TMP D823
TMP D823
TMP D823
1 INTRODUCTION
The global ‘information revolution’ is unequal in form and distribution. While the rapid
adoption of information and communication technologies (ICTs) among the world’s
*Correspondence to: L. Nielsen and C. Hefferanan, Livestock Development Group, School of Agriculture, Policy
and Development, University of Reading, PO Box 237, Earley Gate, Reading, RG6 6AL, UK.
E-mail: [email protected]; [email protected]
y
The views contained within this article, however, are the authors’ own.
Contract/grant sponsor: Department for International Development’s Livestock Production Program.
affluent has forged the so-called ‘knowledge society’, for the poor, access to information, in
such forms, remains limited. Within the literature, this disparity has been described as the
‘digital divide’ (World Bank, 1998; UNDP, 2001). Nonetheless, the discourse on this topic
has been widely framed at the national level with a focus on infrastructure and connectivity
rates. Hence, the focus has largely been on technical connectivity rather than social,
educational or cultural impacts.
Indeed, Mansell (2005) argues, that the attention on improving access and the
connectivity of ICTs in the South is misplaced:
The recent emphasis on information and communication technology ‘‘for develop-
ment’’ places too great a premium on technological innovation and technology
diffusion to establish access to global networks for people in developing countries. . .
Nevertheless, the author has largely overlooked the demand for this connectivity by
citizens of the South. Indeed, at the individual level, the demand for these technologies
by the poor has been at best, underestimated and at worst ignored (Heffernan and Nielsen,
2006). Hence, it is no longer the ‘digital divide’ that should frame the debate on ICTs and
poverty but rather why, given this demand, the diffusion of ICTs among nations and
communities has differed so widely.
To explore the ‘adoption divide’ however, the impact of such tools on the users
themselves, must be understood. At present, there is a dearth of research which explores
how the design and development of these tools affect learning outcomes and knowledge
acquisition among the poor. Indeed, current applications have been mainly modelled after
the visual and narrative frames of 2-D web applications. Consequently, programmes are
often static, with low interaction levels. Little research, however, has been conducted
exploring issues in the content and design of ICTs within a development context
(Heffernan and Nielsen, 2005).
Therefore, the objectives of this article are two; first, to explore the design elements of an
interactive, multi-media learning programme (‘El Promotor’) developed for poor livestock
keepers of the Altiplano. The second objective is to examine the relationship between new
and existing knowledge regarding livestock health and production among users.
First, however, a brief review of the potential role of ICTs on learning is offered.
In recent years, computer and web-based ICTs have been heralded as key tools to
improving the quality of learning (Mansell and Wehn, 1998). First, ICTs can provide
information on demand to suit an individual’s specific learning needs. Second, users can
control the pace of learning and review information until the required level of familiarity
has been obtained.
Within the development literature, the link between ICTs and poverty has been well
established (Flor, 2001). Indeed, countries with high levels of ICT indicators (internet
hosts/1000 persons; telephone lines/1000 persons; TV and PC ownership) generally rank
high in the UN’s Human Development Index (ibid). Further arguments are offered at the
systems level, as McNamara (2003) notes:
. . . [ICTs] enable desired changes in the performance of institutions, markets, in the
livelihoods of poor people and the vulnerabilities they face, in the capacity of
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Int. Dev. 18, 889–900 (2006)
DOI: 10.1002/jid
The Impact of ICTs on Learning 891
individuals and governments since it is these changes, not ICTs that lead to poverty
reduction and sustainable development. These outcomes—fewer people in poverty,
more vibrant developing country economies, more responsive government
institutions, reduced disease and illiteracy, greater gender equality—are the
ultimate measure of the value and impact of ICTs in development.
Thus, the author argues that it is the underlying impacts on human capacity and
communication where ICTs will have the largest impact on poverty. Nevertheless,
there have been some dissenting views regarding this point. Indeed, it has been argued
that if human capacity is not enhanced to accommodate these technologies, inequalities
will increase rather than decrease (Sciadas, 2003). Both arguments while valid,
skirt a bigger issue. It is ultimately the ability of these tools to connect people and places
that will be the largest force for poverty alleviation. And underestimating the capacity of
the poor to utilise and embrace these tools may be the largest barrier to poverty
alleviation.
At the individual level, the literature begins to explore this notion of connectivity. There
are two primary arguments utilised to explain the disappointing impact of ICTs on the poor
themselves. First, as noted above, access by the poor to these forms of communication is
low and second, where the infrastructure is available, the actual software produced is often
inappropriate to the learning needs of the poor (Lloyd-Laney, 2003; Sciadas, 2003;
Soeftestad and Sein, 2003). Nonetheless, with regard to the lack of access, the arguments
are already being challenged. Indeed, in some southern countries network development is
growing at a faster rate than in the North (Fink and Kenny, 2003). As such, in some cases
access to new technologies in the South is bypassing the North (Munya, 2000; Maxfield,
2004).
However, while at the macro-level gains are being made, at the individual level, the poor
design of many ICTs still hampers uptake and adoption rates. A number of reasons have
been offered to explain the lack of uptake. For example, Soeftestad and Sein (2003) argue
that conceptualisation of many ICTs is fundamentally flawed and have led to ill-formulated
projects and programmes:
. . .ICTs come out of a western intellectual and scientific tradition and cannot be
applied as is to non-western settings.
Lloyd-Laney (2003) further remarks:
The materials that are available and accessible are not appropriate to meet their [the
poors’] needs, either in their content (i.e. doesn’t reflect their reality) or packaging
(language, presentation of information etc.). . . Information and knowledge is often
disseminated without understanding the needs of the users, or the contexts in which
they can access and adopt the information. . .
Therefore, it is clear, that ultimately the impact of ICTs will depend upon the relevance
of content and the appropriateness of the design to the individual user.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Int. Dev. 18, 889–900 (2006)
DOI: 10.1002/jid
892 L. Nielsen and C. Heffernan
Altiplano. Therefore, the target population included resource poor farmers with few or
no years of formal education. Equally, the programme needed to be a stand alone and
accommodate users with no previous exposure to computers and/or computer-based
learning.
The software design was informed by the Information Processing (IP) theory of learning
developed by cognitive psychologists (Reiber, 2000). The IP theory relates learning to the
appropriateness of the media to the particular learning style of the individual involved; and
the relevance of the content to the learner (Clark and Sugrue, 1995). Hence, the IP model
identifies how the learner interacts with information to produce knowledge (Reiber, 2000).
Within this theory, uptake is related to the ability of the media to optimise the learners’
attention in order for the material to be committed to long term memory (Salomon, 1979;
Howe, 1980; Jonassen and Grabowski, 1993; Reiber, 2000).
Therefore, to devise the initial content, household interviews were performed with over
800 farmers across three departments in the Altiplano (La Paz, Oruro and Potosi) (LDG,
2003). A number of techniques, both participatory and more quantitative were utilised to
elicit the animal health priorities of the poor. As such, the content regarding animal health
and production was derived from the information demands of the poor, as prioritised by
themselves. The notion was that by ascertaining the priorities of the poor with regard to the
diseases and production problems that they faced, the programme would be directly
relevant to the concerns of users. Historically, within the livestock sector, the low uptake of
technologies and extension material has been attributed to the material meeting the
concerns of decision-makers and not the farmers themselves (LID, 1998). Appropriate
content, however, is only part of the overall design issues relating to ICTs; the tools also
have to suit the learning styles of users. Therefore, the interface was designed based upon
the visual and oral didactic of the Altiplano.
Prior to building the programme, visual referents were assessed for preferences and
suitability. As such, a range of pictorial styles were initially tested among a sub-group of
the overall study. In total, 40 households participated in the visual testing portion of the
research. The results indicated a strong preference for more realistic and less stylised
images. Indeed, informants noted that cartoon images were suitable only for children and
could potentially cause offence. Equally, the majority of participants preferred images
which depicted obese animals and people. When asked to explain their choices, many
individuals equated bodyweight with well-being and subsequently, the happiness of the
animal or person involved.
Further, the study explored the specific visual characteristics associated with both
knowledge of livestock keeping and trustworthiness. As such, a variety of El Promotor
characters were tested for suitability with regard to these key criteria. A depiction was
chosen which best represented these features as decided by the majority of study
participants.
Language was also a consideration. Study participants asked that the introduction to the
programme be offered in their native language (Aymara or Quechua) and that technical
information was provided in Spanish. Reasons offered for this duality were as follows.
Study participants felt that knowing the names of particular diseases and potential
treatments in Spanish was beneficial if there was the need to approach formal services
providers.
The programme can be run on a normal PC and require no extra hardware or software
requirements. At present, El Promotor has been distributed via milk co-operatives, the
Government and NGOs in a project area of over 5000 households.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Int. Dev. 18, 889–900 (2006)
DOI: 10.1002/jid
The Impact of ICTs on Learning 893
The programme was built to accommodate user demands for specific information. Hence,
the software enables users to choose the species and topics of interest. For each species a
menu regarding animal health, feeding and housing was offered. After choosing a broad
subject matter, users could then focus on the specific area of interest. The software utilised
visual and aural cues, thereby no level of literacy was required.
While the programme did utilise animated images, at present, evidence relating to the
impact and uptake of animated versus static images is unclear (Schnotz, 2002; Lewalter,
2003). For example, Reiber (2000) noted that:
Animation makes the cognitive task more concrete and spontaneous by providing the
motion and trajectory attributes directly to the learner.
While Schnotz (2002) suggested that animated pictures can have both beneficial and
inhibiting functions in the process of learning. Conversely, Lewalter (2003) concluded
there was ‘a lack of superiority of dynamic visuals on learning outcome when compared
with static visuals’. To account for these issues, animation was used occasionally to either
enhance meaning or reduce the level of abstraction via emphasis on a particular visual or
demonstration of procedural instructions. As such, animated scenes tended to focus on
treatment descriptions or specific desirable husbandry or management changes.
Nonetheless, the layout and design also had to accommodate traditional frames for
learning. Learning, within the Andean communities has been related to nurturing both in
relation to the environment and with regard to social relationships (Genge, 2001). Indeed,
as the author notes:
. . .within Andean cosmology, knowledge is not at the margins of a nurturing process,
as much as it is nurturing itself. The space where knowledge is expressed is the
chacra, the centre of the Andean universe. Chacra is a space of ‘‘growing plants,
raising animals, and making a cultivated field’’.
Thus, knowledge within Andean society is embedded both within social relationships and
the mother earth or Pachimama (Rist et al., 1999). Therefore, to support such learning,
illustrations had strong referents to the local environment and customary learning. For
example, illustrations included well-known local geographic features as background features.
Depictions of livestock keeping were placed within scenes with community members of
different ages and genders. Finally, the El Promotor was portrayed as a ’teacher’ outside of the
scene and was also represented within the frame interacting with community members. Thus,
the programme utilised both denotative and connotative elements to support El Promotor as a
nurturer within chacra, or the learning didactic of the Andes.
2 METHODS
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Int. Dev. 18, 889–900 (2006)
DOI: 10.1002/jid
894 L. Nielsen and C. Heffernan
Mastitis 16
Liver Fluke 53
Foot and Mouth Disease (FMD) 26
Total 85
As such, a full score was given to an answer which was totally correct, a þ 1 was
accorded responses which while ostensibly correct were sufficiently general to be
applicable to a number of diseases such as ‘weakness’ or ‘fever’. Wrong answers were
accorded 1, while 2 was applied to responses with likely negative consequences for the
animal involved. Indeed, there are many traditional treatments that can be categorised in
this manner such as washing powder up the nostrils or stomach burning for intestinal
parasites etc. (Heffernan and Nielsen, 2005). In order not to create a ‘western scientific
bias’, traditional treatments that were beneficial or helpful also derived full scores.
The following formula was utilised to give participants a single score for each
assessment category before and after exposure to El Promotor in each of the disease
modules:
X
Knowledge Score ¼ ððAC 2Þ þ ðGE 1ÞÞ ððER 1Þ ðED 2ÞÞ (1)
As the numbers of responses in each assessment category could clearly differ (i.e. while
there are many correct symptoms of a disease there may be only one correct treatment), the
data was normalised to support comparisons. Hence, the sum for each category was divided
by the total number of scores possible providing a figure between 1 and 1.
By comparing knowledge scores prior to and after use of the programme, any changes
were attributed to learning. The final learning scores were derived using the following
formula:
The three diseases chosen did not have specific associations with positive traditional or
customary knowledge. Rather the identification and treatment of two of the diseases
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Int. Dev. 18, 889–900 (2006)
DOI: 10.1002/jid
The Impact of ICTs on Learning 895
(mastitis and liver fluke) had clearly identifiable farmer behaviours with specific
consequences such as the lack of hygiene for mastitis or grazing in swampy areas for liver
fluke. Conversely, FMD, a highly contagious epidemic disease had none of these qualities
thereby enabling an alternate perspective.
3 RESULTS
Table 2 offers the results of three learning modules on mastitis, liver fluke and FMD .
The mean learning scores suggest that learning had occurred across all three disease
modules. Indeed, a significant difference (p < 0.001, LSD p < 0.05) was found between
pre-assessment and post-assessment scores listed above.
However, within learning modules knowledge uptake differed depending upon the topic
area i.e. disease causation, symptoms, treatment or prevention as viewed in Table 3.
Overall, the topic had a significant effect on learning in all the disease modules (Mastitis
and FMD (p < 0.001); Liver fluke (p ¼ 0.043).
Indeed, learning on the mastitis module was highest with regard to prevention and lowest
with regard to causation. Interestingly, the opposite is true for the Foot and Mouth disease.
The findings may be explained by the type of previous knowledge held by participants. Foot
and Mouth Disease is a government priority in Bolivia and while not a large problem on the
Altiplano, the government organises annual vaccination campaigns. The campaigns are well
advertised with numerous radio and TV advertisements. Consequently, it is not surprising
that vaccination was recognised as a prevention strategy. Conversely, the correct cause of
Foot and Mouth Disease was less well known and thereby, a large amount of new knowledge
could be gained. The majority of respondents related the disease to poor weather conditions,
the ingestion of low quality food, as well as, an unhygienic environment.
To explore the impact of prior knowledge on learning, prior knowledge scores were correlated
with average learning scores. The resulting correlation coefficients are shown in Table 4.
Learning scores by topic Mastitis (n ¼ 16) Liver fluke (n ¼ 53) FMD (n ¼ 26)
Cause 0.24 0.44 0.46
Symptom 0.41 0.26 0.30
Treatment 0.31 0.28 0.23
Prevention 0.51 0.32 0.25
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Int. Dev. 18, 889–900 (2006)
DOI: 10.1002/jid
896 L. Nielsen and C. Heffernan
Topic of disease module Mastitis (n ¼ 16) Liver fluke (n ¼ 53) FMD (n ¼ 26)
Not surprisingly, the distribution of scores suggests that those participants with lower
pre-assessment scores tended to achieve higher learning scores, and vice versa. However,
when the responses were examined in more detail, a number of interesting findings were
apparent. Study participants with a significant amount of prior knowledge or the ‘high
scorers’, tended to offer less detailed responses. Nevertheless, rather than provide
previously known information, after exposure to the programme, these individual’s
responses tended to focus on the new material. Therefore, those individuals with a high
level of previous knowledge appeared to be prioritising information derived from the
programme (Table 5).
As the table illustrates, while all of the initial descriptions were correct with regard to the
symptoms of mastitis, the responses offered post-exposure were more closely aligned to
those offered in the software programme.
Nevertheless, there were other cases where key messages learned from the programme
augmented the initial correct responses (Table 6).
As the table illustrates, with regard to FMD, as noted above, many individuals knew that
vaccination prevented the disease. However, after use of the programme, users were able to
substantially augment their prior knowledge and offer other actions to control the spread of
the disease.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Int. Dev. 18, 889–900 (2006)
DOI: 10.1002/jid
The Impact of ICTs on Learning 897
Cold weather Contagious (from other cows), airborne disease, contaminated grass/
food/water
Dirty housing/environment Contaminated water, contagious from other cows
Dry/rough food Virus, contagious (from other cows)
Eating bad grass Contaminated grass/food/water
Espigas (barley kernels) Espigas (barley kernels), contagious
Hot weather Contaminated grass/food
Hot weather/cold weather Hot weather
Lack of food/water Contaminated water
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Int. Dev. 18, 889–900 (2006)
DOI: 10.1002/jid
898 L. Nielsen and C. Heffernan
Interestingly, no such conflicts between existing knowledge and new knowledge were
observed with regard to the causation of Mastitis (Table 9).
Indeed, the relationship between a dirty environment and mastitis, while not previously
known, was widely accepted. For mastitis, the new knowledge corresponded to more
general beliefs regarding the relationship between a dirty environment and ill health and as
such, went largely unchallenged.
4 CONCLUSIONS
The study demonstrates that ICTs can enhance the connectivity of the poor with the wider
society in which they reside through exposure to different knowledge frames. Equally, such
tools can provide decision-makers with a clear and unbiased view of the demands of the
poor for information and knowledge.
With regard to the learning assessment, on average, post assessment scores were
significantly higher than pre-assessment scores, which suggest that there was knowledge
uptake. When the effect of prior knowledge on uptake was explored, three levels of
knowledge acquisition could be ascertained. First, it was clear that while those with little
previous knowledge could obtain a more accurate understanding of the disease in question,
those with a greater initial understanding were able to prioritise the most important aspects
of the disease after training. Finally, those who offered erroneous information prior to
training often replaced their old knowledge frame with new and more accurate information.
The study also explored the interface between customary and ‘scientific knowledge’. As
the results demonstrated, on the Altiplano, animal health is intricately connected to the
environment in general and husbandry conditions more specifically. Indeed, cold weather
and contaminated feed were linked to the causation of all of the above diseases both before
and after exposure to the programme. Hence, the study demonstrates that the use of ICTs
such as El Promotor will not instantly replace existing knowledge frames which are
embedded in social structures, but rather should work within these contexts to enhance the
connectivity of the poor to wider knowledge pools. This is not to advocate, however, the
perpetuation of unhelpful or erroneous customary beliefs. Indeed, the poor have the right to
correct knowledge and ICTs that both support customary learning didactics and visual
learning can greatly aid the poor in joining the global knowledge society.
REFERENCES
Clark RE, Sugrue BM. 1995. Research on instructional media, 1978-1988. In Instructional
Technology: Past Present and Future, AnglinG (ed.). Libraries Unlimited: Englewood CO;
348–364.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Int. Dev. 18, 889–900 (2006)
DOI: 10.1002/jid
The Impact of ICTs on Learning 899
Flor A. 2001. ICT and Poverty: The Indisputable Link. World Bank Publications: Washington DC.
Fink C, Kenny C. 2003. W(h)ither the Digital Divide? Development Gateway, World Bank
Publications: Washington DC.
Genge CD. 2001. Nurturing: an alternative learning cosmology. In Unfolding Learning Societies:
Deepening the Dialogues, Jain M, Miller V, Jain S (eds). Vimukt Shiksha, Special Issue, April
2001. http://www.swaraj.org/shikshantar/ls2.pdf [May 2005].
Heffernan C, Nielsen L. 2006. The Livestock Guru: demand-led knowledge transfer for poverty
alleviation. In Proceedings of an International Conference on ICTs and Development, 25–26th
May, University of California: Berkeley CA.
Heffernan C, Nielsen L. 2005. The Livestock Guru: transmitting demand-led information to decision-
makers and the poor. In Proceedings of an International Workshop on Making a Difference:
Improving the livelihood of resource poor small livestock keepers, 12–16th September, DFID
Livestock Production Program, Howick.
Howe MJA. 1980. The Psychology of Human Learning. Harper & Row: New York NY.
Jonassen DH, Grabowski BL. 1993. Handbook of Individual Differences, Learning and Instruction.
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: London.
Livestock In Development 1998. Strategies for Improving DFIDs Impact on Poverty Reduction: A
review of best practice in the livestock sector. Livestock In Development: Somerset.
Livestock Development Group. 2003. Livestock Services and the Poor. Report produced for the
Global Initiative on Livestock Services and the Poor. Livestock Development Group, The
University of Reading, Reading.
Lewalter D. 2003. Cognitive strategies for learning from static and dynamic visuals. Learning &
Instruction 13: 177–189.
Lloyd-Laney M. 2003. Making Knowledge Networks Work for the Poor: Final report, Practical
Action (Intermediate Technology Development Group). ITDG: Warwickshire.
Maxfield A. 2004. Health Communications Insights: Information communications technologies for
the developing world. Health Communication Partnership, John Hopkins Bloomberg School of
Public Health, Centre for Communications Programs: Baltimore MD.
Mansell R. 2005. Networks and partnerships, processes and prospects. In Connecting People and
Places: Challenges and Opportunities for Development. Annual Conference of the Development
Studies Association, 7–9th September, Open University, Milton Keynes.
Mansell R, Wehn U. 1998. Knowledge Societies: Information Technology for Sustainable Devel-
opment. Science Policy Research Unit (SPRU), United Nations Commission for Science and
Technology for Development, Oxford University Press: Oxford.
McNamara SK. 2003. Information and Communication Technologies, Poverty and Development:
Learning from Experience. InfoDev Annual Symposium, December 9–10th, Geneva.
Munya H. 2000. Information and Communication Technologies for Rural Development and Food
Security: Lessons from field experiences in developing countries. SD Dimensions, Sustainable
Development Department, Food and Agricultural Organisation of the United Nations (FAO),
Rome.
Reiber LP. 2000. Computers, Graphics & Learning. www.csulb.edu/llarson/523/cglbook.pdf [May
2005].
Rist S, San Martin J, Tapia N. 1999. Andean cosmovision and self-sustained development. In Food
for Thought: Ancient Visions And new Experiments of Rural People, Haverkort B, Hiemstra W
(eds). Zed Books: London.
Salomon G. 1979. Interaction of Media, Cognition, and Learning: An Exploration of How Symbolic
forms Cultivate Mental Skills and Affect Knowledge Acquisition. Jossey-Bass Publishers: London.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Int. Dev. 18, 889–900 (2006)
DOI: 10.1002/jid
900 L. Nielsen and C. Heffernan
Schnotz W. 2002. Enabling, facilitating, and inhibiting effects in learning from animated pictures. In
Proceedings of the International Workshop on Dynamic Visualizations and Learning, Ploetzner R
(ed.). Knowledge Media Research Center: Tübingen.
Sciadas G. 2003. Monitoring the Digital Divide and Beyond. InfoDev and Orbicom, Canada Institute
for Scientific and Technical Information, NRC Press: Ottawa.
Soeftestad LT, Sein MK. 2003. ICT and Development: East is East and West is West and the Twain
May yet meet. Community-Based Natural Resource Management Network. http://www.cbnrm.net
[May 2005].
UNDP. 2001. Human Development Report 2001: Making new Technologies Work for Human
Development. United Nations Development Programme (UNDP), Oxford University Press:
New York NY.
World Bank. 1998. World Development Report 1998/99: Knowledge for Development. World Bank:
Washington DC.
Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Int. Dev. 18, 889–900 (2006)
DOI: 10.1002/jid