memory
Short transitory memory processed in the hippocampus.
Reference List
American Association for the Advancement of Science (AAAS). (1880). Dr. Paul
Broca. Science, 1(8), 93. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2900242
Bullock, T. H. (1984). Comparative neuroscience holds promise for quiet
revolutions. Science, 225(4661), 473478.
Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1990). Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness.
Seminars in the Neurosciences, 2, 263275.
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and the selection in relation to sex.
London: J. Murray.
Darwin, C. (1859). On the origins of species by means of natural selection, or,
The preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. London, UK: J.
Murray.
Demonet, J. F., Chollet, F., Ramsay, S., Cardebat, D., Nespoulous, J. L., Wise, R., . . .
Frackowiak, R. (1992). The anatomy of phonological and semantic
processing in normal subjects. Brain, 115(6), 17531768.
Edelman, G. (2004). Wider than the sky: The phenomenal gift of consciousness.
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Gardner, R. A., & Gardner, B. T. (1969). Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee.
Science, 165(3894), 664672.
Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Hubel, D. H. (1995). Eye, brain, and vision. Freeman & Co., NY: Scientific American
Library/Scientific American Books.
Kandel, E. R., & Schwartz, J. H. (1982). Molecular biology of learning: Modulation
of transmitter release. Science, 218(4571), 433443.
Knapp, M. L., & Hall, J. A. (2009). Nonverbal communication in human interaction.
Boston, MA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.
Ramn y Cahal, S. (1937). Recollections of my life. Memoirs of the American
Philosophical Society, 8, 19011917.
Rolls, E. T., & Cowey, A. (1970). Topography of the retina and striate cortex and
its relationship to visual acuity in rhesus monkeys and squirrel monkeys.
Experimental Brain Research, 10(3), 298310.
Rosenzweig, M. R., Breedlove, S. M., & Leiman, A. L. (2002). Biological psychology
(3rd ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal
lobes. Science, 283(5408), 16571661.
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1997). Working memory: A view from neuroimaging.
Cognitive psychology, 33, 542.
Van Essen, D. C., Anderson, C. H., & Felleman, D. J. (1992). Information processing
in the primate visual system: An integrated systems perspective. Science,
255(5043), 419423.
Walker, L. A. (1987). A loss for words: The story of deafness in a family. New
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. The Nervous System by Aneeq Ahmad is licensed
under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International
License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
deed.en_US.
The Brain
Diane Beck & Evelina Tapia
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, University of Illinois
nobaproject.com
Abstract
The human brain is responsible for all behaviors, thoughts, and experiences
described in this textbook. This chapter provides an introductory overview of
the brain, including some basic neuroanatomy, and brief descriptions of the
neuroscience methods used to study it.
Learning Objectives
Name and describe the basic function of the brain stem, cerebellum, and
cerebral hemispheres.
Name and describe the basic function of the four cerebral lobes: occipital,
temporal, parietal, and frontal cortex.
Name and describe the most common approaches to studying the human
brain.
Distinguish among four neuroimaging methods: PET, fMRI, EEG, and DOI.
Introduction
Any textbook on psychology would be incomplete without reference to the
brain. Every behavior, thought, or experience described in the other chapters
must be implemented in the brain. A detailed understanding of the human
brain can help us make sense of human experience and behavior. For example,
one well-established fact about human cognition is that it is limited. We cannot
do two complex tasks at once: We cannot read and carry on a conversation at
the same time, text and drive, or surf the Internet while listening to a lecture,
at least not successfully or safely. We cannot even pat our head and rub our
stomach at the same time (with exceptions, see A Brain Divided). Why is this?
Many people have suggested that such limitations reflect the fact that the
behaviors draw on the same resource; if one behavior uses up most of the
resource there is not enough resource left for the other. But what might this
limited resource be in the brain?
The brain uses oxygen and glucose, delivered via the blood. The brain is a
large consumer of these metabolites, using 20% of the oxygen and calories we
consume despite being only 2% of our total weight. However, as long as we are
not oxygen-deprived or malnourished, we have more than enough oxygen and
glucose to fuel the brain. Thus, insufficient brain fuel cannot explain our limited
capacity. Nor is it likely that our limitations reflect too few neurons. The average
human brain contains 100 billion neurons. It is also not the case that we use
only 10% of our brain, a myth that was likely started to imply we had untapped
potential. Modern neuroimaging (see Studying the Human Brain) has shown
that we use all parts of brain, just at different times, and certainly more than
10% at any one time.
nobaproject.com - The Brain
209
Figure 1. An MRI of the human brain delineating three major structures: the cerebral
hemispheres, brain stem, and cerebellum.
If we have an abundance of brain fuel and neurons, how can we explain our
limited cognitive abilities? Why cant we do more at once? The most likely
explanation is the way these neurons are wired up. We know, for instance, that
many neurons in the visual cortex (the part of the brain responsible for
processing visual information) are hooked up in such a way as to inhibit each
other (Beck & Kastner, 2009). When one neuron fires, it suppresses the firing
of other nearby neurons. If two neurons that are hooked up in an inhibitory
way both fire, then neither neuron can fire as vigorously as it would otherwise.
This competitive behavior among neurons limits how much visual information
the brain can respond to at the same time. Similar kinds of competitive wiring
nobaproject.com - The Brain
210
among neurons may underlie many of our limitations. Thus, although talking
about limited resources provides an intuitive description of our limited capacity
behavior, a detailed understanding of the brain suggests that our limitations
more likely reflect the complex way in which neurons talk to each other rather
than the depletion of any specific resource.
Brain Stem
The brain stem is sometimes referred to as the trunk of the brain. It is
responsible for many of the neural functions that keep us alive, including
regulating our respiration (breathing), heart rate, and digestion. In keeping with
its function, if a patient sustains severe damage to the brain stem he or she will
require life support (i.e., machines are used to keep him or her alive). Because
of its vital role in survival, in many countries a person who has lost brain stem
function is said to be brain dead, although other countries require significant
tissue loss in the cortex (of the cerebral hemispheres), which is responsible for
our conscious experience, for the same diagnosis. The brain stem includes the
medulla, pons, midbrain, and diencephalon (which consists of thalamus and
hypothalamus). Collectively, these regions also are involved in our sleepwake
cycle, some sensory and motor function, as well as growth and other hormonal
behaviors.
nobaproject.com - The Brain
211
Figure 2. A sample of neuroanatomy nomenclature. The colored boxes indicate the different
groupings of the seven structures printed in black, with the labels matching the color of the
boxes. The hindbrain, midbrain, and forebrain nomenclature stems from the development of
the vertebrate brain; these three areas differentiate early in embryonic development and later
give rise to the structures listed in black. These three areas further subdivide into the
telencephalon, diencephalon, mesencephalon, metencephalon, and myelencephalon at a
later stage of development.
Cerebellum
The cerebellum is the distinctive structure at the back of the brain. The Greek
philosopher and scientist Aristotle aptly referred to it as the small brain
(parencephalon in Greek, cerebellum in Latin) in order to distinguish it from
the large brain (encephalon in Greek, cerebrum in Latin). The cerebellum
is critical for coordinated movement and posture. More recently, neuroimaging
studies (see Studying the Human Brain) have implicated it in a range of
cognitive abilities, including language. It is perhaps not surprising that the
nobaproject.com - The Brain
212
Cerebral Hemispheres
The cerebral hemispheres are responsible for our cognitive abilities and
conscious experience. They consist of the cerebral cortex and accompanying
white matter (cerebrum in Latin) as well as the subcortical structures of the
basal ganglia, amygdala, and hippocampal formation. The cerebral cortex is the
largest and most visible part of the brain, retaining the Latin name (cerebrum)
for large brain that Aristotle coined. It consists of two hemispheres (literally
two half spheres) and gives the brain its characteristic gray and convoluted
appearance; the folds and grooves of the cortex are called gyri and sulci (gyrus
and sulcus if referring to just one), respectively.
The two cerebral hemispheres can be further subdivided into four lobes:
the occipital, temporal, parietal, and frontal lobes. The occipital lobe is
responsible for vision, as is much of the temporal lobe. The temporal lobe is
also involved in auditory processing, memory, and multisensory integration (e.
g., the convergence of vision and audition). The parietal lobe houses the
somatosensory (body sensations) cortex and structures involved in visual
attention, as well as multisensory convergence zones. The frontal lobe houses
the motor cortex and structures involved in motor planning, language,
judgment, and decision-making. Not surprisingly then, the frontal lobe is
proportionally larger in humans than in any other animal.
The subcortical structures are so named because they reside beneath the
cortex. The basal ganglia are critical to voluntary movement and as such make
contact with the cortex, the thalamus, and the brain stem. The amygdala and
hippocampal formation are part of the limbic system, which also includes some
nobaproject.com - The Brain
213
cortical structures. The limbic system plays an important role in emotion and,
in particular, in aversion and gratification.
A Brain Divided
The two cerebral hemispheres are connected by a dense bundle of white matter
tracts called the corpus callosum. Some functions are replicated in the two
hemispheres. For example, both hemispheres are responsible for sensory and
motor function, although the sensory and motor cortices have a contralateral
(or opposite-side) representation; that is, the left cerebral hemisphere is
responsible for movements and sensations on the right side of the body and
the right cerebral hemisphere is responsible for movements and sensations on
the left side of the body. Other functions are lateralized; that is, they reside
primarily in one hemisphere or the other. For example, for right-handed and
the majority of left-handed individuals, the left hemisphere is most responsible
for language.
There are some people whose two hemispheres are not connected, either
because the corpus callosum was surgically severed (callosotomy) or due to a
genetic abnormality. These split-brain patients have helped us understand the
functioning of the two hemispheres. First, because of the contralateral
representation of sensory information, if an object is placed in only the left or
only the right visual hemifield, then only the right or left hemisphere,
respectively, of the split-brain patient will see it. In essence, it is as though the
person has two brains in his or her head, each seeing half the world.
Interestingly, because language is very often localized in the left hemisphere,
if we show the right hemisphere a picture and ask the patient what she saw,
she will say she didnt see anything (because only the left hemisphere can speak
and it didnt see anything). However, we know that the right hemisphere sees
the picture because if the patient is asked to press a button whenever she sees
the image, the left hand (which is controlled by the right hemisphere) will
nobaproject.com - The Brain
214
respond despite the left hemispheres denial that anything was there. There
are also some advantages to having disconnected hemispheres. Unlike those
with a fully functional corpus callosum, a split-brain patient can simultaneously
search for something in his right and left visual fields (Luck, Hillyard, Mangun,
& Gazzaniga, 1989) and can do the equivalent of rubbing his stomach and
patting his head at the same time (Franz, Eliason, Ivry, & Gazzaniga, 1996). In
other words, they exhibit less competition between the hemispheres.
215
Figure 3. MRI slices of the human brain. Both the outer gray matter and inner white matter
are visible in each image. The brain is a three-dimensional (3-D) structure, but an image is
two-dimensional (2-D). Here, we show example slices of the three possible 2-D cuts through
the brain: a saggital slice (green), a horizontal slice (blue), which is also know as a transverse
or axial slice, and a coronal slice (red). The bottom two images are color coded to match the
illustration of the relative orientations of the three slices in the top image.
216
Neuroanatomy
Dissection of the brain, in either animals or cadavers, has been a critical tool of
neuroscientists since 340 BC when Aristotle first published his dissections. Since
then this method has advanced considerably with the discovery of various
staining techniques that can highlight particular cells. Because the brain can be
sliced very thinly, examined under the microscope, and particular cells
highlighted, this method is especially useful for studying specific groups of
neurons or small brain structures; that is, it has a very high spatial resolution.
Dissections allow scientists to study changes in the brain that occur due to
various diseases or experiences (e.g., exposure to drugs or brain injuries).
Virtual dissection studies with living humans are also conducted. Here, the
brain is imaged using computerized axial tomography (CAT) or MRI scanners;
they reveal with very high precision the various structures in the brain and can
help detect changes in gray or white matter. These changes in the brain can
then be correlated with behavior, such as performance on memory tests, and,
nobaproject.com - The Brain
217
218
Neuroimaging
Neuroimaging tools are used to study the brain in action; that is, when it is
engaged in a specific task. Positron emission topography (PET) records blood
flow in the brain. The PET scanner detects the radioactive substance that is
injected into the bloodstream of the participant just before or while he or she
is performing some task (e.g., adding numbers). Because active neuron
populations require metabolites, more blood and hence more radioactive
substance flows into those regions. PET scanners detect the injected radioactive
substance in specific brain regions, allowing researchers to infer that those
areas were active during the task. Functional magnetic resonance imaging
(fMRI) also relies on blood flow in the brain. This method, however, measures
the changes in oxygen levels in the blood and does not require any substance
to be injected into the participant. Both of these tools have good spatial
resolution (although not as precise as dissection studies), but because it takes
at least several seconds for the blood to arrive to the active areas of the brain,
PET and fMRI have poor temporal resolution; that is, they do not tell us very
precisely when the activity occurred.
Electroencephalography (EEG), on the other hand, measures the electrical
activity of the brain, and therefore, it has a much greater temporal resolution
(millisecond precision rather than seconds) than PET or fMRI. Like tDCS,
electrodes are placed on the participants head when he or she is performing
a task. In this case, however, many more electrodes are used, and they measure
rather than produce activity. Because the electrical activity picked up at any
particular electrode can be coming from anywhere in the brain, EEG has poor
spatial resolution; that is, we have only a rough idea of which part of the brain
generates the measured activity.
nobaproject.com - The Brain
219
Diffuse optical imaging (DOI) can give researchers the best of both worlds:
high spatial and temporal resolution, depending on how it is used. Here, one
shines infrared light into the brain, and measures the light that comes back out.
DOI relies on the fact that the properties of the light change when it passes
through oxygenated blood, or when it encounters active neurons. Researchers
can then infer from the properties of the collected light what regions in the
brain were engaged by the task. When DOI is set up to detect changes in blood
oxygen levels, the temporal resolution is low and comparable to PET or fMRI.
However, when DOI is set up to directly detect active neurons, it has both high
spatial and temporal resolution.
Because the spatial and temporal resolution of each tool varies, strongest
evidence for what role a certain brain area serves comes from converging
evidence. For example, we are more likely to believe that the hippocampal
formation is involved in memory if multiple studies using a variety of tasks and
different neuroimaging tools provide evidence for this hypothesis. The brain is
a complex system, and only advances in brain research will show whether the
brain can ever really understand itself.
220
Outside Resources
Video: Brain Bank at Harvard (National Geographic video)
http://video.nationalgeographic.com/video/science/health-human-body-sci/humanbody/brain-bank-sci/
Video: Frontal Lobes and Behavior (video #25)
http://www.learner.org/resources/series142.html
Video: Organization and Evaluation of Human Brain Function video (video #1)
http://www.learner.org/resources/series142.html
Video: Videos of a split-brain patient
http://youtu.be/ZMLzP1VCANo
Video: Videos of a split-brain patient (video #5)
http://www.learner.org/resources/series142.html
Web: Atlas of the Human Brain: interactive demos and brain sections
http://www.thehumanbrain.info/
Web: Harvard University Human Brain Atlas: normal and diseased brain
scans
http://www.med.harvard.edu/aanlib/home.html
Discussion Questions
1. In what ways does the segmentation of the brain into the brain stem,
cerebellum, and cerebral hemispheres provide a natural division?
2. How has the study of split-brain patients been informative?
3. What is behind the expression use your gray matter, and why is it not
entirely accurate?
4. Why is converging evidence the best kind of evidence in the study of brain
function?
5. If you were interested in whether a particular brain area was involved in a
specific behavior, what neuroscience methods could you use?
6. If you were interested in the precise time in which a particular brain process
occurred, which neuroscience methods could you use?
Vocabulary
Ablation
Surgical removal of brain tissue.
Axial plane
See horizontal plane.
Basal ganglia
Subcortical structures of the cerebral hemispheres involved in voluntary
movement.
Brain stem
The trunk of the brain comprised of the medulla, pons, midbrain, and
diencephalon.
Callosotomy
Surgical procedure in which the corpus callosum is severed (used to control
severe epilepsy).
Case study
A thorough study of a patient (or a few patients) with naturally occurring lesions.
Cerebellum
The distinctive structure at the back of the brain, Latin for small brain.
Cerebral cortex
The outermost gray matter of the cerebrum; the distinctive convolutions
characteristic of the mammalian brain.
Cerebral hemispheres
The cerebral cortex, underlying white matter, and subcortical structures.
Cerebrum
Usually refers to the cerebral cortex and associated white matter, but in some
texts includes the subcortical structures.
Contralateral
Literally opposite side; used to refer to the fact that the two hemispheres of
the brain process sensory information and motor commands for the opposite
side of the body (e.g., the left hemisphere controls the right side of the body).
Converging evidence
Similar findings reported from multiple studies using different methods.
Coronal plane
A slice that runs from head to foot; brain slices in this plane are similar to slices
of a loaf of bread, with the eyes being the front of the loaf.
Electroencephalography (EEG)
A neuroimaging technique that measures electrical brain activity via multiple
electrodes on the scalp.
Frontal lobe
The front most (anterior) part of the cerebrum; anterior to the central sulcus
and responsible for motor output and planning, language, judgment, and
decision-making.
Gray matter
The outer grayish regions of the brain comprised of the neurons cell bodies.
Gyri
(plural) Folds between sulci in the cortex.
Gyrus
A fold between sulci in the cortex.
Horizontal plane
A slice that runs horizontally through a standing person (i.e., parallel to the
floor); slices of brain in this plane divide the top and bottom parts of the brain;
this plane is similar to slicing a hamburger bun.
Lateralized
To the side; used to refer to the fact that specific functions may reside primarily
in one hemisphere or the other (e.g., for the majority individuals, the left
hemisphere is most responsible for language).
Lesion
A region in the brain that suffered damage through injury, disease, or medical
intervention.
Limbic system
Includes the subcortical structures of the amygdala and hippocampal formation
as well as some cortical structures; responsible for aversion and gratification.
Metabolite
A substance necessary for a living organism to maintain life.
Motor cortex
Region of the frontal lobe responsible for voluntary movement; the motor
cortex has a contralateral representation of the human body.
Myelin
Fatty tissue, produced by glial cells (see chapter, Neurons) that insulates the
axons of the neurons; myelin is necessary for normal conduction of electrical
impulses among neurons.
Nomenclature
Naming conventions.
Occipital lobe
The back most (posterior) part of the cerebrum; involved in vision.
Parietal lobe
The part of the cerebrum between the frontal and occipital lobes; involved in
bodily sensations, visual attention, and integrating the senses.
Phrenology
A now-discredited field of brain study, popular in the first half of the 19th century
that correlated bumps and indentations of the skull with specific functions of
the brain.
Sagittal plane
A slice that runs vertically from front to back; slices of brain in this plane divide
the left and right side of the brain; this plane is similar to slicing a baked potato
lengthwise.
Spatial resolution
A term that refers to how small the elements of an image are; high spatial
resolution means the device or technique can resolve very small elements; in
neuroscience it describes how small of a structure in the brain can be imaged.
Split-brain patient
A patient who has had most or all of his or her corpus callosum severed.
Subcortical
Structures that lie beneath the cerebral cortex, but above the brain stem.
Sulci
(plural) Grooves separating folds of the cortex.
Sulcus
A groove separating folds of the cortex.
Temporal lobe
The part of the cerebrum in front of (anterior to) the occipital lobe and below
the lateral fissure; involved in vision, auditory processing, memory, and
integrating vision and audition.
Temporal resolution
A term that refers to how small a unit of time can be measured; high temporal
resolution means capable of resolving very small units of time; in neuroscience
it describes how precisely in time a process can be measured in the brain.
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS)
A neuroscience technique that passes mild electrical current directly through
a brain area by placing small electrodes on the skull.
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)
A neuroscience technique whereby a brief magnetic pulse is applied to the head
that temporarily induces a weak electrical current that interferes with ongoing
activity.
Transverse plane
See horizontal plane.
Visual hemifield
The half of visual space (what we see) on one side of fixation (where we are
looking); the left hemisphere is responsible for the right visual hemifield, and
the right hemisphere is responsible for the left visual hemifield.
White matter
The inner whitish regions of the cerebrum comprised of the myelinated axons
of neurons in the cerebral cortex.
Reference List
Beck, D. M., & Kastner, S. (2009). Top-down and bottom-up mechanisms in
biasing competition in the human brain. Vision Research, 49, 11541165.
Brasil-Neto, J. P. (2012). Learning, memory, and transcranial direct current
stimulation. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 3(80). doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00080.
Feng, W. W., Bowden, M. G., & Kautz, S. (2013). Review of transcranial direct
current stimulation in poststroke recovery. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation,
20, 6877.
Franz, E. A., Eliassen, J. C., Ivry, R. B., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1996). Dissociation of
spatial and temporal coupling in the bimanual movements of callosotomy
patients. Psychological Science, 7, 306310.
Kandal, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., & Jessell, T. M. (Eds.) (2000). Principles of neural
science (Vol. 4). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Kuo, M. F., & Nitsche, M. A. (2012). Effects of transcranial electrical stimulation
on cognition. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 43, 192199.
Luck, S. J., Hillyard, S. A., Mangun, G. R., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1989). Independent
hemispheric attentional systems mediate visual search in split-brain
patients. Nature, 342, 543545.
Swanson, L. (2000). What is the brain? Trends in Neurosciences, 23, 519527.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. The Brain by Diane Beck and Evelina Tapia is
licensed
under
the
Creative
Commons
Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0
Abstract
The goal of this chapter is to introduce you to the topic of hormones and
behavior. This field of study is also called behavioral endocrinology, which is
the scientific study of the interaction between hormones and behavior. This
interaction is bidirectional: hormones can influence behavior, and behavior can
sometimes influence hormone concentrations. Hormones are chemical
messengers released from endocrine glands that travel through the blood
system to influence the nervous system to regulate behaviors such as
aggression, mating, and parenting of individuals.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
This chapter describes the relationship between hormones and behavior. Many
readers are likely already familiar with the general idea that hormones can
affect behavior. Students are generally familiar with the idea that sex-hormone
concentrations increase in the blood during puberty and decrease as we age,
especially after about 50 years of age. Sexual behavior shows a similar pattern.
Most people also know about the relationship between aggression and anabolic
steroid hormones, and they know that administration of artificial steroid
hormones sometimes results in uncontrollable, violent behavior called roid
rage. Many different hormones can influence several types of behavior, but
for the purpose of this chapter, we will restrict our discussion to just a few
examples of hormones and behaviors. For example, are behavioral sex
differences the result of hormones, the environment, or some combination of
factors? Why are men much more likely than women to commit aggressive acts?
Are hormones involved in mediating the so-called maternal instinct?
Behavioral endocrinologists are interested in how the general physiological
effects of hormones alter the development and expression of behavior and
how behavior may influence the effects of hormones. This chapter describes,
both phenomenologically and functionally, how hormones affect behavior.
235
236
237
pathways or, alternatively, turns on or turns off gene activation that regulates
protein synthesis. The newly synthesized proteins may activate or deactivate
other genes, causing yet another cascade of cellular events. Importantly,
sufficient numbers of appropriate hormone receptors must be available for a
specific hormone to produce any effects. For example, testosterone is
important for male sexual behavior. If men have too little testosterone, then
sexual motivation may be low, and it can be restored by testosterone treatment.
However, if men have normal or even elevated levels of testosterone yet display
low sexual drive, then it might be possible for a lack of receptors to be the cause
and treatment with additional hormones will not be effective.
How might hormones affect behavior? In terms of their behavior, one can
think of humans and other animals conceptually as comprised of three
interacting components: (1) input systems (sensory systems), (2) integrators
(the central nervous system), and (3) output systems, or effectors (e.g., muscles).
Hormones do not cause behavioral changes. Rather, hormones influence these
three systems so that specific stimuli are more likely to elicit certain responses
in the appropriate behavioral or social context. In other words, hormones
change the probability that a particular behavior will be emitted in the
appropriate situation (Nelson, 2011). This is a critical distinction that can affect
how we think of hormone-behavior relationships.
We can apply this three-component behavioral scheme to a simple behavior,
singing in zebra finches. Only male zebra finches sing. If the testes of adult male
finches are removed, then the birds reduce singing, but castrated finches
resume singing if the testes are reimplanted, or if the birds are treated with
either testosterone or estradiol. Although we commonly consider androgens
to be male hormones and estrogens to be female hormones, it is common
for testosterone to be converted to estradiol in nerve cells (Figure 1). Thus, many
male-like behaviors are associated with the actions of estrogens! Indeed, all
estrogens must first be converted from androgens because of the typical
biochemical synthesis process. If the converting enzyme is low or missing, then
nobaproject.com - Hormones & Behavior
238
239
Figure 1. Biochemical Pathway for Steroid Hormone Synthesis: It is important to note that
testosterone (an androgen) can be converted to another androgen, DHT, or an estrogen,
estradiol. Too much or too little of the converting enzymes can influence brain and behavior.
240
competitors might be more easily seen or heard. Estrogens also could influence
the central nervous system. Neuronal architecture or the speed of neural
processing could change in the presence of estrogens. Higher neural processes
(e.g., motivation, attention, or perception) also might be influenced. Finally, the
effector organs, muscles in this case, could be affected by the presence of
estrogens. Blood estrogen concentrations might somehow affect the muscles
of a songbirds syrinx (the vocal organ of birds). Estrogens, therefore, could
affect birdsong by influencing the sensory capabilities, central processing
system, or effector organs of an individual bird. We do not understand
completely how estrogen, derived from testosterone, influences birdsong, but
in most cases, hormones can be considered to affect behavior by influencing
one, two, or all three of these components, and this three-part framework can
aid in the design of hypotheses and experiments to explore these issues.
How
might
behaviors
affect
hormones?
The
birdsong
example
demonstrates how hormones can affect behavior, but as noted, the reciprocal
relation also occurs; that is, behavior can affect hormone concentrations. For
example, the sight of a territorial intruder may elevate blood testosterone
concentrations in resident male birds and thereby stimulate singing or fighting
behavior. Similarly, male mice or rhesus monkeys that lose a fight decrease
circulating testosterone concentrations for several days or even weeks
afterward. Comparable results have also been reported in humans.
Testosterone concentrations are affected not only in humans involved in
physical combat, but also in those involved in simulated battles. For example,
testosterone concentrations were elevated in winners and reduced in losers of
regional chess tournaments.
People do not have to be directly involved in a contest to have their
hormones affected by the outcome of the contest. Male fans of both the
Brazilian and Italian teams were recruited to provide saliva samples to be
assayed for testosterone before and after the final game of the World Cup
soccer match in 1994. Brazil and Italy were tied going into the final game, but
nobaproject.com - Hormones & Behavior
241
Brazil won on a penalty kick at the last possible moment. The Brazilian fans
were elated and the Italian fans were crestfallen. When the samples were
assayed, 11 of 12 Brazilian fans who were sampled had increased testosterone
concentrations, and 9 of 9 Italian fans had decreased testosterone
concentrations, compared with pre-game baseline values (Dabbs, 2000).
In some cases, hormones can be affected by anticipation of behavior. For
example, testosterone concentrations also influence sexual motivation and
behavior in women. In one study, the interaction between sexual intercourse
and testosterone was compared with other activities (cuddling or exercise) in
women (van Anders, Hamilton, Schmidt, & Watson, 2007). On three separate
occasions, women provided a pre-activity, post-activity, and next-morning saliva
sample. After analysis, the womens testosterone was determined to be
elevated prior to intercourse as compared to other times. Thus, an anticipatory
relationship exists between sexual behavior and testosterone. Testosterone
values were higher post-intercourse compared to exercise, suggesting that
engaging in sexual behavior may also influence hormone concentrations in
women.
Sex Differences
Hens and roosters are different. Cows and bulls are different. Men and women
are different. Even girls and boys are different. Humans, like many animals, are
sexually dimorphic (di, two; morph, type) in the size and shape of their bodies,
their physiology, and for our purposes, their behavior. The behavior of boys
and girls differs in many ways. Girls generally excel in verbal abilities relative to
boys; boys are nearly twice as likely as girls to suffer from dyslexia (reading
difficulties) and stuttering and nearly 4 times more likely to suffer from autism.
Boys are generally better than girls at tasks that require visuospatial abilities.
Girls engage in nurturing behaviors more frequently than boys. More than 90%
of all anorexia nervosa cases involve young women. Young men are twice as
nobaproject.com - Hormones & Behavior
242
likely as young women to suffer from schizophrenia. Boys are much more
aggressive and generally engage in more rough-and-tumble play than girls
(Berenbaum, Martin, Hanish, Briggs, & Fabes, 2008). Many sex differences, such
as the difference in aggressiveness, persist throughout adulthood. For example,
there are many more men than women serving prison sentences for violent
behavior. The hormonal differences between men and women may account
for adult sex differences that develop during puberty, but what accounts for
behavioral sex differences among children prior to puberty and activation of
their gonads? Hormonal secretions from the developing gonads determine
whether the individual develops in a male or female manner. The mammalian
embryonic testes produce androgens, as well as peptide hormones, that steer
the development of the body, central nervous system, and subsequent behavior
in a male direction. The embryonic ovaries of mammals are virtually quiescent
and do not secrete high concentrations of hormones. In the presence of ovaries,
or in the complete absence of any gonads, morphological, neural, and, later,
behavioral development follows a female pathway.
Gonadal steroid hormones have organizational (or programming) effects
upon brain and behavior (Phoenix, Goy, Gerall, & Young, 1959). The organizing
effects of steroid hormones are relatively constrained to the early stages of
development. An asymmetry exists in the effects of testes and ovaries on the
organization of behavior in mammals. Hormone exposure early in life has
organizational effects on subsequent rodent behavior; early steroid hormone
treatment causes relatively irreversible and permanent masculinization of
rodent behavior (mating and aggressive). These early hormone effects can be
contrasted with the reversible behavioral influences of steroid hormones
provided in adulthood, which are called activational effects. The activational
effects of hormones on adult behavior are temporary and may wane soon after
the hormone is metabolized. Thus, typical male behavior requires exposure to
androgens during gestation (in humans) or immediately after birth (in rodents)
to somewhat masculinize the brain and also requires androgens during or after
nobaproject.com - Hormones & Behavior
243
puberty to activate these neural circuits. Typical female behavior requires a lack
of exposure to androgens early in life which leads to feminization of the brain
and also requires estrogens to activate these neural circuits in adulthood. But
this simple dichotomy, which works well with animals with very distinct sexual
dimorphism in behavior, has many caveats when applied to people.
If you walk through any major toy store, then you will likely observe a couple
of aisles filled with pink boxes and the complete absence of pink packaging of
toys in adjacent aisles. Remarkably, you will also see a strong self-segregation
of boys and girls in these aisles. It is rare to see boys in the pink aisles and vice
versa. The toy manufacturers are often accused of making toys that are gender
biased, but it seems more likely that boys and girls enjoy playing with specific
types and colors of toys. Indeed, toy manufacturers would immediately double
their sales if they could sell toys to both sexes. Boys generally prefer toys such
as trucks and balls and girls generally prefer toys such as dolls. Although it is
doubtful that there are genes that encode preferences for toy cars and trucks
on the Y chromosome, it is possible that hormones might shape the
development of a childs brain to prefer certain types of toys or styles of play
behavior. It is reasonable to believe that children learn which types of toys and
which styles of play are appropriate to their gender. How can we understand
and separate the contribution of physiological mechanisms from learning to
understand sex differences in human behaviors? To untangle these issues,
animal models are often used. Unlike the situation in humans, where sex
differences are usually only a matter of degree (often slight), in some animals,
members of only one sex may display a particular behavior. As noted, often
only male songbirds sing. Studies of such strongly sex-biased behaviors are
particularly valuable for understanding the interaction among behavior,
hormones, and the nervous system.
nobaproject.com - Hormones & Behavior
244
A study of vervet monkeys calls into question the primacy of learning in the
establishment of toy preferences (Alexander & Hines, 2002). Female vervet
monkeys preferred girl-typical toys, such as dolls or cooking pots, whereas male
vervet monkeys preferred boy-typical toys, such as cars or balls. There were no
sex differences in preference for gender-neutral toys, such as picture books or
stuffed animals. Presumably, monkeys have no prior concept of boy or girl
toys. Young rhesus monkeys also show similar toy preferences.
What then underlies the sex difference in toy preference? It is possible that
certain attributes of toys (or objects) appeal to either boys or girls. Toys that
appeal to boys or male vervet or rhesus monkeys, in this case, a ball or toy car,
are objects that can be moved actively through space, toys that can be
incorporated into active, rough and tumble play. The appeal of toys that girls
or female vervet monkeys prefer appears to be based on color. Pink and red
(the colors of the doll and pot) may provoke attention to infants.
Society may reinforce such stereotypical responses to gender-typical toys.
The sex differences in toy preferences emerge by 12 or 24 months of age and
seem fixed by 36 months of age, but are sex differences in toy preference
present during the first year of life? It is difficult to ask pre-verbal infants what
they prefer, but in studies where the investigators examined the amount of
time that babies looked at different toys, eye-tracking data indicate that infants
as young as 3 months showed sex differences in toy preferences; girls preferred
dolls, whereas boys preferred trucks. Another result that suggests, but does
not prove, that hormones are involved in toy preferences is the observation
that girls diagnosed with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH), whose adrenal
glands produce varying amounts of androgens early in life, played with
masculine toys more often than girls without CAH. Further, a dose-response
relationship between the extent of the disorder (i.e., degree of fetal androgen
exposure) and degree of masculinization of play behavior was observed. Are
the sex differences in toy preferences or play activity, for example, the inevitable
consequences of the differential endocrine environments of boys and girls, or
nobaproject.com - Hormones & Behavior
245
are these differences imposed by cultural practices and beliefs? Are these
differences the result of receiving gender-specific toys from an early age, or are
these differences some combination of endocrine and cultural factors? Again,
these are difficult questions to unravel in people.
Even when behavioral sex differences appear early in development, there
seems to be some question regarding the influences of societal expectations.
One example is the pattern of human play behavior during which males are
more physical; this pattern is seen in a number of other species including
nonhuman primates, rats, and dogs. Is the difference in the frequency of roughand-tumble play between boys and girls due to biological factors associated
with being male or female, or is it due to cultural expectations and learning? If
there is a combination of biological and cultural influences mediating the
frequency of rough-and-tumble play, then what proportion of the variation
between the sexes is due to biological factors and what proportion is due to
social influences? Importantly, is it appropriate to talk about normal sex
differences when these traits virtually always arrange themselves along a
continuum rather than in discrete categories?
Sex differences are common in humans and in nonhuman animals. Because
males and females differ in the ratio of androgenic and estrogenic steroid
hormone concentrations, behavioral endocrinologists have been particularly
interested in the extent to which behavioral sex differences are mediated by
hormones. The process of becoming female or male is called sexual
differentiation. The primary step in sexual differentiation occurs at fertilization.
In mammals, the ovum (which always contains an X chromosome) can be
fertilized by a sperm bearing either a Y or an X chromosome; this process is
called sex determination. The chromosomal sex of homogametic mammals
(XX) is female; the chromosomal sex of heterogametic mammals (XY) is male.
Chromosomal sex determines gonadal sex. Virtually all subsequent sexual
differentiation is typically the result of differential exposure to gonadal steroid
hormones. Thus, gonadal sex determines hormonal sex, which regulates
nobaproject.com - Hormones & Behavior
246
247
248
Figure 3. The Average Sex Differences in Human Performance Often Reflect Significant
Overlap Between the Sexes There are often greater differences in performance between
individuals of the same sex (for example, between Alice and Mary in the figure) than between
individuals of the opposite sex (for example, between John and Mary in the figure).
249
Aggressive Behaviors
The possibility for aggressive behavior exists whenever the interests of two or
more individuals are in conflict (Nelson, 2006). Conflicts are most likely to arise
over limited resources such as territories, food, and mates. A social interaction
decides which animal gains access to the contested resource. In many cases, a
submissive posture or gesture on the part of one animal avoids the necessity
of actual combat over a resource. Animals may also participate in threat displays
or ritualized combat in which dominance is determined but no physical damage
is inflicted.
There is overwhelming circumstantial evidence that androgenic steroid
hormones mediate aggressive behavior across many species. First, seasonal
variations in blood plasma concentrations of testosterone and seasonal
variations in aggression coincide. For instance, the incidence of aggressive
behavior peaks for male deer in autumn, when they are secreting high levels
of testosterone. Second, aggressive behaviors increase at the time of puberty,
when the testes become active and blood concentrations of androgens rise.
Juvenile deer do not participate in the fighting during the mating season. Third,
in any given species, males are generally more aggressive than females. This is
certainly true of deer; relative to stags, female deer rarely display aggressive
behavior, and their rare aggressive acts are qualitatively different from the
aggressive behavior of aggressive males. Finally, castration typically reduces
aggression in males, and testosterone replacement therapy restores aggression
to pre-castration levels. There are some interesting exceptions to these general
observations that are outside the scope of this chapter.
As mentioned, males are generally more aggressive than females. Certainly,
human males are much more aggressive than females. Many more men than
women are convicted of violent crimes in North America. The sex differences
in human aggressiveness appear very early. At every age throughout the school
years, many more boys than girls initiate physical assaults. Almost everyone
nobaproject.com - Hormones & Behavior
250
will acknowledge the existence of this sex difference, but assigning a cause to
behavioral sex differences in humans always elicits much debate. It is possible
that boys are more aggressive than girls because androgens promote
aggressive behavior and boys have higher blood concentrations of androgens
than girls. It is possible that boys and girls differ in their aggressiveness because
the brains of boys are exposed to androgens prenatally and the wiring of their
brains is thus organized in a way that facilitates the expression of aggression.
It is also possible that boys are encouraged and girls are discouraged by family,
peers, or others from acting in an aggressive manner. These three hypotheses
are not mutually exclusive, but it is extremely difficult to discriminate among
them to account for sex differences in human aggressiveness.
What kinds of studies would be necessary to assess these hypotheses? It is
usually difficult to separate out the influences of environment and physiology
on the development of behavior in humans. For example, boys and girls differ
in their rough-and-tumble play at a very young age, which suggests an early
physiological influence on aggression. However, parents interact with their male
and female offspring differently; they usually play more roughly with male
infants than with females, which suggests that the sex difference in
aggressiveness is partially learned. This difference in parental interaction style
is evident by the first week of life. Because of these complexities in the factors
influencing human behavior, the study of hormonal effects on sexdifferentiated behavior has been pursued in nonhuman animals, for which
environmental influences can be held relatively constant. Animal models for
which sexual differentiation occurs postnatally are often used so that this
process can be easily manipulated experimentally.
Again, with the appropriate animal model, we can address the questions
posed above: Is the sex difference in aggression due to higher adult blood
concentrations of androgens in males than in females, or are males more
aggressive than females because their brains are organized differently by
perinatal hormones? Are males usually more aggressive than females because
nobaproject.com - Hormones & Behavior
251
Parental Behaviors
Parental behavior can be considered to be any behavior that contributes
directly to the survival of fertilized eggs or offspring that have left the body of
the female. There are many patterns of mammalian parental care. The
developmental status of the newborn is an important factor driving the type
and quality of parental care in a species. Maternal care is much more common
than paternal care. The vast majority of research on the hormonal correlates
of mammalian parental behavior has been conducted on rats. Rats bear altricial
young, and mothers perform a cluster of stereotyped maternal behaviors,
including nest building, crouching over the pups to allow nursing and to provide
nobaproject.com - Hormones & Behavior
252
253
diapers and burping the infants, were also recorded. In these studies, no
relationship between hormone concentrations and maternal responsiveness,
as measured by attitude questionnaires, was found. For example, most women
showed an increasing positive self-image during early pregnancy that dipped
during the second half of pregnancy, but recovered after parturition. A related
dip in feelings of maternal engagement occurred during late pregnancy, but
rebounded substantially after birth in most women. However, when behavior,
rather than questionnaire responses, was compared with hormone
concentrations, a different story emerged. Blood plasma concentrations of
cortisol were positively associated with approach behaviors. In other words,
women who had high concentrations of blood cortisol, in samples obtained
immediately before or after nursing, engaged in more physically affectionate
behaviors and talked more often to their babies than mothers with low cortisol
concentrations. Additional analyses from this study revealed that the
correlation was even greater for mothers that had reported positive maternal
regard (feelings and attitudes) during gestation. Indeed, nearly half of the
variation in maternal behavior among women could be accounted for by cortisol
concentrations and positive maternal attitudes during pregnancy.
Presumably, cortisol does not induce maternal behaviors directly, but it may
act indirectly on the quality of maternal care by evoking an increase in the
mothers general level of arousal, thus increasing her responsiveness to infantgenerated cues. New mothers with high cortisol concentrations were also more
attracted to their infants odors, were superior in identifying their infants, and
generally found cues from infants highly appealing (Fleming, Steiner, & Corter,
1997).
The medial preoptic area is critical for the expression of rat maternal
behavior. The amygdala appears to tonically inhibit the expression of maternal
behavior. Adult rats are fearful of pups, a response that is apparently mediated
by chemosensory information. Lesions of the amygdala or afferent sensory
pathways from the vomeronasal organ to the amygdala disinhibit the
nobaproject.com - Hormones & Behavior
254
255
Outside Resources
Book: Adkins-Regan, E. (2005). Hormones and animal social behavior.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Book: Beach, F. A. (1948). Hormones and behavior. New York: Paul Hoeber.
Book: Pfaff, D. W. (2009). Hormones, brain, and behavior (2nd ed.). New York:
Academic Press.
Book: Pfaff, D. W., Phillips, I. M., & Rubin, R. T. (2005). Principles of hormone/
behavior relations. New York: Academic Press.
Discussion Questions
1. What are some of the problems associated with attempting to determine
causation in a hormonebehavior interaction? What are the best ways to
address these problems?
3. List and describe some behavioral sex differences that you have noticed
between boys and girls. What causes girls and boys to choose different toys?
Do you think that the sex differences you have noted arise from biological
causes or are learned? How would you go about establishing your opinions
as fact?
5. Imagine that you discovered that the brains of architects were different from
those of non-architectsspecifically, that the drawstraightem nuclei of the
right temporal lobe were enlarged in architects as compared with nonarchitects. Would you argue that architects were destined to be architects
because of their brain organization or that experience as an architect
changed their brains? How would you resolve this issue?
Vocabulary
5-reductase
An enzyme required to convert testosterone to 5-dihydrotestosterone.
Aggression
A form of social interaction that includes threat, attack, and fighting.
Aromatase
An enzyme that converts androgens into estrogens.
Chromosomal sex
The sex of an individual as determined by the sex chromosomes (typically XX
or XY) received at the time of fertilization.
Defeminization
The removal of the potential for female traits.
Demasculinization
The removal of the potential for male traits.
Dihydrotestosterone (DHT)
A primary androgen that is an androgenic steroid product of testosterone and
binds strongly to androgen receptors.
Endocrine gland
A ductless gland from which hormones are released into the blood system in
response to specific biological signals.
Estrogen
Any of the C18 class of steroid hormones, so named because of the estrusgenerating properties in females. Biologically important estrogens include
estradiol and estriol.
Feminization
The induction of female traits.
Gonadal sex
The sex of an individual as determined by the possession of either ovaries or
testes. Females have ovaries, whereas males have testes.
Hormone
An organic chemical messenger released from endocrine cells that travels
through the blood to interact with target cells at some distance to cause a
biological response.
Masculinization
The induction of male traits.
Maternal behavior
Parental behavior performed by the mother or other female.
Neurotransmitter
A chemical messenger that travels between neurons to provide communication.
Some neurotransmitters, such as norepinephrine, can leak into the blood
system and act as hormones.
Oxytocin
A peptide hormone secreted by the pituitary gland to trigger lactation, as well
as social bonding.
Parental behavior
Behaviors performed in relation to ones offspring that contributes directly to
the survival of those offspring
Paternal behavior
Parental behavior performed by the father or other male.
Progesterone
A primary progestin that is involved in pregnancy and mating behaviors.
Progestin
A class of C21 steroid hormones named for their progestational (pregnancysupporting) effects. Progesterone is a common progestin.
Prohormone
A molecule that can act as a hormone itself or be converted into another
hormone with different properties. For example, testosterone can serve as a
hormone or as a prohormone for either dihydrotestosterone or estradiol.
Prolactin
A protein hormone that is highly conserved throughout the animal kingdom. It
has many biological functions associated with reproduction and synergistic
actions with steroid hormones.
Receptor
A chemical structure on the cell surface or inside of a cell that has an affinity
for a specific chemical configuration of a hormone, neurotransmitter, or other
compound.
Sex determination
The point at which an individual begins to develop as either a male or a female.
In animals that have sex chromosomes, this occurs at fertilization. Females are
XX and males are XY. All eggs bear X chromosomes, whereas sperm can either
bear X or Y chromosomes. Thus, it is the males that determine the sex of the
offspring.
Sex differentiation
The process by which individuals develop the characteristics associated with
being male or female. Differential exposure to gonadal steroids during early
development causes sexual differentiation of several structures including the
brain.
Target cell
A cell that has receptors for a specific chemical messenger (hormone or
neurotransmitter).
Testosterone
The primary androgen secreted by the testes of most vertebrate animals,
including men.
Reference List
Alexander, G. M. & Hines, M. (2002). Sex differences in response to childrens
toys in nonhuman primates (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus). *Evolution
and Human Behavior*, 23, 467479.
Berenbaum, S. A., Martin, C. L., Hanish, L. D., Briggs, P. T., & Fabes, R. A. (2008).
Sex differences in childrens play. In J. B. Becker, K. J. Berkley, N. Geary, E.
Hampson, J. Herman, & E. Young (Eds.), Sex differences in the brain: From
genes to behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.
Dabbs, J. M. (2000). Heroes, rogues, and lovers: Testosterone and behavior.
Columbus, OH: McGraw Hill.
Fleming, A. S., Steiner, M., & Corter, C. (1997). Cortisol, hedonics, and maternal
responsiveness in human mothers. Hormones and Behavior, 32, 8598.
Fleming, A. S., & Gonzalez, A. (2009). Neurobiology of human maternal care. In
P. T. Ellison & P. B. Gray (Eds.), Endocrinology of social relationships (pp. 294
318). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Goodson, J. L., Saldanha, C. J., Hahn, T. P., Soma, K. K. (2005). Recent advances
in behavioral neuroendocrinology: Insights from studies on birds.
Hormones and Behavior, 48, 46173.
Kidd, K. A., Blanchfield, P. J., Mills, K. H., Palace, V. P., Evans, R. E. Lazorchak, J. M.
& Flick, R. (2007). Collapse of a fish population following exposure to a
synthetic estrogen. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,104,
88978901.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Hormones & Behavior by Randy J. Nelson is
licensed
under
the
Creative
Commons
Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0
Biochemistry of Love
Sue Carter & Stephen Porges
University of North Carolina
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Love is deeply biological. It pervades every aspect of our lives and has inspired
countless works of art. Love also has a profound effect on our mental and
physical state. A broken heart or a failed relationship can have disastrous
effects; bereavement disrupts human physiology and may even precipitate
death. Without loving relationships, humans fail to flourish, even if all of their
other basic needs are met. As such, love is clearly not just an emotion; it is a
biological process that is both dynamic and bidirectional in several dimensions.
Social interactions between individuals, for example, trigger cognitive and
physiological processes that influence emotional and mental states. In turn,
these changes influence future social interactions. Similarly, the maintenance
of loving relationships requires constant feedback through sensory and
cognitive systems; the body seeks love and responds constantly to interactions
with loved ones or to the absence of such interactions. The evolutionary
principles and ancient hormonal and neural systems that support the beneficial
and healing effects of loving relationships are described here.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
Although evidence exists for the healing power of love, only recently has science
turned its attention to providing a physiological explanation for love. The study
of love in this context offers insight into many important topics, including the
biological basis of interpersonal relationships and why and how disruptions in
social bonds have such pervasive consequences for behavior and physiology.
Some of the answers will be found in our growing knowledge of the
neurobiological and endocrinological mechanisms of social behavior and
interpersonal engagement.
268
track.
The evolutionary pathways that led from reptiles to mammals allowed the
emergence of the unique anatomical systems and biochemical mechanisms
that enable social engagement and selectively reciprocal sociality. Reptiles show
minimal parental investment in offspring and form nonselective relationships
between individuals. Pet owners may become emotionally attached to their
turtle or snake, but this relationship is not reciprocal. In contrast, most mammals
show intense parental investment in offspring and form lasting bonds with their
children. Many mammalian speciesincluding humans, wolves, and prairie
volesalso develop long-lasting, reciprocal, and selective relationships
between adults, with several features of what humans experience as love. In
turn, these reciprocal interactions trigger dynamic feedback mechanisms that
foster growth and health.
269
270
271
272
social behavior are constantly adapting: These peptides and the systems that
they regulate are always in flux. In spite of these difficulties, some of the different
functions of oxytocin and vasopressin have been identified.
273
Because the
274
275
276
the tendency to show a pair bond. However, these studies also showed that a
single exposure to a higher level of oxytocin in early life could disrupt the later
capacity to pair bond (Carter et al., 2009).
There is little doubt that either early social experiences or the effects of
developmental exposure to these neuropeptides holds the potential to have
long-lasting effects on behavior. Both parental care and exposure to oxytocin
in early life can permanently modify hormonal systems, altering the capacity
to form relationships and influence the expression of love across the life span.
Our preliminary findings in voles further suggest that early life experiences
affect the methylation of the oxytocin receptor gene and its expression
(Connelly, Kenkel, Erickson, & Carter, 2011). Thus, we can plausibly argue that
love is epigenetic.
277
278
Conclusion
Research in this field is new and there is much left to understand. However, it
is already clear that both love and oxytocin are powerful. Of course, with power
comes responsibility. Although research into mechanisms through which love
or hormones such as oxytocinmay protect us against stress and disease is
in its infancy, this knowledge will ultimately increase our understanding of the
nobaproject.com - Biochemistry of Love
279
way that our emotions impact upon health and disease. The same molecules
that allow us to give and receive love also link our need for others with health
and well-being.
Acknowledgments
C. Sue Carter and Stephen W. Porges are both Professors of Psychiatry at the
University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, and also are Research Professors of
Psychology at Northeastern University, Boston.
Discussions of love and forgiveness with members of the Fetzer Institutes
Advisory Committee on Natural Sciences led to this essay and are gratefully
acknowledged here. We are especially appreciative of thoughtful editorial input
from Dr. James Harris. Studies from the authors laboratories were sponsored
by the National Institutes of Health. We also express our gratitude for this
support and to our colleagues, whose input and hard work informed the ideas
expressed in this article. A version of this paper was previously published in
EMBO Reports in the series on Sex and Society; this paper is reproduced with
the permission of the publishers of that journal.
280
Outside Resources
Book: C. S. Carter, L. Ahnert et al. (Eds.), (2006). Attachment and bonding: A
new synthesis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Discussion Questions
1. If love is so important in human behavior, why is it so hard to describe and
understand?
2. Discuss the role of evolution in understanding what humans call love or
other forms of prosociality.
3. What are the common biological and neuroendocrine elements that appear
in maternal love and adult-adult relationships?
4. Oxytocin and vasopressin are biochemically similar. What are some of the
differences between the actions of oxytocin and vasopressin?
5. How may the properties of oxytocin and vasopressin help us understand
the biological bases of love?
6. What are common features of the biochemistry of love and safety, and
why are these important to human health?
Vocabulary
Epigenetics
Heritable changes in gene activity that are not caused by changes in the DNA
sequence. http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Epigenetics
Oxytocin
A nine amino acid mammalian neuropeptide. Oxytocin is synthesized primarily
in the brain, but also in other tissues such as uterus, heart and thymus, with
local effects. Oxytocin is best known as a hormone of female reproduction due
to its capacity to cause uterine contractions and eject milk. Oxytocin has effects
on brain tissue, but also acts throughout the body in some cases as an
antioxidant or anti-inflammatory.
Vagus nerve
The 10th cranial nerve. The mammalian vagus has an older unmyelinated
branch which originates in the dorsal motor complex and a more recently
evolved, myelinated branch, with origins in the ventral vagal complex including
the nucleus ambiguous.
Reference List
Aragona, B. J, & Wang, Z. (2009). Dopamine regulation of social choice in a
monogamous rodent species. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 15.
Bales, K. L., Kim, A. J., Lewis-Reese, A. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Both oxytocin
and vasopressin may influence alloparental care in male prairie voles.
Hormones and Behavior, 44, 454361.
Bosch, O. J., & Neumann, I. D. (2012). Both oxytocin and vasopressin are
mediators of maternal care and aggression in rodents: from central release
to sites of action. Hormones and Behavior, 61, 293303.
Carter, C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 779818.
Carter, C. S., Boone, E. M., Pournajafi-Nazarloo, H., & Bales, K. L. (2009). The
consequences of early experiences and exposure to oxytocin and
vasopressin are sexually-dimorphic. Developmental Neuroscience, 31, 332
341.
Carter, C. S., DeVries, A. C., & Getz, L. L. (1995). Physiological substrates of
mammalian monogamy: The prairie vole model. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews, 19, 303314.
Carter, C. S., & Altemus, M. (1997). Integrative functions of lactational hormones
in social behavior and stress management. Annals of the New York Academy
of Sciences, Integrative Neurobiology of Affiliation 807, 164174.
Cho, M. M., DeVries, A. C., Williams, J. R., Carter, C. S. (1999). The effects of
Karelina, K., & DeVries, A. C. (2011). Modeling social influences on human health.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 73, 6774.
Kenkel, W.M., Paredes, J., Lewis, G.F., Yee, J.R., Pournajafi-Nazarloo, H., Grippo,
A.J., Porges, S.W., & Carter, C.S. (2013). Autonomic substrates of the response
to pups in male prairie voles. PlosOne Aug 5;8(8):e69965. doi: 10.1371/
journal.pone.0069965.
Kenkel, W.M., Paredes, J., Yee, J. R., Pournajafi-Nazarloo, H., Bales, K. L., & Carter,
C. S. (2012). Exposure to an infant releases oxytocin and facilitates pairbonding in male prairie voles. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 24, 874886.
Keverne, E. B. (2006). Neurobiological and molecular approaches to attachment
and bonding. In C. S. Carter, L. Ahnert et al. (Eds.), Attachment and bonding:
A new synthesis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Pp. 101-117.
Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Domes, G., Kirsch, P., & Heinrichs, M. (2011). Oxytocin
and vasopressin in the human brain: social neuropeptides for translational
medicine. Nature: Reviews in Neuroscience 12, 524538.
Porges, S. W. (2011). The polyvagal theory: Neurophysiological foundations of
emotions, attachment, communication and self-regulation. New York, NY:
Norton.
Szeto, A., Nation, D. A., Mendez, A. J., Dominguez-Bendala, J., Brooks, L. G.,
Schneiderman, N., & McCabe, P. M. (2008). Oxytocin attenuates NADPHdependent superoxide activity and IL-6 secretion in macrophages and
vascular cells. American Journal of Physiology, Endocrinology and
Metabolism 295, E14951501.
Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Gurung, R. A., & Updegraff,
J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: Tend-andbefriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychol Rev, 107, 411429.
Woodard, S. H., Fischman, B. J., Venkat, A., Hudson, M. E., Varala, K., Cameron
S.A., . . . Robinson, G. E. (2011). Genes involved in convergent evolution of
eusociality in bees. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 108, 74727477.
Zhang, L., Hernandez, V. S., Liu, B., Medina, M. P., Nava-Kopp, A. T., Irles, C., &
Morales, M. (2012). Hypothalamic vasopressin system regulation by
maternal separation: Its impact on anxiety in rats. Neuroscience, 215, 135
148.
Zhang, T. Y., & Meaney, M. J. (2010). Epigenetics and the environmental
regulation of the genome and its function. Annual Review of Psychology, 61,
439466.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Biochemistry of Love by Sue Carter and Stephen
Porges is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Evolutionary Theories in
Psychology
David M. Buss
University of Texas at Austin
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Evolutionchange over timeoccurs through the processes of natural and
sexual selection. Its primary products are adaptationssolutions to problems
of survival and reproduction. Sexual selection theory describes evolution due
to mating advantage rather than survival advantage and occurs through two
distinct pathways: intrasexual competition and intersexual selection. Gene
selection theory, the modern formulation of evolutionary biology, occurs
through differential gene replication. Evolutionary psychology synthesizes
evolutionary principles with modern psychology and focuses primarily on
psychological adaptationsinformation processing procedures inside the
head. Two major evolutionary psychological theories are described. Sexual
strategies theory describes the psychology of human mating strategies and the
ways in which women and men differ in those strategies. Error management
theory describes the evolution of cognitive biases in domains ranging from
perception to mating.
Learning Objectives
Identify the core premises of error management theory, and provide two
empirical examples of adaptive cognitive biases.
preferences (for fat and sugar), sweat glands and shivering mechanisms, and
fears of snakes, spiders, darkness, heights, and strangers. These fears helped
our ancestors to avoid the hostile forces of nature. The second major class of
adaptations is composed of those that aided our ancestors in mate competition,
which brings us to a second evolutionary theory proposed by Charles Darwin
sexual selection theory.
292
293
Evolutionary Psychology
Evolutionary psychology applies the lenses of modern evolutionary theory to
understanding the mechanisms of the human mind. It focuses primarily on
psychological adaptationsmechanisms of the mind that evolved to solve
specific problems of survival or reproduction. Most evolutionary psychologists
conceptualize psychological adaptations as information-processing devices:
nobaproject.com - Evolutionary Theories in Psychology
294
inputs, procedures, and outputs. To use a physiological example, a callusproducing adaptation would have input such as repeated friction to the skin;
procedures that dictate the growth of new skin cells to the afflicted area; and
actual calluses as output that function to protect the underlying structures
beneath the skin. A psychological example would be sexual jealousy. A jealousy
adaptation might contain inputs such as a romantic partner flirting with
someone else or a mate poacher flirting with ones partner; the procedures
would be decisions that include assessing the physical formidability of the rival
and gauging ones partners interest in the mate poacher; and the behavioral
output might range from vigilance (e.g., snooping through a partners email) to
violence (e.g., threatening the rival).
As these examples illustrate, evolutionary psychology is fundamentally an
interactionist framework. Neither calluses nor jealousy simply pop up without
the relevant environmental input. Indeed, adaptations are designed to deal with
environmental problems, be they physical (damage due to friction to the skin)
or social (mate poachers trying to lure your romantic partner).
Evolutionary psychology is interactionist in also including cultural input as
central to the activation or suppression of psychological adaptations. For
example, although status within the group is critical in all cultures to acquiring
reproductively relevant resources (including desirable mates), in individualistic
cultures such as the United States, status is heavily determined by individual
accomplishments. In more collectivist cultures, such as Japan, status is more
heavily determined by contributions to the group and the groups success. The
importance placed on virginity, to take another example, is a mate preference
highly susceptible to cultural input. Cultural norms of premarital sex influence
the degree to which men and women value virginity in potential marriage
partners. Evolutionary psychology, in short, does not predict rigid robotic-like
instincts, but rather flexible environmentally contingent and culturally
contingent adaptations.
nobaproject.com - Evolutionary Theories in Psychology
295
296
men, however, because they need not invest much, the costs of making a poor
sexual decision are lower. This logic leads to a powerful set of empirical
predictions: In short-term mating, women should be choosier or more
discriminating than men; that men will, on average, be motivated to engage in
more low-investment sexual strategies; that men will sometimes deceive
women about their long-term intentions in order to have short-term sex; and
that men will lower their mating standards in short-term mating more than
women.
A tremendous body of empirical evidence supports these and related
predictions (Buss & Schmitt, 2011). Men express a desire for a larger number
of sex partners than do women. They let less time elapse before seeking sexual
intercourse. They are more willing to consent to sex with strangers and are less
likely to require emotional involvement in order to have sex. They have more
frequent sexual fantasies and fantasize about a larger variety of sex partners.
They are more likely to regret missed sexual opportunities. And, they lower their
standards in short-term mating, showing a willingness to mate with a larger
variety of women as long as the costs and risks are low.
In long-term mating, however, both sexes invest tremendously in both the
relationship and in resultant children. Consequently, the theory predicts that
both sexes should be extremely choosy when pursuing a long-term mating
strategy. Much empirical research supports this prediction. Many qualities on
which women and men choose long-term mates are highly similarboth want
mates who are intelligent, kind, understanding, healthy, dependable, honest,
loyal, loving, and adaptable.
Women and men differ in their preferences for a few key qualities in longterm mating because they have faced somewhat distinct adaptive problems.
Ancestral women faced the problem of securing a long-term mate willing and
able to invest resources in them and their children. Consequently, modern
women have inherited from their successful ancestral mothers a desire for
mates who possess resources, who have qualities linked with resource
nobaproject.com - Evolutionary Theories in Psychology
297
298
299
EMT has also been used to predict adaptive biases in the domain of mating.
Consider something as simple as a smile. Its an inherently ambiguous cue. It
may signal sexual or romantic interest. Or it may signal mere friendliness.
Because of the heavy costs to men of missing out on the rare chances for
reproduction, EMT predicts that men have a sexual overperception bias, causing
them to over-infer sexual interest by a woman based on minimal ambiguous
cues such as a smile or a touch. In the mating domain, the sexual overperception
bias is among the most robust empirically documented phenomena. Its been
documented in studies in which men and women rated the sexual interest of
people in photographs and videotaped interactions. And, its been documented
in the laboratory in actual social interactions using a speed dating paradigm
(Perilloux, Easton, & Buss, 2012). In short, EMT predicts that men, more than
women, will over-infer sexual interest based on minimal cues, and empirical
research confirms this adaptive mating bias.
Conclusion
Sexual strategies theory and error management theory are two evolutionary
psychological theories that have received much empirical support from dozens
of independent researchers. But, there are many other evolutionary
psychological theories, such as social exchange theory, dominance theory,
resource-holding potential theory, and environmental navigation theory. The
merits of each evolutionary psychological theory must be evaluated separately,
based on the degree to which empirical predictions rendered in advance are
supported by scientific studies. Some will be supported. Some will be rejected
based on evidence. And, many will require modification and increased
complexity in light of new scientific findings and new theoretical developments.
300
Outside Resources
FAQs
http://www.anth.ucsb.edu/projects/human/evpsychfaq.html
Web: Articles and books on evolutionary psychology
http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/homepage/Group/BussLAB/
Web: Main international scientific organization for the study of evolution
and human behavior, HBES
http://www.hbes.com/
Discussion Questions
1. How does change take place over time in the living world?
2. Which two potential psychological adaptations to problems of survival are
not discussed in this chapter?
3. What are the psychological and behavioral implications of the fact that
women bear heavier costs to produce a child than men do?
4. Can you formulate a hypothesis about an error management bias in the
domain of social interaction?
Vocabulary
Adaptations
Evolved solutions to problems that historically contributed to reproductive
success.
In chapters, can "ordered list" formatting feature also be offered in alpha (a, b,
c)?
a
Evolution
Change over time. Is the definition changing?
Intersexual selection
A process of sexual selection by which evolution (change) occurs as a
consequences of the mate preferences of one sex exerting selection pressure
on members of the opposite sex.
Intrasexual competition
A process of sexual selection by which members of one sex compete with each
other, and the victors gain preferential mating access to members of the
opposite sex.
Natural selection
Differential reproductive success as a consequence of differences in heritable
attributes.
Psychological adaptations
Mechanisms of the mind that evolved to solve specific problems of survival or
reproduction; conceptualized as information processing devices.
Sexual selection
The evolution of characteristics because of the mating advantage they give
organisms.
Sexual Strategies Theory
A comprehensive evolutionary theory of human mating that defines the menu
of mating strategies humans pursue (e.g., short-term casual sex, long-term
committed mating), the adaptive problems women and men face when
pursuing these strategies, and the evolved solutions to these mating problems.
Reference List
Buss, D. M. (2012). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind (4th
ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary
hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 12, 149.
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (2011). Evolutionary psychology and feminism. Sex
Roles, 64, 768787.
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary
perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204232.
Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Error management theory: A new
perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 78, 8191.
Haselton, M. G., Nettle, D., & Andrews, P. W. (2005). The evolution of cognitive
bias. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 724
746). New York, NY: Wiley.
Jackson, R. E., & Cormack, J. K. (2008). Evolved navigation theory and the
environmental vertical illusion. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 299
304.
Perilloux, C., Easton, J. A., & Buss, D. M. (2012). The misperception of sexual
interest. Psychological Science, 23, 146151.
Epigenetics in Psychology:
Toward an Understanding of
the Dynamic Interaction
among Genes, Environment,
and the Brain
Ian Weaver
Dalhousie University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Early life experiences exert a profound and long-lasting influence on physical
and mental health throughout life. The efforts to identify the primary causes
of this have significantly benefited from studies of the epigenomea dynamic
layer of information associated with DNA that differs between individuals and
can be altered through various experiences and environments. The epigenome
has been heralded as a key missing piece of the etiological puzzle for
understanding how development of psychological disorders may be influenced
by the surrounding environment, in concordance with the genome.
Understanding the mechanisms involved in the initiation, maintenance, and
heritability of epigenetic states is thus an important aspect of research in current
biology, particularly in the study of learning and memory, emotion, and social
behavior in humans. Moreover, epigenetics in psychology provides a framework
for understanding how the expression of genes is influenced by experiences
and the environment to produce individual differences in behavior, cognition,
personality, and mental health. In this chapter, we survey recent developments
revealing epigenetic aspects of mental health and review some of the challenges
of epigenetic approaches in psychology to help explain how nurture shapes
nature.
Learning Objectives
Explain what the term epigenetics means and the molecular machinery
involved.
Name and discuss important neural and developmental pathways that are
regulated by epigenetic factors, and provide examples of epigenetic effects
on personality traits and cognitive behavior.
Introduction
Early childhood is not only a period of physical growth; it is also a time of mental
development related to changes in the anatomy, physiology, and chemistry of
the nervous system that influence mental health throughout life. Cognitive
abilities associated with learning and memory, reasoning, problem solving, and
developing relationships continue to emerge during childhood. Brain
development is more rapid during this critical or sensitive period than at any
other, with more than 700 neural connections created each second. Herein,
complex geneenvironment interactions (or genotypeenvironment interactions,
GE) serve to increase the number of possible contacts between neurons, as
they hone their adult synaptic properties and excitability. Many weak
connections form to different neuronal targets; subsequently, they undergo
remodeling in which most connections vanish and a few stable connections
remain. These structural changes (or plasticity) may be crucial for the
development of mature neural networks that support emotional, cognitive, and
social behavior. The generation of different morphology, physiology, and
behavioral outcomes from a single genome in response to changes in the
environment forms the basis for phenotypic plasticity, which is fundamental
to the way organisms cope with environmental variation, navigate the present
world, and solve future problems.
The challenge for psychology has been to integrate findings from genetics
and environmental (social, biological, chemical) factors, including the quality of
infantmother attachments, into the study of personality and our
understanding of the emergence of mental illness. These studies have
demonstrated that common DNA sequence variation and rare mutations
account for only a small fraction (1%2%) of the total risk for inheritance of
personality traits and mental disorders (Dick, Riley, & Kendler, 2010; Gershon,
Alliey-Rodriguez, & Liu, 2011). Additionally, studies that have attempted to
examine the mechanisms and conditions under which DNA sequence variation
nobaproject.com - Epigenetics in Psychology: Toward an Understanding of the Dynamic Interaction among Genes, Environment, a
311
nobaproject.com - Epigenetics in Psychology: Toward an Understanding of the Dynamic Interaction among Genes, Environment, a
312
nobaproject.com - Epigenetics in Psychology: Toward an Understanding of the Dynamic Interaction among Genes, Environment, a
313
nobaproject.com - Epigenetics in Psychology: Toward an Understanding of the Dynamic Interaction among Genes, Environment, a
314
nobaproject.com - Epigenetics in Psychology: Toward an Understanding of the Dynamic Interaction among Genes, Environment, a
315
nobaproject.com - Epigenetics in Psychology: Toward an Understanding of the Dynamic Interaction among Genes, Environment, a
316
of poor maternal care. This points to a possible molecular target for treatments
that may reverse or ameliorate the traces of childhood maltreatment.
Several studies have attempted to determine to what extent the findings
from model animals are transferable to humans. Examination of post-mortem
brain tissue from healthy human subjects found that the human equivalent of
the glucocorticoid receptor gene promoter (NR3C1 exon 1F promoter) is also
unique to the individual (Turner, Pelascini, Macedo, & Muller, 2008). A similar
study examining newborns showed that methylation of the glucocorticoid
receptor gene promoter maybe an early epigenetic marker of maternal mood
and risk of increased hormonal responses to stress in infants 3 months of age
(Oberlander et al., 2008). Although further studies are required to examine the
functional consequence of this DNA methylation, these findings are consistent
with our studies in the neonate and adult offspring of low licking and grooming
mothers that show increased DNA methylation of the promoter of the
glucocorticoid receptor gene, decreased glucocorticoid receptor gene
expression, and increased hormonal responses to stress (Weaver et al., 2004).
Examination of brain tissue from suicide victims found that the human
glucocorticoid receptor gene promoter is also more methylated in the brains
of individuals who had experienced maltreatment during childhood (McGowan
et al., 2009). These finding suggests that DNA methylation mediates the effects
of early environment in both rodents and humans and points to the possibility
of new therapeutic approaches stemming from translational epigenetic
research. Indeed, similar processes at comparable epigenetic labile regions
could explain why the adult offspring of high and low licking/grooming mothers
exhibit widespread differences in hippocampal gene expression and cognitive
function (Weaver, Meaney, & Szyf, 2006).
However, this type of research is limited by the inaccessibility of human
brain samples. The translational potential of this finding would be greatly
enhanced if the relevant epigenetic modification can be measured in an
accessible tissue. Examination of blood samples from adult patients with
nobaproject.com - Epigenetics in Psychology: Toward an Understanding of the Dynamic Interaction among Genes, Environment, a
317
nobaproject.com - Epigenetics in Psychology: Toward an Understanding of the Dynamic Interaction among Genes, Environment, a
318
nobaproject.com - Epigenetics in Psychology: Toward an Understanding of the Dynamic Interaction among Genes, Environment, a
319
nobaproject.com - Epigenetics in Psychology: Toward an Understanding of the Dynamic Interaction among Genes, Environment, a
320
(Alarcon et al., 2004), which are profound intellectual disability disorders. Both
MECP2 and CBP are highly expressed in neurons and are involved in regulating
neural gene expression (Chen et al., 2003; Martinowich et al., 2003).
Rett syndrome patients have a mutation in their DNA sequence in a gene
called MECP2. MECP2 plays many important roles within the cell: One of these
roles is to read the DNA sequence, checking for DNA methylation, and to bind
to areas that contain methylation, thereby preventing the wrong proteins from
being present. Other roles for MECP2 include promoting the presence of
particular, necessary, proteins, ensuring that DNA is packaged properly within
the cell and assisting with the production of proteins. MECP2 function also
influences gene expression that supports dendritic and synaptic development
and hippocampus-dependent memory (Li, Zhong, Chau, Williams, & Chang,
2011; Skene et al., 2010). Mice with altered MECP2 expression exhibit genomewide increases in histone acetylation, neuron cell death, increased anxiety,
cognitive deficits, and social withdrawal (Shahbazian et al., 2002). These findings
support a model in which DNA methylation and MECP2 constitute a cell-specific
epigenetic mechanism for regulation of histone modification and gene
expression, which may be disrupted in Rett syndrome.
RTS patients have a mutation in their DNA sequence in a gene called CBP.
One of these roles of CBP is to bind to specific histones and promote histone
acetylation, thereby promoting gene expression. Consistent with this function,
RTS patients exhibit a genome-wide decrease in histone acetylation and
cognitive dysfunction in adulthood (Kalkhoven et al., 2003). The learning and
memory deficits are attributed to disrupted neural plasticity (Korzus, Rosenfeld,
& Mayford, 2004). Similar to RTS in humans, mice with a mutation of CBP
perform poorly in cognitive tasks and show decreased genome-wide histone
acetylation (for review, see Josselyn, 2005). In the mouse brain CBP was found
to act as an epigenetic switch to promote the birth of new neurons in the brain.
Interestingly, this epigenetic mechanism is disrupted in the fetal brains of mice
with a mutation of CBP, which, as pups, exhibit early behavioral deficits following
nobaproject.com - Epigenetics in Psychology: Toward an Understanding of the Dynamic Interaction among Genes, Environment, a
321
removal and separation from their mother (Wang et al., 2010). These findings
provide a novel mechanism whereby environmental cues, acting through
histone modifying enzymes, can regulate epigenetic status and thereby directly
promote neurogenesis, which regulates neurobehavioral development.
Together, these studies demonstrate that misregulation of epigenetic
modifications and their regulatory enzymes is capable of orchestrating
prominent deficits in neuronal plasticity and cognitive function. Knowledge
from these studies may provide greater insight into other mental disorders
such as depression and suicidal behaviors.
nobaproject.com - Epigenetics in Psychology: Toward an Understanding of the Dynamic Interaction among Genes, Environment, a
322
nobaproject.com - Epigenetics in Psychology: Toward an Understanding of the Dynamic Interaction among Genes, Environment, a
323
effects of adversity and determine who is sensitive and who is resilient through
a gene-environment interplay. Genes such as the glucocorticoid receptor
appear to moderate the effects of childhood adversity on mental illness.
Remarkably, epigenetic DNA modifications have been identified that may
underlie the long-lasting effects of environment on biological functions. This
new epigenetic research is pointing to a new strategy to understanding geneenvironment interactions.
The next decade of research will show if this potential can be exploited in
the development of new therapeutic options that may alter the traces that early
environment leaves on the genome. However, as discussed in this chapter, the
epigenome is not static and can be molded by developmental signals,
environmental perturbations, and disease states, which present an
experimental challenge in the search for epigenetic risk factors in psychological
disorders (Rakyan, Down, Balding, & Beck, 2011). The sample size and
epigenomic assay required is dependent on the number of tissues affected, as
well as the type and distribution of epigenetic modifications. The combination
of genetic association maps studies with epigenome-wide developmental
studies may help identify novel molecular mechanisms to explain features of
inheritance of personality traits and transform our understanding of the
biological basis of psychology. Importantly, these epigenetic studies may lead
to identification of novel therapeutic targets and enable the development of
improved strategies for early diagnosis, prevention, and better treatment of
psychological and behavioral disorders.
nobaproject.com - Epigenetics in Psychology: Toward an Understanding of the Dynamic Interaction among Genes, Environment, a
324
Outside Resources
Reference: The Encyclopedia of DNA Elements (ENCODE) project
http://encodeproject.org/ENCODE/
Reference: THREADS - A new way to explore the ENCODE Project
http://www.nature.com/encode/#/threads
Web: Explore, view, and download genome-wide maps of DNA and histone
modifications from the NCBI Epigenomics Portal
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/epigenomics
Web: NOVA ScienceNOW - Introduction to Epigenetics
http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/nova/genes
Web: The University of Utah's Genetic Science Learning Center
http://learn.genetics.utah.edu/content/epigenetics/
Discussion Questions
1. Describe the physical state of the genome when genes are active and
inactive.
2. Often, the physical characteristics of genetically identical twins become
increasingly different as they age, even at the molecular level. Explain why
this is so (use the terms environment and epigenome).
3. Name 34 environmental factors that influence the epigenome and
describe their effects.
4. The rat nurturing example shows us how parental behavior can shape the
behavior of offspring on a biochemical level. Discuss how this relates to
humans and include the personal and social implications.
5. Explain how the food we eat affects gene expression.
6. Can the diets of parents affect their offsprings epigenome?
7. Why is converging evidence the best kind of evidence in the study of brain
function?
8. If you were interested in whether a particular brain area was involved in a
specific behavior, what neuroscience methods could you use?
9. If you were interested in the precise time in which a particular brain process
occurred, which neuroscience methods could you use?
Vocabulary
DNA methylation
Covalent modifications of mammalian DNA occurring via the methylation of
cytosine, typically in the context of the CpG dinucleotide.
DNA methyltransferases (DNMTs)
Enzymes that establish and maintain DNA methylation using methyl-group
donor compounds or cofactors. The main mammalian DNMTs are DNMT1,
which maintains methylation state across DNA replication, and DNMT3a and
DNMT3b, which perform de novo methylation.
Epigenetics
The study of heritable changes in gene expression or cellular phenotype caused
by mechanisms other than changes in the underlying DNA sequence. Epigenetic
marks include covalent DNA modifications and posttranslational histone
modifications.
Epigenome
The genome-wide distribution of epigenetic marks.
Gene
A specific deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA) sequence that codes for a specific
polypeptide or protein or an observable inherited trait.
Genome-wide association study (GWAS)
A study that maps DNA polymorphisms in affected individuals and controls
matched for age, sex, and ethnic background with the aim of identifying causal
genetic variants.
Genotype
The DNA content of a cells nucleus, whether a trait is externally observable or
not.
Histone modifications
Posttranslational modifications of the N-terminal tails of histone proteins that
serve as a major mode of epigenetic regulation. These modifications include
acetylation, phosphorylation, methylation, sumoylation, ubiquitination, and
ADP-ribosylation.
Identical twins
Two individual organisms that originated from the same zygote and therefore
are genetically identical or very similar. The epigenetic profiling of identical twins
discordant for disease is a unique experimental design as it eliminates the DNA
sequence-, age-, and sex-differences from consideration.
Phenotype
The pattern of expression of the genotype or the magnitude or extent to which
it is observably expressedan observable characteristic or trait of an organism,
such as its morphology, development, biochemical or physiological properties,
or behavior.
Reference List
Adams, R. L., McKay, E. L., Craig, L. M., & Burdon, R. H. (1979). Mouse DNA
methylase: methylation of native DNA. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta, 561
(2), 345357.
Alarcon, J. M., Malleret, G., Touzani, K., Vronskaya, S., Ishii, S., Kandel, E. R., &
Barco, A. (2004). Chromatin acetylation, memory, and LTP are impaired in
CBP+/- mice: a model for the cognitive deficit in Rubinstein-Taybi syndrome
and
its
amelioration.
Neuron,
42(6),
947959.
doi:
10.1016/j.
Guan, Z., Giustetto, M., Lomvardas, S., Kim, J. H., Miniaci, M. C., Schwartz, J. H., . . .
Kandel, E. R. (2002). Integration of long-term-memory-related synaptic
plasticity involves bidirectional regulation of gene expression and chromatin
structure. Cell, 111(4), 483493.
Guo, J. U., Ma, D. K., Mo, H., Ball, M. P., Jang, M. H., Bonaguidi, M. A., . . . Song, H.
(2011). Neuronal activity modifies the DNA methylation landscape in the
adult brain. Nature Neuroscience, 14(10), 13451351. doi: 10.1038/nn.2900
Heijmans, B. T., Tobi, E. W., Stein, A. D., Putter, H., Blauw, G. J., Susser, E. S., . . .
Lumey, L. H. (2008). Persistent epigenetic differences associated with
prenatal exposure to famine in humans. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences U S A, 105(44), 1704617049. doi: 0806560105 [pii]
10.1073/pnas.0806560105
Hong, L., Schroth, G. P., Matthews, H. R., Yau, P., & Bradbury, E. M. (1993). Studies
of the DNA binding properties of histone H4 amino terminus. Thermal
denaturation studies reveal that acetylation markedly reduces the binding
constant of the H4 "tail" to DNA. Journal of Biological Chemistry, 268(1), 305
314.
Jenuwein, T., & Allis, C. D. (2001). Translating the histone code. Science, 293
(5532), 10741080. doi: 10.1126/Science.1063127293/5532/1074 [pii]
Jiang, Y. H., Bressler, J., & Beaudet, A. L. (2004). Epigenetics and human disease.
Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics, 5, 479510. doi: 10.1146/
annurev.genom.5.061903.180014
Josselyn, S. A. (2005). What's right with my mouse model? New insights into the
molecular and cellular basis of cognition from mouse models of Rubinstein-
Taybi Syndrome. Learning & Memory, 12(2), 8083. doi: 12/2/80 [pii]10.1101/
lm.93505
Kadonaga, J. T. (1998). Eukaryotic transcription: an interlaced network of
transcription factors and chromatin-modifying machines. Cell, 92(3), 307
313.
Kalkhoven, E., Roelfsema, J. H., Teunissen, H., den Boer, A., Ariyurek, Y., Zantema,
A., . . . Peters, D. J. (2003). Loss of CBP acetyltransferase activity by PHD finger
mutations in Rubinstein-Taybi syndrome. Human Molecular Genetics, 12(4),
441450.
Korzus, E., Rosenfeld, M. G., & Mayford, M. (2004). CBP histone acetyltransferase
activity is a critical component of memory consolidation. Neuron, 42(6), 961
972. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.06.002S0896627304003526 [pii]
Kuo, M. H., & Allis, C. D. (1998). Roles of histone acetyltransferases and
deacetylases in gene regulation. Bioessays, 20(8), 615626. doi:
10.1002/(SICI)15211878(199808)20:8<615::AID-BIES4>3.0.CO;2-H [pii] 10.1002/(SICI)
1521-1878(199808)20:8<615::AID-BIES4>3.0.CO;2-H
Law, J. A., & Jacobsen, S. E. (2010). Establishing, maintaining and modifying DNA
methylation patterns in plants and animals. Nature Reviews Genetics, 11
(3), 204220. doi: nrg2719 [pii]10.1038/nrg2719
Lee, M. G., Wynder, C., Schmidt, D. M., McCafferty, D. G., & Shiekhattar, R. (2006).
Histone H3 lysine 4 demethylation is a target of nonselective antidepressive
medications. Chemistry & Biology, 13(6), 563567. doi: 10.1016/j.
chembiol.2006.05.004
Levenson, J. M., O'Riordan, K. J., Brown, K. D., Trinh, M. A., Molfese, D. L., & Sweatt,
J. D. (2004). Regulation of histone acetylation during memory formation in
the hippocampus. Journal of Biological Chemistry, 279(39), 4054540559.
Li, H., Zhong, X., Chau, K. F., Williams, E. C., & Chang, Q. (2011). Loss of activityinduced phosphorylation of MeCP2 enhances synaptogenesis, LTP and
spatial memory. Nature Neuroscience, 14(8), 10011008. doi: 10.1038/
nn.2866
Lillycrop, K. A., Phillips, E. S., Jackson, A. A., Hanson, M. A., & Burdge, G. C. (2005).
Dietary protein restriction of pregnant rats induces and folic acid
supplementation prevents epigenetic modification of hepatic gene
expression in the offspring. Journal of Nutrition, 135(6), 13821386. doi:
135/6/1382 [pii]
Lillycrop, K. A., Slater-Jefferies, J. L., Hanson, M. A., Godfrey, K. M., Jackson, A. A.,
& Burdge, G. C. (2007). Induction of altered epigenetic regulation of the
hepatic glucocorticoid receptor in the offspring of rats fed a proteinrestricted
diet
during
pregnancy
suggests
that
reduced
DNA
Myers, M. M., Brunelli, S. A., Shair, H. N., Squire, J. M., & Hofer, M. A. (1989).
Relationships between maternal behavior of SHR and WKY dams and adult
blood pressures of cross-fostered F1 pups. Developmental Psychobiology,
22(1), 5567.
Oberlander, T. F., Weinberg, J., Papsdorf, M., Grunau, R., Misri, S., & Devlin, A.
M. (2008). Prenatal exposure to maternal depression, neonatal methylation
of human glucocorticoid receptor gene (NR3C1) and infant cortisol stress
responses. Epigenetics, 3(2), 97106. doi: 6034 [pii]
Ooi, S. K., O'Donnell, A. H., & Bestor, T. H. (2009). Mammalian cytosine
methylation at a glance. Journal of Cell Science, 122(Pt 16), 27872791. doi:
122/16/2787 [pii]10.1242/jcs.015123
Painter, R. C., Roseboom, T. J., & Bleker, O. P. (2005). Prenatal exposure to the
Dutch famine and disease in later life: an overview. Reproductive Toxicology,
20(3), 345352. doi: S0890-6238(05)00088-2 [pii]10.1016/j. Reproductive
Toxicology.2005.04.005
Petronis, A. (2010). Epigenetics as a unifying principle in the aetiology of complex
traits and diseases. Nature, 465(7299), 721727. doi: 10.1038/nature09230
Poulter, M. O., Du, L., Weaver, I. C., Palkovits, M., Faludi, G., Merali, Z., . . . Anisman,
H. (2008). GABAA receptor promoter hypermethylation in suicide brain:
implications for the involvement of epigenetic processes. Biological
Psychiatry, 64(8), 645652. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.028
Rakyan, V. K., Down, T. A., Balding, D. J., & Beck, S. (2011). Epigenome-wide
association studies for common human diseases. Nature Reviews Genetics,
12(8), 529541. doi: 10.1038/nrg3000
Trends
in
Biochemical
Sciences,
22(4),
128132.
doi:
S0968000497010165 [pii]
Wang, J., Weaver, I. C., Gauthier-Fisher, A., Wang, H., He, L., Yeomans, J., . . . Miller,
F. D. (2010). CBP histone acetyltransferase activity regulates embryonic
neural differentiation in the normal and Rubinstein-Taybi syndrome brain.
Developmental Cell, 18(1), 114125. doi: 10.1016/j.devcel.2009.10.023
Weaver, I. C., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., D'Alessio, A. C., Sharma, S., Seckl, J.
R., . . . Meaney, M. J. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior.
Nature Neuroscience, 7(8), 847854. doi: 10.1038/nn1276
Weaver, I. C., Champagne, F. A., Brown, S. E., Dymov, S., Sharma, S., Meaney, M.
J., & Szyf, M. (2005). Reversal of maternal programming of stress responses
in adult offspring through methyl supplementation: altering epigenetic
marking later in life. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(47), 1104511054. doi:
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3652-05.2005
Weaver, I. C., Meaney, M. J., & Szyf, M. (2006). Maternal care effects on the
hippocampal transcriptome and anxiety-mediated behaviors in the
offspring that are reversible in adulthood. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences U S A, 103(9), 34803485. doi: 10.1073/
pnas.0507526103
Wells, J. C. (2003). The thrifty phenotype hypothesis: thrifty offspring or thrifty
mother? Journal of Theoretical Biology, 221(1), 143161.
Wilkinson, M. B., Xiao, G., Kumar, A., LaPlant, Q., Renthal, W., Sikder, D., . . .
Nestler, E. J. (2009). Imipramine treatment and resiliency exhibit similar
chromatin regulation in the mouse nucleus accumbens in depression
models. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(24), 78207832. doi: 10.1523/
JNEUROSCI.0932-09.2009
Wolffe, A. P., & Matzke, M. A. (1999). Epigenetics: regulation through repression.
Science, 286(5439), 481486.
The Nature-Nurture
Question
Eric Turkheimer
University of Virginia
nobaproject.com
Abstract
People have a deep intuition about what has been called the naturenurture
question. Some aspects of our behavior feel as though they originate in our
genetic makeup, while others feel like the result of our upbringing or our own
hard work. The scientific field of behavior genetics attempts to study these
differences empirically, either by examining similarities among family members
with different degrees of genetic relatedness, or, more recently, by studying
differences in the DNA of people with different behavioral traits. The scientific
methods that have been developed are ingenious, but often inconclusive. Many
of the difficulties encountered in the empirical science of behavior genetics turn
out to be conceptual, and our intuitions about nature and nurture get more
complicated the harder we think about them. In the end, it is an
oversimplification to ask how genetic some particular behavior is. Genes and
environments always combine to produce behavior, and the real science is in
the discovery of how they combine for a given behavior.
Learning Objectives
Know the major research designs that can be used to study naturenurture
questions.
Introduction
Three related problems at the borderline of philosophy and empirical science
are fundamental to humans understanding of our relationship to the natural
world: the mindbody problem, the free will problem, and the naturenurture
problem. These great questions have much in common. Everyone, even without
reference to science or formal philosophy, can generate intuitions about them
via introspection and casual observation of the world. Yet, our intuitions about
our relationship with the physical and biological world often feel incomplete
and half-seen. We are in control of our actions in some ways, yet bound to our
bodies in others; it seems obvious that our consciousness is some kind of
property of our physical brains, still it often feels as though our awareness floats
free of raw physicality. This peculiar combination of easy, but incomplete access
to our relationship with nature leaves us fascinated and a little obsessed, like
a cat that climbs into a paper bag and then out again, over and over, mystified
every time by a relationship between inner and outer that it can glimpse but
cant quite grasp.
That some characteristics are inborn while others are acquired is a
fundamental and profound human intuition, and it is worth crediting its
importance and validity before we begin to pick it apart. Of the three great
questions about humans relationships with the natural world, only nature
nurture is sometimes called a debate. In the history of psychology, no other
question has generated so much disagreement and moral indignation: The
most fundamental reason we are so fascinated with naturenurture is that our
most important moral judgments seem to depend on it. We may admire the
athletic skills of a great basketball player, but his height is simply a gift, a payoff
in the genetic lottery in which we have all been involuntarily entered. For the
same reason no one would condemn a short person, much less someone with
a real congenital disability: It is, to state the obvious, not their fault. But we do
credit the concert oboist for her skills, and perhaps her parents and community
nobaproject.com - The Nature-Nurture Question
346
347
monozygotic (MZ), twins result from a single zygote and have the same DNA.
They are essentially clones. Fraternal, or dizygotic (DZ), twins develop from two
zygotes. Fraternal twins are ordinary siblings who happen to have been born
at the same time. They share 50% of their DNA. To analyze naturenurture using
twins, we compare the similarity of MZ and DZ pairs. Identical twins,
unsurprisingly, are almost perfectly similar for height. The heights of fraternal
twins are like any other sibling pairs: more similar to each other than to people
from other families, but hardly identical. This difference in similarity tells us
something about the role genetics plays in the determination of height. Now
consider speaking Spanish. If one identical twin speaks Spanish at home, the
nobaproject.com - The Nature-Nurture Question
348
co-twin with whom she is raised almost certainly does too. But in this case, the
same would be true for a pair of fraternal twins raised together. For languagespeaking, fraternal twins are just as similar as identical twins, so it appears that
the additional genetic similarity of identical twins isnt making much difference.
Twin and adoption designs have much in common; in fact, they are two
instances of a much broader class of methods in which similarity among
individuals is analyzed in terms of how biologically related they are, a scientific
discipline called quantitative genetics. We can do these studies with siblings
and half-siblings, cousins, or with twins who have been separated at birth and
raised separately (Bouchard, Lykken, McGue, & Segal, 1990; such twins are very
rare and play a smaller role than is commonly believed in the science of nature
nurture), or with entire pedigrees of extended families (see Plomin, DeFries,
Knopik, & Neiderhiser, 2012, for a complete introduction to research methods
relevant to naturenurture).
For better or for worse, our thinking about naturenurture has been
intensified because the methods of quantitative genetics produce a number,
called a heritability coefficient, varying from 0 to 1, that appears to provide a
single measure of the role of genetics in a trait. In a general way, a heritability
coefficient measures how strongly differences among individuals for a trait are
related to differences among their genes. But beware: The previous sentences
are qualified with appears to and in a general way because heritability
coefficients, although simple to compute, are deceptively difficult to interpret.
Nevertheless, numbers that seem to provide simple answers to complicated
questions have a strong draw on the human mind, and a great deal of time has
been spent discussing whether the heritability of intelligence or personality or
depression is equal to this number or that.
The final reason naturenurture has continued to fascinate us is that we live
in an era of great scientific discovery in genetics, the equal, certainly, of the time
of Galileo, Copernicus, and Newton with regard to astronomy and physics. When
Francis Galton first started thinking about naturenurture in the late-19th
nobaproject.com - The Nature-Nurture Question
349
century he was very influenced by his cousin, Charles Darwin, but genetics per
se was unknown. Mendels famous work with peas, conducted at about the
same time, went undiscovered for 20 years; quantitative genetics was
developed in the 1920s; DNA discovered by Watson and Crick in the 1950s; the
human genome was completely sequenced at the turn of the 21st century; and
we are now on the cusp of a time when it will be possible to obtain the DNA
sequence of anyone at relatively low cost. No one knows what this new genetic
knowledge will mean for the study of naturenurture, but as we will see in the
next section, answers to naturenurture questions have turned out to be far
more difficult and mysterious than anyone imagined.
350
351
scores in the third grade and to conclude that reading out loud is important to
success in school. It may well be, but the study as described is inconclusive,
because there are genetic as well as environmental pathways between the
parenting practices of mothers and the abilities of children. To establish that
reading aloud causes success, a scientist can either study the problem in
adoptive families in which the genetic pathway is absent or by finding a way to
assign children at random to oral reading conditions.
In many other ways, however, the outcomes of naturenurture studies have
been less decisive. The most disappointing outcome has been that the sorting
of traits from more to less genetic hasnt worked out. In fact, everything has
turned out to be at least moderately heritable, and nothing has turned out to
be perfectly so, without much consistency as to which traits are more heritable
and which are less, once other considerations (such as how accurately the trait
can be measured) are taken into account (Turkheimer, 2000). The problem is
conceptual. The heritability coefficient, and, in fact, the whole quantitative
genetic apparatus that underlies it, does not do what our naturenurture
intuitions want it to do. We want to be informed about how important genes
and environment are to the development of a trait, but that turns out to be a
poorly specified question. First of all, both genes and environment are
absolutely important to every trait: No genes can develop in a vacuum, and
without genes the environment has nothing to work on. Even more important,
because naturenurture questions are always about differences among people,
the answer we obtain for a given trait depends not only on the trait itself, but
also on how and how much people differ on the trait in the population being
studied.
The classic example is the trait of having two arms. No one would doubt that
the development of arms is a profoundly biological and genetic process. But
fraternal twins are just as similar for two-armedness (that is to say, nearly
perfectly similar) as identical twins, and as a result the heritability of having two
arms is essentially zero. This result is not a tip-off that arm development is less
nobaproject.com - The Nature-Nurture Question
352
genetic than we had imagined, rather it occurs because people do not vary in
the genes related to arm development. To the extent people do differ in arm
number, it is likely the result of accidents and, therefore, environmental. For
this reason, we always have to be very careful when asking naturenurture
questions and especially when we try to express the answer in terms of a single
number. The heritability of a trait is not a property of that trait, it is a property
of the trait in a particular context in which relevant genes and environments
vary in some ways but not in others.
Another difficulty with the heritability coefficient as an outcome of nature
nurture studies is that it presumes that it is meaningful to divide up differences
in a trait into two portions, genes and environment, which can be added
together to obtain the total variability. Conceptually, this is a little like asking
how much of the experience of a symphony is attributable to the horns and
how much to the stringsthe ways in which instruments or genes combine is
more complicated than that. More formally, it turns out to be the case that for
many traits genetic differences affect behavior under some environmental
circumstances but not others, a phenomenon called gene-environment
interaction, or G x E. In one well-known example, Caspi et al. (2002) showed
that among maltreated children, those who carried a particular allele of the
MAOA gene showed a predisposition to violence and antisocial behavior, while
those with other alleles did not. The gene had no effect in children who had not
been maltreated. Making matters even more complex are very recent studies
of what is known as epigenetics (see chapter, Epigenetics), a process in which
the DNA itself can modified by environmental events, and those changes
transmitted to children.
Another question we sometimes want to answer when we think about
naturenurture is how susceptible a trait is to being changed, how malleable it
is, or whether we have a choice about it. But, these questions are also much
more complex than they appear at first. Phenylketonuria is an inborn error of
metabolism caused by a single gene; it prevents the body from metabolizing
nobaproject.com - The Nature-Nurture Question
353
354
behaviors but not others. So it is with nature and nurture. What at first appears
to be a straightforward distinction that might be indexed with a single number
becomes more and more multifaceted the closer we examine it. The many
questions we can ask about the relationships among genes, environments, and
human traitshow sensitive are traits to environmental change, and how
common are the environments to which they are sensitive; whether parents or
the broader cultural environment are more relevant; how sensitive traits are to
differences in genetic endowment, and how much do the relevant genes vary
in a particular population; whether a single gene or a great many genes are
involved; whether a trait is more easily described in genetic or more complex
behavioral termsall these questions may have different answers, and the
answer to one tells us little about the answers to the others.
Once we learn the profound and wide-ranging effects of genetic differences
on all human characteristics, especially behavioral ones, it is tempting to
conclude that our cultural, ethical, legal, and personal ways of thinking about
ourselves will have to undergo profound changes to take genetic effects into
account. Perhaps criminal proceedings will have to consider genetic
background. Parents, presented with the genetic sequence of their children,
will be faced with difficult decisions about reproduction. These hopes or fears
are often exaggerated. In some ways, our thinking may need to change,
especially if it was once informed by a superficially empirical reading of the
great American principle that all men are created equal. Human beings differ,
and like all evolved organisms they differ genetically. The Declaration of
Independence predates Darwin and Mendel, but it is hard to imagine that
Jefferson-- whose genius encompassed botany as well as moral philosophy-would have been alarmed to learn about the genetic diversity of organisms. It
must be remembered that for almost all human behavioral characteristics,
modern genetics has taught us that the genesis of human behavior is too
complex to be predicted from even the most complete genetic information,
unless one happens to have an identical twin. The science of nature and nurture
nobaproject.com - The Nature-Nurture Question
355
has demonstrated that genetic differences among people are integral to human
moral equality, freedom, and self-determination, not opposed to them. As
Mordecai Kaplan said about the role of the past in Jewish theology, genetics
gets a vote, not a veto, in the determination of human behavior. We should
indulge our fascination with naturenurture while resisting the temptation to
oversimplify it.
356
Outside Resources
Web: Institute for Behavioral Genetics
http://www.colorado.edu/ibg/
Discussion Questions
1. Is your personality more like one of your parents than the other? If you have
a sibling, is his or her personality like yours? In your family, how did these
similarities and differences develop? What do you think caused them?
2. Can you think of a human characteristic for which genetic differences would
play almost no role? Defend your choice.
3. Do you think the time will come when we will be able to predict almost
everything about someone by examining their DNA on the day they are
born?
4. Identical twins are more similar than fraternal twins for the trait of
aggressiveness, as well as for criminal behavior. Do these facts have
implications for the courtroom? If it can be shown that a violent criminal
had violent parents, should it make a difference in culpability or sentencing?
Vocabulary
Adoption study
A behavior genetic research method that involves comparison of adopted
children to their adoptive and biological parents.
Behavioral genetics
The empirical science of how genes and environments combine to generate
behavior.
Heritability coefficient
An easily misinterpreted statistical construct that purports to measure the role
of genetics in the explanation of differences among individuals.
Quantitative genetics
Scientific and mathematical methods for inferring genetic and environmental
processes based on the degree of genetic and environmental similarity among
organisms.
Twin studies
A behavior genetic research method that involves comparison of the similarity
of identical (monozygotic; MZ) and fraternal (dizygotic; DZ) twins.
Reference List
Bouchard, T. J., Lykken, D. T., McGue, M., & Segal, N. L. (1990). Sources of human
psychological differences: The Minnesota study of twins reared apart.
Science, 250(4978), 223228.
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., Taylor, A. & Poulton,
R. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children.
Science, 297(5582), 851854.
Plomin, R., Corley, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1990). Individual differences
in television viewing in early childhood: Nature as well as nurture.
Psychological Science, 1(6), 371377.
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., Knopik, V. S., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2012). Behavioral
genetics. New York, NY: Worth Publishers.
Scott, J. P., & Fuller, J. L. (1998). Genetics and the social behavior of the dog.
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Turkheimer, E. (2000). Three laws of behavior genetics and what they mean.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9(5), 160164.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. The Nature-Nurture Question by Eric Turkheimer
is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Topic 3
Sensation and Perception
Vision
Simona Buetti & Alejandro Lleras
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Vision is the sensory modality that transforms light into a psychological
experience of the world around you, with minimal bodily effort. This chapter
provides an overview of the most significant steps in this transformation and
strategies that your brain uses to achieve this visual understanding of the
environment.
Learning Objectives
Describe how the eye transforms light information into neural energy.
Describe how the visual system has adapted to deal with different lighting
conditions.
What Is Vision?
Think about the spectacle of a starry night. You look up at the sky, and thousands
of photons from distant stars come crashing into your retina, a light-sensitive
structure at the back of your eyeball. These photons are millions of years old
and have survived a trip across the universe, only to run into one of your
photoreceptors. Tough luck: in one thousandth of a second, this little bit of light
energy becomes the fuel to a photochemical reaction known as
photoactivation. The light energy becomes neural energy and triggers a
cascade of neural activity that, a few hundredths of a second later, will result
in your becoming aware of that distant star. You and the universe united by
photons. That is the amazing power of vision. Light brings the world to you.
Without moving, you know whats out there. You can recognize friends coming
to meet you before you are able to hear them coming, ripe fruits from green
ones on trees without having to taste them and before reaching out to grab
them. You can also tell how quickly a ball is moving in your direction (Will it hit
you? Can you hit it?).
How does all of that happen? First, light enters the eyeball through a tiny
hole known as the pupil and, thanks to the refractive properties of your cornea
and lens, this light signal gets projected sharply into the retina (see Outside
Resources for a detailed description of the eye structure). There, light is
transduced into neural energy by about 200 million photoreceptor cells. This
is where the information carried by the light about distant objects and colors
starts being encoded by our brain. There are two different types of
photoreceptors: rods and cones. Rods give us sensitivity under dim lighting
conditions and allow us to see at night. Cones allow us to see fine details in
bright light and give us the sensation of color. Cones are tightly packed around
the fovea (the central region of the retina behind your pupil) and more sparsely
elsewhere. Rods populate the periphery (the region surrounding the fovea) and
are almost absent from the fovea.
nobaproject.com - Vision
366
But vision is far more complex than just catching photons. The information
encoded by the photoreceptors undergoes a rapid and continuous set of ever
more complex analysis so that, eventually, you can make sense of whats out
there. At the fovea, visual information is encoded separately from tiny portions
of the world (each about half the width of a human hair viewed at arms length)
so that eventually the brain can reconstruct in great detail fine visual differences
from locations at which you are directly looking. This fine level of encoding
requires lots of light and it is slow going (neurally speaking). In contrast, in the
periphery, there is a different encoding strategy: detail is sacrificed in exchange
for sensitivity. Information is summed across larger sections of the world. This
aggregation occurs quickly and allows you to detect dim signals under very low
levels of light, as well as detect sudden movements in your peripheral vision.
367
light back to you, as will two points on the table. On the other hand, two points
that fall on either side of the boundary contour between your hand and the
table will reflect very different light.
The fact that the brain is interested in coding contrast in the world reveals
something deeply important about the forces that drove the evolution of our
brain: encoding the absolute amount of light in the world tells us little about
what is out there. But if your brain can detect the sudden appearance of a
difference in light somewhere in front of you, then it must be that something
new is there. That contrast signal is information. That information may
represent something that you like (food, a friend) or something dangerous
approaching (a tiger, a cliff). The rest of your visual system will work hard to
determine what that thing is, but as quickly as 10ms after light enters your eyes,
ganglion cells in your retinae have already encoded all the differences in light
from the world in front of you.
Contrast is so important that your neurons go out of their way not only to
encode differences in light but to exaggerate those differences for you, lest you
miss them. Neurons achieve this via a process known as lateral inhibition.
When a neuron is firing in response to light, it produces two signals: an output
signal to pass on to the next level in vision, and a lateral signal to inhibit all
neurons that are next to it. This makes sense on the assumption that nearby
neurons are likely responding to the same light coming from nearby locations,
so this information is somewhat redundant. The magnitude of the lateral
inhibitory signal a neuron produces is proportional to the excitatory input that
neuron receives: the more a neuron fires, the stronger the inhibition it produces.
Figure 1 illustrates how lateral inhibition amplifies contrast signals at the edges
of surfaces.
nobaproject.com - Vision
368
Figure 1. Illustration of Lateral Inhibition at work. The top of the figure shows a black stripe
on a white background. The first row of circles illustrates photoreceptors responding in a
graded fashion: the more light hits them, the more they fire. The numbers inside the circles
represent how much these cells are firing, and the thickness of lines is also meant to illustrate
the strength of neural firing. These photoreceptors activate the next layer of neurons in the
retina: bipolar cells. These cells produce lateral inhibition signals, depicted by the horizontal
lines that end with a small circle. The inhibition signals are proportional (here, 10% for ease)
to the excitatory input they receive. Cells receiving 100 units will inhibit their neighbors by 10
units. Cells receiving 20 units will inhibit their neighbors by 2 units. The output of a bipolar
cell will be determined by the input it receives minus all the lateral inhibition signals from its
neighbors. As a result of the inhibition, notice how on the bright side of the edges, the firing
rates are the highest (88) compared to nearby neurons just coding bright light (80). These
higher values near the edge occur because these cells receive a comparatively small amount
of inhibition from their dark-side neighbor (-2). Similarly, on the dark side of the edge, the
firing rates are the lowest (8) of all the dark region (16) because these cells receive a
comparatively large amount of inhibition from their bright-side neighbor (-10). Overall, the
nobaproject.com - Vision
369
image is coded as a black stripe surrounded by brighter light, but now, thanks to lateral
inhibition, all the edges in the image have been emphasized (enhanced), as illustrated by the
perceived luminance profile at the bottom of the image.
370
A similar, but more subtle, adjustment occurs when the change in lighting
is not so drastic. Think about your experience of reading a book at night in your
bed compared to reading outdoors: the room may feel to you fairly well
illuminated (enough so you can read) but the light bulbs in your room are not
producing the billions of photons that you encounter outside. In both cases,
you feel that your experience is that of a well-lit environment. You dont feel
one experience as millions of times brighter than the other. This is because
vision (as much of perception) is not proportional: seeing twice as many photons
does not produce a sensation of seeing twice as bright a light. The visual system
tunes into the current experience by favoring a range of contrast values that is
most informative in that environment (Gardner et al., 2005). This is the concept
of contrast gain: the visual system determines the mean contrast in a scene
and represents values around that mean contrast best, while ignoring smaller
contrast differences. (See the Outside Resources section for a demonstration.)
371
nobaproject.com - Vision
372
Now that you have a basic understanding of how your visual system works,
you can ask yourself the question: why do you have two eyes? Everything that
we discussed so far could be computed with information coming from a single
eye. So why two? Looking at the animal kingdom gives us a clue. Animals who
tend to be prey have eyes located on opposite sides of their skull. This allows
them to detect predators whenever one appears anywhere around them.
Humans, like most predators, have two eyes pointing in the same direction,
encoding almost the exact scene twice. This redundancy gives us a binocular
advantage: having two eyes not only provides you with two chances at catching
a signal in front of you, but the minute difference in perspective that you get
from each eye is used by your brain to reconstruct the sense of threedimensional space. You can get an estimate of how far distant objects are from
you, their size, and their volume. This is no easy feat: the signal in each eye is
a two-dimensional projection of the world, like two separate pictures drawn
upon your retinae. Yet, your brain effortlessly provides you with a sense of
depth by combining those two signals. This 3-D reconstruction process also
relies heavily on all the knowledge you acquired through experience about
nobaproject.com - Vision
373
spatial information. For instance, your visual system learns to interpret how
the volume, distance, and size of objects change as they move closer or farther
from you. (See the Outside Resources section for demonstrations.)
374
375
your head the same red inside your moms head? That is an almost impossible
question to answer that has been debated by philosophers for millennia, yet
recent data suggests that there might in fact be cultural differences in the way
we perceive color. As it turns out, not all cultures categorize colors in the same
way, for example. And some groups see different shades of what we in the
Western world would call the same color, as categorically different colors. The
Berinmo tribe in New Guinea, for instance, appear to experience green shades
that denote leaves that are alive as belonging to an entirely different color
category than the sort of green shades that denote dying leaves. Russians, too,
appear to experience light and dark shades of blue as different categories of
colors, in a way that most Westerners do not. Further, current brain imaging
research suggests that peoples brains change (increase in white-matter
volume) when they learn new color categories! These are intriguing and
suggestive findings, for certain, that seem to indicate that our cultural
environment may in fact have some (small) but definite impact on how people
use and experience colors across the globe.
nobaproject.com - Vision
376
nobaproject.com - Vision
377
Figure 3. Demonstration of color aftereffect. Look at the black dot in the top panel for 15
seconds. Then quickly move your eyes to the center dot in the bottom panel. The normally
gray circles will appear tinted in the opponent color of each corresponding circle. For example,
the top left circle appears green, which is opponent to the red color you saw on the
corresponding circle on the top panel.
378
Concluding Remarks
We are at an exciting moment in our scientific understanding of vision. We have
just begun to get a functional understanding of the visual system. It is not
sufficiently evolved for us to recreate artificial visual systems (i.e., we still cannot
make robots that see and understand light signals as we do), but we are getting
there. Just recently, major breakthroughs in vision science have allowed
researchers to significantly improve retinal prosthetics: photosensitive circuits
that can be implanted on the back of the eyeball of blind people that connect
to visual areas of the brain and have the capacity to partially restore a visual
experience to these patients (Nirenberg & Pandarinath, 2012). And using
functional magnetic brain imaging, we can now decode from your brain activity
the images that you saw in your dreams while you were asleep (Horikawa,
Tamaki, Miyawaki, & Kamitani, 2013)! Yet, there is still so much more to
understand. Consider this: if vision is a construction process that takes time,
whatever we see now is no longer what is front of us. Yet, humans can do
amazing time-sensitive feats like hitting a 90-mph fastball in a baseball game.
It appears then that a fundamental function of vision is not just to know what
is happening around you now, but actually to make an accurate inference about
what you are about to see next (Enns & Lleras, 2008), so that you can keep up
with the world. Understanding how this future-oriented, predictive function of
vision is achieved in the brain is probably the next big challenge in this
fascinating realm of research.
nobaproject.com - Vision
379
Outside Resources
Video: Acquired knowledge and its impact on our three-dimensional
interpretation of the world - 3D Street Art
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=GwNeukAmxJw
Video: Acquired knowledge and its impact on our three-dimensional
interpretation of the world - Anamorphic Illusions
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=tBNHPk-Lnkk
Video: Acquired knowledge and its impact on our three-dimensional
interpretation of the world - Optical Illusion
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=YjmHofJ2da0&feature=related
Web: Amazing library with visual phenomena and optical illusions, explained
http://michaelbach.de/ot/index.html
Web: Anatomy of the eye
http://www.eyecareamerica.org/eyecare/anatomy/
Web: Demonstration of contrast gain adaptation
http://www.med.yale.edu/neurobio/mccormick/contrast/wave.html
Web: Demonstration of illusory contours and lateral inhibition. Mach bands
http://michaelbach.de/ot/lum-MachBands/index.html
Web: Demonstration of illusory contrast and lateral inhibition. The Hermann
grid
http://michaelbach.de/ot/lum_herGrid/
Discussion Questions
1. When running in the dark, it is recommended that you never look straight
at the ground. Why? What would be a better strategy to avoid obstacles?
2. The majority of ganglion cells in the eye specialize in detecting drops in the
amount of light coming from a given location. That is, they increase their
firing rate when they detect less light coming from a specific location. Why
might the absence of light be more important than the presence of light?
Why would it be evolutionarily advantageous to code this type of
information?
3. There is a hole in each one of your eyeballs called the optic disk. This is
where veins enter the eyeball and where neurons (the axons of the ganglion
cells) exit the eyeball. Why do you not see two holes in the world all the
time? Close one eye now. Why do you not see a hole in the world now? To
experience a blind spot, follow the instructions in this website: http://
michaelbach.de/ot/cog_blindSpot/index.html
4. Imagine you were given the task of testing the color-perception abilities of
a newly discovered species of monkeys in the South Pacific. How would you
go about it?
5. An important aspect of emotions is that we sense them in ourselves much
in the same way as we sense other perceptions like vision. Can you think of
an example where the concept of contrast gain can be used to understand
peoples responses to emotional events?
Vocabulary
Binocular advantage
Benefits from having two eyes as opposed to a single eye.
Cones
Photoreceptors that operate in lighted environments and can encode fine visual
details. There are three different kinds (S or blue, M or green and L or red) that
are each sensitive to slightly different types of light. Combined, these three
types of cones allow you to have color vision.
Contrast
Relative difference in the amount and type of light coming from two nearby
locations.
Contrast gain
Process where the sensitivity of your visual system can be tuned to be most
sensitive to the levels of contrast that are most prevalent in the environment.
Dark adaptation
Process that allows you to become sensitive to very small levels of light, so that
you can actually see in the near-absence of light.
Lateral inhibition
A signal produced by a neuron aimed at suppressing the response of nearby
neurons.
Photoactivation
A photochemical reaction that occurs when light hits photoreceptors, producing
a neural signal.
Rods
Photoreceptors that are very sensitive to light and are mostly responsible for
night vision.
Synesthesia
The blending of two or more sensory experiences, or the automatic activation
of a secondary (indirect) sensory experience due to certain aspects of the
primary (direct) sensory stimulation.
Trichromacy theory
Theory that proposes that all of your color perception is fundamentally based
on the combination of three (not two, not four) different color signals.
Vestibulo-ocular reflex
Coordination of motion information with visual information that allows you to
maintain your gaze on an object while you move.
What pathway
Pathway of neural processing in the brain that is responsible for your ability to
recognize what is around you.
Where-and-How pathway
Pathway of neural processing in the brain that is responsible for you knowing
where things are in the world and how to interact with them.
Reference List
Enns, J. T., & Lleras, A. (2008). New evidence for prediction in human vision.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12 327333.
Gardner, J. L., Sun, P., Waggoner, R. A. , Ueno, K., Tanaka, K., & Cheng, K. (2005).
Contrast adaptation and representation in human early visual cortex.
Neuron, 47, 607620.
Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception
and action. Trends in Neuroscience, 15, 2025.
Helmholtz, H. von. (1867). Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik. Leipzig:
Leopold Voss.
Hering, E. (1892). Grundzu ge der Lehre vom Lichtsinn. Berlin, Germany:
Springer.
Horikawa, T., Tamaki, M., Miyawaki, Y., & Kamitani, Y. (2013). Neural decoding
of visual imagery during sleep. Science, 340(6132), 639642.
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction, and
functional architecture in the cats visual cortex. Journal of Physiology, 160,
106154.
Hurley, J. B. (2002). Shedding light on adaptation. Journal of General Physiology,
119, 125128.
Nirenberg, S., & Pandarinath, C. (2012). Retinal prosthetic strategy with the
capacity to restore normal vision. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Vision by Simona Buetti and Alejandro Lleras is
licensed
under
the
Creative
Commons
Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0
Hearing
Andrew J. Oxenham
University of Minnesota
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Hearing allows us to perceive the world of acoustic vibrations all around us,
and provides us with our most important channels of communication. This
chapter reviews the basic mechanisms of hearing, beginning with the anatomy
and physiology of the ear and a brief review of the auditory pathways up to the
auditory cortex. An outline of the basic perceptual attributes of sound, including
loudness, pitch, and timbre, is followed by a review of the principles of tonotopic
organization, established in the cochlea. An overview of masking and frequency
selectivity is followed by a review of the perception and neural mechanisms
underlying spatial hearing. Finally, an overview is provided of auditory scene
analysis, which tackles the important question of how the auditory system is
able to make sense of the complex mixtures of sounds that are encountered
in everyday acoustic environments.
Learning Objectives
Describe the structure and general function of the auditory pathways from
the outer ear to the auditory cortex.
Introduction
Hearing forms a crucial part of our everyday life. Most of our communication
with others, via speech or music, reaches us through the ears. Indeed, a saying,
often attributed to Helen Keller, is that blindness separates us from things, but
deafness separates us from people. The ears respond to acoustic information,
or soundtiny and rapid variations in air pressure. Sound waves travel from
the source and produce pressure variations in the listeners ear canals, causing
the eardrums (or tympanic membranes) to vibrate. This chapter provides an
overview of the events that follow, which convert these simple mechanical
vibrations into our rich experience known as hearing, or auditory perception.
Loudness
The most direct physical correlate of loudness is sound intensity (or sound
pressure) measured close to the eardrum. However, many other factors also
influence the loudness of a sound, including its frequency content, its duration,
and the context in which it is presented. Some of the earliest psychophysical
studies of auditory perception, going back more than a century, were aimed at
examining the relationships between perceived loudness, the physical sound
intensity, and the just-noticeable differences in loudness (Fechner, 1860;
Stevens, 1957). A great deal of time and effort has been spent refining various
nobaproject.com - Hearing
392
Pitch
Pitch plays a crucial role in acoustic communication. Pitch variations over time
provide the basis of melody for most types of music; pitch contours in speech
provide us with important prosodic information in non-tone languages, such
as English, and help define the meaning of words in tone languages, such as
Mandarin Chinese. Pitch is essentially the perceptual correlate of waveform
periodicity, or repetition rate: The faster a waveform repeats over time, the
higher is its perceived pitch. The most common pitch-evoking sounds are known
as harmonic complex tones. They are complex because they consist of more
than one frequency, and they are harmonic because the frequencies are all
integer multiples of a common fundamental frequency (F0). For instance, a
harmonic complex tone with a F0 of 100 Hz would also contain energy at
frequencies of 200, 300, 400 Hz, and so on. These higher frequencies are known
as harmonics or overtones, and they also play an important role in determining
the pitch of a sound. In fact, even if the energy at the F0 is absent or masked,
we generally still perceive the remaining sound to have a pitch corresponding
to the F0. This phenomenon is known as the pitch of the missing fundamental,
and it has played an important role in the formation of theories and models
nobaproject.com - Hearing
393
about pitch (de Cheveign, 2005). We hear pitch with sufficient accuracy to
perceive melodies over a range of F0s from about 30 Hz (Pressnitzer, Patterson,
& Krumbholz, 2001) up to about 45 kHz (Attneave & Olson, 1971; Oxenham,
Micheyl, Keebler, Loper, & Santurette, 2011). This range also corresponds quite
well to the range covered by musical instruments; for instance, the modern
grand piano has notes that extend from 27.5 Hz to 4,186 Hz. We are able to
discriminate changes in frequency above 5,000 Hz, but we are no longer very
accurate in recognizing melodies or judging musical intervals.
Timbre
Timbre refers to the quality of sound, and is often described using words such
as bright, dull, harsh, and hollow. Technically, timbre includes anything that
allows us to distinguish two sounds that have the same loudness, pitch, and
duration. For instance, a violin and a piano playing the same note sound very
different, based on their sound quality or timbre.
An important aspect of timbre is the spectral content of a sound. Sounds
with more high-frequency energy tend to sound brighter, tinnier, or harsher
than sounds with more low-frequency content, which might be described as
deep, rich, or dull. Other important aspects of timbre include the temporal
envelope (or outline) of the sound, especially how it begins and ends. For
instance, a piano has a rapid onset, or attack, produced by the hammer striking
the string, whereas the attack of a clarinet note can be much more gradual.
Artificially changing the onset of a piano note by, for instance, playing a recording
backwards, can dramatically alter its character so that it is no longer
recognizable as a piano note. In general, the overall spectral content and the
temporal envelope can provide a good first approximation to any sound, but it
turns out that subtle changes in the spectrum over time (or spectro-temporal
variations) are crucial in creating plausible imitations of natural musical
instruments (Risset & Wessel, 1999).
nobaproject.com - Hearing
394
Our auditory perception depends on how sound is processed through the ear.
The ear can be divided into three main partsthe outer, middle, and inner ear
(see Figure 1). The outer ear consists of the pinna (the visible part of the ear,
with all its unique folds and bumps), the ear canal (or auditory meatus), and
the tympanic membrane. Of course, most of us have two functioning ears,
which turn out to be particularly useful when we are trying to figure out where
a sound is coming from. As discussed below in the section on spatial hearing,
our brain can compare the subtle differences in the signals at the two ears to
localize sounds in space. However, this trick does not always help: for instance,
a sound directly in front or directly behind you will not produce a difference
nobaproject.com - Hearing
395
between the ears. In these cases, the filtering produced by the pinnae helps us
localize sounds and resolve potential front-back and up-down confusions. More
generally, the folds and bumps of the pinna produce distinct peaks and dips in
the frequency response that depend on the location of the sound source. The
brain then learns to associate certain patterns of spectral peaks and dips with
certain spatial locations. Interestingly, this learned association remains
malleable, or plastic, even in adulthood. For instance, a study that altered the
pinnae using molds found that people could learn to use their new ears
accurately within a matter of a few weeks (Hofman, Van Riswick, & Van Opstal,
1998). Because of the small size of the pinna, these kinds of acoustic cues are
only found at high frequencies, above about 2 kHz. At lower frequencies, the
sound is basically unchanged whether it comes from above, in front, or below.
The ear canal itself is a tube that helps to amplify sound in the region from
about 1 to 4 kHza region particularly important for speech communication.
The middle ear consists of an air-filled cavity, which contains the three
middle-ear bones, known as the incus, malleus, and stapes, or hammer, mallet,
and stirrup, because of their respective shapes. They have the distinction of
being the smallest bones in the body. Their primary function is to transmit the
vibrations from the tympanic membrane to the oval window of the cochlea and,
via a form of lever action, to better match the impedance of the air surrounding
the tympanic membrane with that of the fluid within the cochlea.
The inner ear includes the cochlea, encased in the temporal bone of the
skull, in which the mechanical vibrations of sound are transduced into neural
signals that are processed by the brain. The cochlea is a spiral-shaped structure
that is filled with fluid. Along the length of the spiral runs the basilar membrane,
which vibrates in response to the pressure differences produced by vibrations
of the oval window. Sitting on the basilar membrane is the organ of Corti, which
runs the entire length of the basilar membrane from the base (by the oval
window) to the apex (the tip of the spiral). The organ of Corti includes three
rows of outer hair cells and one row of inner hair cells. The hair cells sense the
nobaproject.com - Hearing
396
vibrations by way of their tiny hairs, or stereocillia. The outer hair cells seem to
function to mechanically amplify the sound-induced vibrations, whereas the
inner hair cells form synapses with the auditory nerve and transduce those
vibrations into action potentials, or neural spikes, which are transmitted along
the auditory nerve to higher centers of the auditory pathways.
One of the most important principles of hearingfrequency analysisis
established in the cochlea. In a way, the action of the cochlea can be likened to
that of a prism: the many frequencies that make up a complex sound are broken
down into their constituent frequencies, with low frequencies creating maximal
basilar-membrane vibrations near the apex of the cochlea and high frequencies
creating maximal basilar-membrane vibrations nearer the base of the cochlea.
This decomposition of sound into its constituent frequencies, and the
frequency-to-place mapping, or tonotopic representation, is a major
organizational principle of the auditory system, and is maintained in the neural
representation of sounds all the way from the cochlea to the primary auditory
cortex. The decomposition of sound into its constituent frequency components
is part of what allows us to hear more than one sound at a time. In addition to
representing frequency by place of excitation within the cochlea, frequencies
are also represented by the timing of spikes within the auditory nerve. This
property, known as phase locking, is crucial in comparing time-of-arrival
differences of waveforms between the two ears (see the section on spatial
hearing, below).
Unlike vision, where the primary visual cortex (or V1) is considered an early
stage of processing, auditory signals go through many stages of processing
before they reach the primary auditory cortex, located in the temporal lobe.
Although we have a fairly good understanding of the electromechanical
properties of the cochlea and its various structures, our understanding of the
processing accomplished by higher stages of the auditory pathways remains
somewhat sketchy. With the possible exception of spatial localization and
neurons tuned to certain locations in space (Harper & McAlpine, 2004; Knudsen
nobaproject.com - Hearing
397
& Konishi, 1978), there is very little consensus on the how, what, and where of
auditory feature extraction and representation. There is evidence for a pitch
center in the auditory cortex from both human neuroimaging studies (e.g.,
Griffiths, Buchel, Frackowiak, & Patterson, 1998; Penagos, Melcher, & Oxenham,
2004) and single-unit physiology studies (Bendor & Wang, 2005), but even here
there remain some questions regarding whether a single area of cortex is
responsible for coding single features, such as pitch, or whether the code is
more distributed (Walker, Bizley, King, & Schnupp, 2011).
398
Spatial Hearing
In contrast to vision, we have a 360 field of hearing. Our auditory acuity is,
however, at least an order of magnitude poorer than vision in locating an object
in space. Consequently, our auditory localization abilities are most useful in
alerting us and allowing us to orient towards sources, with our visual sense
generally providing the finer-grained analysis. Of course, there are differences
nobaproject.com - Hearing
399
between species, and some, such as barn owls and echolocating bats, have
developed highly specialized sound localization systems.
Our ability to locate sound sources in space is an impressive feat of neural
computation. The two main sources of information both come from a
comparison of the sounds at the two ears. The first is based on interaural time
differences (ITD) and relies on the fact that a sound source on the left will
generate sound that will reach the left ear slightly before it reaches the right
ear. Although sound is much slower than light, its speed still means that the
time of arrival differences between the two ears is a fraction of a millisecond.
The largest ITD we encounter in the real world (when sounds are directly to the
left or right of us) are only a little over half a millisecond. With some practice,
humans can learn to detect an ITD of between 10 and 20 s (i.e., 20 millionths
of a second) (Klump & Eady, 1956).
The second source of information is based in interaural level differences
(ILDs). At higher frequencies (higher than about 1 kHz), the head casts an
acoustic shadow, so that when a sound is presented from the left, the sound
level at the left ear is somewhat higher than the sound level at the right ear. At
very high frequencies, the ILD can be as much as 20 dB, and we are sensitive
to differences as small as 1 dB.
As mentioned briefly in the discussion of the outer ear, information
regarding the elevation of a sound source, or whether it comes from in front
or behind, is contained in high-frequency spectral details that result from the
filtering effects of the pinnae.
In general, we are most sensitive to ITDs at low frequencies (below about
1.5 kHz). At higher frequencies we can still perceive changes in timing based on
the slowly varying temporal envelope of the sound but not the temporal fine
structure (Bernstein & Trahiotis, 2002; Smith, Delgutte, & Oxenham, 2002),
perhaps because of a loss of neural phase-locking to the temporal fine structure
at high frequencies. In contrast, ILDs are most useful at high frequencies, where
the head shadow is greatest. This use of different acoustic cues in different
nobaproject.com - Hearing
400
frequency regions led to the classic and very early duplex theory of sound
localization (Rayleigh, 1907). For everyday sounds with a broad frequency
spectrum, it seems that our perception of spatial location is dominated by
interaural time differences in the low-frequency temporal fine structure
(Macpherson & Middlebrooks, 2002).
As with vision, our perception of distance depends to a large degree on
context. If we hear someone shouting at a very low sound level, we infer that
the shouter must be far away, based on our knowledge of the sound properties
of shouting. In rooms and other enclosed locations, the reverberation can also
provide information about distance: As a speaker moves further away, the direct
sound level decreases but the sound level of the reverberation remains about
the same; therefore, the ratio of direct-to-reverberant energy decreases
(Zahorik & Wightman, 2001).
401
these rules of thumb, sounds that are in close proximity, in time or frequency,
tend to be grouped together. Also, sounds that begin and end at the same time
tend to form a single auditory object. Interestingly, spatial location is not always
a strong or reliable grouping cue, perhaps because the location information
from individual frequency components is often ambiguous due to the effects
of reverberation. Several studies have looked into the relative importance of
different cues by trading off one cue against another. In some cases, this has
led to the discovery of interesting auditory illusions, where melodies that are
not present in the sounds presented to either ear emerge in the perception
(Deutsch, 1979), or where a sound element is perceptually lost in competing
perceptual organizations (Shinn-Cunningham, Lee, & Oxenham, 2007).
More recent attempts have used computational and neutrally based
approaches to uncover the mechanisms of auditory scene analysis (e.g., Elhilali,
Ma, Micheyl, Oxenham, & Shamma, 2009), and the field of computational
auditory scene analysis (CASA) has emerged in part as an effort to move towards
more principled, and less heuristic, approaches to understanding the parsing
and perception of complex auditory scenes (e.g., Wang & Brown, 2006). Solving
this problem will not only provide us with a better understanding of human
auditory perception, but may provide new approaches to smart hearing aids
and cochlear implants, as well as automatic speech recognition systems that
are more robust to background noise.
Conclusion
Hearing provides us with our most important connection to the people around
us. The intricate physiology of the auditory system transforms the tiny variations
in air pressure that reach our ear into the vast array of auditory experiences
that we perceive as speech, music, and sounds from the environment around
us. We are only beginning to understand the basic principles of neural coding
in higher stages of the auditory system, and how they relate to perception.
nobaproject.com - Hearing
402
nobaproject.com - Hearing
403
Outside Resources
Audio: Auditory Demonstrations from Richard Warrens lab at the University
of Wisconsin, Milwaukee
http://www4.uwm.edu/APL/demonstrations.html
Audio: Auditory Demonstrations. CD published by the Acoustical Society of
America (ASA). You can listen to the demonstrations here
http://www.feilding.net/sfuad/musi3012-01/demos/audio/
Web: Demonstrations and illustrations of cochlear mechanics can be found
here
http://lab.rockefeller.edu/hudspeth/graphicalSimulations
Web: More demonstrations and illustrations of cochlear mechanics
http://www.neurophys.wisc.edu/animations/
Discussion Questions
1. Based on the available acoustic cues, how good do you think we are at
judging whether a low-frequency sound is coming from in front of us or
behind us? How might we solve this problem in the real world?
2. Outer hair cells contribute not only to amplification but also to the frequency
tuning in the cochlea. What are some of the difficulties that might arise for
people with cochlear hearing loss, due to these two factors? Why do hearing
aids not solve all these problems?
3. Why do you think the auditory system has so many stages of processing
before the signals reach the auditory cortex, compared to the visual system?
Is there a difference in the speed of processing required?
Vocabulary
Cochlea
Snail-shell-shaped organ that transduces mechanical vibrations into neural
signals.
Interaural differences
Differences (usually in time or intensity) between the two ears.
Pinna
Visible part of the outer ear.
Tympanic membrane
Ear drum, which separates the outer ear from the middle ear.
Reference List
Attneave, F., & Olson, R. K. (1971). Pitch as a medium: A new approach to
psychophysical scaling. American Journal of Psychology, 84, 147166.
Bendor, D., & Wang, X. (2005). The neuronal representation of pitch in primate
auditory cortex. Nature, 436, 11611165.
Bernstein, L. R., & Trahiotis, C. (2002). Enhancing sensitivity to interaural delays
at high frequencies by using "transposed stimuli." Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America, 112, 10261036.
Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organization of
sound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Deutsch, D. (1979). Binaural integration of melodic patterns. Perception &
Psychophysics, 25, 399405.
Durlach, N. I., Mason, C. R., Kidd, G., Jr., Arbogast, T. L., Colburn, H. S., & ShinnCunningham, B. G. (2003). Note on informational masking. Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America, 113, 29842987.
Elhilali, M., Ma, L., Micheyl, C., Oxenham, A. J., & Shamma, S. (2009). Temporal
coherence in the perceptual organization and cortical representation of
auditory scenes. Neuron, 61, 317329.
Fechner, G. T. (1860). Elemente der Psychophysik (Vol. 1). Leipzig, Germany:
Breitkopf und Haertl.
Griffiths, T. D., Buchel, C., Frackowiak, R. S., & Patterson, R. D. (1998). Analysis
Moore, B. C. J., Glasberg, B. R., & Baer, T. (1997). A model for the prediction of
thresholds, loudness, and partial loudness. Journal of the Audio Engineering
Society, 45, 224240.
Oxenham, A. J., Micheyl, C., Keebler, M. V., Loper, A., & Santurette, S. (2011). Pitch
perception beyond the traditional existence region of pitch. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences USA, 108, 76297634.
Penagos, H., Melcher, J. R., & Oxenham, A. J. (2004). A neural representation of
pitch salience in non-primary human auditory cortex revealed with fMRI.
Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 68106815.
Pressnitzer, D., Patterson, R. D., & Krumbholz, K. (2001). The lower limit of
melodic pitch. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109, 20742084.
Rayleigh, L. (1907). On our perception of sound direction. Philosophical
Magazine, 13, 214232.
Risset, J. C., & Wessel, D. L. (1999). Exploration of timbre by analysis and
synthesis. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music (2nd ed., pp. 113
168): Academic Press.
Shinn-Cunningham, B. G., Lee, A. K., & Oxenham, A. J. (2007). A sound element
gets lost in perceptual competition. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences USA, 104, 1222312227.
Smith, Z. M., Delgutte, B., & Oxenham, A. J. (2002). Chimaeric sounds reveal
dichotomies in auditory perception. Nature, 416, 8790.
Stevens, S. S. (1957). On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review, 64, 153
181.
Walker, K. M., Bizley, J. K., King, A. J., & Schnupp, J. W. (2011). Multiplexed and
robust representations of sound features in auditory cortex. Journal of
Neuroscience, 31, 1456514576.
Wang, D., & Brown, G. J. (Eds.). (2006). Computational auditory scene analysis:
Principles, algorithms, and applications. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Watson, C. S., & Kelly, W. J. (1978). Informational masking in auditory patterns.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 64, S39.
Zahorik, P., & Wightman, F. L. (2001). Loudness constancy with varying sound
source distance. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 7883.
de Cheveign, A. (2005). Pitch perception models. In C. J. Plack, A. J. Oxenham,
A. N. Popper, & R. Fay (Eds.), Pitch: Neural coding and perception (pp. 169
233). New York, NY: Springer Verlag.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Hearing by Andrew J. Oxenham is licensed under
the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. To
view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.
en_US.
Abstract
Humans are omnivores (able to survive on many different foods).
The
omnivores dilemma is to identify foods that are healthy and avoid poisons.
Taste and smell cooperate to solve this dilemma. Stimuli for both taste and
smell are chemicals. Smell results from a biological system that essentially
permits the brain to store rough sketches of the chemical structures of odor
stimuli in the environment. Thus, people in very different parts of the world
can learn to like odors (paired with calories) or dislike odors (paired with nausea)
that they encounter in their worlds. Taste information is preselected (by the
nature of the receptors) to be relevant to nutrition. No learning is required; we
are born loving sweet and hating bitter. Taste inhibits a variety of other systems
in the brain. Taste damage releases that inhibition, thus intensifying sensations
like those evoked by fats in foods. Ear infections and tonsillectomies both can
damage taste. Adults who have experienced these conditions experience
intensified sensations from fats and enhanced palatability of high-fat foods.
This may explain why individuals who have had ear infections or tonsillectomies
tend to gain weight.
Learning Objectives
Explain the salient properties of taste and smell that help solve the
omnivores dilemma.
415
chemicals (e.g., bacon odor). The olfactory system creates an image for the
mixture and stores it in memory just as it does for the odor of a single molecule
(Shepherd, 2005).
Taste is simpler than olfaction. Bitter and sweet utilize GPCRs, just as
olfaction does, but the number of different receptors is much smaller. For
bitter, 25 receptors are tuned to different chemical structures (Meyerhof et al.,
2010). Such a system allows us to sense many different poisons.
Sweet is even simpler. The primary sweet receptor is composed of two
different G protein-coupled receptors; each of these two proteins ends in large
structures reminiscent of Venus flytraps. This complex receptor has multiple
sites that can bind different structures. The Venus flytrap endings open so that
even some very large molecules can fit inside and stimulate the receptor.
Bitter is inclusive (i.e., multiple receptors tuned to very different chemical
structures feed into common neurons). Sweet is exclusive. There are many
sugars with similar structures, but only three of these are particularly important
to humans (sucrose, glucose, and fructose). Thus, our sweet receptor tunes
out most sugars, leaving only the most important to stimulate the sweet
receptor. However, the ability of the sweet receptor to respond to some nonsugars presents us with one of the great mysteries of taste. Several non-sugar
molecules can stimulate the primary sweet receptor (e.g., saccharine,
aspartame, cyclamate).
416
is eaten later.
metabolism, thus making losing weight even harder. So why did nature give
us artificial sweeteners? We dont know.
One more mystery about sweet deserves comment. The discovery of the
sweet receptor was met with great excitement because many investigators had
searched for it for years. The fact that this complex receptor had multiple sites
to which different molecules could bind explained why many different
molecules taste sweet. However, this is actually a serious problem. No matter
what molecule stimulates this receptor, the neural output from that receptor
is the same. This would mean that the sweetness of all sweet substances would
have to be the same. Yet artificial sweeteners do not taste exactly like sugar.
The answer may lie in the fact that one of the two proteins that makes up the
receptor can act alone, but only strong concentrations of sugar stimulate this
isolated protein receptor. This permits the brain to distinguish between the
sweetness of sugar and the sweetness of non-sugar molecules.
Salty and sour are the simplest tastes; these stimuli ionize (break into
positively and negatively charged particles). The first event in the transduction
series is the movement of the positively charged particle through channels in
the taste cell membrane (Chaudhari & Roper, 2010).
The receptors
mediating salty taste are not mature at birth in humans, but when they are
mature a few weeks after birth, the baby likes dilute salt (although more
concentrated salt will evoke stinging sensations that will be avoided). Sour is
generally disliked (protecting against tissue damage from acid?), but to the
nobaproject.com - Taste and Smell
417
amazement of many parents, some young children appear to actually like the
sour candies available today; this may be related to the breadth of their
experience with fruits (Liem & Mennella, 2003). This hard-wired affect is the
most salient characteristic of taste and this is why we classify only those taste
qualities with hard-wired affect as basic tastes.
418
Those who have experienced a conditioned aversion may have found that
the dislike (even disgust) evoked when a flavor is paired with nausea can
generalize to the smell of the food alone (orthonasal olfaction). Some years
ago, Jeremy Wolfe and Linda Bartoshuk surveyed conditioned aversions among
college students and staff that had resulted from consuming foods/beverages
associated with nausea (Bartoshuk & Wolfe, 1990). In 29% of the aversions,
subjects reported that even the smell of the food/beverage had become
aversive. Other properties of food objects can become aversive as well. In one
unusual case, an aversion to cheese crackers generalized to vanilla wafers
apparently because the containers were similar.
Conditioned aversions
function to protect us from ingesting a food that our brains associate with
illness. Conditioned preferences are harder to form, but they help us learn
what is safe to eat.
Is the affect associated with olfaction ever hard-wired? Pheromones are
said to be olfactory molecules that evoke specific behaviors. Googling human
pheromone will take you to websites selling various sprays that are supposed
to make one more sexually appealing. However, careful research does not
support such claims in humans or any other mammals (Doty, 2010). For
example, amniotic fluid was at one time believed to contain a pheromone that
attracted rat pups to their mothers nipples so they could suckle. Early interest
in identifying the molecule that acted as that pheromone gave way to
understanding that the behavior was learned when a novel odorant, citral
(which smells like lemons), was easily substituted for amniotic fluid (Pedersen,
Williams, & Blass, 1982).
419
420
polka dots. Other tongues can have 10 times as many fungiform papillae,
spaced so closely that there is little space between them. There is a connection
between the density of fungiform papillae and the perception of taste. Those
who experience the most intense taste sensations (we call them supertasters)
tend to have the most fungiform papillae. Incidentally, this is a rare example
in sensory processes of visible anatomical variation that correlates with
function. We can look at the tongues of a variety of individuals and predict
which of them will experience the most intense taste sensations.
The structures that house taste buds innervated by the glossopharyngeal
nerve are called circumvallate papillae. They are relatively large structures
arrayed in an inverted V shape across the back of the tongue. Each of them
looks like a small island surrounded by a moat.
Taste nerves project to the brain, where they send inhibitory signals to one
another.
constancy. Damage to one nerve reduces taste input but also reduces inhibition
on the other nerves (Bartoshuk et al 2005). That release of inhibition intensifies
the central neural signals from the undamaged nerves, thereby maintaining
whole mouth function.
powerful that it actually increases whole mouth taste. The small effect of limited
taste damage is one of the earliest clinical observations. In 1825, Brillat-Savarin
described in his book The Physiology of Taste an interview with an ex-prisoner
who had suffered a horrible punishment: amputation of his tongue. This man,
whom I met in Amsterdam, where he made his living by running errands, had
had some education, and it was easy to communicate with him by writing. After
I had observed that the forepart of his tongue has been cut off clear to the
ligament, I asked him if he still found any flavor in what he ate, and if his sense
of taste had survived the cruelty to which he had been subjected. He replied
that he still possessed the ability to taste fairly well (Brillat-Savarin, 1971, pg.
35). This injury damaged the chorda tympani but spared the glossopharyngeal
nerve.
nobaproject.com - Taste and Smell
421
We now know that taste nerves not only inhibit one another but also inhibit
other oral sensations. Thus, taste damage can intensify oral touch (fats) and
oral burn (chilis). In fact, taste damage appears to be linked to pain in general.
Consider an animal injured in the wild. If pain reduced eating, its chance of
survival would be diminished. However, nature appears to have wired the brain
such that taste input inhibits pain. Eating is reinforced and the animals chances
of survival increase.
422
Outside Resources
Video: Inside the Psychologists Studio with Linda Bartoshuk
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=fO76jgepo74
Video: Linda Bartoshuk at Nobel Conference 46
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=I4tbg_yb4Ms
Video: Test your tongue: the science of taste
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=KvP7xGBzZNs
Discussion Questions
1. In this chapter, we have defined basic tastes in terms of whether or not a
sensation produces hard-wired affect. Can you think of any other definitions
of basic tastes?
2. Do you think omnivores, herbivores, or carnivores have a better chance at
survival?
3. Olfaction is mediated by one cranial nerve. Taste is mediated by three cranial
nerves. Why do you think evolution gave more nerves to taste than to smell?
What are the consequences of this?
Vocabulary
Conditioned aversions and preferences
Likes and dislikes developed through associations with pleasurable or
unpleasurable sensations.
Gustation
The action of tasting; the ability to taste.
Olfaction
The sense of smell; the action of smelling; the ability to smell.
Omnivore
A person or animal that is able to survive by eating a wide range of foods from
plant or animal origin.
Orthonasal olfaction
Perceiving scents/smells introduced via the nostrils.
Retronasal olfaction
Perceiving scents/smells introduced via the mouth/palate.
Reference List
Bartoshuk, L. M., Snyder, D. J., Grushka, M., Berger, A. M., Duffy, V. B,, & Kveton,
J. F. (2005). Taste damage: previously unsuspected consequences. Chemical
Senses, 30(Suppl. 1), i218i219
Bartoshuk, L. M., & Wolfe, J. M. (1990). Conditioned taste aversions in humans:
Are they olfactory aversions? (abstract). Chemical Senses, 15, 551.
Brillat-Savarin, J. A. (1825). The physiology of taste (M.F.K. Fisher, Trans., 1971).
New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf.
Chaudhari, N., & Roper, S. D. (2010). The cell biology of taste. Journal of Cell
Biology, 190, 285296.
Doty, R. L. (2010). The great pheromone myth. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins
University Press.
Liem, D. G., & Mennella, J. A. (2003). Heightened sour preferences during
childhood. Chemical Senses, 28(2), 173180.
Meyerhof, W., Batram, C., Kuhn, C., Brockhoff, A., Chudoba, E., Bufe, B., . . .
Behrens, M. (2010). The molecular receptive ranges of human TAS2R bitter
taste receptors. Chemical Senses, 35, 157170.
Pedersen, P.E., Williams, C.L., & Blass, E.M. (1982). Activation and odor
conditioning of suckling behavior in 3-day-old albino rats. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 8(4), 329341.
Pollan, M. (2006). The omnivore's dilemma. New York, NY: Penguin Books.
Rozin, E., & Rozin, P. (1981). Culinary themes and variations. Natural History, 90,
614.
Rozin, P., & Vollmecke, T.A. (1986). Food likes and dislikes. Annual Review of
Nutrition, 6, 433-456.
Shepherd, G. M. (2005). Outline of a theory of olfactory processing and its
relevance to humans. Chemical Senses, 30(Suppl 1), i3-i5.
Swithers, S. E., & Davidson, T. L. (2008). A role for sweet taste: Calorie predictive
relations in energy regulation by rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 122(1),
161-173.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Taste and Smell by Linda Bartoshuk and Derek
Snyder is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
The sensory systems of touch and pain provide us with information about our
environment and our bodies that is often crucial for survival and well-being.
Moreover, touch is a source of pleasure. In this chapter, we review how
information about our environment and our bodies is coded in the periphery
and interpreted by the brain as touch and pain sensations. We discuss how
these experiences are often dramatically shaped by top-down factors like
motivation, expectation, mood, fear, stress, and context. When wellfunctioning, these circuits promote survival and prepare us to make adaptive
decisions. Pathological loss of touch can result in perceived disconnection from
the body, and insensitivity to pain can be very dangerous, leading to maladaptive
hazardous behavior. On the other hand, chronic pain conditions, in which these
systems start signaling pain in response to innocuous touch or even in the
absence of any observable sensory stimuli, have tremendous negative impact
on the lives of the affected. Understanding how our sensory-processing
mechanisms can be modulated psychologically and physiologically promises
to help researchers and clinicians find new ways to alleviate the suffering of
chronic-pain patients.
Learning Objectives
Describe the social touch hypothesis and the role of affective touch in
development and bonding.
Explain how expectations and context affect pain and touch experiences.
Introduction
Imagine a life free of pain. How would it becalm, fearless, serene? Would you
feel invulnerable, invincible? Getting rid of pain is a popular questa quick
search for pain-free life on Google returns well over 4 million hitsincluding
links to various bestselling self-help guides promising a pain-free life in only 7
steps, 6 weeks, or 3 minutes. Pain management is a billion-dollar market, and
involves much more than just pharmaceuticals. Surely a life with no pain would
be a better one?
432
Figure 1A: Patient with HSAN-V genetic mutation affecting pain nerve growth. Severely affected
12-year-old boy with damages to his left knee and ankles. (Minde et al., 2004)
Well, consider one of the lucky few: 12-year-old Thomas has never felt
deep pain. Not even when a fracture made him walk around with one leg shorter
than the other, so that the bones of his healthy leg were slowly crushed to
destruction underneath the knee joint (see Figure 1A). For Thomas and other
members of a large Swedish family, life without pain is a harsh reality because
of a mutated gene that affects the growth of the nerves conducting deep pain.
Most of those affected suffer from joint damage and frequent fractures to bones
in their feet and hands; some end up in wheelchairs even before they reach
puberty (Minde et al., 2004). It turns out paingenerallyserves us well.
nobaproject.com - Touch and Pain
433
Living without a sense of touch sounds less attractive than being free of pain
touch is a source of pleasure and essential to how we feel. Losing the sense
of touch has severe implicationssomething patient G. L. experienced when
an antibiotics treatment damaged the type of nerves that signal touch from her
skin and the position of her joints and muscles. She reported feeling like shed
lost her physical self from her nose down, making her disembodiedlike she
no longer had any connection to the body attached to her head. If she didnt
look at her arms and legs they could just wander off without her knowing
initially she was unable to walk, and even after she relearned this skill she was
so dependent on her visual attention that closing her eyes would cause her to
land in a hopeless heap on the floor. Only light caresses like those from her
childrens hands can make her feel she has a body, but even these sensations
remain vague and elusive (Olausson et al., 2002; Sacks, 1985).
Sensation
Cutaneous Senses of the Skin Connect the Brain to the
Body and the Outside World
Touch and pain are aspects of the somatosensory system, which provides our
brain with information about our own body (interoception) and properties of
the immediate external world (exteroception) (Craig, 2002). We have
somatosensory receptors located all over the body, from the surface of our skin
to the depth of our joints. The information they send to the central nervous
system is generally divided into four modalities: cutaneous senses (senses of
the skin), proprioception (body position), kinesthesis (body movement), and
nociception (pain, discomfort). We are going to focus on the cutaneous senses,
which respond to tactile, thermal, and pruritic (itchy) stimuli, and events that
cause tissue damage (and hence pain). In addition, there is growing evidence
nobaproject.com - Touch and Pain
434
for a fifth modality specifically channeling pleasant touch (McGlone & Reilly,
2010).
435
Box 1.
436
represented according to where in the body they stem from (see homunculus
illustration, Figure 2). The unpleasant ache you feel after the sharp pin stab is
a separate, simultaneous signal sent from the nociceptors in your foot via thin
C-pain or A-fibers to the insular cortex and other brain regions involved in
processing of emotion and interoception (see Figure 3a for a schematic
representation of this pathway). The experience of stepping on a pin is, in other
words, composed by two separate signals: one discriminatory signal allowing
us to localize the touch stimulus and distinguish whether its a blunt or a sharp
stab; and one affective signal that lets us know that stepping on the pin is bad.
It is common to divide pain into sensorydiscriminatory and affective
motivational aspects (Auvray, Myin, & Spence, 2010). This distinction
corresponds, at least partly, to how this information travels from the peripheral
to the central nervous system and how it is processed in the brain (Price, 2000).
437
Figure 2: Left - The Homunculus: Homunculus means little man, and here you see a scale
model of the human body distorted to reflect the relative space that body parts occupy in the
somatosensory cortex. As you can see, the lips, hands, feet and genitals send more
somatosensory projections to the brain than do any other body parts. Right - Cortical mapping
of the sensory homunculus: The body parts are represented in specific locations on the
somatosensory cortex. Representations map out somatotopically, with the feet located
medially and shoulders and arms laterally to the interhemispheric fissure. Facial structures
are represented in a different location to the scalp and head; the face oriented upside down
with the forehead pointing towards the shoulders.
438
Affective
Aspects
of
Touch
Are
Important
for
or
physical
contact,
the
children
suffered
cognitive
and
439
The skin senses are similar across species, likely reflecting the evolutionary
advantage of being able to tell what is touching you, where its happening, and
whether or not its likely to cause tissue damage. An intriguing line of touch
research suggests that humans, cats, and other animals have a special,
evolutionarily preserved system that promotes gentle touch because it carries
social and emotional significance.On a peripheral level, this system consists of
a subtype of C-fibers that responds not to painful stimuli, but rather to gentle
stroking touchcalled C-tactile fibers. The firing rate of the C-tactile fibers
correlates closely with how pleasant the stroking feelssuggesting they are
coding specifically for the gentle caresses typical of social affiliative touch
(Lken, Wessberg, Morrison, McGlone, & Olausson, 2009). This finding has led
to the social touch hypothesis, which proposes that C-tactile fibers form a
system for touch perception that supports social bonding (Morrison, Lken, &
Olausson, 2010; Olausson, Wessberg, Morrison, McGlone, & Vallbo, 2010). The
discovery of the C-tactile system suggests that touch is organized in a similar
way to pain; fast-conducting A-fibers contribute to sensorydiscriminatory
aspects, while thin C-fibers contribute to affectivemotivational aspects (Lken,
Wessberg, Morrison, McGlone, & Olausson, 2009). However, while these hardwired afferent systems often provide us with accurate information about our
environment and our bodies, how we experience touch or pain depends very
much on top-down sources like motivation, expectation, mood, fear, and stress.
Modulation
Pain Is Necessary for Survival, but Our Brain Can Stop It
if It Needs To
440
In April 2003, the climber Aron Ralston found himself at the floor of Blue John
Canyon in Utah, forced to make an appalling choice: face a slow but certain
deathor amputate his right arm. Five days earlier he fell down the canyon
since then he had been stuck with his right arm trapped between an 800-lb
boulder and the steep sandstone wall. Weak from lack of food and water and
close to giving up, it occurred to him like an epiphany that if he broke the two
bones in his forearm he could manage to cut off the rest with his pocket knife.
The thought of freeing himself and surviving made him so exited he spent the
next 40 minutes completely engrossed in the task: first snapping his bones
using his body as a lever, then sticking his fingers into the arm, pinching bundles
of muscle fibers and severing them one by one, before cutting the blue arteries
and the pale noodle-like nerves. The pain was unimportant. Only cutting
through the thick white main nerve made him stop for a minutethe flood of
pain, he describes, was like thrusting his entire arm into a cauldron of magma.
Finally free, he rappelled down a cliff and walked another 7 miles until he was
rescued by some hikers (Ralston, 2010). How is it possible to do something so
excruciatingly painful to yourself, and still manage to walk, talk, and think
rationally afterwards? The answer lies within the brain, where signals from the
body are interpreted. When we perceive somatosensory and nociceptive signals
from the body, the experience is highly subjective and malleable by motivation,
attention, emotion, and context.
441
Figure 3: Pain processing pathways Left - Ascending pain pathways: An injury is signaled
simultaneously via fast-conducting A or A-fibres and slow-conducting C-pain or A-fibres.
The fast A-fibres signal pressure, stretching and other tissue movements to the somatosensory
cortex via the dorsal column nuclei. The C-pain and A-fibres sends pain information from
nociceptors in the tissue or skin, and transmits these signals to second order neurons in the
dorsal horn of the spinal cord. The second order neurons then cross over to the opposite side,
where they form the ascending spinothalamic tract. This tract projects signals to nuclei in the
medulla and midbrain on the way up to the thalamus (T). The thalamus relays the information
to the somatosensory and insular cortex, as well as cortical regions mediating different aspects
of the pain experience such as affective responses in the cingulate cortex. Right - Descending
pain modulation pathways: Information from the environment and certain motivational states
can activate this topdown pathway. Several areas in the limbic forebrain including the anterior
cingulate and insular cortex, nuclei in the amygdala and the hypothalamus (H), project to the
midbrain periaqueductal grey (PAG), which then modulates ascending pain transmission from
the afferent pain system indirectly through the rostral ventromedial medulla (RVM) in the
brainstem. This modulating system produces analgesia by the release of endogenous opioids,
442
and uses ON- and OFF-cells to exert either inhibitory (green) or facilitatory (red) control of
nociceptive signals at the spinal dorsal horn.
The
MotivationDecision
Model
and
Descending
Modulation of Pain
According to the motivationdecision model, the brain automatically and
continuously evaluates the pros and cons of any situationweighing impending
threats and available rewards (Fields, 2004, 2006). Anything more important for
survival than avoiding the pain activates the brains descending pain
modulatory systema top-down system involving several parts of the brain
and brainstem, which inhibits nociceptive signaling so that the more important
actions can be attended to (Figure 3b). In Arons extreme case, his actions were
likely based on such an unconscious decision processtaking into account his
homeostatic state (his hunger, thirst, the inflammation and decay of his crushed
hand slowly affecting the rest of his body), the sensory input available (the sweet
smell of his dissolving skin, the silence around him indicating his solitude), and
his knowledge about the threats facing him (death, or excruciating pain that
wont kill him) versus the potential rewards (survival, seeing his family again).
Arons story illustrates the evolutionary advantage to being able to shut off pain:
The descending pain modulatory system allows us to go through with
potentially life-saving actions. However, when one has reached safety or
obtained the reward, healing is more important. The very same descending
system can then crank up nociception from the body to promote healing and
motivate us to avoid potentially painful actions. To facilitate or inhibit
nociceptive signals from the body, the descending pain modulatory system uses
a set of ON- or OFF-cells in the brainstem, which regulates how much of the
nociceptive signal reaches the brain. The descending system is dependent on
opioid signaling, and analgesics like morphine relieve pain via this circuit
nobaproject.com - Touch and Pain
443
444
the perception of how unpleasant pain is, while still retaining the ability to
experience the intensity of the sensation (Rainville, Duncan, Price, Carrier, &
Bushnell, 1997; Rainville, Feine, Bushnell, & Duncan, 1992). Social rewards, like
holding the hand of your boyfriend or girlfriend, have pain-reducing effects.
Even looking at a picture of him/her can have similar effectsin fact, seeing a
picture of a person we feel close to not only reduces subjective pain ratings,
but also the activity in pain-related brain areas (Eisenberger et al., 2011). The
most common things to do when wanting to help someone through a painful
experiencebeing present and holding the persons handthus seems to have
a measurably positive effect.
445
446
447
Summary
Sensory experiences connect us to the people around us, to the rest of the
world, and to our own bodies. Pleasant or unpleasant, theyre part of being
human. In this chapter, we have seen how being able to inhibit pain responses
is central to our survivaland in cases like that of climber Aron Ralston, that
ability can allow us to do extreme things. We have also seen how important the
ability to feel pain is to our healthillustrated by young Thomas, who keeps
injuring himself because he simply doesnt notice pain. While Thomas has to
learn to avoid harmful activities without the sensory input that normally guides
us, G. L. has had to learn how to keep approaching and move about in a world
she can hardly feel at all, with a body that is practically disconnected from her
nobaproject.com - Touch and Pain
448
449
Outside Resources
Book: Butler, D. S., Moseley, G. L., & Sunyata. (2003). Explain pain (p. 19).
Australia: Noigroup.
Book: Kringelbach, M. L., & Berridge, K. C. (Eds.). (2010). Pleasures of the brain
(p. 343). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Book: Ralston, A. (2004). Between a rock and a hard place: The basis of the
motion picture 127 Hours. New York, NY: Atria.
Book: Sacks, O. (1998). The man who mistook his wife for a hat: And other
clinical tales. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.
Video: BBC Documentary series Human Senses, Episode 3: Touch and Vision
http://watchdocumentary.org/watch/human-senses-episode-03-touch-and-visionvideo_f3e33c14a.html
Discussion Questions
1. Your friend has had an accident and there is a chance the injury might cause
pain over a prolonged period. How would you support your friend? What
would you say and do to ease the pain, and why do you think it would work?
2. We have learned that touch and pain sensation in many aspects do not
reflect objectively the outside world or the body state. Rather, these
experiences are shaped by various top-down influences, and they can even
occur without any peripheral activation. This is similar to the way other
sensory systems work, e.g., the visual or auditory systems, and seems to
reflect a general way the brain process sensory events. Why do you think
the brain interprets the incoming sensory information instead of giving a
one-to-one readout the way a thermometer and other measuring
instruments would? Imagine you instead had direct unbiased access
between stimuli and sensation. What would be the advantages and
disadvantages of this?
3. Feelings of pain or touch are subjectivethey have a particular quality that
you perceive subjectively. How can we know whether the pain you feel is
similar to the pain I feel? Is it possible that modern scientists can objectively
measure such subjective feelings?
Vocabulary
A-fibers
Fast-conducting sensory nerves with myelinated axons. Larger diameter and
thicker myelin sheaths increases conduction speed. A-fibers conduct touch
signals from low-threshold mechanoreceptors with a velocity of 80 m/s and a
diameter of 10 m; A-fibers have a diameter of 2.5 m and conduct cold,
noxious, and thermal signals at 12 m/s. The third and fastest conducting A-fiber
is the A, which conducts proprioceptive information with a velocity of 120 m/
s and a diameter of 20 m.
Allodynia
Pain due to a stimulus that does not normally provoke pain, e.g., when a light,
stroking touch feels painful.
Analgesia
Pain relief.
C-fibers
C-fibers: Slow-conducting unmyelinated thin sensory afferents with a diameter
of 1 m and a conduction velocity of approximately 1 m/s. C-pain fibers convey
noxious, thermal, and heat signals; C-tactile fibers convey gentle touch, light
stroking.
Chronic pain
Persistent or recurrent pain, beyond usual course of acute illness or injury;
sometimes present without observable tissue damage or clear cause.
C-pain or A-fibers
C-pain fibers convey noxious, thermal, and heat signals
C-tactile fibers
C-tactile fibers convey gentle touch, light stroking
Cutaneous senses
The senses of the skin: tactile, thermal, pruritic (itchy), painful, and pleasant.
Endorphin
An endogenous morphine-like peptide that binds to the opioid receptors in the
brain and body; synthesized in the bodys nervous system.
Exteroception
The sense of the external world, of all stimulation originating from outside our
own bodies.
Interoception
The sense of the physiological state of the body. Hunger, thirst, temperature,
pain, and other sensations relevant to homeostasis. Visceral input such as heart
rate, blood pressure, and digestive activity give rise to an experience of the
bodys internal states and physiological reactions to external stimulation. This
experience has been described as a representation of the material me, and
it is hypothesized to be the foundation of subjective feelings, emotion, and selfawareness.
Nociception
The neural process of encoding noxious stimuli, the sensory input from
nociceptors. Not necessarily painful, and crucially not necessary for the
experience of pain.
Nociceptors
High-threshold sensory receptors of the peripheral somatosensory nervous
system that are capable of transducing and encoding noxious stimuli.
Nociceptors send information about actual or impending tissue damage to the
brain. These signals can often lead to pain, but nociception and pain are not
the same.
Noxious stimulus
A stimulus that is damaging or threatens damage to normal tissues.
Ocial touch hypothesis
Proposes that social touch is a distinct domain of touch. C-tactile afferents form
a special pathway that distinguishes social touch from other types of touch by
selectively firing in response to touch of social-affective relevance; thus sending
affective information parallel to the discriminatory information from the Afibers. In this way, the socially relevant touch stands out from the rest as having
Pain
Defined as an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with
actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage,
according to the International Association for the Study of Pain.
Phantom pain
Pain that appears to originate in an amputated limb.
Placebo effect
Effects from a treatment that are not caused by the physical properties of a
treatment but by the meaning ascribed to it. These effects reflect the brains
own activation of modulatory systems, which is triggered by positive
expectation or desire for a successful treatment. Placebo analgesia is the most
well-studied placebo effect and has been shown to depend, to a large degree,
on opioid mechanisms. Placebo analgesia can be reversed by the
pharmacological blocking of opioid receptors. The word placebo is probably
derived from the Latin word placebit (it will please).
Sensitization
Increased responsiveness of nociceptive neurons to their normal input and/or
recruitment of a response to normally subthreshold inputs. Clinically,
sensitization may only be inferred indirectly from phenomena such as
hyperalgesia or allodynia. Sensitization can occur in the central nervous system
(central sensitization) or in the periphery (peripheral sensitization).
Reference List
Auvray, M., Myin, E., & Spence, C. (2010). The sensory-discriminative and
affective-motivational aspects of pain. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
Reviews, 34(2), 214223.
Committee on Advancing Pain Research, Care, & Institute of Medicine. (2011).
Relieving pain in America: A blueprint for transforming prevention, care,
education, and research. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press.
Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological
condition of the body. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(8), 655666.
Dum, J., & Herz, A. (1984). Endorphinergic modulation of neural reward systems
indicated by behavioral changes. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior,
21(2), 259266.
Dunbar, R. I. (2010). The social role of touch in humans and primates:
behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neuroscience &
Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(2), 260268.
Eippert, F., Bingel, U., Schoell, E. D., Yacubian, J., Klinger, R., Lorenz, J., & Buchel,
C. (2009). Activation of the opioidergic descending pain control system
underlies placebo analgesia. Neuron, 63(4), 533543.
Eippert, F., Finsterbusch, J., Bingel, U., & Buchel, C. (2009). Direct evidence for
spinal cord involvement in placebo analgesia. Science, 326(5951), 404.
Eisenberger, N. I., Master, S. L., Inagaki, T. K., Taylor, S. E., Shirinyan, D.,
Lieberman, M. D., & Naliboff, B. D. (2011). Attachment figures activate a
Leknes, S., Berna, C., Lee, M. C., Snyder, G. D., Biele, G., & Tracey, I. (2013). The
importance of context: When relative relief renders pain pleasant. Pain, 154
(3), 402410.
Leknes, S., & Tracey, I. (2008). A common neurobiology for pain and pleasure.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(4), 314320.
Levine, J. D., Gordon, N. C., & Fields, H. L. (1978). The mechanism of placebo
analgesia. Lancet, 2(8091), 654657.
Liljencrantz, J., Bjrnsdotter, M., Morrison, I., Bergstrand, S., Ceko, M.,
Seminowicz, D. A., . . . Olausson, H. (2013). Altered C-tactile processing in
human dynamic tactile allodynia. Pain, 154(2), 227234.
Lken, L. S., Wessberg, J., Morrison, I., McGlone, F., & Olausson, H. (2009). Coding
of pleasant touch by unmyelinated afferents in humans. Nature
Neuroscience, 12(5), 547548.
McGlone, F., & Reilly, D. (2010). The cutaneous sensory system. Neuroscience
& Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(2), 148159.
Minde, J., Toolanen, G., Andersson, T., Nennesmo, I., Remahl, I. N., Svensson, O.,
& Solders, G. (2004). Familial insensitivity to pain (HSAN V) and a mutation
in the NGFB gene. A neurophysiological and pathological study. Muscle &
Nerve, 30(6), 752760.
Morrison, I., Bjornsdotter, M., & Olausson, H. (2011). Vicarious responses to
social touch in posterior insular cortex are tuned to pleasant caressing
speeds. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(26), 95549562.
Morrison, I., Lken, L. S., & Olausson, H. (2010). The skin as a social organ.
Experimental Brain Research, 204(3), 305314.
Olausson, H., Lamarre, Y., Backlund, H., Morin, C., Wallin, B. G., Starck, G., . . .
Bushnell, M. C. (2002). Unmyelinated tactile afferents signal touch and
project to insular cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 5(9), 900904.
Olausson, H., Wessberg, J., Morrison, I., McGlone, F., & Vallbo, . (2010). The
neurophysiology of unmyelinated tactile afferents. Neuroscience &
Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(2), 185191.
Pechtel, P., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2011). Effects of early life stress on cognitive and
affective
function:
An
integrated
review
of
human
literature.
Ralston, A. (2010, December 31). Aron Ralston's 127 hours: This is going to make
one hell of a story. . . Retrieved from The Telegraph: http://www.telegraph.
co.uk/culture/books/8233734/Aron-Ralstons-127-Hours-This-is-going-to-makeone-hell-of-a-story.-.-..html
Sacks, O. (1985). The disembodied lady. In The man who mistook his wife for a
hat and other clinical tales (pp. 4354) New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.
Simons, D. J., & Land, P. W. (1987). Early experience of tactile stimulation
influences organization of somatic sensory cortex. Nature, 326(6114), 694
697.
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004).
Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain.
Science, 303(5661), 11571162.
Wiech, K., & Tracey, I. (2009). The influence of negative emotions on pain:
Behavioral effects and neural mechanisms. NeuroImage, 47(3), 987994.
Zambreanu, L., Wise, R. G., Brooks, J. C. W., Iannetti, G. D., & Tracey, I. (2005). A
role for the brainstem in central sensitisation in humans. Evidence from
functional magnetic resonance imaging. Pain, 114(3), 397407.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Touch and Pain by Guro E. Lseth, Dan-Mikael
Ellingson, and Siri
NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
The vestibular system functions to detect head motion and position relative to
gravity and is primarily involved in the fine control of visual gaze, posture,
orthostasis, spatial orientation, and navigation. Vestibular signals are highly
processed in many regions of the brain and are involved in many essential
functions. In this chapter, we provide an overview of how the vestibular system
works and how vestibular signals are used to guide behavior.
Learning Objectives
vestibulo-thalamo-cortical
pathways.
commissural system.
Introduction
Remember the dizzy feeling you got as a child after you jumped off the merrygo-round or spun around like a top? These feelings result from activation of
the vestibular system, which detects our movements through space but is not
a conscious sense like vision or hearing. In fact, most vestibular functions are
imperceptible, but vestibular-related sensations such as motion sickness can
pop up rapidly when riding on a roller coaster, having a bumpy plane ride, or
a sailing a boat in rough seas. However, these sensations are really side effects
and the vestibular system is actually extremely important for everyday activities,
with vestibular signals being involved in much of the brains information
processing that controls such fundamental functions as balance, posture, gaze
stabilization, spatial orientation, and navigation, to name a few. In many regions
of the brain, vestibular information is combined with signals from the other
senses as well as with motor information to give rise to motion perception,
body awareness, and behavioral control. Here, we will explore the workings of
the vestibular system and consider some of the integrated computations the
brain performs using vestibular signals to guide our common behavior.
468
469
Figure 1. Receptor hair cells and semicircular canal structure. A) Vestibular hair cell in
the receptor epithelium with stereocilia in the apical surface of the cell. Innervating afferent
and efferent neurons make synaptic contacts with the basal surface of the cell. B) Semicircular
canal structure showing the fluid duct, the hair cell stereocilia embedded in a gelatinous
membrane on top of the hair cells, and the innervating afferent fibers.
As shown in Figure 2, when the head moves toward the receptor hair cells
(e.g., left head turns for the left horizontal semicircular canal), the stereocilia
are bent toward the tallest end and special mechanically gated ion channels in
the tips of the cilia open, which excites (depolarizes) the cell (Shotwell, Jacobs,
& Hudspeth, 1981). Head motion in the opposite direction causes bending
toward the smallest stereocilia, which closes the channels and inhibits
(hyperpolarizes) the cell. The left and right ear semicircular canals have opposite
polarity, so for example, when you turn your head to the left, the receptors in
the left horizontal semicircular canal will be excited while right ear horizontal
canal receptors will be inhibited (Figure 3). The same relationship is true for
nobaproject.com - The Vestibular System
470
the vertical semicircular canals. Vestibular afferent nerve fibers innervate the
base of the hair cell and increase or decrease their neural firing rate as the
receptor cell is excited or inhibited (Dickman and Correia, 1989), respectively,
and then carry these signals regarding head rotational motion to the brain as
part of the vestibulocochlear nerve (Cranial nerve VIII). They enter the brainstem
and terminate in the ipsilateral vestibular nuclei, cerebellum, and reticular
formation (Carleton & Carpenter, 1984; Dickman & Fang, 1996). The primary
vestibular hair cell and afferent neurotransmitters are glutamate and aspartate.
Due to the mechanical properties of the vestibular receptor system, rotational
accelerations of the head are integrated into velocity signals (Van Egmond,
Groen, & Jongkess, 1949) that are then encoded by semicircular canal afferents
(Fernandez & Goldberg, 1971). Detection thresholds for rotational motion have
shown that afferents can discriminate differences in head velocity on the order
of 2 deg/sec, but also are sensitive to a broad range of natural head movements
up to high head speeds in the hundreds of deg/sec (as you might experience
when you make a fast head turn toward a loud sound, or are performing
gymnastics; Sadeghi, Chacron, Taylor, & Cullen, 2007; Yu, Dickman, & Angelaki,
2012).
471
Figure 2. Vestibular receptor cell directional selectivity. Middle) At rest, hair cells release
some neurotransmitter, producing a high, spontaneous firing rate in the innervating afferent
fibers. Left) When the stereocilia are displaced toward the kinocilium, the cell is depolarized
and the afferent firing rate is increased. Right) When the stereocilia are displaced away from
the kinocilium, the cell is hyperpolarized and the afferent firing rate is decreased
472
Figure 3. Receptor hair cells in the otolith organs. Receptor cells have stereocilia
embedded in the gelatinous membrane, which is covered by thousands of calcium carbonite
otoconia. Receptor cells are polarized in opposite directions relative to a central location and
are innervated by VIIIth nerve afferent fibers.
Otolith receptors are sensitive to linear accelerations and tilts of the head
relative to gravity (Fernandez & Goldberg, 1976a). The utricle otolith receptor
lies parallel to the horizontal semicircular canal and the saccule receptor lies
vertical in the head (Hearing Chapter, Figure 1- ). As shown in Figure 4, a special
nobaproject.com - The Vestibular System
473
otolith epithelium contains receptor hair cells whose stereocilia extend into a
gelatin membrane that is covered by a layer of calcium carbonate crystals,
termed otoconia, like rocks piled up to form a jetty (Lindeman, 1969). Otoconia
are not affected by fluid movements but instead are displaced by linear
accelerations, including translations (e.g., forward/backward or upward/
downward motions) or changes in head position relative to gravity. These linear
accelerations produce displacements of the otoconia (due to their high mass),
much like rocks rolling down a hill or your coffee cup falling off the car dashboard
when you push the gas pedal. Movements of the otoconia bend the hair cell
stereocilia and open/close channels in a similar way to that described for the
semicircular canals. However, otolith hair cells are polarized such that the tallest
stereocilia are pointing toward the center of the utricle and away from the center
in the saccule, which effectively splits the receptors into two opposing groups
(Flock, 1964; Lindeman, 1969). In this way, some hair cells are excited and some
inhibited for each linear motion force or head tilt experienced, with the
population of receptors and their innervating afferents being directionally
tuned to all motions or head tilts in 3D space (Fernandez & Goldberg, 1976b).
474
Figure 4. Vestibular response to horizontal plane head rotation. A) When the head is
stationary, afferent fibers on both the sides of the head have equivalent firing so there is no
sense of motion. B) When the head turns to the left, all of the left horizontal semicircular
canal hair cells are excited and afferent fibers increase their firing rate. Conversely, right
horizontal canal afferents decrease their firing rate
All vestibular hair cells and afferents receive connections from vestibular
efferents, which are fibers projecting from the brain out to the vestibular
receptor organs, whose function is not well understood. It is thought that
efferents control the sensitivity of the receptor (Boyle, Carey, & Highstein, 1991).
The primary efferents neurotransmitter is acetylcholine (Anniko & Arnold,
1991).
475
476
information from one side of the head can change the normal resting activity
in the VIIIth nerve afferent fibers and will be interpreted by the brain as a head
rotation, even though the head is stationary. These effects often lead to illusions
of spinning or rotating that can be quite upsetting and may produce nausea or
vomiting. However, over time the commissural fibers provide for vestibular
compensation, a process by which the loss of unilateral vestibular receptor
function is partially restored centrally and behavioral responses, such as the
vestibuloocular reflex (VOR) and postural responses, mostly recover (Beraneck
et al., 2003; Fetter & Zee, 1988,; Newlands, Hesse, Haque, & Angelaki, 2001;
Newlands & Perachio, 1990).
In addition to the commissural pathway, many vestibular nuclei neurons
receive proprioceptive signals from the spinal cord regarding muscle movement
and position, visual signals regarding spatial motion, other multisensory (e.g.,
trigeminal) signals, and higher order signals from the cortex. It is thought that
the cortical inputs regulate fine gaze and postural control, as well as suppress
the normal compensatory reflexes during motion in order to elicit volitional
movements.
semicircular canal and otolith afferents that allow central vestibular neurons
to compute specific properties of head motion (Dickman & Angelaki, 2002). For
example, Einstein (1907) showed that linear accelerations are equivalent
whether they arise from translational motion or from tilts of the head relative
to gravity. The otolith receptors cannot discriminate between the two, so how
is it that we can tell the difference between when we are translating forward
and tilting backward, where the linear acceleration signaled by the otolith
afferents is the same? Vestibular nuclei and cerebellar neurons use convergent
signals from both the semicircular canals and the otolith receptors to
discriminate between tilt and translation, and as a result, some cells encode
head tilt (Zhou, 2006) while other cells encode translational motion (Angelaki,
Shaikh, Green, & Dickman, 2004).
477
Vestibuloocular system
The vestibular system is responsible for controlling gaze stability during motion
(Crane & Demer, 1997). For example, if we want to read the sign in a store
window while walking by, we must maintain foveal fixation on the words while
compensating for the combined rotational and translational head movements
incurred during our stride. The vestibular system regulates compensatory eye,
neck, spinal, and limb movements in order to maintain gaze (Keshner &
Peterson, 1995). One of the major components contributing to gaze stability
is the VOR, which produces reflexive eye movements that are equal in
magnitude and opposite in direction to the perceived head motion in 3D space
(Wilson et al., 1995). The VOR is so accurate and fast that it allows people to
maintain visual fixation on objects of interest while experiencing demanding
motion conditions, such as running, skiing, playing tennis, and driving. In fact,
gaze stabilization in humans has been shown to be completely compensatory
(essentially perfect) for most natural behaviors. To produce the VOR, vestibular
neurons must control each of the six pairs of eye muscles in unison through a
specific set of connections to the oculomotor nuclei (Ezure & Graf, 1984). The
anterior and posterior semicircular canals along with the saccule control vertical
and torsional (turning of the eye around the line of sight) eye movements, while
the horizontal canals and the utricle control horizontal eye movements.
To understand how the VOR works, lets take the example of the
compensatory response for a leftward head turn while reading the words on a
computer screen. The basic pathway consists of horizontal semicircular canal
afferents that project to specific neurons in the vestibular nuclei. These nuclei
cells, in turn, send an excitatory signal to the contralateral abducens nucleus,
which projects through the sixth cranial nerve to innervate the lateral rectus
muscle (Figure 5). Some abducens neurons send an excitatory projection back
across the midline to a subdivision of cells in the ipsilateral oculomotor nucleus,
which, in turn, projects through the third cranial nerve to innervate the right
nobaproject.com - The Vestibular System
478
(ipsilateral) medial rectus muscle. When a leftward head turn is made, the left
horizontal canal vestibular afferents will increase their firing rate and
consequently increase the activity of vestibular nuclei neurons projecting to the
opposite (contralateral) right abducens nucleus. The abducens neurons
produce contraction of the right lateral rectus and, through a separate cell
projection to the left oculomotor nucleus, excite the left medial rectus muscles.
In addition, matching bilateral inhibitory connections relax the left lateral rectus
and right medial rectus eye muscles. The resulting rightward eye movement
for both eyes stabilizes the object of interest upon the retina for greatest visual
acuity.
479
Figure 5.
Vestibuloocular reflex.
semicircular canal receptors are excited, while the right ear receptors are inhibited. The left
excitatory signals excite vestibular nuclei neurons. These cells project across the brain to
excite motor neurons in the right abducens nucleus (VI) that excite the lateral rectus muscle
of the right eye and to cells in the oculomotor nucleus (III) that excite the medial rectus muscle
of the left eye. This moves both eyes to the right to exactly match the leftward head movement
and stabilize visual gaze upon a target of interest. The right ear inhibitory signals cross to
neurons in the left vestibular nucleus that decrease their firing rate. These cells are inhibitory
and decrease their firing rate to further increase the response of rightward motor eye muscle
480
cells.
During linear translations, a different type of VOR also occurs (Paige &
Tomko, 1991). For example, sideways motion to the left results in a horizontal
rightward eye movement to maintain visual stability on an object of interest.
In a similar manner, vertical updown head movements (such as occur while
walking or running) elicit oppositely directed vertical eye movements (Angelaki,
McHenry, & Hess, 2000). For these reflexes, the amplitude of the translational
VOR depends on viewing distance. This is due to the fact that the vergence
angle (i.e., the angle between the lines of sight for each eye) varies as a function
of the inverse of the distance to the viewed visual object (Schwarz, Busettini, &
Miles, 1989). Visual objects that are far away (2 meters or more) require no
vergence angle, but as the visual objects get closer (e.g., when holding your
finger close to your nose), a large vergence angle is needed. During translational
motion, the eyes will change their vergence angle as the visual object moves
from close to farther away (or vice versa). These responses are a result of
activation of the otolith receptors, with connections to the oculomotor nuclei
similar to those described above for the rotational vestibuloocular reflex. With
tilts of the head, the resulting eye movement is termed torsion, and consists of
a rotational eye movement around the line of sight that is in the direction
opposite to the head tilt. As mentioned above, there are major reciprocal
connections between the vestibular nuclei and the cerebellum. It has been well
established that these connections are crucial for adaptive motor learning in
the vestibuloocular reflex (Lisberger, Pavelko, & Broussard, 1994).
481
Vestibulo-spinal network
There are two vestibular descending pathways that regulate body muscle
responses to motion and gravity, consisting of the lateral vestibulo-spinal tract
(LVST) and the medial vestibulo-spinal tract (MVST). Reflexive control of head
and neck muscles arises through the neurons in the medial vestibulospinal tract
(MVST). These neurons comprise the rapid vestibulocollic reflex (VCR) that
serves to stabilize the head in space and participates in gaze control (Peterson,
Goldber, Bilotto, & Fuller, 1985).
482
Vestibulo-autonomic control
Some vestibular nucleus neurons send projections to the reticular formation,
dorsal pontine nuclei, and nucleus of the solitary tract. These connections
regulate breathing and circulation through compensatory vestibular autonomic
responses that stabilize respiration and blood pressure during body motion
and changes relative to gravity. They may also be important for induction of
motion sickness and emesis.
483
& Grusser, 1992). Neurons in the PIVC are multisensory, responding to body
motion, somatosensory, proprioceptive, and visual motion stimuli (Chen,
DeAngelis, & Angelaki, 2011; Grusser, Pause, & Schreiter, 1982). PIVC and areas
3a and 2v are heavily interconnected. Vestibular neurons also have been
observed in the posterior parietal cortex; in area 7, in the ventral intraparietal
area (VIP), the medial intraparietal area (MIP), and the medial superior temporal
area (MST). VIP contains multimodal neurons involved in spatial coding. MIP
and MST neurons respond to body motion through space by multisensory
integration of visual motion and vestibular signals (Gu, DeAngelis, & Angelaki ,
2007) and many MST cells are directly involved in heading perception (Gu,
Watkins, Angelaki, & DeAngelis, 2006). Lesions of the parietal cortical areas can
result in confusions in spatial awareness. Finally, areas involved with the control
of saccades and pursuit eye movements, including area 6, area 8, and the
superior frontal gyrus, receive vestibular signals (Fukushima, Sato, Fukushima,
Shinmei, & Kaneko, 2000). How these different cortical regions contribute to
our perception of motion and spatial orientation is still not well understood.
484
485
Figure 6. Cortical regions of the brain known to be involved with vestibular processing.
A) The frontal eye fields control eye movements and receive vestibular motion information.
Areas 2v and 3a are somatosensory areas that map body location and movement signals.
Area PIVC responds to body and head motion information. The posterior parietal cortex is
involved with motion perception and responds to both visual and vestibular motion cues. B)
The hippocampus and parahippocampul regions are involved with spatial orientation and
navigation functions. All receive vestibular signals regarding body and head motion.
486
Motion sickness
Although a number of conditions can produce motion sickness, it is generally
thought that it is evoked from a mismatch in sensory cues between vestibular,
visual, and proprioceptive signals (Yates, Miller, & Lucot, 1998). For example,
reading a book in a car on a winding road can produce motion sickness, whereby
the accelerations experienced by the vestibular system do not match the visual
input. However, if one looks out the window at the scenery going by during the
same travel, no sickness occurs because the visual and vestibular cues are in
alignment. Sea sickness, a form of motion sickness, appears to be a special
case and arises from unusual vertical oscillatory and roll motion. Human studies
have found that low frequency oscillations of 0.2 Hz and large amplitudes (such
as found in large seas during a storm) are most likely to cause motion sickness,
with higher frequencies offering little problems.
Summary
Here, we have seen that the vestibular system transduces and encodes signals
about head motion and position with respect to gravity, information that is then
used by the brain for many essential functions and behaviors. We actually
understand a great deal regarding vestibular contributions to fundamental
reflexes, such as compensatory eye movements and balance during motion.
More recent progress has been made toward understanding how vestibular
signals combine with other sensory cues, such as vision, in the thalamus and
cortex to give rise to motion perception. However, there are many complex
cognitive abilities that we know require vestibular information to function, such
as spatial orientation and navigation behaviors, but these systems are only just
beginning to be investigated. Future research regarding vestibular system
function will likely be geared to seeking answers to questions regarding how
the brain copes with vestibular signal loss. In fact, according to the National
nobaproject.com - The Vestibular System
487
Institutes of Health, nearly 35% of Americans over the age of 40 (69 million
people) have reported chronic vestibular-related problems. It is therefore of
significant importance to human health to better understand how vestibular
cues contribute to common brain functions and how better treatment options
for vestibular dysfunction can be realized.
488
Discussion Questions
1. If a person sustains loss of the vestibular receptors in one ear due to disease
or trauma, what symptoms would the person suffer? Would the symptoms
be permanent?
2. Often motion sickness is relieved when a person looks at far distance
objects, such as things located on the far horizon. Why does far distance
viewing help in motion sickness while close distance view (like reading a
map or book) make it worse?
3. Vestibular signals combine with visual signals in certain areas of cortex and
assist in motion perception. What types of cues does the visual system
provide for self motion through space? What types of vestibular signals
would be consistent with rotational versus translational motion?
Vocabulary
Abducens nucleus
A group of excitatory motor neurons in the medial brainstem that send
projections through the VIth cranial nerve to control the ipsilateral lateral rectus
muscle. In addition, abducens interneurons send an excitatory projection
across the midline to a subdivision of cells in the ipsilateral oculomotor nucleus,
which project through the IIIrd cranial nerve to innervate the ipsilateral medial
rectus muscle.
Acetylcholine
An organic compound neurotransmitter consisting of acetic acid and choline.
Depending upon the receptor type, acetycholine can have excitatory, inhibitory,
or modulatory effects.
Aspartate
An excitatory amino acid neurotransmitter that is widely used by vestibular
receptors, afferents, and many neurons in the brain.
Compensatory reflexes
A stabilizing motor reflex that occurs in response to a perceived movement,
such as the vestibuloocular reflex, or the postural responses that occur during
running or skiing.
Depolarized
When receptor hair cells have mechanically gated channels open, the cell
increases its membrane voltage, which produces a release of neurotransmitter
to excite the innervating nerve fiber.
Detection thresholds
The smallest amount of head motion that can be reliably reported by an
observer.
Directional tuning
The preferred direction of motion that hair cells and afferents exhibit where a
peak excitatory response occurs and the least preferred direction where no
response occurs. Cells are said to be tuned for a best and worst direction of
motion, with in-between motion directions eliciting a lesser but observable
response.
Gamma-aminobutyric acid
A major inhibitory neurotransmitter in the vestibular commissural system.
Gaze stability
A combination of eye, neck, and head responses that are all coordinated to
maintain visual fixation (fovea) upon a point of interest.
Glutamate
An excitatory amino acid neurotransmitter that is widely used by vestibular
receptors, afferents, and many neurons in the brain.
Hair cells
The receptor cells of the vestibular system. They are termed hair cells due to
the many hairlike cilia that extend from the apical surface of the cell into the
gelatin membrane. Mechanical gated ion channels in the tips of the cilia open
and close as the cilia bend to cause membrane voltage changes in the hair cell
that are proportional to the intensity and direction of motion.
Hyperpolarizes
When receptor hair cells have mechanically gated channels close, the cell
decreases
its
membrane
voltage,
which
produces
less
release
of
Neurotransmitters
A chemical compound used to send signals from a receptor cell to a neuron, or
from one neuron to another. Neurotransmitters can be excitatory, inhibitory,
or modulatory and are packaged in small vesicles that are released from the
end terminals of cells.
Oculomotor nuclei
Includes three neuronal groups in the brainstem, the abducens nucleus, the
oculomotor nucleus, and the trochlear nucleus, whose cells send motor
commands to the six pairs of eye muscles.
Oculomotor nucleus
A group of cells in the middle brainstem that contain subgroups of neurons
that project to the medial rectus, inferior oblique, inferior rectus, and superior
rectus muscles of the eyes through the 3rd cranial nerve.
Otoconia
Small calcium carbonate particles that are packed in a layer on top of the gelatin
membrane that covers the otolith receptor hair cell stereocilia.
Otolith receptors
Two inner ear vestibular receptors (utricle and saccule) that transduce linear
accelerations and head tilt relative to gravity into neural signals that are then
transferred to the brain.
Proprioceptive
Sensory information regarding muscle position and movement arising from
receptors in the muscles, tendons, and joints.
Semicircular canals
A set of three inner ear vestibular receptors (horizontal, anterior, posterior) that
transduce head rotational accelerations into head rotational velocity signals
that are then transferred to the brain. There are three semicircular canals in
each ear, with the major planes of each canal being orthogonal to each other.
Stereocilia
Hairlike projections from the top of the receptor hair cells. The stereocilia are
arranged in ascending height and when displaced toward the tallest cilia, the
mechanical gated channels open and the cell is excited (depolarized). When
the stereocilia are displaced toward the smallest cilia, the channels close and
the cell is inhibited (hyperpolarized).
Torsion
A rotational eye movement around the line of sight that consists of a clockwise
or counterclockwise direction.
Vergence angle
The angle between the line of sight for the two eyes. Low vergence angles
indicate far-viewing objects, whereas large angles indicate viewing of near
objects.
Vestibular compensation
Following injury to one side of vestibular receptors or the vestibulocochlear
nerve, the central vestibular nuclei neurons gradually recover much of their
function through plasticity mechanisms. The recovery is never complete,
however, and extreme motion environments can lead to dizziness, nausea,
problems with balance, and spatial memory.
Vestibular efferents
Nerve fibers originating from a nucleus in the brainstem that project from the
brain to innervate the vestibular receptor hair cells and afferent nerve terminals.
Efferents have a modulatory role on their targets, which is not well understood.
Vestibular system
Consists of a set of motion and gravity detection receptors in the inner ear, a
set of primary nuclei in the brainstem, and a network of pathways carrying
motion and gravity signals to many regions of the brain.
Vestibulocochlear nerve
The VIIIth cranial nerve that carries fibers innervating the vestibular receptors
and the cochlea.
Vestibuloocular reflex
Eye movements produced by the vestibular brainstem that are equal in
magnitude and opposite in direction to head motion. The VOR functions to
maintain visual stability on a point of interest and is nearly perfect for all natural
head movements.
Reference List
Angelaki, D. E., McHenry, M. Q., & Hess, B. J. (2000). Primate translational
vestibuloocular reflexes. I. High-frequency dynamics and three-dimensional
properties during lateral motion. J. Neurophysiol 83, 16371647.
Angelaki, D. E., Shaikh, A. G., Green, A. M., & Dickman, J. D. (2004). Neurons
compute internal models of the physical laws of motion. Nature, 430, 560
564.
Anniko, M., & Arnold, W. (1991). Acetylcholine receptor localization in human
adult cochlear and vestibular hair cells. AC and Otolar, 111, 491499.
Beraneck, M., Hachemaoui, M., Idoux, E., Ris, L., Uno, A., Godaux, E., . . . Vibert,
N. (2003). Long-term plasticity of ipsilesional medial vestibular nucleus
neurons after unilateral labyrinthectomy. J Neurophysiol, 90, 184203.
Blanks, R.H.I., Curthoys, I. S., Bennett, M. L., & Markham, C. H. (1985). Planar
relationships of the semicircular canals in rhesus and squirrel monkeys.
Brain Res, 340, 315324.
Boyle, R., Carey, J. P., & Highstein, S.M. (1991). Morphological correlates of
response dynamics and efferent stimulation in horizontal semicircular canal
afferents of the toadfish, Opsanus tau. J Neurophysiol, 66, 15041511.
Brodal, A. The vestibular nuclei in the macaque monkey. (1984). J. Comp. Neurol.,
227, 252266.
Carleton, S. C., & Carpenter, M. B. (1984). Distribution of primary vestibular
fibers in the brainstem and cerebellum of the monkey. Brain Res, 294, 281
298.
Chen, A., DeAngelis, G. C., & Angelaki, D. E. (2011). A comparison of vestibular
spatiotemporal tuning in macaque parietoinsular vestibular cortex, ventral
intraparietal area, and medial superior temporal area. J. Neurosci., 31, 3082
3094.
Crane, B. T., & Demer, J. L. (1997). Human gaze stabilization during natural
activities: translation, rotation, magnification, and target distance effects. J
Neurophysiol, 78, 21292144.
Dickman, J. D., & Angelaki, D. E. (2002). Vestibular convergence patterns in
vestibular nuclei neurons of alert primates. J. Neurophysiol., 88, 35183533.
Dickman, J. D., & Correia, M. J. (1989). Responses of pigeon horizontal
semicircular canal afferent fibers. I. Step, trapezoid, and low frequency
sinusoid mechanical and rotational stimulation. J Neurophysiol, 62, 1090
1101.
Dickman, J. D., & Fang, Q. (1996). Differential central projections of vestibular
afferents in pigeons. J Comp Neurol, 367, 110121.
Einstein, A. (1907).On the relativity principle and the conclusions drawn from
it. Jahrb. Radioakt, 4, 411462.
Ezure, K., & Graf, K.W. (1984). A quantitative analysis of the spatial organization
of the vestibulo-ocular reflexes in lateral- and frontal-eyed animals. I.
Orientation of semicircular canals and extraocular muscles. Neuroscience,
12, 8594.
mstd and heading perception based on vestibular signals. Nat Neurosci, 10,
10381047.
Gu, Y., Watkins, P. V., Angelaki, D. E., & DeAngelis, G. C. (2006). Visual and
nonvisual contributions to three-dimensional heading selectivity in the
medial superior temporal area. J Neurosci, 26, 7385.
Guldin, W. O., Akbarian, S., & Grusser, O. J. (1992). Cortico-cortical connections
and cytoarchitectonics of the primate vestibular cortex: A study in squirrel
monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). J. Comp. Neurol., 326, 375401.
Hafting, T., Fyhn, M., Molden, S., Moser, M.-B., Moser, E. I. (2005). Microstructure
of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature, 436, 801806.
Iurato, S. (1967). Submicroscopic structure of the inner ear. London, England:
Pergamon Press.
Kasahara, M., & Uchino, Y. (1974). Bilateral semicircular canal inputs to neurons
in cat vestibular nuclei. Exp Brain Res, 20, 285296.
Keshner, E. A., & Peterson, B. W. (1995). Mechanisms controlling human head
stabilization. I. Head-neck dynamics during random rotations in the
horizontal plane. J Neurophysiol, 73, 22932301.
Lindeman, H. H. (1969). Studies on the morphology of the sensory regions of
the vestibular apparatus with 45 figures. Ergeb Anat Entwicklungsgesch, 42,
1113.
Lisberger, S. G., Pavelko, T. A., & Broussard, D. M. (1994). Neural basis for motor
learning in the vestibuloocular reflex of primates. I. Changes in the
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. The Vestibular System by Dora Angelaki and J.
David Dickman is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercialShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
There are profound cultural differences in how people think about, measure,
and use their time. This chapter describes some major dimensions of time that
are most prone to cultural variation.
Learning Objectives
Understand how cultures differ in the views of time and the importance of
these differences for social behavior.
Introduction
It is said that time is money in industrialized economies. Workers are paid by
the hour, lawyers charge by the minute, and advertising is sold by the second
(US$3.3 million for a 30-second commercial, or a little over $110,000 per second,
for the 2012 Super Bowl). Remarkably, the civilized mind has reduced time
the most obscure and abstract of all intangiblesto the most objective of all
quantities: money. With time and things on the same value scale, we can
establish how many of our working hours equal the price of a product in a store.
This way of thinking about time is not universal, however. Beliefs about time
remain profoundly different from culture to culture. Research shows that
cultural differences in time can be as vast as those between languages. In one
particularly telling study of the roots of culture shock, Spradley and Phillips
asked a group of returning Peace Corps volunteers to rank 33 items concerning
the amount of cultural adjustment each had required of them. The list included
a wide range of items familiar to fearful travelers, such as the type of food
eaten, the personal cleanliness of most people, the number of people of
your own race, and the general standard of living. But aside from mastering
the foreign language, the two greatest difficulties for the Peace Corps volunteers
were concerned with social time: the general pace of life, followed by one of
its most significant components, how punctual most people are (Spradley &
Phillips, 1972).
Half a century ago anthropologist Edward Hall described cultural rules of
social time as the silent language (Hall, 1983). These informal patterns of
time are seldom, if ever, made explicit. They exist in the air around us. They
are either familiar and comfortable or unfamiliar and wrong. The world over,
children simply pick up their societys conceptions of early and late, of waiting
and rushing, of the past, the present, and the future, as they mature. No
dictionary clearly defines these rules of time for them or for strangers who
stumble over the maddening incongruities between the time sense they bring
nobaproject.com - Time and Culture
507
508
Sequence
Each culture sets rules concerning the appropriate sequence of tasks and
activities. Is it work before play, or vice versa? Do people take all of their sleep
at night, or is there a siesta in the midafternoon? Is one expected to have coffee
or tea and socialize, and for how long, before getting down to serious business?
There are also customs about sequences over the long run. For example, how
long is the socially accepted period of childhood, if it exists at all, and when is
it time to assume the responsibilities of an adult?
509
Calendars
Many cultures use social activities to define their calendars rather than the
other way around. The calendars of the Nuer people from the Upper Nile in the
Sudan, for example, are based on the seasonal changes in their environment.
They know that the month of kur is occurring because they are building their
fishing dams and cattle camps. When they break camp and return to their
villages, they know it must now be the month of dwat.
nobaproject.com - Time and Culture
510
Most societies have some type of week, but it is not always seven days long.
The Muysca of Columbia had a three-day week. The Incas of Peru had a 10-day
week. Often the length of the week reflects cycles of activities, rather than the
other way around. For many, the market is the main activity requiring group
coordination. The Khasi people hold their markets every eighth day.
Consequently, they have made their week eight days long and named the days
of the week after the places where the main markets occur (Levine, 2005).
psychologists
emphasize
the
significance
of
511
conclusion.
Levine (2012) argues for the value of shifting between each approach
depending on the characteristics of the individuals and the situations involved.
In a corporation, for example, some positions may require tight scheduling of
time (e.g., accountants during tax time). On the other hand, employees in
research and development may be most productive when less tightly controlled.
512
waste of ones time in a larger sense, that it is a wasteful way to spend ones
life. If something more worthy of ones attentionbe it social- or work-related
challenges a planned schedule, it is seen as wasteful to not deviate from the
planned schedule. In fact, the term wasted time may make little sense. A
typical comment may be, There is no such thing as wasted time. If you are not
doing one thing, you are doing something else (Levine, 1997).
Temporal Orientation
There are individual and cultural differences in peoples orientation toward the
past, present, and future. Zimbardo and Boyd (2008) have developed a scale
that distinguishes between six types of temporal frames:
1. Past negativea pessimistic, negative, or aversive orientation toward the
nobaproject.com - Time and Culture
513
past.
2. Past positivea warm, sentimental, nostalgic, and positive construction of
the past.
3. Present hedonistichedonistic orientation attitude toward time and life.
4. Present fatalistica fatalistic, helpless, and hopeless attitude toward the
future and life.
5. Futureplanning for, and achievement of, future goals, characterizing a
general future orientation.
6. Future transcendentalan orientation to the future beyond ones own
death.
Zimbardo and Boyd have found large individual and cultural differences on
both the individual subscales and the patterns of the subscales taken together.
They describe a wide range of consequences of these differences. Time
perspective affects political, economic, personal, social, environmental, and
other domains of life and society. One of the paradoxes, they report, is that
each particular temporal perspective is associated with numerous personal
and social benefits but that, in excess, they are associated with even greater
costs. There are both positive and negative processes associated with each
perspective. Individuals who focus on the past, for example, are often described
with terms such as happy, grateful, patriotic, high self-esteem, and having strong
personal values; on the other hand, past time perspective can be associated
with terms such as depressed, guilty, angry and revengeful, and resistant to
change. Similarly, a focus on the present may be associated with strong social
nobaproject.com - Time and Culture
514
affiliations, joy, sensuality, sexuality, energy, and improvisation; but it may also
be associated with violence, anger, over-fatalism, risk-taking, and addictive
behavior. A focus on the future may be associated with achievement, selfefficacy, healthy behaviors, and hope for change; but also with anxiety, social
isolation, competitiveness, and unhealthy physical consequences ranging from
coronary artery disease to sexual impotence. The authors argue for the
importance of a healthy balance in ones temporal orientation.
Conclusion
Understanding the values and assumptions a culture places on these temporal
dimensions is essential to creating policies that enhance the quality of peoples
lives. The historian Lewis Mumford once observed how each culture believes
that every other space and time is an approximation to or perversion of the
real space and time in which it lives. The truth, however, is there is no single
correct way to think about time. There are different ways of thinking, each with
their pluses and minuses, and all may be of value in given situations.
515
Outside Resources
Video: Dealing with Time
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=fSvC3i4Sqp4
Video: RSA AnimateThe Secret Powers of Time
www.youtube.com/watch?v=A3oIiH7BLmg&feature=related
Discussion Questions
1. Can you give an example of Edward Halls notion of time as a silent
language?
2. Can you give an example of clock time in your own life? Can you give an
example of event time?
3. Are there activities where you might benefit from another cultures approach
to time rather than your usual approach? Give an example.
4. What do you think are the consequences, both positive and negative, of a
faster pace of life?
5. Is it fair to conclude that some cultural time practices are more advanced
than others? That some are healthier than others? Explain.
Vocabulary
Clock time
Scheduling activities according to the time on the clock.
Ma
Japanese way of thinking that emphasizes attention to the spaces between
things rather than the things themselves.
Monochronic (M-time)
Monochronic thinking focuses on doing one activity, from beginning to
completion, at a time.
Pace of life
The frequency of events per unit of time; also referred to as speed or tempo.
Polychronic (P-time)
Polychronic thinking switches back and forth among multiple activities as the
situation demands.
Silent language
Cultural norms of time and time use as they pertain to social communication
and interaction.
Social time
Scheduling by the flow of the activity. Events begin and end when, by mutual
consensus, participants feel the time is right.
Temporal perspective
The extent to which we are oriented toward the past, present, and future.
Reference List
Birth, K. (1999). Any time is Trinidad time: Social meanings and temporal
consciousness. Gainesville, FL: University Press of Florida.
Bluedorn, A. (2002). The human organization of time: Temporal realities and
experience. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford Business Books.
Brislin, R. (2000). Understanding cultures influence on behavior (2nd ed.). Fort
Worth, TX: Harcourt.
Brislin, R., & Kim, E. (2003). Cultural diversity in peoples understanding and uses
of time. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 52(3), 363382.
Hall, E. T. (1983). The dance of life: The other dimension of time. Garden City:
Anchor Press.
Lauer, R. (1981). Temporal man: The meaning and uses of social time. New York,
NY: Praeger.
Levine, R. (2012). Time use and happiness: Implications for social policy. Thimpu,
Bhutan: Centre for Bhutanese Studies.
Levine, R. (2005). A geography of busyness. Social Research, 72, 355370.
Levine, R. (1997). A geography of time. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Levine, R., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). The pace of life in 31 countries. Journal of
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30, 178205.
Spradley, J. P., & Phillips, M. (1972). Culture and stress: A quantitative analysis.
American Anthropologist, 74, 518 529.
Zimbardo, P., & Boyd, J. (2008). The time paradox. New York, NY: Simon &
Schuster.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Time and Culture by Robert V. Levine is licensed
under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International
License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
deed.en_US.
Abstract
We think important objects and events in our world will automatically grab our
attention, but they often dont, particularly when our attention is focused on
something else. The failure to notice unexpected objects or events when
attention is focused elsewhere is now known as inattentional blindness. The
study of such failures of awareness has a long history, but their practical
importance has received increasing attention over the past decade. This chapter
describes the history and status of research on inattentional blindness,
discusses the reasons why we find these results to be counterintuitive, and the
implications of failures of awareness for how we see and act in our world.
Learning Objectives
Do you regularly spot editing errors in movies? Can you multitask effectively,
texting while talking with your friends or watching television? Are you fully aware
of your surroundings? If you answered yes to any of those questions, youre not
alone. And, youre most likely wrong.
More than 50 years ago, experimental psychologists began documenting
the many ways that our perception of the world is limited, not by our eyes and
ears, but by our minds. We appear able to process only one stream of
information at a time, effectively filtering other information from awareness.
To a large extent, we perceive only that which receives the focus of our cognitive
efforts: our attention.
Imagine the following task, known as dichotic listening (e.g., Cherry, 1953;
Moray, 1959; Treisman, 1960): You put on a set of headphones that play two
completely different speech streams, one to your left ear and one to your right
ear. Your task is to repeat each syllable spoken into your left ear as quickly and
accurately as possible, mimicking each sound as you hear it. When performing
this attention-demanding task, you wont notice if the speaker in your right ear
switches to a different language or is replaced by a different speaker with a
similar voice. You wont notice if the content of their speech becomes
nonsensical. In effect, you are deaf to the substance of the ignored speech. But,
that is not because of the limits of your auditory senses. It is a form of cognitive
deafness, due to the nature of focused, selective attention. Even if the speaker
on your right headphone says your name, you will notice it only about one-third
of the time (Conway, Cowan, & Bunting, 2001). And, at least by some accounts,
you only notice it that often because you still devote some of your limited
attention to the ignored speech stream (Holendar, 1986). In this task, you will
tend to notice only large physical changes (e.g., a switch from a male to a female
speaker), but not substantive ones, except in rare cases.
nobaproject.com - Failures of Awareness: The Case of Inattentional Blindness
526
527
528
matches that of the black-shirted players (Simons & Chabris, 1999). However,
even unique items can go unnoticed. In one task, people monitored black
shapes and ignored white shapes that moved around a computer window (Most
et al., 2001). Approximately 30 percent of them failed to detect the bright red
cross traversing the display, even though it was the only colored item and was
visible for five seconds.
Another crucial influence on noticing is the effort you put into the attentiondemanding task. If you have to keep separate counts of bounce passes and
aerial passes, you are less likely to notice the gorilla (Simons & Chabris, 1999),
and if you are tracking faster moving objects, you are less likely to notice (Simons
& Jensen, 2009). You can even miss unexpected visual objects when you devote
your limited cognitive resources to a memory task (Fougnie & Marois, 2007), so
the limits are not purely visual. Instead, they appear to reflect limits on the
capacity of attention. Without attention to the unexpected event, you are
unlikely to become aware of it (Mack & Rock, 1998; Most, Scholl, Clifford, &
Simons, 2005).
Inattentional blindness is not just a laboratory curiosityit also occurs in
the real world and under more natural conditions. In a recent study (Chabris,
Weinberger, Fontaine, & Simons, 2011), Chabris and colleagues simulated a
famous police misconduct case in which a Boston police officer was convicted
of lying because he claimed not to have seen a brutal beating (Lehr, 2009). At
the time, he had been chasing a murder suspect and ran right past the scene
of a brutal assault. In Chabris simulation, subjects jogged behind an
experimenter who ran right past a simulated fight scene. At night, 65 percent
missed the fight scene. Even during broad daylight, 44 percent of observers
jogged right passed it without noticing, lending some plausibility to the Boston
cops story that he was telling the truth and never saw the beating.
nobaproject.com - Failures of Awareness: The Case of Inattentional Blindness
529
530
studies suggest that those people who have a greater working memory capacity
are more likely to notice unexpected objects (Hannon & Richards, 2010;
Richards, Hannon, & Derakshan, 2010). In effect, those who have more
resources available when focusing attention are more likely to spot other
aspects of their world. However, other studies find no such relationship: Those
with greater working memory capacity are not any more likely to spot an
unexpected object or event (Seegmiller, Watson, & Strayer, 2011; Bredemeier
& Simons, 2012). There are theoretical reasons to predict each pattern. With
more resources available, people should be more likely to notice (see
Macdonald & Lavie, 2011). However, people with greater working memory
capacity also tend to be better able to maintain their focus on their prescribed
task, meaning that they should be less likely to notice. At least one study
suggests that the ability to perform a task does not predict the likelihood of
noticing (Simons & Jensen, 2009; for a replication, see Bredemeier & Simons,
2012). In a study I conducted with Melinda Jensen, we measured how well people
could track moving objects around a display, gradually increasing the speed
until people reached a level of 75% accuracy. Tracking ability varied greatly:
Some people could track objects at more than twice the speed others could.
Yet, the ability to track objects more easily was unrelated to the odds of noticing
an unexpected event. Apparently, as long as people try to perform the tracking
task, they are relatively unlikely to notice unexpected events.
What makes these findings interesting and important is that they run counter
to our intuitions. Most people are confident they would notice the chestthumping gorilla. In fact, nearly 90%believe they would spot the gorilla (Levin
& Angelone, 2008), and in a national survey, 78% agreed with the statement,
People generally notice when something unexpected enters their field of view,
even when theyre paying attention to something else (Simons & Chabris, 2010).
Similarly, people are convinced that they would spot errors in movies or changes
to a conversation partner (Levin & Angelone, 2008). We think we see and
remember far more of our surroundings than we actually do. But why do we
nobaproject.com - Failures of Awareness: The Case of Inattentional Blindness
531
532
phone when driving? The reason might well be the same mistaken intuition that
makes inattentional blindness surprising: Drivers simply do not notice how
distracted they are when they are talking on a phone, so they believe they can
drive just as well when talking on a phone even though they cant (Strayer &
Johnston, 2001).
So, what can you do about inattentional blindness? The short answer
appears to be, not much. There is no magical elixir that will overcome the
limits on attention, allowing you to notice everything (and that would not be a
good outcome anyway). But, there is something you can do to mitigate the
consequences of such limits. Now that you know about inattentional blindness,
you can take steps to limit its impact by recognizing how your intuitions will
lead you astray.
First, maximize the attention you do have available by avoiding distractions,
especially under conditions for which an unexpected event might be
catastrophic. The ring of a new call or the ding of a new text are hard to resist,
so make it impossible to succumb to the temptation by turning your phone off
or putting it somewhere out of reach when you are driving. If you know that
you will be tempted and you know that using your phone will increase
inattentional blindness, you must be proactive. Second, pay attention to what
others might not notice. If you are a bicyclist, dont assume that the driver sees
you, even if they appear to make eye contact. Looking is not the same as seeing.
Only by understanding the limits of attention and by recognizing our mistaken
beliefs about what we know to be true can we avoid the modern-day
consequences of those limits.
533
Outside Resources
Article: Scholarpedia article on inattentional blindness
http://www.scholarpedia.org/article/Inattentional_blindness
Video: The original gorilla video
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=vJG698U2Mvo
Video: The sequel to the gorilla video
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=IGQmdoK_ZfY
Web: Website for Chabris & Simons book, The Invisible Gorilla. Includes links
to videos and descriptions of the research on inattentional blindness
http://www.theinvisiblegorilla.com
Discussion Questions
1. Many people, upon learning about inattentional blindness, try to think of
ways to eliminate it, allowing themselves complete situation awareness.
Why might we be far worse off if we were not subject to inattentional
blindness?
2. If inattentional blindness cannot be eliminated, what steps might you take
to avoid its consequences?
3. Can you think of situations in which inattentional blindness is highly likely
to be a problem? Can you think of cases in which inattentional blindness
would not have much of an impact?
Vocabulary
Dichotic listening
A task in which different audio streams are presented to each ear. Typically,
people are asked to monitor one stream while ignoring the other.
Inattentional blindness
The failure to notice a fully visible, but unexpected, object or event when
attention is devoted to something else.
Inattentional deafness
The auditory analog of inattentional blindness. People fail to notice an
unexpected sound or voice when attention is devoted to other aspects of a
scene.
Selective listening
A method for studying selective attention in which people focus attention on
one auditory stream of information while deliberately ignoring other auditory
information.
Reference List
Bredemeier, K., & Simons, D. J. (2012). Working memory and inattentional
blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 239244.
Chabris, C. F., Weinberger, A., Fontaine, M., & Simons, D. J. (2011). You do not
talk about fight club if you do not notice fight club: Inattentional blindness
for a simulated real-world assault. i-Perception, 2, 150153.
Cherry, E. C. (1953). Experiments on the recognition of speech with one and two
ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975979.
Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., & Bunting, M. F. (2001). The cocktail party
phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 331335.
Dalton, P., & Fraenkel, N. (2012). Gorillas we have missed: Sustained
inattentional deafness for dynamic events. Cognition, 124, 367372.
Levin, D. T., & Angelone, B. L. (2008). The visual metacognition questionnaire: A
measure of intuitions about vision. The American Journal of Psychology,
121, 451472.
Macdonald, J. S. P., & Lavie, N. (2011). Visual perceptual load induces
inattentional deafness. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1780
1789.
Mack A., & Rock I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence
to
inattentional
blindness.
Journal
of
Experimental
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Failures of Awareness: The Case of Inattentional
Blindness by Daniel Simons is licensed under the Creative Commons AttributionNonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Multi-Modal Perception
Lorin Lachs
California State University, Fresno
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Most of the time, we perceive the world as a unified bundle of sensations from
multiple sensory modalities. In other words, our perception is multimodal. This
chapter provides an overview of multimodal perception, including information
about its neurobiology and its psychological effects.
Learning Objectives
Perception: Unified
Although it has been traditional to study the various senses independently,
most of the time, perception operates in the context of information supplied
by multiple sensory modalities at the same time. For example, imagine if you
witnessed a car collision. You could describe the stimulus generated by this
event by considering each of the senses independently; that is, as a set of
unimodal stimuli. Your eyes would be stimulated with patterns of light energy
bouncing off the cars involved. Your ears would be stimulated with patterns of
acoustic energy emanating from the collision. Your nose might even be
stimulated by the smell of burning rubber or gasoline. However, all of this
information would be relevant to the same thing: your perception of the car
collision. Indeed, unless someone was to explicitly ask you to describe your
perception in unimodal terms, you would most likely experience the event as
a unified bundle of sensations from multiple senses. In other words, your
perception would be multimodal. The question is whether the various sources
of information involved in this multimodal stimulus are processed separately
by the perceptual system or not.
For the last few decades, perceptual research has pointed to the importance
of multimodal perception: the effects on the perception of events and objects
in the world that are observed when there is information from more than one
sensory modality. Most of this research indicates that, at some point in
perceptual processing, information from the various sensory modalities is
integrated. In other words, the information is combined and treated as a unitary
representation of the world.
544
545
not equal the response to the multimodal stimulus. This superadditive effect
of multisensory integration indicates that there are consequences resulting
from the integrated processing of multimodal stimuli.
The extent of the superadditive effect (sometimes referred to as
multisensory enhancement) is determined by the strength of the response to
the single stimulus modality with the biggest effect. To understand this concept,
imagine someone speaking to you in a noisy environment (such as a crowded
party). When discussing this type of multimodal stimulus, it is often useful to
describe it in terms of its unimodal components: In this case, there is an
auditory component (the sounds generated by the speech of the person
speaking to you) and a visual component (the visual form of the face movements
as the person speaks to you). In the crowded party, the auditory component of
the persons speech might be difficult to process (because of the surrounding
party noise). The potential for visual information about speechlipreading
to help in understanding the speakers message is, in this situation, quite large.
However, if you were listening to that same person speak in a quiet library, the
auditory portion would probably be sufficient for receiving the message, and
the visual portion would help very little, if at all (Sumby & Pollack, 1954). In
general, for a stimulus with multimodal components, if the response to each
component (on its own) is weak, then the opportunity for multisensory
enhancement is very large. However, if one componentby itselfis sufficient
to evoke a strong response, then the opportunity for multisensory
enhancement is relatively small. This finding is called the Principle of Inverse
Effectiveness (Stein & Meredith, 1993) because the effectiveness of
multisensory enhancement is inversely related to the unimodal response with
the greatest effect.
Another important theoretical question about multimodal perception
concerns the neurobiology that supports it. After all, at some point, the
information from each sensory modality is definitely separated (e.g., light comes
in through the eyes, and sound comes in through the ears). How does the brain
nobaproject.com - Multi-Modal Perception
546
take information from different neural systems (optic, auditory, etc.) and
combine it? If our experience of the world is multimodal, then it must be the
case that at some point during perceptual processing, the unimodal information
coming from separate sensory organssuch as the eyes, ears, skinis
combined. A related question asks where in the brain this integration takes
place. We turn to these questions in the next section.
547
548
549
There may be two ways for these multimodal interactions to occur. First, it
could be that the processing of auditory information in relatively late stages of
processing feeds back to influence low-level processing of visual information
in unimodal cortex (McDonald, Teder-Slejrvi, Russo, & Hillyard, 2003).
Alternatively, it may be that areas of unimodal cortex contact each other directly
(Driver & Noesselt, 2008; Macaluso & Driver, 2005), such that multimodal
integration is a fundamental component of all sensory processing.
In fact, the large numbers of multisensory neurons distributed all around
the cortexin multisensory convergence areas and in primary corticeshas
led some researchers to propose that a drastic reconceptualization of the brain
is necessary (Ghazanfar & Schroeder, 2006). They argue that the cortex should
not be considered as being divided into isolated regions that process only one
kind of sensory information. Rather, they propose that these areas only prefer
to process information from specific modalities but engage in low-level
multisensory processing whenever it is beneficial to the perceiver (Vasconcelos
et al., 2011).
550
Multimodal Phenomena
Audiovisual Speech
Multimodal phenomena concern stimuli that generate simultaneous (or nearly
simultaneous) information in more than one sensory modality. As discussed
above, speech is a classic example of this kind of stimulus. When an individual
speaks, she generates sound waves that carry meaningful information. If the
perceiver is also looking at the speaker, then that perceiver also has access to
visual patterns that carry meaningful information. Of course, as anyone who
has ever tried to lipread knows, there are limits on how informative visual speech
information is. Even so, the visual speech pattern alone is sufficient for very
robust speech perception. Most people assume that deaf individuals are much
better at lipreading than individuals with normal hearing. It may come as a
surprise to learn, however, that some individuals with normal hearing are also
remarkably good at lipreading (sometimes called speechreading). In fact, there
is a wide range of speechreading ability in both normal hearing and deaf
populations (Andersson, Lyxell, Rnnberg, & Spens, 2001). However, the
reasons for this wide range of performance are not well understood (Auer &
nobaproject.com - Multi-Modal Perception
551
Bernstein, 2007; Bernstein, 2006; Bernstein, Auer, & Tucker, 2001; Mohammed
et al., 2005).
How does visual information about speech interact with auditory
information about speech? One of the earliest investigations of this question
examined the accuracy of recognizing spoken words presented in a noisy
context, much like in the example above about talking at a crowded party. To
study this phenomenon experimentally, some irrelevant noise (white noise
which sounds like a radio tuned between stations) was presented to
participants. Embedded in the white noise were spoken words, and the
participants task was to identify the words. There were two conditions: one in
which only the auditory component of the words was presented (the auditoryalone condition), and one in both the auditory and visual components were
presented (the audiovisual condition). The noise levels were also varied, so
that on some trials, the noise was very loud relative to the loudness of the
words, and on other trials, the noise was very soft relative to the words. Sumby
and Pollack (1954) found that the accuracy of identifying the spoken words was
much higher for the audiovisual condition than it was in the auditory-alone
condition. In addition, the pattern of results was consistent with the Principle
of Inverse Effectiveness: The advantage gained by audiovisual presentation was
highest when the auditory-alone condition performance was lowest (i.e., when
the noise was loudest). At these noise levels, the audiovisual advantage was
considerable: It was estimated that allowing the participant to see the speaker
was equivalent to turning the volume of the noise down by over half. Clearly,
the audiovisual advantage can have dramatic effects on behavior.
Another phenomenon using audiovisual speech is a very famous illusion
called the McGurk effect (named after one of its discoverers). In the classic
formulation of the illusion, a movie is recorded of a speaker saying the syllables
gaga. Another movie is made of the same speaker saying the syllables baba.
Then, the auditory portion of the baba movie is dubbed onto the visual portion
of the gaga movie. This combined stimulus is presented to participants, who
nobaproject.com - Multi-Modal Perception
552
are asked to report what the speaker in the movie said. McGurk and MacDonald
(1976) reported that 98 percent of their participants reported hearing the
syllable dadawhich was in neither the visual nor the auditory components
of the stimulus. These results indicate that when visual and auditory
information about speech is integrated, it can have profound effects on
perception.
553
Crossmodal Phenomena
Crossmodal phenomena are distinguished from multimodal phenomena in
that they concern the influence one sensory modality has on the perception of
another.
554
Crossmodal Speech
Several crossmodal phenomena have also been discovered for speech stimuli.
These crossmodal speech effects usually show altered perceptual processing
of unimodal stimuli (e.g., acoustic patterns) by virtue of prior experience with
the alternate unimodal stimulus (e.g., optical patterns). For example,
Rosenblum, Miller, and Sanchez (2007) conducted an experiment examining
the ability to become familiar with a persons voice. Their first interesting finding
was unimodal: Much like what happens when someone repeatedly hears a
person speak, perceivers can become familiar with the visual voice of a
nobaproject.com - Multi-Modal Perception
555
speaker. That is, they can become familiar with the persons speaking style
simply by seeing that person speak. Even more astounding was their
crossmodal finding: Familiarity with this visual information also led to increased
recognition of the speakers auditory speech, to which participants had never
had exposure.
Similarly, it has been shown that when perceivers see a speaking face, they
can identify the (auditory-alone) voice of that speaker, and vice versa (Kamachi,
Hill, Lander, & Vatikiotis-Bateson, 2003; Lachs & Pisoni, 2004a, 2004b, 2004c;
Rosenblum, Smith, Nichols, Lee, & Hale, 2006). In other words, the visual form
of a speaker engaged in the act of speaking appears to contain information
about what that speaker should sound like. Perhaps more surprisingly, the
auditory form of speech seems to contain information about what the speaker
should look like.
Conclusion
In this chapter, we have reviewed some of the main evidence and findings
concerning the role of multimodal perception in our experience of the world.
It appears that our nervous system (and the cortex in particular) contains
considerable architecture for the processing of information arriving from
multiple senses. Given this neurobiological setup, and the diversity of
behavioral phenomena associated with multimodal stimuli, it is likely that the
investigation of multimodal perception will continue to be a topic of interest in
the field of experimental perception for many years to come.
556
Outside Resources
Article: A review of the neuroanatomy and methods associated with
multimodal perception:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.015
Journal: Experimental Brain Research Special issue: Crossmodal processing
http://www.springerlink.com/content/0014-4819/198/2-3
Video: McGurk demo
http://youtu.be/aFPtc8BVdJk
Video: The Rubber Hand Illusion
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=sxwn1w7MJvk
Web: Double-flash illusion demo
http://www.cns.atr.jp/~kmtn/soundInducedIllusoryFlash2/
Discussion Questions
1. The extensive network of multisensory areas and neurons in the cortex
implies that much perceptual processing occurs in the context of multiple
inputs. Could the processing of unimodal information ever be useful? Why
or why not?
2. Some researchers have argued that the Principle of Inverse Effectiveness
(PoIE) results from ceiling effects: Multisensory enhancement cannot take
place when one modality is sufficient for processing because in such cases
it is not possible for processing to be enhanced (because performance is
already at the ceiling). On the other hand, other researchers claim that the
PoIE stems from the perceptual systems ability to assess the relative value
of stimulus cues, and to use the most reliable sources of information to
construct a representation of the outside world. What do you think? Could
these two possibilities ever be teased apart? What kinds of experiments
might one conduct to try to get at this issue?
3. In the late 17th century, a scientist named William Molyneux asked the
famous philosopher John Locke a question relevant to modern studies of
multisensory processing. The question was this: Imagine a person who has
been blind since birth, and who is able, by virtue of the sense of touch, to
identify three dimensional shapes such as spheres or pyramids. Now
imagine that this person suddenly receives the ability to see. Would the
person, without using the sense of touch, be able to identify those same
shapes visually? Can modern research in multimodal perception help
answer this question? Why or why not? How do the studies about
crossmodal phenomena inform us about the answer to this question?
Vocabulary
Integrated
The process by which the perceptual system combines information arising from
more than one modality.
Multimodal
Of or pertaining to multiple sensory modalities.
Multimodal perception
The effects that concurrent stimulation in more than one sensory modality has
on the perception of events and objects in the world.
Multisensory enhancement
See superadditive effect of multisensory integration.
Sensory modalities
A type of sense; for example, vision or audition.
Unimodal
Of or pertaining to a single sensory modality.
Unimodal components
The parts of a stimulus relevant to one sensory modality at a time.
Reference List
Andersson, U., Lyxell, B., Rnnberg, J., & Spens, K.-E. (2001). Cognitive correlates
of visual speech understanding in hearing-impaired individuals. Journal of
Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 6(2), 103116. doi: 10.1093/
deafed/6.2.103
Auer, E. T., Jr., & Bernstein, L. E. (2007). Enhanced visual speech perception in
individuals with early-onset hearing impairment. Journal of Speech,
Language, and Hearing Research, 50(5), 11571165. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388
(2007/080)
Bernstein, L. E. (2006). Visual speech perception. In E. Vatikiotis-Bateson, G.
Bailley, & P. Perrier (Eds.), Audio-visual speech processing. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Bernstein, L. E., Auer, E. T., Jr., & Tucker, P. E. (2001). Enhanced speechreading
in deaf adults: Can short-term training/practice close the gap for hearing
adults? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 518.
Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands /`feel/' touch that eyes see.
[10.1038/35784]. Nature, 391(6669), 756756.
Calvert, G. A. (2001). Crossmodal processing in the human brain: Insights from
functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 11101123.
Calvert, G. A., Hansen, P. C., Iversen, S. D., & Brammer, M. J. (2001). Detection of
audio-visual integration sites in humans by application of electrophysiological
criteria to the bold effect. NeuroImage, 14(2), 427438. doi: 10.1006/
nimg.2001.0812
in
Cognitive
Sciences,
10(6),
278-285.
doi:
10.1016/j.
tics.2006.04.008
Kamachi, M., Hill, H., Lander, K., & Vatikiotis-Bateson, E. (2003). "Putting the face
to the voice": Matching identity across modality. Current Biology, 13, 1709
1714.
Lachs, L., & Pisoni, D. B. (2004a). Crossmodal source identification in speech
perception. Ecological Psychology, 16(3), 159187.
Lachs, L., & Pisoni, D. B. (2004b). Crossmodal source information and spoken
word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
& Performance, 30(2), 378396.
Lachs, L., & Pisoni, D. B. (2004c). Specification of crossmodal source information
in isolated kinematic displays of speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America, 116(1), 507518.
Macaluso, E., & Driver, J. (2005). Multisensory spatial interactions: A window
onto functional integration in the human brain. Trends in Neurosciences,
28(5), 264271. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.03.008
McDonald, J. J., Teder-Slejrvi, W. A., Russo, F. D., & Hillyard, S. A. (2003). Neural
substrates of perceptual enhancement by cross-modal spatial attention.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(1), 1019. doi: 10.1162/089892903321107783
McGurk, H., & MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264,
746748.
Mohammed, T., Campbell, R., MacSweeney, M., Milne, E., Hansen, P., & Coleman,
M. (2005). Speechreading skill and visual movement sensitivity are related
in deaf speechreaders. Perception, 34(2), 205216.
Recanzone, G. H. (2003). Auditory influences on visual temporal rate perception.
Journal of Neurophysiology, 89(2), 10781093. doi: 10.1152/jn.00706.2002
Rosenblum, L. D., Miller, R. M., & Sanchez, K. (2007). Lip-read me now, hear me
better later: Cross-modal transfer of talker-familiarity effects. Psychological
Science, 18(5), 392396.
Rosenblum, L. D., Smith, N. M., Nichols, S. M., Lee, J., & Hale, S. (2006). Hearing
a face: Cross-modal speaker matching using isolated visible speech.
Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 8493.
Sekuler, R., Sekuler, A. B., & Lau, R. (1997). Sound alters visual motion perception.
[10.1038/385308a0]. Nature, 385(6614), 308308.
Shams, L., Kamitani, Y., & Shimojo, S. (2000). Illusions. What you see is what you
hear. Nature, 408(6814), 788. doi: 10.1038/35048669
Soto-Faraco, S., Kingstone, A., & Spence, C. (2003). Multisensory contributions
to the perception of motion. Neuropsychologia, 41(13), 18471862. doi:
10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00185-4
Spence, C., Senkowski, D., & Roder, B. (2009). Crossmodal processing. [Editorial
Introductory]. Exerimental Brain Research, 198(2-3), 107111. doi: 10.1007/
s00221-009-19734
Stein, B. E., & Meredith, M. A. (1993). The merging of the senses. Cambridge,
MA: The MIT Press.
Stein, B. E., & Stanford, T. R. (2008). Multisensory integration: Current issues
from the perspective of the single neuron. [10.1038/nrn2331]. Nature
Reviews Neuroscience, 9(4), 255266.
Sumby, W. H., & Pollack, I. (1954). Visual contribution of speech intelligibility in
noise. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 26, 212215.
Vasconcelos, N., Pantoja, J., Belchior, H., Caixeta, F. V., Faber, J., Freire, M. A.
M., . . . Ribeiro, S. (2011). Cross-modal responses in the primary visual cortex
encode complex objects and correlate with tactile discrimination.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(37), 1540815413.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1102780108
Vroomen, J., & De Gelder, B. (2004). Perceptual effects of cross-modal
stimulation: Ventriloquism and the freezing phenomenon. In G. A. Calvert,
C. Spence, & B. E. Stein (Eds.), Handbook of multisensory processes.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Multi-Modal Perception by Lorin Lachs is licensed
under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International
License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
deed.en_US.
Abstract
Eyewitnesses can provide very compelling legal testimony, but rather than
recording experiences flawlessly, their memories are susceptible to a variety of
errors and biases. They (like the rest of us) can make errors in remembering
specific details and can even remember whole events that did not actually
happen. In this chapter, we discuss several of the common types of errors, and
what they can tell us about human memory and its interactions with the legal
system.
Learning Objectives
Describe the kinds of mistakes that eyewitnesses commonly make and some
of the ways that this can impede justice.
570
exonerated (and the real rapist identified) based on DNA evidence. For details
on this case and other (relatively) lucky individuals whose false convictions were
subsequently overturned with DNA evidence, see the Innocence Project website
().
There is also hope, though, that many of the errors may be avoidable if
proper precautions are taken during the investigative and judicial processes.
Psychological science has taught us what some of those precautions might
involve, and we discuss some of that science now.
Misinformation
In an early study of eyewitness memory, undergraduate subjects first watched
a slideshow depicting a small red car driving and then hitting a pedestrian
(Loftus, Miller, & Burns, 1978). Some subjects were then asked leading questions
about what had happened in the slides. For example, subjects were asked, How
fast was the car traveling when it passed the yield sign? But this question was
actually designed to be misleading, because the original slide included a stop
sign rather than a yield sign.
Later, subjects were shown pairs of slides. One of the pair was the original
slide containing the stop sign; the other was a replacement slide containing a
yield sign. Subjects were asked which of the pair they had previously seen.
Subjects who had been asked about the yield sign were likely to pick the slide
showing the yield sign, even though they had originally seen the slide with the
stop sign. In other words, the misinformation in the leading question led to
inaccurate memory.
This phenomenon is called the misinformation effect, because the
misinformation that subjects were exposed to after the event (here in the form
of a misleading question) apparently contaminates subjects memories of what
they witnessed. Hundreds of subsequent studies have demonstrated that
memory can be contaminated by erroneous information that people are
nobaproject.com - Eyewitness Testimony and Memory Biases
571
exposed to after they witness an event (see Frenda, Nichols, & Loftus, 2011;
Loftus, 2005). The misinformation in these studies has led people to incorrectly
remember everything from small but crucial details of a perpetrators
appearance to objects as large as a barn that wasnt there at all.
These studies have demonstrated that young adults (the typical research
subjects in psychology) are often susceptible to misinformation, but that
children and older adults can be even more susceptible (Bartlett & Memon,
2007; Ceci & Bruck, 1995). In addition, misinformation effects can occur easily,
and without any intention to deceive (Allan & Gabbert, 2008). Even slight
differences in the wording of a question can lead to misinformation effects.
Subjects in one study were more likely to say yes when asked Did you see the
broken headlight? than when asked Did you see a broken headlight? (Loftus,
1975).
Other studies have shown that misinformation can corrupt memory even
more easily when it is encountered in social situations (Gabbert, Memon, Allan,
& Wright, 2004). This is a problem particularly in cases where more than one
person witnesses a crime. In these cases, witnesses tend to talk to one another
in the immediate aftermath of the crime, including as they wait for police to
arrive. But because different witnesses are different people with different
perspectives, they are likely to see or notice different things, and thus remember
different things, even when they witness the same event. So when they
communicate about the crime later, they not only reinforce common memories
for the event, they also contaminate each others memories for the event
(Gabbert, Memon, & Allan, 2003; Paterson & Kemp, 2006; Takarangi, Parker, &
Garry, 2006).
The misinformation effect has been modeled in the laboratory. Researchers
had subjects watch a video in pairs. Both subjects sat in front of the same screen,
but because they wore differently polarized glasses, they saw two different
versions of a video, projected onto a screen. So, although they were both
watching the same screen, and believed (quite reasonably) that they were
nobaproject.com - Eyewitness Testimony and Memory Biases
572
watching the same video, they were actually watching two different versions of
the video (Garry, French, Kinzett, & Mori, 2008).
In the video, Eric the electrician is seen wandering through an unoccupied
house and helping himself to the contents thereof. A total of eight details were
different between the two videos. After watching the videos, the co-witnesses
worked together on 12 memory test questions. Four of these questions dealt
with details that were different in the two versions of the video, so subjects had
the chance to influence one another. Then subjects worked individually on 20
additional memory test questions. Eight of these were for details that were
different in the two videos. Subjects accuracy was highly dependent on whether
they had discussed the details previously. Their accuracy for items they had not
previously discussed with their co-witness was 79%. But for items that they had
discussed, their accuracy dropped markedly, to 34%. That is, subjects allowed
their co-witnesses to corrupt their memories for what they had seen.
Identifying Perpetrators
In addition to correctly remembering many details of the crimes they witness,
eyewitnesses often need to remember the faces and other identifying features
of the perpetrators of those crimes. Eyewitnesses are often asked to describe
that perpetrator to law enforcement and later to make identifications from
books of mug shots or lineups. Here, too, there is a substantial body of research
demonstrating that eyewitnesses can make serious, but often understandable
and even predictable, errors (Caputo & Dunning, 2007; Cutler & Penrod, 1995).
In most jurisdictions in the United States, lineups are typically conducted
with pictures, called photo spreads, rather than with actual people standing
behind one-way glass (Wells, Memon, & Penrod, 2006). The eyewitness is given
a set of small pictures of perhaps six or eight individuals who are dressed
similarly and photographed in similar circumstances. One of these individuals
is the police suspect, and the remainder are foils or fillers (people known to
nobaproject.com - Eyewitness Testimony and Memory Biases
573
574
It is hard for the legal system to do much about most of these problems.
But there are some things that the justice system can do to help lineup
identifications go right. For example, investigators can put together highquality, fair lineups. A fair lineup is one in which the suspect and each of the
foils is equally likely to be chosen by someone who has read an eyewitness
description of the perpetrator but who did not actually witness the crime
(Brigham, Ready, & Spier, 1990). This means that no one in the lineup should
stick out, and that everyone should match the description given by the
eyewitness. Other important recommendations that have come out of this
research include better ways to conduct lineups, double blind lineups,
unbiased instructions for witnesses, and conducting lineups in a sequential
fashion (see Technical Working Group for Eyewitness Evidence, 1999; Wells et
al., 1998; Wells & Olson, 2003).
575
Other sorts of memory biases are more complicated and longer lasting. For
example, it turns out that our expectations and beliefs about how the world
works can have huge influences on our memories. Because many aspects of
our everyday lives are full of redundancies, our memory systems take advantage
of the recurring patterns by forming and using schemata, or memory templates
(Alba & Hasher, 1983; Brewer & Treyens, 1981). Thus, we know to expect that
a library will have shelves and tables and librarians, and so we dont have to
spend energy noticing these at the time. The result of this lack of attention,
however, is that one is likely to remember schema-consistent information (such
as tables), and to remember them in a rather generic way, whether or not they
were actually present.
False Memory
Some memory errors are so large that they almost belong in a class of their
own: false memories. Back in the early 1990s a pattern emerged whereby
people would go into therapy for depression and other everyday problems, but
over the course of the therapy develop memories for violent and horrible
victimhood (Loftus & Ketcham, 1994). These patients therapists claimed that
the patients were recovering genuine memories of real childhood abuse, buried
deep in their minds for years or even decades. But some experimental
psychologists believed that the memories were instead likely to be false
created in therapy. These researchers then set out to see whether it would
indeed be possible for wholly false memories to be created by procedures
similar to those used in these patients therapy.
In early false memory studies, undergraduate subjects family members
were recruited to provide events from the students lives. The student subjects
were told that the researchers had talked to their family members and learned
about four different events from their childhoods. The researchers asked if the
now undergraduate students remembered each of these four events
nobaproject.com - Eyewitness Testimony and Memory Biases
576
introduced via short hints. The subjects were asked to write about each of the
four events in a booklet and then were interviewed two separate times. The
trick was that one of the events came from the researchers rather than the
family (and the family had actually assured the researchers that this event had
not happened to the subject). In the first such study, this researcher-introduced
event was a story about being lost in a shopping mall and rescued by an older
adult. In this study, after just being asked whether they remembered these
events occurring on three separate occasions, a quarter of subjects came to
believe that they had indeed been lost in the mall (Loftus & Pickrell, 1995). In
subsequent studies, similar procedures were used to get subjects to believe
that they nearly drowned and had been rescued by a lifeguard, or that they had
spilled punch on the brides parents at a family wedding, or that they had been
attacked by a vicious animal as a child, among other events (Heaps & Nash,
1999; Hyman, Husband, & Billings, 1995; Porter, Yuille, & Lehman, 1999).
More recent false memory studies have used a variety of different
manipulations to produce false memories in substantial minorities and even
occasional majorities of manipulated subjects (Braun, Ellis, & Loftus, 2002;
Lindsay, Hagen, Read, Wade, & Garry, 2004; Mazzoni, Loftus, Seitz, & Lynn, 1999;
Seamon, Philbin, & Harrison, 2006; Wade, Garry, Read, & Lindsay, 2002). For
example, one group of researchers used a mock-advertising study, wherein
subjects were asked to review (fake) advertisements for Disney vacations, to
convince subjects that they had once met the character Bugs Bunny at
Disneylandan impossible false memory because Bugs is a Warner Brothers
character (Braun et al., 2002). Another group of researchers photoshopped
childhood photographs of their subjects into a hot air balloon picture and then
asked the subjects to try to remember and describe their hot air balloon
experience (Wade et al., 2002). Other researchers gave subjects unmanipulated
class photographs from their childhoods along with a fake story about a class
prank, and thus enhanced the likelihood that subjects would falsely remember
the prank (Lindsay et al., 2004).
nobaproject.com - Eyewitness Testimony and Memory Biases
577
Conclusion
To conclude, eyewitness testimony is very powerful and convincing to jurors,
even though it is not particularly reliable. Identification errors occur, and these
errors can lead to people being falsely accused and even convicted. Likewise,
eyewitness memory can be corrupted by leading questions, misinterpretations
of events, conversations with co-witnesses, and their own expectations for what
should have happened. People can even come to remember whole events that
never occurred.
nobaproject.com - Eyewitness Testimony and Memory Biases
578
The problems with memory in the legal system are real. But what can we
do to start to fix them? A number of specific recommendations have already
been made, and many of these are in the process of being implemented (e.g.,
Steblay & Loftus, 2012; Technical Working Group for Eyewitness Evidence, 1999;
Wells et al., 1998). Some of these recommendations are aimed at specific legal
procedures, including when and how witnesses should be interviewed, and how
lineups should be constructed and conducted. Other recommendations call for
appropriate education (often in the form of expert witness testimony) to be
provided to jury members and others tasked with assessing eyewitness
memory. Eyewitness testimony can be of great value to the legal system, but
decades of research now argues that this testimony is often given far more
weight than its accuracy justifies.
579
Discussion Questions
1. Imagine that you are a juror in a murder case where an eyewitness testifies.
In what ways might your knowledge of memory errors affect your use of
this testimony?
2. How true to life do you think television shows such as CSI or Law & Order
are in their portrayals of eyewitnesses?
3. Many jurisdictions in the United States use show-ups, where an eyewitness
is brought to a suspect (who may be standing on the street or in handcuffs
in the back of a police car) and asked, Is this the perpetrator? Is this a good
or bad idea, from a psychological perspective? Why?
Vocabulary
False memories
Memory for an event that never actually occurred, implanted by experimental
manipulation or other means.
Foils
Any member of a lineup (whether live or photograph) other than the suspect.
Misinformation effect
A memory error caused by exposure to incorrect information between the
original event (e.g., a crime) and later memory test (e.g., an interview, lineup,
or day in court).
Mock witnesses
A research subject who plays the part of a witness in a study.
Photo spreads
A selection of normally small photographs of faces given to a witness for the
purpose of identifying a perpetrator.
Schema (plural: schemata)
A memory template, created through repeated exposure to a particular class
of objects or events.
Reference List
Alba, J. W., & Hasher, L. (1983). Is memory schematic? Psychological Bulletin, 93,
203231.
Allan, K., & Gabbert, F. (2008). I still think it was a banana: Memorable lies and
forgettable truths. Acta Psychologica, 127, 299308.
Bartlett, J., & Memon, A. (2007). Eyewitness memory in young and older adults.
In: M.P. Toglia, J.D. Read, D.F. Ross, & R.C.L. Lindsay (Eds.), The handbook of
eyewitness psychology: Volume II: Memory for people (pp. 309338).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Berkowitz, S. R., Laney, C., Morris, E. K., Garry, M., & Loftus, E. F. (2008). Pluto
behaving badly: False beliefs and their consequences. American Journal of
Psychology, 121, 643660.
Bernstein, D. M., Laney, C., Morris, E. K., & Loftus, E. F. (2005). False memories
about food can lead to food avoidance. Social Cognition, 23, 1134.
Bernstein, D. M., & Loftus, E. F., (2009a). How to tell if a particular memory is
true or false. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 370374.
Bernstein, D. M., & Loftus, E. F. (2009b). The consequences of false memories
for food preferences and choices. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4,
135139.
Bornstein, B. H., Deffenbacher, K. A., Penrod, S. D., & McGorty, E. K. (2012).
Effects of exposure time and cognitive operations on facial identification
accuracy: A meta-analysis of two variables associated with initial memory
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Eyewitness Testimony and Memory Biases by
Cara Laney and Elizabeth F. Loftus is licensed under the Creative Commons AttributionNonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Topic 4
Development
Cognitive Development in
Childhood
Robert Siegler
Carnegie Mellon University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
This chapter examines what cognitive development is, major theories about
how it occurs, the roles of nature and nurture, whether it is continuous or
discontinuous, and how research in the area is being used to improve education.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
Cognitive development refers to the growth of thinking that takes place from
birth through adolescence. Defining thinking turns out to be harder than it
appears, because no clear boundaries separate thinking from other mental
activities. Thinking obviously involves the higher mental processes: problem
solving, reasoning, creating, conceptualizing, categorizing, remembering,
planning, and so on. However, thinking also involves other mental processes
that seem more basic and at which even toddlers are skilled: perceiving objects
and events in the external environment, acting skillfully on objects in order to
obtain goals, and understanding and producing language are three such
examples. Yet other parts of human development involve thinking to some
extent but are not usually viewed as central parts of cognitive development,
because thinking is only a relatively small part of the characteristics; peoples
personalities and temperaments are two such examples.
As the name suggests, cognitive development is about change. Childrens
thinking changes in dramatic and surprising ways. Consider DeVriess (1969)
study of whether young children understand the difference between
appearance and reality. To find out, she brought an unusually even-tempered
cat named Maynard to a psychology laboratory and allowed the 3- to 6-yearold participants in the study to pet and play with him. DeVries then put a mask
of a fierce dog on Maynards head, and asked the children what Maynard was.
Despite all of the children having identified Maynard previously as a cat, now
most 3-year-olds said that he was a dog and claimed that he had a dogs bones
and a dogs stomach. In contrast, the 6-year-olds werent fooled; they had no
doubt that Maynard remained a cat. Understanding how childrens thinking
changes so dramatically in just a few years is one of the fascinating challenges
in studying cognitive development.
nobaproject.com - Cognitive Development in Childhood
591
592
The way in which nature and nurture work together can be seen in findings
on visual development. Many people view vision as something that people
either are born with or that is purely a matter of biological maturation, but it
also depends on the right kind of experience at the right time. For example,
development of depth perception, the ability to actively perceive the distance
from oneself of objects in the environment, depends on seeing patterned light
and having normal brain activity in response to the patterned light, in infancy
(Held, 1993). If no patterned light is received, for example when a baby has
severe cataracts or blindness that is not surgically corrected until later in
development, depth perception remains abnormal even after the surgery.
Adding to the complexity of the nature-nurture interaction, childrens genes
lead to their eliciting different treatment from other people, which influences
their cognitive development. For example, infants physical attractiveness and
temperament are influenced considerably by their genetic inheritance, but it is
also the case that parents provide more sensitive and affectionate care to
easygoing and attractive infants than to difficult and less attractive ones, which
can contribute to the infants later cognitive development (Langlois et al., 1995;
van den Boom & Hoeksma, 1994).
Also contributing to the complex interplay of nature and nurture is the role
of children in shaping their own cognitive development. From the first days out
of the womb, children actively choose to attend more to some things and less
to others. For example, even 1-month-olds choose to look at their mothers face
more than at the faces of other women of the same age and general level of
attractiveness (Bartrip, Morton, & de Schonen, 2001). Childrens contributions
to their own cognitive development grow larger as they grow older (Scarr &
McCartney, 1983). When children are young, their parents largely determine
their experiences: whether they will attend day care, the children with whom
they will have play dates, the books to which they have access, and so on. In
contrast, older children and adolescents choose their environments to a larger
degree. Their parents preferences largely determine how 5-year-olds spend
nobaproject.com - Cognitive Development in Childhood
593
time, but 15-year-olds own preferences largely determine when, if ever, they
set foot in a library. Childrens choices often have large consequences. To cite
one example, the more that children choose to read, the more that their reading
improves in future years (Baker, Dreher, & Guthrie, 2000). Thus, the issue is not
whether cognitive development is a product of nature or nurture; rather, the
issue is how nature and nurture work together to produce cognitive
development.
594
The great Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget proposed that childrens thinking
progresses through a series of four discrete stages. By stages, he meant
periods during which children reasoned similarly about many superficially
different problems, with the stages occurring in a fixed order and the thinking
within different stages differing in fundamental ways. The four stages that
Piaget hypothesized were the sensorimotor stage (birth to 2 years), the
preoperational reasoning stage (2 to 6 or 7 years), the concrete operational
reasoning stage (6 or 7 to 11 or 12 years), and the formal operational reasoning
stage (11 or 12 years and throughout the rest of life).
During the sensorimotor stage, childrens thinking is largely realized through
their perceptions of the world and their physical interactions with it. Their
mental representations are very limited. Consider Piagets object permanence
task, which is one of his most famous problems. If an infant younger than 9
months of age is playing with a favorite toy, and another person removes the
nobaproject.com - Cognitive Development in Childhood
595
toy from view, for example by putting it under an opaque cover and not letting
the infant immediately reach for it, the infant is very likely to make no effort to
retrieve it and to show no emotional distress (Piaget, 1954). This is not due to
their being uninterested in the toy or unable to reach for it; if the same toy is
put under a clear cover, infants below 9 months readily retrieve it (Munakata,
McClelland, Johnson, & Siegler, 1997). Instead, Piaget claimed that infants less
than 9 months do not understand that objects continue to exist even when out
of sight.
During the preoperational stage, according to Piaget, children can solve not
only this simple problem (which they actually can solve after 9 months) but
show a wide variety of other symbolic-representation capabilities, such as those
involved in drawing and using language. However, such 2- to 7-year-olds tend
to focus on a single dimension, even when solving problems would require
them to consider multiple dimensions. This is evident in Piagets (1952)
conservation problems. For example, if a glass of water is poured into a taller,
thinner glass, children below age 7 generally say that there now is more water
than before. Similarly, if a clay ball is reshaped into a long, thin sausage, they
claim that there is now more clay, and if a row of coins is spread out, they claim
that there are now more coins. In all cases, the children are focusing on one
dimension, while ignoring the changes in other dimensions (for example, the
greater width of the glass and the clay ball).
Children overcome this tendency to focus on a single dimension during the
concrete operations stage, and think logically in most situations. However,
according to Piaget, they still cannot think in systematic scientific ways, even
when such thinking would be useful. Thus, if asked to find out which variables
influence the period that a pendulum takes to complete its arc, and given
weights that they can attach to strings in order to do experiments with the
pendulum to find out, most children younger than age 12, perform biased
experiments from which no conclusion can be drawn, and then conclude that
whatever they originally believed is correct. For example, if a boy believed that
nobaproject.com - Cognitive Development in Childhood
596
weight was the only variable that mattered, he might put the heaviest weight
on the shortest string and push it the hardest, and then conclude that just as
he thought, weight is the only variable that matters (Inhelder & Piaget, 1958).
Finally, in the formal operations period, children attain the reasoning power
of mature adults, which allows them to solve the pendulum problem and a wide
range of other problems. However, this formal operations stage tends not to
occur without exposure to formal education in scientific reasoning, and appears
to be largely or completely absent from some societies that do not provide this
type of education.
Although Piagets theory has been very influential, it has not gone
unchallenged. Many more recent researchers have obtained findings indicating
that cognitive development is considerably more continuous than Piaget
claimed. For example, Diamond (1985) found that on the object permanence
task described above, infants show earlier knowledge if the waiting period is
shorter. At age 6 months, they retrieve the hidden object if the wait is no longer
than 2 seconds; at 7 months, they retrieve it if the wait is no longer than 4
seconds; and so on. Even earlier, at 3 or 4 months, infants show surprise in the
form of longer looking times if objects suddenly appear to vanish with no
obvious cause (Baillargeon, 1987). Similarly, childrens specific experiences can
greatly influence when developmental changes occur. Children of pottery
makers in Mexican villages, for example, know that reshaping clay does not
change the amount of clay at much younger ages than children who do not
have similar experiences (Price-Williams, Gordon, & Ramirez, 1969).
So, is cognitive development fundamentally continuous or fundamentally
discontinuous? A reasonable answer seems to be, It depends on how you look
at it and how often you look. For example, under relatively facilitative
circumstances, infants show early forms of object permanence by 3 or 4 months,
and they gradually extend the range of times for which they can remember
hidden objects as they grow older. However, on Piagets original object
permanence task, infants do quite quickly change toward the end of their first
nobaproject.com - Cognitive Development in Childhood
597
year from not reaching for hidden toys to reaching for them, even after theyve
experienced a substantial delay before being allowed to reach. Thus, the debate
between those who emphasize discontinuous, stage-like changes in cognitive
development and those who emphasize gradual continuous changes remains
a lively one.
Applications to Education
Understanding how children think and learn has proven useful for improving
education. One example comes from the area of reading. Cognitive
developmental research has shown that phonemic awarenessthat is,
awareness of the component sounds within wordsis a crucial skill in learning
to read. To measure awareness of the component sounds within words,
researchers ask children to decide whether two words rhyme, to decide whether
the words start with the same sound, to identify the component sounds within
words, and to indicate what would be left if a given sound were removed from
a word. Kindergartners performance on these tasks is the strongest predictor
of reading achievement in third and fourth grade, even stronger than IQ or
social class background (Nation, 2008). Moreover, teaching these skills to
randomly chosen 4- and 5-year-olds results in their being better readers years
later (National Reading Panel, 2000).
Another educational application of cognitive developmental research
involves the area of mathematics. Even before they enter kindergarten, the
mathematical knowledge of children from low-income backgrounds lags far
behind that of children from more affluent backgrounds. Ramani and Siegler
(2008) hypothesized that this difference is due to the children in middle- and
upper-income families engaging more frequently in numerical activities, for
example playing numerical board games such as Chutes and Ladders. Chutes
and Ladders is a game with a number in each square; children start at the
number one and spin a spinner or throw a dice to determine how far to move
nobaproject.com - Cognitive Development in Childhood
598
their token. Playing this game seemed likely to teach children about numbers,
because in it, larger numbers are associated with greater values on a variety of
dimensions. In particular, the higher the number that a childs token reaches,
the greater the distance the token will have traveled from the starting point,
the greater the number of physical movements the child will have made in
moving the token from one square to another, the greater the number of
number words the child will have said and heard, and the more time will have
passed since the beginning of the game. These spatial, kinesthetic, verbal, and
time-based cues provide a broad-based, multisensory foundation for
knowledge of numerical magnitudes (the sizes of numbers), a type of
knowledge that is closely related to mathematics achievement test scores
(Booth & Siegler, 2006).
Playing this numerical board game for roughly 1 hour, distributed over a 2week period, improved low-income childrens knowledge of numerical
magnitudes, ability to read printed numbers, and skill at learning novel
arithmetic problems. The gains lasted for months after the game-playing
experience (Ramani & Siegler, 2008; Siegler & Ramani, 2009). An advantage of
this type of educational intervention is that it has minimal if any costa parent
could just draw a game on a piece of paper.
Understanding of cognitive development is advancing on many different
fronts. One exciting area is linking changes in brain activity to changes in
childrens thinking (Nelson et al., 2006). Although many people believe that brain
maturation is something that occurs before birth, the brain actually continues
to change in large ways for many years thereafter. For example, a part of the
brain called the prefrontal cortex, which is located at the front of the brain and
is particularly involved with planning and flexible problem solving, continues
to develop throughout adolescence (Blakemore & Choudhury, 2006). Such new
research domains, as well as enduring issues such as nature and nurture,
continuity and discontinuity, and how to apply cognitive development research
to education, insure that cognitive development will continue to be an exciting
nobaproject.com - Cognitive Development in Childhood
599
600
Outside Resources
Book: Frye, D., Baroody, A., Burchinal, M., Carver, S. M., Jordan, N. C., &
McDowell, J. (2013). Teaching math to young children: A practice guide.
Washington, DC: National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional
Assistance (NCEE), Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of
Education.
Book: Kuhn, D., & Siegler, R. S. (Vol. Eds.). (2006). Volume 2: Cognition,
perception, and language. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.), Handbook
of child psychology (6th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Book: Siegler, R. S., & Alibali, M. W. (2004). Children's thinking (4th ed.). Upper
Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Discussion Questions
1. Why are there different theories of cognitive development? Why dont
researchers agree on which theory is the right one?
2. Do childrens natures differ, or do differences among children only reflect
differences in their experiences?
3. Do you see development as more continuous or more discontinuous?
4. Can you think of ways other than those described in the chapter in which
research on cognitive development could be used to improve education?
Vocabulary
Chutes and Ladders
A numerical board game that seems to be useful for building numerical
knowledge.
Concrete operations stage
Piagetian stage between ages 7 and 12 when children can think logically about
concrete situations but not engage in systematic scientific reasoning.
Conservation problems
Problems pioneered by Piaget in which physical transformation of an object or
set of objects changes a perceptually salient dimension but not the quantity
that is being asked about.
Continuous development
Ways in which development occurs in a gradual incremental manner, rather
than through sudden jumps.
Depth perception
The ability to actively perceive the distance from oneself of objects in the
environment.
Discontinuous development
Discontinuous development
Formal operations stage
Piagetian stage starting at age 12 years and continuing for the rest of life, in
which adolescents may gain the reasoning powers of educated adults.
Nature
The genes that children bring with them to life and that influence all aspects of
their development.
Numerical magnitudes
The sizes of numbers.
Nurture
The environments, starting with the womb, that influence all aspects of
childrens development.
Phonemic awareness
Awareness of the component sounds within words.
Piagets theory
Theory that development occurs through a sequence of discontinuous stages:
the sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete operational, and formal operational
stages.
Reference List
Baillargeon, R. (1987). Object permanence in 3 1/2- and 4 1/2-month-old infants.
Developmental Psychology, 23, 655664.
Baker, L., Dreher, M. J., & Guthrie, J. T., (Eds.). (2000). Engaging young readers:
Promoting achievement and motivation. New York: Guilford.
Bartrip, J., Morton, J., & De Schonen, S. (2001). Responses to mother's face in 3week to 5-month old infants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
19, 219232
Blakemore, S.-J., & Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescent brain:
Implications for executive function and social cognition. Journal of Child
Psychiatry and Psychology, 47, 296312.
Booth, J. L., & Siegler, R. S. (2006). Developmental and individual differences in
pure numerical estimation. Developmental Psychology, 41, 189201.
DeVries, R. (1969). Constancy of genetic identity in the years three to six.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 34, 127.
Diamond, A. (1985). Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as
indicated by infants' performance on AB. Child Development, 56, 868883.
Held, R. (1993). What can rates of development tell us about underlying
mechanisms? In C. E. Granrud (Ed.), Visual perception and cognition in
infancy (pp. 7590). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood
Ramani, G. B., & Siegler, R. S. (2008). Promoting broad and stable improvements
in low-income childrens numerical knowledge through playing number
board games. Child Development, 79, 375394.
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A
theory of genotype-environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424435.
Siegler, R. S., & Ramani, G. B. (2009). Playing linear number board gamesbut
not
circular
onesimproves
low-income
preschoolers
numerical
Abstract
Childhood social and personality development emerges through the interaction
of social influences, biological maturation, and the childs representations of
the social world and the self. This interaction is illustrated in a discussion of the
influence of significant relationships, the development of social understanding,
the growth of personality, and the development of social and emotional
competence in childhood.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
How have I become the kind of person I am today? Every adult ponders this
question from time to time. The answers that readily come to mind include the
influences of parents, peers, temperament, a moral compass, a strong sense
of self, and sometimes critical life experiences such as parental divorce. Social
and personality development encompasses these and many other influences
on the growth of the person. In addition, it addresses questions that are at the
heart of understanding how we develop as unique people. How much are we
products of nature or nurture? How enduring are the influences of early
experiences? The study of social and personality development offers
perspective on these and other issues, often by showing how complex and
multifaceted are the influences on developing children, and thus the intricate
processes that have made you the person you are today (Thompson, 2006a).
Understanding social and personality development requires looking at
children from three perspectives that interact to shape development. The first
is the social context in which each child lives, especially the relationships that
provide security, guidance, and knowledge. The second is biological maturation
that supports developing social and emotional competencies and underlies
temperamental individuality. The third is childrens developing representations
of themselves and the social world. Social and personality development is best
understood as the continuous interaction between these social, biological, and
representational aspects of psychological development.
Relationships
This interaction can be observed in the development of the earliest relationships
between infants and their parents in the first year. Virtually all infants living in
normal circumstances develop strong emotional attachments to those who
care for them. Psychologists believe that the development of these attachments
nobaproject.com - Social and Personality Development in Childhood
613
is as biologically natural as learning to walk and that they are not simply a
byproduct of the parents provision of food or warmth. Rather, attachments
have evolved in humans because they promote the childs motivation to stay
close to those who care for them and, as a consequence, to benefit from the
learning, security, guidance, warmth, and affirmation that close relationships
provide (Cassidy, 2008).
Although nearly all infants develop emotional attachments to their
caregivers, they vary in the security of those attachments. Infants become
securely attached when their parents respond sensitively to them, and
consequently these infants have confidence that their caregivers will provide
support when needed. Infants become insecurely attached in the context of
insensitive care, and these infants tend to respond avoidantly, resistantly, or in
a disorganized manner (Belsky & Pasco Fearon, 2008). The different behaviors
of securely and insecurely attached infants can be observed especially when
the infant needs the caregivers support, and researchers use a standard
laboratory procedure, called the Strange Situation, that involves brief
separations from the caregiver to assess the Strange Situation (Solomon &
George, 2008). Infants can be securely or insecurely attached with mothers,
fathers, and other regular caregivers, and they can differ in their security with
different people. The security of attachment is an important cornerstone of
social and personality development because infants and young children who
are securely attached have been found also to develop stronger friendships
with peers, are more advanced in emotion understanding and early conscience
development, and have a more positive self-concept compared with insecurely
attached children (Thompson, 2008). This is consistent with the view of
attachment theory that the experiences of care leading to a secure or insecure
attachment shapes young childrens developing representations of what people
are like, how to interact with them, and the self.
nobaproject.com - Social and Personality Development in Childhood
614
Parental roles in relation to their children change in other ways also. Parents
increasingly become mediators (or gatekeepers) of their childrens involvement
with peers and activities outside the family. Their communication (and practice)
of values contributes to childrens academic achievement, moral development,
and activity preferences. As children reach adolescence, the parentchild
relationship increasingly becomes one of coregulation, in which each partner
recognizes the childs growing competence and autonomy and together they
rebalance authority relations.
615
Peer Relationships
Parentchild relationships are not the only significant relationships in a childs
life. Peer relationships are also important. This is because social interaction
with another child who is similar in age, skills, and knowledge provokes the
development of many social skills that are valuable for the rest of life (Bukowski,
Buhrmester, & Underwood, 2011). In peer relationships, children learn how to
initiate and maintain social interaction with another child. They learn skills for
managing conflict, such as through turn-taking, compromise, and bargaining.
They play with other childrenplaying with toys and other objects as infants,
pretend play as preschoolers, and later engaging in games with rules in the
primary grades. In each case, play involves the mutual, sometimes complex,
coordination of goals, actions, and understanding. Children develop friendships
with other children with whom they are compatible, and who become sources
of security and support that are different from the support that parents provide.
nobaproject.com - Social and Personality Development in Childhood
616
Social Understanding
As we have seen, childrens experience of relationships at home and the peer
group contributes to an expanding repertoire of social and emotional skills and
also to broadened social understanding. In these relationships, children
develop expectations for specific people (leading, for example, to secure or
insecure attachments to parents), understanding of how to interact with adults
and peers, and developing self-concept based on how others respond to them.
These relationships are also significant forums for emotional development.
Remarkably, young children begin developing social understanding very
early in life. Before the end of the first year, infants are aware that other people
have perceptions, feelings, and other mental states that affect their behavior,
and which are different from the childs own mental states. This can be readily
observed in a process called social referencing, in which an infant looks to the
nobaproject.com - Social and Personality Development in Childhood
617
618
that what Mommy is looking at is in her mind) (Gopnik, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 2001).
This is especially likely to occur in relationships with people whom the child
knows well, consistent with the ideas of attachment theory discussed above.
Growing language skills give young children words with which to represent these
mental states (e.g., mad, wants) and talk about them with others. Thus in
conversation with their parents about everyday experiences, children learn
much about peoples mental states from how adults talk about them (Your
sister was sad because she thought Daddy was coming home.) (Thompson,
2006b). Developing social understanding is, in other words, based on childrens
everyday interactions with others and their careful interpretations of what they
see and hear. There are also some scientists who believe that infants are
biologically prepared to perceive people in a special way, as organisms with an
internal mental life, and this facilitates their interpretation of peoples behavior
with reference to those mental states (Leslie, 1994).
Personality
Parents look into the faces of their newborn infants and wonder what kind of
person this child will become. They scrutinize their babys preferences,
characteristics, and responses to discern the temperamental qualities that form
the basis for developing personality. They are quite right to do so, because
temperament is a foundation for personality growth. But temperament
(defined as early emerging differences in reactivity and self-regulation) is not
the whole story of personality development. Although temperament is
biologically based, it interacts with the influence of experience from the moment
of birth (if not before) to shape personality (Rothbart, 2011). Temperamental
dispositions are affected, for example, by the support or harshness of parental
care. More generally, personality is shaped by the goodness of fit between the
childs temperamental qualities and characteristics of the environment (Chess
& Thomas, 1999). If a child with a temperamentally high activity level grows up
nobaproject.com - Social and Personality Development in Childhood
619
in a family that enjoys physically vigorous activities, this is a good fit or match
that supports positive personality growth. If the same child grows up in a family
that is quiet and sedentary, the result is like gears that do not mesh well, and
we might not expect such positive development. Personality is the result,
therefore, of the continuous interaction of biological dispositions and
experience, and the same is true for many other aspects of social and
personality development.
Personality develops from temperament in other ways (Thompson, Winer,
&
Goodvin,
2010).
As
children
mature
biologically,
temperamental
characteristics unfold and change over time. A newborn is not capable of much
self-control, but as brain-based capacities for self-control progressively mature,
temperamental differences in self-regulation become more apparent. In
addition, personality incorporates many other features of the childs
individuality besides temperament. Childrens developing self-concept, their
motivations to achieve or to socialize, their values and goals, their coping styles,
their sense of responsibility and conscientiousness, and many other qualities
are encompassed into personality. These qualities are influenced by biological
dispositions but even more by the childs experiences with others, particularly
in close relationships, that guide the growth of individual characteristics.
Indeed, personality development begins with the biological foundations of
temperament but becomes increasingly elaborated, extended, and refined over
time. The newborn that parents gazed upon thus becomes an adult with a
personality of depth and nuance.
620
621
badly after misbehaving, and who feel uncomfortable when others misbehave.
In the development of conscience, young children become more socially and
emotionally competent in a manner that provides a foundation for later moral
conduct (Thompson, 2012).
The development of gender and gender identity is likewise an interaction
among social, biological, and representational influences (Ruble, Martin, &
Berenbaum, 2006). Young children learn about gender from parents, peers, and
others in society, and develop their own conceptions of the attributes associated
with maleness or femaleness (called gender schemas). They also negotiate
biological transitions (such as puberty) that cause their sense of themselves
and their sexual identity to mature.
Each of these examples of the growth of social and emotional competence
illustrates not only the interaction of social, biological, and representational
influences, but also how their development unfolds over an extended period.
Early influences are important, but not determinative, because the capabilities
required for mature moral conduct, gender identity, and other outcomes
continue to develop throughout childhood, adolescence, and even the adult
years.
Conclusion
As the preceding sentence suggests, social and personality development
continues through adolescence and the adult years, and it is influenced by the
same constellation of social, biological, and representational influences
discussed for childhood. Changing social relationships and roles, biological
maturation and (much later) decline, and how the individual represents
experience and the self continue to form the bases for development throughout
life. In this respect, when an adult looks forward rather than retrospectively to
ask, what kind of person am I becoming?a similarly fascinating, complex,
multifaceted interaction of developmental processes lies ahead.
nobaproject.com - Social and Personality Development in Childhood
622
623
Outside Resources
Web: Center for the Developing Child, Harvard University
http://developingchild.harvard.edu
Web: Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning
http://casel.org
Discussion Questions
1. If parentchild relationships naturally change as the child matures, would
you expect that the security of attachment might also change over time?
What reasons would account for your expectation?
2. In what ways does a childs developing theory of mind resemble how
scientists create, refine, and use theories in their work? In other words,
would it be appropriate to think of children as informal scientists in their
development of social understanding?
3. If there is a poor goodness of fit between a childs temperament and
characteristics of parental care, what can be done to create a better match?
Provide a specific example of how this might occur.
4. What are the contributions that parents offer to the development of social
and emotional competence in children? Answer this question again with
respect to peer contributions.
Vocabulary
Authoritative
A parenting style characterized by high (but reasonable)
expectations for childrens behavior, good communication, warmth and
nurturance, and the use of reasoning (rather than coercion) as preferred
responses to childrens misbehavior.
Conscience
The cognitive, emotional, and social influences that cause young children to
create and act consistently with internal standards of conduct.
Effortful control
A temperament quality that enables children to be more
successful in motivated self-regulation.
Family Stress Model
A description of the negative effects of family financial
difficulty on child adjustment through the effects of economic stress on parents
depressed mood, increased marital problems, and poor parenting.
Gender schemas
Organized beliefs and expectations about maleness and femaleness that guide
childrens thinking about gender.
Goodness of fit
The match or synchrony between a childs temperament and characteristics of
parental care that contributes to positive or negative personality development.
A good fit means that parents have accommodated to the childs
temperamental attributes, and this contributes to positive personality growth
Reference List
Baumrind, D. (2013). Authoritative parenting revisited: History and current
status. In R. E. Larzelere, A. Sheffield, & A. W. Harrist (Eds.), Authoritative
parenting: Synthesizing nurturance and discipline for optimal child
development (pp. 1134). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Belsky, J., & Pasco Fearon, R. M. (2008). Precursors of attachment security. In J.
Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research,
and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 295316). New York, NY: Guilford.
Bukowski, W. M., Buhrmester, D., & Underwood, M. K. (2011). Peer relations as
a developmental context. In M. K. Underwood & L. H. Rosen (Eds.), Social
development(pp. 153179). New York, NY: Guilford
Cassidy, J. (2008). The nature of the childs ties. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.),
Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd
ed., pp. 322). New York, NY: Guilford.
Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (1999). Goodness of fit: Clinical applications from infancy
through adult life. New York, NY: Brunner-Mazel/Taylor & Francis.
Conger, R. D., Conger, K. J., & Martin, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status, family
processes, and individual development. Journal of Marriage and Family, 72,
685704
Emery, R. E. (1999). Marriage, divorce, and childrens adjustment (2nd ed.).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Feinman, S. (Ed.) (1992). Social referencing and the social construction of reality
in infancy. New York, NY: Plenum.
Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. K. (2001). The scientist in the crib. New York,
NY: HarperCollins.
Kochanska, G. (2002). Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and
their young children: A context for the early development of conscience.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 191195.
Kochanska, G., Kim, S., Barry, R. A., & Philibert, R. A. (2011). Childrens genotypes
interact with maternal responsive care in predicting childrens competence:
Diathesis-stress
or
differential
susceptibility?
Development
and
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Social and Personality Development in Childhood
by Ross Thompson is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercialShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Adolescent Development
Jennifer Lansford
Duke University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Adolescence is a period that begins with puberty and ends with the transition
to adulthood (approximately ages 1020). Physical changes associated with
puberty are triggered by hormones. Cognitive changes include improvements
in complex and abstract thought, as well as development that happens at
different rates in distinct parts of the brain and increases adolescents
propensity for risky behavior because increases in sensation-seeking and
reward motivation precede increases in cognitive control. Adolescents
relationships with parents go through a period of redefinition in which
adolescents become more autonomous, and aspects of parenting, such as distal
monitoring and psychological control, become more salient. Peer relationships
are important sources of support and companionship during adolescence yet
can also promote problem behaviors. Same-sex peer groups evolve into mixedsex peer groups, and adolescents romantic relationships tend to emerge from
these groups. Identity formation occurs as adolescents explore and commit to
different roles and ideological positions. Nationality, gender, ethnicity,
socioeconomic status, religious background, sexual orientation, and genetic
factors shape how adolescents behave and how others respond to them, and
are sources of diversity in adolescence.
Learning Objectives
Adolescence Defined
Adolescence is a developmental stage that has been defined as starting with
puberty and ending with the transition to adulthood (approximately ages 10
20). Adolescence has evolved historically, with evidence indicating that this stage
is lengthening as individuals start puberty earlier and transition to adulthood
later than in the past. Puberty today begins, on average, at age 1011 years for
girls and 1112 years for boys. This average age of onset has decreased
gradually over time since the 19th century by 34 months per decade, which
has been attributed to a range of factors including better nutrition, obesity,
increased father absence, and other environmental factors (Steinberg, 2013).
Completion of formal education, financial independence from parents,
marriage, and parenthood have all been markers of the end of adolescence
and beginning of adulthood, and all of these transitions happen, on average,
later now than in the past. In fact, the prolonging of adolescence has prompted
the introduction of a new developmental period called emerging adulthood
that captures these developmental changes out of adolescence and into
adulthood, occurring from approximately ages 18 to 29 (Arnett, 2000).
This chapter will outline changes that occur during adolescence in three
domains: physical, cognitive, and social. Within the social domain, changes in
relationships with parents, peers, and romantic partners will be considered.
Next, the chapter turns to adolescents psychological and behavioral
adjustment, including identity formation, aggression and antisocial behavior,
anxiety and depression, and academic achievement. Finally, the chapter
summarizes sources of diversity in adolescents experiences and development.
636
Physical Changes
Physical changes of puberty mark the onset of adolescence (Lerner & Steinberg,
2009). For both boys and girls, these changes include a growth spurt in height,
growth of pubic and underarm hair, and skin changes (e.g., pimples). Boys also
experience growth in facial hair and a deepening of their voice. Girls experience
breast development and begin menstruating. These pubertal changes are
driven by hormones, particularly an increase in testosterone for boys and
estrogen for girls.
Cognitive Changes
Major changes in the structure and functioning of the brain occur during
adolescence and result in cognitive and behavioral developments (Steinberg,
2008). Cognitive changes during adolescence include a shift from concrete to
more abstract and complex thinking. Such changes are fostered by
improvements during early adolescence in attention, memory, processing
speed, and metacognition (ability to think about thinking and therefore make
better use of strategies like mnemonic devices that can improve thinking). Early
in adolescence, changes in the brains dopaminergic system contribute to
increases in adolescents sensation-seeking and reward motivation. Later in
adolescence, the brains cognitive control centers in the prefrontal cortex
develop, increasing adolescents self-regulation and future orientation. The
difference in timing of the development of these different regions of the brain
contributes to more risk taking during middle adolescence because adolescents
are motivated to seek thrills that sometimes come from risky behavior, such as
reckless driving, smoking, or drinking, and have not yet developed the cognitive
control to resist impulses or focus equally on the potential risks (Steinberg,
2008). One of the worlds leading experts on adolescent development, Laurence
Steinberg, likens this to engaging a powerful engine before the braking system
nobaproject.com - Adolescent Development
637
is in place. The result is that adolescents are more prone to risky behaviors than
are children or adults.
Social Changes
Parents. Although peers take on greater importance during adolescence, family
relationships remain important too. One of the key changes during adolescence
involves a renegotiation of parentchild relationships. As adolescents strive for
more independence and autonomy during this time, different aspects of
parenting become more salient. For example, parents distal supervision and
monitoring become more important as adolescents spend more time away
from parents and in the presence of peers. Parental monitoring encompasses
a wide range of behaviors such as parents attempts to set rules and know their
adolescents friends, activities, and whereabouts, in addition to adolescents
willingness to disclose information to their parents (Stattin & Kerr, 2000).
Psychological control, which involves manipulation and intrusion into
adolescents emotional and cognitive world through invalidating adolescents
feelings and pressuring them to think in particular ways (Barber, 1996), is
another aspect of parenting that becomes more salient during adolescence
and is related to more problematic adolescent adjustment.
Peers
As children become adolescents, they usually begin spending more time with
their peers and less time with their families, and these peer interactions are
increasingly unsupervised by adults. Childrens notions of friendship often focus
on shared activities, whereas adolescents notions of friendship increasingly
focus on intimate exchanges of thoughts and feelings. During adolescence, peer
groups evolve from primarily single-sex to mixed-sex. Adolescents within a peer
group tend to be similar to one another in behavior and attitudes, which has
nobaproject.com - Adolescent Development
638
639
Romantic relationships
Adolescence is the developmental period during which romantic relationships
typically first emerge. Initially, same-sex peer groups that were common during
childhood expand into mixed-sex peer groups that are more characteristic of
adolescence. Romantic relationships often form in the context of these mixedsex peer groups (Connolly, Furman, & Konarski, 2000). Although romantic
relationships during adolescence are often short-lived rather than long-term
committed partnerships, their importance should not be minimized.
Adolescents spend a great deal of time focused on romantic relationships, and
their positive and negative emotions are more tied to romantic relationships
(or lack thereof) than to friendships, family relationships, or school (Furman &
Shaffer, 2003). Romantic relationships contribute to adolescents identity
formation, changes in family and peer relationships, and adolescents
emotional and behavioral adjustment.
Furthermore, romantic relationships are centrally connected to adolescents
emerging sexuality. Parents, policymakers, and researchers have devoted a
great deal of attention to adolescents sexuality, in large part because of
concerns related to sexual intercourse, contraception, and preventing teen
pregnancies. However, sexuality involves more than this narrow focus. For
example, adolescence is often when individuals who are lesbian, gay, bisexual,
or transgender come to perceive themselves as such (Russell, Clarke, & Clary,
2009). Thus, romantic relationships are a domain in which adolescents
experiment with new behaviors and identities.
640
641
versus adolescence (late starters). According to the theory, early starters are at
greater risk for long-term antisocial behavior that extends into adulthood than
are late starters. Late starters who become antisocial during adolescence are
theorized to experience poor parental monitoring and supervision, aspects of
parenting that become more salient during adolescence. Poor monitoring and
lack of supervision contribute to increasing involvement with deviant peers,
which in turn promotes adolescents own antisocial behavior. Late starters
desist from antisocial behavior when changes in the environment make other
options more appealing. Similarly, Moffitts (1993) life-course persistent versus
adolescent-limited model distinguishes between antisocial behavior that
begins in childhood versus adolescence. Moffitt regards adolescent-limited
antisocial behavior as resulting from a maturity gap between adolescents
dependence on and control by adults and their desire to demonstrate their
freedom from adult constraint. However, as they continue to develop, and
legitimate adult roles and privileges become available to them, there are fewer
incentives to engage in antisocial behavior, leading to desistance in these
antisocial behaviors.
642
Academic achievement
Adolescents spend more waking time in school than in any other context (Eccles
& Roeser, 2011). Academic achievement during adolescence is predicted by
interpersonal
(e.g.,
parental
engagement
in
adolescents
education),
643
Diversity
Adolescent development does not necessarily follow the same pathway for all
individuals. Certain features of adolescence, particularly with respect to
biological changes associated with puberty and cognitive changes associated
with brain development, are relatively universal. But other features of
adolescence depend largely on circumstances that are more environmentally
variable. For example, adolescents growing up in one country might have
different opportunities for risk taking than adolescents in a different country,
and supports and sanctions for different behaviors in adolescence depend on
laws and values that might be specific to where adolescents live. Likewise,
different cultural norms regarding family and peer relationships shape
adolescents experiences in these domains. For example, in some countries,
adolescents parents are expected to retain control over major decisions,
whereas in other countries, adolescents are expected to begin sharing in or
taking control of decision making.
Even within the same country, adolescents gender, ethnicity, immigrant
status, religion, sexual orientation, socioeconomic status, and personality can
shape both how adolescents behave and how others respond to them, creating
diverse developmental contexts for different adolescents. For example, early
puberty (that occurs before most other peers have experienced puberty)
appears to be associated with worse outcomes for girls than boys, likely in part
because girls who enter puberty early tend to associate with older boys, which
in turn is associated with early sexual behavior and substance use. For
adolescents who are ethnic or sexual minorities, discrimination sometimes
presents a set of challenges that nonminorities do not face.
Finally, genetic variations contribute an additional source of diversity in
adolescence. Current approaches emphasize gene X environment interactions,
which often follow a differential susceptibility model (Belsky & Pluess, 2009).
That is, particular genetic variations are considered riskier than others, but
nobaproject.com - Adolescent Development
644
factors.
For
example,
the
association
between
the
Conclusions
Adolescent development is characterized by biological, cognitive, and social
changes. Social changes are particularly notable as adolescents become more
autonomous from their parents, spend more time with peers, and begin
exploring romantic relationships and sexuality. Adjustment during adolescence
is reflected in identity formation, which often involves a period of exploration
followed by commitments to particular identities. Adolescence is characterized
by risky behavior, which is made more likely by changes in the brain in which
reward-processing centers develop more rapidly than cognitive control
systems, making adolescents more sensitive to rewards than to possible
negative consequences. Despite these generalizations, factors such as country
of residence, gender, ethnicity, and sexual orientation shape development in
ways that lead to diversity of experiences across adolescence.
645
Outside Resources
Podcasts: Society for Research on Adolescence website with links to podcasts
on a variety of topics related to adolescent development
http://www.s-r-a.org/sra-news/podcasts
Study: Add Health website on one of the biggest longitudinal studies of
adolescence to date
http://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth
Video: A selection of TED talks on adolescent brain development
http://tinyurl.com/lku4a3k
Web: UNICEF website on adolescents around the world
http://www.unicef.org/adolescence/index.html
Discussion Questions
1. What can parents do to promote their adolescents positive adjustment?
2. In what ways do changes in brain development and cognition make
adolescents particularly susceptible to peer influence?
3. How could interventions designed to prevent or reduce adolescents
problem behavior be developed to take advantage of what we know about
adolescent development?
4. Reflecting on your own adolescence, provide examples of times when you
think your experience was different from those of your peers as a function
of something unique about you.
5. In what ways was your experience of adolescence different from your
parents experience of adolescence? How do you think adolescence may be
different 20 years from now?
Vocabulary
Crowds
Adolescent peer groups characterized by shared reputations or images.
Differential susceptibility
Genetic factors that make individuals more or less responsive to environmental
experiences.
Foreclosure
Individuals commit to an identity without exploration of options.
Homophily
Adolescents tend to associate with peers who are similar to themselves.
Identity achievement
Individuals have explored different options and then made commitments.
Identity diffusion
Adolescents neither explore nor commit to any roles or ideologies.
Moratorium
State in which adolescents are actively exploring options but have not yet made
identity commitments.
Psychological control
Parents manipulation of and intrusion into adolescents emotional and
cognitive world through invalidating adolescents feelings and pressuring them
to think in particular ways.
Reference List
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late
teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55, 469480.
Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revisiting a neglected
construct. Child Development, 67, 32963319.
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis-stress: Differential susceptibility
to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 885908.
Brown, B. B., & Larson, J. (2009). Peer relationships in adolescence. In R. M.
Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp. 74
103). New York, NY: Wiley.
Connolly, J., Furman, W., & Konarski, R. (2000). The role of peers in the emergence
of heterosexual romantic relationships in adolescence. Child Development,
71, 13951408.
Dick, D. M., Meyers, J. L., Latendresse, S. J., Creemers, H. E., Lansford, J. E.,
Huizink, A. C. (2011). CHRM2, parental monitoring, and adolescent
externalizing behavior: Evidence for gene-environment interaction.
Psychological Science, 22, 481489.
Dishion, T. J., & Tipsord, J. M. (2011). Peer contagion in child and adolescent
social and emotional development. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 189
214.
884890.
Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2000). Parental monitoring: A reinterpretation. Child
Development, 71, 10721085.
Steinberg, L. (2013). Adolescence (10th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risktaking. Developmental Review, 28, 78106.
Abstract
The purpose of this chapter is to provide a brief review of attachment theory
a theory designed to explain the significance of the close, emotional bonds
that children develop with their caregivers and the implications of those bonds
for understanding personality development. The chapter discusses the origins
of the theory, research on individual differences in attachment security in
infancy and childhood, and the role of attachment in adult relationships.
Learning Objectives
Explain the way the attachment system works and its evolutionary
significance.
Introduction
Some of the most rewarding experiences in peoples lives involve the
development and maintenance of close relationships. For example, some of
the greatest sources of joy involve falling in love, starting a family, being reunited
with distant loved ones, and sharing experiences with close others. And, not
surprisingly, some of the most painful experiences in peoples lives involve the
disruption of important social bonds, such as separation from a spouse, losing
a parent, or being abandoned by a loved one.
Why do close relationships play such a profound role in human experience?
Attachment theory is one approach to understanding the nature of close
relationships. In this chapter, we review the origins of the theory, the core
theoretical principles, and some ways in which attachment influences human
behavior, thoughts, and feelings across the life course.
657
658
659
Ainsworths work was important for at least three reasons. First, she
provided one of the first empirical demonstrations of how attachment behavior
is organized in unfamiliar contexts. Second, she provided the first empirical
taxonomy of individual differences in infant attachment patterns. According
to her research, at least three types of children exist: those who are secure in
their relationship with their parents, those who are anxious-resistant, and those
who are anxious-avoidant. Finally, she demonstrated that these individual
differences were correlated with infantparent interactions in the home during
the first year of life. Children who appear secure in the strange situation, for
example, tend to have parents who are responsive to their needs. Children who
appear insecure in the strange situation (i.e., anxious-resistant or avoidant)
often have parents who are insensitive to their needs, or inconsistent or
rejecting in the care they provide.
660
661
Attachment in Adulthood
Although Bowlby was primarily focused on understanding the nature of the
infantcaregiver relationship, he believed that attachment characterized
human experience across the life course. It was not until the mid-1980s,
however, that researchers began to take seriously the possibility that
attachment processes may be relevant to adulthood. Hazan and Shaver (1987)
were two of the first researchers to explore Bowlbys ideas in the context of
romantic relationships. According to Hazan and Shaver, the emotional bond
that develops between adult romantic partners is partly a function of the same
motivational systemthe attachment behavioral systemthat gives rise to the
emotional bond between infants and their caregivers. Hazan and Shaver noted
that in both kinds of relationship, people (a) feel safe and secure when the other
person is present; (b) turn to the other person during times of sickness, distress,
or fear; (c) use the other person as a secure base from which to explore the
world; and (d) speak to one another in a unique language, often called
motherese or baby talk. (See Box X.2.)
On the basis of these parallels, Hazan and Shaver (1987) argued that adult
romantic relationships, such as infantcaregiver relationships, are attachments.
According to Hazan and Shaver, individuals gradually transfer attachmentrelated functions from parents to peers as they develop. Thus, although young
children tend to use their parents as their primary attachment figures, as they
reach adolescence and young adulthood, they come to rely more upon close
friends and/or romantic partners for basic attachment-related functions. Thus,
although a young child may turn to his or her mother for comfort, support, and
guidance when distressed, scared, or ill, young adults may be more likely to
turn to their romantic partners for these purposes under similar situations.
nobaproject.com - Attachment Through the Life Course
662
Hazan and Shaver (1987) asked a diverse sample of adults to read the three
paragraphs below and indicate which paragraph best characterized the way
they think, feel, and behave in close relationships:
1. I am somewhat uncomfortable being close to others; I find it difficult to trust
them completely, difficult to allow myself to depend on them. I am nervous
when anyone gets too close, and often, others want me to be more intimate
than I feel comfortable being.
2. I find it relatively easy to get close to others and am comfortable depending
on them and having them depend on me. I dont worry about being
abandoned or about someone getting too close to me.
3. I find that others are reluctant to get as close as I would like. I often worry
that my partner doesnt really love me or wont want to stay with me. I want
to get very close to my partner, and this sometimes scares people away.
663
concluded that the same kinds of individual differences that exist in infant
attachment also exist in adulthood.
664
interest in those who are higher in security than those who are more insecure
(McClure, Lydon, Baccus, & Baldwin, 2010). However, there is also some
evidence that peoples attachment styles mutually shape one another in close
relationships. For example, in a longitudinal study, Hudson, Fraley, Vicary, and
Brumbaugh (2012) found that, if one person in a relationship experienced a
change in security, his or her partner was likely to experience a change in the
same direction.
Relationship Functioning
Research has consistently demonstrated that individuals who are relatively
secure are more likely than insecure individuals to have high functioning
relationshipsrelationships that are more satisfying, more enduring, and less
characterized by conflict. For example, Feeney and Noller (1992) found that
insecure individuals were more likely than secure individuals to experience a
breakup of their relationship. In addition, secure individuals are more likely to
report satisfying relationships (e.g., Collins & Read, 1990) and are more likely
to provide support to their partners when their partners were feeling distressed
(Simpson, Rholes, & Nelligan, 1992).
665
of more than 700 individuals studied from infancy to adulthood that maternal
sensitivity across development prospectively predicted security at age 18.
Simpson, Collins, Tran, and Haydon (2007) found that attachment security,
assessed in infancy in the strange situation, predicted peer competence in
grades 1 to 3, which, in turn, predicted the quality of friendship relationships
at age 16, which, in turn, predicted the expression of positive and negative
emotions in their adult romantic relationships at ages 20 to 23.
It is easy to come away from such findings with the mistaken assumption
that early experiences determine later outcomes. To be clear: Attachment
theorists assume that the relationship between early experiences and
subsequent outcomes is probabilistic, not deterministic. Having supportive and
responsive experiences with caregivers early in life is assumed to set the stage
for positive social development. But that does not mean that attachment
patterns are set in stone. In short, even if an individual has far from optimal
experiences in early life, attachment theory suggests that it is possible for that
individual to develop well-functioning adult relationships through a number of
corrective experiencesincluding relationships with siblings, other family
members, teachers, and close friends. Security is best viewed as a culmination
of a persons attachment history rather than a reflection of his or her early
experiences alone. Those early experiences are considered important not
because they determine a persons fate, but because they provide the
foundation for subsequent experiences.
666
Outside Resources
Survey: Learn more about your attachment patterns via this online survey
http://www.yourpersonality.net/relstructures/
Discussion Questions
1. What kind of relationship did you have with your parents or primary
caregivers when you were young? Do you think that had any bearing on the
way you related to others (e.g., friends, relationship partners) as you grew
older?
2. There is variation across cultures in the extent to which people value
independence. Do you think this might have implications for the
development of attachment patterns?
3. As parents age, it is not uncommon for them to have to depend on their
adult children. Do you think that peoples history of experiences in their
relationships with their parents might shape peoples willingness to provide
care for their aging parents? In other words, are secure adults more likely
to provide responsive care to their aging parents?
4. Some people, despite reporting insecure relationships with their parents,
report secure, well-functioning relationships with their spouses. What kinds
of experiences do you think might enable someone to develop a secure
relationship with their partners despite having an insecure relationship with
other central figures in their lives?
5. Most attachment research on adults focuses on attachment to peers (e.g.,
romantic partners). What other kinds of things may serve as attachment
figures? Do you think siblings, pets, or gods can serve as attachment figures?
Vocabulary
Attachment behavioral system
A motivational system selected over the course of evolution to maintain
proximity between a young child and his or her primary attachment figure.
Attachment behaviors
Behaviors and signals that attract the attention of a primary attachment figure
and function to prevent separation from that individual or to reestablish
proximity to that individual (e.g., crying, clinging).
Attachment figure
Someone who functions as the primary safe haven and secure base for an
individual. In childhood, an individuals attachment figure is often a parent. In
adulthood, an individuals attachment figure is often a romantic partner.
Attachment patterns
(also called attachment styles or attachment orientations) Individual
differences in how securely (vs. insecurely) people think, feel, and behave in
attachment relationships.
Strange situation
A laboratory task that involves briefly separating and reuniting infants and their
primary caregivers as a way of studying individual differences in attachment
behavior.
Reference List
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of
attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York, NY: Basic
Books
Chappell, K. D., & Davis, K. E. (1998). Attachment, partner choice, and perception
of romantic partners: An experimental test of the attachment-security
hypothesis. Personal Relationships, 5, 327342.
Collins, N., & Read, S. (1990). Adult attachment, working models and relationship
quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58,
644-663.
Feeney, J. A., & Noller, P. (1992). Attachment style and romantic love: Relationship
dissolution. Australian Journal of Psychology, 44, 6974.
Fraley, R. C., Roisman, G. I., Booth-LaForce, C., Owen, M. T., & Holland, A. S.
(2013). Interpersonal and genetic origins of adult attachment styles: A
longitudinal study from infancy to early adulthood. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 104, 8817-838.
Frazier, P. A, Byer, A. L., Fischer, A. R., Wright, D. M., & DeBord, K. A. (1996). Adult
attachment style and partner choice: Correlational and experimental
findings. Personal Relationships, 3, 117136.
Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Spangler, G., Suess, G., & Unzner, L. (1985).
Maternal sensitivity and newborns orientation responses as related to
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Attachment Through the Life Course by R. Chris
Fraley is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Emerging Adulthood
Jeffrey Jensen Arnett
Clark University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Emerging adulthood has been proposed as a new life stage between
adolescence and young adulthood, lasting roughly from ages 18 to 25. Five
features make emerging adulthood distinctive: identity explorations, instability,
self-focus, feeling in-between adolescence and adulthood, and a sense of broad
possibilities for the future. Emerging adulthood is found mainly in developed
countries, where most young people obtain tertiary education and median ages
of entering marriage and parenthood are around 30. There are variations in
emerging adulthood within developed countries. It lasts longest in Europe, and
in Asian developed countries, the self-focused freedom of emerging adulthood
is balanced by obligations to parents and by conservative views of sexuality. In
developing countries, although today emerging adulthood exists only among
the middle-class elite, it can be expected to grow in the 21st century as these
countries become more affluent.
Learning Objectives
Explain where, when, and why a new life stage of emerging adulthood
appeared over the past half-century.
Identify the five features that distinguish emerging adulthood from other
life stages.
Introduction
Think for a moment about the lives of your grandparents and greatgrandparents when they were in their twenties. How do their lives at that age
compare to your life? If they were like most other people of their time, their
lives were quite different than yours. What happened to change the twenties
so much between their time and our own? And how should we understand the
1829 age period today?
The theory of emerging adulthood proposes that a new life stage has arisen
between adolescence and young adulthood over the past half-century in
industrialized countries. Fifty years ago, most young people in these countries
had entered stable adult roles in love and work by their late teens or early
twenties. Relatively few people pursued education or training beyond
secondary school, and, consequently, most young men were full-time workers
by the end of their teens. Relatively few women worked in occupations outside
the home, and the median marriage age for women in the United States and
in most other industrialized countries in 1960 was around 20 (Arnett & Taber,
1994; Douglass, 2005). The median marriage age for men was around 22, and
married couples usually had their first child about one year after their wedding
day. All told, for most young people half a century ago, their teenage
adolescence led quickly and directly to stable adult roles in love and work by
their late teens or early twenties. These roles would form the structure of their
adult lives for decades to come.
Now all that has changed. A higher proportion of young people than ever
beforeabout 70% in the United Statespursue education and training
beyond secondary school (National Center for Education Statistics, 2012). The
early twenties are not a time of entering stable adult work but a time of immense
job instability: In the United States, the average number of job changes from
ages 20 to 29 is seven. The median age of entering marriage in the United States
is now 27 for women and 29 for men (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 2011).
nobaproject.com - Emerging Adulthood
677
Consequently, a new stage of the life span, emerging adulthood, has been
created, lasting from the late teens through the mid-twenties, roughly ages 18
to 25.
678
679
International Variations
The five features proposed in the theory of emerging adulthood originally were
based on research involving about 300 Americans between ages 18 and 29 from
various ethnic groups, social classes, and geographical regions (Arnett, 2004).
To what extent does the theory of emerging adulthood apply internationally?
nobaproject.com - Emerging Adulthood
680
The answer to this question depends greatly on what part of the world is
considered. Demographers make a useful distinction between the developing
countries that comprise the majority of the worlds population and the
economically developed countries that are part of the Organization for
Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD), including the United States,
Canada, western Europe, Japan, South Korea, Australia, and New Zealand. The
current population of OECD countries (also called developed countries) is 1.2
billion, about 18% of the total world population (UNDP, 2011). The rest of the
human population resides in developing countries, which have much lower
median incomes; much lower median educational attainment; and much higher
incidence of illness, disease, and early death. Let us consider emerging
adulthood in OECD countries first, then in developing countries.
681
The lives of Asian emerging adults in developed countries such as Japan and
South Korea are in some ways similar to the lives of emerging adults in Europe
and in some ways strikingly different. Like European emerging adults, Asian
emerging adults tend to enter marriage and parenthood around age 30 (Arnett,
2011). Like European emerging adults, Asian emerging adults in Japan and South
Korea enjoy the benefits of living in affluent societies with generous social
welfare systems that provide support for them in making the transition to
adulthoodfor
example,
free
university
education
and
substantial
unemployment benefits.
However, in other ways, the experience of emerging adulthood in Asian
OECD countries is markedly different than in Europe. Europe has a long history
of individualism, and todays emerging adults carry that legacy with them in
their focus on self-development and leisure during emerging adulthood. In
contrast, Asian cultures have a shared cultural history emphasizing collectivism
and family obligations. Although Asian cultures have become more
individualistic in recent decades as a consequence of globalization, the legacy
of collectivism persists in the lives of emerging adults. They pursue identity
explorations and self-development during emerging adulthood, like their
American and European counterparts, but within narrower boundaries set by
their sense of obligations to others, especially their parents (Phinney &
Baldelomar, 2011). For example, in their views of the most important criteria
for becoming an adult, emerging adults in the United States and Europe
consistently rank financial independence among the most important markers
of adulthood. In contrast, emerging adults with an Asian cultural background
especially emphasize becoming capable of supporting parents financially as
among the most important criteria (Arnett, 2003; Nelson, Badger, & Wu, 2004).
This sense of family obligation may curtail their identity explorations in
emerging adulthood to some extent, as they pay more heed to their parents
wishes about what they should study, what job they should take, and where
they should live than emerging adults do in the West (Rosenberger, 2007).
nobaproject.com - Emerging Adulthood
682
683
Figure 1: Gross tertiary enrollment, selected countries, 2007. Source: UNdata (2010). Note.
Gross enrollment ratio is the total enrollment in a specific level of education, regardless of
age, expressed as a percentage of the eligible official school-age population corresponding
to the same level of education in a given school year. For the tertiary level, the population
used is that of the five-year age group following the end of secondary schooling.
684
Conclusion
The new life stage of emerging adulthood has spread rapidly in the past halfcentury and is continuing to spread. Now that the transition to adulthood is
later than in the past, is this change positive or negative for emerging adults
and their societies? Certainly there are some negatives. It means that young
people are dependent on their parents for longer than in the past, and they
take longer to become full contributing members of their societies. A substantial
proportion of them have trouble sorting through the opportunities available to
them and struggle with anxiety and depression, even though most are
optimistic. However, there are advantages to having this new life stage as well.
By waiting until at least their late twenties to take on the full range of adult
responsibilities, emerging adults are able to focus on obtaining enough
education and training to prepare themselves for the demands of todays
information- and technology-based economy. Also, it seems likely that if young
people make crucial decisions about love and work in their late twenties or early
thirties rather than their late teens and early twenties, their judgment will be
more mature and they will have a better chance of making choices that will
work out well for them in the long run.
What can societies do to enhance the likelihood that emerging adults will
make a successful transition to adulthood? One important step would be to
expand the opportunities for obtaining tertiary education. The tertiary
education systems of OECD countries were constructed at a time when the
economy was much different, and they have not expanded at the rate needed
nobaproject.com - Emerging Adulthood
685
to serve all the emerging adults who need such education. Furthermore, in
some countries, such as the United States, the cost of tertiary education has
risen steeply and is often unaffordable to many young people. In developing
countries, tertiary education systems are even smaller and less able to
accommodate their emerging adults. Across the world, societies would be wise
to strive to make it possible for every emerging adult to receive tertiary
education, free of charge. There could be no better investment for preparing
young people for the economy of the future.
686
Outside Resources
Web: Jeffrey Jensen Arnett website
http://www.jeffreyarnett.com
Web: Society for the Study of Emerging Adulthood
http://www.ssea.org
Discussion Questions
1. What kind of variations in emerging adulthood would you predict within
your country? Would there be social class differences? Gender differences?
Ethnic differences?
2. Looking at Figure 1, what contrasts do you observe between OECD countries
and developing countries? Between males and females? What economic
and cultural differences might explain these contrasts?
3. Do you agree or disagree with the authors prediction that emerging
adulthood is likely to become a life stage experienced worldwide in the
decades to come? What factors are likely to determine whether this turns
out to be true?
Vocabulary
Collectivism
Belief system that emphasizes the duties and obligations that each person has
toward others.
Developed countries
The economically advanced countries of the world, in which most of the worlds
wealth is concentrated.
Developing countries
The less economically advanced countries that comprise the majority of the
worlds population. Most are currently developing at a rapid rate.
Emerging adulthood
A new life stage extending from approximately ages 18 to 25, during which the
foundation of an adult life is gradually constructed in love and work. Primary
features include identity explorations, instability, focus on self-development,
feeling incompletely adult, and a broad sense of possibilities.
Individualism
Belief system that exalts freedom, independence, and individual choice as high
values.
OECD countries
Members of the Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development,
comprised of the worlds wealthiest countries.
Tertiary education
Education or training beyond secondary school, usually taking place in a college,
university, or vocational training program.
Reference List
Arnett, J. J. (2012). New horizons in emerging and young adulthood. In A. Booth
& N. Crouter (Eds.), Early adulthood in a family context (pp. 231244). New
York, NY: Springer.
Arnett, J. J. (2011). Emerging adulthood(s): The cultural psychology of a new life
stage. In L.A. Jensen (Ed.), Bridging cultural and developmental psychology:
New syntheses in theory, research, and policy (pp. 255275). New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Arnett, J. J. (2004). Emerging adulthood: The winding road from late teens
through the twenties. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Arnett, J. J. (2003). Conceptions of the transition to adulthood among emerging
adults in American ethnic groups. New Directions for Child and Adolescent
Development, 100, 6375.
Arnett, J. J. & Schwab, J. (2012). The Clark University poll of emerging adults:
Thriving, struggling, & hopeful. Worcester, MA: Clark University.
Arnett, J. J., & Taber, S. (1994). Adolescence terminable and interminable: When
does adolescence end? Journal of Youth & Adolescence, 23, 517537.
Arnett, J.J. (2007). The long and leisurely route: Coming of age in Europe today.
Current History, 106, 130-136.
Ct, J. (2006). Emerging adulthood as an institutionalized moratorium: Risks
and benefits to identity formation. In J. J. Arnett & J. L. Tanner (Eds.), Emerging
adults in America: Coming of age in the 21st century (pp. 85116).
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Emerging Adulthood by Jeffrey Jensen Arnett is
licensed
under
the
Creative
Commons
Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0
Abstract
This chapter focuses on parenthood as a developmental task of adulthood.
Parents take on new roles as their children develop, transforming their identity
as a parent as the developmental demands of their children change. The main
influences on parenting, parent characteristics, child characteristics, and
contextual factors, are described.
Learning Objectives
699
Galinsky (1987) was one of the first to emphasize the development of parents
themselves, how they respond to their childrens development, and how they
grow as parents. Parenthood is an experience that transforms ones identity as
parents take on new roles. Childrens growth and development force parents
to change their roles. They must develop new skills and abilities in response to
childrens development. Galinsky identified six stages of parenthood that focus
on different tasks and goals (see Table 2).
nobaproject.com - The Developing Parent
700
701
their relationship with their child into their other relationships, parents often
have to reshape their conceptions of themselves and their identity. Parenting
responsibilities are the most demanding during infancy because infants are
completely dependent on caregiving.
702
703
Influences on Parenting
Parenting is a complex process in which parents and children influence one
another. There are many reasons that parents behave the way do. The multiple
influences on parenting are still being explored. Proposed influences on
parental behavior include 1) parent characteristics, 2) child characteristics, and
3) contextual and sociocultural characteristics (Belsky, 1984; Demick, 1999) (see
Figure 1).
Parent Characteristics
Parents bring unique traits and qualities to the parenting relationship that affect
their decisions as parents. These characteristics include the age of the parent,
gender, beliefs, personality, developmental history, knowledge about parenting
and child development, and mental and physical health. Parents personalities
affect parenting behaviors. Mothers and fathers who are more agreeable,
conscientious, and outgoing are warmer and provide more structure to their
children. Parents who are more agreeable, less anxious, and less negative also
support their childrens autonomy more than parents who are anxious and less
agreeable (Prinzie, Stams, Dekovic, Reijntjes, & Belsky, 2009). Parents who have
these personality traits appear to be better able to respond to their children
positively and provide a more consistent, structured environment for their
children.
Parents developmental histories, or their experiences as children, also affect
their parenting strategies. Parents may learn parenting practices from their own
parents. Fathers whose own parents provided monitoring, consistent and ageappropriate discipline, and warmth were more likely to provide this constructive
parenting to their own children (Kerr, Capaldi, Pears, & Owen, 2009). Patterns
of negative parenting and ineffective discipline also appear from one generation
to the next. However, parents who are dissatisfied with their own parents
nobaproject.com - The Developing Parent
704
approach may be more likely to change their parenting methods with their own
children.
Child Characteristics
Parenting is bidirectional. Not only do parents affect their children, children
influence their parents. Child characteristics, such as gender, birth order,
temperament, and health status, affect parenting behaviors and roles. For
example, an infant with an easy temperament may enable parents to feel more
effective, as they are easily able to soothe the child and elicit smiling and cooing.
On the other hand, a cranky or fussy infant elicits fewer positive reactions from
his or her parents and may result in parents feeling less effective in the parenting
role (Eisenberg et al., 2008). Over time, parents of more difficult children may
become more punitive and less patient with their children (Clark, Kochanska,
& Ready, 2000; Eisenberg et al., 1999; Kiff, Lengua, & Zalewski, 2011). Parents
who have a fussy, difficult child are less satisfied with their marriages and have
greater challenges in balancing work and family roles (Hyde, Else-Quest, &
Goldsmith, 2004). Thus, child temperament is one of the child characteristics
that influences how parents behave with their children.
Another child characteristic is the gender of the child. Parents respond
differently to boys and girls. Parents often assign different household chores
to their sons and daughters. Girls are more often responsible for caring for
younger siblings and household chores, whereas boys are more likely to be
asked to perform chores outside the home, such as mowing the lawn (Grusec,
Goodnow, & Cohen, 1996). Parents also talk differently with their sons and
daughters, providing more scientific explanations to their sons and using more
emotion words with their daughters (Crowley, Callanan, Tenenbaum, & Allen,
2001).
705
706
Conclusion
Many factors influence parenting decisions and behaviors. These factors
include characteristics of the parent, such as gender and personality, as well as
characteristics of the child, such as age. The context is also important. The
interaction among all these factors creates many different patterns of parenting
behavior. Furthermore, parenting influences not just a childs development, but
also the development of the parent. As parents are faced with new challenges,
they change their parenting strategies and construct new aspects of their
identity. The goals and tasks of parents change over time as their children
develop.
707
Outside Resources
Web: American Psychological Association- Information and Resources on
Parenting
http://www.apa.org/topics/parenting/index.aspx
Discussion Questions
1. Reflect on the way you were raised. Consider the parenting behaviors (e.g.,
rules, discipline strategies, warmth, and support) used in your household
when you were a child. Why do you think your parents behaved this way?
How do these factors fit with the influences on parenting described here?
Provide specific examples of multiple influences on parenting.
2. Think about different parents and grandparents you know. Do the
challenges they face as parents differ based on the age of their children?
Do your observations fit with Galinskys stages of parenting?
3. What type of parent do you envision yourself becoming? If you are a parent,
how do you parent your child/children? How do you think this is similar to
or different than the way you were raised? What influences exist in your life
that will make you parent differently from your own parents?
Vocabulary
Authority stage
Stage from approximately 2 years to age 4 or 5 when parents create rules and
figure out how to effectively guide their childrens behavior.
Bidirectional
The idea that parents influence their children, but their children also influence
the parents; the direction of influence goes both ways, from parent to child,
and from child to parent.
Departure stage
Stage at which parents prepare for a child to depart and evaluate their successes
and failures as parents.
Image-making stage
Stage during pregnancy when parents consider what it means to be a parent
and plan for changes to accommodate a child.
Interdependent stage
Stage during teenage years when parents renegotiate their relationship with
their adolescent children to allow for shared power in decision-making.
Interpretive stage
Stage from age 4or 5 to the start of adolescence when parents help their children
interpret their experiences with the social world beyond the family.
Nurturing stage
Stage from birth to around 18-24 months in which parents develop an
attachment relationship with child and adapt to the new baby.
Temperament
A childs innate personality; biologically based personality, including qualities
such as activity level, emotional reactivity, sociability, mood, and soothability.
Reference List
Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child
Development, 55, 8396.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1989). Ecological systems theory. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Annals
of Child Development, Vol. 6 (pp. 187251). Geenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Clark, L. A., Kochanska, G., & Ready, R. (2000). Mothers personality and its
interaction with child temperament as predictors of parenting behavior.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 274285.
Cohn, D., Passel, J., Wang, W., & Livingston, G. (2011). Barely Half of U.S. Adults
are Married A Record Low. Social & Demographic Trends. Washington,
DC: Pew Research Center.
Conger, R. D., & Conger, K. J. (2002). Resilience in Midwestern families: Selected
findings from the first decade of a prospective longitudinal study. Journal
of Marriage and Family, 64, 361373.
Crowley, K., Callanan, M. A., Tenenbaum, H. R., & Allen, E. (2001). Parents explain
more often to boys than to girls during shared scientific thinking.
Psychological Science, 12, 258261.
Demick, J. (1999). Parental development: Problem, theory, method, and practice.
In R. L. Mosher, D. J. Youngman, & J. M. Day (Eds.), Human Development
Across the Life Span: Educational and Psychological Applications (pp. 177
199). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Dye, J. L. (2010). Fertility of American women: 2008. Current Population Reports
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. The Developing Parent by Marissa L. Diener is
licensed
under
the
Creative
Commons
Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0
Aging
Tara Queen & Jacqui Smith
University of Michigan
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Traditionally, research on aging described only the lives of people over age 65
and the very old. Contemporary theories and research recognizes that
biogenetic and psychological processes of aging are complex and lifelong.
Functioning in each period of life is influenced by what happened earlier and,
in turn, affects subsequent change. We all age in specific social and historical
contexts. Together, these multiple influences on aging make it difficult to define
when middle-age or old age begins. This chapter describes central concepts
and research about adult development and aging. We consider contemporary
questions about cognitive aging and changes in personality, self-related beliefs,
social relationships, and subjective well-being. These four aspects of
psychosocial aging are related to health and longevity.
Learning Objectives
Describe cognitive, psychosocial, and physical changes that occur with age.
Introduction
We are currently living in an aging society (Rowe, 2009). Indeed, by 2030 when
the last of the Baby Boomers reach age 65, the U.S. older population will be
double that of 2010. Furthermore, because of increases in average life
expectancy, each new generation can expect to live longer than their parents
generation and certainly longer than their grandparents generation. As a
consequence, it is time for individuals of all ages to rethink their personal life
plans and consider prospects for a long life. When is the best time to start a
family? Will the education gained up to age 20 be sufficient to cope with future
technological advances and marketplace needs? What is the right balance
between work, family, and leisure throughout life? What's the best age to retire?
How can I age successfully and enjoy life to the fullest when I'm 80 or 90? In
this chapter we will discuss several different domains of psychological research
on aging that will help answer these important questions.
720
highlight the effects of social expectations and the normative timing of life
events and social roles (e.g., becoming a parent, retirement). They also consider
the lifelong cumulative effects of membership in specific cohorts (generations)
and sociocultural subgroups (e.g., race, gender, socioeconomic status) and
exposure to historical events (e.g., war, revolution, natural disasters; Elder,
Johnson, & Crosnoe, 2003; Settersten, 2005). Life span theories complement
the life-course perspective with a greater focus on processes within the
individual (e.g., the aging brain). This approach emphasizes the patterning of
lifelong intra- and inter-individual differences in the shape (gain, maintenance,
loss), level, and rate of change (Baltes, 1987, 1997). Both life course and life
span researchers generally rely on longitudinal studies to examine hypotheses
about different patterns of aging associated with the effects of biogenetic, life
history, social, and personal factors. Cross-sectional studies provide
information about age-group differences, but these are confounded with
cohort, time of study, and historical effects.
Cognitive Aging
Researchers have identified areas of both losses and gains in cognition in older
age. Cognitive ability and intelligence are often measured using standardized
tests and validated measures. The psychometric approach has identified two
categories of intelligence that show different rates of change across the life
span (Schaie & Willis, 1996). Fluid intelligence refers to information processing
abilities, such as logical reasoning, remembering lists, spatial ability, and
reaction time. Crystallized intelligence encompasses abilities that draw upon
experience and knowledge. Measures of crystallized intelligence include
vocabulary tests, solving number problems, and understanding texts.
nobaproject.com - Aging
721
With age, systematic declines are observed on cognitive tasks requiring selfinitiated, effortful processing, without the aid of supportive memory cues (Park,
2000). Older adults tend to perform poorer than young adults on memory tasks
that involve recall of information, where individuals must retrieve information
they learned previously without the help of a list of possible choices. For
example, older adults may have more difficulty recalling facts such as names
or contextual details about where or when something happened (Craik, 2000).
What might explain these deficits as we age? As we age, working memory, or
our ability to simultaneously store and use information, becomes less efficient
(Craik & Bialystok, 2006). The ability to process information quickly also
decreases with age. This slowing of processing speed may explain age
differences on many different cognitive tasks (Salthouse, 2004). Some
researchers have argued that inhibitory functioning, or the ability to focus on
certain information while suppressing attention to less pertinent information,
declines with age and may explain age differences in performance on cognitive
tasks (Hasher & Zacks, 1988). Finally, it is well established that our hearing and
vision decline as we age. Longitudinal research has proposed that deficits in
sensory functioning explain age differences in a variety of cognitive abilities
(Baltes & Lindenberger, 1997).
Fewer age differences are observed when memory cues are available, such
as for recognition memory tasks, or when individuals can draw upon acquired
knowledge or experience. For example, older adults often perform as well if
not better than young adults on tests of word knowledge or vocabulary. With
age often comes expertise, and research has pointed to areas where aging
experts perform as well or better than younger individuals. For example, older
typists were found to compensate for age-related declines in speed by looking
farther ahead at printed text (Salthouse, 1984). Compared to younger players,
older chess experts are able to focus on a smaller set of possible moves, leading
to greater cognitive efficiency (Charness, 1981). Accrued knowledge of everyday
tasks, such as grocery prices, can help older adults to make better decisions
nobaproject.com - Aging
722
723
self-propose
changes
in
self-related
knowledge,
beliefs,
and
724
value young bodies. Feeling younger and being satisfied with ones own aging
are expressions of positive self-perceptions of aging. They reflect the operation
of self-related processes that enhance well-being. Levy (2009) found that older
individuals who are able to adapt to and accept changes in their appearance
and physical capacity in a positive way report higher well-being, have better
health, and live longer.
Social Relationships
Social ties to family, friends, mentors, and peers are primary resources of
information, support, and comfort. Individuals develop and age together with
family and friends and interact with others in the community. Across the life
course, social ties are accumulated, lost, and transformed. Already in early life,
there are multiple sources of heterogeneity in the characteristics of each
person's social network of relationships (e.g., size, composition, and quality).
Life course and life span theories and research about age-related patterns in
social relationships focus on understanding changes in the processes
underlying social connections. Antonucci's Convoy Model of Social Relations
(2001; Kahn & Antonucci, 1980), for example, suggests that the social
connections that people accumulate are held together by exchanges in social
support (e.g., tangible and emotional). The frequency, types, and reciprocity of
the exchanges change with age and in response to need, and in turn, these
exchanges impact the health and well-being of the givers and receivers in the
convoy. In many relationships, it is not the actual objective exchange of support
that is critical but instead the perception that support is available if needed
(Uchino, 2009). Carstensens Socioemotional Selectivity Theory (1993;
Carstensen, Isaacowitz, & Charles, 1999) focuses on changes in motivation for
actively seeking social contact with others. She proposes that with increasing
age our motivational goals change from information gathering to emotion
regulation. To optimize the experience of positive affect, older adults actively
nobaproject.com - Aging
725
restrict their social life to prioritize time spent with emotionally close significant
others. In line with this, older marriages are found to be characterized by
enhanced positive and reduced negative interactions and older partners show
more affectionate behavior during conflict discussions than do middle-aged
partners (Carstensen, Gottman, & Levenson, 1995). Research showing that
older adults have smaller networks compared to young adults and tend to avoid
negative interactions also supports this theory. Similar selective processes are
also observed when time horizons for interactions with close partners shrink
temporarily for young adults (e.g., impending geographical separations).
Much research focuses on the associations between specific effects of longterm social relationships and health in later life. Older married individuals who
receive positive social and emotional support from their partner generally
report better health than their unmarried peers (Antonucci, 2001; Umberson,
Williams, Powers, Liu, & Needham, 2006; Waite & Gallagher, 2000). Despite the
overall positive health effects of being married in old age (compared with being
widowed, divorced, or single), living as a couple can have a "dark side" if the
relationship is strained or if one partner is the primary caregiver. The
consequences of positive and negative aspects of relationships are complex
(Birditt & Antonucci, 2008; Rook, 1998; Uchino, 2009). For example, in some
circumstances, criticism from a partner may be perceived as valid and useful
feedback whereas in others it is considered unwarranted and hurtful. In longterm relationships, habitual negative exchanges might have diminished effects.
Parent-child and sibling relationships are often the most long-term and
emotion-laden social ties. Across the life span, the parent-child tie, for example,
is characterized by a paradox of solidarity, conflict, and ambivalence
(Fingerman, Chen, Hay, Cichy, & Lefkowitz, 2006).
nobaproject.com - Aging
726
727
728
nobaproject.com - Aging
729
Outside Resources
Web: Columbia Aging Society
http://www.agingsocietynetwork.org/
Web: Columbia International Longevity Center
http://www.mailman.columbia.edu/academic-departments/centers/columbiaaging/international-longevity-center-knowledge-transfer
Web: National Institute on Aging
http://www.nia.nih.gov/
Web: Stanford Center Longevity
http://longevity3.stanford.edu/
Discussion Questions
1. How do age stereotypes and intergenerational social interactions shape
quality of life in older adults? What are the implications of the research of
Levy and others?
2. Researchers suggest that there is both stability and change in Big Five
personality traits after age 30. What is stable? What changes?
3. Describe the Social Convoy Model of Antonucci. What are the implications
of this model for older adults?
4. Memory declines during adulthood. Is this statement correct? What does
research show?
5. Is dementia inevitable in old age? What factors are currently thought to be
protective?
6. What are the components of successful aging described by Rowe and Kahn
(1998) and others? What outcomes are used to evaluate successful aging?
Vocabulary
Age identity
How old or young people feel compared to their chronological age; after early
adulthood, most people feel younger than their chronological age.
Autobiographical narratives
A qualitative research method used to understand characteristics and life
themes that an individual considers to uniquely distinguish him- or herself from
others.
Average life expectancy
Mean number of years that 50% of people in a specific birth cohort are expected
to survive. This is typically calculated from birth but is also sometimes recalculated for people who have already reached a particular age (e.g., 65).
Cohort
Group of people typically born in the same year or historical period, who share
common experiences over time; sometimes called a generation (e.g., Baby
Boom Generation).
Convoy Model of Social Relations
Theory that proposes that the frequency, types, and reciprocity of social
exchanges change with age. These social exchanges impact the health and wellbeing of the givers and receivers in the convoy.
Cross-sectional studies
Research method that provides information about age group differences; age
differences are confounded with cohort differences and effects related to
history and time of study.
Crystallized intelligence
Type of intellectual ability that relies on the application of knowledge,
experience, and learned information.
Fluid intelligence
Type of intelligence that relies on the ability to use information processing
resources to reason logically and solve novel problems.
Hedonic well-being
Component of well-being that refers to emotional experiences, often including
measures of positive (e.g., happiness, contentment) and negative affect (e.g.,
stress, sadness).
Heterogeneity
Inter-individual and subgroup differences in level and rate of change over time.
Inhibitory functioning
Ability to focus on a subset of information while suppressing attention to less
relevant information.
Longitudinal studies
Research method that collects information from individuals at multiple time
points over time, allowing researchers to track cohort differences in age-related
change to determine cumulative effects of different life experiences.
Processing speed
The time it takes individuals to perform cognitive operations (e.g., process
information, react to a signal, switch attention from one task to another, find a
specific target object in a complex picture).
Psychometric approach
Approach to studying intelligence that examines performance on tests of
intellectual functioning.
Recall
Type of memory task where individuals are asked to remember previously
learned information without the help of external cues.
Recognition
Type of memory task where individuals are asked to remember previously
learned information with the assistance of cues.
Self-perceptions of aging
An individuals perceptions of their own aging process; positive perceptions of
aging have been shown to be associated with greater longevity and health.
Social network
Network of people with whom an individual is closely connected; social
networks provide emotional, informational, and material support and offer
opportunities for social engagement.
Socioemotional Selectivity Theory
Theory proposed to explain the reduction of social partners in older adulthood;
posits that older adults focus on meeting emotional over information-gathering
goals, and adaptively select social partners who meet this need.
Subjective age
A multidimensional construct that indicates how old (or young) a person feels
and into which age group a person categorizes him- or herself
Successful aging
Includes three components: avoiding disease, maintaining high levels of
cognitive and physical functioning, and having an actively engaged lifestyle.
Working memory
Memory system that allows for information to be simultaneously stored and
utilized or manipulated.
Reference List
Antonucci, T. C. (2001). Social relations: An examination of social networks, social
support and sense of control. In J.E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook
of the psychology of aging (5th ed., pp. 427453). New York: Academic Press.
Baltes, P. B . (1987). Theoretical propositions of lifespan developmental
psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental
Psychology, 23, 611626.
Baltes, P. B. & Lindenberger, U. (1997). Emergence of powerful connection
between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: A new
window to the study of cognitive aging? Psychology and Aging, 12, 1221.
Birditt, K., & Antonucci, T. C. (2008). Life sustaining irritations? Relationship
quality and mortality in the context of chronic illness. Social Science &
Medicine, 67, 1291.
Birren, J. E. & Schroots, J. J. F. (2006). Autobiographical memory and the narrative
self over the life span. In J. E. Birren and K. Warner Schaie (Eds.) Handbook
of the psychology of aging (6th ed. pp. 477499). Burlingham, MA: Elsevier
Academic Press.
Blanchard-Fields, F. (2007). Everyday problem solving and emotion: An adult
development perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16,
2631.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Carstensen, L. L. (1993). Motivation for social contact across the life span: A
theory of socioemotional selectivity. In J. E. Jacobs (Ed.), Nebraska
Symposium on Motivation, 1992: Developmental perspectives on
motivation (pp. 209254). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Carstensen, L. L., Gottman, J. M., & Levensen, R. W. (1995). Emotional behavior
in long-term marriage. Psychology and Aging, 10, 140149.
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously:
A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54, 165181.
Charles, S. T., & Carstensen, L. L. (2010). Social and emotional aging. Annual
review of psychology, 61, 383409.
Charness, N. (1981). Search in chess: Age and skill differences.Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 467.
Christensen, K., Doblhammer, G.,
Elder, G. H., Johnson, M. K., & Crosnoe, R. (2003). The emergence and
development of life course theory. In J. T. Mortimer & M. J. Shanahan (Eds.),
Handbook of the Life Course, 3-19. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum
Publishers.
Fingerman, K. L., Berg, C. A., Smith, J., & Antonucci, T.C. (Eds.) (2011). Handbook
of Life-Span Development. New York: Springer
Fingerman, K. L., Chen, P. C., Hay, E., Cichy, K. E., & Lefkowitz, E. S. (2006).
Ambivalent reactions in the parent and offspring relationship. The Journals
of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 61,
152-160.
Fratiglioni, L., Paillard-Borg, S., & Winblad, B. (2004). An active and socially
integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet
Neurology, 3, 343353
Friedman, H. S., Tucker, J. S., Tomlinson-Keasey, C., Schwartz, J. E., Wingard, D.
L., & Criqui,
M. H. (1993). Does childhood personality predict longevity? Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 176185. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.176
George, L. K. (2010). Still happy after all these years: Research frontiers on
subjective well-being in later life. The Journals of Gerontology Series B:
Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 65, 331.
Hasher, L. & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging:
A review and a new view. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning
and Motivation, (Vol. 22, pp. 193225). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Hertzog, C., Kramer, A. F., Wilson, R. S., & Lindenberger, U. (2009). Enrichment
effects on adult cognitive development: Can the functional capacity of older
adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychological Science in the Public
Interest, 9, 165
Kahn, R. L., & Antonucci, T. C. (1980). Convoys over the life course: Attachment,
roles, and social support. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim (Eds.), Life-span
development and behavior (pp. 253286). New York: Academic Press.
Kramer, A. F. & Erickson, K. I. (2007). Capitalizing on cortical plasticity: The
influence of physical activity on cognition and brain function. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 11, 342348.
Lang, F. R., Baltes, P. B., & Wagner, G. G. (2007). Desired lifetime and end-of-life
desires across ddulthood From 20 to 90: A dual-source information model.
Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 62B, 268276.
Levy, B. (2009). Stereotype Embodiment: A Psychosocial Approach to Aging.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(6), 332336
Lucas, R. E. (2007). Adaptation and the Set-Point Model of Subjective Well-Being
Does Happiness Change After Major Life Events?. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 16, 7579.
Lucas, R. E. & Donnellan, A. B. (2011). Personality development across the life
span: Longitudinal analyses with a national sample from Germany. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 847861
Magai, C. (2008). Long-lived emotions. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L.
Feldman Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions, 376392. New York:
Guilford.
Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41, 954
969.
McAdams, D. P.(2006). The redemptive self: Generativity and the stories
Americans live by. Research in Human Development, 3(2-3), 81100.
Mroczek, D. K. (2001). Age and emotion in adulthood. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 10(3), 8790.
Park, D. C. (2000). The basic mechanisms accounting for age-related decline in
cognitive function. In D.C. Park & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Cognitive Aging: A Primer
(pp. 321). New York: Psychology Press.
Park, D. C. & Gutchess, A. H. (2000). Cognitive aging and everyday life. In D.C.
Park & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Cognitive Aging: A Primer (pp. 217232). New York:
Psychology Press.
Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N., Shiner, R., N., Caspi, A., & Goldberg, L. R. (2007). The
power of personality: The comparative validity of personality traits,
socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for predicting important life
outcomes.
Perspectives
on
Psychological
Science,
2(4),
313345.
doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00047.
Roberts, B. W., & Mroczek, D. K. (2008). Personality trait change in adulthood.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 3135.
Rook, K. S. (1998). Investigating the positive and negative sides of personal
relationships: Through a lens darkly? In W. R. Cupach & B. H. Spitzberg (Eds.),
The dark side of close relationships (pp. 369393). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Ross, M., & Wilson, A. E. (2003). Autobiographical memory and conceptions of
self: Getting better all the time. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
12, 6669.
Rowe, J. W. (2009). Facts and fictions about an aging America. Contexts, 8, 1621.
Rowe, J. W. & Kahn, R. L. (1997). Successful aging. The Gerontologist, 37(4), 433
440.
Rubin, D., & Berntsen, D. (2006). People over 40 feel 20% younger than their
age: Subjective age across the lifespan. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 13,
776780
Ryff, C. D. (1995). Psychological well-being in adult life. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 4(4), 99104
Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. (2000). Interpersonal flourishing: A positive health agenda
for the new millennium. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 3044.
Salthouse, T. A. (2004). What and when of cognitive aging. Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 13, 140144.
Salthouse, T. A. (1984). Effects of age and skill in typing. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 113, 345.
Schaie, K. W. & Willis, S. L. (1996). Psychometric intelligence and aging. In F.
Blanchard-Fields & T.M. Hess (Eds.), Perspectives on Cognitive Change in
Stone, A. A., Schwartz, J. E., Broderick, J. E., & Deaton, A. (2010). A snapshot of
the age distribution of psychological well-being in the United States.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 99859990.
Tentori, K., Osherson, D., Hasher, L., & May, C. (2001). Wisdom and aging:
Irrational preferences in college students but not older adults. Cognition,
81, B87B96.
Uchino, B. N. (2009). What a lifespan approach might tell us about why distinct
measures of social support have differential links to physical health. Journal
of Social and Personal Relationships, 26(1), 5362. DOI: 10.1177/0265407509105521
Umberson, D., Williams, K., Powers, D. A., Liu, H., & Needham, B. (2006). You
make me sick: Marital quality and health over the life course. Journal of
Health and Social Behavior, 47, 116.
Waite, L. J. & Gallagher, M. (2000). The case for marriage: Why married people
are happier, healthier, and better off financially. New York: Doubleday.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Aging by Tara Queen and Jacqui Smith is licensed
under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International
License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
deed.en_US.
Topic 5
Cognition and Language
Attention
Frances Friedrich
University of Utah
nobaproject.com
Abstract
We use the term attention all the time, but what processes or abilities does
that concept really refer to? This chapter will focus on how attention allows us
to select certain parts of our environment and ignore other parts, and what
happens to the ignored information. A key concept is the idea that we are limited
in how much we can do at any one time. So we will also consider what happens
when someone tries to do several things at once, such as driving while using
electronic devices.
Learning Objectives
Learn about different models of when and how selection can occur.
What is attention?
Before we begin exploring attention in its various forms, take a moment to
consider how you think about the concept. How would you define attention, or
how do you use the term? We certainly use the word very frequently in our
everyday language: ATTENTION! USE ONLY AS DIRECTED! warns the label on
the medicine bottle, meaning be alert to possible danger. Pay attention! pleads
the weary seventh-grade teacher, not warning about danger (with possible
exceptions, depending on the teacher) but urging the students to focus on the
task at hand. We may refer to a child who is easily distracted as having an
attention disorder, although we also are told that Americans have an attention
span of about 8 seconds, down from 12 seconds in 2000, suggesting that we
all have trouble sustaining concentration for any amount of time (from How
that number was determined is not clear from the Web site, nor is it clear how
attention span in the goldfish9 seconds!was measured, but the fact that
our average span reportedly is less than that of a goldfish is intriguing, to say
the least.
William James wrote extensively about attention in the late 1800s. An often
quoted passage (James, 1890/1983) beautifully captures how intuitively obvious
the concept of attention is, while it remains very difficult to define in measurable,
concrete terms:
Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession by the mind,
in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously possible
objects or trains of thought. Focalization, concentration of consciousness are
of its essence. It implies withdrawal from some things in order to deal effectively
with others. (pp. 381382)
Notice that this description touches on the conscious nature of attention,
as well as the notion that what is in consciousness is often controlled voluntarily
but can also be determined by events that capture our attention. Implied in this
description is the idea that we seem to have a limited capacity for information
nobaproject.com - Attention
748
nobaproject.com - Attention
749
Selective Attention
The Cocktail Party
Selective attention is the ability to select certain stimuli in the environment to
process, while ignoring distracting information. One way to get an intuitive
sense of how attention works is to consider situations in which attention is
used. A party provides an excellent example for our purposes. Many people
may be milling around, there is a dazzling variety of colors and sounds and
smells, the buzz of many conversations is striking. There are so many
conversations going on; how is it possible to select just one and follow it? You
dont have to be looking at the person talking; you may be listening with great
interest to some gossip while pretending not to hear. However, once you are
engaged in conversation with someone, you quickly become aware that you
cannot also listen to other conversations at the same time. You also are probably
not aware of how tight your shoes feel or of the smell of a nearby flower
arrangement. On the other hand, if someone behind you mentions your name,
you typically notice it immediately and may start attending to that (much more
interesting) conversation. This situation highlights an interesting set of
observations. We have an amazing ability to select and track one voice, visual
object, etc., even when a million things are competing for our attention, but at
the same time, we seem to be limited in how much we can attend to at one
time, which in turn suggests that attention is crucial in selecting what is
important. How does it all work?
nobaproject.com - Attention
750
751
752
753
Distracted Driving
More relevant to our current lifestyles are questions about multitasking while
texting or having cell phone conversations. Research designed to investigate,
under controlled conditions, multitasking while driving has revealed some
surprising results. Certainly there are many possible types of distractions that
could impair driving performance, such as applying makeup using the rearview
mirror, attempting (usually in vain) to stop the kids in the backseat from fighting,
fiddling with the CD player, trying to negotiate a handheld cell phone, a cigarette,
and a soda all at once, eating a bowl of cereal while driving (!). But we tend to
have a strong sense that we CAN multitask while driving, and cars are being
built with more and more technological capabilities that encourage
multitasking. How good are we at dividing attention in these cases?
Most people acknowledge the distraction caused by texting while driving
and the reason seems obvious: Your eyes are off the road and your hands and
at least one hand (often both) are engaged while texting. However, the problem
is not simply one of occupied hands or eyes, but rather that the cognitive
demands on our limited capacity systems can seriously impair driving
performance (Strayer, Watson, & Drews, 2011). The effect of a cell phone
conversation on performance (such as not noticing someones brake lights or
responding more slowly to them) is just as significant when the individual is
having a conversation with a hands-free device as with a handheld phone; the
same impairments do not occur when listening to the radio or a book on tape
(Strayer & Johnston, 2001). Moreover, studies using eye-tracking devices have
shown that drivers are less likely to later recognize objects that they did look at
when using a cell phone while driving (Strayer & Drews, 2007). These findings
demonstrate that cognitive distractions such as cell phone conversations can
produce inattentional blindness, or a lack of awareness of what is right before
your eyes (see also, Simons & Chabris, 1999). Sadly, although we all like to think
that we can multitask while driving, in fact the percentage of people who can
nobaproject.com - Attention
754
Summary
It may be useful to think of attention as a mental resource, one that is needed
to focus on and fully process important information, especially when there is
a lot of distracting noise threatening to obscure the message. Our selective
attention system allows us to find or track an object or conversation in the midst
of distractions. Whether the selection process occurs early or late in the analysis
of those events has been the focus of considerable research, and in fact how
selection occurs may very well depend on the specific conditions. With respect
to divided attention, in general we can only perform one cognitively demanding
task at a time, and we may not even be aware of unattended events even though
they might seem too obvious to miss (check out some examples in the Outside
Resources below). This type of inattention blindness can occur even in welllearned tasks, such as driving while talking on a cell phone. Understanding how
attention works is clearly important, even for our everyday lives.
nobaproject.com - Attention
755
Outside Resources
Video: Here's a wild example of how much we fail to notice when our attention
is captured by one element of a scene.
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ubNF9QNEQLA&feature=related
Video: Try this test to see how well you can focus on a task in the face of a
lot of distraction.
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Ahg6qcgoay4&NR=1
Discussion Questions
1. Discuss the implications of the different models of selective attention for
everyday life. For instance, what advantages and disadvantages would be
associated with being able to filter out all unwanted information at a very
early stage in processing? What are the implications of processing all ignored
information fully, even if you aren't consciously aware of that information?
2. Think of examples of when you feel you can successfully multitask and when
you cant. Discuss what aspects of the tasks or the situation seem to
influence divided attention performance. How accurate do you think you
are in judging your own multitasking ability?
3. What are the public policy implications of current evidence of inattentional
blindness as a result of distracted driving? Should this evidence influence
traffic safety laws? What additional studies of distracted driving would you
propose?
Vocabulary
Dichotic listening
An experimental task in which two messages are presented to different ears.
Divided attention
The ability to flexibly allocate attentional resources between two or more
concurrent tasks.
Inattentional blindness
The failure to notice a fully visible object when attention is devoted to something
else.
Limited capacity
The notion that humans have limited mental resources that can be used at a
given time.
Selective attention
The ability to select certain stimuli in the environment to process, while ignoring
distracting information.
Shadowing
A task in which the individual is asked to repeat an auditory message as it is
presented.
Subliminal perception
The ability to process information for meaning when the individual is not
consciously aware of that information.
Reference List
Bargh, J., & Morsella, E. (2008). The unconscious mind. Perspectives on
Psychological Science, 3(1), 7379.
Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: What
governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 130, 701725.
Broadbent, D. A. (1958). Perception and communication. London, England:
Pergamon Press.
Cheesman, J., & Merikle, P. (1986). Distinguishing conscious from unconscious
perceptual processes. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 40, 343367.
Cheesman, J., & Merikle, P. (1984). Priming with and without awareness.
Perception and Psychophysics, 36, 387395.
Cherry, E. C. (1953). Experiments on the recognition of speech with one and two
ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975979.
Deutsch, J. A., & Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: some theoretical considerations.
Psychological Review, 70, 8090.
Greenwald, A. G. (1992). New Look 3: Unconscious cognition reclaimed.
American Psychologist, 47, 766779.
Hirst, W. C., Neisser, U., & Spelke, E. S. (1978). Divided attention. Human Nature,
1, 5461.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Attention by Frances Friedrich is licensed under
the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. To
view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.
en_US.
Consciousness
Ken Paller & Satoru Suzuki
Northwestern University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Consciousness is the ultimate mystery. What is it and why do we have it? These
questions are difficult to answer, even though consciousness is so fundamental
to our existence. Perhaps the natural world could exist largely as it is without
human consciousnessbut taking away consciousness would essentially take
away our humanity. Psychological science has addressed questions about
consciousness in part by distinguishing neurocognitive functions allied with
conscious experience from those that transpire without conscious experience.
The continuing investigation of these sorts of distinctions is yielding an empirical
basis for new hypotheses about the precursors of conscious experience. Richer
conceptualizations are thus being built, combining first-person and thirdperson perspectives to provide new clues to the mystery of consciousness.
Learning Objectives
Conscious Experiences
Contemplate the unique experience of being you at this moment! You, and only
you, have direct knowledge of your own conscious experiences. At the same
time, you cannot know consciousness from anyone elses inside view. How can
we begin to understand this fantastic ability to have private, conscious
experiences?
In a sense, everything you know is from your own vantage point, with your
own consciousness at the center. Yet the scientific study of consciousness
confronts the challenge of producing general understanding that goes beyond
what can be known from one individuals perspective.
To delve into this topic, some terminology must first be considered. The term
consciousness can denote the ability of a person to generate a series of
conscious experiences one after another. Here we include experiences of
feeling and understanding sensory input, of a temporal sequence of
autobiographical events, of imagination, of emotions and moods, of ideas, of
memoriesthe whole range of mental contents open to an individual.
Consciousness can also refer to the state of an individual, as in a sharp or
dull state of consciousness, a drug-induced state such as euphoria, or a
diminished state due to drowsiness, sleep, neurological abnormality, or coma.
In this chapter, we focus not on states of consciousness or on selfconsciousness, but rather on the process that unfolds in the course of a
conscious experiencea moment of awarenessthe essential ingredient of
consciousness.
nobaproject.com - Consciousness
766
Other Minds
You have probably experienced the sense of knowing exactly what a friend is
thinking. Various signs can guide our inferences about consciousness in others.
We can try to infer whats going on in someone elses mind by relying on the
assumption that they feel what we imagine we would feel in the same situation.
We might account for someones actions or emotional expressions through our
knowledge of that individual and our careful observations of their behavior. In
this way, we often display substantial insight into what they are thinking. Other
times we are completely wrong.
By measuring brain activity using various neuroscientific technologies, we
can acquire additional information useful for deciphering another persons
state of mind. In special circumstances such inferences can be highly accurate,
but limitations on mind reading remain, highlighting the difficulty of
understanding exactly how conscious experiences arise.
A Science of Conciousness
Attempts to understand consciousness have been pervasive throughout
human history, mostly dominated by philosophical analyses focused on the
first-person perspective. Now we have a wider set of approaches that includes
philosophy, psychology, neuroscience, cognitive science, and contemplative
science (Blackmore, 2006; Koch, 2012; Zelazo, Moscovitch, & Thompson, 2007;
Zeman, 2002).
The challenge for this combination of approaches is to give a comprehensive
explanation of consciousness. That explanation would include describing the
benefits of consciousness, particularly for behavioral capabilities that conscious
experiences allow, that trump automatic behaviors. Subjective experiences also
need to be described in a way that logically shows how they result from
precursor events in the human brain. Moreover, a full account would describe
nobaproject.com - Consciousness
767
768
For instance, you may think that if you attentively look at a bright spot, you
must be aware of it. Not so. In a phenomenon known as motion-induced
blindness, bright discs completely vanish from your awareness in full attention.
To experience this for yourself, see this chapter's Outside Resource section for
a demonstration of motion-induced blindness.
You may think that if you deeply analyze an image, decoding its meaning
and making a decision about it, you must be aware of the image. Not necessarily.
When a number is briefly flashed and rapidly replaced by a random pattern,
you may have no awareness of it, despite the fact that your brain allows you to
determine that the number is greater than 5, and then prepare your right hand
for a key press if that is what you were instructed to do (Dehaene et al., 1998).
Thus, neither the brightness of an image, paying full attention to it, nor
deeply analyzing it guarantees that you will be aware of it. What, then, is the
crucial ingredient of visual awareness?
A contemporary answer is that our awareness of a visual feature depends
on a certain type of reciprocal exchange of information across multiple brain
areas, particularly in the cerebral cortex. In support of this idea, directly
activating your visual motion area (known as V5) with an externally applied
magnetic field (transcranial magnetic stimulation) will make you see moving
dots. This is not surprising. What is surprising is that activating your visual
motion area alone does not let you see motion. You will not see moving dots if
the feedback signal from V5 to the primary visual cortex is disrupted by a further
transcranial magnetic stimulation pulse (Pascual-Leone & Walsh, 2001). The
reverberating reciprocal exchange of information between higher-level visual
areas and primary visual cortex appears to be essential for generating visual
awareness.
This idea can also explain why people with certain types of brain damage
lack visual awareness. Consider a patient with brain damage limited to primary
visual cortex who claims not to see anything a problem termed cortical
blindness. Other areas of visual cortex may still receive visual input through
nobaproject.com - Consciousness
769
projections from brain structures such as the thalamus and superior colliculus,
and these networks may mediate some preserved visual abilities that take place
without awareness. For example, a patient with cortical blindness might detect
moving stimuli via V5 activation but still have no conscious experiences of the
stimuli, because the reverberating reciprocal exchange of information cannot
take place between V5 and the damaged primary visual cortex. The preserved
ability to detect motion might be evident only when a guess is required (guess
whether something moved to the left or right)otherwise the answer would
be I didnt see anything. This phenomenon of blindsight refers to blindness
due to a neurological cause that preserves abilities to analyze and respond to
visual stimuli that are not consciously experienced (Lamme, 2001).
If exchanges of information across brain areas are crucial for generating
visual awareness, neural synchronization must play an important role because
it promotes neural communication. A neurons excitability varies over time.
Communication among neural populations is enhanced when their oscillatory
cycles of excitability are synchronized. In this way, information transmitted from
one population in its excitable phase is received by the target population when
it is also in its excitable phase. Indeed, oscillatory neural synchronization in the
beta- and gamma-band frequencies (identified according to the number of
oscillations per second, 1330 Hz and 30100 Hz, respectively) appears to be
closely associated with visual awareness. This idea is highlighted in the Global
Neuronal Workspace Theory of Consciousness (Dehaene & Changeux, 2011),
in which sharing of information among prefrontal, inferior parietal, and occipital
regions of the cerebral cortex is postulated to be especially important for
generating awareness.
A related view, the Information Integration Theory of Consciousness, is that
shared information itself constitutes consciousness (Tononi, 2004). An
organism would have minimal consciousness if the structure of shared
information is simple, whereas it would have rich conscious experiences if the
structure of shared information is complex. Roughly speaking, complexity is
nobaproject.com - Consciousness
770
771
772
773
774
775
776
Yet belief in free choice has been shown to promote moral behavior, and it
is the basis of human notions of justice. The sense of free choice may be a
beneficial trait that became prevalent because it helped us flourish as social
beings.
Understanding Consciousness
Our human consciousness unavoidably colors all of our observations and our
attempts to gain understanding. Nonetheless, scientific inquiries have provided
useful perspectives on consciousness. The advances described above should
engender optimism about the various research strategies applied to date and
about the prospects for further insight into consciousness in the future.
Because conscious experiences are inherently private, they have sometimes
been taken to be outside the realm of scientific inquiry. This view idealizes
science as an endeavor involving only observations that can be verified by
multiple observers, relying entirely on the third-person perspective, or the view
from nowhere (from no particular perspective). Yet conducting science is a
human activity that depends, like other human activities, on individuals and
their subjective experiences. A rational scientific account of the world cannot
avoid the fact that people have subjective experiences.
Subjectivity thus has a place in science. Conscious experiences can be
subjected to systematic analysis and empirical tests to yield progressive
understanding. Many further questions remain to be addressed by scientists
of the future. Is the first-person perspective of a conscious experience basically
the same for all human beings, or do individuals differ fundamentally in their
introspective experiences and capabilities? Should psychological science focus
only on ordinary experiences of consciousness, or are extraordinary
experiences also relevant? Can training in introspection lead to a specific sort
of expertise with respect to conscious experience? An individual with training,
such as through extensive meditation practice, might be able to describe their
nobaproject.com - Consciousness
777
nobaproject.com - Consciousness
778
Outside Resources
1. Video: Demonstration of motion-induced blindness - Look steadily at the
blue moving pattern. One or more of the yellow spots may disappear.
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=4Aye9FWgxUg
4. Video: Clip on the rubber hand illusion, from the BBC science series
"Horizon."
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Qsmkgi7FgEo
Discussion Questions
1. Why has consciousness evolved? Presumably it provides some beneficial
capabilities for an organism beyond behaviors that are based only on
automatic triggers or unconscious processing. What are the likely benefits
of consciousness?
2. How would you explain to a congenitally blind person the experience of
seeing red? Detailed explanations of the physics of light and neurobiology
of color processing in the brain would describe the mechanisms that give
rise to the experience of seeing red, but would not convey the experience.
What would be the best way to communicate the subjective experience
itself?
3. Our visual experiences seem to be a direct readout of information from the
world that comes into our eyes, and we usually believe that our mental
representations give us an accurate and exact re-creation of the world. Is it
possible that what we consciously perceive is not veridical, but is a limited
and distorted view, in large part a function of the specific sensory and
information-processing abilities that the brain affords?
4. When are you most consciouswhile youre calm, angry, happy, or moved;
while absorbed in a movie, video game, or athletic activity; while engaged
in a spirited conversation, making decisions, meditating, reflecting, trying
to solve a difficult problem, day dreaming, or feeling creative? How do these
considerations shed light on what consciousness is?
5. Consciousness may be a natural biological phenomenon and a chief
function of a brain, but consider the many ways in which it is also contingent
on (i) a body linked with a brain, (ii) an outside world, (iii) a social
Vocabulary
Awareness
A conscious experience or the capability of having conscious experiences, which
is distinct from self-awareness, the conscious understanding of ones own
existence and individuality.
Conscious experience
The first-person perspective of a mental event, such as feeling some sensory
input, a memory, an idea, an emotion, a mood, or a continuous temporal
sequence of happenings.
Contemplative science
A research area concerned with understanding how contemplative practices
such as meditation can affect individuals, including changes in their behavior,
their emotional reactivity, their cognitive abilities, and their brains.
Contemplative science also seeks insights into conscious experience that can
be gained from first-person observations by individuals who have gained
extraordinary expertise in introspection.
First-person perspective
Observations made by individuals about their own conscious experiences, also
known as introspection or a subjective point of view. Phenomenology refers to
the description and investigation of such observations.
Third-person perspective
Observations made by individuals in a way that can be independently confirmed
by other individuals so as to lead to general, objective understanding. With
respect to consciousness, third-person perspectives make use of behavioral
and neural measures related to conscious experiences.
Reference List
Blackmore, S. (2006). Conversations on consciousness: What the best minds
think about the brain, free will, and what it means to be human. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Casali, A. G., Gosseries, O., Rosanova, M., Boly, M. Sarasso, S., Casali, K. R.,
Casarotto, S., Bruno, M.-A., Laureys, S., Tononi, G., & Massimini, M. (2013).
A theoretically based index of consciousness independent of sensory
processing and behavior. Science Translational Medicine, 5, 198ra105
Dane, E., Rockmann, K. W., & Pratt, M. G. (2012). When should I trust my gut?
Linking domain expertise to intuitive decision-making effectiveness.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 119, 187194.
Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le ClecH, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., DehaeneLambertz, G., van de Moortele, P.-F., & Le Bihan, D. (1998). Imaging
unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395, 597600.
Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J.-P. (2011). Experimental and theoretical approaches
to conscious processing. Neuron, 70, 200227.
Graziano, M. S. A., & Kastner, S. (2011). Human consciousness and its
relationship to social neuroscience: A novel hypothesis. Cognitive
Neuroscience, 2, 98113.
Koch, C. (2012). Consciousness: Confessions of a romantic reductionist.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Lamme, V. A. F. (2001). Blindsight: The role of feedforward and feedback
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Consciousness by Ken Paller and Satoru Suzuki
is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
The Unconscious
Ap Dijksterhuis
Radboud University Nijmegen
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Unconscious psychological processes have fascinated people for a very long
time. The idea that people must have an unconscious is based on the idea that
(a) there is so much going on in our brains, and the capacity of consciousness
is so small, that there must be much more than just consciousness; and that
(b) unless you believe consciousness is causally disconnected from other bodily
and mental processes, conscious experiences must be prepared by other
processes in the brain of which we are not conscious. Not only logic dictates
that action starts unconsciously, but research strongly suggests this too.
Moreover, unconscious processes are very often highly important for human
functioning, and many phenomena, such as attitude formation, goal pursuit,
stereotyping, creativity, and decision making are impossible to fully understand
without incorporating the role of unconscious processes.
Learning Objectives
792
This is not say that contemporaries of Descartes and later thinkers all agreed
with Descartess dualism. In fact, many of them disagreed and kept on theorizing
about unconscious psychological processes. For instance, the British
philosopher John Norris (16571711) said: We may have ideas of which we are
not conscious. . . . There are infinitely more ideas impressed on our minds than
we can possibly attend to or perceive. Immanual Kant (17241804) agreed:
The field of our sense-perceptions and sensations, of which we are not
conscious . . .is immeasurable. Norris and Kant used a logical argument that
many proponents of the importance of unconscious psychological processes
still like to point at today: There is so much going on in our brains, and the
capacity of consciousness is so small, that there must be much more than just
consciousness.
The most famous advocate of the importance of unconscious processes
arrived at the scene in the late 19th century: the Austrian neurologist Sigmund
Freud. Most people associate Freud with psychoanalysis, with his theory on id,
ego, and superego, and with his ideas on repression, hidden desires, and
dreams. Such associations are fully justified, but Freud also published lesserknown general theoretical work (e.g., Freud, 1915/1963). This theoretical work
sounds, in contrast to his psychoanalytic work, very fresh and contemporary.
For instance, Freud already argued that human behavior never starts with a
conscious process (compare this to the Libet experiment discussed below).
Freud, and also Wilhelm Wundt, pointed at another logical argument for the
necessity of unconscious psychological processes. Wundt put it like this: Our
mind is so fortunately equipped, that it brings us the most important bases for
our thoughts without our having the least knowledge of this work of elaboration.
Only the results of it become conscious. This unconscious mind is for us like an
unknown being who creates and produces for us, and finally throws the ripe
fruits in our lap. In other words, we may become consciously aware of many
different thingsthe taste of a glass of Burgundy, the beauty of the Taj Mahal,
or the sharp pain in our toe after a collision with a bedbut these experiences
nobaproject.com - The Unconscious
793
do not hover in the air before they reach us. They are prepared, somehow and
somewhere. Unless you believe consciousness is causally disconnected from
other bodily and mental processes (for instance if one assumes it is guided by
the gods), conscious experiences must be prepared by other processes in the
brain of which we are not conscious.
The German psychologist Watt (1905), in an appealing experiment, showed
that we are only consciously aware of the results of mental processes. His
participants were repeatedly presented with nouns (e.g., oak) and had to
respond with an associated word as quickly as they could. On some occasions
participants were requested to name a superordinate word (oak-tree), while
on other occasions they were asked to come up with a part (oak-acorn) or a
subordinate (oak-beam) word. Hence, participants thinking was divided into
four stages: the instructions (e.g., superordinate), the presentation of the noun
(e.g., oak), the search for an appropriate association, and the verbalization of
the reply (e.g., tree). Participants were asked to carefully introspect on all four
stages to shed light on the role of consciousness during each stage. The third
stage (searching for an association) is the stage during which the actual thinking
takes place and hence this was considered the most interesting stage. However,
unlike the other stages, this stage was, as psychologists call it, introspectively
blank: Participants could not report anything. The thinking itself was
unconscious, and participants were only conscious of the answer that surfaced.
794
795
796
797
for all of us, even for people who are not explicitly prejudiced, or, in other words,
for people who do not want to stereotype.
798
decisions than the latter. Although they did the exact same thing consciously
again, solving anagramsthe first group made better decisions than the
second group because the first thought unconsciously. Recently, researchers
reported neuroscientific evidence for such unconscious thought processes,
indeed showing that recently encoded information is further processed
unconsciously when people have the goal to do so (Creswell, Bursley, & Satpute,
in press).
People are sometimes surprised to learn that we can do so much, and so
many sophisticated things, unconsciously. However, it is important to realize
that there is no one-to-one relation between attention and consciousness (see
e.g., Dijksterhuis & Aarts, 2010). Our behavior is largely guided by goals and
motives, and these goals determine what we pay attention tothat is, how
many resources our brain spends on somethingbut not necessarily what we
become consciously aware of. We can be conscious of things that we hardly
pay attention to (such as fleeting daydreams), and we can be paying a lot of
attention to something we are temporarily unaware of (such as a problem we
want to solve or a big decision we are facing). Part of the confusion arises
because attention and consciousness are correlated. When one pays more
attention to an incoming stimulus, the probability that one becomes consciously
aware of it increases. However, attention and consciousness are distinct. And
to understand why we can do so many things unconsciously, attention is the
key. We need attention, but for quite a number of things, we do not need
conscious awareness.
These days, most researchers agree that the most sensible approach to learn
about unconscious and conscious processes is to consider (higher) cognitive
operations as unconscious, and test what (if anything) consciousness adds
(Dijksterhuis & Aarts 2010; van Gaal, Lamme, Fahrenfort, & Ridderinkhof, 2011;
for an exception, see Newell & Shanks, in press). However, researchers still
widely disagree about the relative importance or contribution of conscious and
unconscious processes. Some theorists maintain the causal role of
nobaproject.com - The Unconscious
799
800
Outside Resources
Book: A wonderful book about how little we know about ourselves: Wilson,
T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Book: Another wonderful book about free willor its absence?: Wegner, D.
M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Discussion Questions
1. Assess both the strengths and weaknesses of the famous Libet study.
2. Assuming that attention and consciousness are orthogonal, can you name
examples of conscious processes that hardly require attention or of
unconscious processes that require a lot of attention?
3. Do you think some of the priming experiments can also be explained purely
by conscious processes?
4. What do you think could be the main function of consciousness?
5. Some people, scientists included, have a strong aversion to the idea that
human behavior is largely guided by unconscious processes. Do you know
why?
Vocabulary
Cartesian catastrophe
The idea that mental processes taking place outside conscious awareness are
impossible.
Conscious
Having knowledge of something external or internal to oneself; being aware of
and responding to ones surroundings.
Distractor task
A task that is designed to make a person think about something unrelated to
an impending decision.
EEG
(Electroencephalography) The recording of the brains electrical activity over a
period of time by placing electrodes on the scalp.
Eureka experience
When a creative product enters consciousness.
Mere-exposure effects
The result of developing a more positive attitude towards a stimulus after
repeated instances of mere exposure to it.
Priming
The process by which recent experiences increase a traits accessibility.
Unconscious
Not conscious; the part of the mind that affects behavior though it is inaccessible
to the conscious mind.
Reference List
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior:
Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230244.
Bos, M. W., Dijksterhuis, A. & van Baaren, R. B. (2008). On the goal-dependency
of unconscious thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44, 111420.
Creswell, D., Bursley, J. & Satpute, A. (in press). Neural reactivation links
unconscious thought to decision making performance. Social Cognitive and
Affective Neuroscience.
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled
components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 518.
Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2010). Goals, attention, and (un)consciousness.
Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 467490.
Dijksterhuis, A., & Nordgren, L. F. (2006). A theory of unconscious thought.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 95109.
Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (1998). The relation between perception
and behavior or how to win a game of Trivial Pursuit. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 74, 865877.
Freud, S. (1963). General Psychological Theory. New York: Simon & Schuster.
(Original work published 1915.)
Holland, R. W., Hendriks, M., & Aarts, H. (2005). Smells like clean spirit:
Abstract
People form mental concepts of categories of objects, which permit them to
respond appropriately to new objects they encounter. Most concepts cannot
be strictly defined but are organized around the best examples or prototypes,
which have the properties most common in the category. Objects fall into many
different categories, but there is usually a most salient one, called the basiclevel category, which is at an intermediate level of specificity (e.g., chairs, rather
than furniture or desk chairs). Concepts are closely related to our knowledge
of the world, and people can more easily learn concepts that are consistent
with their knowledge. Theories of concepts argue either that people learn a
summary description of a whole category or else that they learn exemplars of
the category. Recent research suggests that there are different ways to learn
and represent concepts and that they are accomplished by different neural
systems.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
Consider the following set of objects: some dust, papers, a computer monitor,
two pens, a cup, and an orange. What do these things have in common? Only
that they all happen to be on my desk as I write this. This set of things can be
considered a category, a set of objects that can be treated as equivalent in some
way. But, most of our categories seem much more informativethey share
many properties. For example, consider the following categories: trucks,
wireless devices, weddings, psychopaths, and trout. Although the objects in a
given category are different from one another, they have many commonalities.
When you know something is a truck, you know quite a bit about it. The
psychology of categories concerns how people learn, remember, and use
informative categories such as trucks or psychopaths.
The mental representations we form of categories are called concepts. There
is a category of trucks in the world, and I also have a concept of trucks in my
head. We assume that peoples concepts correspond more or less closely to
the actual category, but it can be useful to distinguish the two, as when
someones concept is not really correct.
Concepts are at the core of intelligent behavior. We expect people to be able
to know what to do in new situations and when confronting new objects. If you
go into a new classroom and see chairs, a blackboard, a projector, and a screen,
you know what these things are and how they will be used. Youll sit on one of
the chairs and expect the instructor to write on the blackboard or project
something onto the screen. You do this even if you have never seen any of these
particular objects before, because you have concepts of classrooms, chairs,
projectors, and so forth, that tell you what they are and what youre supposed
to do with them. Furthermore, if someone tells you a new fact about the
projectorfor example, that it has a halogen bulbyou are likely to extend this
fact to other projectors you encounter. In short, concepts allow you to extend
what you have learned about a limited number of objects to a potentially infinite
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
812
set of entities.
You know thousands of categories, most of which you have learned without
careful study or instruction. Although this accomplishment may seem simple,
we know that it isnt, because it is difficult to program computers to solve such
intellectual tasks. If you teach a learning program that a robin, a swallow, and
a duck are all birds, it may not recognize a cardinal or peacock as a bird. As well
shortly see, the problem is that objects in categories are often surprisingly
diverse.
Simpler organisms, such as animals and human infants, also have concepts
(Mareschal, Quinn, & Lea, 2010). Squirrels may have a concept of predators, for
example, that is specific to their own lives and experiences. However, animals
likely have many fewer concepts and cannot understand complex concepts
such as mortgages or musical instruments.
Nature of Categories
Traditionally, it has been assumed that categories are well defined. This means
that you can give a definition that specifies what is in and out of the category.
Such a definition has two parts. First, it provides the necessary features for
category membership: What must objects have in order to be in it? Second,
those features must be jointly sufficient for membership: If an object has those
features, then it is in the category. For example, if I defined a dog as a fourlegged animal that barks, this would mean that every dog is four-legged, an
animal, and barks, and also that anything that has all those properties is a dog.
Unfortunately, it has not been possible to find definitions for many familiar
categories. Definitions are neat and clear-cut; the world is messy and often
unclear. For example, consider our definition of dogs. In reality, not all dogs
have four legs; not all dogs bark. I knew a dog that lost her bark with age (this
was an improvement); no one doubted that she was still a dog. It is often possible
to find some necessary features (e.g., all dogs have blood and breathe), but
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
813
Fuzzy Categories
Borderline items. Experiments also showed that the psychological assumptions
of well-defined categories were not correct. Hampton (1979) asked subjects to
judge whether a number of items were in different categories. He did not find
that items were either clear members or clear nonmembers. Instead, he found
many items that were just barely considered category members and others
that were just barely not members, with much disagreement among subjects.
Sinks were barely considered as members of the kitchen utensil category, and
sponges were barely excluded. People just included seaweed as a vegetable
and just barely excluded tomatoes and gourds. Hampton found that members
and nonmembers formed a continuum, with no obvious break in peoples
membership judgments. If categories were well defined, such examples should
be very rare. Many studies since then have found such borderline members
that are not clearly in or clearly out of the category.
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
814
815
Table 1. Examples of Two Categories, with Members Ordered by Typicality (from Rosch &
Mervis, 1975)
You can find out which category members are typical merely by asking
people. Table 1 shows a list of category members in order of their rated typicality.
Typicality is perhaps the most important variable in predicting how people
interact with categories. The following text box is a partial list of what typicality
influences.
We can understand the two phenomena of borderline members and
typicality as two sides of the same coin. Think of the most typical category
member: This is often called the category prototype. Items that are less and
less similar to the prototype become less and less typical. At some point, these
less typical items become so atypical that you start to doubt whether they are
in the category at all. Is a rug really an example of furniture? Its in the home
like chairs and tables, but its also different from most furniture in its structure
and use. From day to day, you might change your mind as to whether this
atypical example is in or out of the category. So, changes in typicality ultimately
lead to borderline members.
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
816
Source of Typicality
Intuitively, it is not surprising that robins are better examples of birds than
penguins are, or that a table is a more typical kind of furniture than is a rug.
But given that robins and penguins are known to be birds, why should one be
more typical than the other? One possible answer is the frequency with which
we encounter the object: We see a lot more robins than penguins, so they must
be more typical. Frequency does have some effect, but it is actually not the most
important variable (Rosch, Simpson, & Miller, 1976). For example, I see both
rugs and tables every single day, but one of them is much more typical as
furniture than the other.
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
817
The best account of what makes something typical comes from Rosch and
Merviss (1975) family resemblance theory. They proposed that items are likely
to be typical if they (a) have the features that are frequent in the category and
(b) do not have features frequent in other categories. Lets compare two
extremes, robins and penguins. Robins are small flying birds that sing, live in
nests in trees, migrate in winter, hop around on your lawn, and so on. Most of
these properties are found in many other birds. In contrast, penguins do not
fly, do not sing, do not live in nests or in trees, do not hop around on your lawn.
Furthermore, they have properties that are common in other categories, such
as swimming expertly and having wings that look and act like fins. These
properties are more often found in fish than in birds.
According to Rosch and Mervis, then, it is not because a robin is a very
common bird that makes it typical. Rather, it is because the robin has the shape,
size, body parts, and behaviors that are very common among birdsand not
common among fish, mammals, bugs, and so forth.
In a classic experiment, Rosch and Mervis (1975) made up two new
categories, with arbitrary features. Subjects viewed example after example and
had to learn which example was in which category. Rosch and Mervis
constructed some items that had features that were common in the category
and other items that had features less common in the category. The subjects
learned the first type of item before they learned the second type. Furthermore,
they then rated the items with common features as more typical. In another
experiment, Rosch and Mervis constructed items that differed in how many
features were shared with a different category. The more features were shared,
the longer it took subjects to learn which category the item was in. These
experiments, and many later studies, support both parts of the family
resemblance theory.
818
Category Hierarchies
Many important categories fall into hierarchies, in which more concrete
categories are nested inside larger, abstract categories. For example, consider
the categories: brown bear, bear, mammal, vertebrate, animal, entity. Clearly,
all brown bears are bears; all bears are mammals; all mammals are vertebrates;
and so on. Any given object typically does not fall into just one categoryit
could be in a dozen different categories, some of which are structured in this
hierarchical manner. Examples of biological categories come to mind most
easily, but within the realm of human artifacts, hierarchical structures can
readily be found: desk chair, chair, furniture, artifact, object.
Brown (1958), a child language researcher, was perhaps the first to note that
there seems to be a preference for which category we use to label things. If
your office desk chair is in the way, youll probably say, Move that chair, rather
than Move that desk chair or piece of furniture. Brown thought that the use
of a single, consistent name probably helped children to learn the name for
things. And, indeed, childrens first labels for categories tend to be exactly those
names that adults prefer to use (Anglin, 1977).
819
820
821
& Brownell, 1985). Experts can differ from novices in which categories are the
most differentiated, because they know different things about the categories,
therefore changing how similar the categories are.
822
823
Why would someone propose such a theory of concepts? One answer is that
in many experiments studying concepts, people learn concepts by seeing
exemplars over and over again until they learn to classify them correctly. Under
such conditions, it seems likely that people eventually memorize the exemplars
(Smith & Minda, 1998). There is also evidence that close similarity to wellremembered objects has a large effect on classification. Allen and Brooks (1991)
taught people to classify items by following a rule. However, they also had their
subjects study the items, which were richly detailed. In a later test, the
experimenters gave people new items that were very similar to one of the old
items but were in a different category. That is, they changed one property so
that the item no longer followed the rule. They discovered that people were
often fooled by such items. Rather than following the category rule they had
been taught, they seemed to recognize the new item as being very similar to
an old one and so put it, incorrectly, into the same category.
Many experiments have been done to compare the prototype and exemplar
theories. Overall, the exemplar theory seems to have won most of these
comparisons. However, the experiments are somewhat limited in that they
usually involve a small number of exemplars that people view over and over
again. It is not so clear that exemplar theory can explain real-world classification
in which people do not spend much time learning individual items (how much
time do you spend studying squirrels? or chairs?). Also, given that some part of
our knowledge of categories is learned through general statements we read or
hear, it seems that there must be room for a summary description separate
from exemplar memory.
Many researchers would now acknowledge that concepts are represented
through multiple cognitive systems. For example, your knowledge of dogs may
be in part through general descriptions such as dogs have four legs. But you
probably also have strong memories of some exemplars (your family dog,
Lassie) that influence your categorization. Furthermore, some categories also
involve rules (e.g., a strike in baseball). How these systems work together is the
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
824
Knowledge
The final topic has to do with how concepts fit with our broader knowledge of
the world. We have been talking very generally about people learning the
features of concepts. For example, they see a number of birds and then learn
that birds generally have wings, or perhaps they remember bird exemplars.
From this perspective, it makes no difference what those exemplars or features
arepeople just learn them. But consider two possible concepts of buildings
and their features in Table 2.
Imagine you had to learn these two concepts by seeing exemplars of them,
each exemplar having some of the features listed for the concept (as well as
some idiosyncratic features). Learning the donker concept would be pretty easy.
It seems to be a kind of underwater building, perhaps for deep-sea explorers.
Its features seem to go together. In contrast, the blegdav doesnt really make
sense. If its in the desert, how can you get there by submarine, and why do
they have polar bears as pets? Why would farmers live in the desert or use
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
825
826
will state that there is something about dogs, perhaps some specific gene or
set of genes, that all dogs have and that makes them bark, have fur, and look
the way they do. Therefore, decisions about whether something is a dog do not
depend only on features that you can easily see but also on the assumed
presence of this cause.
Belief in an essence can be revealed through experiments describing
fictional objects. Keil (1989) described to adults and children a fiendish
operation in which someone took a raccoon, dyed its hair black with a white
stripe down the middle, and implanted a sac of super-smelly yucky stuff under
its tail. The subjects were shown a picture of a skunk and told that this is now
what the animal looks like. What is it? Adults and children over the age of 4 all
agreed that the animal is still a raccoon. It may look and even act like a skunk,
but a raccoon cannot change its stripes (or whatever!)it will always be a
raccoon.
Importantly, the same effect was not found when Keil described a coffeepot
that was operated on to look like and function as a birdfeeder. Subjects agreed
that it was now a birdfeeder. Artifacts dont have an essence.
Signs of essentialism include (a) objects are believed to be either in or out
of the category, with no in-between; (b) resistance to change of category
membership or of properties connected to the essence; and (c) for living things,
the essence is passed on to progeny.
Essentialism is probably helpful in dealing with much of the natural world,
but it may be less helpful when it is applied to humans. Considerable evidence
suggests that people think of gender, racial, and ethnic groups as having
essences, which serves to emphasize the difference between groups and even
justify discrimination (Hirschfeld, 1996). Historically, group differences were
described by inheriting the blood of ones family or group. Bad blood was not
just an expression but a belief that negative properties were inherited and could
not be changed. After all, if it is in the nature of those people to be dishonest
(or clannish or athletic ...), then that could hardly be changed, any more than a
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
827
Conclusion
Concepts are central to our everyday thought. When we are planning for the
future or thinking about our past, we think about specific events and objects
in terms of their categories. If youre visiting a friend with a new baby, you have
some expectations about what the baby will do, what gifts would be appropriate,
how you should behave toward it, and so on. Knowing about the category of
babies helps you to effectively plan and behave when you encounter this child
youve never seen before.
Learning about those categories is a complex process that involves seeing
exemplars (babies), hearing or reading general descriptions (Babies like blackand-white pictures), general knowledge (babies have kidneys), and learning
the occasional rule (all babies have a rooting reflex). Current research is focusing
on how these different processes take place in the brain. It seems likely that
these different aspects of concepts are accomplished by different neural
structures (Maddox & Ashby, 2004).
Another interesting topic is how concepts differ across cultures. As different
cultures have different interests and different kinds of interactions with the
world, it seems clear that their concepts will somehow reflect those differences.
On the other hand, the structure of categories in the world also imposes a
strong constraint on what kinds of categories are actually useful. Some
researchers have suggested that differences between Eastern and Western
modes of thought have led to qualitatively different kinds of concepts (e.g.,
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
828
Norenzayan, Smith, Kim, & Nisbett, 2002). Although such differences are
intriguing, we should also remember that different cultures seem to share
common categories such as chairs, dogs, parties, and jars, so the differences
may not be as great as suggested by experiments designed to detect cultural
effects. The interplay of culture, the environment, and basic cognitive processes
in establishing concepts has yet to be fully investigated.
829
Outside Resources
Debate: The debate about Pluto and the definition of planet is an interesting
one, as it illustrates the difficulty of arriving at definitions even in science.
The Planetary Science Institutes website has a series of press releases about
the Pluto debate, including reactions from astronomers, while it happened.
http://www.psi.edu
Discussion Questions
1. Pick a couple of familiar categories and try to come up with definitions for
them. When you evaluate each proposal (a) is it in fact accurate as a
definition, and( b) is it a definition that people might actually use in
identifying category members?
2. For the same categories, can you identify members that seem to be better
and worse members? What about these items makes them typical and
atypical?
3. Going around the room, point to some common objects (including things
people are wearing or brought with them) and identify what the basic-level
category is for that item. What are superordinate and subordinate
categories for the same items?
4. List some features of a common category such as tables. The knowledge
view suggests that you know reasons for why these particular features occur
together. Can you articulate some of those reasons? Do the same thing for
an animal category.
5. Choose three common categories: a natural kind, a human artifact, and a
social event. Discuss with class members from other countries or cultures
whether the corresponding categories in their cultures differ. Can you make
a hypothesis about when such categories are likely to differ and when they
are not?
Vocabulary
Basic-level category
The neutral, preferred category for a given object, at an intermediate level of
specificity.
Category
A set of entities that are equivalent in some way. Usually the items are similar
to one another.
Concept
The mental representation of a category.
Exemplar
An example in memory that is labeled as being in a particular category.
Psychological essentialism
The belief that members of a category have an unseen property that causes
them to be in the category and to have the properties associated with it.
Typicality
The difference in goodness of category members, ranging from the most
typical (the prototype) to borderline members.
Reference List
Allen, S. W., & Brooks, L. R. (1991). Specializing the operation of an explicit rule.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 319.
Anglin, J. M. (1977). Word, object, and conceptual development. New York, NY:
W. W. Norton.
Berlin, B. (1992). Ethnobiological classification: Principles of categorization of
plants and animals in traditional societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Brown, R. (1958). How shall a thing be called? Psychological Review, 65, 1421.
Gelman, S. A. (2003). The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday
thought. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Hampton, J. A. (1979). Polymorphous concepts in semantic memory. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 441461.
Hirschfeld, L. A. (1996). Race in the making: Cognition, culture, and the child's
construction of human kinds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Horton, M. S., & Markman, E. M. (1980). Developmental differences in the
acquisition of basic and superordinate categories. Child Development, 51,
708719.
Keil, F. C. (1989). Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Wisniewski, E. J., & Murphy, G. L. (1989). Superordinate and basic category names
in discourse: A textual analysis. Discourse Processes, 12, 245261.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Categories and Concepts by Gregory Murphy is
licensed
under
the
Creative
Commons
Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0
Abstract
Humans are not perfect decision makers. Not only are we not perfect, but we
depart from perfection or rationality in systematic and predictable ways. The
understanding of these systematic and predictable departures is core to the
field of judgment and decision making. By understanding these limitations, we
can also identify strategies for making better and more effective decisions.
Learning Objectives
Understand the systematic biases that affect our judgment and decision
making.
Introduction
In his Nobel Prizewinning work, psychologist Herbert Simon (1957; March &
Simon, 1958) argued that our decisions are bounded in their rationality.
According to the bounded rationality framework, human beings try to make
rational decisions, but our cognitive limitations prevent us from being fully
rational. Time and cost constraints limit the quantity and quality of the
information that is available to us. Moreover, we only retain a relatively small
amount of information in our usable memory. And limitations on intelligence
and perceptions constrain the ability of even very bright decision makers to
accurately make the best choice based on the information that is available.
About 15 years after the publication of Simons seminal work, Tversky and
Kahneman (1973, 1974; Kahneman & Tversky, 1979) produced their own Nobel
Prizewinning research, which provided critical information about specific
systematic and predictable biases, or mistakes, that influence judgment
(Kahneman received the prize after Tverskys death). The work of Simon,
Tversky, and Kahneman paved the way to our modern understanding of
judgment and decision making. And their two Nobel prizes signaled the broad
acceptance of the field of behavioral decision research as a mature area of
intellectual study.
842
(location, prestige, faculty, etc.), (3) weight the criteria (rank them in terms of
importance to you), (4) generate alternatives (the schools that admitted you),
(5) rate each alternative on each criterion (rate each school on each criteria that
you identified, and (6) compute the optimal decision. Acting rationally would
require that you follow these six steps in a fully rational manner.
I strongly advise people to think through important decisions such as this
in a manner similar to this process. Unfortunately, we often dont. Many of us
rely on our intuitions far more than we should. And when we do try to think
systematically, the way we enter data into such formal decision-making
processes is often biased.
Fortunately, psychologists have learned a great deal about the biases that
affect our thinking. This knowledge about the systematic and predictable
mistakes that even the best and the brightest make can help you identify flaws
in your thought processes and reach better decisions.
843
844
What is your estimate of the number of Big Four clients per 1,000 that have
significant executive-level management fraud? (Fill in the blank below with the
appropriate number.)
________
management fraud.
Problem 3 (adapted from Tversky & Kahneman, 1981):
Imagine that the United States is preparing for the outbreak of an unusual
Asian disease that is expected to kill 600 people. Two alternative programs to
combat the disease have been proposed. Assume that the exact scientific
estimates of the consequences of the programs are as follows.
1. Program A: If Program A is adopted, 200 people will be saved.
2. Program B: If Program B is adopted, there is a one-third probability that 600
people will be saved and a two-thirds probability that no people will be
saved.
nobaproject.com - Judgement and Decision Making
845
Overconfidence
On the first problem, if you set your ranges so that you were justifiably 98
percent confident, you should expect that approximately 9.8, or nine to 10, of
your ranges would include the actual value. So, lets look at the correct answers:
1. 1,636
2. $19.4 billion
3. 1,438.5
4. 71
5. 8,425
6. 22
7. $6.007 billion
8. 1,423
9. 6,510
10. 52
846
Count the number of your 98% ranges that actually surrounded the true
quantities. If you surrounded nine to 10, you were appropriately confident in
your judgments. But most readers surround only between three (30%) and
seven (70%) of the correct answers, despite claiming 98% confidence that each
range would surround the true value. As this problem shows, humans tend to
be overconfident in their judgments.
Anchoring
Regarding the second problem, people vary a great deal in their final
assessment of the level of executive-level management fraud, but most think
that 10 out of 1,000 is too low. When I run this exercise in class, half of the
students respond to the question that I asked you to answer. The other half
receive a similar problem, but instead are asked whether the correct answer is
higher or lower than 200 rather than 10. Most people think that 200 is high.
But, again, most people claim that this anchor does not affect their final
estimate. Yet, on average, people who are presented with the question that
focuses on the number 10 (out of 1,000) give answers that are about one-half
the size of the estimates of those facing questions that use an anchor of 200.
When we are making decisions, any initial anchor that we face is likely to
influence our judgments, even if the anchor is arbitrary. That is, we insufficiently
adjust our judgments away from the anchor.
Framing
Turning to Problem 3, most people choose Program A, which saves 200 lives
for sure, over Program B. But, again, if I was in front of a classroom, only half
of my students would receive this problem. The other half would have received
the same set-up, but with the following two options:
847
848
Moore, 2013).
Contemporary Developments
Bounded rationality served as the integrating concept of the field of behavioral
decision research for 40 years. Then, in 2000, Thaler (2000) suggested that
decision making is bounded in two ways not precisely captured by the concept
of bounded rationality. First, he argued that our willpower is bounded and that,
as a consequence, we give greater weight to present concerns than to future
concerns. Our immediate motivations are often inconsistent with our long-term
interests in a variety of ways, such as the common failure to save adequately
for retirement or the difficulty many people have staying on a diet. Second,
Thaler suggested that our self-interest is bounded such that we care about the
outcomes of others. Sometimes we positively value the outcomes of others
giving them more of a commodity than is necessary out of a desire to be fair,
for example. And, in unfortunate contexts, we sometimes are willing to forgo
our own benefits out of a desire to harm others.
My colleagues and I have recently added two other important bounds to
the list. Chugh, Banaji, and Bazerman (2005) and Banaji and Bhaskar (2000)
introduced the concept of bounded ethicality, which refers to the notion that
our ethics are limited in ways we are not even aware of ourselves. Second,
Chugh and Bazerman (2007) developed the concept of bounded awareness to
refer to the broad array of focusing failures that affect our judgment, specifically
the many ways in which we fail to notice obvious and important information
that is available to us.
A final development is the application of judgment and decision-making
research to the areas of behavioral economics, behavioral finance, and
behavioral marketing, among others. In each case, these fields have been
transformed by applying and extending research from the judgment and
decision-making literature.
nobaproject.com - Judgement and Decision Making
849
850
baseball executives was limited and systematically biased and that their
intuitions had been incorporated into important decisions in ways that created
enormous mistakes. Lewis (2003) documents that baseball professionals tend
to overgeneralize from their personal experiences, be overly influenced by
players very recent performances, and overweigh what they see with their own
eyes, despite the fact that players multiyear records provide far better data. By
substituting valid predictors of future performance (System 2 thinking), the
Athletics were able to outperform expectations given their very limited payroll.
Another important direction for improving decisions comes from Thaler and
Sunsteins (2008) book Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth, and
Happiness. Rather than setting out to debias human judgment, Thaler and
Sunstein outline a strategy for how decision architects can change
environments in ways that account for human bias and trigger better decisions
as a result. For example, Beshears, Choi, Laibson, and Madrian (2008) have
shown that simple changes to defaults can dramatically improve peoples
decisions. They tackle the failure of many people to save for retirement and
show that a simple change can significantly influence enrollment in 401(k)
programs. In most companies, when you start your job, you need to proactively
sign up to join the companys retirement savings plan. Many people take years
before getting around to doing so. When, instead, companies automatically
enroll their employees in 401(k) programs and give them the opportunity to
opt out, the net enrollment rate rises significantly. By changing defaults, we
can counteract the human tendency to live with the status quo.
Similarly, Johnson and Goldsteins (2003) cross-European organ donation
study reveals that countries that have opt-in organ donation policies, where
the default is not to harvest peoples organs without their prior consent, sacrifice
thousands of lives in comparison to opt-out policies, where the default is to
harvest organs. The United States and too many other countries require that
citizens opt in to organ donation through a proactive effort; as a consequence,
consent rates range between 4.25%44% across these countries. In contrast,
nobaproject.com - Judgement and Decision Making
851
852
Outside Resources
Book: Bazerman, M. H., & Moore, D. (2013). Judgment in managerial decision
making (8th ed.). John Wiley & Sons Inc.
Book: Kahneman, D. (2011) Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York, NY: Farrar,
Straus and Giroux.
Book: Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2008). Nudge: Improving Decisions about
Health,
Discussion Questions
1. Are the biases in this chapter a problem in the real world?
2. How would you use this chapter to be a better decision maker?
3. Can you see any biases in todays newspaper?
Vocabulary
Anchoring
The bias to be affected by an initial anchor, even if the anchor is arbitrary, and
to insufficiently adjust our judgments away from that anchor.
Biases
The systematic and predictable mistakes that influence the judgment of even
very talented human beings.
Bounded awareness
The systematic ways in which we fail to notice obvious and important
information that is available to us.
Bounded ethicality
The systematic ways in which our ethics are limited in ways we are not even
aware of ourselves.
Bounded rationality
Model of human behavior that suggests that humans try to make rational
decisions but are bounded due to cognitive limitations.
Bounded self-interest
The systematic and predictable ways in which we care about the outcomes of
others.
Bounded willpower
The tendency to place greater weight on present concerns rather than future
concerns.
Framing
The bias to be systematically affected by the way in which information is
presented, while holding the objective information constant.
Overconfident
The bias to have greater confidence in your judgment than is warranted based
on a rational assessment.
System 1
Our intuitive decision-making system, which is typically fast, automatic,
effortless, implicit, and emotional.
System 2
Our more deliberative decision-making system, which is slower, conscious,
effortful, explicit, and logical.
Reference List
Alpert, M., & Raiffa, H. (1969). A progress report on the training of probability
assessors. Unpublished Report.
Banaji, M. R., & Bhaskar, R. (2000). Implicit stereotypes and memory: The
bounded rationality of social beliefs. In D. L. Schacter & E. Scarry (Eds.),
Memory, brain, and belief (pp. 139175). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Bazerman, M. H., & Moore, D. (2013). Judgment in managerial decision making
(8th ed.). John Wiley & Sons Inc.
Beshears, J., Choi, J. J., Laibson, D., & Madrian, B. C. (2008). The importance of
default options for retirement saving outcomes: Evidence from the United
States. In S. J. Kay & T. Sinha (Eds.), Lessons from pension reform in the
Americas (pp. 5987). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Chugh, D. (2004). Societal and managerial implications of implicit social
cognition: Why milliseconds matter. Social Justice Research, 17(2), 203222.
Chugh, D., Banaji, M. R., & Bazerman, M. H. (2005). Bounded ethicality as a
psychological barrier to recognizing conflicts of interest. In D. Moore, D. M.
Cain, G. Loewenstein, & M. H. Bazerman (Eds.), Conflicts of Interest (pp. 74
95). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Chugh, D., & Bazerman, M. H. (2007). Bounded awareness: What you fail to see
can hurt you. Mind & Society, 6(1), 118.
Fischhoff, B. (1982). Debiasing. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.),
Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 422444). New York,
NY: Cambridge University Press.
Johnson, E. J., & Goldstein, D. (2003). Do defaults save lives? Science 302(5649),
13381339.
Joyce, E. J., & Biddle, G. C. (1981). Are auditors judgments sufficiently regressive?
Journal of Accounting Research, 19(2), 323349.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision
under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263292.
Lewis, M. (2003). Moneyball: The art of winning an unfair game. New York, NY:
W.W. Norton & Company Ltd.
March, J. G., & Simon, H. A. (1958). Organizations. Oxford: Wiley.
Simon, H. A. (1957). Models of man, social and rational: Mathematical essays
on rational human behavior in a social setting. New York, NY: John Wiley &
Sons.
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning:
Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23,
645726.
Thaler, R. H. (2000). From homo economicus to homo sapiens. Journal of
Economics Perspectives, 14, 133141.
Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2008). Nudge: Improving decisions about health,
wealth, and happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology
of choice. Science, New Series, 211(4481), 453458.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and
biases. Science, New Series, 185(4157), 11241131.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency
and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5(2), 207232.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Judgement and Decision Making by Max H.
Bazerman is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
People form mental concepts of categories of objects, which permit them to
respond appropriately to new objects they encounter. Most concepts cannot
be strictly defined but are organized around the best examples or prototypes,
which have the properties most common in the category. Objects fall into many
different categories, but there is usually a most salient one, called the basiclevel category, which is at an intermediate level of specificity (e.g., chairs, rather
than furniture or desk chairs). Concepts are closely related to our knowledge
of the world, and people can more easily learn concepts that are consistent
with their knowledge. Theories of concepts argue either that people learn a
summary description of a whole category or else that they learn exemplars of
the category. Recent research suggests that there are different ways to learn
and represent concepts and that they are accomplished by different neural
systems.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
Consider the following set of objects: some dust, papers, a computer monitor,
two pens, a cup, and an orange. What do these things have in common? Only
that they all happen to be on my desk as I write this. This set of things can be
considered a category, a set of objects that can be treated as equivalent in some
way. But, most of our categories seem much more informativethey share
many properties. For example, consider the following categories: trucks,
wireless devices, weddings, psychopaths, and trout. Although the objects in a
given category are different from one another, they have many commonalities.
When you know something is a truck, you know quite a bit about it. The
psychology of categories concerns how people learn, remember, and use
informative categories such as trucks or psychopaths.
The mental representations we form of categories are called concepts. There
is a category of trucks in the world, and I also have a concept of trucks in my
head. We assume that peoples concepts correspond more or less closely to
the actual category, but it can be useful to distinguish the two, as when
someones concept is not really correct.
Concepts are at the core of intelligent behavior. We expect people to be able
to know what to do in new situations and when confronting new objects. If you
go into a new classroom and see chairs, a blackboard, a projector, and a screen,
you know what these things are and how they will be used. Youll sit on one of
the chairs and expect the instructor to write on the blackboard or project
something onto the screen. You do this even if you have never seen any of these
particular objects before, because you have concepts of classrooms, chairs,
projectors, and so forth, that tell you what they are and what youre supposed
to do with them. Furthermore, if someone tells you a new fact about the
projectorfor example, that it has a halogen bulbyou are likely to extend this
fact to other projectors you encounter. In short, concepts allow you to extend
what you have learned about a limited number of objects to a potentially infinite
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
864
set of entities.
You know thousands of categories, most of which you have learned without
careful study or instruction. Although this accomplishment may seem simple,
we know that it isnt, because it is difficult to program computers to solve such
intellectual tasks. If you teach a learning program that a robin, a swallow, and
a duck are all birds, it may not recognize a cardinal or peacock as a bird. As well
shortly see, the problem is that objects in categories are often surprisingly
diverse.
Simpler organisms, such as animals and human infants, also have concepts
(Mareschal, Quinn, & Lea, 2010). Squirrels may have a concept of predators, for
example, that is specific to their own lives and experiences. However, animals
likely have many fewer concepts and cannot understand complex concepts
such as mortgages or musical instruments.
Nature of Categories
Traditionally, it has been assumed that categories are well-defined. This means
that you can give a definition that specifies what is in and out of the category.
Such a definition has two parts. First, it provides the necessary features for
category membership: What must objects have in order to be in it? Second,
those features must be jointly sufficient for membership: If an object has those
features, then it is in the category. For example, if I defined a dog as a fourlegged animal that barks, this would mean that every dog is four-legged, an
animal, and barks, and also that anything that has all those properties is a dog.
Unfortunately, it has not been possible to find definitions for many familiar
categories. Definitions are neat and clear-cut; the world is messy and often
unclear. For example, consider our definition of dogs. In reality, not all dogs
have four legs; not all dogs bark. I knew a dog that lost her bark with age (this
was an improvement); no one doubted that she was still a dog. It is often possible
to find some necessary features (e.g., all dogs have blood and breathe), but
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
865
Fuzzy Categories
Borderline items. Experiments also showed that the psychological assumptions
of well-defined categories were not correct. Hampton (1979) asked subjects to
judge whether a number of items were in different categories. He did not find
that items were either clear members or clear nonmembers. Instead, he found
many items that were just barely considered category members and others
that were just barely not members, with much disagreement among subjects.
Sinks were barely considered as members of the kitchen utensil category, and
sponges were barely excluded. People just included seaweed as a vegetable
and just barely excluded tomatoes and gourds. Hampton found that members
and nonmembers formed a continuum, with no obvious break in peoples
membership judgments. If categories were well defined, such examples should
be very rare. Many studies since then have found such borderline members
that are not clearly in or clearly out of the category.
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
866
867
Table 1. Examples of Two Categories, with Members Ordered by Typicality (from Rosch &
Mervis, 1975)
You can find out which category members are typical merely by asking
people. Table 1 shows a list of category members in order of their rated typicality.
Typicality is perhaps the most important variable in predicting how people
interact with categories. The following text box is a partial list of what typicality
influences.
We can understand the two phenomena of borderline members and
typicality as two sides of the same coin. Think of the most typical category
member: This is often called the category prototype. Items that are less and
less similar to the prototype become less and less typical. At some point, these
less typical items become so atypical that you start to doubt whether they are
in the category at all. Is a rug really an example of furniture? Its in the home
like chairs and tables, but its also different from most furniture in its structure
and use. From day to day, you might change your mind as to whether this
atypical example is in or out of the category. So, changes in typicality ultimately
lead to borderline members.
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
868
Source of Typicality
Intuitively, it is not surprising that robins are better examples of birds than
penguins are, or that a table is a more typical kind of furniture than is a rug.
But given that robins and penguins are known to be birds, why should one be
more typical than the other? One possible answer is the frequency with which
we encounter the object: We see a lot more robins than penguins, so they must
be more typical. Frequency does have some effect, but it is actually not the most
important variable (Rosch, Simpson, & Miller, 1976). For example, I see both
rugs and tables every single day, but one of them is much more typical as
furniture than the other.
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
869
The best account of what makes something typical comes from Rosch and
Merviss (1975) family resemblance theory. They proposed that items are likely
to be typical if they (a) have the features that are frequent in the category and
(b) do not have features frequent in other categories. Lets compare two
extremes, robins and penguins. Robins are small flying birds that sing, live in
nests in trees, migrate in winter, hop around on your lawn, and so on. Most of
these properties are found in many other birds. In contrast, penguins do not
fly, do not sing, do not live in nests or in trees, do not hop around on your lawn.
Furthermore, they have properties that are common in other categories, such
as swimming expertly and having wings that look and act like fins. These
properties are more often found in fish than in birds.
According to Rosch and Mervis, then, it is not because a robin is a very
common bird that makes it typical. Rather, it is because the robin has the shape,
size, body parts, and behaviors that are very common among birdsand not
common among fish, mammals, bugs, and so forth.
In a classic experiment, Rosch and Mervis (1975) made up two new
categories, with arbitrary features. Subjects viewed example after example and
had to learn which example was in which category. Rosch and Mervis
constructed some items that had features that were common in the category
and other items that had features less common in the category. The subjects
learned the first type of item before they learned the second type. Furthermore,
they then rated the items with common features as more typical. In another
experiment, Rosch and Mervis constructed items that differed in how many
features were shared with a different category. The more features were shared,
the longer it took subjects to learn which category the item was in. These
experiments, and many later studies, support both parts of the family
resemblance theory.
870
Category Hierarchies
Many important categories fall into hierarchies, in which more concrete
categories are nested inside larger, abstract categories. For example, consider
the categories: brown bear, bear, mammal, vertebrate, animal, entity. Clearly,
all brown bears are bears; all bears are mammals; all mammals are vertebrates;
and so on. Any given object typically does not fall into just one categoryit
could be in a dozen different categories, some of which are structured in this
hierarchical manner. Examples of biological categories come to mind most
easily, but within the realm of human artifacts, hierarchical structures can
readily be found: desk chair, chair, furniture, artifact, object.
Brown (1958), a child language researcher, was perhaps the first to note that
there seems to be a preference for which category we use to label things. If
your office desk chair is in the way, youll probably say, Move that chair, rather
than Move that desk chair or piece of furniture. Brown thought that the use
of a single, consistent name probably helped children to learn the name for
things. And, indeed, childrens first labels for categories tend to be exactly those
names that adults prefer to use (Anglin, 1977).
871
872
873
& Brownell, 1985). Experts can differ from novices in which categories are the
most differentiated, because they know different things about the categories,
therefore changing how similar the categories are.
874
875
Why would someone propose such a theory of concepts? One answer is that
in many experiments studying concepts, people learn concepts by seeing
exemplars over and over again until they learn to classify them correctly. Under
such conditions, it seems likely that people eventually memorize the exemplars
(Smith & Minda, 1998). There is also evidence that close similarity to wellremembered objects has a large effect on classification. Allen and Brooks (1991)
taught people to classify items by following a rule. However, they also had their
subjects study the items, which were richly detailed. In a later test, the
experimenters gave people new items that were very similar to one of the old
items but were in a different category. That is, they changed one property so
that the item no longer followed the rule. They discovered that people were
often fooled by such items. Rather than following the category rule they had
been taught, they seemed to recognize the new item as being very similar to
an old one and so put it, incorrectly, into the same category.
Many experiments have been done to compare the prototype and exemplar
theories. Overall, the exemplar theory seems to have won most of these
comparisons. However, the experiments are somewhat limited in that they
usually involve a small number of exemplars that people view over and over
again. It is not so clear that exemplar theory can explain real-world classification
in which people do not spend much time learning individual items (how much
time do you spend studying squirrels? or chairs?). Also, given that some part of
our knowledge of categories is learned through general statements we read or
hear, it seems that there must be room for a summary description separate
from exemplar memory.
Many researchers would now acknowledge that concepts are represented
through multiple cognitive systems. For example, your knowledge of dogs may
be in part through general descriptions such as dogs have four legs. But you
probably also have strong memories of some exemplars (your family dog,
Lassie) that influence your categorization. Furthermore, some categories also
involve rules (e.g., a strike in baseball). How these systems work together is the
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
876
Knowledge
The final topic has to do with how concepts fit with our broader knowledge of
the world. We have been talking very generally about people learning the
features of concepts. For example, they see a number of birds and then learn
that birds generally have wings, or perhaps they remember bird exemplars.
From this perspective, it makes no difference what those exemplars or features
arepeople just learn them. But consider two possible concepts of buildings
and their features in Table 2.
Imagine you had to learn these two concepts by seeing exemplars of them,
each exemplar having some of the features listed for the concept (as well as
some idiosyncratic features). Learning the donker concept would be pretty easy.
It seems to be a kind of underwater building, perhaps for deep-sea explorers.
Its features seem to go together. In contrast, the blegdav doesnt really make
sense. If its in the desert, how can you get there by submarine, and why do
they have polar bears as pets? Why would farmers live in the desert or use
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
877
878
will state that there is something about dogs, perhaps some specific gene or
set of genes, that all dogs have and that makes them bark, have fur, and look
the way they do. Therefore, decisions about whether something is a dog do not
depend only on features that you can easily see but also on the assumed
presence of this cause.
Belief in an essence can be revealed through experiments describing
fictional objects. Keil (1989) described to adults and children a fiendish
operation in which someone took a raccoon, dyed its hair black with a white
stripe down the middle, and implanted a sac of super-smelly yucky stuff under
its tail. The subjects were shown a picture of a skunk and told that this is now
what the animal looks like. What is it? Adults and children over the age of 4 all
agreed that the animal is still a raccoon. It may look and even act like a skunk,
but a raccoon cannot change its stripes (or whatever!)it will always be a
raccoon.
Importantly, the same effect was not found when Keil described a coffeepot
that was operated on to look like and function as a birdfeeder. Subjects agreed
that it was now a birdfeeder. Artifacts dont have an essence.
Signs of essentialism include (a) objects are believed to be either in or out
of the category, with no in-between; (b) resistance to change of category
membership or of properties connected to the essence; and (c) for living things,
the essence is passed on to progeny.
Essentialism is probably helpful in dealing with much of the natural world,
but it may be less helpful when it is applied to humans. Considerable evidence
suggests that people think of gender, racial, and ethnic groups as having
essences, which serves to emphasize the difference between groups and even
justify discrimination (Hirschfeld, 1996). Historically, group differences were
described by inheriting the blood of ones family or group. Bad blood was not
just an expression but a belief that negative properties were inherited and could
not be changed. After all, if it is in the nature of those people to be dishonest
(or clannish or athletic ...), then that could hardly be changed, any more than a
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
879
Conclusion
Concepts are central to our everyday thought. When we are planning for the
future or thinking about our past, we think about specific events and objects
in terms of their categories. If youre visiting a friend with a new baby, you have
some expectations about what the baby will do, what gifts would be appropriate,
how you should behave toward it, and so on. Knowing about the category of
babies helps you to effectively plan and behave when you encounter this child
youve never seen before.
Learning about those categories is a complex process that involves seeing
exemplars (babies), hearing or reading general descriptions (Babies like blackand-white pictures), general knowledge (babies have kidneys), and learning
the occasional rule (all babies have a rooting reflex). Current research is focusing
on how these different processes take place in the brain. It seems likely that
these different aspects of concepts are accomplished by different neural
structures (Maddox & Ashby, 2004).
Another interesting topic is how concepts differ across cultures. As different
cultures have different interests and different kinds of interactions with the
world, it seems clear that their concepts will somehow reflect those differences.
On the other hand, the structure of categories in the world also imposes a
strong constraint on what kinds of categories are actually useful. Some
researchers have suggested that differences between Eastern and Western
modes of thought have led to qualitatively different kinds of concepts (e.g.,
nobaproject.com - Categories and Concepts
880
Norenzayan, Smith, Kim, & Nisbett, 2002). Although such differences are
intriguing, we should also remember that different cultures seem to share
common categories such as chairs, dogs, parties, and jars, so the differences
may not be as great as suggested by experiments designed to detect cultural
effects. The interplay of culture, the environment, and basic cognitive processes
in establishing concepts has yet to be fully investigated.
881
Outside Resources
Debate: The debate about Pluto and the definition of planet is an interesting
one, as it illustrates the difficulty of arriving at definitions even in science.
The Planetary Science Institutes website has a series of press releases about
the Pluto debate, including reactions from astronomers, while it happened.
http://www.psi.edu
Discussion Questions
1. Pick a couple of familiar categories and try to come up with definitions for
them. When you evaluate each proposal (a) is it in fact accurate as a
definition, and( b) is it a definition that people might actually use in
identifying category members?
2. For the same categories, can you identify members that seem to be better
and worse members? What about these items makes them typical and
atypical?
3. Going around the room, point to some common objects (including things
people are wearing or brought with them) and identify what the basic-level
category is for that item. What are superordinate and subordinate
categories for the same items?
4. List some features of a common category such as tables. The knowledge
view suggests that you know reasons for why these particular features occur
together. Can you articulate some of those reasons? Do the same thing for
an animal category.
5. Choose three common categories: a natural kind, a human artifact, and a
social event. Discuss with class members from other countries or cultures
whether the corresponding categories in their cultures differ. Can you make
a hypothesis about when such categories are likely to differ and when they
are not?
Vocabulary
Basic-level category
The neutral, preferred category for a given object, at an intermediate level of
specificity.
Category
A set of entities that are equivalent in some way. Usually the items are similar
to one another.
Concept
The mental representation of a category.
Exemplar
An example in memory that is labeled as being in a particular category.
Psychological essentialism
The belief that members of a category have an unseen property that causes
them to be in the category and to have the properties associated with it.
Typicality
The difference in goodness of category members, ranging from the most
typical (the prototype) to borderline members.
Reference List
Allen, S. W., & Brooks, L. R. (1991). Specializing the operation of an explicit rule.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 319.
Anglin, J. M. (1977). Word, object, and conceptual development. New York, NY:
W. W. Norton.
Berlin, B. (1992). Ethnobiological classification: Principles of categorization of
plants and animals in traditional societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Brown, R. (1958). How shall a thing be called? Psychological Review, 65, 1421.
Gelman, S. A. (2003). The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday
thought. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Hampton, J. A. (1979). Polymorphous concepts in semantic memory. Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 441461.
Hirschfeld, L. A. (1996). Race in the making: Cognition, culture, and the child's
construction of human kinds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Horton, M. S., & Markman, E. M. (1980). Developmental differences in the
acquisition of basic and superordinate categories. Child Development, 51,
708719.
Keil, F. C. (1989). Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Wisniewski, E. J., & Murphy, G. L. (1989). Superordinate and basic category names
in discourse: A textual analysis. Discourse Processes, 12, 245261.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Categories and Concepts by Gregory Murphy is
licensed
under
the
Creative
Commons
Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0
Abstract
Humans have the capacity to use complex language, far more than any other
species on Earth. We cooperate with each other to use language for
communication; language is often used to communicate about and even
construct and maintain our social world. Language use and human sociality are
inseparable parts of Homo sapiens as a biological species.
Learning Objectives
Describe the process by which people can share new information by using
language.
Introduction
Imagine two men of 30-something age, Adam and Ben, walking down the
corridor. Judging from their clothing, they are young businessmen, taking a
break from work. They then exchange the utterances.
If you are watching this scene and hearing their conversation, what can you
guess from this? First of all, youd guess that Gary bought a ring for Mary,
whoever Gary and Mary might be. Perhaps you would infer that Gary is getting
married to Mary. What else can you guess? Perhaps that Adam and Ben are
fairly close colleagues, and both of them know Gary and Mary reasonably well.
In other words, you can guess the social relationships surrounding the people
who are engaging in the conversation and the people whom they are talking
about.
Language is used in our everyday life. If psychology is a science of behavior,
scientific investigation of language use must be one of the most central topics
this is because language use is ubiquitous in human lives. Every human group
has a language; human infants (except those who have unfortunate disabilities)
learn at least one language without being taught explicitly. Even when children
who dont have much language to begin with are brought together, they can
begin to develop and use their own language. There is at least one known
instance where children who had had little language were brought together
and developed their own language spontaneously with minimum input from
adults. In Nicaragua in the 1980s, deaf children who were separately raised in
various locations were brought together to schools for the first time. Teachers
tried to teach them Spanish with little success. However, they began to notice
that the children were using their hands and gestures, apparently to
communicate with each other. Linguists were brought in to find out what was
happeningit turned out the children had developed their own sign language
by themselves. That was the birth of a new language, Nicaraguan Sign Language
(Kegl, Senghas, & Coppola, 1999). Language is ubiquitous, and we humans are
nobaproject.com - Language and Language Use
894
895
people take turns to assume the roles of speaker and listener, and actively
engage in the exchange of meaning.
Common ground helps people coordinate their language use. For instance,
when a speaker says something to a listener, he or she takes into account their
common ground, that is, what the speaker thinks the listener knows. Adam said
what he did because he knew Ben would know who Gary was. Hed have said,
A friend of mine is getting married, to another colleague who wouldnt know
Gary. This is called audience design (Fussell & Krauss, 1992); speakers design
their utterances for their audiences by taking into account the audiences
knowledge. If their audiences are seen to be knowledgeable about an object
(such as Ben about Gary), they tend to use a brief label of the object (i.e., Gary);
for a less knowledgeable audience, they use more descriptive words (e.g., a
friend of mine) to help the audience understand their utterances (Box 1).
896
897
describe this situation using language, others in the conversation begin to use
similar words and grammar, and many other aspects of language use converge.
As you all do so, similar situation models begin to be built in everyones mind
through the mechanism known as priming. Priming occurs when your thinking
about one concept (e.g., ring) reminds you about other related concepts (e.g.,
marriage, wedding ceremony). So, if everyone in the conversation knows
about Gary, Mary, and the usual course of events associated with a ring
engagement, wedding, marriage, etc. everyone is likely to construct a shared
situation model about Gary and Mary. Thus, making use of our highly developed
interpersonal ability to imitate (i.e., executing the same action as another
person) and cognitive ability to infer (i.e., one idea leading to other ideas), we
humans coordinate our common ground, share situation models, and
communicate with each other.
898
899
900
for emotive stories (Box 2). If gossip is repeatedly transmitted and spread, it
can reach a large number of people. When stories travel through
communication chains, they tend to become conventionalized (Bartlett, 1932).
A Native American tale of the War of the Ghosts recounts a warriors encounter
with ghosts traveling in canoes and his involvement with their ghostly battle.
He is shot by an arrow but doesnt die, returning home to tell the tale. After his
narration, however, he becomes still, a black thing comes out of his mouth, and
he eventually dies. When it was told to a student in England in the 1920s and
retold from memory to another person, who, in turn, retold it to another and
so on in a communication chain, the mythic tale became a story of a young
warrior going to a battlefield, in which canoes became boats, and the black
thing that came out of his mouth became simply his spirit (Bartlett, 1932). In
other words, information transmitted multiple times was transformed to
something that was easily understood by many, that is, information was
assimilated into the common ground shared by most people in the linguistic
community. More recently, Kashima (2000) conducted a similar experiment
using a story that contained sequence of events that described a young couples
interaction that included both stereotypical and counter-stereotypical actions
(e.g., a man watching sports on TV on Sunday vs. a man vacuuming the house).
After the retelling of this story, much of the counter-stereotypical information
was dropped, and stereotypical information was more likely to be retained.
Because stereotypes are part of the common ground shared by the community,
this finding too suggests that conversational retellings are likely to reproduce
conventional content.
901
902
903
904
Conclusion
Language and language use constitute a central ingredient of human
psychology. Language is an essential tool that enables us to live the kind of life
we do. Can you imagine a world in which machines are built, farms are
cultivated, and goods and services are transported to our household without
language? Is it possible for us to make laws and regulations, negotiate contracts,
and enforce agreements and settle disputes without talking? Much of
contemporary human civilization wouldnt have been possible without the
human ability to develop and use language. Like the Tower of Babel, language
can divide humanity, and yet, the core of humanity includes the innate ability
for language use. Whether we can use it wisely is a task before us in this
globalized world.
905
Discussion Questions
1. In what sense is language use innate and learned?
2. Is language a tool for thought or a tool for communication?
3. What sorts of unintended consequences can language use bring to your
psychological processes?
Vocabulary
Audience design
Constructing utterances to suit the audiences knowledge.
Common ground
Information that is shared by people who engage in a conversation.
Ingroup
Group to which a person belongs.
Lexicon
Words and expressions.
Linguistic intergroup bias
A tendency for people to characterize positive things about their ingroup using
more abstract expressions, but negative things about their outgroups using
more abstract expressions.
Outgroup
Group to which a person does not belong.
Priming
A stimulus presented to a person reminds him or her about other ideas
associated with the stimulus.
Sapir-Whorf hypothesis
The hypothesis that the language that people use determines their thoughts.
Situation model
A mental representation of an event, object, or situation constructed at the time
of comprehending a linguistic description.
Social brain hypothesis
The hypothesis that the human brain has evolved, so that humans can maintain
larger ingroups.
Social networks
Networks of social relationships among individuals through which information
can travel.
Syntax
Rules by which words are strung together to form sentences.
Reference List
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social
psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., & Cleland, A. A. (2000). Syntactic co-ordination
in dialogue. Cognition, 75, B1325.
Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Clark, H. H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. (1986). Referring as a collaborative process.
Cognition, 22, 139.
Dunbar, R. (1996). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. Boston,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neorcortical size, group size and language
in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 681735.
Dunbar, R. I. M., Duncan, N. D. C., & Nettle, D. (1995). Size and structure of freely
forming conversational groups. Human Nature, 6, 6778.
Dunbar, R. I. M., Marriott, A., & Duncan, N. D. C. (1997). Human conversational
behaviour. Human Nature, 8, 231246.
Fussell, S. R., & Krauss, R. M. (1992). Coordination of knowledge in
communication: Effects of speakers assumptions about what others know.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 378391.
Giles, H., Coupland, N., & Coupland, J. (1991) Accommodation theory:
Lieberman, M., Eisenberger, N. I., Crockett, M. J., Tom, S. M., Pfeifer, J. H., & Way,
B. W. (2007). Putting feelings into words. Psychological Science, 18, 421428.
Lyubomirsky, S., Sousa, L., & Dickerhoof, R. (2006). The costs and benefits of
writing, talking, and thinking about lifes triumphs and defeats. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 692708.
Maass, A., Salvi, D., Arcuri, L., & Semin, G. (1989). Language use in intergroup
contexts: The linguistic intergroup bias. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 57, 981993.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Seagal, J. (1999). Forming a story: The health benefits of
narrative. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 12431254.
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of
dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 169226.
Sapir, E. (1921). Language: An introduction to the study of speech. New York,
NY: Harcourt Brace.
Semin, G., & Fiedler, K. (1988). The cognitive functions of linguistic categories
in describing persons: Social cognition and language. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 54, 558568.
Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality (J. B. Carroll, Ed.). Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Language and Language Use by Yoshihisa
Kashima is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Theory of Mind
Bertram Malle
Brown University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
One of the most remarkable human capacities is to perceive and understand
mental states. This capacity, often labeled theory of mind, consists of an array
of psychological processes that play essential roles in human social life. We
review some of these roles, examine what happens when the capacity is
deficient, and explore the many processes that make up the capacity to
understand minds.
Learning Objectives
Enumerate the many domains of social life in which theory of mind is critical.
Describe and explain some of the many concepts and processes that
comprise the human understanding of minds.
Introduction
One of the most fascinating human capacities is the ability to perceive and
interpret other peoples behavior in terms of their mental states. Having an
appreciation for the workings of another persons mind is considered a
prerequisite for natural language acquisition (Baldwin & Tomasello, 1998),
strategic social interaction (Zhang, Hedden, & Chia, 2012), reflexive thought
(Bogdan, 2000), and moral judgment (Guglielmo, Monroe, & Malle, 2009). This
capacity develops from early beginnings in the first year of life to the adults
fast and often effortless understanding of others thoughts, feelings, and
intentions. And though we must speculate about its evolutionary origin, we do
have indications that the capacity evolved sometime in the last few million years.
In this chapter we will focus on two questions: What is the role of
understanding others minds in human social life? And what is known about
the mental processes that underlie such understanding? For simplicity, we will
label this understanding theory of mind, even though it is not literally a
theory that people have about the mind; rather, it is a capacity that some
scholars prefer to label mentalizing or mindreading. But we will go behind
all these labels by breaking down the capacity into distinct components: the
specific concepts and mental processes that underlie the human understanding
of minds.
First, lets get clear about the roles that this understanding plays in social life.
916
movements in terms of mental states, perceivers can parse this complex scene
into intentional actions of reaching and giving (Baird & Baldwin, 2001); they can
interpret the actions as instances of offering and trading; and with an
appropriate cultural script, they know that all that was going on was a customer
pulling out her credit card with the intention to pay the cashier behind the
register. Peoples theory of mind thus frames and interprets perceptions of
human behavior in a particular wayas perceptions of agents who can act
intentionally and who have desires, beliefs, and other mental states that guide
their actions (Perner, 1991; Wellman, 1990).
Not only would social perceivers without a theory of mind be utterly lost in
a simple payment interaction; without a theory of mind, there would probably
be no such things as cashiers, credit cards, and payment (Tomasello, 2003).
Plain and simple, humans need to understand minds in order to engage in the
kinds of complex interactions that social communities (small and large) require.
And it is these complex social interactions that have given rise, in human cultural
evolution, to houses, cities, and nations; to books, money, and computers; to
education, law, and science.
The list of social interactions that rely deeply on theory of mind is long; here
are a few highlights.
Teaching another person new actions or rules by taking into account what
the learner knows or doesnt know and how one might best make him
understand.
Figuring out our social standing by trying to guess what others think and
feel about us.
917
918
So what is this magical potion that allows most people to gain quick and
automatic access to other peoples minds and to recognize the meaning
underlying human behavior? Scientific research has accumulated a good deal
of knowledge in the past few decades, and here is a synopsis of what we know.
919
Figure 1. Some of the major tools of theory of mind, with the bottom showing simple,
automatic, early developing, and evolutionarily old processes, and the top showing complex,
more deliberate, late developing, and evolutionarily recent processes.
920
921
conversation take on similar gestures, body positions, even tone of voice? They
synchronize their behaviors by way of (largely) unconscious imitation. Such
synchrony can happen even at very low levels, such as negative physiological
arousal (Levenson & Ruef, 1992), though the famous claim of synchrony in
womens menstrual cycles is a myth (Yang & Schank, 2006). Interestingly, people
who enjoy an interaction synchronize their behaviors more, and increased
synchrony (even manipulated in an experiment) makes people enjoy their
interaction more (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). Some research findings suggest
that synchronizing is made possible by brain mechanisms that tightly link
perceptual information with motor information (when I see you move your arm,
my arm-moving program is activated). In monkeys, highly specialized so-called
mirror neurons fire both when the monkey sees a certain action and when it
performs that same action (Rizzolatti, Fogassi, & Gallese, 2001). In humans,
however, things are a bit more complex. In many everyday settings, people
perceive uncountable behaviors and fortunately dont copy all of them (just
consider walking in a crowdhundreds of your mirror neurons would fire in a
blaze of confusion). Human imitation and mirroring is selective, triggering
primarily actions that are relevant to the perceivers current state or aim.
Automatic empathy builds on imitation and synchrony in a clever way. If
Bill is sad and expresses this emotion in his face and body, and if Elena watches
or interacts with Bill, then she will subtly imitate his dejected behavior and,
through well-practiced associations of certain behaviors and emotions, she will
feel a little sad as well (Sonnby-Borgstrm, Jnsson, & Svensson, 2003). Thus,
she empathizes with himwhether she wants to or not. Try it yourself. Type
sad human faces into your Internet search engine and select images from
your results. Look at 20 photos and pay careful attention to what happens to
your face and to your mood. Do you feel almost a pull of some of your facial
muscles? Do you feel a tinge of melancholy?
922
923
the stern interrogator? I would feel scared . . . An even simpler form of such
modeling is the assumption that the other thinks, feels, wants what we do
which has been called the like-me assumption (Meltzoff, 2007) or the
inclination toward social projection(Krueger, 2007). In a sense, this is an
absence of perspective taking, because we assume that the others perspective
equals our own. This can be an effective strategy if we share with the other
person the same environment, background, knowledge, and goals, but it gets
us into trouble when this presumed common ground is in reality lacking. Lets
say you know that Brianna doesnt like Freds new curtains, but you hear her
exclaim to Fred, These are beautiful! Now you have to predict whether Fred
can figure out that Brianna was being sarcastic. It turns out that you will have
a hard time suppressing your own knowledge in this case and you may
overestimate how easy it is for Fred to spot the sarcasm (Keysar, 1994). Similarly,
you will overestimate how visible that pimple is on your chineven though it
feels big and ugly to you, in reality very few people will ever notice it (Gilovich
& Savitsky, 1999). So the next time when you spot a magnificent bird high up
in the tree and you get impatient with your friend who just cant see what is
clearly obvious, remember: its obvious to you.
What all these examples show is that people use their own current state
of knowledge, concern, or perceptionto grasp other peoples mental states.
And though they often do so correctly, they also get things wrong at times. This
is why couples counselors, political advisors, and Buddhists agree on at least
one thing: we all need to try harder to recognize our egocentrism and actively
take other peoples perspectivethat is, grasp their actual mental states, even
if (or especially when) they are different from our own.
924
925
926
F
i
nobaproject.com - Theory of Mind
u
g
927
re
2.
Tools in Summary
We have seen that the human understanding of other minds relies on many
tools. People process such information as motion, faces, and gestures and
categorize it into such concepts as agent, intentional action, or fear. They rely
on relatively automatic psychological processes, such as imitation, joint
attention, and projection. And they rely on more effortful processes, such as
simulation and mental-state inference. These processes all link behavior that
humans observe to mental states that humans infer. If we call this stunning
capacity a theory, it is a theory of mind and behavior.
928
929
within seconds or less. Whats so special about that? Well, it takes years for a
child to develop this capacity, and it took our species a few million years to
evolve it. Thats pretty special.
930
Outside Resources
Blog: On the debate about menstrual synchrony
http://blogs.scientificamerican.com/context-and-variation/2011/11/16/menstrualsynchrony/
Blog: On the debates over mirror neurons
http://blogs.scientificamerican.com/guest-blog/2012/11/06/whats-so-special-aboutmirror-neurons/
Book: First and last chapters of Zunshine, L. (2006). Why we read fiction:
Theory of mind and the novel. Columbus, OH: Ohio State University Press.
https://ohiostatepress.org/Books/Book PDFs/Zunshine Why.pdf
Movie: A movie that portrays the social difficulties of a person with autism:
Adam (Fox Searchlight Pictures, 2009)
http://www.imdb.com/title/tt1185836/?ref_=fn_tt_tt_1
Video: Autism, theory of mind, and false-belief test
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=6bsvm7q3lWo
Video: TED talk on autism
http://www.ted.com/talks/temple_grandin_the_world_needs_all_kinds_of_minds.
html
Video: TED talk on empathy
http://blog.ted.com/2011/04/18/a-radical-experiment-in-empathy-sam-richardsat-ted-com/
Discussion Questions
1. Recall a situation in which you tried to infer what a person was thinking or
feeling but you just couldnt figure it out, and recall another situation in
which you tried the same but succeeded. Which tools were you able to use
in the successful case that you didnt or couldnt use in the failed case?
2. Mindfulness training improves keen awareness of ones own mental states.
Look up a few such training programs (easily found online) and develop a
similar training program to improve awareness of other peoples minds.
3. In the near future we will have robots that closely interact with people. Which
theory of mind tools should a robot definitely have? Which ones are less
important? Why?
4. Humans assume that everybody has the capacity to make choices and
perform intentional actions. But in a sense, a choice is just a series of brain
states, caused by previous brain states and states of the world, all governed
by the physical laws of the universe. Is the concept of choice an illusion?
5. The capacity to understand others minds is intimately related to another
unique human capacity: language. How might these two capacities have
evolved? Together? One before the other? Which one?
Vocabulary
Automatic empathy
A social perceiver unwittingly taking on the internal state of another person,
usually because of mimicking the persons expressive behavior and thereby
feeling the expressed emotion.
False-belief test
An experimental procedure that assesses whether a perceiver recognizes that
another person has a false beliefa belief that contradicts reality.
Intention
An agents mental state of committing to perform an action that the agent
believes will bring about a desired outcome.
Intentionality
The quality of an agents performing a behavior intentionallythat is, with skill
and awareness and executing an intention (which is in turn based on a desire
and relevant beliefs).
Joint attention
Two people attending to the same object and being aware that they both are
attending to it.
Mimicry
Copying others behavior, usually without awareness.
Mirror neurons
Neurons identified in monkey brains that fire both when the monkey performs
a certain action and when it perceives another agent performing that action.
Projection
A social perceivers assumption that the other person wants, knows, or feels
the same as the perceiver wants, know, or feels.
Simulation
The process of representing the other persons mental state.
Synchrony
Two people displaying the same behaviors or having the same internal states
(typically because of mutual mimicry).
Theory of mind
The human capacity to understand minds, a capacity that is made up of a
collection of concepts (e.g., agent, intentionality) and processes (e.g., goal
detection, imitation, empathy, perspective taking).
Reference List
Baird, J. A., & Baldwin, D. A. (2001). Making sense of human behavior: Action
parsing and intentional inference. In B. F. Malle, L. J. Moses, & D. A. Baldwin
(Eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp.
193206). Cambridge, MA:MIT Press.
Baldwin, D. A., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Word learning: A window on early
pragmatic understanding. In E. V. Clark (Ed.), The proceedings of the twentyninth annual child language research forum (pp. 323). Chicago, IL: Center
for the Study of Language and Information.
Blackburn, J., Gottschewski, K., George, E., & L, N. (2000, May). A discussion
about theory of mind: From an autistic perspective. Proceedings of Autism
Europes 6th International Congress. Glasgow. Retrieved from http://
archive.autistics.org/library/AE2000-ToM.html.
Bogdan, R. (2000). Minding minds: Evolving a reflexing mind by interpreting
others. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception
behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 76, 893910.
Epley, N., Morewedge, C. K., & Keysar, B. (2004). Perspective taking in children
and adults: Equivalent egocentrism but differential correction. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 760768.
Gergely, G., Ndasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Br, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance
at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165193.
Gilovich, T., & Savitsky, K. (1999). The spotlight effect and the illusion of
transparency: Egocentric assessments of how we are seen by others.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 165168.
Guglielmo, S., Monroe, A. E., & Malle, B. F. (2009). At the heart of morality lies
folk psychology. Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy, 52, 449
466.
Johnson, S. C. (2000). The recognition of mentalistic agents in infancy. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 2228.
Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.),
Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 15, pp. 192240). Lincoln:
University of Nebraska Press.
Keysar, B. (1994). The illusory transparency of intention: Linguistic perspective
taking in text. Cognitive Psychology, 26, 165208.
Krueger, J. I. (2007). From social projection to social behaviour. European Review
of Social Psychology, 18, 135.
Levenson, R. W., & Ruef, A. M. (1992). Empathy: A physiological substrate. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 234246.
Malle, B. F. (2008). The fundamental tools, and possibly universals, of social
cognition. In R. M. Sorrentino & S. Yamaguchi (Eds.), Handbook of motivation
and cognition across cultures (pp. 267296). New York, NY: Elsevier/
Academic Press.
Malle, B. F. (2004). How the mind explains behavior: Folk explanations, meaning,
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Theory of Mind by Bertram Malle is licensed
under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International
License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
deed.en_US.
Topic 6
Learning and Memory
Abstract
Learning is a complex process that defies easy definition and description. This
chapter reviews some of the philosophical issues involved with defining learning
and describes in some detail the characteristics of learners and of encoding
activities that seem to affect how well people can acquire new memories,
knowledge, or skills. At the end, we consider a few basic principles that guide
whether a particular attempt at learning will be successful or not.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
What do you do when studying for an exam? Do you read your class notes and
textbook (hopefully not for the very first time)? Do you try to find a quiet place
without distraction? Do you use flash cards to test your knowledge? The choices
you make reveal your theory of learning, but there is no reason for you to limit
yourself to your own intuitions. There is a vast and vibrant science of learning,
in which researchers from psychology, education, and neuroscience study basic
principles of learning and memory.
In fact, learning is a much broader domain than you might think. Consider:
Is listening to music a form of learning? More often, it seems listening to music
is a way of avoiding learning. But we know that your brains response to auditory
information changes with your experience with that information, a form of
learning called auditory perceptual learning (Polley, Steinberg, & Merzenich,
2006). Each time we listen to a song, we hear it differently because of our
experience. When we exhibit changes in behavior without having intended to
learn something, that is called implicit learning (Seger, 1994), and when we
exhibit changes in our behavior that reveal the influence of past experience
even though we are not attempting to use that experience, that is called implicit
memory (Richardson-Klavehn & Bjork, 1988). These topics will be covered in
the chapter on Memory.
Other well-studied forms of learning include the types of learning that are
general across species. We cant ask a slug to learn a poem or a lemur to learn
to bat left-handed, but we can assess learning in other ways. For example, we
can look for a change in our responses to things when we are repeatedly
stimulated. If you live in a house with a grandfather clock, you know that what
was once an annoying and intrusive sound is now probably barely audible to
you. Similarly, poking an earthworm again and again is likely to lead to a
reduction in its retraction from your touch. These phenomena are forms of
nobaproject.com - Factors Influencing Learning
946
Learners
People bring numerous individual differences with them into memory
experiments, and many of these variables affect learning. In the classroom,
motivation matters (Pintrich, 2003), though experimental attempts to induce
motivation with money yield only modest benefits (Heyer & OKelly, 1949).
Learners are, however, quite able to allocate more effort to learning prioritized
over unimportant materials (Castel, Benjamin, Craik, & Watkins, 2002).
In addition, the organization and planning skills that a learner exhibits matter
a lot (Garavalia & Gredler, 2002), suggesting that the efficiency with which one
organizes self-guided learning is an important component of learning. We will
return to this topic soon.
nobaproject.com - Factors Influencing Learning
947
948
Encoding Activities
What we do when were learning is very important. Weve all had the experience
of reading something and suddenly coming to the realization that we dont
remember a single thing, even the sentence that we just read. How we go about
encoding information determines a lot about how much we remember.
You might think that the most important thing is to try to learn. Interestingly,
this is not true, at least not completely. Trying to learn a list of words, as
compared to just evaluating each word for its part of speech (i.e., noun, verb,
adjective) does help you recall the wordsthat is, it helps you remember and
write down more of the words later. But it actually impairs your ability to
recognize the wordsto judge on a later list which words are the ones that you
studied (Eagle & Leiter, 1964). So this is a case in which incidental learning
that is, learning without the intention to learnis better than intentional
learning.
Such examples are not particularly rare and are not limited to recognition.
Nairne, Pandeirada, and Thompson (2008) showed, for example, that survival
processingthinking about and rating each word in a list for its relevance in a
survival scenarioled to much higher recall than intentional learning (and also
higher, in fact, than other encoding activities that are also known to lead to high
levels of recall). Clearly, merely intending to learn something is not enough.
How a learner actively processes the material plays a large role; for example,
reading words and evaluating their meaning leads to better learning than
reading them and evaluating the way that the words look or sound (Craik &
Lockhart, 1972). These results suggest that individual differences in motivation
will not have a large effect on learning unless learners also have accurate ideas
about how to effectively learn material when they care to do so.
nobaproject.com - Factors Influencing Learning
949
950
One final factor that merits discussion is the role of testing. Educators and
students often think about testing as a way of assessing knowledge, and this is
indeed an important use of tests. But tests themselves affect memory, because
retrieval is one of the most powerful ways of enhancing learning (Roediger &
Butler, 2013). Self-testing is an underutilized and potent means of making
learning more durable.
951
Transfer-appropriate processing
Sometimes, it doesnt make sense to talk about whether a particular encoding
activity is good or bad for learning. Rather, we can talk about whether that
activity is good for learning as revealed by a particular test. For example,
although reading words for meaning leads to better performance on a test of
recall or recognition than paying attention to the pronunciation of the word, it
leads to worse performance on a test that taps knowledge of that pronunciation,
such as whether a previously studied word rhymes with another word (Morris,
Bransford, & Franks, 1977). The principle of transfer-appropriate processing
states that memory is better when the test taps the same type of knowledge
as the original encoding activity. When thinking about how to learn material,
we should always be thinking about the situations in which we are likely to need
access to that material. An emergency responder who needs access to learned
procedures under conditions of great stress should learn differently from a
hobbyist learning to use a new digital camera.
952
But the value of forgetting is even greater than that. There is lots of evidence
that some forgetting is a prerequisite for more learning. For example, the
previously discussed benefits of distributing practice opportunities may arise
in part because of the greater forgetting that takes places between those spaced
learning events. It is for this reason that some encoding activities that are
difficult and lead to the appearance of slow learning actually lead to superior
learning in the long run (Bjork, 2011). When we opt for learning activities that
enhance learning quickly, we must be aware that these are not always the same
techniques that lead to durable, long-term learning.
Conclusion
To wrap things up, lets think back to the questions we began the chapter with.
What might you now do differently when preparing for an exam? Hopefully, you
will think about testing yourself frequently, developing an accurate sense of
what you do and do not know, how you are likely to use the knowledge, and
using the scheduling of tasks to your advantage. If you are learning a new skill
or new material, using the scientific study of learning as a basis for the study
and practice decisions you make is a good bet.
953
Discussion Questions
1. How would you best design a computer program to help someone learn a
new foreign language? Think about some of the principles of learning
outlined in this chapter and how those principles could be instantiated in
rules in a computer program.
2. Would you rather have a really good memory or really good metacognition?
How might you train someone to develop better metacognition if he or she
doesnt have a very good memory, and what would be the consequences
of that training?
3. In what kinds of situations not discussed here might you find a benefit of
forgetting on learning?
Vocabulary
Chunk
The process of grouping information together using our knowledge.
Classical conditioning
Describes stimulus-stimulus associative learning.
Encoding
The pact of putting information into memory.
Habituation
Occurs when the response to a stimulus decreases with exposure.
Implicit learning
Occurs when we acquire information without intent that we cannot easily
express.
Implicit memory
Memory tests in which the effect of prior experiences is measured in a context
in which the subject is not instructed to remember those prior experiences.
Incidental learning
Any type of learning that happens without the intention to learn.
Intentional learning
Any type of learning that happens when motivated by intention.
Metacognition
Describes the knowledge and skills people have in monitoring and controlling
their own learning and memory.
Nonassociative learning
Occurs when a single repeated exposure leads to a change in behavior.
Operant conditioning
Describes stimulus-response associative learning.
Perceptual learning
Occurs when aspects of our perception changes as a function of experience.
Sensitization
Occurs when the response to a stimulus increases with exposure
Transfer-appropriate processing
A principle that states that memory performance is superior when a test taps
the same cognitive processes as the original encoding activity.
Working memory
The form of memory we use to hold onto information temporarily, usually for
the purposes of manipulation.
Reference List
Ashcraft, M. H., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory,
math anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 130, 224237.
Baddeley, A. D., & Longman, D. J. A. (1978). The influence of length and frequency
of training session on the rate of learning to type. Ergonomics, 21, 627635.
Bahrick, H. P., Bahrick, L. E., Bahrick, A. S., & Bahrick, P. O. (1993). Maintenance
of foreign language vocabulary and the spacing effect. Psychological
Science, 4, 316321.
Bjork, R. A. (2011). On the symbiosis of learning, remembering, and forgetting.
In A. S. Benjamin (Ed.), Successful remembering and successful forgetting:
A Festschrift in honor of Robert A. Bjork (pp. 122). London, UK: Psychology
Press.
Castel, A. D., Benjamin, A. S., Craik, F. I. M., & Watkins, M. J. (2002). The effects
of aging on selectivity and control in short-term recall. Memory & Cognition,
30, 10781085.
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology,
4, 5581.
Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for
memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671
684.
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working
memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19,
450466.
Eagle, M., & Leiter, E. (1964). Recall and recognition in intentional and incidental
learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 5863.
Garavalia, L. S., & Gredler, M. E. (2002). Prior achievement, aptitude, and use of
learning strategies as predictors of college student achievement. College
Student Journal, 36, 616626.
Hall, K. G., Domingues, D. A., & Cavazos, R. (1994). Contextual interference
effects with skilled baseball players. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78, 835
841.
Heyer, A. W., Jr., & OKelly, L. I. (1949). Studies in motivation and retention: II.
Retention of nonsense syllables learned under different degrees of
motivation. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 27, 143
152.
Jenkins, J. J. (1979). Four points to remember: A tetrahedral model of memory
experiments. In L. S. Cermak & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Levels of processing and
human memory (pp. 429446). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kane, M. J., Conway, A. R. A., Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2008). Variation in
working memory capacity as variation in executive attention and control.
In A. R. A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. J. Kane, A. Miyake, & J. N. Towse (Eds.),
Variation in Working Memory (pp. 2248). New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
Kimball, D. R., Smith, T. A., & Muntean, W. J. (2012). Does delaying judgments of
learning really improve the efficacy of study decisions? Not so much. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 923954.
Kornell, N., & Metcalfe, J. (2006). Study efficacy and the region of proximal
learning framework.
Pinsker, H., Kupfermann, I., Castelluci, V., & Kandel, E. (1970). Habituation and
dishabituation of the gill-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Science, 167, 1740
1742.
Pintrich, P. R. (2003). A motivational science perspective on the role of student
motivation in learning and teaching contexts. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 95, 667686.
Polley, D. B., Steinberg, E. E., & Merzenich, M. M. (2006). Perceptual learning
directs auditory cortical map reorganization through top-down influences.
The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 49704982.
Ramirez, G., & Beilock, S. L. (2011). Writing about testing worries boosts exam
performance in the classroom. Science, 331, 211213.
Richardson-Klavehn, A. & Bjork, R.A. (1988). Measures of memory. Annual
Review of Psychology, 39, 475543.
Roediger, H. L., & Butler, A.C. (2013). Retrieval practice (testing) effect. In H. L.
Pashler (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the mind. Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publishing Co.
Seger, C. A. (1994). Implicit learning. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 163196.
Simon, H. A. (1974). How big is a chunk? Science, 4124, 482488.
Taylor, K., & Rohrer, D. (2010). The effects of interleaved practice. Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 24, 837848.
Tullis, J. G., & Benjamin, A. S. (2012). Consequences of restudy choices in younger
and older learners. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 743749.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Factors Influencing Learning by Aaron Benjamin
is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
Basic principles of learning are always operating and always influencing human
behavior. This chapter discusses the two very fundamental forms of learning
that are represented in classical (Pavlovian) and instrumental (operant)
conditioning. Through them, we respectively learn to associate (1.) stimuli in
the environment or (2.) our own behaviors with significant events such as
rewards and punishers. The two types of learning have been intensively studied
because they have powerful effects on behavior and because they provide
methods that allow scientists to analyze learning processes rigorously. This
chapter describes some of the most important things you need to know about
classical and instrumental conditioning, and it illustrates some of the many
ways they help us understand normal and disordered behavior in humans.
Learning Objectives
Understand some important facts about each that tell us how they work.
966
conditioned response (CR), and the natural response to the food itself is the
unconditioned response (UR). Modern studies of classical conditioning use a
very wide range of CSs and USs and measure a wide range of conditioned
responses.
The second form of conditioning was first studied by Edward Thorndike and
later extended by B. F. Skinner. It is known as instrumental or operant
conditioning. This form of conditioning occurs when a behavior is associated
with the occurrence of a significant event. In the best-known example, a rat
learns to press a lever in a box in the laboratory (a Skinner box) when leverpressing produces food pellets. The behavior is an operant because it
operates on the environment; it is also instrumental for making the food occur.
The food pellet is called a reinforcer, because it strengthens the response it is
made a consequence of. (A reinforcer is any event that does this.) Operant
conditioning research studies how the effects of a behavior influence the
probability it will occur again. According to the law of effect, when a behavior
has a positive (satisfying) effect or consequence, it is likely to be repeated in the
future. When a behavior has a negative (annoying) consequence, it is less likely
to be repeated in the future. Effects that increase behaviors are reinforcers;
effects that decrease them are punishers.
An important idea behind the study of operant conditioning is that it
provides a method for studying how consequences influence voluntary
behavior. The rats lever-pressing for food is voluntary in the sense that the rat
is free to make and repeat the response whenever it wants to. No one forces it
to lever-press, and there is no stimulus, like Pavlovs bell, that directly causes it
to occur. One of the lessons of operant conditioning research, though, is that
voluntary behavior is strongly influenced by its consequences.
nobaproject.com - Conditioning and Learning
967
968
969
970
971
bill is perhaps the number that is printed on it. In Canada, the number occurs
together with a unique color. Because of blocking, Americans often dont learn
the color of the $20 bill. (It turns out that the Canadian $20 bill is green.) The
number gives them all the information they need; there is no prediction error
for the learning process to correct.
Classical conditioning is strongest if the CS and US are intense or salient. It
is also best if the CS and US are relatively new and the organism hasnt been
exposed to them a lot before. It is also especially strong if the organisms biology
has prepared it to associate a particular CS and US. For example, rats and
humans are naturally inclined to associate an illness with a flavor, rather than
with a light or tone. (This sorting tendency, which is set up by evolution, is called
preparedness.) There are many factors that affect the strength of classical
conditioning, and these have been the subject of much research and theory
(see Rescorla & Wagner, 1972; Pearce & Bouton, 2001). Behavioral
neuroscientists have also used classical conditioning to investigate many of the
basic brain processes that are involved in learning (see Fanselow & Poulos,
2005; Thompson & Steinmetz, 2009).
972
Realistic use of extinction in the clinic must accept one important fact about
it, however. This is that extinction does not necessarily destroy the original
learning (see Bouton, 2004). For example, if time is allowed to pass after
extinction has occurred, presentation of the CS can evoke some responding
again. This is called spontaneous recovery. Another important phenomenon
is the renewal effect: After extinction, if the CS is tested in a new context, such
as a different room or location, responding can also return. These effects have
been interpreted to suggest that extinction inhibits rather than erases the
learned behavior, and this inhibition is mainly expressed in the context in which
it is learned (see context in the Key Vocabulary section below).
This does not mean that extinction is a bad treatment for behavior disorders.
Instead, clinicians can make it effective by using basic research on learning to
help defeat these relapse effects (see Craske et al., 2008). For example, if
extinction therapies are conducted in the contexts where a person might be in
most danger of relapsing (at work, for example), the success of therapy can be
enhanced.
973
974
presence of a chair, and so on. Pigeons can learn the discrimination readily,
and under the right conditions, they will also peck the correct button when
shown pictures of new flowers, cars, chairs, and people that they have never
seen before. The birds have learned to categorize the sets of stimuli. Stimuluscontrol methods can be used to study how such categorization is learned.
Another thing to know about operant conditioning is that making the response
always requires you to choose that behavior over others. The student who
drinks beer on Thursday nights chooses to drink instead of staying at home
and studying with his girlfriend. The rat chooses to press the lever instead of
sleeping or scratching its ear in the back of the box. The alternative behaviors
are each associated with their own reinforcers. And the tendency to perform a
particular action depends exquisitely on both the reinforcers earned for it and
the reinforcers earned for its alternatives.
nobaproject.com - Conditioning and Learning
975
To investigate this idea, choice has been studied in the Skinner box by making
two levers available for the rat (or two buttons available for the pigeon), each
of which has its own reinforcement or payoff rate. A thorough study of choice
in situations like this has led to a rule called the quantitative law of effect (see
Herrnstein, 1970), which can be understood without going into quantitative
detail. The law acknowledges the fact that the effects of reinforcing one behavior
depend crucially on how much reinforcement is earned for the behaviors
alternatives. In general, a given reinforcer will be less reinforcing if there are
many alternative reinforcers in the environment. For this reason, alcohol, sex,
or drugs may be less likely to be extremely powerful reinforcers, and create
risky excesses in behavior, if a persons environment is also full of other sources
of reinforcement.
976
977
know this is to repeat the reinforcer devaluation experiment just described with
instrumental responses that have been given extensive and repeated practice
(see Holland, 2004). After all the practice, the instrumental response is no longer
sensitive to reinforcer devaluation: Even after a strong aversion is learned to
sucrose, the rat continues to perform the response that used to produce it,
quite impervious to its past consequences! The rat just responds automatically.
Habits are very common in human experience. You do not need to think much
about how to make your coffee in the morning or how to brush your teeth.
Instrumental behaviors can eventually become habitual. This lets us get the job
done while being free to think about other things.
978
The third association in the diagram is the one between the stimulus and
the response (S R). As discussed above, after a lot of practice, the stimulus
may begin to elicit the response directly. This is habit learning, where the
response occurs relatively automatically, without much mental processing of
nobaproject.com - Conditioning and Learning
979
the relation between the action and the outcome and the outcomes current
value.
The final link in the figure is one between the stimulus and the responseoutcome association [S (R O)]. More than just entering into a simple
association with the R or the O, the stimulus can signal that the R O relationship
is now in effect. This is what we mean when we say that the stimulus can set
the occasion for the operant response: It sets the occasion for the responsereinforcer relationship. Through this mechanism, the painter might begin to
paint when given the right tools and the opportunity enabled by the canvas.
The canvas theoretically signals that the behavior of painting will now be
reinforced by positive consequences.
The figure provides a framework that you can use to understand almost any
learned behavior you observe in yourself, your family, or your friends. If you
would like to understand it more deeply, consider taking a course on learning
in the future, which will give you a fuller appreciation of how Pavlovian learning,
instrumental learning, habit learning, and occasion setting actually work and
interact.
980
Outside Resources
Article: Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Pavlovian conditioning: Its not what you think
it is. American Psychologist, 43, 151160.
Book: Domjan, M. (2010). The principles of learning and behavior (6th ed.).
Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Discussion Questions
1. Describe three examples of Pavlovian (classical) conditioning that you have
seen in your own behavior, or that of your friends or family, in the past few
days.
4. In the modern world, processed foods are highly available and have been
engineered to be highly palatable and reinforcing. Discuss how Pavlovian
and instrumental conditioning can work together to explain why people
often eat too much.
5. How does blocking challenge the idea that pairings of a CS and US are
sufficient to cause Pavlovian conditioning? What is important in creating
Pavlovian learning?
6. How does the reinforcer devaluation effect challenge the idea that
reinforcers merely stamp in the operant response? What does the effect
tell us that animals actually learn in operant conditioning?
Vocabulary
Blocking
In classical conditioning, the finding that no conditioning occurs to a stimulus
if it is combined with a previously conditioned stimulus during conditioning
trials. Suggests that information, surprise value, or prediction error is important
in conditioning.
Categorize
To sort or arrange different items into classes or categories.
Classical conditioning
The procedure in which an initially neutral stimulus (the conditioned stimulus,
or CS) is paired with and an unconditioned stimulus (or US). The result is that
the conditioned stimulus begins to elicit a conditioned response (CR). Classical
conditioning is nowadays considered important as both a behavioral
phenomenon and as a method to study simple associative learning. Same as
Pavlovian conditioning.
Conditioned compensatory response
In classical conditioning, a conditioned response that opposes, rather than is
the same as, the unconditioned response. It functions to reduce the strength
of the unconditioned response. Often seen in conditioning when drugs are used
as unconditioned stimuli.
Conditioned response (CR)
The response that is elicited by the conditioned stimulus after classical
conditioning has taken place.
Goal-directed behavior
Instrumental behavior that is influenced by the animals knowledge of the
association between the behavior and its consequence and the current value
of the consequence. Sensitive to the reinforcer devaluation effect.
Habit
Instrumental behavior that occurs automatically in the presence of a stimulus
and is no longer influenced by the animals knowledge of the value of the
reinforcer. Insensitive to the reinforcer devaluation effect.
Instrumental conditioning
Process in which animals learn about the relationship between their behaviors
and their consequences. Also known as operant conditioning.
Law of effect
The idea that instrumental or operant responses are influenced by their effects.
Responses that are followed by a pleasant state of affairs will be strengthened
and those that are followed by discomfort will be weakened. Nowadays, the
term refers to the idea that operant or instrumental behaviors are lawfully
controlled by their consequences.
Operant
A behavior that is controlled by its consequences. The simplest example is the
rats lever-pressing, which is controlled by the presentation of the reinforcer.
Operant conditioning
See instrumental conditioning.
Pavlovian conditioning
See classical conditioning.
Prediction error
When the outcome of a conditioning trial is different from that which is predicted
by the conditioned stimuli that are present on the trial (i.e., when the US is
surprising). Prediction error is necessary to create Pavlovian conditioning (and
associative learning generally). As learning occurs over repeated conditioning
trials, the conditioned stimulus increasingly predicts the unconditioned
stimulus, and prediction error declines. Conditioning works to correct or reduce
prediction error.
Preparedness
The idea that an organisms evolutionary history can make it easy to learn a
particular association. Because of preparedness, you are more likely to
associate the taste of tequila, and not the circumstances surrounding drinking
it, with getting sick. Similarly, humans are more likely to associate images of
spiders and snakes than flowers and mushrooms with aversive outcomes like
shocks.
Punisher
A stimulus that decreases the strength of an operant behavior when it is made
a consequence of the behavior.
Stimulus control
When an operant behavior is controlled by a stimulus that precedes it.
Taste aversion learning
The phenomenon in which a taste is paired with sickness, and this causes the
organism to rejectand dislikethat taste in the future.
Unconditioned response (UR)
In classical conditioning, an innate response that is elicited by a stimulus before
(or in the absence of) conditioning.
Unconditioned stimulus (US)
In classical conditioning, the stimulus that elicits the response before
conditioning occurs.
Reference List
Balleine, B. W. (2005). Neural basis of food-seeking: Affect, arousal, and reward
in corticostratolimbic circuits. Physiology & Behavior, 86, 717730.
Bernstein, I. L. (1991). Aversion conditioning in response to cancer and cancer
treatment. Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 185191.
Bouton, M. E. (2004). Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learning
& Memory, 11, 485494.
Colwill, R. M., & Rescorla, R. A. (1986). Associative structures in instrumental
learning. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation,
(Vol. 20, pp. 55104). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Craske, M. G., Kircanski, K., Zelikowsky, M., Mystkowski, J., Chowdhury, N., &
Baker, A. (2008). Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 527.
Dickinson, A., & Balleine, B. W. (1994). Motivational control of goal-directed
behavior. Animal Learning & Behavior, 22, 118.
Fanselow, M. S., & Poulos, A. M. (2005). The neuroscience of mammalian
associative learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 207234.
Herrnstein, R. J. (1970). On the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
of Behavior, 13, 243266.
Holland, P. C. (2004). Relations between Pavlovian-instrumental transfer and
reinforcer devaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Spreat, S., & Spreat, S. R. (1982). Learning principles. In V. Voith & P. L. Borchelt
(Eds.), Veterinary clinics of North America: Small animal practice (pp. 593
606). Philadelphia, PA: W. B. Saunders.
Thompson, R. F., & Steinmetz, J. E. (2009). The role of the cerebellum in classical
conditioningof discrete behavioral responses. Neuroscience, 162, 732755.
Timberlake, W. L. (2001). Motivational modes in behavior systems. In R. R.
Mowrer & S. B. Klein (Eds.), Handbook of contemporary learning theories
(pp. 155210). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Wasserman, E. A. (1995). The conceptual abilities of pigeons. American Scientist,
83, 246255.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Conditioning and Learning by Mark E. Bouton
is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
Memory is a single term but it reflects a number of different abilitiesholding
information briefly while working with it (working memory), remembering
episodes of ones life (episodic memory), and our general knowledge of facts
of the world (semantic memory), among other types. Remembering episodes
involves three processes: encoding information (perceiving it and relating it to
past knowledge), storing it (maintaining it over time), and then retrieving it
(accessing the information when needed). Failures can occur at any stage,
leading to forgetting or to having false memories. The key to improving ones
memory is to improve processes of encoding and to use techniques that
guarantee effective retrieval. Good encoding techniques include relating new
information to what one already knows, forming mental images, and creating
associations among information that needs to be remembered. The key to good
retrieval is developing effective cues, ones that will lead the rememberer back
to the encoded information. Classic mnemonic systems, known since the time
of the ancient Greeks and still used by some today, can greatly improve ones
memory abilities.
Learning Objectives
Describe why the classic mnemonic device of the method of loci works so
well.
Introduction
On a spring day in 2013, Simon Reinhard sat in a room at Washington University
in St. Louis with about 60 people in the audience. The task given to the group
was to remember digits, in particular increasingly long series of digits. On the
first round, a computer generated 10 random digits6 1 9 4 8 5 6 3 7 1on a
screen for 10 seconds. After the series disappeared, Simon typed them into his
computer while those in the audience wrote them on sheets of paper and
checked them against Simons answers on the screen. (The computer checked
Simon. He was perfect.) Simon asked how many people got them all correct; a
smattering of hands went up. In the next phase, 20 digits appeared on the
screen for 20 seconds. Simon got them all correct again. No one in the audience
(mostly professors, graduate students, and undergraduate students) recalled
the 20 digits perfectly. Then came 30 digits studied for 30 seconds; once again,
Simon got them all correct and no one else did. For a final trial, 50 digits appeared
on the screen for 50 seconds and again Simon got them all right while the people
in the audience watched in amazement. Simon would have been happy to keep
goinghis record in this task, called forward digit spanis 240 digits (and in
the case where he set this record, the situation was a bit more difficult because
the digits were read aloud at a one-second rate rather than seen on a screen).
When most of us witness a performance like that of Simon Reinhard, we
think one of two things: First, maybe hes cheating somehow. (No, he is not.)
Second, Simon must have completely different abilities from the rest of
humankind. After all, psychologists established many years ago that the normal
memory span for adults is about 7 digits, with some of us able to get a few more
and others of us getting a few less (Miller, 1956). That is why the first phone
numbers were limited to 7 digitspsychologists determined that many errors
were caused (costing the phone company money) when the number is
increased to even 8 digits. But in normal testing, no one gets 50 digits correct
in a row, much less 240. So does Simon Reinhard simply have a photographic
nobaproject.com - Memory (Encoding, Storage, Retrieval)
996
memory? The answer is no. Simon has taught himself strategies for
remembering that have greatly increased his capacity for remembering digits
as well as for virtually any other type of materialwords, faces and names,
poetry, historical dates, and so on. Twelve years ago, before he started training
his memory abilities, he had a digit span of 7, just like most of us. Simon has
been training his abilities for about 10 years as of this writing and has risen to
be in the top two of memory athletes, as aficionados of this sport like to call
themselves. In 2012, he came in second in the World Memory Championships
(composed of 11 tasks) held in London. He currently ranks second in the world,
behind another German competitor, Johannes Mallow.
In this chapter, we reveal what psychologists and others have learned about
memory, and we will also give you the general principles by which you can
improve your own memory for factual material. At the end of the chapter, we
give you references that would permit you to learn much more about this topic
than we can cover in a brief chapter. First we provide an overview of types of
memory, because although we use a single termmemorywe often mean
quite different things by it.
Varieties of Memory
For most of us, remembering digits relies on short-term memory or working
memory, the ability to hold information in mind for a brief time and work with
it (e.g., multiplying 24 x 17 without using paper would rely on working memory).
Another type of memory is episodic memory, the ability to remember the
episodes of your life. If you were given the task of recalling everything you did
2 days ago, that would be a test of episodic memory; you would be required to
mentally travel through the day in your mind and note the main events of your
day. Semantic memory is your storehouse of more or less permanent
knowledge, the meanings of words in the language (e.g., the meaning of
parasol) and the huge collection of facts about the world (that Millard Fillmore
nobaproject.com - Memory (Encoding, Storage, Retrieval)
997
was a U.S. President and that Benjamin Franklin was not). Collective memory
refers to the kind of memory that people in a group share (whether family,
community, schoolmates, citizens of a state or a country). People born and
raised in Texas often share a strong Texan identity; they collectively remember
the Alamo, the Battle of San Jacinto, Sam Houston, the 12 years when Texas
was an independent country, the fabled Texas Rangers, and so on. These items
and others constitute the collective memory of Texans.
The kinds of memories listed in the previous paragraphs represent only
some of the distinctions that psychologists make. This chapter is largely about
episodic memory, remembering the events in ones life. We shall refer to
relatively recent events; remembering the events across the course of ones
entire life (e.g., your year in sixth grade) is usually referred to as
autobiographical memory. Psychologists debate the classification of types of
memories and which ones rely on other types (Tulving, 2007), but for this
chapter we will focus on episodic memory, which is the type of memory that
most people have in mind when they hear the word memory. For example,
when people say that an older relative is losing her memory due to Alzheimers
disease, the type of memory loss they observe is the ability to remember events
or episodic memory. Semantic memory is actually preserved in early-stage
Alzheimers disease.
998
recognize her face and have it serve as a cue to retrieve the name. Any successful
act of remembering requires that all three stages be intact. However, two types
of errors can also occur, as we are all too well aware. Forgetting is one type
you see the person you met at the party and you cannot recall her nameyou
just draw a blank. The other error is misremembering (false recall or false
recognition). You might see someone who looks like Lyn Goff and call the person
that name (false recognition of the face). Or you might see the real Lyn Goff,
recognize her face, but then call her the name of some other woman you met
at the party (misrecall her name).
Whenever forgetting or misremembering occurs, we can ask what stage in
the learning/memory process the failure occurred, although it is often difficult
to answer this question with precision. One reason for this ambiguity is that
the three stages are not as discrete as implied by our description above. Rather,
all three stages depend on one another. How we encode information
determines how it will be stored and what cues will be effective when we try to
retrieve it. The act of retrieval also changes the way information is subsequently
remembered, usually aiding later recall of the retrieved information. The central
point for now is that the three stages of encoding, storage, and retrieval affect
one another and are inextricably bound together.
Encoding
Encoding refers to the initial experience of perceiving and learning events.
Psychologists often study remembering by using tasks such as having students
study a list of pictures or words (although sentences, stories and videos are
also used). Encoding in these situations is usually straightforward, at least at
first glanceyou see some words but not others. However, in life encoding is
much more challenging. As you walk across campus, you see myriad sights and
soundsfriends pass by, as do strangers, people are off playing Frisbee on the
lawn, others are lying on the grass reading or talking, music is in the air and you
nobaproject.com - Memory (Encoding, Storage, Retrieval)
999
recognize the song, which leads you to remember having heard the same song
at a party last week. The physical and mental environments are much too rich
for you to encode all the happenings and sights and sounds around you or the
internal thoughts you have in response to those sights and sounds. So an
important first principle of encoding is that it is selective: You attend to some
events in your environment and you ignore others. A second point about
encoding is that it is promiscuousyou are always encoding the events of your
life; every day is filled with events that you encode and can remember, at least
for a while. We attend to the world, trying to understand it. Normally this
presents no problem, as our days are filled with routine happenings. But if
something happens that seems strangeduring your daily walk across campus,
you see a giraffe off to the sidethen you pay close attention and try to
understand why you are seeing what you are seeing. You may ask a friend and
discover that people from the local zoo are visiting and trying to drum up student
volunteers. The giraffe came with them to help attract attention to the cause.
After your typical walk across campus from your dorm to class (one without
appearance of a giraffe), you would be able to remember the events reasonably
well if you were asked. You could say whom you bumped into and to whom you
said hello, what song was playing from a radio and so on. However, suppose
someone asked you to recall this particular journey a month later. You would
not have a chance. You could doubtless recall a typical walk across campus, but
not this particular walk (unless something distinctive had happened, like the
appearance of the giraffe). Yet if you did see a giraffe while you were walking
across campus, you would remember that event for a long time, perhaps for
the rest of your life. You would tell your friends about it and on later occasions
when you saw a giraffe, you might be reminded of that day one was on campus.
Psychologists have long pinpointed distinctivenesshaving an event stand
out as quite different from a background of similar eventsas a key to
remembering events (Hunt, 2003). In addition, when vivid memories are tinged
with strong emotional content, they often seem to leave an indelible mark on
nobaproject.com - Memory (Encoding, Storage, Retrieval)
1000
us. People who ran the Boston Marathon in 2013 have doubtless developed
vivid memories of the event.
Even those of us who were not directly involved in those events may have
vivid memories of the events, including memories of having first heard about
the event. For example, you may remember how you heard that there had been
a bombing at the marathon (e.g., where you were, who told you). The term
flashbulb memory was originally coined by Brown and Kulik (1977) to indicate
this sort of vivid memory of finding out an important piece of news, where the
memory seems to have been captured with the vividness of a photograph
illuminated with a flash. The moment that people received the news seems
etched in memory with great clarity. Later research has shown that although
people have great confidence in their memories of such emotional, distinctive
events, accuracy is far from perfect (Talarico & Rubin, 2003), so the great
confidence is somewhat misplaced. Nonetheless, all other things being equal,
distinctive events are well remembered.
Events do not leap from the world into a persons mind. We might say that
we went to a party and remember it, but what we remember is (at best) what
we encoded. As noted above, the process of encoding is selective and in complex
situations, relatively few of many possible happenings are attended to and
encoded. The process of encoding always involves recodingthat is, taking the
information from one form as it is given to us and then converting in a way that
makes sense to us. For example, you might try to remember the names of the
U.S. Great Lakes by memorizing the acronym HOMES (Huron, Ontario, Michigan,
Erie, and Superior). The process of recoding the lakes into a single word can
help us to remember. However, recoding can also introduce errors when we
add material while encoding and then remember the new material as if it had
literally been presented (as discussed below).
nobaproject.com - Memory (Encoding, Storage, Retrieval)
1001
Psychologists have studied many recoding strategies that can be used during
study to improve retention. First, as we study, we should think of the meaning
of the events (Craik & Lockhart, 1972) and we should try to relate new events
to information we already know, to form associations that can help us retrieve
information later. Second, imagining events also makes them more memorable.
Creating vivid images of information (even verbal information) can greatly
improve later retention (Bower & Reitman, 1972). Imagery forms part of the
technique that Simon Reinhard uses to remember huge numbers of digits, but
we can all use images to encode information more effectively. The basic idea
behind good encoding tactics is to form distinctive memories (ones that stand
out) and to form links or associations among memories to help later retrieval.
Using these strategies together greatly aids retention of many sorts of material
(Hunt & McDaniel, 1993). It is effortful to use study strategies such as the ones
described here, but the effort is well worth it in terms of enhanced learning and
retention.
We emphasized earlier that encoding can be selectivepeople cannot
encode all information to which they are exposed. However, recoding can also
add information that was not seen or heard. Several of the techniques described
above, such as forming associations and making inferences, can happen
without our awareness. This is one reason why people can remember events
that were not literally there, because during the process of recoding we add the
events. One common way of inducing false memories in the laboratory employs
a word-list technique (Deese, 1959; Roediger & McDermott, 1995). Students
heard lists of 15 words like door, glass, pane, shade, ledge, sill, house, open,
curtain, frame, view, breeze, sash, screen, and shutter. After hearing many such
lists, students were given a test in which they got words from the lists (e.g., door,
pane, frame) and words unrelated to any of the lists. One of the words on the
test was window, which was not studied but which was related to the words
that were. That is, many of the words in the list are associated to window
although that word is not in the list. When subjects were tested, the correctly
nobaproject.com - Memory (Encoding, Storage, Retrieval)
1002
recognized the studied words (door, etc.) 72% of the time. That seems
reasonably accurate. However, when window was on the test, they falsely
recognized it as having been on the list 84% of the time (Stadler, Roediger, &
McDermott, 1999). The same happened with many other lists the authors used,
and the phenomenon is referred to as the DRM (for Deese-RoedigerMcDermott) effect. One idea to explain such results is that while students
listened to items in the list, the items often triggered the students to think about
window even though window was never presented. In this way, people seem
to encode events that were not actually a part of their experience.
Because humans are creative, we are always going beyond the information
we are givenwe associate events, we make inferences about what is
happening. Sometimes we can remember the inferences we make as if the
inferred statements had actually been experienced. Brewer (1977) gave people
sentences to remember that were designed to elicit pragmatic inferences.
Consider The baby stayed awake all night and The karate champion hit the
cinder block. After hearing or seeing such sentences and being given a memory
test later, students tended to remember the statements as having been The
baby cried all night and The karate champion broke the cinder block. These
remembered statements are not logical inferences in that it is perfectly possible
that a karate champion could hit a cinder block without breaking it. Nonetheless,
the pragmatic conclusion from hearing such a sentence is that likely the block
was broken. Similarly, babies do not tend to stay awake all night without a fair
amount of crying, although this conclusion, too, is a pragmatic inference. It may
well be the meaning the speaker intended to convey, but if one looks strictly at
what was said, there was no crying involved. The students remembered the
inference they made while hearing the sentence as if it had actually occurred
(see too McDermott & Chan, 2006).
nobaproject.com - Memory (Encoding, Storage, Retrieval)
1003
Storage
Every experience we have changes our brains. That seems a bold, maybe even
strange, claim at first sight, but it has to be true. We encode our experiences
and these experiences must be represented in the nervous system, so they
change it. Psychologists (and neurobiologists) say that experiences leave
memory traces or engrams (the two terms are synonyms). The basic idea is
that events create engrams through a process of consolidation, the neural
changes that occur after learning over time to create the memory trace of an
experience. Although neurobiologists are concerned with exactly what neural
processes change when memories are created, for psychologists the term
memory trace simply refers to the change in the nervous system that represents
our experience. The exact nature of that change is hard to discern by
psychological means alone (Tulving & Bower, 1975). Something must change
and persist over time to permit us to display our learning in the future, and the
term memory trace serves that function for psychologists.
Although the need for a concept like engram or memory trace is
indisputable, it is wrong to take the term too far. Psychologists and
neuroscientists sometimes have a tendency to do so, but it is important to
realize that memory traces are not little packets of information that lie dormant
in the brain, waiting to be called forward to give an accurate report of past
experience. We know this cannot be so, exactly, because remembering is often
imprecise and is fraught with error. Memory traces do not act like video or audio
recordings, capturing experience with great fidelity. Thus, it wrong to think that
remembering involves reading out a faithful record of past experience when
nobaproject.com - Memory (Encoding, Storage, Retrieval)
1004
1005
Retrieval
Retrieval of information is necessary for its use. If information is encoded and
stored but cannot be retrieved, it is of no use. This is one reason that Endel
Tulving argued that the key process in memory is retrieval (1991, p. 91). Why
should retrieval be given more prominence that encoding or storage? After all,
successful remembering involves all three processes. That is true, but the
bottleneck in learning and memory is the retrieval process. As discussed in
previous sections of the chapter, we basically encode and store nearly
everything to which we attendthousands of events, conversations, sights, and
sounds are encoded every day, creating memory traces (although no one knows
for sure how long they last). Most of this information is wasted, in the sense
that it will never be retrieved. We access in the future only a tiny part of what
we learned from our past, although those other events may shape our
knowledge in more subtle ways than in conscious retrieval. Most of our
memories will never be used, in the sense of brought back to mind consciously.
This fact seems so typical that we rarely reflect on it. All those events that
happened to you in the fourth grade that seemed so important then? Now,
many years later, you would struggle to remember even a few. Are traces of
the memories still there in some latent form? With currently available methods,
it is impossible to know, and it is difficult to see how this situation will change
significantly in the near future.
nobaproject.com - Memory (Encoding, Storage, Retrieval)
1006
1007
One proviso is that the cue cannot match too many other experiences
(Nairne, 2002; Watkins, 1975). Consider a lab experiment. Suppose you study
99 words and a single picture (of a penguin) is item 50 in the list. Afterwards,
the cue recall the picture would provoke penguin perfectly. No one would
miss it. However, if the word penguin were placed in the same spot with other
99 words, its recall would be only 2%3%. This outcome shows the power of
distinctiveness we discussed in the section on encodingone picture is
perfectly recalled from among 99 words because it stands out. Now consider
what would happen if the experiment were repeated but the number of pictures
in the 100-item list were increased from 1 to 5 and then 10 and then 25, and
so on. Although the picture of the penguin would still be in the list each time,
the probability of the cue recall the pictures as being useful for the penguin
picture would drop correspondingly. Watkins (1975) referred to this outcome
as reflecting the cue overload principleto be effective, a retrieval cue cannot
be overloaded with too many memories. For the cue recall the picture to be
effective, it is best if the cue only matches one item in the target set (as in the
one-picture, 99-word case).
To sum up the last two paragraphs, for a retrieval cue to be effective, a match
must exist between the cue and the desired target memory; further, the cuetarget relationship should be distinctive to produce the best retrieval. We will
see how the encoding specificity principle can work in practice in the next section
of the chapter.
Psychologists can measure memory performance by using production tests
(involving recall) or recognition tests (involving selection of correct from
incorrect information, as in a multiple-choice test or a true/false test). For
example, to go back to our example of people studying a list of 100 words, one
group could be asked to recall the list in any order (a test called free recall) and
on another test a different group of people might see the 100 studied words
mixed in with 100 nonstudied words and be asked to circle the old or studied
words in the list. In this situation, the recognition test would permit greater
nobaproject.com - Memory (Encoding, Storage, Retrieval)
1008
performance (more correct) than a recall test (although the problem of just
guessing is generally much more serious in recognition testswhat if a person
circled 175 items of the 200 items, or even circled all 200? They would get many
(or all of them) right but they would also get many wrong.
We usually think of recognition tests as being quite easy, because the cue
for retrieval represents a copy of the actual event that was presented for
studying. What could be a better cue than the exact target that the person is
trying to access? In many cases, this statement is true, but at the same time, a
recognition test does not provide a perfect index of what is stored in memory.
That is, you can fail to recognize a target staring you right in the face, but be
able to recall it later with a different set of cues (Watkins & Tulving, 1975).
Suppose I were to give you the task of recognizing the surnames of famous
American under two different conditions. In one case I gave you the actual last
names of people, whereas in the other case I give you a different cue. You might
think that the actual last name cue would always be best, but research has
shown that this is sometimes not so (Muter, 1984). For example, I might give
you names such as Franklin, Ross, Washington, and Bell. Subjects might well
say that Franklin and Washington are famous Americans, whereas Ross and
Bell are not. However, when given a cued recall test using first names, people
often recall items (produce them) that they had failed to recognize. For example,
a cue like Alexander Graham ________ will often lead to recall of Bell even
though people failed to recognize Bell as a famous American name. Yet when
given the cue George people may not come up with Washington because
George is a common name that matches many people (the cue overload
principle at work). This strange factthat recall can sometimes lead to better
performance than recognitioncan be explained by the encoding specificity
principle. Alexander Graham _________ as a cue matches the way the famous
inventor is stored in memory better than does his surname, Bell, by itself (even
though it is the target). Further, the match is quite distinctive with Alexander
Graham ___________ , but the cue George _________________ is much more
nobaproject.com - Memory (Encoding, Storage, Retrieval)
1009
1010
1011
2 is a shoe.
3 is a tree.
1012
4 is a door.
5 is knives.
6 is sticks.
7 is oven.
17 is penny-seven, go to heaven.
8 is plate.
9 is wine.
10 is hen.
It would probably take you less than 10 minutes to learn this list and practice
recalling it several times (remember to use retrieval practice!). If you took this
trouble, you would have a set of peg words on which you can hang memories,
so this mnemonic device is called the peg word technique. If you now need to
remember some discrete itemssay a grocery list or points you want to make
in a speech or simply a list of words you are giventhis method will let you do
so in a very precise yet flexible way. Suppose you had to remember bread,
peanut butter, bananas, lettuce, and so on. The way to use the method is to
form a vivid image of what you want to remember and imagine it interacting
with all your peg words (or as many as you need). For example, for these items,
you might imagine a large gun (the first peg word) shooting a loaf of bread, then
a jar of peanut butter inside a shoe, then large bunches of bananas hanging
from a tree, then a door slamming on a head of lettuce with leaves flying
everywhere. The idea is to provide good, distinctive encoding of the information
you need to remember while you are learning the information. If you do this,
then retrieval at some point later is relatively easy. You know your cues perfectly
(one is gun, etc.), so you simply go through your cue word list and look in your
minds eye at the image stored there (bread, in this case).
This peg word method sounds strange at first, but it works quite well even
with little training (Roediger, 1980). One proviso, though, is that the items to be
remembered need to be presented relatively slowly at first until you have
practice associating them to the cue word. People get faster with time. Another
interesting aspect of this technique is that it is just as easy to recall the items
nobaproject.com - Memory (Encoding, Storage, Retrieval)
1013
in backwards order as forwards; if you want to show off, you could recall the
odd-numbered items in a forward order and the even-numbered items in a
backwards order. The reason is that the 20 peg words provide direct access to
the memorized items.
How did Simon Reinhard remember those digits? Essentially he has a much
more complex system based on these same principles. In his case, he uses
memory palaces (elaborate scenes with discrete places) combined with huge
sets of images for digits. For example, imagine mentally walking through the
house or apartment where you grew up and identifying as many discrete scenes
as possible. Simon has hundreds of such memory palaces as this that he can
use. Next, for remembering digits, he has memorized a set of 10,000 images.
Every four-digit number for him immediately brings forth a mental image, so
6187 might be Michael Jackson. When Simon hears all the numbers coming at
him, he places an image of every four digits into the places in his memory palace.
He can do this at the incredibly rapid rate of faster than 4 digits per 4 seconds
when they are flashed visually, as in the demonstration at the beginning. As
noted, his record is 240 digits presented and recalled in their exact order. Simon
also holds the world record in the event called speed cards, which is memorizing
a random deck of cards. Simon was able to do this in 21.19 seconds! Again, he
uses his memory palaces and he encodes groups of cards into one image.
Many books exist on how to improve memory using mnemonic devices, but
all involve forming distinctive encoding operations and then having an infallible
set of memory cues. We should add that to develop and use these memory
systems beyond the basic peg system outlined above takes a great amount of
time and concentration. The World Memory Championships are held every year
and the records keep improving. However, for most common purposes, just
keep in mind that to remember well you need to encode information in a
distinctive way and to have good cues for retrieval. You can adapt a system that
will meet most any purpose.
1014
Outside Resources
Book: Brown, P.C., Roediger, H. L. & McDaniel, M. A. (2014). Smarter, sooner,
longer: Effective strategies for learning and remembering. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Discussion Questions
1. Mnemonists like Simon Reinhard develop mental journeys, which enable
them to use the method of loci. Develop your own journey, which contains
20 places, in order, that you know well. One example might be: the front
walkway to your parents apartment; their doorbell; the couch in their living
room; etc. Be sure to use a set of places that you know well and that have
a natural order to them (e.g., the walkway comes before the doorbell). Now
you are more than halfway toward being able to memorize a set of 20 nouns,
in order, rather quickly. As an optional second step, have a friend make a
list of 20 such nouns and read them to you, slowly (e.g., one every 5 seconds).
Use the method to attempt to remember the 20 items.
2. Recall a recent argument or misunderstanding you have had about memory
(e.g., a debate over whether your girlfriend/boyfriend had agreed to
something). In light of what you have just learned about memory, how do
you think about it? Is it possible that the disagreement can be understood
by one of you making a pragmatic inference?
3. Think about what youve just learned in this chapter and about how you
study for tests. On the basis of what you have just learned, is there
something that you want to try that might help your study habits?
Vocabulary
Autobiographical memory
Memory for the events of ones life.
Consolidation
The process occurring after encoding that is believed to stabilize memory traces.
Cue overload principle
The principle stating that the more memories that are associated to a particular
retrieval cue, the less effective the cue will be in prompting retrieval of any one
memory.
Distinctiveness
The principle that unusual events (in a context of similar events) will be recalled
and recognized better than uniform (nondistinctive) events.
Encoding specificity principle
The hypothesis that a retrieval cue will be effective to the extent that information
encoded from the cue overlaps or matches information in the engram or
memory trace.
Engrams
A term indicating the change in the nervous system representing an event; also,
memory trace.
Episodic memory
Memory for events in a particular time and place.
Flashbulb memory
Vivid personal memories of receiving the news of some momentous (and
usually emotional) event.
Memory traces
A term indicating the change in the nervous system representing an event.
Misinformation effect
When erroneous information occurring after an event is remembered as having
been part of the original event.
Mnemonic devices
A strategy for remembering large amounts of information, usually involving
imaging events occurring on a journey or with some other set of memorized
cues.
Recoding
The ubiquitous process during learning of taking information in one form and
converting it to another form, usually one more easily remembered.
Retrieval
The process of accessing stored information.
Retroactive interference
The phenomenon whereby events that occur after some particular event of
interest will usually cause forgetting of the original event.
Semantic memory
The more or less permanent store of knowledge that people have.
Storage
The stage in the learning/memory process that bridges encoding and retrieval;
the persistence of memory over time.
Reference List
Anderson, M. C., Bjork, R., & Bjork, E. L. (1994). Remembering can cause
forgetting:
Retrieval
dynamics
in
long-term
memory.
Journal
of
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation (Vol. 24, pp. 187188). New York:
Norton.
Pyc, M. A., & Rawson, K. A. (2009). Testing the retrieval effort hypothesis: Does
greater difficulty correctly recalling information lead to higher levels of
memory? Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 437447.
Roediger, H. L. (1980). The effectiveness of four mnemonics in ordering recall.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 558.
Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory
tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17, 249255.
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories:
Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental
Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 21, 803814.
Stadler, M. A., Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1999). Norms for word lists
that create false memories. Memory & Cognition, 27, 494500.
Talarico, J. M., & Rubin, D. C. (2003). Confidence, not consistency, characterizes
flashbulb memories. Psychological Science, 14, 455461.
Tulving, E. (2007). Are there 256 different kinds of memory? In J.S. Nairne (Ed.),
The foundations of remembering: Essays in honor of Henry L. Roediger, III
(pp. 3952). New York: Psychology Press.
Tulving, E. (1991). Interview. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 8994
Tulving, E., & Bower, G. H. (1975). The logic of memory representations. The
Abstract
This chapter explores the causes of everyday forgetting and considers
pathological forgetting in the context of amnesia. Forgetting is viewed as an
adaptive process that allows us to be efficient in terms of the information we
retain.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
Chances are that you have experienced memory lapses and been frustrated by
them. You may have had trouble remembering the definition of a key term on
an exam or found yourself unable to recall the name of an actor from one of
your favorite TV shows. Maybe you forgot to call your aunt on her birthday or
you routinely forget where you put your cell phone. Oftentimes, the bit of
information we are searching for comes back to us, but sometimes it does not.
Clearly, forgetting seems to be a natural part of life. Why do we forget? And is
forgetting always a bad thing?
Causes of Forgetting
One very common and obvious reason why you cannot remember a piece of
information is because you did not learn it in the first place. If you fail to encode
information into memory, you are not going to remember it later on. Usually,
encoding failures occur because we are distracted or are not paying attention
to specific details. For example, people have a lot of trouble recognizing an
actual penny out of a set of drawings of very similar pennies, or lures, even
though most of us have had a lifetime of experience handling pennies
(Nickerson & Adams, 1979). However, few of us have studied the features of a
penny in great detail, and since we have not attended to those details, we fail
to recognize them later. Similarly, it has been well documented that distraction
during learning impairs later memory (e.g., Craik, Govoni, Naveh-Benjamin, &
Anderson, 1996). Most of the time this is not problematic, but in certain
situations, such as when you are studying for an exam, failures to encode due
to distraction can have serious repercussions.
nobaproject.com - Forgetting and Amnesia
1028
1029
password hints favorite food and Moms birthday, you would easily be able
to retrieve it. Retrieval hints can bring back to mind seemingly forgotten
memories (Tulving & Pearlstone, 1966). One real-life illustration of the
importance of retrieval cues comes from a study showing that whereas people
have difficulty recalling the names of high school classmates years after
graduation, they are easily able to recognize the names and match them to the
appropriate faces (Bahrick, Bahrick, & Wittinger, 1975). The names are powerful
enough retrieval cues that they bring back the memories of the faces that went
with them. The fact that the presence of the right retrieval cues is critical for
remembering adds to the difficulty in proving that a memory is permanently
forgotten as opposed to temporarily unavailable.
Retrieval failures can also occur because other memories are blocking or
getting in the way of recalling the desired memory. This blocking is referred to
as interference. For example, you may fail to remember the name of a town
you visited with your family on summer vacation because the names of other
towns you visited on that trip or on other trips come to mind instead. Those
memories then prevent the desired memory from being retrieved. Interference
is also relevant to the example of forgetting a password: passwords that we
have used for other websites may come to mind and interfere with our ability
to retrieve the desired password. Interference can be either proactive, in which
old memories block the learning of new related memories, or retroactive, in
which new memories block the retrieval of old related memories. For both types
of interference, competition between memories seems to be key (Mensink &
Raaijmakers, 1988). Your memory for a town you visited on vacation is unlikely
to interfere with your ability to remember an Internet password, but it is likely
to interfere with your ability to remember a different towns name. Competition
between memories can also lead to forgetting in a different way. Recalling a
desired memory in the face of competition may result in the inhibition of related,
competing memories (Levy & Anderson, 2002). You may have difficulty recalling
the name of Kennebunkport, Maine, because other Maine towns, such as Bar
nobaproject.com - Forgetting and Amnesia
1030
Harbor, Winterport, and Camden, come to mind instead. However, if you are
able to recall Kennebunkport despite strong competition from the other towns,
this may actually change the competitive landscape, weakening memory for
those other towns names, leading to forgetting of them instead.
1031
Adaptive Forgetting
We have explored five different causes of forgetting. Together they can account
for the day-to-day episodes of forgetting that each of us experience. Typically,
we think of these episodes in a negative light and view forgetting as a memory
failure. Is forgetting ever good? Most people would reason that forgetting that
occurs in response to a deliberate attempt to keep an event out of mind is a
good thing. No one wants to be constantly reminded of falling on their face in
front of all of their friends. However, beyond that, it can be argued that forgetting
is adaptive, allowing us to be efficient and hold onto only the most relevant
memories (Bjork, 1989; Anderson & Milson, 1989). Shereshevsky, or S, the
mnemonist studied by Alexander Luria (1968), was a man who almost never
forgot. His memory appeared to be virtually limitless. He could memorize a
table of 50 numbers in under 3 minutes and recall the numbers in rows,
columns, or diagonals with ease. He could recall lists of words and passages
that he had memorized over a decade before. Yet Shereshevsky found it difficult
to function in his everyday life because he was constantly distracted by a flood
of details and associations that sprung to mind. His case history suggests that
remembering everything is not always a good thing. You may occasionally have
trouble remembering where you parked your car, but imagine if every time you
had to find your car, every single former parking space came to mind. The task
would become impossibly difficult to sort through all of those irrelevant
memories. Thus, forgetting is adaptive in that it makes us more efficient. The
price of that efficiency is those moments when our memories seem to fail us
(Schacter, 1999).
1032
Amnesia
Clearly, remembering everything would be maladaptive, but what would it be
like to remember nothing? We will now consider a profound form of forgetting
called amnesia that is distinct from more ordinary forms of forgetting. Most of
us have had exposure to the concept of amnesia through popular movies and
television. Typically, in these fictionalized portrayals of amnesia, a character
suffers some type of blow to the head and suddenly has no idea who they are
and can no longer recognize their family or remember any events from their
past. After some period of time (or another blow to the head), their memories
come flooding back to them. Unfortunately, this portrayal of amnesia is not
very accurate. What does amnesia typically look like?
The most widely studied amnesic patient was known by his initials H. M.
(Scoville & Milner, 1957). As a teenager, H. M. suffered from severe epilepsy,
and in 1953, he underwent surgery to have both of his medial temporal lobes
removed to relieve his epileptic seizures. The medial temporal lobes
encompass the hippocampus and surrounding cortical tissue. Although the
surgery was successful in reducing H. M.s seizures and his general intelligence
was preserved, the surgery left H. M. with a profound and permanent memory
deficit. From the time of his surgery until his death in 2008, H. M. was unable
to learn new information, a memory impairment called anterograde amnesia.
H. M. could not remember any event that occurred since his surgery, including
highly significant ones, such as the death of his father. He could not remember
a conversation he had a few minutes prior or recognize the face of someone
who had visited him that same day. He could keep information in his shortterm, or working, memory, but when his attention turned to something else,
that information was lost for good. It is important to note that H. M.s memory
impairment was restricted to declarative memory, or conscious memory for
facts and events. H. M. could learn new motor skills and showed improvement
on motor tasks even in the absence of any memory for having performed the
nobaproject.com - Forgetting and Amnesia
1033
1034
Conclusion
Just as the case study of the mnemonist Shereshevsky illustrates what a life
with a near perfect memory would be like, amnesiac patients show us what a
life without memory would be like. Each of the mechanisms we discussed that
explain everyday forgettingencoding failures, decay, insufficient retrieval
cues, interference, and intentional attempts to forgethelp to keep us highly
efficient, retaining the important information and for the most part, forgetting
the unimportant. Amnesiac patients allow us a glimpse into what life would be
like if we suffered from profound forgetting and perhaps show us that our
everyday lapses in memory are not so bad after all.
1035
Outside Resources
Web: Self-experiment, Penny demo
http://www.exploratorium.edu/exhibits/common_cents/
Web: The Brain Observatorys Project HM
http://thebrainobservatory.ucsd.edu/hm
Web: The Man Who Couldnt Remember
http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/nova/body/corkin-hm-memory.html
Discussion Questions
1. Is forgetting good or bad? Do you agree with the authors that forgetting is
an adaptive process? Why or why not?
2. Can we ever prove that something is forgotten? Why or why not?
3. Which of the five reasons for forgetting do you think explains the majority
of incidences of forgetting? Why?
4. How is real-life amnesia different than amnesia that is portrayed on TV and
in film?
Vocabulary
Anterograde amnesia
Inability to form new memories for facts and events after the onset of amnesia.
Consolidation
Process by which a memory trace is stabilized and transformed into a more
durable form.
Decay
The fading of memories with the passage of time.
Declarative memory
Conscious memories for facts and events.
Dissociative amnesia
Loss of autobiographical memories from a period in the past in the absence of
brain injury or disease.
Encoding
Process by which information gets into memory.
Interference
Other memories get in the way of retrieving a desired memory
Reference List
Anderson, J. R., & Milson, R. (1989). Human memory: An adaptive perspective. *
Psychological
Review*, 96, 703719.
Bahrick, H. P., Bahrick, P. O., & Wittinger, R. P. (1975). Fifty years of memory for
names and
faces: A cross-sectional approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 104,
5475.
Corkin, S. (2002). Whats new with the amnesic patient H. M.? *Nature Reviews
Neuroscience*, 3, 153160.
Craik, F. I. M., Govoni, R., Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Anderson, N. D. (1996). The
effects of
Hertel, P. T., & Calcaterra, G. (2005). Intentional forgetting benefits from thought
substitution.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 484489.
Levy, B. J., & Anderson, M. C. (2002). Inhibitory processes and the control of
memory retrieval.
Luria, A. R. (1968). The mind of a mnemonist: A little book about a vast memory
(L. Solataroff,
Trans.). New York: Basic Books.
Reed, J. M. & Squire, L. R. (1998). Retrograde amnesia for facts and events:
Findings from four
new cases. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 39433954.
Schacter, D. L. (1999). The seven sins of memory: Insights from psychology and
cognitive
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Forgetting and Amnesia by Nicole Dudukovic
and Brice Kuhl is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercialShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Topic 7
Social
Social Neuroscience
Tiffany A. Ito & Jennifer T. Kubota
University of Colorado Boulder, New York University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
This chapter provides an overview of the new field of social neuroscience, which
combines the use of neuroscience methods and theories to understand how
other people influence our thoughts, feelings, and behavior. The chapter
reviews research measuring neural and hormonal responses to understand
how we make judgments about other people and react to stress. Through these
examples, it illustrates how social neuroscience addresses three different
questions: (1) how our understanding of social behavior can be expanded when
we consider neural and physiological responses, (2) what the actual biological
systems are that implement social behavior (e.g., what specific brain areas are
associated with specific social tasks), and (3) how biological systems are
impacted by social processes.
Learning Objectives
Describe how measures of brain activity such as EEG and fMRI are used to
make inferences about social processes.
Discuss the ways in which other people can cause stress and also protect
us against stress.
Psychology has a long tradition of using our brains and body to better
understand how we think and act. For example, in 1939 Heinrich Kluver and
Paul Bucy removed (i.e. lesioned) the temporal lobes in some rhesus monkeys
and observed the effect on behavior. Included in these lesions was a subcortical
area of the brain called the amygdala. After surgery, the monkeys experienced
profound behavioral changes, including loss of fear. These results provided
initial evidence that the amygdala plays a role in emotional responses, a finding
that has since been confirmed by subsequent studies (Phelps & LeDoux, 2005;
Whalen & Phelps, 2009).
As such, social
neuroscience studies the same topics as social psychology, but does so from a
multilevel perspective that includes the study of the brain and body. Figure 1
shows the scope of social neuroscience with respect to the older fields of social
psychology and neuroscience. Although the field is relatively new the term
first appeared in 1992 (Cacioppo & Berntson, 1992) it has grown rapidly, thanks
to technological advances making measures of the brain and body cheaper and
more powerful than ever before, and to the recognition that neural and
physiological information are critical to understanding how we interact with
other people.
Social neuroscience can be thought of as both a methodological approach
(using measures of the brain and body to study social processes) and a
theoretical orientation (seeing the benefits of integrating neuroscience into the
study of social psychology). The overall approach in social neuroscience is to
nobaproject.com - Social Neuroscience
1049
1050
others.
The potential costs of social categorization make it important to understand
how social categorization occurs. Is it rare or does it occur often? Is it something
we can easily stop, or is it hard to override? One difficultly answering these
questions is that people are not always consciously aware of what they are
doing. In this case, we might not always realize when we are categorizing
someone. Another concern is that even when people are aware of their
behavior, they can be reluctant to accurately report it to an experimenter. In
the case of social categorization, subjects might worry they will look bad if they
accurately report classifying someone into a group associated with negative
stereotypes. For instance, many racial groups are associated with some negative
stereotypes, and subjects may worry that admitting to classifying someone into
one of those groups means they believe and use those negative stereotypes.
Social neuroscience has been useful for studying how social categorization
occurs without having to rely on self-report measures, instead measuring brain
activity differences that occur when people encounter members of different
social groups. Much of this work has recorded the electroencephalogram, or
EEG. EEG is a measure of electrical activity generated by the brains neurons.
Comparing this electrical activity at a given point in time against what a person
is thinking and doing at that same time allows us to make inferences about
brain activity associated with specific psychological states. One particularly nice
feature of EEG is that it provides very precise timing information about when
brain activity occurs. EEG is measured non-invasively with small electrodes that
rest on the surface of the scalp. This is often done with a stretchy elastic cap,
like the one shown in Figure 2, into which the small electrodes are sewn.
Researchers simply pull the cap onto the subjects head to get the electrodes
into place; wearing it is similar to wearing a swim cap. The subject can then be
asked to think about different topics or engage in different tasks as brain activity
is measured.
nobaproject.com - Social Neuroscience
1051
1052
to study using more traditional self-report measures. Using EEGs has, therefore,
been helpful in providing interesting new insights into social behavior.
1053
affect the alignment of the oxygen molecules within the blood (i.e., how they
are tilted). As the oxygen molecules move in and out of alignment with the
magnetic fields, their nuclei produce energy that can be detected with special
sensors placed close to the head. Recording fMRI involves having the subject
lay on a small bed that is then rolled into the scanner. While fMRI does require
subjects to lie still within the small scanner and the large magnets involved are
noisy, the scanning itself is safe and painless. Like EEG, the subject can then
be asked to think about different topics or engage in different tasks as brain
activity is measured. If we know what a person is thinking or doing when fMRI
detects a blood flow increase to a particular brain area, we can infer that part
of the brain is involved with the thought or action. fMRI is particularly useful
for identifying which particular brain areas are active at a given point in time.
The conclusion that the mPFC is associated with the self comes from studies
measuring fMRI while subjects think about themselves (e.g., saying whether
traits are descriptive of themselves). Using this knowledge, other researchers
have looked at whether the same brain area is active when people make
inferences about others. Mitchell, Neil Macrae, and Banaji (2005) showed
subjects pictures of strangers and had them judge either how pleased the
person was to have his or her picture taken or how symmetrical the face
appeared. Judging whether someone is pleased about being photographed
requires making an inference about someones internal feelings we call this
mentalizing. By contrast, facial symmetry judgments are based solely on
physical appearances and do not involve mentalizing. A comparison of brain
activity during the two types of judgments shows more activity in the mPFC
when making the mental versus physical judgments, suggesting this brain area
is involved when inferring the internal beliefs of others.
There are two other notable aspects of this study. First, mentalizing about
others also increased activity in a variety of regions important for many aspects
of social processing, including a region important in representing biological
motion (superior temporal sulcus or STS), an area critical for emotional
nobaproject.com - Social Neuroscience
1054
processing (amygdala), and a region also involved in thinking about the beliefs
of others (temporal parietal junction, TPJ) (Gobbini & Haxby, 2007; Schultz,
Imamizu, Kawato, & Frith, 2004) (Figure 4). This finding shows that a distributed
and interacting set of brain areas is likely to be involved in social processing.
Second, activity in the most ventral part of the mPFC (the part closer to the belly
rather than toward the top of the head), which has been most consistently
associated with thinking about the self, was particularly active when subjects
mentalized about people they rated as similar to themselves. Simulation is
thought to be most likely for similar others, so this finding lends support to the
conclusion that we use simulation to mentalize about others. After all, if you
encounter someone who has the same musical taste as you, you will probably
assume you have other things in common with him. By contrast, if you learn
that someone loves music that you hate, you might expect him to differ from
you in other ways (Srivastava, Guglielmo, & Beer, 2010). Using a simulation of
our own feelings and thoughts will be most accurate if we have reason to think
the persons internal experiences are like our own. Thus, we may be most likely
to use simulation to make inferences about others if we think they are similar
to us.
This research is a good example of how social neuroscience is revealing the
functional neuroanatomy of social behavior. That is, it tells us which brain
areas are involved with social behavior. The mPFC (as well as other areas such
as the STS, amygdala, and TPJ) is involved in making judgments about the self
and others. This research also provides new information about how inferences
are made about others. Whereas some have doubted the widespread use of
simulation as a means for making inferences about others, the activation of
the mPFC when mentalizing about others, and the sensitivity of this activation
to similarity between self and other, provides evidence that simulation occurs.
1055
1056
contribution of this work has been in understanding the conditions under which
other people can cause stress. In one study, Dickerson, Mycek, and Zaldivar
(2008) asked undergraduates to deliver a speech either alone or to two other
people. When the students gave the speech in front of others, there was a
marked increase in cortisol compared with when they were asked to give a
speech alone. This suggests that like chronic physical stress, everyday social
stressors, like having your performance judged by others, induces a stress
response. Interestingly, simply giving a speech in the same room with someone
who is doing something else did not induce a stress response. This suggests
that the mere presence of others is not stressful, but rather it is the potential
for them to judge us that induces stress.
Worrying about what other people think of us is not the only source of social
stress in our lives. Other research has shown that interacting with people who
belong to different social groups than us what social psychologists call
outgroup members can increase physiological stress responses. For example,
cardiovascular responses associated with stress like contractility of the heart
ventricles and the amount of blood pumped by the heart (what is called cardiac
output) are increased when interacting with outgroup as compared with
ingroup members (i.e., people who belong to the same social group we do)
(Mendes, Blascovich, Likel, & Hunter, 2002). This stress may derive from the
expectation that interactions with dissimilar others will be uncomfortable
(Stephan & Stephan, 1985) or concern about being judged as unfriendly and
prejudiced if the interaction goes poorly (Plant & Devine, 2003).
The research just reviewed shows that events in our social lives can be
stressful, but are social interactions always bad for us? No. In fact, while others
can be the source of much stress, they are also a major buffer against stress.
Research on social support shows that relying on a network of individuals in
tough times gives us tools for dealing with stress and can ward off loneliness
(Cacioppo & Patrick, 2008). For instance, people who report greater social
support show a smaller increase in cortisol when performing a speech in front
nobaproject.com - Social Neuroscience
1057
Conclusions
Human beings are intensely social creatures our lives are intertwined with
other people and our health and well-being depend on others. Social
neuroscience helps us to understand the critical function of how we make sense
of and interact with other people. This chapter provides an introduction to what
social neuroscience is and what we have already learned from it, but there is
much still to understand. As we move forward, one exciting future direction will
be to better understand how different parts of the brain and body interact to
produce the numerous and complex patterns of social behavior that humans
display. We hinted at some of this complexity when we reviewed research
showing that while the mPFC is involved in mentalizing, other areas such as the
STS, amygdala, and TPJ are as well. There are likely additional brain areas
involved as well, interacting in ways we do not yet fully understand. These brain
areas in turn control other aspects of the body to coordinate our responses
during social interactions. Social neuroscience will continue to investigate these
questions, revealing new information about how social processes occur, while
also increasing our understanding of basic neural and physiological processes.
1058
Outside Resources
Video: See a demonstration of fMRI data being collected.
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=lLORKtkf2n8
Video: See an example of EEG data being collected.
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=BKVv6v-Hd0A
Video: View two tasks frequently used in the lab to create stress giving a
speech in front of strangers, and doing math computations out loud in front
of others. Notice how some subjects show obvious signs of stress, but in
some situations, cortisol changes suggest that even people who appear calm
are experiencing a physiological response associated with stress.
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=aYI6lCeeT5g
Video: Watch a video used by Fritz Heider and Marianne Simmel in a landmark
study on social perception published in 1944. Their goal was to investigate
how we perceive other people, and they studied it by seeing how readily we
apply people-like interpretations to non-social stimuli.
http://intentionperception.org/wp-content/uploads/2013/02/Heider_Flash.swf
Discussion Questions
1. Categorizing someone as a member of a social group can activate group
stereotypes. EEG research suggests that social categorization occurs quickly
and often automatically. What does this tell us about the likelihood of
stereotyping occurring? How can we use this information to develop ways
to stop stereotyping from happening?
2. Watch this video, similar to what was used by Fritz Heider and Marianne
Simmel in a landmark study on social perception published in 1944, and
imagine telling a friend what happened in the video.
http://
intentionperception.org/wp-content/uploads/2013/02/Heider_Flash.swf. After
watching the video, think about the following: Did you describe the motion
of the objects solely in geometric terms (e.g., a large triangle moved from
the left to the right), or did you describe the movements as actions of
animate beings, maybe even of people (e.g., the circle goes into the house
and shuts the door)? In the original research, 33 of 34 subjects described
the action of the shapes using human terms. What does this tell us about
our tendency to mentalize?
3. Consider the types of things you find stressful. How many of them are social
in nature (e.g., are related to your interactions with other people)? Why do
you think our social relations have such potential for stress? In what ways
can social relations be beneficial and serve as a buffer for stress?
Vocabulary
Amygdala
A region located deep within the brain in the medial area (toward the center)
of the temporal lobes (parallel to the ears). If you could draw a line through
your eye sloping toward the back of your head and another line between your
two ears, the amygdala would be located at the intersection of these lines. The
amygdala is involved in detecting relevant stimuli in our environment and has
been implicated in emotional responses.
Automatic process
When a thought, feeling, or behavior occurs with little or no mental effort.
Typically, automatic processes are described as involuntary or spontaneous,
often resulting from a great deal of practice or repetition.
Cortisol
A hormone made by the adrenal glands, within the cortex. Cortisol helps the
body maintain blood pressure and immune function. Cortisol increases when
the body is under stress.
Electroencephalogram
A measure of electrical activity generated by the brains neurons.
Mentalizing
The act of representing the mental states of oneself and others. Mentalizing
allows humans to interpret the intentions, beliefs, and emotional states of
others.
Neuroendocrinology
The study of how the brain and hormones act in concert to coordinate the
physiology of the body.
Outgroup
A social group to which an individual does not identify or belong.
Simulation
Imaginary or real imitation of other peoples behavior or feelings.
Social categorization
The act of mentally classifying someone into a social group (e.g., as female,
elderly, a librarian).
Social support
A subjective feeling of psychological or physical comfort provided by family,
friends, and others.
Stereotypes
The beliefs or attributes we associate with a specific social group. Stereotyping
refers to the act of assuming that because someone is a member of a particular
group, he or she possesses the groups attributes. For example, stereotyping
occurs when we assume someone is unemotional just because he is man, or
particularly athletic just because she is African American.
Stress
A threat or challenge to our well-being. Stress can have both a psychological
component, which consists of our subjective thoughts and feelings about being
threatened or challenged, as well as a physiological component, which consists
of our bodys response to the threat or challenge (see fight or flight response).
Superior temporal sulcus
The sulcus (a fissure in the surface of the brain) that separates the superior
temporal gyrus from the middle temporal gyrus. Located in the temporal lobes
(parallel to the ears), it is involved in perception of biological motion or the
movement of animate objects.
Sympathetic nervous system
A branch of the autonomic nervous system that controls many of the bodys
internal organs. Activity of the SNS generally mobilizes the bodys fight or flight
response.
Temporal parietal junction
The area where the temporal lobes (parallel to the ears) and partial lobes (at
the top of the head toward the back) meet. This area is important in mentalizing
and distinguishing between the self and others.
Reference List
AlAbsi, M., Hugdahl, K., & Lovallo, W. (2002). Adrenocortical stress responses
and altered working
memory performance. Psychophysiology, 39(1), 9599.
Arnsten, A. F. T. (2009). Stress signaling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex
structure and function.
Nature Neuroscience Reviews, 10(6), 410422.
Black, P. (2002). Stress and the inflammatory response: A review of neurogenic
inflammation. *Brain,
Behavior, & Immunity, 16*, 622653.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1992). Social psychological contributions to
the decade of the brain: Doctrine of multilevel analysis. American
Psychologist, 47, 10191028.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Patrick, B. (2008). Loneliness: Human nature and the need for
social connection. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.
Carruthers, P. and Smith, P. (1996). Theories of Theories of Mind. New York, NY:
Cambridge University Press.
Davidson, R. J., Pizzagalli, D., Nitschke, J. B., & Putnam, K. (2002). Depression:
Perspectives from affective neuroscience. Annual Review of Psychology, 53,
545574.
Dickerson, S. S., Gable, S. L., Irwin, M. R., Aziz, N., & Kemeny, M. E. (2009). Socialevaluative threat and
proinflammatory
cytokine
regulation
an
experimental
laboratory
Kelley, W. M., Macrae, C. N., Wyland, C. L., Caglar, S., Inati, S., & Heatherton, T.
F. (2002). Finding the
self? An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14,
785794.
Lazarus, R. S., (1974). Psychological stress and coping in adaptation and illness. *
International Journal of
Psychiatry in Medicine, 5*, 321333.
Mendes, W. B., Blascovich, J., Lickel, B., & Hunter, S. (2002). Challenge and threat
during social interactions with White and Black men. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 28, 939952.
Mitchell, J. P., Neil Macrae, C., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). Forming impressions of
people versus inanimate
objects: social-cognitive processing in the medial prefrontal cortex.
Neuroimage, 26(1), 251257.
Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (2003). The antecedents and implications of interracial
anxiety. *Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29*, 790801.
Schultz, J., Imamizu, H., Kawato, M., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Activation of the human
superior temporal
gyrus during observation of goal attribution by intentional objects. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 16951705.
Srivastava, S., Guglielmo, S., & Beer, J. S. (2010). Perceiving others personalities:
Examining the
dimensionality, assumed similarity to the self, and stability of perceiver
effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 520.
Whalen, P. J., & Phelps, E. A. (2009). The human amygdala. New York, NY: The
Guilford Press.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Social Neuroscience by Tiffany A. Ito and Jennifer
T. Kubota is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
Social cognition is the area of social psychology that examines how people
perceive and think about their social world. This chapter provides an overview
of key topics within social cognition and attitudes, including judgmental
heuristics, social prediction, affective and motivational influences on judgment,
and explicit and implicit attitudes.
Learning Objectives
Learn
Understand
Determine
Understand
the
Introduction
Imagine that you are walking on the street and see a man push an elderly woman
to the ground. You might immediately ask yourself, Why did he do that? This
is because we know intuitively that we can better understand the behavior of
others if we know the thoughts that contributed to the behavior. The area of
social psychology that focuses on how people think about others and their
social world is called social cognition. Researchers in this area study how people
make sense of themselves and others to make judgments, form attitudes, and
make predictions about the future. Much of the research in social cognition has
demonstrated that people are adept at distilling large amounts of information
into smaller, more usable chunks and that they possess many cognitive tools
that allow them to efficiently navigate their environments. This research has
also illuminated the many social factors that can influence these judgments
and predictions. Not only can our past experiences, expectations, motivations,
and moods impact our reasoning, but many of our decisions and behaviors can
be driven by unconscious processes and implicit attitudes that we are unaware
of having. The goal of this chapter is to highlight the mental tools we use to
navigate and make sense of a complex social world and describe some of the
emotional, motivational, and cognitive factors that affect our reasoning.
1073
that we develop from encounters with the person, object, or event, as well as
from information that we glean from secondhand sources. Rather than
spending copious amounts of time learning about each individual object that
we encounter (e.g., each new dog that we see), we can rely on our schemas to
tell us that a newly encountered dog probably barks, likes to run, and enjoys
dog treats. In this way, our schemas greatly reduce the amount of cognitive
work that we need to do and allow us to go beyond the information given
(Bruner, 1957).
We can hold schemas about almost any entityindividual people (person
schemas), ourselves (self-schemas), and recurring events (event schemas or
scripts). Each of these types of schemas is useful in their own way. For example,
event schemas allow us to navigate new situations in an efficient and seamless
manner. A script for dining at a restaurant would indicate that one should wait
to be seated by the host or hostess, that food should be ordered from a menu,
and that one is expected to pay the check at the end of the meal. Because the
majority of dining situations conform to this general format, most diners just
need to follow their scripts to know what is expected of them and how they
should behave, which greatly reduces their cognitive workload.
Another important way in which we simplify our social world is by employing
heuristics, which are mental shortcuts that reduce complex problem-solving
to more simple rule-based decisions. A common decision that people are faced
with is judging whether an object belongs to a particular category. To make this
classification, people may rely on the representativeness heuristic to arrive at
a quick decision (Kahneman & Tversky, 1972, 1973). Rather than engaging in
an in-depth consideration of the objects attributes, one can simply judge the
likelihood of the object belonging to the category based on the extent to which
the object appears similar to ones mental representation of the category. For
example, a perceiver may quickly judge a female to be a librarian based on the
fact that the female appears to be quiet, bookish, and introverted, which fits
the perceivers representation of what librarians are like. In many situations, an
nobaproject.com - Social Cognition and Attitudes
1074
1075
as their own future thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, and how these predictions
can impact their decisions.
1076
predict occupational choice than career inventories (Shrauger & Osberg, 1981).
Yet, it is not always the case that we hold greater insight into ourselves. While
self-reported personality traits predict certain behavioral tendencies to a
greater extent than a peers personality report of oneself, for certain behaviors,
peer reports are more accurate than self-reports (Kolar, Funder, & Colvin, 1996;
Vazire, 2010). Similarly, although we are generally aware of our knowledge, our
abilities, and our future actions, these perceptions are often overly positive,
and we display overconfidence in their accuracy (Metcalfe, 1998). For example,
we underestimate how much time it will take us to complete a task, whether it
is writing a paper, finishing a project at work, or building a bridgea
phenomenon known as the planning fallacy (Buehler, Griffin, & Ross, 1994).
The planning fallacy helps explain why so many college students end up pulling
all-nighters to finish writing assignments or study for exams. The tasks simply
end up taking longer than expected. On the positive side, the planning fallacy
can also lead individuals to pursue ambitious projects that may turn out to be
worthwhile, but would have been avoided had they known just how long it
would take.
The other important factor that affects our decision-making is our ability to
predict how we will feel about certain outcomes. In addition to predicting
whether we will feel positively or negatively, we also make predictions about
how strongly and for how long we will feel that way. Research demonstrates
that these predictions of ones future feelingsknown as affective forecasting
are accurate in some ways and limited in others (Gilbert & Wilson, 2007). We
are adept at predicting whether an event or future situation will make us feel
positively or negatively (Wilson & Gilbert, 2003), but often incorrectly predict
the strength or duration of those emotions. Predictions about future feelings
are influenced by the impact bias, the tendency for a person to overestimate
the intensity of their future feelings. For example, from comparing peoples
estimates of how they would feel after a specific event to the actual feelings of
people who have experienced this event, we know that people overestimate
nobaproject.com - Social Cognition and Attitudes
1077
how badly they would feel after a negative event, such as losing a job and
overestimate how happy they would feel after a positive event, such as winning
the lottery (Brickman, Coates, & Janoff-Bullman, 1978). People also exhibit a
durability bias. The durability bias refers to the tendency for people to
overestimate for how long positive and negative events will affect them. This
bias is much greater for predictions regarding negative events than positive
events and occurs because people are generally unaware of the many
psychological mechanisms that help them adapt to and cope with these events
(Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg, & Wheatley, 1998; Wilson, Wheatley, Meyers,
Gilbert, & Axsom, 2000).
In summary, individuals form impressions of themselves and others, make
predictions about the future, and use these judgments to inform their decisions.
However, these judgments are shaped by the tendency to view ourselves in an
overly positive light and our inability to appreciate the extent to which we
habituate to both positive and negative events. In the next section, we will
discuss how motivations, moods, and desires also shape social judgment.
1078
1079
memory (Blaney, 1986; Bower 1981, 1991; DeSteno, Petty, Wegener, & Rucker,
2000; Forgas, Bower, & Krantz, 1984; Schwarz, Strack, Kommer, & Wagner, 1987).
The mood that we were in when the memory was recorded becomes a retrieval
cue so that our present mood primes these congruent memories, making them
come to mind more easily (Fiedler, 2001). Furthermore, because the availability
of events can affect their perceived frequency (the availability heuristic), the
biased retrieval of congruent memories can then impact the subsequent
judgments we make (Tversky & Kahneman, 1973). For example, if you are
retrieving many sad memories, you might conclude that you have had a tough,
depressing life.
In addition to our moods influencing the specific memories that we retrieve,
our moods can also influence global judgments that we make. This can
sometimes lead to inaccuracies when our current mood is irrelevant to the
judgment at hand. In a classic study demonstrating this effect, the researchers
found that study participants rated themselves as less satisfied with their lives
in general if they were asked on a day when it happened to be raining versus
sunny (Schwarz & Clore, 1983). However, this occurred only if the participants
were not aware of how the weather might be influencing their mood. In essence,
participants were in worse moods on rainy days than sunny days, and, if
unaware of the weather, participants incorrectly used their mood as evidence
of their overall life satisfaction.
In summary, our mood and motivations can influence both the way we think
and the decisions that we ultimately make. Mood can shape our thinking even
when the mood is irrelevant to the judgment, and our motivations can influence
our thinking even if we have no particular preference as to the outcome. Just
as we might be unaware of how our reasoning is influenced by our motives and
moods, research has found that our behaviors can be determined by
unconscious processes rather than intentional decisions, an idea we will explore
in the next section.
1080
Automaticity
Do we actively choose and control all our behaviors or do some of these
behaviors occur automatically? A large body of evidence now suggests that
many of our behaviors are, in fact, automatic. A behavior or process is
considered automatic if it is unintentional, uncontrollable, occurs outside of
conscious awareness, or is cognitively efficient (Bargh & Chartrand, 1999). A
process may be considered automatic even if it does not have all these features;
for example, driving is a fairly automatic process, but is clearly intentional.
Processes can become automatic through repetition, practice, or repeated
associations. For example, although driving can be very difficult and cognitively
effortful at the start, over time it becomes a relatively automatic process and
components of it can occur outside conscious awareness.
In addition to practice leading to the learning of automatic behaviors, some
automatic processes, such as fear responses, appear to be innate. For example,
people quickly detect negative stimuli, such as negative words, even when those
stimuli are presented subliminally (Dijksterhuis & Aarts, 2003; Pratto & John,
1991). This could represent an evolutionarily adaptive response that makes
individuals more likely to detect danger in their environment. Other innate
automatic processes may have evolved due to their pro-social outcomes. The
chameleon effectwhere individuals nonconsciously mimic the postures,
mannerisms, facial expressions, and other behaviors of their interaction
partnersis an example of how people may engage in certain behaviors
without conscious intention or awareness (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). Automatic
mimicry has been shown to lead to more positive social interactions and to
increase liking between the mimicked person and the mimicking person.
When concepts and behaviors have been repeatedly associated with each
other, the concept or behavior can be primedmade more cognitively
accessibleby exposing participants to the strongly associated concept. For
example, by presenting participants with the concept of a doctor, associated
nobaproject.com - Social Cognition and Attitudes
1081
1082
1083
those reaction times. One common implicit measure is the Implicit Association
Test (IAT;Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998),
which measures how quickly the participant can pair a concept (e.g., black and
white) with an attribute (e.g., good or bad). The participants response time in
pairing the concept with the attribute indicates how strongly they associate the
two. Another common implicit measure is the evaluative priming task(Fazio,
Jackson, Dunton, & Williams, 1995), which measures how quickly the participant
can label the valence of the attitude object when it appears immediately after
a positive or negative image. The more quickly a participant can label the
attitude object after being primed with a positive versus negative image
indicates how positively they evaluate the object.
Individuals implicit attitudes are sometimes inconsistent with their explicitly
held attitudes. Hence, implicit measures may reveal biases that participants do
not report on explicit measures. As a result, implicit attitude measures are
especially useful for examining the pervasiveness and strength of controversial
attitudes and stereotypic associations, such as racial biases or associations
between race and violence. For example, research using the IAT has shown that
about 66% of white respondents have a negative bias toward blacks (Nosek,
Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002), that bias on the IAT against Blacks is associated
with more discomfort during an interracial interaction (McConnell, & Leibold,
2001), and that implicit associations linking Blacks to violence are associated
with a greater tendency to shoot unarmed Black targets in a video game (Payne,
2001). Thus, even though individuals are often unaware of their implicit
attitudes, these attitudes can have serious implications for their behavior,
especially when these individuals do not have the cognitive resources available
to override the attitudes influence.
1084
Conclusion
Decades of research on social cognition and attitudes has examined many of
the tricks and tools that we use to efficiently process the limitless amounts
of social information that we encounter in our lives. These tools are quite useful
for organizing the information that we encounter and arriving at quick
decisions. When you see an individual engage in a behavior, such as seeing a
man push an elderly lady to the ground, you form judgments about his
personality, predictions about the likelihood of him engaging in similar
behaviors in the future, as well as predictions about the elderly womans feelings
and how you would feel if you were in her position. As the research presented
in this chapter demonstrates, we are adept and efficient at making these
judgments and predictions, but these judgments are not made in a vacuum.
Ultimately, our perception of the social world is a subjective experience, and,
consequently, our decisions are influenced by our experiences, expectations,
emotions, motivations, and current contexts. By being aware of when our
judgments are most accurate and how our judgments are shaped by these
social influences, we are in a much better position to appreciate, and potentially
counter, their effects.
1085
Outside Resources
Video: Daniel Gilbert discussing affective forecasting.
http://www.dailymotion.com/video/xebnl3_dan-gilbert-on-what-affective-forec_people#.
UQlwDx3WLm4
Video: Focus on heuristics.
http://education-portal.com/academy/lesson/heuristics.html
Web: BBC Horizon documentary How to Make Better Decisions that discusses
many chapter topics (Part 1).
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ul-FqOfX-t8
Web: Implicit Attitudes Test.
https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/
Discussion Questions
1. Describe your event-schema, or script, for an event that you encounter
regularly (e.g., dining at a restaurant). Now, attempt to articulate a script for
an event that you have encountered only once or a few times. How are these
scripts different? How confident are you in your ability to navigate these
two events?
2. Think of a time when you made a decision that you thought would make
you very happy (e.g., purchasing an item). To what extent were you accurate
or inaccurate? In what ways were you wrong, and why do you think you
were wrong?
3. What is an issue you feel strongly about (e.g., abortion, death penalty)? How
would you react if research demonstrated that your opinion was wrong?
What would it take before you would believe the evidence?
4. Take an implicit association test at the Project Implicit website (https://
implicit.harvard.edu/implicit). How do your results match or mismatch your
explicit attitudes.
Vocabulary
Affective forecasting
Predicting how one will feel in the future after some event or decision.
Attitude
A psychological tendency that is expressed by evaluating a particular entity with
some degree of favor or disfavor.
Automatic
A behavior or process has one or more of the following features: unintentional,
uncontrollable, occurring outside of conscious awareness, and cognitively
efficient.
Availability heuristic
A heuristic in which the likelihood of an object belonging to a category is
evaluated based on the extent to which the object appears similar to ones
mental representation of the category.
Chameleon effect
The tendency for individuals to nonconsciously mimic the postures,
mannerisms, facial expressions, and other behaviors of ones interaction
partners.
Directional goals
Directional goals
Durability bias
A bias in affective forecasting in which one underestimates for how long one
will feel an emotion after some event.
Explicit attitude
An attitude that is consciously held and can be reported on by the person
holding the attitude.
Heuristics
A mental shortcut or rule of thumb that reduces complex mental problems to
more simple rule-based decisions.
Hot cognition
The mental processes that are influenced by desires and feelings.
Impact bias
A bias in affective forecasting in which one underestimates the strength or
intensity of emotion one will experience after some event.
Implicit attitude
An attitude that a person cannot verbally or overtly state.
Mood-congruent memory
The tendency to be better able to recall memories that have a mood similar to
our current mood.
Motivated skepticism
A form of bias that can result from having a directional goal in which one is
skeptical of evidence despite its strength because it goes against what one
wants to believe.
Planning fallacy
A cognitive bias in which one underestimates how long it will take to complete
a task.
Primed
A process by which a concept or behavior is made more cognitively accessible
or likely to occur through the presentation of an associated concept.
Representativeness heuristic
A heuristic in which the frequency or likelihood of an event is evaluated based
on how easily instances of it come to mind.
Schema
A mental model or representation that organizes the important information
about a thing, person, or event (also known as a script).
Social cognition
The study of how people think about the social world.
Stereotypes
Our general beliefs about the traits or behaviors shared by group of people.
Reference List
Ambady, N., Bernieri, F. J., & Richeson, J. A. (2000). Toward a histology of social
behavior: Judgmental accuracy from thin slices of the behavioral stream.
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 201271. San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Ambady, N., Hallahan, M., & Conner, B. (1999). Accuracy of judgments of sexual
orientation from thin slices of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 77, 538547.
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1993). Half a minute: Predicting teacher evaluations
from thin slices of nonverbal behavior and physical attractiveness. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 431441.
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being.
American Psychologist, 54, 462479.
Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 99,
229246.
Bower, G. H. (1991). Mood congruity of social judgments. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.),
Emotion and social judgments (pp. 3153). New York, NY: Pergamon.
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129148.
Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bullman, R. (1978). Lottery winners and
accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 36, 917927.
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes (p. 1). Fort Worth,
TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers.
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in
automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona
fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 10131027.
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research:
Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297327.
Fiedler, K. (2001). Affective influences on social information processing. In J. P.
Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 163185).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Forgas, J. P., Bower, G. H., & Krantz, S. (1984). The influence of mood on
perceptions of social interactions. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 20, 497513.
Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. (1998).
Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 617638.
Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: Experiencing the future.
Science, 317, 13511354.
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. K. L. (1998). Measuring individual
differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 14641480.
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G., & Tannenbaum, P. (1957). The measurement of meaning.
Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.
Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and
controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 81, 181192.
Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power
of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
61, 380391.
Schwarz, N., Strack, F., Kommer, D., & Wagner, D. (1987). Soccer, rooms, and the
quality of your life: Mood effects on judgments of satisfaction with life in
general and with specific domains. Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 6979.
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of wellbeing: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513523.
Shrauger, J. S., & Osberg, T. M. (1981). The relative accuracy of self-predictions
and judgments by others in psychological assessment. Psychological
Bulletin, 90, 322351.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency
and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207232.
Vazire, S. (2010). Who knows what about a person? The self-other asymmetry
(SOKA) model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 281300.
Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1997). Cognitive and social consequences
of the need for cognitive closure. European Review of Social Psychology, 18,
133173.
Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T. P., Meyers, J. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Axsom, D. (2000).
Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 821836.
Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. Advances in
Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 345411.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Social Cognition and Attitudes by Yanine D. Hess
and Cynthia L. Pickett is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercialShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
This chapter introduces several major principles in the process of persuasion.
It offers an overview of the different paths to persuasion. It then describes how
mindless processing makes us vulnerable to undesirable persuasion and some
of the tricks that may be used against us.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
Persuasion has been defined as the process by which a message induces
change in beliefs, attitudes, or behaviors (Myers, 2011). It can take a multitude
of forms with profoundly different consequences. It may, for example, differ in
whether it targets public compliance or private acceptance, is short-term or
long-term, whether it involves slowly escalating commitments or sudden
interventions and, most of all, in the benevolence of its intentions. When
persuasion is well-meaning, we might call it education. When it is manipulative,
it might be called mind control (Levine, 2003).
Whatever the content, however, there is a similarity to the form of the
process itself. As the advertising commentator Sid Bernstein once observed,
Of course, you sell candidates for political office the same way you sell soap
or sealing wax or whatever; because, when you get right down to it, thats the
only way anything is sold (Levine, 2003).
Persuasion is one of the most studied of all social psychology phenomena.
This chapter provides an introduction to several of its most important
components.
1102
The peripheral route relies on superficial cues that have little to do with
logic. The peripheral approach is the salesmans way of thinking. It requires a
target who isnt thinking carefully about what you are saying. It requires low
effort from the target and often exploits rule-of-thumb heuristics that trigger
mindless reactions (see below). It may be intended to persuade you to do
something you do not want to do and might later be sorry you did.
Advertisements, for example, may show celebrities, cute animals, beautiful
scenery, or provocative sexual images that have nothing to do with the product.
The peripheral approach is also common in the darkest of persuasion programs,
such as those of dictators and cult leaders. Adolph Hitler, who developed
perhaps the most horrific of propaganda machines, observed that the message
must be limited to a very few points and must harp on these in slogans until
the last member of the public understands.
1103
1104
Authority
From earliest childhood, we learn to rely on authority figures for sound decision
making because their authority signifies status and power, as well as expertise.
These two facets often work together. Authorities such as parents and teachers
are not only our primary sources of wisdom while we grow up, but they control
us and our access to the things we want. In addition, we have been taught to
believe that respect for authority is a moral virtue. As adults, it is natural to
transfer this respect to societys designated authorities, such as judges, doctors,
bosses, and religious leaders. We assume their positions give them special
nobaproject.com - Persuasion: So Easily Fooled
1105
access to information and power. Usually we are correct, so that our willingness
to defer to authorities becomes a convenient shortcut to sound decision
making. Uncritical trust in authority may, however, lead to bad decisions.
Perhaps the most famous study ever conducted in social psychology
demonstrated that, when conditions were set up just so, two-thirds of a sample
of psychologically normal men were willing to administer potentially lethal
shocks to a stranger when an apparent authority in a laboratory coat ordered
them to do so (Milgram, 1974; Burger, 2009).
Uncritical trust in authority can be problematic for several reasons. First,
even if the source of the message is a legitimate, well-intentioned authority,
they may not always be correct. Second, when respect for authority becomes
mindless, expertise in one domain may be confused with expertise in general.
To assume there is credibility when a successful actor promotes a cold remedy,
or when a psychology professor offers his views about politics, can lead to
problems. Third, the authority may not be legitimate. It is not difficult to fake a
college degree or professional credential or to buy an official-looking badge or
uniform.
Honesty
Honesty is the moral dimension of trustworthiness. Persuasion professionals
have long understood how critical it is to their efforts. Marketers, for example,
dedicate exorbitant resources to developing and maintaining an image of
honesty. A trusted brand or company name becomes a mental shortcut for
consumers. It is estimated that some 50,000 new products come out each year.
Forrester Research, a marketing research company, calculates that children
have seen almost six million ads by the age of 16. An established brand name
helps us cut through this volume of information. It signals we are in safe
territory. The real suggestion to convey, advertising leader Theodore
MacManus observed in 1910, is that the man manufacturing the product is an
nobaproject.com - Persuasion: So Easily Fooled
1106
honest man, and the product is an honest product, to be preferred above all
others (Fox, 1997).
Likability
If we know that celebrities arent really experts, and that they are being paid to
say what theyre saying, why do their endorsements sell so many products?
Ultimately, it is because we like them. More than any single quality, we trust
people we like. Roger Ailes, a public relations adviser to Presidents Reagan and
George H.W. Bush, observed: If you could master one element of personal
communication that is more powerful than anything . . . it is the quality of being
likeable. I call it the magic bullet, because if your audience likes you, theyll
forgive just about everything else you do wrong. If they dont like you, you can
hit every rule right on target and it doesnt matter.
The mix of qualities that make a person likable are complex and often do
not generalize from one situation to another. One clear finding, however, is that
physically attractive people tend to be liked more. In fact, we prefer them to a
disturbing extent: Various studies have shown we perceive attractive people as
smarter, kinder, stronger, more successful, more socially skilled, better poised,
better adjusted, more exciting, more nurturing, and, most important, of higher
moral character. All of this is based on no other information than their physical
appearance (e.g., Dion, Berscheid, & Walster, 1972).
1107
1108
only preferred the toy cars more but were convinced the endorser was an expert
about the toys. This held true for children of all ages. In addition, they believed
the toy race cars were bigger, faster, and more complex than real race cars they
saw on film. They were also less likely to believe the commercial was staged
(Ross et al., 1984).
Word of Mouth
Imagine you read an ad that claims a new restaurant has the best food in your
city. Now, imagine a friend tells you this new restaurant has the best food in
the city. Who are you more likely to believe? Surveys show we turn to people
around us for many decisions. A 1995 poll found that 70% of Americans rely on
personal advice when selecting a new doctor. The same poll found that 53% of
moviegoers are influenced by the recommendation of a person they know. In
another survey, 91% said theyre likely to use another persons recommendation
when making a major purchase.
nobaproject.com - Persuasion: So Easily Fooled
1109
The Maven
More persuasive yet, however, is to involve peers face-to-face. Rather than
overinvesting in formal advertising, businesses and organizations may plant
seeds at the grassroots level hoping that consumers themselves will then spread
the word to each other. The seeding process begins by identifying so-called
information hubsindividuals the marketers believe can and will reach the
most other people.
The seeds may be planted with established opinion leaders. Software
companies, for example, give advance copies of new computer programs to
professors they hope will recommend it to students and colleagues.
Pharmaceutical companies regularly provide travel expenses and speaking fees
to researchers willing to lecture to health professionals about the virtues of
their drugs. Hotels give travel agents free weekends at their resorts in the hope
theyll later recommend them to clients seeking advice.
There is a Yiddish word, maven, which refers to a person whos an expert or
a connoisseur, as in a friend who knows where to get the best price on a sofa
or the co-worker you can turn to for advice about where to buy a computer.
They (a) know a lot of people, (b) communicate a great deal with people, (c) are
more likely than others to be asked for their opinions, and (d) enjoy spreading
the word about what they know and think. Most important of all, they are
trusted. As a result, mavens are often targeted by persuasion professionals to
nobaproject.com - Persuasion: So Easily Fooled
1110
Reciprocity
There is no duty more indispensable than that of returning a kindness, wrote
Cicero. Humans are motivated by a sense of equity and fairness. When someone
does something for us or gives us something, we feel obligated to return the
favor in kind. It triggers one of the most powerful of social norms, the reciprocity
rule, whereby we feel compelled to repay, in equitable value, what another
nobaproject.com - Persuasion: So Easily Fooled
1111
Social Proof
If everyone is doing it, it must be right. People are more likely to work late if
others on their team are doing the same, to put a tip in a jar that already contains
money, or eat in a restaurant that is busy. This principle derives from two
extremely powerful social forcessocial comparison and conformity. We
compare our behavior to what others are doing and, if there is a discrepancy
between the other person and ourselves, we feel pressure to change (Cialdini,
2008).
The principle of social proof is so common that it easily passes unnoticed.
Advertisements, for example, often consist of little more than attractive social
models appealing to our desire to be one of the group (They pick Pepsi, time
after time). Sometimes social cues are presented with such specificity that it
is as if the target is being manipulated by a puppeteerfor example, the laugh
tracks on situation comedies that instruct one not only when to laugh but how
to laugh. Studies find these techniques work. Fuller and Skeehy-Skeffington
(1974), for example, found that audiences laughed longer and more when a
laugh track accompanied the show than when it did not, even though
nobaproject.com - Persuasion: So Easily Fooled
1112
respondents knew the laughs they heard were connived by a technician from
old tapes that had nothing to do with the show they were watching. People are
particularly susceptible to social proof (a) when they are feeling uncertain, and
(b) if the people in the comparison group seem to be similar to ourselves. As P.
T. Barnum once said, Nothing draws a crowd like a crowd.
1113
1114
1115
1116
Sagarin and his colleagues have developed a more aggressive version of this
technique that they refer to as stinging (Sagarin, Cialdini, Rice, & Serna, 2002).
Their studies focused on the popular advertising tactic whereby well-known
authority figures are employed to sell products they know nothing about, for
example, ads showing a famous astronaut pontificating on Rolex watches. In a
first experiment, they found that simply forewarning people about the
deviousness of these ads had little effect on peoples inclination to buy the
product later. Next, they stung the subjects. This time, they were immediately
confronted with their gullibility. Take a look at your answer to the first question.
Did you find the ad to be even somewhat convincing? If so, then you got fooled. ...
Take a look at your answer to the second question. Did you notice that this
stockbroker was a fake? They were then asked to evaluate a new set of ads.
The sting worked. These subjects were not only more likely to recognize the
manipulativeness of deceptive ads; they were also less likely to be persuaded
by them.
Anti-vulnerability trainings such as these can be helpful. Ultimately, however,
the most effective defense against unwanted persuasion is to accept just how
vulnerable we are. One must, first, accept that it is normal to be vulnerable and,
second, to learn to recognize the danger signs when we are falling prey. To be
forewarned is to be forearmed.
Conclusion
This chapter has provided a brief introduction to the psychological processes
and subsequent tricks involved in persuasion. It has emphasized the
peripheral route of persuasion because this is when we are most vulnerable to
psychological manipulation. These vulnerabilities are side effects of normal
and usually adaptive psychological processes. Mindless heuristics offer
shortcuts for coping with a hopelessly complicated world. They are necessities
for human survival. All, however, underscore the dangers that accompany any
nobaproject.com - Persuasion: So Easily Fooled
1117
mindless thinking.
1118
Outside Resources
Book: Ariely, D. (2008). Predictably irrational. New York, NY: Harper.
Book: Cialdini, R. B. (2008). Influence: Science and practice (5th ed.). Boston,
MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Book: Gass, R., & Seiter, J. (2010). Persuasion, social influence, and compliance
gaining (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson.
Book: Kahneman, D. (2012). Thinking fast and slow. New York, NY: Farrar,
Straus & Giroux.
Book: Tavris, C., & Aronson, E. (2011). Mistakes were made (but not by me).
New York, NY: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.
http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/americanexperience/features/introduction/jonestownintroduction/
Video: Philip Zimbardos now-classic video, Quiet Rage, offers a powerful,
insightful description of his famous Stanford prison study
http://www.prisonexp.org/documentary.htm
Video: The documentary Outfoxed provides an excellent example of how
persuasion can be masked as news and education.
http://www.outfoxed.org/
Video: The video, The Science of Countering Terrorism: Psychological
Perspectives, a talk by psychologist Fathali Moghaddam, is an excellent
introduction to the process of terrorist recruitment and thinking
http://sciencestage.com/v/32330/fathali-moghaddam-science-cafe-the-scienceof-countering-terrorism-psychological-perspectives.html
Discussion Questions
1. Imagine you are commissioned to create an ad to sell a new beer. Can you
give an example of an ad that would rely on the central route? Can you give
an example of an ad that would rely on the peripheral route?
2. The reciprocity principle can be exploited in obvious ways, such as giving a
customer a free sample of a product. Can you give an example of a less
obvious way it might be exploited? What is a less obvious way that a cult
leader might use it to get someone under his or her grip?
3. Which trick in this chapter are you, personally, most prone to? Give a
personal example of this. How might you have avoided it?
Vocabulary
Central route to persuasion
Persuasion that employs direct, relevant, logical messages.
Heuristics
Mental shortcuts that enable people to make decisions and solve problems
quickly and efficiently.
Psychological reactance
A reaction to people, rules, requirements, or offerings that are perceived to limit
freedoms.
Social proof
The mental shortcut based on the assumption that, if everyone is doing it, it
must be right.
The norm of reciprocity
The normative pressure to repay, in equitable value, what another person has
given to us.
The rule of scarcity
People tend to perceive things as more attractive when their availability is
limited, or when they stand to lose the opportunity to acquire them on favorable
terms.
The triad of trust
We are most vulnerable to persuasion when the source is perceived as an
authority, as honest and likable.
Trigger features
Specific, sometimes minute, aspects of a situation that activate fixed action
patterns.
Reference List
Barrett, D. (2010). Supernormal stimuli: How primal urges overran their
evolutionary purpose. New York, NY: W.W. Norton.
Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. New York, NY: Academic
Press.
Brehm, S. S., & Weinraub, M. (1977). Physical barriers and psychological
reactance: Two-year-olds responses to threats to freedom. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 830836.
Burger, J. M. (2009). Replicating Milgram: Would people still obey today?
American Psychologist, 64(1), 111.
Burger, J. M. (1986). Increasing compliance by improving the deal: The thatsnot-all technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 277283.
Cialdini, R. B. (2008). Influence: Science and practice (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn
and Bacon.
Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 285290
Fox, Stephen (1997). The mirror makers: A history of American advertising and
its creators. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press.
Fuller, R. G., & Sheehy-Skeffington, A. (1974). Effects of group laughter on
responses to humorous materials: A replication and extension.
Psychological Reports, 35, 531534.
Sagarin, B. J., Cialdini, R. B., Rice, W. E., & Serna, S. B. (2002). Dispelling the illusion
of invulnerability: The motivations and mechanisms of resistance to
persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 526541.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Persuasion: So Easily Fooled by Robert V. Levine
is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
We often change our attitudes and behaviors to match the attitudes and
behaviors of the people around us. One reason for this conformity is a concern
about what other people think of us. This process was demonstrated in a classic
study in which college students deliberately gave wrong answers to a simple
visual judgment task rather than go against the group. Another reason we
conform to the norm is because other people often have information we do
not, and relying on norms can be a reasonable strategy when we are uncertain
about how we are supposed to act. Unfortunately, we frequently misperceive
how the typical person acts, which can contribute to problems such as the
excessive binge drinking often seen in college students. Obeying orders from
an authority figure can sometimes lead to disturbing behavior. This danger was
illustrated in a famous study in which participants were instructed to administer
painful electric shocks to another person in what they believed to be a learning
experiment. Despite vehement protests from the person receiving the shocks,
most participants continued the procedure when instructed to do so by the
experimenter. The findings raise questions about the power of blind obedience
in deplorable situations such as atrocities and genocide. They also raise
concerns about the ethical treatment of participants in psychology
experiments.
Learning Objectives
Conformity
When he was a teenager, my son often enjoyed looking at photographs of me
and my wife taken when we were in high school. He laughed at the hairstyles,
the clothing, and the kind of glasses people wore back then. And when he was
through with his ridiculing, we would point out that no one is immune to
fashions and fads and that someday his children will probably be equally
amused by his high school photographs and the trends he found so normal at
the time.
Everyday observation confirms that we often adopt the actions and attitudes
of the people around us. Trends in clothing, music, foods, and entertainment
are obvious. But our views on political issues, religious questions, and lifestyles
also reflect to some degree the attitudes of the people we interact with. Similarly,
decisions about behaviors such as smoking and drinking are influenced by
whether the people we spend time with engage in these activities. Psychologists
refer to this widespread tendency to act and think like the people around us as
conformity.
What causes all this conformity? To start, humans may possess an inherent
tendency to imitate the actions of others. Although we usually are not aware
of it, we often mimic the gestures, body posture, language, talking speed, and
many other behaviors of the people we interact with. Researchers find that this
mimicking increases the connection between people and allows our
interactions to flow more smoothly (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999).
Beyond this automatic tendency to imitate others, psychologists have
identified two primary reasons for conformity. The first of these is normative
influence. When normative influence is operating, people go along with the
crowd because they are concerned about what others think of them. We dont
want to look out of step or become the target of criticism just because we like
different kinds of music or dress differently than everyone else. Fitting in also
brings rewards such as camaraderie and compliments.
nobaproject.com - Conformity and Obedience
1131
1132
1133
1134
Obedience
Although we may be influenced by the people around us more than we
recognize, whether we conform to the norm is up to us. But sometimes
decisions about how to act are not so easy. Sometimes we are directed by a
more powerful person to do things we may not want to do. Researchers who
study obedience are interested in how people react when given an order or
command from someone in a position of authority. In many situations,
obedience is a good thing. We are taught at an early age to obey parents,
teachers, and police officers. Its also important to follow instructions from
judges, firefighters, and lifeguards. And a military would fail to function if
soldiers stopped obeying orders from superiors. But, there is also a dark side
to obedience. In the name of following orders or just doing my job, people
can violate ethical principles and break laws. More disturbingly, obedience often
is at the heart of some of the worst of human behaviormassacres, atrocities,
and even genocide.
It was this unsettling side of obedience that led to some of the most famous
and most controversial research in the history of psychology. Milgram (1963,
1965, 1974) wanted to know why so many otherwise decent German citizens
went along with the brutality of the Nazi leaders during the Holocaust. These
inhumane policies may have originated in the mind of a single person, Milgram
(1963, p. 371) wrote, but they could only be carried out on a massive scale if a
very large number of persons obeyed orders.
To understand this obedience, Milgram conducted a series of laboratory
investigations. In all but one variation of the basic procedure, participants were
men recruited from the community surrounding Yale University, where the
research was carried out. These citizens signed up for what they believed to be
an experiment on learning and memory. In particular, they were told the
research concerned the effects of punishment on learning. Three people were
involved in each session. One was the participant.
nobaproject.com - Conformity and Obedience
1136
Thats all. Get me out of here. I told you I had heart trouble. My hearts starting
to bother me now. Get me out of here, please. My hearts starting to bother me.
I refuse to go on. Let me out.
The experimenters role was to encourage the participant to continue. If at
any time the teacher asked to end the session, the experimenter responded
with phrases such as, The experiment requires that you continue, and You
have no other choice, you must go on. The experimenter ended the session
only after the teacher stated four successive times that he did not want to
continue. All the while, the learners protests became more intense with each
shock. After 300 volts, the learner refused to answer any more questions, which
led the experimenter to say that no answer should be considered a wrong
answer. After 330 volts, despite vehement protests from the learner following
previous shocks, the teacher heard only silence, suggesting that the learner
was now physically unable to respond. If the teacher reached 450 voltsthe
end of the generatorthe experimenter told him to continue pressing the 450
volt lever for each wrong answer. It was only after the teacher pressed the 450volt lever three times that the experimenter announced that the study was over.
If you had been a participant in this research, what would you have done?
Virtually everyone says he or she would have stopped early in the process. And
most people predict that very few if any participants would keep pressing all
the way to 450 volts. Yet in the basic procedure described here, 65 percent of
the participants continued to administer shocks to the very end of the session.
These were not brutal, sadistic men. They were ordinary citizens who
nonetheless followed the experimenters instructions to administer what they
believed to be excruciating if not dangerous electric shocks to an innocent
person. The disturbing implication from the findings is that, under the right
circumstances, each of us may be capable of acting in some very
uncharacteristic and perhaps some very unsettling ways.
nobaproject.com - Conformity and Obedience
1137
1138
1139
Outside Resources
Video: An example of information influence in a field setting
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=4yFeaS60nWk
Video: Scenes from a recent partial replication of Milgrams obedience
studies
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=HwqNP9HRy7Y
Video: Scenes from a recent replication of Aschs conformity experiment
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=VgDx5g9ql1g
Web: Website devoted to scholarship and research related to Milgrams
obedience studies
http://www.stanleymilgram.com
Discussion Questions
1. In what ways do you see normative influence operating among you and
your peers? How difficult would it be to go against the norm? What would
it take for you to not do something just because all your friends were doing
it?
2. What are some examples of how informational influence helps us do the
right thing? How can we use descriptive norm information to change
problem behaviors?
3. Is conformity more likely or less likely to occur when interacting with other
people through social media as compared to face-to-face encounters?
4. When is obedience to authority a good thing and when is it bad? What can
be done to prevent people from obeying commands to engage in truly
deplorable behavior such as atrocities and massacres?
5. In what ways do Milgrams experimental procedures fall outside the
guidelines for research with human participants? Are there ways to conduct
relevant research on obedience to authority without violating these
guidelines?
Vocabulary
Conformity
Changing ones attitude or behavior to match a perceived social norm.
Descriptive norm
The perception of what most people do in a given situation.
Informational influence
Conformity that results from a concern to act in a socially approved manner as
determined by how others act.
Normative influence
Conformity that results from a concern for what other people think of us.
Obedience
Responding to an order or command from a person in a position of authority.
Reference List
Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one
against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs, 70 (9, Whole No.
416).
Berndt, T. J. (1979). Developmental changes in conformity to peers and parents.
Developmental Psychology, 15, 608616.
Bond, R. (2005). Group size and conformity. Group Processes & Intergroup
Relations, 8, 331354.
Bond, R., & Smith, P. B. (1996). Culture and conformity: A meta-analysis of studies
using Aschs (1952b, 1956) line judgment task. Psychological Bulletin, 119,
111137.
Borsari, B., & Carey, K. B. (2003). Descriptive and injunctive norms in college
drinking: A meta-analytic integration. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 64, 331
341.
Burger, J. M. (2009). Replicating Milgram: Would people still obey today?
American Psychologist, 64, 111.
Burger, J. M., Bell, H., Harvey, K., Johnson, J., Stewart, C., Dorian, K., & Swedroe,
M. (2010). Nutritious or delicious? The effect of descriptive norm information
on food choice. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29, 228242.
Burger, J. M., LaSalvia, C. T., Hendricks, L. A., Mehdipour, T., & Neudeck, E. M.
(2011). Partying before the party gets started: The effects of descriptive
norms on pre-gaming behavior. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 33,
220227.
Burger, J. M., & Shelton, M. (2011). Changing everyday health behaviors through
descriptive norm manipulations. Social Influence, 6, 6977.
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perceptionbehavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 76, 893910.
Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., & Kallgren, C. A. (1990). A focus theory of normative
conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 10151026.
Crutchfield, R. S. (1955). Conformity and character. American Psychologist, 10,
191198.
Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational
social influences upon individual judgment. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 51, 629636.
Goldstein, N. J., Cialdini, R. B., & Griskevicius, V. (2008). A room with a viewpoint:
Using social norms to motivate environmental conservation in hotels.
Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 472482.
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority: An experimental view. New York,
NY: Harper & Row.
Milgram, S. (1965). Some conditions of obedience and disobedience to
authority. Human Relations, 18, 5776.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Conformity and Obedience by Jerry M. Burger
is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
Friendship and love, and more broadly, the relationships that people cultivate
in their lives, are some of the most valuable treasures a person can own. This
chapter explores ways in which we try to understand how friendships form,
what attracts one person to another, and how love develops. It also explores
how the Internet influences how we meet people and develop deep
relationships. Finally, this chapter will examine social support and how this can
help many through the hardest times and help make the best times even better.
Learning Objectives
Review research that suggests that friendships are important for our health
and well-being.
Introduction
The importance of relationships has been examined by researchers for decades.
Many researchers point to sociologist mile Durkheims classic study of suicide
and social ties (1951) as a starting point for this work. Durkheim argued that
being socially connected is imperative to achieving personal well-being. In fact,
he argued that a person who has no close relationships is likely a person who
is at risk for suicide. It is those relationships that give a person meaning in their
life. In other words, suicide tends to be higher among those who become
disconnected from society. What is interesting about that notion is when people
are asked to describe the basic necessities for lifepeople will most often say
food, water, and shelter, but seldom do people list close relationships in the
top three. Yet time and time again, research has demonstrated that we are
social creatures and we need others to survive and thrive. Another way of
thinking about it is that close relationships are the psychological equivalent of
food and water; in other words, these relationships are necessary for survival.
Baumeister and Leary (1995) maintain that humans have basic needs and one
of them is the need to belong; these needs are what makes us human and give
a sense of purpose and identity to our lives (Brissette, Cohen, & Seeman, 2000;
Ryff, 1989).
Given that close relationships are so vital to well-being, it is important to ask
how interpersonal relationships begin. What makes us like or love one person
but not another? Why is it that when bad things happen, we frequently want to
talk to our friends or family about the situation? Though these are difficult
questions to answer because relationships are complicated and unique, this
chapter will examine how relationships begin; the impact of technology on
relationships; and why coworkers, acquaintances, friends, family, and intimate
partners are so important in our lives.
1151
Proximity
Often we stumble upon friends or romantic partners; this happens partly due
to how close in proximity we are to those people. Specifically, proximity or
physical nearness has been found to be a significant factor in the development
of relationships. For example, when college students go away to a new school,
they will make friends consisting of classmates, roommates, and teammates (i.
e., people close in proximity). Proximity allows people the opportunity to get to
know one other and discover their similaritiesall of which can result in a
friendship or intimate relationship. Proximity is not just about geographic
distance, but rather functional distance, or the frequency with which we cross
paths with others. For example, college students are more likely to become
closer and develop relationships with people on their dorm-room floors
because they see them (i.e., cross paths) more often than they see people on
a different floor. How does the notion of proximity apply in terms of online
relationships? Deb Levine (2000) argues that in terms of developing online
relationships and attraction, functional distance refers to being at the same
place at the same time in a virtual world (i.e., a chat room or Internet forum)
crossing virtual paths.
1152
Familiarity
One of the reasons why proximity matters to attraction is that it breeds
familiarity; people are more attracted to that which is familiar. Just being around
someone or being repeatedly exposed to them increases the likelihood that we
will be attracted to them. We also tend to feel safe with familiar people, as it is
likely we know what to expect from them. Dr. Robert Zajonc (1968) labeled this
phenomenon the mere-exposure effect. More specifically, he argued that the
more often we are exposed to a stimulus (e.g., sound, person) the more likely
we are to view that stimulus positively. Moreland and Beach (1992)
demonstrated this by exposing a college class to four women (similar in
appearance and age) who attended different numbers of classes, revealing that
the more classes a woman attended, the more familiar, similar, and attractive
she was considered by the other students.
There is a certain comfort in knowing what to expect from others;
consequently research suggests that we like what is familiar. While this is often
on a subconscious level, research has found this to be one of the most basic
principles of attraction (Zajonc, 1980). For example, a young man growing up
with an overbearing mother may be attracted to other overbearing women not
because he likes being dominated but rather because it is what he considers
normal (i.e., familiar).
Similarity
When you hear about couples such as Sandra Bullock and Jesse James, or Kim
Kardashian and Kanye West, do you shake your head thinking this wont last?
It is probably because they seem so different. While many make the argument
that opposites attract, research has found that is generally not true; similarity
is key. Sure, there are times when couples can appear fairly different, but overall
we like others who are like us. Ingram and Morris (2007) examined this
nobaproject.com - Love, Friendship, and Social Support
1153
Reciprocity
Another key component in attraction is reciprocity; this principle is based on
the notion that we are more likely to like someone if they feel the same way
toward us. In other words, it is hard to be friends with someone who is not
friendly in return. Another way to think of it is that relationships are built on
give and take; if one side is not reciprocating, then the relationship is doomed.
Basically, we feel obliged to give what we get and to maintain equity in
relationships. Researchers have found that this is true across cultures
(Gouldner, 1960).
Friendship
Research has found that close friendships can protect our mental and physical
health when times get tough. For example, Adams, Santo, and Bukowski (2011)
asked fifth- and sixth-graders to record their experiences and self-worth, and
to provide saliva samples for 4 days. Children whose best friend was present
during or shortly after a negative experience had significantly lower levels of
nobaproject.com - Love, Friendship, and Social Support
1154
the stress hormone cortisol in their saliva compared to those who did not have
a best friend present. Having a best friend also seemed to protect their feelings
of self-worth. Children who did not identify a best friend or did not have an
available best friend during distress experienced a drop in self-esteem over the
course of the study.
Workplace friendships
Friendships often take root in the workplace, due to the fact that people are
spending as much, or more, time at work than they are with their family and
friends (Kaufman & Hotchkiss, 2003). Often, it is through these relationships
that people receive mentoring and obtain social support and resources, but
they can also experience conflicts and the potential for misinterpretation when
sexual attraction is an issue. Indeed, Elsesser and Peplau (2006) found that
many workers reported that friendships grew out of collaborative work projects,
and these friendships made their days more pleasant.
In addition to those benefits, Riordan and Griffeth (1995) found that people
who worked in an environment where friendships could develop and be
maintained were more likely to report higher levels of job satisfaction, job
involvement, and organizational commitment, and they were less like to leave
that job. Similarly, a Gallup poll revealed that employees who had close friends
at work were almost 50% more satisfied with their jobs than those who did not
(Armour, 2007).
Internet friendships
What influence does the Internet have on friendships? It is not surprising that
people use the Internet with the goal of meeting and making new friends (Fehr,
2008; McKenna, 2008). Researchers have wondered if the issue of not being
face-to-face reduces the authenticity of relationships, or if the Internet really
nobaproject.com - Love, Friendship, and Social Support
1155
Love
Is all love the same? Are there different types of love? Examining these questions
more closely, Robert Sternbergs (2004; 2007) work has focused on the notion
that all types of love are comprised of three distinct areas: intimacy, passion,
and commitment. Intimacy includes caring, closeness, and emotional support.
The passion component of love is comprised of physiological and emotional
arousal; these can include physical attraction, emotional responses that
promote physiological changes, and sexual arousal. Lastly, commitment refers
to the cognitive process and decision to commit to love another person and
the willingness to work to keep that love over the course of your life. The
elements involved in intimacy (caring, closeness, and emotional support) are
generally found in all types of close relationshipsfor example, a mothers love
for a child or the love that friends share. Interestingly, this is not true for passion.
nobaproject.com - Love, Friendship, and Social Support
1156
Figure 1: Triangular Theory of Love. Adapted from Wikipedia Creative Commons, 2013
Taking this theory a step further, anthropologist Helen Fisher explained that
she scanned the brains (using fMRI) of people who had just fallen in love and
observed that their brain chemistry was going crazy, similar to the brain of an
addict on a drug high (Cohen, 2007). Specifically, serotonin production increased
by as much as 40% in newly in-love individuals. Further, those newly in love
tended to show obsessive-compulsive tendencies. Conversely, when a person
experiences a breakup, the brain processes it in a similar way to quitting a heroin
habit (Fisher, Brown, Aron, Strong, & Mashek, 2009). Thus, those who believe
that breakups are physically painful are correct! Another interesting point is
that long-term love and sexual desire activate different areas of the brain. More
specifically, sexual needs activate the part of the brain that is particularly
sensitive to innately pleasurable things such as food, sex, and drugs (i.e., the
striatuma rather simplistic reward system), whereas love requires
conditioningit is more like a habit. When sexual needs are rewarded
consistently, then love can develop. In other words, love grows out of positive
rewards, expectancies, and habit (Cacioppo, Bianchi-Demicheli, Hatfield &
nobaproject.com - Love, Friendship, and Social Support
1157
Rapson, 2012).
1158
Social Support
When bad things happen, it is important for people to know that others care
about them and can help them out. Unsurprisingly, research has found that
this is a common thread across cultures (Markus & Kitayma, 1991; Triandis,
1995) and over time (Reis, Sheldon, Gable, Roscoe, & Ryan, 2000); in other words,
social support is the active ingredient that makes our relationships particularly
beneficial. But what is social support? One way of thinking about social support
is that it consists of three discrete conceptual components.
1159
people around to support them if they needed it helped them all to some
degree.
Perceived support has also been linked to well-being. Brannan and
colleagues (2012) found that perceived support predicted each component of
well-being (high positive affect, low negative affect, high satisfaction with life)
among college students in Iran, Jordan, and the United States. Similarly, Cohen
and McKay (1984) found that a high level of perceived support can serve as a
buffer against stress. Interestingly enough, Dr. Cohen found that those with
higher levels of social support were less likely to catch the common cold. The
research is clearperceived social support increases happiness and well-being
and makes our live better in general (Diener & Seligman, 2002; Emmons & Colby,
1995).
1160
mentor support did not meet teachers needs, instead making them feel afraid
and embarrassed to receive mentor support.
Quality or Quantity?
With so many mixed findings, psychologists have asked whether it is the quality
of social support that matters or the quantity (e.g., more people in my support
network). Interestingly, research by Friedman and Martin (2011) examining
1,500 Californians over 8 decades found that while quality does matter,
individuals with larger social networks lived significantly longer than those with
smaller networks. This research suggests we should count the number of our
friends / family membersthe more, the better, right? Not necessarily: Dunbar
(1992; 1993) argued that we have a cognitive limit with regard to how many
people with whom we can maintain social relationships. The general consensus
is about 150we can only really know (maintain contact and relate to) about
150 people. Finally, research shows that diversity also matters in terms of ones
network, such that individuals with more diverse social networks (i.e., different
types of relationships including friends, parents, neighbors, and classmates)
were less likely to get the common cold compared to those with fewer and less
diverse networks (Cohen, Doyle, Turner, Alper, & Skoner, 2003). In sum, it is
important to have quality relationships as well as quantityand as the Beatles
said, all you need is lovelove is all you need.
1161
Outside Resources
Movie: Official Website of Catfish the Movie
http://www.iamrogue.com/catfish
Video: Ted Talk from Helen Fisher on the brain in love
http://www.ted.com/talks/helen_fisher_studies_the_brain_in_love.html
Web: Groundbreaking longitudinal study on longevity from Howard S.
Friedman and Leslie R. Martin
http://www.howardsfriedman.com/longevityproject/
Discussion Questions
1. What is more importantperceived social support or received social
support? Why?
2. We understand how the Internet has changed the dating scenehow might
it further change how we become romantically involved?
3. Can you love someone whom you have never met?
4. Do you think it is the quality or quantity of your relationships that really
matters most?
Vocabulary
Functional distance
The frequency with which we cross paths with others.
Mere-exposure effect
The notion that people like people/places/things merely because they are
familiar with them.
Perceived social support
A persons perception that others are there to help them in times of need.
Proximity
Physical nearness.
Received social support
The actual act of receiving support (e.g., informational, functional).
Support support network
The people who care about and support a person.
Reference List
Adams, R. E., Santo, J., & Bukowski, W. M. (2011). The presence of a best friend
buffers the effects of negative experiences. Developmental Psychology, 47
(6), 17861791. doi:10.1037/a0025401
Aristotle. (n.d.). In poverty and other misfortunes of life. BrainyQuote.com.
Retrieved July 25, 2013, from http://www.brainyquote.com/quotes/quotes/
a/aristotle148482.html
Armour, S. (2007, August 2). Friendships and work: A good or bad partnership?
USA Today. Retrieved from http://usatoday30.usatoday.com/money/
workplace/2007-08-01-work-friends_N.htm
Bargh, J. A., McKenna, K. Y. A, & Fitsimons, G. G. (2002). Can you see the real
me? Activation and expression of the true self on the Internet. Journal of
Social Issues, 58, 3348.
Baumeister, R. & Leary, M. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin,
117(3), 497529.
Benford, P. (2008). The use of Internet-based communication by people with
autism (Doctoral dissertation, University of Nottingham).
Brannan, D., Biswas-Diener, R., Mohr, C. D., Mortazavi, S., & Stein, N. (2012).
Friends and family, a cross-cultural investigation of social support and
subjective well-being. Journal of Positive Psychology, 8(1), 6575.
Brannan, D., & Bleisten, T. (2012). Novice ESOL teachers perceptions of social
Finkel, E. J., Burnette J. L., & Scissors L. E. (2007). Vengefully ever after: Destiny
beliefs, state attachment anxiety, and forgiveness. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 92, 871886.
Fisher, H. E., Brown, L. L., Aron, A., Strong, G., & Mashek, D. (2009). Reward,
addiction, and emotion regulation systems associated with rejection in love.
Journal of Neurophysiology, 104, 5160.
Friedman, H. S. & Martin, L. R. (2011). The Longevity Project: Surprising
Discoveries for Health and Long Life from the Landmark Eight-Decade Study.
New York, NY: Hudson Street Press.
Gottlieb, B. H. (1985). Social support and community mental health. In S. Cohen
& S. Syme (Eds.), Social Support and Health (pp. 303326). Orlando, FL:
Academic Press.
Gouldner, A. W. (1960). The norm of reciprocity: A preliminary statement.
American Sociological Review, 25, 169186.
Ingram, P., & Morris, M. W. (2007). Do people mix at mixers? Structure,
homophily, and the life of the party. Administrative Science Quarterly, 52,
558585.
Kaufman, B. E., & Hotchkiss, J. L. 2003. The economics of labor markets (6th
ed.). Mason, OH: Thomson South-Western.
Levine, D. (2000). Virtual attraction: What rocks your boat. Cyberpsychology &
Behavior, 3(4), 565573. doi:10.1089/109493100420179
Madden, M. & Lenhart, A. (2006). Americans who are seeking romance use the
Internet to help them in their search, but there is still widespread public
concern about safety of online dating. Pew/Internet and American Life
Project. Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.org/Reports/2006/OnlineDating.
aspx
Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for
cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224
253.
McCann Hamilton, V. (2007) Human relations: The art and science of building
effective relationships. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
McKenna, K. A. (2008) MySpace or your place: Relationship initiation and
development in the wired and wireless world. In S. Sprecher, A. Wenzel, &
J. Harvey (Eds.), Handbook of relationship initiation (pp. 235247). New York,
NY: Psychology Press.
McKenna, K. Y. A., Green, A. S., & Gleason, M. E. J. (2002). Relationship formation
on the Internet: Whats the big attraction? Journal of Social Issues, 58, 931.
Mckillip, J., & Redel, S. L. (1983). External validity of matching on physical
attractiveness for same- and opposite-sex couples. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 13, 328337.
Moreland, R. L., & Beach, S. R. (1992). Exposure effects in the classroom: The
development of affinity among students. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 28, 255276.
Reis, H. T., Sheldon, K. M., Gable, S. L., Roscoe, R., & Ryan, R. (2000). Daily well
being: The role of autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Personality and
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Love, Friendship, and Social Support by Debi
Brannan and Cynthia D. Mohr is licensed under the Creative Commons AttributionNonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
This chapter assumes that a thorough understanding of people requires a
thorough understanding of groups. Each of us is an autonomous individual
seeking our own objectives, yet we are also members of groupsgroups that
constrain us, guide us, and sustain us. Just as each of us influences the group
and the people in the group, so, too, do groups change each one of us. Joining
groups satisfies our need to belong, gain information and understanding
through social comparison, define our sense of self and social identity, and
achieve goals that might elude us if we worked alone. Groups are also practically
significant, for much of the worlds work is done by groups rather than by
individuals. Success sometimes eludes our groups, but when group members
learn to work together as a cohesive team their success becomes more certain.
People also turn to groups when important decisions must be made, and this
choice is justified as long as groups avoid such problems as group polarization
and groupthink.
Learning Objectives
Use theories of social facilitation to predict when a group will perform tasks
slowly or quickly (e.g., students eating a meal as a group, workers on an
assembly line, or a study group).
List and discuss the factors that facilitate and impede group performance
and decision making.
individuals? In many cases the answers are not what common sense and folk
wisdom might suggest.
1176
1177
Affiliation in Groups
Groups not only satisfy the need to belong, they also provide members with
information, assistance, and social support. Leon Festingers theory of social
comparison (1950, 1954) suggested that in many cases people join with others
to evaluate the accuracy of their personal beliefs and attitudes.
Stanley
1178
frequently discover that we are members of the better group, and so can take
pride in our superiority. By denigrating other groups, we elevate both our
personal and our collective self-esteem (Crocker & Major, 1989).
Mark Learys sociometer model goes so far as to suggest that self-esteem
is part of a sociometer that monitors peoples relational value in other peoples
eyes (2007, p. 328). He maintains self-esteem is not just an index of ones sense
of personal value, but also an indicator of acceptance into groups. Like a gauge
that indicates how much fuel is left in the tank, a dip in self-esteem indicates
exclusion from our group is likely. Disquieting feelings of self-worth, then,
prompt us to search for and correct characteristics and qualities that put us at
risk of social exclusion. Self-esteem is not just high self-regard, but the selfapprobation that we feel when included in groups (Leary & Baumeister, 2000).
1179
1180
Figure 1: The "competition machine" Triplett used to study the impact of competition on
performance. Triplett's study was one of the first labratory studies conducted in the field of
social psychology. Triplett, N. (1898)
1181
usually only occur when the task requires the person to perform dominant
responses, i.e., ones that are well-learned or based on instinctive behaviors. If
the task requires nondominant responses, i.e., novel, complicated, or untried
behaviors that the organism has never performed before or has performed
only infrequently, then the presence of others inhibits performance. Hence,
students write poorer quality essays on complex philosophical questions when
they labor in a group rather than alone (Allport, 1924), but they make fewer
mistakes in solving simple, low-level multiplication problems with an audience
or a coactor than when they work in isolation (Dashiell, 1930).
Social facilitation, then, depends on the task: other people facilitate
performance when the task is so simple that it requires only dominant
responses, but others interfere when the task requires nondominant
responses.
influence when social facilitation, not social interference, occurs. Studies of the
challenge-threat response and brain imaging, for example, confirm that we
respond physiologically and neurologically to the presence of others
(Blascovich, Mendes, Hunter, & Salomon, 1999). Other people also can trigger
evaluation apprehension, particularly when we feel that our individual
performance will be known to others, and those others might judge it negatively
(Bond, Atoum, & VanLeeuwen, 1996). The presence of other people can also
cause perturbations in our capacity to concentrate on and process information
(Harkins, 2006). Distractions due to the presence of other people have been
shown to improve performance on certain tasks, such as the Stroop task, but
undermine performance on more cognitively demanding tasks(Huguet,
Galvaing, Monteil, & Dumas, 1999).
1182
Social Loafing
Groups usually outperform individuals. A single student, working alone on a
paper, will get less done in an hour than will four students working on a group
project. One person playing a tug-of-war game against a group will lose. A crew
of movers can pack up and transport your household belongings faster than
you can by yourself. As the saying goes, Many hands make light the work
(Littlepage, 1991; Steiner, 1972).
Groups, though, tend to be underachievers. Studies of social facilitation
confirmed the positive motivational benefits of working with other people on
well-practiced tasks in which each members contribution to the collective
enterprise can be identified and evaluated. But what happens when tasks
require a truly collective effort? First, when people work together they must
coordinate their individual activities and contributions to reach the maximum
level of efficiencybut they rarely do (Diehl & Stroebe, 1987). Three people in
a tug-of-war competition, for example, invariably pull and pause at slightly
different times, so their efforts are uncoordinated. The result is coordination
loss: the three-person group is stronger than a single person, but not three
times as strong. Second, people just dont exert as much effort when working
on a collective endeavor, nor do they expend as much cognitive effort trying to
solve problems, as they do when working alone. They display social loafing
(Latan, 1981).
Bibb Latan, Kip Williams, and Stephen Harkins (1979) examined both
coordination losses and social loafing by arranging for students to cheer or clap
either alone or in groups of varying sizes. The students cheered alone or in 2or 6-person groups, or they were lead to believe they were in 2- or 6-person
groups (those in the pseudo-groups wore blindfolds and headsets that played
masking sound). As Figure 2 indicates, groups generated more noise than
solitary subjects, but the productivity dropped as the groups became larger in
size. In dyads, each subject worked at only 66% of capacity, and in 6-person
nobaproject.com - The Psychology of Groups
1183
groups at 36%. Productivity also dropped when subjects merely believed they
were in groups. If subjects thought that one other person was shouting with
them, they shouted 82% as intensely, and if they thought five other people were
shouting, they reached only 74% of their capacity. These loses in productivity
were not due to coordination problems; this decline in production could be
attributed only to a reduction in effortto social loafing (Latan et al., 1979,
Experiment 2).
1184
Figure 2: Sound pressure per person as a function of group or pseudo group size. Latane,
B. (1981)
1185
Teamwork
Social loafing is no rare phenomenon. When sales personnel work in groups
with shared goals, they tend to take it easy if another salesperson is nearby
who can do their work (George, 1992). People who are trying to generate new,
creative ideas in group brainstorming sessions usually put in less effort and are
thus less productive than people who are generating new ideas individually
(Paulus & Brown, 2007). Students assigned group projects often complain of
inequity in the quality and quantity of each members contributions: Some
people just dont work as much as they should to help the group reach its
learning goals (Neu, 2012). People carrying out all sorts of physical and mental
tasks expend less effort when working in groups, and the larger the group, the
more they loaf (Karau & Williams, 1993).
Groups can, however, overcome this impediment to performance through
teamwork. A group may include many talented individuals, but they must learn
how to pool their individual abilities and energies to maximize the teams
performance. Team goals must be set, work patterns structured, and a sense
of group identity developed. Individual members must learn how to coordinate
their actions, and any strains and stresses in interpersonal relations need to be
identified and resolved (Salas, Rosen, Burke, & Goodwin, 2009).
Researchers have identified two key ingredients to effective teamwork: a
shared mental representation of the task and group unity. Teams improve their
performance over time as they develop a shared understanding of the team
and the tasks they are attempting. Some semblance of this shared mental
model is present nearly from its inception, but as the team practices, differences
among the members in terms of their understanding of their situation and their
team diminish as a consensus becomes implicitly accepted (Tindale, Stawiski,
& Jacobs, 2008).
nobaproject.com - The Psychology of Groups
1186
Effective teams are also, in most cases, cohesive groups (Dion, 2000). Group
cohesion is the integrity, solidarity, social integration, or unity of a group. In
most cases, members of cohesive groups like each other and the group and
they also are united in their pursuit of collective, group-level goals. Members
tend to enjoy their groups more when they are cohesive, and cohesive groups
usually outperform ones that lack cohesion.
This cohesion-performance relationship, however, is a complex one. Metaanalytic studies suggest that cohesion improves teamwork among members,
but that performance quality influences cohesion more than cohesion
influences performance (Mullen & Copper, 1994; Mullen, Driskell, & Salas, 1998;
see Figure 3). Cohesive groups also can be spectacularly unproductive if the
groups norms stress low productivity rather than high productivity (Seashore,
1954).
1187
Figure 3: The relationship between group cohesion and performance over time. Groups
that are cohesive do tend to perform well on tasks now (Time1) and in the future (Time 2).
Notice, though, that the relationship between Performance at Time 1 and Cohesiveness at
Time 2 is greater (r=.51) than the relationship between Cohesion at Time 1 and Performance
at Time 2 (r=.25). These findings suggest that cohesion improves performance, but that a
group that performs well is likely to also become more cohesive. Mullen, Driskell, & Salas (1998)
Group Development
In most cases groups do not become smooth-functioning teams overnight. As
Bruce Tuckmans (1965) theory of group development suggests, groups usually
pass through several stages of development as they change from a newly
formed group into an effective team. As noted in Table 1, in the forming phase,
the members become oriented toward one another. In the storming phase, the
group members find themselves in conflict, and some solution is sought to
improve the group environment. In the norming, phase standards for behavior
nobaproject.com - The Psychology of Groups
1188
and roles develop that regulate behavior. In the performing, phase the group
has reached a point where it can work as a unit to achieve desired goals, and
the adjourning phase ends the sequence of development; the group disbands.
Throughout these stages groups tend to oscillate between the task-oriented
issues and the relationship issues, with members sometimes working hard but
at other times strengthening their interpersonal bonds (Tuckman & Jensen,
1977).
1189
Table 1: Sources based on Tuckman (1965) and Tuckman & Jensen (1977)
1190
what the group has to offer, but the group also investigates you. During this
investigation stage you are still an outsider: interested in joining the group, but
not yet committed to it in any way. But once the group accepts you and you
accept the group, socialization begins: you learn the groups norms and take
on different responsibilities depending on your role. On a sports team, for
example, you may initially hope to be a star who starts every game or plays a
particular position, but the team may need something else from you. In time,
though, the group will accept you as a full-fledged member and both sides in
the processyou and the group itselfincrease their commitment to one
another. When that commitment wanes, however, your membership may come
to an end as well.
1191
Group Polarization
Lets say you are part of a group assigned to make a presentation. One of the
group members suggests showing a short video that, although amusing,
includes some provocative images. Even though initially you think the clip is
inappropriate, you begin to change your mind as the group discusses the idea.
The group decides, eventually, to throw caution to the wind and show the clip
and your instructor is horrified by your choice.
This hypothetical example is consistent with studies of groups making
decisions that involve risk. Common sense notions suggest that groups exert
a moderating, subduing effect on their members. However, when researchers
looked at groups closely, they discovered many groups shift toward more
extreme decisions rather than less extreme decisions after group interaction.
Discussion, it turns out, doesnt moderate peoples judgments after all. Instead,
it leads to group polarization: judgments made after group discussion will be
more extreme in the same direction as the average of individual judgments
made prior to discussion (Myers & Lamm, 1976). If a majority of members feel
that taking risks is more acceptable than exercising caution, then the group will
become riskier after a discussion. For example, in France, where people
generally like their government but dislike Americans, group discussion
improved their attitude toward their government but exacerbated their
negative opinions of Americans (Moscovici & Zavalloni, 1969).
Similarly,
prejudiced people who discussed racial issues with other prejudiced individuals
became even more negative, but those who were relatively unprejudiced
exhibited even more acceptance of diversity when in groups (Myers & Bishop,
1970).
1192
Groupthink
Groups sometimes make spectacularly bad decisions. In 1961, a special advisory
committee to President John F. Kennedy planned and implemented a covert
invasion of Cuba at the Bay of Pigs that ended in total disaster. In 1986, NASA
carefully, and incorrectly, decided to launch the Challenger space shuttle in
temperatures that were too cold.
nobaproject.com - The Psychology of Groups
1193
membership much more in cohesive groups, they are less likely to abandon
the group, and they work harder in pursuit of the groups goals. But extreme
cohesiveness can be dangerous. When cohesiveness intensifies, members
become more likely to accept the goals, decisions, and norms of the group
without reservation. Conformity pressures also rise as members become
reluctant to say or do anything that goes against the grain of the group, and
nobaproject.com - The Psychology of Groups
1194
Isolation. Groupthink groups too often work behind closed doors, keeping
out of the limelight. They isolate themselves from outsiders and refuse to
modify their beliefs to bring them into line with societys beliefs. They avoid
leaks by maintaining strict confidentiality and working only with people who
are members of their group.
Biased leadership. A biased leader who exerts too much authority over
group members can increase conformity pressures and railroad decisions.
In groupthink groups, the leader determines the agenda for each meeting,
sets limits on discussion, and can even decide who will be heard.
minimizing
the
possibility
of
negative
outcomes,
1195
potential for violence during football games. Could these kinds of groups
experience groupthink?
discussed above are present, combined with other contributing causal factors,
such as cohesiveness, isolation, biased leadership, and stress.
To avoid
value, but also emotional, psychological value. In groups we find others who
appreciate and value us. In groups we gain the support we need in difficult
times, but also have the opportunity to influence others. In groups we find
evidence of our self-worth, and secure ourselves from the threat of loneliness
and despair. For most of us, groups are the secret source of well-being.
1196
Outside Resources
Audio: This American Life. Episode 109 deals with the motivation and
excitement of joining with others at summer camp.
http://www.thisamericanlife.org/radio-archives/episode/109/notes-on-camp
Audio: This American Life. Episode 158 examines how people act when they
are immersed in a large crowd.
http://www.thisamericanlife.org/radio-archives/episode/158/mob-mentality
Audio: This American Life. Episode 61 deals with fiascos, many of which are
perpetrated by groups.
http://www.thisamericanlife.org/radio-archives/episode/61/fiasco
Audio: This American Life. Episode 74 examines how individuals act at
conventions, when they join with hundreds or thousands of other people
who are similar in terms of their avocations or employment.
http://www.thisamericanlife.org/radio-archives/episode/74/conventions
Journal Article: The Dynamogenic Factors in Pacemaking and Competition
presents Norman Tripletts original paper on what would eventually be
known as social facilitation.
http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Triplett/
Video: Flash mobs illustrate the capacity of groups to organize quickly and
complete complex tasks. One well-known example of a pseudo-flash mob is
the rendition of Do Re Mi from the Sound of Music in the Central Station
of Antwerp in 2009.
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=7EYAUazLI9k
Discussion Questions
1. What are the advantages and disadvantages of sociality? Why do people
often join groups?
2. Is self-esteem shaped by your personality qualities or by the value and
qualities of groups to which you belong?
3. In what ways does membership in a group change a persons self-concept
and social identity?
4. What steps would you take if you were to base a self-esteem enrichment
program in schools on the sociometer model of self-worth?
5. If you were a college professor, what would you do to increase the success
of in-class learning teams?
6. What are the key ingredients to transforming a working group into a true
team?
7. Have you ever been part of a group that made a poor decision and, if so,
were any of the symptoms of groupthink present in your group?
Vocabulary
Collective self-esteem
Feelings of self-worth that are based on evaluation of relationships with others
and membership in social groups.
Common knowledge effect
The tendency for groups to spend more time discussing information that all
members know (shared information) and less time examining information that
only a few members know (unshared).
Group cohesion
The solidarity or unity of a group resulting from the development of strong and
mutual interpersonal bonds among members and group-level forces that unify
the group, such as shared commitment to group goals.
Group polarization
The tendency for members of a deliberating group to move to a more extreme
position, with the direction of the shift determined by the majority or average
of the members predeliberation preferences.
Groupthink
A set of negative group-level processes, including illusions of vulnerability, selfcensorship, and pressures to conform, that occur when highly cohesive groups
seek concurrence when making a decision.
Ostracism
Excluding one or more individuals from a group by reducing or eliminating
contact with the person, usually by ignoring, shunning, or explicitly banishing
them.
Shared mental model
Knowledge, expectations, conceptualizations, and other cognitive representations
that members of a group have in common pertaining to the group and its
members, tasks, procedures, and resources.
Social comparison
The process of contrasting ones personal qualities and outcomes, including
beliefs, attitudes, values, abilities, accomplishments, and experiences, to those
of other people.
Social facilitation
Improvement in task performance that occurs when people work in the
presence of other people.
Social identity theory
A theoretical analysis of group processes and intergroup relations that assumes
groups influence their members self-concepts and self-esteem, particularly
when individuals categorize themselves as group members and identify with
the group.
Social loafing
The reduction of individual effort exerted when people work in groups
compared with when they work alone.
Sociometer model
A conceptual analysis of self-evaluation processes that theorizes self-esteem
functions to psychologically monitor of ones degree of inclusion and exclusion
in social groups.
Teamwork
The process by which members of the team combine their knowledge, skills,
abilities, and other resources through a coordinated series of actions to produce
an outcome.
Reference List
Allport, F. H. (1924). Social psychology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for
interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation.
Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497529.
Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Hunter, S. B., & Salomon, K. (1999). Social
facilitation as challenge and threat. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 77, 6877.
Bond, C. F., Atoum, A. O., & VanLeeuwen, M. D. (1996). Social impairment of
complex learning in the wake of public embarrassment. Basic and Applied
Social Psychology, 18, 3144.
Buote, V. M., Pancer, S. M., Pratt, M. W., Adams, G., Birnie-Lefcovitch, S., Polivy,
J., & Wintre, M. G. (2007). The importance of friends: Friendship and
adjustment among 1st-year university students. Journal of Adolescent
Research, 22(6), 665689.
Crocker, J., & Luhtanen, R. (1990). Collective self-esteem and ingroup bias.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 6067.
Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). Social stigma and self-esteem: The self-protective
properties of stigma. Psychological Review, 96, 608630.
Darwin, C. (1859/1963). The origin of species. New York: Washington Square
Press.
Tindale, R. S., Stawiski, S., & Jacobs, E. (2008). Shared cognition and group
learning. In V. I. Sessa & M. London (Eds.), Work group learning:
Understanding, improving and assessing how groups learn in organizations
(pp. 7390). New York: Taylor & Francis Group.
Triplett, N. (1898). The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competition.
American Journal of Psychology, 9, 507533.
Tuckman, B. W. (1965). Developmental sequences in small groups. Psychological
Bulletin, 63, 384399.
Tuckman, B. W., & Jensen, M. A. C. (1977). Stages of small group development
revisited. Group and Organizational Studies, 2, 419427.
Williams, K. D. (2007). Ostracism. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 425452.
Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269274.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. The Psychology of Groups by Donelson R. Forsyth
is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Prejudice, Discrimination,
and Stereotyping
Susan T. Fiske
Princeton University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
People often are biased against people not from their own group, showing
prejudice (emotional bias), stereotypes (cognitive bias), and discrimination
(behavioral bias). Bias used to be more open, but during the 20th century, bias
became more subtle (automatic, ambiguous, and ambivalent). In the 21st
century, social group categories have become more complex, perhaps
transforming older biases.
Learning Objectives
Understand 21st century biases that may break down as identities get more
complicated.
Introduction
Mother to son:
Whats your pet peeve about people misunderstanding you? Everyone wants
to be seen for who they are, not as just another typical X, even in your own
family. So why do people put other people into groups, and what happens when
we do? This chapter focuses on biases against social groups, which social
psychologists sort into emotional prejudices, mental stereotypes, and
behavioral discrimination. These three aspects of bias are related, but they
each can occur separately from the others (Dovidio & Gaertner, 2010; Fiske,
1998). For example, sometimes people resent a social group (prejudice) without
knowing even the most superficial things about it (stereotypes).
This chapter shows that todays biases are not the same as yesterdays biases
in many ways, but in other ways, the more things change, the more they stay
the same. We start with old-fashioned biases that might have belonged to our
grandparents and great-grandparentsplus a few people who never left the
bad old days. Then we will discuss late 20th century biases that affected our
parents and still linger today. Finally, we will talk about todays 21st century
biases that challenge fairness and respect for all.
1213
prejudices. For example, just 80 years ago, American college students thought
Turkish people were cruel, very religious, and treacherous (Katz & Braly, 1933).
Where did they get those ideas, assuming that few of them had every met
anyone from Turkey? Old-fashioned stereotypes were overt and unapologetic.
Blatant biases are conscious beliefs, feelings, and behavior that people are
perfectly willing to admit, are mostly hostile, and openly favor their own group.
Blatant biases tend to run in packs: People who openly hate one outgroup also
hate many others. On the other hand, people who reject overt prejudice tend
to be tolerant of most groups. Two scales illustrate this pattern, so we turn to
them next.
1214
The point is that SDOa preference for inequality as normal and natural
also predicts endorsing the superiority of certain groups: Whites, men, nativeborn residents, heterosexuals, and Christians. This means seeing minorities,
women,
immigrants,
homosexuals,
and
non-Christians
as
inferior.
Understandably, the listed groups tend to score, respectively, higher and lower
on SDO. The SDO gender difference (men higher, women lower) appears all
over the world.
SDO rests on a fundamental belief that the world is tough and competitive,
so it correlates with seeing groups as battling each other for limited resources,
with winners and losers. SDO sees intergroup relations as economic combat
(see Table 1).
Right-wing Authoritarianism
Right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) focuses on value conflicts instead of the
economic ones that SDO targets. RWA endorses respect for obedience and
authority in the service of group conformity (Altemeyer, 1988). RWA values
group unity over individual preferences, to maintain group values in the face
of deviance. Despite its name, it is not necessarily limited to people on the right
(conservatives), but it does correlate with a preference for order, clarity, and
conventional values that more often fit conservative beliefs. RWA focuses on
nobaproject.com - Prejudice, Discrimination, and Stereotyping
1215
Automatic Biases
Most people like themselves well enough, and most people identify themselves
as members of certain groups but not others. Logic suggests that they would
therefore like their own groups well enough, whether those groups are
hometown, school, religion, gender, or ethnicity. Liking yourself and your groups
is human nature. The larger issue is that own-group preference suggests liking
other groups less. This trade-off is relatively automatic, that is, unintended,
immediate, and irresistible.
nobaproject.com - Prejudice, Discrimination, and Stereotyping
1216
1217
Ambiguous Biases
As the IAT indicates, peoples biases often stem from the spontaneous tendency
to favor their own, at the expense of the other. Outgroup disliking stems from
ingroup liking (Brewer & Brown, 1998). Given the chance, people reward their
own groupeven when it does not benefit them personally. Mostly, people do
not punish the other group as much as neglect it in favor of the ingroup. Social
identity theory (Tajfel, Billig, Bundy, & Flament, 1971) describes peoples
tendency to favor their own group (the ingroup) over another group (the
outgroup).
outgroup. And we see each as more homogeneous than they are. This results
in the perception that they really differ from us, and they are all alike, besides.
People categorize people into groups as spontaneously as we categorize
furniture or fruit. The difference is that we inhabit one of the categories, as selfcategorization theory points out (Turner, 1975). Because these group
categories are not neutral, we favor people like usand incidentally disfavor
those others. Ingroup favoritism is an ambiguous form of bias because it
disfavors the outgroup by exclusion. In hiring, for example, this can result in
not getting the job. All this stems from the natural human tendency to be more
comfortable with people like yourself.
nobaproject.com - Prejudice, Discrimination, and Stereotyping
1218
Ambivalent Biases
People often have mixed feelings toward other groups. Not all stereotypes are
all bad (Asians are sometimes considered the model minority), and emotional
prejudices may be ambivalent (pitying people with disabilities may seem
charitable, but disrespects them at the same time). Sexist people may feel
themselves benevolent toward traditional women and hostile only toward
nontraditional women. Racist people may feel paternalistic sympathy toward
Black or Latino people but also fault their supposed work ethic. Ageist people
may feel fond of older people but resent their taking up shared resources. A
simple way to understand these mixed feelings, across a variety of groups,
results from the Stereotype Content Model (Fiske, Cuddy, & Glick, 2007).
When people learn about a new group, they first want to know its intentions
for good or ill. Like the guard at night: Who goes there, friend or foe? If the
other group has good, cooperative intentions, they are warm and trustworthy,
on our side; otherwise, they are cold competitors or exploiters, not on our side.
After intentions, we want to know whether they are competent to act on their
intentions (if they are incompetent, they matter less). These two simple
dimensionswarmth and competencetogether map how groups relate to
each other in society.
nobaproject.com - Prejudice, Discrimination, and Stereotyping
1219
American groups who seem both warm and competent include citizens and
the middle class. Even people who dont belong to these groups know that these
are the groups our society admires. At the opposite extreme are homeless
people and drug addicts, stereotyped as not having good intentions (perhaps
exploitative, seen as not trying to play by the rules), and likewise being
incompetent (unable) to do anything useful. These groups reportedly make
society more disgusted than any other groups do. However, these extreme
ingroups and outgroups are only half the map.
Some group stereotypes are mixed, high on one dimension and low on the
other. Groups stereotyped as competent but not warm, for example, include
rich people and outsiders good at business (Jewish or Asian people, in the U.S.
at this time). Groups seen as competent but cold make people feel some envy,
admitting that they may have some talent but resenting them for not being
people like us. The model minority stereotype includes excessive
competence but deficient sociability. Being stereotyped this way is not all good.
The other mixed combination is high warmth but low competence. Groups
who fit this combination include older people and disabled people. Others
report pitying them, but only so long as they stay in their place. Grey panthers
or disability-rights activists give up the pity, although they may gain some
respect.
Altogether, these four kinds of stereotypes and their associated emotional
prejudices (pride, disgust, envy, pity) occur all over the world, for each societys
own groups. These maps of the group terrain predict specific types of
discrimination for specific kinds of groups, underlining how bias is not exactly
equal opportunity.
1220
1221
Outside Resources
Web: Website exploring the causes and consequences of prejudice.
http://www.understandingprejudice.org/
Discussion Questions
1. Do you know more people from different kinds of social groups than your
parents did?
2. How often do you hear people criticizing groups without knowing anything
about them?
3. Take the IAT. Could you feel that some associations are easier than others?
4. What groups illustrate ambivalent biases, seemingly competent but cold,
or warm but incompetent?
5. Do you or someone you know believe that group hierarchies are inevitable?
Desirable?
6. How can people learn to get along with people who seem different from
them?
Vocabulary
Automatic bias
Automatic biases are unintended, immediate, and irresistible.
Aversive racism
Aversive racism is unexamined racial bias that the person does not intend and
would reject, but that avoids inter-racial contact.
Blatant biases
Blatant biases are conscious beliefs, feelings, and behavior that people are
perfectly willing to admit, are mostly hostile, and openly favor their own group.
Discrimination
Discrimination is behavior that advantages or disadvantages people merely
based on their group membership.
Implicit Association Test
Implicit Association Test (IAT) measures relatively automatic biases that favor
own group relative to other groups.
Prejudice
Prejudice is an evaluation or emotion toward people merely based on their
group membership.
Right-wing authoritarianism
Right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) focuses on value conflicts but endorses
respect for obedience and authority in the service of group conformity.
Self-categorization theory
Self-categorization theory develops social identity theorys point that people
categorize themselves, along with each other into groups, favoring their own
group.
Social dominance orientation
Social dominance orientation (SDO) describes a belief that group hierarchies
are inevitable in all societies and even good, to maintain order and stability.
Social identity theory
Social identity theory notes that people categorize each other into groups,
favoring their own group.
Stereotype Content Model
Stereotype Content Model shows that social groups are viewed according to
their perceived warmth and competence.
Stereotypes
Stereotype is a belief that characterizes people based merely on their group
membership.
Subtle biases
Subtle biases are automatic, ambiguous, and ambivalent, but real in their
consequences.
Reference List
Altemeyer, B. (2004). Highly dominating, highly authoritarian personalities. The
Journal of Social Psychology, 144(4), 421-447. doi:10.3200/SOCP.144.4.421-448
Altemeyer, B. (1988). Enemies of freedom: Understanding right-wing
authoritarianism. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Bodenhausen, G. V., & Peery, D. (2009). Social categorization and stereotyping
in vivo: The VUCA challenge. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3
(2), 133-151. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00167.x
Brewer, M. B., & Brown, R. J. (1998). Intergroup relations. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T.
Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology, Vols. 1 and 2
(4th ed.) (pp. 554-594). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled
components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), 5-18.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2010). Intergroup bias. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert,
& G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology, Vol. 2 (5th ed.) (pp.
1084-1121). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
Fiske, S. T. (1998). Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination. In D. T. Gilbert,
S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology, Vols. 1
and 2 (4th ed.) (pp. 357-411). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social
cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2),
77-83. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.005
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., &
Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, selfesteem, and self-concept. Psychological Review, 109(1), 3-25.
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.1.3
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual
differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464-1480. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464
Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2009).
Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of
predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 17-41.
doi:10.1037/a0015575
Katz, D., & Braly, K. (1933). Racial stereotypes of one hundred college students.
The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 28(3), 280-290. doi:10.1037/
h0074049
Rudman, L. A., & Ashmore, R. D. (2007). Discrimination and the implicit
association test. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10(3), 359-372.
doi:10.1177/1368430207078696
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social
hierarchy and oppression. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., & Flament, C. (1971). Social categorization
and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1(2),
149-178. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420010202
Turner, J. C. (1975). Social comparison and social identity: Some prospects for
intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 5(1), 5-34.
doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420050102
Word, C. O., Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1974). The nonverbal mediation of selffulfilling prophecies in interracial interaction. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 10(2), 109-120. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(74)90059-6
Abstract
This chapter discusses the causes and consequences of human aggression and
violence. Both internal and external causes are considered. Effective and
ineffective techniques for reducing aggression are also discussed.
Learning Objectives
Explain whether people think the world is less violent now than in the past,
and whether it actually is less violent. If there is a discrepancy between
perception and reality, how can it be resolved?
Introduction
Aggression is indeed the dark side of human nature. Although aggression may
have been adaptive in our ancient past, it hardly seems adaptive today. For
example, on 14 December 2012 Adam Lanza, age 20, first killed his mother in
their home, and then went to an elementary school in Newtown, Connecticut
and began shooting, killing 20 children and 6 school employees, before killing
himself. When incidents such as these happen, we want to know what caused
them. Although it is impossible to know what motivated a particular individual
such as Lanza to commit the Newtown school shooting, for decades researchers
have studied the internal and external factors that influence aggression and
violence. We consider some of these factors in this chapter.
Before we get too far, lets begin by defining the term aggression. Laypeople
and researchers often use the term aggression differently. Laypeople might
describe a salesperson that tries really hard to sell them something as
aggressive. The salesperson does not, however, want to harm potential
customers. Most researchers define aggression as any behavior intended to
harm another person who does not want to be harmed (Baron & Richardson,
1994). This definition includes three important features. First, aggression is a
behavioryou can see it. Aggression is not an internal response, such as having
angry feelings or aggressive thoughts (although such internal responses can
increase the likelihood of actual aggression). Second, aggression is intentional
rather than accidental. For example, a dentist might intentionally give a patient
a shot of Novocain (which hurts!), but the goal is to help rather than harm the
patient. Third, the victim wants to avoid the harm. Thus, suicide and
sadomasochistic sex play would not be called aggression because the victim
actively seeks to be harmed.
nobaproject.com - Aggression and Violence
1233
Researchers and laypeople also differ in their use of the term violence. A
meteorologist might call a storm violent if it has intense winds, rain, thunder,
lightning, or hail. Researchers define violence as aggression intended to cause
extreme physical harm (e.g., injury, death). Thus, all violent acts are aggressive,
but not all aggressive acts are violent. For example, screaming and swearing at
another person is aggressive, but not violent.
Figure 1. Rate of battle deaths in state-based armed conflicts, 1946-2008. Civilian and
military battle deaths in state-based armed conflicts, divided by world population. Sources:
UCDP/PRIO Armed Conflict Dataset; see Human Security Report Project (2007), based on data
from Lacina and Gleditsch (2005), updated in 2010 by Tara Cooper. Best estimate used when
available; otherwise the geometric mean of the High and Low estimates is used. World
population figures from U.S. Census Bureau (2010). Population data for 1946-1949 were taken
from McEvedy and Jones (1978), and multiplied by 1.01 to make them commensurate with
the rest. From Pinker (2011, p. 301). Copyright permission granted by Steven Pinker.
The good news is that the level of violence in the world is decreasing over
timeby millennia, century, and even decade (Pinker, 2011). Studies of body
counts, such as the proportion of prehistoric skeletons with axe and arrowhead
wounds, suggest that prehistoric societies were far more violent than those
today. Estimates show that if the wars of the 20th century had killed the same
proportion of the population as ancient tribal wars did, then the death toll would
have been 20 times higher2 billion rather than 100 million. More recent data
show that murder rates in Europe have decreased dramatically since the Middle
nobaproject.com - Aggression and Violence
1234
Ages. For example, estimated murders in England dropped from 24 per 100,000
in the 14th century to 0.6 per 100,000 by the early 1960s. The major decline in
violence occurred in the 17th century during the Age of Reason, which began
in the Netherlands and England and then spread to other European countries.
Global violence has also steadily decreased since the middle of the 20th century.
For example, the number of battle deaths in interstate wars has declined from
more than 65,000 per year in the 1950s to fewer than 2,000 per year in the
2000s. There have also been global declines in the number of armed conflicts
and combat deaths, the number of military coups, and the number of deadly
violence campaigns waged against civilians. For example, Figure 1 shows the
number of battle deaths per 100,000 people per year over 60 years (see Pinker,
2011, p. 301). As can be seen, battle deaths of all types (civil, colonial, interstate,
internationalized civil) have decreased over time. The claim that violence has
decreased dramatically over time may seem hard to believe in todays digital
age when we are constantly bombarded by scenes of violence in the media. In
the news media, the top stories are the most violent onesIf it bleeds it leads,
so the saying goes. Citizen journalists around the world also use social media
to show and tell the world about unjustified acts of violence. Because violent
images are more available to us now than ever before, we incorrectly assume
that violence levels are also higher. Our tendency to overestimate the amount
of violence in the world is due to the availability heuristic, which is the tendency
to judge the frequency or likelihood of an event by the ease with which relevant
instances come to mind. Because we are frequently exposed to scenes of
violence in the mass media, acts of violence are readily accessible in memory
and come to mind easily, so we assume violence is more common than it actually
is.
Human aggression is very complex and is caused by multiple factors. We
will consider a few of the most important internal and external causes of
aggression. Internal causes include anything the individual brings to the
situation that increases the probability of aggression. External causes include
nobaproject.com - Aggression and Violence
1235
Internal Factors
Age
At what age are people most aggressive? You might be surprised to learn that
toddlers 1 to 3 years old are most aggressive. Toddlers often rely on physical
aggression to resolve conflict and get what they want. In free play situations,
researchers have found that 25 percent of their interactions are aggressive
(Tremblay, 2000). No other group of individuals (e.g., Mafia, street gangs) resorts
to aggression 25 percent of the time. Fortunately for the rest of us, most toddler
aggression isnt severe enough to qualify as violence because they dont use
weapons, such as guns and knives. As children grow older, they learn to inhibit
their aggressive impulses and resolve conflict using nonaggressive means, such
as compromise and negotiation. Although most people become less aggressive
over time, a small subset of people becomes more aggressive over time. The
most dangerous years for this small subset of people (and for society as a whole)
are late adolescence and early adulthood. For example, 18- to 24-year-olds
commit most murders in the U.S. (U.S. Federal Bureau of Investigation, 2012).
Gender
At all ages, males tend to be more physically aggressive than females. However,
it would be wrong to think that females are never physically aggressive. Females
do use physical aggression, especially when they are provoked by other females
(Collins, Quigley, & Leonard, 2007). Among heterosexual partners, women are
actually slightly more likely than men to use physical aggression (Archer, 2000).
nobaproject.com - Aggression and Violence
1236
However, when men do use physical aggression, they are more likely than
women to cause serious injuries and even death to their partners. When people
are strongly provoked, gender differences in aggression shrink (Bettencourt &
Miller, 1996).
Females are much more likely than males to engage in relational aggression,
defined as intentionally harming another persons social relationships, feelings
of acceptance, or inclusion within a group (Crick & Grotpeter, 1995). Examples
of relational aggression include gossiping, spreading rumors, withdrawing
affection to get what you want, excluding someone from your circle of friends,
and giving someone the silent treatment.
1237
term Machiavellianism comes from the Italian philosopher and writer Niccol
Machiavelli, who advocated using any means necessary to gain raw political
power, including aggression and violence.
External Factors
Frustration and Other Unpleasant Events
One of the earliest theories of aggression proposed that aggression is caused
by frustration, which was defined as blocking goal-directed behavior (Dollard
et al., 1939).For example, if you are standing in a long line to purchase a ticket,
it is frustrating when someone crowds in front of you.This theory was later
nobaproject.com - Aggression and Violence
1238
expanded to say that all unpleasant events, not just frustrations, cause
aggression (Berkowitz, 1989). Unpleasant events such as frustrations,
provocations, social rejections, hot temperatures, loud noises, bad air (e.g.,
pollution, foul odors, secondhand smoke), and crowding can all cause
aggression. Unpleasant events automatically trigger a fightflight response.
Weapons
Obviously, using a weapon can increase aggression and violence, but can just
seeing a weapon increase aggression? To find out, researchers sat angry
participants at a table that had a shotgun and a revolver on itor, in the control
condition, badminton racquets and shuttlecocks (Berkowitz & LePage, 1967).
The items on the table were supposedly part of a different study, but the
researcher had forgotten to put them away. The participant was supposed to
decide what level of electric shock to deliver to a person pretending to be
another participant, and the electric shocks were used to measure aggression.
The experimenter told participants to ignore the items on the table, but
apparently they could not. Participants who saw the guns gave more shocks
than did participants who saw the sports items. Several other studies have
replicated this so-called weapons effect, including some conducted outside the
lab (Carlson, Marcus-Newhall, & Miller, 1990). For example, one study found
that motorists were more likely to honk their horns at another driver stalled in
a pickup truck with a rifle visible in his rear window than in response to the
same delay from the same truck, but with no gun (Turner, Layton, & Simons,
1975). When you think about it, you would have to be pretty stupid to honk
your horn at a driver with a rifle in his truck. However, drivers were probably
responding in an automatic rather than a deliberate manner. Other research
has shown drivers who have guns in their vehicles are more aggressive drivers
than those without guns in their vehicles (Hemenway, Vriniotis, & Miller, 2006).
1239
Violent Media
There are plenty of aggressive cues in the mass media, such as in TV programs,
films, and video games. In the U.S., the Surgeon General warns the public about
threats to their physical and mental health. Most Americans know that the U.
S. Surgeon General issued a warning about cigarettes in 1964: Warning: The
Surgeon General Has Determined That Cigarette Smoking Is Dangerous to Your
Health. However, most Americans do not know that the U.S. Surgeon General
issued a warning regarding violent TV programs in 1972: It is clear to me that
the causal relationship between televised violence and antisocial behavior is
sufficient to warrant appropriate and immediate remedial action. . . . There
comes a time when the data are sufficient to justify action. That time has come
(Steinfeld, 1972). Since then, hundreds of additional studies have shown that
all forms of violent media can increase aggression (e.g., Anderson & Bushman,
2002). Violent video games might even be more harmful than violent TV
programs, for at least three reasons. First, playing a video game is active,
whereas watching a TV program is passive. Active involvement enhances
learning. One study found that boys who played a violent video game were
more aggressive afterward than were boys who merely watched the same game
(Polman, Orobio de Castro, & van Aken, 2008). Second, video game players are
more likely to identify with a violent character than TV watchers. If the game
involves a first-person shooter, players have the same visual perspective as the
killer. If the game is third person, the player controls the characters actions
from a more distant visual perspective. In either case, the player is linked to a
violent character. Research has shown that people are more aggressive when
they identify with a violent character (e.g., Konijn, Nije Bijvank, & Bushman,
2007). Third, violent games directly reward players for violent behavior by
awarding points or by allowing them to advance in the game. In some games,
players are also rewarded through verbal praise, such as hearing Impressive!
after killing an enemy. In TV programs, reward is not directly tied to the viewers
nobaproject.com - Aggression and Violence
1240
behavior. It is well known that rewarding behavior increases its frequency. One
study found that players were more aggressive after playing a violent game
that rewarded violent actions than after playing the same game that punished
violent actions (Carnagey & Anderson, 2005). The evidence linking violent video
games to aggression is compelling. A comprehensive review found that violent
games increase aggressive thoughts, angry feelings, and aggressive behaviors
and decrease empathic feelings and prosocial behaviors (Anderson et al., 2010).
Similar effects were obtained for males and females, regardless of their age,
and regardless of what country they were from.
Alcohol
Alcohol has long been associated with aggression and violence. In fact,
sometimes alcohol is deliberately used to promote aggression. It has been
standard practice for many centuries to issue soldiers some alcohol before they
went into battle, both to increase aggression and reduce fear (Keegan, 1993).
There is ample evidence of a link between alcohol and aggression, including
evidence from experimental studies showing that consuming alcohol can cause
an increase in aggression (e.g., Lipsey, Wilson, Cohen, & Derzon, 1997). Most
theories of intoxicated aggression fall into one of two categories: (a)
pharmacological theories that focus on how alcohol disrupts cognitive
processes, and (b) expectancy theories that focus on how social attitudes about
alcohol facilitate aggression. Normally, people have strong inhibitions against
behaving aggressively, and pharmacological models focus on how alcohol
reduces these inhibitions. To use a car analogy, alcohol increases aggression
by cutting the brake line rather than by stepping on the gas. How does alcohol
cut the brake line? Alcohol disrupts cognitive executive functions that help us
organize, plan, achieve goals, and inhibit inappropriate behaviors (Giancola,
2000). Alcohol also reduces glucose, which provides energy to the brain for selfcontrol (Gailliot & Baumeister, 2007). Alcohol has a myopic effect on attention
nobaproject.com - Aggression and Violence
1241
1242
Reducing Aggression
Most people are greatly concerned about the amount of aggression in society.
Aggression directly interferes with our basic needs of safety and security. Thus,
it is urgent to find ways to reduce aggression. Because there is no single cause
for aggression, it is difficult to design effective treatments. A treatment that
works for one individual may not work for another individual. And some
extremely aggressive people, such as psychopaths, are considered to be
untreatable. Indeed, many people have started to accept the fact that
aggression and violence have become an inevitable, intrinsic part of our society.
This being said, there certainly are things that can be done to reduce aggression
and violence. Before discussing some effective methods for reducing
aggression, two ineffective methods need to be debunked: catharsis and
punishment.
Catharsis
The term catharsis dates back to Aristotle and means to cleanse or purge.
Aristotle taught that viewing tragic plays gave people emotional release from
negative emotions. In Greek tragedy, the heroes didnt just grow old and retire
they are often murdered. Sigmund Freud revived the ancient notion of
catharsis by proposing that people should express their bottled-up anger. Freud
believed if they repressed it, negative emotions would build up inside the
individual and surface as psychological disorders. According tocatharsis theory,
acting aggressively or even viewing aggression purges angry feelings and
aggressive impulses into harmless channels. Unfortunately for catharsis theory,
research shows the opposite often occurs (e.g., Geen & Quanty, 1977).
nobaproject.com - Aggression and Violence
1243
If venting anger doesnt get rid of it, what does? All emotions, including anger,
consist of bodily states (e.g., arousal) and mental meanings. To get rid of anger,
you can focus on either of those. Anger can be reduced by getting rid of the
arousal state, such as by relaxing, listening to calming music, or counting to 10
before responding. Mental tactics can also reduce anger, such as by reframing
the situation or by distracting oneself and turning ones attention to more
pleasant topics. Incompatible behaviors can also help get rid of anger. For
example, petting a puppy, watching a comedy, kissing your lover, or helping
someone in need, because those acts are incompatible with anger and,
therefore, they make the angry state impossible to sustain (e.g., Baron, 1976).
Viewing the provocative situation from a more distant perspective, such as that
of a fly on the wall, also helps (Mischkowski, Kross, & Bushman,2012).
Punishment
Most cultures assume that punishment is an effective way to deter aggression
and violence. Punishment is defined as inflicting pain or removing pleasure for
a misdeed. Punishment can range in intensity from spanking a child to executing
a convicted killer. Parents use it, organizations use it, and governments use it,
but does it work? Today, aggression researchers have their doubts. Punishment
is most effective when it is: (a) intense, (b) prompt, (c) applied consistently and
with certainty, (d) perceived as justified, and (e) possible to replace the
undesirable punished behavior with a desirable alternative behavior
(Berkowitz, 1993). Even if punishment occurs under these ideal conditions, it
may only suppress aggressive behavior temporarily, and it has several
undesirable long-term consequences. Most important, punishment models the
aggressive behavior it seeks to prevent. Longitudinal studies have shown that
children who are physically punished by their parents at home are more
aggressive outside the home, such as in school (e.g., Lefkowitz, Huesmann, &
Eron, 1978). Because punishment is unpleasant, it can also trigger aggression
nobaproject.com - Aggression and Violence
1244
Successful Interventions
Although specific aggression intervention strategies cannot be discussed in any
detail here, there are two important general points to be made. First, successful
interventions target as many causes of aggression as possible and attempt to
tackle them collectively. Interventions that are narrowly focused at removing a
single cause of aggression, however well conducted, are bound to fail. In general,
external causes are easier to change than internal causes. For example, one
can reduce exposure to violent media or alcohol consumption, and make
unpleasant situations more tolerable (e.g., use air conditioners when it is hot,
reduce crowding in stressful environments such as prisons and psychiatric
wards).
Second, aggression problems are best treated in early development, when
people are still malleable. As was mentioned previously, aggression is very
stable over time, almost as stable as intelligence. If young children display
excessive levels of aggression (often in the form of hitting, biting, or kicking), it
places them at high risk for becoming violent adolescents and even violent
adults. It is much more difficult to alter aggressive behaviors when they are part
of an adult personality, than when they are still in development.
Yoda warned that anger, fear, and aggression are the dark side of the Force.
They are also the dark side of human nature. Fortunately, aggression and
violence are decreasing over time, and this trend should continue. We also know
a lot more now than ever before about what factors increase aggression and
how to treat aggressive behavior problems. When Luke Skywalker was going to
enter the dark cave on Degobah (the fictional Star Wars planet), Yoda said, Your
weapons, you will not need them. Hopefully, there will come a time in the nottoo-distant future when people all over the world will no longer need weapons.
1245
Outside Resources
Book: Bushman, B. J., & Huesmann, L. R. (2010). Aggression. In S. T. Fiske, D.
T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (5th ed.) (pp.
833-863). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Discussion Questions
1. Discuss whether different examples (hypothetical and real) meet the
definition of aggression and the definition of violence.
2. Why do people deny the harmful effects of violent media when the research
evidence linking violent media to aggression is so conclusive?
3. Consider the various causes of aggression described in this chapter and
elsewhere, and discuss whether they can be changed to reduce aggression,
and if so how.
Vocabulary
Aggression
Any behavior intended to harm another person who does not want to be
harmed.
Availability heuristic
The tendency to judge the frequency or likelihood of an event by the ease with
which relevant instances come to mind.
Catharsis
Greek term that means to cleanse or purge. Applied to aggression, catharsis is
the belief that acting aggressively or even viewing aggression purges angry
feelings and aggressive impulses into harmless channels.
Hostile attribution bias
The tendency to perceive ambiguous actions by others as aggressive.
Hostile expectation bias
The tendency to assume that people will react to potential conflicts with
aggression.
Hostile perception bias
The tendency to perceive social interactions in general as being aggressive.
Punishment
Inflicting pain or removing pleasure for a misdeed. Punishment decreases the
likelihood that a behavior will be repeated.
Relational aggression
Intentionally harming another persons social relationships, feelings of
acceptance, or inclusion within a group.
Violence
Aggression intended to cause extreme physical harm, such as injury or death.
Weapons effect
The increase in aggression that occurs as a result of the mere presence of a
weapon.
Reference List
Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B.J., Sakamoto, A.,
Rothstein, H. R., & Saleem, M. (2010). Violent video-game effects on
aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in Eastern and Western
countries. Psychological Bulletin, 136 (pp. 151-173).
Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Media violence and societal violence.
Science, 295 (pp. 2377-2378).
Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (1997). External validity of trivial experiments:
The case of laboratory aggression. Review of General Psychology, 1 (pp.
19-41).
Archer, J. (2000). Sex differences in aggression between heterosexual partners:
A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 126 (pp. 651-680).
Baron, R. A. (1976). The reduction of human aggression: A field study of the
influence of incompatible reactions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 6
(pp. 260-274).
Baron, R. A., & Richardson, D. R. (1994). Human aggression (2nd ed.). New York:
Plenum Press.
Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination and
reformulation. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59-73.
Berkowitz, L., & LePage, A. (1967). Weapons as aggression-eliciting stimuli.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 202-207.
Lane.
Mischkowski, D., Kross, E., & Bushman, B. J. (2012). Flies on the wall are less
aggressive: Self-distanced reflection reduces angry feelings, aggressive
thoughts, and aggressive behaviors. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 48, 1187-1191.
Olweus, D. (1979). The stability of aggressive reaction patterns in males: A review.
Psychological Bulletin, 86, 852-875.
Paulhus, D. L, & Williams, K. M. (2002). The dark triad of personality: Narcissism,
Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36
(6) (pp. 556-563).
Pinker, S. (2011). The better angels of our nature. New York: Viking.
Polman, J., Orobio de Castro, B., & van Aken, M. (2008). Experimental study of
the differential effects of playing versus watching violent video games on
childrens aggressive behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 34(3) (pp. 256-264).
Steele, C. M., & Josephs, R. A. (1990). Alcohol myopia: Its prized and dangerous
effects. American Psychologist, 45 (pp. 921-933).
Steinfeld, J. (1972). Statement in hearings before Subcommittee on
Communications of Committee on Commerce (United States Senate, Serial
#9252) (pp. 25-27). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Subra, B., Muller, D., Bgue, L., Bushman, B. J., & Delmas, F. (2010). Effects of
alcohol and weapon cues on aggressive thoughts and behaviors. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(8) (pp. 1052-1057).
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Aggression and Violence by Brad J. Bushman is
licensed
under
the
Creative
Commons
Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0
Abstract
People often act to benefit other people, and these acts are examples of
prosocial behavior. Such behaviors may come in many guises: helping an
individual in need; sharing personal resources; volunteering time, effort, and
expertise; cooperating with others to achieve some common goals. The focus
of this chapter is on helpingprosocial acts in dyadic situations in which one
person is in need and another provides the necessary assistance to eliminate
the others need. Although people are often in need, help is not always given.
Why not? The decision of whether or not to help is not as simple and
straightforward as it might seem, and many factors need to be considered by
those who might help. In this chapter, we will try to understand how the decision
to help is made by answering the question: Who helps when and why?
Learning Objectives
Learn which situational and social factors affect when a bystander will help
another in need.
Discover whether we help others out of a sense of altruistic concern for the
victim, for more self-centered and egoistic motives, or both.
Introduction
Go to YouTube and search for episodes of Primetime: What Would You Do?
You will find video segments in which apparently innocent individuals are
victimized, while onlookers typically fail to intervene. The events are all staged,
but they are very real to the bystanders on the scene. The entertainment offered
is the nature of the bystanders responses, and viewers are outraged when
bystanders fail to intervene. They are convinced that they would have helped.
But would they? Viewers are overly optimistic in their beliefs that they would
play the hero. Helping may occur frequently, but help is not always given to
those in need. So when do people help, and when do they not? All people are
not equally helpfulwho helps? Why would a person help another in the first
place? Many factors go into a persons decision to helpa fact that the viewers
do not fully appreciate. This chapter will answer the question: Who helps when
and why?
1260
most. Trying to understand why people do not always help became the focus
of bystander intervention research (e.g., Latan & Darley, 1970).
To answer the question regarding when people help, researchers have
focused on
1. how bystanders come to define emergencies,
2. when they decide to take responsibility for helping, and
3. how the costs and benefits of intervening affect their decisions of whether
to help.
1261
1262
Who Helps?
Do you know someone who always seems to be ready, willing, and able to help?
Do you know someone who never helps out? It seems there are personality
and individual differences in the helpfulness of others. To answer the question
of who chooses to help, researchers have examined 1) the role that sex and
gender play in helping, 2) what personality traits are associated with helping,
and 3) the characteristics of the prosocial personality.
1263
1264
Crowley, 1986). There may be some specialization in the types of help given by
the two sexes, but it is nice to know that there is someone out thereman or
womanwho is able to give you the help that you need, regardless of what
kind of help it might be.
1265
tied to those in need, understand the problems the victim is experiencing, and
have a heightened sense of moral obligation to be helpful. This factor has been
shown to be highly correlated with the trait of agreeableness discussed
previously. The second characteristic, helpfulness, is more behaviorally
oriented. Those high on the helpfulness factor have been helpful in the past,
and because they believe they can be effective with the help they give, they are
more likely to be helpful in the future.
Why Help?
Finally, the question of why a person would help needs to be asked. What
motivation is there for that behavior? Psychologists have suggested that 1)
evolutionary forces may serve to predispose humans to help others, 2) egoistic
concerns may determine if and when help will be given, and 3) selfless, altruistic
motives may also promote helping in some cases.
1266
But, we do not restrict our relationships just to our own family members.
We live in groups that include individuals who are unrelated to us, and we often
help them too. Why? Reciprocal altruism (Trivers, 1971) provides the answer.
Because of reciprocal altruism, we are all better off in the long run if we help
one another. If helping someone now increases the chances that you will be
helped later, then your overall chances of survival are increased. There is the
chance that someone will take advantage of your help and not return your
favors. But people seem predisposed to identify those who fail to reciprocate,
and punishments including social exclusion may result (Buss, 2004). Cheaters
will not enjoy the benefit of help from others, reducing the likelihood of the
survival of themselves and their kin.
Evolutionary forces may provide a general inclination for being helpful, but
they may not be as good an explanation for why we help in the here and now.
What factors serve as proximal influences for decisions to help?
1267
1268
Altruistic help
Although many researchers believe that egoism is the only motivation for
helping, others suggest that altruismhelping that has as its ultimate goal the
improvement of anothers welfaremay also be a motivation for helping under
the right circumstances. Batson (2011) has offered the empathyaltruism
model to explain altruistically motivated helping for which the helper expects
no benefits. According to this model, the key for altruism is empathizing with
the victim, that is, putting oneself in the shoes of the victim and imagining how
the victim must feel. When taking this perspective and having empathic
concern, potential helpers become primarily interested in increasing the wellbeing of the victim, even if the helper must incur some costs that might
otherwise be easily avoided. The empathyaltruism model does not dismiss
egoistic motivations; helpers not empathizing with a victim may experience
personal distress and have an egoistic motivation, not unlike the feelings and
motivations explained by the arousal: costreward model. Because egoistically
motivated individuals are primarily concerned with their own costbenefit
outcomes, they are less likely to help if they think they can escape the situation
with no costs to themselves. In contrast, altruistically motivated helpers are
willing to accept the cost of helping to benefit a person with whom they have
empathizedthis self-sacrificial approach to helping is the hallmark of
altruism (Batson, 2011).
Although there is still some controversy about whether people can ever act
for purely altruistic motives, it is important to recognize that, while helpers may
derive some personal rewards by helping another, the help that has been given
is also benefitting someone who was in need. The residents who offered food,
blankets, and shelter to stranded runners who were unable to get back to their
hotel rooms because of the Boston Marathon bombing undoubtedly received
positive rewards because of the help they gave, but those stranded runners
who were helped got what they needed badly as well. In fact, it is quite
nobaproject.com - Helping and Prosocial Behavior
1269
remarkable how the fates of people who have never met can be so intertwined
and complementary. Your benefit is mine; and mine is yours (Dovidio et al.,
2006, p. 143).
Conclusion
We started this chapter by asking the question, Who helps when and why? As
we have shown, the question of when help will be given is not quite as simple
as the viewers of What Would You Do? believe. The power of the situation that
operates on potential helpers in real time is not fully considered. What might
appear to be a split-second decision to help is actually the result of consideration
of multiple situational factors (e.g., the helpers interpretation of the situation,
the presence and ability of others to provide the help, the results of a cost
benefit analysis) (Dovidio et al., 2006). We have found that men and women
tend to help in different waysmen are more impulsive and physically active,
while women are more nurturing and supportive. Personality characteristics
such as agreeableness and the prosocial personality orientation also affect
peoples likelihood of giving assistance to others. And, why would people help
in the first place? In addition to evolutionary forces (e.g., kin selection, reciprocal
altruism), there is extensive evidence to show that helping and prosocial acts
may be motivated by selfish, egoistic desires; by selfless, altruistic goals; or by
some combination of egoistic and altruistic motives. (For a fuller consideration
of the field of prosocial behavior, we refer you to Dovidio et al. [2006].)
1270
Outside Resources
Book: Batson, C.D. (2009). Altruism in humans. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Book: Dovidio, J. F., Piliavin, J. A., Schroeder, D. A., & Penner, L. A. (2006). The
social psychology of prosocial behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Book: Mikuliner, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2010). Prosocial motives, emotions, and
behavior: The better angels of our nature. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Discussion Questions
1. Pluralistic ignorance suggests that inactions by other observers of an
emergency will decrease the likelihood that help will be given. What do you
think will happen if even one other observer begins to offer assistance to a
victim?
2. In addition to those mentioned in the chapter, what other costs and rewards
might affect a potential helpers decision of whether to help? Receiving help
to solve some problem is an obvious benefit for someone in need; are there
any costs that a person might have to bear as a result of receiving help from
someone?
3. What are the characteristics possessed by your friends who are most
helpful? By your friends who are least helpful? What has made your helpful
friends and your unhelpful friends so different? What kinds of help have
they given to you, and what kind of help have you given to them? Are you
a helpful person?
4. Do you think that sex and gender differences in the frequency of helping
and the kinds of helping have changed over time? Why? Do you think that
we might expect more changes in the future?
5. What do you think is the primary motive for helping behavior: egoism or
altruism? Are there any professions in which people are being pure
altruists, or are some egoistic motivations always playing a role?
6. There are other prosocial behaviors in addition to the kind of helping
discussed here. People volunteer to serve many different causes and
organizations. People come together to cooperate with one another to
achieve goals that no one individual could reach alone. How do you think
the factors that affect helping might affect prosocial actions such as
volunteering and cooperating? Do you think that there might be other
factors that make people more or less likely to volunteer their time and
energy or to cooperate in a group?
Vocabulary
Agreeableness
A core personality trait that includes such dispositional characteristics as being
sympathetic, generous, forgiving, and helpful, and behavioral tendencies
toward harmonious social relations and likeability.
Altruism
A motivation for helping that has the improvement of anothers welfare as its
ultimate goal, with no expectation of any benefits for the helper.
Bystander intervention
The phenomenon whereby people intervene to help others in need even if the
other is a complete stranger and the intervention puts the helper at risk.
Costbenefit analysis
A decision-making process that compares the cost of an action or thing against
the expected benefit to help determine the best course of action.
Diffusion of responsibility
When deciding whether to help a person in need, knowing that there are others
who could also provide assistance relieves bystanders of some measure of
personal responsibility, reducing the likelihood that bystanders will intervene.
Egoism
A motivation for helping that has the improvement of the helpers own
circumstances as its primary goal.
Empathic concern
According to Batsons empathyaltruism hypothesis, observers who empathize
with a person in need (that is, put themselves in the shoes of the victim and
imagine how that person feels) will experience empathic concern and have an
altruistic motivation for helping.
Empathyaltruism model
An altruistic theory proposed by Batson (2011) that claims that people who put
themselves in the shoes of a victim and imagining how the victim feel will
experience empathic concern that evokes an altruistic motivation for helping.
Helpfulness
A component of the prosocial personality orientation; describes individuals who
have been helpful in the past and, because they believe they can be effective
with the help they give, are more likely to be helpful in the future.
Helping
Prosocial acts that typically involve situations in which one person is in need
and another provides the necessary assistance to eliminate the others need.
Kin selection
According to evolutionary psychology, the favoritism shown for helping our
blood relatives, with the goals of increasing the likelihood that some portion of
our DNA will be passed on to future generations.
Negative state relief model
An egoistic theory proposed by Cialdini et al. (1982) that claims that people have
learned through socialization that helping can serve as a secondary
reinforcement that will relieve negative moods such as sadness.
Other-oriented empathy
A component of the prosocial personality orientation; describes individuals who
have a strong sense of social responsibility, empathize with and feel emotionally
tied to those in need, understand the problems the victim is experiencing, and
have a heightened sense of moral obligations to be helpful.
Personal distress
According to Batsons empathyaltruism hypothesis, observers who take a
detached view of a person in need will experience feelings of being worried
and upset and will have an egoistic motivation for helping to relieve that
distress.
Pluralistic ignorance
Relying on the actions of others to define an ambiguous need situation and to
then erroneously conclude that no help or intervention is necessary.
Prosocial behavior
Social behavior that benefits another person.
Prosocial personality orientation
A measure of individual differences that identifies two sets of personality
characteristics (other-oriented empathy, helpfulness) that are highly correlated
with prosocial behavior.
Reciprocal altruism
According to evolutionary psychology, a genetic predisposition for people to
help those who have previously helped them.
Reference List
Batson, C. D. (2011). Altruism in humans. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Becker, S. W., & Eagly, A. H. (2004). The heroism of women and men. American
Psychologist, 59, 163178.
Burnstein, E., Crandall, C., & Kitayama, S. (1994). Some neo-Darwinian decision
rules for altruism: Weighing cues for inclusive fitness as a function of the
biological importance of the decision. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 67, 773789.
Buss, D. M. (2004). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind.
Boston, MA: Allyn Bacon.
Cialdini, R. B., Darby, B. K. & Vincent, J. E. (1973). Transgression and altruism: A
case for hedonism. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 9, 502516.
Cialdini, R. B., Kenrick, D. T., & Baumann, D. J. (1982). Effects of mood on prosocial
behavior in children and adults. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), The development of
prosocial behavior (pp. 339359). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Cialdini, R. B., & Kenrick, D. T. (1976). Altruism as hedonism: A social
developmental perspective on the relationship of negative mood state and
helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 907914.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1998). Trait theories in personality. In D. F. Barone,
M. Hersen, & V. B. Van Hasselt (Eds.), Advanced Personality (pp. 103121).
New York, NY: Plenum.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Helping and Prosocial Behavior by Dennis L.
Poepsel and David A. Schroeder is licensed under the Creative Commons AttributionNonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Positive Relationships
Nathaniel M. Lambert
Brigham Young University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Most research in the realm of relationships has examined that which can go
wrong in relationships (e.g., conflict, infidelity, intimate partner violence). I
summarize much of what has been examined about what goes right in a
relationship and call these positive relationship deposits. Some research
indicates that relationships need five positive interactions for every negative
interaction. Active-constructive responding, gratitude, forgiveness, and time
spent together are some sources of positive deposits in ones relational bank
account. These kinds of deposits can reduce the negative effects of conflict on
marriage and strengthen relationships.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
The status of close relationships in America can sometimes look a bit grim.
More than half of marriages now end in divorce in the United States (Pinsof,
2002). Infidelity is the leading cause of divorce (Priviti & Amato, 2004) and is on
the rise across all age groups (Allen et al., 2008). Cybersex has likely contributed
to the increased rates of infidelity, with some 65% of those who look for sex
online having intercourse with their Internet partner offline as well. Research
on intimate partner violence indicates that it occurs at alarmingly high rates,
with over one-fifth of couples reporting at least one episode of violence over
the course of a year (Schafer, Caetano, & Clark, 1998). These and other issues
that arise in relationships (e.g., substance abuse, conflict) represent significant
obstacles to close relationships. With so many problems that plague
relationships, how can a positive relationship be cultivated? Is there some magic
bullet or ratio? Yes, kind of.
1287
opportunities for deposits than for withdrawals. Conversely, even though there
may be fewer negatives, Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Finkenauer, and Vohs (2001)
argue quite persuasively that bad events overpower good events in ones life,
which suggests that the negative withdrawals are more salient and more
impactful. This further accentuates the need to ensure that we have a healthy
store of positive deposits that can help to counteract these more impactful
account withdrawals. Positive deposits that accumulate over time should
provide a buffer against the withdrawals that happen in every relationship. In
other words, the inevitable occasional conflict is not nearly so bad for the
relationship when it occurs in a partnership that is otherwise highly positive.
What opportunities does relationships science suggest are effective
opportunities each day to make positive relationship deposits?
1288
1289
1290
If partners listen and are enthusiastic about the good news of the other,
they build a stronger relationship. If they ignore the good news, change the
subject, devalue the good news, or refocus the good news to be about
themselves, they may make a withdrawal from the account. Being aware of this
research and findings can help individuals to focus on better providing helpful
responses to those they care about.
1291
Gratitude
Relationship researchers report that expressing gratitude on a regular basis is
an important means by which positive deposits may be made into relationship
bank accounts. In a recent study, participants were randomly assigned to write
about daily events, express gratitude to a friend, discuss a positive memory
with a friend, or think grateful thoughts about a friend twice a week for three
weeks. At the conclusion of the three weeks, those who were randomly assigned
to express gratitude to their friend reported higher positive regard for their
friend and more comfort voicing relationship concerns than did those in the
two control conditions (Lambert & Fincham, 2011). Also, those who expressed
gratitude to a close relationship partner reported greater perceived communal
strength (e.g., caring, willingness to sacrifice) than participants in all control
conditions (Lambert, Clark, Durtschi, Fincham, & Graham, 2010). Similarly,
Algoe, Fredrickson, and Gable (2013) found that benefactors positive
perceptions of beneficiaries were increased when gratitude was expressed for
the benefit, and these perceptions enhanced relationship quality. These studies
suggest that expressing gratitude to someone you are close to is an important
way of making positive relationship deposits.
Forgiveness
Forgiveness is something else you can do regularly to aid relationship
satisfaction (e.g., Fincham, 2000; Paleari, Regalia, & Fincham, 2003) and
commitment (e.g., Finkel, Rusbult, Kumashiro, & Hannon, 2002; Karremans &
Van Lange, 2008). Unresolved conflict can put couples at risk of developing the
negative cycle of interaction that causes further harm to relationships. For
instance, one study found that lack of forgiveness is linked to ineffective conflict
resolution (Fincham, Beach, & Davila, 2004). For instance, if Cindy cannot forgive
Joe, Cindy will struggle to effectively resolve other disagreements in their
nobaproject.com - Positive Relationships
1292
relationship. Yet, those who do forgive report much better conflict resolution
a year later (Fincham, Beach, & Davila, 2007). It appears that forgiveness can
be an important way of building emotional capital in the relationship. Not
forgiving the people in your life can block positive deposits to the relationship
bank account.
1293
1294
Discussion Questions
1. What are some of the main challenges that face relationships today?
2. How would you describe the concept of an emotional bank account?
3. What are some ways people can make deposits to their relationship bank
accounts?
4. What do you think are the most effective ways for making positive
relationship deposits?
5. What are some of the most powerful relationship deposits that others have
made into your relationship bank account?
6. What would you consider to be some challenging or engaging activities that
you would consider doing more of with a close relationship partner?
7. Are there relationships of yours that have gotten into a negative spiral and
could profit from positive relationship deposits?
Vocabulary
Active-constructive responding
Demonstrating sincere interest and enthusiasm for the good news of another
person.
Capitalization
Seeking out someone else with whom to share your good news.
Relationship bank account
An account you hold with every person in which a positive deposit or a negative
withdrawal can be made during every interaction you have with the person.
Self-expansion model
Seeking to increase ones capacity often through an intimate relationship.
Reference List
Algoe, S. B., Fredrickson, B. L. & Gable, S. L. (2013) The social functions of the
emotion of gratitude via expression. Emotion, 13, 605-609.
Allen, E. S., Atkins, D. C., Baucom, D. H., Snyder, D. K., Gordon, K. C., & Glass, S.
P. (2005). Intrapersonal, interpersonal, and contextual factors in engaging
in and responding to extramarital involvement. Clinical Psychology: Science
and Practice, 12, 101130.
Aron, A., Norman, C., Aron, E., McKenna, C., & Heyman, R. (2000). Couples shared
participation in novel and arousing activities and experienced relationship
quality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 273284.
Aron, A., & Aron, E. N. (1996). Self and self-expansion in relationships. In G. J. O.
Fletcher & J. Fitness (Eds.), Knowledge structures in close relationships: A
social psychological approach (pp. 325344). Mahway, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is
stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323-370.
Covey, S. R. (1989). The seven habits of highly effective people. New York, NY:
Simon and Schuster.
Driver, J., & Gottman, J. (2004). Daily marital interactions and positive affect
during marital conflict among newlywed couples. Family Process, 43, 301
314.
Fincham, F., Beach, S., & Davila, J. (2004). Conflict resolution in marriage and
Janicki, D., Kamarck, T., Shiffman, S., & Gwaltney, C. (2006). Application of
ecological momentary assessment to the study of marital adjustment and
social interactions during daily life. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 168
172.
Karremans, J. C., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2008). Forgiveness in interpersonal
relationships: Its malleability and powerful consequences. European Review
of Social Psychology, 19, 202241.
Lambert, N. M., Clarke, M. S., Durtschi, J. A., Fincham, F. D., & Graham, S. M.
(2010). Benefits of expressing gratitude: Expressing gratitude to a partner
changes ones view of the relationship. Psychological Science, 21, 574580.
Lambert, N. M., & Fincham, F. D. (2011). Expressing gratitude to a partner leads
to more relationship maintenance behavior. Emotion, 11, 5260.
Paleari, G., Regalia, C., & Fincham, F. D. (2003). Adolescents willingness to forgive
parents: An empirical model. Parenting: Science and Practice, 3, 155174.
Patrick, S., Sells, J. N., Giordano, F. G., & Tollerud, T. R. (2007). Intimacy,
differentiation, and personality variables as predictors of marital
satisfaction. The Family Journal, 15, 359367.
Pinsof, W. M. (2002). The death of till death us do part: The transformation of
pair-bonding in the 20th century. Family Process, 41(2), 135157.
Prager, K. J., & Buhrmester, D. (1998). Intimacy and need fulfillment in couple
relationships. Journal of Social & Personal Relationships, 15, 435469.
Schafer, J., Caetano, R., & Clark, C. L. (1998). Rates of intimate partner violence
Abstract
More attractive people elicit more positive first impressions. This effect is called
the attractiveness halo, and it is shown when judging those with more attractive
faces, bodies, or voices. Moreover, it yields significant social outcomes, including
advantages to attractive people in domains as far-reaching as romance,
friendships, family relations, education, work, and criminal justice. Physical
qualities that increase attractiveness include youthfulness, symmetry,
averageness, masculinity in men, and femininity in women. Positive expressions
and behaviors also raise evaluations of a persons attractiveness. Cultural,
cognitive, evolutionary, and overgeneralization explanations have been offered
to explain why we find certain people attractive. Whereas the evolutionary
explanation predicts that the impressions associated with the halo effect will
be accurate, the other explanations do not. Although the research evidence
does show some accuracy, it is too weak to satisfactorily account for the positive
responses shown to more attractive people.
Learning Objectives
Learn the
Know what
attractiveness.
Understand
Learn about
attractiveness.
1305
Attractiveness not only elicits positive trait impressions, but it also provides
advantages in a wide variety of social situations. In a classic study, attractiveness,
rather than measures of personality or intelligence, predicted whether
individuals randomly paired on a blind date wanted to contact their partner
again (Walster, Aronson, Abrahams, & Rottman, 1966). Although attractiveness
has a greater influence on mens romantic preferences than womens (Feingold,
1990), it has significant effects for both sexes. Attractive men and women
become sexually active earlier than their less attractive peers. Also,
attractiveness in men is positively related to the number of short-term, but not
long-term, sexual partners, whereas the reverse is true for women (Rhodes,
Simmons, & Peters, 2005). These results suggest that attractiveness in both
sexes is associated with greater reproductive success, since success for men
depends more on short-term mating opportunitiesmore mates increases the
probability of offspringand success for women depends more on long-term
mating opportunitiesa committed mate increases the probability of offspring
survival. Of course, not everyone can win the most attractive mate, and research
shows a matching effect. More attractive people expect to date individuals
higher in attractiveness than do unattractive people (Montoya, 2008), and actual
romantic couples are similar in attractiveness (Feingold, 1988). The appeal of
attractive people extends to platonic friendships. More attractive people are
more popular with their peers, and this is shown even in early childhood
(Langlois et al., 2000).
nobaproject.com - Attraction and Beauty
1306
The attractiveness halo is also found in situations where one would not
expect it to make such a difference. For example, research has shown that
strangers are more likely to help an attractive than an unattractive person by
mailing a lost letter containing a graduate school application with an attached
photograph (Benson, Karabenick, & Lerner, 1976). More attractive job
applicants are preferred in hiring decisions for a variety of jobs, and attractive
people receive higher salaries (Dipboye, Arvey, & Terpstra, 1977; Hamermesh
& Biddle, 1994; Hosoda, Stone-Romero, & Coats, 2003). Facial attractiveness
also affects political and judicial outcomes. More attractive congressional
candidates are more likely to be elected, and more attractive defendants
convicted of crimes receive lighter sentences (Stewart, 1980; Verhulst, Lodge,
& Lavine, 2010). Body attractiveness also contributes to social outcomes. A
smaller percentage of overweight than normal-weight college applicants are
admitted despite similar high school records (Canning & Mayer, 1966), parents
are less likely to pay for the education of their heavier weight children (Crandall,
1991), and overweight people are less highly recommended for jobs despite
equal qualifications (Larkin & Pines, 1979). Voice qualities also have social
outcomes. College undergraduates express a greater desire to affiliate with
other students who have more attractive voices (Miyake & Zuckerman, 1993),
and politicians with more attractive voices are more likely to win elections
(Gregory & Gallagher, 2002; Tigue, Borak, OConnor, Schandl, & Feinberg, 2012).
These are but a few of the research findings clearly demonstrating that we are
unable to adhere to the conventional wisdom not to judge a book by its cover.
1307
1308
in women and more masculine, lower-pitched voices are more attractive in men
(Collins, 2000; Puts, Barndt, Welling, Dawood, & Burriss, 2011). In the case of
bodies, features that increase attractiveness include a more sex-typical waistto-hip rationarrower waist than hips for women but not for menas well as
a physique that is not emaciated or grossly obese. Negative reactions to obesity
are present from a young age. For example, a classic study found that when
children were asked to rank-order their preferences for children with various
disabilities who were depicted in pictures, the overweight child was ranked the
lowest, even lower than a child who was missing a hand, one who was seated
in a wheelchair, and one with a facial scar (Richardson, Goodman, Hastorf, &
Dornbusch, 1961).
Although there are many physical qualities that influence attractiveness, no
single quality seems to be a necessary or sufficient condition for high
attractiveness. A person with a perfectly symmetrical face may not be attractive
if the eyes are too close together or too far apart. One can also imagine a woman
with beautiful skin or a man with a masculine facial features who is not attractive.
Even a person with a perfectly average face may not be attractive if the face is
the average of a population of 90-year-olds. These examples suggest that a
combination of features are required for high attractiveness. In the case of
mens attraction to women, a desirable combination appears to include
perceived youthfulness, sexual maturity, and approachability (Cunningham,
1986). In contrast, a single quality, like extreme distance from the average face,
is sufficient for low attractiveness. Although certain physical qualities are
generally viewed as more attractive, anatomy is not destiny. Attractiveness is
positively related to smiling and facial expressivity (Riggio & Friedman, 1986),
and there also is some truth to the maxim pretty is as pretty does. Research
has shown that students are more likely to judge an instructors physical
appearance as appealing when his behavior is warm and friendly than when it
is cold and distant (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977), and people rate a woman as more
physically attractive when they have a favorable description of her personality
nobaproject.com - Attraction and Beauty
1309
For example, the long neck on the woman shown in Figure 1 is unlikely to
be judged attractive by Westerners. Yet, long necks have been preferred in a
traditional Myanmar tribe, because they are thought to resemble a mythological
dragon who spawned them. Despite cultural variations like this, research has
provided strong evidence against the claim that attractiveness is only due to
social learning. Indeed, young infants prefer to look at faces that adults have
nobaproject.com - Attraction and Beauty
1310
1311
preference would further predict that the people who are most attractive
depend on our learning experiences, since what is average or prototypical in a
face, voice, or body will depend on the people we have seen. Consistent with
an effect of learning experiences, young infants prefer face morphs that are an
average of faces they have previously seen over morphs that are an average of
novel faces (Rubenstein, Kalakanis, & Langlois, 1999). Short-term perceptual
experiences can influence judgments of attractiveness even in adults. Brief
exposure to a series of faces with the same distortion increases the rated
attractiveness of new faces with that distortion (Rhodes, Jeffery, Watson,
Clifford, & Nakayama, 2003), and exposure to morphs of human and
chimpanzee faces increases the rated attractiveness of new human faces
morphed with a small degree of chimpanzee face (Principe & Langlois, 2012).
1312
Figure 2. Top. An averaged face created from 32 individual faces. Bottom left. Original face
from Martinez & Benevente (1998). Bottom middle. Original face morphed toward the average
face. Bottom right. Original face morphed away from the average face.
One reason average stimuli, including faces, may be preferred is that they
are easy to categorize, and when a stimulus is easy to categorize, it elicits positive
emotion (Winkielman, Halberstadt, Fazendeiro, & Catty, 2006). Another possible
reason average stimuli may be preferred is that we may be less apprehensive
about familiar-looking stimuli (Zajonc, 2001). All other things equal, we prefer
stimuli we have seen before over novel ones, a mere-exposure effect, and we
also prefer stimuli that are similar to those we have seen before, a generalized
mere-exposure
effect.
Consistent
with
reduced
apprehensiveness
1313
participants saw, but also new faces from the familiarized other-race category
(Zebrowitz & Zhang, 2012). Such a generalized mere-exposure effect also could
explain the preference for average stimuli, which look more familiar, although
the effect may be more reliable for judgments of likeability than attractiveness
(Rhodes, Halberstadt, & Brajkovich, 2001; Rhodes, Halberstadt, Jeffery, &
Palermo,
2005).
Whether
due
to
ease
of
categorization
or
less
apprehensiveness, the cognitive explanation holds that certain people are more
attractive because perceptual learning has rendered them more familiar.
1314
1315
Although the level of attractiveness provides a valid cue to low, but not high,
intelligence or health, it is important to bear in mind that attractiveness is only
a weak predictor of these traits, even in the range where it has some validity.
The finding that low, but not high, attractiveness can be diagnostic of actual
traits is consistent with another explanation for why we find particular people
attractive. This has been dubbed anomalous face overgeneralization, but it
could equally apply to anomalous voices or bodies. The evolutionary account
has typically assumed that as attractiveness increases, so does fitness, and it
has emphasized the greater fitness of highly attractive individuals, a good genes
effect (Buss, 1989). In contrast, the overgeneralization hypothesis argues that
the level of attractiveness provides an accurate index only of low fitness. On
this account, the attractiveness halo effect is a by-product of reactions to low
fitness. More specifically, we overgeneralize the adaptive tendency to use low
attractiveness as an indication of lower-than-average health and intelligence,
and we mistakenly use higher-than-average attractiveness as an indication of
higher-than-average health and intelligence (Zebrowitz & Rhodes, 2004). The
overgeneralization hypothesis differs from the evolutionary hypothesis in
another important respect. It is concerned with the importance of detecting
low fitness not only when choosing a mate, but also in other social interactions.
This is consistent with the fact that the attractiveness halo effect is present in
many domains.
Whereas the cultural, cognitive, and overgeneralization accounts of
attractiveness do not necessarily predict that the halo effect in impressions will
be accurate, the evolutionary good genes account does. As we have seen,
there is some support for this prediction, but the effects are too weak and
circumscribed to fully explain the strong halo effect in response to highly
attractive people. In addition, it is important to recognize that whatever accuracy
there is does not necessarily imply a genetic link between attractiveness and
adaptive traits, such as health or intelligence. One non-genetic mechanism is
an influence of environmental factors. For example, the quality of nutrition and
nobaproject.com - Attraction and Beauty
1316
Conclusions
Although it may seem unfair, attractiveness confers many advantages. More
attractive people are favored not only as romantic partners but, more
surprisingly, by their parents, peers, teachers, employers, and even judges and
voters. Moreover, there is substantial agreement about who is attractive, with
infants and perceivers from diverse cultures showing similar responses.
Although this suggests that cultural influences cannot completely explain
attractiveness, experience does have an influence. There is controversy about
why certain people are attractive to us. The cognitive account attributes higher
attractiveness to the ease of processing prototypes or the safety associated
with familiar stimuli. The evolutionary account attributes higher attractiveness
to the adaptive value of preferring physical qualities that signal better health
or genetic fitness when choosing mates. The overgeneralization account
attributes higher attractiveness to the overgeneralization of an adaptive
avoidance of physical qualities that signal poor health or low genetic fitness.
Although there is debate as to which explanation is best, it is important to realize
that all of the proposed mechanisms may have some validity.
1317
Outside Resources
Video: Multiple videos realted to the science of beauty
http://dsc.discovery.com/search.htm?terms=science+of+beauty
Video: Multiple videos related to the science of sex appeal
http://dsc.discovery.com/search.htm?terms=science+of+sex+appeal
Discussion Questions
1. Why do you think the attractiveness halo exists even though there is very
little evidence that attractive people are more intelligent or healthy?
2. What cultural influences affect whom you perceive as attractive? Why?
3. How do you think evolutionary theories of why faces are attractive apply in
a modern world, where people are much more likely to survive and
reproduce, regardless of how intelligent or healthy they are?
4. Which of the theories do you think provides the most compelling
explanation for why we find certain people attractive?
Vocabulary
Anomalous face overgeneralization hypothesis
Proposes that the attractiveness halo effect is a
by-product of reactions to low fitness. People overgeneralize the adaptive
tendency to use low attractiveness as an indicator of negative traits, like low
health or intelligence, and mistakenly use higher-than-average attractiveness
as an indicator of high health or intelligence.
Mere-exposure effect
The tendency to prefer stimuli that have been seen before over novel ones.
There also is a generalized mere-exposure effect shown in a preference for
stimuli that are similar to those that have been seen before.
Morph
A face or other image that has been transformed by a computer program so
that it is a mixture of multiple images.
Prototype
A typical, or average, member of a category. Averageness increases
attractiveness.
Reference List
Benson, P. L., Karabenick, S. A., & Lerner, R. M. (1976). Pretty pleases: The effects
of physical attractiveness, race, and sex on receiving help. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 12, 409415.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary
hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 149.
Canning, H., & Mayer, J. (1966). Obesityits possible effect on college
acceptance. New England Journal of Medicine, 275, 11721174.
Collins, S. A. (2000). Men's voices and women's choices. Animal Behaviour, 60,
773780.
Crandall, C. S. (1991). Do heavy-weight students have more difficulty paying for
college? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 606611.
Cunningham, M. R. (1986). Measuring the physical in physical attractiveness:
Quasi-experiments on the sociobiology of female facial beauty. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 925935.
Cunningham, M. R., Roberts, A. R., Barbee, A. P., Druen, P. B., & Wu, C. (1995).
'Their ideas of beauty are, on the whole, the same as ours': Consistency and
variability in the cross-cultural perception of female physical attractiveness.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 261279.
Dipboye, R. L., Arvey, R. D., & Terpstra, D. E. (1977). Sex and physical
attractiveness of raters and applicants as determinants of rsum
evaluations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 62, 288294.
Eagly, A. H., Ashmore, R. D., Makhijani, M. G., & Longo, L. C. (1991). What is
beautiful is good, but . . . : A meta-analytic review of research on the physical
attractiveness stereotype. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 109128.
Feingold, A. (1990). Gender differences in effects of physical attractiveness on
romantic attraction: A comparison across five research paradigms. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 981993.
Feingold, A. (1988). Matching for attractiveness in romantic partners and samesex friends: A meta-analysis and theoretical critique. Psychological Bulletin,
104, 226235.
Folstad, I., & Karter, A. J. (1992). Parasites, bright males, and the
immunocompetence handicap. American Naturalist, 139, 603622.
Foroud, T. W., Wetherill, L., Vinci-Booher, S., Moore, E. S., Ward, R. E., Hoyme, H.
E. Jacobson, S. W. (2012). Relation over time between facial measurements
and cognitive outcomes in fetal alcohol-exposed children. Alcoholism:
Clinical and Experimental Research, 36, 16341646.
Franklin, R. G., Jr., & Adams, R. B., Jr. (2010). The two halves of beauty: Laterality
and the duality of facial attractiveness. Brain and Cognition, 72, 300305.
Franklin, R. G., Jr., & Adams, R. B., Jr. (2009). A dual-process account of female
facial attractiveness preferences: Sexual and nonsexual routes. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 11561159.
Gregory, S. W., & Gallagher, T. J. (2002). Spectral analysis of candidates' nonverbal
vocal communication: Predicting U.S. presidential election outcomes. Social
Psychology Quarterly, 65, 298308.
Gross, A. E., & Crofton, C. (1977). What is good is beautiful. Sociometry, 40, 85
90.
Halberstadt, J. B. (2006). The generality and ultimate origins of the attractiveness
of prototypes. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 166183.
Hamermesh, D. S., & Biddle, J. E. (1994). Beauty and the labor market. American
Economic Review, 84, 11741194.
Hebl, M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1997). The stigma of obesity: The differences are
black and white. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 417426.
Hildebrandt, K. A., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (1979). Facial feature determinants of
perceived infant attractiveness. Infant Behavior and Development, 2, 329
339.
Hosoda, M., Stone-Romero, E. F., & Coats, G. (2003). The effects of physical
attractiveness on job-related outcomes: A meta-analysis of experimental
studies. Personnel Psychology, 56, 431-462.
Kramer, S., Zebrowitz, L. A., San Giovanni, J. P., & Sherak, B. (1995). Infants'
preferences for attractiveness and babyfaceness. In B. G. Bardy, R. J.
Bootsma, & Y. Guiard (Eds.), Studies in Perception and Action III (pp. 389
392). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.
Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A., Hallam, M., & Smoot,
M. (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical
review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 390423.
Langlois, J. H., Roggman, L. A., Casey, R. J., Ritter, J. M., Rieser-Danner, L. A., &
Montoya, R. M. (2008). I'm hot, so I'd say you're not: The influence of objective
physical attractiveness on mate selection. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 34, 13151331.
Mueller, U., & Mazur, A. (1996). Facial dominance of West Point cadets as a
predictor of later military rank. Social Forces, 74, 823850.
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal
reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231259.
Penn, D. J., Damjanovich, K., & Potts, W. K. (2002). MHC heterozygosity confers
a selective advantage against multiple-strain infections. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 99, 1126011264.
Principe, C. P., & Langlois, J. H. (2012). Shifting the prototype: Experience with
faces influences affective and attractiveness preferences. Social Cognition,
30, 109120.
Puts, D. A., Barndt, J. L., Welling, L. L., Dawood, K., & Burriss, R. P. (2011).
Intrasexual
competition
among
women:
Vocal
femininity
affects
Rhodes, G., Halberstadt, J., & Brajkovich, G. (2001) Generalization of mereexposure effects to averaged composite faces. Social Cognition, 19, 5770.
Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Watson, T. L., Clifford, C. W. G., & Nakayama, K. (2003)
Fitting the mind to the world: Face adaptation and attractiveness
aftereffects. Psychological Science, 14, 558566.
Rhodes, G. Simmons, L. W. Peters, M. (2005).
Verhulst, B., Lodge, M., & Lavine, H. (2010). The attractiveness halo: Why some
candidates are perceived more favorably than others. Journal of Nonverbal
Behavior, 34, 111117.
Walster, E., Aronson, V., Abrahams, D., & Rottman, L. (1966). Importance of
physical attractiveness in dating behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 4, 508516.
Whitehead, R. D., Ozakinci, G., Stephen, I. D., & Perrett, D. I. (2012). Appealing
to vanity: Could potential appearance improvement motivate fruit and
vegetable consumption? American Journal of Public Health, 102, 207211.
Winkielman, P., Halberstadt, J., Fazendeiro, T., & Catty, S. (2006). Prototypes are
attractive because they are easy on the mind. Psychological Science, 17,
799806.
Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 224228.
Zebrowitz, L. A., Wang, R., Bronstad, P. M., Eisenberg, D., Undurraga, E., ReyesGarca, V., & Godoy, R., (2012). First impressions from faces among U.S. and
culturally isolated Tsimane people in the Bolivian rainforest. Journal of
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43, 119134.
Zebrowitz, L. A. & Rhodes, G. (2004). Sensitivity to bad genes and the
anomalous face overgeneralization effect: Accuracy, cue validity, and cue
utilization in judging intelligence and health. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior.
28, 167185.
Zebrowitz, L. A., & Zhang, E., (2012). Neural evidence for reduced
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Attraction and Beauty by Robert G. Franklin and
Leslie Zebrowitz is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercialShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Industrial/Organizational (I/
O) Psychology
Berrin Erdogan & Talya N. Bauer
Portland State University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
This chapter provides an introduction to industrial and organizational (I/O)
psychology. I/O psychology is an area of psychology that specializes in the
scientific study of behavior in organizational settings and the application of
psychology to understand work behavior. The U.S. Department of Labor
estimates that I/O psychology, as a field, will grow 26% by the year 2018. I/O
psychologists typically have advanced degrees such as a Ph.D. or masters
degree and may work in academic, consulting, government, military, or private
for-profit and not-for-profit organizational settings. Depending on the state in
which they work, I/O psychologists may be licensed. They might ask and answer
questions such as What makes people happy at work? What motivates
employees at work? What types of leadership styles result in better
performance of employees? Who are the best applicants to hire for a job?
One hallmark of I/O psychology is its basis in data and evidence to answer such
questions, and I/O psychology is based on the scientist-practitioner model. The
key individuals and studies in the history of I/O psychology are addressed in
this chapter. Further, professional I/O associations are discussed, as are the
key areas of competence developed in I/O masters programs.
1334
At this point you may be asking yourself: Does psychology really need a
special field to study work behaviors? In other words, wouldnt the findings of
general psychology be sufficient to understand how individuals behave at work?
The answer is an underlined no. Employees behave differently at work
compared with how they behave in general. While some fundamental principles
of psychology definitely explain how employees behave at work (such as
selective perception or the desire to relate to those who are similar to us),
organizational settings are unique. To begin with, organizations have a
hierarchy. They have job descriptions for employees. Individuals go to work not
only to seek fulfillment and to remain active, but also to receive a paycheck and
satisfy their financial needs. Even when they dislike their jobs, many stay and
continue to work until a better alternative comes along. All these constraints
suggest that how we behave at work may be somewhat different from how we
nobaproject.com - Industrial/Organizational (I/O) Psychology
1335
would behave without these constraints. According to the U.S. Bureau of Labor
Statistics, in 2011, more than 149 million individuals worked at least part time
and spent many hours of the week workingsee Figure 1 for a breakdown (U.
S. Department of Labor, 2011). In other words, we spend a large portion of our
waking hours at work. How happy we are with our jobs and our careers is a
primary predictor of how happy and content we are with our lives in general
(Erdogan, Bauer, Truxillo, & Mansfield, 2012). Therefore, the I/O psychology field
has much to offer to individuals and organizations interested in increasing
employee productivity, retention, and effectiveness while at the same time
ensuring that employees are happy and healthy.
1336
Figure 1. Average Hours Worked by Full Time and Part Time Workers
It seems that I/O psychology is useful for organizations, but how is it helpful
to you? Findings of I/O psychology are useful and relevant to everyone who is
planning to work in an organizational setting. Note that we are not necessarily
taking about a business setting. Even if you are planning to form your own band,
or write a novel, or work in a not-for-profit organization, you will likely be working
in, or interacting with, organizations. Understanding why people behave the
way they do will be useful to you by helping you motivate and influence your
coworkers and managers, communicate your message more effectively,
negotiate a contract, and manage your own work life and career in a way that
fits your life and career goals.
nobaproject.com - Industrial/Organizational (I/O) Psychology
1337
1338
conduct their own research in their own companies, and some companies
employ many I/O psychologists.Google is one company that collects and
analyzes data to deal with talent-related issues. Google uses an annual
Googlegeist (roughly translating to the spirit of Google) survey to keep tabs on
how happy employees are. When survey results as well as turnover data showed
that new mothers were twice as likely to leave the company as the average
employee, the company made changes in its maternity leave policy and
mitigated the problem (Manjoo, 2013). In other words, I/O psychologists both
contribute to the science of workplace behavior by generating knowledge and
solve actual problems organizations face by designing the workplace
recruitment, selection, and workforce management policies using this
knowledge.
While the scientist-practitioner model is the hoped-for ideal, not everyone
agrees that it captures the reality. Some argue that practitioners are not always
up to date about what scientists know and, conversely, that scientists do not
study what practitioners really care about often enough (Briner & Rousseau,
2011). At the same time, consumers of research should be wary, as there is
some pseudo-science out there. The issues related to I/O psychology are
important to organizations, which are sometimes willing to pay a lot of money
for solutions to their problems, with some people trying to sell their most recent
invention in employee testing, training, performance appraisal, and coaching
to organizations. Many of these claims are not valid, and there is very little
evidence that some of these products, in fact, improve the performance or
retention of employees. Therefore, organizations and consumers of I/O-related
knowledge and interventions need to be selective and ask to see such evidence
(which is not the same as asking to see the list of other clients who purchased
their products!).
1339
1340
associations for I/O psychologists. For example, European Association for Work
and Organizational Psychology (EAWOP) is the premiere organization for I/O
psychologists in Europe, where I/O psychology is typically referred to as work
and organizational psychology. A global federation of I/O psychology
organizations, named the Alliance for Organizational Psychology, was recently
established. It currently has three member organizations (SIOP, EAWOP, and
the Organizational Psychology Division of the International Association for
Applied Psychology, or Division 1), with plans to expand in the future. The
Association for Psychological Science (APS) is another association to which many
I/O psychologists belong.
Those who work in the I/O field may be based at a university, teaching and
researching I/O-related topics. Some private organizations employing I/O
psychologists include DDI, HUMRRO, Valtera, and Kenexa. These organizations
engage in services such as testing, performance management, and
administering attitude surveys. Many organizations also hire in-house
employees with expertise in I/O psychologyrelated fields to work in
departments including human resource management or people analytics.
According to a 2011 membership survey of SIOP, the largest percentage of
members were employed in academic institutions, followed by those in
consulting or independent practice, private sector organizations, and public
sector organizations (Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology,
2011). Moreover, the majority of respondents (86%) were not licensed.
1341
The term founding father of I/O psychology is usually associated with Hugo
Munsterberg of Harvard University. His 1913 book on Psychology and Industrial
Efficiency, is considered to be the first textbook in I/O psychology. The book is
the first to discuss topics such as how to find the best person for the job and
how to design jobs to maintain efficiency by dealing with fatigue.
One of his contemporaries, Frederick Taylor, was not a psychologist and is
considered to be a founding father not of I/O psychology but of scientific
management. Despite his non-psychology background, his ideas were
important to the development of the I/O psychology field, because they evolved
at around the same time, and some of his innovations, such as job analysis,
later became critically important aspects of I/O psychology. Taylor was an
engineer and management consultant who pioneered time studies where
management observed how work was being performed and how it could be
performed better. For example, after analyzing how workers shoveled coal, he
decided that the optimum weight of coal to be lifted was 21 pounds, and he
designed a shovel to be distributed to workers for this purpose. He instituted
mandatory breaks to prevent fatigue, which increased efficiency of workers.
His book Principles of Scientific Management was highly influential in pointing
out how management could play a role in increasing efficiency of human factors.
Lillian Gilbreth was an engineer and I/O psychologist, arguably completing
the first Ph.D. in I/O psychology. She and her husband, Frank Gilbreth, developed
Taylors ideas by conducting time and motion studies, but also bringing more
humanism to these efforts. Gilbreth underlined the importance of how workers
felt about their jobs, in addition to how they could perform their jobs more
efficiently. She was also the first to bring attention to the value of observing job
candidates while they performed their jobs, which is the foundation behind
work sample tests. The Gilbreths ran a successful consulting business based
on these ideas. Her advising of GE in kitchen redesign resulted in foot-pedal
trash cans and shelves in refrigerator doors. Her life with her husband and 12
kids is detailed in a book later made into a 1950 movie, Cheaper by the Dozen,
nobaproject.com - Industrial/Organizational (I/O) Psychology
1342
1343
1344
We have now reviewed what I/O psychology is, what I/O psychologists do,
the history of I/O, associations related to I/O psychology, and accomplishments
of I/O psychologists. Those interested in finding out more about I/O psychology
are encouraged to visit the outside resources below to learn more.
1345
Topic 8
Personality
The Psychodynamic
Perspective
Robert Bornstein
Adelphi University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Originating in the work of Sigmund Freud, the psychodynamic perspective
emphasizes unconscious psychological processes (for example, wishes and
fears of which were not fully aware), and contends that childhood experiences
are crucial in shaping adult personality. The psychodynamic perspective has
evolved considerably since Freuds time, and now includes innovative new
approaches such as object relations theory and neuropsychoanalysis. Some
psychodynamic concepts have held up well to empirical scrutiny while others
have not, and aspects of the theory remain controversial, but the
psychodynamic perspective continues to influence many different areas of
contemporary psychology.
Learning Objectives
Define the concept of ego defense, and give examples of commonly used
ego defenses.
Introduction
Have you ever done something that didnt make sense? Perhaps you waited
until the last minute to begin studying for an exam, even though you knew that
delaying so long would ensure that you got a poor grade. Or maybe you spotted
a person you liked across the roomsomeone about whom you had romantic
feelingsbut instead of approaching that person you headed the other way
(and felt ashamed about it afterward). If youve ever done something that didnt
seem to make senseand who among us hasntthe psychodynamic
perspective on personality might be useful for you. It can help you understand
why you chose not to study for that test, or why you ran the other way when
the person of your dreams entered the room.
Psychodynamic theory (sometimes called psychoanalytic theory) explains
personality in terms of unconscious psychological processes (for example,
wishes and fears of which were not fully aware), and contends that childhood
experiences are crucial in shaping adult personality. Psychodynamic theory is
most closely associated with the work of Sigmund Freud, and with
psychoanalysis, a type of psychotherapy that attempts to explore the patients
unconscious thoughts and emotions so that the person is better able to
understand him- or herself.
Freuds work has been extremely influential, its impact extending far beyond
psychology (several years ago Time magazine selected Freud as one of the most
important thinkers of the 20th century). Freuds work has been not only
influential, but quite controversial as well. As you might imagine, when Freud
suggested in 1900 that much of our behavior is determined by psychological
forces of which were largely unawarethat we literally dont know whats going
on in our own mindspeople were (to put it mildly) displeased (Freud,
1900/1953a). When he suggested in 1905 that we humans have strong sexual
feelings from a very early age, and that some of these sexual feelings are
directed toward our parents, people were more than displeasedthey were
nobaproject.com - The Psychodynamic Perspective
1351
1352
including those occurring during the first weeks or months of lifeset in motion
personality processes that affect us years, even decades, later (Blatt & Levy,
2003; McWilliams, 2009). This is especially true of experiences that are outside
the normal range (for example, losing a parent or sibling at a very early age).
1353
evolved over more than 50 years (he began in 1885, and continued until he died
in 1939), there were numerous revisions along the way. Thus, it is most accurate
to think of psychodynamic theory as a set of interrelated models that
complement and build upon each other. Three are particularly important: the
topographic model, the psychosexual stage model, and the structural model.
The preconscious
1354
orientation.
1355
superego. The id is the seat of drives and instincts, whereas the ego represents
the logical, reality-oriented part of the mind, and the superego is basically your
consciencethe moral guidelines, rules, and prohibitions that guide your
behavior. (You acquire these through your family and through the culture in
which you were raised.)
According to the structural model, our personality reflects the interplay of
these three psychic structures, which differ across individuals in relative power
and influence. When the id predominates and instincts rule, the result is an
impulsive personality style. When the superego is strongest, moral prohibitions
reign supreme, and a restrained, overcontrolled personality ensues. When the
ego is dominant, a more balanced set of personality traits develop (Eagle, 2011;
McWilliams, 2009).
1356
1357
a mental image of mom or dad as harsh and judgmental, you might instead
become a self-critical person, and feel that you can never live up to other
peoples standards . . . or your own (Luyten & Blatt, 2013).
Object relations theory has increased many psychologists interest in
studying psychodynamic ideas and concepts, in part because it represents a
natural bridge between the psychodynamic perspective and research in other
areas of psychology. For example, developmental and social psychologists also
believe that mental representations of significant people play an important role
in shaping our behavior. In developmental psychology you might read about
this in the context of attachment theory (which argues that attachmentsor
bondsto significant people are key to understanding human behavior; Fraley,
2002). In social psychology, mental representations of significant figures play
an important role in social cognition (thoughts and feelings regarding other
people; Bargh & Morsella, 2008; Robinson & Gordon, 2011).
1358
Evidence
that it has
psychology
defenses that we use). It turns out that certain defenses are more adaptive
than others: Rationalization and sublimation are healthier (psychologically
speaking) than repression and reaction formation (Cramer, 2006). Denial
is, quite literally,
mental
shape our
1359
1360
2000).
1361
1362
1363
1364
Outside Resources
Institution: Institute for Psychoanalytic Training and Research (IPTAR) - A
branch of the International Psychoanalytic Association, IPTAR plays an active
role in supporting empirical research on psychoanalytic theory and therapy.
http://www.iptar.org/
Institution: The American Psychoanalytic Association - The American
Psychoanalytic Association supports psychodynamic training and research,
and sponsors a number of workshops (as well as two annual meetings) each
year.
http://www.apsa.org/
Institution:
The
American
Psychological
Association
Division
of
Discussion Questions
1. What is psychic causality?
2. What are the main differences between the preconscious and the
unconscious in Freuds topographic model?
3. What are the three key structures in the structural model of the mindand
what does each structure do?
4. Which ego defense do you think is more adaptive: reaction formation or
sublimation? Why?
5. How do people raised in individualistic societies differ from those raised in
more sociocentric societies with respect to their self-concepthow do they
perceive and describe themselves?
6. According to object relations theory, how do early relationships with our
parents and other significant figures affect later friendships and romantic
relationships?
7. Which field has the potential to benefit more from the emerging new
discipline of neuropsychoanalysis: neuroscience, or psychoanalysis? Why?
Vocabulary
Ego defenses
Mental strategies, rooted in the ego, that we use to manage anxiety when we
feel threatened (some examples include repression, denial, sublimation, and
reaction formation).
Neuropsychoanalysis
An integrative, interdisciplinary domain of inquiry seeking to integrate
psychoanalytic and neuropsychological ideas and findings to enhance both
areas of inquiry (you can learn more by visiting the webpage of the International
Neuropsychoanalysis Society at http://www.neuropsa.org.uk/).
Psychic causality
The assumption that nothing in mental life happens by chancethat there is
no such thing as a random thought or feeling.
(The
Reference List
Bargh, J. A., & Morsella, E. (2008). The unconscious mind. Perspectives on
Psychological Science, 3, 73-79.
Blatt, S. J., & Levy, K. N. (2003). Attachment theory, psychoanalysis, personality
development, and psychopathology. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 23, 104-152.
Bond, M. (2004). Empirical studies of defense style: Relationships with
psychopathology and change. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 12, 263-278.
Bornstein, R. F. (2010). Psychoanalytic theory as a unifying framework for 21st
century personality assessment. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 27, 133-152.
Bornstein, R. F. (2009). Heisenberg, Kandinsky, and the heteromethod
convergence problem: Lessons from within and beyond psychology. Journal
of Personality Assessment, 91, 1-8.
Bornstein, R. F. (2006). A Freudian construct lost and reclaimed: The
psychodynamics of personality pathology. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 23,
339-353.
Bornstein, R. F. (2005). Reonnecting psychoanalysis to mainstream psychology:
Challenges and opportunities. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 22, 323-340.
Cramer, P. (2006). Protecting the self: Defense mechanisms in action. New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
Cramer, P. (2000). Defense mechanisms in psychology today: Further processes
for adaptation. American Psychologist, 55, 637646.
Personality Traits
Edward Diener & Richard E. Lucas
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Michigan State University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Personality traits reflect peoples characteristic patterns of thoughts, feelings,
and behaviors. Personality traits imply consistency and stabilitysomeone who
scores high on a specific trait like Extraversion is expected to be sociable in
different situations and over time. Thus, trait psychology rests on the idea that
people differ from one another in terms of where they stand on a set of basic
trait dimensions that persist over time and across situations. The most widely
used system of traits is called the Five-Factor Model. This system includes five
broad traits that can be remembered with the acronym OCEAN: Openness,
Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism. Each of the
major traits from the Big Five can be divided into facets to give a more finegrained analysis of someone's personality. In addition, some trait theorists
argue that there are other traits that cannot be completely captured by the FiveFactor Model. Critics of the trait concept argue that people do not act
consistently from one situation to the next and that people are very influenced
by situational forces. Thus, one major debate in the field concerns the relative
power of peoples traits versus the situations in which they find themselves as
predictors of their behavior.
Learning Objectives
List and describe the Big Five (OCEAN) personality traits that comprise
the Five-Factor Model of personality.
Describe each of the Big Five personality traits, and the low and high end
of the dimension.
Give examples of each of the Big Five personality traits, including both a low
and high example.
Describe how traits and social learning combine to predict your social
activities.
Describe your theory of how personality traits get refined by social learning.
Introduction
When we observe people around us, one of the first things that strikes us is
how different people are from one another. Some people are very talkative
while others are very quiet. Some are active whereas others are couch potatoes.
Some worry a lot, others almost never seem anxious. Each time we use one of
these words, words like talkative, quiet, active, or anxious, to describe
those around us, we are talking about a persons personalitythe characteristic
ways that people differ from one another. Personality psychologists try to
describe and understand these differences.
Although there are many ways to think about the personalities that people
have, Gordon Allport and other personologists claimed that we can best
understand the differences between individuals by understanding their
personality traits. Personality traits reflect basic dimensions on which people
differ (Matthews, Deary, & Whiteman, 2003). According to trait psychologists,
there are a limited number of these dimensions (dimensions like Extraversion,
Conscientiousness, or Agreeableness), and each individual falls somewhere on
each dimension, meaning that they could be low, medium, or high on any
specific trait.
An important feature of personality traits is that they reflect continuous
distributions rather than distinct personality types. This means that when
personality psychologists talk about Introverts and Extraverts, they are not
really talking about two distinct types of people who are completely and
qualitatively different from one another. Instead, they are talking about people
who score relatively low or relatively high along a continuous distribution. In
fact, when personality psychologists measure traits like Extraversion, they
typically find that most people score somewhere in the middle, with smaller
numbers showing more extreme levels. The figure below shows the distribution
of Extraversion scores from a survey of thousands of people. As you can see,
most people report being moderately, but not extremely, extraverted, with
nobaproject.com - Personality Traits
1377
Figure 1. Distribution of Extraversion Scores in a Sample Higher bars mean that more
people have scores of that level. This figure shows that most people score towards the middle
of the extraversion scale, with fewer people who are highly extraverted or highly introverted.
There are three criteria that are characterize personality traits: (1)
consistency, (2) stability, and (3) individual differences.
1. To have a personality trait, individuals must be somewhat consistent across
situations in their behaviors related to the trait. For example, if they are
talkative at home, they tend also to be talkative at work.
2. Individuals with a trait are also somewhat stable over time in behaviors
related to the trait. If they are talkative, for example, at age 30, they will also
tend to be talkative at age 40.
3. People differ from one another on behaviors related to the trait. Using
speech is not a personality trait and neither is walking on two feetvirtually
all individuals do these activities, and there are almost no individual
nobaproject.com - Personality Traits
1378
differences. But people differ on how frequently they talk and how active
they are, and thus personality traits such as Talkativeness and Activity Level
do exist.
A challenge of the trait approach was to discover the major traits on which
all people differ. Scientists for many decades generated hundreds of new traits,
so that it was soon difficult to keep track and make sense of them. For instance,
one psychologist might focus on individual differences in friendliness,
whereas another might focus on the highly related concept of sociability.
Scientists began seeking ways to reduce the number of traits in some systematic
way and to discover the basic traits that describe most of the differences
between people.
The way that Gordon Allport and his colleague Henry Odbert approached
this was to search the dictionary for all descriptors of personality (Allport &
Odbert, 1936). Their approach was guided by the lexical hypothesis, which
states that all important personality characteristics should be reflected in the
language that we use to describe other people. Therefore, if we want to
understand the fundamental ways in which people differ from one another, we
can turn to the words that people use to describe one another. So if we want
to know what words people use to describe one another, where should we
look? Allport and Odbert looked in the most obvious placethe dictionary.
Specifically, they took all the personality descriptors that they could find in the
dictionary (they started with almost 18,000 words but quickly reduced that list
to a more manageable number) and then used statistical techniques to
determine which words went together. In other words, if everyone who said
that they were friendly also said that they were sociable, then this might
mean that personality psychologists would only need a single trait to capture
individual differences in these characteristics. Statistical techniques were used
to determine whether a small number of dimensions might underlie all of the
nobaproject.com - Personality Traits
1379
1380
Scores on the Big Five traits are mostly independent. That means that a
persons standing on one trait tells very little about their standing on the other
traits of the Big Five. For example, a person can be extremely high in Extraversion
and be either high or low on Neuroticism. Similarly, a person can be low in
nobaproject.com - Personality Traits
1381
Agreeableness and be either high or low in Conscientiousness. Thus, in the FiveFactor Model, you need five scores to describe most of an individuals
personality.
In the Appendix to this module, we present a short scale to assess the FiveFactor Model of personality (Donnellan, Oswald, Baird, & Lucas, 2006). You can
take this test to see where you stand in terms of your Big Five scores. John
Johnson has also created a helpful website that has personality scales that can
be used and taken by the general public:
After seeing your scores, you can judge for yourself whether you think such
tests are valid.
Traits are important and interesting because they describe stable patterns
of behavior that persist for long periods of time (Caspi, Roberts, & Shiner, 2005).
Importantly, these stable patterns can have broad-ranging consequences for
many areas of our life (Roberts, Kuncel, Shiner, Caspi, & Goldberg, 2007). For
instance, think about the factors that determine success in college. If you were
asked to guess what factors predict good grades in college, you might guess
something like intelligence. This guess would be correct, but we know much
more about who is likely to do well. Specifically, personality researchers have
also found the personality traits like Conscientiousness play an important role
in college and beyond, probably because highly conscientious individuals study
hard, get their work done on time, and are less distracted by nonessential
activities that take time away from school work. In addition, highly conscientious
people are often healthier than people low in conscientiousness because they
are more likely to maintain healthy diets, to exercise, and to follow basic safety
procedures like wearing seat belts or bicycle helmets. Over the long term, this
consistent pattern of behaviors can add up to meaningful differences in health
and longevity. Thus, personality traits are not just a useful way to describe
people you know; they actually help psychologists predict how good a worker
someone will be, how long he or she will live, and the types of jobs and activities
nobaproject.com - Personality Traits
1382
the person will enjoy. Thus, there is growing interest in personality psychology
among psychologists who work in applied settings, such as health psychology
or organizational psychology.
1383
To give you a sense of what these narrow units are like, Figure 3 shows facets
for each of the Big Five traits. It is important to note that although personality
researchers generally agree about the value of the Big Five traits as a way to
summarize ones personality, there is no widely accepted list of facets that
should be studied. The list below, based on work by researchers Paul Costa and
Jeff McCrae, thus reflects just one possible list among many. It should, however,
give you an idea of some of the facets making up each of the Five-Factor Model.
Facets can be useful because they provide more specific descriptions of what
a person is like. For instance, if we take our friend who loves parties but hates
public speaking, we might say that this person scores high on the
gregariousness and warmth facets of extraversion, while scoring lower on
nobaproject.com - Personality Traits
1384
1385
their heightened awareness of the threats in the world around them (Gray,
1981. This model has since been updated; see Gray & McNaughton, 2000). These
early theories have led to a burgeoning interest in identifying the physiological
underpinnings of the individual differences that we observe.
Another revision of the Big Five is the HEXACO model of traits (Ashton &
Lee, 2007). This model is similar to the Big Five, but it posits slightly different
versions of some of the traits, and its proponents argue that one important
class of individual differences was omitted from the Five-Factor Model. The
HEXACO adds Honesty-Humility as a sixth dimension of personality. People high
in this trait are sincere, fair, and modest, whereas those low in the trait are
manipulative, narcissistic, and self-centered. Thus, trait theorists are agreed
that personality traits are important in understanding behavior, but there are
still debates on the exact number and composition of the traits that are most
important.
There are other important traits that are not included in comprehensive
models like the Big Five. Although the five factors capture much that is important
about personality, researchers have suggested other traits that capture
interesting aspects of our behavior. In Figure 4 below we present just a few, out
of hundreds, of the other traits that have been studied by personologists.
1386
1387
Not all of the above traits are currently popular with scientists, yet each of
them has experienced popularity in the past. Although the Five-Factor Model
has been the target of more rigorous research than some of the traits above,
these additional personality characteristics give a good idea of the wide range
of behaviors and attitudes that traits can cover.
1388
1389
reticent to speak up during class and may even act like a wallflower at a different
party. But this does not mean that personality does not exist, nor does it mean
that peoples behavior is completely determined by situational factors. Indeed,
research conducted after the person-situation debate shows that on average,
the effect of the situation is about as large as that of personality traits.
However, it is also true that if psychologists assess a broad range of behaviors
across many different situations, there are general tendencies that emerge.
Personality traits give an indication about how people will act on average, but
frequently they are not so good at predicting how a person will act in a specific
situation at a certain moment in time. Thus, to best capture broad traits, one
must assess aggregate behaviors, averaged over time and across many different
types of situations. Most modern personality researchers agree that there is a
place for broad personality traits and for the narrower units such as those
studied by Walter Mischel.
Appendix
The Mini-IPIP Scale
(Donnellan, Oswald, Baird, & Lucas, 2006)
Instructions: Below are phrases describing peoples behaviors. Please use
the rating scale below to describe how accurately each statement describes
you. Describe yourself as you generally are now, not as you wish to be in the
future. Describe yourself as you honestly see yourself, in relation to other
people you know of the same sex as you are, and roughly your same age. Please
read each statement carefully, and put a number from 1 to 5 next to it to describe
how accurately the statement describes you.
nobaproject.com - Personality Traits
1390
1 = Very inaccurate
2 = Moderately inaccurate
3 = Neither inaccurate nor accurate
4 = Moderately accurate
5 = Very accurate
1. _______ Am the life of the party (E)
2. _______ Sympathize with others feelings (A)
3. _______ Get chores done right away (C)
4. _______ Have frequent mood swings (N)
5. _______ Have a vivid imagination (O)
6. _______Dont talk a lot (E)
7. _______ Am not interested in other peoples problems (A)
8. _______ Often forget to put things back in their proper place (C)
9. _______ Am relaxed most of the time (N)
10. ______ Am not interested in abstract ideas (O)
11. ______ Talk to a lot of different people at parties (E)
12. ______ Feel others emotions (A)
1391
Scoring: The first thing you must do is to reverse the items that are worded
in the opposite direction. In order to do this, subtract the number you put for
that item from 6. So if you put a 4, for instance, it will become a 2. Cross out the
score you put when you took the scale, and put the new number in representing
your score subtracted from the number 6.
Items to be reversed in this way: 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 15, 16, 17, 18, 19, 20
Next, you need to add up the scores for each of the five OCEAN scales
(including the reversed numbers where relevant). Each OCEAN score will be the
sum of four items. Place the sum next to each scale below.
1392
1393
Outside Resources
Web: International Personality Item Pool
http://ipip.ori.org/
Web: John Johnson personality scales
http://www.personal.psu.edu/j5j/IPIP/ipipneo120.htm
Web: Personality trait systems compared
http://www.personalityresearch.org/bigfive/goldberg.html
Web: Sam Gosling website
http://homepage.psy.utexas.edu/homepage/faculty/gosling/samgosling.htm
Discussion Questions
1. Consider different combinations of the Big Five, such as O (Low), C (High),
E (Low), A (High), and N (Low). What would this person be like? Do you know
anyone who is like this? Can you select politicians, movie stars, and other
famous people and rate them on the Big Five?
2. How do you think learning and inherited personality traits get combined in
adult personality?
3. Can you think of instances where people do not act consistentlywhere
their personality traits are not good predictors of their behavior?
4. Has your personality changed over time, and in what ways?
5. Can you think of a personality trait not mentioned in this chapter that
describes how people differ from one another?
6. When do extremes in personality traits become harmful, and when are they
unusual but productive of good outcomes?
Vocabulary
Agreeableness
A personality trait that reflects a persons tendency to be compassionate,
cooperative, warm, and caring to others. People low in agreeableness tend to
be rude, hostile, and to pursue their own interests over those of others.
Conscientiousness
A personality trait that reflects a persons tendency to be careful, organized,
hardworking, and to follow rules.
Continuous distributions
Characteristics can go from low to high, with all different intermediate values
possible. One does not simply have the trait or not have it, but can possess
varying amounts of it.
Extraversion
A personality trait that reflects a persons tendency to be sociable, outgoing,
active, and assertive.
Facets
Broad personality traits can be broken down into narrower facets or aspects of
the trait. For example, extraversion has several facets, such as sociability,
dominance, risk-taking and so forth.
Factor analysis
A statistical technique for grouping similar things together according to how
highly they are associated.
Five-Factor Model
(also called the Big Five) The Five-Factor Model is a widely accepted model of
personality traits. Advocates of the model believe that much of the variability
in peoples thoughts, feelings, and behaviors can be summarized with five broad
traits. These five traits are Openness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion,
Agreeableness, and Neuroticism.
HEXACO model
The HEXACO model is an alternative to the Five-Factor Model. The HEXACO
model includes six traits, five of which are variants of the traits included in the
Big Five (Emotionality [E], Extraversion [X], Agreeableness [A], Conscientiousness
[C], and Openness [O]). The sixth factor, Honesty-Humility [H], is unique to this
model.
Independent
Two characteristics or traits are separate from one another-- a person can be
high on one and low on the other, or vice-versa. Some correlated traits are
relatively independent in that although there is a tendency for a person high
on one to also be high on the other, this is not always the case.
Lexical hypothesis
The lexical hypothesis is the idea that the most important differences between
people will be encoded in the language that we use to describe people.
Therefore, if we want to know which personality traits are most important, we
can look to the language that people use to describe themselves and others.
Neuroticism
A personality trait that reflects the tendency to be interpersonally sensitive and
the tendency to experience negative emotions like anxiety, fear, sadness, and
anger.
Openness to Experience
A personality trait that reflects a persons tendency to seek out and to appreciate
new things, including thoughts, feelings, values, and experiences.
Personality
Enduring predispositions that characterize a person, such as styles of thought,
feelings and behavior.
Personality traits
Enduring dispositions in behavior that show differences across individuals, and
which tend to characterize the person across varying types of situations.
Person-situation debate
The person-situation debate is a historical debate about the relative power of
personality traits as compared to situational influences on behavior. The
situationist critique, which started the person-situation debate, suggested that
people overestimate the extent to which personality traits are consistent across
situations.
Reference List
Allport, G. W., & Odbert, H. S. (1936). Trait names: A psycholexical study.
Psychological Monographs, 47, 211.
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages
of the HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Social
Psychological Review, 11, 150166.
Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., & Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development: Stability
and change. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 56, 453484.
Donnellan, M. B., Oswald, F. L., Baird, B. M., & Lucas, R. E. (2006). The mini-IPIP
scales: Tiny-yet-effective measures of the Big Five factors of personality.
Psychological Assessment, 18, 192203.
Eysenck, H. J. (1981). A model for personality.New York: Springer Verlag.
Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative description of personality: The Big Five
personality traits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1216
1229.
Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysencks theory of personality. In H. J. Eysenck
(Ed.), A Model for Personality (pp. 246-276). New York: Springer Verlag.
Gray, J. A. & McNaughton, N. (2000). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry
into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system (second edition).
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Matthews, G., Deary, I.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Personality Traits by Edward Diener and Richard
E. Lucas is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
For human beings, the self is what happens when I encounters Me. The
central psychological question of selfhood, then, is this: How does a person
apprehend and understand who he or she is? Over the past 100 years,
psychologists have approached the study of self (and the related concept of
identity) in many different ways, but three central metaphors for the self
repeatedly emerge. First, the self may be seen as a social actor, who enacts
roles and displays traits by performing behaviors in the presence of others.
Second, the self is a motivated agent, who acts upon inner desires and
formulates goals, values, and plans to guide behavior in the future. Third, the
self eventually becomes an autobiographical author, too, who takes stock of
lifepast, present, and futureto create a story about who I am, how I came
to be, and where my life may be going. This chapter briefly reviews central ideas
and research findings on the self as an actor, an agent, and an author, with an
emphasis on how these features of selfhood develop over the human life
course.
Learning Objectives
Describe how a sense of self as a social actor emerges around the age of 2
years and how it develops going forward.
Describe the development of the selfs sense of motivated agency from the
emergence of the childs theory of mind to the articulation of life goals and
values in adolescence and beyond.
Define the term narrative identity, and explain what psychological and
cultural functions narrative identity serves.
Introduction
In the Temple of Apollo at Delphi, the ancient Greeks inscribed the words: Know
thyself. For at least 2,500 years, and probably longer, human beings have
pondered the meaning of the ancient aphorism. Over the past century,
psychological scientists have joined the effort. They have formulated many
theories and tested countless hypotheses that speak to the central question of
human selfhood: How does a person know who he or she is?
The ancient Greeks seemed to realize that the self is inherently reflexive
it reflects back on itself. In the disarmingly simple idea made famous by the
great psychologist William James (1892/1963), the self is what happens when
I reflects back upon Me. The self is both the I and the Meit is the knower,
and it is what the knower knows when the knower reflects upon itself. When
you look back at yourself, what do you see? When you look inside, what do you
find? Moreover, when you try to change your self in some way, what is it that
you are trying to change? The philosopher Charles Taylor (1989) describes the
self as a reflexive project. In modern life, Taylor agues, we often try to manage,
discipline, refine, improve, or develop the self. We work on our selves, as we
might work on any other interesting project. But what exactly is it that we work
on?
Imagine for a moment that you have decided to improve yourself. You might,
say, go on a diet to improve your appearance. Or you might decide to be nicer
to your mother, in order to improve that important social role. Or maybe the
problem is at workyou need to find a better job or go back to school to prepare
for a different career. Perhaps you just need to work harder. Or get organized.
Or recommit yourself to religion. Or maybe the key is to begin thinking about
your whole life story in a completely different way, in a way that you hope will
bring you more happiness, fulfillment, peace, or excitement.
nobaproject.com - Self and Identity
1405
Although there are many different ways you might reflect upon and try to
improve the self, it turns out that many, if not most, of them fall roughly into
three broad psychological categories (McAdams & Cox, 2010). The I may
encounter the Me as (a) a social actor, (b) a motivated agent, or (c) an
autobiographical author.
1406
For human beings, the sense of the self as a social actor begins to emerge
around the age of 18 months. Numerous studies have shown that by the time
they reach their second birthday most toddlers recognize themselves in mirrors
and other reflecting devices (Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979; Rochat, 2003). What
they see is an embodied actor who moves through space and time. Many
children begin to use words such as me and mine in the second year of life,
suggesting that the I now has linguistic labels that can be applied reflexively to
itself: I call myself me. Around the same time, children also begin to express
social emotions such as embarrassment, shame, guilt, and pride (Tangney,
Stuewig, & Mashek, 2007). These emotions tell the social actor how well he or
she is performing in the group. When I do things that win the approval of others,
I feel proud of myself. When I fail in the presence of others, I may feel
embarrassment or shame. When I violate a social rule, I may experience guilt,
which may motivate me to make amends.
Many of the classic psychological theories of human selfhood point to the
second year of life as a key developmental period. For example, Freud
(1923/1961) and his followers in the psychoanalytic tradition traced the
emergence of an autonomous ego back to the second year. Freud used the
term ego (in German das Ich, which also translates into the I) to refer to an
executive self in the personality. Erikson (1963) argued that experiences of trust
and interpersonal attachment in the first year of life help to consolidate the
autonomy of the ego in the second. Coming from a more sociological
perspective, Mead (1934) suggested that the I comes to know the Me through
reflection, which may begin quite literally with mirrors but later involves the
reflected appraisals of others. I come to know who I am as a social actor, Mead
argued, by noting how other people in my social world react to my
performances. In the development of the self as a social actor, other people
function like mirrorsthey reflect who I am back to me.
nobaproject.com - Self and Identity
1407
Research has shown that when young children begin to make attributions
about themselves, they start simple (Harter, 2006). At age 4, Jessica knows that
she has dark hair, knows that she lives in a white house, and describes herself
to others in terms of simple behavioral traits. She may say that she is nice, or
helpful, or that she is a good girl most of the time. By the time, she hits fifth
grade (age 10), Jessica sees herself in more complex ways, attributing traits to
the self such as honest, moody, outgoing, shy, hard-working, smart,
good at math but not gym class, or nice except when I am around my annoying
brother. By late childhood and early adolescence, the personality traits that
people attribute to themselves, as well as those attributed to them by others,
tend to correlate with each other in ways that conform to a well-established
taxonomy of five broad trait domains, repeatedly derived in studies of adult
personality and often called the Big Five: (1) extraversion, (2) neuroticism, (3)
agreeableness, (4) conscientiousness, and (5) openness to experience (Roberts,
Wood, & Caspi, 2008). By late childhood, moreover, self-conceptions will likely
also include important social roles: I am a good student, I am the oldest
daughter, or I am a good friend to Sarah.
Traits and roles, and variations on these notions, are the main currency of
the self as social actor (McAdams & Cox, 2010). Trait terms capture perceived
consistencies in social performance. They convey what I reflexively perceive to
be my overall acting style, based in part on how I think others see me as an
actor in many different social situations. Roles capture the quality, as I perceive
it, of important structured relationships in my life. Taken together, traits and
roles make up the main features of my social reputation, as I apprehend it in
my own mind (Hogan, 1982).
If you have ever tried hard to change yourself, you may have taken aim at
your social reputation, targeting your central traits or your social roles. Maybe
you woke up one day and decided that you must become a more optimistic
and emotionally upbeat person. Taking into consideration the reflected
appraisals of others, you realized that even your friends seem to avoid you
nobaproject.com - Self and Identity
1408
because you bring them down. In addition, it feels bad to feel so bad all the
time: Wouldnt it be better to feel good, to have more energy and hope? In the
language of traits, you have decided to work on your neuroticism. Or maybe
instead, your problem is the trait of conscientiousness: You are undisciplined
and dont work hard enough, so you resolve to make changes in that area. Selfimprovement efforts such as theseaimed at changing ones traits to become
a more effective social actorare sometimes successful, but they are very hard
kind of like dieting. Research suggests that broad traits tend to be stubborn,
resistant to change, even with the aid of psychotherapy. However, people often
have more success working directly on their social roles. To become a more
effective social actor, you may want to take aim at the important roles you play
in life. What can I do to become a better son or daughter? How can I find new
and meaningful roles to perform at work, or in my family, or among my friends,
or in my church and community? By doing concrete things that enrich your
performances in important social roles, you may begin to see yourself in a new
light, and others will notice the change, too. Social actors hold the potential to
transform their performances across the human life course. Each time you walk
out on stage, you have a chance to start anew.
1409
1410
1411
on our values and life goals. Perhaps you grew up as a traditional Catholic, but
now in college you believe that the values inculcated in your childhood no longer
function so well for you. You no longer believe in the central tenets of the
Catholic Church, say, and are now working to replace your old values with new
ones. Or maybe you still want to be Catholic, but you feel that your new take
on faith requires a different kind of personal ideology. In the realm of the
motivated agent, moreover, changing values can influence life goals. If your
new value system prioritizes alleviating the suffering of others, you may decide
to pursue a degree in social work, or to become a public interest lawyer, or to
live a simpler life that prioritizes people over material wealth. A great deal of
the identity work we do in adolescence and young adulthood is about values
and goals, as we strive to articulate a personal vision or dream for what we
hope to accomplish in the future.
1412
1413
about life from a sequence of chapters and scenes (Habermas & de Silveira,
2008). For example, a 16-year-old may be able to explain to herself and to others
how childhood experiences in her family have shaped her vocation in life. Her
parents were divorced when she was 5 years old, the teenager recalls, and this
caused a great deal of stress in her family. Her mother often seemed anxious
and depressed, but she (the now-teenager when she was a little girlthe storys
protagonist) often tried to cheer her mother up, and her efforts seemed to
work. In more recent years, the teenager notes that her friends often come to
her with their boyfriend problems. She seems to be very adept at giving advice
about love and relationships, which stems, the teenager now believes, from her
early experiences with her mother. Carrying this causal narrative forward, the
teenager now thinks that she would like to be a marriage counselor when she
grows up.
Unlike children, then, adolescents can tell a full and convincing story about
an entire human life, or at least a prominent line of causation within a full life,
explaining continuity and change in the storys protagonist over time. Once the
cognitive skills are in place, young people seek interpersonal opportunities to
share and refine their developing sense of themselves as storytellers (the I) who
tell stories about themselves (the Me). Adolescents and young adults author a
narrative sense of the self by telling stories about their experiences to other
people, monitoring the feedback they receive from the tellings, editing their
stories in light of the feedback, gaining new experiences and telling stories about
those, and on and on, as selves create stories that, in turn, create new selves
(McLean et al., 2007). Gradually, in fits and starts, through conversation and
introspection, the I develops a convincing and coherent narrative about the Me.
Contemporary research on the self as autobiographical author emphasizes
the strong effect of culture on narrative identity (Hammack, 2008). Culture
provides a menu of favored plot lines, themes, and character types for the
construction of self-defining life stories. Autobiographical authors sample
selectively from the cultural menu, appropriating ideas that seem to resonate
nobaproject.com - Self and Identity
1414
well with their own life experiences. As such, life stories reflect the culture,
wherein they are situated as much as they reflect the authorial efforts of the
autobiographical I.
As one example of the tight link between culture and narrative identity,
McAdams (2013) and others (e.g., Kleinfeld, 2012) have highlighted the
prominence of redemptive narratives in American culture. Epitomized in such
iconic cultural ideals as the American dream, Horatio Alger stories, and
narratives of Christian atonement, redemptive stories track the move from
suffering to an enhanced status or state, while scripting the development of a
chosen protagonist who journeys forth into a dangerous and unredeemed
world (McAdams, 2013). Hollywood movies often celebrate redemptive quests.
Americans are exposed to similar narrative messages in self-help books, 12step programs, Sunday sermons, and in the rhetoric of political campaigns.
Over the past two decades, the worlds most influential spokesperson for the
power of redemption in human lives may be Oprah Winfrey, who tells her own
story of overcoming childhood adversity while encouraging others, through her
media outlets and philanthropy, to tell similar kinds of stories for their own lives
(McAdams, 2013). Research has demonstrated that American adults who enjoy
high levels of mental health and civic engagement tend to construct their lives
as narratives of redemption, tracking the move from sin to salvation, rags to
riches, oppression to liberation, or sickness/abuse to health/recovery
(McAdams, Diamond, de St. Aubin, & Mansfield, 1997; McAdams, Reynolds,
Lewis, Patten, & Bowman, 2001; Walker & Frimer, 2007). In American society,
these kinds of stories are often seen to be inspirational.
At the same time, McAdams (2011, 2013) has pointed to shortcomings and
limitations in the redemptive stories that many Americans tell, which mirror
cultural biases and stereotypes in American culture and heritage. McAdams
has argued that redemptive stories support happiness and societal
engagement for some Americans, but the same stories can encourage moral
righteousness and a nave expectation that suffering will always be redeemed.
nobaproject.com - Self and Identity
1415
For better and sometimes for worse, Americans seem to love stories of personal
redemption and often aim to assimilate their autobiographical memories and
aspirations to a redemptive form. Nonetheless, these same stories may not
work so well in cultures that espouse different values and narrative ideals
(Hammack, 2008). It is important to remember that every culture offers its own
storehouse of favored narrative forms. It is also essential to know that no single
narrative form captures all that is good (or bad) about a culture. In American
society, the redemptive narrative is but one of many different kinds of stories
that people commonly employ to make sense of their lives.
What is your story? What kind of a narrative are you working on? As you look
to the past and imagine the future, what threads of continuity, change, and
meaning do you discern? For many people, the most dramatic and fulfilling
efforts to change the self happen when the I works hard, as an autobiographical
author, to construct and, ultimately, to tell a new story about the Me. Storytelling
may be the most powerful form of self-transformation that human beings have
ever invented. Changing ones life story is at the heart of many forms of
psychotherapy and counseling, as well as religious conversions, vocational
epiphanies, and other dramatic transformations of the self that people often
celebrate as turning points in their lives (Adler, 2012). Storytelling is often at
the heart of the little changes, too, minor edits in the self that we make as we
move through daily life, as we live and experience life, and as we later tell it to
ourselves and to others.
Conclusion
For human beings, selves begin as social actors, but they eventually become
motivated agents and autobiographical authors, too. The I first sees itself as an
embodied actor in social space; with development, however, it comes to
appreciate itself also as a forward-looking source of self-determined goals and
values, and later yet, as a storyteller of personal experience, oriented to the
nobaproject.com - Self and Identity
1416
1417
Outside Resources
Web: The website for the Foley Center for the Study of Lives, at Northwestern
University. The site contains research materials, interview protocols, and
coding manuals for conducting studies of narrative identity.
http://www.sesp.northwestern.edu/foley/
Discussion Questions
1. Back in the 1950s, Erik Erikson argued that many adolescents and young
adults experience a tumultuous identity crisis. Do you think this is true
today? What might an identity crisis look and feel like? And, how might it be
resolved?
2. Many people believe that they have a true self buried inside of them. From
this perspective, the development of self is about discovering a
psychological truth deep inside. Do you believe this to be true? How does
thinking about the self as an actor, agent, and author bear on this question?
3. Psychological research shows that when people are placed in front of
mirrors they often behave in a more moral and conscientious manner, even
though they sometimes experience this procedure as unpleasant. From the
standpoint of the self as a social actor, how might we explain this
phenomenon?
4. By the time they reach adulthood, does everybody have a narrative identity?
Do some people simply never develop a story for their life?
5. What happens when the three perspectives on selfthe self as actor, agent,
and authorconflict with each other? Is it necessary for peoples selfascribed traits and roles to line up well with their goals and their stories?
6. William James wrote that the self includes all things that the person
considers to be mine. If we take James literally, a persons self might extend
to include his or her material possessions, pets, and friends and family.
Does this make sense?
7. To what extent can we control the self? Are some features of selfhood easier
to control than others?
8. What cultural differences may be observed in the construction of the self?
How might gender, ethnicity, and class impact the development of the self
as actor, as agent, and as author?
Vocabulary
Autobiographical reasoning
The ability, typically developed in adolescence, to derive substantive
conclusions about the self from analyzing ones own personal experiences.
Big Five
A broad taxonomy of personality trait domains repeatedly derived from studies
of trait ratings in adulthood and encompassing the categories of (1) extraversion
vs. introversion, (2) neuroticism vs. emotional stability, (3) agreeable vs.
disagreeableness, (4) conscientiousness vs. nonconscientiousness, and (5)
openness to experience vs. conventionality. By late childhood and early
adolescence, peoples self-attributions of personality traits, as well as the trait
attributions made about them by others, show patterns of intercorrelations
that confirm with the five-factor structure obtained in studies of adults.
Ego
Sigmund Freuds conception of an executive self in the personality. Akin to this
chapters notion of the I, Freud imagined the ego as observing outside reality,
engaging in rational though, and coping with the competing demands of inner
desires and moral standards.
Identity
Sometimes used synonymously with the term self, identity means many
different things in psychological science and in other fields (e.g., sociology). In
this chapter, I adopt Erik Eriksons conception of identity as a developmental
task for late adolescence and young adulthood. Forming an identity in
adolescence and young adulthood involves exploring alternative roles, values,
goals, and relationships and eventually committing to a realistic agenda for life
that productively situates a person in the adult world of work and love. In
Theory of mind
Emerging around the age of 4, the childs understanding that other people have
minds in which are located desires and beliefs, and that desires and beliefs,
thereby, motivate behavior.
Reference List
Adler, J. M. (2012). Living into the story: Agency and coherence in a longitudinal
study of narrative identity development and mental health over the course
of psychotherapy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 367
389.
Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Norton.
Freud, S. (1923/1961). The ego and the id. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard
edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 19).
London, UK:
Hogarth.
Freund, A. M., & Riediger, M. (2006). Goals as building blocks of personality and
development in adulthood. In D. K. Mroczek & T. D. Little (Eds.), Handbook
of personality development (pp. 353372). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Habermas, T., & Bluck, S. (2000). Getting a life: The emergence of the life story
in adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 748769.
Habermas, T., & de Silveira, C. (2008). The development of global coherence in
life narrative across adolescence: Temporal, causal, and thematic aspects.
Developmental Psychology, 44, 707721.
Hammack, P. L. (2008). Narrative and the cultural psychology of identity.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 222247.
Harter, S. (2006). The self. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.) & W. Damon & R. M. Lerner
(Series Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and
personality development (pp. 505570). New York, NY: Wiley.
Hogan, R. (1982). A socioanalytic theory of personality. In M. Paige (Ed.),
Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 29, pp. 5589). Lincoln, NE:
University of Nebraska Press.
James, W. (1892/1963). Psychology. Greenwich, CT: Fawcett.
Josselson, R. (1996). Revising herself: The story of womens identity from college
to midlife. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Kleinfeld, J. (2012). The frontier romance: Environment, culture, and Alaska
bad things turn good and good things turn bad: Sequences of redemption
and contamination in life narrative, and their relation to psychosocial
adaptation in midlife adults and in students. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 27, 472483.
McAdams, D. P., & Cox, K. S. (2010). Self and identity across the life span. In M.
E. Lamb & A. M. Freund (Eds.), The handbook of life-span development: Vol.
2. Social and emotional development (pp. 158207). New York, NY: Wiley.
McLean, K. C., Pasupathi, M., & Pals, J. L. (2007). Selves creating stories creating
selves: A process model of self-development. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 11, 262278.
McLean, K. C., & Fournier, M. A. (2008). The content and process of
autobiographical reasoning in narrative identity. Journal of Research in
Personality, 42, 527545.
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago
Press.
Roberts, B. W., Wood, D., & Caspi, A. (2008). The development of personality
traits in adulthood. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook
of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 375398). New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Robins, R. W., Tracy, J. L., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2008). Naturalizing the self. In O.
P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory
and research (3rd ed., pp. 421447). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Rochat, P. (2003). Five levels of self-awareness as they unfold early in life.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Self and Identity by Dan P. McAdams is licensed
under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International
License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
deed.en_US.
Gender
Christia Spears Brown & Jennifer A. Jewell
University of Kentucky
nobaproject.com
Abstract
This chapter discusses gender and its related concepts, including sex, gender
roles, gender identity, sexual orientation, and sexism. In addition, this chapter
includes a discussion of differences that exist between males and females and
how these real gender differences compare to the stereotypes society holds
about gender differences. In fact, there are significantly fewer real gender
differences than one would expect relative to the large number of stereotypes
about gender differences. This chapter then discusses theories of how gender
roles develop and how they contribute to strong expectations for gender
differences. Finally, the chapter concludes with a discussion of some of the
consequences of relying on and expecting gender differences, such as gender
discrimination, sexual harassment, and ambivalent sexism.
Learning Objectives
Discuss gender differences that exist, as well as those that do not actually
exist.
Understand and explain different theories of how gender roles are formed.
Introduction
Before we discuss gender in detail, it is important to understand what gender
actually is. The terms sex and gender are frequently used interchangeably,
though they have different meanings. In this context, sex refers to the biological
category of male or female, as defined by physical differences in genetic
composition and in reproductive anatomy and function. On the other hand,
gender refers to the cultural, social, and psychological meanings that are
associated with masculinity and femininity (Wood & Eagly, 2002). You can think
of male and female as distinct categories of sex (a person is typically born a
male or a female), but masculine and feminine as continuums associated
with gender (everyone has a certain degree of masculine and feminine traits
and qualities).
Beyond sex and gender, there are a number of related terms that are also
often misunderstood. Gender roles are the behaviors, attitudes, and
personality traits that are designated as either masculine or feminine in a given
culture. In American culture, we commonly think of gender roles in terms of
gender stereotypes, or the beliefs and expectations people hold about the
typical characteristics, preferences, and behaviors of men and women.A
persons gender identity refers to their psychological sense of being male or
female. In contrast, a persons sexual orientation is the direction of their
emotional and erotic attraction toward members of the opposite sex, the same
sex, or both sexes. These are important distinctions, and though we will not
discuss each of these terms in detail, it is important to recognize that sex,
gender, gender identity, and sexual orientation do not always correspond with
one another. A person can be biologically male but have a female gender
identity while being attracted to women, or any other combination of identities
and orientations.
nobaproject.com - Gender
1434
Gender Differences
Differences between males and females can be based on (a) actual gender
differences (i.e., men and women are actually different in some abilities), (b)
gender roles (i.e., differences in how men and women are supposed to act), or
(c) gender stereotypes (i.e., differences in how we think men and women are).
Sometimes gender stereotypes and gender roles reflect actual gender
differences, but sometimes they do not.
What are actual gender differences? In terms of language and language skills,
girls develop language skills earlier and know more words than boys; this does
not, however, translate into long-term differences. Girls are also more likely
than boys to offer praise, to agree with the person theyre talking to, and to
elaborate on the other persons comments; boys, in contrast, are more likely
than girls to assert their opinion and offer criticisms (Leaper & Smith, 2004).In
terms of temperament,boys are slightly less able to suppress inappropriate
responses and slightly more likely to blurt things out than girls (Else-Quest,
Hyde, Goldsmith, & Van Hulle, 2006).With respect to aggression,boys exhibit
higher rates of unprovoked physical aggression than girls, but no difference in
provoked aggression) (Hyde, 2005). Some of the biggest differences involve the
play styles of children. Boys frequently play organized rough-and-tumble games
in large groups, while girls often play less physical activities in much smaller
groups (Maccoby, 1998).There are also differences in the rates of depression,
with girls much more likely than boys to be depressed after puberty. After
puberty, girls are also more likely to be unhappy with their bodies than boys.
However, there is considerable variability between individual males and
individual females. Also, even when there are mean level differences, the actual
size of most of these differences is quite small. This means, knowing someones
gender does not help much in predicting his or her actual traits. For example,
in terms of activity level, boys are considered more active than girls. However,
42% of girls are more active than the average boy (but so are 50% of boys; see
nobaproject.com - Gender
1435
Figure 1. While our gender stereotypes paint males and females as drastically different
from each other, even when a difference exists, there is considerable overlap in the presence
of that trait between genders. This graph shows the average difference in self-esteem between
boys and girls. Boys have a higher average self-esteem than girls, but the average scores are
much more similar than different. Taken from Hyde (2005).
Many domains we assume differ across genders are really based on gender
stereotypes and not actual differences. Based on large meta-analyses, the
analyses of thousands of studies across more than one million people, research
has shown: Girls are not more fearful, shy, or scared of new things than boys;
boys are not more angry than girls and girls are not more emotional than boys;
boys do not perform better at math than girls; and girls are not more talkative
than boys (Hyde, 2005).
nobaproject.com - Gender
1436
In the following sections, well investigate gender roles, the part they play in
creating these stereotypes, and how they can affect the development of real
gender differences.
Gender Roles
As mentioned earlier, gender roles are well-established social constructions
that may change from culture to culture and over time.In American culture, we
commonly think of gender roles in terms of gender stereotypes, or the beliefs
and expectations people hold about the typical characteristics, preferences,
and behaviors of men and women.
By the time we are adults, our gender roles are a stable part of our
personalities, and we usually hold many gender stereotypes. When do children
start to learn about gender? Very early. By their first birthday, children can
distinguish faces by gender. By their second birthday, they can label others
gender and even sort objects into gender-typed categories. By the third
birthday, children can consistently identify their own gender (see Martin, Ruble,
& Szkrybalo, 2002, for a review). At this age, children believe sex is determined
by external attributes, not biological attributes. Between 3 and 6 years of age,
children learn that gender is constant and cant change simply by changing
external attributes, having developed gender constancy. During this period,
children also develop strong and rigid gender stereotypes. Stereotypes can refer
to play (e.g., boys play with trucks, and girls play with dolls), traits (e.g., boys are
strong, and girls like to cry), and occupations (e.g., men are doctors and women
are nurses). These stereotypes stay rigid until children reach about age 8 or 9.
Then they develop cognitive abilities that allow them to be more flexible in their
thinking about others.
nobaproject.com - Gender
1437
Figure 2. Children develop the ability to classify gender very early in life.
How do our gender roles and gender stereotypes develop and become so
strong? Many of our gender stereotypes are so strong because we emphasize
gender so much in culture (Bigler & Liben, 2007). For example, males and
females are treated differently before they are even born. When someone
learns of a new pregnancy, the first question asked is Is it a boy or a girl?
Immediately upon hearing the answer, judgments are made about the child:
Boys will be rough and like blue, while girls will be delicate and like pink.
Developmental intergroup theory postulates that adults heavy focus on
gender leads children to pay attention to gender as a key source of information
about themselves and others, to seek out any possible gender differences, and
to form rigid stereotypes based on gender that are subsequently difficult to
change.
There are also psychological theories that partially explain how children form
their own gender roles after they learn to differentiate based on gender. The
first of these theories is gender schema theory.Gender schema theory argues
that children are active learners who essentially socialize themselves. In this
case, children actively organize others behavior, activities, and attributes into
gender categories, which are known as schemas. These schemas then affect
nobaproject.com - Gender
1438
what children notice and remember later. People of all ages are more likely to
remember schema-consistent behaviors and attributes than schemainconsistent behaviors and attributes. So, people are more likely to remember
men, and forget women, who are firefighters. They also misremember schemainconsistent information. If research participants are shown pictures of
someone standing at the stove, they are more likely to remember the person
to be cooking if depicted as a woman, and the person to be repairing the stove
if depicted as a man. By only remembering schema-consistent information,
gender schemas strengthen more and more over time.
A second theory that attempts to explain the formation of gender roles in
children is social learning theory. Social learning theory argues that gender
roles are learned through reinforcement, punishment, and modeling. Children
are rewarded and reinforced for behaving in concordance with gender roles
and punished for breaking gender roles. In addition, social learning theory
argues that children learn many of their gender roles by modeling the behavior
of adults and older children and, in doing so, develop ideas about what
behaviors are appropriate for each gender. Social learning theory has less
support than gender schema theoryresearch shows that parents do reinforce
gender-appropriate play, but for the most part treat their male and female
children similarly (Lytton & Romney, 1991).
1439
unwanted touching or comments, being the target of jokes, having their body
parts rated, or being called names related to sexual orientation.
Different treatment by gender begins with parents. A meta-analysis of
research from the United States and Canada found that parents most frequently
treated sons and daughters differently by encouraging gender-stereotypical
activities (Lytton & Romney, 1991). Fathers, more than mothers, are particularly
likely to encourage gender-stereotypical play, especially in sons. Parents also
talk to their children differently based on stereotypes. For example, parents
talk about numbers and counting twice as often with sons than daughters
(Chang, Sandhofer, & Brown, 2011) and talk to sons in more detail about science
than with daughters. Parents are also much more likely to discuss emotions
with their daughters than their sons.
Children do a large degree of socializing themselves. By age 3, children play
in gender-segregated play groups and expect a high degree of conformity.
Children who are perceived as gender atypical (i.e., do not conform to gender
stereotypes) are more likely to be bullied and rejected than their more genderconforming peers.
Gender stereotypes typically maintain gender inequalities in society. The
concept of ambivalent sexism recognizes the complex nature of gender
attitudes, in which women are often associated with positive and negative
qualities (Glick & Fiske, 2001). It has two components. First, hostile sexism refers
to the negative attitudes of women as inferior and incompetent relative to men.
Second, benevolent sexism refers to the perception that women need to be
protected, supported, and adored by men. There has been considerable
empirical support for benevolent sexism, possibly because it is seen as more
socially acceptable than hostile sexism. Gender stereotypes are found not just
in American culture. Across cultures, males tend to be associated with stronger
and more active characteristics than females (Best, 2001).
nobaproject.com - Gender
1440
In recent years, gender and related concepts have become a common focus
of social change and social debate. Many societies, including American society,
have seen a rapid change in perceptions of gender roles, media portrayals of
gender, and legal trends relating to gender. For example, there has been an
increase in childrens toys attempting to cater to both genders (such as Legos
marketed to girls), rather than catering to traditional stereotypes. Nationwide,
the drastic surge in acceptance of homosexuality and gender questioning has
resulted in a rapid push for legal change to keep up with social change. Laws
such as Dont Ask, Dont Tell and the Defense of Marriage Act (DOMA), both
of which were enacted in the 1990s, have met severe resistance on the grounds
of being discriminatory toward sexual minority groups and have been accused
of unconstitutionality less than 20 years after their implementation. Change in
perceptions of gender is also evident in social issues such as sexual harassment,
a term that only entered the mainstream mindset in the 1991 Clarence Thomas/
Anita Hill scandal. As societys gender roles and gender restrictions continue to
fluctuate, the legal system and the structure of American society will continue
to change and adjust.
nobaproject.com - Gender
1441
Figure 3. Gender and related topics have become common subjects for social and legal
discussion in America in recent years, and this trend is likely to continue.
nobaproject.com - Gender
1442
Outside Resources
Video: Human Sexuality is Complicated
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=xXAoG8vAyzI
Web: Big Think with Professor of Neuroscience Lise Eliot
http://bigthink.com/users/liseeliot
Web: Understanding Prejudice: Sexism
http://www.understandingprejudice.org/links/sexism.htm
Discussion Questions
1. What are the differences and associations among gender, sex, gender
identity, and sexual orientation?
2. Are the gender differences that exist innate (biological) differences or are
they caused by other variables?
3. Discuss the theories relating to the development of gender roles and gender
stereotypes. Which theory do you support? Why?
4. Using what youve read in this chapter: a. Why do you think gender
stereotypes are so inflated compared with actual gender differences? b.
Why do you think people continue to believe in such strong gender
differences despite evidence to the contrary?
5. Brainstorm additional forms of gender discrimination aside from sexual
harassment. Have you seen or experienced gender discrimination
personally?
6. How is benevolent sexism detrimental to women, despite appearing
positive?
Vocabulary
Ambivalent sexism
A concept of gender attitudes that encompasses both positive and negative
qualities.
Benevolent sexism
The positive element of ambivalent sexism, which recognizes that women are
perceived as needing to be protected, supported, and adored by men.
Gender
The cultural, social, and psychological meanings that are associated with
masculinity and femininity.
Gender constancy
The awareness that gender is constant and does not change simply by changing
external attributes; develops between 3 and 6 years of age.
Gender discrimination
Differential treatment on the basis of gender.
Gender identity
A persons psychological sense of being male or female.
Gender roles
The behaviors, attitudes, and personality traits that are designated as either
masculine or feminine in a given culture.
Gender stereotypes
The beliefs and expectations people hold about the typical characteristics,
preferences, and behaviors of men and women.
Hostile sexism
The negative element of ambivalent sexism, which includes the attitudes that
women are inferior and incompetent relative to men.
Schemas
The gender categories into which, according to gender schema theory, children
actively organize others behavior, activities, and attributes.
Sex
Biological category of male or female as defined by physical differences in
genetic composition and in reproductive anatomy and function.
Sexual harassment
A form of gender discrimination based on unwanted treatment related to sexual
behaviors or appearance.
Sexual orientation
Refers to the direction of emotional and erotic attraction toward members of
the opposite sex, the same sex, or both sexes.
Social learning theory
This theory of how children form their own gender roles argues that gender
roles are learned through reinforcement, punishment, and modeling.
Reference List
Best, D. L. (2001). Gender concepts: Convergence in cross-cultural research and
methodologies. Cross-Cultural Research: The Journal of Comparative Social
Science, 35(1), 2343. doi: 10.1177/106939710103500102
Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2007). Developmental intergroup theory: Explaining
and reducing children's social stereotyping and prejudice. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 16(3), 162166. doi: 10.1111/
j.1467-8721.2007.00496.x
Chang, A. Sandhofer, C., & Brown, C. S. (2011). Gender biases in early number
exposure to preschool-aged children. Journal of Language and Social
Psychology. doi: 10.1177/0261927X11416207
Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., Goldsmith, H. H., & Van Hulle, C. A. (2006). Gender
differences in temperament: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(1),
3372. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.33
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile and benevolent
sexism as complementary justifications for gender inequality. American
Psychologist, 56(2), 109118. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.2.109
Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist,
60(6), 581592. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581
Leaper, C., & Smith, T. E. (2004). A meta-analytic review of gender variations in
childrens language use: Talkativeness, affiliative speech, and assertive
speech. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 9931027. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.993
Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents differential socialization of boys and
girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 267296. doi:
10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.267
Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together.
Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.
Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early
gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128(6), 903933. doi:
10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.903
Tzuriel, D., & Egozi, G. (2010). Gender differences in spatial ability of young
children: The effects of training and processing strategies. Child
Development, 81(5), 14171430. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01482.x
Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of the behavior of
women and men: Implications for the origins of sex differences.
Psychological Bulletin, 128(5), 699727. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.5.699
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Gender by Christia Spears Brown and Jennifer
A. Jewell is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
This chapter describes different ways to address questions about personality
stability across the lifespan. Definitions of the major types of personality
stability are provided, and evidence concerning the different kinds of stability
and change are reviewed. The mechanisms thought to produce personality
stability and personality change are identified and explained.
Learning Objectives
Describe
the
transformation.
mechanisms
behind
the
possibility
of
personality
Introduction
Personality psychology is about how individuals differ from each other in their
characteristic ways of thinking, feeling, and behaving. Some of the most
interesting questions about personality attributes involve issues of stability and
change. Are shy children destined to become shy adults? Are the typical
personality attributes of adults different from the typical attributes of
adolescents? Do people become more self-controlled and better able to
manage their negative emotions as they become adults? What mechanisms
explain personality stability and what mechanisms account for personality
change?
1454
1455
1456
1457
a longitudinal study when all participants start the study at roughly the same
age.
A number of large-scale, cross-sectional studies have evaluated age
differences in personality (Anusic, Lucas, & Donnellan, 2012; Lucas & Donnellan,
2009; McCrae & Costa, 2003; Soto, John, Gosling, & Potter, 2011; Srivastava,
John, Gosling, & Potter, 2003) as have a number of longitudinal studies (Lucas
& Donnellan, 2011; Specht, Egloff, & Schmukle, 2011; Terracciano, McCrae,
Brant, & Costa, 2005; Wortman, Lucas, & Donnellan, in press). Fortunately, many
of the general trends from these different designs converge on the same basic
set of findings. Most notably, Roberts, Walton, and Viechtbauer (2006) combined
the results of 92 longitudinal studies to provide an overview of absolute changes
in personality across the lifespan. They used the Big Five taxonomy (e.g., John,
Naumann, & Soto, 2008) to categorize the different personality attributes
examined in the individual studies to make sense of the vast literature.
The Big Five domains include extraversion (attributes such as assertive,
confident, independent, outgoing, and sociable), agreeableness (attributes
such as cooperative, kind, modest, and trusting), conscientiousness (attributes
such as hard working, dutiful, self-controlled, and goal-oriented), neuroticism
(attributes such as anxious, tense, moody, and easily angered), and openness
(attributes such as artistic, curious, inventive, and open-minded). The Big Five
is one of the most common ways of organizing the vast range of personality
attributes that seem to distinguish one person from the next. This organizing
framework made it possible for Roberts et al. (2006) to draw broad conclusions
from the literature.
In general, average levels of extraversion (especially the attributes linked to
self-confidence and independence), agreeableness, and conscientiousness
appear to increase with age whereas neuroticism appears to decrease with age
(Roberts et al., 2006). Openness also declines with age, especially after mid-life
(Roberts et al., 2006). These changes are often viewed as positive trends given
that higher levels of agreeableness and conscientiousness and lower levels of
nobaproject.com - Personality Stability and Change
1458
1459
Vaidya, Gray, Haig, Mroczek, & Watson, 2008) and existing studies suggest that
personality changes differ across people (Roberts & Mroczek, 2008). These new
research methods work best when researchers collect more than two waves of
longitudinal data covering longer spans of time. This kind of research design is
still somewhat uncommon in psychological studies but it will likely characterize
the future of research on personality stability.
Differential stability. The evaluation of differential stability requires a
longitudinal study. The simplest strategy is to follow a large sample of
participants of the same age and measure their personality attributes at two
points separated by a meaningful span of time. The researcher then calculates
the correlation between scores at the first assessment and scores at the second
assessment (a coefficient sometimes called a test-retest correlation or even a
stability coefficient). As you know, a correlation coefficient is a numerical
summary of the linear association between two variables. Correlations around
.1 or .1 are often called small associations, whereas correlations around .50
and .50 (or larger) are often called large associations (Cohen, 1988).
Roberts and DelVecchio (2000) summarized 3,217 test-retest correlations
for a wide range of personality attributes reported in 152 longitudinal studies.
They used statistical methods to equate the different test-retest correlations
to a common interval of about seven years. This allowed them to compare
results from studies of differing lengths of time because not all studies followed
participants for the same interval of time. Roberts and DelVecchio found that
differential stability increased with age. The correlations ranged from about .30
for samples involving young children to about .70 for samples involving older
adults. Ferguson (2010) updated and replicated this basic pattern. This pattern
of increasing stability with age is called the cumulative continuity principle of
personality development (Caspi et al., 2005). This general pattern holds for
both women and men and applies to a wide range of different personality
attributes ranging from extraversion to openness and curiosity. It is important
to emphasize, however, that the observed correlations are never perfect at any
nobaproject.com - Personality Stability and Change
1460
age (i.e., the correlations do not reach 1.0). This indicates that personality
changes can occur at any time in the lifespan; it just seems that greater
inconsistency is observed in childhood and adolescence than in adulthood.
1461
In general, the picture that emerges from the literature is that personality
traits are relatively enduring attributes that become more stable from childhood
to adulthood. Nonetheless, the stability of personality attributes is not perfect
at any period in the lifespan. This is an important conclusion because it
challenges two extreme perspectives that have been influential in psychological
research. More than 100 years ago, the famous psychologist William James
remarked that character (personality) was set like plaster for most people by
age 30.
1462
leisure time very differently than more cautious individuals. Some prefer
extreme sports whereas others prefer less intense experiences. Reactive
personenvironment transactions occur when individuals react differently to
the same objective situation because of their personalities. A large social
gathering represents a psychologically different context to the highly
extraverted person compared with the highly introverted person. Evocative
personenvironment transactions occur whenever individuals draw out or
evoke certain kinds of responses from their social environments because of
their personality attributes. A warm and secure individual invites different kinds
of responses from peers than a cold and aloof individual.
Current researchers make distinctions between the mechanisms likely to
produce personality stability and the mechanisms likely to produce changes
(Roberts, 2006; Roberts et al., 2008). Brent Roberts coined the helpful acronym
ASTMA to aid in remembering many of these mechanisms: Attraction (A),
selection (S), manipulation (M), and attrition (A) tend to produce personality
stability, whereas transformation (T) explains personality change.
Individuals sometimes select careers, friends, social clubs, and lifestyles
because of their personality attributes. This is the active process of attraction
individuals are attracted to environments because of their personality
attributes. Situations that match with our personalities seem to feel right (e.
g., Cesario, Grant, & Higgins, 2004). On the flipside of this process, gatekeepers,
such as employers, admissions officers, and even potential relationship
partners, often select individuals because of their personalities. Extraverted
and outgoing individuals are likely to make better salespeople than quiet
individuals who are uncomfortable with social interactions. All in all, certain
individuals are admitted by gatekeepers into particular kinds of environments
because of their personalities. Likewise, individuals with characteristics that are
a bad fit with a particular environment may leave such settings or be asked to
leave by gatekeepers. A lazy employee will not last long at a demanding job.
These examples capture the process of attrition (dropping out). The processes
nobaproject.com - Personality Stability and Change
1463
1464
bias or a hostile attribution of intent; see Crick & Dodge, 1996; Orobio de Castro,
Veerman, Koops, Bosch, & Monshouwer, 2002). If a stranger runs into you and
you spill your hot coffee all over a clean shirt, how do you interpret the situation?
Do you believe the other person was being aggressive, or were you just unlucky?
A rude, caustic, or violent response might invite a similar response from the
individual who ran into you. The basic point is that personality attributes help
shape reactions to and responses from the social world, and these processes
often (but not always) end up reinforcing dispositional tendencies.
Although a number of mechanisms account for personality continuity by
generating a match between the individuals characteristics and the
environment, personality change or transformation is nonetheless possible.
Recall that differential stability is not perfect. The simplest mechanism for
producing change is a cornerstone of behaviorism: Patterns of behavior that
produce positive consequences (pleasure) are repeated, whereas patterns of
behavior that produce negative consequences (pain) will diminish (Thorndike,
1933). Social settings may have the power to transform personality if the
individual is exposed to different rewards and punishments and the setting
places limitations on how a person can reasonably behave (Caspi & Moffitt,
1993). For example, environmental contexts that limit agency and have very
clear reward structures such as the military might be particularly powerful
contexts for producing lasting personality changes (e.g., Jackson, Thoemmes,
Jonkmann, Ldke, & Trautwein, 2012).
It is also possible that individuals might change their personality attributes
by actively striving to change their behaviors and emotional reactions with help
from outsiders. This idea lies at the heart of psychotherapy. As it stands, the
conditions that produce lasting personality changes are an active area of
research. Personality researchers have historically sought to demonstrate the
existence of personality stability, and they are now turning their full attention
to the conditions that facilitate personality change. There are currently a few
examples of interventions that end up producing short-term personality
nobaproject.com - Personality Stability and Change
1465
changes (Jackson, Hill, Payne, Roberts, & Stine-Morrow, 2012), and this is an
exciting area for future research (Edmonds, Jackson, Fayard, & Roberts, 2008).
Insights about personality change are important for creating effective
interventions designed to foster positive human development. Finding ways to
promote self-control, emotional stability, creativity, and an agreeable
disposition would likely lead to improvements for both individuals and society
as a whole because these attributes predict a range of consequential life
outcomes (Ozer & Benet-Martnez, 2006; Roberts et al., 2007)
Conclusion
There are multiple ways to evaluate personality stability. The existing evidence
suggests that personality attributes are relatively enduring attributes that show
predictable average-level changes across the lifespan. Personality stability is
produced by a complicated interplay between individuals and their social
settings. Many personality attributes are linked to life experiences in a mutually
reinforcing cycle: Personality attributes seem to shape environmental contexts,
and those contexts often then accentuate and reinforce those very personality
attributes. Even so, personality change or transformation is possible because
individuals respond to their environments. Individuals may also want to change
their personalities. Personality researchers are now beginning to address
important questions about the possibility of lasting personality changes
through intervention efforts.
Throughout most of this chapter I will use the term stability to refer to
continuity, stability/change, and consistency/inconsistency.
1466
Discussion Questions
1. Why is it difficult to give a simple answer to the question of whether
personality is stable across the lifespan?
2. What happens during young adulthood that might explain findings about
average changes in personality attributes?
3. Why does differential stability increase during adulthood?
4. What are some concrete examples of the ASTMA processes?
5. Can you explain the corresponsive principle of personality development?
Provide several clear examples.
6. Do you think dramatic personality changes are likely to happen in
adulthood? Why or why not?
7. What kinds of environments might be particularly powerful for changing
personality? What specific features of these environments seem to make
them powerful for producing change?
8. Is it easy to change your personality in adulthood? What steps do you think
are needed to produce noticeable and lasting changes in your personality?
What steps are needed to change the personalities of others?
9. Do you find the evidence that personality attributes are relatively enduring
attributes reflects a largely positive aspect of adult development or a more
unpleasant aspect? Why?
Vocabulary
Absolute stability
Consistency in the level or amount of a personality attribute over time.
Age effects
Differences in personality between groups of different ages that are related to
maturation and development instead of birth cohort differences.
Attraction
A connection between personality attributes and aspects of the environment
that occurs because individuals with particular traits are drawn to certain
environments.
Attrition
A connection between personality attributes and aspects of the environment
that occurs because individuals with particular traits drop out from certain
environments.
Birth cohort
Individuals born in a particular year or span of time.
Cohort effects
Differences in personality that are related to historical and social factors unique
to individuals born in a particular year.
Corresponsive principle
The idea that personality traits often become matched with environmental
conditions such that an individuals social context acts to accentuate and
reinforce their personality attributes.
Cross-sectional study/design
A research design that uses a group of individuals with different ages (and birth
cohorts) assessed at a single point in time.
Cumulative continuity principle
The generalization that personality attributes show increasing stability with age
and experience.
Differential stability
Consistency in the rank-ordering of personality across two or more
measurement occasions.
Evocative personenvironment transactions
The interplay between individuals and their contextual circumstances that
occurs whenever attributes of the individual draw out particular responses from
others in their environment.
Group level
A focus on summary statistics that apply to aggregates of individuals when
studying personality development. An example is considering whether the
average score of a group of 50 year olds is higher than the average score of a
group of 21 year olds when considering a trait like conscientiousness.
Heterotypic stability
Consistency in the underlying psychological attribute across development
regardless of any changes in how the attribute is expressed at different ages.
Homotypic stability
Consistency of the exact same thoughts, feelings, and behaviors across
development.
Individual level
A focus on individual level statistics that reflect whether individuals show
stability or change when studying personality development. An example is
evaluating how many individuals increased in conscientiousness versus how
many decreased in conscientiousness when considering the transition from
adolescence to adulthood.
Longitudinal study/design
A research design that follows the same group of individuals at multiple time
points.
Manipulation
A connection between personality attributes and aspects of the environment
that occurs whenever individuals with particular traits actively shape their
environments.
Maturity principle
The generalization that personality attributes associated with the successful
fulfillment of adult roles increase with age and experience.
Personenvironment transactions
The interplay between individuals and their contextual circumstances that ends
up shaping both personality and the environment.
Selection
A connection between personality attributes and aspects of the environment
that occurs whenever individuals with particular attributes choose particular
kinds of environments.
Stress reaction
The tendency to become easily distressed by the normal challenges of life.
Transformation
The term for personality changes associated with experience and life events.
Reference List
Anusic, I., Lucas, R. E., & Donnellan, M. B. (2012). Cross-sectional age differences
in personality: Evidence from nationally representative samples from
Switzerland and the United States. Journal of Research in Personality, 46,
116120.
Caspi, A., Bem, D. J., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (1989). Continuities and consequences of
interactional styles across the life course. Journal of Personality, 57, 375
406.
Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., & Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development: Stability
and change. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 453484.
Caspi, A., & Bem, D. J. (1990). Personality continuity and change across the life
course. In L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp.
549575). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (1993). When do individual differences matter? A
paradoxical theory of personality coherence. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 247
271.
Cesario, J., Grant, H., & Higgins, E. T. (2004). Regulatory fit and persuasion:
Transfer from feeling right. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
86, 388404.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms
Kotov, R., Gamez, W., Schmidt, F., & Watson, D. (2010). Linking big personality
traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: A meta-analysis.
Psychological Bulletin 136, 768821.
Lucas, R. E., & Donnellan, M. B. (2011). Personality development across the
lifespan: Longitudinal analyses with a national sample from Germany.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 847861.
Lucas, R. E., & Donnellan, M. B. (2009). Age differences in personality: Evidence
from a nationally representative sample of Australians. Developmental
Psychology, 45, 13531363.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (2003). Personality in adulthood: A five-factor
theory perspective (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. (2001). Structural models of personality and their
relation to antisocial behavior: A meta-analytic review. Criminology, 39, 765
798.
Mroczek, D. K., & Spiro, A., III (2007). Personality change influences mortality in
older men. Psychological Science, 18, 371376.
Orobio de Castro, B., Veerman, J. W., Koops, W., Bosch, J. D., & Monshouwer, H.
J. (2002). Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior: A metaanalysis. Child Development, 73, 916934.
Ozer, D. J., & Benet-Martnez, V. (2006). Personality and the prediction of
consequential outcomes. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 401421.
Rindfuss, R. R. (1991). The young adult years: Diversity, structural change, and
Wortman, J., Lucas, R. E., & Donnellan, M. B. (in press). Stability and change in
the Big Five personality domains: Evidence from a longitudinal study of
Australians. Psychology and Aging.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Personality Stability and Change by M. Brent
Donnellan is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Personality Assessment
David Watson
University of Notre Dame
nobaproject.com
Abstract
This chapter provides a basic overview to the assessment of personality. It
discusses objective personality tests (based on both self-report and informant
ratings), projective and implicit tests, and behavioral/performance measures.
It describes the basic features of each method, as well as reviewing the
strengths, weaknesses, and overall validity of each approach.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
Personality is the field within psychology that studies the thoughts, feelings,
behaviors, goals, and interests of normal individuals. It therefore covers a very
wide range of important psychological characteristics. Moreover, different
theoretical models have generated very different strategies for measuring these
characteristics. For example, humanistically oriented models argue that people
have clear, well-defined goals and are actively striving to achieve them
(McGregor, McAdams, & Little, 2006). It, therefore, makes sense to ask them
directly about themselves and their goals. In contrast, psychodynamically
oriented theories propose that people lack insight into their feelings and
motives, such that their behavior is influenced by processes that operate
outside of their awareness (e.g., McClelland, Koestner, & Weinberger, 1989;
Meyer & Kurtz, 2006). Given that they are unaware of these processes, it does
not make sense to ask directly about them. One, therefore, needs to adopt an
entirely different approach to identify these nonconscious factors.
Not surprisingly, researchers have adopted a wide range of approaches to
measure important personality characteristics. The most widely used strategies
will be summarized in the following sections.
Objective Tests
Definition
Objective tests (Loevinger, 1957; Meyer & Kurtz, 2006) represent the most
familiar and widely used approach to assessing personality. Objective tests
involve administering a standard set of items, each of which is answered using
a limited set of response options (e.g., true or false; strongly disagree, slightly
disagree, slightly agree, strongly agree). Responses to these items then are
nobaproject.com - Personality Assessment
1484
1485
The items included in self-report measures may consist of single words (e.
g., assertive), short phrases (e.g., am full of energy), or complete sentences (e.
g., I like to spend time with others). Table 1 presents a sample self-report
measure assessing the general traits comprising the influential five-factor
model
(FFM)
of
personality:
neuroticism,
extraversion,
openness,
1486
1487
1488
Informant ratings
Another approach is to ask someone who knows a person well to describe his
or her personality characteristics. In the case of children or adolescents, the
informant is most likely to be a parent or teacher. In studies of older participants,
informants may be friends, roommates, dating partners, spouses, children, or
bosses (Oh et al., 2011; Vazire & Carlson, 2011; Watson et al., 2000).
Generally speaking, informant ratings are similar in format to self-ratings.
As was the case with self-report, items may consist of single words, short
phrases, or complete sentences. Indeed, many popular instruments include
parallel self- and informant-rating versions, and it often is relatively easy to
convert a self-report measure so that it can be used to obtain informant ratings.
Table 2 illustrates how the self-report instrument shown in Table 1 can be
converted to obtain spouse-ratings (in this case, having a husband describe the
personality characteristics of his wife).
1489
1490
2011). Indeed, informants typically have strong incentives for being accurate in
their judgments. As Funder and Dobroth (1987, p. 409), put it, Evaluations of
the people in our social environment are central to our decisions about who to
befriend and avoid, trust and distrust, hire and fire, and so on.
Informant personality ratings have demonstrated a level of validity in
relation to important life outcomes that is comparable to that discussed earlier
for self-ratings. Indeed, they outperform self-ratings in certain circumstances,
particularly when the assessed traits are highly evaluative in nature (e.g.,
intelligence, charm, creativity; see Vazire & Carlson, 2011). For example, Oh et
al. (2011) found that informant ratings were more strongly related to job
performance than were self-ratings. Similarly, Oltmanns and Turkheimer (2009)
summarized evidence indicating that informant ratings of Air Force cadets
predicted early, involuntary discharge from the military better than self-ratings.
Nevertheless, informant ratings also are subject to certain problems and
limitations. One general issue is the level of relevant information that is available
to the rater (Funder, 2012). For instance, even under the best of circumstances,
informants lack full access to the thoughts, feelings, and motives of the person
they are rating. This problem is magnified when the informant does not know
the person particularly well and/or only sees him or her in a limited range of
situations (Funder, 2012; Beer & Watson, 2010).
Informant ratings also are subject to some of the same response biases
noted earlier for self-ratings. For instance, they are not immune to the reference
group effect. Indeed, it is well-established that parent ratings often are subject
to a sibling contrast effect, such that parents exaggerate the true magnitude
of differences between their children (Pinto, Rijsdijk, Frazier-Wood, Asherson,
& Kuntsi, 2012). Furthermore, in many studies, individuals are allowed to
nominate (or even recruit) the informants who will rate them. Because of this,
it most often is the case that informants (who, as noted earlier, may be friends,
relatives, or romantic partners) like the people they are rating. This, in turn,
means that informants may produce overly favorable personality ratings.
nobaproject.com - Personality Assessment
1491
Indeed, their ratings actually can be more favorable than the corresponding
self-ratings (Watson & Humrichouse, 2006). This tendency for informants to
produce unrealistically positive ratings has been termed the letter of
recommendation effect (Leising, Erbs, & Fritz, 2010) and the honeymoon effect
when applied to newlyweds (Watson & Humrichouse, 2006).
1492
the
broad
traits
of
neuroticism,
extraversion,
openness,
1493
1494
1998), which can be used to assess motives such as the need for achievement.
The validity of the Rorschach has been a matter of considerable controversy
(Lilienfeld et al., 2000; Mihura, Meyer, Dumitrascu, & Bombel, 2012; Society for
Personality Assessment, 2005). Most reviews acknowledge that Rorschach
scores do show some ability to predict important outcomes. Its critics, however,
argue that it fails to provide important incremental information beyond other,
more easily acquired information, such as that obtained from standard selfreport measures (Lilienfeld et al., 2000).
Validity evidence is more impressive for the TAT. In particular, reviews have
concluded that TAT-based measures of the need for achievement show (a)
significant validity to predict important criteria and (b) provide important
information beyond that obtained from objective measures of this motive
(McClelland et al., 1989; Spangler, 1992). Furthermore, given the relatively weak
associations between objective and projective measures of motives, McClelland
et al. (1989) argue that they tap somewhat different processes, with the latter
assessing implicit motives (Schultheiss, 2008).
Implicit Tests
In recent years, researchers have begun to use implicit measures of personality
(Back, Schmuckle, & Egloff, 2009; Vazire & Carlson, 2011). These tests are based
on the assumption that people form automatic or implicit associations between
certain concepts based on their previous experience and behavior. If two
concepts (e.g., me and assertive) are strongly associated with each other, then
they should be sorted together more quickly and easily than two concepts (e.
g., me and shy) that are less strongly associated. Although validity evidence for
these measures still is relatively sparse, the results to date are encouraging:
Back et al. (2009), for example, showed that implicit measures of the FFM
personality traits predicted behavior even after controlling for scores on
objective measures of these same characteristics.
nobaproject.com - Personality Assessment
1495
1496
At the same time, however, this approach also has some disadvantages. This
assessment strategy clearly is much more cumbersome and labor intensive
than using objective tests, particularly self-report. Moreover, similar to
projective tests, behavioral measures generate a rich set of data that then need
to be scored in a reliable and valid way. Finally, even the most ambitious study
only obtains relatively small samples of behavior that may provide a somewhat
distorted view of a persons true characteristics. For example, your behavior
during a getting acquainted conversation on a single given day inevitably will
reflect a number of transient influences (e.g., level of stress, quality of sleep the
previous night) that are idiosyncratic to that day.
Conclusion
No single method of assessing personality is perfect or infallible; each of the
major methods has both strengths and limitations. By using a diversity of
approaches, researchers can overcome the limitations of any single method
and develop a more complete and integrative view of personality.
1497
Discussion Questions
1. Under what conditions would you expect self-ratings to be most similar to
informant ratings? When would you expect these two sets of ratings to be
most different from each other?
2. The findings of Gosling, et al. (2002) demonstrate that we can obtain
important clues about students personalities from their dorm rooms. What
other aspects of peoples lives might give us important information about
their personalities?
3. Suppose that you were planning to conduct a study examining the
personality trait of honesty. What method or methods might you use to
measure it?
Vocabulary
Big Five
Five, broad general traits that are included in many prominent models of
personality. The five traits are neuroticism (those high on this trait are prone to
feeling sad, worried, anxious, and dissatisfied with themselves), extraversion
(high scorers are friendly, assertive, outgoing, cheerful, and energetic),
openness to experience (those high on this trait are tolerant, intellectually
curious, imaginative, and artistic), agreeableness (high scorers are polite,
considerate, cooperative, honest, and trusting), and conscientiousness (those
high on this trait are responsible, cautious, organized, disciplined, and
achievement-oriented).
High-stakes testing
Settings in which test scores are used to make important decisions about
individuals. For example, test scores may be used to determine which
individuals are admitted into a college or graduate school, or who should be
hired for a job. Tests also are used in forensic settings to help determine whether
a person is competent to stand trial or fits the legal definition of sanity.
Honeymoon effect
The tendency for newly married individuals to rate their spouses in an
unrealistically positive manner. This represents a specific manifestation of the
letter of recommendation effect when applied to ratings made by current
romantic partners. Moreover, it illustrates the very important role played by
relationship satisfaction in ratings made by romantic partners: As marital
satisfaction declines (i.e., when the honeymoon is over), this effect disappears.
Implicit motives
These are goals that are important to a person, but that he/she cannot
consciously express. Because the individual cannot verbalize these goals
directly, they cannot be easily assessed via self-report. However, they can be
measured using projective devices such as the Thematic Apperception Test
(TAT).
Letter of recommendation effect
The general tendency for informants in personality studies to rate others in an
unrealistically positive manner.
contrast effect refers to the tendency of parents to exaggerate the true extent
of differences between their children.
Reliablility
The consistency of test scores across repeated assessments. For example, testretest reliability examines the extent to which scores change over time.
Self-enhancement bias
The tendency for people to see and/or present themselves in an overly favorable
way. This tendency can take two basic forms: defensiveness (when individuals
actually believe they are better than they really are) and impression
management (when people intentionally distort their responses to try to
convince others that they are better than they really are). Informants also can
show enhancement biases. The general form of this bias has been called the
letter-of-recommendation effect, which is the tendency of informants who like
the person they are rating (e.g., friends, relatives, romantic partners) to describe
them in an overly favorable way. In the case of newlyweds, this tendency has
been termed the honeymoon effect.
Sibling contrast effect
The tendency of parents to use their perceptions of all of their children as a
frame of reference for rating the characteristics of each of them. For example,
suppose that a mother has three children; two of these children are very
sociable and outgoing, whereas the third is relatively average in sociability.
Because of operation of this effect, the mother will rate this third child as less
sociable and outgoing than he/she actually is. More generally, this effect causes
parents to exaggerate the true extent of differences between their children.
This effect represents a specific manifestation of the more general reference
group effect when applied to ratings made by parents.
Validity
Evidence related to the interpretation and use of test scores. A particularly
important type of evidence is criterion validity, which involves the ability of a
test to predict theoretically relevant outcomes. For example, a presumed
measure of conscientiousness should be related to academic achievement
(such as overall grade point average).
Reference List
Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2009). Predicting actual behavior from
the explicit and implicit self-concept of personality. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 97, 533548.
Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D. A., Taylor, G. J. (1994). The Twenty-Item Toronto
Alexithymia Scale: I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor
structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 2332.
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Funder, D. C. (2007). Psychology as the science
of self-reports and finger movements: Whatever happened to actual
behavior? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 396403.
Beer, A., & Watson, D. (2010). The effects of information and exposure on selfother agreement. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 3845.
Cattell, R. B., Eber, H. W, & Tatsuoka, M. M. (1980). Handbook for the Sixteen
Personality Factor Questionnaire (16PF). Champaign, IL: Institute for
Personality and Ability Testing.
Exner, J. E. (2003). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system (4th ed.). New York,
NY: Wiley.
Frank, L. K. (1939). Projective methods for the study of personality. Journal of
Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 8, 389413.
Friedman, H. S., Kern, K. L., & Reynolds, C. A. (2010). Personality and health,
subjective well-being, and longevity. Journal of Personality, 78, 179215.
Oh, I.-S., Wang, G., & Mount, M. K. (2011). Validity of observer ratings of the fivefactor model of personality traits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 96, 762773.
Oltmanns, T. F., & Turkheimer, E. (2009). Person perception and personality
pathology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 3236.
Patrick, C. J., Curtin, J. J., & Tellegen, A. (2002). Development and validation of a
brief form of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Psychological
Assessment, 14, 150163.
Paunonen, S. V., & LeBel, E. P. (2012). Socially desirable responding and its elusive
effects on the validity of personality assessments. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 103, 158175.
Pinto, R., Rijsdijk, F., Frazier-Wood, A. C., Asherson, P., & Kuntsi, J. (2012). Bigger
families fare better: A novel method to estimate rater contrast effects in
parental ratings on ADHD symptoms. Behavior Genetics, 42, 875885.
Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality
and academic performance. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 322338.
Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N. R., Shiner, R., Caspi, A., & Goldberg, L. R. (2007). The
power of personality: The comparative validity of personality traits,
socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for predicting important life
outcomes. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 313345.
Rorschach, H. (1942) (Original work published 1921). Psychodiagnostik
[Psychodiagnostics]. Bern, Switzerland: Bircher.
under
the
Creative
Commons
Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0
Self-Regulation and
Conscientiousness
Roy F. Baumeister
Florida State University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Self-regulation means changing oneself based on standards, that is, ideas of
how one should or should not be. It is a centrally important capacity that
contributes to socially desirable behavior, including moral behavior. Effective
self-regulation requires knowledge of standards for proper behavior, careful
monitoring of ones actions and feelings, and the ability to make desired
changes.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
Self-regulation is the capacity to alter ones responses. It is broadly related to
the term self-control. The term regulate means to change somethingbut
not just any change, rather change to bring it into agreement with some idea,
such as a rule, a goal, a plan, or a moral principle. To illustrate, when the
government regulates how houses are built, that means the government
inspects the buildings to check that everything is done up to code or according
to the rules about good building. In a similar fashion, when you regulate
yourself, you watch and change yourself to bring your responses into line with
some ideas about how they should be.
People regulate four broad categories of responses. They control their
thinking, such as in trying to concentrate or to shut some annoying earworm
tune out of their mind. They control their emotions, as in trying to cheer
themselves up or to calm down when angry (or to stay angry, if thats helpful).
They control their impulses, as in trying not to eat fattening food, trying to hold
ones tongue, or trying to quit smoking. Last, they try to control their task
performances, such as in pushing themselves to keep working when tired and
discouraged, or deciding whether to speed up (to get more done) or slow down
(to make sure to get it right).
1514
for a few minutes and then Ill be back. You can have this treat any time, but if
you can wait until I come back, you can have two of them. Two treats are better
than one, but to get the double treat, the child had to wait. Self-regulation was
required to resist that urge to gobble down the marshmallow on the table so
as to reap the larger reward.
Many situations in life demand similar delays for best results. Going to
college to get an education often means living in poverty and debt rather than
getting a job to earn money right away. But in the long run, the college degree
increases your lifetime income by hundreds of thousands of dollars. Very few
nonhuman animals can bring themselves to resist immediate temptations so
as to pursue future rewards, but this trait is an important key to success in
human life.
Benefits of Self-Control
People who are good at self-regulation do better than others in life. Follow-up
studies with Mischels samples found that the children who resisted temptation
and delayed gratification effectively grew into adults who were better than
others in school and work, more popular with other people, and who were rated
as nicer, better people by teachers and others (Mischel, Shoda, & Peake, 1988;
Shoda, Mischel, & Peake, 1990). College students with high self-control get
better grades, have better close relationships, manage their emotions better,
have fewer problems with drugs and alcohol, are less prone to eating disorders,
are better adjusted, have higher self-esteem, and get along better with other
people, as compared to people with low self-control (Tangney, Baumeister, &
Boone, 2004). They are happier and have less stress and conflict (Hofmann,
Vohs, Fisher, Luhmann, & Baumeister, 2013). Longitudinal studies have found
that children with good self-control go through life with fewer problems, are
more successful, are less likely to be arrested or have a child out of wedlock,
and enjoy other benefits (Moffitt et al., 2011). Criminologists have concluded
nobaproject.com - Self-Regulation and Conscientiousness
1515
that low self-control is aif not thekey trait for understanding the criminal
personality (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990; Pratt & Cullen, 2000).
Some researchers have searched for evidence that too much self-control
can be bad (Tangney et al., 2004)but without success. There is such a thing
as being highly inhibited or clinically over-controlled, which can impair
initiative and reduce happiness, but that does not appear to be an excess of
self-regulation. Rather, it may stem from having been punished excessively as
a child and, therefore, adopting a fearful, inhibited approach to life. In general,
self-control resembles intelligence in that the more one has, the better off one
is, and the benefits are found through a broad range of life activities.
1516
eating more (Polivy, 1976). Alcohol has been found to impair all sorts of selfregulation, partly because intoxicated persons fail to keep track of their behavior
and compare it to their standards.
The combination of standards and monitoring was featured in an influential
theory about self-regulation by Carver and Scheier (1981, 1982, 1998). Those
researchers started their careers studying self-awareness, which is a key human
trait. The study of self-awareness recognized early on that people do not simply
notice themselves the way they might notice a tree or car. Rather, selfawareness always seemed to involve comparing oneself to a standard. For
example, when a man looks in a mirror, he does not just think, Oh, there I am,
but more likely thinks, Is my hair a mess? Do my clothes look good? Carver
and Scheier proposed that the reason for this comparison to standards is that
it enables people to regulate themselves, such as by changing things that do
not measure up to their standards. In the mirror example, the man might comb
his hair to bring it into line with his standards for personal appearance. Good
students keep track of their grades, credits, and progress toward their degree
and other goals. Athletes keep track of their times, scores, and achievements,
as a way to monitor improvement.
The process of monitoring oneself can be compared to how a thermostat
operates. The thermostat checks the temperature in the room, compares it to
a standard (the setting for desired temperature), and if those do not match, it
turns on the heat or air conditioner to change the temperature. It checks again
and again, and when the room temperature matches the desired setting, the
thermostat turns off the climate control. In the same way, people compare
themselves to their personal standards, make changes as needed, and stop
working on change once they have met their goals. People feel good not just
when they reach their goals but even when they deem they are making good
progress (Carver & Scheier, 1990). They feel bad when they are not making
sufficient progress.
nobaproject.com - Self-Regulation and Conscientiousness
1517
That brings up the third ingredient, which is the capacity to change oneself.
In effective self-regulation, people operate on themselves to bring about these
changes. The popular term for this is willpower, which suggests some kind of
energy is expended in the process. Psychologists hesitate to adopt terms
associated with folk wisdom, because there are many potential implications.
Here, the term is used to refer specifically to some energy that is involved in
the capacity to change oneself.
Consistent with the popular notion of willpower, people do seem to expend
some energy during self-regulation. Many studies have found that after people
exert self-regulation to change some response, they perform worse on the next
unrelated task if it too requires self-regulation (Hagger, Wood, Stiff, &
Chatzisarantis, 2010). That pattern suggests that some energy such as willpower
was used up during the first task, leaving less available for the second task. The
term for this state of reduced energy available for self-regulation is ego
depletion (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Muraven, & Tice, 1998). As people go about
their daily lives, they have to resist many desires and impulses and must control
themselves in other ways, and so over the course of a typical day many people
gradually become ego depleted. The result is that they become increasingly
likely to give in to impulses and desires that they would have resisted
successfully earlier in the day (Hofmann, Vohs, & Baumeister, 2012). During the
state of ego depletion, people become less helpful and more aggressive, prone
to overeat, misbehave sexually, express more prejudice, and in other ways do
things that they may later regret.
Thus, a persons capacity for self-regulation is not constant, but rather it
fluctuates. To be sure, some people are generally better than others at
controlling themselves (Tangney et al., 2004). But even someone with excellent
self-control may occasionally find that control breaks down under ego
depletion. In general, self-regulation can be improved by getting enough sleep
and healthy food, and by minimizing other demands on ones willpower.
nobaproject.com - Self-Regulation and Conscientiousness
1518
Conscientiousness
Conscientiousness is a stable dimension of personality, which means that some
people are typically higher on it than others. Being a personality trait does not
mean that it is unchangeable. Most people do show some changes over time,
particularly becoming higher on conscientiousness as they grow older. Some
psychologists look specifically at the trait of self-control, which is understood
(and measured) in personality psychology in a very specific, narrowly focused,
well-defined sense. Conscientiousness, in contrast, is one of five super-traits
that supposedly account for all the other traits, in various combinations. The
trait self-control is one big part of conscientiousness, but there are other parts.
Two aspects of conscientiousness that have been well documented are being
orderly and being industrious (Roberts, Lejuez, Krueger, Richards, & Hill, 2012).
Orderliness includes being clean and neat, making and following plans, and
being punctual (which is helpful with following plans!). Low conscientious means
the opposite: being disorganized, messy, late, or erratic. Being industrious not
nobaproject.com - Self-Regulation and Conscientiousness
1519
only means working hard but also persevering in the face of failures and
difficulties, as well as aspiring to excellence. Most of these reflect good selfcontrol.
Conscientious people are careful, disciplined, responsible, and thorough,
and they tend to plan and think things through before acting. People who are
low in conscientiousness tend to be more impulsive and spontaneous, even
reckless. They are easygoing and may often be late or sloppy, partly because
they are not strongly focused on future goals for success and not highly
concerned to obey all rules and stay on schedule. Psychologists prefer not to
make a value judgment about whether it is better to be high or low in any
personality trait. But when it comes specifically to self-control, it is difficult to
resist the conclusion that high self-control is better, both for the person and
for society at large.
Some aspects of conscientiousness have less apparent connection to selfcontrol, however. People high in conscientiousness tend to be decisive. They
are often formal, in the sense of following social norms and rules, such as
dressing properly, waiting ones turn, or holding doors for others. They tend to
respect traditions and traditional values.
Conscientious people behave differently from people who score low on that
trait. People scoring low on conscientiousness are more likely than others to
report driving without wearing seatbelts, daydreaming, swearing, telling dirty
jokes, and picking up hitchhikers (Hirsh, DeYoung, & Peterson, 2009). In terms
of more substantial life outcomes, people low on conscientiousness are more
likely than others to get divorced, presumably because they make bad choices
and misbehave during the marriage such as by saying hurtful things, getting
into arguments and fights, and behaving irresponsibly (Roberts, Jackson,
Fayard, Edmonds, & Meints, 2009). People low on conscientiousness are more
likely than others to lose their jobs, to become homeless, to do time in prison,
to have money problems, and to have drug problems.
nobaproject.com - Self-Regulation and Conscientiousness
1520
Conscientious people make better spouses. They are less likely than others
to get divorced, partly because they avoid many behaviors that undermine
intimacy, such as abusing their partners, drinking excessively, or having
extramarital affairs (Roberts et al., 2009).
Encompassing self-control, conscientiousness is the personality trait with
the strongest effect on life or death: People high on that trait live longer than
others (Deary, Weiss, & Batty, 2010). Why? Among other things, they avoid many
behavior patterns associated with early death, including alcohol abuse, obesity
and other eating problems, drug abuse, smoking, failure to exercise, risky sex,
suicide, violence, and unsafe driving (Bogg & Roberts, in press). They also visit
physicians more regularly and take their prescribed medicines more reliably
than people low in conscientiousness. Their good habits help avoid many lifethreatening diseases.
1521
Outside Resources
Book: For more advanced and in-depth coverage, consult The Handbook of
Self-Regulation (2nd Edition), edited by Kathleen Vohs and Roy Baumeister.
This book contains different chapters by different experts in the field,
covering large amounts of research findings.
Book: To read more, the easiest and most fun source would be The New York
Times bestseller Willpower: Rediscovering the Greatest Human Strength, by
Roy Baumeister and John Tierney, published by Penguin. This is intended not
as a purely scientific work but as an entertaining summary for the general
public.
Discussion Questions
1. Why do you think criminals are often poor at self-regulation?
2. On average, children growing up without both parents present do worse at
many things, from math achievement in school to the likelihood of being
arrested for crimes. Might self-control be part of the explanation? Why?
3. Many people make New Years resolutions to change themselves in various
ways, but often they fail at these. Why?
4. Is good self-control something one is born with or something that is learned?
5. How would a parent teach his or her children to have good self-control?
6. Why are people with good self-control happier than other people?
Vocabulary
Conscientiousness
A personality trait consisting of self-control, orderliness, industriousness, and
traditionalism.
Ego depletion
The state of diminished willpower or low energy associated with having exerted
self-regulation.
Monitoring
Keeping track of a target behavior that is to be regulated.
Self-regulation
The process of altering ones responses, including thoughts, feelings, impulses,
actions, and task performance.
Standards
Ideas about how things should (or should not) be.
Reference List
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion:
Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 74, 12521265. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252
Baumeister, R. F., & Tierney, J. (2011). Willpower: Rediscovering the greatest
human strength. New York, NY: Penguin Press.
Bogg, T., & Roberts, B.W. (in press). The case for conscientiousness: Evidence
and implications for a personality trait marker of health and longevity.
Annals of Behavioral Medicine.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control
theory approach to human behavior. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. E. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and
negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 1935.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New
York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A useful conceptual
framework for personality-social, clinical, and health psychology.
Psychological Bulletin, 92, 111135.
Deary, I. J., Weiss, A., & Batty, G. D. (2010). Intelligence and personality as
predictors of illness and death: How researchers in differential psychology
and chronic disease epidemiology are collaborating to understand and
address health inequalities. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 11,
5379.
Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory of crime. Stanford, CA:
Stanford University Press.
Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C. & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010). Ego depletion
and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 136, 495525.
Heckman, J., Pinto, R., & Savelyev, P. (in press). Understanding the mechanisms
through which an influential early childhood program boosted adult
outcomes. American Economic Review.
Hirsh, J. B., DeYoung, C. G., & Peterson, J. B. (2009). Metatraits of the Big Five
differentially predict engagement and restraint of behavior. Journal of
Personality, 77, 10851102.
Hofmann, W., Vohs, K. D., Fisher, R., Luhmann, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2013).
Yes, but are they happy? Effects of trait self-control on affective well-being
and life satisfaction. Manuscript submitted for publication. University of
Chicago.
Hofmann, W., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012). What people desire, feel
conflicted about, and try to resist in everyday life. Psychological Science, 23,
582-588. doi: 10.1177/0956797612437426
Mischel, W. (1974). Processes in delay of gratification. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 249292). San Diego,
CA: Academic Press.
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Peake, P. (1988). The nature of adolescent competencies
predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 54, 687696.
Moffitt, T. E., Arsenault, L. Belsky, D., Dickson, N. Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H.,
Houts, R., Poulton, R., Roberts, B. W., Ross, S., Sears, M. R., Thomson, W. M.,
& Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health,
wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
108, 26932698.
Oaten, M., & Cheng, K. (2006). Improved self-control: The benefits of a regular
program of academic study. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28, 116.
Polivy, J. (1976). Perception of calories and regulation of intake in restrained and
unrestrained subjects. Addictive Behaviors, 1, 237-243.
Pratt, T. C., & Cullen, F. T. (2000). The empirical status of Gottfredson and Hirschis
general theory of crime: A meta-analysis. Criminology, 38, 931964.
Roberts, B. W., Jackson, J. J., Fayard, J. V., Edmonds, G., & Meints, J. (2009).
Conscientiousness. In M. Leary & R. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual
differences in social behavior (pp. 369381). New York, NY: Guilford.
Roberts, B. W., Lejuez, C., Krueger, R. F., Richards, J. M., & Hill, P. L. (2012). What
is conscientiousness and how can it be assessed? Developmental
Psychology. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/a0031109
Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent cognitive and
self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification:
Identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental Psychology, 26, 978986.
Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts
good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success.
Journal of Personality, 72, 271322.
Creativity
Dean Keith Simonton
University of California, Davis
nobaproject.com
Abstract
An idea or solution is considered creative if it is original, useful, and surprising.
However, depending on who actually judges these three criteria, we must
distinguish personal little-c creativity from consensual Big-C Creativity. In
any case, psychologists who investigate creativity most often adopt one of three
perspectives. First, they can ask how creators think, and thus focus on the
cognitive processes behind creativity. Second, they can ask who is creative, and
hence investigate the personal characteristics of highly creative people. Third,
they can ask about the social context, and, thereby, examine the environments
that influence creativity. Although psychologists have made major advances in
the study of creativity, many exciting and important questions remain to be
answered.
Learning Objectives
Comprehend
an idea is
Appreciate
creative.
some of the cognitive processes that provide the basis for
creativity.
Know
Understand
What do the following have in common: the drug penicillin, the Eiffel Tower, the
film Lord of the Rings, the General Theory of Relativity, the hymn Amazing Grace,
the iPhone, the novel Don Quixote, the painting The Mona Lisa, a recipe for
chocolate fudge, the soft drink Coca-Cola, the video game Wii Sports, the West
Coast offense in football, and the zipper? You guessed right! All of the named
items were products of the creative mind. Not one of them existed until
somebody came up with the idea. Creativity is not something that you just pick
like apples from a tree. Because creative ideas are so special, creators who come
up with the best ideas are often highly rewarded with fame, fortune, or both.
Nobel Prizes, Oscars, Pulitzers, and other honors bring fame, and big sales and
box office bring fortune. Yet what is creativity in the first place?
1533
The third and last criterion is surprise. The idea should be surprising, or at
least nonobvious (to use the term used by the Patent Office). For instance, a
solution that is a straightforward derivation from acquired expertise cannot be
considered surprising even if it were original. Einsteins relativity theory was not
a step-by-step deduction from classical physics but rather the theory was built
upon a new foundation that challenged the very basis of traditional physics.
When applying these three criteria, it is critical to recognize that originality,
usefulness, and surprise are all quantitative rather than qualitative attributes
of an idea. Specifically, we really have to speak of degree to which an idea
satisfies each of the three criteria. In addition, the three attributes should have
a zero point, that is, it should be possible to speak of an idea lacking any
originality, usefulness, or surprise whatsoever. Finally, we have to assume that
if an idea scores zero on any one criterion then it must have zero creativity as
well. For example, someone who reinvents the wheel is definitely producing a
useful idea, but the idea has zero originality and hence no creativity whatsoever.
Similarly, someone who invented a parachute made entirely out of steel
reinforced concrete would get lots of credit for originalityand surprise!but
none for usefulness.
Yet, certainly, we have to ask: Who makes these judgments? The person who
generated the idea or other people who the person expects to appreciate the
idea? If the former, we can speak of subjective or personal little-c creativity,
and if the later, we have objective or consensual Big-C Creativity (Simonton,
in press). This distinction is important because sometimes personal and
consensual assessments do not have to agree. Such disagreements are
especially conspicuous in neglected geniuses, such as the poet Emily
Dickinson, the painter Vincent Van Gogh, and the scientist Gregor Mendelall
producing ideas that received only posthumous recognition for their creativity.
nobaproject.com - Creativity
1534
1535
1536
Table 1. The Creative Personality Scale of the Gough (1979) Adjective Check List. Note. The
Adjective Check List actually contains 300 adjectives arranged in alphabetical order from
absent-minded to zany, but the above adjectives are the only ones scored.
1537
1538
just because you become an expert in your field it does not mean that youll be
creative, too.
1539
The above effect is most likely transient. It is doubtful that those participants
exposed to such incongruous experiences would exhibit any long-term change
in their creativity. But what would happen if the exposure was much longer,
years rather than minutes? Then the benefit might endure a lifetime. An
example is the long-term benefits that accrue to persons who have acquired
multicultural experiences, such as living in a foreign country for a significant
amount of time (Leung, Maddux, Galinsky, & Chiu, 2008). Daily life abroad
exposes a person to different ways of doing everyday activities. Moreover,
because the visitor quickly learns that when in Rome do as the Romans do,
the exposure becomes direct rather than vicarious (Maddux, Adam, & Galinsky,
2010). To be sure, not everybodys creativity benefits from multicultural
environments. The person also has to score high on openness to experience
(Leung & Chiu, 2008). Otherwise, they will close themselves off from the
potential stimulation, and then just gripe about the peculiar customs of the
natives rather than actively practice those customssuch as making a totally
different breakfast!
Finally, both little-c and Big-C creativitybut especially the latterare more
likely to appear in specific sociocultural systems (Simonton, 2003a). Some
political, social, cultural, and economic environments are supportive of
exceptional creativity, whereas others tend to suppress if not destroy creativity.
For this reason, the history of any civilization or nation tends to have Dark
Ages as well as Golden Ages. Early medieval Europe illustrates the former,
while Renaissance Italy exemplifies the latter. It would take us too far beyond
introductory psychology to discuss all of the relevant factors. Yet, one factor fits
nicely with what was discussed in the previous paragraph. Highly creative
societies are far more likely to be multicultural, with abundant influences from
other civilizations. For instance, Japanese civilization tended to undergo a revival
of creativity after the infusion of new ideas from other civilizations, including
Korean, Chinese, Indian, and European (Simonton, 1997). This influx involved
not just Japanese living abroad but also non-Japanese immigrating to Japan.
nobaproject.com - Creativity
1540
Conclusion
Creativity certainly must be considered a crucial human behavior. Indeed, like
language, creativity sets Homo sapiens well apart from even our closest
evolutionary relatives. It is virtually impossible to imagine a world in which all
of the products of the creative mind were removed. I couldnt even type this
very sentence at this instant. Even the alphabet was invented. Creativity
permeates every aspect of modern life: technology, science, literature, the visual
arts, music, cooking, sports, politics, war, business, advertising ... well, I could
go on and on. Fortunately, psychologists have made major strides in
understanding the phenomenon. In fact, some of the best studies of creativity
are also excellent examples of scientific creativity. At the same time, it remains
clear that we still have a long ways to go before we know everything we need
to know about the psychology of creativity. Hence, creativity research has a
bright future.
nobaproject.com - Creativity
1541
Outside Resources
Video: Amy Tan: Where does creativity hide?
http://www.ted.com/talks/amy_tan_on_creativity.html
Video: Creativity science
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=EL4bVMeuxlM&feature=youtu.be
Video: How to be creative
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=oq0ijANlIeM
Web: American Creativity Association
http://www.aca.cloverpad.org/
Web: Be More Creative
http://www.bemorecreative.com/
Web: Creating Minds
http://creatingminds.org/
Web: Creative Quotations
http://www.creativequotations.com/
Web: Creativity at Work
http://www.creativityatwork.com/
Discussion Questions
1. To be
computers programmer?
that mean
creative?
egotistical,
For example, will you become more creative if you become more
individualistic, informal, reflective, self-confident, sexy, and
unconventional? Or, how about widening your interests and becoming more
open to experience? Which comes first, the personality or the capacity?
Vocabulary
Big-C Creativity
Creative ideas that have an impact well beyond the everyday life of home or
work. At the highest level, this kind of creativity is that of the creative genius.
Convergent thinking
The opposite of divergent thinking, the capacity to narrow in on the single
correct answer or solution to a given question or problem (e.g., giving the right
response on an intelligence tests).
Divergent thinking
The opposite of convergent thinking, the capacity for exploring multiple
potential answers or solutions to a given question or problem (e.g., coming up
with many different uses for a common object).
Latent inhibition
The ability to filter out extraneous stimuli, concentrating only on the information
that is deemed relevant. Reduced latent inhibition is associated with higher
creativity.
Little-c creativity
Creative ideas that appear at the personal level, whether the home or the
workplace. Such creativity needs not have a larger impact to be considered
creative.
Multicultural experiences
Individual exposure to two or more cultures, such as obtained by living abroad,
emigrating to another country, or working or going to school in a culturally
diverse setting.
Openness to experience
One of the factors of the Big Five Model of personality, the factor assesses the
degree that a person is open to different or new values, interests, and activities.
Originality
When an idea or solution has a low probability of occurrence.
Remote associations
Associations between words or concepts that are semantically distant and thus
relatively unusual or original.
Unusual uses
A test of divergent thinking that asks participants to find many uses for
commonplace objects, such as a brick or paperclip.
Reference List
Carson, S., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Reliability, validity, and factor
structure of the Creative Achievement Questionnaire. Creativity Research
Journal, 17, 3750.
Carson, S. H. (2011). Creativity and psychopathology: A shared vulnerability
model. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 144-153.
Ericsson, K. A. (1996). The acquisition of expert performance: An introduction
to some of the issues. In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.), The road to expert performance:
Empirical evidence from the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 1
50). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The natural history of creativity. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic
creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 290309.
Gough, H. G. (1979). A creative personality scale for the adjective check list.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 13981405.
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York, NY: McGrawHill.
Harris, J. A. (2004). Measured intelligence, achievement, openness to
experience, and creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 913
929.
functions,
and
social
network
size
predict
creative
through collaboration (pp. 304328). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Simonton, D. K. (2003b). Creativity assessment. In R. Fernndez-Ballesteros
(Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychological assessment (Vol. 1, pp. 276280).
London, UK: Sage Publications.
Simonton, D. K. (1997). Foreign influence and national achievement: The impact
of open milieus on Japanese civilization. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 72, 8694.
Simonton, D. K., & Damian, R. I. (2013). Creativity. In D. Reisberg (Ed.), Oxford
handbook of cognitive psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Torrance, E.P. (1974). Torrance tests of creative thinking: Norms-technical
manual. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service, Inc.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Creativity by Dean Keith Simonton is licensed
under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International
License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
deed.en_US.
Self-Efficacy
James E Maddux & Evan Kleiman
George Mason University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
The term self-efficacy refers to your beliefs about your ability to effectively
perform the tasks needed to attain a valued goal. Self-efficacy does not refer
to your abilities but to how strongly you believe you can use your abilities to
work toward goals. Self-efficacy is not a unitary construct or trait; rather, people
have self-efficacy beliefs in different domains, such as academic self-efficacy,
problem-solving self-efficacy, and self-regulatory self-efficacy. Stronger selfefficacy beliefs are associated with positive outcomes, such as better grades,
greater athletic performance, happier romantic relationships, and a healthier
lifestyle.
Learning Objectives
Define self-efficacy.
1555
Self-efficacy may sound similar to a concept you may be familiar with already
self-esteembut these are very different notions. Self-esteem refers to how
much you like or esteem yourselfto what extent you believe you are a good
and worthwhile person. Self-efficacy, however, refers to your self-confidence
to perform well and to achieve in specific areas of life such as school, work, and
relationships. Self-efficacy does influence self-esteem because how you feel
about yourself overall is greatly influenced by your confidence in your ability to
perform well in areas that are important to you and to achieve valued goals.
For example, if performing well in athletics is very important to you, then your
self-efficacy for athletics will greatly influence your self-esteem; however, if
performing well in athletics is not at all important you to you, then your selfefficacy for athletics will probably have little impact on your self-esteem.
1556
means very often. Then all of the answers are summed together to create a
total stress score, with higher scores equating to higher levels of stress. It is
very important to develop tools to measure self-efficacy that take peoples
subjective beliefs about their self-efficacy and turn them into the most objective
possible measure. This means that one persons score of 6 out of 10 on a
measure of self-efficacy will be similar to another persons score of 6 out of 10
on the same measure.
We will discuss two broad types of self-report measures for self-efficacy. The
first category includes measures of general self-efficacy (e.g., Schwarzer &
Jerusalem, 1995; Sherer et al., 1982). These scales ask people to rate themselves
on general items, such as It is easy for me to stick to my aims and accomplish
my goals and I can usually handle whatever comes my way. If you remember
from earlier in this chapter, however, self-efficacy is not a global trait, so there
are problems with lumping all types of self-efficacy together in one measure.
Thus, the second category of self-efficacy measures includes task-specific
measures of self-efficacy. Rather than gauge self-efficacy in general, these
measures ask about a persons self-efficacy beliefs about a particular task. There
can be an unlimited number of these types of measures. Task-specific measures
of self-efficacy describe several situations relating to a behavior and then ask
the participant to write down how confidently he or she feels about doing that
behavior. For example, a measure of dieting self-efficacy would list a variety of
situations where it can be hard to stick to a dietsuch as during vacations,
when bored, or when going out to eat with others who are not on a diet. A
measure of exercise self-efficacy would list a variety of situations where it can
be hard to exercisesuch as when feeling depressed, when feeling tired, and
when you are with other people who do not want to exercise. Finally, a measure
of childrens or teens self-regulatory self-efficacy would include a variety of
situations where it can be hard to resist impulsessuch as controlling temper,
resisting peer pressure to smoke cigarettes, and defying pressure to have
unprotected sex. Most studies agree that the task-specific measures of selfnobaproject.com - Self-Efficacy
1557
efficacy are better predictors of behavior than the general measures of selfefficacy (Bandura, 2006).
nobaproject.com - Self-Efficacy
1558
These five types of self-efficacy influence can take many real-world forms
that almost everyone has experienced. You may have had previous
performance experiences affect your academic self-efficacy when you did well
on a test and believed that you would do well on the next test. A vicarious
performance may have affected your athletic self-efficacy when you saw your
best friend skateboard for the first time and thought that you could skateboard
well, too. Verbal persuasion could have affected your academic self-efficacy
when a teacher that you respect told you that you could get into the college of
your choice if you studied hard for the SATs. Its important to know that not all
people are equally likely to influence your self-efficacy though verbal
persuasion. People who appear trustworthy or attractive, or who seem to be
experts, are more likely to influence your self-efficacy than are people who do
not possess these qualities (Petty & Brinol, 2010). Thats why a teacher you
respect is more likely to influence your self-efficacy than a teacher you do not
respect. Imaginal performances are an effective way to increase your selfefficacy. For example, imagining yourself doing well on a job interview actually
leads to more effective interviewing (Knudstrup, Segrest, & Hurley, 2003).
Affective states and physical sensations abound when you think about the times
you have given presentations in class. For example, you may have felt your
heart racing while giving a presentation. If you believed your heart was racing
because you had just had a lot of caffeine, it likely would not affect your
performance. If you believed your heart was racing because you were doing a
poor job, you might believe that you cannot give the presentation well. This is
because you associate the feeling of anxiety with failure and expect to fail when
you are feeling anxious.
nobaproject.com - Self-Efficacy
1559
1560
because you did or did not believe in your abilities to do well? Many researchers
have considered how self-efficacy works in academic settings, and the short
answer is that academic self-efficacy affects every possible area of academic
achievement (Pajares, 1996).
Students who believe in their ability to do well academically tend to be more
motivated in school (Schunk, 1991). When self-efficacious students attain their
goals, they continue to set even more challenging goals (Schunk, 1990). This
can all lead to better performance in school in terms of higher grades and taking
more challenging classes (Multon, Brown, & Lent, 1991). For example, students
with high academic self-efficacies might study harder because they believe that
they are able to use their abilities to study effectively. Because they studied
hard, they receive an A on their next test. Teachers self-efficacies also can affect
how well a student performs in school. Self-efficacious teachers encourage
parents to take a more active role in their childrens learning, leading to better
academic performance (Hoover-Dempsey, Bassler, & Brissie, 1987).
Although there is a lot of research about how self-efficacy is beneficial to
school-aged children, college students can also benefit from self-efficacy.
Freshmen with higher self-efficacies about their ability to do well in college tend
to adapt to their first year in college better than those with lower self-efficacies
(Chemers, Hu, & Garcia, 2001). The benefits of self-efficacy continue beyond
the school years: people with strong self-efficacy beliefs toward performing well
in school tend to perceive a wider range of career options (Lent, Brown, & Larkin,
1986). In addition, people who have stronger beliefs of self-efficacy toward their
professional work tend to have more successful careers (Stajkovic & Luthans,
1998).
One question you might have about self-efficacy and academic performance
is how a students actual academic ability interacts with self-efficacy to influence
academic performance. The answer is that a students actual ability does play
a role, but it is also influenced by self-efficacy. Students with greater ability
perform better than those with lesser ability. But, among a group of students
nobaproject.com - Self-Efficacy
1561
with the same exact level of academic ability, those with stronger academic selfefficacies outperform those with weaker self-efficacies. One study (Collins,
1984) compared performance on difficult math problems among groups of
students with different levels of math ability and different levels of math selfefficacy. Among a group of students with average levels of math ability, the
students with weak math self-efficacies got about 25% of the math problems
correct. The students with average levels of math ability and strong math selfefficacies got about 45% of the questions correct. This means that by just having
stronger math self-efficacy, a student of average math ability will perform 20%
better than a student with similar math ability but weaker math self-efficacy.
You might also wonder if self-efficacy makes a difference only for people with
average or below-average abilities. Self-efficacy is important even for aboveaverage students. In this study, those with above-average math abilities and
low math self-efficacies answered only about 65% of the questions correctly;
those with above-average math abilities and high math self-efficacies answered
about 75% of the questions correctly.
Healthy Behaviors
Think about a time when you tried to improve your health, whether through
dieting, exercising, sleeping more, or any other way. Would you be more likely
to follow through on these plans if you believed that you could effectively use
your skills to accomplish your health goals? Many researchers agree that people
with stronger self-efficacies for doing healthy things (e.g., exercise self-efficacy,
dieting self-efficacy) engage in more behaviors that prevent health problems
and improve overall health (Strecher, DeVellis, Becker, & Rosenstock, 1986).
People who have strong self-efficacy beliefs about quitting smoking are able to
quit smoking more easily (DiClemente, Prochaska, & Gibertini, 1985). People
who have strong self-efficacy beliefs about being able to reduce their alcohol
consumption are more successful when treated for drinking problems (Maisto,
nobaproject.com - Self-Efficacy
1562
Connors, & Zywiak, 2000). People who have stronger self-efficacy beliefs about
their ability to recover from heart attacks do so more quickly than those who
do not have such beliefs (Ewart, Taylor, Reese, & DeBusk, 1983).
One group of researchers (Roach Yadrick, Johnson, Boudreaux, Forsythe, &
Billon, 2003) conducted an experiment with people trying to lose weiht. All
people in the study participated in a weight loss program that was designed for
the U.S. Air Force. This program had already been found to be very effective,
but the researchers wanted to know if increasing peoples self-efficacies could
make the program even more effective. So, they divided the participants into
two groups: one group received an intervention that was designed to increase
weight loss self-efficacy along with the diet program, and the other group
received only the diet program. The researchers tried several different ways to
increase self-efficacy, such as having participants read a copy of Oh, The Places
Youll Go! by Dr. Seuss (1990), and having them talk to someone who had
successfully lost weight. The people who received the diet program and an
intervention to increase self-efficacy lost an average of 8.2 pounds over the 12
weeks of the study; those participants who had only the diet program lost only
5.8 pounds. Thus, just by increasing weight loss self-efficacy, participants were
able to lose over 50% more weight.
Studies have found that increasing a persons nutritional self-efficacy can
lead them to eat more fruits and vegetables (Luszczynska, Tryburcy, &
Schwarzer, 2006). Self-efficacy plays a large role in successful physical exercise
(Maddux & Dawson, 2014). People with stronger self-efficacies for exercising
are more likely to plan on beginning an exercise program, actually beginning
that program (DuCharme & Brawley, 1995), and continuing it (Marcus, Selby,
Niaura, & Rossi, 1992). Self-efficacy is especially important when it comes to
safe sex. People with greater self-efficacies about condom usage are more likely
to engage in safe sex (Kaneko, 2007), making them more likely to avoid sexually
transmitted diseases, such as HIV (Forsyth & Carey, 1998).
nobaproject.com - Self-Efficacy
1563
Athletic Performance
If you are an athlete, self-efficacy is especially important in your life. Professional
and amateur athletes with stronger self-efficacy beliefs about their athletic
abilities perform better than athletes with weaker levels of self-efficacy
(Wurtele, 1986). This holds true for athletes in all types of sports, including track
and field (Gernigon & Delloye, 2003), tennis (Sheldon & Eccles, 2005), and golf
(Bruton, Mellalieu, Shearer, Roderique-Davies, & Hall, 2013). One group of
researchers found that basketball players with strong athletic self-efficacy
beliefs hit more foul shots than did basketball players with weak self-efficacy
beliefs (Haney & Long, 1995). These researchers also found that the players
who hit more foul shots had greater increases in self-efficacy after they hit the
foul shots compared to those who hit fewer foul shots and did not experience
increases in self-efficacy. This is an example of how we gain self-efficacy through
performance experiences.
Self-Regulation
One of the major reasons that higher self-efficacy usually leads to better
performance and greater success is that self-efficacy is an important
component of self-regulation. Self-regulation is the complex process through
which you control your thoughts, emotions, and actions (Gross, 1998). It is
crucial to success and well-being in almost every area of your life. Every day,
you are exposed to situations where you might want to act or feel a certain way
that would be socially inappropriate or that might be unhealthy for you in the
long run. For example, when sitting in a boring class, you might want to take
out your phone and text your friends, take off your shoes and take a nap, or
perhaps scream because you are so bored. Self-regulation is the process that
you use to avoid such behaviors and instead sit quietly through class. Selfregulation takes a lot of effort, and it is often compared to a muscle that can
nobaproject.com - Self-Efficacy
1564
nobaproject.com - Self-Efficacy
1565
Collective Efficacy
Collective efficacy is a concept related to self-efficacy. Collective efficacy refers
to the shared beliefs among members of a group about the groups ability to
effectively perform the tasks needed to attain a valued goal (Bandura, 1997).
Groups and teams that have higher collective efficacies perform better than
groups and teams with lower collective efficacies (Marks, 1999). Collective
efficacy is especially important during tasks that require a lot of teamwork (KatzNavon & Erez, 2005). For example, when you have to do a group project that
involves each group member contributing a portion of the final project, your
groups performance will be much better if all members share the belief that
your group can perform the necessary tasks together. Collective efficacy plays
a role in romantic relationships. Married couples who strongly believe in their
ability to accomplish shared goals are happier than couples with weaker efficacy
beliefs (Kaplan & Maddux, 2002). Although collective efficacy is an important
part of how well a team or group performs, self-efficacy also plays a role in team
situations. For example, better decision-making self-efficacy predicts better
performance in team sports, such as baseball (Hepler & Feltz, 2012).
Conclusion
Self-efficacy refers to your beliefs about your ability to effectively perform the
tasks needed to attain a valued goal and it affects your daily life in many ways.
Self-efficacious adolescents perform better at school and self-efficacious adults
perform better at work. These individuals have happier romantic relationships
and work better in teams. People with strong self-efficacies have better health
than those with weak self-efficacies; they are more likely to engage in behaviors
that prevent health problems and actually increase their health. They are more
likely to begin and continue exercise, have safer sex, and eat better foods. Higher
self-efficacy is also useful for getting out of bad habits. People with strong selfnobaproject.com - Self-Efficacy
1566
efficacies are able to lose weight, quit smoking, and cut down on alcohol
consumption more successfully than can people with low self-efficacies. As
illustrated by the well-known childrens book The Little Engine That Could (Piper,
1930),telling yourself I think I can can be a powerful motivator and can increase
your chances for success.
Our own final words on self-efficacy also draw from childrens literature.
Many people receive a copy of Oh, The Places Youll Go! when they reach a major
milestone, such as graduating high school to go on to college or graduating
college to enter the workforce. Whether or not you or whoever gave you the
book knew it, Oh, The Places Youll Go! is all about self-efficacy. This book speaks
directly to readers by talking about all of the challenges they might face on their
journeys. Throughout the book, the narrator continues to assure readers that
they will be able to use their abilities to effectively handle these challenges. So,
we leave you with Dr. Seuss wise words: Youre on your own. And you know
what you know. And you are the guy wholl decide where to go. And will you
succeed? Yes! You will, indeed! 98 and 3/4 percent guaranteed.
nobaproject.com - Self-Efficacy
1567
Outside Resources
Video: Association for Psychological Science presents an interview with
Albert Bandura
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=-_U-pSZwHy8
Video: Self-efficacys role and sources
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=wrzzbaomLmc
Web: Professor Frank Pajares self-efficacy site.
http://www.uky.edu/~eushe2/Pajares/self-efficacy.html
Discussion Questions
1. Can you think of ways your own self-efficacy beliefs play a role in your daily
life? In which areas do you have strong self-efficacy? In which areas would
you like your self-efficacy to be a bit stronger? How could you increase your
self-efficacy in those areas?
2. Can you think of a time when a teacher, coach, or parent did something to
encourage your self-efficacy? What did he or she do and say? How did it
enhance your self-efficacy?
3. What are some ways that you can help strengthen the self-efficacies of the
people in your life?
4. Can you think of a time when collective efficacy played a role in your team
or group activities? What did you notice about being on a team or in a group
that had high collective efficacy? What about a team or group with low
collective efficacy?
Vocabulary
Collective efficacy
The shared beliefs among members of a group about the groups ability to
effectively perform the tasks needed to attain a valued goal.
Imaginal performances
When imagining yourself doing well increases self-efficacy.
Performance experiences
When past successes or failures lead to changes in self-efficacy.
Self-efficacy
The belief that you are able to effectively perform the tasks needed to attain a
valued goal.
Self-regulation
The complex process through which people control their thoughts, emotions,
and actions.
Self-report measure
A type of questionnaire in which participants answer questions whose answers
correspond to numerical values that can be added to create an overall index of
some construct.
Task-specific measures of self-efficacy
Measures that ask about self-efficacy beliefs for a particular task (e.g., athletic
self-efficacy, academic self-efficacy).
Verbal persuasion
When trusted people (friends, family, experts) influence your self-efficacy for
better or worse by either encouraging or discouraging you about your ability
to succeed.
Vicarious performances
When seeing other people succeed or fail leads to changes in self-efficacy.
Reference List
Bandura, A. (2006). Guide for constructing self-efficacy scales. In F. Pajares & T.
C. Urdan (Eds.), Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp. 307337). Charlotte,
NC: Information Age Publishing.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Worth
Publishers.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change.
Psychological Review, 84(2), 191215. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191
Bandura, A., & Locke, E. A. (2003). Negative self-efficacy and goal effects
revisited. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(1), 8799. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.1.87
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion:
Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 74(5), 12521265. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252
Bruton, A. M., Mellalieu, S. D., Shearer, D., Roderique-Davies, G., & Hall, R. (2013).
Performance accomplishment information as predictors of self-efficacy as
a function of skill level in amateur golf. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology,
25(2), 197208. doi:10.1080/10413200.2012.705802
Cervone, D., Jiwani, N., & Wood, R. (1991). Goal setting and the differential
influence of self-regulatory processes on complex decision-making
performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(2), 257266.
Chemers, M. M., Hu, L., & Garcia, B. F. (2001). Academic self-efficacy and first
year college student performance and adjustment. Journal of Educational
Social
and
Clinical
Psychology,
21(2),
157164.
doi:10.1521/
jscp.21.2.157.22513
Katz-Navon, T. Y., & Erez, M. (2005). When collective- and self-efficacy affect team
performance the role of task interdependence. Small Group Research, 36
(4), 437465. doi:10.1177/1046496405275233
Knudstrup, M., Segrest, S. L., & Hurley, A. E. (2003). The use of mental imagery
in the simulated employment interview situation. Journal of Managerial
Psychology, 18(6), 573591. doi:10.1108/02683940310494395
Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Larkin, K. C. (1986). Self-efficacy in the prediction of
academic performance and perceived career options. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 33(3), 265269. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.33.3.265
Luszczynska, A., Tryburcy, M., & Schwarzer, R. (2006). Improving fruit and
vegetable consumption: A self-efficacy intervention compared with a
combined self-efficacy and planning intervention. Health Education
Research, 22(5), 630638. doi:10.1093/her/cyl133
Maddux, J. E. (Ed.) (1995). Self efficacy, adaptation, and adjustment: Theory,
research, and application. New York: Plenum
Maddux, J. E., & Dawson, K. (2014). Promoting physical fitness in adulthood: A
focus on exercise. In T. Gullotta & M. Bloom (Eds), Encyclopedia of primary
prevention and health promotion (2nd ed., 821-827). New York: Kluwer/
Plenum.
Maddux, J. E., & Gosselin, J. T. (2011). Self-efficacy. In D. S. Dunn (Ed.), Oxford
bibliographies online: Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.
Retrived
from:
http://www.oxfordbibliographies.com/view/document/
obo-9780199828340/obo-9780199828340-0088.xml?rskey=twCabi&result=105
Maddux, J. E., & Volkmann, J. R. (2010). Self-efficacy and self-regulation. In R.
Hoyle (Ed.), Handbook of personality and self-regulation (315-321). New
York: Wiley-Blackwell.
Maisto, S. A., Connors, G. J., & Zywiak, W. H. (2000). Alcohol treatment changes
in coping skills, self-efficacy, and levels of alcohol use and related problems
1 year following treatment initiation. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 14
(3), 257266. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.14.3.257
Marcus, B. H., Selby, V. C., Niaura, R. S., & Rossi, J. S. (1992). Self-efficacy and the
stages of exercise behavior change. Research Quarterly for Exercise and
Sport, 63(1), 6066. doi:10.1080/02701367.1992.10607557
Marks, M. A. (1999). A test of the impact of collective efficacy in routine and
novel performance environments. Human Performance, 12(34), 295309.
doi:10.1080/08959289909539873
Multon, K. D., Brown, S. D., & Lent, R. W. (1991). Relation of self-efficacy beliefs
to academic outcomes: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 38(1), 3038. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.38.1.30
Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of
Educational Research, 66(4), 543578. doi:10.3102/00346543066004543.
Petty, R., & Brinol, P. (2010). Attitude change. In R. F. Baumeister & E. J. Finkel
(Eds.), Advanced social psychology: The state of the science. Oxford
University Press.
Piper, W. (1930). The little engine that could. New York: Platt & Munk.
Roach, J. B., Yadrick, M. K., Johnson, J. T., Boudreaux, L. J., Forsythe, W. A., & Billon,
W. (2003). Using self-efficacy to predict weight loss among young adults.
Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 103(10), 13571359.
Schunk, D. H. (1991). Self-efficacy and academic motivation. Educational
Psychologist, 26(34), 207231. doi:10.1080/00461520.1991.9653133
Schunk, D. H. (1990). Goal setting and self-efficacy during self-regulated
learning. Educational Psychologist, 25(1), 7186. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep2501_6
Schunk, D. H., & Miller, S. D. (2002). Self-efficacy and adolescents motivation.
In F. Pajares & T. C. Urdan (Eds.), Academic Motivation of Adolescents (pp.
2952). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1995). Generalized self-efficacy scale. Measures
in Health Psychology: A Users Portfolio. Causal and Control Beliefs, 1, 3537.
Seuss, Dr. (1990). Oh, the places youll go! New York: Random House.
Sheldon, J. P., & Eccles, J. S. (2005). Physical and psychological predictors of
perceived ability in adult male and female tennis players. Journal of Applied
Sport Psychology, 17(1), 4863. doi:10.1080/10413200590907568
Sherer, M., Maddux, J.E., Mercadante, B., Prentice Dunn, S., Jacobs, B., & Rogers,
R.W. (1982). The self efficacy scale: Construction and validation.
Psychological Reports, 51, 663671.
Stajkovic, A. D., & Luthans, F. (1998). Self-efficacy and work-related performance:
Strecher, V. J., DeVellis, B. M., Becker, M. H., & Rosenstock, I. M. (1986). The role
of self-efficacy in achieving health behavior change. Health Education &
Behavior, 13(1), 7392. doi:10.1177/109019818601300108
Vancouver, J. B., More, K. M., & Yoder, R. J. (2008). Self-efficacy and resource
allocation: Support for a discontinuous model. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 93(1), 35-47. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.1.35.
Vecchio, G. M., Gerbino, M., Pastorelli, C., Del Bove, G., & Caprara, G. V. (2007).
Multi-faceted self-efficacy beliefs as predictors of life satisfaction in late
adolescence. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(7), 18071818.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.05.018
Wentzel, K. R., Barry, C. M., & Caldwell, K. A. (2004). Friendships in middle school:
Influences on motivation and school adjustment. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 96(2), 195203. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.2.195
Social
and
jscp.1986.4.3.290
Clinical
Psychology,
4(3),
290301.
doi:10.1521/
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Self-Efficacy by James E Maddux and Evan Kleiman
is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Intellectual Abilities,
Interests, and Mastery
David Lubinski
Vanderbilt University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Psychologists interested in the study of human individuality have found that
accomplishments in education, the world of work, and creativity are a joint
function of talent, passion, and commitmentor how much effort and time
one is willing to invest in personal development when the opportunity is
provided. This chapter reviews models and measures that psychologists have
designed to assess intellect, interests, and energy for personal development.
The chapter begins with a model for organizing these three psychological
domains, which is useful for understanding talent development. This model is
not only helpful for understanding the many different ways that positive
development may unfold among people, but it is also useful for conceptualizing
personal development and ways of selecting opportunities in learning and work
settings that are more personally meaningful. Data supporting this model are
reviewed.
Learning Objectives
Discuss why the model of talent development offered in this chapter places
equal emphasis on assessing the person and assessing the environment.
List personal attributes other than interests and abilities that are important
to individual accomplishment.
1583
between such settings). Here, the dominant models of intellectual abilities and
educationaloccupational interests are assembled. Because this review will be
restricted to measures of individual differences that harbor real-world
significance, these two models are linked to corresponding features of learning
and work environments, ability requirements and incentive or reward
structures, which set standards for meeting expectations (performance) and
rewarding valued performance (compensation). Correspondence between
abilities and ability requirements constitutes satisfactoriness (competence),
whereas correspondence between an interests and reward structures
constitutes satisfaction (fulfillment). To the extent that satisfactoriness and
satisfaction co-occur, the individual is motivated to maintain contact with the
environment and the environment is motivated to retain the individual; if one
of these dimensions is dis-correspondent, the individual is motivated to leave
the environment or the environment is motivated to dismiss.
1584
Figure 1. The above is an adaptation of the Theory of Work Adjustment (Dawis & Lofquist,
1984), following Lubinski and Benbow (2000) to highlight its general role in talent development
over the life span. The radex scaling of cognitive abilities (upper left) and the RIASEC hexagon
of interests (lower left) outline personal attributes relevant to learning and work. The letters
within the cognitive ability arrangement denote different regions of concentration, whereas
their accompanying numbers increase as a function of complexity. Contained within the
RIASEC is a simplification of this hexagon. Following Prediger (1982), it amounts to a twodimensional structure of independent dimensions: people/things and data/ideas, which
underlie RIASEC. The dotted line running down the individual and environment sectors
underscores the idea that equal emphasis is placed on assessing personal attributes (abilities
and interests) and assessing the environment (abilities requirements and reward structure).
Correspondence between abilities and ability requirements constitutes satisfactoriness
whereas correspondence between interests and reinforce systems constitutes satisfaction.
Jointly, these two dimensions predict tenure or a longstanding relationship between the
individual and the environment.
1585
Cognitive Abilities
Over the past several decadesthe past 20 years in particulara remarkable
consensus has emerged that cognitive abilities are organized hierarchically
(Carroll, 1993). A general outline of this hierarchy is represented graphically by
a radex (Guttman, 1954), depicted in the upper left region of Figure 1. This
illustrates the reliable finding that cognitive ability assessments covary as a
function of their content or complexity (Corno, Cronbach et al., 2002; Lubinski
& Dawis, 1992; Snow & Lohman, 1989). Cognitive ability tests can be scaled in
nobaproject.com - Intellectual Abilities, Interests, and Mastery
1586
this space based on how highly they covary with one another. The more that
two tests share complexity and content, the more they covary and the closer
they are to one another as points within the radex. Test complexity is scaled
from the center of the radex (g) out, and, along lines emanating from the origin,
complexity decreases but test content remains the same. Test content is scaled
around the circular bands with equal distance from the center of the radex and,
progressing around these bands, the relative density of test content changes
from spatial/mechanical to verbal/linguistic to quantitative/numerical, but test
complexity remains constant. Therefore, test content varies within each band
(but complexity remains constant), whereas test complexity varies between
bands (but on lines from the origin to the periphery, content remains constant).
Because the extent to which tests covary is represented by how close together
they are within this space (Lubinski & Dawis, 1992; Snow & Lohman, 1989; Wai,
Lubinski, & Benbow, 2009), this model is helpful in organizing the many different
kinds of specific ability tests.
As Piaget astutely pointed out, Intelligence is what you use when you dont
know what to do, and this model affords an excellent overview of the content
and sophistication of thought applied to familiar and novel problem-solving
tasks. Mathematical, spatial, and verbal reasoning constitute the chief specific
abilities with implications for different choices and performance after those
choices in learning and work settings (Corno et al., 2002; Dawis, 1992;
Gottfredson, 2003; Lubinski, 2004; Wai et al., 2009). The content of measures
or tests of these specific abilities index individual differences in different
modalities of thought: reasoning with numbers, words, and figures or shapes.
Yet, despite this disparate content and focus, contrasting specific ability tests
are all positively correlated, because they all index an underlying general
property of intellectual thought.
nobaproject.com - Intellectual Abilities, Interests, and Mastery
1587
This general (common) dimension, identified over 100 years ago (Spearman,
1904) and corroborated by a massive quantity of subsequent research (Carroll,
1993; Jensen, 1998), is general mental ability, the general factor, or simply g
(Gottfredson, 1997). General mental ability represents the complexity/
sophistication of a persons intellectual repertoire (Jensen, 1998; Lubinski &
Dawis, 1992). The more complex a test is, regardless of its content, the better
a measure of g it is. Further, because g underlies all cognitive reasoning
processes, any test that assesses a specific ability is also, to some extent, a
measure of g (Lubinski, 2004). In school, work, and a variety of everyday life
circumstances, assessments of this general dimension covary more broadly
and deeper than any other measure of human individuality (Hunt, 2011; Jensen,
1998; Lubinski, 2000, 2004).
Measures of g manifest their life importance by going beyond educational
settings (where they covary with educational achievement assessments in the
.70.80 range), by playing a role in shaping phenomena within Freuds two
important life domains, arbeiten and lieben, working and loving (or, resource
acquisition and mating). Measures of g covary .20.60 with work performance
as a function of job complexity, .30.40 with income, and .20 with criminal
behavior, .40 with SES of origin, and .50.70 with achieved SES; assortative
mating correlations on g are around .50 (Jensen, 1998; Lubinski, 2004; Schmidt
& Hunter, 1998). Furthermore, Malcolm Gladwell (2008) notwithstanding, there
does not appear to be an ability threshold; that is, the idea that after a certain
point more ability does not matter. More ability does matter.
Although other determinants are certainly needed (interests, persistence,
opportunity), more ability does make a difference in learning, working, and
creating, even among the top 1% of ability, or IQ equivalents ranging from
approximately 137 to over 200 (see Figure 2). When appropriate assessment
and criterion measures are utilized to capture the breadth of ability and
accomplishment differences among the profoundly talented, individual
differences within the top 1% of ability are shown to matter a great deal. In the
nobaproject.com - Intellectual Abilities, Interests, and Mastery
1588
1589
Figure 2. Frey and Detterman (2004) have shown that the SAT composite is an excellent
measure of general intelligence for high ability samples; here, intellectually precocious youth
were assessed on this composite at age 13 and separated into quartiles (Lubinski, 2009). The
mean age 13 SAT composite scores for each quartile are displayed in parentheses along the
x-axis. Odds ratios comparing the likelihood of each outcome in the top (Q4) and bottom (Q1)
SAT quartiles are displayed at the end of every respective criterion line. An asterisk indicates
that the 95% confidence interval for the odds ratio did not include 1.0, meaning that the
likelihood of the outcome in Q4 was significantly greater than in Q1. These SAT assessments
by age 13 were conducted before the re-centering of the SAT in the mid-1990s (i.e., during the
1970s and early 1980s); at that time, cutting scores for the top 1 in 200 were SAT-M 500, SATV 430; for the top 1 in 10,000, cutting scores were SAT-M 700, SAT-V 630 by age 13.
1590
To reveal how general and specific abilities operate over the course of
development, Figure 3 contains data from over 400,000 high schools students
assessed between grades 9 through 12, and tracked for 11 years. Specifically,
Figure 3 graphs the general and specific ability profiles of students earning
terminal degrees in nine disciplines (Wai et al., 2009). Given that highly
congruent findings were observed for all four cohorts (g rades 9 through 12),
the cohorts were combined. High general intelligence and an intellectual
orientation dominated by high mathematical and spatial abilities, relative to
verbal ability, were salient characteristics of individuals who pursued advanced
education credentials in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics
(STEM). These participants occupy a region in the intellectual space defined by
the dimensions of ability level and ability pattern different from participants
who earn undergraduate and graduate degrees in other domains.
1591
Figure 3. Average z scores of participants on verbal, spatial, and mathematical ability for
terminal bachelors degrees, terminal masters degrees, and doctoral degrees are plotted by
field. The groups are plotted in rank order of their normative standing on g (verbal [V] + spatial
[S] + mathematical [M]) along the x-axis, and the line with the arrows from each field pointing
to it indicates on the continuous scale where they are in general mental ability in z-score units.
This figure is standardized in relation to all participants with complete ability data at the time
of initial testing. Respective Ns for each group (men + women) were as follows for bachelors,
masters, and doctorates, respectively: engineering (1,143, 339, 71), physical science (633, 182,
202), math/computer science (877, 266, 57), biological science (740, 182, 79), humanities
(3,226, 695, 82), social science (2,609, 484, 158), arts (615, 171 [masters only]), business (2,386,
191 [masters + doctorate]), and education (3,403, 1,505 [masters + doctorate]). For education
and business, masters degrees and doctorates were combined because the doctorate
samples for these groups were too small to obtain stability (N = 30). For the specific N for each
degree by sex that composed the major groupings, see Appendix A in Wai et al. (2009).
1592
Two major differences distinguish the STEM from the non-STEM educational
groups. First, students who ultimately secure educational credentials in STEM
domains are more capable than those earning degrees in other areas, especially
in nonverbal intellectual abilities. Within all educational domains, more
advanced degrees are associated with more general and specific abilities.
Second, for all three STEM educational groupings (and the advanced degrees
within these groupings), spatial ability > verbal abilitywhereas for all others,
ranging from education to biology, spatial ability < verbal ability (with business
being an exception). Young adolescents who subsequently secured advanced
educational credentials in STEM manifested a spatialverbal ability pattern
opposite that of those who ultimately earned educational credentials in other
areas. These same patterns play out in occupational arenas in predictable ways
(Kell, Lubinski, Benbow, & Steiger, 2013b). In the past decade, individual
differences within the top 1% of ability have revealed that these patterns
portend important outcomes for technical innovation and creativity, with
respect to both ability level (Lubinski, 2009; Park et al., 2008) and pattern (Kell
et al. 2013a, Kell et al., 2013b; Park et al., 2007). Level of general ability has
predictive validity for the magnitude of accomplishment (how extraordinary
they are), whereas ability pattern has predictive validity for the nature of
accomplishments (the domains they occur in).
Interests
Just because people can do something well doesnt mean they like doing it.
Psychological information on motivational differences (personal passions) is
needed to understand attractions and aversions, different ways to create a
meaningful life, and how differential development unfolds. Even people with
the same intellectual equipment vary widely in their motivational proclivities.
Paraphrasing Plato, different horses drive intellectual development down
different life paths. The lower left region of Figure 1 provides the dominant
nobaproject.com - Intellectual Abilities, Interests, and Mastery
1593
1594
skilled
educationalvocational
counselors
routinely
combine
Mastery
As all parents of more than one child know, there are huge individual differences
in the extent to which individuals embrace opportunities for positive
development. Seasoned faculty at top institutions for graduate training have
observed the same phenonemonamong highly select graduate students, task
commitment varies tremendously. Even among the intellectual elite, individual
differences in accomplishments stem from more than abilities, interests, and
opportunity; conative determinants are critical catalysts. Galton (1869) called
it zeal, Hull (1928) called it industriousness, and Webb (1915) called it will.
Such labels as grit or strivers are sometimes used to define resources that
people call upon to mobilize their abilities and interests over protracted
intervals. Conative factors are distinct from abilities and preferences, having
more to do with individual differences in energy or psychological tempo rather
than the content of what people can do or how rapidly they learn. Indeed,
characteristic
across
scientific
studies
of
expertise
and
world-class
1595
1596
1597
Figure 4: Hours worked per week (top) and hours willing to work per week in ideal job
(bottom) for top STEM Graduate Students (GS) and Profoundly Gifted Talent Search (TS)
participants now in their mid-thirties (from Lubinski et al., 2006).
Conclusion
Since Spearman (1904) advanced the idea of general intelligence, a steady
stream of systematic scientific knowledge has accrued in the psychological
study of human individuality. We have learned that the intellect is organized
hierarchically, that interests are multidimensional and only covary slightly with
abilities, and that individual differences are huge in terms of investing in
personal development. When these aspects of human psychological diversity
are combined with commensurate attention devoted to opportunities for
learning, work, and personal growth, a framework for understanding human
development begins to take shape. Because frameworks may be found that
emphasize only one set of these determinants, this essay closes with the
recommendationbased on the empirical evidenceto stress all three.
1598
Outside Resources
Book: Human Cognitive Abilities, by John Carroll constitutes a definitive
treatment of the nature and hierarchical organization of cognitive abilities,
based on a conceptual and empirical analysis of the past centurys factor
analytic research.
http://www.amazon.com/Human-Cognitive-Abilities-Factor-Analytic-Studies/
dp/0521382750/ref=sr_1_1?s=books&ie=UTF8&qid=1390944516&sr=1-1&keywords=
human+cognitive+abilities
Book: The g Factor, by Arthur Jensen, explicates the depth and breadth of
the central dimension running through all cognitive abilities, the summit of
Carrolls (1993) hierarchical organization: general intelligence (or g).
Revealed here is the practical and scientific significance for coming to terms
with a rich array of critical human outcomes found in schools, work, and
everyday life.
http://www.amazon.com/The-Factor-Evolution-Behavior-Intelligence/dp/0275961036
Discussion Questions
1. Why are abilities and interests insufficient for conceptualizing educational
and occupational development?
2. Why does the model of talent development discussed in this chapter place
equal emphasis on assessing the individual and assessing the environment.
3. What is the most widely agreed on empirical finding, among investigators
who study the development of truly outstanding careers?
4. Besides what you can do and what you like, what other factors are important
to consider when making choices about your personal development in
learning and work settings?
Vocabulary
g or general mental ability
The general factor common to all cognitive ability measures, a very general
mental capacity that, among other things, involves the ability to reason, plan,
solve problems, think abstractly, comprehend complex ideas, learn quickly, and
learn from experience. It is not merely book learning, a narrow academic skill,
or test-taking smarts. Rather, it reflects a broader and deeper capability for
comprehending our surroundingscatching on, making sense of things, or
figuring out what to do (Gottfredson, 1997, p. 13).
Satisfaction
Correspondence between an individuals needs or preferences and the rewards
offered by the environment.
Satisfactoriness
Correspondence between an individuals abilities and the ability requirements
of the environment.
Specific abilities
Cognitive abilities that contain an appreciable component of g or general ability,
but also contain a large component of a more content-focused talent such as
mathematical, spatial, or verbal ability; patterns of specific abilities channel
development down different paths as a function of an individuals relative
strengths and weaknesses.
Under-determined or misspecified causal models
Psychological frameworks that miss or neglect to include one or more of the
critical determinants of the phenomenon under analysis.
Reference List
Ackerman, P. L. (1996). A theory of adult intellectual development: Process,
personality, interests, and knowledge. Intelligence, 22, 227257.
Ackerman, P. L., & Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Intelligence, personality, and
interests: Evidence for overlapping traits. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 218
245.
Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (1997). Genetic influence on mental abilities, personality,
vocational interests, and work attitudes. International Review of Industrial
and Organizational Psychology, 12, 373395.
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Ceci, S. J., & Williams, W. M. (2011). Understanding current causes of womens
underrepresentation in science. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 108, 31573162.
Corno, L., Cronbach, L. J., et al. (Eds.) (2002). Remaking the concept of aptitude:
Extending the legacy of Richard E. Snow. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dawis, R. V. (1992). The individual differences tradition in counseling psychology.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 39, 719.
Dawis, R. V. (1991). Vocational interests, values and preferences. In M. D.
Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial & organizational
psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 833872). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologists Press.
194199.
Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2006). Study of mathematically precocious youth
after 35 years: Uncovering antecedents for the development of math
science expertise. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 316345.
Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2000). States of excellence. American Psychologist,
55, 137150.
Lubinski, D., & Dawis, R. V. (1992). Aptitudes, skills, and proficiencies. In M. D.
Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), The handbook of industrial/organizational
psychology (2nd ed., pp. 159). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Lubinski, D., & Humphreys, L. G. (1997). Incorporating general intelligence into
epidemiology and the social sciences. Intelligence, 24, 159201.
Park, G., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2008). Ability differences among people
who have commensurate degrees matter for scientific creativity.
Psychological Science, 19, 957961.
Park, G., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2007). Contrasting intellectual patterns
for creativity in the arts and sciences: Tracking intellectually precocious
youth over 25 years. Psychological Science, 18, 948952.
Prediger, D. J. (1982). Dimensions underlying Hollands hexagon: Missing link
between interests and occupations? Journal of Vocational Behavior, 21, 259
287.
Rounds, J. B., & Tracey, T. J. (1990). From trait-and-factor to personenvironment
fit counseling: Theory and process. In W. B. Walsh & S. H. Osipow (Eds.),
preference
dimensions
among
intellectually
Strong, E. K., Jr. (1943). Vocational interests for men and women. Stanford, CA:
Stanford University Press.
Su, R., Rounds, J., & Armstrong, P. I. (2009). Men and things, women and people:
A meta-analysis of sex differences in interests. Psychological Bulletin, 35,
859884.
Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Spatial ability for STEM domains:
Aligning over fifty years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its
importance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 817835.
Webb, R. M., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2007). Spatial ability: A neglected
dimension in talent searches for intellectually precocious youth. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 99, 397420.
Wilson, E. O. (1998). Consilience: The unity of knowledge. New York, NY: Knopf.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Intellectual Abilities, Interests, and Mastery by
David Lubinski is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercialShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Topic 9
Emotions and Motivation
Affective Neuroscience
Eddie Harmon-Jones & Cindy Harmon-Jones
University of New South Wales
nobaproject.com
Abstract
This chapter provides a brief overview of the neuroscience of emotion. It
integrates findings from human and animal research to describe the brain
networks and associated neurotransmitters involved in basic affective systems.
Learning Objectives
Name five emotional systems and their associated neural structures and
neurotransmitters.
1614
than the differences. We often use the term organism to refer to the individual
who is experiencing an emotion or showing evidence of particular neural
activations. An organism could be a rat, a monkey, or a human.
Across species, emotional responses are organized around the organisms
survival and reproductive needs. Emotions influence perception, cognition, and
behavior to help organisms survive and thrive (Farb, Chapman, & Anderson,
2013). Networks of structures in the brain respond to different needs, with some
overlap between different emotions. Specific emotions are not located in a
single structure of the brain. Instead, emotional responses involve networks of
activation, with many parts of the brain activated during any emotional process.
In fact, the brain circuits involved in emotional reactions include nearly the
entire brain (Berridge & Kringelbach, 2013). Brain circuits located deep within
the brain below the cerebral cortex are primarily responsible for generating
basic emotions (Berridge & Kringelbach, 2013; Panksepp & Biven, 2012). In the
past, research attention was focused on specific brain structures that will be
reviewed here, but future research may find that additional areas of the brain
are also important in these processes.
1615
Basic Emotions
Desire: The neural systems of reward seeking
One of the most important affective neuronal systems relates to feelings of
desire, or the appetite for rewards. Researchers refer to these appetitive
processes using terms such as wanting (Berridge & Kringelbach, 2008),
seeking (Panksepp & Biven, 2012), or behavioural activation sensitivity (Gray,
1987). When the appetitive system is aroused, the organism shows enthusiasm,
nobaproject.com - Affective Neuroscience
1616
interest, and curiosity. These neural circuits motivate the animal to move
through its environment in search of rewards such as appetizing foods,
attractive sex partners, and other pleasurable stimuli. When the appetitive
system is underaroused, the organism appears depressed and helpless.
Much evidence for the structures involved in this system comes from animal
research using direct brain stimulation. When an electrode is implanted in the
lateral hypothalamus or in cortical or mesencephalic regions to which the
hypothalamus is connected, animals will press a lever to deliver electrical
stimulation, suggesting that they find the stimulation pleasurable. The regions
in the desire system also include the amygdala, nucleus accumbens, and frontal
cortex (Panksepp & Biven, 2012). The neurotransmitter dopamine, produced
in the mesolimbic and mesocortical dopamine circuits, activates these regions.
It creates a sense of excitement, meaningfulness, and anticipation. These
structures are also sensitive to drugs such as cocaine and amphetamines,
chemicals that have similar effects to dopamine (Panksepp & Biven, 2012).
Research in both humans and nonhuman animals shows that the left frontal
cortex (compared to the right frontal cortex) is more active during appetitive
emotions such as desire and interest. Researchers first noted that persons who
had suffered damage to the left frontal cortex developed depression, whereas
those with damage to the right frontal cortex developed mania (Goldstein,
1939). The relationship between left frontal activation and approach-related
emotions has been confirmed in healthy individuals using EEG and fMRI
(Berkman & Lieberman, 2010). For example, increased left frontal activation
occurs in 2- to 3-day-old infants when sucrose is placed on their tongues (Fox
& Davidson, 1986), and in hungry adults as they view pictures of desirable
desserts (Gable & Harmon-Jones, 2008). In addition, greater left frontal activity
in appetitive situations has been found to relate to dopamine (Wacker, Mueller,
Pizzagalli, Hennig, & Stemmler, 2013).
1617
1618
1619
and fear are near one another, but separate (Panksepp & Biven, 2012). They
extend from the medial amygdala, through specific parts of the hypothalamus,
and into the periaqueductal gray of the midbrain. The anger circuits are linked
to the appetitive circuits, such that lack of an anticipated reward can provoke
rage. In addition, when humans are angered, they show increased left frontal
cortical activation, supporting the idea that anger is an approach-related
emotion (Harmon-Jones et al., 2013). The neurotransmitters involved in rage
are not yet well understood, but Substance P may play an important role
(Panksepp & Biven, 2012). Other neurochemicals that may be involved in anger
include testosterone (Peterson & Harmon-Jones, 2012) and argininevasopressin (Heinrichs, von Dawans, & Domes, 2009). Several chemicals inhibit
the rage system, including opioids and high doses of antipsychotics, such as
chlorpromazine (Panksepp & Biven, 2012).
1620
1621
1622
regions. For example, extensive research has examined the involvement of the
amygdala in fear, but research has also shown that the amygdala is active during
uncertainty (Whalen, 1998) as well as positive emotions (Anderson et al., 2003;
Schulkin, 1990).
Conclusion
Research in affective neuroscience has contributed to knowledge regarding
emotional, motivational, and behavioral processes. The study of the basic
emotional systems of nonhuman animals provides information about the
organization and development of more complex human emotions. Although
much still remains to be discovered, current findings in affective neuroscience
have already influenced our understanding of drug use and abuse,
psychological disorders such as panic disorder, and complex human emotions
such as desire and enjoyment, grief and love.
1623
Outside Resources
Video: A 1-hour interview with Jaak Panksepp, the father of affective
neuroscience
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=u4ICY6-7hJo
Video: A 15-minute interview with Kent Berridge on pleasure in the brain
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=51rGE1DgIo0
Video: A 5-minute interview with Joseph LeDoux on the amygdala and fear
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=fDD5wvFMH6U
Web: Brain anatomy interactive 3D model
http://www.pbs.org/wnet/brain/3d/index.html
Discussion Questions
1. The neural circuits of liking are different from the circuits of wanting.
How might this relate to the problems people encounter when they diet,
fight addictions, or try to change other habits?
2. The structures and neurotransmitters that produce pleasure during social
contact also produce panic and grief when organisms are deprived of social
contact. How does this contribute to an understanding of love?
3. Research shows that stressful environments increase the area of the
nucleus accumbens that is sensitive to fear, whereas preferred
environments increase the area that is sensitive to rewards. How might
these changes be adaptive?
Vocabulary
Affect
An emotional process; includes moods, subjective feelings, and discrete
emotions.
Amygdala
Two almond-shaped structures located in the medial temporal lobes of the
brain.
Hypothalamus
A brain structure located below the thalamus and above the brain stem.
Neuroscience
The study of the nervous system.
Nucleus accumbens
A region of the basal forebrain located in front of the preoptic region.
Periaqueductal gray
The gray matter in the midbrain near the cerebral aqueduct.
Preoptic region
A part of the anterior hypothalamus.
Stria terminalis
A band of fibers that runs along the top surface of the thalamus.
Thalamus
A structure in the midline of the brain located between the midbrain and the
cerebral cortex.
Visual cortex
The part of the brain that processes visual information, located in the back of
the brain.
Reference List
Anderson, A. K., Christoff, K., Stappen, I., Panitz, D., Ghahremani, D. G., Glover,
G., . . . Sobel, N. (2003). Dissociated neural representations of intensity and
valence in human olfaction. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 196202.
Berkman, E. T., & Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Approaching the bad and avoiding
the good: Lateral prefrontal cortical asymmetry distinguishes between
action and valence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(9), 19701979.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21317
Berridge, K. C., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2013). Neuroscience of affect: brain
mechanisms of pleasure and displeasure. Current Opinion in Neurobiology,
23, 294303. doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.017
Berridge, K. C., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2008). Affective neuroscience of pleasure:
Reward in humans and animals. Psychopharmacology, 199, 457480. doi:
10.1007/s00213-008-1099-6
Blanchard, D. C., & Blanchard, R. J. (2003). What can animal aggression research
tell us about human aggression? Hormones and Behavior, 44, 171177.
Farb, N.A.S., Chapman, H. A., & Anderson, A. K. (2013). Emotions: Form follows
function. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 393398. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.015
Fox, N. A., & Davidson, R. J. (1986). Taste-elicited changes in facial signs of
emotion and the asymmetry of brain electrical activity in human newborns.
Neuropsychologia, 24, 417422.
situationally-induced
anger
relates
to
situationally-induced
Functions of Emotions
Hyisung Hwang & David Matsumoto
San Francisco State University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Emotions play a crucial role in our lives because they have important functions.
This chapter describes those functions, dividing the discussion into three areas:
the intrapersonal, the interpersonal, and the social and cultural functions of
emotions. The section on the intrapersonal functions of emotion describes the
roles that emotions play within each of us individually; the section on the
interpersonal functions of emotion describes the meanings of emotions to our
relationships with others; and the section on the social and cultural functions
of emotion describes the roles and meanings that emotions have to the
maintenance and effective functioning of our societies and cultures at large. All
in all we will see that emotions are a crucially important aspect of our
psychological composition, having meaning and function to each of us
individually, to our relationships with others in groups, and to our societies as
a whole.
Learning Objectives
Give examples of the role and function of emotion in each of the three areas
described.
Introduction
It is impossible to imagine life without emotion. We treasure our feelingsthe
joy at a ball game, the pleasure of the touch of a loved one, or the fun with
friends on a night out. Even negative emotions are important, such as the
sadness when a loved one dies, the anger when violated, the fear that
overcomes us in a scary or unknown situation, or the guilt or shame toward
others when our sins are made public. Emotions color life experiences and give
those experiences meaning and flavor.
In fact, emotions play many important roles in peoples lives and have been
the topic of scientific inquiry in psychology for well over a century (Cannon,
1927; Darwin, 1872; James, 1890). This chapter explores why we have emotions
and why they are important. Doing so requires us to understand the function
of emotions, and this chapter does so below by dividing the discussion into
three sections. The first concerns the intrapersonal functions of emotion, which
refer to the role that emotions play within each of us individually. The second
concerns the interpersonal functions of emotion, which refer to the role
emotions play between individuals within a group. The third concerns the social
and cultural functions of emotion, which refer to the role that emotions play
in the maintenance of social order within a society. All in all, we will see that
emotions inform us of who we are, what our relationships with others are like,
and how to behave in social interactions. Emotions give meaning to events;
without emotions, those events would be mere facts. Emotions help coordinate
interpersonal relationships. And emotions play an important role in the cultural
functioning of keeping human societies together.
1637
1638
order to prevent the chaos of competing systems operating at the same time,
allowing for coordinated responses to environmental stimuli (Levenson, 1999).
For instance, when we are afraid, our bodies shut down temporarily unneeded
digestive processes, resulting in saliva reduction (a dry mouth); blood flows
disproportionately to the lower half of the body; the visual field expands; and
air is breathed in, all preparing the body to flee. Emotions initiate a system of
components that includes subjective experience, expressive behaviors,
physiological reactions, action tendencies, and cognition, all for the purposes
of specific actions; the term emotion is, in reality, a metaphor for these
reactions.
One common misunderstanding many people have when thinking about
emotions, however, is the belief that emotions must always directly produce
action. This is not true. Emotion certainly prepares the body for action; but
whether people actually engage in action is dependent on many factors, such
as the context within which the emotion has occurred, the target of the emotion,
the perceived consequences of ones actions, previous experiences, and so forth
(Baumeister, Vohs, DeWall, & Zhang, 2007; Matsumoto & Wilson, 2008). Thus,
emotions are just one of many determinants of behavior, albeit an important
one.
1639
1640
this communicative signal value, they help solve social problems by evoking
responses from others, by signaling the nature of interpersonal relationships,
and by providing incentives for desired social behavior (Keltner, 2003).
1641
research, married couples visited a laboratory after having not seen each other
for 24 hours, and then engaged in intimate conversations about daily events
or issues of conflict. Discrete expressions of contempt, especially by the men,
and disgust, especially by the women, predicted later marital dissatisfaction
and even divorce.
1642
produced the greatest inhibition, followed by disgust, with neutral the least.
This study was later replicated (Hertenstein & Campos, 2004) using joy and
disgust expressions, altering the method so that the infants were not allowed
to touch the toy (compared with a distractor object) until one hour after
exposure to the expression. At 14 months of age, significantly more infants
touched the toy when they saw joyful expressions, but fewer touched the toy
when the infants saw disgust.
1643
enormous potential for social chaos, which can easily occur if individuals are
not coordinated well and relationships not organized systematically.
One of the important functions of culture is to provide this necessary
coordination and organization. Doing so allows individuals and groups to
negotiate the social complexity of human social life, thereby maintaining social
order and preventing social chaos. Culture does this by providing a meaning
and information system to its members, which is shared by a group and
transmitted across generations, that allows the group to meet basic needs of
survival, pursue happiness and well-being, and derive meaning from life
(Matsumoto & Juang, 2013). Culture is what allowed the banana from southeast
Asia to appear on your table.
1644
1645
1646
1647
Outside Resources
Web: See how well you can read other peoples facial expressions of emotion
http://www.humintell.com/free-demos/
Discussion Questions
1. When emotions occur, why do they simultaneously activate certain
physiological and psychological systems in the body and deactivate others?
2. Why is it difficult for people to act rationally and think happy thoughts when
they are angry? Conversely, why is it difficult to remember sad memories
or have sad thoughts when people are happy?
3. Youre walking down a deserted street when you come across a stranger
who looks scared. What would you say? What would you do? Why?
4. Youre walking down a deserted street when you come across a stranger
who looks angry. What would you say? What would you do? Why?
5. Think about the messages children receive from their environment (such
as from parents, mass media, the Internet, Hollywood movies, billboards,
and storybooks). In what ways do these messages influence the kinds of
emotions that children should and should not feel?
Vocabulary
Cultural display rules
These are rules that are learned early in life that specify the management and
modification of emotional expressions according to social circumstances.
Cultural display rules can work in a number of different ways. For example, they
can require individuals to express emotions as is (i.e., as they feel them), to
exaggerate their expressions to show more than what is actually felt, to tone
down their expressions to show less than what is actually felt, to conceal their
feelings by expressing something else, or to show nothing at all.
Interpersonal
This refers to the relationship or interaction between two or more individuals
in a group. Thus, the interpersonal functions of emotion refer to the effects of
ones emotion on others, or to the relationship between oneself and others.
Intrapersonal
This refers to what occurs within oneself. Thus, the intrapersonal functions of
emotion refer to the effects of emotion to individuals that occur physically inside
their bodies and psychologically inside their minds.
Social referencing
This refers to the process whereby individuals look for information from others
to clarify a situation, and then use that information to act. Thus, individuals will
often use the emotional expressions of others as a source of information to
make decisions about their own behavior.
Reference List
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., DeWall, N., & Zhang, L. (2007). How emotion shapes
behavior: Feedback, anticipation, and reflection, rather than direct
causation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(2), 167203.
Bradshaw, D. (1986). Immediate and prolonged effectiveness of negative
emotion expressions in inhibiting infants' actions (Unpublished doctoral
dissertation). Berkeley, CA: University of California, Berkeley.
Cannon, W. B. (1927). The JamesLange theory of emotions: A critical
examination and an alternative theory. American Journal of Psychology, 39,
106124.
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). Evolutionary psychology and the emotions. In
M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp.
91115). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of emotion in man and animals. New York,
NY: Oxford University Press.
Dimberg, U., & Ohman, A. (1996). Behold the wrath: Psychophysiological
responses to facial stimuli. Motivation & Emotion, 20(2), 149182.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Miller, P. A., Fultz, J., Shell, R., Mathy, R. M., & Reno,
R. R. (1989). Relation of sympathy and distress to prosocial behavior: A
multimethod study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 5566.
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity
of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 205
235.
Esteves, F., Dimberg, U., & Ohman, A. (1994). Automatically elicited fear:
Conditioned skin conductance responses to masked facial expressions.
Cognition and Emotion, 8(5), 393413.
Friesen, W. V. (1972). Cultural differences in facial expressions in a social
situation: An experimental test of the concept of display rules (Unpublished
doctoral dissertation). San Francisco, CA: University of California, San
Francisco.
Gottman, J. M., Levenson, R. W., & Woodin, E. (2001). Facial expressions during
marital conflict. Journal of Family Communication, 1, 3757.
Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1992). Marital processes predictive of later
dissolution: Behavior, physiology, and health. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 63(2), 221223.
Hertenstein, M. J., & Campos, J. J. (2004). The retention effects of an adult's
emotional displays on infant behavior. Child Development, 75(2), 595613.
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: Holt.
Keltner, D. (2003). Expression and the course of life: Studies of emotion,
personality, and psychopathology from a social-functional perspective. In
P. Ekman, J. Campos, R. J. Davidson, & F.B.M. De Waal (Eds.), Emotions inside
out: 130 years after Darwin's The expression of the emotions in man and
animals (Vol. 1000, pp. 222243). New York, NY: New York Academy of
Sciences.
Klinnert, M. D., Campos, J. J., & Sorce, J. F. (1983). Emotions as behavior regulators:
Social referencing in infancy. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion:
Theory, research, and experience (pp. 5786). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Levenson, R. W. (1999). The intrapersonal functions of emotion. Cognition and
Emotion, 13(5), 481504.
Marsh, A. A., Ambady, N., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). The effects of fear and anger
facial expressions on approach- and avoidance-related behaviors. Emotion,
5(1), 119124.
Matsumoto, D. (2001). Culture and emotion. In D. Matsumoto (Ed.), The
handbook of culture and psychology (pp. 171194). New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Matsumoto, D., Hirayama, S., & LeRoux, J. A. (2006). Psychological skills related
to adjustment. In P.T.P. Wong & L.C.J. Wong (Eds.), Handbook of multicultural
perspectives on stress and coping (pp. 387405). New York, NY: Springer.
Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., Nakagawa, S., Alexandre, J., Altarriba, J., Anguas-Wong,
A. M., et al. (2008). Culture, emotion regulation, and adjustment. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 94(6), 925937.
Matsumoto, D., & Hwang, H. C. (2013). Assessing cross-cultural competence: A
review of available tests. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 44(6), 849
873.
Matsumoto, D., & Juang, L. (2013). Culture and psychology (5th ed.). Belmont,
CA: Cengage.
U.
(Ed.).
(2009).
Cultural
transmission:
Developmental,
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Functions of Emotions by Hyisung Hwang and
David Matsumoto is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercialShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
Emotions dont just feel good or bad, they also contribute crucially to peoples
well-being and health. In general, experiencing positive emotions is good for
us, whereas experiencing negative emotions is bad for us. However, recent
research on emotions and well-being suggests this simple conclusion is
incomplete and sometimes even wrong. Taking a closer look at this research,
the present chapter provides a more complex relationship between emotion
and well-being. At least three aspects of the emotional experience appear to
affect how a given emotion is linked with well-being: the intensity of the emotion
experienced, the fluctuation of the emotion experienced, and the context in
which the emotion is experienced. While it is generally good to experience more
positive emotion and less negative emotion, this is not always the guide to the
good life.
Learning Objectives
1661
texts of thinkers such as Charles Darwin (1872) and Aristotle (1999). However,
modern psychological research has provided empirical evidence that feelings
are not just inconsequential byproducts. Rather, each emotion experience,
however fleeting, has effects on cognition, behavior, and the people around us.
For example, feeling happy is not only pleasant, but is also useful to feel when
in social situations because it helps us be friendly and collaborative, thus
promoting our positive relationships. Over time, the argument goes, these
effects add up to have tangible effects on peoples well-being (good mental and
physical health).
A variety of research has been inspired by the notion that our emotions are
involved in, and maybe even causally contribute to, our well-being. This research
has shown that people who experience more frequent positive emotions and
less frequent negative emotions have higher well-being (e.g., Fredrickson, 1998;
Lyubomirksy, King, & Diener, 2005), including increased life satisfaction (Diener,
Sandvik, & Pavot, 1991), increased physical health (Tugade, Fredrickson, &
Barrett, 2004; Veenhoven, 2008), greater resilience to stress (Folkman &
Moskowitz, 2000; Tugade & Fredrickson, 2004), better social connection with
others (Fredrickson, 1998), and even longer lives (Veenhoven, 2008). Notably,
the effect of positive emotion on longevity is about as powerful as the effect of
smoking! Perhaps most importantly, some research directly supports that
emotional experiences cause these various outcomes rather than being just a
consequence of them (Fredrickson, Cohn, Coffey, Pek, & Finkel, 2008;
Lyubomirsky et al., 2005).
At this point, you might be tempted to conclude that you should always
strive to experience as much positive emotion and as little negative emotion
as possible. However, recent research suggests that this conclusion may be
premature. This is because this conclusion neglects three central aspects of the
emotion experience. First, it neglects the intensity of the emotion: Positive and
negative emotions might not have the same effect on well-being at all
intensities. Second, it neglects how emotions fluctuate over time: Stable
nobaproject.com - Emotion Experience and Well-Being
1662
emotion experiences might have quite different effects from experiences that
change a lot. Third, it neglects the context in which the emotion is experienced:
The context in which we experience an emotion might profoundly affect
whether the emotion is good or bad for us. So, to address the question Which
emotions should we feel? we must answer, It depends! We next consider
each of the three aspects of feelings, and how they influence the link between
feelings and well-being.
So, which emotions are the best ones to feel? Take a moment to think
about how you might answer this question. At first glance, the answer might
seem obvious. Of course, we should experience as much positive emotion and
as little negative emotion as possible! Why? Because it is pleasant to experience
positive emotions and it is unpleasant to experience negative emotions (Russell
& Barrett, 1999). The conclusion that positive feelings are good and negative
feelings are bad might seem so obvious as not to even warrant the question,
much less bona fide psychological research. In fact, the very labels of positive
and negative imply the answer to this question. However, for the purposes of
this chapter, it may be helpful to think of positive and negative as descriptive
terms used to discuss two different types of experiences, rather than a true
value judgment. Thus, whether positive or negative emotions are good or bad
for us is an empirical question.
As it turns out, this empirical question has been on the minds of theorists
and researchers for many years. Such psychologists as Alice Isen, Charles
Carver, Michael Scheier, and, more recently, Barbara Fredrickson, Dacher
Keltner, Sonja Lyubomirsky, and others began asking whether the effects of
feelings could go beyond the obvious momentary pleasure or displeasure. In
other words, can emotions do more for us than simply make us feel good or
bad? This is not necessarily a new question; variants of it have appeared in the
texts of thinkers such as Charles Darwin (1872) and Aristotle (1999). However,
modern psychological research has provided empirical evidence that feelings
are not just inconsequential byproducts. Rather, each emotion experience,
nobaproject.com - Emotion Experience and Well-Being
1663
however fleeting, has effects on cognition, behavior, and the people around us.
For example, feeling happy is not only pleasant, but is also useful to feel when
in social situations because it helps us be friendly and collaborative, thus
promoting our positive relationships. Over time, the argument goes, these
effects add up to have tangible effects on peoples well-being (good mental and
physical health).
A variety of research has been inspired by the notion that our emotions are
involved in, and maybe even causally contribute to, our well-being. This research
has shown that people who experience more frequent positive emotions and
less frequent negative emotions have higher well-being (e.g., Fredrickson, 1998;
Lyubomirksy, King, & Diener, 2005), including increased life satisfaction (Diener,
Sandvik, & Pavot, 1991), increased physical health (Tugade, Fredrickson, &
Barrett, 2004; Veenhoven, 2008), greater resilience to stress (Folkman &
Moskowitz, 2000; Tugade & Fredrickson, 2004), better social connection with
others (Fredrickson, 1998), and even longer lives (Veenhoven, 2008). Notably,
the effect of positive emotion on longevity is about as powerful as the effect of
smoking! Perhaps most importantly, some research directly supports that
emotional experiences cause these various outcomes rather than being just a
consequence of them (Fredrickson, Cohn, Coffey, Pek, & Finkel, 2008;
Lyubomirsky et al., 2005).
At this point, you might be tempted to conclude that you should always
strive to experience as much positive emotion and as little negative emotion
as possible. However, recent research suggests that this conclusion may be
premature. This is because this conclusion neglects three central aspects of the
emotion experience. First, it neglects the intensity of the emotion: Positive and
negative emotions might not have the same effect on well-being at all
intensities. Second, it neglects how emotions fluctuate over time: Stable
emotion experiences might have quite different effects from experiences that
change a lot. Third, it neglects the context in which the emotion is experienced:
The context in which we experience an emotion might profoundly affect
nobaproject.com - Emotion Experience and Well-Being
1664
whether the emotion is good or bad for us. So, to address the question Which
emotions should we feel? we must answer, It depends! We next consider
each of the three aspects of feelings, and how they influence the link between
feelings and well-being.
1665
1666
1999). These associations tend to hold even when controlling for average levels
of positive or negative emotion, which means that beyond the overall intensity
of positive or negative emotion, the fluctuation of ones emotions across time
is associated with well-being. While it is not entirely clear why fluctuations are
linked to worse well-being, one explanation is that strong fluctuations are
indicative of emotional instability (Kuppens, Oravecz, & Tuerlinckx, 2010).
Of course, this should not be taken to mean that we should rigidly feel the
exact same way every minute of every day, regardless of context. After all,
psychological flexibilityor the ability to adapt to changing situational demands
and experience emotions accordinglyhas generally demonstrated beneficial
links with well-being (Bonanno, Papa, Lalande, Westphal, & Coifman, 2004;
Kashdan, & Rottenberg, 2010). The question remains, however: what exact
amount of emotional fluctuation constitutes unhealthy instability and what
amount of emotional fluctuation constitutes healthy flexibility.
Again, then, we must qualify the conclusion that it is always better to
experience more positive emotions and less negative emotions. The degree to
which emotions fluctuate across time plays an important role. Overall, relative
stability (but not rigidity) in emotion experience appears to be optimal for wellbeing.
1667
experienced.
1668
1669
1670
Other emotions
Up until now, we have treated emotional experiences as though people can
only experience one emotion at a time. However, it should be kept in mind that
positive and negative emotions are not simply the opposite of one another.
Instead, they tend to be independent of one another, which means that a person
can feel positive and negative emotions at the same time (Larsen, McGraw,
Mellers, & Cacioppo, 2004). For example, how does it feel to win a prize when
you expected a greater prize? Given what might have been, situations like this
can elicit both happiness and sadness. Or, take schadenfreude (a German
term for deriving pleasure from someone elses misfortune), or aviman (an
Indian term for prideful, loving anger), or nostaligia (an English term for
affectionate sadness about something from the past): these terms capture the
notion that people can feel both positively and negatively within the same
emotional experience. And as it turns out, the other emotions that someone
feels (e.g., sadness) during the experience of an emotion (e.g., happiness)
influence whether that emotion experience has a positive or negative effect on
well-being.
Importantly, the extent to which someone experiences different emotions
at the same timeor mixed emotionsmay be beneficial for their well-being.
Early support for this theory was provided by a study of bereaved spouses. In
the study, participants were asked to talk about their recently deceased spouse,
which undoubtedly elicited strong negative emotions. However, some
nobaproject.com - Emotion Experience and Well-Being
1671
Conclusion
Are emotions just fleeting experiences with no consequence beyond our
momentary comfort or discomfort? A variety of research answers a firm no
emotions are integral predictors of our well-being. This chapter examined how,
exactly, emotion experience might be linked to well-being. The obvious answer
to this question is: of course, experiencing as much positive emotions and as
little negative emotions as possible is good for us. But although this is true in
general, recent research suggests that this obvious answer is incomplete and
sometimes even wrong. As philosopher Robert Solomon said, Living well is not
just maximizing the good feelings and minimizing the bad. () A happy life is
not necessarily filled with happy moments (2007, p. 86).
1672
Outside Resources
Journal: If you are interested in direct access to research on emotion, take a
look at the journal Emotion
http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/emo/index.aspx
Video: Check out videos of expert emotion researchers discussing their work
http://www.youtube.com/playlist?list=PLh9mgdi4rNew731mjIZn43G_Y5otqKzJA
Video: See psychologist Daniel Gilbert and other experts discussing current
research on emotion in the PBS series This Emotional Life
http://video.pbs.org/program/this-emotional-life/
Discussion Questions
1. Much research confirms the relative benefits of positive emotions and
relative costs of negative emotions. Could positive emotions be detrimental,
or could negative emotions be beneficial? Why or why not?
2. We described some contexts that influence the effects of emotional
experiences on well-being. What other contexts might influence the links
between emotions and well-being? Age? Gender? Culture? How so?
3. How could you design an experiment that tests(A) When and why it is
beneficial to feel a negative emotion such as sadness? (B) How is the
coherence of emotion behavior and emotion experience linked to wellbeing? (C) How likely a person is to feel mixed (as compared to simple)
emotions?
Vocabulary
Emotion
An experiential, physiological, and behavioral response to a personally
meaningful stimulus.
Emotion coherence
The degree to which emotional responses (subjective experience, behavior,
physiology, etc.) converge with one another.
Emotion fluctuation
The degree to which emotions vary or change in intensity over time.
Well-being
The experience of mental and physical health and the absence of disorder.
Reference List
Aristotle. (1999). Nicomachean ethics (M. Ostwald, Trans.). Upper Saddle River,
NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Westphal M., & Coifman, K. (2004). The
importance of being flexible: The ability to both enhance and suppress
emotional expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psychological
Science, 15, 482487.
Bonanno, G. A., & Keltner, D. (1997). Facial expressions of emotion and the
course of conjugal bereavement. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 126
137.
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, O. J. (2000). Measuring emotion: Behavior, feeling, and
physiology. In R. D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion
(pp. 242276). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Cyders, M. A., & Smith, G. T. (2008). Emotion-based dispositions to rash action:
Positive and negative urgency. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 807828.
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of emotions in man and animals. New York,
NY: Philosophical Library.
Davidson, R. J. (1998). Affective style and affective disorders: Perspectives from
affective neuroscience. Cognition & Emotion, 12, 307330.
Diener, E., Sandvik, E., & Pavot, W. (1991). Happiness is the frequency, not the
intensity, of positive versus negative affect. Subjective well-being: An
interdisciplinary perspective, 21, 119139.
Eid, M., & Diener, E. (1999). Intraindividual variability in affect: Reliability, validity,
and personality correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76,
662676.
Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of
emotion. In J. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 207282).
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
English, T., & John, O. P. (2013). Understanding the social effects of emotion
regulation: The mediating role of authenticity for individual differences in
suppression. Emotion, 13, 314.
Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2000). Positive affect and the other side of coping.
American Psychologist, 55, 647654.
Ford, B. Q., & Tamir, M. (2012). When getting angry is smart: Emotional
preferences and emotional intelligence. Emotion, 12, 685.
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General
Psychology, 2, 300319.
Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M. (2008). Open
hearts build lives: Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness
meditation, build consequential personal resources. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 95, 1045-1062.
Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M. (2008). Open
hearts build lives: Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness
meditation, build consequential personal resources. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 95, 1045-1062.
Fridlund, A. J., Ekman, P., & Oster, H. (1987). Facial expressions of emotion. In
A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Nonverbal behavior and communication
(2nd ed., pp. 143223). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Grant, A. M., & Schwartz, B. (2011). Too much of a good thing: The challenge
and opportunity of the inverted U. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
6, 6176.
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation
processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of
personality and social psychology, 85, 348-362.
Gruber, J. (2011). When feeling good can be bad: Positive emotion persistence
(PEP) in bipolar disorder. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20,
217221.
Gruber, J., Culver, J. L., Johnson, S. L., Nam, J., Keller, K. L., & Ketter, T. K. (2009).
Do positive emotions predict symptomatic change in bipolar disorder?
Bipolar Disorders, 11, 330336.
Gruber, J., Kogan, A., Quoidbach, J., & Mauss, I. B. (2013). Happiness is best kept
stable: Positive emotion variability is associated with poorer psychological
health. Emotion, 13, 16.
Hershfield, H. E., Scheibe, S., Sims, T. L., & Carstensen, L. L. (2013). When feeling
bad can be good: Mixed emotions benefit physical health across adulthood.
Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 5461.
Johnson, S. L. (2005). Mania and dysregulation in goal pursuit. Clinical
Psychology Review, 25, 241262.
Marsh, A. A., Finger, E. C., Mitchell, D. G. V., Reid, M. E., Sims, C., Kosson, D. S., ...
Blair, R. J. R. (2008). Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in
children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive
behavior disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 712720.
Martin, L. R., Friedman, H. S., Tucker, J. S., Tomlinson-Keasey, C., Criqui, M. H., &
Schwartz, J. E. (2002). A life course perspective on childhood cheerfulness
and its relation to mortality risk. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
28, 11551165.
Mauss, I. B., Levenson, R. W., McCarter, L., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2005).
The tie that binds? Coherence among emotion experience, behavior, and
autonomic physiology. Emotion, 5, 175190.
Mauss, I. B., Savino, N. S., Anderson, C. L., Weisbuch, M., Tamir, M., &
Laudenslager, M. L. (2012a). The pursuit of happiness can be lonely.
Emotion, 12, 908912.
Mauss, I. B., Shallcross, A. J., Troy, A. S., John, O. P., Ferrer, E., Wilhelm, F. H., &
Gross, J. J. (2011). Dont hide your happiness! Positive emotion dissociation,
social connectedness, and psychological functioning. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 100, 738748.
Mauss, I. B., Tamir, M., Anderson, C. L., & Savino, N. S. (2011). Can seeking
happiness make people unhappy? Paradoxical effects of valuing happiness.
Emotion, 11, 807815.
Oishi, S., Diener, E., & Lucas, R.E. (2007). Optimal level of well-being: Can people
be too happy? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 346360.
12, 807816.
Trull, T. J., Solhan, M. B., Tragesser, S. L., Jahng, S., Wood, P. K., Piasecki, T. M., &
Watson, D. (2008). Affective instability: Measuring a core feature of
borderline personality disorder with ecological momentary assessment.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 647661.
Tugade, M., Fredrickson, B. L., & Barrett, L. F. (2004). Psychological resilience and
positive emotional granularity: Examining the benefits of positive emotions
on coping and health. Journal of Personality, 72, 11611190.
Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, L. L. (2004). Resilient individuals use positive
emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 320333.
Veenhoven, R. (2008). Healthy happiness: Effects of happiness on physical
consequences for preventive health care. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9,
449469.
van Kleef, E., van Dijk, G., Steinel, W., & van Beest, I. (2008). A social functional
approach to emotions in bargaining: When communicating anger pays and
when it backfires. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 600614.
hman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an
evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483
522.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Emotion Experience and Well-Being by Brett
Ford and Iris B. Mauss is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercialShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Emotional Intelligence
Marc Brackett, Sarah Delaney & Peter Salovey
Yale University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
In this chapter, we review the construct of emotional intelligence by examining
its underlying theoretical model, measurement tools, validity, and applications
in real-world settings. We use empirical research from the past few decades to
support and discuss competing definitions of emotional intelligence and
possible future directions for the field.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
Imagine you are waiting in line to buy tickets to see your favorite band. Knowing
tickets are limited and prices will rise quickly, you showed up 4 hours early.
Unfortunately, so did everyone else. The line stretches for blocks and hasnt
moved since you arrived. It starts to rain. You are now close to Will Call when
you notice three people jump ahead of you to join their friends, who appear to
have been saving a spot for them. They talk loudly on their cellphones as you
inch forward, following the slow procession of others waiting in line. You finally
reach the ticket counter only to have the clerk tell you the show is sold out. You
notice the loud group off to the side, waving their tickets in the air. At this exact
moment, a fiery line of emotion shoots through your whole body. Your heart
begins to race, and you feel the urge to either slam your hands on the counter
or scream in the face of those you believe have slighted you. What are these
feelings, and what will you do with them?
Emotional intelligence (EI) involves the idea that cognition and emotion are
interrelated. From this notion stems the belief that emotions influence decision
making, relationship building, and everyday behavior. After spending hours
waiting eagerly in the pouring rain and having nothing to show for it, is it even
possible to squelch such intense feelings of anger due to injustice? From an EI
perspective, emotions are active mental processes that can be managed, so
long as individuals develop the knowledge and skills to do so. But how, exactly,
do we reason with our emotions? In other words, how intelligent is our emotion
system?
To begin, well briefly review the concept of standard, or general, intelligence.
The late American psychologist, David Wechsler, claimed that intelligence is the
global capacity of an individual to think rationally, act purposefully, and deal
effectively with their environment (Wechsler, 1944). If we choose to accept this
definition, then intelligence is an operational process through which we learn
to utilize our internal abilities in order to better navigate our surroundingsa
nobaproject.com - Emotional Intelligence
1687
process that is most certainly similar to, if not impacted by, our emotions. In
1990, Drs. Peter Salovey and John D. Mayer first explored and defined EI. They
explained EI as the ability to monitor ones own and others feelings and
emotions, to discriminate among them and use this information to guide ones
thinking and actions (Salovey & Mayer, 1990). EI, according to these
researchers, asserts that all individuals possess the ability to leverage their
emotions to enhance thinking, judgment, and behavior. This chapter aims to
unpack this theory by exploring the growing empirical research on EI, as well
as what can be learned about its impact on our daily lives.
History of EI
Traditionally, many psychologists and philosophers viewed cognition and
emotion as separate domains, with emotion posing a threat to productive and
rational thinking. Have you ever been told not to let your emotions get in the
way of your decisions? This separation of passion and reason stretches as far
back as early ancient Greece (Lyons, 1999). Additionally, mid-20th century
scholars explained emotions as mentally destabilizing forces (Young, 1943). Yet,
there are traces throughout history where the intersection of emotion and
cognition has been theoretically questioned. In 350 B.C.E., the famous Greek
philosopher Aristotle wrote, some men . . . if they have first perceived and seen
what is coming and have first roused themselves and their calculative faculty,
are not defeated by their emotion, whether it be pleasant or painful( Aristotle,
trans. 2009, Book VII, Chapter 7, Section 8). Still, our social interactions and
experiences suggest this belief has undergone centuries of disregard, both in
Western and Eastern cultures. These are the same interactions that teach us
to toughen up and keep our emotions hidden. So, how did we arrive at EIa
scientific theory that claims all individuals have access to a calculative faculty
through emotion?
nobaproject.com - Emotional Intelligence
1688
In the early 1970s, many scientists began to recognize the limitations of the
Intelligence Quotient (IQ)the standardized assessment of intelligence. In
particular, they noticed its inability to explain differences among individuals
unrelated to just cognitive ability alone. These frustrations led to the
advancement of more inclusive theories of intelligence such as Gardners
multiple intelligences theory (1983/1993) and Sternbergs triarchic theory of
intelligence (1985). Researchers also began to explore the influence of moods
and emotions on thought processes, including judgment (Isen, Shalker, Clark,
& Karp, 1978) and memory (Bower, 1981). It was through these theoretical
explorations and empirical studies that the concept of EI began to take shape.
Today, the field of EI is extensive, encompassing varying perspectives and
measurement tools. Some attribute this growth to Daniel Golemans
popularization of the construct in his 1995 book, Emotional Intelligence: Why
It Can Matter More Than IQ. Generating public appeal, he focused on EIs
connection to personal and professional success. Golemans model of EI
includes a blend of emotion-related skills, traditional cognitive intelligence, and
distinct personality traits. This embellished conceptualization of EI, followed by
an increase in EI literature, contributed, at least in part, to conflicting definitional
and measurement models within the field.
1689
1. Perception of Emotion
Perception of emotion refers to peoples capacity to identify emotions in
themselves and others using facial expressions, tone of voice, and body
language (Brackett et al., 2013). Those skilled in the perception of emotion also
are able to express emotion accordingly and communicate emotional needs.
For example, lets return to our opening scenario. After being turned away at
the ticket booth, you slowly settle into the reality that you cannot attend the
concert. A group of your classmates, however, managed to buy tickets and are
discussing their plans at your lunch table. When they ask if you are excited for
the opening band, you shrug and pick at your food. If your classmates are skilled
at perception of emotion, then they will read your facial expression and body
language and determine that you might be masking your true feelings of
nobaproject.com - Emotional Intelligence
1690
3. Understanding of Emotion
EI also includes the ability to differentiate between emotional states, as well as
their specific causes and trajectories. Feelings of sadness or disappointment
can result from the loss of a person or object, such as your concert tickets.
Standing in the rain, by most standards, is merely a slight annoyance. However,
nobaproject.com - Emotional Intelligence
1691
waiting in the rain for hours in a large crowd will likely result in irritation or
frustration. Feeling like you have been treated unfairly when someone cuts in
line and takes the tickets you feel you deserved can cause your unpleasantness
to escalate into anger and resentment. People skilled in this area are aware of
this emotional trajectory and also have a strong sense of how multiple emotions
can work together to produce another. For instance, it is possible that you may
feel contempt for the people who cut in front of you in line. However, this feeling
of contempt does not arise from anger alone. Rather, it is the combination of
anger and disgust by the fact that these individuals, unlike you, have disobeyed
the rules. Successfully discriminating between negative emotions is an
important skill related to understanding of emotion, and it may lead to more
effective emotion management (Feldman Barret, Gross, Christensen, &
Benvenuto, 2001).
4. Management of Emotion
Emotion management includes the ability to remain open to a wide range of
emotions, recognize the value of feeling certain emotions in specific situations,
and understand which short- and long-term strategies are most efficient for
emotion regulation (Gross, 1998). Anger seems an appropriate response to
falling short of a goal (concert tickets) that you pursued both fairly and patiently.
In fact, you may even find it valuable to allow yourself the experience of this
feeling. However, this feeling will certainly need to be managed in order to
prevent aggressive, unwanted behavior. Coming up with strategies, such as
taking a deep breath and waiting until you feel calm before letting the group
ahead of you know they cut in line, will allow you to regulate your anger and
prevent the situation from escalating. Using this strategy may even let you gain
insight into other perspectivesperhaps you learn they had already purchased
their tickets and were merely accompanying their friends.
1692
1693
1694
these areas difficult even when combined with emotional skills. For example,
characteristics such as agreeableness and neuroticism, while contributing to
personal and professional success, are seen as innate traits that are likely to
remain static over time. Distinguishing EI from personality traits helps us better
target the skills that can improve desirable outcomes (Brackett et al., 2013).
Approaching EI with language that provides the opportunity for personal growth
is crucial to its application. Because the ability model aligns with this approach,
the remainder of this chapter will focus on ability EI and the ways in which it
can be applied both in professional and academic settings.
Outcomes
Historically, emotions have been thought to have no place in the classroom or
workplace (Sutton & Wheatly, 2003). Yet today, we know empirical research
supports the belief that EI has the potential to influence decision making, health,
relationships, and performance in both professional and academic settings (e.
g., Brackett et al., 2013; Brackett, Rivers, & Salovey, 2011).
A. Workplace
Research conducted in the workplace supports positive links between EI and
enhanced job performance, occupational well-being, and leadership
effectiveness. In one study, EI was associated with performance indicators such
as company rank, percent merit increase, ratings of interpersonal facilitation,
and affect and attitudes at work (Lopes, Grewal, Kadis, Gall, & Salovey, 2006).
Similar correlations have been found between EI and a variety of managerial
simulations involving problem solving, determining employee layoffs, adjusting
claims, and negotiating successfully (Day & Carroll, 2004; Feyerherm & Rice,
2002; Mueller & Curhan, 2006). Emotion management is seen as most likely to
affect job performance by influencing social and business interactions across
nobaproject.com - Emotional Intelligence
1695
B. Schools
When applied in educational settings, theoretical foundations of EI are often
integrated into social and emotional learning (SEL) programs. SEL is the
process of merging thinking, feeling, and behaving. These skills enable
individuals to be aware of themselves and of others, make responsible
decisions, and manage their own behaviors and those of others (Elias et al.,
1997; Elbertson, Brackett, & Weissberg, 2010). SEL programs are designed to
enhance the climate of a classroom, school, or district, with the ultimate goal
of enhancing childrens social and emotional skills and improving their academic
outcomes (Greenberg et al., 2003). Adopting curricula that focus on these
elements is believed to enable success in academics, relationships, and,
ultimately, in life (Becker & Luthar, 2002; Catalino, Berglundh, Ryan, Lonczek, &
Hawkins, 2004).
Take a moment to think about the role of a teacher. How might emotions
impact the climate of a classroom? If a teacher enters a classroom feeling
anxious, disgruntled, or unenthused, these states will most likely be noticed,
and felt, by the students. If not managed well, these negative emotions can hurt
the classroom dynamic and prevent student learning (Travers, 2001). Research
nobaproject.com - Emotional Intelligence
1696
suggests that the abilities to perceive, use, understand, and manage emotions
are imperative for effective teaching (Reyes, Brackett, Rivers, White, & Salovey,
2012; Brackett, Reyes, Rivers, Elbertson, & Salovey, 2011; Hargreaves, 2001). In
a study that examined the relationship between emotion regulation and both
job satisfaction and burnout among secondary-school teachers, researchers
found that emotion regulation among teachers was associated with positive
affect, support from principals, job satisfaction, and feelings of personal
accomplishment (Brackett, Palomera, Mojsa-Kaja, Reyes, & Salovey, 2010).
EI, when embedded into SEL programs, has been shown to contribute
positively to personal and academic success in students (Durlak, Weissberg,
Dymnicki, Tayloer, & Schellinger, 2011). Research also shows that strong
emotion regulation can help students pay attention in class, adjust to the school
environment, and manage academic anxiety (Lopes & Salovey, 2004; Mestre,
Guil, Lopes, Salovey, & Gil-Olarte, 2006). A recent randomized control trial of
RULER also found that, after one year, schools that used RULERcompared
with those that used only the standard curriculumwere rated by independent
observers as having higher degrees of warmth and connectedness between
teachers and students, more autonomy and leadership, less bullying among
students, and teachers who focused more on students interests and
motivations (Rivers, Brackett, Reyes, Elbertson, & Salovey, 2013).
1697
particularly those that stretch beyond the measurement debate and question
the validity of the EI construct when defined too broadly (Locke, 2005). In order
to advance EI research, there is a great need for investigators to address these
issues by reconciling disparate definitions and refining existing measures.
Potential considerations for future research in the field should include deeper
investigation into the genetic (versus acquired) and fluid (versus crystallized)
aspects of EI. The cultural implications and differences of EI also are important
to consider. Studies should expand beyond the United States and Europe in
order for the theory of EI to be cross-culturally valid and for its applications and
outcomes to be achieved more universally. Greater attention should also be
paid to developmental trajectories, gender differences, and how EI operates in
the workplace and educational settings (Brackett et al., 2013).
Although further explorations and research in the field of EI are needed,
current findings indicate a fundamental relationship between emotion and
cognition. Returning to our opening question, what will you do when denied
concert tickets? One of the more compelling aspects of EI is that it grants us
reign over our own emotionsforces once thought to rule the self by denying
individual agency. But with this power comes responsibility. If you are enraged
about not getting tickets to the show, perhaps you can take a few deep breaths,
go for a walk, and wait until your physiological indicators (shaky hands or
accelerated heartbeat) subside. Once youve removed yourself, your feeling of
rage may lessen to annoyance. Lowering the intensity level of this feeling (a
process known as down regulating) will help re-direct your focus on the situation
itself, rather than the activated emotion. In this sense, emotion regulation allows
you to objectively view the point of conflict without dismissing your true feelings.
Merely down regulating the emotional experience facilitates better problem
solving. Now that you are less activated, what is the best approach? Should you
talk to the ticket clerk? Ask to see the sales manager? Or do you let the group
know how you felt when they cut the line? All of these options present better
solutions than impulsively acting out rage.
nobaproject.com - Emotional Intelligence
1698
1699
Outside Resources
Book: Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York, NY: Bantam.
Discussion Questions
1. What are the four emotional abilities that comprise EI, and how do they
relate to each other?
2. What are three possible implications for using ability-based and mixed or
trait-based models of EI?
3. Discuss the ways in which EI can contribute positively to the workplace and
classroom settings.
Vocabulary
Ability model
An approach that views EI as a standard intelligence that utilizes a distinct set
of mental abilities that (1) are intercorrelated, (2) relate to other extant
intelligences, and (3) develop with age and experience (Mayer & Salovey, 1997).
Emotional intelligence
The ability to monitor ones own and others feelings and emotions, to
discriminate among them and to use this information to guide ones thinking
and actions. (Salovey & Mayer, 1990). EI includes four specific abilities:
perceiving, using, understanding, and managing emotions.
Four-Branch Model
An ability model developed by Drs. Peter Salovey and John Mayer that includes
four main components of EI, arranged in hierarchical order, beginning with
basic psychological processes and advancing to integrative psychological
processes. The branches are (1) perception of emotion, (2) use of emotion to
facilitate thinking, (3) understanding emotion, and (4) management of emotion.
Performance assessment
A method of measurement associated with ability models of EI that evaluate
the test takers ability to solve emotion-related problems.
Self-report assessment
A method of measurement associated with mixed and trait models of EI, which
evaluates the test takers perceived emotion-related skills, distinct personality
traits, and other characteristics.
Social and emotional learning (SEL)
The real-world application of EI in an educational setting and/or classroom that
involves curricula that teach the process of integrating thinking, feeling, and
behaving in order to become aware of the self and of others, make responsible
decisions, and manage ones own behaviors and those of others (Elias et al.,
1997)
Reference List
Bar-On, R. (2006). The Bar-On model of emotional-social intelligence (ESI).
Psicometha, 18
(Suppl.), 1325.
Bass, B. M. (1985). Leadership and performance beyond expectations. New York,
NY: Free Press.
Bass, B. M., & Riggio, R. E. (2006). Transformational leadership (2nd ed.) Mahwah,
NJ:
Erlbaum.
Becker, B.E., & Luthar, S.S. (2002). Social-emotional factors affecting
achievement outcomes among disadvantaged studets: Closing the
achievement gap. Educational Psychologist, 37, 197-214.
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129148.
Boyatzis, R., & Sala, F. (2004). The Emotional Competency Inventory (ECI). In G.
Geher (Ed.),
Measuring emotional intelligence: Common ground and controversy (pp.
143178). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science.
Brackett, M. A., Bertoli, M., Elbertson, N., Bausseron, E., Castillo, R., and Salovey,
P. (2013).
Reconceptualizing the cognition-emotion link. In M. D. Robinson, E. R.
Watkins, E. Harmon-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of Cognition and Emotion (pp.
365-379). New York, NY: Guilford.
Brackett, M. A., Palomera, R., Mojsa-kaja, J., Reyes, M. R., & Salovey, P. (2010).
Emotion-regulation ability, burnout, and job satisfaction among British
secondary-school teachers. Psychology in the Schools, 47, 406417.
Brackett, M. A., Reyes, M. R., Rivers, S. E., Elbertson, N. A., & Salovey, P. (2011).
Emotional climate, teacher affiliation, and student conduct. Journal of
Classroom Interaction, 46, 2736.
Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., Shiffman, S., Lerner, N., & Salovey, P. (2006). Relating
emotional
abilities to social functioning: A comparison of self-report and performance
measures of emotional intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 91, 780795.
Brackett, M. A., & Mayer, J. D. (2003). Convergent, discriminant, and incremental
validity of
competing measures of emotional intelligence. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 29, 11471158.
Brackett, M.A., Rivers, S.E., & Salovey, P. (2011). Emotional Intelligence,
Implications for Personal, Social, Academic, and Workplace Success. Social
and Personality Psychology Compass, 5, 88-103.
Catalino, R.F., Berglund, L., Ryan, J.A.M., Lonczek, H.S., & Hawkins, J.D. (2004).
Positive youth development in the United States: Research findings on
evaluations of positive youth development programs. The Annals of
American Academy of Political and Social Science, 591, 98-124. Doi:
10.1177/0002716203260102.
Cherniss, C. (2010). Emotional intelligence: Toward clarification of a concept. *
Industrial and
Organizational Psychology, 3*, 110126.
Day, A. L., & Carroll, S. A. (2008). Faking emotional intelligence (EI): Comparing
response distortion on ability and trait-based EI measures, Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 29, 761784.
Day, A. L., & Carroll, S. A.(2004). Using an ability-based measure of emotional
intelligence to
predict individual performance, group performance, and group citizenship
behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 14431458.
Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Tayloer, R. D., & Schellinger, K. B.
(2011), The impact of enhancing students social and emotional learning: A
meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Development,
82, 405432.
Elbertson, N. A., Brackett, M. A., & Weissberg, R. P. (2010). School-based social
and emotional learning (SEL) programming: Current perspectives. In A.
Hargreaves, M. Fullan, D. Hopkins, & A. Lieberman (Eds.), The second
international handbook of educational change (pp. 10171032). New York,
NY: Springer.
Elias, M. J., Zins, J. E., Weissberg, R. P., Frey, K. S., Greenberg, M. T., Haynes, N.
M., Shriver, T. P. (1997). Promoting social and emotional learning: Guidelines
for educators. Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum
Development.
Feldman Barrett, L., Gross, J., Christensen, T. C., & Benvenuto, M. (2001). Knowing
what youre
feeling and knowing what to do about it: Mapping the relation between
emotion differentiation and emotion regulation. Cognition and Emotion,
15, 713724.
Feyerherm, A. E., & Rice, C. I. (2002). Emotional intelligence and team
performance: The good, the bad and the ugly. International Journal of
Organizational Analysis, 10, 343362.
Gardner, H. (1993). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences (10th
Anniversary Edition). New York, NY: Basic. (Original work published 1983)
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York, NY: Bantam.
Greenberg, M. T., Weissberg, R. P., OBrien, M. U., Zins, J. E., Fredericks, L., Resnik,
H., & Elias, M. J. (2003). Enhancing school-based prevention and youth
development through coordinated social, emotional, and academic
learning. American Psychologist, 58, 466474. Doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.58.6-7.466
Gross, J.J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative
review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271-299.
Hargreaves, A. (2001). The emotional geographies of teachers relations with
colleagues. International Journal of Educational Research, 35, 503527.
Isen, A. M., Shalker, T. E., Clark, M., & Karp, L. (1978). Affect, accessibility of
material in memory, and behavior: A cognitive loop? Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 36, 112.
Leban, W., & Zulauf, C. (2004). Linking emotional intelligence abilities and
transformational leadership styles. Leadership Organization Development
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey &
D. J. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development and emotional intelligence:
Educational implications (pp. 334). New York, NY: Basic.
Mestre, J. M., Guil, R., Lopes, P. N., Salovey, P., & Gil-Olarte, P. (2006). Emotional
intelligence and social and academic adaptation to school. Psicothema, 18
(Suppl.), 112117.
Mueller, J. S., & Curhan, J. R. (2006). Emotional intelligence and counterpart
mood induction in a negotiation. International Journal of Conflict
Management, 17, 110128.
OBoyle, E. H., Humphrey, R. H., Pollack, J. M., Hawver, T. H., & Story, P. A. (2010).
The relation between emotional intelligence and job performance: A metaanalysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32, 788818.
Paulhus, D. L., Lysy, D. C., & Yik, M. S. M. (1998). Self-report measures of
intelligence: Are they useful as proxy IQ tests? Journal of Personality, 66,
525554.
Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2003). Trait emotional intelligence: Behavioural
validation in two studies of emotion recognition and reactivity to mood
induction. European Journal of Personality, 17, 39-57.
Reyes, M. R., Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., White, M., & Salovey, P. (2012).
Classroom emotional climate, student engagement, and academic
achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 700712.
Rivers, S. E., Brackett, M. A., Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (2007). Measuring
emotional intelligence as a set of mental abilities. In G. Matthews, M.
Rivers, S.E., Brackett, M.A., Reyes, M.R., Elbertson, N.A., & Salovey, P. (2013).
Improving the social and emotional climate of classroms: A clustered
randomized controlled trial testing The RULER Approach. Prevention
Science, 14, 77-87.
Sutton, R. E., & Wheatly, K. F. (2003). Teachers emotions and teaching: A review
of the literature and directions for future research. Educational Psychology
Review, 15, 327358.
Young, P. T. (1943). Emotion in man and in animal: Its nature and relation to
attitude and motive. New York, NY: Wiley.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Emotional Intelligence by Marc Brackett, Sarah
Delaney, and Peter Salovey is licensed under the Creative Commons AttributionNonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
How do peoples cultural ideas and practices shape their emotions (and other
types of feelings)? In this chapter, I describe findings from studies comparing
North American (United States, Canada) and East Asian (Chinese, Japanese,
Korean) contexts. These studies reveal both cultural similarities and differences
in various aspects of emotional life. I discuss the scientific and practical
importance of these findings and conclude with directions for future research.
Learning Objectives
Learn about recent empirical findings and theories of culture and emotion.
Imagine that you are traveling in a country that you have never been to before.
Everything seems different: the sights, the smells, and the sounds. People are
speaking a language you do not understand, and wearing clothes that are
different from yours. But they greet you with a smile, and you sense that despite
the differences that you observe, deep down inside, these people have the
same feelings that you do.
But is this true? While most scholars agree that members of different
cultures may vary in the foods they eat, the languages they speak, and the
holidays they celebrate, scholars disagree about the extent to which culture
shapes peoples emotions and feelings, including what people feel, what they
express, and what they do during an emotional event. Understanding how
culture shapes peoples emotional lives and what impact emotion has on
psychological health and well-being in different cultures will not only advance
the study of human behavior, but will also benefit multicultural societies. Across
a variety of settingsacademic, business, medicalpeople worldwide are
coming into more contact with people from different cultures than their own.
In order to communicate and function effectively with people from other
cultures, people must understand the ways in which their cultural ideas and
practices shape their emotions.
Historical Background
In the 1950s and 1960s, social scientists tended to fall into one of two camps.
The universalist camp claimed that despite cultural differences in customs and
traditions, at a fundamental level, all humans feel similarly. Universalists
believed that because emotions evolved in response to the environments of
our primordial ancestors, emotions are the same across different human
cultures. Indeed, people often describe their emotions as automatic, natural,
physiological, and instinctual, supporting the view that emotions are hardwired and universal.
nobaproject.com - Culture and Emotion
1715
1716
Figure 1. Facial expressions associated with happiness, sadness, disgust, and anger based
on the Facial Action Coding System.
1717
context) (Masuda, Ellsworth, Mesquita, Leu, Tanida, & Van de Veerdonk, 2008),
and the degree to which people focus on different features of the face (Yuki,
Maddux, & Matsuda, 2007) when perceiving peoples emotions.
But how does culture shape other aspects of emotional life, such as how
people emotionally respond to different situations, how people want to feel,
and what makes people happy? Today, most scholars would agree that
emotions and other related feeling states are multifaceted, and that both
cultural similarities and differences can be found for each facet of emotion.
Thus, rather than classify emotions as universal or socially constructed, scholars
are attempting to identify the specific ways in which these different aspects of
emotional life are similar and are different across cultures. These endeavors
are yielding new insights into the cultural shaping of emotion.
1718
1719
1720
more intensely than did their Hmong counterparts, even though they reported
feeling happy, proud, and in love at similar levels of intensity. Similar patterns
have emerged in studies comparing European Americans with Chinese
Americans during different emotion-eliciting tasks (Tsai et al., 2002; Tsai,
Levenson, & McCoy, 2006; Tsai, Levenson, & Carstensen, 2000). Thus, while the
physiological aspects of emotional responding appear to be more similar across
cultures, the facial expressive aspects of emotional responding appear to be
more different across cultures.
These differences in facial expression during positive events are consistent
with findings from cross-cultural studies of display rules, and stem from the
models of self described above. In North American contexts that promote an
independent self, individuals must express their emotions in order to influence
others. In contrast, in East Asian contexts that promote an interdependent self,
individuals must control and suppress their emotions in order to adjust to
others.
1721
1722
1723
1724
Figure 2: Adapted from Feldman, Barrett, and Russell (1999); Larsen and Diener ((1992); Russell
(1991); Thayer (1989); Watson and Tellegen (1985)
1725
Figure 3: Sample Hong Kong Chinese (left) and European American (right) Facebook pages.
Again, these differences in ideal affect are related to the independent and
interdependent selves described above. Independent selves want to influence
others. Influencing others requires action (or doing something), and action
involves high arousal states. In contrast, interdependent selves want to adjust
to others. Adjusting to others requires suspending action and attending to
others, which both involve low arousal states. Thus, the more that individuals
and cultures want to influence others (as they do in North American contexts),
the more they value excitement, enthusiasm, and other high arousal positive
states, and the more that individuals and cultures want to adjust to others (as
they do in East Asian contexts), the more they value calm, peacefulness, and
other low arousal positive states (Tsai, Miao, Seppala, Fung, & Yeung, 2007).
Because ideal affect functions as a guide for behavior and as a way of
evaluating ones emotional states, cultural differences in ideal affect can result
in different emotional lives. For example, in several studies, we have observed
that people engage in activities (e.g., choose recreational activities, listen to
music) that are consistent with their ideal affect, and therefore, cultural
nobaproject.com - Culture and Emotion
1726
1727
1728
Americans are often overlooked for top leadership positions (Hyun, 2005).
In addition to averting cultural misunderstandings, understanding cultural
similarities and differences in emotion may teach individuals about other paths
to psychological health and well-being. For instance, findings from a recent
series of studies suggests that calm states are easier to elicit than excitement
states, suggesting that one way of increasing happiness in cultures that value
excitement may be to increase the value placed on calm states (Chim, Tsai,
Hogan, & Fung, 2013).
1729
Could
These
Cultural
Differences
Be
Due
to
Temperament?
An alternative explanation for the cultural differences in emotion described
above is that the group differences are due to temperamental factors; that is,
biological predispositions to respond in a certain way. Could it be that European
Americans are just more emotional than East Asians because of their genetic
differences? Indeed, most models of emotion acknowledge that temperament
and culture both play roles in emotional life, and yet few if any models indicate
nobaproject.com - Culture and Emotion
1730
1731
Figure 4: Affect valuation theory. Darker lines indicate stronger predicted relationships.
Summary
Based on studies comparing North American and East Asian cultural contexts,
there is clear evidence for both cultural similarities and differences in emotional
functioning, and most of the differences can be traced to different cultural
models of the self. Future research is needed to reveal the myriad other ways
in which culture shapes peoples emotional lives.
1732
Outside Resources
Audio Interview: The Really Big Questions What Are Emotions? Interview
with Paul Ekman, Martha Nussbaum, Dominique Moisi, and William Reddy
http://www.trbq.org/index.php?option=com_content&view=category&layout=
blog&id=16&Itemid=43
Book: Ed Diener and Robert Biswas-Diener: Happiness: Unlocking the
Mysteries of Psychological Wealth
Book: Hazel Markus: Clash: 8 Cultural Conflicts That Make Us Who We Are
Discussion Questions
1. What cultural ideas and practices related to emotion were you exposed to
when you were a child? What cultural ideas and practices related to emotion
are you currently exposed to as an adult? How do you think they shape your
emotional experiences and expressions?
2. How can researchers avoid inserting their own beliefs about emotion in
their research?
3. Most of the studies described above are based on self-report measures.
What are some of the advantages and disadvantages of using self-report
measures to understand the cultural shaping of emotion? How might the
use of other behavioral methods (e.g., neuroimaging) address some of these
limitations?
4. Do the empirical findings described above change your beliefs about
emotion? How?
5. Imagine you are a manager of a large American company that is beginning
to do work in China and Japan. How will you apply your current knowledge
about culture and emotion to prevent misunderstandings between you and
your Chinese and Japanese employees?
Vocabulary
Affect
Feelings that can be described in terms of two dimensions, the dimensions of
arousal and valence (Figure 3). For example, high arousal positive states refer
to excitement, elation, and enthusiasm. Low arousal positive states refer to
calm, peacefulness, and relaxation. Whereas actual affect refers to the states
that people actually feel, ideal affect refers to the states that people ideally
want to feel.
Culture
Shared, socially transmitted ideas (e.g., values, beliefs, attitudes) that are
reflected in and reinforced by institutions, products, and rituals.
Emotions
Changes in subjective experience, physiological responding, and behavior in
response to a meaningful event. Emotions tend to occur on the order of seconds
(in contract to moods which may last for days).
Feelings
A general term used to describe a wide range of states that include emotions,
moods, traits and that typically involve changes in subjective experience,
physiological responding, and behavior in response to a meaningful event.
Emotions typically occur on the order of seconds, whereas moods may last for
days, and traits are tendencies to respond a certain way across various
situations.
Independent self
A model or view of the self as distinct from others and as stable across different
situations. The goal of the independent self is to express and assert the self,
and to influence others. This model of self is prevalent in many individualistic,
Western contexts (e.g., the United States, Australia, Western Europe).
Interdependent self
A model or view of the self as connected to others and as changing in response
to different situations. The goal of the interdependent self is to suppress
personal preferences and desires, and to adjust to others. This model of self is
prevalent in many collectivistic, East Asian contexts (e.g., China, Japan, Korea).
Reference List
Bylsma, L., Morris, B., & Rottenberg, J. (2008). A meta-analysis of emotional
reactivity in major depressive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(4),
676691.
Chentsova-Dutton, Y. E., Chu, J. P., Tsai, J. L., Rottenberg, J., Gross, J. J., & Gotlib,
I. H. (2007). Depression and emotional reactivity: Variation among Asian
Americans of East Asian descent and European Americans. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 116(4), 776785.
Chentsova-Dutton, Y. E., Tsai, J. L., & Gotlib, I. (2010). Further evidence for the
cultural norm hypothesis: Positive emotion in depressed and control
European American and Asian American women. Cultural Diversity and
Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16, 284295.
Cheryan, S., & Monin, B. (2005). Where are you really from?: Asian Americans
and identity denial. Journal of personality and social psychology, 89(5), 717
731.
Chim, L., Moon, A., Ang, J., & Tsai, J. L. (2013). Magazine ads, Facebook pages,
and company websites reflect cultural differences in ideal affect. In T.
Masuda (Chair). Culture and mind: Implications for art, design, and
advertising. Symposium held at International Association for Cross-Cultural
Psychology, Los Angeles, CA.
Chim, L., Tsai, J.L., Hogan, C., & Fung, H. H. (2013). Enhancing happiness by
valuing calm. Manuscript in preparation.
Cousins, S. D. (1989). Culture and self-perception in Japan and the United States.
10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
Masuda, T., Ellsworth, P. C., Mesquita, B., Leu, J., Tanida, S., & Van de Veerdonk,
E. (2008). Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception
of facial emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(3), 365
381. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.3.365
Miyamoto, Y., Uchida, Y., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2010). Culture and mixed emotions:
Co-occurrence of positive and negative emotions in Japan and the United
States. Emotion, 10(3), 404415. doi: 10.1037/a0018430
Morling, B., Kitayama, S., & Miyamoto, Y. (2002). Cultural practices emphasize
influence in the United States and adjustment in Japan. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 311323.
Oishi, S., Diener, E. F., Lucas, R. E., & Suh, E. M. (1999). Cross-cultural variations
in predictors of life satisfaction: Perspectives from needs and values.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(8), 980990.
Perunovic, W., Heller, D., & Rafaeli, E. (2007). Within-person changes in the
structure of emotion: The role of cultural identification and language.
Psychological Science, 18, 607613.
Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial
expressions? A review of the cross-cultural studies. Psychological Bulletin,
115(1), 102141. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.102
Safdar, S., Friedlmeier, W., Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., Kwantes, C. T., Kakai, H.,
& Shigemasu, E. (2009). Variations of emotional display rules within and
across cultures: A comparison between Canada, USA, and Japan. Canadian
Tsai, J. L., Levenson, R. W., & Carstensen, L. L. (2000). Autonomic, subjective, and
expressive responses to emotional films in older and younger Chinese
Americans and European Americans. Psychology and Aging, 15(4), 684693.
Tsai, J. L., Louie, J., Chen, E. E., & Uchida, Y. (2007). Learning what feelings to
desire: Socialization of ideal affect through childrens storybooks.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1730.
Tsai, J. L., Miao, F. F., Seppala, E., Fung, H. H., & Yeung, D. (2007). Influence and
adjustment goals: Sources of cultural differences in ideal affect. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 11021117.
Tsai, J. L., Sims, T., Thomas, E., & Fung, H. H. (2013). Ideal affect across the life
span: A comparison of European American, Chinese American, and Hong
Kong Chinese. Manuscript in progress.
Weisz, J., Rothbaum, F., & Blackburn, T. (1984). Standing out and standing in:
The psychology of control in American and Japan. American Psychologist,
39, 955969.
Yuki, M., Maddux, W. W., & Masuda, T. (2007). Are the windows to the soul the
same in the East and West? Cultural differences in using the eyes and mouth
as cues to recognize emotions in Japan and the United States. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 43(2), 303311.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Culture and Emotion by Jeanne Tsai is licensed
under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International
License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/
deed.en_US.
Knowledge Emotions:
Feelings that Foster
Learning, Exploring, and
Reflecting
Paul Silvia
University North Carolina, Greensboro
nobaproject.com
Abstract
When people think of emotions they usually think of the obvious ones, such as
happiness, fear, anger, and sadness. This chapter looks at the knowledge
emotions, a family of emotional states that foster learning, exploring, and
reflecting. Surprise, interest, confusion, and awe come from events that are
unexpected, complicated, and mentally challenging, and they motivate learning
in its broadest sense, be it learning over the course of seconds (finding the
source of a loud crash, as in surprise) or over a lifetime (engaging with hobbies,
pastimes, and intellectual pursuits, as in interest). The chapter reviews research
on each emotion, with an emphasis on causes, consequences, and individual
differences. As a group, the knowledge emotions motivate people to engage
with new and puzzling things rather than avoid them. Over time, engaging with
new things, ideas, and people broadens someones experiences and cultivates
expertise. The knowledge emotions thus dont gear up the body like fear, anger,
and happiness do, but they do gear up the minda critical task for humans,
who must learn essentially everything that they know.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
What comes to mind when you think of emotions? Its probably the elation of
happiness, the despair of sadness, or the freak-out fright of fear. Emotions such
as happiness, anger, sadness, and fear are important emotions, but human
emotional experience is vastpeople are capable of experiencing a wide range
of feelings.
This chapter considers the knowledge emotions, a profoundly important
family of emotions associated with learning, exploring, and reflecting. The
family of knowledge emotions has four main members: surprise, interest,
confusion, and awe. These are considered knowledge emotions for two reasons.
First, the events that bring them about involve knowledge: These emotions
happen when something violates what people expected or believed. Second,
these emotions are fundamental to learning: Over time, they build useful
knowledge about the world.
1748
lifespan. Interest, for example, motivates people to learn about things over
days, weeks, and years. Finding something interesting motivates for its own
sake learning and is probably the major engine of human competence (Izard,
1977; Silvia, 2006).
What causes emotions to happen in the first place? Although it usually feels
like something in the worlda good hug, a snake slithering across the driveway,
a hot-air balloon shaped like a question markcauses an emotion directly,
emotion theories contend that emotions come from how we think about what
is happening in the world, not what is literally happening. After all, if things in
the world directly caused emotions, everyone would always have the same
emotion in response to something. Appraisal theories (Ellsworth & Scherer,
2003; Lazarus, 1991) propose that each emotion is caused by a group of
appraisals, which are evaluations and judgments of what events in the world
mean for our goals and well-being: Is this relevant to me? Does it further or
hinder my goals? Can I deal with it or do something about it? Did someone do
it on purpose? Different emotions come from different answers to these
appraisal questions.
With that as a background, in the following sections well consider the nature,
causes, and effects of each knowledge emotion. Afterward, we will consider
some of their practical implications.
nobaproject.com - Knowledge Emotions: Feelings that Foster Learning, Exploring, and Reflecting
1749
Surprise
Nothing gets peoples attention like something startling. Surprise, a simple
emotion, hijacks a persons mind and body and focuses them on a source of
possible danger (Simons, 1996). When theres a loud, unexpected crash, people
stop, freeze, and orient to the source of the noise. Their minds are wiped clean
after something startling, people usually cant remember what they had been
nobaproject.com - Knowledge Emotions: Feelings that Foster Learning, Exploring, and Reflecting
1750
Interest
People are curious creatures. Interestan emotion that motivates exploration
and learning (Silvia, 2012)is one of the most commonly experienced emotions
in everyday life (Izard, 1977). Humans must learn virtually everything they know,
from how to cook pasta to how the brain works, and interest is an engine of
this massive undertaking of learning across the lifespan.
The function of interest is to engage people with things that are new, odd,
or unfamiliar. Unfamiliar things can be scary or unsettling, which makes people
avoid them. But if people always avoided new things they would learn and
experience nothing. Its hard to imagine what life would be like if people werent
curious to try new things: We would never feel like watching a different movie,
nobaproject.com - Knowledge Emotions: Feelings that Foster Learning, Exploring, and Reflecting
1751
1752
and paragraphs are easier to remember when they are interesting (Sadoski,
2001; Schiefele, 1999)and for broader academic successpeople get better
grades and feel more intellectually engaged in classes they find interesting
(Krapp, 1999, 2002; Schiefele, Krapp, & Winteler, 1992).
Individual differences in interest are captured by trait curiosity(Kashdan,
2004; Kashdan et al., 2009). People low in curiosity prefer activities and ideas
that are tried and true and familiar; people high in curiosity, in contrast, prefer
things that are offbeat and new. Trait curiosity is a facet of openness to
experience, a broader trait that is one of the five major factors of personality
(McCrae, 1996; McCrae & Sutin, 2009). Not surprisingly, being high in openness
to experience involves exploring new things and findings quirky things
appealing. Research shows that curious, open people ask more questions in
class, own and read more books, eat a wider range of food, andnot
surprisingly, given their lifetime of engaging with new thingsare a bit higher
in intelligence (DeYoung, 2011; Kashdan & Silvia, 2009; Peters, 1978; Raine,
Reynolds, Venables, & Mednick, 2002).
Confusion
Sometimes the world is weird. Interest is a wonderful resource when people
encounter new and unfamiliar things, but those things arent always
comprehensible. Confusion happens when people are learning something that
is both unfamiliar and hard to understand. In the appraisal space shown in
Figure 1, confusion comes from appraising an event as high in novelty,
complexity, and unfamiliarity as well as appraising it as hard to comprehend
(Silvia, 2010, 2013).
Confusion, like interest, promotes thinking and learning. This isnt an obvious
ideaour intuitions would suggest that confusion makes people frustrated and
thus more likely to tune out and quit. But as odd as it sounds, making students
confused can help them learn better. In an approach to learning known as
nobaproject.com - Knowledge Emotions: Feelings that Foster Learning, Exploring, and Reflecting
1753
1754
Awe
Awea state of fascination and wonderis the deepest and probably least
common of the knowledge emotions. When people are asked to describe
profound experiences, such as the experience of beauty or spiritual
transformation, awe is usually mentioned (Cohen, Gruber, & Keltner, 2010).
People are likely to report experiencing awe when they are alone, engaged with
art and music, or in nature (Shiota, Keltner, & Mossman, 2007).
Awe comes from two appraisals (Keltner & Haidt, 2003). First, people
appraise something as vast, as beyond the normal scope of their experience.
Thus, like the other knowledge emotions, awe involves appraising an event as
inconsistent with ones existing knowledge, but the degree of inconsistency is
huge, usually when people have never encountered something like the event
before (Bonner & Friedman, 2011). Second, people engage in accommodation,
which is changing their beliefsabout themselves, other people, or the world
in generalto fit in the new experience. When something is massive (in size,
scope, sound, creativity, or anything else) and when people change their beliefs
to accommodate it, theyll experience awe.
A mild, everyday form of awe is chills, sometimes known as shivers or thrills.
Chills involve getting goosebumps on the skin, especially the scalp, neck, back,
and arms, usually as a wave that starts at the head and moves downward. Chills
are part of strong awe experiences, but people often experience them in
response to everyday events, such as compelling music and movies (Maruskin,
Thrash, & Elliot, 2012; Nusbaum & Silvia, 2011). Music that evokes chills, for
example, tends to be loud, have a wide frequency range (such as both low and
high frequencies), and major dynamic shifts, such as a shift from quiet to loud
or a shift from few to many instruments (Huron & Margulis, 2010).
nobaproject.com - Knowledge Emotions: Feelings that Foster Learning, Exploring, and Reflecting
1755
Like the other knowledge emotions, awe motivates people to engage with
something outside the ordinary. Awe is thus a powerful educational tool. In
science education, it is common to motivate learning by inspiring wonder. One
example comes from a line of research on astronomy education, which seeks
to educate the public about astronomy by using awe-inspiring images of deep
space (Arcand, Watzke, Smith, & Smith, 2010). When people see beautiful and
striking color images of supernovas, black holes, and planetary nebulas, they
usually report feelings of awe and wonder. These feelings then motivate them
to learn about what they are seeing and their scientific importance (Smith et
al., 2011).
Regarding individual differences, some people experience awe much more
often than others. One study that developed a brief scale to measure awe
the items included statements such as I often feel awe and I feel wonder
almost every dayfound that people who often experience awe are much
higher in openness to experience (a trait associated with openness to new things
and a wide emotional range) and in extraversion (a trait associated with positive
emotionality) (Shiota, Keltner, & John, 2006). Similar findings appear for when
people are asked how often they experience awe in response to the arts
(Nusbaum & Silvia, in press). For example, people who say that they often feel
a sense of awe and wonder when listening to music are much higher in
openness to experience (Silvia & Nusbaum, 2011).
1756
confusion, and awe first signal that something awry has happened that deserves
our attention. They then motivate us to engage with the new things that strain
our understanding of the world and how it works. Emotions surely aid fighting
and fleeing, but for most of the hours of most of our days, they mostly aid in
learning, exploring, and reflecting.
nobaproject.com - Knowledge Emotions: Feelings that Foster Learning, Exploring, and Reflecting
1757
Outside Resources
Video: A talk with Todd Kashdan, a well-known scholar in the field of curiosity
and positive psychology, centered on curiosity
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=IPpVxZRqRc8
Video: More from Todd
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=UunaTEpWrME
Web: Aesthetics and Astronomy, a project that uses wonder and beauty to
foster knowledge about the science of space
http://astroart.cfa.harvard.edu/
Web: The Emotion Computing Group, an interdisciplinary team that studies
how to measure confusion and harness it for deeper learning, among other
intriguing things
https://sites.google.com/site/memphisemotivecomputing/Home
Discussion Questions
1. Research shows that people learn more quickly and deeply when they are
interested. Can you think of examples from your own life when you learned
from interest versus from extrinsic rewards (e.g., good grades, approval
from parents and peers)? Was learning more enjoyable or effective in one
case?
2. How would you redesign a psychology lecture to harness the power of the
knowledge emotions? How could you use interest, confusion, and awe to
grab students attention and motivate them to reflect and learn?
3. Psychology, like all the sciences, is fueled by wonder. For psychology, the
wonder is about human nature and behavior. What, to you, is the most
wondrous, amazing, and awe-inspiring idea or finding from the science of
psychology? Does reflecting on this amazing fact motivate you to want to
know more about it?
Vocabulary
Accommodation
Changing one's beliefs about the world and how it works in light of new
experience.
Appraisal structure
The set of appraisals that bring about an emotion.
Appraisal theories
Evaluations that relate what is happening in the environment to peoples values,
goals, and beliefs. Appraisal theories of emotion contend that emotions are
caused by patterns of appraisals, such as whether an event furthers or hinders
a goal and whether an event can be coped with.
Awe
An emotion associated with profound, moving experiences. Awe comes about
when people encounter an event that is vast (far from normal experience) but
that can be accommodated in existing knowledge.
Chills
A feeling of goosebumps, usually on the arms, scalp, and neck, that is often
experienced during moments of awe.
Confusion
An emotion associated with conflicting and contrary information, such as when
people appraise an event as unfamiliar and as hard to understand. Confusion
motivates people to work through the perplexing information and thus fosters
deeper learning.
Coping potential
People's beliefs about their ability to handle challenges.
Facial expressions
Part of the expressive component of emotions, facial expressions of emotion
communicate inner feelings to others.
Functionalist theories of emotion
Theories of emotion that emphasize the adaptive role of an emotion in handling
common problems throughout evolutionary history.
Impasse-driven learning
An approach to instruction that motivates active learning by having learners
work through perplexing barriers.
Interest
An emotion associated with curiosity and intrigue, interest motivates engaging
with new things and learning more about them. It is one of the earliest emotions
to develop and a resource for intrinsically motivated learning across the life
span.
Intrinsically motivated learning
Learning that is for its own sakesuch as learning motivated by curiosity and
wonderinstead of learning to gain rewards or social approval.
Knowledge emotions
A family of emotions associated with learning, reflecting, and exploring. These
emotions come about when unexpected and unfamiliar events happen in the
environment. Broadly speaking, they motivate people to explore unfamiliar
things, which builds knowledge and expertise over the long run.
Openness to experience
One of the five major factors of personality, this trait is associated with higher
curiosity, creativity, emotional breadth, and open-mindedness. People high in
openness to experience are more likely to experience interest and awe.
Surprise
An emotion rooted in expectancy violation that orients people toward the
unexpected event.
Trait curiosity
Stable individual-differences in how easily and how often people become
curious.
Reference List
Arcand, K. K., Watzke, M., Smith, L. F., & Smith, J. K. (2010). Surveying aesthetics
and astronomy: A project exploring the publics perception of astronomical
images and the science within. CAP Journal, 10, 1316.
Bakker, M. J., van Dijk, J. G., van den Maagdenberg, A. M. J. M., & Tijssen, M. A.
J. (2006). Startle syndromes. The Lancet Neurology, 5, 513524.
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Bonner, E. T., & Friedman, H. L. (2011). A conceptual clarification of the
experience of awe: An interpretative phenomenological analysis. The
Humanistic Psychologist, 39, 222235.
Cohen, A. B., Gruber, J., & Keltner, D. (2010). Comparing spiritual transformations
and experiences of profound beauty. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality,
2, 127135
Craig, S. D., DMello, S., Witherspoon, A., & Graesser, A. (2008). Emote aloud
during learning with AutoTutor: Applying the facial action coding system to
cognitive-affective states during learning. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 777
788.
DeYoung, C. G. (2011). Intelligence and personality. In R. J. Sternberg & S. B.
Kaufman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of intelligence (pp. 711737).
New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Durso, F. T., Geldbach, K. M., & Corballis, P. (2012). Detecting confusion using
facial electromyography. Human Factors, 54, 6069.
DMello, S., Lehman, B., Pekrun, R., & Graesser, A. (in press). Confusion can be
beneficial for learning. Learning and Instruction.
Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In R. J.
Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective
sciences (pp. 572595). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Hidi, S. (2001). Interest, reading, and learning: Theoretical and practical
considerations. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 191209.
Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest
development. Educational Psychologist, 41, 111127.
Huron, D., & Margulis, E. H. (2010). Musical expectancy and thrills. In P. N. Juslin
& J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research,
applications (pp. 575604). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York, NY: Plenum.
Kashdan, T. B. (2004). Curiosity. In C. Peterson & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.),
Character strengths and virtues: A handbook and classification (pp. 125
141). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Kashdan, T. B., & Silvia, P. J. (2009). Curiosity and interest: The benefits of thriving
on novelty and challenge. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of
positive psychology (2nd ed., pp. 367374). New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
Kashdan, T. B., & Silvia, P. J. (2009). Curiosity and interest: The benefits of thriving
on novelty and challenge. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of
positive psychology (2nd ed., pp. 367374). New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
Kashdan, T.B., Gallagher, M. W., Silvia, P. J., Winterstein, B. P., Breen, W. E., Terhar,
D., & Steger, M. F. (2009). The curiosity and exploration inventoryII:
Development, factor structure, and psychometrics. Journal of Research in
Personality, 43, 987998.
Keltner, D., & Gross, J. J. (1999). Functional accounts of emotions. Cognition and
Emotion, 13, 467480.
Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (2003). Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic
emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 297314.
Krapp, A. (2002). An educational-psychological theory of interest and its relation
to self-determination theory. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of
self-determination research (pp. 405427). Rochester, NY: University of
Rochester Press.
Krapp, A. (1999). Interest, motivation and learning: An educationalpsychological perspective. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 14,
2340.
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
Maruskin, L. A., Thrash, T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2012). The chills as a psychological
construct: Content universe, factor structure, affective composition,
elicitors, trait antecedents, and consequences. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 103, 135157.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Knowledge Emotions: Feelings that Foster
Learning, Exploring, and Reflecting by Paul Silvia is licensed under the Creative Commons
Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this
license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
This chapter provides an overview of the main theories and findings on goals
and motivation. We address the origins, manifestations, and types of goals, and
the various factors that influence motivation in goal pursuit. We further address
goal conflict and, specifically, the exercise of self-control in protecting long-term
goals from momentary temptations.
Learning Objectives
Describe the factors that influence motivation in the course of goal pursuit.
Give examples of goal activation effects, self-regulation processes, and selfcontrol processes.
Introduction
Whether your goal is to pass a course, eat healthily, or land on Mars, you need
a certain level of motivation to stay on course and achieve it. A goal is the
cognitive representation of a desired state (Fishbach & Ferguson 2007;
Kruglanski, 1996), whereas motivation refers to the psychological driving force
that enables action in the pursuit of that goal (Lewin, 1935). The desired end
state of a goal can be clearly defined (e.g., step on the surface of Mars), or more
abstract and representing a motivational state that is never fully completed (e.
g., eat healthily). Motivation can stem from the benefits associated with the
process of pursuing a goal (intrinsic motivation). For example, you might be
driven by the desire to have a fulfilling experience while working on your Mars
mission. Motivation can also come from the benefits associated with achieving
a goal such as fame and fortune (extrinsic motivation) (Deci & Ryan, 1985).
Social psychologists recognize that goal pursuit and motivation do not
depend solely on an individuals personality but rather are products of personal
characteristics and situational factors. Indeed, cues in a persons immediate
environmentincluding images, words, sounds, and the presence of other
peoplecan activate or prime a goal. This activation can be conscious, such
that the person is aware of environmental cues that made her want to pursue
a goal (i.e., why?) and the resulting goal-directed judgments and behaviors (i.
e., how?). However, this activation can also occur outside a persons awareness
and lead to nonconscious goal pursuit. In this case, the person is unaware of
why she is pursuing a goal or does not even realize that she is pursuing a goal.
In this chapter we review key aspects of goals and motivation. We first discuss
the origins and manifestation of goals. We then review factors that influence
individuals motivation in the course of pursuing a goal such as studying an 800page book for an exam (self-regulation). Finally, we discuss what motivates
individuals to keep following their goals when faced with other conflicting
desiresfor example, when a tempting opportunity to socialize on Facebook
nobaproject.com - Motives and Goals
1774
Goal Priming
What makes people adhere to a goal in any given context? Cues in the immediate
environment (e.g., objects, images, words, and sounds) can influence the
pursuit of goals to which people are already committed (Bargh, 1990; Custers,
Aarts, Oikawa, & Elliot, 2009; Frster, Liberman, & Friedman, 2007). In memory,
goals are organized in associative networks, which connect each goal to
corresponding means (i.e., activities or objects that contribute to goal
attainment; Kruglanski et al., 2002). For example, the goal to stay physically fit
may be associated with several means, including the nearby gym, ones bicycle,
nobaproject.com - Motives and Goals
1775
or a training partner. Cues related to the goal or means will activate or prime
that goal pursuit. For example, the presence of ones training partner or the
word workout in a puzzle can activate the goal of staying physically fit and,
hence, increase a persons motivation to exercise. Soon after goal priming, the
motivation to act on a goal peaks, and then slowly declines after some delay,
as the person moves away from the prime or after she pursues the goal (Bargh,
Gollwitzer, Lee-Chai, Barndollar, & Trotschel, 2001).
1776
1777
1778
1779
1780
found that children who were able to wait longer in the experiment performed
better academically and socially and had better psychological coping skills as
adolescents.
1781
People are more likely to identify a self-control conflict and exercise selfcontrol when they think of a choice as part of a broader pattern of repeated
behavior rather than as one isolated choice. Indeed, when considering broader
decision patterns, consistent temptations become more problematic for longterm interests (Rachlin, 2000; Read, Loewenstein, & Kalyanaraman, 1999).
Moreover, conflict identification is more likely if people see their current choices
as similar to their future choices, in a dynamic of highlighting.
1782
Conclusion
In this chapter, we adopted a social-cognitive approach to review some of the
main theories and findings on goals and motivation. We described the principles
of goal priming and how goals influence perceptions, feelings, and actions. We
then summarized the principles of self-regulation, including phases,
orientations, and fluctuations in the course of goal pursuit. Finally, we discussed
key research on self-control, including the antecedents and processes involved
in overcoming temptation.
1783
Discussion Questions
1. What is the difference between goal and motivation?
2. What is the difference between self-regulation and self-control?
3. How do positive and negative feelings inform goal pursuit in a cybernetic
self-regulation process?
4. Describe the characteristics of the deliberative mindset that allows
individuals to decide between different goals. How might these
characteristics hinder the implemental phase of self-regulation?
5. You just read a chapter on Goals and Motivation, and you believe it is a
sign of commitment to the goal of learning about social psychology. Define
commitment in this context. How would interpreting your efforts as a sign
of commitment influence your motivation to read more about social
psychology? By contrast, how would interpreting your efforts as a sign of
progress influence your motivation to read more?
6. Mel and Alex are friends. Mel has a prevention focus self-regulatory
orientation, whereas Alex has a promotion focus. They are both training for
a marathon and are looking for motivational posters to hang in their
respective apartments. While shopping, they find a poster with the following
Confucius quote: The will to win, the desire to succeed, the urge to reach
your full potential ... . These are the keys that will unlock the door to personal
excellence. Who is this poster more likely to help stay motivated for the
marathon (Mel or Alex)? Why? Find or write a quote that might help the
other friend.
Vocabulary
Balancing between goals
Shifting between a focal goal and other goals or temptations by putting less
effort into the focal goalusually with the intention of coming back to the focal
goal at a later point in time.
Commitment
The sense that a goal is both valuable and attainable
Deliberative phase
The first of the two basic stages of self-regulation in which individuals decide
which of many potential goals to pursue at a given point in time.
Ego-depletion
The exhaustion of physiological and/or psychological resources following the
completion of effortful self-control tasks, which subsequently leads to reduction
in the capacity to exert more self-control.
Extrinsic motivation
Motivation stemming from the benefits associated with achieving a goal such
as obtaining a monetary reward.
Goal
The cognitive representation of a desired state (outcome).
Goal priming
The activation of a goal following exposure to cues in the immediate
environment related to the goal or its corresponding means (e.g., images,
words, sounds).
Highlighting a goal
Prioritizing a focal goal over other goals or temptations by putting more effort
into the focal goal.
Implemental phase
The second of the two basic stages of self-regulation in which individuals plan
specific actions related to their selected goal.
Intrinsic motivation
Motivation stemming from the benefits associated with the process of pursuing
a goal such as having a fulfilling experience.
Means
Activities or objects that contribute to goal attainment.
Motivation
The psychological driving force that enables action in the course of goal pursuit.
Nonconscious goal activation
When activation occurs outside a persons awareness, such that the person is
unaware of the reasons behind her goal-directed thoughts and behaviors.
Prevention focus
One of two self-regulatory orientations emphasizing safety, responsibility, and
security needs, and viewing goals as oughts. This self-regulatory focus seeks
to avoid losses (the presence of negatives) and approach non-losses (the
absence of negatives).
Progress
The perception of reducing the discrepancy between ones current state and
ones desired state in goal pursuit.
Promotion focus
One of two self-regulatory orientations emphasizing hopes, accomplishments,
and advancement needs, and viewing goals as ideals. This self-regulatory
focus seeks to approach gains (the presence of positives) and avoid non-gains
(the absence of positives).
Self-control
The capacity to control impulses, emotions, desires, and actions in order to
resist a temptation and adhere to a valued goal.
Self-regulation
The processes through which individuals alter their emotions, desires, and
actions in the course of pursuing a goal.
Reference List
Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2006). See what you want to see: Motivational
influences on visual perception. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 91(4), 612625.
Bargh, J. A. (1990). Conditional automaticity. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,
28(6), 486486.
Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Trotschel, R. (2001).
The automated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral
goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 10141027.
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being.
American Psychologist, 54(7), 462479.
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion:
Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 74(5), 12521265.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New
York, NY: Cambridge University Press
Custers, R., Aarts, H., Oikawa, M., & Elliot, A. (2009). The nonconscious road to
perceptions of performance: Achievement priming augments outcome
expectancies and experienced self-agency. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 45(6), 12001208.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). The general causality orientations scaleSelfdetermination in personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 19(2), 109
134.
Ferguson, M. J., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). Liking is for doing: The effects of goal pursuit
on automatic evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87
(5), 557572.
Fishbach, A., Friedman, R. S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Leading us not unto
temptation: Momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 296309.
Fishbach, A., Zhang, Y., & Koo, M. (2009). The dynamics of self-regulation.
European Review of Social Psychology, 20, 15344.
Fishbach, A., & Converse, B. A. (2010). Identifying and battling temptation. In K.
D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research,
theory, and applications (2nd ed., pp. 244260). New York, NY: Guilford
Press.
Fishbach, A., & Ferguson, M. F. (2007). The goal construct in social psychology.
In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of
basic principles (pp. 490515). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Fishbach, A. & Trope, Y., (2007). Implicit and explicit mechanisms of
counteractive self-control. In J. Shah and W. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of
motivation science (pp. 281294). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1974). Attitudes toward objects as predictors of single
and multiple behavioral criteria. Psychological Review, 81(1), 5974.
Frster, J., Liberman, N., & Friedman, R. S. (2007). Seven principles of goal
Liberman, N., & Frster, J. (2008). Expectancy, value and psychological distance:
A new look at goal gradients. Social Cognition, 26(5), 515533.
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of
behavior. New York, NY: Henry Holt.
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratification in children.
Science, 244(4907), 933938.
Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The control of perception. Oxford, UK: Aldine.
Rachlin, H. (2000). The science of self-control. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Read, D., Loewenstein, G., & Kalyanaraman, S. (1999). Mixing virtue and vice:
Combining the immediacy effect and the diversification heuristic. Journal
of Behavioral Decision Making, 12(4), 257273.
Read, D., Loewenstein, G., & Rabin, M. (1999). Choice bracketing. Journal of Risk
and Uncertainty, 19(1-3), 171197.
Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1979). Role of category accessibility in the interpretation
of information about personsSome determinants and implications.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(10), 16601672.
Thaler, R. H., & Shefrin, H. M. (1981). An economic-theory of self-control. Journal
of Political Economy, 89(2), 392406.
Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-regulatory failure: A resourcedepletion approach. Psychological Science, 11(3), 249254.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Motives and Goals by Ayelet Fishbach and
Maferima Tour-Tillery is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercialShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Drive States
Sudeep Bhatia & George Loewenstein
Carnegie Mellon University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Our thoughts and behaviors are strongly influenced by affective experiences
known as drive states. These drive states motivate us to fulfill goals that are
beneficial to our survival and reproduction. This chapter provides an overview
of key drive states, including information about their neurobiology and their
psychological effects.
Learning Objectives
Outline the neurobiological basis of drive states such as hunger and arousal.
Introduction
What is the longest youve gone without eating? A couple of hours? An entire
day? How did it feel? Humans rely critically on food for nutrition and energy,
and the absence of food can create drastic changes not only in physical
appearance, but in thoughts and behaviors. If you fasted for a day, you probably
noticed how hunger can take over your mind: Direct your attention to the foods
that you could be eating (a cheesy slice of pizza, or perhaps some sweet and
cold ice cream), and motivate you to obtain and consume these foods. And
once you have eaten and your hunger has satisfied, your thoughts and
behaviors return to normal.
Hunger is a drive state, an affective experience that motivates organisms
to fulfill goals that are generally beneficial to their survival and reproduction.
Like other drive states, such as thirst or sexual arousal, hunger has a profound
impact on the functioning of the mind. It affects psychological processes, such
as perception, attention, emotion, and motivation, and influences the behaviors
that these processes generate.
1798
Different drive states have different triggers. Most drive states respond to
both internal and external cues, but the mix of internal and external, and the
specific cues, differ between drives. Hunger, for example, depends on internal,
visceral signals as well as sensory signals such as the sight or smell of tasty
food. Different drive states also result in different cognitive and feeling states
and are associated with different behaviors. Yet despite these differences, there
are a number of properties common to all drive states.
Homeostasis
Humans, like all organisms, need to maintain a stable state in their various
physiological systems. For example, the excessive loss of body water results in
dehydration, a dangerous and potentially fatal state, but too much water can
be damaging as well: A moderate and stable level of body fluid is ideal. The
tendency of an organism to maintain this stability across all the different
physiological systems in the body is called homeostasis.
Homeostasis consists of two main ingredients. First, the state of the system
being regulated must be monitored and compared to an ideal level, or a set
point. Second, there need to be mechanisms for moving the system back to
this set point, that is, to restore homeostasis, when deviations from it are
detected.
Many homeostatic mechanisms, such as blood circulation and immune
responses, are automatic and nonconscious. Others, however, involve
deliberate action. Most drive states motivate action to restore homeostasis
using both a carrot and a stick. The stick is the bad feeling, such as hunger,
thirst, or the misery of cold or heat, that occurs when one departs from the set
point. The carrot is the pleasure derived from any activity that moves the system
back toward the set point. For example, when body temperature declines below
the set point, any activity that helps to restore homeostasis, such as putting
ones hand in warm water, feels pleasurable, and likewise, when the body
nobaproject.com - Drive States
1799
temperature is above the set point, anything that cools it feels pleasurable.
1800
Hunger
Hunger is a paradigmatic drive state that results in thoughts and behaviors
related to the consumption of food. Hunger is generally triggered by low glucose
levels in the blood (Rolls, 2000), and behaviors resulting from hunger aim to
restore homeostasis regarding glucose and its presence in the body. Various
other internal and external cues can also cause hunger. For example, the
chemical composition of the food training from the stomach serves as an
internal cue for the body to initiate the search for food (Greenberg, Smith, &
Gibbs, 1990). External cues include the time of day, estimated time until the
next feeding (hunger increases immediately prior to food consumption), and
nobaproject.com - Drive States
1801
the sight, smell, taste, and even touch of food and food-related stimuli. Note
that while hunger is a generic feeling, it has nuances that can produce the eating
of specific foods that correct for nutritional imbalances, which we may not be
conscious of.
The hypothalamus plays a very important role in eating behavior. It is
responsible for synthesizing and secreting various hormones. The lateral
hypothalamus (LH) is concerned largely with hunger. Lesions of the LH can
eliminate all eating to the point where animals will starve to death unless kept
alive by force feeding (Anand & Brobeck, 1951). Additionally, artificially
stimulating the LH, using electrical currents, can generate eating behavior if
food is available (Andersson, 1951).
Activation of the LH cannot only increase the desirability of food but can
also reduce the desirability of nonfood-related items. Brendl, Markman, and
Messner (2003), for example, found that participants who were given a handful
of popcorn to trigger hunger not only had higher ratings of food products, but
also had lower ratings of nonfood products, compared with participants whose
appetites were not similarly primed.
Hunger is only part of the story of when and why we eat. An analogous
process, satiation, relates to the decline of hunger and the eventual termination
of eating behavior. In fact, hunger and satiation are two distinct processes,
controlled by different circuits in the brain and triggered by different cues.
Distinct from the LH, which plays an important role in hunger, the ventromedial
hypothalamus (VMH) plays an important role in satiety. While lesions of the
VMH can make an animal overeat to the point of obesity, the relationship
between the LH and the VMB is quite complicated. Rats with VMH lesions can
be quite finicky about their food (Teitelbaum, 1955).
Other brain areas, besides the LH and VMH, also play important roles in
eating behavior. The sensory cortices (visual, olfactory, and taste), for example,
are important in identifying food items. These areas provide informational value
and not hedonic evaluations. While many of their functions are roughly stable
nobaproject.com - Drive States
1802
Sexual Arousal
A second drive state, especially critical to reproduction, is sexual arousal. Sexual
arousal results in thoughts and behaviors related to sexual activity. As with
hunger, it is generated by a large range of internal and external mechanisms
that are triggered either after the extended absence of sexual activity or by the
immediate presence and possibility of sexual activity (or by cues commonly
associated with such possibilities). Unlike hunger, however, these mechanisms
can differ substantially between males and females, indicating important
evolutionary difference in the biological functions that sexual arousal serves
for different sexes.
Sexual arousal and pleasure in males, for example, is strongly related to the
preoptic area, a region in the anterior hypothalamus. If the preoptic area is
damaged, male sexual behavior is severely impaired. Interestingly, damage to
the preoptic area does not affect certain types of sexual motivations. For
example, rats that have had prior sexual experiences will still seek out sexual
partners after their preoptic area is lesioned. However, once having secured a
sexual partner, rats with lesioned preoptic areas will show no further inclination
to actually initiate sex.
nobaproject.com - Drive States
1803
1804
Conclusion
Drive states are evolved motivational mechanisms designed to ensure that
organisms take self-beneficial actions. In this chapter, we have reviewed key
properties of drive states, such as homeostasis and the narrowing of attention.
We have also discussed, in detail, two important drive stateshunger and
sexual arousaland explored their underlying neurobiology and the ways in
which various environmental and biological factors affect their properties.
There are many drive states, besides hunger and sexual arousal, which affect
humans on a daily basis. Fear, thirst, exhaustion, exploratory and maternal
drives, and drug cravings are all drive states that have been studied by
researchers (see e.g., Buck, 1999; Van Boven & Loewenstein, 2003). While these
drive states share the properties discussed in this chapter, each also has unique
features that allows it to effectively fulfill its evolutionary functions.
One key difference between drive states is the extent to which they are
triggered by internal as opposed to external stimuli. Thirst, for example, is
induced both by decreased fluid levels and an increased concentration of salt
in the body. Fear, on the other hand, is induced by perceived threats in the
external environment. Drug cravings are triggered both by internal homeostatic
mechanisms and by external visual, olfactory, and contextual cues. Other drive
states, such as those pertaining to maternity, are triggered by specific events
in the organisms life. Differences such as these make the study of drive states
a scientifically interesting and important endeavor. Drive states are rich in their
diversity, and many questions involving their neurocognitive underpinnings,
environmental determinants, and behavioral effects, have yet to be answered.
One final thing to consider, not discussed in this chapter, relates to the realworld consequences of drive states. Hunger, sexual arousal, and other drive
states are all psychological mechanisms that have evolved gradually over
millions of years. We share these drive states not only with our human ancestors
but with animals such as monkeys, dogs, and rats. It is not surprising then that
nobaproject.com - Drive States
1805
these drive states, at times, lead us to behave in ways that are ill-suited to our
modern lives. Consider, for example, the obesity epidemic that is affecting
countries around the world. Like other diseases of affluence, obesity is a product
of drive states that are too easily fulfilled: homeostatic mechanisms that worked
well when food was scarce backfire when a meal rich in fat and sugar is readily
available. Unrestricted sexual arousal can have a similarly perverse effect on
our well-being. Countless politicians have sacrificed their entire lifes work (not
to mention their marriages) by satisfying adulterous sexual impulses toward
colleagues, staffers, prostitutes, and others over whom they have social or
financial power. It not an overstatement to say that many problems of the 21st
century, from school massacres to obesity to drug addiction, are influenced by
the mismatch between our drive states and our uniquely modern ability to fulfill
them at a moments notice.
1806
Outside Resources
Web: An open textbook chapter on homeostasis
http://en.wikibooks.org/wiki/Human_Physiology/Homeostasis
Web: Motivation and emotion in psychology
http://allpsych.com/psychology101/motivation_emotion.html
Web: The science of sexual arousal
http://www.apa.org/monitor/apr03/arousal.aspx
Discussion Questions
1. The ability to maintain homeostasis is important for an organisms survival.
What are the ways in which homeostasis ensures survival? Do different drive
states accomplish homeostatic goals differently?
2. Drive states result in the narrowing of attention toward the present and
toward the self. Which drive states lead to the most pronounced narrowing
of attention toward the present? Which drive states lead to the most
pronounced narrowing of attention toward the self?
3. What are important differences between hunger and sexual arousal, and
in what ways do these differences reflect the biological needs that hunger
and sexual arousal have been evolved to address?
4. Some of the properties of sexual arousal vary across males and females.
What other drives states affect males and females differently? Are there
drive states that vary with other differences in humans (e.g., age)?
Vocabulary
Drive state
Affective experiences that motivate organisms to fulfill goals that are generally
beneficial to their survival and reproduction.
Homeostasis
The tendency of an organism to maintain a stable state across all the different
physiological systems in the body.
Homeostatic set point
An ideal level that the system being regulated must be monitored and compared
to.
Hypothalamus
A portion of the brain involved in a variety of functions, including the secretion
of various hormones and the regulation of hunger and sexual arousal.
Lordosis
A physical sexual posture in females that serves as an invitation to mate.
Preoptic area
A region in the anterior hypothalamus involved in generating and regulating
male sexual behavior.
Reward value
A neuropsychological measure of an outcomes affective importance to an
organism.
Reference List
Anand, B. K., & Brobeck, J. R. (1951). Hypothalamic control of food intake in rats
and cats. The Yale journal of biology and medicine, 24(2), 123.
Andersson, B. (1951). The effect and localization of electrical stimulation of
certain parts of the brain stem in sheep and goats. Acta Physiologica
Scandinavica, 23(1), 823.
Ariely, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2006). The heat of the moment: The effect of sexual
arousal on sexual decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making,
19(2), 8798
Biderman, A. D. (1960). Social-psychological needs and involuntary behavior
as illustrated by compliance in interrogation. Sociometry, 23(2), 120147.
Brendl, C. M., Markman, A. B., & Messner, C. (2003). The devaluation effect:
Activating a need devalues unrelated objects. Journal of Consumer
Research, 29(4), 463473.
Buck, R. (1999). The biological affects: A typology. Psychological Review, 106,
301336.
Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the
organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66(3).
Gawin, F. H. (1991). Cocaine addiction: psychology and neurophysiology.
Science, 251(5001), 15801586.
Giordano, L. A., Bickel, W. K., Loewenstein, G., Jacobs, E. A., Marsch, L., & Badger,
G. J. (2002). Mild opioid deprivation increases the degree that opioiddependent outpatients discount delayed heroin and money. Psychopharmacology,
163(2), 174182.
Greenberg, D., Smith, G. P., & Gibbs, J. (1990). Intraduodenal infusions of fats
elicit satiety in sham-feeding rats. American Journal of PhysiologyRegulatory, Integrative, and Comparative Physiology, 259(1), 110118.
Heath, R. G. (1964). Pleasure response of human subjects to direct stimulation
of the brain: Physiologic and psychodynamic considerations. In R. G. Heath
(Ed.), The role of pleasure in behavior (pp. 219243). New York, NY: Hoeber.
Kow, L. M., & Pfaff, D. W. (1998). Mapping of neural and signal transduction
pathways for lordosis in the search for estrogen actions on the central
nervous system. Behavioural Brain Research, 92(2), 169180
Olds, J., & Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical
stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47(6), 419.
Panksepp, J. (2004). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and
animal emotions (Vol. 4). New York, NY: Oxford University Press U.S.
Rolls, E. T. (2000). The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3),
284-294.
Teitelbaum, P. (1955). Sensory control of hypothalamic hyperphagia. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 48(3), 156.
Van Boven, L., & Loewenstein, G. (2003). Projection of transient drive states.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Drive States by Sudeep Bhatia and George
Loewenstein is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Topic 10
Psychological Disorders
Abstract
This chapter is divided into three parts. The first is a brief introduction to various
criteria we use to define or distinguish between normality and abnormality.
The second, largest part is a history of mental illness from the Stone Age to the
20th century, with a special emphasis on the recurrence of three causal
explanations for mental illnesssupernatural, somatogenic, and psychogenic
factors. This part briefly touches upon trephination, the Greek theory of hysteria
within the context of the four bodily humors, witch hunts, asylums, moral
treatment,
mesmerism,
catharsis,
the
mental
hygiene
movement,
Learning Objectives
Explain the differences in treatment facilities for the mentally ill (e.g., mental
hospitals, asylums, community mental health centers).
Describe the reform efforts of Dix and Beers and the outcomes of their work.
1818
1819
suffered from too much blood and thus blood-letting would be the necessary
treatment. Hippocrates classified mental illness into one of four categories
epilepsy, mania, melancholia, and brain feverand like other prominent
physicians and philosophers of his time, he did not believe mental illness was
shameful or that mentally ill individuals should be held accountable for their
behavior. Mentally ill individuals were cared for at home by family members
and the state shared no responsibility for their care. Humorism remained a
recurrent somatogenic theory up until the 19th century.
While Greek physician Galen (AD 130201) rejected the notion of a uterus
having an animistic soul, he agreed with the notion that an imbalance of the
four bodily fluids could cause mental illness. He also opened the door for
psychogenic explanations for mental illness, however, by allowing for the
experience of psychological stress as a potential cause of abnormality. Galens
psychogenic theories were ignored for centuries, however, as physicians
attributed mental illness to physical causes throughout most of the millennium.
By the late Middle Ages, economic and political turmoil threatened the
power of the Roman Catholic church. Between the 11th and 15th centuries,
supernatural theories of mental disorders again dominated Europe, fueled by
natural disasters like plagues and famines that lay people interpreted as
brought about by the devil. Superstition, astrology, and alchemy took hold, and
common treatments included prayer rites, relic touching, confessions, and
atonement. Beginning in the 13th century the mentally ill, especially women,
began to be persecuted as witches who were possessed. At the height of the
witch hunts during the 15th through 17th centuries, with the Protestant
Reformation having plunged Europe into religious strife, two Dominican monks
wrote the Malleus Maleficarum (1486) as the ultimate manual to guide witch
hunts. Johann Weyer and Reginald Scot tried to convince people in the mid- to
late-16th century that accused witches were actually women with mental
illnesses and that mental illness was not due to demonic possession but to
faulty metabolism and disease, but the Churchs Inquisition banned both of
nobaproject.com - History of Mental Illness
1820
their writings. Witch-hunting did not decline until the 17th and 18th centuries,
after more than 100,000 presumed witches had been burned at the stake
(Schoeneman, 1977; Zilboorg & Henry, 1941).
Modern treatments of mental illness are most associated with the
establishment of hospitals and asylums beginning in the 16th century. Such
institutions mission was to house and confine the mentally ill, the poor, the
homeless, the unemployed, and the criminal. War and economic depression
produced vast numbers of undesirables and these were separated from society
and sent to these institutions. Two of the most well-known institutions, St. Mary
of Bethlehem in London, known as Bedlam, and the Hpital Gnral of Paris
which included La Salptrire, La Piti, and La Bictrebegan housing mentally
ill patients in the mid-16th and 17th centuries. As confinement laws focused
on protecting the public from the mentally ill, governments became responsible
for housing and feeding undesirables in exchange for their personal liberty.
Most inmates were institutionalized against their will, lived in filth and chained
to walls, and were commonly exhibited to the public for a fee. Mental illness
was nonetheless viewed somatogenically, so treatments were similar to those
for physical illnesses: purges, bleedings, and emetics.
While inhumane by todays standards, the view of insanity at the time likened
the mentally ill to animals (i.e., animalism) who did not have the capacity to
reason, could not control themselves, were capable of violence without
provocation, did not have the same physical sensitivity to pain or temperature,
and could live in miserable conditions without complaint. As such, instilling fear
was believed to be the best way to restore a disordered mind to reason.
By the 18th century, protests rose over the conditions under which the
mentally ill lived, and the 18th and 19th centuries saw the growth of a more
humanitarian view of mental illness. In 1785 Italian physician Vincenzo
Chiarughi (17591820) removed the chains of patients at his St. Boniface
hospital in Florence, Italy, and encouraged good hygiene and recreational and
occupational training. More well known, French physician Philippe Pinel (1745
nobaproject.com - History of Mental Illness
1821
1822
1870s and especially on the early 20th century discoveries of vaccines for
cholera, syphilis, and typhus, the mental hygiene movement reverted to a
somatogenic theory of mental illness.
European psychiatry in the late 18th century and throughout the 19th
century,
however,
struggled
between
somatogenic
and
psychogenic
1823
their efficacy in treating mental illness is due to factors shared among all of the
approaches (not particular elements specific to each approach): the therapistpatient alliance, the therapists allegiance to the therapy, therapist competence,
and placebo effects (Luborsky et al., 2002; Messer & Wampold, 2002).
In contrast, the leading somatogenic treatment for mental illness can be
found in the establishment of the first psychotropic medications in the mid-20th
century. Restraints, electro-convulsive shock therapy, and lobotomies
continued to be employed in American state institutions until the 1970s, but
they quickly made way for a burgeoning pharmaceutical industry that has
viewed and treated mental illness as a chemical imbalance in the brain.
Both etiological theories coexist today in what the psychological discipline
holds as the biopsychosocial model of explaining human behavior. While
individuals may be born with a genetic predisposition for a certain psychological
disorder, certain psychological stressors need to be present for them to develop
the disorder. Sociocultural factors such as sociopolitical or economic unrest,
poor living conditions, or problematic interpersonal relationships are also
viewed as contributing factors. However much we want to believe that we are
above the treatments described above, or that the present is always the most
enlightened time, let us not forget that our thinking today continues to reflect
the same underlying somatogenic and psychogenic theories of mental illness
discussed throughout this cursory 9,000-year history.
1824
1825
Outside Resources
Web: Images from the History of Medicine. Search "mental illness"
http://ihm.nlm.nih.gov/luna/servlet/view/all
Web: Science Museum Brought to Life
http://www.sciencemuseum.org.uk/broughttolife/themes/menalhealthandillness.
aspx
Web: The UCL Center for the History of Medicine
http://www.ucl.ac.uk/histmed/
Web: The Wellcome Library. Search "mental illness".
http://wellcomelibrary.org/
Web: US National Library of Medicine
http://vsearch.nlm.nih.gov/vivisimo/cgi-bin/query-meta?query=mental+illness&v:
project=nlm-main-website
Discussion Questions
1. What does it mean to say that someone is mentally ill? What criteria are
usually considered to determine whether someone is mentally ill?
2. Describe the difference between supernatural, somatogenic, and
psychogenic theories of mental illness and how subscribing to a particular
etiological theory determines the type of treatment used.
3. How did the Greeks describe hysteria and what treatment did they
prescribe?
4. Describe humorism and how it explained mental illness.
5. Describe how the witch hunts came about and their relationship to mental
illness.
6. Describe the development of treatment facilities for the mentally insane,
from asylums to community mental health centers.
7. Describe the humane treatment of the mentally ill brought about by
Chiarughi, Pinel, and Tuke in the late 18th and early 19th centuries and how
it differed from the care provided in the centuries preceding it.
8. Describe William Tukes treatment of the mentally ill at the York Retreat
within the context of the Quaker Society of Friends. What influence did Tukes
treatment have in other parts of the world?
9. What are the 20th-century treatments resulting from the psychogenic and
somatogenic theories of mental illness?
10. Describe why a classification system is important and how the leading
classification system used in the United States works. Describe some
concerns with regard to this system.
Vocabulary
Animism
The belief that everyone and everything had a soul and that mental illness
was due to animistic causes, for example, evil spirits controlling an individual
and his/her behavior.
Asylum
A place of refuge or safety established to confine and care for the mentally ill;
forerunners of the mental hospital or psychiatric facility.
Biopsychosocial model
A model in which the interaction of biological, psychological, and sociocultural
factors is seen as influencing the development of the individual.
Cathartic method
A therapeutic procedure introduced by Breuer and developed further by Freud
in the late 19th century whereby a patient gains insight and emotional relief
from recalling and reliving traumatic events.
Cultural relativism
The idea that cultural norms and values of a society can only be understood on
their own terms or in their own context.
Etiology
The causal description of all of the factors that contribute to the development
of a disorder or illness.
Hysteria
Term used by the ancient Greeks and Egyptians to describe a disorder believed
to be caused by a womans uterus wandering throughout the body and
interfering with other organs (today referred to as conversion disorder, in which
psychological problems are expressed in physical form).
Maladaptive
Term referring to behaviors that cause people who have them physical or
emotional harm, prevent them from functioning in daily life, and/or indicate
that they have lost touch with reality and/or cannot control their thoughts and
behavior (also called dysfunctional).
Mesmerism
Derived from Franz Anton Mesmer in the late 18th century, an early version of
hypnotism in which Mesmer claimed that hysterical symptoms could be treated
through animal magnetism emanating from Mesmers body and permeating
the universe (and later through magnets); later explained in terms of high
suggestibility in individuals.
Psychogenesis
Developing from psychological origins.
Somatogenesis
Developing from physical/bodily origins.
Supernatural
Developing from origins beyond the visible observable universe.
Syndrome
Involving a particular group of signs and symptoms.
Traitement moral (moral treatment)
A therapeutic regimen of improved nutrition, living conditions, and rewards for
productive behavior that has been attributed to Philippe Pinel during the French
Revolution, when he released mentally ill patients from their restraints and
treated them with compassion and dignity rather than with contempt and
denigration.
Trephination
The drilling of a hole in the skull, presumably as a way of treating psychological
disorders.
Reference List
Bell, L. V. (1980). Treating the mentally ill: From colonial times to the present.
New York: Praeger.
Forrest, D. (1999). Hypnotism: A history. New York: Penguin.
Grob, G. N. (1994). The mad among us: A history of the care of Americas mentally
ill. New York: Free Press.
Luborsky, L., Rosenthal, R., Diguer, L., Andrusyna, T. P., Berman, J. S., Levitt, J.
T., . . . Krause, E. D. (2002). The dodo bird verdict is alive and wellmostly.
Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 9, 212.
Messer, S. B., & Wampold, B. E. (2002). Let's face facts: Common factors are
more potent than specific therapy ingredients. Clinical Psychology: Science
and Practice, 9(1), 2125.
Micale, M. S. (1985). The Salptrire in the age of Charcot: An institutional
perspective on medical history in the late nineteenth century. Journal of
Contemporary History, 20, 703731.
Restak, R. (2000). Mysteries of the mind. Washington, DC: National Geographic
Society.
Schoeneman, T. J. (1977). The role of mental illness in the European witch hunts
of the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries: An assessment. Journal of the
History of the Behavioral Sciences, 13(4), 337351.
Tseng, W. (1973). The development of psychiatric concepts in traditional Chinese
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. History of Mental Illness by Ingrid G. Farreras is
licensed
under
the
Creative
Commons
Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0
Therapeutic Orientations
Hannah Boettcher, Stefan G. Hofmann & Q. Jade Wu
Boston University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
In the past century, a number of psychotherapeutic orientations have gained
popularity for treating mental illnesses. This chapter outlines some of the bestknown therapeutic approaches and explains the history, techniques,
advantages, and disadvantages associated with each. The most effective
modern approach is cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT). We also discuss
psychoanalytic therapy, person-centered therapy, and mindfulness-based
approaches. Drug therapy and emerging new treatment strategies will also be
briefly explored.
Learning Objectives
For each therapeutic approach, consider: history, goals, key techniques, and
empirical support.
Introduction
Nearly half of all Americans will experience mental illness at some point in their
lives, and mental health problems affect more than one-quarter of the
population in any given year (Kessler et al., 2005). Fortunately, a range of
psychotherapies, primarily cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) approaches, exist
to treat mental illnesses.
This chapter provides an overview of some of the best-known schools of
thought in psychotherapy. The most effective contemporary approach is CBT.
Other approaches are psychoanalytic therapies, person-centered therapies,
and acceptance and mindfulness-based therapies. The effectiveness of these
treatments is not as clear as they are for CBT. Sometimes, adding psychiatric
medications can enhance the effects of psychotherapies. As you read, note the
advantages and disadvantages of each approach, and consider the degree to
which they have demonstrated effectiveness through empirical research.
1838
Techniques in Psychoanalysis
Psychoanalysts and psychodynamic therapists employ several techniques to
explore patients unconscious mind. During free association, the patient shares
any and all thoughts that come to mind, without attempting to organize or
censor these thoughts in any way. The analyst uses his or her expertise to
discern patterns or underlying meaning in the patients thoughts. Another
common approach is to discuss childhood relationships with caregivers, which
are thought to determine the way a person relates to others and predict later
psychiatric difficulties. Psychoanalysts may also discuss their patients dreams.
nobaproject.com - Therapeutic Orientations
1839
In Freudian theory, dreams contain not only manifest or actual content, but
also latent content, or symbolic underlying meaning that can be discovered
through dream analysis (Freud, 1900/1955).
In psychoanalytic and psychodynamic therapy, the therapist plays a
receptive roleinterpreting the patients thoughts and behavior based on
clinical experience and psychoanalytic theory. Sometimes, psychoanalytic
therapists will seat patients facing away from them to promote freer selfdisclosure. Freudian theory suggests that patients may displace feelings for
people in their lives onto the therapist, a process known as transference, and
that
therapists
may
also
displace
emotions
onto
patients,
called
countertransference. The therapist views his or her relationship with the patient
as another reflection of the patients mind.
Advantages
and
Disadvantages
of
Psychoanalytic
Therapy
Psychoanalysis was once the only type of psychotherapy available, but presently
the number of therapists practicing this approach is decreasing in the United
States and around the world. Psychoanalysis is not appropriate for some types
of patients, including those with severe psychopathology or mental retardation.
Further, psychoanalysis is often expensive because treatment usually lasts
many years. Still, some patients and therapists find the prolonged and detailed
analysis very rewarding.
Perhaps the greatest disadvantage of psychoanalysis and related
approaches is the lack of empirical support for their effectiveness. The limited
research that has been conducted on these treatments suggests that they do
not reliably lead to better mental health outcomes (e.g., Driessen et al., 2010).
More recently, some review studies seem to suggest that long-term
psychodynamic therapies might be beneficial (e.g., Leichsenring & Rabung,
2008). However, other authors have questioned the validity of this review.
nobaproject.com - Therapeutic Orientations
1840
1841
1842
1843
Techniques in CBT
Beck and Ellis strove to help patients identify maladaptive appraisalsthose
that were unhelpful or contributed to a mental health problemand substitute
more adaptive appraisals. This technique of reappraisal, or cognitive
restructuring, became a fundamental aspect of CBT. Another important
treatment target of CBT is the clients maladaptive behavior that maintains the
psychopathology. In treatment, therapists and patients work together to
develop healthy behavioral habits. For many mental health problems, especially
anxiety disorders, CBT incorporates what is known as exposure therapy. During
exposure therapy, a patient confronts a problematic situation and remains fully
engaged in the experience instead of avoiding it. The goal is to reduce fear
associated with the situation through extinction learning, a neurobiological and
cognitive process by which the patient unlearns the irrational fear. For
example, exposure therapy for someone with social anxiety might involve giving
formal presentations to a group of strangers. After repeated exposures, he or
she will experience less physiological fear and maladaptive thoughts about
public speaking, breaking the cycle of avoidance.
1844
1845
1846
artery disease, and fibromyalgia (Hofmann, Sawyer, Witt & Oh, 2010).
Mindfulness and acceptance figure prominently into some cognitivebehavioral therapies, particularly dialectical behavior therapy (DBT) (e.g.,
Linehan, Amstrong, Suarez, Allmon, & Heard, 1991). DBT is often used in the
treatment of borderline personality disorder, and it is founded on the dialectical
worldview of incorporating both acceptance and change in therapeutic
progress. DBT therapists employ cognitive-behavioral techniques as well as
mindfulness exercises.
Another form of treatment that also uses mindfulness techniques is
acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) (Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999).
This intervention is based on Skinnerian behaviorism. In this treatment, patients
are taught to observe their thoughts from a detached perspective (Hayes et al.,
1999). ACT encourages patients not to attempt changing or avoiding what
thoughts and emotions they observe within themselves. However, the
differences among ACT, CBT, and other mindfulness-based treatments are a
topic of current controversy in the literature.
1847
1848
Pharmacological Treatments
Psychiatric medications are frequently used as a treatment for mental
disorders, including schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, depression, and anxiety
disorders. Psychological drugs are commonly used, in part, because they can
be prescribed by general practitioners, whereas only trained professionals are
qualified to deliver effective psychotherapy. While drugs and CBT therapists
tend to be almost equally effective, choosing the best intervention depends on
the disorder and individual being treated, as well as other factors, such as
treatment availability and comorbidity(i.e., having multiple mental or physical
disorders at once). Although many new types of drugs have been introduced
in recent decades, there is still much we do not understand about their
mechanism in the brain. Further research efforts are needed to refine our
understanding of both pharmacological and behavioral treatments.
Conclusion
Several schools of thought have emerged for treating mental health problems.
Cognitive behavioral therapy is the treatment with the most empirical support.
Other popular, but less supported approaches, include psychodynamic
therapies, person-centered therapy, mindfulness-based treatments, and
acceptance and commitment therapy. Recent advances in research and
nobaproject.com - Therapeutic Orientations
1849
technology are allowing clinicians to treat more patients more effectively than
ever before.
1850
Discussion Questions
1. Psychoanalytic theory is no longer the dominant therapeutic approach,
because it lacks empirical support. Yet many consumers continue to seek
psychoanalytic or psychodynamic treatments. Do you think psychoanalysis
still has a place in mental health treatment? If so, why?
2. What might be some advantages and disadvantages of technological
advances in psychological treatment? What will psychotherapy look like 100
years from now?
3. Some people have argued that all therapies are about equally effective, and
that they all affect change through common factors such as the involvement
of a supportive therapist. Does this claim sound reasonable to you? Why or
why not?
4. When choosing a psychological treatment for a specific patient, what factors
besides the treatments demonstrated efficacy should be taken into
account?
Vocabulary
Acceptance and commitment therapy
A therapeutic approach designed to foster nonjudgmental observation of ones
own mental processes.
Automatic thoughts
Thoughts that occur spontaneously; often used to describe problematic
thoughts that maintain mental disorders.
Comorbidity
Describes a state of having more than one psychological or physical disorder
at a given time.
Dialectical worldview
A perspective in DBT that emphasizes the joint importance of change and
acceptance.
Exposure therapy
A form of intervention in which the patient engages with a problematic (usually
feared) situation without avoidance or escape.
Free association
In psychodynamic therapy, a process in which the patient reports all thoughts
that come to mind without censorship, and these thoughts are interpreted by
the therapist.
Integrative or eclectic psychotherapy
Also called integrative psychotherapy, this term refers to approaches combining
multiple orientations (e.g., CBT with psychoanalytic elements).
Mindfulness
A process that reflects a nonjudgmental, yet attentive, mental state.
Mindfulness-based therapy
A form of psychotherapy grounded in mindfulness theory and practice, often
involving meditation, yoga, body scan, and other features of mindfulness
exercises.
Person-centered therapy
A therapeutic approach focused on creating a supportive environment for selfdiscovery.
Psychoanalytic therapy
Sigmund Freuds therapeutic approach focusing on resolving unconscious
conflicts.
Psychodynamic therapy
Treatment applying psychoanalytic principles in a briefer, more individualized
format.
Reappraisal, or Cognitive restructuring
The process of identifying, evaluating, and changing maladaptive thoughts in
psychotherapy.
Schema
A mental representation or set of beliefs about something.
Unconditional positive regard
In person-centered therapy, an attitude of warmth, empathy and acceptance
adopted by the therapist in order to foster feelings of inherent worth in the
patient.
Reference List
Baer, R. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual and
empirical review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 125143.
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., Segal,
Z. V., Abbey, S., Speca, M., Velting, D., & Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: A
proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice,
11, 230241.
Butler, A. C., Chapman, J. E., Forman, E. M., & Beck, A. T. (2006). The empirical
status of cognitive behavioral therapy: A review of meta-analyses. Clinical
Psychology Review, 26, 1731.
Cuijpers, P., Driessen, E., Hollon, S.D., van Oppen, P., Barth, J., & Andersson, G.
(2012). The efficacy of non-directive supportive therapy for adult
depression: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 280291.
Driessen, E., Cuijpers, P., de Maat, S. C. M., Abbass, A. A., de Jonghe, F., & Dekker,
J. J. M. (2010). The efficacy of short-term psychodynamic psychotherapy for
depression: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 2536.
Ellis, A. (1957). Rational psychotherapy and individual psychology. Journal of
Individual Psychology, 13, 3844.
Freud, S. (1955). Studies on hysteria. London, UK: Hogarth Press (Original work
published 1895).
Freud, S. (1955). The interpretation of dreams. London, UK: Hogarth Press
(Original work published 1900).
Freud. S. (1955). Beyond the pleasure principle. H London, UK: Hogarth Press
(Original work published 1920).
Friedli, K., King, M. B., Lloyd, M., & Horder, J. (1997). Randomized controlled
assessment of non-directive psychotherapy versus routine generalpractitioner care. Lancet, 350,
16621665.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and Commitment
Therapy. New
York, NY: Guilford Press.
Hofmann, S. G., Asnaani, A., Vonk, J. J., Sawyer, A. T., & Fang, A. (2012). The efficacy
of cognitive behavioral therapy: A review of meta-analyses. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 36, 427440.
Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A., & Oh, D. (2010). The effect of mindfulnessbased therapy on anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 169183
Kabat-Zinn J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present,
and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 144156.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic
pain patients
based
on
the
practice
of
mindfulness
meditation:
Theoretical
Schizophrenia Spectrum
Disorders
Deanna M. Barch
Washington University in St. Louis
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Schizophrenia and the other psychotic disorders are some of the most impairing
forms of psychopathology, frequently associated with a profound negative
effect on the individuals educational, occupational, and social function. Sadly,
these disorders often manifest right at time of the transition from adolescence
to adulthood, just as young people should be evolving into independent young
adults. The spectrum of psychotic disorders includes schizophrenia,
schizoaffective disorder, delusional disorder, schizotypal personality disorder,
schizophreniform disorder, brief psychotic disorder, as well as psychosis
associated with substance use or medical conditions. In this chapter, we
summarize the primary clinical features of these disorders, describe the known
cognitive and neurobiological changes associated with schizophrenia, describe
potential risk factors and/or causes for the development of schizophrenia, and
describe currently available treatments for schizophrenia.
Learning Objectives
1862
There are many different types or themes to delusions. The most common
delusions are persecutory and involve the belief that individuals or groups are
trying to hurt, harm, or plot against the person in some way. These can be
people that the person knows (people at work, the neighbors, family members),
or more abstract groups (the FBI, the CIA, aliens, etc.). Other types of delusions
include grandiose delusions, where the person believes that they have some
special power or ability (e.g., I am the new Buddha, I am a rock star); referential
delusions, where the person believes that events or objects in the environment
have special meaning for them (e.g., that song on the radio is being played
specifically for me); or other types of delusions where the person may believe
that others are controlling their thoughts and actions, their thoughts are being
broadcast aloud, or that others can read their mind (or they can read other
peoples minds).
When you see a person on the street talking to themselves or shouting at
other people, they are experiencing hallucinations. These are perceptual
experiences that occur even when there is no stimulus in the outside world
generating the experiences. They can be auditory, visual, olfactory (smell),
gustatory (taste), or somatic (touch). The most common hallucinations in
psychosis (at least in adults) are auditory, and can involve one or more voices
talking about the person, commenting on the persons behavior, or giving them
orders. The content of the hallucinations is frequently negative (you are a loser,
that drawing is stupid, you should go kill yourself) and can be the voice of
someone the person knows or a complete stranger. Sometimes the voices
sound as if they are coming from outside the persons head. Other times the
voices seem to be coming from inside the persons head, but are not
experienced the same as the persons inner thoughts or inner speech.
Talking to someone with schizophrenia is sometimes difficult, as their speech
may be difficult to follow, either because their answers do not clearly flow from
your questions, or because one sentence does not logically follow from another.
This is referred to as disorganized speech, and it can be present even when
nobaproject.com - Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders
1863
the person is writing. Disorganized behavior can include odd dress, odd
makeup (e.g., lipstick outlining a mouth for 1 inch), or unusual rituals (e.g.,
repetitive hand gestures). Abnormal motor behavior can include catatonia,
which refers to a variety of behaviors that seem to reflect a reduction in
responsiveness to the external environment. This can include holding unusual
postures for long periods of time, failing to respond to verbal or motor prompts
from another person, or excessive and seemingly purposeless motor activity.
Some of the most debilitating symptoms of schizophrenia are difficult for
others to see. These include what people refer to as negative symptoms or
the absence of certain things we typically expect most people to have. For
example, anhedonia or amotivation reflect a lack of apparent interest in or drive
to engage in social or recreational activities. These symptoms can manifest as
a great amount of time spent in physical immobility. Importantly, anhedonia
and amotivation do not seem to reflect a lack of enjoyment in pleasurable
activities or events (Cohen & Minor, 2010; Kring & Moran, 2008; Llerena, Strauss,
& Cohen, 2012) but rather a reduced drive or ability to take the steps necessary
to obtain the potentially positive outcomes (Barch & Dowd, 2010). Flat affect
and reduced speech (alogia) reflect a lack of showing emotions through facial
expressions, gestures, and speech intonation, as well as a reduced amount of
speech and increased pause frequency and duration.
In many ways, the types of symptoms associated with psychosis are the
most difficult for us to understand, as they may seem far outside the range of
our normal experiences. Unlike depression or anxiety, many of us may not have
had experiences that we think of as on the same continuum as psychosis.
However, just like many of the other forms of psychopathology described in
this book, the types of psychotic symptoms that characterize disorders like
schizophrenia are on a continuum with normal mental experiences. For
example, work by Jim van Os in the Netherlands has shown that a surprisingly
large percentage of the general population (10%+) experience psychotic-like
symptoms, though many fewer have multiple experiences and most will not
nobaproject.com - Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders
1864
continue to experience these symptoms in the long run (Verdoux & van Os,
2002). Similarly, work in a general population of adolescents and young adults
in Kenya has also shown that a relatively high percentage of individuals
experience one or more psychotic-like experiences (~19%) at some point in their
lives (Mamah et al., 2012; Ndetei et al., 2012), though again most will not go on
to develop a full-blown psychotic disorder.
Schizophrenia is the primary disorder that comes to mind when we discuss
psychotic disorders (see Table 1 for diagnostic criteria), though there are a
number of other disorders that share one or more features with schizophrenia.
In the remainder of this chapter, we will use the terms psychosis and
schizophrenia somewhat interchangeably, given that most of the research has
focused on schizophrenia. In addition to schizophrenia (see Table 1), other
psychotic disorders include schizophreniform disorder (a briefer version of
schizophrenia), schizoaffective disorder (a mixture of psychosis and
depression/mania symptoms), delusional disorder (the experience of only
delusions), and brief psychotic disorder (psychotic symptoms that last only a
few days or weeks).
1865
1866
1867
addition, people with more cognitive problems have worse function in everyday
life (Bowie et al., 2008; Bowie, Reichenberg, Patterson, Heaton, & Harvey, 2006;
Fett et al., 2011).
Some people with schizophrenia also show deficits in what is referred to as
social cognition, though it is not clear whether such problems are separate from
the cognitive problems described above or the result of them (Hoe, Nakagami,
Green, & Brekke, 2012; Kerr & Neale, 1993; van Hooren et al., 2008). This includes
problems with the recognition of emotional expressions on the faces of other
individuals (Kohler, Walker, Martin, Healey, & Moberg, 2010) and problems
inferring the intentions of other people (theory of mind) (Bora, Yucel, & Pantelis,
2009b). Individuals with schizophrenia who have more problems with social
cognition also tend to have more negative and disorganized symptoms
(Ventura, Wood, & Hellemann, 2011), as well as worse community function (Fett
et al., 2011).
The advent of neuroimaging techniques such as structural and functional
magnetic resonance imaging and positron emission tomography opened up
the ability to try to understand the brain mechanisms of the symptoms of
schizophrenia as well as the cognitive impairments found in psychosis. For
example, a number of studies have suggested that delusions in psychosis may
be associated with problems in salience detection mechanisms supported by
the ventral striatum (Jensen & Kapur, 2009; Jensen et al., 2008; Kapur, 2003;
Kapur, Mizrahi, & Li, 2005; Murray et al., 2008) and the anterior prefrontal cortex
(Corlett et al., 2006; Corlett, Honey, & Fletcher, 2007; Corlett, Murray, et al.,
2007a, 2007b). These are regions of the brain that normally increase their
activity when something important (aka salient) happens in the environment.
If these brain regions misfire, it may lead individuals with psychosis to
mistakenly attribute importance to irrelevant or unconnected events. Further,
there is good evidence that problems in working memory and cognitive control
in schizophrenia are related to problems in the function of a region of the brain
called the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) (Minzenberg, Laird, Thelen,
nobaproject.com - Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders
1868
Carter, & Glahn, 2009; Ragland et al., 2009). These problems include changes
in how the DLPFC works when people are doing working-memory or cognitivecontrol tasks, and problems with how this brain region is connected to other
brain regions important for working memory and cognitive control, including
the posterior parietal cortex (e.g., Karlsgodt et al., 2008; J. J. Kim et al., 2003;
Schlosser et al., 2003), the anterior cingulate (Repovs & Barch, 2012), and
temporal cortex (e.g., Fletcher et al., 1995; Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2001). In
terms of understanding episodic memory problems in schizophrenia, many
researchers have focused on medial temporal lobe deficits, with a specific focus
on the hippocampus (e.g., Heckers & Konradi, 2010). This is because there is
much data from humans and animals showing that the hippocampus is
important for the creation of new memories (Squire, 1992). However, it has
become increasingly clear that problems with the DLPFC also make important
contributions to episodic memory deficits in schizophrenia (Ragland et al.,
2009), probably because this part of the brain is important for controlling our
use of memory.
In addition to problems with regions such as the DLFPC and medial temporal
lobes in schizophrenia described above, magnitude resonance neuroimaging
studies have also identified changes in cellular architecture, white matter
connectivity, and gray matter volume in a variety of regions that include the
prefrontal and temporal cortices (Bora et al., 2011). People with schizophrenia
also show reduced overall brain volume, and reductions in brain volume as
people get older may be larger in those with schizophrenia than in healthy
people (Olabi et al., 2011). Taking antipsychotic medications or taking drugs
such as marijuana, alcohol, and tobacco may cause some of these structural
changes. However, these structural changes are not completely explained by
medications or substance use alone. Further, both functional and structural
brain changes are seen, again to a milder degree, in the first-degree relatives
of people with schizophrenia (Boos, Aleman, Cahn, Pol, & Kahn, 2007; Brans et
al., 2008; Fusar-Poli et al., 2007; MacDonald, Thermenos, Barch, & Seidman,
nobaproject.com - Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders
1869
2009). This again suggests that that neural changes associated with
schizophrenia are related to a genetic risk for this illness.
1870
you have other risk factors (Casadio, Fernandes, Murray, & Di Forti, 2011; Luzi,
Morrison, Powell, di Forti, & Murray, 2008). The likelihood of developing
schizophrenia is also higher for kids who grow up in urban settings (March et
al., 2008) and for some minority ethnic groups (Bourque, van der Ven, & Malla,
2011). Both of these factors may reflect higher social and environmental stress
in these settings. Unfortunately, none of these risk factors is specific enough
to be particularly useful in a clinical setting, and most people with these risk
factors do not develop schizophrenia. However, together they are beginning to
give us clues as the neurodevelopmental factors that may lead someone to be
at an increased risk for developing this disease.
An important research area on risk for psychosis has been work with
individuals who may be at clinical high risk. These are individuals who are
showing attenuated (milder) symptoms of psychosis that have developed
recently and who are experiencing some distress or disability associated with
these symptoms. When people with these types of symptoms are followed over
time, about 35% of them develop a psychotic disorder (T. D. Cannon et al., 2008),
most frequently schizophrenia (Fusar-Poli, McGuire, & Borgwardt, 2012). In
order to identify these individuals, a new category of diagnosis, called
Attenuated Psychotic Syndrome, was added to Section III (the section for
disorders in need of further study) of the DSM-V (see Table 1 for symptoms)
(APA, 2013). However, adding this diagnostic category to the DSM-V created a
good deal of controversy (Batstra & Frances, 2012; Fusar-Poli & Yung, 2012).
Many scientists and clinicians have been worried that including risk states in
the DSM-V would create mental disorders where none exist, that these
individuals are often already seeking treatment for other problems, and that it
is not clear that we have good treatments to stop these individuals from
developing to psychosis. However, the counterarguments have been that there
is evidence that individuals with high-risk symptoms develop psychosis at a
much higher rate than individuals with other types of psychiatric symptoms,
and that the inclusion of Attenuated Psychotic Syndrome in Section III will spur
nobaproject.com - Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders
1871
important research that might have clinical benefits. Further, there is some
evidence that non-invasive treatments such as omega-3 fatty acids and
intensive family intervention may help reduce the development of full-blown
psychosis (Preti & Cella, 2010) in people who have high-risk symptoms.
Treatment of Schizophrenia
The currently available treatments for schizophrenia leave much to be desired,
and the search for more effective treatments for both the psychotic symptoms
of schizophrenia (e.g., hallucinations and delusions) as well as cognitive deficits
and negative symptoms is a highly active area of research. The first line of
treatment for schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders is the use of
antipsychotic medications. There are two primary types of antipsychotic
medications, referred to as typical and atypical. The fact that typical
antipsychotics helped some symptoms of schizophrenia was discovered
serendipitously more than 60 years ago (Carpenter & Davis, 2012; Lopez-Munoz
et al., 2005). These are drugs that all share a common feature of being a strong
block of the D2 type dopamine receptor. Although these drugs can help reduce
hallucinations, delusions, and disorganized speech, they do little to improve
cognitive deficits or negative symptoms and can be associated with distressing
motor side effects. The newer generation of antipsychotics is referred to as
atypical antipsychotics. These drugs have more mixed mechanisms of action
in terms of the receptor types that they influence, though most of them also
influence D2 receptors. These newer antipsychotics are not necessarily more
helpful for schizophrenia but have fewer motor side effects. However, many of
the atypical antipsychotics are associated with side effects referred to as the
metabolic syndrome, which includes weight gain and increased risk for
cardiovascular illness, Type-2 diabetes, and mortality (Lieberman et al., 2005).
nobaproject.com - Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders
1872
1873
Outside Resources
Book: Ben Behind His Voices: One familys journal from the chaos of
schizophrenia to hope (2011). Randye Kaye. Rowman and Littlefield.
Book: Henrys Demons: Living with schizophrenia, a father and sons story
(2011). Henry and Patrick Cockburn. Scribner Macmillan.
Book: The Center Cannot Hold: My journey through madness (2008). Elyn R.
Saks. Hyperion.
Book: The Quiet Room: A journal out of the torment of madness (1996). Lori
Schiller. Grand Central Publishing.
Web: National Alliance for the Mentally Ill. This is an excellent site for learning
more about advocacy for individuals with major mental illnesses such as
schizophrenia.
http://www.nami.org/
Web: National Institute of Mental Health. This website has information on
NIMH-funded schizophrenia research.
http://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/topics/schizophrenia/index.shtml
Web: Schizophrenia Research Forum. This is an excellent website that
contains a broad array of information about current research on
schizophrenia.
http://www.schizophreniaforum.org/
Discussion Questions
1. Describe the major differences between the major psychotic disorders.
5. What are some of the factors associated with increased risk for developing
schizophrenia? If we know whether or not someone has these risk factors,
how well can we tell whether they will develop schizophrenia?
8. Are there any treatments besides antipsychotic medications that help any
of the symptoms of schizophrenia? If so, what are they?
Vocabulary
Alogia
A reduction in the amount of speech and/or increased pausing before the
initiation of speech.
Anhedonia/amotivation
A reduction in the drive or ability to take the steps or engage in actions necessary
to obtain the potentially positive outcome.
Catatonia
Behaviors that seem to reflect a reduction in responsiveness to the external
environment. This can include holding unusual postures for long periods of
time, failing to respond to verbal or motor prompts from another person, or
excessive and seemingly purposeless motor activity.
Delusions
False beliefs that are often fixed, hard to change even in the presence of
conflicting information, and often culturally influenced in their content.
Diagnostic criteria
The specific criteria used to determine whether an individual has a specific type
of psychiatric disorder. Commonly used diagnostic criteria are included in the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder, 5th Edition (DSM-V) and
the Internal Classification of Disorders, Version 9 (ICD-9).
Disorganized behavior
Behavior or dress that is outside the norm for almost all subcultures. This would
include odd dress, odd makeup (e.g., lipstick outlining a mouth for 1 inch), or
unusual rituals (e.g., repetitive hand gestures).
Disorganized speech
Speech that is difficult to follow, either because answers do not clearly follow
questions or because one sentence does not logically follow from another.
Dopamine
A neurotransmitter in the brain that is thought to play an important role in
regulating the function of other neurotransmitters.
Episodic memory
The ability to learn and retrieve new information or episodes in ones life.
Flat affect
A reduction in the display of emotions through facial expressions, gestures, and
speech intonation.
Functional capacity
The ability to engage in self-care (cook, clean, bathe), work, attend school, and/
or engage in social relationships.
Hallucinations
Perceptual experiences that occur even when there is no stimulus in the outside
world generating the experiences. They can be auditory, visual, olfactory (smell),
gustatory (taste), or somatic (touch).
Magnetic resonance imaging
A set of techniques that uses strong magnets to measure either the structure
of the brain (e.g., gray matter and white matter) or how the brain functions
when a person performs cognitive tasks (e.g., working memory or episodic
memory) or other types of tasks.
Neurodevelopmental
Processes that influence how the brain develops either in utero or as the child
is growing up.
Positron emission tomography
A technique that uses radio-labelled ligands to measure the distribution of
different neurotransmitter receptors in the brain or to measure how much of
a certain type of neurotransmitter is released when a person is given a specific
type of drug or does a particularly cognitive task.
Processing speed
The speed with which an individual can perceive auditory or visual information
and respond to it.
Psychopathology
Illnesses or disorders that involve psychological or psychiatric symptoms.
Working memory
The ability to maintain information over a short period of time, such as 30
seconds or less.
Reference List
APA. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, Fifth Edition
(5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., Macdonald, A., Sabb, F. W., Noll, D. C., & Cohen, J. D.
(1999). Prefrontal cortex and context processing in medication-naive firstepisode patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 36(13), 217
218.
Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2003). Context processing deficit in
schizophrenia: Diagnostic specificity, 4-week course, and relationships to
clinical symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 132143.
Barch, D. M., & Ceaser, A. E. (2012). Cognition in schizophrenia: Core
psychological and neural mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16, 27
34.
Barch, D. M., & Dowd, E. C. (2010). Goal representations and motivational drive
in schizophrenia: The role of prefrontal-striatal interactions. Schizophrenia
Bulletin, 36(5), 919934. doi: sbq068 [pii] 10.1093/schbul/sbq068
Batstra, L., & Frances, A. (2012). Diagnostic inflation: Causes and a suggested
cure. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 200(6), 474479. doi:
10.1097/NMD.0b013e318257c4a2
Boos, H. B., Aleman, A., Cahn, W., Pol, H. H., & Kahn, R. S. (2007). Brain volumes
in relatives of patients with schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 64(3), 297304.
Bora, E., Fornito, A., Radua, J., Walterfang, M., Seal, M., Wood, S. J., . . . Pantelis,
C. (2011). Neuroanatomical abnormalities in schizophrenia: A multimodal
voxelwise meta-analysis and meta-regression analysis. Schizophrenia
Research, 127(13), 4657. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.12.020
Bora, E., Yucel, M., & Pantelis, C. (2009a). Cognitive functioning in schizophrenia,
schizoaffective disorder and affective psychoses: Meta-analytic study. The
British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 195(6), 475482.
doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.055731
Bora, E., Yucel, M., & Pantelis, C. (2009b). Theory of mind impairment in
schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 109(13), 19. doi:
10.1016/j.schres.2008.12.020
Bourque, F., van der Ven, E., & Malla, A. (2011). A meta-analysis of the risk for
psychotic disorders among first- and second-generation immigrants.
Psychological Medicine, 41(5), 897910. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001406
Bowie, C. R., Leung, W. W., Reichenberg, A., McClure, M. M., Patterson, T. L.,
Heaton, R. K., & Harvey, P. D. (2008). Predicting schizophrenia patients' realworld behavior with specific neuropsychological and functional capacity
measures.
Biological
Psychiatry,
63(5),
505511.
doi:
10.1016/j.
biopsych.2007.05.022
Bowie, C. R., Reichenberg, A., Patterson, T. L., Heaton, R. K., & Harvey, P. D. (2006).
Determinants of real-world functional performance in schizophrenia
subjects: Correlations with cognition, functional capacity, and symptoms.
The American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(3), 418425. doi: 10.1176/appi.
ajp.163.3.418
Brans, R. G., van Haren, N. E., van Baal, G. C., Schnack, H. G., Kahn, R. S., &
Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2008). Heritability of changes in brain volume over time
in twin pairs discordant for schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry,
65(11), 12591268. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.11.1259
Cannon, M., Jones, P. B., & Murray, R. M. (2002). Obstetric complications and
schizophrenia: Historical and meta-analytic review. The American Journal
of Psychiatry, 159(7), 10801092.
Cannon, T. D., Cadenhead, K., Cornblatt, B., Woods, S. W., Addington, J., Walker,
E., . . . Heinssen, R. (2008). Prediction of psychosis in youth at high clinical
risk: A multisite longitudinal study in North America. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 65(1), 2837.
Carpenter, W. T., Jr., & Davis, J. M. (2012). Another view of the history of
antipsychotic drug discovery and development. Molecular Psychiatry, 17
(12), 11681173. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.121
Casadio, P., Fernandes, C., Murray, R. M., & Di Forti, M. (2011). Cannabis use in
young people: The risk for schizophrenia. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
Reviews. doi: S0149-7634(11)00073-X [pii] 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.007
Cohen, A. S., & Minor, K. S. (2010). Emotional experience in patients with
schizophrenia revisited: Meta-analysis of laboratory studies. Schizophrenia
Bulletin, 36(1), 143150. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn061
Corlett, P. R., Honey, G. D., Aitken, M. R., Dickinson, A., Shanks, D. R., Absalom,
A. R., . . . Fletcher, P. C. (2006). Frontal responses during learning predict
vulnerability to the psychotogenic effects of ketamine: Linking cognition,
brain activity, and psychosis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(6), 611621.
Fusar-Poli, P., McGuire, P., & Borgwardt, S. (2012). Mapping prodromal psychosis:
A critical review of neuroimaging studies. European Psychiatry: The Journal
of the Association of European Psychiatrists, 27(3), 181191. doi: 10.1016/
j.eurpsy.2011.06.006
Fusar-Poli, P., Perez, J., Broome, M., Borgwardt, S., Placentino, A., Caverzasi, E., . . .
McGuire, P. (2007). Neurofunctional correlates of vulnerability to psychosis:
A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
Reviews, 31(4), 465484.
Fusar-Poli, P., & Yung, A. R. (2012). Should attenuated psychosis syndrome be
included in DSM-5? Lancet, 379(9816), 591592. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736
(11)61507-9
Gejman, P. V., Sanders, A. R., & Kendler, K. S. (2011). Genetics of schizophrenia:
New findings and challenges. Annual Review of Genomics and Human
Genetics. doi: 10.1146/annurev-genom-082410-101459
Gottesman, I. I., & Shields, J. (1967). A polygenic theory of schizophrenia.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America, 58(1), 199205.
Green, M. F. (2006). Cognitive impairment and functional outcome in
schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 67
Suppl 9, 38; discussion 3642.
Heckers, S., & Konradi, C. (2010). Hippocampal pathology in schizophrenia.
Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, 4, 529553.
Hoe, M., Nakagami, E., Green, M. F., & Brekke, J. S. (2012). The causal relationships
Psychological
Medicine,
39(2),
197198.
doi:
10.1017/
S0033291708003899
Kapur, S. (2003). Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: A framework linking
biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 160(1), 1323.
Kapur, S., Mizrahi, R., & Li, M. (2005). From dopamine to salience to psychosis
linking biology, pharmacology and phenomenology of psychosis.
Schizophrenia Research, 79(1), 5968. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.01.003
Karlsgodt, K. H., van Erp, T. G., Poldrack, R. A., Bearden, C. E., Nuechterlein, K.
H., & Cannon, T. D. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging of the superior
longitudinal fasciculus and working memory in recent-onset schizophrenia.
Biological Psychiatry, 63(5), 512518.
Lopez-Munoz, F., Alamo, C., Cuenca, E., Shen, W. W., Clervoy, P., & Rubio, G.
(2005). History of the discovery and clinical introduction of chlorpromazine.
Annals of Clinical Psychiatry: Official Journal of the American Academy of
Clinical Psychiatrists, 17(3), 113135.
Luzi, S., Morrison, P. D., Powell, J., di Forti, M., & Murray, R. M. (2008). What is
the mechanism whereby cannabis use increases risk of psychosis?
Neurotoxicity Research, 14(23), 105112. doi: 10.1007/BF03033802
MacDonald, A. W., III, Thermenos, H. W., Barch, D. M., & Seidman, L. J. (2009).
Imaging genetic liability to schizophrenia: Systematic review of FMRI studies
of patients' nonpsychotic relatives. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35(6), 1142
1162.
Mamah, D., Mbwayo, A., Mutiso, V., Barch, D. M., Constantino, J. N., Nsofor, T., . . .
Ndetei, D. M. (2012). A survey of psychosis risk symptoms in Kenya.
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 53(5), 516524. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.08.003
March, D., Hatch, S. L., Morgan, C., Kirkbride, J. B., Bresnahan, M., Fearon, P., &
Susser, E. (2008). Psychosis and place. Epidemiologic Reviews, 30, 84100.
doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxn006
Mesholam-Gately, R. I., Giuliano, A. J., Goff, K. P., Faraone, S. V., & Seidman, L. J.
(2009). Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: A meta-analytic
review. Neuropsychology, 23(3), 315336. doi: 10.1037/a0014708
Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Poline, J., Kohn, P. D., Holt, J. L., Egan, M. F., Weinberger,
D. R., & Berman, K. F. (2001). Evidence for abnormal cortical functional
connectivity during working memory in schizophrenia. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 158, 18091817.
Miller, B., Messias, E., Miettunen, J., Alaraisanen, A., Jarvelin, M. R., Koponen,
H., . . . Kirkpatrick, B. (2011). Meta-analysis of paternal age and schizophrenia
risk in male versus female offspring. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37(5), 1039
1047. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq011
Minzenberg, M. J., Laird, A. R., Thelen, S., Carter, C. S., & Glahn, D. C. (2009).
Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function
in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(8), 811822. doi:
10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.91
Murray, G. K., Corlett, P. R., Clark, L., Pessiglione, M., Blackwell, A. D., Honey,
G., . . . Fletcher, P. C. (2008). Substantia nigra/ventral tegmental reward
prediction error disruption in psychosis. Molecular Psychiatry, 13(3), 267
276.
Ndetei, D. M., Muriungi, S. K., Owoso, A., Mutiso, V. N., Mbwayo, A. W.,
Khasakhala, L. I., . . . Mamah, D. (2012). Prevalence and characteristics of
psychotic-like experiences in Kenyan youth. Psychiatry Research, 196(23),
235242. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.053
Nuechterlein, K. H., Subotnik, K. L., Green, M. F., Ventura, J., Asarnow, R. F., Gitlin,
M. J., . . . Mintz, J. (2011). Neurocognitive predictors of work outcome in
recent-onset schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37 Suppl 2, S3340. doi:
10.1093/schbul/sbr084
Olabi, B., Ellison-Wright, I., McIntosh, A. M., Wood, S. J., Bullmore, E., & Lawrie,
S. M. (2011). Are there progressive brain changes in schizophrenia? A metaanalysis of structural magnetic resonance imaging studies. Biological
Psychiatry, 70(1), 8896. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.01.032
Owen, M. J., Craddock, N., & O'Donovan, M. C. (2010). Suggestion of roles for
both common and rare risk variants in genome-wide studies of
schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(7), 667673. doi: 10.1001/
archgenpsychiatry.2010.69
Preti, A., & Cella, M. (2010). Randomized-controlled trials in people at ultra high
risk of psychosis: a review of treatment effectiveness. Schizophrenia
Research, 123(1), 3036. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.07.026
Ragland, J. D., Laird, A. R., Ranganath, C., Blumenfeld, R. S., Gonzales, S. M., &
Glahn, D. C. (2009). Prefrontal activation deficits during episodic memory in
schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(8), 863874.
Repovs, G., & Barch, D. M. (2012). Working memory related brain network
connectivity in individuals with schizophrenia and their siblings. Frontiers
in Human Neuroscience, 6, 137. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00137
Rutter, M., Kim-Cohen, J., & Maughan, B. (2006). Continuities and discontinuities
in psychopathology between childhood and adult life. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, and Aallied Disciplines, 47(34), 276295. doi:
10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01614.x
Schlosser, R., Gesierich, T., Kaufmann, B., Vucurevic, G., Hunsche, S., Gawehn,
J., & Stoeter, P. (2003). Altered effective connectivity during working memory
performance in schizophrenia: A study with fMRI and structural equation
modeling. Neuroimage, 19(3), 751763.
Snitz, B. E., Macdonald, A. W., 3rd, & Carter, C. S. (2006). Cognitive deficits in
unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients: A meta-analytic
review of putative endophenotypes. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32(1), 179194.
Squire, L.R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings
with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195231.
Sullivan, P. F., Kendler, K. S., & Neale, M. C. (2003). Schizophrenia as a complex
trait: Evidence from a meta-analysis of twin studies. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 60(12), 11871192. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.12.1187
Ventura, J., Hellemann, G. S., Thames, A. D., Koellner, V., & Nuechterlein, K. H.
(2009). Symptoms as mediators of the relationship between neurocognition
and functional outcome in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophrenia
Research, 113(23), 189199. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.03.035
Ventura, J., Thames, A. D., Wood, R. C., Guzik, L. H., & Hellemann, G. S. (2010).
Disorganization and reality distortion in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of
the relationship between positive symptoms and neurocognitive deficits.
Schizophrenia Research, 121(13), 114. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.05.033
Ventura, J., Wood, R. C., & Hellemann, G. S. (2011). Symptom domains and
neurocognitive functioning can help differentiate social cognitive processes
in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin. doi: 10.1093/
schbul/sbr067
Verdoux, H., & van Os, J. (2002). Psychotic symptoms in non-clinical populations
and the continuum of psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 54(12), 5965.
van Hooren, S., Versmissen, D., Janssen, I., Myin-Germeys, I., a Campo, J.,
Mengelers, R., . . . Krabbendam, L. (2008). Social cognition and
neurocognition as independent domains in psychosis. Schizophrenia
Research, 103(13), 257265. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.02.022
Abstract
Anxiety is a natural part of life and, at normal levels, helps us to function at our
best. However, for people with anxiety disorders, anxiety is overwhelming and
hard to control. Anxiety disorders develop out of a blend of biological (genetic)
and psychological factors that, when combined with stress, may lead to the
development of ailments. Primary anxiety-related diagnoses include
generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder, specific phobia, social anxiety
disorder (social phobia), posttraumatic stress disorder, and obsessivecompulsive disorder. In this chapter, we summarize the main clinical features
of each of these disorders and discuss their similarities and differences with
everyday experiences of anxiety.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
What is anxiety? Most of us feel some anxiety almost every day of our lives.
Maybe you have an important test coming up for school. Or maybe theres that
big game next Saturday, or that first date with someone new you are hoping to
impress. Anxiety can be defined as a negative mood state that is accompanied
by bodily symptoms such as increased heart rate, muscle tension, a sense of
unease, and apprehension about the future (APA, 2013; Barlow, 2002). Anxiety
is what motivates us to plan for the future, and in this sense, anxiety is actually
a good thing. Its that nagging feeling that motivates us to study for that test,
practice harder for that game, or be at our very best on that date. But some
people experience anxiety so intensely that it is no longer helpful or useful.
They may become so overwhelmed and distracted by anxiety that they actually
fail their test, fumble the ball, or spend the whole date fidgeting and avoiding
eye contact. If anxiety begins to interfere in the persons life in a significant way,
it is considered a disorder.
Anxiety and closely related disorders emerge from triple vulnerabilities,a
combination of biological, psychological, and specific factors that increase our
risk for developing a disorder (Barlow, 2002; Surez, Bennett, Goldstein, &
Barlow, 2009). Biological vulnerabilities refer to specific genetic and
neurobiological factors that might predispose someone to develop anxiety
disorders. No single gene directly causes anxiety or panic, but our genes may
make us more susceptible to anxiety and influence how our brains react to
stress (Drabant et al., 2012; Gelernter & Stein, 2009; Smoller, Block, & Young,
2009). Psychological vulnerabilities refer to the influences that our early
experiences have on how we view the world. If we were confronted with
unpredictable stressors or traumatic experiences at younger ages, we may
come to view the world as unpredictable and uncontrollable, even dangerous
(Chorpita & Barlow, 1998; Gunnar & Fisher, 2006). Specific vulnerabilities refer
to how our experiences lead us to focus and channel our anxiety (Surez et al.,
nobaproject.com - Anxiety and Related Disorders
1897
1898
making it one of the most common anxiety disorders (see Table 1).
What makes a person with GAD worry more than the average person?
Research shows that individuals with GAD are more sensitive and vigilant toward
possible threats than people who are not anxious (Aikins & Craske, 2001; Barlow,
2002; Bradley, Mogg, White, Groom, & de Bono, 1999). This may be related to
early stressful experiences, which can lead to a view of the world as an
unpredictable, uncontrollable, and even dangerous place. Some have
suggested that people with GAD worry as a way to gain some control over these
otherwise uncontrollable or unpredictable experiences and against uncertain
nobaproject.com - Anxiety and Related Disorders
1899
1900
1901
Craske, 2007; Brown, White, & Barlow, 2005; Craske & Barlow, 2008; Shear et
al., 1997).
The individual may also have experienced an overwhelming urge to escape
during the unexpected panic attack. This can lead to a sense that certain places
or situationsparticularly situations where escape might not be possibleare
not safe. These situations become external cues for panic. If the person begins
to avoid several places or situations, or still endures these situations but does
so with a significant amount of apprehension and anxiety, then the person also
has agoraphobia (Barlow, 2002; Craske & Barlow, 1988; Craske & Barlow, 2008).
Agoraphobia can cause significant disruption to a persons life, causing them
to go out of their way to avoid situations, such as adding hours to a commute
to avoid taking the train or only ordering take-out to avoid having to enter a
grocery store. In one tragic case seen by our clinic, a woman suffering from
agoraphobia had not left her apartment for 20 years and had spent the past
10 years confined to one small area of her apartment, away from the view of
the outside. In some cases, agoraphobia develops in the absence of panic
attacks and therefor is a separate disorder in DSM-5. But agoraphobia often
accompanies panic disorder.
About 4.7% of the population has met criteria for PD or agoraphobia over
their lifetime (Kessler, Chiu, Demler, Merikangas, & Walters, 2005; Kessler et al.,
2006) (see Table 1). In all of these cases of panic disorder, what was once an
adaptive natural alarm reaction now becomes a learned, and much feared, false
alarm.
Specific Phobia
The majority of us might have certain things we fear, such as bees, or needles,
or heights (Myers et al., 1984). But what if this fear is so consuming that you
cant go out on a summers day, or get vaccines needed to go on a special trip,
or visit your doctor in her new office on the 26th floor? To meet criteria for a
nobaproject.com - Anxiety and Related Disorders
1902
1903
1904
What causes someone to fear social situations to such a large extent? The
person may have learned growing up that social evaluation in particular can be
dangerous, creating a specific psychological vulnerability to develop social
anxiety (Bruch & Heimberg, 1994; Lieb et al., 2000; Rapee & Melville, 1997). For
example, the persons caregivers may have harshly criticized and punished
them for even the smallest mistake, maybe even punishing them physically. Or,
someone might have experienced a social trauma that had lasting effects, such
as being bullied or humiliated. Interestingly, one group of researchers found
that 92% of adults in their study sample with social phobia experienced severe
teasing and bullying in childhood, compared with only 35% to 50% among
people with other anxiety disorders (McCabe, Antony, Summerfeldt, Liss, &
Swinson, 2003). Someone else might react so strongly to the anxiety provoked
by a social situation that they have an unexpected panic attack. This panic attack
then becomes associated (conditioned response) with the social situation,
causing the person to fear they will panic the next time they are in that situation.
This is not considered PD, however, because the persons fear is more focused
on social evaluation than having unexpected panic attacks, and the fear of
having an attack is limited to social situations. As many as 12.1% of the general
population suffer from social phobia at some point in their lives (Kessler,
Berglund, et al., 2005), making it one of the most common anxiety disorders,
second only to specific phobia (see Table 1).
1905
continue on with their lives (Friedman, 2009). For some, however, the months
and years following a trauma are filled with intrusive reminders of the event, a
sense of intense fear that another traumatic event might occur, or a sense of
isolation and emotional numbing. They may engage in a host of behaviors
intended to protect themselves from being vulnerable or unsafe, such as
constantly scanning their surroundings to look for signs of potential danger,
never sitting with their back to the door, or never allowing themselves to be
anywhere alone. This lasting reaction to trauma is what characterizes
posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD).
A diagnosis of PTSD begins with the traumatic event itself. An individual
must have been exposed to an event that involves actual or threatened death,
serious injury, or sexual violence. To receive a diagnosis of PTSD, exposure to
the event must include either directly experiencing the event, witnessing the
event happening to someone else, learning that the event occurred to a close
relative or friend, or having repeated or extreme exposure to details of the event
(such as in the case of first responders). The person subsequently reexperiences the event through both intrusive memories and nightmares. Some
memories may come back so vividly that the person feels like they are
experiencing the event all over again, what is known as having a flashback. The
individual may avoid anything that reminds them of the trauma, including
conversations, places, or even specific types of people. They may feel
emotionally numb or restricted in their ability to feel, which may interfere in
their interpersonal relationships. The person may not be able to remember
certain aspects of what happened during the event. They may feel a sense of
a foreshortened future, that they will never marry, have a family, or live a long,
full life. They may be jumpy or easily startled, hypervigilant to their
surroundings, and quick to anger.
1906
Sonnega, Bromet, Hughes, & Nelson, 1995). Whereas PTSD was previously
categorized as an Anxiety Disorder, in the most recent version of the DSM
(DSM-5; APA, 2013) it has been reclassified under the more specific category of
Trauma- and Stressor-Related Disorders.
A person with PTSD is particularly sensitive to both internal and external
cues that serve as reminders of their traumatic experience. For example, as we
saw in PD, the physical sensations of arousal present during the initial trauma
can become threatening in and of themselves, becoming a powerful reminder
of the event. Someone might avoid watching intense or emotional movies in
order to prevent the experience of emotional arousal. Avoidance of
conversations, reminders, or even of the experience of emotion itself may also
be an attempt to avoid triggering internal cues. External stimuli that were
present during the trauma can also become strong triggers. For example, if a
woman is raped by a man wearing a red t-shirt, she may develop a strong alarm
reaction to the sight of red shirts, or perhaps even more indiscriminately to
anything with a similar color red. A combat veteran who experienced a strong
smell of gasoline during a roadside bomb attack may have an intense alarm
reaction when pumping gas back at home. Individuals with a psychological
vulnerability toward viewing the world as uncontrollable and unpredictable may
particularly struggle with the possibility of additional future, unpredictable
traumatic events, fueling their need for hypervigilance and avoidance, and
perpetuating the symptoms of PTSD.
Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder
Have you ever had a strange thought pop into your mind, such as picturing the
stranger next to you naked? Or maybe you walked past a crooked picture on
the wall and couldnt resist straightening it. Most people have occasional strange
thoughts and may even engage in some compulsive behaviors, especially
when they are stressed (Boyer & Linard, 2008; Fullana et al., 2009). But for
nobaproject.com - Anxiety and Related Disorders
1907
most people, these thoughts are nothing more than a passing oddity, and the
behaviors are done (or not done) without a second thought. For someone with
obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), however, these thoughts and
compulsive behaviors dont just come and go. Instead, strange or unusual
thoughts are taken to mean something much more important and real, maybe
even something dangerous or frightening. The urge to engage in some behavior,
such as straightening a picture, can become so intense that it is nearly
impossible not to carry it out, or causes significant anxiety if it cant be carried
out. Further, someone with OCD might become preoccupied with the possibility
that the behavior wasnt carried out to completion and feel compelled to repeat
the behavior again and again, maybe several times before they are satisfied.
To receive a diagnosis of OCD, a person must experience obsessive thoughts
and/or compulsions that seem irrational or nonsensical, but that keep coming
into their mind. Some examples of obsessions include doubting thoughts (such
as doubting a door is locked or an appliance is turned off), thoughts of
contamination (such as thinking that touching almost anything might give you
cancer), or aggressive thoughts or images that are unprovoked or nonsensical.
Compulsions may be carried out in an attempt to neutralize some of these
thoughts, providing temporary relief from the anxiety the obsessions cause, or
they may be nonsensical in and of themselves. Either way, compulsions are
distinct in that they must be repetitive or excessive, the person feels driven
to carry out the behavior, and the person feels a great deal of distress if they
cant engage in the behavior. Some examples of compulsive behaviors are
repetitive washing (often in response to contamination obsessions), repetitive
checking (locks, door handles, appliances often in response to doubting
obsessions), ordering and arranging things to ensure symmetry, or doing things
according to a specific ritual or sequence (such as getting dressed or ready for
bed in a specific order). To meet diagnostic criteria for OCD, engaging in
obsessions and/or compulsions must take up a significant amount of the
persons time, at least an hour per day, and must cause significant distress or
nobaproject.com - Anxiety and Related Disorders
1908
impairment in functioning. About 1.6% of the population has met criteria for
OCD over the course of a lifetime (Kessler, Berglund, et al., 2005) (see Table 1).
Whereas OCD was previously categorized as an Anxiety Disorder, in the most
recent version of the DSM (DSM-5; APA, 2013) it has been reclassified under the
more specific category of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders.
People with OCD often confuse having an intrusive thought with their
potential for carrying out the thought. Whereas most people when they have
a strange or frightening thought are able to let it go, a person with OCD may
become stuck on the thought and be intensely afraid that they might somehow
lose control and act on it. Or worse, they believe that having the thought is just
as bad as doing it. This is called thought-action fusion. For example, one patient
of ours was plagued by thoughts that she would cause harm to her young
daughter. She experienced intrusive images of throwing hot coffee in her
daughters face or pushing her face underwater when she was giving her a bath.
These images were so terrifying to the patient that she would no longer allow
herself any physical contact with her daughter and would leave her daughter
in the care of a babysitter if her husband or another family was not available
to supervise her. In reality, the last thing she wanted to do was harm her
daughter, and she had no intention or desire to act on the aggressive thoughts
and images, nor does anybody with OCD act on these thoughts, but these
thoughts were so horrifying to her that she made every attempt to prevent
herself from the potential of carrying them out, even if it meant not being able
to hold, cradle, or cuddle her daughter. These are the types of struggles people
with OCD face every day.
1909
over
the
years.
Medications
(anti-anxiety
drugs
and
1910
1911
Outside Resources
American Psychological Association (APA)
http://www.apa.org/topics/anxiety/index.aspx
National Institutes of Mental Health (NIMH)
http://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/topics/anxiety-disorders/index.shtml
Web: Anxiety and Depression Association of America (ADAA)
http://www.adaa.org/
Web: Center for Anxiety and Related Disorders (CARD)
http://www.bu.edu/card/
Discussion Questions
1. Name and describe the three main vulnerabilities contributing to the
development of anxiety and related disorders. Do you think these disorders
could develop out of biological factors alone? Could these disorders develop
out of learning experiences alone?
2. Many of the symptoms in anxiety and related disorders overlap with
experiences most people have. What features differentiate someone with
a disorder versus someone without?
3. What is an alarm reaction? If someone experiences an alarm reaction when
they are about to give a speech in front of a room full of people, would you
consider this a true alarm or a false alarm?
4. Many people are shy. What differentiates someone who is shy from
someone with social anxiety disorder? Do you think shyness should be
considered an anxiety disorder?
5. Is anxiety ever helpful? What about worry?
Vocabulary
Agoraphobia
A sort of anxiety disorder distinguished by feelings that a place is uncomfortable
or may be unsafe because it is significantly open or crowded.
Anxiety
A mood state characterized by negative affect, muscle tension, and physical
arousal in which a person apprehensively anticipates future danger or
misfortune.
Biological vulnerability
A specific genetic and neurobiological factor that might predispose someone
to develop anxiety disorders.
Conditioned response
A learned reaction following classical conditioning, or the process by which an
event that automatically elicits a response is repeatedly paired with another
neutral stimulus (conditioned stimulus), resulting in the ability of the neutral
stimulus to elicit the same response on its own.
External cues
Stimuli in the outside world that serve as triggers for anxiety or as reminders
of past traumatic events.
Fight or flight response
A biological reaction to alarming stressors that prepares the body to resist or
escape a threat.
Flashback
Sudden, intense re-experiencing of a previous event, usually trauma-related.
Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD)
Excessive worry about everyday things that is at a level that is out of proportion
to the specific causes of worry.
Internal bodily or somatic cues
Physical sensations that serve as triggers for anxiety or as reminders of past
traumatic events.
Interoceptive avoidance
Avoidance of situations or activities that produce sensations of physical arousal
similar to those occurring during a panic attack or intense fear response.
Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD)
A disorder characterized by the desire to engage in certain behaviors excessively
or compulsively in hopes of reducing anxiety. Behaviors include things such as
cleaning, repeatedly opening and closing doors, hoarding, and obsessing over
certain thoughts.
Panic disorder (PD)
A condition marked by regular strong panic attacks, and which may include
significant levels of worry about future attacks.
Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)
A sense of intense fear, triggered by memories of a past traumatic event, that
another traumatic event might occur. PTSD may include feelings of isolation
and emotional numbing.
Psychological vulnerabilities
Influences that our early experiences have on how we view the world.
Reinforced response
Following the process of operant conditioning, the strengthening of a response
following either the delivery of a desired consequence (positive reinforcement)
or escape from an aversive consequence.
SAD performance only
Social anxiety disorder which is limited to certain situations that the sufferer
perceives as requiring some type of performance.
Social anxiety disorder (SAD)
A condition marked by acute fear of social situations which lead to worry and
diminished day to day functioning.
Specific vulnerabilities
How our experiences lead us to focus and channel our anxiety.
Thought-action fusion
The tendency to overestimate the relationship between a thought and an action,
such that one mistakenly believes a bad thought is the equivalent of a bad
action.
Reference List
APA. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association.
Aikins, D. E., & Craske, M. G. (2001). Cognitive theories of generalized anxiety
disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 24(1), 57-74, vi.
Antony, M. M., & Barlow, D. H. (2002). Specific phobias. In D. H. Barlow (Ed.),
Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety and panic
(2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Arrindell, W. A., Eisemann, M., Richter, J., Oei, T. P., Caballo, V. E., van der Ende,
J., . . . Cultural Clinical Psychology Study, Group. (2003). Phobic anxiety in 11
nations. Part I: Dimensional constancy of the five-factor model. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 41(4), 461-479.
Barlow, D. H. (2002). Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of
anxiety and panic (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
Barlow, D. H., Allen, L.B., & Basden, S. (2007). Pscyhological treatments for panic
disorders, phobias, and generalized anxiety disorder. In P.E. Nathan & J.M.
Gorman (Eds.), A guide to treatments that work (3rd ed.). New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Barlow, D. H., Ellard, K. K., Fairholme, C. P., Farchione, T. J., Boisseau, C. L., Allen,
L. B., & Ehrenreich-May, J. (2011). Unified Protocol for the Transdiagnostic
Treatment of Emotional Disorders (Workbook). New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Barlow, D. H., Gorman, J. M., Shear, M. K., & Woods, S. W. (2000). Cognitivebehavioral therapy, imipramine, or their combination for panic disorder: A
randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association,
283(19), 2529-2536.
Barlow, D. H., & Craske, M. G. (2007). Mastery of your anxiety and panic (4th
ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Barlow, D. H., & Liebowitz, M. R. (1995). Specific and social phobias. In H. I. Kaplan
& B. J. Sadock (Eds.), Comprehensive textbook of psychiatry: VI (pp.
1204-1217). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.
Bogels, S. M., Alden, L., Beidel, D. C., Clark, L. A., Pine, D. S., Stein, M. B., & Voncken,
M. (2010). Social anxiety disorder: questions and answers for the DSM-V.
Depression and Anxiety, 27(2), 168-189. doi: 10.1002/da.20670
Borkovec, T. D., Alcaine, O.M., & Behar, E. (2004). Avoidance theory of worry and
generalized anxiety disorder. In R.G. Heimberg, Turk C.L. & D.S. Mennin
(Eds.), Generalized Anxiety Disorder: Advances in research and practice (pp.
77-108). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Borkovec, T. D., Hazlett-Stevens, H., & Diaz, M.L. (1999). The role of positive
beliefs about worry in generalized anxiety disorder and its treatment.
Clinical Psychology and Pscyhotherapy, 6, 69-73.
Borkovec, T. D., & Hu, S. (1990). The effect of worry on cardiovascular response
to phobic imagery. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28(1), 69-73.
Boyer, P., & Linard, P. (2008). Ritual behavior in obsessive and normal
individuals: Moderating anxiety and reorganizing the flow of action. Current
Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Jin, R., Ruscio, A. M., Shear, K., & Walters, E. E. (2006).
The epidemiology of panic attacks, panic disorder, and agoraphobia in the
National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63
(4), 415-424. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.4.415
Kessler, R. C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E., Hughes, M., & Nelson, C. B. (1995).
Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives
of General Psychiatry, 52(12), 1048-1060.
Lieb, R., Wittchen, H. U., Hofler, M., Fuetsch, M., Stein, M. B., & Merikangas, K.
R. (2000). Parental psychopathology, parenting styles, and the risk of social
phobia in offspring: a prospective-longitudinal community study. Archives
of General Psychiatry, 57(9), 859-866.
McCabe, R. E., Antony, M. M., Summerfeldt, L. J., Liss, A., & Swinson, R. P. (2003).
Preliminary examination of the relationship between anxiety disorders in
adults and self-reported history of teasing or bullying experiences. Cognitive
Behavior Therapy, 32(4), 187-193. doi: 10.1080/16506070310005051
Myers, J. K., Weissman, M. M., Tischler, C. E., Holzer, C. E., Orvaschel, H., Anthony,
J. C., . . . Stoltzman, R. (1984). Six-month prevalence of psychiatric disorders
in three communities. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 959-967.
Ost, L. G. (1992). Blood and injection phobia: background and cognitive,
physiological, and behavioral variables. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
101(1), 68-74.
Otto, M. W., Tolin, D. F., Simon, N. M., Pearlson, G. D., Basden, S., Meunier, S.
A., . . . Pollack, M. H. (2010). Efficacy of d-cycloserine for enhancing response
to cognitive-behavior therapy for panic disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 67
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Anxiety and Related Disorders by David H. Barlow
and Kristen K. Ellard is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercialShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Mood Disorders
Anda Gershon & Renee Thompson
Stanford University, Washington University in St. Louis
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Everyone feels down or euphoric from time to time, but this is different from
having a mood disorder such as major depressive disorder or bipolar disorder.
Mood disorders are extended periods of depressed, euphoric, or irritable
moods that in combination with other symptoms cause the person significant
distress and interfere with his or her daily life, often resulting in social and
occupational difficulties. In this chapter, we describe major mood disorders,
including their symptom presentations, general prevalence rates, and how and
why the rates of these disorders tend to vary by age, gender, and race. In
addition, biological and environmental risk factors that have been implicated
in the development and course of mood disorders, such as heritability and
stressful life events, are reviewed. Finally, we provide an overview of treatments
for mood disorders, covering treatments with demonstrated effectiveness, as
well as new treatment options showing promise.
Learning Objectives
The actress Brooke Shields published a memoir titled Down Came the Rain: My
Journey through Postpartum Depression in which she described her struggles
with depression following the birth of her daughter. Despite about one in 20
women experiencing depression after the birth of a baby (American Psychiatric
Association [APA], 2013), postpartum depressionrecently renamed perinatal
depressioncontinues to be veiled by stigma, owing in part to a widely held
expectation that motherhood should be a time of great joy. In an opinion piece
in the New York Times, Shields revealed that entering motherhood was a
profoundly overwhelming experience for her.
1929
1930
1931
Mood Disorders
Two major types of unipolar disorders described by the DSM-5 (APA, 2013) are
major depressive disorder and persistent depressive disorder (PDD;
dysthymia). MDD is defined by one or more MDEs, but no history of manic or
hypomanic episodes. Criteria for PDD are feeling depressed most of the day
for more days than not, for at least two years. At least two of the following
symptoms are also required to meet criteria for PDD:
2. insomnia or hypersomnia
4. low self-esteem
6. feelings of hopelessness
1932
1933
It is important to note that the DSM-5 was published in 2013, and findings
based on the updated manual will be forthcoming. Consequently, the research
presented below was largely based on a similar, but not identical,
conceptualization of mood disorders drawn from the DSM-IV (APA, 2000).
1934
1935
Bipolar Disorders
The lifetime prevalence rate of bipolar spectrum disorders in the general U.S.
population is estimated at approximately 4.4%, with BD I constituting about 1%
of this rate (Merikangas et al., 2007). Prevalence estimates, however, are highly
dependent on the diagnostic procedures used (e.g., interviews vs. self-report)
and whether or not sub-threshold forms of the disorder are included in the
estimate. BD often co-occurs with other psychiatric disorders. Approximately
65% of people with BD meet diagnostic criteria for at least one additional
psychiatric disorder, most commonly anxiety disorders and substance use
disorders (McElroy et al., 2001). The co-occurrence of BD with other psychiatric
disorders is associated with poorer illness course, including higher rates of
suicidality (Leverich et al., 2003). A recent cross-national study sample of more
than 60,000 adults from 11 countries, estimated the worldwide prevalence of
nobaproject.com - Mood Disorders
1936
1937
Depressive Disorders
Research across family and twin studies has provided support that genetic
factors are implicated in the development of MDD. Twin studies suggest that
familial influence on MDD is mostly due to genetic effects and that individualspecific environmental effects (e.g., romantic relationships) play an important
role, too. By contrast, the contribution of shared environmental effect by siblings
is negligible (Sullivan, Neale & Kendler, 2000). The mode of inheritance is not
fully understood although no single genetic variation has been found to
increase the risk of MDD significantly. Instead, several genetic variants and
environmental factors most likely contribute to the risk for MDD (Lohoff, 2010).
One environmental stressor that has received much support in relation to
MDD is stressful life events. In particular, severe stressful life eventsthose
that have long-term consequences and involve loss of a significant relationship
(e.g., divorce) or economic stability (e.g., unemployment) are strongly related
to depression (Brown & Harris, 1989; Monroe et al., 2009). Stressful life events
are more likely to predict the first MDE than subsequent episodes (Lewinsohn,
Allen, Seeley, & Gotlib, 1999). In contrast, minor events may play a larger role
in subsequent episodes than the initial episodes (Monroe & Harkness, 2005).
nobaproject.com - Mood Disorders
1938
1939
Bipolar Disorders
Although there have been important advances in research on the etiology,
course, and treatment of BD, there remains a need to understand the
mechanisms that contribute to episode onset and relapse. There is compelling
evidence for biological causes of BD, which is known to be highly heritable
(McGuffin, Rijsdijk, Andrew, Sham, Katz, & Cardno, 2003). It may be argued that
a high rate of heritability demonstrates that BD is fundamentally a biological
phenomenon. However, there is much variability in the course of BD both within
a person across time and across people (Johnson, 2005). The triggers that
determine how and when this genetic vulnerability is expressed are not yet
understood; however, there is evidence to suggest that psychosocial triggers
may play an important role in BD risk (e.g., Johnson et al., 2008; Malkoff-Schwartz
et al., 1998).
In addition to the genetic contribution, biological explanations of BD have
also focused on brain function. Many of the studies using fMRI techniques to
characterize BD have focused on the processing of emotional stimuli based on
the idea that BD is fundamentally a disorder of emotion (APA, 2000). Findings
show that regions of the brain thought to be involved in emotional processing
and regulation are activated differently in people with BD relative to healthy
controls (e.g., Altshuler et al., 2008; Hassel et al., 2008; Lennox, Jacob, Calder,
Lupson, & Bullmore, 2004).
However, there is little consensus as to whether a particular brain region
becomes more or less active in response to an emotional stimulus among
people with BD compared with healthy controls. Mixed findings are in part due
to samples consisting of participants who are at various phases of illness at the
time of testing (manic, depressed, inter-episode). Sample sizes tend to be
nobaproject.com - Mood Disorders
1940
1941
medications.
SSRIs,
the
most
commonly
prescribed
1942
and insomnia.
Other
biological
treatments
for
people
with
depression
include
1943
emotions affect and are affected by each other. Interpersonal Therapy for
Depression focuses largely on improving interpersonal relationships by
targeting problem areas, specifically unresolved grief, interpersonal role
disputes, role transitions, and interpersonal deficits. Finally, there is also some
support for the effectiveness of Short-Term Psychodynamic Therapy for
Depression (Leichsenring, 2001). The short-term treatment focuses on a limited
number of important issues, and the therapist tends to be more actively
involved than in more traditional psychodynamic therapy.
Bipolar Disorders
Patients with BD are typically treated with pharmacotherapy. Antidepressants
such as SSRIs and SNRIs are the primary choice of treatment for depression,
whereas for BD, lithium is the first line treatment choice. This is because SSRIs
and SNRIs have the potential to induce mania or hypomania in patients with
BD. Lithium acts on several neurotransmitter systems in the brain through
complex mechanisms, including reduction of excitatory (dopamine and
glutamate)
neurotransmission,
and
increasing
of
inhibitory
(GABA)
neurotransmission (Lenox & Hahn, 2000). Lithium has strong efficacy for the
treatment of BD (Geddes, Burgess, Hawton, Jamison, & Goodwin, 2004).
However, a number of side effects can make lithium treatment difficult for
patients to tolerate. Side effects include impaired cognitive function (Wingo,
Wingo, Harvey, & Baldessarini, 2009), as well as physical symptoms such as
nausea, tremor, weight gain, and fatigue (Dunner, 2000). Some of these side
effects can improve with continued use; however, medication noncompliance
remains an ongoing concern in the treatment of patients with BD.
Anticonvulsant medications (e.g., carbamazepine, valproate) are also
commonly used to treat patients with BD, either alone or in conjunction with
nobaproject.com - Mood Disorders
1944
lithium.
There are several adjunctive treatment options for people with BD.
Interpersonal and social rhythm therapy (IPSRT; Frank et al., 1994) is a
psychosocial intervention focused on addressing the mechanism of action
posited in social zeitgeber theory to predispose patients who have BD to
relapse, namely sleep disruption. A growing body of literature provides support
for the central role of sleep dysregulation in BD (Harvey, 2008). Consistent with
this literature, IPSRT aims to increase rhythmicity of patients lives and
encourage vigilance in maintaining a stable rhythm. The therapist and patient
work to develop and maintain a healthy balance of activity and stimulation such
that the patient does not become overly active (e.g., by taking on too many
projects) or inactive (e.g., by avoiding social contact). The efficacy of IPSRT has
been demonstrated in that patients who received this treatment show reduced
risk of episode recurrence and are more likely to remain well (Frank et al., 2005).
Conclusion
Everyone feels down or euphoric from time to time. For some people, these
feelings can last for long periods of time and can also co-occur with other
symptoms that, in combination, interfere with their everyday lives. When people
experience an MDE or a manic episode, they see the world differently. During
an MDE, people often feel hopeless about the future, and may even experience
suicidal thoughts. During a manic episode, people often behave in ways that
are risky or place them in danger. They may spend money excessively or have
unprotected sex, often expressing deep shame over these decisions after the
episode. MDD and BD cause significant problems for people at school, at work,
and in their relationships and affect people regardless of gender, age,
nationality, race, religion, or sexual orientation. If you or someone you know is
suffering from a mood disorder, it is important to seek help. Effective treatments
are available and continually improving. If you have an interest in mood
nobaproject.com - Mood Disorders
1945
1946
Outside Resources
Books: Recommended memoirs include A Memoir of Madness by William
Styron (MDD); Noonday Demon: An Atlas of Depression by Andrew Solomon
(MDD); and An Unquiet Mind: A Memoir of Moods and Madness by Kay
Redfield (BD).
Web: Visit the Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies to find a
list of the recommended therapists and evidence-based treatments.
http://www.abct.org
Web: Visit the Depression and Bipolar Support Alliance for educational
information and social support options.
http://www.dbsalliance.org/
Discussion Questions
1. What factors might explain the large gender difference in the prevalence
rates of MDD?
2. Why might American ethnic minority groups experience more persistent
BD than European Americans?
3. Why might the age of onset for MDD be decreasing over time?
4. Why might overnight travel constitute a potential risk for a person with BD?
5. What are some reasons positive life events may precede the occurrence of
manic episode?
Vocabulary
Anhedonia
Loss of interest or pleasure in activities one previously found enjoyable or
rewarding.
Attributional style
The tendency by which a person infers the cause or meaning of behaviors or
events.
Chronic stress
Discrete or related problematic events and conditions which persist over time
and result in prolonged activation of the biological and/or psychological stress
response (e.g., unemployment, ongoing health difficulties, marital discord).
Early adversity
Single or multiple acute or chronic stressful events, which may be biological or
psychological in nature (e.g., poverty, abuse, childhood illness or injury),
occurring during childhood and resulting in a biological and/or psychological
stress response.
Grandiosity
Inflated self-esteem or an exaggerated sense of self-importance and self-worth
(e.g., believing one has special powers or superior abilities).
Hypersomnia
Excessive daytime sleepiness, including difficulty staying awake or napping, or
prolonged sleep episodes.
Psychomotor agitation
Increased motor activity associated with restlessness, including physical actions
(e.g., fidgeting, pacing, feet tapping, handwringing).
Psychomotor retardation
A slowing of physical activities in which routine activities (e.g., eating, brushing
teeth) are performed in an unusually slow manner.
Social zeitgeber
Zeitgeber is German for time giver. Social zeitgebers are environmental cues,
such as meal times and interactions with other people, that entrain biological
rhythms and thus sleep-wake cycle regularity.
Socioeconomic status (SES)
A persons economic and social position based on income, education, and
occupation.
Suicidal ideation
Recurring thoughts about suicide, including considering or planning for suicide,
or preoccupation with suicide.
Reference List
Abramson, L. Y, Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. (1978). Learned helplessness
in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
87, 4974. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.87.1.49
Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression:
A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review, 96, 358373.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.56.3.431
Altshuler, L., Bookheimer, S., Townsend, J., Proenza, M. A., Sabb, F., Mintz, J., &
Cohen, M. S. (2008). Regional brain changes in bipolar I depression: A
functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Bipolar Disorders, 10, 708
717. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00617.x
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects.
New York, NY: Hoeber.
Blazer, D., George, L. K., Landerman, R., Pennybacker, M., Melville, M. L.,
Woodbury, M., et al. (1985). Psychiatric disorders. A rural/urban comparison.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 651656. PMID: 4015306. doi: 10.1001/
archpsyc.1985.01790300013002
Breslau, J., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Kendler, K. S., Su, M., Williams, D., & Kessler, R.
and
Psychopathology,
21,
593620.
doi:
10.1017/
S0954579409000327
Dunner, D. L. (2000). Optimizing lithium treatment. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,
61(S9), 7681.
Ehlers, C. L., Frank, E., & Kupfer, D. J. (1988). Social zeitgebers and biological
rhythms: a unified approach to understanding the etiology of depression.
ps.55.8.931
Leichsenring, F. (2001). Comparative effects of short-term psychodynamic
psychotherapy and cognitive-behavioral therapy in depression: A metaanalytic approach. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 401419. doi: 10.1016/
S0272-7358(99)00057-4
Lennox, B. R., Jacob, R., Calder, A. J., Lupson, V., & Bullmore, E. T. (2004).
Behavioural and neurocognitive responses to sad facial affect are
attenuated in patients with mania. Psychological Medicine, 34, 795802.
doi: 10.1017/S0033291704002557
Lenox, R. H., & Hahn C. G. (2000). Overview of the mechanism of action of lithium
in the brain: fifty-year update. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 61 (S9), 515.
Leverich, G. S., Altshuler, L. L., Frye, M. A., Suppes, T., Keck, P. E. Jr, McElroy, S.
L., et al. (2003). Factors associated with suicide attempts in 648 patients with
bipolar disorder in the Stanley Foundation Bipolar Network. Journal of
Clinical Psychiatry, 64, 506515. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v64n0503
Leverich, G. S., Post, R. M., Keck, P. E. Jr, Altshuler, L. L., Frye, M. A., Kupka, R. W.,
et al. (2007). The poor prognosis of childhood-onset bipolar disorder. Journal
of
Pediatrics,
150,
485490.
PMID:
17452221.
doi:
10.1016/j.
jpeds.2006.10.070
Lewinsohn, P. M., Allen, N. B., Seeley, J. R., & Gotlib, I. H. (1999). First onset versus
recurrence of depression: differential processes of psychosocial risk. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 483489. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.108.3.483
Lewinsohn, P. M., Seeley, J. R., Buckley, M. E., & Klein, D. N. (2002). Bipolar disorder
Merikangas, K. R., Akiskal, H. S., Angst, J., Greenberg, P. E., Hirschfeld, R. M.,
Petukhova, M., et al. (2007). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of bipolar
spectrum disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey replication. Archives
of General Psychiatry, 64, 543552. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.543
Merikangas, K. R., Jin, R., He, J. P., Kessler, R. C., Lee, S., Sampson, N. A., et al.
(2011). Prevalence and correlates of bipolar spectrum disorder in the world
mental health survey initiative. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 241251.
doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.12
Minsky, S., Vega, W., Miskimen, T., Gara, M., & Escobar, J. (2003). Diagnostic
patterns in Latino, African American, and European American psychiatric
patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 637644. doi: 10.1001/
archpsyc.60.6.637
Monroe, S. M., & Harkness, K. L. (2011). Recurrence in major depression: A
conceptual analysis. Psychological Review, 118, 655674. doi: 10.1037/
a0025190
Monroe, S. M., & Harkness, K. L. (2005). Life stress, the Kindling hypothesis,
and the recurrence of depression: Considerations from a life stress
perspective. Psychological Review, 112, 417445. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.417
Monroe, S.M., Slavich, G.M., Georgiades, K. (2009). The social environment and
life stress in depression. In Gotlib, I.H., Hammen, C.L (Eds.) Handbook of
depression (2nd ed., pp. 340-360). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Hilt, L. M. (2009). Gender differences in depression. In
I. H. Gotlib & Hammen, C. L. (Eds.), Handbook of depression (2nd ed., pp.
386404). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Perlis, R. H., Miyahara, S., Marangell, L. B., Wisniewski, S. R., Ostacher, M.,
DelBello, M. P., et al. (2004). Long-term implications of early onset in bipolar
disorder: data from the first 1000 participants in the systematic treatment
enhancement program for bipolar disorder (STEP-BD). Biological Psychiatry,
55, 875881. PMID: 15110730. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.10.003
Rosa, M. A., Gattaz, W. F., Pascual-Leone, A., Fregni, F., Rosa, M. O., Rumi, D. O.,
Marcolin, M. A. (2006). Comparison of repetitive transcranial magnetic
stimulation and electroconvulsive therapy in unipolar non-psychotic
refractory depression: a randomized, single-blind study. International
Journal
of
Neuropsychopharmacology,
9,
667676.
doi:
10.1017/
S1461145706007127
Schulze-Rauschenbach, S. C., Harms, U., Schlaepfer, T. E., Maier, W., Falkai, P., &
Wagner, M. (2005). Distinctive neurocognitive effects of repetitive
transcranial magnetic stimulation and electroconvulsive therapy in major
depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 410416. doi: 10.1192/
bjp.186.5.410
Shields, B. (2005). Down Came the Rain: My Journey Through Postpartum
Depression. New York: Hyperion.
Sullivan, P., Neale, M. C., & Kendler, K. S. (2000). Genetic epidemiology of major
depression: Review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157,
15521562. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1552
Twenge, J. M., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2002). Age, gender, race, SES, and birth
cohort differences on the Childrens Depression Inventory: A meta-analysis.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 578588. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.111.4.578
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Mood Disorders by Anda Gershon and Renee
Thompson is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Personality Disorders
Cristina Crego & Thomas Widiger
University of Kentucky
nobaproject.com
Abstract
The purpose of this chapter is to define what is meant by a personality disorder,
identify the five domains of general personality (i.e., neuroticism, extraversion,
openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness), identify the six personality
disorders proposed for retention in the 5th edition of the Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) (i.e., borderline, antisocial,
schizotypal, avoidant, obsessive-compulsive, and narcissistic), summarize the
etiology for antisocial and borderline personality disorder, and identify the
treatment for borderline personality disorder (i.e., dialectical behavior therapy
and mentalization therapy).
Learning Objectives
Introduction
Everybody has their own unique personality; that is, their characteristic manner
of thinking, feeling, behaving, and relating to others (John, Robins, & Pervin,
2008). Some people are typically introverted, quiet, and withdrawn; whereas
others are more extraverted, active, and outgoing. Some individuals are
invariably conscientiousness, dutiful, and efficient; whereas others might be
characteristically undependable and negligent. Some individuals are
consistently anxious, self-conscious, and apprehensive; whereas others are
routinely relaxed, self-assured, and unconcerned. Personality traits refer to
these characteristic, routine ways of thinking, feeling, and relating to others.
There are signs or indicators of these traits in childhood, but they become
particularly evident when the person is an adult. Personality traits are integral
to each persons sense of self, as they involve what people value, how they think
and feel about things, what they like to do, and, basically, what they are like
most every day throughout much of their lives.
There are literally hundreds of different personality traits. All of these traits
can be organized into the broad dimensions referred to as the Five-Factor
Model (John, Naumann, & Soto, 2008). These five broad domains are inclusive;
there does not appear to be any traits of personality that lie outside of the FiveFactor Model. This even applies to traits that you may use to describe yourself.
Table I provides illustrative traits for both poles of the five domains of this model
of personality. A number of the traits that you see in this table may describe
you. If you can think of some other traits that describe yourself, you should be
able to place them somewhere in this table.
1965
1966
Table I: Illustrative traits for both poles across Five-Factor Model personality dimensions.
1967
Description
Each of the 10 DSM-IV-TR (and DSM-5) personality disorders is a constellation
of maladaptive personality traits, rather than just one particular personality
trait (Lynam & Widiger, 2001). In this regard, personality disorders are
syndromes. For example, avoidant personality disorder is a pervasive pattern
of social inhibition, feelings of inadequacy, and hypersensitivity to negative
evaluation (APA, 2000), which is a combination of traits from introversion (e.g.,
socially withdrawn, passive, and cautious) and neuroticism (e.g., selfconsciousness, apprehensiveness, anxiousness, and worrisome). Dependent
personality disorder includes submissiveness, clinging behavior, and fears of
separation (APA, 2000), for the most part a combination of traits of neuroticism
(anxious, uncertain, pessimistic, and helpless) and maladaptive agreeableness
(e.g., gullible, guileless, meek, subservient, and self-effacing). Antisocial
personality disorder is, for the most part, a combination of traits from
antagonism (e.g., dishonest, manipulative, exploitative, callous, and merciless)
and low conscientiousness (e.g., irresponsible, immoral, lax, hedonistic, and
rash). See the 1967 movie, Bonnie and Clyde, starring Warren Beatty, for a nice
portrayal of someone with antisocial personality disorder.
Some of the DSM-IV-TR personality disorders are confined largely to traits
within one of the basic domains of personality. For example, obsessivecompulsive personality disorder is largely a disorder of maladaptive
conscientiousness, including such traits as workaholism, perfectionism,
punctilious, ruminative, and dogged; schizoid is confined largely to traits of
introversion (e.g., withdrawn, cold, isolated, placid, and anhedonic); borderline
personality disorder is largely a disorder of neuroticism, including such traits
as emotionally unstable, vulnerable, overwhelmed, rageful, depressive, and
self-destructive (watch the 1987 movie, Fatal Attraction, starring Glenn Close,
for a nice portrayal of this personality disorder); and histrionic personality
disorder is largely a disorder of maladaptive extraversion, including such traits
nobaproject.com - Personality Disorders
1968
1969
traits (APA, 2012; Skodol, 2012). For example, it has been suggested that
antisocial personality disorder includes impairments in identity (e.g.,
egocentrism), self-direction, empathy, and capacity for intimacy, which are said
to be different from such traits as arrogance, impulsivity, and callousness (APA,
2012).
Validity
It is quite possible that in future revisions of the DSM some of the personality
disorders included in DSM-IV-TR and DSM-5 will no longer be included. In fact,
for DSM-5 it was originally proposed that four be deleted. The personality
disorders that were slated for deletion were histrionic, schizoid, paranoid, and
dependent (APA, 2012). The rationale for the proposed deletions was in large
part because they are said to have less empirical support than the diagnoses
that were at the time being retained (Skodol, 2012). There is agreement within
the field with regard to the empirical support for the borderline, antisocial, and
schizotypal personality disorders (Mullins-Sweat, Bernstein, & Widiger, 2012;
Skodol, 2012). However, there is a difference of opinion with respect to the
empirical support for the dependent personality disorder (Bornstein, 2012;
Livesley, 2011; Miller, Widiger, & Campbell, 2010; Mullins-Sweat et al., 2012).
Little is known about the specific etiology for most of the DSM-IV-TR
personality disorders. Because each personality disorder represents a
constellation of personality traits, the etiology for the syndrome will involve a
complex interaction of an array of different neurobiological vulnerabilities and
dispositions with a variety of environmental, psychosocial events. Antisocial
personality disorder, for instance, is generally considered to be the result of an
interaction of genetic dispositions for low anxiousness, aggressiveness,
impulsivity, and/or callousness, with a tough, urban environment, inconsistent
parenting, poor parental role modeling, and/or peer support (Hare, Neumann,
& Widiger, 2012). Borderline personality disorder is generally considered to be
nobaproject.com - Personality Disorders
1970
Treatment
Personality disorders are relatively unique because they are often egosyntonic; that is, most people are largely comfortable with their selves, with
their characteristic manner of behaving, feeling, and relating to others. As a
result, people rarely seek treatment for their antisocial, narcissistic, histrionic,
paranoid, and/or schizoid personality disorder. People typically lack insight into
the maladaptivity of their personality.
One clear exception though is borderline personality disorder (and perhaps
as well avoidant personality disorder). Neuroticism is the domain of general
personality structure that concerns inherent feelings of emotional pain and
suffering, including feelings of distress, anxiety, depression, self-consciousness,
helplessness, and vulnerability. Persons who have very high elevations on
neuroticism (i.e., persons with borderline personality disorder) experience life
as one of pain and suffering, and they will seek treatment to alleviate this severe
nobaproject.com - Personality Disorders
1971
emotional distress. People with avoidant personality may also seek treatment
for their high levels of neuroticism (anxiousness and self-consciousness) and
introversion (social isolation). In contrast, narcissistic individuals will rarely seek
treatment to reduce their arrogance; paranoid persons rarely seek treatment
to reduce their feelings of suspiciousness; and antisocial people rarely (or at
least willfully) seek treatment to reduce their disposition for criminality,
aggression, and irresponsibility.
Nevertheless, maladaptive personality traits will be evident in many
individuals seeking treatment for other mental disorders, such as anxiety,
mood, or substance use. Many of the people with a substance use disorder will
have antisocial personality traits; many of the people with mood disorder will
have borderline personality traits. The prevalence of personality disorders
within clinical settings is estimated to be well above 50% (Torgersen, 2012). As
many as 60% of inpatients within some clinical settings are diagnosed with
borderline personality disorder (APA, 2000). Antisocial personality disorder may
be diagnosed in as many as 50% of inmates within a correctional setting (Hare
et al., 2012). It is estimated that 10% to 15% of the general population meets
criteria for at least one of the 10 DSM-IV-TR personality disorders (Torgersen,
2012), and quite a few more individuals are likely to have maladaptive
personality traits not covered by one of the 10 DSM-IV-TR diagnoses.
The presence of a personality disorder will often have an impact on the
treatment of other mental disorders, typically inhibiting or impairing
responsivity. Antisocial persons will tend to be irresponsible and negligent;
borderline persons can form intensely manipulative attachments to their
therapists; paranoid patients will be unduly suspicious and accusatory;
narcissistic patients can be dismissive and denigrating; and dependent patients
can become overly attached to and feel helpless without their therapists.
nobaproject.com - Personality Disorders
1972
It is unclear why specific and explicit treatment manuals have not been
developed for the other personality disorders. This may reflect a regrettable
assumption that personality disorders are unresponsive to treatment. It may
also reflect the complexity of their treatment. As noted earlier, each DSM-IV-TR
1973
Conclusions
It is evident that all individuals have a personality, as indicated by their
characteristic way of thinking, feeling, behaving, and relating to others. For some
people, these traits result in a considerable degree of distress and/or
impairment, constituting a personality disorder. A considerable body of
research has accumulated to help understand the etiology, pathology, and/or
treatment for some personality disorders (i.e., antisocial, schizotypal,
borderline, dependent, and narcissistic), but not so much for others (e.g.,
nobaproject.com - Personality Disorders
1974
histrionic, schizoid, and paranoid). However, researchers and clinicians are now
shifting toward a more dimensional understanding of personality disorders,
wherein each is understood as a maladaptive variant of general personality
structure, thereby bringing to bear all that is known about general personality
functioning to an understanding of these maladaptive variants.
1975
Outside Resources
Web: DSM-5 website discussion of personality disorders
http://www.dsm5.org/ProposedRevision/Pages/PersonalityDisorders.aspx
Discussion Questions
1. Do you think that any of the personality disorders, or some of their specific
traits, are ever good or useful to have?
2. If someone with a personality disorder commits a crime, what is the right
way for society to respond? For example, does or should meeting diagnostic
criteria for antisocial personality disorder mitigate (lower) a persons
responsibility for committing a crime?
3. Given what you know about personality disorders and the traits that
comprise each one, would you say there is any personality disorder that is
likely to be diagnosed in one gender more than the other? Why or why not?
4. Do you believe that personality disorders can be best understood as a
constellation of maladaptive personality traits, or do you think that there is
something more involved for individuals suffering from a personality
disorder?
5. The authors suggested Clyde Barrow as an example of antisocial personality
disorder and Blanche Dubois for histrionic personality disorder. Can you
think of a person from the media or literature who would have at least some
of the traits of narcissistic personality disorder?
Vocabulary
Antisocial
A pervasive pattern of disregard and violation of the rights of others. These
behaviors may be aggressive or destructive and may involve breaking laws or
rules, deceit or theft.
Avoidant
A pervasive pattern of social inhibition, feelings of inadequacy, and
hypersensitivity to negative evaluation.
Borderline
A pervasive pattern of instability of interpersonal relationships, self-image, and
affects, and marked impulsivity.
Dependent
A pervasive and excessive need to be taken care of that leads to submissive
and clinging behavior and fears of separation.
Five-Factor Model
Five broad domains or dimensions that are used to describe human personality.
Histrionic
A pervasive pattern of excessive emotionality and attention seeking.
Narcissistic
A pervasive pattern of grandiosity (in fantasy or behavior), need for admiration,
and lack of empathy.
Obsessive-compulsive
A pervasive pattern of preoccupation with orderliness, perfectionism, and
mental and interpersonal control, at the expense of flexibility, openness, and
efficiency.
Paranoid
A pervasive distrust and suspiciousness of others such that their motives are
interpreted as malevolent.
Personality
Characteristic, routine ways of thinking, feeling, and relating to others.
Personality disorders
When personality traits result in significant distress, social impairment, and/or
occupational impairment.
Schizoid
A pervasive pattern of detachment from social relationships and a restricted
range of expression of emotions in interpersonal settings.
Schizotypal
A pervasive pattern of social and interpersonal deficits marked by acute
discomfort with, and reduced capacity for, close relationships as well as
perceptual distortions and eccentricities of behavior.
Reference List
Allik, J. (2005). Personality dimensions across cultures. Journal of Personality
Disorders, 19, 212232.
American Psychiatric Association (2012). Rationale for the proposed changes
to the personality disorders classification in DSM-5. Retrieved from http://
www.dsm5.org/ProposedRevision/Pages/PersonalityDisorders.aspx.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders. Text revision (4th ed., rev. ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2001). Practice guidelines for the treatment
of patients with borderline personality disorder. Washington, DC: Author.
Beck, A. T., Freeman, A., Davis, D., and Associates (1990). Cognitive therapy of
personality disorders, (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Bornstein, R. F. (2012). Illuminating a neglected clinical issue: Societal costs of
interpersonal dependency and dependent personality disorder. Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 68, 766781.
Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., & Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development: Stability
and change. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 453484.
DeYoung, C. G., Hirsh, J. B., Shane, M. S., Papademetris, X., Rajeevan, N., & Gray,
J. (2010). Testing predictions from personality neuroscience: Brain structure
and the Big Five. Psychological Science, 21, 820828.
Gunderson, J. G. (2010). Commentary on Personality traits and the classification
Lynam, D. R., & Widiger, T. A. (2001). Using the five factor model to represent
the DSM-IV personality disorders: An expert consensus approach. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 401412.
Miller, J. D., Widiger, T. A., & Campbell, W. K. (2010). Narcissistic personality
disorder and the DSM-V. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 640649.
Millon, T. (2011). Disorders of personality. Introducing a DSM/ICD spectrum
from normal to abnormal (3rd ed.). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
Mullins-Sweatt; Bernstein; Widiger. Retention or deletion of personality
disorder diagnoses for DSM-5: an expert consensus approach. Journal of
personality disorders 2012;26(5):689-703.
Perry, J. C., & Bond, M. (2000). Empirical studies of psychotherapy for personality
disorders. In J. Gunderson and G. Gabbard (Eds.), Psychotherapy for
personality disorders (pp. 131). Washington DC: American Psychiatric
Press.
Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consistency of
personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review of
longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 325.
Shedler, J., Beck, A., Fonagy, P., Gabbard, G. O., Gunderson, J. G., Kernberg, O., ...
Westen, D. (2010). Personality disorders in DSM-5. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 167, 10271028.
Skodol, A. (2012). Personality disorders in DSM-5. Annual Review of Clinical
Psychology, 8, 317344.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Personality Disorders by Cristina Crego and
Thomas Widiger is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercialShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Dissociative Disorders
Dalena van Heugten - van der Kloet
Maastricht University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
In psychopathology, dissociation happens when thoughts, feelings, and
experiences of our consciousness and memory do not collaborate well with
each other. This chapter provides an overview of dissociative disorders,
including the definitions of dissociation, its origins and competing theories, and
their relation to traumatic experiences and sleep problems.
Learning Objectives
How can the two models be combined into one conceptual scheme?
Introduction
Think about the last time you were daydreaming. Perhaps it was while you were
driving or attending class. Some portion of your attention was on the activity
at hand, but most of your conscious mind was wrapped up in fantasy. Now
imagine that you could not control your daydreams. What if they intruded your
waking consciousness unannounced, causing you to lose track of reality or
experience the loss of time. Imagine how difficult it would be for you. This is
similar to what people who suffer from dissociative disorders may experience.
Of the many disorders listed in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), dissociative
disorders rank as among the most puzzling and controversial. Dissociative
disorders encompass an array of symptoms ranging from memory loss
(amnesia) for autobiographical events, to changes in identity and the
experience of everyday reality (American Psychiatric Association, 2000).
Is it real?
Lets start with a little history. Multiple personality disorder, or dissociative
identity disorderas it is known nowused to be a mere curiosity. The disorder
was rarely diagnosed until the 1980s. Thats when multiple personality disorder
became an official diagnosis in the DSM-III. From then on, the numbers of
multiples increased rapidly. In the 1990s, there were hundreds of people
diagnosed with multiple personality in every major city in the United States
(Hacking, 1995). How could this epidemic be explained?
One possible explanation might be the media attention that was given to
the disorder. It all started with the book The Three Faces of Eve (Thigpen &
Cleckley, 1957). This book, and later the movie, was one of the first to speak of
multiple personality disorder. However, it wasnt until years later, when the
fictional as told to book of Sybil (Schreiber, 1973) became known worldwide,
nobaproject.com - Dissociative Disorders
1988
that the prototype of what it was like to be a multiple personality was born.
Sybil tells the story of how a clinicianCornelia Wilburunravels the different
personalities of her patient Sybil during a long course of treatment (over 2,500
office hours!). She was one of the first to relate multiple personality to childhood
sexual abuse. Probably, this relation between childhood abuse and dissociation
has fueled the increase of numbers of multiples from that time on. It motivated
therapists to actively seek for clues of childhood abuse in their dissociative
patients. This suited well within the mindset of the 1980s, as childhood abuse
was a sensitive issue then in psychology as well as in politics (Hacking, 1995).
From then on, many movies and books were made on the subject of multiple
personality, and nowadays, we see patients with dissociative identity disorder
as guests visiting the Oprah Winfrey show, as if they were our modern-day circus
acts.
Defining dissociation
The DSM-IV-TR defines dissociation as a disruption in the usually integrated
function of consciousness, memory, identity, or perception of the environment
(American Psychiatric Association, 2000, p. 519). A distinction is often made
between dissociative states and dissociative traits (e.g., Bremner, 2010;
Bremner & Brett, 1997). State dissociation is viewed as a transient symptom,
which lasts for a few minutes or hours (e.g., dissociation during a traumatic
event). Trait dissociation is viewed as an integral aspect of personality.
Dissociative symptoms occur in patients but also in the general population,
like you and me. Therefore, dissociation has commonly been conceptualized
as ranging on a continuum, from nonsevere manifestations of daydreaming to
more severe disturbances typical of dissociative disorders (Bernstein & Putnam,
1986). The dissociative disorders include:
1. Dissociative Amnesia (extensive forgetting typically associated with highly
nobaproject.com - Dissociative Disorders
1989
aversive events);
2. Dissociative Fugue (short-lived reversible amnesia for personal identity,
involving unplanned travel or wandering);
3. Depersonalization Disorder (DPD; feeling as though one is an outside
observer of ones body); and
4. Dissociative Identity Disorder (DID; experiencing two or more distinct
identities that recurrently take control over ones behavior) (American
Psychiatric Association, 2000).
1990
1991
Measuring dissociation
The Dissociative Experiences Scale(DES) (Bernstein & Putnam, 1986; Carlson &
Putnam, 2000; Wright & Loftus, 1999) is the most widely used self-report
measure of dissociation. A self-report measure is a type of in which a person
completes a survey or with or without the help of an investigator. This scale
measures dissociation with items such as (a) Some people sometimes have
the experience of feeling as though they are standing next to themselves or
watching themselves do something, and they actually see themselves as if they
were looking at another person and (b) Some people find that sometimes they
are listening to someone talk, and they suddenly realize that they did not hear
part or all of what was said.
The DES is suitable only as a screening tool. When somebody scores a high
level of dissociation on this scale, this does not necessarily mean that he or she
is suffering from a dissociative disorder. It does, however, give an indication to
investigate the symptoms more extensively. This is usually done with a
structured clinical interview, called the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV
Dissociative Disorders (Steinberg, 1994), which is performed by an experienced
clinician.
1992
with or avoid the impact of highly aversive experiences (e.g., Spiegel et al., 2011).
In this view, individuals rely on dissociation to escape from painful memories
(Gershuny & Thayer, 1999). Once they have learned to use this defensive coping
mechanism, it can become automatized and habitual, even emerging in
response to minor stressors (Van der Hart & Horst, 1989). The idea that
dissociation can serve a defensive function can be traced back to Pierre Janet
(1899/1973), one of the first scholars to link dissociation to psychological trauma
(Hacking, 1995).
The PTM casts the clinical observation that dissociative disorders are linked
to a trauma history in straightforward causal terms, that is, one causes the other
(Gershuny & Thayer, 1999). For example, Vermetten and colleagues (Vermetten,
Schmahl, Lindner, Loewenstein, & Bremner, 2006) found that the DID patients
in their study all suffered from posttraumatic stress disorder and concluded
that DID should be conceptualized as an extreme form of early-abuserelated
posttraumatic stress disorder (Vermetten et al., 2006).
1993
1994
1995
1996
First, Watsons (2001) basic findings have been reproduced time and again.
This means that the same results (namely that dissociation and sleep problems
are related) have been found in lots of different studies, using different groups,
and different materials. All lead to the conclusion that unusual sleep
experiences and dissociative symptoms are linked.
Second, the connection between sleep and dissociation is specific. It seems
that unusual sleep phenomena that are difficult to control, including nightmares
and waking dreams, are related to dissociative symptoms, but lucid dreaming
dreams that are controllableare only weakly related to dissociative
symptoms. For example, dream recall frequency was related to dissociation
(Suszek & Kopera, 2005). Individuals who reported three or more nightmares
over a three-week period showed higher levels of dissociation compared to
individuals reporting two nightmares or less (Levin & Fireman, 2002), and a
relation was found between dream intensity and dissociation (Yu et al., 2010).
Third, the sleep-dissociation link is apparent not only in general population
groupspeople such as you and mebut also in patient groups. Accordingly,
one group of researchers reported nightmare disorder in 17 out of 30 DID
patients (Agargun et al., 2003). They also found a 27.5% prevalence of nocturnal
dissociative episodes in patients with dissociative disorders (Agargun et al.,
2001). Another study investigated a group of borderline personality disorder
patients and found that 49% of them suffered from nightmare disorder.
Moreover, the patients with nightmare disorder displayed higher levels of
dissociation than patients not suffering from nightmare disorder (Semiz,
Basoglu, Ebrinc, & Cetin, 2008). Additionally, Ross (2011) found that patients
suffering from DID reported higher rates of sleepwalking compared to a group
of psychiatric outpatients and a sample from the general population.
To sum up, there seems to be a strong relationship between dissociative
symptoms and unusual sleep experiences that is evident in a range of
phenomena, including waking dreams, nightmares, and sleepwalking.
1997
1998
1999
Outside Resources
Book: Schreiber, F. R. (1973). Sybil. Chicago: Regnery.
Web: Video depicting the controversy regarding the existence of DID and
show you some debate between clinicians and researchers on the topics of
brain imaging, recovered memories, and false memories. Brain scan
switching on command.
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zhM0xp5vXqY
Web: Video depicting the controversy regarding the existence of DID and
show you some debate between clinicians and researchers on the topics of
brain imaging, recovered memories, and false memories. False memory
syndrome.
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=K4NZ7_Hn-rI
Web: Video depicting the controversy regarding the existence of DID and
show you some debate between clinicians and researchers on the topics of
brain imaging, recovered memories, and false memories. Revisiting the
memory wars.
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=VcFRZsD8DLk
Web: Video illustrating patients suffering from DID. The woman with seven
personalities.
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=7TlYGivBGYE
Discussion Questions
1. Why are dissociation and trauma related to each other?
2. How is dissociation related to sleep problems?
3. Are dissociative symptoms induced or merely increased by sleep
disturbances?
4. Do you have any ideas regarding treatment possibilities for dissociative
disorders?
5. Does DID really exist?
Vocabulary
Amnesia
The loss of memory.
Anxiety disorder
A group of diagnoses in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) classification system where anxiety is central to the
persons dysfunctioning. Typical symptoms include excessive rumination,
worrying, uneasiness, apprehension, and fear about future uncertainties either
based on real or imagined events. These symptoms may affect both physical
and psychological health. The anxiety disorders are subdivided into panic
disorder, specific phobia, social phobia, posttraumatic stress disorder,
obsessive-compulsive disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder.
Borderline Personality Disorder
This personality disorder is defined by a chronic pattern of instability. This
instability manifests itself in interpersonal relationships, mood, self-image, and
behavior that can interfere with social functioning or work. It may also cause
grave emotional distress.
Cognitive failures
Every day slips and lapses, also called absentmindedness.
Consciousness
The quality or state of being aware of an external object or something within
oneself. It has been defined as the ability to experience or to feel, wakefulness,
having a sense of selfhood, and the executive control system of the mind.
Cross-sectional design
Research method that involves observation of all of a population, or a
representative subset, at one specific point in time.
Defensive coping mechanism
An unconscious process, which protects an individual from unacceptable or
painful ideas, impulses, or memories.
DES
Dissociative Experiences Scale.
DID
Dissociative identity disorder, formerly known as multiple personality disorder,
is at the far end of the dissociative disorder spectrum. It is characterized by at
least two distinct, and dissociated personality states. These personality states
or alters - alternately control a persons behavior. The sufferer therefore
experiences significant memory impairment for important information not
explained by ordinary forgetfulness.
Dissociation
A disruption in the usually integrated function of consciousness, memory,
identity, or perception of the environment.
Fantasy proneness
The tendency to extensive fantasizing or daydreaming.
General population
A sample of people representative of the average individual in our society.
Insomnia
A sleep disorder in which there is an inability to fall asleep or to stay asleep as
long as desired. Symptoms also include waking up too early, experience many
awakenings during the night, and not feeling rested during the day.
Lucid dreams
Any dream in which one is aware that one is dreaming.
Mood disorder
A group of diagnoses in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) classification system where a disturbance in the persons
mood is the primary dysfunction. Mood disorders include major depressive
disorder, bipolar disorder, dysthymic and cyclothymic disorder.
Nightmares
An unpleasant dream that can cause a strong negative emotional response
from the mind, typically fear or horror, but also despair, anxiety, and great
sadness. The dream may contain situations of danger, discomfort,
psychological or physical terror. Sufferers usually awaken in a state of distress
and may be unable to return to sleep for a prolonged period of time.
Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder
This anxiety disorder is characterized by intrusive thoughts (obsessions), by
repetitive behaviors (compulsions), or both. Obsessions produce uneasiness,
fear, or worry. Compulsions are then aimed at reducing the associated anxiety.
Examples of compulsive behaviors include excessive washing or cleaning;
repeated checking; extreme hoarding; and nervous rituals, such as switching
the light on and off a certain number of times when entering a room. Intrusive
thoughts are often sexual, violent, or religious in nature...
Prevalence
The number of cases of a specific disorder present in a given population at a
certain time.
PTM
Post-traumatic model of dissociation.
Recurrent dreams
The same dream narrative or dreamscape is experienced over different
occasions of sleep.
Schizophrenia
This mental disorder is characterized by a breakdown of thought processes and
emotional responses. Symptoms include auditory hallucinations, paranoid or
bizarre delusions, or disorganized speech and thinking. Sufferers from this
disorder experience grave dysfunctions in their social functioning and in work.
SCID-D
Structural Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Dissociative Disorders.
Self-report measure
A type of psychological test in which a person fills out a survey or questionnaire
with or without the help of an investigator.
Sleep deprivation
A sufficient lack of restorative sleep over a cumulative period so as to cause
physical or psychiatric symptoms and affect routine performances of tasks.
Sleep paralysis
Sleep paralysis occurs when the normal paralysis during REM sleep manifests
when falling asleep or awakening, often accompanied by hallucinations of
danger or a malevolent presence in the room.
Sleep-wake cycle
A daily rhythmic activity cycle, based on 24-hour intervals, that is exhibited by
many organisms.
State
When a symptom is acute, or transient, lasting from a few minutes to a few
hours.
Trait
When a symptom forms part of the personality or character.
Trauma
An event or situation that causes great distress and disruption, and that creates
substantial, lasting damage to the psychological development of a person.
Vivid dreams
A dream that is very clear, where the individual can remember the dream in
great detail.
Reference List
Abrams, M. P., Mulligan, A. D., Carleton, R. N., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2008).
Prevalence and correlates of sleep paralysis in adults reporting childhood
sexual abuse. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 15351541.
Agargun, M. Y., Kara H., Ozer, O. A., Selvi, Y., Kiran, U., & Ozer, B. (2003). Clinical
importance of nightmare disorder in patients with dissociative disorders.
Psychiatry Clinical Neuroscience, 57, 575579.
Agargun, M. Y., Kara, H., Ozer, O. A., Semiz, U., Selvi, Y., Kiran, U., & Tombul, T.
(2001). Characteristics of patients with nocturnal dissociative disorders.
Sleep and Hypnosis, 3, 131134.
Allen, J. G., & Coyne, L. (1995). Dissociation and the vulnerability to psychotic
experiences. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 183, 615622.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
Arlow, J. (1966). Depersonalization and derealization. In: R. Loewenstein, L. M.
Newman, M. Schur, & A. J. Solnit (Eds.), PsychoanalysisA general psychology
(pp. 456478). New York, NY: International Universities Press, Inc.
Barrett, D. (1995). The dream character as a prototype for the multiple
personality alter. Dissociation, 8, 61-68.
Barrett, D. (1994). Dreaming as a normal model for multiple personality
disorder. In S.J. Lynn & J.W. Rhue (Eds.), Dissociation: Clinical and theoretical
perspectives (pp. 123135). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Giesbrecht, T., Lynn, S. J., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Merckelbach, H. (2008). Cognitive
processes in dissociation: An analysis of core theoretical assumptions.
Psychological Bulletin, 134, 617647.
Giesbrecht, T., Smeets, T., Leppink, J., Jelicic, M., & Merckelbach, H. (2007). Acute
dissociation after 1 night of sleep loss. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116,
599606.
Hacking, I. (1995). Rewriting the soul: Multiple personality and the sciences.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Kihlstrom, J. F. (2005). Dissociative disorders. Annual Review of Clinical
Psychology, 10, 127.
Koffel, E., & Watson, D. (2009). The two-factor structure of sleep complaints and
its relation to depression and anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118,
183194.
Lee, W. E., Kwok, C. H. T., Hunter, E. C. M., Richards, M. & David, A. S. (2010).
Prevalence and childhood antecedents of depersonalization syndrome in
a UK birth cohort. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, (in press).
Levin, R., & Fireman, G. (2002). Nightmare prevalence, nightmare distress, and
self-reported psychological disturbance. Sleep, 25, 205212.
Levitan, H. L. (1967). Depersonalization and the dream. The Psychoanalytic
Quaterly, 36, 157-171.
McNally, R. J., & Clancy, S. A. (2005). Sleep paralysis in adults reporting repressed,
recovered, or continuous memories of childhood sexual abuse. Journal of
Author.
Rauschenberg, S.L., Lynn, S.J. (1995). Fantasy proneness, DSM-III-r axis I
psychopathology and dissociation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104,
373-380.
Ross, C. A. (2011). Possession experiences in Dissociative Identity Disorder: A
preliminary study. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 12, 393400.
Ross, C. A., Anderson, G., Fleisher, W. P., & Norton, G. R. (1991). The frequency
of Multiple Personality Disorder among psychiatric-inpatients. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 17171720.
Rufer, M., Fricke, S., Held, D., Cremer, J., & Hand, I. (2006). Dissociation and
symptom dimensions of obsessive-compulsive disorderA replication
study. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 256, 146
150.
Sandberg, D., & Lynn, S.J. (1992). Dissociative experiences, psychopathology and
adjustment, and child and adolescent maltreatment in female college
students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 717723.
Sar, V., Tutkun, H., Alyanak, B., Bakim, B., & Baral, I. (2000). Frequency of
dissociative
disorders
among
psychiatric
outpatients
in
Turkey.
Van der Hart, O., & Horst, R. (1989). The dissociation theory of Pierre Janet.
Journal of Traumatic Stress, 2, 211.
Van der Kloet, D., Giesbrecht, T., Lynn, S.J., Merckelbach, & de Zutter, A. (2011).
Sleep normalization and decrease in dissociative experiences: Evaluation
in an inpatient sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Online First
Publication, August 15, 2011. doi: 10.1037/a0024781
Vermetten, E., Schmahl, C., Lindner, S., Loewenstein, R.J. & Bremner, J.D. (2006).
Hippocampal and amygdalar volumes in dissociative identity disorder.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 630636.
Watson, D. (2001). Dissociations of the night: Individual differences in sleeprelated experiences and their relation to dissociation and schizotypy. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 526535.
Yu, J. H., Ross, C. A., Keyes, B. B., Li, Y., Dai, Y. F., Zhang, T. H., Wang, L. L., Fang,
Q., & Xiao, Z. P. (2010). Dissociative disorders among Chinese inpatients
diagnosed with schizophrenia. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 11, 358
372.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Dissociative Disorders by Dalena van Heugten
- van der Kloet is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercialShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
People with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) suffer from a profound social
disability. Social neuroscience is the study of the parts of the brain that support
social interactions or the social brain. This chapter provides an overview of
ASD and focuses on understanding how social brain dysfunction leads to ASD.
Our increasing understanding of the social brain and its dysfunction in ASD will
allow us to better identify the genes that cause ASD and will help us to create
and pick out treatments to better match individuals. Because social brain
systems emerge in infancy, social neuroscience can help us to figure out how
to diagnose ASD even before the symptoms of ASD are clearly present. This is
a hopeful time because social brain systems remain malleable well into
adulthood and thus open to creative new interventions that are informed by
state-of-the-art science.
Learning Objectives
2019
2020
2021
Figure 1. The red lines indicate the scan paths (collection of eye movements) used by
people with (right column) and without (left column) autism to explore faces. Modified from
Pelphrey et al., (2002).
2022
2023
2024
2025
The integration of imaging methods is critical for this endeavor. Using face
perception as an example, the combination of fMRI and ERP could identify who,
of those individuals with ASD, shows anomalies in the FG and then determine
the stage of information processing at which these impairments occur. Because
different processing stages often reflect discrete cognitive processes, this level
of understanding could encourage treatments that address specific processing
deficits at the neural level.
For example, differences observed in the early processing stages might
reflect problems with low-level visual perception, while later differences would
indicate problems with higher-order processes, such as emotion recognition.
These same principles can be applied to the broader network of social brain
regions and, combined with measures of behavioral functioning, could offer a
comprehensive profile of brain-behavior performance for a given individual. A
fundamental goal for this kind of subgroup approach is to improve the ability
to tailor treatments to the individual.
Another objective is to improve the power of other scientific tools. Most
studies of individuals with ASD compare groups of individuals, for example,
individuals on with ASD compared to typically developing peers. However,
studies have also attempted to compare children across the autism spectrum
by group according to differential diagnosis (e.g., Aspergers disorder versus
autistic disorder), or by other behavioral or cognitive characteristics (e.g.,
cognitively able versus intellectually disabled or anxious versus non-anxious).
Yet, the power of a scientific study to detect these kinds of significant,
meaningful, individual differences is only as strong as the accuracy of the factor
used to define the compared groups.
The identification of distinct subgroups within the autism spectrum
according to information about the brain would allow for a more accurate and
detailed exposition of the individual differences seen in those with ASD. This is
especially critical for the success of investigations into the genetic basis of ASD.
As mentioned before, the genes discovered thus far account for only a small
nobaproject.com - Autism: Insights from the Study of the Social Brain
2026
or
biomarkers,
can
help
guide
genetic
research.
2027
studies of infant siblings of children with ASD and a comparison group of infant
siblings without familial risks. Such designs gather longitudinal information
about developmental trajectories across the first three years of life for both
groups followed by clinical diagnosis at approximately 36 months.
These studies are problematic in that many of the social features of autism
do not emerge in typical development until after 12 months of age, and it is not
certain that these symptoms will manifest during the limited periods of
observation involved in clinical evaluations or in pediatricians offices.
Moreover, across development, but especially during infancy, behavior is widely
variable and often unreliable, and at present, behavioral observation is the only
means to detect symptoms of ASD and to confirm a diagnosis. This is quite
problematic because, even highly sophisticated behavioral methods, such as
eye tracking (see Figure 1), do not necessarily reveal reliable differences in
infants with ASD (Ozonoff et al., 2010). However, measuring the brain activity
associated with social perception can detect differences that do not appear in
behavior until much later. The identification of biomarkers utilizing the imaging
methods we have described offers promise for earlier detection of atypical
social development.
ERP measures of brain response predict subsequent development of autism
in infants as young as six months old who showed normal patterns of visual
fixation (as measured by eye tracking) (Elsabbagh et al., 2012). This suggests
the great promise of brain imaging for earlier recognition of ASD. With earlier
detection, treatments could move from addressing existing symptoms to
preventing their emergence by altering the course of abnormal brain
development and steering it toward normality.
2028
2029
Outside Resources
Web: American Psychiatric Associations website for the 5th edition of the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
http://www.dsm5.org
Web: Autism Science Foundation - organization supporting autism research
by providing funding and other assistance to scientists and organizations
conducting, facilitating, publicizing and disseminating autism research. The
organization also provides information about autism to the general public
and serves to increase awareness of autism spectrum disorders and the
needs of individuals and families affected by autism.
http://www.autismsciencefoundation.org/
Web: Autism Speaks - Autism science and advocacy organization
http://www.autismspeaks.org/
Discussion Questions
1. How can neuroimaging inform our understanding of the causes of autism?
2. What are the ways in which neuroimaging, including fMRI and ERP, may
benefit efforts to diagnosis and treat autism?
3. How can an understanding of the social brain help us to understand ASD?
4. What are the core symptoms of ASD, and why is the social brain of particular
interest?
5. What are some of the components of the social brain, and what functions
do they serve?
Vocabulary
Endophenotypes
A characteristic that reflects a genetic liability for disease and a more basic
component of a complex clinical presentation. Endophenotypes are less
developmentally malleable than overt behavior.
Social brain
The set of neuroanatomical structures that allows us to understand the actions
and intentions of other people.
Reference List
Allison, T., Puce, A., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues:
Role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4(7), 267278.
Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., et al. (1996). Electrophysiological studies
of face perception in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(6), 551
565.
Brothers, L. (1990). The social brain: A project for integrating primate behavior
and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts in Neuroscience, 1, 2751.
Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children
with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of
Autism & Developmental Disorders, 28(6), 479485.
Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., & McPartland, J. (2005). Understanding the nature of
face processing impairment in autism: Insights from behavioral and
electrophysiological studies. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(3), 403
424.
Elsabbagh, M., Mercure, E., Hudry, K., Chandler, S., Pasco, G., Charman, T., et al.
(2012). Infant neural sensitivity to dynamic eye gaze is associated with later
emerging autism. Current Biology, 22(4), 338342.
Geschwind, D. H., & Levitt, P. (2007). Autism spectrum disorders: Developmental
disconnection syndromes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17(1), 103111.
Goren, C. C., Sarty, M., & Wu, P. Y. (1975). Visual following and pattern
discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. Pediatrics, 56(4), 544
549.
Gottesman I. I., & Shields, J. (1973) Genetic theorizing and schizophrenia. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 122, 1530.
Hobson, R. (1986). The autistic childs appraisal of expressions of emotion.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27(3), 321342.
Johnson, M. H. (2006). Biological motion: A perceptual life detector? Current
Biology, 16(10), R376377.
Kaiser, M. D., Hudac, C. M., Shultz, S., Lee, S. M., Cheung, C., Berken, A. M., et al.
(2010). Neural signatures of autism. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(49), 2122321228.
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2,
217250.
Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with
autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion.
Nature, 459(7244), 257261.
Maestro, S., Muratori, F., Cavallaro, M. C., Pei, F., Stern, D., Golse, B., et al. (2002).
Attentional skills during the first 6 months of age in autism spectrum
disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 41(10), 12391245.
McCleery, J. P., Akshoomoff, N., Dobkins, K. R., & Carver, L. J. (2009). Atypical face
versus object processing and hemispheric asymmetries in 10-month-old
infants at risk for autism. Biological Psychiatry, 66(10), 950957.
McPartland, J. C., Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., Panagiotides, H., & Carver, L. J. (2004).
Event-related brain potentials reveal anomalies in temporal processing of
faces in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 45(7), 12351245.
McPartland, J. C., Webb, S. J., Keehn, B., & Dawson, G. (2011). Patterns of visual
attention to faces and objects in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of
Autism and Develop Disorders, 41(2), 148157.
Minshew, N. J., & Williams, D. L. (2007). The new neurobiology of autism: Cortex,
connectivity, and neuronal organization. Archives of Neurology, 64(7), 945
950.
Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A
study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 24, 247-257.
Osterling, J. A., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. A. (2002). Early recognition of 1-yearold infants with autism spectrum disorder versus mental retardation.
Development & Psychopathology, 14(2), 239251.
Ozonoff, S., Iosif, A. M., Baguio, F., Cook, I. C., Hill, M. M., Hutman, T., et al. (2010).
A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(3),
256266.
Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B. D., & Piven, J.
(2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism &
Developmental Disorders, 32(4), 249261.
Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, R. K., Anderson, A. W., Volkmar, F.,
et al. (2000). Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face
discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(4), 331340.
Simion, F., Regolin, L., & Bulf, H. (2008). A predisposition for biological motion
in the newborn baby. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105
(2), 809813.
de Haan, M., Johnson, M. H., & Halit, H. (2003). Development of face-sensitive
event-related potentials during infancy: A review. International Journal of
Psychophysiology, 51(1), 4558.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Autism: Insights from the Study of the Social
Brain by Kevin A. Pelphrey is licensed under the Creative Commons AttributionNonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Psychopathy
Chris Patrick
Florida State University
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Psychopathy (or psychopathic personality) is a topic that has long fascinated
the public at large as well as scientists and clinical practitioners. However, it
has also been subject to considerable confusion and scholarly debate over the
years. This chapter reviews alternative conceptions of psychopathy that have
been proposed historically, and reviews major instruments currently in use for
the assessment of psychopathic tendencies in clinical and nonclinical samples.
An integrative theoretic framework, the Triarchic model, is presented that
provides a basis for reconciling differing historic conceptions and assessment
approaches. Implications of the model for thinking about causal hypotheses
of psychopathy, and for resolving longstanding points of contention in the field,
are discussed.
Learning Objectives
Become familiar with the Triarchic model of psychopathy and its constituent
constructs of boldness, meanness, and disinhibition.
Introduction
For many in the public at large, the term psychopath conjures up images of
ruthless homicidal maniacs and criminal masterminds. This impression is
reinforced on an ongoing basis by depictions of psychopathic individuals in
popular books and films, such as No Country for Old Men, Silence of the Lambs,
and Catch Me if You Can, and by media accounts of high-profile criminals ranging
from Charles Manson to Jeffrey Dahmer to Bernie Madoff. However, the concept
of psychopathy (psychopathic personality) held by experts in the mental
health field differs sharply from this common public perceptionemphasizing
distinct dispositional tendencies as opposed to serious criminal acts of one sort
or another. This chapter reviews historic and contemporary conceptions of
psychopathy as a clinical disorder, describes methods for assessing it, and
discusses how a new conceptual model can help to address key questions
regarding its nature and origins that have long been debated. It will be seen
from this review that the topic remains no less fascinating or socially relevant
when considered from a clinicalscientific perspective.
Historic Conceptions
Early writers characterized psychopathy as an atypical form of mental illness
in which rational faculties appeared normal but everyday behavior and social
relationships are markedly disrupted. French physician Philippe Pinel
(1806/1962) documented cases of what he called manie sans delire (insanity
without delirium), in which dramatic episodes of recklessness and aggression
occurred in individuals not suffering from obvious clouding of the mind. German
psychiatrist Julius Koch (1888) introduced the disease-oriented term
psychopathic to convey the idea that conditions of this type had a strong
constitutional-heritable basis. In his seminal book The Mask of Sanity, which
focused on patients committed for hospital treatment, American psychiatrist
nobaproject.com - Psychopathy
2041
2042
2043
Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI; Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996), which was
developed to index personality dispositions embodied within historic
conceptions of psychopathy. Its current revised form (PPI-R; Lilienfeld & Widows,
2005) contains 154 items, organized into eight facet scales.
nobaproject.com - Psychopathy
2044
Like the items of the PCL-R, the subscales of the PPI cohere around two
distinguishable factors: a fearless dominance (FD) factor reflecting social
potency, stress immunity, and fearlessness, and a self-centered impulsivity (SCI)
factor reflecting egocentricity, exploitativeness, hostile rebelliousness, and lack
of planning. However, unlike the factors of the PCL-R, the two PPI factors are
uncorrelated, and thus even more distinct in their external correlates. Scores
on PPI-FD are associated with indices of positive psychological adjustment (e.
g., higher well-being; lower anxiety and depression) and measures of narcissism
(low) empathy, and thrill/adventure seeking (Benning, Patrick, Blonigen, Hicks,
& Iacono, 2005). Given this, PPI-FD has been interpreted as capturing a more
adaptive expression of dispositional fearlessness (i.e., boldness; see below)
than the interpersonal-affective factor of the PCL-Rwhich can be viewed as
tapping a more pathologic (antagonistic or mean) expression of fearlessness.
Scores on PPI-SCI, like Factor 2 of the PCL-R, are associated with multiple
indicators of deviancyincluding impulsivity and aggressiveness, child and
adult antisocial behavior, substance abuse problems, heightened distress and
dysphoria, and suicidal ideation.
2045
Core
ingredients
of
psychopathy:
disinhibition,
2046
Definitions
Disinhibition as described in the Triarchic model encompasses tendencies
toward impulsiveness, weak behavioral restraint, hostility and mistrust, and
difficulties in regulating emotion. Meanness entails deficient empathy, lack of
affiliative capacity, contempt toward others, predatory exploitativeness, and
empowerment through cruelty and destructiveness. Referents for disinhibition
and meanness include the finding of distinct I/CP and CU factors in the child
psychopathy literature and corresponding evidence for distinct disinhibitory
and callous-aggression factors underlying impulse control (externalizing)
problems in adults (Krueger, Markon, Patrick, Benning, & Kramer, 2007). The
third construct in the model, Boldness,encompasses dominance, social
assurance, emotional resiliency, and venturesomeness. Referents for this
construct include the mask elements of Cleckleys conception, Lykkens (1995)
low fear theory of psychopathy, the FD factor of the PPI, and developmental
research on fearless temperament as a possible precursor to psychopathy
(Patrick et al., 2009).
From the perspective of the Triarchic model, Cleckleys conception of
psychopathy emphasized boldness and disinhibition, whereas criminally
oriented conceptions (and affiliated measures, including the PCL-R and APSD)
emphasize meanness and disinhibition more so. According to the model,
individuals high in disinhibitory tendencies would warrant a diagnosis of
psychopathy if also high in boldness or meanness (or both), but individuals high
on only one of these tendencies would not. Individuals with differing relative
elevations on these three symptomatic components would account for
contrasting variants (subtypes) of psychopathy as described in the literature
(Hicks, Markon, Patrick, Krueger, & Newman, 2004; Karpman, 1941; Skeem,
Johansson, Andershed, Kerr, & Louden, 2007).
nobaproject.com - Psychopathy
2047
Table 1. Sample items from the Triarchic Psychopathy Measure (TriPM; Patrick, 2010)
Although the TriPM is relatively new, promising evidence for its convergent
and discriminant validity has begun to appear (e.g., Sellbom & Phillips, 2013;
Strickland et al., in press; see also Venables & Patrick, 2012). Given that the
inventory is freely available online, and that several foreign-language
nobaproject.com - Psychopathy
2048
Causal factors
Considerable research has been devoted over many years to investigation of
causal factors in psychopathy. Existing theories are of two types: (1) theories
emphasizing core deficits in emotional sensitivity or responsiveness, and (2)
theories positing basic impairments in cognitive-attentional processing (Patrick
& Bernat, 2009). In support of these alternative theories, differing
neurobiological correlates of psychopathy have been reported. One of the most
consistent entails a lack of normal enhancement of the startle blink reflex to
abrupt noises occurring during viewing of aversive foreground stimuli (e.g.,
scary or disturbing pictorial images) as compared with neutral or pleasant
stimuli (see Figure 1). This result, akin to a failure to jump upon hearing a trash
can tip while walking alone in a dark alley, has been interpreted as reflecting a
lack of normal defensive (fear) reactivity. Another fairly consistent finding
involves reduced amplitude of brain potential response to intermittent target
stimuli, or following incorrect responses, within cognitive performance tasks
indicative of reduced cortical-attentional processing or impaired action
monitoring (Patrick & Bernat, 2009). Yet other research using functional
neuroimaging has demonstrated deficits in basic subcortical (amygdala)
reactivity to interpersonal distress cues (e.g., fearful human faces) in highnobaproject.com - Psychopathy
2049
psychopathic individuals (Jones, Laurens, Herba, Barker, & Viding, 2009; Marsh
et al., 2008).
The Triarchic model may prove to be of use for reconciling alternative causal
models of psychopathy that have been proposed based on contrasting
neurobiological and behavioral findings. For example, lack of startle
enhancement during aversive cuing has been tied specifically to the
interpersonal-affective factor of the PCL-R and the counterpart FD factor of the
PPI (Figure 1)suggesting a link to the boldness component of psychopathy.
By contrast, reduced brain potential responses in cognitive tasks appear more
related to impulsive-externalizing tendencies associated with the disinhibition
component of psychopathy (Carlson, Thi, & McLaron, 2009; Patrick & Bernat,
2009). On the other hand, the finding of reduced subcortical response to
affective facial cues has been tied to the CU traits factor of child/adolescent
psychopathy, a referent for meanness in the Triarchic model. However, further
research is needed to determine whether this finding reflects fear deficits
common to meanness and boldness, or deficits in affiliative capacity or empathy
specific to meanness.
nobaproject.com - Psychopathy
2050
Figure 1. Evidence for lack of normal augmentation of the defensive startle reflex during
viewing of aversive visual images in high-psychopathic individuals.
Upper plot: Mean magnitude of startle blink responses to noise probes occurring during
viewing of pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant picture stimuli in two male prisoner groups: (1)
prisoners scoring high on the antisocial behavior (AB) factor of the Psychopathy ChecklistRevised (PCL-R; Hare, 2003) but not the interpersonal-affective (IA) factor (labeled High PCLR AB only in the plot; n = 18), and (2) prisoners high on both factors of the PCL-R (labeled
High PCL-R IA & AB; n = 17).
nobaproject.com - Psychopathy
2051
Lower plot: Mean magnitude of startle blink responses to noise probes occurring during
viewing of pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant picture stimuli in two subgroups of young males
from a large community sample (overall N = 307): (1) lowest 10% of scorers (n = 31) within the
sample on the fearless dominance factor of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI;
Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996), labeled Low PPI-FD in the plot, and (2) highest 10% of scorers
(n = 31) on the PPI fearless dominance factor of the PPI, labeled High PPI-FD in the plot.
In both plots, blink means for each picture type are presented in z-score units (M = 0, SD
= 1) derived by standardizing raw blink magnitude scores across trials for each individual
subject. Data in the upper plot are from Patrick, Bradley, and Lang (1993); data in the lower
plot are from Benning, Patrick, and Iacono (2005).
nobaproject.com - Psychopathy
2052
2053
analytic studies of criminal offenders exhibiting high overall scores on the PCLR have demonstrated one subtype characterized by low anxiety in particular,
and another exhibiting high anxiety along with very high levels of impulsivity
and aggression (Hicks et al., 2004; Skeem et al., 2007). The implication is that
low anxiousness is central to one variant of criminal psychopathy (the bolddisinhibited, or primary type) but not to another variant (the disinhibitedmean, aggressive-externalizing, or secondary type).
A further key question is whether violent/aggressive tendencies are typical
of psychopathic individuals and should be included in the definition of the
disorder. Cleckleys (1941/1976) view was that such tendencies should be
regarded as the exception rather than as the rule (p. 262). However,
aggressiveness is central to criminally oriented conceptions of psychopathy,
and the PCL-R includes an item reflecting hot-temperedness and aggression
(poor behavioral controls) along with other items scored in part based on
indications of cruelty and violence. In the Triarchic model, tendencies toward
aggression are represented in both the disinhibition and meanness constructs,
and a mean-disinhibited type of psychopath clearly exists, marked by the
presence of salient aggressive behavior (Frick & Marsee, 2006; Hicks et al., 2004).
Thus, Cleckleys idea of aggression as ancillary to psychopathy may apply more
to a variant of psychopathy that entails high boldness in conjunction with high
disinhibition (Hicks et al., 2004).
Another question is whether criminal or antisocial behavior more broadly
represents a defining feature of psychopathy, or a secondary manifestation
(Cooke, Michie, Hart, & Clark, 2004). From the standpoint of the Triarchic model,
antisocial behavior arises from the complex interplay of different deviancepromoting influencesincluding dispositional boldness, meanness, and
disinhibition. However, whether approaches can be developed for classifying
antisocial behaviors in ways that relate more selectively to these and other
distinct influences (e.g., through reference to underlying motives, spontaneity
versus premeditation) is an important topic to be addressed in future research.
nobaproject.com - Psychopathy
2054
2055
(Verona & Vitale, 2006). Some intriguing evidence exists for thisincluding twin
research findings demonstrating a genetic association between dispositional
boldness (as indexed by estimated scores on PPI-FD) and a composite index of
externalizing problems in male but not female participants (Blonigen, Hicks,
Patrick, Krueger, Iacono, & McGue, 2005). However, more extensive research
along these lines, examining all facets of the Triarchic model in relation to
behavioral outcomes of differing kinds, will be required to effectively address
the question of gender-moderated expression.
A final intriguing question is whether successful psychopaths exist. Hall
and Benning (2006) hypothesized that successful psychopathy entails a
preponderance of certain causal influences (resulting in particular symptomatic
features) over others. Drawing on known correlates of PPI-FD (e.g., Benning et
al., 2005; Ross, Benning, Patrick, Thompson, & Thurston, 2009) and theories
positing separate etiologic mechanisms for differing features of psychopathy
(Fowles & Dindo, 2009; Patrick & Bernat, 2009), these authors proposed that
the presence of dispositional fearlessness (boldness) may be conducive to
success when not accompanied by high externalizing proneness (disinhibition).
For example, high-bold/low-disinhibited individuals could be expected to
achieve higher success in occupations calling for leadership and/or courage
because their psychopathic tendencies are manifested mainly in terms of social
effectiveness, affective resilience, and venturesomeness.
Data relevant to this idea come from an intriguing study by Lilienfeld,
Waldman, Landfield, Rubenzer, and Faschingbauer (2012), who used
personality trait ratings of former U.S. presidents provided by expert historians
to estimate scores on the FD and SCI factors of the PPI (Ross et al., 2009). They
found that higher estimated levels of PPI-FD (boldness) predicted higher ratings
of
presidential
performance,
persuasiveness,
leadership,
and
crisis
2056
nobaproject.com - Psychopathy
2057
Outside Resources
Book: (Fictional novels or biographies/biographical novels) Capote, T. (1966).
In cold blood. New York, NY: Random House.
Book: (Reference) Babiak, P., & Hare, R. D. (2006). Snakes in suits. New York,
NY: HarperCollins.
Book: (Reference) Blair, R. J. R., Mitchell, D., & Blair, K. (2005). The psychopath:
Emotion and the brain. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Book: (Reference) Raine, A. (2013). The anatomy of violence. New York, NY:
Random House.
Book: (Reference) Salekin, R., & Lynam, D. T. (2010). Handbook of child and
adolescent psychopathy. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Movie: (Psychopathic criminals) Demme, J., Saraf, P., Saxon, E. (Producers), &
Sena, D. (Director). (1993). Kalifornia. United States: Gramercy.
Movie: (Psychopathic criminals) Ward, F., Bozman, R., Utt, K., Demme, J.
(Producers), & Armitrage, G. (Director). (1990). Miami blues. United States:
Orion.
Web: Hervey Cleckleys classic book The Mask of Sanity is no longer in print
at this time, but a version authorized for nonprofit educational use by his
estate can be viewed online at
http://www.quantumfuture.net/store/sanity_1.PdF
Web: The Triarchic Psychopathy Measure can be accessed online at
https://www.phenxtoolkit.org/index.php?pageLink=browse.protocoldetails&id=121601
Web: The website for the Aftermath Foundation, a nonprofit organization
that provides information and support for victims and family members of
psychopathic individuals, is
http://www.aftermath-surviving-psychopathy.org/
Web: The website for the Society for Scientific Study of Psychopathy, the
major association for researchers in this area, is
http://www.psychopathysociety.org/index.php?lang=en-US
Discussion Questions
1. What did Cleckley mean when he characterized psychopathy as involving a
Mask of Sanity?
2. Compare and contrast the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R), the
Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD), and the Psychopathic
Personality Inventory (PPI), in terms of the samples they are designed for,
the way in which they are administered, and the content and factor structure
of their items.
3. Identify and define the three facet constructs of the Triarchic model of
psychopathy. Discuss how these facet constructs relate to the factors of
the PCL-R, ASPD, and PPI. Discuss how U.S. President Teddy Roosevelt and
fictional character Anton Chigurh from the film No Country for Old Men
might compare in terms of scores on the three Triarchic constructs.
4. Identify alternative types of theories that have been proposed regarding
the cause of psychopathy, and how these can be viewed from the
perspective of the Triarchic model.
5. Identify two longstanding issues of debate regarding the nature/definition
of psychopathy and how these issues are addressed by the Triarchic model.
Vocabulary
Antisocial personality disorder
Counterpart diagnosis to psychopathy included in the third through fifth
editions of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM; APA,
2000). Defined by specific symptoms of behavioral deviancy in childhood (e.g.,
fighting, lying, stealing, truancy) continuing into adulthood (manifested as
repeated rule-breaking, impulsiveness, irresponsibility, aggressiveness, etc.).
Psychopathy
Synonymous with psychopathic personality, the term used by Cleckley
(1941/1976), and adapted from the term psychopathic introduced by German
psychiatrist Julius Koch (1888) to designate mental disorders presumed to be
heritable.
Triarchic model
Model formulated to reconcile alternative historic conceptions of psychopathy
and differing methods for assessing it.
Conceives of psychopathy as
Reference List
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Blonigen, D. M., Hicks, B. M., & Iacono, W. G. (2005).
Estimating facets of psychopathy from normal personality traits: A step
toward community-epidemiological investigations. Assessment, 12, 318.
Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., & Iacono, W. G. (2005). Psychopathy, startle blink
modulation, and electrodermal reactivity in twin men. Psychophysiology,
42, 753-762.
Blonigen, D. M., Hicks, B. M., Patrick, C. J., Krueger, R. F., Iacono, W. G., & McGue,
M. K. (2005). Psychopathic personality traits: Heritability and genetic overlap
with internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Psychological
Medicine, 35, 637648.
Carlson, S. R., Thi, S., & McLaron, M. E. (2009). Visual P3 amplitude and selfreported psychopathic personality traits: Frontal reduction is associated
with self-centered impulsivity. Psychophysiology, 46, 100113.
Cleckley, H. (1976). The mask of sanity (5th ed.). St. Louis, MO: Mosby. (Original
edition published in 1941)
Cooke, D. J., Michie, C., Hart, S. D., & Clark, D. A. (2004). Reconstructing
psychopathy: Clarifying the significance of antisocial and socially deviant
behavior in the diagnosis of psychopathic personality disorder. Journal of
Personality Disorders, 18, 337357.
traits.
Washington,
DC:
American
Psychiatric
org/index.php?pageLink=browse.protocoldetails&id=121601
Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Emotion in the criminal
psychopath: Startle reflex modulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102,
82-92.
Patrick, C. J., Fowles, D. C., & Krueger, R. F. (2009). Triarchic conceptualization of
psychopathy: Developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and
meanness. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 913938.
Patrick, C. J., & Bernat, E. M. (2009). Neurobiology of psychopathy: A two-process
theory. In: G. G. Berntson & J. T. Cacioppo (Eds.), Handbook of neuroscience
for the behavioral sciences (pp. 11101131). New York, NY: John Wiley &
Sons.
Patrick, C. J., & Bernat, E. M. (2009). Neurobiology of psychopathy: A two-process
theory. In: G. G. Berntson & J. T. Cacioppo (Eds.), Handbook of neuroscience
for the behavioral sciences (pp. 11101131). New York, NY: John Wiley &
Sons.
Pinel, P. (1962). A treatise on insanity (D. Davis, translator). New York, NY: Hafner.
(Original edition published in 1806)
Pressman, E. R. (Producer), & Stone, O. (Director). (1987). Wall Street. United
States: 20th Century Fox.
Ross, S. R., Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Thompson, A., & Thurston, A. (2009).
Factors of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory: Criterion-related validity
and relationship to the BIS/BAS and Five-Factor models of personality.
Assessment, 16, 7187.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Psychopathy by Chris Patrick is licensed under
the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. To
view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.
en_US.
Abstract
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a psychiatric disorder that is
most often diagnosed in school-aged children. Many children with ADHD find
it difficult to focus on tasks and follow instructions, and these characteristics
can lead to problems in school and at home. How children with ADHD are
diagnosed and treated is a topic of controversy, and many people, including
scientists and nonscientists alike, hold strong beliefs about what ADHD is and
how people with the disorder should be treated. This chapter will familiarize
the reader with the scientific literature on ADHD. First, we will review how ADHD
is diagnosed in children, with a focus on how mental health professionals
distinguish between ADHD and normal behavior problems in childhood.
Second, we will describe what is known about the causes of ADHD. Third, we
will describe the treatments that are used to help children with ADHD and their
families. The chapter will conclude with a brief discussion of how we expect
that the diagnosis and treatment of ADHD will change over the coming decades.
Learning Objectives
Introduction
Childhood is a stage of life characterized by rapid and profound development.
Starting at birth, children develop the skills necessary to function in the world
around them at a rate that is faster than any other time in life. This is no small
accomplishment! By the end of their first decade of life, most children have
mastered the complex cognitive operations required to comply with rules, such
as stopping themselves from acting impulsively, paying attention to parents
and teachers in the face of distraction, and sitting still despite boredom. Indeed,
acquiring self-control is an important developmental task for children (Mischel,
Shoda, & Rodriguez, 1989), because they are expected to comply with directions
from adults, stay on task at school, and play appropriately with peers. For
children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), however,
exercising self-control is a unique challenge. These children, oftentimes despite
their best intentions, struggle to comply with adults instructions, and they are
often labeled as problem children and rule breakers. Historically, people
viewed these children as willfully noncompliant due to moral or motivational
defect (Still, 1902). However, scientists now know that the noncompliance
observed in children with ADHD can be explained by a number of factors,
including neurological dysfunction.
The goal of this chapter is to review the classification, causes, consequences,
and treatment of ADHD. ADHD is somewhat unique among the psychiatric
disorders in that most people hold strong opinions about the disorder, perhaps
due to its more controversial qualities. When applicable, we will discuss some
of the controversial beliefs held by social critics and laypeople, as well as
scientists who study the disorder. Our hope is that a discussion of these
controversies will allow you to reach your own conclusions about the legitimacy
of the disorder.
2075
2076
2077
2078
One goal of this chapter will be to examine whether ADHD meets the criteria
of a true disorder. The first criterion states that children with ADHD should
show impairment in major functional domains. This is certainly true for children
with ADHD. These children have lower academic achievement compared with
their peers. They are more likely to repeat a grade or be suspended and less
nobaproject.com - ADHD and Behavior Disorders in Children
2079
likely to graduate from high school (Loe & Feldman, 2007). Children with ADHD
are often unpopular among their peers, and many of these children are actively
disliked and socially rejected (Landau, Milich, & Diener, 1998). Children with
ADHD are likely to experience comorbid psychological problems such as
learning disorders, depression, anxiety, and oppositional defiant disorder. As
they grow up, adolescents and adults with ADHD are at risk to abuse alcohol
and other drugs (Molina & Pelham, 2003) and experience other adverse
outcomes (see Box 1). In sum, there is sufficient evidence to conclude that
children diagnosed with ADHD are significantly impaired by their symptoms.
It is also important to determine that a childs symptoms are not caused by
normal patterns of development. Many of the behaviors that are diagnostic of
ADHD in some children would be considered developmentally appropriate for
a younger child. This is true for many psychological and psychiatric disorders
in childhood. For example, bedwetting is quite common in 3-year-old children;
at this age, most children have not gained control over nighttime urination. For
this reason, a 3-year-old child who wets the bed would not be diagnosed with
enuresis (i.e., the clinical term for chronic bedwetting), because his or her
behavior is developmentally appropriate. Bedwetting in an 8-year-old child,
however, is developmentally inappropriate. At this age, children are expected
to remain dry overnight, and failure to master this skill would prevent children
from sleeping over at friends houses or attending overnight camps. A similar
example of developmentally appropriate versus inappropriate hyperactivity
and noncompliance is provided in Box 2.
2080
2081
Most experts believe that genetic and neurophysiological factors cause the
majority of ADHD cases. Indeed, ADHD is primarily a genetic disordertwin
studies find that whether or not a child develops ADHD is due in large part (75%)
to genetic variations (Faraone et al., 2005). Further, children with a family history
of ADHD are more likely to develop ADHD themselves (Faraone & Biederman,
1994). Specific genes that have been associated with ADHD are linked to
neurotransmitters
such
as
dopamine
and
serotonin.
In
addition,
neuroimagining studies have found that children with ADHD show reduced
brain volume in some regions of the brain, such as the prefrontal cortex, the
corpus callosum, the anterior cingulate cortex, the basal ganglia, and the
cerebellum (Seidman, Valera, & Makris, 2005). Among their other functions,
these regions of the brain are implicated in organization, impulse control, and
motor activity, so the reduced volume of these structures in children with ADHD
may cause some of their symptoms.
Although genetics appear to be a main cause of ADHD, recent studies have
shown that environmental risk factors may cause a minority of ADHD cases.
Many of these environmental risk factors increase the risk for ADHD by
disrupting early development and compromising the integrity of the central
nervous system. Environmental influences such as low birth weight,
malnutrition, and maternal alcohol and nicotine use during pregnancy can
increase the likelihood that a child will develop ADHD (Mick, Biederman,
Faraone, Sayer, & Kleinman, 2002). Additionally, recent studies have shown that
exposure to environmental toxins, such as lead and pesticides, early in a childs
life may also increase risk of developing ADHD (Nigg, 2006).
2082
2083
2084
Medication
The most common method of treating ADHD is to prescribe stimulant
medications such as Adderall. These medications treat many of the core
symptoms of ADHDtreated children will show improved impulse control,
time-on-task, and compliance with adults, and decreased hyperactivity and
disruptive behavior. However, there are also negative side effects to stimulant
medication, such as growth and appetite suppression, increased blood
pressure, insomnia, and changes in mood (Barkley, 2006). Although these side
effects can be unpleasant for children, they can often be avoided with careful
monitoring and dosage adjustments.
Opinions differ on whether stimulants should be used to treat children with
ADHD. Proponents argue that stimulants are relatively safe and effective, and
that untreated ADHD poses a much greater risk to children (Barkley, 2006).
Critics argue that because many stimulant medications are similar to illicit drugs,
such as cocaine and methamphetamine, long-term use may cause
cardiovascular problems or predispose children to abuse illicit drugs. However,
longitudinal studies have shown that people taking these medications are not
more likely to experience cardiovascular problems or to abuse drugs
nobaproject.com - ADHD and Behavior Disorders in Children
2085
(Biederman, Wilens, Mick, Spencer, & Faraone, 1999; Cooper et al., 2011). On
the other hand, it is not entirely clear how long-term stimulant treatment can
affect the brain, particularly in adults who have been medicated for ADHD since
childhood.
Finally, critics of psychostimulant medication have proposed that stimulants
are increasingly being used to manage energetic but otherwise healthy children.
It is true that the percentage of children prescribed stimulant medication has
increased since the 1980s. This increase in use is not unique to stimulant
medication, however. Prescription rates have similarly increased for most types
of psychiatric medication (Olfson, Marcus, Weissman, & Jensen, 2002). As
parents and teachers become more aware of ADHD, one would expect that
more children with ADHD will be identified and treated with stimulant
medication. Further, the percentage of children in the United States being
treated with stimulant medication is lower than the estimated prevalence of
children with ADHD in the general population (Nigg, 2006).
2086
2087
2088
2089
gain access to illicit medication, policy makers and researchers can make efforts
to curtail the rate of stimulant misuse. For example, because drug diversion is
a major source of illicit stimulants, policymakers have enacted prescription
monitoring programs to keep track of patients prescription-seeking behavior
(Office of Drug Control Policy, 2011), and, in some cases, patients are required
to pass drug screens before receiving their prescriptions. To address
malingering, researchers are working to develop psychological tests that can
identify individuals who are faking symptoms (Jasinski et al., 2011). Finally,
pharmacologists are working to develop stimulant medications that do not
carry the same risk of abuse as the currently available drugs (e.g.,
lisdexamfetamine) (Biederman et al., 2007).
Although all of these measures will reduce illicit users access to stimulant
medication, it is important to consider how the policies will affect access among
people who need these medications to treat their ADHD symptoms.
Prescription tracking programs may reduce physicians willingness to prescribe
stimulants out of fear of being investigated by law enforcement. Patients with
ADHD with comorbid substance abuse problems may be denied access to
stimulant medication because they are considered high risk for drug diversion.
Similarly, lengthy psychological evaluations to assess for malingering and
mandated drug screenings may be prohibitively expensive for less affluent
individuals with ADHD. These measures to reduce illicit drug use are necessary
from a public health perspective, but as we move forward and enact policies to
reduce stimulant abuse, it will be equally important to consider impact of such
legislation on patients access to treatment.
2090
2091
Discussion Questions
1. Does ADHD meet the definition of a psychiatric disorder?
2. Explain the difference between developmentally appropriate and
developmentally inappropriate behavior problems.
3. Do you believe that it is ethical to prescribe stimulant medication to children?
Why or why not? What are the risks associated with withholding stimulant
medication from children with ADHD?
4. How should society balance the need to treat individuals with ADHD using
stimulants with public health concerns about the abuse of these same
medications?
Vocabulary
Contingency management
A reward or punishment that systematically follows a behavior. Parents can use
contingencies to modify their childrens behavior.
Drug diversion
When a drug that is prescribed to treat a medical condition is given to another
individual who seeks to use the drug illicitly.
Malingering
Fabrication or exaggeration of medical symptoms to achieve secondary gain
(e.g., receive medication, avoid school).
Oppositional defiant disorder
A childhood behavior disorder that is characterized by stubbornness, hostility,
and behavioral defiance. This disorder is highly comorbid with ADHD.
Parent management training
A treatment for childhood behavior problems that teaches parents how to use
contingencies to more effectively manage their childrens behavior.
Pathologizes
To define a trait or collection of traits as medically or psychologically unhealthy
or abnormal.
Reference List
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (4th ed., text revision.). Washing DC: Author.
Barkley, R. A. (2006). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for
diagnosis and treatment (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Barkley, R. A., & Cunningham, C. E. (1979). Effects of methylphenidate on the
mother-child interactions of hyperactive children. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 36, 201208.
Biederman, J., Boellner, S. W., Childress, A., Lopez, F. A., Krishnan, S., & Zhang,
Y. X. (2007). Lisdexamfetamine dimesylate and mixed amphetamine salts
extended-release in children with ADHD: A double-blind, placebocontrolled, crossover analog classroom study. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 970
976.
Biederman, J., Wilens, T., Mick, E., Spencer, T., & Faraone, S. V. (1999).
Pharmacotherapy of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder reduces risk
for substance use disorder. Pediatrics, 104, 20.
Califano, J. (2004). Youve got drugs! Prescription drug pushers on the Internet.
A CASA white paper. The National Center on Addiction and Substance Abuse.
New York, NY: Columbia University.
Campbell, S. B. (2002). Behavior problems in preschool children. (2nd ed.). New
York, NY: Guilford Press.
Cooper, W. O., Habel, L. A., Sox, C. M., Chan, K. A., Arbogast, P. G., Cheetham, C.,
Hinshaw, S. P., Owens, E. B., Wells, K. C., Kraemer, H. C., Abikoff, H. B., Arnold,
E. L., Wigal, T. (2000). Family processes and treatment outcome in the
MTA: Negative/ineffective parenting practices in relation to multimodal
treatment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 555568.
Jasinski, L. J., Harp, J. P., Berry, D. T. R., Shandera-Ochsner, A. L., Mason, L. H., &
Ranseen, J. D. (2011). Using symptom validity tests to detect malingered
ADHD in college students. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 25, 14151428.
Jensen, P. S., Hinshaw, S. P., Swanson, J. M., Greenhill, L. L., Conners, C. K., Arnold,
L. E., Wigal, T. (2001). Findings from the NIMH Multimodal Treatment Study
of ADHD (MTA): Implications and applications for primary care providers.
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 22, 60-73.
Kazdin, A. E. (2005). Parent management training: Treatment for oppositional,
aggressive, and antisocial behavior in children and adolescents. New York,
NY: Oxford University Press.
Landau, S., Milich, R., & Diener, M. B. (1998). Peer relations of children with
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 14, 83
105.
Loe, I. M., & Feldman, H. M. (2007). Academic and educational outcomes of
children with ADHD. Ambulatory Pediatrics, 7, 8290.
Luman, M., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). The impact of reinforcement
contingencies on AD/HD: A review and theoretical appraisal. Clinical
Psychology Review, 25, 183213.
MTA Cooperative Group (1999). A 14-month randomized clinical trial of
Setlik, J., Bond, G. R., & Ho, M. (2009). Adolescent prescription ADHD medication
abuse Is rising along with prescriptions for these medications. Pediatrics,
124, 875880.
Still, G. F. (1902). Some abnormal psychical conditions in children: The
Goulstonian lectures. Lancet, 1, 10081012.
Teter, C. J., McCabe, S. E., Cranford, J. A., Boyd, C. J., & Guthrie, S. K. (2005).
Prevalence and motives for illicit use of prescription stimulants in an
undergraduate student sample. Journal of American College Health, 53,
253262.
Thome, J., Ehlis, A. C., Fallgatter, A. J., Krauel, K., Lange, K. W., Riederer, P.,
Gerlach, M. (2012). Biomarkers for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD). A consensus report of the WFSBP task for on biological markers
and the World Federation of ADHD. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry,
13, 379400.
Volkow, N. D., & Swanson, J. M. (2003). Variables that affect the clinical use and
abuse of methylphenidate in the treatment of ADHD. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 160, 19091918.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. ADHD and Behavior Disorders in Children by
Richard Milich and Walter Roberts is licensed under the Creative Commons AttributionNonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Psychopharmacology
Susan Barron
University of Kentucky
nobaproject.com
Abstract
Psychopharmacology is the study of how drugs affect behavior. If a drug
changes your perception, or the way you feel or think, the drug exerts effects
on your brain and nervous system. We call drugs that change the way you think
or feel psychoactive or psychotropic drugs, and almost everyone has used a
psychoactive drug at some point (yes, caffeine counts). Understanding some
of the basics about psychopharmacology can help us better understand a wide
range of things that interest psychologists and others. For example, the
pharmacological treatment of certain neurodegenerative diseases such as
Parkinsons disease tells us something about the disease itself.
The
Finally,
understanding something about the actions of drugs of abuse and their routes
of administration can help us understand why some psychoactive drugs are so
addictive. In this chapter, we will provide an overview of some of these topics
as well as discuss some current controversial areas in the field of
psychopharmacology.
Learning Objectives
How does the route of administration affect how rewarding a drug might be?
Introduction
Psychopharmacology, the study of how drugs affect the brain and behavior, is
a relatively new science, although people have probably been taking drugs to
change how they feel from early in our history (think of early history of eating
fermented fruit, ancient beer recipes, chewing on the leaves of the cocaine plant
for
stimulant
properties
as
just
some
examples).
The
word
2104
Table 1.
A very useful link at the end of this chapter shows the various steps involved
in neurotransmission and some ways drugs can alter this.
nobaproject.com - Psychopharmacology
2105
Table 2.
observed when someone takes a particular drug. The reality is that no drugs
currently available work only exactly where we would like in the brain or only
on a specific neurotransmitter. In many cases, individuals are sometimes
prescribed one psychotropic drug but then may also have to take additional
drugs to reduce the side effects caused by the initial drug.
nobaproject.com - Psychopharmacology
Sometimes
2106
individuals stop taking medication because the side effects can be so profound.
Drug Administration
There are many ways to take drugs, and these routes of drug administration
can have a significant impact on how quickly that drug reaches brain. The most
common route of administration is oral administration, which is relatively slow
and perhaps surprisingly often the most variable and complex route of
administration. Drugs enter the stomach and then get absorbed by the blood
supply and capillaries that line the small intestine. The rate of absorption can
be affected by a variety of factors including the quantity and the type of food
in the stomach (e.g., fats vs. proteins). This is why the medicine label for some
drugs (like antibiotics) may specifically state foods that you should or should
NOT consume within an hour of taking the drug because they can affect the
rate of absorption. Two of the most rapid routes of administration include
inhalation (i.e., smoking or gaseous anesthesia) and intravenous (IV) in which
nobaproject.com - Psychopharmacology
2107
the drug is injected directly into the vein and hence the blood supply. Both of
these routes of administration can get the drug to brain in less than 10 seconds.
IV administration also has the distinction of being the most dangerous because
if there is an adverse drug reaction, there is very little time to administer any
antidote, as in the case of an IV heroin overdose..
Why might how quickly a drug gets to the brain be important? If a drug
activates the reward circuits in the brain AND it reaches the brain very quickly,
the drug has a high risk for abuse and addiction.
Psychostimulants like
amphetamine or cocaine are examples of drugs that have high risk for abuse
because they are agonists at DA neurons involved in reward AND because these
drugs exist in forms that can be either smoked or injected intravenously. Some
argue that cigarette smoking is one of the hardest addictions to quit, and
although part of the reason for this may be that smoking gets the nicotine into
the brain very quickly (and indirectly acts on DA neurons), it is a more
complicated story. For drugs that reach the brain very quickly, not only is the
drug very addictive, but so are the cues associated with the drug (see Rohsenow,
Niaura, Childress, Abrams, & Monti, 1990). For a crack user, this could be the
pipe that they use to smoke the drug. For a cigarette smoker, however, it could
be something as normal as finishing dinner or waking up in the morning (if that
is when the smoker usually has a cigarette). For both the crack user and the
cigarette smoker, the cues associated with the drug may actually cause craving
that is alleviated by (you guessed it) lighting a cigarette or using crack (i.e.,
relapse). This is one of the reasons individuals that enroll in drug treatment
programs, especially out-of-town programs, are at significant risk of relapse if
they later find themselves in proximity to old haunts, friends, etc. But this is
much more difficult for a cigarette smoker. How can someone avoid eating?
Or avoid waking up in the morning, etc. These examples help you begin to
understand how important the route of administration can be for psychoactive
drugs.
nobaproject.com - Psychopharmacology
2108
Drug Metabolism
Metabolism involves the breakdown of psychoactive drugs, and this occurs
primarily in the liver. The liver produces enzymes (proteins that speed up a
chemical reaction), and these enzymes help catalyze a chemical reaction that
breaks down psychoactive drugs.
psychoactive drugs are broken down by the same family of enzymes, the
cytochrome P450 superfamily. There is not a unique enzyme for each drug;
rather, certain enzymes can break down a wide variety of drugs. Tolerance to
the effects of many drugs can occur with repeated exposure; that is, the drug
produces less of an effect over time, so more of the drug is needed to get the
same effect. This is particularly true for sedative drugs like alcohol or opiatebased painkillers. Metabolic tolerance is one kind of tolerance and it takes place
in the liver. Some drugs (like alcohol) cause enzyme induction an increase in
the enzymes produced by the liver. For example, chronic drinking results in
alcohol being broken down more quickly, so the alcoholic needs to drink more
to get the same effect of course, until so much alcohol is consumed that it
damages the liver (alcohol can cause fatty liver or cirrhosis).
2109
drug levels can build up to potentially toxic levels. In this case, the effects can
persist for extended periods of time after the consumption of grapefruit juice.
As of 2013, there are at least 85 drugs shown to adversely interact with grapefruit
juice (Bailey, Dresser, & Arnold, 2013). Some psychotropic drugs that are likely
to interact with grapefruit juice include carbamazepine (Tegretol), prescribed
for bipolar disorder; diazepam (Valium), used to treat anxiety, alcohol
withdrawal, and muscle spasms; and fluvoxamine (Luvox), used to treat
obsessive compulsive disorder and depression. A link at the end of this chapter
gives the latest list of drugs reported to have this unusual interaction.
2110
2111
psychopharmacology.
2112
nobaproject.com - Psychopharmacology
2113
2114
nobaproject.com - Psychopharmacology
2115
Outside Resources
Video: Neurotransmission
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=FR4S1BqdFG4
Web: Description of how some drugs work and the brain areas involved - 1
http://www.drugabuse.gov/news-events/nida-notes/2007/10/impacts-drugsneurotransmission
Web: Description of how some drugs work and the brain areas involved - 2
http://learn.genetics.utah.edu/content/addiction/drugs/mouse.html
Web: Information about how neurons communicate and the reward
pathways
http://learn.genetics.utah.edu/ontent/addiction/reward/neurontalk.html
Web: National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism
http://www.niaaa.nih.gov/
Web: National Institute of Drug Abuse
http://www.drugabuse.gov/
Web: National Institute of Mental Health
http://www.nimh.nih.gov/index.shtml
Web: Neurotransmission
http://science.education.nih.gov/supplements/nih2/addiction/activities/
lesson2_neurotransmission.htm
Discussion Questions
1. What are some of the issues surrounding prescribing medications for
children and adolescents? How might this be improved?
2. What are some of the factors that can affect relapse to an addictive drug?
3. How might prescribing medications for depression be improved in the
future to increase the likelihood that a drug would work and minimize side
effects?
Vocabulary
Agonists
A drug that increases or enhances a neurotransmitters effect.
Antagonist
A drug that blocks a neurotransmitters effect.
Enzyme
A protein produced by a living organism that allows or helps a chemical reaction
to occur.
Enzyme induction
Process through which a drug can enhance the production of an enzyme.
Metabolism
Breakdown of substances.
Neurotransmitter
A chemical substance produced by a neuron that is used for communication
between neurons.
Pharmacokinetics
The action of a drug through the body, including absorption, distribution,
metabolism, and excretion.
Polypharmacy
The use of many medications.
Psychoactive drugs
A drug that changes mood or the way someone feels.
Psychotropic drug
A drug that changes mood or emotion, usually used when talking about drugs
prescribed for various mental conditions (depression, anxiety, schizophrenia,
etc.).
Synapse
The tiny space separating neurons.
Reference List
Bailey D. G., Dresser G., & Arnold J. M. (2013). Grapefruit-medication
interactions: forbidden fruit or avoidable consequences? Canadian Medical
Association Journal, 185, 309316.
Brown, M. J., & Mezuk, B. (2012). Brains, bones, and aging: psychotropic
medications and bone health among older adults. Current Osteoporosis
Reports, 10, 303311.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2011) Prevalence of autism
spectrum disorders autism and developmental disabilities monitoring
network, 14 sites, United States, 2008. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly
Report 61(SS03) 119.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2013) Mental health surveillance
among children United States, 20052011. Morbidity and Mortality
Weekly Report 62 Suppl, 1-35.
Hilmer, N., & Gnjidict, D. (2008). The effects of polypharmacy in older adults.
Clinical Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 85, 8688.
Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2008). Effectiveness of antidepressants: an evidence myth
constructed from a thousand randomized trials? Philosophy, Ethics and
Humanities in Medicine, 3,14.
Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Demler, O., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Prevalence, severity,
and comorbidity of twelve-month DSM-IV disorders in the National
Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). Archives of General Psychiatry, 62,
617627.
Moreno, C., Laje, G., Blanco, C., Jiang, H., Schmidt, A. B., & Olfson, M., (2007).
National trends in the outpatient diagnosis and treatment of bipolar
disorder in youth. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(9), 10321039.
Pollock, B. G., Forsyth, C. E., & Bies, R. R. (2008). The critical role of clinical
pharmacology in geriatric psychopharmacology. Clinical Pharmacology &
Therapeutics, 85, 8993.
Rohsenow, D. J., Niaura, R. S., Childress, A. R., Abrams, D. B., &, Monti, P. M.
(1990). Cue reactivity in addictive behaviors: Theoretical and treatment
implications. International Journal of Addiction, 25, 957993.
Schwartz, J. B., & Abernethy, D. R. (2008). Aging and medications: Past, present,
future. Clinical Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 85, 310.
World Health Organization. (2004). Promoting mental health: concepts,
emerging evidence, practice (Summary Report). Geneva, Switzerland:
Author. Retrieved from http://www.who.int/mental_health/evidence/en/
promoting_mhh.pdf
Zhou, S. F. (2009). Polymorphism of human cytochrome P450 2D6 and its clinical
significance: Part II. Clinical Pharmacokinetics, 48, 761804.
Topic 11
Well-Being
Abstract
Our emotions, thoughts, and behaviors play an important role in our health.
Not only do they influence our day-to-day health practices, but they can also
influence how our body functions. This chapter provides an overview of health
psychology, which is a field devoted to understanding the connections between
psychology and health.
Learning Objectives
2128
important role in our overall health (Cohen & Herbert, 1996; Taylor, 2012).
Health psychology relies on the Biopsychosocial Model of Health. This
model posits that biology, psychology, and social factors are just as important
in the development of disease as biological causes (e.g., germs, viruses), which
is consistent with the World Health Organization (1946) definition of health.
This model replaces the older Biomedical Model of Health, which primarily
considers the physical, or pathogenic, factors contributing to illness. Thanks to
advances in medical technology, there is a growing understanding of the
physiology underlying the mindbody connection, and in particular, the role
that different feelings can have on our bodys function. Health psychology
researchers working in the fields of psychosomatic medicine and
psychoneuroimmunology, for example, are interested in understanding how
psychological factors can get under the skin and influence our physiology in
order to better understand how factors like stress can make us sick.
2129
researchers will then evaluate you for several days by asking you questions
about your symptoms, monitoring how much mucus you are producing by
weighing your used tissues, and taking body fluid samplesall to see if you are
objectively ill with a cold. Now, the interesting thing is that not everyone who
has drops of cold virus put in their nose develops the illness. Studies like this
one find that people who are less stressed and those who are more positive at
the beginning of the study are at a decreased risk of developing a cold (Cohen,
Tyrrell, & Smith, 1991; Cohen, Alper, Doyle, Treanor, & Turner, 2006) (see Figure
1 for an example).
2130
Importantly, it is not just major life stressors (e.g., a family death, a natural
disaster) that increase the likelihood of getting sick. Even small daily hassles
like getting stuck in traffic or fighting with your girlfriend can raise your blood
pressure, alter your stress hormones, and even suppress your immune system
function (DeLongis, Folkman, & Lazarus, 1988; Twisk, Snel, Kemper, & van
Machelen, 1999).
It is clear that stress plays a major role in our mental and physical health,
but what exactly is it? The term stress was originally derived from the field of
mechanics where it is used to describe materials under pressure. The word
was first used in a psychological manner by researcher Hans Selye. He was
examining the effect of an ovarian hormone that he thought caused sickness
in a sample of rats. Surprisingly, he noticed that almost any injected hormone
produced this same sickness. He smartly realized that it was not the hormone
under investigation that was causing these problems, but instead, the aversive
experience of being handled and injected by researchers that led to high
physiological arousal and, eventually, to health problems like ulcers. Selye
(1946) coined the term stressor to label a stimulus that had this effect on the
body and developed a model of the stress response called the General
Adaptation Syndrome. Since then, psychologists have studied stress in a
myriad of ways, including stress as negative events (e.g., natural disasters or
major life changes like dropping out of school), as chronically difficult situations
(e.g., taking care of a loved one with Alzheimers), as short-term hassles, as a
biological fight-or-flight response, and even as clinical illness like post-traumatic
stress disorder (PTSD). It continues to be one of the most important and wellstudied psychological correlates of illness, because excessive stress causes
potentially damaging wear and tear on the body and can influence almost any
imaginable disease process.
2131
Coping Strategies
How individuals cope with the stressors they face can have a significant impact
on health. Coping is often classified into two categories: problem-focused
coping or emotion-focused coping (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989).
Problem-focused coping is thought of as actively addressing the event that is
causing stress in an effort to solve the issue at hand. For example, say you have
an important exam coming up next week. A problem-focused strategy might
be to spend additional time over the weekend studying to make sure you
understand all of the material. Emotion-focused coping, on the other hand,
regulates the emotions that come with stress. In the above examination
example, this might mean watching a funny movie to take your mind off the
nobaproject.com - The Healthy Life
2132
anxiety you are feeling. In the short term, emotion-focused coping might reduce
feelings of stress, but problem-focused coping seems to have the greatest
impact on mental wellness (Billings & Moos, 1981; Herman-Stabl, Stemmler, &
Petersen, 1995). That being said, when events are uncontrollable (e.g., the death
of a loved one), emotion-focused coping directed at managing your feelings, at
first, might be the better strategy. Therefore, it is always important to consider
the match of the stressor to the coping strategy when evaluating its plausible
benefits.
2133
believe they can complete tasks and reach their goals. Just as feeling in control
can reduce stress and improve health, higher self-efficacy can reduce stress
and negative health behaviors, and is associated with better health (OLeary,
1985).
Social Relationships
Research has shown that the impact of social isolation on our risk for disease
and death is similar in magnitude to the risk associated with smoking regularly
(Holt-Lunstad, Smith, & Layton, 2010; House, Landis, & Umberson, 1988). In
fact, the importance of social relationships for our health is so significant that
some scientists believe our body has developed a physiological system that
encourages us to seek out our relationships, especially in times of stress (Taylor
et al., 2000). Social integration is the concept used to describe the number of
social roles that you have (Cohen & Wills, 1985), as well as the lack of isolation.
For example, you might be a daughter, a basketball team member, a Humane
Society volunteer, a coworker, and a student. Maintaining these different roles
can improve your health via encouragement from those around you to maintain
a healthy lifestyle. Those in your social network might also provide you with
social support (e.g., when you are under stress). This support might include
emotional help (e.g., a hug when you need it), tangible help (e.g., lending you
money), or advice. By helping to improve health behaviors and reduce stress,
social relationships can have a powerful, protective impact on health, and in
some cases, might even help people with serious illnesses stay alive longer
(Spiegel, Kraemer, Bloom, & Gottheil, 1989).
2134
2135
rapidly (Kubzansky, Sparrow, Vokonas, & Kawachi, 2001; Nes & Segerstrom,
2006; Scheier & Carver, 1985; Segerstrom, Taylor, Kemeny, & Fahey, 1998).
Figure 2. This figure illustrates one possible way that positive affect protects individuals
against disease. Positive affect can reduce stress perceptions (a), thereby improving health
behaviors (b) and lowering physiological stress responses (c) (e.g., decreased cardiovascular
reactivity, lower stress hormones, non-suppressed immune activity). As a result, there is likely
to be less incidence of disease (d, e). (Adapted from Pressman & Cohen, 2005)
2136
Stress Management
About 20 percent of Americans report having stress, with 1833 year-olds
reporting the highest levels (American Psychological Association, 2012). Given
that the sources of our stress are often difficult to change (e.g., personal
finances, current job), a number of interventions have been designed to help
reduce the aversive responses to duress. For example, relaxation activities and
forms of meditation are techniques that allow individuals to reduce their stress
via breathing exercises, muscle relaxation, and mental imagery. Physiological
arousal from stress can also be reduced via biofeedback, a technique where
the individual is shown bodily information that is not normally available to them
(e.g., heart rate), and then taught strategies to alter this signal. This type of
intervention has even shown promise in reducing heart and hypertension risk,
as well as other serious conditions (e.g., Moravec, 2008; Patel, Marmot, & Terry,
1981). But reducing stress does not have to be complicated! For example,
exercise is a great stress reduction activity (Salmon, 2001) that has a myriad of
health benefits.
2137
compromised due to high stress and the elevated frequency of exposure to the
illnesses of your fellow students in lecture halls, cafeterias, and dorms.
Figure 3: A popular joke about how difficult it is to stay balanced and healthy during college.
Psychologists study both health behaviors and health habits. The former
are behaviors that can improve or harm your health. Some examples include
regular exercise, flossing, and wearing sunscreen, versus negative behaviors
like drunk driving, pulling all-nighters, or smoking. These behaviors become
habits when they are firmly established and performed automatically. For
example, do you have to think about putting your seatbelt on or do you do it
automatically? Habits are often developed early in life thanks to parental
encouragement or the influence of our peer group.
While these behaviors sound minor, studies have shown that those who
engaged in more of these protective habits (e.g., getting 78 hours of sleep
regularly, not smoking or drinking excessively, exercising) had fewer illnesses,
felt better, and were less likely to die over a 912-year follow-up period (Belloc
nobaproject.com - The Healthy Life
2138
behaviors can even influence academic performance. For example, poor sleep
quality and quantity are related to weaker learning capacity and academic
performance (Curcio, Ferrara, & De Gennaro, 2006). Due to the effects that
health behaviors can have, much effort is put forward by psychologists to
understand how to change unhealthy behaviors, and to understand why
individuals fail to act in healthy ways. Health promotion involves enabling
individuals to improve health by focusing on behaviors that pose a risk for future
illness, as well as spreading knowledge on existing risk factors. These might be
genetic risks you are born with, or something you developed over time like
obesity, which puts you at risk for Type 2 diabetes and heart disease, among
other illnesses.
information online, many people now use the Internet for health information
and 38% percent report that this influences their decision to see a doctor (Fox
& Jones, 2009).
2139
2140
2141
disease, to name a few. The National Institutes of Health have called for
researchers to use the knowledge we have about risk factors to design effective
interventions to reduce the prevalence of preventable illness. Additionally,
there are a growing number of individuals across developed countries with
multiple chronic illnesses and/or lasting disabilities, especially with older age.
Addressing their needs and maintaining their quality of life will require skilled
individuals who understand how to properly treat these populations. Health
psychologists will be on the forefront of work in these areas.
With this focus on prevention, it is important that health psychologists move
beyond studying risk (e.g., depression, stress, hostility, low socioeconomic
status) in isolation, and move toward studying factors that confer resilience and
protection from disease. There is, fortunately, a growing interest in studying
the positive factors that protect our health (e.g., Diener & Chan, 2011; Pressman
& Cohen, 2005; Richman, Kubzansky, Maselko, Kawachi, Choo, & Bauer, 2005)
with evidence strongly indicating that people with higher positivity live longer,
suffer fewer illnesses, and generally feel better. Seligman (2008) has even
proposed a field of Positive Health to specifically study those who exhibit
above average healthsomething we do not think about enough. By shifting
some of the research focus to identifying and understanding these healthpromoting factors, we may capitalize on this information to improve public
health.
Innovative interventions to improve health are already in use and continue
to be studied. With recent advances in technology, we are starting to see great
strides made to improve health with the aid of computational tools. For
example, there are hundreds of simple applications (apps) that use email and
text messages to send reminders to take medication, as well as mobile apps
that allow us to monitor our exercise levels and food intake (in the growing
mobile-health, or m-health, field). These m-health applications can be used to
raise health awareness, support treatment and compliance, and remotely
collect data on a variety of outcomes. Also exciting are devices that allow us to
nobaproject.com - The Healthy Life
2142
monitor physiology in real time; for example, to better understand the stressful
situations that raise blood pressure or heart rate. With advances like these,
health psychologists will be able to serve the population better, learn more
about health and health behavior, and develop excellent health-improving
strategies that could be specifically targeted to certain populations or
individuals. These leaps in equipment development, partnered with growing
health psychology knowledge and exciting advances in neuroscience and
genetic research, will lead health researchers and practitioners into an exciting
new time where, hopefully, we will understand more and more about how to
keep people healthy.
2143
Outside Resources
App: 30 iPhone apps to monitor your health
http://www.hongkiat.com/blog/iphone-health-app/
Quiz: Hostility
http://www.mhhe.com/socscience/hhp/fahey7e/wellness_worksheets/
wellness_worksheet_090.html
Self-assessment: Perceived Stress Scale
http://www.ncsu.edu/assessment/resources/perceived_stress_scale.pdf
Self-assessment: Whats your real age (based on your health practices and
risk factors)?
http://www.realage.com
Video: Try out a guided meditation exercise to reduce your stress
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Zh-klfBJlHc
Web: American Psychosomatic Society
http://www.psychosomatic.org/home/index.cfm
Web: APA Division 38, Health Psychology
http://www.health-psych.org
Web: Society of Behavioral Medicine
http://www.sbm.org
Discussion Questions
1. What psychological factors contribute to health?
2. Which psychosocial constructs and behaviors might help protect us from
the damaging effects of stress?
3. What kinds of interventions might help to improve resilience? Who will these
interventions help the most?
4. How should doctors use research in health psychology when meeting with
patients?
5. Why do clinical health psychologists play a critical role in improving public
health?
Vocabulary
Adherence
In health, it is the ability of a patient to maintain a health behavior prescribed
by a physician. This might include taking medication as prescribed, exercising
more, or eating less high-fat food.
Behavioral medicine
A field similar to health psychology that integrates psychological factors (e.g.,
emotion, behavior, cognition, and social factors) in the treatment of disease.
This applied field includes clinical areas of study, such as occupational therapy,
hypnosis, rehabilitation or medicine, and preventative medicine.
Biofeedback
The process by which physiological signals, not normally available to human
perception, are transformed into easy-to-understand graphs or numbers.
Individuals can then use this information to try to change bodily functioning (e.
g., lower blood pressure, reduce muscle tension).
Chronic disease
A health condition that persists over time, typically for periods longer than three
months (e.g., HIV, asthma, diabetes).
Control
Feeling like you have the power to change your environment or behavior if you
need or want to.
Daily hassles
Irritations in daily life that are not necessarily traumatic, but that cause
difficulties and repeated stress.
Emotion-focused coping
Coping strategy aimed at reducing the negative emotions associated with a
stressful event.
Health
According to the World Health Organization, it is a complete state of physical,
mental, and social well-being and not merely the absence of disease or infirmity.
Health behavior
Any behavior that is related to healtheither good or bad.
Hostility
An experience or trait with cognitive, behavioral, and emotional components.
It often includes cynical thoughts, feelings of emotion, and aggressive behavior.
Mindbody connection
The idea that our emotions and thoughts can affect how our body functions.
Problem-focused coping
A set of coping strategies aimed at improving or changing stressful situations.
Psychoneuroimmunology
A field of study examining the relationship among psychology, brain function,
and immune function.
Psychosomatic medicine
An interdisciplinary field of study that focuses on how biological, psychological,
and social processes contribute to physiological changes in the body and health
over time.
Resilience
The ability to bounce back from negative situations (e.g., illness, stress) to
normal functioning or to simply not show poor outcomes in the face of adversity.
In some cases, resilience may lead to better functioning following the negative
experience (e.g., post-traumatic growth).
Self-efficacy
The belief that one can perform adequately in a specific situation.
Social integration
The size of your social network, or number of social roles (e.g., son, sister,
student, employee, team member).
Social support
The perception or actuality that we have a social network that can help us in
times of need and provide us with a variety of useful resources (e.g., advice,
love, money).
Stress
A pattern of physical and psychological responses in an organism after it
perceives a threatening event that disturbs its homeostasis and taxes its abilities
to cope with the event.
Stressor
An event or stimulus that induces feelings of stress.
Type A Behavior
Type A behavior is characterized by impatience, competitiveness, neuroticism,
hostility, and anger.
Type B Behavior
Type B behavior reflects the absence of Type A characteristics and is represented
by less competitive, aggressive, and hostile behavior patterns.
Reference List
Adamson, J., Ben-Shlomo, Y., Chaturvedi, N., & Donovan, J. (2008). Ethnicity,
socio-economic position and genderdo they affect reported healthcare
seeking behaviour? Social Science & Medicine, 57, 895904.
American Psychological Association (2012). Stress in American 2012 [Press
release]. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/news/press/releases/stress/2012/
generations.aspx
Belloc, N. B., & Breslow, L. (1972). Relationship of physical health status and
health practices. Preventive Medicine, 1, 409421.
Billings, A. G., & Moos, R. H. (1981). The role of coping responses and social
resources in attenuating the stress of life events. Journal of Behavioral
Medicine, 4, 139157.
Breslow, L., & Enstrom, J. E. (1980). Persistence of health habits and their
relationship to mortality. Preventive Medicine, 9, 469483.
Briscoe, M. E. (1987). Why do people go to the doctor? Sex differences in the
correlates of GP consultation. Social Science & Medicine, 25, 507513.
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies:
A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 56, 267283.
Cohen, S., Alper, C. M., Doyle, W. J., Treanor, J. J., & Turner, R. B. (2006). Positive
emotional style predicts resistance to illness after experimental exposure
to rhinovirus or influenza A virus. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 809815.
Cohen, S., Janicki-Deverts, D., & Miller, G. E. (2007). Psychological stress and
disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 298, 16851687.
Cohen, S., Tyrrell, D. A., & Smith, A. P. (1991). Psychological stress and
susceptibility to the common cold. New England Journal of Medicine, 325,
606612.
Cohen, S., & Herbert, T. B. (1996). Health psychology: Psychological factors and
physical disease from the perspective of human psychoneuroimmunology.
Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 113142.
Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering
hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310357.
Cole-Lewis, H., & Kershaw, T. (2010). Text messaging as a tool for behavior
change in disease prevention and management. Epidemiologic Reviews, 32,
5669.
Curcio, G., Ferrara, M., & De Gennaro, L. (2006). Sleep loss, learning capacity
and academic performance. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 10, 323337.
DeLongis, A., Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1988). The impact of daily stress on
health and mood: Psychological and social resources as mediators. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 486495.
Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses:
a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological
Bulletin, 130, 355391.
Dunbar-Jacob, J., & Mortimer-Stephens, M. (2001). Treatment adherence in
147, 598611.
Salmon, P. (2001). Effects of physical exercise on anxiety, depression, and
sensitivity to stress: A unifying theory. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(1), 33
61.
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: assessment
and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology,
4, 219247.
Schoenfeld, E. R., Greene, J. M., Wu, S. Y., & Leske, M. C. (2001). Patterns of
adherence to diabetes vision care guidelines: Baseline findings from the
Diabetic Retinopathy Awareness Program. Ophthalmology, 108, 563571.
Schulz, R., & Hanusa, B.H. (1978). Long-term effects of control and predictabilityenhancing interventions: Findings and ethical issues. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 36, 11941202.
Segerstrom, S. C., Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., & Fahey, J. L. (1998). Optimism is
associated with mood, coping, and immune change in response to stress.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 16461655.
Seligman, M. E. P. (2008). Positive health. Applied Psychology, 57, 318.
Selye, H. (1946). The general adaptation syndrome and the diseases of
adaptation. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology, 6, 117230.
Sieber, W. J., Rodin, J., Larson, L., Ortega, S., Cummings, N., Levy, S., ... Herberman,
R. (1992). Modulation of human natural killer cell activity by exposure to
uncontrollable stress. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 6, 141156.
Spiegel, D., Kraemer, H., Bloom, J., & Gottheil, E. (1989). Effect of psychosocial
treatment on survival of patients with metastatic breast cancer. The Lancet,
334, 888891.
Taylor, S. E. (2012) Health psychology (8th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Gurung, R. A., & Updegraff,
J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: Tend-andbefriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review, 107, 411429.
Twisk, J. W., Snel, J., Kemper, H. C., & van Mechelen, W. (1999). Changes in daily
hassles and life events and the relationship with coronary heart disease risk
factors: A 2-year longitudinal study in 2729-year-old males and females.
Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 46, 229240.
Wallston, B. S., & Wallston, K. A. (1978). Locus of control and health: a review of
the literature. Health Education & Behavior, 6, 107117.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. The Healthy Life by Emily Hooker and Sarah
Pressman is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Abstract
Subjective well-being (SWB) is the scientific term for happiness and life
satisfactionthinking and feeling that your life is going well, not badly. Scientists
rely primarily on self-report surveys to assess the happiness of individuals, but
they have validated these scales with other types of measures. Peoples levels
of subjective well-being are influenced by both internal factors, such as
personality and outlook, and external factors, such as the society in which they
live. Some of the major determinants of subjective well-being are a persons
inborn temperament, the quality of their social relationships, the societies they
live in, and their ability to meet their basic needs. To some degree people adapt
to conditions so that over time our circumstances may not influence our
happiness as much as one might predict they would. Importantly, researchers
have also studied the outcomes of subjective well-being and have found that
happy people are more likely to be healthier and live longer, to have better
social relationships, and to be more productive at work. In other words, people
high in subjective well-being seem to be healthier and function more effectively
compared to people who are chronically stressed, depressed, or angry. Thus,
happiness does not just feel good, but it is good for people and for those around
them.
Learning Objectives
Be able to list two internal causes of subjective well-being and two external
causes of subjective well-being.
Describe the types of societies that experience the most and least
happiness, and why they do.
Introduction
When people describe what they most want out of life, happiness is almost
always on the list, and very frequently it is at the top of the list. When people
describe what they want in life for their children, they frequently mention health
and wealth, occasionally they mention fame or successbut they almost always
mention happiness. People will claim that whether their kids are wealthy and
work in some prestigious occupation or not, I just want my kids to be happy.
Happiness appears to be one of the most important goals for people, if not the
most important. But what is it, and how do people get it?
In this chapter I describe happiness or subjective well-being (SWB) as a
processit results from certain internal and external causes, and in turn it
influences the way people behave, as well as their physiological states. Thus,
high SWB is not just a pleasant outcome but is an important factor in our future
success. Because scientists have developed valid ways of measuring
happiness, they have come in the past decades to know much about its causes
and consequences.
Types of Happiness
Philosophers debated the nature of happiness for thousands of years, but
scientists have recently discovered that happiness means different things.
Three major types of happiness are high life satisfaction, frequent positive
feelings, and infrequent negative feelings (Diener, 1984). Subjective wellbeing is the label given by scientists to the various forms of happiness taken
together. Although there are additional forms of SWB, the three in the table
below have been studied extensively. The table also shows that the causes of
the different types of happiness can be somewhat different.
nobaproject.com - Happiness: The Science of Subjective Well-Being
2161
You can see in the table that there are different causes of happiness, and
that these causes are not identical for the various types of SWB. Therefore,
there is no single key, no magic wandhigh SWB is achieved by combining
several different important elements (Diener & Biswas-Diener, 2008). Thus,
people who promise to know the key to happiness are oversimplifying.
Some people experience all three elements of happinessthey are very
satisfied, enjoy life, and have only a few worries or other unpleasant emotions.
Other unfortunate people are missing all three. Most of us also know individuals
who have one type of happiness but not another. For example, imagine an
elderly person who is completely satisfied with her lifeshe has done most
everything she ever wantedbut is not currently enjoying life that much
because of the infirmities of age. There are others who show a different pattern,
for example, who really enjoy life but also experience a lot of stress, anger, and
worry. And there are those who are having fun, but who are dissatisfied and
believe they are wasting their lives. Because there are several components to
happiness, each with somewhat different causes, there is no magic single cureall that creates all forms of SWB. This means that to be happy, individuals must
acquire each of the different elements that cause it.
2162
2163
2164
satisfaction. Others, colored red, are very low. The grey areas in the map are
places we could not collect happiness datathey were just too dangerous or
inaccessible.
Figure 1
Can you guess what might make some societies happier than others? Much
of North America and Europe have relatively high life satisfaction, and much of
Africa is low in life satisfaction. For life satisfaction living in an economically
developed nation is helpful because when people must struggle to obtain food,
shelter, and other basic necessities, they tend to be dissatisfied with lives.
However, other factors, such as trusting and being able to count on others, are
also crucial to the happiness within nations. Indeed, for enjoying life our
relationships with others seem more important than living in a wealthy society.
One factor that predicts unhappiness is conflictindividuals in nations with
high internal conflict or conflict with neighboring nations tend to experience
nobaproject.com - Happiness: The Science of Subjective Well-Being
2165
low SWB.
2166
2167
Besides the internal and external factors that influence happiness, there are
psychological influences as wellsuch as our aspirations, social comparisons,
and adaptation. Peoples aspirations are what they want in life, including
income, occupation, marriage, and so forth. If peoples aspirations are high,
they will often strive harder, but there is also a risk of them falling short of their
aspirations and being dissatisfied. The goal is to have challenging aspirations
but also to be able to adapt to what actually happens in life.
Ones outlook and resilience are also always very important to happiness.
Every person will have disappointments in life, fail at times, and have problems.
Thus, happiness comes not to people who never have problemsthere are no
such individualsbut to people who are able to bounce back from failures and
adapt to disappointments. This is why happiness is never caused just by what
happens to us but always includes our outlook on life.
2168
Adaptation to Circumstances
The process of adaptation is important in understanding happiness. When
good and bad events occur, people often react strongly at first, but then their
reactions adapt over time and they return to their former levels of happiness.
For instance, many people are euphoric when they first marry, but over time
they grow accustomed to the marriage and are no longer ecstatic. The marriage
becomes commonplace and they return to their former level of happiness. Few
of us think this will happen to us, but the truth is that it usually does. Some
people will be a bit happier even years after marriage, but nobody carries that
initial high through the years.
People also adapt over time to bad events. However, people take a long time
to adapt to certain negative events such as unemployment. People become
unhappy when they lose their work, but over time they recover to some extent.
But even after a number of years, unemployed individuals sometimes have
lower life satisfaction, indicating that they have not completely habituated to
the experience. However, there are strong individual differences in adaptation,
too. Some people are resilient and bounce back quickly after a bad event, and
others are fragile and do not ever fully adapt to the bad event. Do you adapt
quickly to bad events and bounce back, or do you continue to dwell on a bad
event and let it keep you down?
An example of adaptation to circumstances is shown in Figure 3, which shows
the daily moods of Harry, a college student who had Hodgkins lymphoma (a
form of cancer). As can be seen, over the 6-week period when I studied Harrys
moods, they went up and down. A few times his moods dropped into the
negative zone below the horizontal blue line. Most of the time Harrys moods
were in the positive zone above the line. But about halfway through the study
Harry was told that his cancer was in remissioneffectively curedand his
moods on that day spiked way up. But notice that he quickly adaptedthe
effects of the good news wore off, and Harry adapted back toward where he
nobaproject.com - Happiness: The Science of Subjective Well-Being
2169
was before. So even the very best news one can imaginerecovering from
cancerwas not enough to give Harry a permanent high. Notice too, however,
that Harrys moods averaged a bit higher after cancer remission. Thus, the
typical pattern is a strong response to the event, and then a dampening of this
joy over time. However, even in the long run, the person might be a bit happier
or unhappier than before.
2170
Measuring Happiness
SWB researchers have relied primarily on self-report scales to assess happiness
how people rate their own happiness levels on self-report surveys. People
respond to numbered scales to indicate their levels of satisfaction, positive
feelings, and lack of negative feelings. You can see where you stand on these
nobaproject.com - Happiness: The Science of Subjective Well-Being
2171
scales by going to or by filling out the Flourishing Scale below. These measures
will give you an idea of what popular scales of happiness are like.
2172
2173
2174
Table 4
2175
Outside Resources
Video: Recipes for Happiness - Interview with Ed Diener
http://tinyurl.com/qao7vs6
Video: Recipes for Happiness - Interview with Ruut Veenhoven
http://tinyurl.com/osmeu3b
Web: Barbara Fredricksons website on positive emotions
http://www.unc.edu/peplab/news.html
Web: Ed Dieners website
http://internal.psychology.illinois.edu/~ediener/
Web: International Positive Psychology Association
http://www.ippanetwork.org/
Web: Positive Acorn Positive Psychology website
http://positiveacorn.com/
Web: Sonja Lyubomirskys website on happiness
http://sonjalyubomirsky.com/
Web: University of Pennsylvania Positive Psychology Center website
http://www.ppc.sas.upenn.edu/
Web: World Database on Happiness
http://www1.eur.nl/fsw/happiness/
Discussion Questions
1. Which do you think is more important, the top-down personality influences
on happiness or the bottom-up situational circumstances that influence
it? In other words, discuss whether internal sources such as personality and
outlook or external factors such situations, circumstances, and events are
more important to happiness. Can you make an argument that both are
very important?
2. Do you know people who are happy in one way but not in others? People
who are high in life satisfaction, for example, but low in enjoying life or high
in negative feelings? What should they do to increase their happiness across
all three types of subjective well-being?
3. Certain sources of happiness have been emphasized in this book, but there
are others. Can you think of other important sources of happiness and
unhappiness? Do you think religion, for example, is a positive source of
happiness for most people? What about age or ethnicity? What about health
and physical handicaps? If you were a researcher, what question might you
tackle on the influences on happiness?
4. Are you satisfied with your level of happiness? If not, are there things you
might do to change it? Would you function better if you were happier?
5. How much happiness is helpful to make a society thrive? Do people need
some worry and sadness in life to help us avoid bad things? When is
satisfaction a good thing, and when is some dissatisfaction a good thing?
6. How do you think money can help happiness? Interfere with happiness?
What level of income will you need to be satisfied?
Vocabulary
Adaptation
The fact that after people first react to good or bad events, sometimes in a
strong way, their feelings and reactions tend to dampen down over time and
they return toward their original level of subjective well-being.
Bottom-up or external causes of happiness
Situational factors outside the person that influence his or her subjective wellbeing, such as good and bad events and circumstances such as health and
wealth.
Happiness
The popular word for subjective well-being. Scientists sometimes avoid using
this term because it can refer to different things, such as feeling good, being
satisfied, or even the causes of high subjective well-being.
Life satisfaction
A person reflects on their life and judges to what degree it is going well, by
whatever standards that person thinks are most important for a good life.
Negative feelings
Undesirable and unpleasant feelings that people tend to avoid if they can.
Moods and emotions such as depression, anger, and worry are examples.
Positive feelings
Desirable and pleasant feelings. Moods and emotions such as enjoyment and
love are examples.
Subjective well-being
The name that scientists give to happinessthinking and feeling that our lives
are going very well.
Subjective well-being scales
Self-report surveys or questionnaires in which participants indicate their levels
of subjective well-being, by responding to items with a number that indicates
how well off they feel.
Top-down or internal causes of happiness
The persons outlook and habitual response tendencies that influence their
happinessfor example, their temperament or optimistic outlook on life.
Reference List
Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542575.
Diener, E., Inglehart, R., & Tay, L. (2012). Theory and validity of life satisfaction
scales. Social Indicators Research, in press.
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being:
Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276302.
Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D., Oishi, S., & Biswas-Diener,
R. (2009). New measures of well-being: Flourishing and positive and negative
feelings. Social Indicators Research, 39, 247266.
Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2008). Happiness: Unlocking the mysteries of
psychological wealth. Malden, MA: Wiley/Blackwell.
Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Beyond money: Toward an economy of
well-being. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5, 131.
Diener, E., & Tay, L. (2012). The remarkable benefits of happiness for successful
and healthy living. Report of the Well-Being Working Group, Royal
Government of Bhutan. Report to the United Nations General Assembly:
Well-Being and Happiness: A New Development Paradigm.
Lyubomirsky, S. (2013). The myths of happiness: What should make you happy,
but doesnt, what shouldnt make you happy, but does. New York, NY:
Penguin.
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Happiness: The Science of Subjective Well-Being
by Edward Diener is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercialShareAlike 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Positive Psychology
Robert A. Emmons
University of California, Davis
nobaproject.com
Abstract
A brief history of the positive psychology movement is presented, and key
themes within positive psychology are identified. Three important positive
psychology topics are gratitude, forgiveness, and humility. Ten key findings
within the field of positive psychology are put forth, and the most important
empirical findings regarding gratitude, forgiveness, and humility are discussed.
Assessment techniques for these three strengths are described, and
interventions for increasing gratitude, developing forgiveness, and becoming
more humble are briefly considered.
Learning Objectives
Describe what positive psychology is, who started it, and why it came into
existence.
Identify some of the most important findings from the science of positive
psychology with respect to forgiveness, gratitude, and humility.
Explore how positive psychology might make a difference in how you think
about your own life, the nature of human nature, and what is really
important to you.
Introduction
Positive psychology is a popular movement that began in the late 1990s. It is
the branch of psychology that has as its primary focus the on the strengths,
virtues, and talents that contribute to successful functioning and enable
individuals and communities to flourish. Core topics include happiness,
resiliency, well-being, and states of flow and engagement. It was spearheaded
by a former president of the American Psychological Association, Martin
Seligman.
Throughout most of its history, psychology was concerned with identifying
and remedying human ills. It has largely focused on decreasing maladaptive
emotions and behaviors, while generally ignoring positive and optimal
functioning. In contrast, the goal of positive psychology is to identify and
enhance the human strengths and virtues that make life worth living. Unlike
the positive thinking or new thought movements that are associated with
people like Norman Vincent Peale or Rhonda Byrne (The Secret), positive
psychology pursues scientifically informed perspectives on what makes life
worth living. It is empirically based. It focuses on measuring aspects of the
human condition that lead to happiness, fulfillment, and flourishing. The
science of happiness is covered in other chapters within this section of this
book. Therefore, aside from key findings summarized in Table 1, the emphasis
in this chapter will be on other topics within positive psychology.
Moving from an exclusive focus on distress, disorder, and dysfunction,
positive psychology shifts the scientific lens to a concentration on well-being,
health, and optimal functioning. Positive psychology provides a different
vantage point through which to understand human experience. Recent
developments have produced a common framework and that locates the study
of positive states, strengths and virtues in relation to each other and links them
to important life outcomes. Recent developments suggest that problems in
psychological functioning may be more profitably dealt with as the absence,
nobaproject.com - Positive Psychology
2187
2188
for myself. The strength of gratitude is measured with such items as At least
once a day, I stop and count my blessings.
Within the United States, the most commonly endorsed strengths are
kindness, fairness, honesty, gratitude and judgment (Park, Peterson & Seligman,
2006). Worldwide, the following strengths were most associated with positive
life satisfaction: hope, zest, gratitude and love. The researchers called these
strengths of the heart. Moreover, strengths associated with knowledge, such
as love of learning and curiosity, were least correlated with life satisfaction (Park,
Peterson & Seligman, 2005).
2189
2190
Forgiveness, gratitude, and humility are three key strengths that have been the
focus of sustained research programs within positive psychology. What have
we learned about each of these and why do these matter for human flourishing?
Forgiveness
Forgiveness is essential to harmonious long-term relationships between
individuals, whether between spouses or nations, dyads or collectives. At the
level of the individual, forgiveness of self can help one achieve an inner peace
as well as peace with others and with God. Wrongdoing against others can
result in guilt, and self-loathing.
"Forgiveness may ultimately be the most powerful weapon for breaking the
dreadful cycle of violence."
Research is answering fundamental questions about what forgiveness is
and isnt, how it develops, what are its physiological correlates and physical
effects, whether it is always beneficial, and how peopleif they are so motivated
might be helped to forgive. Forgiveness is not excusing, condoning, tolerating,
or forgetting that one has been hurt because of the actions of another.
nobaproject.com - Positive Psychology
2191
Gratitude
Gratitude is a feeling of appreciation or thankfulness in response to receiving
a benefit. The emerging science of gratitude has produced some important
findings. From childhood to old age, accumulating evidence documents the
wide array of psychological, physical, and relational benefits associated with
gratitude (Wood, Froh, & Geraghty, 2010). Gratitude is important not only
because it helps us feel good, but also because it inspires us to do good.
Gratitude heals, energizes, and transforms lives in a myriad of ways consistent
with the notion that virtue is both its own reward and produces other rewards
nobaproject.com - Positive Psychology
2192
(Emmons, 2007).
To give a flavor of these research findings, dispositional gratitude has been
found to be positively associated qualities such as empathy, forgiveness, and
the willingness to help others. For example, people who rated themselves as
having a grateful disposition perceived themselves as having more socially
helpful characteristics, expressed by their empathetic behavior, and emotional
support for friends within the last month (McCullough, Emmons, & Tsang, 2002).
In our research, when people report feeling grateful, thankful, and appreciative
in their daily lives, they also feel more loving, forgiving, joyful, and enthusiastic.
Notably, the family, friends, partners and others who surround them
consistently report that people who practice gratitude are viewed as more
helpful, more outgoing, more optimistic, and more trustworthy (Emmons &
McCullough, 2003).
Expressing gratitude for lifes blessings that is, a sense of wonder,
thankfulness and appreciation is likely to elevate happiness for a number of
reasons. Grateful thinking fosters the savoring of positive life experiences and
situations, so that people can extract the maximum possible satisfaction and
enjoyment from their circumstances. Counting ones blessings may directly
counteract the effects of hedonic adaptation, the process by which our
happiness level returns, again and again, to its set range, by preventing people
from taking the good things in their lives for granted. If we consciously remind
ourselves of our blessings, it should become harder to take them for granted
and adapt to them. And the very act of viewing good things as gifts itself is likely
to be beneficial for mood. How much does it matter? Consider these eyepopping statistics. People are 25% happier if they keep gratitude journals, sleep
1/2 hour more per evening, and exercise 33% more each week compared to
persons who are not keeping journals. They achieve up to a 10% reduction in
systolic blood pressure, and decrease their dietary fat intake by up to 20%. Lives
marked by frequent positive emotions of joy, love and gratitude are up to 7
years longer than lives bereft of these pleasant feelings.
nobaproject.com - Positive Psychology
2193
The science of gratitude has also revealed some surprising findings. For
example, students who practice gratitude increase their grade point average.
Occasional gratitude journaling boosts well-being more than the regular
practice of counting blessings. Remembering ones sorrows, failures, and other
painful experiences is more beneficial to happiness than recalling only
successes. Becoming aware that a very pleasant experience is about to end
enhances feelings of gratitude for it. Thinking about the absence of something
positive in your life produces more gratitude and happiness than imagining its
presence.
To assess your own level of gratefulness, take the test below.
2194
2195
Humility
What is humility and why does it matter? Although the etymological roots of
humility are in lowliness and self-abasement (from the Latin term humilis
meaning lowly, humble, or literally on the ground and from the Latin term
humus meaning "earth"), the emerging consensus among scholars is that
humility is a psychological and intellectual virtue, or a character strength. There
is no simple definition but it seems to involve the following elements: A clear
and accurate (not underestimated) sense of ones abilities and achievements;
the ability to acknowledge ones mistakes, imperfections, gaps in knowledge,
and limitations (often with reference to a higher power); an openness to new
ideas, contradictory information, and advice keeping ones abilities and
accomplishments in perspective; relatively low self-focus or an ability to forget
the self; appreciation of the value of all things, as well as the many different
ways that people and things can contribute to our world. In contemporary
society, it is easy to overlook the merits of humility. In politics, business and
sports, the egoists command our attention. Show me someone without an
ego, said real estate mogul Donald Trump, and Ill show you a loser. In contrast,
the primary message of this book is that the unassuming virtue of humility,
rather than representing weakness or inferiority, as is commonly assumed, is
a strength of character that produces positive, beneficial results for self and
society. Successful people are humble people. They are more likely to flourish
in life, in more domains, than are people who are less humble (Exline & Hill,
2196
2012).
Do you think you are you a humble person? For obvious reasons, you cannot
rate your own level of humility. Its an elusive concept to get at scientifically. I
am very humble is self-contradictory. This has not discouraged personality
psychologists from developing questionnaires to get at it, albeit indirectly. For
example, to what extent do you identify with each of the following statements:
1. I generally have a good idea about the things I do well or do poorly.
2. I have difficulty accepting advice from other people.
3. I try my best in things, but I realize that I have a lot of work to do in many
areas.
4. I am keenly aware of what little I know about the world.
People who say they feel humble when they are praised report that the
experience made them want to be nice to people, increase their efforts, and
challenge themselves
Humble people are more admired and the trait of humility is viewed
positively by most
2197
Humble teachers are rated as more effective and humble lawyers as more
likeable by jurors
2198
Outside Resources
Web: Authentic Happiness.
http://www.authentichappiness.sas.upenn.edu
Web: The International Positive Psychology Association (IPPA).
http://www.ippanetwork.org/
Discussion Questions
1. Can you think of people in your life who are very humble? What do they do
or say that expresses their humility? To what extent do you think it would
be good if you were more humble? To what extent do you think it would be
good if you were less humble?
2. How can thinking gratefully about an unpleasant event from your past help
you to deal positively with it? As the result of this event, what kinds of things
do you now feel thankful or grateful for? How has this event benefited you
as a person? How have you grown? Were there personal strengths that grew
out of your experience?
3. Mahatma Gandhi once said, The weak can never forgive. Forgiveness is the
attribute of the strong. What do you think he meant by this? Do you agree
or disagree? What are some of the obstacles you have faced in your own
life when trying to forgive others?
Vocabulary
Character strength
A positive trait or quality deemed to be morally good and is valued for itself as
well as for promoting individual and collective well-being.
Flourishing
To live optimally psychologically, relationally, and spiritually.
Forgiveness
The letting go of negative thoughts, feelings, and behaviors toward an offender.
Gratitude
A feeling of appreciation or thankfulness in response to receiving a benefit.
Humility
Having an accurate view of selfnot too high or lowand a realistic appraisal
of ones strengths and weaknesses, especially in relation to other people.
Positive psychology
The science of human flourishing. Positive Psychology is an applied science with
an emphasis on real world intervention.
Pro-social
Thoughts, actions, and feelings that are directed towards others and which are
positive in nature.
Reference List
Biswas-Diener, R. (2011). Applied positive psychology: Progress and challenges.
European Health Psychologist, 13, 2426.
Culligan, K. (2002). Prayer and forgiveness: Can psychology help? Spiritual Life,
89,78.
Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus
burdens: An
experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily
life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 377389.
Emmons, R. A. (2007). Thanks! How the new science of gratitude can make you
happier. Boston. MA: Houghton-Mifflin.
Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens:
An
experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily
life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 377389.
Enright, R. D. (2001). Forgiveness is a choice. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Exline, J. J., & Hill, P. C. (2012). Humility: A consistent and robust predictor of
generosity. Journal of Positive Psychology, 7, 208218.
McCullough, M. E., Emmons, R. A., & Tsang, J. (2002). The grateful disposition:
A conceptual and empirical topography. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 82, 112127.
McCullough, M. E., Root, L. M., & Cohen A. D. (2006). Writing about the benefits
of an interpersonal transgression facilitates forgiveness. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 887897.
Park, N., Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2006). Character strengths in fiftyfour nations and the fifty U.S. states. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 3,
118129.
Park, N., Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Strengths of character and
well-being: A closer look at hope and modesty. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 23, 603619.
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Character strengths and virtues: A
handbook and classification. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Wood, A. M., Froh, J. J., & Geraghty, A. W. (2010). Gratitude and well-being: A
review and theoretical integration. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 890905.
under
the
Creative
Commons
Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike
4.0
Abstract
This chapter asks two questions: Is happiness good? and Is happier better?
(i.e., is there any benefit to be happier, even if one is already moderately happy?)
The answer to the first question is by and large yes. The answer to the second
question is, it depends. That is, the optimal level of happiness differs,
depending on specific life domains. In terms of romantic relationships and
volunteer activities, happier is indeed better. In contrast, in terms of income,
education, and political participation, the moderate level of happiness is the
best; beyond the moderate level of happiness, happier is not better.
Learning Objectives
Learn about the levels of happiness that are associated with the highest
levels of outcomes.
Introduction
Are you happy? If someone asked this question, how would you answer it? Some
of you will surely say yes immediately, while others will hesitate to say yes. As
scientific research on happiness over the past 30 years has repeatedly shown,
there are huge individual variations among levels of happiness (Diener, Suh,
Lucas, & Smith, 1999). Not surprisingly, some people are very happy, while
others are not so happy, and still others are very unhappy. What about the next
question: Do you want to be happy? If someone asked this question, I would
bet the vast majority of college students (in particular, Americans) would
immediately say yes. Although there are large individual differences in the actual
levels of happiness, nearly everyone wants to be happy, and most of us want
to be happier, even if we are already fairly happy (Oishi, Diener, & Lucas, 2007).
The next important questions then are Is happiness good? and Is happier
better? (i.e., is there any benefit to being happier, even if one is already
moderately happy?) This chapter will tackle these two questions.
Is happiness good? The ancient philosopher Aristotle thought so. He argued
that happiness is the ultimate goal of human beings, because everything else,
ranging from being respected by others, to being with a wonderful partner, to
living in a fabulous house, is all instrumental, namely, to achieve some other
goals (Thomson, 1953). In contrast, all other aspirations (e.g., money, health,
reputation, friendship) are instrumental goals pursued in order to meet higher
goals, including happiness. Thus, according to Aristotle, it is only rational that
happiness is the ultimate objective in life. There are of course plenty of thinkers
who disagree with Aristotle and see happiness as a frivolous pursuit. For
instance, the famous French novelist Gustav Flaubert is believed to have said:
To be stupid, selfish, and have good health are three requirements for
happiness, though if stupidity is lacking, all is lost (Diener & Biswas-Diener,
2008, p. 19). Flaubert clearly associated happiness with selfishness and
thoughtlessness. So what does the science of happiness tell us about the utility
nobaproject.com - Optimal Levels of Happiness
2208
of happiness?
There are two major reviews on this topic so far (Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener,
2005; Veenhoven, 1989). Both reviews found that happiness is good; happy
people tend to be more likely to be successful at work (Cropanzano & Wright,
1999; Roberts, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2003), tend to be more likely to find romantic
partners (Lucas, Clark, Georgellis, & Diener, 2003), tend to be better citizens,
engage in more prosocial behaviors (Carlson, Charlin, & Miller, 1988), and tend
to be healthier and live longer than unhappy people (Pressman & Cohen, 2012).
The correlation between happiness and various life outcomes is almost
never negative. That is, harmful effects of happiness are rare. However, the
effect size is modest at best (r = .20.30), with a great deal of heterogeneity,
suggesting that important moderators (particularly individual differences)
remain to be discovered. Thus, although happiness is generally associated with
positive life outcomes, the next important question is whether it is wise to seek
greater happiness when one is already reasonably happy.
A major review on the question of Is happier better? revealed that the
answer depends on life domains (Oishi, Diener, & Lucas, 2007). In achievementrelated domains such as income and education, once one is moderately happy,
greater levels of happiness were not associated with better outcomes. In
contrast, in relationship-related life domains, even if one is moderately happy,
greater levels of happiness were indeed positively associated with better
outcomes. I will describe specific findings below.
In one study, researchers (Diener, Nickerson, Lucas, & Sandvik, 2002)
followed college students from their freshman year until middle adulthood
(when they were in their late 30s). When participants were incoming college
freshman, they reported their cheerfulness. Nineteen years later, at about age
37, the same participants also reported their annual income. There was a
positive association between cheerfulness in the freshman year and income 19
years later; specifically, the participants who were in the highest 10% of
cheerfulness in 1976 earned an average of $62,681 in 1995. In contrast, the
nobaproject.com - Optimal Levels of Happiness
2209
Figure 1. Happiness at age 18 and income at age 33 (from Oishi & Koo, 2008). Income is
in Australian dollars.
Diener et al.s (2002) finding has been replicated in other large longitudinal
studies. For instance, Oishi et al. (2007) analyzed the Australian Youth in
Transition study, a longitudinal study of nationally representative cohorts of
young people in Australia and found the non-linear effect of happiness on later
income. Participants in the Australian study indicated their life satisfaction
(satisfaction with life as a whole) when they were 18 years old. They also
reported their gross income when they were 33 years old. Like American data,
Australian data also showed that teenagers satisfied with their lives were later
earning more money than those unsatisfied. However, Australians who were
moderately satisfied when they were 18 years old were making the most in
their 30s rather than those who were very satisfied with their lives. Respondents
nobaproject.com - Optimal Levels of Happiness
2210
from the Australian Youth in Transition Study also reported the number of years
of schooling they completed beyond primary education when they were 26
years old. Similar to the income findings, the highest levels of education were
reported by those individuals who had moderate levels of satisfaction when
they were 18 years old. The very satisfied teenagers did not pursue as much
education later as teenagers who were moderately satisfied. One reason why
moderately satisfied individuals later made the most money could be due to
the years of education that people pursued: Very satisfied teenagers do not
seem to pursue more education and, therefore, somewhat limiting their earning
in their 30s.
Oishi et al. (2007) also analyzed two other longitudinal data sets: the German
Socio-Economic Panel Study (GSOEP) and the British Household Panel Study
(BHPS). Both studies used nationally representative samples whose participants
were followed longitudinally. These two data sets again showed that people
who were satisfied with their lives early on were making more money years
later than those who were not satisfied with their lives. However, again, the
relationship between earlier life satisfaction and later income was not linear.
That is, as in Australian data depicted in Figure 1, those who were most satisfied
early on were not making as much money as those who were moderately
satisfied.
In short, four large, longitudinal studies conducted in the United States,
United Kingdom, Australia, and Germany all converged to indicate that
happiness is good up to a certain point; however, higher levels of happiness
beyond a moderate level are not associated with higher incomes or more
education.
What about other life domains? Is the moderate level of happiness also
associated with the best outcome in terms of, say, romantic relationships? The
respondents in the Australian Youth in Transition Study also reported the length
of their current intimate relationship later. In contrast to the income and
education findings, individuals from the very satisfied teenagers were involved
nobaproject.com - Optimal Levels of Happiness
2211
in longer intimate relationships later than were individuals from the second
and third most-satisfied teenagers.
Figure 1. Happiness at age 18 and length of romantic relationships at age 33 (from Oishi
& Koo, 2008)
There are now many other data sets available to test the issues of the optimal
levels of happiness. For instance, the World Values Survey, which was
administered in 1981, 1990, 1995, and 2000, includes 118,519 respondents from
96 countries and regions around the world (see www.worldvaluessurvey.org
for more information about the survey questions and samples). Respondents
rated their overall life satisfaction on a 10-point scale (All things considered,
how satisfied are you with your life as a whole these days?). They also indicated
their income (in deciles from the lowest 10% in the nation to the highest 10%
of the nation), highest education completed, their relationship status (i.e.,
whether they were currently in a stable long-term relationship), volunteer work
they participated in (respondents indicated which, if any, of the 15 types of
volunteer work they were involved in), and political actions they had taken (e.
g., signing a petition, joining in boycotts). These questions were embedded in
more than 200 questions about values and beliefs. Here, we consider income,
highest education completed, relationship status, volunteer work, and political
participation.
nobaproject.com - Optimal Levels of Happiness
2212
The World Values Survey data also showed that the highest levels of income
and education were reported by moderately satisfied individuals (8 or 9 on the
10-point scale) rather than very satisfied (10 on the 10-point scale). Similarly,
the greatest level of political participation was reported by moderately satisfied
individuals rather than by the most satisfied individuals. In contrast, the highest
proportion of respondents in a stable intimate relationship was observed
among the very satisfied rather than moderately satisfied individuals.
Similarly, the highest level of volunteer activities was observed among the very
satisfied individuals.
So far, the research shows that the optimal level of happiness differs,
depending on specific life domains. In terms of romantic relationships and
volunteer activities, happier is indeed better. In contrast, in terms of income,
education, and political participation, the moderate level of happiness is the
best; beyond the moderate level of happiness, happier is not better. Why is it
best to be happiest possible in terms of romantic relationships and volunteer
activities, whereas best to be moderately happy in terms of achievement?
At this point, this mechanism has not been empirically demonstrated. Thus,
the following is just a speculation, or educated guess. First, with regard to the
nonlinear association between happiness and achievement, the main reason
why the very high level of happiness might not be associated with the highest
level of achievement is that complete satisfaction with current conditions might
prevent individuals from energetically pursuing challenge in achievement
domains such as education and income. After all, the defining characteristics
of need for achievement are high standards of excellence and constant striving
for perfection (McClelland, 1961). Similarly, if individuals are completely
satisfied with the current political situation, they might be less likely to actively
participate in the political process. Achievement domains also have very clear
objective criteria, in the form of either monetary value, degree, or skill levels.
Improvement motivation (e.g., self-criticism, self-improvement) serves well in
the achievement domains because this mindset makes clear what needs to be
nobaproject.com - Optimal Levels of Happiness
2213
2214
Conclusion
In summary, the optimal levels of happiness differ, depending on life domains.
In terms of income and education, the optimal levels of happiness seem to be
moderate levels. That is, individuals who are moderately happy are likely to
attain the higher levels of education and earn the most in the future. In contrast,
in terms of romantic relationships and volunteer activities, the optimal levels
of happiness seem to be the highest levels. Individuals who are very happy are
likely to stay in a good romantic relationship or volunteer. The divergent optimal
levels of happiness for relationship and achievement domains suggest that it
is generally difficult to have an extremely high level of overall happiness, good
romantic relationships, and high achievements. To this end, it is not surprising
that icons of improvement motivationTom Cruise, Kobe Bryant, Donald
Trump, and Martha Stewartall had marital problems, while achieving
unprecedented success in their respective fields. It should be noted, however,
that the successes of Warren Buffet, Bill Gates, and Barack and Michele Obama,
among others at work and love, give us some hope that it is possible to have it
all, if you have talent in your chosen field, are passionate about it, and can switch
your motivational strategies between work and love.
2215
Discussion Questions
1. Why do you think that the optimal level of happiness is the moderate level
of happiness for future income, and the highest education achieved? Can
you think of any other reasons than the ones described in this chapter why
this is the case?
2. Do you think that the optimal level of happiness differs, not only across life
domains, but across cultures? If so, how? In which culture, might the optimal
level of happiness be lower? Higher? Why?
3. What might be the optimal level of happiness for health and longevity?
Highest possible level of happiness or moderate level? Why do you think so?
Vocabulary
Happiness
A state of well-being characterized by relative permanence, by dominantly
agreeable emotion ranging in value from mere contentment to deep and
intense joy in living, and by a natural desire for its continuation.
Life domains
Various domains of life, such as finances and job.
Life satisfaction
The degree to which one is satisfied with ones life overall.
Optimal level
The level that is the most favorable for an outcome.
Reference List
Carlson, M., Charlin, V., & Miller, N. (1988). Positive mood and helping behavior:
A test of six hypotheses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55,
211229.
Cropanzano, R., & Wright, T. A. (1999). A 5-year study of change in the
relationship between well-being and job performance. Consulting
Psychology Journal: Practice and Research, 51, 252265.
Diener, E., Nickerson, C., Lucas, R. E., & Sandvik, E. (2002). Dispositional affect
and job outcomes. Social Indicators Research, 59, 229259
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being:
Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276302.
Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2008). Happiness: Unlocking the mysteries of
psychological wealth. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2003). Reexamining
adaptation and the set point model of happiness: Reactions to changes in
marital status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 527539.
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive
affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 803
855.
McClelland, D. C. (1961). The achieving society. New York, NY: D. Van Nostrand
Co.
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (2003). Reflections on the self-fulfilling
effects of positive illusions. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 289295.
Oishi, S., Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (2007). The optimal level of well-being: Can
we be too happy? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 346360.
Oishi, S., & Koo, M. (2008). Two new questions about happiness: Is happiness
good? and Is happier better? In M. Eid & R. J. Larsen. (Eds.), Handbook of
subjective well-being (pp. 290306). New York: Oxford University Press.
Pressman, S. D., & Cohen, S. (2012). Positive emotion use and longevity in
famous deceased psychologists. Health Psychology, 31, 297305.
Roberts, B. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Work experiences and personality
development in young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 84, 582593.
Thomson, J. A. K. (1953). The ethics of Aristotle: The Nicomachean ethics.
London, U.K.: Penguin Books.
Veenhoven, R. (1989). How harmful is happiness? Consequences of enjoying
life or not. Rotterdam: University Pers Roterdam.
Copyright 2014 by Diener Education Fund. Optimal Levels of Happiness by Shigehiro Oishi
is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0
International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/
by-nc-sa/4.0/deed.en_US.
Index
5-reductase, 247
Ability models, 1690
ablate, 218
Absolute stability, 1456
acceptance and commitment therapy, 1847
accommodation, 1755
action potential, 159
Active personenvironment transactions, 1462
active-constructive responding, 1289
adaptation, 2169
adaptations, 292
adherence, 2140
adoption study, 348
Affective, 1614
affective forecasting, 1077
A-fibers, 436
age effects, 1457
age identity, 724
aggression, 1233
aggressive behavior, 250
agnosias, 186
agonists, 2104
agoraphobia, 1902
agreeableness, 1265
Agreeableness, 1380
allodynia, 445
alogia, 1864
altruism, 1269
attrition, 1463
audience design, 896
authoritative, 615
authority stage, 702
autobiographical memory, 998
autobiographical narratives, 724
autobiographical reasoning, 1413
automatic, 1081, 1216
Automatic empathy, 922
automatic process, 1052
automatic thoughts, 1843
autonomic nervous systems, 182
availability heuristic, 1075, 1235
average life expectancy, 728
aversive racism, 1219
avoidant, 1968
awareness, 766
Awe, 1755
axon, 151
balance, 1779
balls, 555
basal ganglia, 213
behavioral genetics, 348
behavioral medicine, 2141
Behaviorism, 128
benevolent sexism, 1440
biases, 842
bidirectional, 705
Big Five, 1493
Big Five, 1408
Dependent, 1968
dependent variable, 29
depolarization, 84
depth perception, 593
DES, 1992
descending pain modulatory system, 443
descriptive norms, 1133
developed countries, 681
developing countries, 681
Developmental intergroup theory, 1438
diagnostic criteria, 1865
dialectical behavior therapy (DBT), 1847
dialectical worldview, 1847
diary method, 52
dichotic listening, 526, 751
Differential stability, 1456
Differential stability, 1460
Diffuse optical imaging (DOI), 220
Diffusion, 156
diffusion of responsibility, 1262
directional goals, 1079
discontinuous., 594
discrimination, 1213
discriminative stimulus, 974
Disorganized behavior, 1864
disorganized speech, 1863
dissociation, 1989
dissociative amnesia, 1035
distinctiveness, 1000
distractor task, 798
identity, 1411
Identity diffusion, 641
image-making stage, 701
Imaginal performances, 1559
immunocytochemistry, 189
impact bias, 1077
impasse-driven learning, 1754
implemental, 1777
Implicit Association Test, 1217
Implicit Association Test, 1084
implicit attitude, 1083
implicit learning, 946
implicit measures of attitudes, 1083
implicit memory, 946
implicit motives, 1495
inattentional blindness, 528
inattentional deafness, 530
incidental learning, 949
independent, 1381
independent self, 1721
independent variable, 29
individual differences, 127
individual level, 1456
individualism, 682
Information processing theories, 592
informational influence, 1133
ingroup, 899, 1057
inhibitory functioning, 722
insomnia, 1996
instrumental, 967
integrated, 544
integrative or eclectic psychotherapy, 1849
intentional, 921
intentional learning, 949
intentionality, 921
interaural level differences (ILDs), 400
interaural time differences (ITD), 400
interdependent self, 1721
interdependent stage, 703
Interest, 1751
interference, 1030
internal, 2161
internal bodily or somatic cues, 1901
internal validity, 49
interoception, 434
interoceptive avoidance, 1901
interpersonal, 1637
interpretive stage, 702
intersexual selection, 293
intra- and inter-individual differences, 721
intrapersonal, 1637
intrasexual competition, 293
intrinsic, 1774
intrinsically motivated, 1752
introspection, 124
invasive, 78
I/O psychology, 1334
ion channels, 156
ionotropic receptors, 162
joint attention, 923
malingering, 2089
manipulate, 1464
margin of error, 107
masculinization, 243
maternal behavior, 237
maturity principle of adult personality development, 1459
Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT), 1693
McGurk effect, 552
means, 1775
medial prefrontal cortex, 1053
medial temporal lobes, 1033
medial vestibulo-spinal tract, 482
medulla oblongata, 188
memory traces, 1004
mentalizing, 1054
mere-exposure effect, 1153, 1313
mere-exposure effects, 796
mesmerism, 1823
Metabolism, 2109
metabolites, 209
Metacognition, 951
mimicry, 921
mindbody connection, 2129
mindfulness, 1846
mindfulness-based therapy, 1846
mirror neurons, 922
misinformation effect, 571, 1005
mixed models, 1693
mnemonic devices, 1012
mock witnesses, 574
monitoring, 1516
monochronic (M-time), 511
mood disorders, 1996
mood-congruent memory, 1080
morph, 1311
motivated skepticism, 1079
motivation, 1774
motor cortex, 213
motor strip, 184
multicultural experiences, 1540
multimodal, 544
multimodal perception, 544
multimodal phenomena, 551
multisensory convergence zones, 547
multisensory enhancement, 546
myelin, 215
myelin sheath, 152
Narcissistic, 1969
narrative identity, 1413
natural selection, 292
Nature, 592
need for closure, 1079
negative feelings, 2161
negative state relief model, 1267
neural crest, 181
neural impulse, 123
neural induction, 180
neural plasticity, 85
neuroblasts, 181
neurodevelopmental, 1871
neuroendocrinology, 1056
neuroepithelium), 181
neuropsychoanalysis, 1360
neuroscience, 1614
neuroscience methods, 78
Neuroticism, 1380
neurotransmitter, 2104
neurotransmitters, 153, 236
nightmares, 1996
nociception, 434
nociceptors, 435
nomenclature, 211
nonassociative learning, 947
nonconscious, 1774
noninvasive, 78
normative influence, 1131
noxious stimuli, 445
nucleus, 151
nucleus accumbens, 1618
numerical magnitudes, 599
Nurture, 592
nurturing stage, 701
obedience, 1135
object permanence task, 595
object relations theory, 1357
obsessive-compulsive, 1968
obsessive-compulsive disorder, 1991
obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), 1908
occipital lobe, 187, 213
OECD countries, 681
omnivores, 415
O*Net, 1344
Openness, 1380
openness to experience, 1540, 1753
operant, 967
operant conditioning, 947, 967
operational definitions, 29
Opponent Process theory of color, 375
oppositional defiant disorder, 2080
optimal levels, 2212
originality, 1533
other-oriented empathy, 1265
outgroup, 1057
outgroups, 899
overconfident, 847
oxygenated hemoglobin, 79
oxytocin, 237, 270
pace of life, 515
pain, 432
panic disorder (PD), 1901
paranoid, 1970
parasympathetic nervous system (PNS), 85
parasympathetic nervous systems., 182
parent management training, 2086
Parental behavior, 252
parietal lobe, 185, 213
participant demand, 31
paternal, 252
pathologizes, 2076
Pavlovian conditioning, 966
pro-social, 2191
prosocial behavior, 1265
prosocial personality orientation, 1265
prototype, 1311
proximity, 1152
psychic causality, 1353
psychoactive drugs, 2104
psychoanalytic therapy, 1839
psychodynamic therapy, 1839
psychogenic, 1818
psychological adaptations, 294
psychological essentialism, 826, 878
psychological reactance, 1115
Psychological vulnerabilities, 1897
psychometric approach, 721
psychoneuroimmunology, 2129
psychopathology, 1864
psychopathy, 2041
psychophysics, 124
Psychophysiological methods, 78
psychosexual stage model, 1355
psychosomatic medicine, 2129
psychotropic drug, 2106
PTM, 1992
punishers, 967
Punishment, 1244
p-value, 105
qualitative changes, 594
quantitative changes, 594
quantitative genetics, 349
self-control, 1775
Self-efficacy, 2133
self-enhancement bias, 1488
self-esteem, 1411
self-expansion model, 1293
self-interest is bounded, 849
self-perceptions of aging, 725
self-regulation, 1774
Self-regulation, 1514
Self-report assessments, 1694
self-report measure, 1994
self-report scales, 2171
Semantic memory, 997
sensitization, 947
Sensitization, 446
sensorimotor stage, 595
sensory modalities, 544
set point, 1799
sex, 1434
sex determination, 246
sexual differentiation, 246
sexual harassment, 1439
sexual orientation, 1434
sexual selection, 293
Sexual strategies theory, 296
shadowing, 751
sibling contrast effect, 1491
significance, 106
silent language, 507
simulation, 923, 1053
Acknowledgments
The Diener Education Fund would like to acknowledge the following individuals and
companies for their contribution to the Noba Project: The staff of Positive Acorn, including
Robert Biswas-Diener as managing editor and Jessica Bettelheim as Project Manager;
Sockeye Creative for their work on brand and identity development, web design, and digital
strategy; Experience Lab for digital user experience design; The Other Firm for web and
software development; Arthur Mount for illustrations; Dan Mountford for artwork; Chad
Hurst for photography; EEI Communications for manuscript proofreading; Marissa Diener,
Shigehiro Oishi, Daniel Simons, Robert Levine, Lorin Lachs, and Thomas Sander for their
feedback and suggestions in the early stages of the project.
Thank you all!