Optical Illusions
Optical Illusions
Optical Illusions
Optical Illusions
Print version (PDF) Read first! 1. Collection of Optical Illusions
q q q q
Read first!
"The Blue Jay, as we clearly see, Is so much like the green Bay tree That one might say the only clue Lies in their dif-fer-ence of hue, And if you have a color sense, You'll see at once this difference." ---- R. W. Wood 1917
q q
q q
Phenomena of Contrast Gestalt Laws Geometrical-Optical Illusions New Geometrical-Optical Illusions Perspective, Constancy of Size Subjective Contours, Foreground - Background Perceptual Conflicts Impossible Figures and More Motion Perception Pinwheel Want to Get Hypnotized?
Image and text taken from S. Coren, C. Porac & L. M. Ward, Sensation and Perception.
2. Introduction to Perception
q q q q q q q
Many of us have at least once come across optical illusions in our lifetime. Beside being fun to solve, optical illusions have the power to puzzle and bewilder because we have an implicit trust that what our senses tell us is physical reality. Illusions are a nice window into how the brain works. "The mind sees and the mind hears. The rest is blind and deaf." said Epicharmus 450 years before the birth of Christ. It is surprising to find out that the eye cannot see, it merely perceives information which is passed down to the brain where innumerable processes of classification, comparison, and decision-making are initiated. In this section we have gathered many instances where the senses (the brain?) tell lies. You can find them grouped for your easier access in the menu on the left side. Probably you have seen some of the listed optical illusions before, such as the "classical" - the geometrical-optical - illusions, since they have been known a fairly long time. Beside the collection of optical illusions you can find a brief introduction to perception containing some explanations for these puzzling effects. Compiled by Ramona Winkler, edited and expanded by Gerd Waloszek (SAP User Experience) Note on the Source of Images The images in this online book come from different sources:
q
Definition of Perception Perceptual Organization Perceptual Constancy Context Effects Depth Perception Motion Perception Individual Differences in Perceiving
3. References
q q q q
Readings: Authors A-D Readings: Authors E-K Readings: Authors L-Z Links
top
q
Some of them can be found on many Websites and seem to be treated as if they were in the public domain; typically, we added "Source: unknown" to these images Other images are taken from books, such as Roger Shepard's book Mind Sights: Original Visual Illusions, Ambiguities, and Other Anomalies. Of course, the copyright belongs to the respective authors. The remainder of the images has been redrawn by us according to originals found in journals or textbooks; these images are not in the public domain but you may ask us for permission to use them.
In all cases, where the images were not redrawn by us, we added the respective source information. Status q Version 1.3.3 (May 2007): Note on the source of images added q Version 1.3.2 (May 2006): High-resolution versions of the Liar and Triangle illusions added q Version 1.3.1 (April 2006): Minor technical and typographical updates q Version 1.3: New induced motion images added q Version 1.2: Comments from Walter H. Ehrenstein jr, University of Dortmund, added; a couple of minor changes made
This paper can be found in Resources on the SAP Design Guild Website (www.sapdesignguild.org).
Read first!
Read first!
"The Blue Jay, as we clearly see, Is so much like the green Bay tree That one might say the only clue Lies in their dif-fer-ence of hue, And if you have a color sense, You'll see at once this difference." ---- R. W. Wood 1917
Image and text taken from S. Coren, C. Porac & L. M. Ward, Sensation and Perception.
Many of us have at least once come across optical illusions in our lifetime. Beside being fun to solve, optical illusions have the power to puzzle and bewilder because we have an implicit trust that what our senses tell us is physical reality. Illusions are a nice window into how the brain works. "The mind sees and the mind hears. The rest is blind and deaf." said Epicharmus 450 years before the birth of Christ. It is surprising to find out that the eye cannot see, it merely perceives information which is passed down to the brain where innumerable processes of classification, comparison, and decision-making are initiated. In this section we have gathered many instances where the senses (the brain?) tell lies. You can find them grouped for your easier access in the menu on the left side. Probably you have seen some of the listed optical illusions before, such as the "classical" - the geometrical-optical - illusions, since they have been known a fairly long time. Beside the collection of optical illusions you can find a brief introduction to perception containing some explanations for these puzzling effects. Compiled by Ramona Winkler, edited and expanded by Gerd Waloszek (SAP User Experience) Note on the Source of Images The images in this online book come from different sources:
q
Some of them can be found on many Websites and seem to be treated as if they were in the public domain; typically, we added "Source: unknown" to these images Other images are taken from books, such as Roger Shepard's book Mind Sights: Original Visual Illusions, Ambiguities, and Other Anomalies. Of course, the copyright belongs to the respective authors.
Read first!
q
The remainder of the images has been redrawn by us according to originals found in journals or textbooks; these images are not in the public domain but you may ask us for permission to use them.
In all cases, where the images were not redrawn by us, we added the respective source information. Status q Version 1.3.3 (May 2007): Note on the source of images added q Version 1.3.2 (May 2006): High-resolution versions of the Liar and Triangle illusions added q Version 1.3.1 (April 2006): Minor technical and typographical updates q Version 1.3: New induced motion images added q Version 1.2: Comments from Walter H. Ehrenstein jr, University of Dortmund, added; a couple of minor changes made
This paper can be found in Resources on the SAP Design Guild Website (www.sapdesignguild.org).
top
Source: Optical Illusions
Phenomena of Contrast
Phenomena of Contrast
Hermann Grid Illusion | New Version | Springer - Subjective Diagonals | Simultaneous Contrast | Checker-Shadow Illusion | Koffka Ring | Ehrenstein's Pattern | Bezold Effect: Color Assimilation For an explanation of many of the contrast phenomena, see Context Effects.
While scanning over the left matrix you probably see gray blobs in the intersections of the black crosses formed by the white squares (vice versa for the right matrix). These blobs can be explained by reference to receptive fields and lateral inhibition. Note: Concerning the Hermann grid, its history and follow-up, see see Bernd Lingelbach and Walter Ehrenstein's recent review (in German) in: www.leinroden.de/304herfold.htm
Phenomena of Contrast
Simultaneous Contrast
Phenomena of Contrast
Place your mouse pointer over the image in order to envision the illusion!
The small squares within the larger ones are all exactly the same color gray (just move your mouse over the image and convince yourself). As you can see, the lightness of the background effects the way in which we perceive the lightness of the small squares: the very left small square seems darker than the very right one. The perception of this kind of display is a result of lateral interactions.
Checker-Shadow Illusion
In this illusion from E. H. Adelson called checker-shadow illusion, the squares marked A and B are the
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/contrast_phenomena.html (3 von 5)08.05.2007 10:28:30
Phenomena of Contrast
same shade of gray (we checked that in an image processing program). Like in the simultaneous contrast example above, the environment of a square determines its perceived lightness.
Koffka Ring
Place your mouse pointer over the image in order to envision the illusion!
The gray ring on the black and gray or black and white ground appears to be uniformly gray. However, if you divide the ring into two parts, the gray color appears to be different depending on the color of the ground.
Ehrenstein's Pattern
In the first image the non-existing circles appear to be very bright. In the next image they seem to be very dark.
Phenomena of Contrast
The blue or yellow areas adjacent to the green triangles (on the left side) influence the appearance of the green triangles. The yellow makes the green appear lighter and the blue makes the green appear darker. Moreover, it is as if a blending of colors occurs - the green triangles have a light bluish tinge in the yellow environment. This phenomenon is the opposite of a contrast effect where one expects nearby colors to accentuate the differences between adjacent areas. (You can notice the same effects with different colors on the right side of the image)
top
Source: Optical Illusions
Gestalt Laws
Gestalt Laws
Proximity | Closure | Similarity | Good Continuation For an explanation of the Gestalt laws, see Perceptual Organization - Gestalt Laws of Grouping.
Proximity
Is the left center circle bigger than the right center circle? No, they are both the same size.
Closure
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/gestalt_laws.html (1 von 3)08.05.2007 10:28:31
Gestalt Laws
We are so accustomed to seeing closure that we sometimes close things that aren't.
Similarity
Good Continuation
Gestalt Laws
top
Source: Optical Illusions
Geometrical-Optical Illusions
Geometrical-Optical Illusions
Hering's Optical Illusions | Ehrenstein's Optical Illusions | Meyer's Optical Illusions | Zllner's Optical Illusions | Mller-Lyer's Optical Illusions | Poggendorf's Optical Illusions
The slanted lines cause the illusion that the blue lines are not parallel. In fact they are!
Place your mouse pointer over each image in order to envision the illusion!
Geometrical-Optical Illusions
Place your mouse pointer over the image in order to envision the illusion!
You probably perceive the middle lines as bowing out slightly. In fact they are parallel!
Geometrical-Optical Illusions
Parallel lines intersected by a pattern of short diagonal lines appear to diverge. Explanation: the brain is attempting to interpret this image as if it were part of a three-dimensional scene. Place your mouse pointer over the image in order to envision the illusion!
A variant of Zllner's optical illusion based on parallel bars. Note that the strength of the illusion depends on the pattern, especially on the slope of the diagonal pattern lines.
Geometrical-Optical Illusions
Probably the most famous and most studied illusion was created by German psychiatrist Franz Mller-Lyer in 1889. Although your eyes tell you that the left horizontal line is longer than the right one, they are equal in length.
The single line if continued joins with the lower of the pair, not the top.
top
Source: Optical Illusions
Kindergarten Illusion | Mnsterberg Illusion | Simplified Mnsterberg Illusion | Rope Illusion | TaylorWoodhouse Illusion The following newer geometric-optical illusions are presented without comments. Most of them are caused by conflicting local and global image features.
Kindergarten Illusion
Mnsterberg Illusion
Rope Illusion
Taylor-Woodhouse Illusion
top
Source: Optical Illusions
Size Constancy | Ponzo's Optical Illusions For an explanation of the constancy phenomena, see Perceptual Constancy.
Size Constancy
Place your mouse pointer over the image in order to envision the illusion!
Which creature appears bigger? If you measure them you'll probably be surprised to find out that they are both the same size. The creature appears to increase in size as it gets further away due to what is called 'perspective'. Perspective is evoked in this image by the pattern, and the lines on the wall which tend to converge on to a common point in the distance. Source: Roger Shepard (1990). Mind Sights: Original Visual Illusions, Ambiguities, and Other
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/perspective.html (1 von 2)08.05.2007 10:28:33
The two slanted lines arranged like an inverted V evoke the impression of perspective. Thus, the upper objects (line, circle, barrel), which are framed by the diagonally converging line segments, are perceived to differ in length from the twin objects beneath.
top
Source: Optical Illusions
Subjective Contours
When you look at the picture, do you see a white triangle floating in front of the black circles? Most people do, although there is not one there! The way the circle parts are cut out, makes us think a triangle is there! Download also high-resolution TIFF versions of this illusion:
q
1800px, very large, variant 1800px, variant very large, ZIP archive of all hi-res images
Here, we have a similar phenomenon with a white circle floating above converging lines. Again, the way the lines are cut out, makes us think a circle is there!
Foreground - Background
For an explanation of the foreground-background phenomenon, see Perceptual Organization - Figure and Background.
You can see either a vase or two faces, depending on whether you define the background as being the white or the black color. Source: unknown
A bearded man's face with ivory leaves or a couple kissing? Source: unknown
Is it the word "Liar" or a man's face? Source: unknown Download also high-resolution TIFF versions of this illusion:
q
1800px, very large, inverted 1800px, inverted very large, ZIP archive of all hi-res images
Source: unknown; on http://mathworld.wolfram.com/YoungGirl-OldWomanIllusion.html (Wolfram Research) you will find more on the history of this illusion (the drawing was created in 1915 by the British cartoonist W.E. Hill).
top
Source: Optical Illusions
Geometrical Figures
This illusion is called the Necker Cube. Keep your eyes on the blue dot. Is it in front, or in back of the cube?
At the top of the picture you see happy faces, at the bottom the faces are sad. If you turn the picture
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/impossible_figures.html (2 von 4)08.05.2007 10:28:35
Do you trust your counting abilities? How many feet does this elephant have?
Stroop Effect
Try calling out loud the color of each word as fast as you can! This optical illusion is called the 'Stroop Effect'.
top
Source: Optical Illusions
Motion Perception
Motion Perception
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/motion.jpg
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/motion.jpg08.05.2007 10:28:47
Motion Perception
Motion Perception
Pinwheel
Pinwheel
This animated optical illusion called "Pinwheel" was created by Troy Zerr, 1995.
Proceed as follows 1. Download the file pinwheel.zip. 2. Execute the program on your PC. 3. Concentrate on the center of your screen for about half a minute. 4. Now take a look at any nearby object. Do you notice a change in your perception?
top
Source: Optical Illusions
Definition of Perception
Definition of Perception
2. Introduction to Perception:
Perception is the process by which organisms interpret and organize sensation to produce a meaningful experience of the world. Sensation usually refers to the immediate, relatively unprocessed result of stimulation of sensory receptors in the eyes, ears, nose, tongue, or skin. Perception, on the other hand, better describes one's ultimate experience of the world and typically involves further processing of sensory input. In practice, sensation and perception are virtually impossible to separate, because they are part of one continuous process. Thus, perception in humans describes the process whereby sensory stimulation is translated into organized experience. That experience, or percept, is the joint product of the stimulation and of the process itself. Relations found between various types of stimulation (e.g., light waves and sound waves) and their associated percepts suggest inferences that can be made about the properties of the perceptual process; theories of perceiving then can be developed on the basis of these inferences. Because the perceptual process is not itself public or directly observable (except to the perceiver himself, whose percepts are given directly in experience), the validity of perceptual theories can be checked only indirectly. Historically, systematic thought about perceiving was the province of philosophy. Philosophical interest in perception stems largely from questions about the sources and validity of what is called human knowledge (epistemology). Epistemologists ask whether a real, physical world exists independently of human experience and, if so, how its properties can be learned and how the truth or accuracy of that experience can be determined. They also ask whether there are innate ideas or whether all experience originates through contact with the physical world, mediated by the sense organs. As a scientific enterprise, however, the investigation of perception has especially developed as part of the larger discipline of psychology. For the most part, psychology bypasses the questions about perceiving raised by philosophy in favour of problems that can be handled by its special methods. The remnants of such philosophical questions, however, do remain; researchers are still concerned, for example, with the relative contributions of innate and learned factors to the perceptual process. Such fundamental philosophical assertions as the existence of a physical world, however, are taken for granted among most scientific students of perceiving. Typically, researchers in perception simply accept the apparent physical world particularly as it is described in those branches of physics concerned with electromagnetic energy, optics, and mechanics. The problems they consider relate to the process whereby percepts are formed from the interaction of physical energy (for example, light) with the perceiving organism. Of further interest is the degree of correspondence between percepts and the physical objects to which they ordinarily relate. How accurately, for example, does the visually perceived size of an object match its physical size as measured (e.g., with a yardstick)?
Text taken from q Peter Lindsay & Donald A. Norman: Human Information Processing: An Introduction to Psychology, 1977. q ENCYCLOPDIA BRITANNICA
Perceptual Organization
Perceptual Organization
Gestalt Laws of Grouping | Figure and Background Organizing raw sensory stimuli into meaningful experiences involves cognition, a set of mental activities that includes thinking, knowing, and remembering. Knowledge and experience are extremely important to perception, because they help us make sense of the input to our sensory systems.
2. Introduction to Perception:
Perceptual Organization
background. Gestalt psychologists have devised ambiguous figure-ground relationsips - that is, drawings in which the figure and ground can be reversed - to illustrate their point that the whole is different from the sum of its parts. The "figure and ground" illusion is commonly experienced when one gazes at the illustration of a black vase the outline of which is created by two white profiles. At any moment one will be able to see either the black vase (in the centre area) as "figure" or the white profiles on each side (in which case the black is seen as "ground"). The fluctuations of figure and ground may occur even when one fails deliberately to shift attention, appearing without conscious effort. Seeing one aspect apparently excludes seeing the other. Although such illustrations may fool our visual systems, people are rarely confused about what they see. In real world, vases do not change into faces as we look at them. Instead, our perceptions are remarkably stable. Note: The Gestaltist's concept is "figure-ground segregation" is not only referring to foregroundbackground, but also covers situations, e.g., in which you look through a window outside at a tree. The frame of the window is then the ground the tree the "figure", although it is behind the "ground." See: Ehrenstein, W. (1930). Untersuchungen ber Figur-Grund-Fragen. Zeitschrift fr Psychologie, 117, 339-412.
Text taken from q ENCYCLOPDIA BRITANNICA Note from q Walter H. Ehrenstein jr., Leibniz Research Center for Human Factors, Dortmund University
top
Source: Optical Illusions
Perceptual Constancy
Perceptual Constancy
2. Introduction to Perception:
Perceptual constancy denotes the tendency of animals and humans to see familiar objects as having standard shape, size, colour, or location regardless of changes in the angle of perspective, distance, or lighting. The impression tends to conform to the object as it is or is assumed to be, rather than to the actual stimulus. Perceptual constancy is responsible for the ability to identify objects under various conditions, which seem to be "taken into account" during a process of mental reconstitution of the known image. Even though the retinal image of a receding automobile shrinks in size, the normal, experienced person perceives the size of the object to remain constant. Indeed, one of the most impressive features of perceiving is the tendency of objects to appear stable in the face of their continually changing stimulus features. Though a dinner plate itself does not change, its image on the retina undergoes considerable changes in shape and size as the perceiver and plate move. What is noteworthy is stability in perception despite gross instability in stimulation. Such matches between the object as it is perceived and the object as it is understood to actually exist (regardless of transformations in the energy of stimulation) are called perceptual constancies. Dimensions of visual experience that exhibit constancy include size, shape, brightness, and colour. Perceptual constancy tends to prevail for these dimensions as long as the observer has appropriate contextual cues; for example, perception of size constancy depends on cues that allow one a valid assessment of his distance from the object. With distance accurately perceived, the apparent size of an object tends to remain remarkably stable, especially for highly familiar objects that have a standard size. Psychologists have proposed several explanations for the phenomenon of size constancy. First, people learn the general size of objects through experience and use this knowledge to help judge size. For example, we know that insects are smaller than people and that people are smaller than elephants. In addition, people take distance into consideration when judging the size of an object. Thus, if two objects have the same retinal image size, the object that seems farther away will be judged as larger. Even infants seem to possess size constancy. Another explanation for size constancy involves the relative sizes of objects. According to this explanation, we see objects as the same size at different distances because they stay the same size relative to surrounding objects. For example, as we drive toward a stop sign, the retinal image sizes of the stop sign relative to a nearby tree remain constant - both images grow larger at the same rate. The experience of constancy may break down under extreme conditions. If distance is sufficiently great, for example, the perceived size of objects will decrease; thus, viewed from an airplane in flight, there seem to be "toy" houses, cars, and people below.
Perceptual Constancy
q
ENCYCLOPDIA BRITANNICA
Source of Figure q Roger Shepard (1990). Mind Sights: Original Visual Illusions, Ambiguities, and Other Anomalies. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
top
Source: Optical Illusions
Context Effects
Context Effects
2. Introduction to Perception:
One of the simplest instance of relational (or context) effects in perception is that of brightness contrast. Thus, the apparent brightness of a stimulus depends not only on its own luminance but also on that of the surrounding stimulation. The same gray square looks whiter against a dark background and blacker when placed in a bright surround. Analogous context effects are evident in many commonplace experiences. A man of average height seems to be a runt when he is on a basketball court with much taller players; yet the same man looms like a giant when refereeing a game played by little boys. It is known that a typical winter's day seems delightfully balmy when temperatures rise after a week of subfreezing weather. To the Gestaltist, contrast effects dramatize the relational nature of perception. They also play a significant role in a more recently developed adaptation-level theory, which also provides a general perceptual model. At the core of the model is the notion that the manner in which a stimulus is perceived depends not only on its own physical characteristics but also on those of surrounding stimuli and of stimuli previously experienced by the observer. In other words, the perceiver is said to be perceptually adapted to past sensory stimuli; his adaptation level forms a kind of zero point against which any new stimulus is perceived. An example is provided by the almost overwhelming silence one experiences when the sound of an air conditioner (to which he has adapted) suddenly ceases. Note: Concerning the Hermann grid, its history and follow-up, see see Bernd Lingelbach and Walter Ehrenstein's recent review (in German) in: www.leinroden.de/304herfold.htm
Text taken from q ENCYCLOPDIA BRITANNICA Note from q Walter H. Ehrenstein jr., Leibniz Research Center for Human Factors, Dortmund University
top
Source: Optical Illusions
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/intro_contexteffects.html08.05.2007 10:28:39
Depth Perception
Depth Perception
2. Introduction to Perception:
Depth perception is the ability to see the world in three dimensions and to perceive distance. Although this ability may seem simple, depth perception is remarkable when you consider that the images projected on each retina are two-dimensional. From these flat images, we construct a vivid threedimensional world. To perceive depth, we depend on two main sources of information: binocular disparity, a depth cue that requires both eyes; and monocular cues, which allow us to perceive depth with just one eye.
Binocular Disparity
Perhaps the most important perceptual cues of distance and depth depend on so-called binocular disparity. Because our eyes are spaced apart, the left and right retinas receive slightly different images. This difference in the left and right images is called binocular disparity. The brain integrates these two images into a single three-dimensional image, allowing us to perceive depth and distance. The phenomenon of binocular disparity functions primarily in near space because with objects at considerable distances from the viewer the angular difference between the two retinal images diminishes.
Monocular Disparity
Monocular cues are cues to depth that are effective when viewed with only one eye. Although there are many kinds of monocular cues, the most important are interposition, atmospheric perspective, texture gradient, linear perspective, size cues, height cues, and motion parallax. Interposition: Probably the most important monocular cue is interposition, or overlap. When one object overlaps or partly blocks our view of another object, we judge the covered object as being farther away from us. Atmospheric Perspective: The air contains microscopic particles of dust and moisture that make distant objects look hazy or blurry. This effect is called atmospheric perspective, and we use it to judge distance. Texture Gradient: A texture gradient arises whenever we view a surface from a slant, rather than directly from above. The texture becomes denser and less detailed as the surface recedes into the background, and this information helps us to judge depth. Linear Perspective: Linear perspective refers to the fact that parallel lines, such as railroad tracks, appear to converge with distance, eventually reaching a vanishing point at the horizon. The more the lines converge, the farther away they appear. Size Cues: Another visual cue to apparent depth is closely related to size constancy. If we assume that two objects are the same size, we perceive the object that casts a smaller retinal image as farther away than the object that casts a larger retinal image. This depth cue is known as relative size, because we
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/intro_depth.html (1 von 2)08.05.2007 10:28:40
Depth Perception
consider the size of an object's retinal image relative to other objects when estimating its distance. Another depth cue involves the familiar size of objects. Through experience, we become familiar with the standard size of certain objects. Knowing the size of these objects helps us judge our distance from them and from objects around them. Height Cues: We perceive points nearer to the horizon as more distant than points that are farther away fom the horizon. This means that below the horizon, objects higher in the visual field appear farther away than those that are lower. Above the horizon, objects lower in the visual field appear farther away than those that are higher. This depth cue is called relative hight, because when judging an object's distance, we consider its height in our visual field relative to other objects. Motion Parallax: Motion parallax appears when objects at different distances from you appear to move at different rates when you are in motion. The rate of an object's movement provides a cue to its distance. The more distant objects appear to move in a more slower pace.
top
Source: Optical Illusions
Motion Perception
Motion Perception
2. Introduction to Perception:
One explanation of motion perception involves a form of unconscious inference. That is, when we walk around or move our head in a particular way, we unconsciously expect that images of stationary objects will move on our retina. We discount such movement on the retina as due to our own bodily motion and perceive the objects as stationary. In contrast, when we are moving and the image of an object does not move on our retina, we perceive that object as moving. Movement is also at the heart of a set of observations of considerable significance in the historical development of Gestalt theory. These observations concern circumstances in which people perceive movement in the absence of actual physical motion of the stimulus. One familiar instance of this class of events is referred to as the phi phenomenon. In simplest form, the phi phenomenon can be demonstrated by successively turning two adjacent lights on and off. Given appropriate temporal and spatial relations between the two lights, an observer will perceive the first light as if it were moving from its location to that of the second light. People may perceive motion when none actually exists. The motion-picture screen, for example, presents a series of briefly flashed, still images; the movement people see is a creation of their own perceptual systems. Note:Optical phenomena of motion perception are as or even more striking than as of static patterns, see Walter Ehrenstein's recent review "Basics of Seeing Motion" in: www.abonet.com.br/abo/665/ abo665.htm
Text taken from: q ENCYCLOPDIA BRITANNICA Note from q Walter H. Ehrenstein jr., Leibniz Research Center for Human Factors, Dortmund University
top
Source: Optical Illusions
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/intro_motion.html08.05.2007 10:28:40
2. Introduction to Perception:
Cultural Influences
Beyond sex differences in perceiving that seem to be culturally imposed, there is evidence for more general cultural influences on perception. The burden of much research is to show that the type of physical environment people construct for themselves or choose to inhabit can influence their style of perceiving.
top
Source: Optical Illusions
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/intro_indivdifferences.html08.05.2007 10:28:40
3. References:
Authors A-D | Authors E-K | Authors L-Z Author(s) Agostini, Franco Akins, Kathleen, ed. Armstrong, Tim Armstrong, Tim Arnheim, Rudolf Arnheim, Rudolf Backhaus, Werner G.K.; Kliegl, Reinhold; Werner, John S. Banta, Chris Title Visual Games Perception Colour Perception: A Practical Approach to Colour Theory Year 1986 1996 1991 Publisher Facts on File Oxford University Press Tarquin Publications Tarquin Publications University of California Press University of California Press de Gruyter
Make Moving Patterns: How to 1982 Make Optical Illusions of Your Own Art and Visual Perception: A Psychology of the Creative Eye Visual Thinking Color Vision Perspective From Different Disciplines Seeing is Believing? Haunted Shacks, Mystery Spots, & other Delightful Phenomena Mechanisms of the Mind Can You Believe Your Eyes: Over 250 Illusions and Other Visual Oddities Principles of Visual Perception Handbook of Perception and Human Performance, Vol. 1 Visual Perception: Physiology, Psychology, and Ecology How to Really Fool Yourself: Illusions for all Your Senses Vision: In the Eye of the Beholder Sensation and Perception, International Edition 1974 1989 1998
1995
Funhouse Press
Blakemore, Colin Block, J. Richard; Yuker, Harold E. Bloomer, Carolyn Boff, Kenneth R.; Kaufman, Lloyd; Thomas, James P. (Eds.) Bruce, Vicki; Green, Patrick Cobb, Vicki Cole, K. C. Coren, Stanley; Porac, Clare; Ward, Lawrence M
1979 1992
1990 1986
Cornsweet, Tom DiSpezio, Michael Anthony Doherty, Paul; Rathjen, Don Doherty, Paul; Rathjen, Don
Visual Perception Optical Illusion Magic: Visual Tricks & Amusements The Chesire Cat & other EyePopping Experiments on How We See the World The Magic Wand and other Bright Experiments on Light and Color
1995
top
Source: Optical Illusions
3. References:
Authors A-D | Authors E-K | Authors L-Z Author(s) Title Basics of seeing motion Ehrenstein, Walter H. jr. Year 2003 Publisher Arquivos Brasileiros de Oftalmologia, Volume 66 (5): www.abonet.com.br/ abo/665/abo66505.htm Zeitschrift fr Psychologie 117, 339412 Tarquin Publications Benedikt Taschen Verlag Tarquin Publications Harry Abrams Harry Abrams John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Ehrenstein, Walter H.
1930
Ernst, Bruno Ernst, Bruno Ernst, Bruno Escher, M. C. Locher, J. L.; Escher, M. C. Falletta, Nicholas
1986
The Eye Beguiled: Optical Illusions 1992 The Magic Mirror of M. C. Escher Escher on Escher: Exploring the Infinite His Life and Complete Graphic Work The Paradoxicon: A Collection of Contradictory Challenges, Problematical Puzzles, and Impossible Illustrations Seeing the Light: Opticis in Nature, Photography, Color, Vision,and Holography The Nature of Visual Illusion Seeing: Illusion, Brain, and Mind Puzzles, Paradoxes and Brain Teasers: Optical Illusions Sensation and Perception, 5th Edition Theories of Visual Perception Even Odder Perceptions Eye and Brain: The Psychology of Seeing 1985 1989 1982 1990
1986
Harper Row
Fineman, Mark Frisby, John Gibilisco, Stan Goldstein, E. Bruce Gordon, Ian Gregory, Richard Gregory, Richard
Dover Publications Oxford University Press Tab Books Brooks/Cole Publishing Co. John Wiley & Sons Routledge Oxford University Press
Illusion in Nature and Art The Moon Illusion Appearance & Reality : A Visual Handbook for Artists, Designers, and Makers Dialogues on Perception Human Color Vision, 2nd Edition Organization in Vision: Essays on Gestalt Perception Sight and Mind: An Introduction to Visual Perception Gestalt Psychology
Julesz, Bela Kaiser, Peter K.; Boynton, Robert M. Kanisza, Gaetano Kaufman, Lloyd Khler, Wolfgang
Massachusetts, MIT Press Optical Society of America Praeger Publishers Oxford University Press Liveright
top
Source: Optical Illusions
3. References:
Authors A-D | Authors E-K | Authors L-Z Author(s) Leeman, Fred Title Hidden Images: Games of Perception, Anamorphic Art, Illusion from the Renaissance to the Present Fundamentals of Sensation & Perception, 2nd Edition Das Hermann-Gitter und die Folgen Human Information Processing: An Introduction to Psychology, 2nd Edition The Workings of the Brain Development, Memory, and Perception: Readings from Scientific American Magazine Visual Illusions, Their Causes, Characteristics & Applications An Introduction to the Biology of Vision Fiendishly Difficult Visual Perception Puzzles Early Vision and Beyond The Logic of Perception Sensation and Perception: An Integrated Approach The Mind's Eye The Art of Optical Illusions Perceptionl, Third Edition 1977 Year 1976 Publisher Harry Abrams
Levine, Michael W.; Shefner, Jeremy M. Bernd Lingelbach, Walter H. Ehrenstein jr. Lindsay, Peter H.; Norman, Donald A. LLinas, Rodolfo
1991
Brooks/Cole Publishing Co. Institut fr Augenoptik Aalen: www.leinroden. de/304herfold.htm Academic Press
1990
W. H. Freeman
Luckiesh, M. McIlwain, James T. Moscovich, Ivan Papathomas, Thomas, et. al. Rock, Irvin Schiffman, Harvey Scientific American Seckel, Al Sekuler, Robert; Blake, Randolph
Dover Publications Cambridge University Press Sterling Publications Massachusetts, MIT Press Massachusetts, MIT Press John Wiley & Sons W. H. Freeman Carlton Books McGraw Hill
Shepard, Roger
1990
W. H. Freeman
Slocum, Jerry; Botermans, Jack Solso, Robert Southall, James P.C. (Ed.) Wade, Nicholas Wade, Nicholas Wandell, Brian Yantis, Steven
Optical Illusions and Other Puzzles 1995 Cognition and the Visual Arts Helmholtz's Treatise on Physiological Optics, Volumes I and II The Art and Science of Visual Illusions Visual Allusions: Pictures of Perception Foundations of Vision Visual Perception : Essential Readings (Key Readings in Cognition) A Vision of the Brain 1994 1962
Routledge & Kegan Paul Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Sinauer Associates Psychology Pr
Zeki, Semir
1993
top
Source: Optical Illusions
3. References:
q q q q q q q q
A Web Collection of Optical Illusions Galleries of Illusions and EluZions Optical Illusions, Scientific Toys, Visual Effects, and Even a Little Magic Collection of Optical and Sensory Illusions - Introductory Level and Advanced Level A Study of Different Geometric Effects The Joy of Visual Perception: A Web Book Mighty Optical Illusions Encyclopedia Britannica on Perception
top
Source: Optical Illusions
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/links.html08.05.2007 10:28:42
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/kindergarten.gif
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/kindergarten.gif08.05.2007 10:28:43
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/muensterberg.gif
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/muensterberg.gif08.05.2007 10:28:43
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/muensterberg-vereinf.gif
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/muensterberg-vereinf.gif08.05.2007 10:28:43
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/seiltaeuschung.gif
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/seiltaeuschung.gif08.05.2007 10:28:43
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/TaylorWoodhouse2.gif
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/TaylorWoodhouse2.gif08.05.2007 10:28:44
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/TaylorWoodhouse3.gif
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/TaylorWoodhouse3.gif08.05.2007 10:28:44
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/rollers.gif
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/rollers.gif08.05.2007 10:28:48
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/donwave.gif
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/donwave.gif08.05.2007 10:28:50
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/brownmo.gif
file:///Y|/resources/optical_illusions/images/brownmo.gif08.05.2007 10:28:50