Discover millions of ebooks, audiobooks, and so much more with a free trial

From $11.99/month after trial. Cancel anytime.

John Bowlby - From Psychoanalysis to Ethology: Unravelling the Roots of Attachment Theory
John Bowlby - From Psychoanalysis to Ethology: Unravelling the Roots of Attachment Theory
John Bowlby - From Psychoanalysis to Ethology: Unravelling the Roots of Attachment Theory
Ebook316 pages4 hours

John Bowlby - From Psychoanalysis to Ethology: Unravelling the Roots of Attachment Theory

Rating: 4 out of 5 stars

4/5

()

Read preview

About this ebook

This accessible book draws on unique evidence from oral histories and little-known archive material to shed new light on the working relationships which led to John Bowlby’s shift from psychoanalysis to ethology as a frame of reference – and ultimately to the development of attachment theory.
  • A unique exploration of the origins of Bowlby’s ideas and the critical transformation in his thinking – offers an alternative to standard accounts of the origin of attachment theory
  • Explores the significance of Bowlby’s influential working relationships with Robert Hinde, Harry Harlow, James Robertson and Mary Ainsworth
  • Provides students, academics, and practitioners with clear insights into the development of attachment theory
  • Accessible to general readers interested in psychology and psychoanalysis
LanguageEnglish
PublisherWiley
Release dateMar 21, 2011
ISBN9781119996262
John Bowlby - From Psychoanalysis to Ethology: Unravelling the Roots of Attachment Theory

Related to John Bowlby - From Psychoanalysis to Ethology

Related ebooks

Psychology For You

View More

Related articles

Reviews for John Bowlby - From Psychoanalysis to Ethology

Rating: 4 out of 5 stars
4/5

1 rating0 reviews

What did you think?

Tap to rate

Review must be at least 10 words

    Book preview

    John Bowlby - From Psychoanalysis to Ethology - Frank C. P. van der Horst

    Contents

    About the Author

    Foreword

    Acknowledgments

    Introduction

    1 Biographical Notes and Early Career

    Childhood, Upbringing, and Schooling

    Studying Medicine at Cambridge

    Bowlby and Progressive Schooling

    Different Schools of Psychoanalysis in Interbellum Britain⁵

    Medical and Psychoanalytic Training

    From Kleinian Psychoanalysis to Real Life

    Bowlby’s Forty-four Juvenile Thieves

    Personal Life During the Pre-war Years

    Concluding Remarks

    2 Loneliness in Infancy: The WHO Report and Issues of Separation

    Separation as a Theme

    Issues of Separation: Evacuation of Children

    Issues of Separation: Observations in Residential Nurseries

    Issues of Separation: Visits to Children in Hospital

    Issues of Separation: Studies on the Hospitalization Effect

    Bowlby and the WHO Report on Deprivation

    Concluding Remarks

    3 Working with James Robertson: The Importance of Observation

    Research Work at the Tavistock in Post-War Years

    James Robertson’s Background and Interest in Separation

    The French Connection

    Identification of Typical Responses to Separation: Protest, Despair, Denial

    A Picture is Worth a Thousand Words

    Divergence of Opinions Between Robertson and Bowlby

    Other Contributions by the Separation Research Unit: Heinicke’s Brief Separations

    Concluding Remarks: Unexplained Observations

    4 Bowlby’s Acquaintance with Ethology: The Work of Lorenz, Tinbergen, and Hinde

    First Acquaintance with Ethology

    The Rise of Ethology as a Discipline

    Lorenz’s The Companion in the Bird’s World⁴

    Tinbergen’s The Study of Instinct and the Four Whys

    Bowlby’s Personal Relationships with Lorenz and Tinbergen

    Early Interactions Between Bowlby and Hinde

    Hinde’s Early Career and Bowlby’s Subsequent Influence

    Hinde’s Influence on Bowlby

    Theoretical Issues: Instinct and Psychoanalysis

    Theoretical Issues: Environment of Evolutionary Adaptedness

    Applying Ethology to Attachment Behavior: Tinbergen’s Four Whys

    Concluding Remarks

    5 From Theoretical Claims to Empirical Evidence: Harry Harlow and the Nature of Love

    Toward Empirical Confirmation

    Harlow’s Early Career

    Ethology and Animal Psychology: Contrasting Approaches to Animal Behavior

    Harlow and Bowlby Become Acquainted in 1957

    Mutual Referencing after 1957

    Harlow and Bowlby Finally Meet in 1958

    Ethology Further Emphasized in Bowlby’s Work

    Mutual Contacts: The CIBA Symposia from 1959 to 1965

    Bowlby’s Writings in the Early 1960s: Using Harlow’s Empirical Findings as a Secure Base

    Harlow’s Research in the 1960s: Seeking Empirical Evidence for Bowlby’s Theoretical Claims

    Bowlby’s Continuing Interest in Harlow’s Work

    Sociogram

    Concluding Remarks

    6 Mary Ainsworth’s Role in the Study of Attachment

    Ainsworth’s Contributions to Attachment Theory

    Ainsworth’s Early Life and Career

    William Blatz and Security Theory

    Working on Separation at the Tavistock Clinic with Bowlby and Robertson

    Naturalistic Observation in Uganda

    Back to Baltimore and Back in Touch with Bowlby

    Ainsworth at the CIBA Symposia in the 1960s

    The Origin and Development of the Secure Base Concept

    Developing the Strange Situation Procedure

    Concluding Remarks

    Conclusions

    The Role of Observation and Experiment

    Bowlby’s Method of Theory Construction

    Theory Construction and the Nature of Theory Change

    Bowlby’s Scientific Descent

    References

    Name Index

    Subject Index

    c01f001.eps

    This edition first published 2011

    © 2011 Frank C. P. van der Horst

    Wiley-Blackwell is an imprint of John Wiley & Sons, formed by the merger of Wiley’s global Scientific, Technical, and Medical business with Blackwell Publishing.

    Registered Office

    John Wiley & Sons Ltd, The Atrium, Southern Gate, Chichester, West Sussex,

    PO19 8SQ, UK

    Editorial Offices

    The Atrium, Southern Gate, Chichester, West Sussex, PO19 8SQ, UK

    9600 Garsington Road, Oxford, OX4 2DQ, UK

    350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148-5020, USA

    For details of our global editorial offices, for customer services, and for information about how to apply for permission to reuse the copyright material in this book please see our website at www.wiley.com/wiley-blackwell.

    The right of Frank C. P. van der Horst to be identified as the author of this work has been asserted in accordance with the UK Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.

    All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, except as permitted by the UK Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988, without the prior permission of the publisher.

    Wiley also publishes its books in a variety of electronic formats. Some content that appears in print may not be available in electronic books.

    Designations used by companies to distinguish their products are often claimed as trademarks. All brand names and product names used in this book are trade names, service marks, trademarks or registered trademarks of their respective owners. The publisher is not associated with any product or vendor mentioned in this book. This publication is designed to provide accurate and authoritative information in regard to the subject matter covered. It is sold on the understanding that the publisher is not engaged in rendering professional services. If professional advice or other expert assistance is required, the services of a competent professional should be sought.

    Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

    Van der Horst, Frank C.P.

    John Bowlby : from psychoanalysis to ethology : unravelling the roots of attachment theory / Frank C.P. van der Horst.

    p. cm.

    Includes bibliographical references and index.

    ISBN 978-0-470-68364-4 (cloth)

    1. Bowlby, John. 2. Attachment behavior. 3. Attachment behavior in children. 4. Psychoanalysis. 5. Human behavior. I. Title.

    BF575.A86V36 2011

    155.4′18–dc22

    2010046385

    A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library.

    This book is published in the following electronic formats: ePDF 9781119993117; Wiley Online Library 9781119993100

    To Francisca

    About the Author

    Frank C.P. van der Horst was born on August 5, 1977 in Delft, the Netherlands. He obtained a Master’s in History and one in Education and Child Studies from Leiden University and was offered the chance to do research on a crossroads of both disciplines. Under the supervision of Professors Van der Veer and Van IJzendoorn – both notable experts in the field – he expanded on the historical and theoretical work at the Centre for Child and Family Studies at Leiden University and focused on the cross-fertilization of ethology and attachment theory, resulting in his Ph.D. thesis John Bowlby and ethology: A study of cross-fertilization. During this project, he discovered many hitherto unknown data in private and public archives and used oral histories to relate the historical development of John Bowlby’s ideas.

    Currently, Van der Horst works as a psychologist at De Waag Rotterdam, an outpatient clinic for forensic psychiatry, and as a researcher at the Centre for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University. His research aims at describing the history of ideas in the behavioral sciences, such as the ideas and work of John Bowlby, René Spitz, William Goldfarb, Jean Piaget, and Harry Harlow. His aim is also to further unravel the roots of attachment theory in, for example, the work of James Robertson and that of developmental psychologist Mary Ainsworth. These studies are necessary to complete our picture of the historical embeddedness of theoretical innovations in developmental psychology.

    Foreword

    by Professor Jerome Kagan

    A question guaranteed to pique the interest of scientists and non-scientists asks, for each phenomenon, to what degree was this event constrained by its past and, therefore, potentially predictable? That is why millions of viewers will watch documentaries describing the origin of our universe or the evolution of life forms. The imprint of the past on the present assumes a special significance when we ask why some adults believe they are less happy than their friends. Economists and political scientists who gather questionnaire evidence from adults in varied societies report that extreme poverty and serious illness are a major cause of unhappiness. Philosophers, on the other hand, argue that a failure to honor one’s ethical standards is the enemy of continued serenity or joy. A reasonable proportion of biologists and psychologists share the belief held by Alice James, the younger sister of William and Henry James, that a sanguine or melancholic mood is due, in part, to one’s genes.

    The strong claim that the experiences of early childhood make a major contribution to adult moods, and by inference quality of adaptation, is relatively recent in the history of ideas. This notion began to gain credence in Europe during the eighteenth century after the size of the middle class had grown and many mothers could stay at home rather than work in the field or at a loom. As a result, society needed to find a critical function for these women and influential commentators nominated a mother’s love for her child as the elixir necessary for healthy psychological development. A large proportion of contemporary psychiatrists and psychologists are certain that the quality of the infant’s emotional relationship with its primary caretaker, preferably the biological mother, affected the probability that the adult will or will not be vulnerable to frequent bouts of anxiety or depression.

    John Bowlby tweaked consensual opinion when he replaced the infant’s need for the sensory pleasures accompanying feeding, which Freud had nominated as formative, with the more psychological need for a feeling of security that a loving mother provides. Bowlby’s childhood may have prepared him for this hypothesis. As the fourth child in an upper-middle-class, English family whose care was entrusted to nursemaids who tended the young Bowlby and his siblings in rooms on the top floor of the family’s spacious home, Bowlby recalled seeing his mother for about an hour a day and his father once a week. In addition, his favorite nurse left the household when Bowlby was only four years old. Bowlby spent a long, productive career trying to prove that the quality of the interaction between the mother and infant constrained the child’s future in a serious way.

    Frank van der Horst’s beautifully crafted, even-handed history of the 50-year interval between Bowlby’s intuition that infants deprived of maternal love were at risk for an unhappy life and Mary Ainsworth’s 1978 volume, Patterns of Attachment, provides readers with a trail of facts so rich in detail each can decide on the validity of Bowlby’s bold idea.

    The Bowlby narrative can be divided into four relatively discrete stages. The first centers around Bowlby’s brief employment at one of the many progressive schools established in the 1920s as an educational application of Freud’s ideas. Most of the children attending this school were emotionally disturbed and some had experienced early separation from parents or obvious neglect. Bowlby regarded these facts as supporting the writings of Ian Suttie, with whom Bowlby was familiar, arguing that a mother’s love for the child was as vital for its psychological development as good nutrition was for its physical growth.

    The second stage began in 1946 when Bowlby joined the staff of the Tavistock Clinic where he met James Robertson, a social worker, who shared Bowlby’s distress over the policy of hospitals forbidding mothers from visiting their young children hospitalized for surgery because the staff wanted to prevent the mothers from bringing infections into the ward. Robertson’s 1952 film, A Two-Year-Old Goes to Hospital, had an emotional impact on audiences far greater than either Bowlby or Robertson had anticipated. I showed this film for several years to large undergraduate classes at Harvard and always noted tears flowing down the faces of the stunned audiences when the film ended.

    Van der Horst documents the significance of Bowlby’s reading of the ethological research of Lorenz, Tinbergen, and Hinde and the fruitful collaboration with the American psychologist Harry Harlow. Harlow’s experiments on rhesus monkeys, which implied that the opportunity to cling to something soft was more critical than food in establishing a bond to a surrogate object, and the ethological investigations allowed Bowlby to detect the relevance of the concept of a releaser and the reactions it evoked. He applied these ideas to the mother–infant relationship by arguing that the face, hair, and hands of the mother acted as releasers for the infant’s grasping, clinging, smiling, and babbling which, through their implementation, established an attachment bond between infant and caretaker. Bowlby regarded the attachment process as analogous to the imprinting of newly hatched goslings which follow the first moving object they see.

    Mary Ainsworth’s research in Uganda and her invention of the Strange Situation at Johns Hopkins University represent the final stage in Van der Horst’s reconstruction. Ainsworth joined Bowlby’s staff at the Tavistock because her husband decided to work in London for a short period and she needed a job. Ainsworth had been mentored by William Blatz at the University of Toronto and the latter shared Bowlby’s conviction that the infant’s security was critical for healthy development. Hence, Ainsworth came to Bowlby prepared to share his fundamental premises. She began to develop her ideas systematically after spending 1954–55 in Uganda observing 28 Ganda infants. This experience led her to posit the three categories of secure attachment, insecure attachment, and non-attachment. Ainsworth acknowledged in the 1967 book Infancy in Uganda that the infant’s temperament can influence the mother–child relationship and she wrote, We must concede that there are genetically based individual differences between babies … It is quite impossible to differentiate genetic, prenatal, and perinatal influences from environmental influences (p. 387).

    Ainsworth described one infant she had classified as non-attached because the girl did not cry or attempt to follow the mother when she left the room, even though this child struck her as happy and comfortable. I suspect this girl belonged to the temperamental group my colleagues and I call low-reactive. Ainsworth also described a small group of infants classified as insecurely attached because they cried frequently – this is category she would later call type C, insecure-resistant. Ainsworth had noticed that these children were chronically fussy babies who cried not only when their mother left them, but also when they were held by the mother. Always a careful observer, Ainsworth informs readers that two of these infants were chronically malnourished. Crying because of hunger should not be equated with crying because of an insecure attachment. In a telling sentence toward the end of the book Ainsworth suggested, Therefore, the warmth of the mother and her observed affectionate contact behavior do not explain the differences between groups (p. 394), and she later adds, There is no evidence that care by several people necessarily interferes with the development of healthy attachment (p. 395).

    However, Ainsworth and her three colleagues failed to mention the role of temperament when they summarized the results of the study of Baltimore infants in Patterns of Attachment. This investigation, based on extensive observations of a small group of infants at home over the course of the first year and their reactions in the Strange Situation at one year, was the origin of the currently popular attachment categories called type B, securely attached, and types A and C, insecurely attached.

    The events of these four stages came together like a perfect storm in the Bowlby and Ainsworth volumes to raise the consciousness of Americans and Europeans to the possibly dangerous consequences of an infant’s insecure attachment to its parents. Historians will praise Van der Horst for a critically neutral narrative, free of the author’s prejudices, that allows each reader to judge the validity of the Bowlby–Ainsworth inferences.

    Nonetheless, it is possible to discern beneath the surface of the seamless prose the possible effects of the historical context in which Bowlby formed his ideas. Wise commentators on human nature have been brooding on the causes of human unhappiness for a long time. Because assigning significance to a mother who failed to be sensitive to her infant’s need for security did not emerge as an explanation until the twentieth century, it is appropriate to speculate that the unique pattern of conditions during the last century that made quality of attachment an attractive explanatory concept. The changes in the family during the twentieth century were discontinuous with the form that dominated Europe and North America during the prior two centuries. Young mothers began to enter the work force in large numbers after 1945 and needed surrogate care for their young children. This disruption in the usual form of infant care worried many citizens who automatically assumed that this novel arrangement probably had malevolent consequences. Richard Kearsley, Philip Zelazo, and I shared that belief in the late 1970s when we studied the effect of day care on Boston infants during their first 29 months.

    It is also relevant that the first half of the last century was marked by two destructive wars, the horrors of the Holocaust, an undisguised challenge to the concept of God (a Time magazine cover during this period declared, God is Dead), increased geographic mobility, and erosion in the moral authority of traditional elites. It is reasonable to assume that this blizzard of events provoked bouts of uncertainty in many adults who naturally searched for an explanation of their cognitive dissonance. Because Americans and Europeans regard the results of scientific research as providing the most reliable access to correct explanations, and Western scientists typically look to distant origins to account for the present, it was easy to be persuaded by statements from respected scientists that the causes of the contemporary angst lay in the events of early childhood. I suspect, however, that this explanation had its roots in the insecurity of the adults who projected their mood on to the young child. Some nineteenth-century scholars were convinced that the origin of adult greed could be seen in the newborn’s reflex grasping of a pencil placed in the palm. If middle-class adults from the majority ethnic and religious groups in their society, who suffered neither extreme poverty nor bigotry, believe that their current tensions and anxieties are due, in part, to their infant experiences, most will be able to retrieve enough evidence to construct an explanation that places some of the blame on their parents. Equally unhappy African-American and Hispanic-American adults who were raised in poverty in urban ghettoes usually blame their mood on their society not their mothers.

    The evidence does indicate that serious neglect or abuse of infants can have undesirable effects on their future psychological development. No one quarrels with that opinion. But the vast majority of infants escape these real threats to psychological growth. It is less obvious that the normal variation in maternal sensitivity to infants has the power to constrain the future indefinitely, unless adults, reflecting on their childhoods, decide that they should have had more loving care than they believe they received. Millions of Chinese infants born during Mao Zedong’s reign rarely saw their parents and spent most of their days in day care centers with many infants and few caretakers. But I know of no evidence suggesting that the 50- to 70-year-olds currently living in the People’s Republic of China, who spent their early years in these centers, are significantly more anxious, depressed, addicted, or incarcerated than the generations born 30 years earlier or later. Indeed, today’s headlines imply that feelings of insecurity may be more salient today in a capitalist China with a one child policy and fewer day care centers than in the China of the 1960s.

    This foreword is not the place to review the extensive evidence bearing on the validity of Bowlby’s hypothesis and the child’s behavior in the Strange Situation. However, it is fair to suggest that the available evidence neither proves nor refutes Bowlby’s creative synthesis with certainty. The existing data are simply inadequate for a consensus on these questions and, therefore, impossible to decide whether we should reject or accept these ideas. But no matter what future research reveals, scientists, parents, and other citizens will profit from Van der Horst’s coherent, gracefully written, even-handed, and richly detailed description of how these important ideas rose to a position of prominence.

    Acknowledgments

    The present book is the fruit of my work at the Centre for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University, the Netherlands, between 2004 and 2010. During these years, I was guided by Professor René van der Veer, whom I consider my scientific mentor. His warm-hearted, enthusiastic, and humane support was of immeasurable importance to the outcome of my scientific work. I am grateful to him for keeping up with my ignorance.

    The many discussions with René – back and forth from our offices to the coffee machine – have contributed much to the result that lies in front of you. On the basis of a rough estimation of the distance we covered, I calculated that during these discussions, we walked from Leiden to Paris, and back. During this trip to the City of Light we consumed several thousands cups of coffee. I am very thankful that we were spared from both blisters and ulcers.

    Over the years, many people have contributed to my research on the roots of attachment theory. I am particularly indebted to Inge Bretherton for reading and extensively commenting on earlier drafts of this book. Her knowledgeability with respect to the history of attachment theory is unprecedented. Several people were willing to be interviewed on the cross-fertilization of attachment theory and ethology: Inge Bretherton, Robert Hinde, Joan Stevenson-Hinde, Helen LeRoy, Howard Steele, Stephen Suomi, and Everett Waters. I thank Marinus van IJzendoorn for his suggestions and comments to several chapters in this book. René van

    Enjoying the preview?
    Page 1 of 1