Theorizing in Social Science: The Context of Discovery
()
About this ebook
All social scientists learn the celebrated theories and frameworks of their predecessors, using them to inform their own research and observations. But before there can be theory, there must be theorizing. Theorizing in Social Science introduces the reader to the next generation of theory construction and suggests useful ways for creating social theory.
What makes certain types of theories creative, and how does one go about theorizing in a creative way? The contributors to this landmark collection—top social scientists in the fields of sociology, economics, and management—draw on personal experiences and new findings to provide a range of answers to these questions. Some turn to cognitive psychology and neuroscience's impact on our understanding of human thought, others encourage greater dialogue between and across the arts and sciences, while still others focus on the processes by which observation leads to conceptualization. Taken together, however, the chapters collectively and actively encourage a shift in the place of theory in social science today. Appealing to students and scientists across disciplines, this collection will inspire innovative approaches to producing, teaching, and learning theory.
Related to Theorizing in Social Science
Related ebooks
Abductive Analysis: Theorizing Qualitative Research Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsProcessual Sociology Rating: 5 out of 5 stars5/5Theory for the Working Sociologist Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsDoing Effective Fieldwork: A Textbook for Students of Qualitative Field Research in Higher-Learning Institutions Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsSeven Rules for Social Research Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5The Logic of Social Research Rating: 5 out of 5 stars5/5Methods That Matter: Integrating Mixed Methods for More Effective Social Science Research Rating: 5 out of 5 stars5/5Retellings: Opportunities for Feminist Research in Rhetoric and Composition Studies Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsAn Introduction to Models in the Social Sciences Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsMaking Sense of Social Theory Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsContributions to L'Année Sociologique Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Fieldwork Is Not What It Used to Be: Learning Anthropology's Method in a Time of Transition Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsAnalysis and Interpretation of Ethnographic Data: A Mixed Methods Approach Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsAvant-Garde Sociology: An Introduction to Sociology Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsImprovising Theory: Process and Temporality in Ethnographic Fieldwork Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5The New Blackwell Companion to Social Theory Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5The Sociocultural Turn in Psychology: The Contextual Emergence of Mind and Self Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsHybridity: The Cultural Logic Of Globalization Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsThe Blackwell Companion to Sociology Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsA Durkheimian Quest: Solidarity and the Sacred Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsMastered by the Clock: Time, Slavery, and Freedom in the American South Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsEssays in Sociological Theory Rating: 5 out of 5 stars5/5Anthropology of the Self: The Individual in Cultural Perspective Rating: 3 out of 5 stars3/5Religion, Populism, and Modernity: Confronting White Christian Nationalism and Racism Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratings@ is for Activism: Dissent, Resistance and Rebellion in a Digital Culture Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsMeaning and Identity: An Interdisciplinary Perspective Rating: 3 out of 5 stars3/5Social Structure & Person Rating: 5 out of 5 stars5/5Anthropology and Modern Life Rating: 5 out of 5 stars5/5Social Knowledge in the Making Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsEthnographies of Movement, Sociality and Space: Place-Making in the New Northern Ireland Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratings
Social Science For You
Come As You Are: Revised and Updated: The Surprising New Science That Will Transform Your Sex Life Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5The Art of Witty Banter: Be Clever, Quick, & Magnetic Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5A People's History of the United States Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Dumbing Us Down - 25th Anniversary Edition: The Hidden Curriculum of Compulsory Schooling Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5The Fourth Turning Is Here: What the Seasons of History Tell Us about How and When This Crisis Will End Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5All About Love: New Visions Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5My Secret Garden: Women's Sexual Fantasies Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Verbal Judo, Second Edition: The Gentle Art of Persuasion Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Weapons of Mass Instruction: A Schoolteacher's Journey Through the Dark World of Compulsory Schooling Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Men Explain Things to Me Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5On Tyranny: Twenty Lessons from the Twentieth Century Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Close Encounters with Addiction Rating: 5 out of 5 stars5/5The Little Book of Hygge: Danish Secrets to Happy Living Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Devil in the Grove: Thurgood Marshall, the Groveland Boys, and the Dawn of a New America Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5You're Not Listening: What You're Missing and Why It Matters Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5The Like Switch: An Ex-FBI Agent's Guide to Influencing, Attracting, and Winning People Over Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Living Resistance: An Indigenous Vision for Seeking Wholeness Every Day Rating: 5 out of 5 stars5/5Unhumans: The Secret History of Communist Revolutions (and How to Crush Them) Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Three Women Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5
Reviews for Theorizing in Social Science
0 ratings0 reviews
Book preview
Theorizing in Social Science - Richard Swedberg
Stanford University Press
Stanford, California
© 2014 by the Board of Trustees of the Leland Stanford Junior University.
All rights reserved.
No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying and recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system without the prior written permission of Stanford University Press.
Printed in the United States of America on acid-free, archival-quality paper
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Theorizing in social science : the context of discovery / edited by Richard Swedberg.
pages cm
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 978-0-8047-8941-7 (cloth : alk. paper)
ISBN 978-0-8047-9109-0 (pbk. : alk. paper)
1. Social sciences—Philosophy. I. Swedberg, Richard, editor of compilation.
H61.T4646 2014
300.1—dc23
2013036393
ISBN 978-0-8047-9119-9 (e-book)
THEORIZING IN SOCIAL SCIENCE
The Context of Discovery
Edited by Richard Swedberg
Stanford Social Sciences
An Imprint of Stanford University Press
Stanford, California
For Mayer Zald (1931–2012), our colleague and friend
Contents
Preface
Richard Swedberg
Contributors
1. From Theory to Theorizing
Richard Swedberg
2. Intuitionist Theorizing
Karin Knorr Cetina
3. Analogy, Cases, and Comparative Social Organization
Diane Vaughan
4. The Unsettlement of Communities of Inquiry
Isaac Ariail Reed and Mayer N. Zald
5. Three Frank Questions to Discipline Your Theorizing
Daniel B. Klein
6. Mundane Theorizing, Bricolage, and Bildung
Stephen Turner
7. The Counterfactual Imagination
Roland Paulsen
8. The Work of Theorizing
Karl E. Weick
9. Susan Sontag and Heteroscedasticity
James G. March
Afterword
Neil Gross
Notes
Bibliography
Index
Preface
Richard Swedberg
THE PURPOSE OF THIS BOOK IS TO START A DISCUSSION OF THE need for more creative theorizing in social science. We need better and bolder theory; and the key to producing it lies in the way that theory is being produced and how it is being taught to the next generation of social scientists. It primarily lies in the process of theorizing. In order to end up with better theory, in brief, we need to shift our main concern from theory to theorizing.
There is some reason to believe that the time is now ripe for this sea change from theory to theorizing. One important reason for this has to do with the emergence a few decades ago of cognitive science, especially cognitive psychology. Cognitive scientists have by now made good inroads into the mysteries of human thought processes; and the findings point in a very different direction from the kind of logical reasoning that for a long time has stood at the center of traditional theory in the social sciences. There exist many ways of thinking other than formal reasoning: with images, analogies, metaphors, and what in everyday language is called intuition.
In an earlier book a co-editor and I wanted to draw attention to the role of social mechanisms (Hedström and Swedberg 1997). We felt that much social science had become too focused on the analysis of variables and did not pay enough attention to the concrete ways in which social actions are linked together, via social mechanisms. In this volume the focus has been shifted to the general process through which theory, including social mechanisms, is produced in the first place.
Before you have a theory, you need to theorize—so how do you go about theorizing in a creative way? The authors of the chapters in this book represent different social sciences; and they all offer different answers to this question. This diversity is healthy in itself. It also testifies to the experimental nature of this volume, which does not have as its goal to summarize the current state of theory in social science, but to point in novel directions by focusing on the process of theorizing.
What unites the authors is a deeply felt desire to break with the present situation: a virtual standstill of theory in social science. The standstill over the past few decades is evident at least in comparison with the powerful development of methods over the same time period, not to mention the explosive rate of innovations in fields such as cognitive science, neuroscience, and genetics.
To change the course of theory in social science—how you produce it and how you teach it—is a huge task and necessarily collective in nature. It is our hope that the reader will want to join this enterprise and help to move it along. The project of creating a new wave of bold and interesting theorizing in social science is large enough, and at the moment also open enough, for a large number of people to participate. What some would regard as drawbacks—the early, sketchy, and unfinished nature of this project—innovators will see as opportunities and what makes it exciting and inviting.
The book is organized in the following manner. The first chapter, From Theory to Theorizing,
presents an overview of the project of theorizing. It discusses how theorizing differs from theory, what kind of theorizing is needed to innovate, and that one goes about theorizing (the different elements that make up the process of theorizing).
A somewhat longer version of this chapter was originally sent to all of the participants, who were asked to react to it in their own way (Swedberg 2012). The idea was not to comment on its content but to use its core argument as an inspiration to carry the project of theorizing forward, in the direction that each author thought best. The result is a plethora of ideas and suggestions, mixed with reflections on the authors’ own experiences of theorizing in their research.
At first the reader may get the impression that the chapters are very far apart. But as Neil Gross discusses in the Afterword (Chapter 10), this is not the case. More precisely, there are two broad themes that all of the authors touch on. These are: what makes certain types of theorizing creative, and how to rein in and steer one’s imagination in a creative direction when theorizing.
Several different factors can help to make theorizing creative. The general nature of human thought, especially as investigated by cognitive psychology and neuroscience, is one of these (see Chapters 1, 2). The reason for this is that the way human beings think comes much closer to what we have traditionally viewed as creative thought. We use, as earlier mentioned, analogies and metaphors, and we use intuition as a matter of course.
Opening up theorizing to the arts and to the general creativity of human beings is another way of making it more innovative (see Chapters 7, 9). Traditionally science and art have not been particularly close, but maybe the time has come to start pulling them closer together. Creative theorizing is furthermore a collective enterprise; and the focus should be on the community rather than on the talented but lonely individual. Creative theorizing fares best when many people theorize and when a creative attitude toward theorizing has become a public good (see Chapter 4).
The second major theme in this book is linked to the first, in the sense that it is concerned with creativity as well. The emphasis, however, is on the specific ways in which creative impulses can be turned into creative theorizing, and not just dissipate. You want to let loose the imagination but also steer it into interesting social science.
How to do this can be very difficult in practice. It is, for example, important to aim at the right level of theorizing (see Chapter 6). It is similarly crucial to choose the right topic and to deal with the empirical material in a special way (Chapters 5, 8). It is also important to understand the craft of theorizing: how to handle analogies, create an explanation, and the like (Chapters 1, 3). While it may not be possible to develop specific rules for how to theorize well, there nonetheless are certain steps that need to be taken, and these can be learned as well as taught.
But even if there do exist a few themes that tie the individual chapters together, each of them also brings something special to the project of creative theorizing. In the rest of this Preface I therefore say something about each chapter, so that the reader is able to easily find what he or she might be looking for or something that looks interesting.
In the first chapter—From Theory to Theorizing,
I outline and present the basic ideas behind the project of creative theorizing in social science. For example, I make an argument for why it is necessary to focus on what happens at the stage before the final formulation of theory, since this is where the key idea is conceived and then hammered out.
I also attempt to show how one goes about theorizing, what steps need to be taken before formulating a theory. I suggest that one should begin by observing, then name the phenomenon and create one or several concepts that capture it. After this, the theory needs to be built out, with the help of analogies, metaphors, typologies, and more. The final stage is when a tentative explanation is produced.
At the heart of Chapter 2—Intuitionist Theorizing
by Karin Knorr Cetina—is the important shift that has taken place in our view of how thinking should be understood, thanks to cognitive psychology and neuroscience, and how this affects the view of what theorizing is. While we have previously looked mainly at and valued the logical and clear type of reasoning, we are today moving in a different direction. This different direction is still not set, but in the meantime Knorr Cetina suggests that we may want to look at the role of what she calls the inner processor in theorizing. This processor draws on long-term memory rather than the working memory, which has a much more limited capacity and only handles what we can recall consciously. The processor is implicit rather than explicit and intuitive rather than reflexive. It operates extremely quickly once it gets going—but it also pretty much comes and goes as it likes. To better understand theorizing in social science, the author concludes, we need to better understand our inner intuitive processor.
While Chapter 2 is mainly theoretical, Chapter 3—Analogy, Cases, and Comparative Social Organization
by Diane Vaughan—is more practical in nature. Many of the other authors in this volume advocate the use of some special technique or tool when theorizing. According to Vaughan, analogies are especially useful for this purpose. They can be used, she says, not only to come up with an explanation, but also to choose a topic in the first place and to describe the topic.
To illustrate the usefulness of what she calls analogical theorizing, Vaughan uses her own well-known study of seemingly disparate topics, from how people end a relationship to the fate of the space shuttle Challenger in 1986 (Vaughan 1986, 1996). Vaughan’s approach to the use of analogies, it should also be noted, is reminiscent of that of James Clerk Maxwell, who was an avid fan of analogies (Maxwell 1884). Like Maxwell, Vaughan does not advocate the simple use of analogies: comparing the phenomenon you are studying to some analogous phenomenon and theorizing the result. Instead she suggests that analogies should be seen as a tool for approaching a solution step by step.
Implicit in the arguments in the first three chapters is that the theorizer is an individual. The idea in Chapter 4, The Unsettlement of Communities of Inquiry
by Isaac Ariail Reed and Mayer N. Zald, is different. These two authors emphasize that there is a necessary collective dimension to creative theorizing. Without acknowledging this collective dimension, they say, the theorizing project will fail, or it will surely be much less successful than it could have been.
It is particularly when a human community is going through important changes that the theorizing of social scientists can flourish, according to the authors. The community of scholars must as a consequence not be isolated from the larger community. It is only when the two are organically linked to each other that the full potential of theorizing in social science can be realized.
Although all of the contributors to this volume are deeply interested in how to raise the level of theorizing in social science, some are also very critical of the current attitude toward theory. Chapter 5—Three Frank Questions to Discipline Your Theorizing
by Daniel B. Klein—is a case in point, with its critique of how theory is sometimes handled in economics.
According to Klein, theorizing does not mean that you can theorize in just about any way you want. Theorizing in social science will only be successful if it fulfills three conditions. First, theorizing has to deal with a real-world problem of some consequence ("Theory of what?). Second, it has to provide a better explanation than the existing one (
Why should we care?). And third, a good argument has to be presented why the new explanation is better than the existing one (
What merit in your explanation?").
Chapter 6, "Mundane Theorizing, Bricolage, and Bildung" by Stephen Turner, approaches the topic of how to theorize well in a different way from Klein. In Turner’s view, there exist a few different ways of theorizing, depending on the ambition and skill of the social scientist. The first is what he calls mundane theorizing. This method, for example, might extend an existing theory to something that it did not originally cover. This may seem simple, but it can be hard to do.
The next level of difficulty occurs when one tries to bring two or more theories to bear on each other, in an effort to produce some kind of new synthesis. But creating a synthesis or bricolage of this type does not constitute the most creative and advanced type of theorizing, according to Turner. This is instead so-called high theory, or Bildung, which is characterized by three features. First, the theorizer must be able to look at other theories from the viewpoint of their producers. Second, he or she must be sensitive to the weaknesses of a theory. And third, a first-class theorizer must be ruthlessly honest when evaluating a theory.
People who engage in high theory derive a great deal of pleasure from what they do, says Turner. They also feel a distinct sense of community with other high theorizers, whether they agree with them or not. And they are absolutely passionate about what they do.
Chapter 7, The Counterfactual Imagination
by Roland Paulsen, is written by a sociologist who shares the concern of many of the contributors to this book, namely that the current way of theorizing in the social sciences is often conducted in the wrong way. He is, however, alone in being interested primarily in counterfactuals and the creative role that these can play in theorizing.
The core of Paulsen’s argument is that social scientists need to train their counterfactual imagination. If they do this, he says, they will be able to handle some important social science topics in a much better way than they currently do. We cannot, for example, fully understand power, if we do not understand the counterreaction—or counterfactual reaction—that power produces. Similarly, it is through the use of the counterfactual imagination that power can be transcended.
Chapter 8 is called The Work of Theorizing,
and its author, Karl E. Weick, is one of the few social scientists who has been intensely interested in theorizing throughout his career. More than twenty-five years ago he suggested the following: Theory cannot be improved until we improve the theorizing process, and we cannot improve the theorizing process until we describe it more explicitly, operate it more self-consciously, and de-couple it from validation more deliberately
(Weick 1989: 516).
The part of the theorizing process that Weick discusses in this book has to do with how one makes the transition from the stage of observation to the formulation of concepts and a theory—or more precisely, how one goes from social reality to a really live kind of theory without producing the narrow knowledge-in-hindsight that social science so often seems to end up with.
Weick’s answer is that we have to realize that the transition from observation to a theory is best understood as the result of a very delicate and tension-filled process in several steps. Weick also provides the potential theorizer with a useful list of fourteen items to keep in mind when theorizing.
James G. March, the author of the next chapter, Susan Sontag and Heteroscedasticity,
has also been concerned with theorizing for much of his career. And like Karl Weick, he shares the conviction that art can be of help in producing good social theory. In Chapter 9 he notes that while we today can produce students who are skilled in methods, we have failed to teach them how to handle the aspects that especially concern artists, authors, and poets. Both social science methods and artistic qualities, however, are needed to produce a truly creative social science. We therefore need to get a handle on the kind of techniques that artists use and adapt them for the purpose of social scientists.
Is this a realistic project? And if so, how can it be accomplished? March hesitates, but not when it comes to the urgency of these ideas. What he finds difficult is instead to outline what these new techniques should look like and how to teach them. In the meantime, however, he says there are some themes and focal points that are important for students of social science to understand. These include ambiguity, contradiction, context in meaning, and the role of affirmation in construction (or the quality of beauty).
This volume ends with an Afterword by Neil Gross, who raises some additional critical issues. He notes that the ideas of pragmatism, especially the work of Charles Peirce, have influenced many of the chapter authors. He also issues three warnings to those who want to get involved in the project of creative theorizing, and says that unless these are heeded the whole project may be endangered. These are as follows: you should focus on the way that social scientists have actually behaved when theorizing; you should not divert your energy to interdisciplinary efforts; and it is important to face the fact that so far theorizing has mainly been a male enterprise.
Finally, let me draw attention to the fact that one of the contributors to this book departed this world just after submitting his chapter. Mayer Zald (1931–2012), co-author of Chapter 4, will be sorely missed. Mayer was a brilliant social scientist, a beloved colleague, and extremely generous to his colleagues and students with praise as well as time and concern. It is fully in character, we think, that his last work contains a plea that, when we discuss theorizing, we should realize that true and humane theorizing in social science always has its roots in the larger community.
For encouragement, help, and suggestions I would like to first thank Mabel Berezin. I am also grateful to many other people who have helped to shape the project of creative theorizing in social science, including Andy Abbott, Margareta Bertilsson, Angie Boyce, Mikael Carleheden, Christoffer Carlsson, Nicolas Eilbaum, Laura Ford, Emily Goldman, Emily Hoagland, Darcy Pan, Lambros Roumbanis, and Hans Zetterberg. The comments of two anonymous reviewers were very helpful. I have, finally, learned much from the students in my classes on theorizing at Cornell University, Copenhagen University, and Stockholm University. For financial support I gratefully acknowledge my debt to Cornell’s Department of Sociology and its Institute for the Social Sciences (ISS).
Contributors
Karin Knorr Cetina is George Wells Beadle Distinguished Service Professor of Sociology and Anthropology at the University of Chicago and principal investigator of the project Scopic Media,
University of Constance. Major publications include Epistemic Cultures (Harvard University Press, 2003), Handbook of the Sociology of Financial Markets (edited with Alex Preda, Oxford University Press 2012), and Maverick Markets: The Global Currency Market as a Cultural Form (forthcoming).
Neil Gross is professor of sociology at the University of British Columbia. His most recent books are Why Are Professors Liberal and Why Do Conservatives Care? (Harvard University Press, 2013), and Social Knowledge in the Making (University of Chicago Press, 2011, co-edited with Charles Camic and Michèle Lamont).
Daniel B. Klein is professor of economics at George Mason University, where he leads the graduate program in Adam Smith studies, and a fellow of the Ratio Institute in Stockholm. He is chief editor of Econ Journal Watch, author of Knowledge and Coordination: A Liberal Interpretation (Oxford University Press, 2012), and editor of What Do Economists Contribute? (NYU Press/Palgrave, 1999).
James G. March is professor emeritus at Stanford University where he has been on the faculty since 1970. Other of his books published by the Stanford University Press include: Explorations in Organizations, The Dynamics of Rules (with Martin Schulz and Xueguang Zhou) and The Roots, Rituals, and Rhetoric of Change (with Mie Augier).
Roland Paulsen is a postdoctoral research fellow in the Department of Business Administration, Lund University. He received his PhD in sociology from Uppsala University; his main fields of study are the sociology of work and critical theory. He is the author of Arbetssamhället: Hur arbetet överlevde teknologin (2010) and Empty Labor: Subjectivity and Idleness at Work (2013).
Isaac Ariail Reed is assistant professor of sociology at the University of Colorado at Boulder. He is the author of Interpretation and Social Knowledge: On the Use of Theory in the Human Sciences (University of Chicago Press, 2011).
Richard Swedberg is professor of sociology at Cornell University. His two primary specialties are economic sociology and social theory. He is the author of several books, including The Art of Social Theory (forthcoming). He is also co-editor (with Neil Smelser) of The Handbook of Economic Sociology (Princeton University Press, 1st ed. 1994, 2nd ed. 2005) and (with Peter Hedström) of Social Mechanisms (Cambridge University Press 1998).
Stephen Turner is Distinguished University Professor in Philosophy at the University of South Florida. His books include The Impossible Science, with Jon Turner (1990) and The Social Theory of Practices (1994). His most recent book is Explaining the Normative (2010). Two collections of his essays are forthcoming: The Politics of Expertise and Understanding the Tacit.
Diane Vaughan is professor of sociology at Columbia University. In addition to the three books analyzed in her chapter, two are in progress: Dead Reckoning: System Effects, Boundary Work, and Risk in Air Traffic Control and Theorizing: Analogy, Cases, and Comparative Social Organization.
Karl E. Weick is Rensis Likert Distinguished University Professor of Organizational Behavior and Psychology, Emeritus, at the Stephen M. Ross School of Business at the University of Michigan. He is the author of many articles and books, including Sensemaking in Organizations (1995), Managing the Unexpected (with Kathleen M. Sutcliffe, 2001, 2006), and Making Sense of the Organization (vol. 1: 2001, vol. 2: 2009).
Mayer N. Zald (1931–2012) was professor emeritus, Department of Sociology and Schools of Business and Social Work, at the University of Michigan. His work focused on social movement theory, organizational theory, and sociology as human science. He wrote or edited twenty-one books, most recently Social Movements and the American Health System, with Jane Benaszak-Holl and Sandra Levitsky (Oxford University Press, 2010). In 2008 he received the John D. McCarthy Award for Lifetime Achievement in the Scholarship of Social Movements and Collective Behavior.
1
From Theory to Theorizing
Richard Swedberg
SINCE THE MID-TWENTIETH CENTURY THE SOCIAL SCIENCES HAVE made great advances in the kind of methods that they use. In the area of theory, however, the situation is quite different. The development since World War II has been quite uneven in this respect; sociologists and other social scientists are today very methodologically competent, but considerably less skillful in the way they handle theory. The major journals contain many solidly executed articles, though creative and theoretically sophisticated articles are less common.
Why is this the case? And can the situation be changed? Can the theory part be brought up to par with the methods part in today’s social science? One answer that I suggest we may want to explore is the option of placing more emphasis on theorizing than on theory; and in this way start to close the gap between the two.
Roughly speaking, the expression to theorize
refers to what one does to produce a theory and to the thought process before one is ready to consider it final. While theorizing is primarily a process, theory is the end product. The two obviously belong together and complement each other. But to focus mainly on theory, which is what is typically done today, means that the ways in which a theory is actually produced are often neglected. This is true both for the individual researcher and for social science as a whole.
Emphasizing the role of theorizing also has huge consequences for the way that theory is taught, a topic that is of great importance and deserves a volume of its own. For example, when sociological theory is often taught today, the student gets to know what Durkheim, Weber, Bourdieu, and others said—knowledge that will supposedly come in handy once the student undertakes future research projects. But teaching theorizing is very different; here the goal is for the student to learn to theorize on his or her own. The point is to learn to develop theories for one’s own empirical work, not just use someone else’s ideas.
The emphasis on each individual doing his or her own theorizing means that each individual must draw on his or her unique set of knowledge and experience. I refer to the central role of the individual in the theorizing process as personalism; the term refers to the fact that theorizing will only be successful if one delves deeply into one’s own self and experiences. You have to know theory to theorize, but to theorize well you also need to relate to it in a personal way.
Also, just as the individual is always exposed to the risk of failing when he or she does anything authentic, the same is true for theorizing (Kierkegaard [1846] 1962). Repeating other people’s theory entails little risk, unlike theorizing on one’s own. This is part of the meaning of Weber’s statement that the scientific worker has to take into his bargain the risk that enters into all scientific work: Does an ‘idea’ occur or does it not?
(Weber 1946: 136).
There exist many ways of theorizing, including induction, deduction, generalizing, model-building, using analogies, and others. Some of these, I argue, are especially useful for theorizing in sociology and social science. In discussing and presenting the different types of theorizing I will often use the work of Charles S. Peirce as my guide. The writings by Peirce, especially How to Theorize
and Training in Reasoning,
are extremely suggestive for theorizing (e.g., Peirce 1934, 1992d/1998). I have similarly found many relevant insights in cognitive science.
But it is also clear that much of what has been written on theorizing has been forgotten and that no one has tried to pull together the most important texts or tried to piece together the tradition of theorizing that I have attempted to describe in this introductory chapter. The writings that do exist are scattered throughout the enormous literature in social science, in autobiographical accounts by social scientists, and in their correspondence.¹
Finally, throughout this chapter I point out the many obstacles that currently exist to creative theorizing. These epistemological obstacles, as I will call them (following Gaston Bachelard), are of different kinds (see, e.g., Bachelard [1934] 1984). Some of them make it hard to deal effectively with data in the process of theorizing. Others encourage the social scientist to rely far too much on existing theory and skip the element of theorizing or reduce it to a minimum.
The Distinction between the Context of Discovery and the Context of Justification
In approaching the topic of theorizing in social science, it is convenient to take as one’s point of departure the well-known distinction in the philosophy of science between the context of discovery and the context of justification. In doing so, it is possible to show that both the current neglect of theorizing and the related overemphasis on theory have much to do with the tendency in today’s social science to largely ignore the context of discovery, and instead to focus most of the attention on the context of justification.
The distinction between the context of discovery and the context of justification received its most influential formulation in the 1930s through the work of Hans Reichenbach and Karl Popper. Today the distinction is still around, even if it has been criticized over the years and is far from generally accepted (Hoyningen-Huene 1987; Schickore and Steinle 2006). It should be pointed out that the argument in this chapter does not rest on the notion that these two concepts are each other’s absolute opposites or that there exists a sharp conceptual line between the two. Nonetheless, the distinction represents a useful point of departure for the discussion.
Both Reichenbach and Popper worked on ways to improve empiricism as a philosophy of science. Reichenbach coined the terms context of discovery
and context of justification,
while Popper helped to diffuse them by giving them a central place in his influential work The Logic of Scientific Discovery (Popper 1935: 4–6; Popper 1959: 31–32, 315; Reichenbach 1938: 6–7, 281; Reichenbach 1951: 231). Both used the distinction primarily with the natural sciences in mind, not the social sciences.
Reichenbach defined the context of discovery as the form in which [thinking processes] are subjectively performed,
and the context of justification as the form in which thinking processes are communicated to other persons
(Reichenbach 1938: 6). While science can address issues in the context of justification in a satisfactory way, the same is not true for the context of discovery. "The act of discovery escapes logical analysis" (Reichenbach 1951: 231, emphasis added).
Popper similarly argued that everything that precedes the formulation of a theory is of no interest to science and logic. It belongs at best to empirical psychology
(Popper 1935: 4–5; Popper 1959: 31–2). This meant in practice that what accounts for the emergence of new theories cannot be studied. In his influential work Popper kept hammering away at this message: it is impossible to study theoretical creativity; the only place for science is in the context of justification (Popper 1982: 47–48).²
In terms of theorizing in the social sciences, what is important in Reichenbach and Popper’s distinction is that attention was now directed away from the context of discovery and toward the context of justification. A theory that cannot be verified (Reichenbach) or falsified (Popper) is not scientific; and it therefore becomes imperative to establish the link between theory and facts according to