This study investigated how symbolization (verbalization) during exposure to a model's behavior influences later reproduction of that behavior in children. Children were assigned to one of three conditions: facilitating symbolization by verbalizing the model's actions, passive observation, or competing symbolization by counting aloud. Half the children received incentives to learn and half did not. Those who verbalized the model's actions during exposure reproduced more of the behaviors later than passive observers, who outperformed those in the competing symbolization condition. Observational learning was not influenced by incentives. The study tested the idea that symbolization enhances learning by strengthening representational responses.
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0 ratings0% found this document useful (0 votes)
121 views
Observational Learning
This study investigated how symbolization (verbalization) during exposure to a model's behavior influences later reproduction of that behavior in children. Children were assigned to one of three conditions: facilitating symbolization by verbalizing the model's actions, passive observation, or competing symbolization by counting aloud. Half the children received incentives to learn and half did not. Those who verbalized the model's actions during exposure reproduced more of the behaviors later than passive observers, who outperformed those in the competing symbolization condition. Observational learning was not influenced by incentives. The study tested the idea that symbolization enhances learning by strengthening representational responses.
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 9
OIsevvalionaI Leavning as a Funclion oJ SnIoIizalion and Incenlive Sel
AulIov|s) AIIevl Banduva, Joan E. Ovusec and Fvances L. MenIove
Bevieved vovI|s) Souvce CIiId BeveIopnenl, VoI. 37, No. 3 |Sep., 1966), pp. 499-506 FuIIisIed I Wiley-Blackwell on IeIaIJ oJ lIe Society for Research in Child Development SlaIIe UBL http://www.jstor.org/stable/1126674 . Accessed 02/10/2012 0846 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. . Wiley-Blackwell and Society for Research in Child Development are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Child Development. http://www.jstor.org OBSERVATIONAL LEARNING AS A FUNCTION OF SYMBOLIZATION AND INCENTIVE SET ALBERT BANDURA, JOAN E. GRUSEC, and FRANCES L. MENLOVE Stanford University This study investigated the effects of symbolization on delayed repro- duction of modeling stimuli in a test of the contiguity-mediational theory of observational learning. During exposure to the behavior of a film-medi- ated model, 1 group of children engaged in concurrent verbalization, a second group observed passively, while a third group engaged in com- peting symbolization. Half of the children in each of the treatment condi- tions observed the model's behavior under a positive incentive set; the re- maining Ss were provided no incentive to learn the model's responses. Ss who generated verbal equivalents of the modeling stimuli during exposure subsequently reproduced more matching responses than the passive view- ers, who, in turn, showed a higher level of acquisition than children in the competing symbolization treatment. Observational learning, however, was not influenced by incentive set. Most conceptualizations of imitative or observational learning have been developed and tested largely on the basis of a limited paradigm re- quiring observing Ss to perform matching responses and to secure positive reinforcers as a precondition for their acquisition (Baer & Sherman, 1964; Miller & Dollard, 1941; Skinner, 1953). These theories, however, fail to ac- count for the occurrence of delayed reproduction of modeling behavior originally learned by observers under exposure conditions permitting no overt performance of matching responses (Bandura, 1962; 1965a; Bandura & Walters, 1963). Since observers can acquire only perceptual and other implicit responses resembling the. sequences of modeling stimuli while they are occurring, symbolic processes which mediate subsequent behavioral re- production must play a prominent role in observational learning. Recent theoretical analyses of observational learning (Bandura, 1965c; This work was supported by Public Health Service research grant M-5162 from the National Institute of Mental Health. Author's address: Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif. 94305. CHILD DEVELOPMENT Sheffield, 1961) emphasize the role of stimulus contiguity and associated cognitive or representational responses in the acquisition process. According to contiguity-mediational theory, during the period of exposure modeling stimuli elicit in observing Ss configurations and sequences of sensory ex- periences which, on the basis of past associations, become centrally inte- grated and structured into perceptual responses. There is some evidence from research in sensory conditioning (Conant, 1964; Leuba, 1940; Naruse & Abonai, 1953) to suggest that, in the course of observation, transitory sensory and perceptual phenomena can be con- verted to retrievable images of the corresponding stimulus events. In addi- tion to imaginal responses, the observer acquires, once verbal labels have become attached to objective stimuli, verbal equivalents of the model's be- havior during the period of exposure (Bandura, 1965b; Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963). The latter findings thus provide some basis for assuming that symbolic or representational responses in the form of images and verbal as- sociates of the model's behavior constitute the enduring learning products of observational experiences. It is likewise assumed in the contiguity-media- tional theory that symbolic matching responses possess cue-producing prop- erties that are capable of eliciting, some time after observation, overt re- sponses corresponding to those that were modeled, provided the requisite components exist in the observer's behavioral repertoire. In order to test the proposition advanced in the above theory that sym- bolization enhances observational learning, a modeling study was conducted in which an attempt was made to manipulate the relevant symbolic re- sponses. Viewers' verbalizations of modeling stimuli would be expected to influence mediating or representational response processes. Therefore, dur- ing the response-acquisition phase of the experiment, one group of children engaged in facilitative verbalization designed to enhance the development of imaginal and verbal associates of the model's behavior. A second group of children simply observed passively, while the third group of children as- signed to a competing symbolization condition produced interfering verbal responses intended to counteract the establishment of representational re- sponses. The degree of observational learning may also be partly governed by incentive-related sets which exert selective control over the direction, in- tensity, and frequency of observing responses. It is also entirely possible that different symbolization instructions could create in observers differential anticipations as to the possibility of their later being called upon to dem- onstrate what they had learned from the modeled stimulus presentation. Such self-induced sets, if operative, might affect attending behavior and thus confound the effects of symbolization processes. Hence, in the present study half of the Ss in each of the three observational treatments were of- fered positive incentives to learn the model's response patterns, while the remaining children were provided no incentives. 500 BANDURA, GRUSEC, AND MENLOVE It was predicted that the number of matching responses acquired ob- servationally by children in the facilitative symbolization condition would exceed the corresponding scores for the passive observers, who, in turn, would show a higher level of acquisition than Ss in the competing symboli- zation treatment. It was also hypothesized, for reasons given above, that in- centive set would enhance observational learning. METHOD Subjects The Ss were 36 boys and 36 girls ranging in age from 6 to 8 years. They were drawn from two elementary schools in a lower-middle-class com- munity. Design and Procedure The investigation utilized a 2 x 2 x 3 factorial design. Children of each sex were randomly assigned to either the facilitative symbolization, the passive observation, or the competing symbolization conditions. Half of the Ss in each of the latter experimental treatments participated in the in- centive-set condition, while the remaining children were assigned to the no-incentive-set group. Thus, there were six groups of six Ss each. The Ss in the facilitative symbolization group were instructed to ver- balize every action of the model as it was being performed in the movie. With children in the competing symbolization group, E explained that she was interested in determining whether children could pay close attention to a movie while engaging in another activity simultaneously. They were therefore to count "1 and a 2, and a 3, and a 4, and a 5" repeatedly, at the same time that they watched closely the filmed presentation. The Ss in the passive observation group were instructed simply to pay close attention to the movie. Children who were assigned to the incentive-set condition were also in- formed that following the movie they would be asked to demonstrate what they had learned and that they would receive candy treats for each item that they reproduced correctly. On the other hand, Ss in the no-incentive- set group were told that they would return to their classroom immediately following the movie. Following the instructional procedures, E turned on the movie projec- tor, which displayed the modeling stimuli on a glass lens screen in the tele- vision console. 501 CHILD DEVELOPMENT The movie consisted of a 4-minute color film in which an adult male model exhibited a series of relatively novel patterns of behavior and often utilized play materials in unusual ways. Since this study was concerned with issues of response acquisition, it was necessary to devise unusual response sequences that have virtually a zero probability of occurring spontaneously in preschool children and hence meet the criterion of novel responses. In the opening scene the model entered the room with his right hand cupped over his eyes. He then clasped his hands behind his back as he surveyed the various play materials. In the first action sequence the model built a tower with blocks ar- ranged in a unique manner and set a plastic juice dispenser, which was to serve as a target, on top of the tower. After picking up a dart gun and a baton, the model paced off a specified number of steps from the target, placed the baton on the floor to mark the range, got down on one knee, and fired the dart gun at the plastic container. The block tower was then dis- assembled in a distinctive way. In the next action sequence, which was di- rected toward a large Bobo doll, the model sat on the doll, punched it in the nose, pummeled it with a mallet, tossed rubber balls at it, lassoed it with a hoola hoop, and dragged it with the improvised lariat to a far corner of the room. The model then picked up two bean bags and paced off back- ward a designated number of steps from the target, which consisted of an upright board with three large holes. After reaching the starting point, the model tossed the bean bags between his legs with his back to the target. He then retrieved the bags, paced backward, squatted with his back to the target, and tossed a bean bag over each shoulder. The closing scene showed the model walking across the room whirling the hoola hoop over his right arm. Delayed imitative performance is determined not only by observational variables but also by rehearsal processes which typically improve retention. An incentive set may, therefore, influence the amount of behavioral repro- duction by both augmenting and channeling observing responses during acquisition, and actuating deliberate implicit rehearsal of matching re- sponses immediately after exposure. Since the present experiment was pri- marily concerned with issues of response acquisition, and the occurrence of differential anticipatory rehearsal would obscure results, children in all groups were assigned the task of counting out loud during the brief period between the end of the movie and reproduction. By this procedure, an at- tempt was made to hold interpolated activities constant for all groups and to prevent facilitative rehearsal of responses. Immediately after the comple- tion of the movie, E announced that they would now proceed to another room in the mobile laboratory, and that she was interested in seeing how high they could count while they walked to the other end of the trailer. Since the interpolated task was highly dissimilar to the modeled activities, it was not expected to reduce retention to any great extent. 502 BANDURA, GRUSEC, AND MENLOVE Test for Acquisition The test for acquisition was administered by a second female experi- menter in a room containing the stimulus items that were utilized by the model in the filmed performance. In order to control for any possible E in- fluences, the person who conducted this phase of the study did not know to which treatment conditions Ss had been assigned. The children were asked by E to demonstrate all of the model's re- sponses they could recall, and were praised and rewarded with candy for each matching response correctly reproduced. A standardized cuing pro- cedure was employed to insure the same order of reproduction for all Ss. They were asked to show the way in which the model behaved in the open- ing scene of the movie; to demonstrate what he did with the dart gun, the Bobo doll, and the bean bags; and to portray the model's behavior in the closing scene of the film. The children were thus given a fragmentary cue and asked to reproduce the entire response sequence in which the par- ticular stimulus object was employed. The E recorded the children's matching responses on a checklist con- taining the 38 responses that had been exhibited by the model. In order to provide an estimate of interscorer reliability, the performances of ten chil- dren were recorded simultaneously but independently by another rater who observed the test sessions through a one-way mirror from an adjoining ob- servation room. The two raters were in perfect agreement on 96 per cent of the specific matching responses that were scored. RESULTS Table 1 presents the mean number of matching responses achieved by children in the various treatment conditions. In each incentive condition, TABLE 1 MEAN NUMBER OF MATCHING RESPONSES REPRODUCED AS A FUNCTION OF SYMBOLIZATION AND INCENTIVE-SET CONDITIONS OBSERVATIONAL CONDITIONS Facilitative Passive Competing INCENTIVE Symbolization Observation Symbolization No Incentive Set: Boys........... 16.8 14.5 11.5 Girls........... 17.5 13.2 6.0 Total......... 17.2 13.8 8.7 Incentive Set: Boys........... 16.2 15.3 13.0 Girls.......... 14.8 11.7 9.8 Total......... 15.5 13.5 11.4 503 CHILD DEVELOPMENT for both boys and girls, the mean reproduction scores attained by the ac- tive symbolizers exceeds the corresponding means for the passive observers who, in turn, show a higher level of acquisition than Ss in the competing symbolization treatment. Analysis of variance of these data reveals that symbolization is a highly significant source of variance (F = 13.01; p < .001). Further comparisons of pairs of means by the t test indicate that Ss who generated verbal equivalents of the modeling stimuli during presenta- tion subsequently reproduced significantly more matching responses than children who either observed passively (t = 2.18; p < .025) or engaged in competing symbolizations (t = 5.12; p < .001). Moreover, the passive viewers acquired more of the model's repertoire of behavior than children who engaged in verbalizations designed to interfere with the development of representational responses (t = 2.94; p < .01). It is of interest that boys reproduced a significantly higher number of matching responses than girls (F = 5.70; p < .05). Contrary to prediction, however, observational learning was not influenced by incentive set nor were there any significant interaction effects. DISCUSSION Although the results of the present study provide confirmatory evi- dence for the facilitative role of symbolization in observational learning, alternative interpretations of these findings might be examined. It is con- ceivable that the method utilized for preventing the acquisition of repre- sentational responses may have interfered with observation of the modeling stimuli. Considering, however, that the modeling stimuli were projected on a large television screen directly in front of the S seated in a dark room, it is improbable that, under such conditions of highly focused attention, concur- rent competing verbalization could reduce appreciably the occurrence of observing responses. Indeed, the replies of children in the incentive-set condition to questions in the postexperiment interview indicate that they exerted strong efforts to observe and to retain the responses exhibited by the model ("While I was counting, I was paying attention to the thing. I thought back about it in my head ... I look at it all the time so I remember it all the time"). The marked external control of observing responses through the tele- vised mode of stimulus presentation in all likelihood accounts to some ex- tent for the absence of a significant incentive effect on the acquisition of matching behavior. As several of the children put it, "I didn't look all over the place. I just watched the movie and kept my eyes on it. . ... I kept watching. I couldn't take my eyes off it." In situations where a person is exposed to multiple models exhibiting diverse patterns of behavior, knowledge of the reinforcement contingencies 504 BANDURA, GRUSEC, AND MENLOVE associated with the corresponding response patterns and anticipation of positive or negative reinforcement for subsequent reproduction may exert selective control over the nature and frequency of attending responses. The effects of incentive set on observational learning would, therefore, be most clearly elucidated by a comparative study utilizing stimulus situations ranging from (a) highly focused observation of a single sequence of model- ing stimuli or (b) controlled exposure to multiple models requiring selective attentiveness to competing social cues presented simultaneously to (c) self- selection of frequency and duration of exposure to specific types of models. The latter condition, which corresponds most closely to observational learn- ing in naturalistic situations, would probably maximize the influence of re- inforcement-oriented set. The hypothesis concerning the influence of anticipated positive rein- forcement for matching responses on observational learning was based on presumed perceptual sensitizing effects of an incentive set induced prior to exposure. The fact, however, that Ss who expected to be subsequently tested and rewarded for their imitative behavior generally achieved slightly lower acquisition scores than their uninformed counterparts suggests that the benefits of incentive set may have been offset by detrimental effects. There were indications in the interview data, elicited by questions about the children's thoughts during the film-viewing, that the incentive-set in- structions generated achievement anxieties in some of the children. ("I was thinking what should I tell you and what should I do.... I was just think- ing, I hope I remember everything.") Observational learning could be ad- versely affected by implicit rehearsal of preceding events and disruptive thoughts concerning an impending test if these competing cognitive ac- tivities occur while the modeling stimuli are being presented at a relatively rapid rate. Any deleterious consequences should be greatest, as was the case, among the active symbolizers who were in the optimal film-viewing condition and, therefore, would most likely be hindered by the additional motivational effects of expectation of a performance test following the ex- posure session. The small but nevertheless significant sex differences in acquisition scores is probably due, in part, to the fact that the modeled responses in- volved physical masculine-typed activities. The girls' more adverse reaction to the incentive set may also have been a contributing factor. A single brief exposure to a continuous series of modeling stimuli is likely to produce some interference in the acquisition and correct sequenc- ing of novel matching responses. Dramatic intrusions from the various se- quential patterns were occasionally evident in Ss' behavioral reproductions, as in the case of the child who fired the dart gun at the Bobo doll, when, in fact, the plastic container served as the modeled target. Massed exposure conditions may thus result in the development of some erroneous modeling responses and the loss, through interference, of previously acquired ones. 505 CHILD DEVELOPMENT Simultaneous competing verbalizations during observation of modeling displays would not be expected to interfere too extensively with the de- velopment of visual imagery, particularly when the modeling stimuli are highly salient, as in the present experiment. It would, therefore, be of con- siderable theoretical significance to determine whether any matching re- sponses could be reproduced if, in addition to preventing the development of verbal associates, visual imaginal responses were likewise precluded, masked, or obliterated. Such imagery-interference procedures would pro- vide the most decisive evidence as to whether representational mediators are necessary for long-term retention and delayed behavioral reproduction of modeling stimuli. REFERENCES Baer, D. M., & Sherman, J. A. Reinforcement control of generalized imitation in young children. J. exp. child Psychol., 1964, 1, 37-49. Bandura, A. Social learning through imitation. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: 1962. Lincoln: Univer. of Nebraska Pr., 1962. Pp. 211-269. Bandura, A. Behavioral modifications through modeling procedures. In L. Krasner & L. P. Ullmann (Eds.), Research in behavior modification. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1965. Pp. 310-340. (a) Bandura, A. Influence of models' reinforcement contingencies on the acquisition of imitative responses. J. Pers. soc. Psychol., 1965, 1, 589-595. (b) Bandura, A. Vicarious processes: a case of no-trial learning. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology. Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press, 1965. Pp. 1-55. (c) Bandura, A., Ross, Dorothea, & Ross, Sheila A. Vicarious reinforcement and imi- tative learning. J. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1963, 67, 601-607. Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. Social learning and personality development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1963. Conant, M. B. Conditioned visual hallucinations. Unpublished manuscript, Stan- ford Univer., 1964. Leuba, C. Images as conditioned sensations. J. exp. Psychol., 1940, 26, 345-351. Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J. Social learning and imitation. New Haven, Conn.: Yale Univer. Pr., 1941. Naruse, G., & Abonai, T. Decomposition and fusion of mental images in the drowsy and posthypnotic hallucinatory state. J. clin. exp. Hypnosis, 1953, 1, 23-41. Sheffield, F. D. Theoretical considerations in the learning of complex sequential tasks from demonstration and practice. In A. A. Lumsdaine (Ed.), Student responses in programmed instructions: a symposium. Washington, D.C.: Na- tional Academy of Sciences, National Research Council, 1961. Pp. 13-52. Skinner, B. F. Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan, 1953. 506