This study experimentally tested two hypotheses about Lawrence Kohlberg's theory of moral development stages: 1) That the stages form an invariant sequence, so exposure to reasoning one stage above a subject's own would influence them more than exposure further above or below. 2) That each stage represents a reorganization of preceding stages, so exposure to a stage directly above would influence more than exposure below. Subjects were assessed at a pretest stage, exposed to reasoning either one below, directly above, or two above their stage, then reassessed. Results supported the hypotheses by finding exposure directly above the most effective at changing subjects' posttest stage assignments.
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0 ratings0% found this document useful (0 votes)
117 views8 pages
Turiel
This study experimentally tested two hypotheses about Lawrence Kohlberg's theory of moral development stages: 1) That the stages form an invariant sequence, so exposure to reasoning one stage above a subject's own would influence them more than exposure further above or below. 2) That each stage represents a reorganization of preceding stages, so exposure to a stage directly above would influence more than exposure below. Subjects were assessed at a pretest stage, exposed to reasoning either one below, directly above, or two above their stage, then reassessed. Results supported the hypotheses by finding exposure directly above the most effective at changing subjects' posttest stage assignments.
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 8
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
1966, Vol. 3, No. 6, 611-618
AN EXPERIMENTAL TEST OF THE SEQUENTIALITY OF DEVELOPMENTAL STAGES IN THE CHILD'S MORAL JUDGMENTS x ELLIOT TURIEL 2 Yale University 2 developmental propositions of Kohlberg's theory of moral judgments were tested: (a) that the stages form an invariant sequence, and, thus, more learn- ing results from exposure to the stage directly above one's level than to stages further above; (b) that passage from 1 stage to the next involves integra- tion of the previous stages, and, thus, more learning results from exposure to the stage directly above than to the stage 1 below. First, Ss' stages were determined in a pretest. 44 Ss of Kohlberg's Stages 2 , 3, and 4 were equally distributed among 3 experimental groups and 1 control group. In the treat- ment conditions, Ss were exposed to either the stage 1 below, 1 above, or 2 above the initial dominant stage. The control group was not administered a treatment condition. In a posttest the influence of the treatment conditions was assessed. The results confirmed the hypotheses since exposure to the stage directly above was the most effective treatment. Moral development has been approached from different viewpoints. Developmental theories such as Piaget's (1948) focus on the cognitive processes underlying moral re- sponses and assume that the organization of these processes is different at different stages of development. The greater part of develop- mental research on morality has stemmed from Piaget's theory of moral stages, stages supported only to a limited extent by subse- quent investigations (see Kohlberg, 1963b). Kohlberg (1958, 1963a) has postulated the following set of moral stages, which are based on children's reasoning in response to hypo- thetical moral conflicts (Kohlberg, 1963a) : Stage 1: Punishment and obedience orientation. Stage 2: Naive instrumental hedonism. Stage 3: Good-boy morality of maintaining good relations, approval of others. Stage 4: Authority-maintaining morality. study is based on a dissertation presented to Yale University in candidacy for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. It was conducted while the author held a United States Public Health Service predoctoral fellowship. The author wishes to express his gratitude to the members of the dissertation committee: Edward Zigler, Irvin Child, Merrill Carl- smith, and Robert Abelson. The author is also indebted to Lawrence Kohlberg for his invaluable advice and aid. Thanks are due to Rita Senf for her critical reading of the manuscript. 2 Now at Bank Street College of Education and the Center for Urban Education, 33 West 42nd Street, New York, New York. Stage 5: Morality of contract and democratically accepted law. Stage 6: Morality of individual principles of con- science [pp. 13-14]. While space does not permit a detailed definition of Kohlberg's stages nor of his methods for the elicitation and stage clas- sification of responses, the Method section should clarify the nature of his data. Kohlberg postulated that his stages define a sequence normally followed by each indi- vidual. The sequence of the stages is hypothe- sized to be invariant, with the attainment of a mode of thought dependent upon the attainment of the preceding mode, requiring a reorganization of the preceding modes of thought. Evidence for this hypothesis (Kohl- berg, 1963a, pp. 15-17) consists, first, of findings of age differences, in various cultures, consistent with the notion of sequence and, second, of findings of a "Guttman quasi- simplex" pattern in the correlations between the various types of thought, a pattern expected if they form a developmental order. While this evidence supports the validity of the stages as forming a fixed sequence, there has been no experimental evidence. The aim of the present research was to subject Kohlberg's hypotheses to an experimental test. In particular, the concept of developmental sequence suggests some hypotheses regarding developmental change and learning of new 611 612 ELLIOT TURIEL moral concepts. The plan of the study was to select subjects at varying developmental stages, expose them to moral reasoning that differed from their dominant stage, and then test the amount of learning and generaliza- tion of the new concepts. First, part of the Kohlberg moral judgment interview was ad- ministered to determine the subject's domi- nant stage. With the remaining part of the Kohlberg interview, the subject was then ex- posed to concepts corresponding to a stage differing from his own. Some subjects were exposed to the stage that was one below their own, some to the stage one above, and some to the stage two above. Finally the subject was retested on the entire interview. If Kohl- berg's stages do form a fixed developmental sequence, so that the attainment of a mode of thought is dependent on the attainment of the preceding mode, then it is expected that subjects exposed to the stage directly above their dominant stage would show more usage of that stage on the retest than would subjects exposed to stages two above or one below. Thus this study was designed to test the following two hypotheses: 1. That Kohlberg's stages form an invari- ant sequence so that an individual's existing mode of thought determines which new con- cepts he can learn. It was expected that sub- jects exposed to reasoning corresponding to a stage directly above their dominant stage would be influenced more than those exposed to reasoning corresponding to a stage further above. 2. That each stage represents a reorganiza- tion of the preceding stages, and in effect is a displacement of those stages. If each stage is a reorganization of the preceding stages, rather than an addition to them, then a tendency to reject lower stages would be ex- pected, so that subjects exposed to a stage one above would be influenced more than those exposed to a stage one below their own. METHOD Subjects This experiment used 44 seventh-grade boys from the New Haven public schools, between the ages of 12-0 and 13-7. These boys, chosen at random from the school files, were from the middle socio- economic class, as determined by their parents' occupation and education level. Scoring Methods An individual's developmental stage is determined by using Kohlberg's (1958) moral judgment inter- view, which contains nine hypothetical conflict stories and corresponding sets of probing questions. The following story is an example: In Europe, a woman was near death from a special kind of cancer. There was one drug that the doctors thought might save her. It was a f orm of radium that a druggist in the same town had recently discovered. The drug was expensive to make, but the druggist was charging ten times what the drug cost him to make. He paid $200 for the radium and charged $2,000 for a small dose of the drug. The sick woman's husband, Heinz, went to everyone he knew to borrow the money, but he could only get together about $1,000, which is half of what it cost. He told the druggist that his wife was dying and asked him to sell cheaper or let him pay later. But the druggist said: "No, I discovered the drug and I'm going to make money from it." So Heinz got desperate and broke into the man's store to steal the drug for his wife. Should the husband have done that? Two scoring procedures are available for deter- mining a subject's scores on each of the six stages. (The stage with the highest score represents his dominant stage.) The first, a more global method, involves the use of rating f orms devised by Kohlberg (19S8). A second scoring procedure uses detailed coding forms (Kohlberg, 1958) for each of the nine situations of the interview. These coding f orms were constructed and standardized on the basis of re- sponses given by a large number of subjects. Each response listed in the coding f orms has a stage assigned to it. A subject's responses to a given situa- tion are divided into "thought-content" units, and each unit is assigned to a stage, as determined by the stage classification of that unit in the coding form. In this way the total number of units assigned to each stage is determined. Design and Procedure There were three steps in the experimental pro- cedure. The subject's dominant stage was determined by a pretest interview. In the experimental session subjects were exposed, through role playing, to con- cepts that were either one below, one above, or two above their initial dominant stages. These experi- mental treatments will be referred to as 1, +1, and +2 treatments, respectively. The treatment groups were equated on IQ via the Ammons Full-Scale Picture Vocabulary Test. In a posttest interview the subjects' stage scores were reassessed to determine the influence of the treatment. Pretest selection interview. During the first meet- ing each subject was individually administered six STAGES IN MORAL JUDGMENTS 613 of the nine situations of the Kohlberg interview in order to determine his initial stage scores. A tentative assessment of each subject's scores was made using Kohlberg's global rating forms. Only those subjects whose scores on the dominant stage were twice as large as their scores on the next most dominant stage were retained. In all, 21 subjects were discarded, while the 48 retained were equally distributed among Kohlberg's Stages 2, 3, and 4. Since the global rating system did not provide the sensitivity desired for the experiment, the protocols of subjects retained were rescored using Kohlberg's detailed coding forms. Only those subjects who then scored higher on their dominant stage, as determined by the global ratings, than on any other stage were retained. Four subjects were thus discarded, leaving a total of 44. Experimental treatment conditions. All subjects of a given dominant stage were randomly assigned to the control group or to three experimental groups (JV =11 per group). In the experimental treatments, administered 2 weeks after the pretest, subjects were exposed to moral reasoning in individual role- playing situations with an adult experimenter. In one treatment the reasoning presented was one stage below the initial dominant stage ( 1 treatment); the second treatment group was exposed to reasoning that was one stage above (+1 treatment); and in a third treatment the reasoning presented was two stages above (+2 treatment). Members of the con- trol group were not seen by the experimenter for any kind of treatment. Through role playing of the three remaining stories of the Kohlberg interview, experimental subjects were exposed to the new moral concepts. After each story was read the subject played the role of the main character in the story, and as the main char- acter he was to seek advice about the problem from two friends. The experimenter played the parts of the two friends. The subject first asked one "friend" for "advice," with that friend's advice favoring one side of the conflict, and then asked the second friend, who favored the other side of the conflict. The reasoning was always at the stage appropriate to the subject's treatment condition. All the argu- ments used in the role playing were constructed by closely following the coding forms and thus are based on specific coded responses. Illustrative examples of the treatment-condition arguments are based on the Kohlberg situation in which the husband's conflict is between stealing a drug or letting his wife die. The following two argu- ments, containing Stage 3 reasoning, represent what a Stage 2 subject in the +1 treatment was exposed to in this situation: (a) You really shouldn't steal the drug. There must be some better way of getting it. You could get help from someone. Or else you could talk the druggist into letting you pay later. The druggist is trying to support his family; so he should get some profit from his business. Maybe the druggist should sell it for less, but still you shouldn't just steal it. (6) You should steal the drug in this case. Stealing isn't good, but you can't be blamed for doing it. You love your wife and are trying to save her life. Nobody would blame you for doing it. The person who should really be blamed is the druggist who was just being mean and greedy. The experimenter, while administering the treat- ment, did not know the subject's stage, since he had not scored the pretest, and did not know the experi- mental group of the subject; hence administration of the treatments was blind. The only exceptions were subjects exposed to "Stage 1" concepts who must have been in Stage 2, and those exposed to "Stage 6," who must have been Stage 4 subjects. The possibility of the experimenter recalling the sub- jects' stages since he previously interviewed them is unlikely because there were many lengthy interviews, and because a subject's stage is determined using the scoring guides. Pastiest interview. The posttest consisted of the six pretest situations plus the three situations of the experimental treatments; it was administered to the experimental subjects 1 week after the treatment, and to the control subjects 3 weeks after the pre- test. Subjects were called to the experimental room individually, where they were told they would be asked questions regarding stories similar to the ones they had previously heard. (The repetition of some of the stories and questions did not seem to affect the subjects' willingness to respond. They generally responded with the same interest and concentration as in the pretest.) Reliability and Scoring of Protocols The results reported in this paper are based on the scores obtained through the detailed coding. The interviews were coded by the experimenter more than a year after their administration. The scorer had no knowledge of the identity of the protocol he was coding, nor of its experimental condition. All the pretests were scored separately from the posttests. The coding was carried out on a situation- by-situation basis rather than on a subject-by- subject basis; after all subjects' responses to the first situation were coded, all subjects' responses to the next situation were coded, and so on. One estimate of the reliability of Kohlberg's de- tailed coding system is based on the independent coding by two judges of responses obtained from 17 subjects not used in this experiment. Scores for each subject consisting of the percentage of the statements falling into each of the six stages were calculated. A weighted score per subject was then obtained by multiplying the number of points at each stage by the number of the stage, summing these products, and dividing the sum by the total number of points. The product-moment correlation between the scores of the two judges was .94. A measure of interjudge agreement on the scoring of the subjects in this experiment was obtained from the correlation between the scores of the author, who used the detailed coding system, and 614 ELLIOT TURIEL those of another scorer who used the global rating system. Under both scoring systems a subject re- ceives a number of points on each stage, which can be converted into a single score by the procedure described above. A product-moment correlation of .78 was found for the original 48 subjects. Since the two scoring systems differ slightly, this correlation is a conservative estimate of the interjudge reliability of the detailed coding system. RESULTS The analysis of the posttest interview, which included all nine moral judgment situations, was divided into the following two parts: 1. Stage scores were obtained from the posttest responses to the three situations used in the treatments and not in the pretest. Since the experimental subjects were directly influ- enced on those three situations, these scores, which will be referred to as "direct scores," represent the amount of direct influence of the treatment. 2. Posttest stage scores for the six situa- tions used in the pretest represent the amount of indirect influence, or the tendency to gen- eralize the treatment influence to situations differing from those on which subjects were directly influenced. The measure reflecting in- direct influence is the difference between a subject's pretest and posttest scores on each stage. These change scores will be referred to as "indirect scores." TABLE 1 MEAN DIRECT POSTTEST STAGE SCORES (!N PROPOR- TIONS) ON THE STAGES ONE BELOW ( 1), THE SAME As (0), ONE ABOVE (+1), AND Two ABOVE (+2) THE PRETEST DOMINANT STAGE Stage level relative to pretest dominant stage" -1 0 +1 +2 Condition groups b i treatment .336n .283 .13121 .057 +1 treatment .183,, .346 .26622 .102 +2 treatment .209 13 .374 .14S 2 , .099 Control .240,4 .395 .122 24 .085 Note. Dunnett < tests were computed for each boldface figure against each of the other three figures in the same row. Tests significant at the .05 level, Group 11 > 13; at the .025 level, 11 > 12; at the .005 level, 22 > 21, 22 > 23, 22 > 24. n Each subject had received pretest scores at each develop- mental stage, the highest of these indicating his dominant stage. On the posttest, for each individual the proportion of his total score was calculated for each level listed in the left column. b N 11 in each group. Direct Scores The analysis of the direct scores involved the percentage of usage for each subject of the stage that is: one below the initial domi- nant stage ( 1 scores), at the same stage as the initial dominant stage (0 scores), one above the initial dominant stage ( + 1 scores), and two above the initial dominant stage ( + 2 scores). 3 The hypothesis was that an individual ac- cepts concepts one stage above his own domi- nant position more readily than he accepts those two stages above, or those one stage below. Two specific hypotheses result from this general hypothesis that the +1 treatment would be the most effective: (a) that the +1 treatment causes more movement to +1 than the +2 treatment causes movement to +2 or the 1 treatment to 1, and (b) that the + 1 treatment causes more 4-1 movement than does any other treatment. Test 0} Hypothesis a. Table 1 presents (in boldface type), for each experimental group, the mean amount of usage of concepts at the same stage as that of the treatment condition. Table 1 also presents the control group mean scores on the stages that are one below ( 1 scores), one above ( + 1 scores), and two above ( + 2 scores) their dominant stage. The experimental groups' scores may not reflect solely the influence of the experimental manipulations. To determine how much of these scores reflects factors other than the treatments, it is necessary to correct for the change that would have occurred independ- ently of the experimental manipulations. The best estimate of this change is provided by the control group, which had no treatment. It may be assumed that the scores of the control group are due to statistical regression and other artifactual sources. 4 8 The other scores, such as those of the stage two below or three above the dominant stage, are not reported because they did not show significant differences between the groups and do not add to the understanding of the problem. 4 It may be a function of skewness that the 1 score of the control group was considerably larger than the +1 or +2 scores. Of a subject's series of scores one stage has the largest score while its adjacent stages have the next largest scores, with the more distant stages to the dominant stage having STAGES IN MORAL JUDGMENTS 615 The experimental groups' scores were cor- rected by subtracting from those scores the corresponding control group scores. This sub- traction was done in the following manner: The 1 mean of the control group was sub- tracted from the 1 mean of the 1 treat- ment group; the +1 mean of the control group was subtracted from the +1 mean of the +1 treatment group; the +2 mean of the control group was subtracted from the +2 mean of the +2 treatment group. The three corrected means ( 1 = .096, +1 = .144, + 2 =.014) obtained in this way are presumably free of artifacts and thus repre- sent the amount of influence of the experi- mental treatments. The corrected means show that, as hy- pothesized, the direct influence of the +1 treatment was greater than that of the other two treatments. The corrected mean of the + 1 treatment group was shown to be signifi- cantly greater than the corrected mean of the +2 treatment group by a one- tailed t test (t = 3.55, p < .DOS). 5 The one-tailed t test of the difference between the corrected means smaller scores. The subjects of this experiment tended to use the stages below the dominant stage more than those above, resulting in a positively skewed distribution on the six situations of the pretest. When the other three situations are included, more usage of the stage directly below the dominant stage, resulting in less skewness, would be expected. The control group and the experimental groups were originally very similar. There were no sig- nificant differences between the combined scores of the experimental groups and the scores of the control group, with the t values ranging from .10 to .65. We also compared each experimental group with the control group and f ound no significant differences. 'Having subtracted the appropriate control score from the experimental condition score we then com- puted a t test for the difference between the corrected means. The standard error for this / test is complicated by the fact that we subtracted correlated groups from independent groups. However, the appropriate standard error may be shown to be : where : si 2 =the MSw for the +1 scores multiplied by 2 /n s = the MS W for the +2 scores multiplied by 2 /n n = the number of subjects in each group TCICJ = the correlation between the +1 and +2 scores of the control group. (We are indebted to Robert Abelson and Merrill Carlsmith for the derivation of this expression.) of the +1 treatment group and the 1 treat- ment group reached a borderline level of significance (t = 1.43, p < .10). The corrected mean of the 1 treatment group was significantly greater than the cor- rected mean of the + 2 treatment group (* = 2.03, p< .05). Test of Hypothesis b. We have demon- strated that the amount of usage of the treat- ment condition stage was greater in the +1 treatment group than in the other two experi- mental groups. While this result is necessary to demonstrate the greater influence of the +1 treatment, the +1 scores of the +1 treat- ment group must also be compared with the + 1 scores of the other groups. Table 1 contains the +1 scores of each of the four groups. The differences between the +1 score of the +1 treatment group and the + 1 scores of the other groups were tested using Dunnett's t statistic, which is appropri- ate in simultaneously testing one group mean against each of several others (Winer, 1962). These t tests indicated that the +1 treatment was the most effective condition in moving subjects up one stage, since the +1 score of the +1 treatment group was significantly larger than the +1 scores of any other group (Table 1). Other findings. Table 1 also presents the 1, 0, and +2 scores for the four groups. The 1 score of the 1 treatment group was larger than the 1 scores of the other groups. However, the Dunnett t test indicates that the difference between the 1 score of the 1 treatment group and the 1 score of the control group did not reach significance (t= 1.66). The differences between the -1 score of the 1 treatment group and the 1 scores of the +1 and the +2 treatment groups were both significant (Table 1). Using Dunnett t tests, comparisons of the +2 score of the +2 treatment group with the +2 scores of the control group (t < 1), of the 1 treatment group (t = 1.16), and the + 1 treatment group (t < 1), indicated that the +2 treatment did not show a significant effect. Congruent with the hypothesis, the con- trol group and the +2 treatment group showed the greatest usage of the dominant 616 ELLIOT TURIEL stage (0 scores). An analysis of variance com- paring the control and the + 2 treatment groups on the one hand, with the 1 and + 1 treatment groups on the other hand, showed a significant difference (F = 4.72, dj- 1/32, p< .OS). Conclusions regarding the direct scores, (a) The +1 treatment had a direct effect, an effect greater than that of either the 1 or + 2 treatment, (b) Although not reaching an acceptable significance level, there was some suggestion that the 1 treatment had an effect in moving subjects down one stage. (c) The +2 treatment did not show a sig- nificantly greater effect than the control condition or the other experimental treat- ments in moving subjects up two stages. Indirect Scores The analysis of the indirect scores was similar to that of the direct scores. The in- direct score is not a rating of responses on the posttest, but rather a measure of change from pretest to posttest. For each subject's stage scores we subtracted the pretest from the posttest scores and obtained change scores. As indicated by Table 2, the pattern of results of the indirect scores was consistent with the hypotheses and with the results on the direct scores. The evidence is only sug- gestive since significant findings were mini- mal. A one-tailed t test (t = 2.70, p < .025) showed that the corrected mean of the +1 treatment group (.052) was significantly larger than that of the +2 treatment group TABLE 2 MEAN INDIRECT POSTTEST STAGE SCORES (IN PROPOR- TIONS) ON THE STAGES ONE BELOW ( 1), THE SAME As (0), ONE ABOVE (+1), AND Two ABOVE (+2) THE PRETEST DOMINANT STAGE Stage level relative to pretest dominant stage -1 0 + 1 +2 Condition groups' 1 -1 treatment + .057 -.045 -.004 -.016 +l treatment + .001 -.043 + .045 -.002 +2 treatment + .009 -.022 + .016 +.008 Control + .045 -.061 -.007 +.010 a N = 11 in each group. (-.002). The one-tailed t test of the differ- ence between the corrected means of the +1 treatment group (.052) and the 1 treatment group (.012) reached a borderline level of significance (t = 1.46, p < .10). Although the +1 score of the +1 treatment group was larger than the +1 scores of the other groups, none of these differences was significant. No other relevant differences were significant. DISCUSSION The analysis of the direct scores showed that the +1 treatment was the most effective of the three treatments, with the +2 treat- ment being the least effective. The similarity between the patterns of the indirect and the direct scores suggests that the differential effect of the treatments represented something more than memorization of the specific ver- balizations used in the treatments and that some change occurred in generalized moral concepts. This conclusion remains tentative since the results on the indirect scores were minimally significant and since the same interview form was used in the test-retest procedure. The findings support Kohlberg's schema of stages as representing a developmental con- tinuum, in which each individual passes through the stages in the prescribed sequence. If the stages do form a developmental sequence, then it should be easier for subjects to understand and utilize concepts that are directly above their dominant stage than concepts that are two stages above. The developmental interpretation is also strengthened by the finding that subjects assimilated the next higher stage more readily than the lower stage, even though they could understand the concepts of the lower stage as well as, if not better than, those of the higher stage. Hence, we have an indication that the attainment of a stage of thought involves a reorganization of the preceding modes of thought, with an integration of each previous stage with, rather than an addition to, new elements of the later stages. Causal Factors of Changes in Stage The subjects exposed to the stage one above their dominant stage did learn to use STAGES IN MORAL JUDGMENTS 617 some new modes of thought. A factor causing the use of new modes of thought may be cognitive conflict. Indeed, Smedslund's work with the concept of conservation (Smedslund, 1961a, 1961c, 1961d) indicates that cognitive conflict may lead to reorganization of struc- ture. The concept of cognitive conflict is similar to the concept of disequilibrium, which Piaget and Inhelder have presented rather obscurely (Inhelder & Piaget, 1958; Piaget, 19SO). They seem to be saying that movement from one structure to the next occurs when the system, by being challenged, is put into a state of disequilibrium. Thus change in structure would involve the estab- lishment of a new equilibrium after the occurrence of disequilibrium. Such a viewpoint is relevant to our study. Since subjects were exposed to new modes of thought through arguments justifying both sides of a moral conflict, they did not really receive solutions to the problems. Such a situation, which exposed subjects to cogent reasons justifying two contradictory positions, could have resulted in cognitive conflict arising from an active concern with both sides of the issue. When the arguments were too "simple," as in the 1 treatment, the sub- jects may not have become actively involved. When the arguments were too "complicated," as in the +2 treatment, the subjects may not have understood them. However, exposure to concepts one stage above, concepts within a subject's grasp, allowed him contact with new contradictory ideas requiring thought. Perhaps coping with concepts that had some meaning to the subjects led to new modes of thinking, or to a greater use of the stage that was one above the initial stage. Related Studies A study having a direct relation to the present research, by Bandura and McDonald (1963), attempts to demonstrate that Piaget's (1948) sequence of moral development changes is a function of reinforcement con- tingencies and imitative learning. The study assumed that Piaget's stages of moral de- velopment could be defined as a stage of "objective responsibility" (moral judgment in terms of the material damage or conse- quences), followed by a stage of "subjective responsibility" (judgment in terms of inten- tion). Following one of Piaget's procedures, Bandura and McDonald assigned children to stages in terms of responses to paired storied acts, one being a well-intentioned act result- ing in considerable material damage, and the other a maliciously motivated act resulting in very little material damage. Their experimental treatments attempted to influence the subjects by reinforcing adult models who expressed judgments in opposition to the child's orientation, and by reinforcing any of the child's own responses that run counter to his dominant mode. Two measures of learning of the opposite orientation were obtained: the amount of learning during ex- perimental treatment, and a posttest response to new stories immediately following the treatment. They showed that children could be influenced to judge on the orientation op- posite to their initial one. Bandura and Mc- Donald viewed this evidence as "throwing considerable doubt on the validity of a devel- opmental stage theory of morality." An adequate test of a stage theory of morality must deal with stages that are truly representative of mental structure rather than with specific verbal responses. Empirical tests of Piaget's moral judgment theory indicate that the stages do not meet the necessary criteria (Kohlberg, 1963b). However, the Bandura and McDonald study does not pro- vide an adequate test of Piaget's theory be- cause his two stages are not those of objective and subjective responsibility, but rather are those of heteronomous and autonomous orien- tations. The heteronomous and autonomous stages are each represented by 11 observable aspects (Kohlberg, 1963b) of children's defi- nitions of right and wrong, of which the dimension of objective-subjective responsi- bility is only one. By studying only one dimension as manifested in children's choices between two alternatives Bandura and Mc- Donald dealt with isolated surface responses, and not with the concept of stage or mental structure. In their experimental treatment one of two possible answers was reinforced Therefore, the induced changes did not repre- sent underlying structures, but instead repre- 618 ELLIOT TURIEL sented switches to what the subjects thought were the correct answers. 6 Another important deficiency in their pro- cedure was the administration of the posttest immediately after the experimental treatment. As Smedslund (1961b) has demonstrated, the test of duration over time is a main criterion for distinguishing between cognitive structure and superficially learned responses. There was a small decrease in subjective responses given by objective children from the experimental treatment to the posttest, while there was no such decrease in the objective responses of subjective children. This finding, that down- ward movement was more stable than upward movement, is in contrast to our findings, in which upward movement was more stable. It is not surprising that the learning of surface verbal responses related to a lower stage can be retained for a short time. It is interesting that the learning of responses related to a higher stage was not entirely retained, even for such a short period of time. In the present research we have worked with responses assumed to reflect mental structure and have found that the concept of developmental stage or mental structure has much relevance to the understanding of children's moral thinking. We suggest that the effectiveness of environmental influences depends on the relation between the type of concept encountered and developmental level. 6 It must be pointed out that in the contrasting pairs of stories a well-intentioned act always resulted in much material damage, while the maliciously motivated act always resulted in little material damage. A child in the objective stage could easily have learned to more frequently designate, as being worse, that story which contained less material damage, thinking that it was the expected answer. Thus he could have given the "higher stage" answer without having learned the concept of intention. REFERENCES BANDURA, A., & MCDONALD, F. J. Influence of social reinforcement and the behavior of models in shaping children's moral judgments. Journal of Abormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 274-281. INHELDER, B., & PIAOET, J. The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. New York: Basic Books, 1958. KOHLBERG, L. The development of modes of moral thinking in the years ten to sixteen. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago, 1958. KOHLBERG, L. The development of children's orienta- tions toward a moral order: I. Sequence in the development of moral thought. Vita Humana, 1963, 6, 11-33. (a) KOHLBERG, L. Moral development and identification. In H. W. Stevenson (Ed.), Yearbook of the Na- tional Society for the Study of Education: Pt. I. Child psychology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1963. Pp. 277-332. (b) PIAGET, J. The moral judgment of the child. (Orig. publ. 1932) Glencoe, 111.: Free Press, 1948. PIAGET, J. The psychology of intelligence. (Orig. publ. 1947) New York: Harcourt, Brace, 1950. SMEDSLUND, J. The acquisition of conservation of substance and weight in children: II. External reinforcement of conservation of weight and of the operations of addition and subtraction. Scandi- navian Journal of Psychology, 1961, 2, 71-84. (a) SMEDSLUND, J. The acquisition of conservation of substance and weight in children: III. Extinction of conservation of weight acquired "normally" and by means of empirical controls on a balance. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1961, 2, 85- 87. (b) SMEDSLUND, J. The acquisition of conservation of substance and weight in children: V. Practice in conflict situations without external reinforcement. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1961, 2, 156- 160. (c) SMEDSLUND, J. The acquisition of conservation of substance and weight in children: VI. Practice on continuous vs. discontinuous material in problem situations without external reinforcement. Scandi- navian Journal of Psychology, 1961, 2, 203-210. (d) WINER, B. J. Statistical principles in experimental design. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1962. (Received June 1, 1965)
(Proceedings of the International Colloquium in the Philosophy of Science, London, 1965, Volume 4) Imre Lakatos (Ed.), Alan Musgrave (Ed.)-Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge-Cambridge University Pr