Department of Psychology, University of York, UK Purpose. This study was aimed at developing a measure of the capacity to use social support effectively. Methods. The responses of 329 participants to a preliminary 130-item scale derived from a scenario study were subjected to principal axis factoring. This resulted in a 48- item Interpersonal Trust Questionnaire (ITQ) comprising three factors labelled fear of disclosure (FOD), social coping (SC) and social intimacy (SI). A second sample of 380 participants then completed the nal form of the ITQ together with the rehearsal (rumination) and emotional inhibition scales from the Emotion Control Questionnaire (ECQ; Roger & Najarian, 1989), the Social Support Questionnaire (SSQ; Sarason, Levine, Basham & Sarason, 1983) and the Inventory of Socially Supportive Behaviours (ISSB: Barrera, 1981). Data on deteriorationin health status over a period of adaptation were also available for a subsample of these participants. An additional sample provided scores on the ITQ and the Network Orientation Scale (NOS; Vaux, Burda & Stewart, 1986). Results. The scales were found to correlate in predictable ways with measures of both social support and emotion control. However, there were gender differences conrming the preference amongst women for emotional rather than informational support. Regression analyses using deterioration in health status as the dependent variable showed an effect for FOD, but this was signicant only amongst women. Conclusions. The ndings indicate the importance of taking both individual differences and availability of support into account, and it is hoped that the new ITQ scale will offer the means for including relevant individual differences in future studies of the role of social support in moderating stress responses. The deleterious effects of stress on health and well-being have been widely reported in the literature, but assuming that stress causes illness raises questions about the mechanisms which might be involved in linking cognitive processes such as perceived threat with physical symptomatology. Psychoimmunological research has suggested that the most plausible candidate mechanism involves the sustained activation of the hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenal axis (Jemmott & Locke, 1984), and there is certainly evidence for the role of corticosteroids in compromising immune function (Asterita, 1985). However, in view of the wide individual differences in cognitive and physiological responses to stress, physiological mechanisms alone provide only a partial answer. One explanation is that moderator variables such as personality may be involved. For 165 British Journal of Health Psychology (1999), 4, 165179 Printed in Great Britain 1999 The British Psychological Society *Requests for reprints should be addressed to Derek Roger, Department of Psychology, University of York, Heslington, York YO1 5DD, UK. example, Kobasas (1979) research on hardiness had suggested that having an internal locus of control may buffer individuals against the impact of stressful life events. Unfortunately, Denney & Frisch (1981) failed to support the hypothesized moderating effect of locus of control on stress, and when situational control is taken into account, having an internal locus of control has been shown to be associated with a greater susceptibility to stress amongst individuals who experienced high levels of uncon- trollable life events (Meadows, 1989). Extraversion and neuroticism have also been investigated in the context of stress, but the ndings have similarly been equivocal: while Totman, Kiff, Reed & Craig (1980) and Broadbent, Broadbent, Phillpotts & Wallace (1984) reported higher levels of susceptibility to experimentally induced colds amongst introverts as compared to extraverts, the effect was not replicated in a more carefully controlled study by Cohen, Tyrrell & Smith (1993). The latter authors also included a measure of locus of control in their study, and again they found no moderating effects for perceived control on outcomes. Reviewing these inconclusive ndings, Roger (1995) has argued that the personality constructs used in earlier studies were inappropriate because they had not been developed specically in the context of stress research. Roger and his colleagues suggested that inhibiting emotion or ruminating over emotional distress might contribute to delayed recovery, and the Emotion Control Questionnaire (ECQ; Roger & Najarian, 1989; Roger & Nesshoever, 1987) was developed to assess these tendencies. The ECQ comprises subscales for rehearsal (or emotional rumination) as well as emotional inhibition, and validation studies of the ECQ have shown that rumination is particularly strongly related to impaired physiological and psychological adaptation following stress (Nieland & Roger, 1993; Roger &Jamieson, 1988; Roger &Najarian, 1998). Scores on the emotional inhibition subscale were also implicated in these studies, and Kaiser, Hinton, Krohne, Stewart & Burton (1995) have shown independently that emotional inhibition is signicantly related to delayed muscle tension recovery following stress. In addition to personality, social support has frequently been investigated as a potential moderator of the impact of stress, and the benecial effects of social support have been reported for general health and well-being (Cobb, 1976; Cohen & Wills, 1985; Thoits, 1985) and for facilitating adaptation and adjustment to change (Cobb, 1976; Sarason, 1981). However, it has also been suggested that high levels of social support may have negative consequences in mental health settings (Sandler & Barrera, 1984), and in a comparison of the effects of positive and negative interactions on health, negative interactions were found to outweigh the benecial effects of social support (Fiore, Becker & Coppel, 1983). These conicting ndings stem in part from a lack of consensus over the denition of social support, which is often referred to as a unitary construct when in fact it appears to be multidimensional, broad in scope and highly heterogeneous. More precise denitions of social support based on functional categories such as emotional support, information support and instrumental support have been proposed (House & Kahn, 1985; Turner, 1981), but the different functions have been found to be highly correlated. In an attempt to resolve some of these issues, Barrera (1981; 1986) and others (for example, Gottlieb, 1985; Sarason, Sarason & Pierce, 1990) have proposed placing research on social support into three categories labelled social integration, enacted or received support, and perceived support. Social integration refers to the objective Angela Forbes and Derek Roger 166 assessment of the number or density of people in an individuals social network (Berkman, 1985), while enacted support assesses the amount of supportive behaviour received over a specied period of time (Tardy, 1985). However, neither of these measures provides an assessment of the quality of the support received, and it has frequently been argued that satisfaction with supports is far more important than the number available (Barrera, 1981). Measures of perceived support provide a subjective assessment of satisfaction as well as availability (Cohen & Hoberman, 1983; Procidano & Heller, 1983; Sarason, Levine, Basham & Sarason, 1983), and have been found to exhibit a far stronger relationship with health than either of the other two measures (Cohen & Hoberman, 1983). Gottlieb (1985) has concluded that individuals who have a strong sense of perceived support are likely to make less threat-focused appraisals of potentially stressful situations, which results in their ability to cope more independently. The emphasis on cognitive evaluation ts well with the current literature on the relationship between stress and illness, in that the appraisal of a potential stressor and of the coping resources available will inuence whether or not the situation is dened as stressful (Folkman, Schaefer & Lazarus, 1979). However, the appraisal process is strongly inuenced by individual differences which may act as moderator variables, and one question which arises is whether perceived support is a function of objective evaluations of the quality of supports, or whether it has more to do with personality. As early as 1984, Lefcourt, Martin & Saleh (1984) had shown that people with an internal locus of control derived greater benets from social support than externals, and Sarason, Sarason & Shearin (1986) have taken the further step of suggesting that support might be regarded as a personality variable in its own right. A number of studies have suggested that the expression of emotion serves to reduce or attenuate physiological arousal associated with stress (Mendolia & Kleck, 1993), and that it represents a stable individual difference (Kring, Smith & Neale, 1994). These ndings have direct implications for social support, since it is in the context of support that emotion is expressedindeed, one might argue that the function of social supports is to provide the opportunity for the expression of emotion. As early as 1976, Tolsdorf dened what he called network orientation as an individuals attitudes about the usefulness of others in helping to cope with problems, and Vaux, Burda & Stewart (1986) used Tolsdorfs ideas to develop the Network Orientation Scale (NOS), which was designed specically to assess individual differences in the propensity to draw upon social supports. Unfortunately, item selection for the NOS was based on item total correlations rather than factor analysis, and the selection criteria were not consistent across the samples which were used. Internal consistency for the scale was also modest, and retest reliability was low (.18 over a 3-week inter-test interval). The network orientation model has recently been adapted by Gruen, Schuldberg, Nelson, Epstein, Weiss & Quinlan (1994) to form the Network Utilization Scale (NUS). These authors reported a signicant relationship between scores on the new scale and depressive symptomatology, but they acknowledge that further work is needed on the scale, particularly with respect to retest reliability and validation against observational data and other forms of psychopathology. The reduction in stress-related physiological arousal through the expression of emotion implies that social support is used as a coping strategy. Coping processes are 167 Stress, social support and fear of disclosure typically classied as either rational or emotional, but within this broad classication a variety of different strategies have been identied. Folkman & Lazarus (1985), for example, describe eight factors in their Ways of Coping Questionnaire (WCQ), but half of them comprised fewer than ve items each. In response to these and other psycho- metric shortcomings in the WCQ, Endler & Parker (1990) devised the Multidimensional Coping Inventory (MCI), which comprises three factors: task (or rational), emotional and avoidance. Roger, Jarvis & Najarian (1993) expanded this model with the inclusion of an additional detachment factor, and subsequent research by Roger, Najarian & Nash (1996) demonstrated that the detached and emotional factors combine into a single bipolar factor which signicantly predicts worsening health status during stress. The aim of the present study was to shed further light on individual differences in the capacity to self-disclose and express emotion in an adaptive manner in the context of social support. The study reports on the development of a new instrument, the Interpersonal Trust Questionnaire (ITQ). The relationship of the ITQ to existing indices of social support, coping and other relevant measures of individual differences is examined, including the NOS, as well as the extent to which inhibiting emotional expression inuences health during periods of adaptation. Method Participants Two samples of participants were included in the study. The rst sample was used for the construction and factorial validation of the new scale, and comprised 329 participants of which 242 were females (mean age 20.52 years; SD 4.01) and 75 were males (mean age 22.19 years; SD 6.78). They were all undergraduate students, drawn from a wide range of academic disciplines, excluding psychology, at the University of York, the University of Ripon and York St. John in York, and the University of Humberside in Hull. The second sample of 380 was used for the validation studies, and comprised 149 males (mean age 20.38 years; SD 5.78) and 231 females (mean age 19.65 years; SD 4.22). All were undergraduatesat the University of York. Test construction In order to generate items for the preliminary item pool, a scenario-basedquestionnairewas completed by an independent sample of 43 undergraduates from the University of York, 19 males and 24 females (mean age 18.98 years; SD 1.60). The questionnaireconsisted of 10 scenarios describing potentially stressful situations that were intendedto be of relevance to the populationin question, based on a methodology developedby the second author (Roger, Jarvis & Najarian, 1993; Roger & Nesshoever, 1987). The participants were asked to give as much informationas possible about the way in which they would respond to the situations described. Their responses were used to generate the items for the initial pool, and the scenario technique has the advantage of providing questionnaire items which are more contemporaneous and less biased than conventional trawls through literature and existing scales. The responses generated by the scenarios were combined with those from a review of the literature to generate a 130-item pilot questionnaire. The response format was a 4-point Likert scale ranging from strongly agree to strongly disagree. The questionnairewas sent out to the 329 students comprisingthe rst sample, and their returns were subjectedto principal axis factor analysis. Ascree test (Cattell, 1966) indicated a three-factor terminal solution, which was produced by Varimax orthogonal rotation. Only three items which loaded on more than one factor were omitted, and using a criterion of 0.4, the three-factor solution included 48 items, 27 on the rst factor, 12 on the second factor and 9 on the third factor. The loadings are shown in Table 1 (the items in the scale appear in the Appendix). Angela Forbes and Derek Roger 168 169 Stress, social support and fear of disclosure Table 1. Factor loadings for the three-factor solution Loading Item Factor 1 (FOD) Factor 2 (SC) Factor 3 (SI) 16 + .692 10 + .685 23 + .683 9 + .623 40 + .620 29 + .598 12 + .583 36 + .579 27 + .564 25 + .555 19 + .550 47 + .543 2 + .540 43 + .539 7 + .506 45 + .477 34 + .471 14 + .456 38 + .452 6 + .440 17 + .445 41 2 .439 4 2 .434 32 + .424 31 + .413 21 + .406 28 + .404 26 + .674 22 + .652 37 + .622 11 2 .595 18 + .571 1 + .556 15 + .549 39 + .548 3 + .522 46 + .461 8 + .411 48 + .407 33 + .626 13 + .600 5 + .557 42 + .545 24 + .460 30 + .448 20 + .439 35 + .424 44 + .419 Note. FOD = fear of disclosure; SC = social coping; SI = social intimacy. The two highest loadingitems on the rst factor were 16 (I worry too much about what others think of me to conde in them) and 10 (To discuss my problems with somebody feels good at the time but afterwards I worry about what I have said), and this factor was labelled fear of disclosure (FOD). The two highest loading items on the second factor were 26 (I like to talk problems over to get them off my chest) and 22 (When I feel upset about something I feel the need to talk to somebody about it), and this factor was labelled social coping (SC). Items 33 (A good friend is somebody you can be honest with) and 13 (To be able to give and take in a friendship is important) loaded highest on the third factor, which was labelled social intimacy (SI). Four-factor and two-factor solutions were also examined. The items on the rst three factors of the four- factor solution were virtually identical to those for the three-factor solution. Only six items loaded on the fourth factor, all of which referredexplicitly to the familyfor example, the highest loading was for item55 in the initial pool, I feel that I can turn to my family for help whenever I need to. In fact, this item and the item, I can conde in at least one family member of my family, both loaded below criterion on the third factor of the three-factor solution (.380 and .372, respectively). These items may well be salient to the student population used, but in view of a low coefcient alpha for the fourth factor and the small number of items, the four-factor solution was rejected in favour of three factors. The second factor on the two-factor solution was very similar to FOD, while the rst factor appeared to combine some elements of the second and third factors from the three-factor solution. In fact, rotating only the items on the rst factor solution to a two-factor solution yielded factors very similar to SC and SI, but with fewer items that either factor from the three-factor solution. Separate factor analyses for the male and female samples producedalmost identical three-factor structures. The analysis was also repeated using an oblique rotation, but this did not signicantly alter the orthogonal structure. There was no increase in double loads over the three which occurred in the orthogonal rotation, thus conrming that the three factors were indeed independent. Responses to the full item pool were subsequently collected from an additional sample of undergraduatestudents, and these were used to increase the original sample to a total of 543 participants, 176 males (mean age 20.96 years; SD6.33) and 367 females (mean age 20.19 years; SD 4.47). A scree test again indicated a three-factor structure, and a maximum likelihood conrmatory factor analysis of these data yielded a structure indistinguishable from the earlier solution. Testing the t of the three-factor model yielded a chi-square value of 2299.15 (p < .0001). One of the shortcomings of the initial factor analysis was the low participant:variable ratio, but the robustness of the factor structure is conrmed by the replication of the ndings with the substantially larger sample. The fact that the analyses extracted only approximately one third of items from the initial pool was not surprising in view of the scenario technique used to generate items, which tends to be exhaustive and uncontaminated by experimenter bias in item selection. All three of the factors had good face validity and were internally consistent, and in viewof the convergent structure across analyses and samples, a three-factor solution was accepted as representing the best t. Correlations were computed between the factors for the original sample, and SC was found to correlate signicantly inversely with FOD (r(327) = 2 .338; p < .001) and signicantly positively with SI (r(327) = .416; p < .001). The FOD and SI factors did not correlate signicantly (r(327) = 2 .104). Separate correlation matrices for equal samples of 75 males and females yielded broadly similar results. The FOD and SI factors did correlate signicantly for males (r(73) = 2 .297; p < .01) but not for females (r(74) = 2 .111), but the correlation was modest, accounting for less than 10% of the common variance. The mean scores for the male and female samples were compared using independent t tests (in all cases, d.f. = N1 + N2 2 2). There was no signicant difference in the means for males and females for FOD (t(315) = .52; n.s.), but the means for both SC and SI did differ signicantly between groups, with females scoring higher than males on SC (t(315) = 3.06; p < .05) and on SI (t(315) = 3.54; p < .001). The mean scores for the samples and the relevant t ratios are displayed in Table 2. Results Reliability Internal consistency. The internal consistency for all of the factors was highly satisfactory, with alpha coefcients of .879 for FOD, .771 for SC and .778 for SI. Angela Forbes and Derek Roger 170 Test retest reliability. Following an inter-test interval of 10 weeks, the questionnaire was sent out to 156 of the students from the University of York who could be contacted, 45 males and 111 females. Returns were received from 134 subjects (40 males and 94 females), representing 86% of the total. The retest coefcients for FOD (.846) and SC (.732) were highly satisfactory, although the coefcient for SI was somewhat more modest (.609). Separate sets of retest coefcients for males and females were comparable to those obtained for the overall sample. Interpersonal trust, emotion control, coping and social support To investigate the relationship of the ITQ to relevant measures of personality, coping and social support, the participants scores were correlated with the following indices: (a) The rehearsal (rumination) and emotional inhibition scales from the Emotion Control Questionnaire (ECQ; Roger & Najarian, 1989), each of which comprises 14 items. ECQ rehearsal measures the tendency to ruminate about emotional upset, and as the name implies, ECQ emotional inhibition assesses the tendency to inhibit the expression of emotion. (b) The detachment scale derived from the Coping Styles Questionnaire (CSQ; Roger, Jarvis & Najarian, 1993). The 22-item scale is bipolar, comprising detached and emotional coping, and is keyed for higher scores to reect a more detached coping style. (c) The Social Support Questionnaire (SSQ; Sarason et al., 1983), which is primarily a measure of perceived emotional support comprising 27 items. For each item, the participant is asked to state the number of people (up to a maximum of nine) who provide them with the type of support specied, and to give a summary rating of overall satisfaction with the support perceived to be available. There are thus two scores for each subject: SSQN (the sum of the number of people listed divided by the number of items), and SSQS (the mean satisfaction score). (d) The Inventory of Socially Supportive Behaviours (ISSB; Barrera, 1981). The ISSB consists of 40 items. It provides a measure of enacted or received support and assesses the amount of support received over the past month. The overall sample of 380 included in the validation studies were divided into two groups. The rst group of 150, comprising 42 males (mean age 20.69 years; SD 5.88) and 171 Stress, social support and fear of disclosure Table 2. Comparison of mean scores for males (N = 75) and females (N = 242) on the three- factor solution Gender Means SD d.f. t ratio Factor Fear of disclosure Male 33.88 8.39 315 .52 n.s. Female 34.68 11.70 Social coping Male 23.68 4.76 315 3.06** Female 25.61 4.75 Social intimacy Male 21.08 3.02 315 3.54*** Female 22.48 2.87 **p < .05; ***p < .001. 108 females (mean age 19.74 years; SD 3.02), completed the ITQ, the ECQ and the CSQ detachment scale. The second group of 230 participants included 107 males (mean age 20.06 years; SD 5.71) and 123 females (mean age 19.56 years; SD 5.42), and these participants all completed the ITQ and the ISSB. A subsample of this group, comprising 126 participants, 55 males (mean age 19.87 years; SD 4.20) and 71 females (mean age 19.83 years; SD 6.26), also completed the SSQ in addition to the ITQ and ISSB. The mean scores for the ITQ scales were almost identical to those obtained for the questionnaire construction sample reported in Table 2, with females again scoring higher on SC and SI than males. The means for the remaining scales were consistent with previously reported data, and as expected there was a trend for females to obtain lower scores on emotional inhibition and lower scores on detached coping. The correlations between the three ITQ scales and the ECQ, CSQ, SSQ and ISSB scales are shown in Table 3 below, for the total samples and for males and females separately. As had been anticipated, the correlations between the ITQ factors and the emotional inhibition scale from the ECQ were all signicant and consistent, with FOD correlating positively and both SC and SI correlating negatively. The pattern of correlations for the Angela Forbes and Derek Roger 172 Table 3. Correlations between the three ITQ factors and the ECQ, CSQ, detachment, SSQ and ISSB scales ITQ factors FOD SC SI ECQ and CSQ ECQ E-I Males (N = 42) .277 2 .434** 2 .190 Females (N = 108) .375*** 2 .421*** 2 .188 Total (N = 150) .345*** 2 .441*** 2 .222** ECQ R Males (N = 42) .351* 2 .081 2 .091 Females (N = 108) .161 .068 .029 Total (N = 150) .206* .046 .029 CSQ Det. Males (N = 42) 2 .557*** .054 2 .113 Females (N = 108) 2 .459*** .068 .029 Total (N = 150) 2 .436*** .048 2 .124 SSQ and ISSB SSQ N Males (N = 55) 2 .483*** .462*** .314* Females (N = 71) 2 .200 .074 .095 Total (N = 126) 2 .345*** .345*** .255** SSQ S Males (N = 55) 2 .527*** .326* .280* Females (N = 71) 2 .332* .014 .054 Total (N = 126) 2 .468*** .328*** .262** ISSB Males (N = 107) 2 .222* .342*** .225* Females (N = 123) 2 .268** .317*** .189* Total (N = 230) .253*** .369*** .231** Note. ITQ: FOD = fear of disclosure; SC = social coping; SI = social intimacy. ECQ/CSQ: E-I = emotional inhibition; R = rehearsal; Det. = detachment. overall sample was broadly similar for males and females separately, although the correlation between FOD and ECQ rehearsal (r(148) = .206; p < .05) for the total sample was clearly carried by the correlation for males. ECQ emotional inhibition correlated higher with FOD for females (r(106) = .375; p < .01) than for males (r(40) = .277; p < .05), although the difference between the coefcients was not signicant (z = 0.59; n.s.). The correlations between the ITQ and ISSB showed similar consistencies, but the correlations between the ITQ factors and the SSQ were stronger for males than for females. Indeed, only SSQS and FOD correlated signicantly for females (r(69) = 2 .332; p < .05), whereas all of the ITQ factors correlated signicantly with SSQN and SSQS for males. Interpersonal trust and network orientation In addition to the emotion control and social support scales, data were obtained from an independent sample of undergraduate students who completed only the ITQ and Vaux, Burda & Stewarts (1986) Network Orientation Scale (NOS), a 20-item questionnaire based on a 4-point Likert scale. As has been noted, the network orientation construct has been used to devise the more recent Network Utilization Scale (NUS; Gruen et al., 1994), but in view of the preliminary nature of the scale the NOS was chosen in preference for the present study. The sample compromised 178 participants, 102 females (mean age 19.36 years; SD 4.4) and 76 males (mean age 2.78 years; SD 6.7). The NOS and ITQ were included with a package of unrelated scales which were sent out by mail to members of a volunteer undergraduate participant panel at the University of York. Vaux (1985a) reported a three-factor structure for the NOS, but the analysis was based on a small sample of 80. The responses to the NOS in the present study were therefore initially subjected to factor analysis, and the scree plot suggested a unifactor or perhaps a two-factor solution rather than the three factors extracted by Vaux. A one-factor extraction included 18 of the 20 items, with only items 16 and 17 failing to load above the .30 criterion, and the factor accounted for 27.7% of the variance. However, the internal consistency was extremely low (coefcient alpha = .10), and the items were therefore rotated to a two-factor Varimax orthogonal terminal solution. This yielded factors with 10 and 9 items, respectively, with the previously omitted item 17 entering the second factor. The highest-loading items on the rst factor were 2 (Friends often have good advice to give) and 14 (It really helps when you are angry to tell a friend what has happened), and the two highest loadings on the second factor were 15 (Some things are too personal to talk to anyone about) and 18 (If you conde in other people, they take advantage of you). The two factors clearly reect a distinction between positive (factor 1) and negative or defensive attitudes (factor 2) about disclosure and conding, but there were four double-loading items of factor 1 and ve on factor 2. The number of double loads increased with an oblique rotation, and the internal consistency remained very modest for both factor 1 (coefcient alpha =.468) and factor 2 (coefcient alpha =.360). Despite these unsatisfactory ndings, the scores from the 18-item unifactor solution were correlated with scores from the three ITQ factors. The NOS correlated signicantly with FOD (r(176) = .398; p < .01) and with SC (r(176) = 2 .180; p < .05) but not with SI. Slightly higher correlations between the rst factor from the NOS and FOD and 173 Stress, social support and fear of disclosure between the second factor and SC conrmed the apparent correspondence between them, but in view of the psychometric inadequacies of the NOS, this was not explored further. Interpersonal trust and health status during adaptation As part of the larger programme of research to which this study is related, additional data were available for the sample of 126 participants who completed the ITQ, the SSQ and the ISSB. The additional data for this sample included measures of health status, and returns from repeated administrations were obtained from a subsample of 102, compris- ing 44 males (mean age 19.95 years; SD 4.38) and 52 females (mean age 19.19; SD 4.98). Health status was assessed over a period of adaptation, which was dened as the 4 months between entering university as rst-year undergraduates and the middle of the second term of their rst academic year. Previous studies have shown that this is a period of signicant demand for adaptation to a new social environment, and that deterioration in health status during this time is inuenced by a variety of individual difference variables (Roger, 1996). The students were contacted for the rst time immediately after entry to the University of York, and were asked to complete a health inventory entitled the Health Status Checklist (HSC; Meadows, 1989). The HSC is based on an extensive GP survey covering 30 common illnesses, and yields overall scores based on stability, deterioration or improvement across repeated administrations, thus allowing for the partialling out of initial health status. The HSC was rst administered at the start of the study to provide a baseline, and administered again after an interval of ve months. A difference score reecting relative deterioration in health status was calculated, using the initial administration as a baseline to take account of initial values. The participants scores on the ITQ, the ISSB and both subscales of the SSQ (SSQN and SSQS) were then entered into stepwise multiple regression analyses as independent variables, with deterioration in health status as the dependent variable. In view of the differential effects for gender in the correlations between the ITQ factors and the independent variables used in the concurrent validation exercise, the data for males and females were analysed separately. Results for females showed that scores on FOD contributed signicantly, with an adjusted R 2 of .106 and a standardized beta of .358 (t(1, 41) = 2.426; p < .02). The remaining independent variables were excluded in the analysis, and none were included in the stepwise regression for males. Combining the data for males and females in the present study yielded similar effects to those obtained for females. Discussion and conclusions The factor analysis of a preliminary item pool based on participants perceptions of social support produced a stable three-factor structure which included 48 items. The factors were labelled fear of disclosure (FOD), social coping (SC) and social intimacy (SI). All three were internally consistent and apart from SI were also stable over time. The retest reliability coefcient for SI fell below .70, but this factor may well be tapping attitudes towards social support or friendship rather than a stable predisposition to use support in a particular way, and may thus reect changes in friendship patterns over the inter-test Angela Forbes and Derek Roger 174 interval. The factors were moderately correlated, but were found to be empirically discriminable when compared with other personality scales. Comparisons between the mean scores for the male and female samples included in the study indicated signicant gender differences for the SC and SI scales, with females scoring signicantly higher than males. These factors primarily assess emotional support, and research has found that while females are more likely to use emotional support, males are more likely to use informational or tangible support (Ashton & Fuehrer, 1993). However, it has been suggested (Sarason, Shearin, Pierce & Sarason, 1987) that the difference in results for males and females is an artifact of the questionnaire items being biased towards a feminine stereotype. Previous studies have found that women are more likely to emphasize self-disclosure within a relationship, whereas within male relationships there is a tendency to inhibit self-disclosure (Lowenthal & Haven, 1968). The absence of signicant gender differences in the mean scores for the FOD factor in the present study was therefore surprising, but the results may have been affected by the student population which was used. Work currently in progress is attempting to discover whether there are systematic gender differences in the ITQ factors, using sex-role inventories across gender rather than relying on simple gender classication. The investigation of the questionnaires concurrent validity showed that FOD correlated signicantly positively with emotional inhibition and rehearsal from the ECQ. Fear of disclosure is associated with an inability or difculty in conding in others, particularly concerning matters of an emotional nature, and it is not surprising that there was a positive correlation with emotional inhibition, but in fact the coefcient accounts for little more than 12% of the common variance, indicating that the scales are not assessing identical constructs. Emotional inhibition also correlated inversely with SI and especially SC, which may be explained in part by a degree of item overlap between the emotional inhibition and SC scales. Neither SI nor SC correlated with ECQ rehearsal. All three factors correlated signicantly with SSQN, SSQS and ISSB for the overall sample. For the components of the SSQ, the highest correlation was between FOD and SSQS, which suggests that participants with a greater fear of disclosing emotionally upsetting information may be less satised with the amount of support they perceive to be available. This could be because they have fewer supportive others to draw upon, or there may be a third variable such as neuroticism which is affecting their responses. Interestingly, there were substantial gender differences in the separate correlation matrices computed for the SSQ, with only one signicant coefcient remaining for females (an inverse correlation between FOD and SSQS). The SSQN score is a simple measure of the frequency or number of supports available, and frequency measures have been shown to be less satisfactory indices of social support than the satisfaction with supports assessed by SSQS (Barrera, 1981). What the results suggest is that the quality of the support is more important for females than it is for males. These gender differences appear consistent with those reported by Ashton & Fuerher (1993), discussed earlier. They are also conrmed to some extent by the multiple regression analyses carried out as part of the present study, where deterioration in health status was signicantly associated with scores on the FOD component of the ITQ for females but not for males. The ability to make effective use of social support has been found to predict mental health outcomes (for example, Tolsdorf, 1976; Vaux, 1985b), and 175 Stress, social support and fear of disclosure the present ndings indicate that this may also be true for physical well-being, at least as far as females are concerned. There has been little agreement over the individual differences which might be involved in these effects, and these present study clearly implicates fear of disclosure as an important variable. Again, gender differences are highly signicant, and form the focus of current research at the University of York. The present study also offered the opportunity to explore further the Network Orientation Scale (NOS; Vaux, Burda & Stewart, 1986), which was developed as an index of individuals propensity to call upon social support. The ndings suggested a unifactor structure, but the internal consistency was very low. Extending the terminal solution to two factors yielded a structure which corresponded broadly to the FOD and SC factors from the ITQ, but the factors were psychometrically weak and the solution included a large number of double loadings. The single factor correlated signicantly positively with FOD and negatively with SC, but the correlations were modest, accounting for less than 16% of the common variance in the rst case and less than 4% in the second. In view of the psychometric inadequacies of the NOS and the conrmatory data from the validation of the ITQ, the latter scale represents a substantially better instrument for assessing the capacity to use social support. Research on social support has tended to focus on attitudes towards support rather than on individual differences which might moderate its effectiveness. Fear of intimacy had been mooted as a factor which might affect the ability to use social support (see Emmons & Colby, 1995), and the denition of support used for generating the item pool for the ITQ was based on the ability to conde in others. The results indicate that fear of disclosure is a key individual difference which needs to be taken into account, and the new scale offers the opportunity for developing more focused, interactive models for explaining the role of social support in moderating stress responses. References Ashton, W. A. & Fuehrer, A. (1993). Effects of gender and gender role identication of participant and type of social support resource on support seeking. Sex Roles, 28, 461476. Asterita, M. F. (1985). The physiology of stress. New York: Human Sciences Press. Barrera, M. (1981). Social support in the adjustment of pregnant adolescents: Assessment issues. In B. H. Gottlieb (Ed.), Social networks and social support, pp. 6996. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Barrera, M. (1986). Distinctions between social support concepts, measures, and models. American Journal of Community Psychology, 14, 413445. Berkman, L. F. (1985). The relationshipof social networks and social support to morbidity and mortality. In S. Cohen & S. L. Syme (Eds), Social support and health, pp. 241262. London: Academic Press. Broadbent, D. E., Broadbent, M. H. P., Phillpotts, R. J. &Wallace, J. (1984). Some further studies on the prediction of experimental colds in volunteers by psychological factors. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 28, 511523. Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number of factors. Multivariate Behavioural Research, 1, 245276. Cobb, S. (1976). Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 38, 300314. Cohen, S. & Hoberman, H. (1983). Positive events and social supports as buffers of life change stress. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 13, 99125. Cohen, S., Tyrell, D. A. J. & Smith, A. P. (1993). Negative life events, perceived stress, negative affect, and susceptibility to the common cold. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 131140. Cohen, S. & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310357. Denney, D. R. & Frisch, M. B. (1981). The relation of neuroticismin relation to life stress and illness. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 25, 303307. Angela Forbes and Derek Roger 176 Emmons, R. A. & Colby, P. M. (1995). Emotional conict and well-being: Relation to perceivedavailability, daily utilisation, and observer reports of social support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 947959. Endler, N. S. & Parker, J. D. A. (1990). Multidimensional assessment of coping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 844854. Fiore, J., Becker, J. & Coppel, D. (1983). Social network interactions: A buffer or a stress. American Journal of Community Psychology, 11, 423439. Folkman, S. & Lazarus, R. S. (1985). If it changes it must be a process: A study of emotion and coping during three stages of a college examination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 150170. Folkman, S., Schaefer, C. & Lazarus, R. S. (1979). Cognitive processes as mediators of stress and coping. In V. Hamilton & D. M. Warburton (Eds), Human Stress and Cognition: An information-processing approach, pp. 265298. London: Wiley. Gottlieb, B. H. (1985). Social support and the study of personal relationship. Journal of Social and Personal Relationship, 2, 351375. Gruen, R. J., Schuldberg, D., Nelson, E. A., Epstein, L., Weiss, L. & Quinlan, D. M. (1994). Network orientation and depressive symptomatology: Development of the Network Utilization Scale. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13, 352365. House, J. S. & Kahn, R. L. (1985). Measures and concepts of social support. In S. Cohen & S. L. Syme (Eds), Social support and health, pp. 83108. London: Academic Press. Jemmott, J. B. & Locke, S. E. (1984). Psychosocial factors, immunologic mediation, and human susceptibility to infectious diseases: How much do we know? Psychological Bulletin, 95, 78108. Kaiser, J., Hinton, J. W., Krohne, H. W., Stewart, R. & Burton, R. (1995). Coping dispositions and physiological recovery from a speech preparation stressor. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 111. Kobasa, S. (1979). Stressful life events, personality and health: An inquiry into hardiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 111. Kring, A. M., Smith, D. A. & Neale, J. M. (1994). Individual differences in dispositional expressiveness: Development and validationof the Emotional Expressivity Scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 934949. Lefcourt, H. M., Martin, R. A. & Saleh, W. E. (1984). Locus of control and social support: Interative moderators of stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 378389. Lowenthal, M. F. & Haven, C. (1968). Interaction and adaptation: Intimacy as a critical variable. American Sociological Review, 33, 2030. Meadows, M. (1989). Personality, stress and health. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of York, England. Mendolia, M. & Kleck, R. E. (1993). Effects of talking about a stressful event on arousal: Does what we talk about make a difference? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 283292. Nieland, M. & Roger, D. (1993). Emotion control and analgesia in labour. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 841843. Procidano, M. E. & Heller, K. (1983). Measures of perceived social support from friends and from family: Three validation studies. American Journal of Community Psychology, 11, 124. Roger, D. (1995). Emotion control coping strategies and adaptive behaviour. Stress and Emotion, 15, 255264. Roger, D. (1996). The role of cognitive rumination, coping styles and self-esteemin moderating adaptational responses to stress. Paper presented at the 8th European Conference on Personality, Ghent. Roger, D. & Jamieson, J. (1988). Individual differences in delayed heart-rate recovery following stress: The role of extraversion, neuroticismand emotional control. Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 721726. Roger, D., Jarvis, G. & Najarian, B. (1993). Detachment and coping: The construction and validation of a new scale for measuring coping strategies. Personality and Individual Differences, 15, 619626. Roger, D. & Najarian, B. (1989). The construction and validation of a new scale for measuring emotional control. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 845853. Roger, D. & Najarian, B. (1998). The relationship between emotional rumination and cortisol secretion under stress. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 531538. Roger, D., Najarian, B. & Nash, P. (1996). The interactive effects of self-esteem, cognitive rumination and coping styles in moderating the relationship between stress and illness. Paper presented to the Annual Conference of the British Psychological Society Special Group in Health Psychology, York. 177 Stress, social support and fear of disclosure Roger, D. & Nesshoever, W. (1987). The construction and preliminary validation of a scale for measuring emotion control. Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 527534. Sandler, I. N. &Barrera, M. (1984). Toward a multimethod approach to assessing the effects of social support. American Journal of Community Psychology, 12, 3752. Sarason, B. R., Sarason, I. G. & Pierce, G. R. (1990). Traditional views of social support and their impact on assessment. In B. R. Sarason, I. G. Sarason & G. R. Pierce (Eds), Social support: An interactional view, pp. 925. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Sarason, B. R., Shearin, E. N., Pierce, G. R. & Sarason, I. G. (1987). Interrelationshipamong social support measures: Theoretical and practical implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 813832. Sarason, I. G. (1981). Test anxiety, stress and social support. Journal of Personality, 49, 101114. Sarason, I. G., Levine, H. M., Basham, R. B. & Sarason, B. R. (1983). Assessing social support: The Social Support Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 127139. Sarason, I. G., Sarason, B. R. & Shearin, E. N. (1986). Social support as an individual difference variable: Its stability, origins, and relational aspects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 813832. Tardy, C. H. (1985). Social support measurement. American Journal of Community Psychology, 13, 187202. Thoits, P. (1985). Conceptual, methodological and theoretical problems in studying social support as a buffer against life stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 23, 145159. Tolsdorf, C. (1976). Social networks, support and coping: An exploratorystudy. Family Process, 15, 407417. Totman, R., Kiff, J., Reed, S. E. &Craig, J. W. (1980). Predictingexperimental colds in volunteers. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 24, 155163. Turner, R. J. (1981). Social support as a contingencyin psychological well-being. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 22, 357367. Vaux, A. (1985a). Factor structure of the Network Orientation Scale. Psychological Reports, 57, 11811182. Vaux, A. (1985b). Variations in social support associated with gender, ethnicity, and age. Journal of Social Issues, 41, 89110. Vaux, A., Burda, P. & Stewart, D. (1986). Orientation towards utilization of support resources. Journal of Community Psychology, 14, 159170. Received 4 November 1997; revised version received 9 September 1998 Appendix Items comprising the Interpersonal Trust Questionnaire 1. It is important to have somebody who I can talk things over with. 2. Sometimes I am unable to conde even in someone who is close to me. 3. I like to discuss even trivial problems to reassure myself that I am making sensible decisions. 4. I am not afraid to ask somebody for help. 5. A good friend is somebody that is understanding. 6. I am unable to trust anybody with my problems. 7. Nobody can understand what I am going through so why should I try and explain. 8. Talking things over helps to focus on the positive aspects of a situation. 9. Sometimes I want to talk things over with a friend but I just cannot. 10. To discuss my problems with somebody feels good at the time but afterwards I worry about what I have said. 11. Some people need somebody to conde in but I prefer to solve my own problems. 12. I regret having told somebody something that is personal. 13. To be able to give and take in a friendship is important. 14. As I get older I nd it continuously more difcult to conde in people. 15. It is important to me to have somebody that will listen to my problems. 16. I worry too much about what others think of me to conde in them. 17. It is easy to be friendly but difcult to make friends. 18. I feel better when I have talked to my friends about my problems. 19. There are some situations which I am unable to conde in anybody. 20. I like to feel that people can conde in me. Angela Forbes and Derek Roger 178 21. In the past I have been hurt by people breaking a condence. 22. When I feel upset about something I feel the need to talk to somebody about it. 23. I worry about what I have told people. 24. I am selective in who I conde in. 25. People will not be interested in my problems. 26. I like to talk problems over to get them off my chest. 27. People will not want to know me if they know what I am really like. 28. I am often given advice that I do not want. 29. I am afraid that if I conde in someone they will tell my problems to others. 30. I have somebody that I can turn to when I am in need of help. 31. People who I have thought were my friends have let me down in the past. 32. I have nobody that I feel that I can conde in. 33. A good friend is somebody that you can be honest with. 34. I do not want everybody to know what I am really like. 35. I prefer to conde in somebody who I feel knows me well. 36. I am afraid that people will laugh at me if I tell them my problems. 37. In the past I have found a problem easier to solve if I have talked it over with somebody. 38. If I trust someone it will only result in upset. 39. It is good to hear problems out loud. 40. Everybody seems so sure of themselves they will think that I am being foolish. 41. I nd it easy to trust people. 42. It is important to be there for someone if they need you. 43. I do not want to alienate people by showing them when I am upset. 44. It is good to have people who can give you encouragement. 45. I have friends who I know would help me but I nd it difcult to ask. 46. There are some things that I would be unable to cope with on my own. 47. I feel vulnerable if I have to ask other people for help. 48. It helps to discuss a problem even if it is impossible to reach a solution. 179 Stress, social support and fear of disclosure