Bradford L. Yates - Media Literacy and Attitude Change (2009)
Bradford L. Yates - Media Literacy and Attitude Change (2009)
Bradford L. Yates - Media Literacy and Attitude Change (2009)
By
BRADFORD L. YATES
A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY
UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA
2000
Copyright 2000
by
Bradford
L.
Yates
This dissertation
is
dedicated to the
memory of my
Daddy.
father,
my
Mama and
love
you so very
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Many times
for
thought
had
set
myself over ten years ago. However, the unending support of those around
I
me
is
the
reason
my
life to date.
would
like to
thank those
who have
helped
true.
First
and foremost,
must thank
my
guidance, support, and motivation over the past four and a half years. Whether
her laughing or crying, Debbie
came
to
was always
words
to rebalance
my
me
my work.
me
that
I
There
could
told her
to
make
it,
was
my
gratitude.
I,
too,
owe a
to
me
the foundations
my
career.
Thank you
to Dr.
John
my
entire doctoral
it
in his office or
on the basketball
court.
Many thanks to
Dr. Julie
Dodd
for
times.
Thank you
Swain
for challenging
me to
find
my
the process.
was
fortunate to
work with
This dissertation would not have been completed without the help of my cousin.
Phyllis Jackson.
me
to conduct
my
study
at
in
fifth
grade
is
my
love.
also
want
to
fourth and fifth grade teachers: Lorey Baggett, Teresa Huggins, Kristie
McHugh, Barbara
Morris, Pamela Royster, Janie Williams, and Roslyn Williams. Their cooperation and
participation helped
make
also
who
participated in
my experiment.
to Jerry
Without them,
this
Thanks
allowing
for
me to
conduct
my
Boys and
appreciate
Wes
Lawson
me
to run
my
of P.
and
fifth
grade students.
also
am
K. Yonge for their participation. Thank you to Robyn Purvis for taking an interest
study and for helping
in
my
me
carry out
my
pretests at P. K.
Yonge.
this ordeal.
Many
thanks to
my
fellow
Naeemah
Clark,
me
with
my
me
A special
thank you
is
and Tony Fargo for being there during the lowest and highest points
thank them for being
frustrations, for
in this process.
my
friend, for
keeping
me
me
vent
my
for never
wavering
in their
support of my efforts.
if
it
my
late
undergraduate advisor and friend. Rex's encouragement, advice, and love pushed
strive for excellence in
me
I
to
my
academic
pursuits.
He was
like a
second father
to
me.
thank
him
for his
am
support.
my
life
when
things were
dark.
my
Her encouragement,
heart and loving
appreciate her
warm
spirit.
To
I
her
give
all
my
love.
my
gratitude to
my
I
for their
love.
Although
am
come
I
to fruition,
am
saddened that
my
father
was not
its
completion.
miss him
terribly.
On many
occasions
my
encouragement by quoting
my
reminded
me of his
infinite
that he
was
still
by
my
side.
My
mother's constant prayers, daily messages of support, and regular supply of inspirational
cards were just what
needed to keep
to give up,
me
I
wanted
heard
my
mother and
inspiration,
and
never would
have completed
am truly
for.
in life.
how to
and
spiritually,
without
fail.
praise the
Lord
for blessing
my
life
epitome of love.
to the
love
dearly.
I,
Lord
for
carrying
me
am unworthy
truly blessed
my
life.
TABLE OF CONTENTS
ESge
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
ABSTRACT
CHAPTERS
1
iv
INTRODUCTION
Empirical Studies Focusing on Media Literacy
2
5
ELM as a Framework
2
LITERATURE REVIEW
Media Literacy Critical Viewing Skills Programs The Current Media Literacy Movement
Evolution of Persuasion Research
Elaboration Likelihood
9
11
17
24
Model
26 39 47
53
Toward Advertisements
Research Questions
57 58 60 60 66
78 79
The Hypotheses
3
METHODS
An Overview
Operational Definitions of Key Variables
RESULTS
Descriptive Analysis
79 79
83
The Scales
Manipulation Checks
Tests of Hypotheses
87
DISCUSSION
Summary of Results
Post-Hoc Analysis
Study Limitations
Implications
92
93
94
98
101
1
Future Research
03
APPENDICES
A
B C
105
107
108 115
D
E
127
150
REFERENCES
BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH
159
173
of the University of Florida in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy
MEDIA LITERACY AND ATTITUDE CHANGE: ASSESSING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF MEDIA LITERACY TRAINING ON CHILDREN'S RESPONSES TO PERSUASIVE MESSAGES WITHIN THE FRAMEWORK OF THE ELABORATION LIKELIHOOD MODEL OF PERSUASION
Bradford L. Yates
August 2000
Chair:
Deborah M. Treise
for
Most commercials
are constructions
that attempt to
sell
how
to evaluate
and
analyze critically the values and ideologies that accompany commercial messages. In
short, children
need to be media
literate.
This dissertation adds to the small but growing body of literature that examines
the effectiveness
of media
literacy training
on children's responses
to persuasive
(ELM) of
a moderating
is
variable in the persuasion process and whether such training affects children's attitudes
to test five
TV
It
was hypothesized
that subjects
exposed
to
media
literacy training
would follow
opposed
product
when exposed
to
Test results suggested that media literacy training was not a moderating variable
in the
ELM.
Additionally, no evidence
was found
media
literacy,
argument
quality,
attitudes.
However,
attitudes
media
produce differences
in
among
subjects.
more negative
attitudes
TV
The
makes
subjects
more
skeptical of
commercial messages because they are more aware of the techniques used by advertisers
to try
If children
techniques used by advertisers, then they will be better equipped to analyze commercials
more
critically
literacy training built into existing school curricula could be very effective at creating
critical
viewers.
CHAPTER INTRODUCTION
1
in the
average American child sees in excess of 30,000 television commercials for various
products each year (Condry, Bence,
& Scheibe,
1988).
By
much
in the
p. 9).
is
(Pungente, 1996). Since most commercials and other media messages are constructions of reality
that
sell
to understand
how to
ideas
(Melamed,
learn
how
to identify
such ideological
messages and analyze the underlying values that are communicated. Although several researchers have argued
that
media
improve students' evaluation of media messages (Considine, 1990; Duncan, 1989; Kahn
& Master,
Ommeren,
& Riffe,
1990), few
of this investigation
is
of media
literacy training
on
children's
& O'Hagan,
1985; Austin
& Johnson,
1997a; Austin
&
& Nach-Ferguson,
1995). Recent
& Freeman,
1996; Austin
& Johnson,
1997a; Austin
&
& Meili,
1994; Austin
& Nach-Ferguson,
& Nass,
on campaign
priorities.
in
effects research.
as active decision-makers
who
make
decisions based
on
theory of media decision-making and identifies logical and emotional decision-making processes of children. Message designers can use the model to identify the most effective
point of intervention. Austin and Johnson (1997a, 1997b) found that children's
is
their studies,
media
literacy training
was
identified as
children's understanding
media
literacy training
on
third-graders' "perceptions
323) using a
schools were
Solomon four-group
style
in three
A fourth
school was
split into
Two
children watched
Buy Me That! A
TV Advertising,
a half-hour
on
advertising techniques. After the video the youngsters discussed the techniques with the
Consumer Reports
to
series
ads.
were shown
the skills
to
each group
learned in the
in
advertisement.
of
mediation, focusing on the extent to which the ad showed "(a) what was
real, (b)
what
was
right or
wrong, and
(c)
& Johnson,
1997b,
p. 323).
bookmark
style
handouts to reinforce
"(a)
which include
&
p. 332).
media All groups (control and experimental) took a posttest immediately after the
literacy lesson
activity.
three
months
later.
posttest,
which was administered by the teacher without assistance from the researcher.
was
to
(e.g.,
to
own
item A?").
Short-term effects were found for understanding of persuasive intent, perceptions desirability, social norms for alcohol use, and predrinking behavior.
of realism,
Results retained significance at delayed posttest for perceived realism and Johnson, 1997b, identification. Some gender differences existed. (Austin
&
p.
323)
Austin and Johnson (1997a) also examined the immediate and delayed effects of
general and specific media literacy training on third-graders' decision-making processes
A Solomon
four-group style experiment with two treatment levels (general and specific) assessed the
effectiveness
of in-school media
literacy training.
purpose media literacy videotape about television advertising; however, one treatment
showed alcohol
showed
clips
media
literacy training.
Immediate
intent,
of persuasive
and
less
viewing of characters as
less similar to
people they
knew
in real life
of positive
to
choose an alcohol-
were found on
their perceptions
of television's
realism and their views of social norms related to alcohol. Delayed effects were
examined and confirmed on expectancies and behavior. The treatment was more
effective
when
alcohol-specific, and
it
also
was more
effective
among
girls
than boys
(Austin
& Johnson,
1997a,
p. 17).
literacy as
an effective means of
making framework. The present study, which builds on Austin and Johnson's
attempts to understand
research,
how media
literacy helps to
established
model of persuasion.
ELM as a Framework
In order to measure effectively the impact of media literacy training
on
children's
(Petty
& Cacioppo,
The
investigation.
determine
if
media
literacy skills
& Petty,
& Goldman,
1981a;
Petty, Harkins.
& Williams.
& Brock,
could play a moderating role in the persuasion process. These include personal
on
the
persuasion process. Therefore, this study aims to test the moderating effect of media
literacy training
on
children's attitude
in
product advertisements.
The
into a general
framework
of persuasion
in
it
also has
communications (Petty
& Cacioppo,
& Schumann,
1983) and
his
become popular
in the field
ELM
terms of issues
(e.g.,
&
et al.,
1983).
attitude
( 1
its
its
integrative merits.
ELM overcomes a critical limitation of traditional hierarchy-of-effects models by relaxing the assumption that cognitively complex changes in consumer attitudes are necessary for effective advertising. By identifying two distinct
The
routes to attitude change, the
advertising ((Droge, 1989), "while accounting for the potential influence of attitude
& Lutz,
& Hensel.
1991,
p. 60).
Lutz.
( 1
983 ) found
that attitude
ELM framework.
that attitude
toward the ad had stronger effects on attitude change than brand cognitions
under low involvement and low knowledge conditions. Additionally, attitude toward the ad was found to have a greater impact on attitude than brand cognitions under high
elaboration likelihood conditions (Petty
& Cacioppo,
revealed that subjects' attitudes toward an advertised product were influenced more by
their attitude
Lord, Lee, and Sauer (1995), tested two competing hypotheses relative to the
formation of attitude toward the ad and found that "results supported the combinedinfluence hypothesis across varying levels of processing motivation and opportunity with
differences in the relative magnitude of argument and cue effects consistent with the
The
this
framework
is
& Hensel,
1991, p. 62).
More
specifically, the
it
is
is
focused on college-age students, and no studies have examined the moderating role of
media
literacy skills.
much
It is
evident that children need to be equipped with skills that help them evaluate the
to
at
is
necessary.
CHAPTER 2
LITERATURE REVIEW
of the media
The discussion
also
and
Media Literacy
What
comes
via the
individuals
know about
the world
beyond
their
immediate surroundings
not present their messages in a neutral and value-free way; they shape and distort reality
reality
pose a problem for society because individuals, especially children, are unable to
It is
through
media
media.
10
Several researchers
made
& Master,
that
Ommeren,
&
They argued
critically
aware of what
it
schools.
New Mexico,
Support and advocacy groups such as the Center for Media Education, the Center
for
Media
for
Media
Literacy, the
National
Media Citizenship
Project,
created to push for a media literate society. National conferences, like the annual
Media
Education Conference and the Media Literacy Citizenship Project conference, have
brought together educators, media professionals and concerned citizens
create a unified voice for
in
an
effort to
media
literacy.
At the Aspen
Institute's'
National Leadership
in 1992, participants
media
literacy:
media literate person and everyone should have the opportunity to become one can decode, evaluate, analyze and produce both print and electronic media. The fundamental objective of media literacy is critical autonomy in relationship to all media. Emphases in media literacy training range widely, including informed
a
"The Aspen
Institute is
[Its]
making
in the fields
accomplishes this by bringing together representatives of industry, government, the media, the academic
world, nonprofits, and others to assess the impact of modern communications and information systems on
democratic societies. The Program also promotes research and distributes conference reports
the communications and information fields and to the broader public" (Aspen Institute,
1
to' leaders in
996, available
http://www.aspeninst.org/Index.html).
11
media
literate.
Media
how to
literacy
of school media
for students to
learn to
responsibility, cooperation,
No
in life,
they
will
their strengths
interests,
literacy
is
PTA and
building a
is
more media
literate population.
Critical
Viewing
Skills
Programs
literacy
movement has
last
Viewing
Skills" Education:
Major Media
( 1
99 1 ),
is
an excellent
Many of the
central issues
were
12
first
investigated in
such projects was eliminated during the early 1980s. These early federal programs
the foundation for future research by scholars and public interest groups about the
laid
potential benefits
literacy
programs.
is
literacy.
It
was
the
all
communication
(e.g.,
(e.g.,
critically
read or viewed over the years in an attempt to better understand what then-
how they
Despite this history of critical reading and viewing, educational institutions were
how to
evaluate critically
mass media experiences. In the 1920s and 1930s, film appreciation courses began
to
spread throughout the United States and England, due in part to the growing number of
It
was not
until the
98 1 ). Finally, in the
late
970s,
its
own
13
designs for teaching better understanding and use of television were developed
but not widely used" (Brown, 1991, p. 58).
An UNESCO
meeting
in
Norway
in
962
laid the
framework
for "critical
viewing
skills" education.
Due
to the scope
that educators
how to
use the
medium
constructively
skills"
I.
To
screen.
II.
of
III.
To
help viewers to
in their
may
benefit
truly democratic
education, namely, to help the individual to respect and uphold truth and, on the
basis of the richest possible personal development, to share and enjoy with his
fellow
heart.
men the
treasures
which our
p.
(Hodgkinson, 1964,
later the
78)
Eleven years
Foundation saw a need for increased and improved mass media instruction within public
schools (Ford Foundation, 1975).
The
funded by the Ford, Markle, and National Science Foundations recommended several
14
Among the
of media
and
structure, the
values within television content, standards for criticism of content, and production skills
Projects
viewing
skills
in
(USOE) funded
critical
viewing
skills
was funded
them only
for
two
years.
by
relating
to television.
fiction; identifying
points of view; understanding the style and content of various types of programming; and
TV
programming and
the printed
word (Lloyd-
& Strand,
research that found a link between television violence and subsequent aggressive
& Ross,
of the four
seed projects, was established for teachers of grades K-5 as well as students' parents and
critical
viewing
skills
(CVS)
activities
were designed
it
for classroom
and
home
was
difficult to
add
15
new
SEDL
existing courses.
skills.
how to
WNET
13,
New
television station
WNET
in 1978,
formalized curriculum materials and conducted workshops for educators and community
leaders across the nation.
Ten
training sessions
were
set
up
and
them conduct
their
own CVS
arranged for community leaders and public librarians to help them train parents and
children in critical
home
viewing.
WNET's critical
viewing
skills
for students in grades 6-8, but could be adapted for children at different cognitive levels.
Creators of the program intended the concepts and materials to be part of the language
arts or social studies curricula as well as to
be used
at
home.
critical writing
and thinking
For example,
ideas, specific
program and
identify
details, literary
1991).
16
Far West Laboratory for Educational Research and Development (Grades 9-12)
CVS
USOE project
was
to identify
TV
skills
appropriate for teenagers; second, develop and field test course materials for teachers,
students and families; third, create materials and provide workshops for educators,
parents,
and leaders of organizations; and fourth, publish and distribute those materials
their initial curriculum,
five areas
of critical viewing
and manage
to recognize
television effects
on one's own
and
them
to question
TV
their
USOE
devoted $400,000 to
its
at
on July
31, 1981.
The
project
was
medium
itself.
built
around
activities.
The
first
structure
17
advertising, while
module
three
explored the news media and governmental constraints. The project's director. Donis
to
be successful.
More
specifically, critical
viewing
skills training
community
p. 3).
leaders, to
CVS projects,
funding in the wake of a deep economic recession. The recession cultivated the
know how
to
compete
in the global
among
literate.
Since media education was associated with the recreational nature of television, critical
viewing
literacy
skills
frill
programs replaced
movement
in the
in the late
990s
like
Kubey
& Baker,
1999).
Telemedia
IS
been created to educate the public about the need for media education. The National
for
media
literacy in
K-12
examined
all
one or
more elements
literacy
components
For
in
arts curricula.
...
communication
arts area,
one of the
theatre standards for grades preK-2 requires that "the student understands context
by
analyzing the role of theatre, film, television, and electronic media in the past and
present" (Florida Department of Education, 1998).
The
language arts
in
&
Wulfemeyer, 2000)
made 10
(Kubey
&
Baker, 1999).
Further examples of media literacy around the nation are found in Minneapolis,
literacy grades
on
and
in
North Carolina,
that has
19
been
in
is
the
Newspaper
in
in
Education program
is
is
p.
develop
critical
reading
skills,
and
N1E
to use
newspapers
math, and
many
other
subjects
(ANPA,
1990).
Newspapers
to
Writing
Skills,
medium to
help students
For example, teachers can use the sports and business sections of the newspaper to
make
skills
in
a poem, essay, or novel and write a newspaper article about the event
(ANPA,
in
1990).
school
& Dickson,
Freedom Forum,
1
1994).
Dvorak
et al.
(1
in extra-curricular
and community
activities.
worthy of
permanent
Students
who took
it
recognized language arts competencies than did either required English courses or
other English electives. These indications from academically superior students
affirm the worthiness of Journalism as a course at the heart of the language arts
(Dvorak
et al.,
1994, p. 49)
20
digital
new
media. Katz
(1996) suggests that children must have access to the technology and be taught to use
safely
it
in
way
is
entertainment.
Media education
literacy
is
by Katz.
Media
Critical
Community
PTA (Parent
Teacher Association).
Cable
in the
1995). With the passage of the Children's Television Act of 1990 and the subsequent
debate over
how to
enforce the requirements of the Act, more attention has been given to
to
on a
daily basis.
New Mexico
New
States.
media
literacy
movement
in the
United
In 1993, the
funded the
New
(NMMLP)
(Darlington, 1996).
Hugh
The
the
Downs of ABCs
Project
is
Downs
Center.
now
sponsored by Albuquerque
New
Mexico
State
Critical
Viewing
Project, a partnership
Association, Cable in the Classroom, and the National Cable Television Association, provides free "Taking Charge of Your TV" workshops to help families view television carefully and critically. Since this
nationwide
initiative
began
in
1994, over 800 local cable operators, parents, and educators have been
workshops
in their
communities.
support of national education organizations including the National Education Association, the American Association of School Administrators, the National Association of Secondary School Principals, the
National Association of Elementary School Principals and the National School Boards Association" (Cable
in the
Albuquerque Academy
is
,000
houses the
New
Project.
21
NMMLP
is
to
the most
media
literacy
practices
and
The
NNMLP
to help
and
CD-ROMs,
teachers and parents develop and refine media literacy skills in children as well as
themselves. The Project has been recognized by local and national media as one of the
"state
995b,
p.
1 ).
NMMLP's effort to
create a
media
literate citizenry
has been
The Project's
director.
Bob McCarmon,
is
essentially a
media
literacy
salesperson,
who
workshops
to public
1996).
As
part
resonating in
New
Project, 1995b, p.
1).
must reclaim from the media the task of teaching children lessons for
formative years and beyond
come
into the
home and
Literacy
Project, 1995b).
Media
literacy exists to
22
On
U.
a national level, media literacy finds support from top government officials.
S. Secretary
said,
stretch their
...
Television
is
when
.
it
comes
to understanding the
It
Takes
A Village
literacy.
She suggests
what and
argues
watch television
of media
literacy training.
is
to
produce
critical thinkers
who can
evaluate and
critical
consumers of
and be trained
themselves.
for
and Media Literacy (National Communication Association, 1998) are the result of the
call for a
in the
geography and science by the National Education Goals Panel (National Education Goals
Panel, 1992) as well as the national education reform legislation, "Goals 2000: Educate
act
in several subjects,
23
Acceptance by school
serve as a framework for each state, school districts, or local schools to use to develop
all-inclusive; rather, they provide the opportunity for
curricula;
they leave the decision about adopting and implementing the standards up to the
individual states and their respective school districts.
flexibility
may
be the catalyst to get other states and school districts to adopt the standards and help
students
become media
literate.
It is
clear that
only those trained in the designated areas should be allowed to teach the specific
skills
literacy.
for the
media
literacy
movement from
1
who
how to
in the
media
all
subjects
is
in
accord with
them
24
In order to
comprehend more
this study,
it
of persuasion research
order
to understand
how
established
attitudes
much of the
(Petty
early
research
& Priester,
media
of an
indirect effects
to
Sheatsley
message flow;
that
media tend
opposed
to
new
ones.
communication. They believed the media were not as powerful as early researchers had
thought. Katz and Lazarsfeld (1955) argued that media tend to influence opinion leaders
who
The
studies of Carl
Hovland
also contributed to
the indirect effects view of the media. Hovland, Lumsdaine, and Sheffield (1949)
little
effect
on mass changes
in attitude
and behavior.
power of the
military films
(Hovland
et al., 1949).
Subsequent research
25
been popular
in social
psychological
on media influence
(Petty
& Priester,
objects,
& Priester,
1994,
p. 94).
Attitude
is
important because
& Priester,
1994). Although a
number of theories of
attitude
model
is
model of
influence elaborated by
McGuire (1985,
1989).
inputs to the persuasion process. These include (1) source, (2) message, (3) recipient, (4)
not complete.
shown
Many of the
steps
& Priester,
occur without attitude change and attitudes can change without learning of specific
information (Petty, Baker,
& Gleicher,
1991; Petty
& Priester,
model
is
26
their exposure,
attention, interest,
it is
not
it
(Petty
& Priester,
1994).
Petty,
response theory in order to address these two drawbacks. Cognitive response theory
suggests that the level of "yielding
related to the idiosyncratic cognitive responses (pro
is
and counter-arguments) generated to the message rather than learning of the message,
and persistence of persuasion
is
related to
memory
1994,
& Priester,
p. 98).
However, the cognitive response theory only focused on individuals who were
active
failed to consider
& Priester,
& Cacioppo,
1981a; Petty
& Cacioppo,
1986a; Petty
& Cacioppo,
1986b) developed the Elaboration Likelihood Model of persuasion, which holds that
there are
two routes
to persuasion, central
and peripheral.
Elaboration Likelihood
Model
tested in a
number of
The
ELM posits that there are two routes to persuasion, a central route
2.1).
The
central route
of persuasion
is
characterized
by active cognitive processing and leads to attitude changes that are more enduring and
predictive of future behavior (Cialdini, Petty,
& Cacioppo,
1981; Petty
& Cacioppo,
1980).
is
negative cues
27
of behavior (Petty
et al..
1983).
literature will
that
ELM.
The
of persuasive
et al.,
1983,
p. 135).
all
in
& Cacioppo,
986a; Petty
& Priester,
994).
An
individual's motivation
and
ability to process
information and generate favorable and/or unfavorable thoughts characterize the central
route. Other characteristics include
existing attitudes
(Cummings
& Venkatesan,
Ho viand,
Janis,
& Kelley,
28
PERSUASIVE COMMUNICATION
Attitude
is
relatively temporary,
susceptible,
and unpredictive of
behavior
MOTIVATED TO PROCESS?
Personal relevance need
;
for cognition;
personal responsibility.
ABILITY TO PROCESS?
Distraction; repetition; prior knowledge;
sources;
number of
arguments.
message comprehensibility.
argument quality)
Unfavorable
Favorable
Thoughts
Predominate
Thoughts
Predominate
Yes
(favorable)
predictive of behavior
& Cacioppo,
986b).
29
is
to external
messages (Cacioppo
& Petty,
et al.,
1981b;
way
summary
evaluation (Azjen
& Fishbein,
1980; Lutz
& Bettman,
1977; Troutman
&
Shanteau, 1976). Careful processing leads to an attitude that becomes part of a person's
belief structure.
& Priester,
1994).
is
attitudes.
Attitudes changed
relatively
change
until
they
al.,
1981; Petty et
al.,
1980;
Petty
& Priester,
1994,
p.
100-101).
The
information are low along this route. This route relies on simple cues within the
persuasion context to influence attitude change. These cues include,
among
others,
source attractiveness, perceived expertise, and the number of arguments. Attitude change
via the peripheral route of persuasion can be highly effective; however, research has
shown
that
less resistant to
& Priester,
& Cacioppo,
986a).
30
is
is
person associating the attitude issue or product with positive or negative cues. Also,
attitude
change
may
result
argued position based on simple cues within the persuasion context (Petty
1983).
may
to the persuasive
communication.
level
to process the
when
message was
1989; Petty
& Unnava.
& Cacioppo,
1979b).
expressed their attitudes toward an issue after exposure to a mediated message with either
strong or
expertise.
The communication
the central
31
route).
Low persona]
al.,
relevance would
make
more
potent (Petty et
1981a).
to persuasion
Petty et
al.
not
Personal involvement
is
one variable
that influences
how much
consideration
is
and Petty (1976) found that subjects generated more supportive arguments
anticipation of a discussion with an opponent
in
an
when
was of high
personal relevance. Petty and Cacioppo (1979a, 1979b) reported that issues of high
rather than
subjects'
manipulation of source credibility had more impact under low involvement conditions
than high. After exposing subjects to a message containing two or six arguments from a
likable or dislikable source,
Chaiken
( 1
On
32
source had a greater impact under low involvement conditions. However. Chaiken's
findings do not provide
two routes
message
manipulation employed by Chaiken (number of arguments) has the ability to serve as a simple cue much in the same way that a source manipulation (likable or
dislikable source) can serve as a cue. (Petty
et al.,
1981a,
p.
849)
test
Petty,
that
the most
more by the
A2
weak) x 2
Subjects
was used
were exposed
at the University
of
among
exam requirement
for graduation)
would be
instituted the
following year. The other half were told the changes would not take effect for 10 years,
thus creating low involvement. Additionally, source expertise
was
varied
among
the
subjects as well as argument quality. Strong arguments included statistics, data, etc. in
relied
on quotes,
personal opinion, and examples. The messages contained eight arguments each and were
1981a).
al.,
33
Two
attitude
broadcasts. Subjects
were asked
concept
of Comprehensive Exams' on
four
unfavorable/favorable)" (Petty et
al.,
exams on an
1 1
-point
all
and
converted and averaged into an index of attitude toward comprehensive exams. Subjects
also rated the quality
cover
story.
They answered
of the manipulations.
the
list
as
recall.
.92).
Findings suggested that under certain circumstances factors other than message
content, like source credibility and attractiveness, might be
more important
as
is
messages low
results, Petty
in personal relevance
and
peripheral cues must be examined to determine the path to persuasion. "Each type of
persuasion occurs in some instances, and the level of personal involvement with an issue
et al.,
1981a,
p.
853).
34
The authors offered two reasons why increased personal relevance might be
associated with increased reliance
on message arguments
is
when
more thoroughly
Few
when
is
&
Cacioppo, 1981b).
Besides motivation, ability
a second reason personal relevance might be
is
have a better schema for thinking about things relevant to them. Thus, people
greater ability to process messages relevant to
may have
them
(Petty et
al.,
1981a).
two
and
"that
al.,
1981a,
p. 854).
They suggested
that future
potential moderators
of
is
The moderating
Petty et
role
of involvement
test the
al.
moderating role of
at the University
involvement
in the
ELM. Over
of
low
weak argument) x 2
Subjects were told they were part of a study concerning the evaluation
of magazine and newspaper ads conducted by the psychology department and journalism
school (Petty etal., 1983).
35
first
advertisements.
level
of
1983).
To
razor,
which happened
Additionally,
would be
test
marketed
in
Midwestern
cities,
by offering a brand of
were
make
a decision about a
in their area.
Low
involvement subjects were led to believe they would not have to make a decision
in the
about razors
it
al.,
Argument
the razor
quality
was
either strong or
was
is
"scientifically designed"
and
"Handle
weak arguments ad
characterized the razor as "designed for beauty" and listed five statements about the
product
(e.g.,
minimum of rust")
(Petty et
al.,
1983).
Celebrity and non-celebrity endorsements were used to vary the peripheral cue. Celebrity endorsed ads featured famous golf and tennis athletes. Non-celebrity ads
featured average looking people unfamiliar to the subjects (Petty et
al.,
1983).
36
first
page,
to
recall the
in
it
product category descriptors. These brand recall and recognition measures were used for
"their practical
importance and for purposes of comparison with the attitude data" (Petty
After responding to questions about one of the legitimate ads in the booklet,
subjects answered questions about the crucial
placed early in the booklet to avoid subject fatigue and boredom and achieve
maximum
and their overall impression of the product. These measures were averaged into a general
positive or negative attitude toward the product (Petty et
al.,
1983).
Next, subjects answered questions consistent with the cover story and then were
instructed to
their thoughts while they
list all
activity
was designed
to
list
that the
et al., 1983).
when involvement
is
a greater impact on attitudes under low involvement conditions rather than high. These
37
persuasion (Petty et
al.,
1983).
Forewarning of subjects
Petty and Cacioppo (1979a) studied the effects
may
& Cacioppo,
1979a
The
researchers' hypothesis,
1966), suggested that the reactance to foreknowledge of persuasive intent would affect
& Cacioppo,
believed the greater the personal relevance of the attitude under attack, the greater the
intent or not).
Within
each group subjects experienced one of three involvement levels (one high and two low).
Subjects read booklet covers that either warned them of the impending persuasive
communication or
not.
in one's
exams were
upcoming graduating
class.
38
in
upcoming graduating
Thus, the personal relevance of the editorials was greatly reduced (Petty
& Cacioppo.
1979a).
After hearing the editorials, the subjects expressed their attitudes toward the
thought and ideas about the radio editorial and rated them as positive, negative or neutral
in favor
(i.e.,
completed
ancillary questions
and attempted
to recall all
& Cacioppo,
1979a).
The
results
of persuasive
intent
on
is
issues
of high involvement
inhibits persuasion
based on reactance theory, was also confirmed. Forewarning of persuasive intent inhibits
persuasion on highly involving issues by producing counter-arguments, not only before
the
message (Petty
& Cacioppo,
&
Cacioppo, 1979a).
Interestingly, in the
show more
attitude
& Cacioppo,
more
1979a).
critical
is
information processing changes because subjects become less objective and more intent
39
their attitudinal
& Cacioppo,
high rather than low involvement conditions and under warned rather than unwarned
conditions.
"This suggests that both involvement and warning increase the importance of
in
& Cacioppo.
1979a,
p. 176).
is
influential
researchers in the area of cognitive development. His developmental stages have been
used time and again to explain and categorize children's mental and physical growth.
Piaget's
work
in cognitive
development suggests
to
move
through the steps necessary to get them to where they need to go can create readiness.
clear
is
an established pattern of
set forth
by
numerous research
studies.
skills to
process and
evaluate persuasive messages will provide vital information for the design of this study.
it
relates to television
is
it
relates to
communication
it is
summarize
Piaget's theory
40
on which
to build.
Piaget (1983;
& Inhelder,
1964) and other stage theorists believe there are distinct qualitative
in contrast to learning theorists
is
who
suggest there
is
According
(i.e.,
the capability
of learning with
key to
believed physical action and experience with the environment were important for
learning.
He
schemata. Sensorimotor schemata refer to things like walking and opening doors;
cognitive schemata refer to concepts, images, and the ability to reason from causes to
refer broadly to
effects;
communication
skills.
Schemata
in
newborn
coordinate into larger schemata. For example, grasping schemata are the coordination of
first,
determine
if the
which
is
is
Adaptation
an on-going process
and control
their
their
Learning theorists,
like
Bloom (Bloom,
& Krathwohl,
1956),
Gagne
(1977), and
Briggs (Gagne
& Briggs,
is
differences in what
based- on 1 979), have developed typologies that classify types of learning being learned. These typologies suggest that specific types of learning take place
41
when
they encounter
new
They assimilate by
new
situations.
Accommodation and
maintain a sense of equilibration, or balance, as they explore and attach order and
meaning
(Good
& Brophy,
is
1990).
(Good
& Brophy,
1990,
p. 72).
Schemata development
stage
moves
into thought
skills,
negation, identity,
reversibility
in the
fact.
Developmental Differences
Attention
Among
is
Children
Attention level
its
Studies
show
that
children pay attention to the formal features of television (e.g., program pace, cuts, sound
effects,
&
& Wright,
is
what content
grammar of program
content.
42
Age
and sound effects appeal to peculiar voices, upbeat music, rhyming, auditory changes,
younger children (Anderson
& Levin,
1976; Calvert
& Gersh,
they tend to depend not rely on such salient features unless they are informative;
learned signals (Calvert, Huston, Watkins,
learn that the end
on
& Wright,
of a dramatic show
is
when revelations
are
are
resolved.
They understand
of the program
is
attention.
As
demands of a
show grow
in
importance (Van Evra, 1998). Wright and colleagues (1984) noted that
young viewers,
in establishing initial
attention,
which
is
less
more
is
active
1984). This
explains
why
much
as
young viewers.
most
is
more
still
is
"These data run counter to the idea that children passively absorb television information, and they suggest that children's cognitive processing
abilities
43
fairly sophisticated"
(Anderson
& Smith,
1984; Pezdek
& Hartman,
1983, cited in
Van
Evra, 1998, p.
6).
among
Hayes
&
Kelly, 1984;
real;
they lack the ability to understand narrative or dramatic structure; and they don't
on
age.
Young
children's
affected
by an
and
effect events
(Hayes
& Kelly,
fire
1984). For
does not
difficult
time distinguishing between program content and commercials and real and imaginary
characters (Canadian Association of Broadcasters, 1985).
an action figure to
"life,"
make
TV life
more
According
to a study
by Calvert (1988), 10-year old children were better able to understand flashbacks than
six-year olds. Calvert concluded that formal features, like flashbacks, influence
children's understanding
who watched
less television
44
overall tended to
make more
more
television a child
in
made
despite the
(1987) suggested that the age of a child and amount of TV viewing during preschool are
key factors
beyond the
of most children, and those younger than 7 or 8 rarely grasp abstract lessons
in
embedded
& Roberts,
on perception
are based
or 8 changes occur and conceptual and symbolic skills develop. These help youngsters
mediate television content and understand the complexities of television portrayals better
(Rubin, 1986).
Hoffher and Cantor (1985) point out that young children rely on more perceptual
features and information (e.g., personal appearance) than conceptual information and
actual behavior
of social interaction on
television
is
Swanson, Meyers,
& Murphy,
comprehend
television characters'
based on
their
own
interpersonal experience
(Van Evra,
1998).
Retention
among
is
of auditory
input, but
45
such encoding
is
an age-related
skill
They
rely
more
is
1998).
in various
media
affect
younger children more than older ones. Meringoff, Vibbert, Char, Fernie, Banker, and
Gardner (1983) reported that preschooler's memory of figurative language improved
greatly after a story
was read
al.
to
them
as
compared
However, Meringoff et
do from
may be more
sensitive
to
on a
either
embedded
in
a computer
game or given
via simple
in the
game
included in the comparison. Rehearsal in either context led to greater recall, but those
playing the
game remember
may have
made
difficult
changes
children
and
interest value
to
spontaneous rehearsal
in
who do
not want to
remember
(Oyen
& Bebko,
skills,
1996).
schema
processed
and
coordination in a study by
46
that
younger
children develop basic television skills early, but general cognitive skills
become more
Although effective learners process information via visual recognition and verbal
encoding,
it
is
older children
who
easily
(Van Evra, 1998). Beentjes and van der Voort (1993) found
from television
stories than print stories.
more
stories
and print
recall
was
tested;
on delayed
tests.
is
recalled.
"Sesame
Street"
less than
1%
after viewing.
human portrayals
than for
The children
more accurately
than complex ones. Despite verbal labels, the preschoolers did not remember the
emotional states of the Muppets or cartoon characters, nor did they mention the physical
states
of the characters
(Hayes
& Casey,
1992).
among
it
is
is
they do not have a vast amount of experiences, fantasies, and relationships to shape their
viewing experience
memories of
47
is
(Winn, 1985).
little
of
television,
it is
Young
on
tests
recall,
more of television
Odom,
who
when
results for
open-ended
recall
young viewers'
recall
recall
measures (Van Evra, 1998). Such findings were reported by Hayes and Kelly
(1984)
when
a recognition measure
was used
in
recall, unlike
recognition,
is
a voluntary process.
He
lot
about
television because
of repetition and
little.
is
of media
literacy training in
of persuasive messages
(i.e.,
television advertisements),
it
is
necessary to discuss the literature relating to children and advertising. Although a review
literature
is
investigation, a
4S
Martin
(1
intent
previous research. The meta-analysis examined studies that addressed these questions:
a commercial?" and
"What
is
"Why
is
a commercial on television/What
is
a commercial
Twenty-one
articles
met the
criteria
in
the meta-analysis. Although most studies suggest a positive relationship between age and
children's understanding
of the
intent
of advertising,
it is
Young (1990)
into
two
attention to advertisements
ability to distinguish
understanding of ad intent
interpretation
d.
e.
of advertising content
and
recall,
memory
awareness of
advertisements)
f.
2.
What
a. b.
c.
on knowledge, attitudes, and values on other people (e.g., parents) on choice/consumption behavior or other types of behavior
(e.g.,
literature
is
useful
it
provides a clearer picture of the numerous areas of research that have been
explored.
The present
first
investigation
is
set forth
by
Young. The
(i.e.,
media
literacy training).
The second
49
category deals with what the child does with the processed information. The focus within
this
category
is
on
attitudes.
on the
relationship
intent
of advertising,
type of intent assessed, type of ad exposure, publication year, and publication type.
The
first
three moderator effects are most relevant to the present study; publication year and
& Rose,
means of
is
intent.
may be
& Donohue,
1987). Children's inability to articulate their understanding of ad intent has led to the
conclusion that they do not understand the intent of advertising intent. This
problematic because they
is
may
it
(Macklin, 1987).
revealed a significant difference between the effect size for verbal assessment and the
effect size for nonverbal assessment.
it
is
problematic to draw
conclusions from the entire set of studies because several study characteristics, especially
differences in methods, moderate the relationship between age and understanding of ad
50
The
more
appropriate for younger children than verbal assessments. Nonverbal measures appear to
(Martin. 1997).
According
to
lets
the audience
know the
is
persuasive function
messages are biased; and 4) realize biased messages must be interpreted differently than
informational messages (Macklin,
1
974)
made
similar distinctions.
They suggested
intent:
assistive
(i.e.,
ads inform us) and 2) persuasive (ads encourage us to buy products). Other
types of intent identified by Blosser and Roberts (1985) are the intent to inform, teach,
entertain, sell,
and persuade.
Despite the differences put forth by Blosser and Roberts (1985), Martin (1997)
literature
intent.
She noted
little
and persuasive
intent.
distinctions
between
intent
and informational/assistive
intent.
51
would be
clearly
is
dependent upon
commercials"
(p. 17).
It is
young ones,
tell
them about
their
Some
studies (e.g.,
Ward, Wackman,
&
They
simply ask general questions about children's understanding of ad intent via surveys or
personal interviews. Other studies
(e.g.,
Rubin,
of commercials only or
Vance,
to
Still
& Hudleson,
(i.e.,
strength of the relationship between age and understanding of ad intent" (p. 207). Seeing
an advertisement
may
more
may
ad's informative
or persuasive
intent.
According
52
two
types of intent
lie
on a
Her meta-analysis
reveals
that
children's understanding
of advertising
on
their perspective.
If they
its
intent as
is
way
1
to
make
a profit (Martin,
997).
Martin (1997) also argued for further development of reliable nonverbal measures
to assess children's (especially
younger
children's) understanding
of ad
intent.
Macklin
et al
measure persuasive/selling
the Trix Rabbit wants
Asking kids
to point to
what
them
to
their understanding
of
advertising's selling intent (Macklin, 1987). Martin (1997) suggested that researchers
"...continue to develop nonverbal measures with the goals of determining whether these
some
other type of information, and the extent to which the results are affected by the nature of
the task" (p. 214). Furthermore, additional verbal assessment measures should be
53
Attitude
Toward Advertisements
is
to
determine changes
in attitude
toward
is
may
develop an attitude toward the actual advertisement that could influence their attitudes
it
is
that has
& Baldinger,
1991
& Olson,
A Meta-Analvsis
In their meta-analysis
attitude
toward
advertisements,
among
on
ad attitudes. Results also provided support for the indirect influence of ad attitudes on
& Stayman,
1992).
This meta-analysis offered several insights into the effects of ad attitudes. The
attitude,
attitude
was not
&
Stayman, 1992).
54
However, the
path
may
& Stayman.
1992).
Moderator analyses yielded useful findings. Although Peter and Churchill (1986)
suggest that multiple-item scales should be more reliable for measuring ad attitude,
results indicated that multi- versus single-item scales
had
little
effect
on average
correlations.
Significance
for
Additionally, student samples were found to produce large effects sizes, especially
between ad
attitude
in
effects sizes led the authors to caution against generalization to other populations
(Brown
& Stayman,
1992).
ways. Use of novel brands in studies tended to produce stronger relationships between ad
attitude
and brand attitude and purchase intentions. Familiar brands tend to weaken the
& Stayman,
1992).
and brand cognitions when products other than consumer nondurables were used
studies.
in the
that increased
priced durable products (e.g., automobile, and refrigerator) might account for the stronger
relationship.
that
more accurate
generalizations.
advertising
in other
55
material,
to the ad).
on
attitudes"
(Brown
& Stayman,
print ads, the
1992,
p.
nonimbedded
feelings
ad.
and instructions
to
in smaller effects
of
on ad
attitude.
ad attitudes and the downstream constructs of brand cognitions, brand attitudes, and purchase intentions" (Brown
& Stayman,
1992,
p. 47).
earlier
results
(Madden, Allen,
& Twible,
(Brown
& Stayman,
1992,
attitude relationship
large
effects sizes
within-subjects design produced the opposite effect on the ad attitude and feelings
relationship
(Brown
& Stayman,
1992).
between ad
attitude
and
(i.e.,
model (Brown
&
Stayman, 1992).
56
research examined in the meta-analysis dealt mainly with evaluative judgments. Other
researchers have examined the significant contribution of affective responses
on ad
of affective
attitudes.
Batra and
Ray
(1
firsts
responses on ad attitudes. Stayman and Aaker (1988) demonstrated that the affective responses of warmth, amusement, and irritation could affect attitude toward the brand
without affecting ad attitudes. Edell and Burke (1987) reported that feelings created by
the actual advertisement are, by themselves, useful in explaining advertising effects.
Such
more than an
scales
in
brand
attitude,
attitudes.
among
feelings elicited
judgments of the
ads' characteristics,
in
& Edell,
989, p.
and
indirectly affect
(Burke
& Edell,
& Ray,
1986; Edell
&
& Aaker,
57
on
attitudes
(1995) used more natural settings and included facial expression measures.
on the following
variables: attitude
attitude.
in
room) using a
real
program and
real
Affective reactions were measured through facial expressions and traditional verbal
measures. Verbal measures indicate that affective responses contribute to ad attitude and
facial
measures do
not. Findings
show
that unfamiliar
brands are more influenced by verbal affective reactions generated by the advertisement.
(facial
postexposure brand attitude. The results indicate that verbal measures are better
indicators
of verbal
is
Research Questions
This study explores the effects of media literacy training on children's attitudes
toward specified products within the framework of the Elaboration Likelihood Model
(ELM) of persuasion
(Petty
& Cacioppo,
1986a; Petty
& Cacioppo,
1986b)
in
order to
is
the question:
58
effects
of media
literacy training
on
children's attitudes
toward persuasive
messages?" While
this
Does media
central route?
2)
What
role
on
The
first
ELM as a whole.
They suggest
that
media
in
we
suspect that any variable that increases the likelihood that people
and able to engage in the difficult task of evaluating the message arguments increases the likelihood of the central route to persuasion. On the other hand, any variable that reduces a person's motivation and/or ability to think about the message content would make the peripheral route more likely.
will be motivated
To
of media
of
children following the central route to persuasion for an advertised product the following
HI
Media
H2:
No media
of children following
59
of media
that
literacy
on changing
children's attitude
is
of
H3: Media
literacy training, as
compared
to
when many
strong
H4: Media
literacy training, as
compared
to
no
when few
strong
H5: Media
literacy training, as
compared
to
no
H6: Media
literacy training, as
compared
to
CHAPTER 3
METHODS
An Overview
A2
to
test the
central focus
of the
by which
if
media
As
discussed in Chapter
1,
training will
1989;
Kahn
& Master,
al.,
media
literacy training
on
children's decision-
alcohol.
of persuasive content
is
media
literacy training
was
identified as
an effective means of
60
61
Subjects
fifth
grade classes
at
in
A for the
grades.
As an
who
gift certificate
drawing.
of
$25, $15, and $10. Additionally, the class in each grade that returned the most parental
consent forms was awarded an ice cream party. Furthermore, a substantial monetary
made
and distributed
evenly
among
The
total
number of subjects
1,
1999 and
fifth
2,
Proce dure
The same procedure was followed on both days of testing. Students numbered off
by twos
in their
homeroom
class.
Nye
the Science
Guy" and
Appendix
that
Each card
also
go to the
62
5
.
Afterwards, students
filled
for the
questionnaire) that
how
to
each page, subjects were instructed to turn to the next page when they were told to do
The
first
page, which included basic demographic questions, instructed students that they
the
to
respond as best that they could. The second page asked students to
their thoughts
about the commercial and then go back and rate those thoughts as positive or negative.
The
third
to
list
The
fourth and fifth pages included measures of attitude toward the product and attitude
six
that
measured subjects'
in general.
manipulation checks for the media literacy training lesson while pages
questions about peripheral cues contained in the commercial.
and
1 1
asked
The
last
final
page served as a
which asked
if
Nye
the Science
Guy"
There were only two rooms available for use when the
student
was
sent to
in the
fifth grade students were tested. Therefore, each room, approximately half of the students were sent
outside while one version of the Pringles Potato Chips commercial aired.
The students
in the
classroom
to
questionnaires were used to ensure that the version of the commercial corresponded with the teacher's
Colored questionnaires corresponded with level of argument quality and number of arguments commercial as well as whether subjects were in the control or experimental group.
in
each
63
filling
all
items were
completed, subjects placed the index card they had received inside their questionnaire
and the teacher collected them. Students were debriefed and thanked for their
participation in the study.
Product of Interest
A pretest
was conducted
to identify the
in the
of interest
pretest).
in
personal relevance
was desired
Eighteen subjects
rate 21
aged 8-12 years old were administered a 23-page questionnaire that asked them to
first
information and provided examples of how to complete the form. The same 12 items
all
21 products. The
first six
The
scale
was highly
reliable,
Measure Involvement (GSMI), which was developed by Tray lor and Joseph( 984) 9
1
The
well (a
.88).
chips were identified as a product that elicited moderate attitudes but high involvement.
Therefore, a potato chips commercial
was sought.
The
ELM purports that subjects who have the motivation and ability to scrutinize a message will
message about the product.
Initial
It
is
produce the
ability necessary to
follow the central route. Subjects must have moderate attitudes so that
strong positive or strong negative attitudes
'
64
The Commercial
Several commercials were reviewed and a nationally broadcast Pringles Potato
10
was
'
is
"Once
stop."
To
test the
levels
The
original
off-air
on high
quality videotape.
of Florida
Design
received media literacy training or watched the control video about caves. Subjects in
both the control and experimental groups viewed one of the four versions of the Pringles
Potato Chips commercial.
1) strong
argument
10
was necessary
to effectively manipulate
arguments.
There were approximately 1 6 males and 4 females depicted in the commercial. The racially diverse characters (African American, Asian, and Caucasian) were seen in various costumes including a caveman, pirate, deserted islander, and astronaut.
'
65
quality x high
number of arguments;
low number of
arguments: 3) weak argument quality x high number of arguments; and 4) weak argument
quality x
vs.
weak
vs.
experimental
No media
Weak
literacy training
Media
literacy training
Few arguments
Strong
Weak
Many arguments
Strong
X2X2
factorial
experimental design.
To
among
Random
assignment to one of the four groups was employed to be able to infer treatment-caused
changes
in this study.
is vital
to equate control
can rule out dangerous threats to internal validity, including testing, instrumentation,
regression, and the interaction
it
creates
common
statistical
models. However,
threats to validity.
that randomization
all
'"
in
in
television production.
66
the best
available
method
From
these groups, subjects were assigned to the four groups representing the
levels
table.
Based on the
was
The
media
literacy training.
attitude
toward
toward
and attention
Table
3.1).
Table
3.1.
Independent Variables
1 )
Dependent Variables
1)
Argument
quality
2) 3)
Number of arguments
Media
literacy training
Favorable thoughts
b) 2)
3)
Unfavorable thoughts
4)
5)
TV
advertising
67
Independent Variables
To
test
selected commercial
follows.
Argument Quality
Manipulation of argument quality
either a strong
.
Argument
quality
was operationalized
as
explain that
when
will
always be
some
differences.
framework of the
ELM
is
to develop
or weak. Following the steps suggested by Petty and Cacioppo (1981a, 1986b), this
study generated "strong" and "weak" arguments for use in the Pringles Potato Chips
commercial.
Pilot-testing
arguments
either strong or
A group of subjects
similar
to those
in the actual
in
a pilot
test.
responding to a four-item semantic differential scale (see Appendix F for the pilot-test
questionnaire).
common
(Osgood, 1965). To measure the quality of arguments, subjects were asked to rate each
68
3
)
on four dimensions
14
:
it;
Doesn't
make sense/Makes
sense;
argument quality-rating
test.
Subjects
The mean score was calculated by averaging responses on the four dimensions used
rate
to
each argument. The median score was 4.75 with means ranging from
to 7.
The
means
were selected as strong arguments. To assess whether the strongest and weakest
arguments were significantly different from one another, a multiple analysis of variance
among means
(F
161)
5.86,
p<
.000). Pairwise
(M
(M =
opposite direction.
Weak argument
eight
(M =
(M =
weak and
six strong
arguments
were used to develop voice-over copy for the Pringles Potato Chips commercial.
Persuasiveness of commercial
comparison, the persuasiveness of each commercial version was assessed using the same
13
numbers
and
7,
represented a very strong feeling on the negative (bad) side and positive (good) side, respectively.
Numbers 2 and 6
5 indicated fairly
weak
and positive side, respectively. Numbers 3 and and positive side, respectively, and number 4 represented
in
order to
age-appropriate.
make sense/Makes
15
After testing for persuasiveness. Petty and Cacioppo (1986b) suggest testing for believability with another panel of subjects in order to develop strong and weak arguments "that do not strain credulity"
134).
(p.
However, due
to a limited
believability
tested at the
same
time.
69
four-item semantic differential scale used in the pilot-test for argument quality (see page
12 of the questionnaire in Appendix D). This measure focused
on the
overall perceived
persuasiveness of the commercial rather than the level of quality of each individual
argument.
Number of Arguments
Using the theme of the Pringles Potato Chips commercial ("Once you pop, the fun
don't stop") and the pilot-test of argument quality, four voice-overs were written. Petty
and Cacioppo (1986b) suggest that number of arguments can be used as a peripheral cue
to persuasion.
They
number of arguments
(6-8) and a
low
number of arguments
(1-3) (Petty
& Cacioppo,
used to represent a high number and two arguments represented a low number.
Two
items served as a
commercial. The
first
item asked
if the
if the
are as follows:
can't
That's because Pringles Potato Chips taste great. They're so delicious, and
they crunch
taste,
Once
you pop,
70
Strong argument quality x low number of arguments Once you pop. the fun
:
won't stop! That's what happens when you pop open a can of Pringles Potato
Chips. Pringles are really great snacks. They're less greasy than other chips, and
today.
Weak argument
stop!
quality x high
can't
That's because Pringles Potato Chips are good for you. They're really
fat
snacks. Pringles are fun to eat because they stay in one piece.
like
bags of chips. Pop open a can of Pringles today. Once you pop, the fun don't
stop!
Weak argument
stop!
quality x
good
recall, subjects
were asked
down
as
many arguments
remember from
the Pringles
Potato Chips commercial they saw. Subjects were given three minutes to complete the
task.
Three
reliability
was high (a =
.94).
Recall items were coded as correct (1) if they appeared in the appropriate
71
(0).
Summing
the
overall
argument
recall score.
As
in the
Pringles commercial.
One
item asked
if the
commercial presented a
lot
of reasons
for
eating Pringles, while the other asked if the commercial presented few reasons for eating
Pringles.
lecture
products and
how to
weak arguments
1)
in
students will
be able to identify persuasive techniques used by advertisers and 2) students will be able
to distinguish
in
Consumer
Reports'
Buy Me
The
three half-hour
to 12, specifically)
by television advertisers
These programs provide parents and children with helpful hints for managing the sales
pitch
and Johnson
( 1
997a; 1997b)
used the
of these videos
in their
literacy training
on
The
selected broadcast
commercials were used to reinforce and help the subjects apply the principles introduced
72
in the
Buy
Me
minute media literacy training session while the other 75 subjects watched an episode of
"Bill
Nye
the Science
in
content.
Subject Variables
According to the
ELM.
in
persuasion, the person must be motivated to process a message and have the ability to do
so (Petty
& Priester,
1994).
By design,
all
of
of
upon
their level
because of the personal relevance of Pringles Potato Chips, which were used
experiment.
in the
As noted above,
personal relevance
General Scale to Measure Involvement with Products (GSMI) developed by Traylor and
GSMI
is
a six-item scale
& Mobley,
1993,
p. 141).
The
scale
is
is
unidimensional.
Summing
The
the
GSMI
one
16
,
score (Bearden et
al.,
1993).
six
are as follows: 1)
When
me
lot
about a
express
person by seeing what brand of this product he uses; 3) This product helps
me
The
original item
an opinion of me."
73
who
lot
is
this
product
tells
me
When I
me
the
way
want them
to see
me.
Ability to process
as
media
thereof.
Level of subject ability was established by whether or not they received training.
Subjects were asked questions on the posttest about their media literacy training as a
first
persuasive tactics, editing, camera angles, humor, color, strength of claims, attractiveness
latter eight
items
were techniques
17
potentially used
by advertisers
to convince
consumers
to
buy products.
Subjects were asked to indicate which methods they believed were used by advertisers.
Three bogus items were included to clearly distinguish lesson content from irrelevant
information.
I8
The
scale
tested.
Dependent Variables
Active cognitive processing
was operationalized
as favorable
and unfavorable
1981).
& Petty,
Twelve
six- inch
all
of
task.
Afterward, subjects were asked to rate their thoughts as good, bad, or not sure by
The
The bogus items were bad language, ugly people, and mad
people.
74
circling
in the
their thoughts.
Research
on
attitude
change suggests there are three dimensions that characterize the classification
of responses (Cacioppo
& Petty,
1981).
The
polarity dimension,
which
is
comprised of
was used
in this
experiment. Three
independent coders
who were
listings.
Coders
reliability
(a =
An
overall
attitude score
of the stimulus
(Pringles Potato Chips commercial), and statements of positive affect about the product
and/or commercial. Example of favorable thoughts include, "I want some," "I
am
mere
recall items.
& Petty,
1981).
commercial), and statements of negative affect about the product and/or commercial.
include, "It
"Pringles are not the best." Thoughts could not be mere recall items. There should be
"
Interestingly,
when
in
75
evidence of a negative disposition toward Pringles Potato Chips and/or the commercial
for Pringles
(Cacioppo
& Petty,
1981
).
is
getting married," "Dancing," and "Pirates." Neutral thoughts positive or negative disposition
show no evidence of a
toward Pringles Potato Chips and/or the commercial for Pringles (Cacioppo
1981).
& Petty.
was measured by a
differential scale.
The
scale
is
to
Stayman and
Batra (1991) reported Cronbach alphas of .90 and .94 for an eight-item and ten-item
version of the scale, respectively. Sample items included bad/good,
unfavorable/favorable, disagreeable/agreeable, unpleasant/pleasant, negative/positive,
low quality/high
quality,
and
worthless/valuable.
was measured by
Several researchers have used varying versions of the scale to measure consumers'
20
attitude
toward an advertisement
(Burke
& Edell,
1989; Edell
& Burke,
1987; Wells,
Leavitt,
& McConville,
97 1 Zinkhan, Locander,
;
& Leigh,
1986). Previous
2
'
to indicate
how
well a
list
20
in
21
Informal testing revealed subjects did not comprehend the term "valuable" well enough to include
it
in
14 items to
3.
76
of words describes the advertisement they are exposed to by placing a number next to the
word. For example,
if
a subject believes a
word
the ad at
all
places a
"
was changed
to
make
it
consistent
with the other scales used to measure attitude toward advertising in general. The
items included (1) Not believable/Believable; (2)
list
of
Not
for
me/For me;
(3)
Not
Not meaningful/Meaningfiil
to
me;
(7)
Phony/Not phony;
(1 1)
Did not
like
ad/Liked ad; (12) Did not enjoy ad/Enjoyed ad; and (13) Did not find ad to be
to
good/Found ad
be good.
was adapted
it
was appropriate
for the
were
measured by the
to
first set
of items.
23
The
list
included: (1)
TV advertising
is
a good
way
available; (2)
TV advertising results
in
24
;
(3) in general,
TV
picture
you can
trust
TV
more
22
The original scale included 10-items, but this item, "TV advertising helps raise our standard of living," was eliminated because it was determined subjects would not comprehend "standard of living.''
23 24
The
original item
77
on
TV25
(5)
TV advertisements help me
find products
that
match
my
TV
advertising helps
26
;
me know which
product
am
looking for
(7)
TV
advertising gives
me
good idea about products by showing the kinds of people who use them;
advertising helps
(8)
TV
to
me buy
27
;
and (9)
am willing
pay
more
on TV.
that
The second
information.
28
set
TV
Alwitt and Prabhaker's (1992) scale included the following items: (1)
most
TV
make me
feel a certain
way
(e.g.,
29
;
(2) today's
TV
much
the people
and
(3) today's
TV
ads
tell
who
use a product than about what the product does for you.
The
deceptive.
32
third
and
final set
that
TV
advertising
is
TV ads try to
work on
(affect) people's
emotions (feelings)
(2) there
is
TV
advertising;
25
original item
trust
brands advertised on
TV more than
26
original item
was "TV
advertising helps
me know which
am
looking for."
27
original item
28
29
The The
original item
was "Most
TV ads today TV
30
original item
was "Today
much
" The
original item
was "Today's
TV
ads
tell
who
33
The
original item
was "Most
on people's emotions."
78
(3)
TV
life
as
it
really
is;
and (4)
TV
series
of questions
(i.e..
Two
questions,
cue of interest
if the
for
As noted
two items
also
were used as a
if
wanted were
had friends
like
attractive.
credibility
if
knew
lot
about Pringles,
if
truth.
statistics
of the data
reliabilities
of the scales will be discussed and the hypotheses will be tested using
(ANOVA).
34
CHAPTER 4
RESULTS
Analysis of variance
to test effects
of argument quality,
on
attitude
toward
TV
the participating
and
reliabilities
of the
Demographic Data
One hundred
(n
Of those, 54
percent
= 68) were
to 12
(M =
10,
SD =
.79).
in the fourth
fifth graders.
percent Asian, and the remaining four-percent reported being Native American or Other.
The Scales
This study relied primarily on six measures for the
initial tests
of hypotheses.
Four
scales, attitude
attitude
79
80
35
,
Two
other scales were used as manipulation checks for the independent variables. Four
peripheral cues and the media literacy training manipulation check were not previously
tested.
A reliability analysis,
correlation coefficients
in
inter- item
internal consistency
of
all scales.
Scale properties for the media literacy manipulation check and attention to
peripheral cues also were scrutinized via factor analysis. Factor analysis
is
a procedure
if more basic,
& Cormier,
Principal
to analyze the
scales.
factor scores
Attitude
Toward
The
attitude
is
attitude
toward a
of the ten-item
scale
of the
scale.
was
retained as a
measure of attitude toward the product. The average inter-item correlation for the
items
was
.47.
81
Attitude
Toward
the
Ad
Scale
Reliability analysis
of the
attitude
reliable
scale (a
.92).
Deleting any of the items would have decreased the reliability. The
mean
inter-item correlation
was
.47.
The 13-item
scale
was
ad.
Toward
TV
Advertising Scale
TV
advertising in general.
reliability.
Average inter-item
was
.33.
A three-item scale that measured perceptions that TV advertising does not offer
information had a relatively low reliability (a
.34).
(a =
.38).
Mean
inter-item correlation
was
.15.
Given
was
discarded.
TV
advertising
is
reliability.
.27.
Scale
was used
to
measure
the effectiveness
of the media
in
literacy lesson.
Principal
components
two
.0.
Seven of the
TV
advertising
is
deceptive.
82
first factor,
Eigenvalue of 3.9. Six items loaded on the second factor, which explained 19.2 percent
of the variance and had an Eigenvalue of 2.5. The seven items that loaded on the
factor had a reliability
first
seven-
item scale was retained. The six items that loaded on the second factor had a reliability
of .75 and a mean inter-item correlation of .34. Although deletion of one item would
have increased the
reliability slightly
(a =
was not
significant.
The
six-
Attention to Peripheral
Cue
Scale
of the manipulation of
analysis with varimax rotation produced one factor that explained 60.1 percent of the
variance (Eigenvalue
.2).
Reliability analysis
.34,
which
is
likely
due
to
The
correlation of the
two
items
was
.20.
of interest
scale.
components
83
two
.0.
first
on the second
factor.
The
The second
.7
of the four-item
scale,
that addressed
how well
of .76.
.44.
lower alpha. Therefore, the four-item scale was retained as one additional measure of
attention to peripheral cues. Reliability analysis
of the three-item
scale,
which included
items that assessed the credibility of the commercial announcer, revealed a Cronbach's
mean
inter-item correlation
all
was
retained as a second
additional measure
of attention
scale
Manipulation Checks
of
media
84
exposed to the lesson; they watched and discussed a television program about caves.
It
was suggested
that subjects
exposed
to
media
literacy training
ability
ability
the
Cacioppo,
986b). In contrast, subjects not exposed to the media literacy lesson would
have
would be more
(Petty
& Cacioppo,
To
test
1986b).
to
literacy lesson
36
3 statements
about
to the
media
literacy lesson
were expected
to score higher
on
exposed.
As noted above,
media
literacy
factor analysis
and
reliability analysis
An
independent-samples
found no
significant difference
item scale
(t
(142)
-.80,
p<
(t
(139)
-1.64,
p=
.10).
was
unsuccessful.
Argument Quality
Argument
(weak
test
quality
the level
of argument quality
vs. strong)
within the four versions of the Pringles Potato Chips commercial. Pilot
5.86,
p < .000)
that the
arguments
* Although
media
sixteen items
were presented
to subjects, only 13
were used
of the
literacy lesson
85
used in the commercial versions were considered strong or weak by subjects similar to
those
who
Number of Arguments
Subjects in the different treatment groups were exposed to varying numbers of
arguments within the versions of the Pringles Potato Chips commercials. Therefore,
(six)
or a low number
of arguments
in the various
commercial versions.
attention to the
It
was expected
control group
in the
commercial than the quality of the arguments. In contrast, subjects group would not pay as much attention to the number of arguments;
scrutinize the quality
experimental
rather, they
would
of the arguments.
to
To
test
lot of)
lot of)
(arguments/claims) for eating Pringles Potato Chips." Subjects' responses to the second
to a
were expected
exposed
to
to score higher
on the
first
An
independent samples
subjects
t-
to a high
86
Mean
among
number (M =
4.78,
(144)
-.78,
The two-item
scale
produced a low
this
Further analysis compared the means of subjects on the single item. "There are
many
(a lot)
among
subjects
(M =
(145)
-1.57,
g=
.117).
When means of
subjects' responses
item,
(not a lot of) reasons (arguments/claims) for eating Pringles Potato Chips," no significant
difference
was found (M =
(145)
.17,
g<
.87).
These results confirm that the manipulation check for number of arguments was not
successful.
As mentioned
peripheral cues.
earlier,
two
different scales
One-way
if
which assessed
difference
among
.021).
the experimental
(M =
3.1 1)
and control
(M =
5.43,
p=
(M
= 3.08) and
control
g = .008)
of the announcer.
87
Persuasiveness of Commercial
As
was assessed
for
potential comparisons.
test,
was
employed. However, subjects were not asked what they thought about each individual
argument; rather, they were asked
how
Pringles Potato Chips commercial was. Subjects rated the overall commercial
on the
three-way
significant
(M =
(M =
3.79) groups (F
(1,
123)
p=
.002).
No
interactions or additional
main
effects
were found.
Tests of Hypotheses
To
test the
"Does media
HI
Media
H2:
No media
of children following
88
Stated concisely,
it
was expected
would
follow the central route to persuasion while subjects in the control condition would
follow the peripheral route.
To
test
= 73)
were compared
one-way
revealed
and control
(M =
1-03)
(1,
.73,
g>
.39).
Therefore, Hypothesis
The same
ANOVA results
and control
among
the experimental
(M = 4.50)
(M =
1.22,
p=
.271). Thus,
either.
of media
literacy training
on changing
children's attitude
that
was of great
personal relevance.
The hypotheses
H3: Media
literacy training, as
compared
to
no
when many
strong
H4: Media
literacy training, as
compared
to
no
when few
strong
89
H5: Media
literacy training, as
compared to no
compared
to
no
it
was expected
between treatment
(media
argument
It
quality,
was thought
attitude
exposed to
media
literacy training
it
was believed
attitude
that
media
literacy training
measures when weak arguments were used despite the number of arguments.
To
of arguments)
way
interactions.
interesting
findings.
A two-way
was found
by
number of arguments (M =
7,
M=
-89; control
x low number;
M=
-92,
-39,
8.71,
p=
.004).
Main
effects
were found
for treatment
group
121)
(M = 5.21,
18.29,
control;
(1,
toward the ad
(M = 4.73,
control;
M = 3.59,
90
18
1.6
1.4
(U
o o
CO
<D
1.2
1
T3
0.92
0.8
| <
0.6
04
0.2
ft39-
Control
Experimental
Treatment Group
Figure
4.
Two-way
interaction
91
experimental; F
( 1
1 1
6.82,
p<
.000).
for treatment
group on the perceptions of the personal and social benefits or costs of TV advertising
scale
(M =
4.07, control;
133)
16.56,
p<
.000),
which
scales that
toward
TV
advertising in general.
However,
TV advertising
is
attitude
toward
TV
main
group
that
(M = 4.89,
control;
M = 5.06, experimental; F
(1,
136)
.47,
p<
.50).
Both
scales
group
(M =
3.77, control;
M = 3.13, experimental; F
F
(1,
(1,
139)
= 4.69, p =
.032;
M = 3.80, control; M =
3.07, experimental;
139)
7.21,
p=
.008).
A main effect was found for argument quality on attitude toward the product (M =
4.95, strong quality;
(1,
121)
= 4.76, p is
.03), attitude
toward
advertising
deceptive)
(M =
5.30, strong
quality;
p.
.007),
and attention
to
an
(M =
M=
3.05,
weak
quality;
(1,
139)
7.51,
p=
.007).
A summation of the
is
presented in the
next chapter, along with a discussion of the results and limitations of the study. The
chapter will conclude with practical implications and suggestions for future research.
CHAPTER 5
DISCUSSION
More
specifically,
determine
Two
in all,
were presented
five
in detail in
Chapter 2
to
and media
literacy training.
tested
processing, attitude toward the product, attitude toward the advertisement, attitude toward
TV
tests
below. Post-hoc analyses designed to provide insight into the findings follows the
summary. Study
of the research.
92
93
Summary of Results
Tests of Hypotheses
The
training
first
literacy
persuasion and no exposure to media literacy training would increase the likelihood that
subjects will follow the peripheral route to persuasion.
It
was predicted
that
media
literacy training
arguments more
first
critically,
thus using central processing. However, no differences were found for the
two hypotheses.
on changing children's
attitudes
toward a
that a
relevance.
It
was predicted
argument
subjects.
quality,
As with
in attitude
were
found for subjects based on level of treatment, argument quality, or number of arguments.
In sum. the relationships predicted about the effectiveness of media literacy
training
on children's responses
to persuasive
Elaboration Likelihood
showed
that there
of treatment,
argument
A summary of the
findings
is
presented
in the
next section.
94
Post-Hoc Analysis
when
A two-way
interaction
was found
for treatment
8.71,
p=
.004).
showed
that
(M =
1.89) had
more
favorable thoughts while watching the Pringles Potato Chips commercial than other
control group and experimental group subjects. Experimental subjects exposed to a high
number of arguments (M =
exposed
to a
-86)
(M =
.51)
to a high
number of arguments (M =
result
of media
one might
expect the number of favorable thoughts to be less than control group subjects. However,
the findings reveal that control group subjects exposed to a high
number of arguments
had
less favorable
arguments produced more favorable thoughts for experimental subjects, while a low
Therefore,
is difficult to
suggest.
Both manipulation
checks for these findings were unsuccessful; therefore, no definitive conclusions can be drawn.
Main
attitude
toward the ad, and perceptions of the personal and social benefits or costs of TV
advertising.
These
media
literacy training
95
children's attitudes.
The
more
commercial than
as well.
One
is
that
media
literacy training
made
subjects
skeptical
of the message and the commercial because they were more aware of the
techniques used by advertisers to try and persuade viewers. The attitude toward the ad
attitude
product and vice versa. These two measures suggest that experimental subjects had
Two
in
TV
would not be
in
is
It
was expected
that there
any differences between groups on the dependent variable, attitude toward advertising
general, because the
that both
groups thought
TV
advertising
was deceptive (M =
4.89, control;
M = 5.06,
(M =
3.04) had
4.07).
that the
may have
it
commercial suggested.
96
Subjects diverged on their perceptions of the personal and social benefits or costs
is
may have
occurred. Responses from subjects exposed to media literacy training might have been
Given the
for attitude
results,
it is
difficult to
draw
measures
toward advertising
in general.
Interestingly,
main
of interest was number of arguments, other items were included that measured
subjects liked the kids in the Pringles commercial and the credibility
how much
of the announcer.
(M =
3.
(M =
to
be
less credible
(M =
(M =
3.80).
These
be used to offer support for the hypotheses that suggested that media literacy training
that a lack
attention to
potential areas
of future
a moderating variable in
to liking the
more
97
Main
attitude
effects
were found
for
argument quality on
attitude
toward advertising
TV
advertising
deceptive).
(M =
4.95) had
more favorable
attitudes
toward Pringles Potato Chips than subjects exposed to weak quality arguments
4.42). This finding suggests that the strong
(M =
arguments contained
in the
commercial
to
arguments of a strong
quality.
quality
produced very
distinct strong
found among the groups provide additional evidence that the quality of the arguments
were
distinctively strong
and weak.
for
one of the
is
attitude
toward advertising
is
in
TV
advertising
deceptive)
intriguing.
Strong
quality
argument subjects
(M =
5.30)
felt
TV
advertising
quality
(M = 4.65).
This finding
is difficult
one
would expect
favorable attitudes.
The findings
felt
weaker arguments
that advertising
was
less
is
needed to determine
if the results
how
in
are an anomaly.
An odd
finding
emerged
for
to peripheral
cues measures
(M =
3.85) liked
more than
weak arguments (M =
3.05).
fairly
weak
98
commercial.
One
is
that subjects
exposed
to
weak arguments
among
two groups
for
announcer
be an aberration.
Study Limitations
The Manipulations
for
media
literacy training
is
no
statistical
evidence to
the groups
made
a difference
among
of subjects.
Media
literacy training .
was
pretested
was not
as effective as expected.
One reason
for
the lack of effectiveness might be the setting of the lesson for the fourth graders (n
90).
was a
large
room without
desks.
used
in the
lesson initiated enthusiastic reactions, which included laughing, shouting, and physical
moved
(e.g.,
experimenter for doing so. Four subjects had to be separated for continuous talking and
physical contact. Therefore, the lack of a traditional classroom setting
may have
classroom teacher was not present during the presentation by the experimenter. All of the
99
The
presence of a teacher might have alleviated behavioral problems and helped focus
students' attention.
Fifth graders (n=58)
were exposed
a teacher present.
is
media
literacy training
scheduling restraints limited the amount of time that could be spent with the students.
future study should expose the students to
media
literacy training
Multiple exposures would be more like typical lessons taught in other subjects. Teachers
typically spend several class periods
on various
units.
An
of the
lesson.
Subjects were
taught media literacy training skills for 50-60 minutes. Perhaps an hour-long lesson for
fourth and fifth graders
is
items were directly related to the content of the lesson, the scale had not been previously
tested.
Factor analysis revealed that two dimensions were being measured by the scale
Further testing
is
items.
needed
to
A final explanation is that media literacy training does not work within the
context used in this study.
Media
literacy training
may
ELM. One
lesson did not provide the ability necessary to produce central processing within subjects.
100
in the
lesson
was designed
The need
is
is
it
may have
trained subjects to be
lesson
is
needed to determine
if content variations
are
First, the
number of arguments
if there
in the Pringles
asked
commercial.
A more appropriate
measure would have been a single self-report measure. Subjects could have been asked,
"How many
more
direct
arguments were
and easier to
in the
interpret.
is
that subjects
may
media
between
all
the claims
made
in the
& Cacioppo,
1984; Petty et
al.,
it
number of arguments.
The Scales
Several of the scales used in the experiment were previously used in other attitude
studies.
had high
reliabilities.
tested.
101
media
literacy training
cues scales. Factor analyses revealed more than one dimension being measured by the
items in the scales. Further scale development and testing
is
and reliable
scales.
The Subjects
The
contradictory results.
The
TV
advertising
is
deceptive
among
However, the
of the
call the
an
media
may have
influenced the
skeptical
more negative
among
the groups.
Implications
Despite the limitations of the study, the findings suggest that there are potential
differences between experimental and control subjects' attitudes toward the advertised
product, but definitive conclusions about media literacy training's effectiveness cannot be
drawn. However, results of this study can provide a foundation for future research.
102
First,
perhaps the manipulation check for media literacy training worked, but not
in the
way
expected.
It
was predicted
that
media
literacy training
critically
commercial messages
and objective manner. However, the findings suggest that media literacy training
may
have contributed to more negative attitudes for experimental subjects toward Pringles
Potato Chips as well as the commercial. This implies that the media literacy training
may
make
children
more
skeptical
indicate that
all
subjects,
whether exposed
be
deceptive. If this
is
It is
is
to
be skeptics.
The
overall goal
of media
literacy in
terms of advertising
to
make
children scrutinize
become
better consumers.
If children
can become
make
is
make other
decisions throughout
life.
is
the
the dominant
and
and
viewing
skills
by using
all
103
consumers; therefore,
to be critical thinkers.
it
makes sense
it
to use the
Therefore,
is
this
it
more
skepticism.
Second,
if a single
among groups
media
school curricula could be very effective at creating critical viewers. Although 48 of the
50
states already
(Kubey
& Baker,
more
efforts
literacy training
on
media
literacy
makes a
it
critically,
and
skills that
can be
Future Research
This study
a foundation for future experimental studies that will test the
is
on
Given the
results
to provide
more
definitive
literacy
training
on
Future research must focus on validating the media literacy training manipulation
check
scale.
A study could
manipulation check scale used in this study. Four groups of subjects could be presented
104
fill
fifth
group. Findings would validate the scale and provide evidence of its reliability or
needed to provide
statistical
does work.
By
more
definitive
One change
made
to the study is to
make
it
at the start
of the
to
expose subjects
to the
media
literacy lesson
more than
once.
A4x2
are
exposed to media
expect larger effects for subjects exposed for two weeks, but
it
would be
interesting to see
how
great the difference between groups might be. Furthermore, delayed posttest effects
if they
were
lasting or not.
Measuring
is
The
suggests media literacy training has an effect on children's attitudes toward persuasive
whether media
of persuasion.
literacy
is
Model
Parental Consent
Dear Parent/Guardian,
My name is Bradford L. Yates and I am a doctoral candidate in the College of at the University of Florida under the supervision of Dr. Debbie Treise, associate professor of advertising, conducting research on television commercials and children's attitudes toward consumer products. The purpose of this
Journalism and Communications
research study
is
of media
literacy training
on children's
responses to commercial advertising. Media literacy training helps students learn to evaluate more critically media messages. The results of this study may help teachers and
parents prepare children to evaluate
more
critically television
may
may
in the
lesson while the other half will watch and discuss an educational program, "Bill
will take approximately
Nye
the
Science Guy," which airs in syndication on selected broadcast channels. These activities
45-60 minutes. Children will then watch a commercial and answer several questions about its content, but they do not have to answer any question they do not wish to answer. Trained adult assistants will help your child fill out the questionnaire. This should take about 20 minutes. The activities will take place in the
same
location (school or
community
names on the questionnaires for matching purposes, be kept confidential to the extent provided by law. Their names will be replaced with code numbers. Results will only be reported in the form of group data.
children will be asked to write their
their identity will
Participation or non-participation in this study will not affect the children's grades or
placement
activities,
in any programs. Although this project is not part of the regular curriculum or your child will not need to make up any work. Non-participants will watch an
the right to
withdraw consent
for
your child's
no known risks or immediate No monetary compensation is offered for participation, but your child will receive a small item (pen or sticker) and a bookmark that highlights key
participation at any time without consequence. There are
points on how to become more media literate. Group results of this study will be May 2000 upon request. Results will be shared with media communication and education scholars at academic conferences and in academic journals. If you have any questions about this research project, please contact me at (352) 376-1874 or my
available in
105
106
Florida, Gainesville,
may be directed to the UFIRB office. Box FL 3261 1-2250; ph: 392-0433.
12250. University of
Sincerely,
Bradford L. Yates
voluntarily give
my
consent for
my
child,
children's attitudes.
Name
Signature
Date
Institutional
20,
2000
Caves
Question Guide for
"'Bill
Nye
The following questions are to be used to stimulate discussion about the video. Please allow the students to share their own thoughts about the video as much as possible. Discuss the video for 20-25
minutes.
in
a cave?
Allow students
to respond.
2.
What
is
a cave? (Answer:
A natural
hollow space
in
the earth)
3.
an underground space
formed? (Answer: Caves form wherever materials or rock or ice are carried away from stiff enough to support a roof. There are caves under ice, under water, and
4.
What
is
limestone? (Answer:
It is
is
made of ancient
seashells.
years of water flowing through the ground like a river, limestone gets washed
the surface and a cave forms.)
5.
What
and amphibians.
and they
live
mostly
in caves.)
What
is
the temperature
in
is
cool and
it
same
all
year round.
The weather
to be
7.
What
to
all starts
They are rocks formed by dripping water that is on the surface. Water drips through earth and leaves minerals behind
Stalactites hold tight to the ceiling. Stalagmites
form
stalactites
and stalagmites.
8.
What
These creatures
live outside
of the cave. Examples include bats, birds such as owls, bears, skunks, and moths. Bats sleep in caves during the day and fly out at night. Bears go into caves in the winter to hibernate (sleep). Birds, skunks, and moths live near the mouth of cave where there is a little bit of light.)
9.
What are troglophiles? (Answer: They are cave lovers. These creatures love the deep, dark, wet environment of a cave. Earthworms are troglophiles.) What
fish,
10.
are troglodytes? (Answer: They are true cave dwellers such as insects, spiders, amphibians, and crustaceans. These creatures can't live anywhere else. They typically have no color and are
often blind;
at all.)
107
media
have
greater ability than subjects not exposed resulting in the greater likelihood that the central
route of the
1.
2.
weak arguments
in
commercials
Introduction
will begin by introducing him/herself and explain to the students what going to happen for the next 45-50 minutes. He will introduce the topic by asking the
The experimenter
is
students if they like commercials and what their favorite ones are.
Do you
What
all like
to
are
some of your
Today we
methods, ways) advertisers use to make us want the products we see in their ads. Commercials can provide useful and helpful information but sometimes commercials leave things out about products or mislead us. We need
the techniques
,
to be able to tell
when
when they
leave
108
[09
things out.
One way
to help us be
more aware
is
to understand
how
advertisers
persuade or convince us to buy their products. We already know about some of the techniques (ways) advertisers use to persuade
us to buy their products because
we use them ourselves. Think about this: If you want your parents to buy you a Christmas gift don't you tell them about the best features of the toy and how great it will be for you to have it? You might even say that everyone else has it so you have to have it, too. Well, advertisers do the same thing when they try to persuade or convince us to buy their products. They tell us how great their products are and how wonderful we will feel if we use their
from you and I when we try to convince buy us something or do something for us. Therefore, since you about some of the methods (ways) that can be used to persuade, then you should be able to identify those techniques (ways) in commercials and make better decisions when you buy things. Let's talk about some of these techniques so you can look out for them when you watch commercials. Remember, the more you know about commercials, the better chance you have of
products. So, they are no different
someone
already
to
know
The experimenter will then play a brief montage of commercials and kids' reactions from the Consumer Reports' video, Buy Me Thai Too! as an attention-getting device. (Video clip includes the montage of commercials and kids' reactions as well as a closer look at the Typhoon II remote controlled car commercial). After the video, the experimenter will ask the students the following questions and take a few minutes to listen to responses.
What do you think about the Typhoon II after seeing the video clip? What were some of the persuasive techniques (or ways) the advertiser used make the Typhoon II look so appealing? (Take a few moments and let the students respond)
to
will begin
how much
sugar
with edited clips from Buy Me That! and Buy Me Thai 3!, The first part of the clip explores how advertisers sell some of the things advertisers do not include in their commercials is in cereal). The second part of the clip is a series of children's
head bites talking about cereal commercials and the nutritious value of advertised cereals.
The
third
segment
eat
is
Do you
(Show
Candy
clip
and then
listen to
what
let
110
"A
fruity part
balanced breakfast,
breakfast, right?
So, you learned that even though the commercial says the cereal is part of a we don't even have to eat the cereal to have a nutritious
Well, this
is
just
try
and use
to get us to
want
Do you
Well,
in
all
few words)
know what an argument is? It is very important to understand this. terms of commercials, an argument (or claim) is a statement (or just a that is used to make you like the product and hopefully buy it. For
"A
fruity part
of a good breakfast."
to look for
am going
to play
want you
arguments or
claims
made about
(Show
or claims
made
in this
commercial?
on a
Very good! You have listed (#) arguments in the commercial. Do you think it better to have lots of arguments in the commercial or is it better to have just a
few? (Allow students to respond) Well, lots of arguments can be good, but what if the arguments are not strong ones? Would it be better to have a few really strong claims about the product
rather than lots
is
found
in the Tartar
came up
with.
Is
new
only
2) 3)
Is the
mouthwash
up
Has
the
4)
5) 6)
Kills
Is better
same germ killing power as regular Listerine germs that cause bad breath, plague, and the gum disease gingivitis than brushing alone (when compared to regular toothpaste)
starts,
What brushing
Can we be
up?
it
to
back up
claim? (Does
show
several
of teeth
that
have
it
germs
that cause
) )
11!
isn't
enough"
(Allow students
advertisement)
to
respond to these queries, then show another commercial and how strong the claims are as they scrutinize the
Let's
made
watch another commercial. This time pay close attention to the claims being in the commercial and try and figure out if they are really strong arguments weak ones. Also, pay attention to how many claims are made. (Show Swiffer Sweeper commercial)
(claims) or pretty
What were the claims the advertiser made about Were they strong claims or weak claims? How many claims were made?
(Allow students to respond to these questions)
Swiffer Sweeper commercial claims
1
Simple
to
4)
and durable) it sweeps (easy to use) and trap hair, crumbs, and common allergies 5) Toss the cloth (disposable. .not a lot of extra work to use it) 6) Swiffer's the one, consider it done (meaning swiffer is all that you need)
2) 3)
It It
smashes
crashes
stunt
2) 3) 4) 5) 6)
7) 8)
The meanest
It It
You
It
It is It
flips
9) 10)
1
The madder it gets the badder it gets, until it throws an all out tantrum l)Use Tyco 6.0 V premium pack for maximum stunt power
12)
Maximum
heat
lots
of claims
in
it,
make the Tantrum seem really What were some of those techniques (ways)? (Allow students
112
Let's look at the commercial again. Listen to the driving (upbeat) music and how man speaks. Notice the quick cuts (a way of editing pictures together) from one scene to another. The commercial is very up beat and exciting, which makes you believe that the Tantrum will be just as exciting.
quickly the
This commercial
is
techniques (methods) to convince us to buy their products. The claims about the
Tantrum are not very strong, so the advertiser decided to use special editing and cool camera angles to make the Tantrum seem like a great toy. This remote controlled toy is probably not that different from other remote controlled toys, so
the advertiser tries to use these special
TV
camera angles)
toys.
to
make
the
Tantrum seem
Did the
Trix, Listerine,
techniques
Reinforce
we identified in the Tantrum commercial? (Allow students to respond. how other techniques (methods) can be used to persuade with or
we
learned so far about advertisers and commercials?
without arguments.)
So, what have
We've learned
the hope that
that advertisers
make arguments
we
and buy their products. They are trying We've also learned that there can
we have
learned that
commercials use other persuasive techniques with or without arguments. As commercial watchers and product buyers we need to pay close attention to the
arguments
quick cuts,
in
commercials and to the other persuasive techniques (e.g., music, colors, pacing) so we can make wise decisions about the products we
to buy.
This segment will focus on more methods (peripheral cues) besides arguments that
advertisers use to help sell products.
McDonald's commercial
the discussion
Buy Me That
3!
is
commercials about sports drinks will be shown. One of the key points of that sports drinks are often endorsed by star athletes, which implies that
will
perform
We know that commercials make claims about products, but are we always told what those claims are? (Show McDonald's commercial featuring Mark McGwire)
it mean when you see Mark McGwire of the McDonald's commercial?
What does
St.
Louis Cardinals
in
(Take a moment to
let
113
That's right.
We
if
we
eat at
it
a baseball like
this short
Mark McGwire.
clip.
know
McDonald's we would be able to hit hasn't worked for me. Take a look at
video
(Show video
clip
from Buy
Me
That 3!)
What
show us?
(Allow students
drink Gatorade
to respond)
all
important for us to
not
the
mean
same
that
because a celebrity
individual if we
drink.
Michael Jordan has several commercials. Let's look (Show Michael Jordan's MCI Long Distance)
Michael Jordan
batteries?
at
one of them.
is connected to a lot of products. Can you name some others? McDonald's, Gatorade, Nike, Hanes, Michael Jordan cologne, Ray-o-vac
Why do
is
in so
many commercials?
one of the most well known individuals in the world. Since most of us recognize him and like him, advertisers believe we would want to use the same products he does. But, we know that just because we use the same products as Michael, we "won't be like Mike." We will still be who we are. So,
them
for a reason.
As we
images, quick cuts, catchy music, and even humor. They also
make
the setting or
talk
at four
commercials and
Jr.)
a clip of Pepsi
Jif
AT&T commercial with David Arquette - Football) AT&T commercial with David Arquette - Hip-hop, bouncing car)
products and they also use quick cuts, eye-catching images, attractive
result in implied (i.e., they are not said) claims or
in
people, catchy music, bright colors, and really nice locations. These techniques
(ways or methods)
about the products
arguments
commercials.
It is
techniques and really think about what commercials are saying. Don't just accept
114
commercials' claims. Be sure to analyze the claims and think about them before deciding to buy products. Pay attention to the other persuasive techniques like
quick cuts and bright colors that might be used as well. Here's a little assignment for you... when you watch commercials count the number of claims made and
determine
if they are
we
will help
Thank you
for
your attention.
room where
number of arguments))
want
to
There are no right or wrong answers. Answer the questions as best you can. Just mark answers that reflect what YOU THINK!
Read and
Name:
School Grade:
How old
are vou?
Circle One:
Boy
White
Native American
Girl
Circle One:
Black
Hispanic
Asian
Other
ID#:
(the
nun
->
Wait
until
vou are
told to
do so
115
116
Write down all of the thoughts you had while watching the commercial about Pringles Potato Chips. You will have 3 minutes. Print your answers. Only write one thought per box. You don't have to use all of the boxes. After the 3 minutes, go back and tell me if your thoughts about the Pringles commercial are GOOD (positive) or BAD (negative) or NOT SURE (undecided). CIRCLE (C) or (B) or (NS) in each box.
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
NS
->
117
Write down as many arguments (claims) that you remember from the Cringles Potato Chips commercial. You will have 3 minutes. Print your answers. Only write one argument per box. You don't have to use all of the
boxes.
-^
118
number that
best represents
how you
feel
about
for:
that that
that
have a
have a have a
have a have a have a have
VERY STRONG FEELING on the positive (good) side STRONG FEELING on the positive (good) side
FAIRLY FAIRLY
WEAK FEELING on the positive (good) side WEAK FEELING on the negative (bad) side
side
STRONG FEELING on the negative (bad) side a VERY STRONG FEELING on the negative (bad)
think about Pringles Potato Chips based on the commercial you just saw? (Check one box on each line to indicate what you think)
1
What do you
Bad
Unfavorable
a a
Good
Favorable
Disagreeable
a a a
Agreeable
Unpleasant
Pleasant
Negative
Positive
Dislike
Like
Useless
a
a a a
Useful
Not
beneficial
Beneficial
Low quality
Worthless
High quality
Valuable
->
11"
number that
is
if you
think the ad
not at
all
believable,
check
1).
Be sure
Number 7 means Number 6 means Number 5 means Number 4 means Number 3 means Number 2 means Number 1 means
that
that
that that that
that
that
VERY STRONG FEELING on the positive (good) side have a STRONG FEELING on the positive (good) side
have a have a have a have a have a have
FAIRLY FAIRLY
STRONG FEELING on the negative (bad) side a VERY STRONG FEELING on the negative (bad)
What do you
Not believable
Not
for
Believable
me
For
me
Not informative
Not
interesting
Informative
Interesting
Irritating
a
a a a a a
LI
Not
irritating
Not meaningful
Meaningful
Phony
Ridiculous
Not phony
Not ridiculous
Terrible
Not
terrible
Worth remembering
Did not
like
ad
a a a a
Liked ad
Enjoyed ad
Did not
find
Found ad
be good
to
ad to be good
->
120
number that
best represents
TV advertising (commercials)
for:
that that
that
that that
that
that
VERY STRONG FEELING on the positive (good) side STRONG FEELING on the positive (good) side
FAIRLY
FAIRLY
STRONG FEELING on the negative (bad) side VERY STRONG FEELING on the negative (bad)
about what products and services are
4
5
TV advertising
available.
Disagree
is
a good
way
to learn
12Q
J
you and your family.
5
Agree
TV advertising
12
Disagree
Agree
In general,
TV advertising
Disagree
12J 12G
Agree
You can
trust
TV more
5
advertised on TV.
Disagree
U
match
5
Agree
TV advertisements help me
my
6
Disagree
12U
helps
Agree
TV advertising
am
looking
for.
me know which
Disagree
12U
Agree
TV advertising gives me a
who
use them.
Disagree
12
Agree
->
121
TV
advertising helps
me buy
1
Disagree
I
Q
pay more for a product that
2
3 4
is
a
advertised on
5
Agree
am
willing to
TV.
7
Disagree
Agree
feel a
ads today are not about products themselves, but just certain way (e.g., happy, excited, good).
3
Most
TV
make me
Disagree
G
as
a
much information
3
4
Agree
Today's
as they could.
6
Disagree
Agree
Today's TV ads tell you more about the people who use a product than about what the product does for you.
I
Disagree
Q
to
a
(affect) people's
Agree
Most
TV ads try
work on
1
emotions (feelings)
5
Disagree
a
more
truth in today's
4
Agree
There
is
TV advertising
5
Disagree
-J
Agree
TV commercials do
not show
2
life
as
it
really
is.
Disagree
Agree
TV advertising
lot alike.
mostly
tries to create
12
Disagree
Agree
->
122
Please check the box under the number that best represents disagree with the following statements.
for:
have a have a
have a have a have a
VERY STRONG FEELING on the positive good) side STRONG FEELING on the positive (good) side
(
FAIRLY FAIRLY
have a have a
Advertisers use some of the same persuasive methods (ways) to convince us to buy
products that
we
Disagree
12O
which
is
6
LI
Agree
Through
editing,
12
Disagree
Agree
to
make products
12
Disagree
Agree
Advertisers sometimes
feel
so
we
will
be
in a
12
Disagree
Agree
to
make products
stand out so
we
will notice
them
in
Disagree
12 Q
O
Agree
TV
Disagree
12O
Agree
->
[23
Advertisers try to
make products
them
12
Disagree
Q
will
Agree
TV
commercials make you believe that you if you use the product he/she does.
be
like the
person
in
the commercial
Disagree
12
Q
lots
Agree
TV commercials
Quick
edits (cut
persuade us to buy products in Which of the following ways (methods) are used
of ways.
in
TV commercials?
fast) 6
7
to
3
another really
4 5
Disagree
Agree
Bad language
1
Disagree
Agree
Catchy music
1
Disagree
a
1
Agree
Ugly people
2
3
Disagree
Agree
Humor (make
us laugh)
2
Disagree
a
i
a
3
Agree
Arguments
2
Disagree
a
i
Agree
Mad
people
2
3
Disagree
a
i
Agree
Famous people
2
3 4 5 6
Disagree
Agree
->
124
Dumber that
best represents
disagree with the following statements about the Pringles Potato Chips commercial you just
saw.
for:
that
that that
that
have a have a
have a
VERY STRONG FEELING on the positive (good) side STRONG FEELING on the positive (good) side
FAIRLY FAIRLY
STRONG FEELING on the negative (bad) side have a VERY STRONG FEELING on the negative (bad)
(a lot of) reasons
12
Disagree
Agree
12
Disagree
I
Agree
12
Disagree
I
Agree
would
like to
in
12
Disagree
I
Agree
have friends
Disagree
12Q 12
Agree
The
(pretty/handsome).
3
Disagree
Agree
->
125
The man
in
the commercial,
lot
about Pringles
Potato Chips.
12
Disagree
LI LI
I
4
LI
LI
LI
Agree
believe
what
the
man
in
12
Disagree
L)
4
LI
6 LJ
LI
LI
Agree
The man
in
is
12
Disagree
Agree
->
126
number to
indicate
how
Pringles Potato Chips commercial was. Persuasive means the commercial convincing arguments about Pringles Potato Chips.
made
for:
VERY STRONG FEELING on the positive good) side STRONG FEELING on the positive (good) side
(
FAIRLY
good) side
or the other
NEUTRAL FEELING
FAIRLY
way
3 means that you have a 2 means that you have a 1 means that you have a
STRONG FEELING on the negative (bad) side VERY STRONG FEELING on the negative (bad)
How
persuasive was the commercial for Pringles Potato Chips? (Check one box on
Doesn't make
Makes me
want Pringles
me want
Doesn't
sense
Pringles
make
Makes
sense
Not believable
a a 3
Believable
Weak
Strong
Guy today?
(Circle one)
Yes
No
Stop!
Good Job!
If you
want to know what you think about certain products... want to tell me what you think, please fill in the blanks below.
Name:
School Grade:
Girl:
best represents
number
how you
feel
Snack Cakes
Bad
1
Good
2
3
(4)
Unfavorable
1
Favorable
(5)
You
number
or disagree with 6
certain way.
Agree
3 5
(2)
Let's
Turn
127
128
Circle the
number
how
much you
represents
sentences below.
the
other people see me eating Snack Cakes, they think of me in a certain way.
When
Bad
Good
Disagree
Agree
4
5
12
12 12
12
You can
Disagree
Unfavorable
4
Favorable
tell
Disagreeable
Agreeable
12
Disagree
am.
12
Negative
Agree
Positive
12
Disagree
12
Dislike
Agree
Like
12
Go
to
12
7
tells
me
Disagree
Agree
3
12
When
the
I
eat
I
way
Disagree
12
Next Page
->
12')
Circle the
number
how
much you
Circle the
sentences below.
the
represents
When
way.
me wearing me in a certain
Bad
Good
3
12
12 12 12
Negative
Disagree
Agree
3
12
7
Unfavorable
4
Favorable
6
You can tell a lot about a person by seeing what brand of Sneakers he/she wears.
Disagree
Disagreeable
Agreeable
4
5 6 7
Agree
3
12
Disagree
Sneakers help
Unpleasant
4
Pleasant
me
express
who I am.
Agree
12
Disagree
Positive
12
Dislike
Agree
3
Like
4 6
12
12
me
Disagree
tells
Agree
3
12
I
others see
me
the
want them
to see
me.
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Next Page
130
2.
Video Games
number that best how you feel about
Circle the
number
how
much you
Circle the
sentences below.
the
represents
When
Good
me
playing Video
in a certain
way.
Bad
Disagree
Agree
12
12 12
12
You can
Disagree
Unfavorable
Favorable
tell
Disagreeable
Agreeable
4 6
Agree
3
12
Disagree
Unpleasant
Pleasant
me
express
who I am.
12
Negative
Agree
3
Positive
12
Disagree
12
Dislike
Agree
3
Like
4
12
Go
to
12
7
tells
Games
me
Top
Disagree of Next
Column
Agree
3
12
When
the
I
play Video
I
way
see
me
Disagree
12
Next Page
->
131
3.
Cereal
number that best how you feel about
above.
Circle the
number
how
much you
Circle the
sentences below.
the
represents
product
listed
When
me
eating Cereal,
they think of
me
in
a certain way.
Bad
Good
Disagree
Agree
12
12 12 12
Negative
12
You can
Disagree
Unfavorable
Favorable
tell
Disagreeable
Agreeable
4 6
7
Agree
3
12
Disagree
Unpleasant
Pleasant
Cereal helps
me
express
who 1 am.
Agree
Positive
12
Cereal
is
12
Dislike
"me".
Agree
Disagree
Like
12
12
lot
tells
me
Disagree
12
When
I
I
me
the
way
want them
me.
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Next Page
->
132
4.
Pizza
number that best how you feel about
Circle the
number
how
much you
Circle the
sentences below.
the
represents
When
Good
me
eating Pizza,
they think of
me
in
a certain way.
Bad
Disagree
Agree
3
12 12 12
12
Disagree
Unfavorable
Favorable
4
5
You can tell a lot about a person by what kind of Pizza he/she eats.
Agree
3
seeing
Disagreeable
Agreeable
4
6 7
12
Disagree
Unpleasant
Pleasant
Pizza helps
me
express
who I am.
Agree
12
Negative
Positive
12
Pizza
is
12
Dislike
"me".
Agree
3
Disagree
Like
4
12
12
7
lot
tells
me
Disagree
12
When
I
me
the
way
want them
Disagree
me.
Agree
3
12
Next Page
->
133
5.
Candy
number that best how you feel about
Circle the
number
how
much you
Circle the
sentences below.
the
represents
When
Good
me
eating Candy,
they think of
me
in
a certain way.
Bad
Disagree
Agree
3
12
12
Disagree
Unfavorable
Favorable
12
12
You can tell a lot about a person by what kind of Candy he/she eats.
Agree
seeing
Disagreeable
Agreeable
12
Candy
Disagree
Unpleasant
Pleasant
helps
me
express
who I am.
Agree
12
Negative
Positive
12
Candy
is
12
Dislike
"me".
Agree
3
Disagree
Like
12
12
lot
Candy
tells
me
Disagree
12
When
I
eat
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Next Page
134
6.
Chewing
Gum
the
Circle the
number
how
much you
Circle the
represents
sentences below.
product
listed
When other people see me with Chewing Gum, they think of me in a certain way.
Disagree
Bad
Good
3
Agree
3
12
12
Disagree
Unfavorable
Favorable
12
12
You can tell a lot about a person by seeing what kind of Chewing Gum he/she chews.
Agree
3
Disagreeable
Agreeable
4
5
12
Chewing
am.
Disagree
4
helps
6
express
Gum
me
who
Unpleasant
Pleasant
12
Negative
Agree
3
Positive
12
Chewing
Disagree
4
is
12
Dislike
Gum
"me".
Agree
Like
4 6
12
12
7
tells
Gum
me
Disagree
Agree
->
12
Disagree
When I chew Chewing Gum, others see me the way I want them to see me.
Agree
4
5
12
Next Page
l.i
7.
Potato Chips
number that best how you feel about
Circle the
number
how
much you
Circle the
sentences below.
the
represents
When
Good
me
in
eating Potato
a certain way.
me
Bad
Disagree
Agree
4 6
12 12
12 12
Negative
12
You can
Disagree
Unfavorable
Favorable
tell
Disagreeable
Agreeable
4
6 7
12
Disagree
Unpleasant
3
Pleasant
me
express
who I am.
Agree
3
Positive
12
Disagree
12
Dislike
Agree
Like
12
Go
to
12
7
tells
me
Disagree
Agree
12
When
the
I
me
way
want them
to see me.
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Next Page
->Q
136
8.
Soda
number that best how you feel about
Circle the
number
how
much you
Circle the
sentences below.
the
represents
When
Good
me
drinking Soda,
they think of
me
in
a certain way.
Bad
Disagree
Agree
3
12
12
12
Disagree
Unfavorable
4
Favorable
You can tell a lot about a person by what brand of Soda he/she drinks.
Agree
seeing
Disagreeable
Agreeable
12 12
Negative
12
Disagree
Unpleasant
Pleasant
am.
Agree
3
Positive
12
Soda
is
12
Dislike
"me".
Agree
3
Disagree
Like
12
Go
to
12
7
tells
me
Top
Disagree of Next
Column -
Agree
3
12
When
I
me
the
way
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Next Page
->
137
9.
Roller Blades
number that best how you feel about
above.
Circle the
number
much you
Circle the
sentences below.
the
represents
product
listed
When
me
using Roller
Bad
Good
Disagree
Agree
12 12 12 12
Negative
12
You can
Disagree
4
a
lot
Unfavorable
Favorable
tell
Disagreeable
Agreeable
12
Disagree
Unpleasant
me
express
who I am.
Pleasant
Agree
Positive
12
Disagree
12
Dislike
Agree
Like
4
12
Go
to
12
7
tells
me
a lot
Disagree
12
When
the
I
me
way
want them
to see me.
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Next Page
->
138
10.
Dolls
number that best how you feel about
Circle the
number
how
much you
Circle the
sentences below.
the
represents
When
Good
me
playing with
me
in a certain
way.
Bad
Disagree
Agree
12 12 12 12
Negative
12
You can
what
Disagree
Unfavorable
Favorable
tell
Disagreeable
Agreeable
4 6
Agree
3
12
Dolls help
Unpleasant
4
Pleasant
Disagree
me
express
who I am.
Agree
Positive
12
Dolls are
12
Dislike
"me".
Agree
3
Disagree
Like
3
12
Go
to
12
7
tells
me
Top of Next Column
lot
Disagree
12
When
way
I I
me the
want them
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Next Page
->
139
1 1
Action Figures
number that best how you feel about
Circle the
number
how
much you
Circle the
sentences below.
the
represents
When
Bad
Good
3
12
12
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Disagree
7 seeing
Unfavorable
Favorable
You can tell a lot about a person by what Action Figures he/she has.
Agree
Disagreeable
Agreeable
4 6
12 12
Negative
12
Disagree
6
express
Unpleasant
Pleasant
me
who I am.
Agree
3
Positive
12
Disagree
12
Dislike
Like
4
6
Agree
3
12
Go
to
12
Disagree
Seeing somebody else play with Action Figures tells me a lot about that person.
Agree
3
12
Disagree
When I play with Action Figures, others see me the way I want them to see me.
Agree
3
12
Next Page
->
140
12.
Stuffed Animals
number that best how you feel about
above.
Circle the
number
how
sentences below.
the
represents
product
When
me
playing with
listed
me
in a
Bad
Good
3
certain way.
12
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Disagree
Unfavorable
Favorable
4 6
12 12
You can tell a lot about a person by what Stuffed Animals he/she has.
Agree
seeing
Disagreeable
Agreeable
12
am.
Disagree
Unpleasant
Pleasant
me
express
who
12
Negative
Agree
3
Positive
12
Disagree
12
Dislike
Like
Agree
12
12
Animals
Disagree
me
lot
12
Disagree
When I play with Stuffed Animals, others see me the way I want them to see me.
Agree
3
12
Next Page
->
141
13.
French Fries
number that best how you feel about
above.
Circle the
number
how
sentences below.
the
represents
product
listed
When
Good
me
in
eating French
me
a certain way.
Bad
Disagree
Agree
3
12 12
12
Disagree
Unfavorable
Favorable
You can tell a lot about a person by seeing what kind of French Fries he/she eats.
Agree
3
Disagreeable
Agreeable
12 12
Negative
12
Disagree
Unpleasant
Pleasant
me
express
who I am.
Agree
3
Positive
12
Disagree
12
Dislike
Agree
3
Like
12
12
tells
Seeing somebody else eat French Fries me a lot about that person.
Disagree
Agree
3
12
When
the
I
me
way
want them
to see
me.
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Next Page
142
14.
Hamburgers
number that best how you feel about
Circle the
number
how
much you
Circle the
sentences below.
the
represents
When
way.
me
eating
in a certain
Bad
Good
3
12 12
12
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Disagree
Unfavorable
Favorable
You can tell a lot about a person by seeing what kind of Hamburgers he/she eats.
Agree
3
Disagreeable
Agreeable
4
5
12
Disagree
Unpleasant
Pleasant
am.
12
Negative
Positive
12
Disagree
12
Dislike
Like
4
12
12
tells
me
Disagree
Agree
3
12
When
the
I
eat
I
way
see
me
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Next Page
->
143
15.
Sports Drinks
number that best how you feel about
Circle the
number
how
sentences below.
the
represents
When
Good
me
drinking
me
in a
Bad
certain way.
12
12
Disagree
Agree
3
Unfavorable
4
Favorable
6
12
You can
Disagree
4
a
lot
Disagreeable
Agreeable
Agree
3
12
12
Negative
12
Disagree
Unpleasant
4
Pleasant
me
express
who 1 am.
Agree
3
Positive
12
Disagree
12
Dislike
Like
4
Agree
3
12
12
tells
me
Disagree
Agree
12
Disagree
When I drink Sports Drinks, others see me the way I want them to see me.
Agree
12
Next Page
->
144
6.
Board Games
number that best how you feel about
Circle the
number
how
much you
Circle the
sentences below.
the
represents
When
Good
me
in
playing Board
a certain way.
Bad
Disagree
Agree
12
12
You can
Disagree
Unfavorable
Favorable
tell
12
12
Disagreeable
Agreeable
Agree
12
Disagree
Unpleasant
Pleasant
12
Negative
Positive
12
Disagree
12
Dislike
Agree
3
Like
4
12
Go
to
12
tells
Games
me
Disagree
Agree
3
12
When
the
I
play Board
I
way
see
me
Disagree
12
Next Page
->
145
17.
Yo-yos
number that best how you feel about
above.
Circle the
number
how
sentences below.
the
represents
product
listed
When
Good
other people see me playing with Yo-yos, they think of me in a certain way.
Disagree
Bad
Agree
3
12
12
Disagree
Unfavorable
Favorable
12 12 12
Negative
You can tell a lot about a person by what kind of Yo-yos he/she has.
Agree
3
seeing
Disagreeable
Agreeable
4
5 6
12
Disagree
Unpleasant
Pleasant
Yo-yos help
me
express
who I am.
Agree
Positive
12
Disagree
12
Dislike
Agree
3
Like
12
Go
to
12
tells
me
Disagree
Agree
3
12
When
the
I
me
way
want them
to see
me.
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Next Page
->Q
146
18.
Bicycles
number that best how you feel about
above.
Circle the
number
how
sentences below.
the
represents
product
listed
When
Good
Bad
Disagree
12 12
12
Disagree
Unfavorable
4
Favorable
6
7
You can tell a lot about a person by seeing what kind of Bicycle he/she has.
Agree
3
Disagreeable
Agreeable
4
6 7
12 12
Negative
12
Disagree
Unpleasant
4
Pleasant
Bicycles help
me
express
who I am.
Agree
Positive
12
Disagree
12
Dislike
Bicycles are
3
"me".
Agree
Like
4
12
Go
to
12
me
a
lot
tells
Disagree
12
When
way
I I
me
the
want them
me.
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Next Page
->
147
19.
Skateboards
number that best how you feel about
above.
Circle the
number
how
sentences below.
the
represents
product
listed
When
Good
me
riding a
in a certain
Bad
12
12
Agree
3
Unfavorable
4
Favorable
6
12
Disagree
You can tell a lot about a person by seeing what kind of Skateboard he/she has.
Agree
3
Disagreeable
Agreeable
12 12
Negative
12
Disagree
Unpleasant
4
Pleasant
Skateboards help
me
express
who I am.
Agree
4
6
Positive
12
Disagree
12
Dislike
Like
4 6
12
Go
to
12
tells
Disagree
Agree
3
12
When
the
I
me
way
want them
to see me.
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Next Page
I4S
20.
Juice
number that best how you feel about
above.
Circle the
number
how
much you
Circle the
sentences below.
the
represents
product
listed
When
Good
me
drinking Juice,
they think of
me
in a certain
way.
Agree
Bad
Disagree
12
12 12 12
Negative
12
You can
Disagree
Unfavorable
Favorable
tell
Disagreeable
Agreeable
12
Disagree
Unpleasant
4
Pleasant
Juice helps
me
express
who I am.
Agree
Positive
12
Juice
is
12
Dislike
"me".
Agree
3
Disagree
Like
12
Go
to
12
Disagree
tells
me
Top
of Next
Column
Agree
3
12
When
way
I I
me
the
want them
me.
Disagree
Agree
3
12
Next Page
->
144
Circle the
number
how
much you
Circle the
sentences below.
the
represents
When
Good
me
playing CDs,
Bad
Disagree
Agree
3
12
12
Disagree
Unfavorable
Favorable
12 12 12
Negative
You can tell a lot about a person by what kind of CDs he/she has.
Agree
3
seeing
Disagreeable
Agreeable
12
CDs
help
4
express
Unpleasant
Pleasant
me
who I am.
Agree
Disagree
3
Positive
12
CDs
Disagree
12
Dislike
are "me".
Agree
3
Like
4
12
Go
to
12
Disagree
CDs
tells
me
a lot
12
When
I
me
the
way
want them
Disagree
me.
Agree
3
12
Stop!
Good Job!
APPENDIX F
want
to
There are no right or wrong answers. the questions as best you can. Just mark answers that reflect what YOU THINK!
Answer
Read and
Name:
School Grade:
How old
are vou?
Circle One:
Boy
White
Native American
Girl
Circle One:
Black
Hispanic
Other
->
150
151
DIRECTIONS:
Please tell me how persuasive (believable) you think each statement about Pringles Potato Chips is. Persuasive means how convincing an argument or claim about a product is. For example, if a commercial says Pringles Potato Chips are the best potato chip ever made, then I might think that is a very persuasive claim. It might make me want to buy Pringles. So, I would rate that statement very high. If a commercial says Pringles cost a lot, then I would rate that statement really low because it doesn't make me want to buy Pringles. I don't want to know what you think about Pringles based on how you feel about Pringles from eating them before (prior experience). I want to know how persuasive (believable) or convincing you think each statement is. Let's look at an
example.
EXAMPLE
Here are what the numbers stand
for:
VERY STRONG FEELING on the positive (good) side STRONG FEELING on the positive (good) side
FAIRLY
means that 4 means that 3 means that 2 means that 1 means that
5
have
WEAK FEELING on the positive (good) side WEAK FEELING on the negative (bad) side
STRONG FEELING on the negative (bad) side VERY STRONG FEELING on the negative (bad) side
what you think about the argument
for Pringles Potato
line to indicate
Doesn't
make
it
a
a a a
a a
Makes me
want
sense
it
me want
Doesn't
sense
make
Makes
Believable
Not believable
Weak
Strong
->
152
VERY STRONG FEELING on the positive STRONG FEELING on the positive (good)
FAIRLY
(good) side
side
means
that
you have a
WEAK FEELING on the positive (good) side WEAK FEELING on the negative (bad) side
side
that
that
that
STRONG FEELING on the negative (bad) side VERY STRONG FEELING on the negative (bad)
Doesn't make
Makes me
want
it
me want
sense
it
Doesn't make
Makes
sense
Not believable
Believable
Weak
Strong
Doesn't
make
it
a
a a
Makes me
want
sense
Believable
it
me want
Doesn't
sense
make
Makes
Not believable
Weak
Strong
fat.
Doesn't make
a
a
Makes me
want
sense
it
me want it
Doesn't
sense
make
Makes
Not believable
a
a
a a
Believable
Weak
Strong
->
153
Doesn't make
a a
LI
a
a
LI
Makes me
want
sense
it
me want it
Doesn't make
sense
Makes
Not believable
a
a
Believable
Weak
Strong
Doesn't
make
it
Makes me
want
it
me want
Doesn't
sense
make
a a a
Makes
sense
Believable
Not believable
Weak
Strong
made with
1
Doesn't
make
it
Makes me
want
it
me want
Doesn't
sense
make
Makes
sense
Not believable
Believable
Weak
Strong
ingredients.
Doesn't
make
it
Makes me
want
it
me want
Doesn't
sense
make
Makes
sense
Believable
Not believable
a
a
Weak
Strong
->
154
Doesn't
make
make
a
a
Makes me
want
sense
it
me want it
Doesn't
sense
Makes
Not believable
Q
a
a
a
Believable
Weak
Strong
Doesn't
make make
Q
a
a
a
a a a
Makes me
want
sense
Believable
it
me want it
Doesn't
sense
Makes
Not believable
Weak
a
chips.
Strong
Four out of five kids choose Pringles Potato Chips over other
2
3 4
Doesn't make
Q
a
Makes me
want
sense
it
me want
Doesn't
sense
it
make
Makes
Believable
Not believable
Weak
Strong
a can of Pringles Potato Chips you can "t stop eating them.
Doesn't
make
it
Makes me
want
it
me want
Doesn't
sense
make
Makes
sense
Not believable
Believable
Weak
Strong
->
155
Doesn't
make
make
Makes me
want
sense
it
me want it
Doesn't
sense
Makes
Not believable
a a a
Believable
Weak
Strong
Doesn't
make
Makes me
want
it
me want it
Doesn't make
sense
Makes
sense
Not believable
a
a a a
Believable
Weak
Strong
Doesn't
make
it
Makes me
want
it
me want
Doesn't
sense
make
Makes
sense
Believable
Not believable
Weak
Strong
Doesn't
make
make
a
a
Makes me
want
sense
it
me want it
Doesn't
sense
Makes
Believable
Not believable
Weak
Strong
->
156
Doesn't make
Makes me
want
it
me want it
Doesn't make
sense
Makes
sense
Believable
Not believable
Weak
a
make
1
Strong
lunch better.
Doesn't make
LI
Makes me
want
sense
it
me want it
Doesn't
sense
make
Makes
Not believable
a
a
Believable
Weak
Strong
Doesn't make
a
a a
Makes me
want
sense
it
me want it
Doesn't make
sense
Makes
Believable
Not believable
Weak
Strong
Doesn't
make
a
a a a
Makes me
want
sense
it
me want it
Doesn't make
sense
Makes
Believable
Not believable
Weak
Strong
->
157
Doesn't make
Makes me
want
it
me want it
Doesn't make
sense
a a a
Makes
sense
Believable
Not believable
Weak
Strong
Doesn't
make
make
Makes me
want
it
me want it
Doesn't
sense
Makes
sense
Believable
Not believable
Weak
Strong
Doesn't make
a
a
Makes me
want
sense
it
me want
sense
it
Doesn't make
Makes
Not believable
Believable
Weak
Strong
Doesn't
make
make
Makes me
want
it
me want it
Doesn't
sense
Makes
sense
Believable
Not believable
a
a
a
a
Weak
Strong
->
15S
Doesn't
make
it
a a a a a
Makes me
want
sense
it
me want
sense
Doesn't make
Makes
Believable
Not believable
Weak
Strong
Doesn't
make
it
Makes me
want
it
me want
Doesn't
sense
make
Makes
sense
Not believable
Believable
Weak
Strong
Doesn't make
Makes me
want
it
me want
Doesn't
sense
it
make
Makes
sense
Believable
Not believable
Weak
Strong
Stop!
Good Job!
REFERENCES
Aiken. P.
P.,
Leathar, D.
S.,
& O'Hagan.
F. (1985). Children's
perceptions of
21
.
& Medicine.
785-797.
and belief dimensions of attitudes to television advertising. Journal of Advertising Research. 32 (5). 30-42.
P. R. (1992). Functional
& Prabhaker,
Anderson. D. R.,
"Sesame
Street."
Anderson, D. R.,
& Smith, R.
(1984).
Young
children's
TV
cognitive continuity. In F.
J. Morrison, C. Lord, D. P. Keating (Eds.), A pplied developmental psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 1 16-163). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
&
ANPA.
tool Washington.
.
DC: American
Aspen
Institute. (1996).
media
Austin, E.
literacy
W.
Austin, E.
young audiences: Developmental considerations in designing health messages. In E. Maibach & R. L. Parrott (Eds.), Designing health messages: Approaches from communication theory and public health practice (pp. 1 14-144). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Freeman, C. (1996, May). Effects of media, parents and peers on African- American adolescents' efficacy toward media celebrities. Paper presented Mass Communication Division of the International Communication
W.
(1995). Reaching
Austin, E. W.,
&
at the
Association, Chicago.
Austin, E. W.,
literacy training
children's decision
making about
alcohol. Journal
of Health
Communication. 2 17-42.
159
60
Austin. E. W.,
Immediate and delayed effects of media graders' decision making for alcohol. Health
Austin, E. W.,
& Meili, H. K.
38 417.
Austin, E. W.,
& Nach-Ferguson. B. (1995). Sources and influences of young school-age children's general and brand-specific knowledge about alcohol. Health Communication. 7 1-20.
.
F.,
& Nass, C.
I.
on
Communication Research.
545-564.
Azjen,
I.,
Englewood
Babrow, A.
NJ: Prentice-Hall.
& Murphy, M. A. (1988). Person perception and children's impressions of television and real peers. Communication Research, 1 (4), 545-564.
O'Keefe, B.
J.,
Swanson, D.
L.,
Meyers, R. A.,
Ross, S. A. (1963). Imitation of film-mediated aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 66 , 3-11.
&
Batra, R.,
Ray, M. L. (1986). Affective responses mediating acceptance of advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (September). 234-249.
&
Beagles-Roos,
J.,
& Gat,
I.
on
children's
Bearden,
W.
scales: Multi-item
F. (1993). Handbook of marketing measures for marketing and consumer behavior research
.
& Mobley, M.
Newbury
Beentjes,
J.
Park,
CA: Sage.
H. A. (1993). Television viewing
vs. reading:
,
W.,
Mental
effort, retention,
inferential learning.
Bettman,
J.
R. (1979).
Memory
factors in
consumer choice:
A review.
Journal of
Bloom,
W.,
& Krathwohl, D.
Green.
(1956).
educational objectives:
The
Taxonomy of Handbook I:
Cognitive domain
161
Blosser, B.
J..
&
Roberts. D. F. (1985).
intent.
Age
message
& Rose. G. M.
TV
knowledge of advertiser
Brehm,
J.
W.
A.
(1966).
Brown.
J.
99
).
projects in the United States and selected countries Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Brown,
S. P., & Stayman, D. M. (1992). Antecedents and consequences of attitude toward the ad: A meta-analysis. Journal of Consumer Research. 19 34-5 1
.
Burke. M.
C.
& Edell,
J.
Burnkrant, R.,
processing of message content. Personality and Social Psycho loav Bulletin. 628-638.
Cable
in the
Classroom. (1996). The family and community critical viewing project Available http://www.ciconline.com/home.htm (retrieved February 25, 1996).
.
Cacioppo,
J.
T.,
& Petty, R.
E. (1979). Effects
Psvcholoev. 37 . 97-109.
Cacioppo,
J. T., Petty, R. E. (1980). Persuasiveness of communications is affected by exposure frequency and message quality: A theoretical and empirical analysis of
&
J.
H. Leigh
97-122).
& C.
Ann
and research
Press.
in advertising (pp.
Cacioppo,
J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1981). Social psychological procedures for cognitive response assessment: The thought-listing technique. In T. V. Merluzzi, C. R.
Glass.
& M.
New
York:
Guilford Press.
Calvert, S. L. (1988). Television production feature effects
Calvert, S. L.,
& Gersh, T. L. (1987). The selective use of sound effects and visual inserts
comprehension. Journal of Applied Developmental
162
C,
Watkins. B. A.,
& Wright,
J.
Campbell, D.
T.,
&
.
Stanley,
J.
MA: Houghton
Canadian Association of Broadcasters. (1985). Broadcast advertising and children Ottawa, Ont., Canada: Author.
Chaiken.
S. (1980). Heuristic
Psychology. 39 752-766.
,
Christenson, P. G.,
in the
formation of
Howe
(Ed.),
79-99).
New
Herman, P., Kozlowski, L., Petty, R. E. (1976). Elastic shifts of opinion: Determinants of direction and durability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34 663-672.
,
&
Cialdini, R.
Petty, R. E.,
& Cacioppo,
J.
.
Bence,
P.,
& Scheibe, C.
Media
television. Journal
Considine, D.
M.
990).
Can we
M.
(1995). Are
we there
yet?
An
States. (1989).
Buy me
that!
New
Cook, T. D.,
Houghton
Mifflin.
Gower.
Cummings, W.
H.,
behavior:
& Venkatesan, M. (1976). Cognitive dissonance and consumer A review of the evidence. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
63
Darlington.
J.
how to
be media savvy.
The Sunday
validity. Journal
of
Boston: Boston
Donohue,
Henke, L.
L.,
& Donohue,
W. A.
(1980).
Do
kids
Doob,
L. (1935). Propaganda,
its
New York:
Holt.
Dorr, A. (1980).
a child. I thought as a child. In S. B. Withey & R. P. Abeles and social behavior: Beyond violence and children (pp. 191230). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
When I was
(Eds.). Television
Droge, C. (1989). Shaping the route to attitude change: Central versus peripheral processing through comparative versus noncomparative advertising. Journal of Marketing Research. 26 (May). 193-204.
Duncan. B. (1989). Media literacy at the crossroads: Some issues, probes and questions. The History and Social Science Teacher. 24 (4). 205-209.
Dvorak,
J.,
Lain, L.,
What
research
tells
ERIC
Clearinghouse on
J.
A.,
& Burke, M.
effects. Journal
C. (1987). The power of feelings in understanding advertising of Consumer Research. 14 (December). 421-433.
Festinger, L. (1957).
Press.
Stanford,
CA: Stanford
University
www.firn.edu/doe/curric/prekl2/over.htm (retrieved
November
3,1998).
VA:
Freedom Forum. (1994). Death by cheeseburger: High school journalism and beyond Arlington, VA: The Freedom Forum.
.
in the
1990s
164
ed.).
New York:
Gagne,
R..
& Briggs, L.
New
York:
Good,
T. L.,
ed.).
& Brophy. J.
New
York: Longman.
Greenwald, A. G. (1968). Cognitive learning, cognitive response to persuasion, and attitude change. In A. G. Greenwald, T. Brock, T. Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological
&
New
York: Academic.
I.,
& Baldinger, A. L. (1991). The ARF copy research validity project. Journal
& Casey, D. & Kelly,
M.
(1992).
Young
The
retention
of
Hayes, D.
S.,
S. B. (1984).
Young
children's processing
Psychology. 38 505-514.
,
Hirsch, B. Z.,
& Kulberg,
J.
M.
critical
approach
to
cinema and
UNESCO.
(1985). Developmental differences in responses to a television
Hoffner.
C.
& Cantor, C.
2U 6),
1065-
1074.
Holbrook. M. B. (1978). Beyond attitude structure: Toward the information determinants of attitude. Journal of Marketing Research. 1 (Novemher1. 545-556.
Hovland, C. I, Janis, I., Kelley, H. (1953). Communication and persuasion Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
&
New
Hovland, C.
Lumsdaine, A., & Sheffield, F. (1949). Experiments on mass communication Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
I.,
.
Huck,
S.
W & Cormier,
W. H.
New
York: HarperCollins.
165
Huston. A.
C.
J.
C, Welch,
R..
& Ross. R
(1984). Children's
Huston, A.
C,
& Wright,
J.
Bryant
& D. R.
New York:
Hyman,
H.,
&
Sheatsley, P. (1947).
Some
reasons
why
information campaigns
James, K. E.,
& Hensel,
P.
J.
The malicious
strain
of
&
Scileppi,
J.
change
to high-
Kahn, T.
M, & Master,
told.
D. (1992). Multimedia literacy at Rowland: A good story, well Technological Horizons in Education Journal, 19 (7), 77-83.
(1955). Personal influence
Katz, D.,
& Lazarsfeld, P. R.
The
rights
New York:
.
Free Press.
Katz,
J.
(1996).
of kids
Wired
Available
7,
1999).
Kubey,
R.,
1999).
Has media
literacy
found a curricular
Kunkel, D. (1988). Children and host-selling television commercials. Communication Research. 15 (February). 71-92.
Lasswell, H. (1927). Propaganda techniques in the world war
Lazarsfeld, P. R., Berelson. B.,
New
The
people's choice
New
York:
Columbia University
Press.
Macmillan.
P., Strand, T. (1980). Developing a curriculum for teenagers. Journal of Communication. 30 (3). 119-125.
&
Lord, K.
R, Lee, M.-S., Sauer, P. L. (1995). The combined influence hypothesis: Central and peripheral antecedents of attitude toward the ad. Journal of
&
166
Lutz. R.
J.,
&
Bettman,
J.
A bicentennial
review. In A. Woodside,
industrial
J. Sheth. P. D. Bennett (Eds.), Consumer and buying behavior (pp. 137-150). New York: Elsevier North-Holland.
&
Lutz,
A.
983). Attitude toward the ad as mediator ( of advertising effectiveness: Determinants and consequences. In R. P. Bagozzi & M. Tybout (Eds.), Advances in consumer research (Vol. 10, pp. 532-539). Ann
J.,
MacKenzie,
S. B.,
& Belch, G. E.
MacKenzie,
S. B.,
& Lutz, R.
Do
J.
Macklin,
M.
C. (1983).
children understand
TV
23(February/March), 63-70.
Macklin, M. C. (1985).
Do young
Madden,
T.
J.,
Allen, C. T.,
& Twible,
An
M.
A meta-
analysis. Journal
of Public Policy
& Marketing.
16 (21 205-216.
McGuire, W.
J.
(1976).
Some
consumer
choice. Journal
McGuire, W.
J.
& E. Aronson
New York:
(Eds.),
Random House.
McGuire, W.
Park,
(1989). Theoretical foundations of campaigns. In R. E. Rice Atkin (Eds.), Public communication campaigns (2nd ed., pp. 43-65).
J.
& C.
K.
Newbury
CA: Sage.
J.
Meadowcroft,
M.,
& Reeves, B.
Melamed,
History and
167
medium on
M.
M,
S..
& Gardner.
H.
(1
983).
How
is
children's learning
J.
medium
methodologically. In
Bryant
1
from television distinctive? Exploiting the & D. R. Anderson (Eds.), Children's 79). New York: Academic Press.
New York:
Academic.
& Olson,
J.
advertising effects
on brand
of Marketing Research.
14, 1998).
National Education Goals Panel. (1992). National education goals panel, executive
summary: The national education goals report-building a nation of learners Washington, DC: National Education Goals Panel.
New Mexico Media Literacy Project. (1995). Progress report: Media literacy soars New Mexico; Nation takes notice as NMMLP activities grow. New Mexico
Media Literacy Project Newsletter. Winter
.
in
1-9.
http://www.nmmlp.org
August
15, 1998).
Nissen, B. (1996).
ABC
Odom,
R. D. (1972). Effects of perceptual salience on the recall of relevant and incidental dimensional values: developmental study. Journal of Experimental
S. Steiner
New York:
Holt, Rinehart,
168
Oyen, A.,
& Bebko,
children's
M. (1996). The effects of computer games and lesson contexts on mnemonic strategies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 62
J.
.
173-189.
Papageorgis, D. (1968). Warning and persuasion. Psychological Bulletin. 70 271-282.
.
Peter.
J. P.,
& Gilbert A.
Churchill,
J.
(1986). Relationships
among
research design
Journal of
A meta-analysis.
H. E. Sypher,
& W.
& Cacioppo,
J.
to persuasion. Journal
Petty,
T. (1979a). Effects of forewarning of persuasive intent and involvement on cognitive responses and persuasion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 5 (21 173-176.
J.
Petty, R. E.,
& Cacioppo,
J.
T.
( 1
1915-1926.
Petty, R. E.,
of issue involvement on attitudes in an advertising context. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the Division 23
J.
& Cacioppo,
T. (1980). Effects
& Cacioppo, J. T. (1981a). Attitudes and persuasion: Classic and contemporary approaches Dubuque, IA: William C Brown.
.
Petty, R. E.,
& Cacioppo,
J.
& Cacioppo,
J.
&
consumer psychology
Petty, R. E.,
MA:
Lexington Books.
& Cacioppo, J. T. (1984). The effects of involvement on responses to argument quantity and quality: Central and peripheral routes to persuasion.
169
Petty. R. E.,
J.
T. (1986a).
New York:
Springer Verlag.
Petty. R. E.,
J.
T. (1986b).
The elaboration
likelihood
model of persuasion.
York: Academic.
J.
T.,
& Goldman,
advertising effectiveness:
.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Schumann, D. (1983). Central and peripheral routes The moderating role of involvement. Journal of Consumer Research, 10 135-146.
to
& Williams,
An
cognitive effort
on
attitudes:
Petty, R. E.,
Ostrom, T.
.
M, &
in
& Priester,
effects:
elaboration likelihood
Media
J. R. (1994). Mass media attitude change: Implications of the model of persuasion. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Advances in theory and research (pp. 91-122). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Petty, R. E., Wells, G. L.,
& Brock, T. C.
Pezdek, K.,
& Hartman, E. F. (1983). Children's television viewing: Attention and comprehension of auditory versus visual information. Child Development, 54
1015-1023.
J.
Piaget,
Mussen
(Ed.),
(4th
ed., Vol. 1,
New York:
Wiley.
Piaget,
& Inhelder, B. (1964). The early growth of loeic in the child Routledge & Kegan Paul.
J.,
J.
London:
Pungente,
Rhine, R.
on media
literacy.
Emergency
Librarian.
24 (2), 9-11.
J., & Severance, L. J. (1970). Ego-involvement, discrepancy, source credibility, and attitude change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 16 175-190.
.
170
Prevention in
Human
Robertson, T.
S.,
& Rossiter,
J.
An
of Consumer Research.
K June).
The
13-20.
Robinson,
J.
L.
history of the
Roedder, D. L.
( 1
98 1 ). Age differences
in children's
An
153.
Rubel, C. (1996,
March
11, 1996).
How to watch a TV
spot.
Marketing
Rubin, A.
News TM.
M. (1986). Age and family control influences on children's The Southern Speech Communication Journal. 52 (1). 35-51.
television viewing.
Scholten,
M.
effectiveness. Journal
Shimp, T. A. (1981). Attitude toward the ad as a mediator of consumer brand choice. Journal of Advertising. 10 (21 9-15.
Stayman, D. M.,
Aaker, D. A. (1988). Are all the effects of ad-induced feelings mediated by A-ad? Journal of Consumer Research. 15 (3). 368-373.
&
Stayman, D. M.,
& Batra, R.
D
memory.
2g(May), 232-239.
S. (1981).
Stutts,
M.
Program-commercial separators in children's television: Do they help a child tell the difference between Bugs Bunny and the Quik Rabbit? Journal of Advertising. 10 (2). 16-25, 48.
A., Vance,
& Hudleson,
Surgeon's General Scientific Advisory Committee. (1972). Television and growing up: The impact of televised violence. Report to the surgeon general Washington,
.
DC: United
Traylor,
States
Government Printing
Office.
M.
B.,
& Joseph, W. B.
(1984). Measuring
Developing a general
scale.
Psychology
& Marketing.
fSummerl
65-77.
171
Troutman. C.
M, & Shanteau, J.
(1976).
Do consumers
&
Wulfemeyer, K. T. (2000). Teaching media studies as high & Mass Communication Educator. 54 (4). 67-76.
What work requires of school-A SCANS report for America 2000 Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Labor.
skills:
Van
Evra,
J.
Ward,
S.,
Wackman, D.
B.,
& Wartella, E.
Cambridge,
MA:
Ward,
S.,
Hills,
J.,
Beverly
Wells,
W.
C,
(December! 11-17.
statistics:
How to
New York:
New York:
Penguin.
S. (Ed.). (1981). Studying visual communication Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.
.
Wright,
J.
C, Huston, A. C,
on
Ross, R.
Raeissi, P.,
Effects
children's attention
P., Calvert, S. L., Rolandelli, D., Weeks, L. A., Pace and continuity of television programs: and comprehension. Developmental Psychology.
20(4), 653-666.
Wright, P. L. (1980). Message-evoked thoughts: Persuasion research using thought verbalizations. Journal of Consumer Research. 7 ( September). 151-175.
Wulfemeyer, K.
T.,
Sneed, D.,
Ommeren,
R. V.,
instruction in high school social science classes: survey of southern California teachers. Paper presented at the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass
Mass media
Communication, Minneapolis,
MN.
172
Zinkhan, G.
M,
Locander,
W.
B.,
& Leigh,
J.
Zuckerman, P., Ziegler, M, & Stevenson, H. W. (1978). Children's viewing of television and recognition memory of commercials. Child Development. 49 96-104.
.
BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH
Bradford L. Yates was born
received the
in
in 1989.
He
earned the M.S. in radio/television from Syracuse University in 1991 and the
M.Ed,
in
studies,
13, in
at
NBC's WRC-TV 4
Washington, DC.
He worked
in
Lynchburg College as an
instructor
For
in addition to
On LC,"
at
TV
and the
communications
of West Georgia.
173
certify that
have read
this
in
is
my
opinion
it
conforms
to the
fully adequate, in
Deborah M.
Treise, Chair
Communications
I
certify that
have read
this
my
opinion
it
fully adequate, in
of Doctor of Philosophy.
Michael
F.
Weigold
Communications
I
certify that
have read
this
in
my
opinion
it
conforms
in
to the
is fully
adequate,
of Philosophy.
Jujieji.
Dodd
Communications
I certify that I have read this study and that in my opinion it conforms to the acceptable standards of scholarly presentation and is fully adequate, in scope and quality, as a dissertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy
Colleen R. Swain
Assistant Professor of Teaching and
Learning
This dissertation was submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the College of Journalism and Communications and to the Graduate School and was accepted as partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.
August 2000
A^-t-l^i
H~y
,.,^
Communications
"N^ERSITV OF FLORIDA