Sociobiology of Caviomorph Rodents: An Integrative Approach
4/5
()
About this ebook
Fully integrative approach to the socibiology of caviomorph rodents
- Brings together research on social systems with that on epigenetic, neurendocrine and developmental mechanisms of social behavior
- Describes the social systems of many previously understudied caviomorph species, identifying the fitness costs and benefits of social living in current day populations as well as quantified evolutionary patterns or trends
- Highlights potential parallels and differences with other animal models
Related to Sociobiology of Caviomorph Rodents
Related ebooks
Metacommunity Ecology Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsModeling Populations of Adaptive Individuals Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsPhase Transitions Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5The Symbiotic Habit Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsPhylogenies in Ecology: A Guide to Concepts and Methods Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsMolecular Panbiogeography of the Tropics Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsThe Evolutionary Strategies that Shape Ecosystems Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsEvolution in Changing Environments: Some Theoretical Explorations Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsThe Ecological World View Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsScale, Heterogeneity, and the Structure and Diversity of Ecological Communities Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsScaling in Ecology with a Model System Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsEcology and Evolution of Dung Beetles Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsGroup Selection in Predator-Prey Communities Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsThe Geographic Mosaic of Coevolution Rating: 3 out of 5 stars3/5Adaptive Diversification Rating: 5 out of 5 stars5/5Caste and Ecology in the Social Insects Rating: 3 out of 5 stars3/5The Ecosystem Approach: Complexity, Uncertainty, and Managing for Sustainability Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsFood Webs and Biodiversity: Foundations, Models, Data Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsCompetition and the Structure of Bird Communities Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Geographic Variation, Speciation and Clines Rating: 5 out of 5 stars5/5Evolutionary Ecology of Parasites: Second Edition Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsInteraction and Coevolution Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsSome Adaptations of Marsh-Nesting Blackbirds Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsDiscontinuities in Ecosystems and Other Complex Systems Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsThe Coevolutionary Process Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsEstimating Species Trees: Practical and Theoretical Aspects Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsThe Wiley Handbook of Evolutionary Neuroscience Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsFundamentals of Microbiome Science: How Microbes Shape Animal Biology Rating: 3 out of 5 stars3/5The Shape of Life: Genes, Development, and the Evolution of Animal Form Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsEvolutionary Biogeography: An Integrative Approach with Case Studies Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratings
Biology For You
The Source: The Secrets of the Universe, the Science of the Brain Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Sapiens: A Brief History of Humankind Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Lifespan: Why We Age—and Why We Don't Have To Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Dopamine Detox: Biohacking Your Way To Better Focus, Greater Happiness, and Peak Performance Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Gut: The Inside Story of Our Body's Most Underrated Organ (Revised Edition) Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Why We Sleep: Unlocking the Power of Sleep and Dreams Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5The Grieving Brain: The Surprising Science of How We Learn from Love and Loss Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5The Soul of an Octopus: A Surprising Exploration into the Wonder of Consciousness Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Rethinking Narcissism: The Bad---and Surprising Good---About Feeling Special Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5The Laws of Connection: The Scientific Secrets of Building a Strong Social Network Rating: 0 out of 5 stars0 ratingsPeptide Protocols: Volume One Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Other Minds: The Octopus, the Sea, and the Deep Origins of Consciousness Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5The Sixth Extinction: An Unnatural History Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5The Art of Thinking Clearly Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5"Cause Unknown": The Epidemic of Sudden Deaths in 2021 & 2022 Rating: 5 out of 5 stars5/5Suicidal: Why We Kill Ourselves Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5The Winner Effect: The Neuroscience of Success and Failure Rating: 5 out of 5 stars5/5Ultralearning: Master Hard Skills, Outsmart the Competition, and Accelerate Your Career Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Woman: An Intimate Geography Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Homo Deus: A Brief History of Tomorrow Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Emotional Blackmail: When the People in Your Life Use Fear, Obligation, and Guilt to Manipulate You Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Anatomy 101: From Muscles and Bones to Organs and Systems, Your Guide to How the Human Body Works Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Lies My Gov't Told Me: And the Better Future Coming Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5The Obesity Code: the bestselling guide to unlocking the secrets of weight loss Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5Our Kind of People: Inside America's Black Upper Class Rating: 3 out of 5 stars3/5A Crack In Creation: Gene Editing and the Unthinkable Power to Control Evolution Rating: 4 out of 5 stars4/5
Reviews for Sociobiology of Caviomorph Rodents
19 ratings1 review
- Rating: 5 out of 5 stars5/5A godsend for this Humanities major, plunged headfirst into orgo. Klein approaches organic chemistry as one might a novel, the plot of which serves as the frame upon which to build the level of understanding necessary to do well in the class. This made a lot more sense to me than rote memorization, which seemed to work for my classmates, but frustrated me to no end.
Book preview
Sociobiology of Caviomorph Rodents - Luis A. Ebensperger
Acknowledgments
The editors of this book are highly indebted to former Wiley editor Suzanne Albrecht for her immediate and unconditional support in materializing this editorial project. We also appreciate the positive reception and suggestions to the original project from three anonymous reviewers. Subsequently, all chapters of this book have benefited from the constructive and useful suggestions made by the following colleagues: Sabine Begall, Daniel Blumstein, Robbie Burger, Patrick Gouat, James Hare, Melissa Holmes, Barbara König, John Koprowski, Karen Mabry, Lisa McGraw, Betty McGuire, Peter Meserve, Daniel Olazabal, Steve Phelps, Neville Pillay, Janet Randall, Carsten Schradin, Jane Waterman, and one anonymous reviewer.
Notes on contributors
Annaliese K. Beery (Ph.D., University of California-Berkeley, USA).
Dr. Beery is a neuroendocrinologist with interests in social behavior, stress, and comparative approaches to understanding the brain and behavior.
Joseph R. Burger (Ph.D., University of New Mexico, USA).
Dr. Burger is an evolutionary ecologist with an interest in field and macroecological approaches to studying social behavior and life history in mammals.
Valentina Colonnello (Ph.D., Sapienza University of Rome, Italy).
Dr. Colonnello is a developmental psychobiologist and clinical psychologist. Her research focuses on the formation of social bonds and developmental trajectories, within a cross-species affective neuroscience perspective.
Elizabeth R. Congdon (Ph.D., University of Missouri-St. Louis, USA).
Dr. Congdon is a science educator and behavioral ecologist specializing in natal dispersal and conservation of Neotropical mammals. She has most recently focused her research on capybaras, both in their native range and as a potential invasive species in the southern United States.
Luis A. Ebensperger (Ph.D., Boston University, USA).
Since 1997, Professor Ebensperger's research has focused on the current function and evolution of group-living and cooperation of caviomorph rodents and other mammals.
Gabriel Francescoli (Ph.D., Universidad de la República, Uruguay).
Dr. Francescoli is head of the Ethology Section at the Sciences College, Montevideo, Uruguay. His research interests are related to the evolution of animal communication systems, and to subterranean rodent communication strategies.
Loren D. Hayes (Ph.D., Miami University, USA).
Professor Hayes is a behavioral ecologist with an interest in caviomorph rodent sociality. Since 2005, his primary research focus has been on Octodon degus sociality in Chile.
Emilio Herrera (Ph.D., Oxford University, UK).
Dr. Herrera gained his B.Sc. in Biology at Universidad Simón Bolívar in Venezuela in 1980 and then went on to get a D.Phil. in Zoology at Oxford with a thesis on capybara social behavior (1986). After a one-year post doc at the Smithsonian in Panama, he returned to Universidad Simón Bolívar where he is a professor.
Yasmin Kamal (M.D./Ph.D. student, Dartmouth Medical School, USA).
Yasmin is a student at Dartmouth Medical School with an interest in neuroendocrinology and genetics.
Brian Keane (Ph.D., Purdue University, USA).
Dr. Keane is a behavioral ecologist who combines field studies with molecular genetics methods to assess questions regarding social behavior.
Eileen A. Lacey (Ph.D., University of Michigan-Ann Arbor, USA).
Dr. Lacey is a behavioral ecologist specializing in studies of rodent social behavior. Currently, her research focuses on the ecological, evolutionary, and neuroendocrine bases for variation in social structure among tuco-tucos (genus Ctenomys) and closely related caviomorph taxa.
Christine R. Maher (Ph.D., University of California-Davis, USA).
Dr. Maher is a behavioral ecologist with a long-term interest in intraspecific variation in social behavior in mammals.
Ruth C. Newberry (Ph.D., University of Edinburgh, UK).
Dr. Newberry is an applied ethologist at the Norwegian University of Life Sciences, specializing in environmental enrichment, social behavior, and animal welfare.
Selene Nogueira (Ph.D., University of São Paulo, Brazil).
Dr. Nogueira is Full Professor in the Department of Biological Science, the coordinator of the Applied Ethology Laboratory at Universidade Estadual de Santa Cruz, Ilhéus–Bahia, Brazil. Her research is dedicated to studying social behavior, animal communication, and animal welfare.
Agustina Novillo (Ph.D., Instituto Argentino de Investigaciones de Zonas Áridas, IADIZA, Mendoza, Argentina).
Dr. Novillo is Assistant Researcher of the National Council for Science & Technology of Argentina (CONICET). Her research has been oriented towards the biogeography and ecology of Andean rodents, with a particular focus on patterns of biodiversity along elevation gradients.
Agustina Ojeda (Ph.D., Instituto Argentino de Investigaciones de Zonas Áridas, IADIZA, Mendoza, Argentina).
Dr. Ojeda is Assistant Researcher of the National Council for Science & Technology of Argentina (CONICET). Her research centers on the study of the patterns and processes that shape the distribution of genetic variation in small desert mammals, particularly in geographically isolated systems, and she uses different approaches, from phylogeography and molecular systematic to genetics landscape.
Ricardo A. Ojeda (Ph.D., Instituto Argentino de Investigaciones de Zonas Áridas, IADIZA, Mendoza, Argentina).
Dr. Ojeda is Principal Researcher of the National Council for Science & Technology of Argentina (CONICET) and his research has been oriented towards the biogeography and ecology of desert mammals, patterns of biodiversity, and the ecological diversification of rodents.
Jaak Panksepp (Ph.D., University of Massachusetts, USA).
Dr. Panksepp is a psychobiologist/neuroscientist whose research during the first half of his career was at Bowling Green State University, OH, and devoted heavily to brain social-emotional systems. He is currently Professor of Neuroscience at Washington State University. He is the author of Affective Neuroscience (Oxford, 1998) and Archaeology of Mind with Lucy Biven (Norton, 2012).
Cristian E. Schleich (Ph.D., Universidad de Mar del Plata, Argentina).
Dr Schleich is a behavioral researcher whose interests are focused on the behavioral ecology and sensory biology of solitary subterranean rodents, particularly Ctenomys.
Raúl Sobrero (Ph.D., P. Universidad Católica de Chile, Chile).
Dr. Sobrero combines ecological and evolutionary perspectives, integrating behavioral, brain anatomy, ecological and phylogenetic studies on wild caviomorph rodents to understand the causes and consequences of animals' responses to physical and social conditions.
Nancy G. Solomon (Ph.D., University of Illinois at Champaign-Urbana, USA).
Dr. Solomon is a behavioral ecologist who has studied small mammal social behavior and reproduction. She has primarily worked with voles in studies conducted in natural populations, semi-natural populations, and laboratory settings.
Zuleyma Tang-Martínez (Ph.D., University of California-Berkeley, USA).
Dr. Tang-Martínez has worked on chemical communication, kin and individual discrimination, dispersal, and social behavior of rodents for approximately 44 years. She supervised E.R. Congdon's research on capybaras (with E. Herrera as field advisor) in her native Venezuela.
Rodrigo A. Vásquez (Ph.D., Oxford University, UK).
The research carried out by Dr. Vásquez is focused on behavioral intra-specific variability at different levels, and the integrative study of behavior, including energetics, endocrinology, and genetics, using small mammals (mainly Octodon degus) and birds.
Introduction
Luis A. Ebensperger¹ & Loren D. Hayes²
¹Departamento de Ecología, Facultad de Ciencias Biológicas, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile
²Department of Biology, Geology, and Environmental Sciences, University of Tennessee at Chattanooga, Chattanooga, TN, USA
I.1 Social behavior of caviomorph rodents and book aims
Social behavior involves the actions directed toward, or in response to conspecifics and the fitness consequences for all individuals involved (Wersinger 2009; Székely et al. 2011). Given that social interactions are diverse in nature and extent, social behavior is similarly diverse. Thus, social behavior encompasses a variety of agonistic (including aggressive) behaviors that result in the establishment of dominance hierarchies and territoriality, but also a similarly diverse array of affiliative interactions. Affiliative interactions takes place in different contexts, including courtship and other sexual interactions that result in mating systems, parent–offspring interactions that result in parental care patterns, or the relatively permanent association of adult conspecifics that result in sociality (or group-living) and different forms of cooperation.
In the late 1990s and early 2000s, some researchers began to argue that generalizations about rodent social behavior were premature due to the lack of information coming from the caviomorph or New World hystricognath rodents, a socially diverse group of South American rodents (Ebensperger 1998; Tang-Martínez 2003). We propose that a greater focus on caviomorph rodents as subject models of social behavior would contribute greatly to collaborative and integrative research on this area. Caviomorph rodents exhibit a diverse range of social behaviors and life history attributes, and are found in a wide range of habitats. Caviomorphs span from solitary living (Adler 2011) to highly social (Herrera et al. 2011), and live in kin-biased (Lacey & Wieczorek 2004) or non-kin biased (Quirici et al. 2011) groups. Some species provide communal care to offspring (Ebensperger et al. 2007) while others attempt to avoid contact with non-descendant offspring held in communal crèches (Taber & Mcdonald 1992; Campos et al. 2001). Mating systems are equally diverse, with some species exhibiting monogamy and territoriality, while others exhibit polygyny, or promiscuity (Adrian & Sachser 2011). In terms of life history, caviomorph rodents exhibit a mixture of fast
and slow
traits; many have long gestation periods and produce small litters of precocial offspring, yet reach sexual maturity at a young age and exhibit low survival (Kraus et al. 2005). High mortality rates effectively make some species semelparous (Ebensperger et al. 2013). Finally, caviomorph rodents are ecologically diverse, occurring in habitats such as high and low altitude shrublands, tropical and temperate forests, and coastal areas. Habitats range from arboreal to semiaquatic to subterranean. Numerous species have wide geographical ranges, increasing the potential for social and life history flexibility.
Historically, the caviomorph rodents have offered diverse opportunities to studies focused primarily on functional or evolutionary explanations of social behavior. However, caviomorphs are also emerging as good model organisms for integrative research (Colonnello et al. 2011; Ardiles et al. 2013). Researchers have started to make in-roads into the neural mechanisms underlying social variation (e.g. Seidel et al. 2011; Uekita & Okanoya 2011), facilitating comparative analyses that cast mechanism in an evolutionary context (Beery et al. 2008). In some species, we are beginning to understand the neuroendocrine (Ebensperger et al. 2011, 2013) and immunological (Ebensperger et al. 2015) responses to social, reproductive, and environmental conditions. Molecular tools are available for some species, setting the stage for work on sociogenomics and mechanisms underlying reproductive success. Thus, we believe that the time is right to move caviomorph sociobiology into an integrative framework (Hayes et al. 2011). We hope that this book encourages researchers to continue the momentum, increasing the level of international interest in these curious organisms.
The main aim of this book is to provide a comprehensive understanding of caviomorph rodent social systems, focusing on advances and future work in the major themes mentioned above. For each topic addressed in this book we asked contributors to provide relevant information on caviomorph rodents, establish potential parallels (or differences) with other animal models, and place this information into a hypothesis-driven and integrative framework. To the best of our ability, we integrated these individual or topic-based efforts to provide an overall conceptual framework for future studies of social behavior. However, before we introduce these wonderfully diverse and interesting organisms, we first discuss how research approaches to social behavior are evolving. We use this framework to highlight how caviomorph animal models may enrich and contribute to speed up our knowledge on animal social behavior.
I.2 Research approaches to social behavior
Niko Tinbergen's (1963) seminal paper was pivotal to widening the scope of animal behavior research and encompassing proximate causation (i.e. genetic, neurological, and hormonal mechanisms), developmental effects, function, and evolution or historical underpinnings of social behavior. Since then, relatively separate lines of research have addressed all four types of questions. In this sense, behavioral ecologists have made great progress to determine the ecological causes and fitness consequences of several aspects of social behavior, including mating systems (Shuster & Wade 2003), parental care (Clutton-Brock 1991; Royle et al. 2012), sociality (Krause & Ruxton 2002), and cooperation (Dugatkin 1997). On the other hand, behavioral geneticists, neurobiologists and endocrinologists have produced information about how genes, gene expression mechanisms, and neuroendocrinological pathways predispose or prone individuals to exhibit different forms of social behavior (e.g. Becker et al. 2002; Anholt & Mackay 2010).
Historically, the ecological-evolutionary work pursued by behavioral ecologists and mechanistic work pursued by geneticists, neurobiologists, and physiologists have followed relatively independent trajectories. Likely, long-term research goals that characterized each field were a major contributor to this end. For example, a large amount of research on the mechanistic underpinnings of social behavior has been based on a few laboratory animal models kept under unnatural conditions and aimed at extrapolations on human-oriented social and sexual behaviors as well as related pathologies (Anholt & Mackay 2010). In contrast, behavioral ecologists have relied on an array of wild animal models and their natural environments, but generally have been less sensitive to the importance of proximate causes (e.g. Krebs & Davies 1993; Davies et al. 2012). As a result, functional and evolutionary implications of behavior from laboratory animal models are complicated, and a lack of detailed genotypic or neuroendocrinological knowledge of wild animal models limits their use to mechanistic studies.
A major push to integrate social behavior research from widely diverse disciplines should be credited to Wilson (1976), who coined the term sociobiology
to embrace the approach called to examine the biological basis of social behavior. Although Wilson did not develop these issues with great detail in the individual chapters of his seminal book (Sociobiology: The New Synthesis, 1976), it was apparent that the aims of sociobiology included genetics, development, and physiological bases of social behavior. Subsequently, behavioral ecologists have highlighted the relevance of mechanisms to fully understand current function and the evolution of social and other behaviors. Thus, mechanisms were placed in a context of constraints and function, with explicit fitness and evolutionary consequences (e.g. Real 1994; Dukas 1998). On the other hand, mechanistically oriented scholars increasingly have shown an explicit interest in placing mechanisms into ecologically and evolutionary relevant settings. This trend is evident from recent reviews and textbooks addressing how genetic, neurological, or endocrinological substrates of social behavior have diverged or converged to explain current within and between species differences in social behavior (e.g. Adkins-Regan 2005; Donaldson & Young 2008; Robinson et al. 2008; Zupanc 2010; O'Connell & Hofmann 2011; Choleris et al. 2013). As a result, we see a growing trend in Behavioral Ecology and other historically separate disciplines to converge into integrative approaches to explain and predict variation in social behavior (Monaghan 2014). However, what is meant precisely by integrative research?
Tinbergen (1963) suggested that a deep understanding of animal behavior would be achieved through research addressing different, yet complementary perspectives. In practice, this view was a powerful framework to stimulate multidisciplinarity
(sensu Tress et al. 2004), meaning that disciplines such as behavioral genetics, endocrinology, and behavioral ecology have been focused on similar behavioral traits or phenomena, yet in parallel, keeping multiple disciplinary objectives (Blumstein et al. 2010; Bateson & Laland 2013). Instead, integration
implies bringing together elements or aspects of multiple disciplines that traditionally have been seen as separate entities. Thus, integration demands interdisciplinarity
where historically independent disciplines using different methods and approaches converge upon common goals (Tress et al. 2004). More importantly, however, integration in research requires connections and interactions among elements or processes that lie at different biological levels to become explicit (Pigliucci 2003; Perry & Burggren 2007). Identification of these connections should result in an integrated knowledge and new theory about the biological phenomena of interest (Tress et al. 2004). For instance, genomics examines variation associated with complete genomes and aims to understand how this variation interacts with environmental and life history contexts to cause physiological or behavioral differences as emergent outputs (Kültz et al. 2013).
Multidisciplinary approaches to animal behavior are clearly responsible for documenting the extraordinary diverse and complex nature of behavioral phenomena that is evident in almost any textbook on animal behavior or behavioral biology. However, it is this complexity that now demands multidisciplinary approaches to turn interdisciplinary. Interdisciplinary approaches carry the promise to provide integration, implying a clear understanding of how components of underlying mechanisms interact with environmental conditions at different levels to impact an individual's phenotype and fitness (Moore et al. 2011; O'Connell & Hofmann 2011; Hofmann et al. 2014). Several advantages of integrating mechanisms and function are likely to emerge. First, integration expands our knowledge of where information on mechanisms and function are seen as complementary. Second, detailed knowledge of mechanisms may reveal the nature and extent to which mechanisms limit or promote reproductive success, and so contribute to explaining individual and population differences in behavior (e.g. Real 1994; Dukas 1998). Finally, knowledge on mechanisms may highlight novel hypotheses about function, difficult to anticipate without this previous knowledge (e.g. Hennessy et al. 2009; Cirulli et al. 2010).
I.3 Terminology
Social systems include the array of associations and interactions between the sexes, within and outside family bonds. Thus, a complete description of social systems should include dominance relationships, courtship and intersexual bonds, space use and territoriality, parental care patterns, and the stability of and extent to which adult conspecifics exhibit sociality (i.e. live in groups) and cooperate. While most known organisms exhibit one or more aspects of social behavior, only some social systems include sociality. Other factors such as group composition and the parental care system (uniparental, biparental, communal) play important roles in social system dynamics. These characteristics are not independent of one another. For example, in mammals, there is evidence that particular breeding strategies (one lactating female vs. multiple lactating females in a group) are coupled with particular mating systems (Lukas & Clutton-Brock 2012), and one potential advantage of group-living is that individuals can collectively defend a territory. Thus, a challenge to studying social systems is determining the distinctions—and links—between these different components. In this book, contributors will use terms like sociality or group-living, breeding strategies, and mating systems to describe different aspects of caviomorph rodent social systems. Below, we define these key terms (see the Glossary for a complete list of terms, highlighted in bold on their first occurrence throughout the book).
I.3.1 Sociality (or group-living)
Sociality or group-living involves spatial and temporal proximity of group members that results from a mutual attraction to conspecifics rather than from attraction to a same resource or physical condition (Parrish et al. 1997; Krause & Ruxton 2002). Animal groups range from short-term to relatively long-term associations (Parrish et al. 1997) and this variation is determined in part by the strength to which group members are attracted to each other (i.e. an intrinsic factor). Temporal duration and emerging group stability are also influenced by ecological conditions that limit the ability of individuals of living independently (Emlen 1995), or by demographic processes linked to mortality (Ebensperger et al. 2009). Sociality is usually defined based on the degree of the spatial overlap of individuals (Krause & Ruxton 2002) and quantified based on the total number of individuals in a group (e.g. group size). Researchers also use metrics that account for the extent of socially affiliative interactions within groups (e.g. social bonding). Recent advances in social network theory (Whitehead 2008) have improved our ability to incorporate both spatial and behavioral interactions in sociality metrics (e.g. Wey et al. 2013).
I.3.2 Mating systems and parental care
An animal's mating system involves the association between males and females during breeding activity, and includes the relative effort of males and females in parental care (Bolhuis & Giraldeau 2005). Parental care in turn involves behaviors (and other forms of investment) exhibited by parents that increase the survival and condition of their offspring, typically at a cost to their own fitness (Royle et al. 2012). Thus, mating systems are characterized by the number of sexual partners that individual male and females have, and the extent to which parental care is shared between both sexes. Monogamy occurs when one member of each sex breeds together as a pair either serially or over a lifetime. In polygynous systems, males mate with multiple females, defending the females (female-defense polygyny) or resources (resource-defense polygyny) that they use. Promiscuity characterizes systems in which males and females mate multiply. Rarely, a species is polyandrous, a system in which multiple males mate with one female. In reality, mating systems are more complex and include two components—social and genetic. The social mating describes the associative behavior of individuals (e.g. whether individuals remain in pairs). In contrast, genetic mating systems describe the reproductive outcomes of the behavior. It is well known that cheating
occurs, resulting in genetic mating systems that differ from the social mating system (Birkhead & Møller 1992).
Parental care strategies are typically defined based on the numbers of parents providing care to offspring. In monogamous species, males and females typically share parental care duties similarly, referred to as biparental care. In contrast, females are typically the main providers of parental care in most other mating systems. In some species, females are the only care-givers (uniparental care) whereas in others multiple females care for offspring in communal litters or crèches (communal care).
I.3.3 Territoriality and dominance hierarchies
Territoriality occurs when one individual or groups of individuals prevent conspecifics or members of other groups gaining access to a proportion of their range areas. Typically, territory defense results in exclusive access to the resources (e.g. food, refuges) within the defended area at the expense of energetic and time costs involved in such defense (Maher & Lott 1995). The motivation for territoriality may differ between the sexes. For example, in some social mammals (e.g. house mice, Mus musculus), males defend territories including groups of communal females while females communally defend litters from intruder males (Manning et al. 1995). It seems important here to note that territorial animals may exclude conspecifics from an area with the use of a combination of social interactions, including direct fight and chases, and scent markings.
In social species, agonistic interactions between individuals may result in dominance hierarchies (Drews 1993; Pusey & Packer 1997). Dominance relationships in social systems are important because these relationships determine the rank and therefore, priority of access of individuals to critical resources (and its effects on condition). In addition, social rank in some social species determines the extent to which each individual contributes with direct reproduction and offspring care. Dominance relationships can also affect the stability of groups, possibly explaining the evolution of fission–fusion societies with high rates of group membership turnover.
Hierarchies can range from linear associations (pecking-order) characterized by transitive relationships, to more complex associations characterized by intransitive relationships (Earley & Dugatkin 2010). Regardless, rank may be influenced by intrinsic factors, including body size, weaponry, or age. In addition, hierarchies are determined by motivational factors such as energy reserves, residency or tenure, or experience during previous contests (Pusey & Packer 1997; Broom et al. 2009; Earley & Dugatkin 2010).
I.3.4 Cooperation and breeding strategies
Different forms of cooperation may evolve once individuals live in groups, including cooperative hunting, communal breeding, or social inspection of predators (Dugatkin 1997). We restrict the use of cooperation to social interactions that result in net inclusive fitness benefits to all participants. Theoretically, cooperation can evolve through several mechanisms, including kin selection, reciprocity, by-product mutualism, or trait (group) selection (Dugatkin 1997). Here, we follow Dugatkin (1997) in that individuals behave in a manner that makes cooperation possible. Thus, different aspects of social behavior may result in cooperation, including individual coordination to scan for approaching predators, coordination during group foraging to locate and attain patchily distributed food resources, or communal defense of a territory.
In addition, cooperation may result in different breeding strategies,
meaning that the degree to which direct reproduction and parental effort are shared (reproductive skew) within groups may vary (Brown 1987; Hayes 2000; Silk 2007). Usually, researchers refer to the breeding status of females and communal care (or alloparental care) of non-breeders in groups, a distinction that unless otherwise stated, will be made in this book. Singular breeding is a strategy in which groups consist of one or a few reproductive females, one or more breeding males (i.e. high reproductive skew) and non-breeders that provide alloparental care to non-descendant offspring (Brown 1987; Silk 2007). Plural breeding is a strategy in which multiple females (and males) in the same social group breed (i.e. low reproductive skew). Plural breeding may occur with and without communal care of offspring (Silk 2007). In some insects and very few mammals, social groups consist of distinct classes, including a reproductive female, one or more reproductive males and non-breeders (Wilson 1976), a condition referred to as eusociality.
Other researchers have used the terms cooperative breeders, communal breeders, and social breeders, which are based on the types of cooperation or care provided to offspring, to describe the breeding structure of mammals (Lukas & Clutton-Brock 2012). In this scheme, cooperative breeders are species in which a proportion of females do not breed regularly and provide alloparental care to non-descendant offspring. Communal breeders are species in which most adult females breed and rear offspring communally. Social breeders are species in which females live in groups, but do not rear offspring communally or cooperate. In this chapter, we use Silk's (2007) classification scheme, which includes singular breeders and plural breeders with and without communal care. Thus, singular breeders are the same as cooperative breeders, plural breeders with communal care are the same as communal breeders and plural breeders without communal care are the same as social breeders (Silk 2007; Lukas & Clutton-Brock 2012).
I.4 Structure of the book
We aim to provide relevant information on caviomorph rodent social systems, establish potential parallels (or differences) with other animal models, and place this information into a hypothesis-driven and integrative framework. To this end, the book includes contributions from experts on mechanistic and functional aspects of social behavior. In Chapter 1, Ojeda et al. describe the general biology of caviomorph rodents with an emphasis on geographic distribution, life history, life styles, ecology, and evolutionary history. This chapter provides a context for readers interested in social behavior but not familiar with rodents. In Chapter 2, Maher and Burger highlight how caviomorph social systems vary within and across species. These authors relate species variation to differences in ecological lifestyles, including diet, habitat mode, space use, and activity period. Within species variation seem linked to differences in resource distribution, predation risk, or population density. They highlight how the uniqueness of these social systems offer potentially fruitful lines for future research. In Chapter 3, Beery et al. address the neural underpinnings of different aspects of social behavior, with the aim to provide a basis to explain (predict) intra- and inter-species variation in conspecific tolerance, conspecific seeking propensity, partner preference, or extent of parental care in current day forms. In Chapter 4, Colonnello et al. provide a developmental background and take current laboratory-based studies to examine the strength of social bonds, sensitivity to social environment, and the developmental and epigenetic factors involved in the expression of social behavior. In Chapter 5, Lacey examines the proximate and functional causes, and its consequences on social and genetic structure of species, with an emphasis on caviomorph rodents. In Chapter 6, Francescoli et al. review the different modalities of communication that evolved in caviomorph rodents. The chapter investigates how the use of different communication modalities (chemical, visual, auditory) and complexity may be further influenced by group living. In Chapter 7, Ebensperger and Hayes examine how ecological and life-history factors explain current sociality and have driven this aspect of social behavior in caviomorphs. This chapter highlights how studies on caviomorphs add to the multi-dimensional nature of variation in sociality. Solomon and Keane dedicate Chapter 8 to discussing some problems with trying to define and quantify sociality. Then, these authors examine some of the ultimate hypotheses proposed to explain sociality in rodents to highlight how the study of caviomorphs is likely to provide independent tests of hypotheses for theory based on non-caviomorph rodents. In Chapter 9, Vásquez addresses how social behavior traits of caviomorphs are potentially cooperative, and how different evolutionary mechanisms may explain them. Herrera leads Chapter 10 to examine how mating systems vary across caviomorphs, and the extent to which this variation reflects ecological factors and phylogeny. Tang-Martínez and Congdon use Chapter 11 to provide an update on what is known about parent–offspring and sibling–sibling relationships in caviomorphs. These authors want the chapter to highlight relationships among these social traits, life history traits (e.g. levels of precociality), and life styles (e.g. fossoriality). The next chapter is a review by Hayes and Ebensperger, and reveals how little we know about the fitness consequences of group living in caviomorphs. However, Chapter 12 also shows how fitness effects of group-living vary across the few better-known species. In Chapter 13, Ebensperger and Hayes integrate major themes from previous chapters and provide an explicit integrative framework for future studies of social behavior.
Acknowledgments
We, the editors are highly indebted to former Wiley commissioning editor, Suzanne Albrecht for her immediate and unconditional support in making this editorial project materialize. We also appreciate the positive reception and suggestions to the overall project from three anonymous reviewers, and are thankful to Wiley-Blackwell for its firm and constant support. In particular, we thank Kelvin Matthews for guiding us through the final preparations of the book. Luis A. Ebensperger has been supported by FONDECYT grants 3970028, 1020861, 1060499, 1090302, and 1130091. NSF grants 0553910, 0853719 and 1261026 funded Loren D. Hayes.
References
Adkins-Regan, E. (2005) Hormones and Animal Social Behavior. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Adler, G.H. (2011) Spacing patterns and social mating systems of echimyid rodents. Journal of Mammalogy92, 31–8.
Adrian, O. & Sachser, N. (2011) Diversity of social and mating systems in cavies: a review. Journal of Mammalogy92, 39–53.
Anholt, R.H. & Mackay, T.F.C. (2010) Principles of Behavioral Genetics. Academic Press, London.
Ardiles, A.O., Ewer, J., Acosta, M.L., et al. (2013) Octodon degus (Molina 1782): a model in comparative biology and biomedicine. Cold Spring Harbor Protocols8, 312–18.
Bateson, P. & Laland, K.N. (2013) Tinbergen's four questions: an appreciation and an update. Trends in Ecology and Evolution28, 712–18.
Becker, J.B., Breedlove, S.M., Crews, D. & McCarthy, M.M. (2002) Behavioral Endocrinology. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Beery, A.K., Lacey, E.A. & Francis, D.D. (2008) Oxytocin and vasopressin receptor distributions in a solitary and a social species of tuco-tuco (Ctenomys haigi and Ctenomys sociabilis). The Journal of Comparative Neurology50, 1847–59.
Birkhead, T.R. & Møller, A.P. (1992) Sperm Competition in Birds: Evolutionary Causes and Consequences. Academic Press, London.
Blumstein, D.T., Ebensperger, L.A., Hayes, L.D. et al. (2010) Towards an integrative understanding of social behavior: new models and new opportunities. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience4, 1–9.
Bolhuis, J.J. & Giraldeau, L.A. (2005) The Behavior of Animals: Mechanisms, Function, and Evolution. Blackwell Publishing, Malden, MA.
Broom, M., Koenig, A. & Borries, C. (2009) Variation in dominance hierarchies among group-living animals: modeling stability and the likelihood of coalitions. Behavioral Ecology20, 844–55.
Brown, J.L. (1987) Helping and Communal Breeding in Birds: Ecology and Evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Campos, C.M., Tognelli, M.F. & Ojeda, R.A. (2001) Dolichotis patagonum. Mammalian Species652, 1–5.
Choleris, E., Pflaff, D.W. & Kavaliers, M. (2013) Oxytocin, Vasopressin and Related Peptides in the Regulation of Behavior. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Cirulli, F., Berry, A., Tommaso Bonsignore, L. et al. (2010) Early life influences on emotional reactivity: evidence that social enrichment has greater effects than handling on anxiety-like behaviors, neuroendocrine responses to stress and central BDNF levels. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews34, 808–20.
Clutton-Brock, T.H. (1991) The Evolution of Parental Care. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Colonnello, V., Iacobucci, P., Fuchs, T., Newberry, R.C. & Panksepp, J. (2011) Octodon degus: a useful animal model for social-affective neuroscience research: basic description of separation distress, social attachments and play. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews35, 1854–63.
Davies, N.B., Krebs, J.R. & West, S.A. (2012) An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology. Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford.
Donaldson, Z.R. & Young L.J. (2008) Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science322, 900–4.
Drews, C. (1993) The concept and definition of dominance in animal behaviour. Behaviour125, 283–313.
Dugatkin, L.A. (1997) Cooperation among Animals. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Dukas, R. (1998) Cognitive Ecology: The Evolutionary Ecology of Information Processing and Decision Making. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Earley, R.L. & Dugatkin, L.A. (2010) Behavior of groups. In: Evolutionary Behavioral Ecology (Westneat, D.F. & Fox, C.W., eds.). Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 285–307.
Ebensperger, L.A. (1998) Sociality in rodents: the New World fossorial hystricognaths as study models. Revista Chilena de Historia Natural71, 65–77.
Ebensperger, L.A., Chesh, A.S., Castro, R.A. et al. (2009) Instability rules social groups in the communal breeder rodent Octodon degus. Ethology115, 540–54.
Ebensperger L.A., Hurtado, M.J. & León, C. (2007) An experimental examination of the consequences of communal versus solitary breeding on maternal condition and the early post-natal growth and survival of degu, Octodon degus, pups. Animal Behaviour73, 185–94.
Ebensperger, L.A., León, C., Ramírez-Estrada, J. et al. (2015) Immunocompetence of breeding females is sensitive to cortisol levels but not to communal rearing in the degu (Octodon degus). Physiology & Behavior140, 61–70.
Ebensperger, L.A., Ramírez-Estrada, J., León C. et al. (2011) Sociality, glucocorticoids and direct fitness in the communally rearing rodent, Octodon degus. Hormones and Behavior60, 346–52.
Ebensperger, L.A., Tapia, D., Ramírez-Estrada, J. et al. (2013) Fecal cortisol levels predict breeding but not survival of females in the short-lived rodent, Octodon degus. General and Comparative Endocrinology186, 164–71.
Emlen, S.T. (1995) An evolutionary theory of the family. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA92, 8092–9.
Hayes, L.D. (2000) To nest communally or not to nest communally: a review of rodent communal nesting and nursing. Animal Behaviour59, 677–88.
Hayes L.D., Burger, J.R., Soto-Gamboa, M., Sobrero, R. & Ebensperger, L.A. (2011) Towards an integrative model of sociality in caviomorph rodents. Journal of Mammalogy92, 65–77.
Hennessy, M.B., Kaiser, S. & Sachser, N. (2009) Social buffering of the stress response: diversity, mechanisms, and functions. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology30, 470–82.
Herrera, E.A., Salas, V., Congdon, E.R., Corriale, M.J. & Tang-Martínez, Z. (2011) Capybara social structure and dispersal patterns: variations on a theme. Journal of Mammalogy92, 12–20.
Hofmann, H.H., Beery, A.K., Blumstein, D.T. et al. (2014) An evolutionary framework for studying mechanisms of social behavior. Trends in Ecology and Evolution29, 581–9.
Kraus, C., Thomson, D.L., Künkele, J. & Trillmich, F. (2005) Living slow and dying young? Life-history strategy and age-specific survival rates in a precocial small mammal. Journal of Animal Ecology74, 171–80.
Krause, J. & Ruxton, G.D. (2002) Living in Groups. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Krebs, J.R. & Davies, N.B. (1993) An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford.
Kültz, D., Clayton, D.F., Robinson, G.E. et al. (2013) New frontiers for organismal biology. BioScience63, 464–71.
Lacey, E.A. & Wieczorek, J.R. (2004) Kinship in colonial tuco-tucos: evidence from group composition and population structure. Behavioral Ecology15, 988–96.
Lukas, D. & Clutton-Brock, T.H. (2012) Cooperative breeding and monogamy in mammalian societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B279, 2151–6.
Maher, C.R. & Lott, D.L. (1995) Definitions of territoriality used in the study of variation in vertebrate spacing systems. Animal Behaviour49, 1581–97.
Manning, C.J., Dewsbury, D.A., Wakeland, E.K. & Potts, W.K. (1995) Communal nesting and nursing in house mice, Mus musculus domesticus. Animal Behaviour50, 741–51.
Monaghan, P. (2014) Behavioral ecology and the successful integration of function and mechanism. Behavioral Ecology25, 1019–21.
Moore, A.J., Székely, T. & Komdeur, J. (2011) Prospects for research in social behavior: systems biology meets behavior. In: Social Behaviour: Genes, Ecology and Evolution (Székely, T., Moore, A.J. & Komdeur, J., eds.). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 538–50.
O'Connell, L.A. & Hofmann, H.A. (2011) Genes, hormones, and circuits: an integrative approach to study the evolution of social behavior. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology32, 320–35.
Parrish, J.K., Hamner, W.M. & Prewitt, C.T. (1997) Introduction: From individuals to aggregations: unifying properties, global framework, and the holy grails of congregation. In: Animal Groups in Three Dimensions (Parrish, J.K. & Hamner, W.M., eds.). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 1–13.
Perry, S.F. & Burggren, W.W. (2007) Why respiratory biology? The meaning and significance of respiration and its integrative study. Integrative and Comparative Biology47, 506–9.
Pigliucci, M. (2003) From molecules to phenotypes? The promise and limits of integrative biology. Basic and Applied Ecology4, 297–306.
Pusey, A.E. & Packer, C. (1997) The ecology of relationships. In: Behavioral Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach (Krebs, J.R. & Davies, N.B., eds.). Blackwell Publishing, Oxford. pp. 254–83.
Quirici, V., Faugeron, S., Hayes, L.D. & Ebensperger, L.A. (2011) Absence of kin structure in a population of the communal rearing rodent Octodon degus. Behavioral Ecology22, 248–54.
Real, L.A. (1994) Behavioral Mechanisms in Evolutionary Ecology. Chicago University Press, Chicago.
Robinson, G.E., Fernald, R.D. & Clayton, D.F. (2008) Genes and social behavior. Science322, 896–900.
Royle, N.J., Smiseth, P.T. & Kölliker, M. (2012) The Evolution of Parental Care. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Seidel, K., Poeggel, G., Holetschka, R., Helmeke, C. & Braun, K. (2011) Paternal deprivation affects the development of corticotrophin-releasing factor-expressing neurones in prefrontal cortex, amygdala and hippocampus of the biparental Octodon degus. Journal of Neuroendocrinology23, 1166–76.
Shuster, S.M. & Wade, M.J. (2003) Mating Systems and Strategies. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Silk, J.B. (2007) The adaptive value of sociality in mammalian groups. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B362, 539–59.
Székely, T., More, A.J. & Komdeur, J. (2011) Introduction: the uphill climb of sociobiology: towards a new synthesis. In: Social Behaviour: Genes, Ecology and Evolution (Székely, T., More, A.J. & Komdeur, J., eds.). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 1–4.
Taber, A.B. & Macdonald, D.W. (1992) Communal breeding in the mara, Dolichotis patagonurn. Journal of Zoology London227, 439–52.
Tang-Martínez, Z. (2003) Emerging themes and future challenges: forgotten rodents, neglected questions. Journal of Mammalogy84, 1212–27.
Tinbergen, N. (1963) On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie20, 410–33.
Tress, G., Tress, B. & Fry, G. (2004) Clarifying integrative research concepts in landscape ecology. Landscape Ecology20, 479–93.
Uekita, T. & Okanoya, K. (2011) Hippocampus lesions induced deficits in social and spatial recognition in Octodon degus. Behavioural Brain Research219, 302–9.
Wersinger, S.R. (2009) The neuroscience of social behavior. Institute for Laboratory Animal Research Journal50, 1–4.
Wey, T.W., Burger, J.R., Ebensperger, L.A. & Hayes, L.D. (2013) Reproductive correlates of social network variation in plural breeding degus (Octodon degus). Animal Behaviour85, 1407–14.
Whitehead, H. (2008) Analyzing Animal Societies: Quantitative Methods for Vertebrate Social Analysis. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Wilson, E.O. (1976) Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Zupanc, G.K.H. (2010) Behavioral Neurobiology: An Integrative Approach. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Chapter 1
The caviomorph rodents
distribution and ecological diversification
Ricardo A. Ojeda, Agustina A. Ojeda & Agustina Novillo
Grupo de Investigaciones de la Biodiversidad (GiB), IADIZA (Instituto Argentino de Investigaciones de Zonas Áridas), CCT-Mendoza, CONICET, Mendoza, Argentina
1.1 The caviomorph radiation
The mammal fauna of the Neotropical Region can be divided into three distinct faunal strata reflecting younger and older faunal contingents (Simpson 1980; Webb & Marshall 1982). The first stratum is comprised of the ancient fauna of the early Cenozoic, which includes armadillos, giant anteaters, marsupials, and a diverse group of autochthonous ungulates. The second stratum is made up of allochthonous lineages, which includes hystricognath (caviomorph) rodents, primates, procyonids, cricetid rodents, emballonurid and vespertilionid bats, and trichechid sirenians. The third stratum includes other more recent taxa. Hystricognath rodents probably arrived from Africa between 45.4 and 36.7 Ma (Poux et al. 2006). The oldest known records of this South American endemic lineage are small (30–120g) caviomorph rodents, which occurred during the Middle Eocene in Peru (about 41 Ma; Antoine et al. 2012). Caviomorphs diversified rapidly into major clades during the Eocene-Oligocene (Antoine et al. 2012; Bertrand et al. 2012) and radiated in different directions, in terms of body size, ecomorphological, physiological, and behavioral traits. As a result, these animals exhibit ecological convergence with several orders of mammals such as hyracoids, ungulates, and lagomorphs (Eisenberg 1981; Mares & Ojeda 1982; Lessa et al. 2008). The rapid adaptive radiation and successful establishment of caviomorphs may have been due to colonization of a new region (South America) which was largely lacking competitors (ecological equivalents), with a diversity of food, habitat resources, and empty
niches. As Patterson and Pascual (1972) noted, these proto-caviomorphs, found a vaccuum leading to rapid diversification in the early Oligocene, when it is already possible to recognize all four caviomorph superfamilies
(p. 278).
The four superfamilies, Cavioidea, Chinchilloidea, Erethizontoidea, and Octodontoidea include 13 families, 56 genera and 246 extant species (Upham & Patterson 2012). The superfamily Cavioidea includes the Dasyproctidae (agouties), Cuniculidae (pacas), Caviidae (cavies and maras), and Hydrochoeridae (capybaras); the superfamily Erethizontoidea (New World porcupines) is monotypic with the Erethizontidae; Chinchilloidea includes the Chinchillidae (chinchillas, mountain and plains viscachas) and Dinomyidae; finally, the Octodontoidea includes the Abrocomidae, Octodontidae, Ctenomyidae, and Echimyidae (including Myocastoridae and Capromyidae) (Rowe et al. 2010; Upham & Patterson 2012; Pérez & Pol 2012). Within the Caviomorpha, the basal split consisted of the separation of the superfamilies Cavioidea + Erethizontoidea from Chinchilloidea + Octodontoidea, estimated at 37.9 Ma (middle to late Eocene). The Chinchilloidea/Octodontoidea divergence was inferred at 35.0 Ma (late Eocene), while the Cavioidea/Erethizontoidea separation occurred at the end of the Eocene (33.9 Ma) (Voloch et al. 2013).
Caviomorph rodents radiated and occupied a wide spectrum of landscapes, elevations, and habitats of the Neotropics, with one species, Erethizon dorsatum, ranging into the Neartic Region. Five families are mainly distributed thoughout the tropical region (Erethizontidae, Cuniculidae, Echymidae, Dinomyidae, and Dasyproctidae). The Myocastoridae, Ctenomyidae, and Chinchillidae are distributed in temperate regions, and Octodontidae and Abrocomidae are mostly distributed along the Andes. The family Caviidae exhibits the broadest distribution, covering most of South America. Overall, there are two major areas with the highest species density. One area includes the Amazonian biome, in the central-western region of tropical South America (between 0 and 10°S), and occupying the lowlands of western Brazil and the tropical Andean Forest of Ecuador and Peru. The other occurs within the Atlantic Forest along the eastern border of southern Brazil, (Upham & Patterson 2012; Ojeda et al. 2013; Ojeda et al. 2015).
Caviomorph rodents have diversified into an extraordinary variety of lifestyles and feeding niches, hereafter macroniches (sensu Eisenberg 1981; Chapter 2 in this book). A simplified illustrated version of some representatives of this functional diversity is shown in Fig. 1.1. Recent analyses on hypothetical ancestral areas and ecological states of major clades of caviomorphs and their diversification in new "adaptive zones" during their evolution in South America have suggested the Amazonia region as the ancestral area in the diversification of proto-caviomorphs, and that they were most likely associated with saxicolous modes of life and a diet based on grasses, leaves and fruits (Ojeda et al. 2015).
nfgz001anfgz001bnfgz001cFigure 1.1 Representative genera of major caviomorph macroniches combining modes of life, such as substrate use, and feeding habits. Original drawings by Benjamín Bender.
1.2 The families
1.2.1 Erethizontidae
Diversification and taxonomic relationships. Erethizontidae (New World porcupines) includes 7% and 6% of caviomorph genera and species respectively, with one genus distributed in North America (Erethizon). The family is comprised of 15 species belonging to at least three genera (Coendou, Erethizon, Chaetomys) (Voss 2011). The genus Sphiggurus is a synonym of Coendou, although other authors recognize Sphiggurus and Echinoprocta as distinct genera from the spiny Coendou (Bonvicino et al. 2002; Woods & Kilpatrick 2005). The Superfamilies Erethizontoidea and Cavioidea split in the early Oligocene and are strongly supported as sister groups (Upham & Patterson 2012; Fabre et al. 2013).
Distribution. The family Erethizontidae is distributed from the Arctic of North America to northern Mexico and east to the Appalachian mountains, and from southern Mexico through Ecuador and northern Argentina as its southernmost range (Fig. 1.2a). Only the genus Erethizon inhabits forests and wooded regions of the Neartic Region. Species density in South America is highest at the eastern margins of the continent, mainly in the Atlantic forest and Cerrado ecoregions, and decreases towards northern and southern geographic ranges (Fig. 1.2a). Historical biogeographical analyses suggest the Amazonia ecoregion as the most likely ancestral area of the Superfamilies Erethizontoidea and Cavioidea (Upham & Patterson 2012; Ojeda et al. 2015).
Niche use. Reconstructions of ancestral state macroniches suggest that New World porcupines evolved from an arboreal form with a diet based on fruits and leaves (Ojeda et al. 2015). The extant genera are arboreal (Chaetomys, Coendou, Sphiggurus) and scansorial (Echinoprocta, Erethizon) with herbivorous and frugivorous diets and a body mass ranging from 1,300 to 14,000 g (Table 1.1).
Sociality. New World porcupines have been classified as solitary (Coendou), and solitary and colonial (Erethizon).
nfgz002anfgz002bFigure 1.2 Geographic distribution of caviomorph families and species density.
Table 1.1 Ecoregions (see Fig. 1.2), macroniches, body mass (g), and general social behavior of caviomorph genera
Notes: Detailed information about caviomorph social systems can be found in Maher and Burger (Chapter 2 in this book).
1.2.2 Chinchillidae
Diversification and taxonomic relationships. The family Chinchillidae (Chinchillas, mountain viscachas, and plains viscachas) is composed of three genera, Chinchilla, Lagidium, and Lagostomus and seven species (Woods & Kilpatrick 2005). Chinchillidae and Dinomyidae (Dinomys) are strongly considered a monophyletic group forming the superfamily Chinchilloidea (Huchon & Douzery 2001; Spotorno et al. 2004; Poux et al. 2006; Huchon et al. 2007; Rowe et al. 2010; Upham & Patterson 2012).
Distribution. Species of Chinchillidae are distributed along the central and southern Andes and throughout most of Patagonia in southern South America, extending latitudinally from 5–40° S (Fig. 1.2b). Most species of Chinchilla and Lagidium are restricted to the arid landscapes of the Andes (Altiplano and Puna) and Patagonia, inhabiting rocky surfaces and among boulders. However, the plains viscacha, Lagostomus maximus, occurs in the Pampas, the Monte desert, and the Chacoan thorn scrub. The highest density of species is located in the arid Andes of Peru and Bolivia (Fig. 1.2b).
Niche use. Reconstructions of ancestral state macroniches suggest that Chinchillidae evolved from an herbivorous, saxicollous ancestor. Some species retained this condition whereas others (e.g. Lagostomus maximus) evolved a fossorial mode of life. The body size of chinchillids ranges between 390 g and 8840 g. Suggested pairs of ecological equivalents are: Lagostomus, Cynomys, Chinchilla, Otospermophilus, Sciurotamias, Lagidium (see Mares & Ojeda 1982).
Sociality. Chinchillas, mountain viscachas, and plains viscachas live in social groups of up to several hundred individuals (Table 1.1; Chapter 2 in this book).
1.2.3 Dinomyidae
Diversification and taxonomic relationships. This family belongs to the group of the giant, long-headed rodents which were quite diverse in the Late Miocene (Rinderknecht et al. 2011). The extant representative belongs to a monotypic genus known as pacarana, Dinomys branickii. Based on recent molecular analyses, Dinomys is strongly allied with members of the Chinchillidae (Huchon & Douzery 2001; Spotorno et al. 2004; Poux et al. 2006; Huchon et al. 2007; Rowe et al. 2010; Upham & Patterson 2012) and constitutes a monophyletic Chinchilloidea superfamily, with Lagostomus occasionally considered as sister species to Dinomys (Upham & Patterson 2012).
Distribution. The pacarana has a latitudinal geographic range from about 10° S to 18° S, through the tropical forest of western Amazonia and the Andean foothills of Venezuela, Colombia, Ecuador, and Peru to the Bolivian Yungas (Fig. 1.2c). The fossil distribution of the family was broader, with specimens known from Argentina, Colombia, Venezuela, and Uruguay (Rinderknecht et al. 2011).
Niche use. The natural history of the pacarana is practically unknown, and most behavioral repertoire has been recorded from captive animals (White & Alberico 1992). Eisenberg and Redford (1999) suggested that pacaranas are nocturnal, eat a diet of fruits, leaves and herbs, and are climbers (Table 1.1).
Sociality. Pacaranas live in social groups of up to five individuals (White & Alberico 1992, and references therein). Pacaranas display an elaborate communication system, with foot stomping, teeth chattering and different vocalizations (Collins & Eisenberg 1972; Eisenberg 1974).
1.2.4 Caviidae
Diversification and taxonomic relationships. The caviids are a diverse clade divided into three widely divergent extant lineages: Caviinae (cavies), Dolichotinae (maras), and Hydrochoerinae (capybaras). These three major lineages are well differentiated from a morphological and ecological perspective (Pérez & Pol 2012). The family is comprised of six genera (Cavia, Galea, Microcavia, Dolichotis, Hydrochoerus, and Kerodon) and 18 species (Woods & Kilpatrick 2005). Their main diversification occurred during the middle to late Miocene and they became morphologically and taxonomically diverse in the late Miocene (Pérez & Pol 2012). The available fossil record suggests that the initial split of Caviidae into three major lineages may have occurred around 11.8–13.5 Ma and that three modern and morphologically distinct lineages of Caviidae (Hydrochoerinae, Dolichotinae, and Caviinae) were already present and diverse about 6.1–9.07 Ma (Pérez & Pol 2012).
Distribution. Caviids are distributed across the South American continent and their habitats include almost all major South American biomes, from moist savannas to thorn forests and desert scrublands. The highest species density occurs in the dry south-central Andes in the Cerrado and Caatinga ecoregions (Fig. 1.2d).
Niche use. The ancestral proto-caviid is hypothesized to have been terrestrial with a diet of grasses and leaves (Ojeda et al. 2015). Their macroniche radiation was primarily in the modes of locomotion, with terrestrial (Dolichotis), semiaquatic (Hydrochoerus), semifossorial (Microcavia), and saxicolous (Kerodon) species. However, this characterization for