The document discusses color perception from both physical and psychological perspectives. Physically, color depends on the spectral composition of light sources and how surfaces reflect light. Psychologically, humans perceive color in terms of hue, saturation and brightness. The trichromacy theory and opponent-process theory explain how signals from three cone photoreceptors are processed in the brain to yield the psychological experience of color. Color constancy demonstrates how perceived color remains stable despite changes in illumination.
The document discusses color perception from both physical and psychological perspectives. Physically, color depends on the spectral composition of light sources and how surfaces reflect light. Psychologically, humans perceive color in terms of hue, saturation and brightness. The trichromacy theory and opponent-process theory explain how signals from three cone photoreceptors are processed in the brain to yield the psychological experience of color. Color constancy demonstrates how perceived color remains stable despite changes in illumination.
-The psychological dimensions of color appearance (hue, saturation, brightness) -The relationship between the psychological and physical dimensions of color (Trichromacy Color opponency) - Other influences on color perception (color constancy, top-down effects) Sir Isaac Newton Newtons Prism Experiment (1704) White light is composed of multiple colors Light Monochromatic light: one wavelength (like a laser) Physical parameters for monochromatic light 1. Wavelength 2. Intensity Heterochromatic light: many wavelengths (normal light sources) For heterochromatic light The spectral composition gives the intensity at each wavelength Monochromatic light Heterochromatic light Spectral composition of two common (heterochromatic) illuminants The spectral components of light entering the eye is the product of the illuminant and the surface reflectance of objects. Reflectance of some common surfaces and pigments 350 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 750 800 Surface reflectance of some common objects 350 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 750 800 Spectral composition of light entering the eye after being reflected from a surface = X Spectral composition of the illuminant Reflectance of the surface Consider a ripe orange illuminated with a bright monochromatic blue (420 nm) light. What color will the banana appear to be? a. bright orange b. dark/dim orange c. black d. dark/dim blue Wavelength R e l a t i v e
a m o u n t
o f
l i g h t Blue (monochromatic) Spectral composition of light entering the eye after being reflected from a surface = X Spectral composition of the illuminant Reflectance of the surface Wavelength R e l a t i v e
a m o u n t
o f
l i g h t White light Blue (monochromatic) Orange (monochromatic) -The physical dimensions of color -The psychological dimensions of color appearance (hue, saturation, brightness) -The relationship between the psychological and physical dimensions of color (Trichromacy Color Opponency) - Other influences on color perception (color constancy, top-down effects) Color Vision Color Physical dimensions of color Spectral composition light source X reflectance Psychological dimensions of color 1. Hue (e.g., red, blue, green) 2. Saturation (e.g., pastel, deep & rich) 3. Brightness (e.g., dim, bright) Newton, 1703 Newton noticed that the color spectrum could be represented in a circle. Psychological dimensions of color 1. hue Psychological dimensions of color 2. saturation Psychological dimensions of color 3. brightness Psychological dimensions of color Psychological dimensions of color The color solid. Graphics programs allow you to pick colors by hue, saturation and brightness from a color circle. 350 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 750 800 How do we get from the physical properties of light: To the psychological dimensions of color? 400 500 600 700 400 500 600 700 400 500 600 700 Hue: peak (center) of spectral distribution 400 500 600 700 400 500 600 700 400 500 600 700 Saturation: spread (variance) of spectral distribution Brightness: height of spectral distribution 400 500 600 700 400 500 600 700 400 500 600 700 400 500 600 700 400 500 600 700 Bright, saturated blue Bright, partially desaturated red (pink) 400 500 600 700 Dark, desaturated green These rules of hue, saturation and brightness are useful for simple (univariate) spectral distributions: But what about any arbitrary distribution? 400 500 600 700 two primary colors + = ? three primary colors + = ? + 400 500 600 700 random ? Color mixtures Subtractive mixtures Occur with paints (pigments) and filters For pigments: In the mixture, the only wavelengths reflected by the mixture are those that are reflected by all components in the mixture. Cyan paint Yellow paint White light Cyan reflected White light Yellow reflected + = White light Subtractive mixtures For filters: wavelengths in the mixture are those that are passed by every filter in use. The spectrum above is created on an RGB computer monitor by additive mixing Additive mixtures You can create additive mixtures by putting pixels very close together Thats how TVs and computer monitors work. And stadium scoreboards Georges Seurat, The Channel of Gravelines (1890) Georges Seurat, the French Pointillist Painter knew about this! Closeup, Georges Seurat, The Channel of Gravelines (1890) Pointillism painters mixed colors additively rather than subtractively! TVs with three phosphors work because almost any color can be generated by adding different amounts of the three primary colors. TVs with three colors (phosphors) work because almost any color can be generated by adding different amounts of the three primary colors. Why? Because we have three types of photoreceptors. Wavelength (nm) A b s o r p t i o n
( % ) 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 750 Hofer, H. et al. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:9669-9679 Physiology of color vision The normal retina contains three kinds of cones (S, M and L), each maximally sensitive to a different part of the spectrum. Trichromatic theory of color vision Young-Helmholtz Theory (1802,1852). Our ability to distinguish between different wavelengths depends on the operation of three different kinds of cone receptors, each with a unique spectral sensitivity. Each wavelength of light produces a unique pattern of activation in the three cone mechanisms. Perceived color is based on the relative amount of activitythe pattern of activityin the three cone mechanisms. The Principal of Univariance For example, the M cones will respond equally to a dim green light as a bright red light. As far as the M cones are concerned, these lights look the same. the absorption of a photon of light by a cone produces the same effect no matter what the wavelength. A given cone system will respond the same to a dim light near peak wavelength as a bright light away from the peak. 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700 800 S M L A given light will excite the L, M, and S cones differently 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700 800 S M L 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700 800 S M L We can add lights to predict L, M and S responses to different amounts of 3 primaries 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700 800 S M L 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700 800 S M L 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700 800 S M L We can also predict L, M and S responses to different levels of saturation 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700 800 S M L 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700 800 S M L 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700 800 S M L Given almost any light, its possible to find intensities of the 3 primaries that produce the same L, M and S responses 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700 800 S M L 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700 800 S M L 400 500 600 700 S M L Because of the principle of univariance, two different spectra that produce the same L, M and S cone responses will look exactly the same. These pairs are called metamers. They make a metameric match 400 500 600 700 S M L 400 500 600 700 So the theory of trichromacy explains why we only need three primaries to produce a variety of colors. But what does an arbitrary sum of primaries look like? In 1878, Hering argued that trichromacy wasnt enough. He asked: Why dont we ever see yellowish blues? Or reddish greens? Color aftereffects blue green red yellow Why are red and blue opposites? Why are yellow and green opposites? blue green red yellow Color aftereffects Hering proposed the Opponent Process Theory Color vision is based on the activity of two opponent-process mechanisms: 1. A RED/GREEN opponent mechanism. 2. A BLUE/YELLOWopponent mechanism -1 1 S M L The Red-Green opponent system subtracts M from L cone responses Red-Green -1 1 1 S M L The Blue-Yellow opponent system subtracts S from L+M cone responses Blue-Yellow Now with RG = L-M and BY = (L+M) S, we can predict the appearance of any arbitrary spectrum of light. Yellow: 560 nm Blue: 450 nm Red: 650 nm Green: 520 nm 400 500 600 700 S M L R-G B-Y 400 500 600 700 S M L R-G B-Y 400 500 600 700 S M L R-G B-Y 400 500 600 700 S M L R-G B-Y Sum of primaries: Now with RG = L-M and BY = (L+M) S, we can predict the appearance of any arbitrary spectrum of light. 400 500 600 700 S M L R-G B-Y Or more complex spectra: 400 500 600 700 S M L R-G B-Y 400 500 600 700 S M L R-G B-Y Our experience of color is shaped by physiological mechanisms, both in the receptors and in opponent neurons. But, color perception depends on more than wavelength Light/dark adaptive state (Purkinje shift) Adaptation aftereffects Simultaneous contrast effects Color constancy Examples: Adaptation aftereffects Simultaneous contrast effects The reflectance curve of a sweater (green curve) and the wavelengths reflected from the sweater when it is illuminated by daylight (white) and by tungsten light (yellow). Color Constancy, is when the perceived color of objects does not vary much with changes in the illumination, even though these changes cause huge changes in the spectral light entering the eye. Yellow light X blue surface = gray light entering eye blue Blue light X yellow surface = gray light entering eye yellow Example 1: same wavelengths entering the eye, different perceived color Color Constancy, is when the perceived color of objects does not vary much with changes in the illumination, even though these changes cause huge changes in the spectral light entering the eye. Yellow light X gray surface = yellow light entering eye gray Blue light X gray surface = blue light entering eye gray Example 2: different wavelengths entering the eye, same perceived color Color Constancy Somehow, the visual system knows the spectrum of the light source, and takes that into account when determining the reflectance properties of a surface. Blue squares on the left are physically the same as the yellow squares on the right! Blue squares on the left are physically the same as the yellow squares on the right! Blue squares on the left are physically the same as the yellow squares on the right! Whats going on with this illusion? Remember, the light entering your eye is a combination of the light source and the reflectance properties of the object. Whats important to you is the reflectance properties, not the light source. The images on the left and right are drawn to look like the same object, just illuminated by two different lights (yellow on left, blue on right). The blue checks on the left and the yellow checks on the right are both physically gray. But with color constancy, the visual system knows that gray light under yellow illumination must be caused by a blue surface (left), and the gray light under blue illumination must be caused by a yellow surface (right). Another similar example. Center squares are physically the same, but look different. The image is rendered to look like the two objects are illuminated by different colored lights. Chromatic adaptation supports color constancy Color Deficiency (Color Blindness) Monochromat - person who needs only one wavelength to match any color Dichromat - person who needs only two wavelengths to match any color Anomalous trichromat - needs three wavelengths in different proportions than normal trichromat Unilateral dichromat - trichromatic vision in one eye and dichromatic in other Color Experience for Monochromats Monochromats have: A very rare hereditary condition Only rods and no functioning cones Ability to perceive only in white, gray, and black tones Univariance True color-blindness Poor visual acuity Very sensitive eyes to bright light Dichromats only two cone types Dichromats are missing one of the three cone systems, so there are three types of dichromats. Wavelength (nm) A b s o r p t i o n
( % ) 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 750 Protanopes missing L cones Deuteranopes missing M cones Tritanopes missing S cones 1. Protanopia affects 1% of males and .02% of females They are missing the long-wavelength pigment Individuals see short-wavelengths as blue Neutral point (gray) occurs at 492nm Above neutral point, they see yellow Dichromats only two cone types 2. Deuteranopia affects 1% of males and .01% of females They are missing the medium wavelength pigment Individuals see short-wavelengths as blue Neutral point (gray) occurs at 498nm Above neutral point, they see yellow Dichromats only two cone types 3. Tritanopia affects .002% of males and .001% of females They are most probably missing the short wavelength pigment Individuals see short wavelengths as blue Neutral point (gray) occurs at 570nm Above neutral point, they see red Dichromats only two cone types Color Processing in the Cortex There is no single module for color perception Cortical cells in V1, V2, and V4 respond to some wavelengths or have opponent responses fMRI experiments on color vision show responses to color all over the visual cortex, but particularly strong responses in area V4. V4 seems to be necessary for color vision. Damage to V4 leads to cerebral achromatopsia complete color blindness, even though the cones are normal.