3
Ethnic identities
Magda Borkowska, James Nazroo, Nissa Finney and Joseph Harrison
Key findings
Ethnic identity is important to people alongside a strong sense of belonging to British
society but standardised measures of ethnicity do not fully capture the complex ways
that people describe their ethnicity.
• The free-text ethnic identity responses demonstrate that the standardised ethnic
categories do not allow people to accurately express complex ethnic origins and
migration experiences; they exclude identities from certain parts of the world and
subnational, place-based identities.
• Ethnic identity is important for most people from minority backgrounds. This is
especially true for those from Black African, Black Caribbean, Pakistani, White Irish and
Jewish backgrounds. Ethnic identity is the least important for White British people,
followed by people from White Eastern European, White Other, and Mixed White and
Asian backgrounds.
• Religious belonging varies considerably across ethnic groups. People from Bangladeshi,
Pakistani, Black African, Arab and Indian backgrounds most frequently report having
a religion. Those from White British, Mixed White and Asian, and Mixed White and
Black Caribbean backgrounds most frequently declare having no religious affiliation.
• Strong religious attachment is more common when people identify with minority
religions and when there tends to be a consistency between ethnic identity and
religious affiliation.
• Most people from ethnic minority backgrounds participate in practices linked to
their ethnicity or religion. White British are the least likely to report participation
in such practices, followed by White Irish and White Eastern Europeans. Eating food
associated with one’s ethnic or religious background is the most popular practice
across ethnic groups.
• A sense of belonging to British society is very high across all groups. A particularly high
sense of belonging is reported by those from Bangladeshi, Indian, Pakistani, Black African,
Black Other, Arab, Jewish and White British backgrounds. A strong sense of belonging
to English, Scottish and Welsh societies is somewhat less common among people from
ethnic minority backgrounds compared to those from a White British background.
30
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
Ethnic identities
Introduction
In the UK, we have become used to filling in ethnicity classification
forms for a range of administrative purposes and are commonly offered a
standardised set of categories derived from the census. The use of a common
set of categories has the advantage of tracking ethnic and racial inequalities
over time, offers consistency across datasets and enables comparisons with
the population census. However, there is a risk that much is missed by the
standardisation of ethnic categories. For example, we cannot accurately
capture the increasingly diverse, changing population using the limited
number of standardised ethnic categories. We also do not know how strongly
people identify with their ethnic, racial, national or religious groups and
what these identities mean for them in everyday life.
This chapter explores articulations of and attachment to ethnic and
religious identities. Additionally, the sense of belonging to British, English,
Scottish and Welsh societies is examined across ethnic groups. This is
possible with the Evidence for Equality National Survey (EVENS) data
because, in addition to including standardised ethnic categories, EVENS
enabled people to describe their ethnic identity in their own words and to
indicate how significant ethnic, religious, national and subnational identities
were to them. The survey also asked them about their everyday practices
related to ethnic and religious identifications. By examining responses on
ethnic identification, we can reflect upon what is (and is not) captured by
standardised ethnic group categorisation.
Theoretical conceptualisations of ethnicity acknowledge that ethnic
identities are socially constructed and shaped by many factors, including
ancestry or country of origin, skin colour, religious beliefs, culture and
language (Aspinall, 1997). Most importantly, however, ethnic identity also
refers to a subjective sense of belonging to a particular ethnic community.
Similar to other group identities, the sense of belonging to an ethnic group
is a dynamic and fluid process rather than a fixed construct. Just like other
group identities, it is also highly context-dependent and relative to a frame
of reference as outlined by social identity theory (Tajfel and Turner, 1979).
Over time, there has been a growing recognition among researchers
that ethnic identity is a complex and multidimensional phenomenon that
extends beyond simple self-identification with a particular ethnic identity
label. To measure such a complex construct across different ethnic groups,
Phinney (1992) developed a widely used multidimensional psychological
scale, the Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure, which comprises three main
subscales: (1) self-identification and the extent of positive feelings towards
one’s group; (2) the extent of having a developed, secure ethnic identity;
and (3) participation in activities associated with one’s ethnic identity. The
questions included in EVENS tap into domains (1) and (3).
31
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
Racism and Ethnic Inequality in a Time of Crisis
Having positive ethnic and/or religious identities might be associated
with many practical and emotional benefits. There is a general agreement
that positive attachment to ethnic identity is likely to increase psychosocial
functioning, that is, it might positively affect psychological wellbeing and
self-esteem, and can protect members of ethnic minority groups from
the negative consequences of experiencing racial discrimination (Roberts
et al, 1999; Umaña-Taylor, 2011). For minority groups, participating in
ethnicity- and/or religion-related practices might provide a safe space for
people to interact with others, build a positive sense of self and foster a sense
of belonging. Furthermore, religious institutions have long served as hubs of
social and civic life as well as places offering practical advice and charitable
activities. As noted by Nicholson (2018), for migrant communities, churches,
mosques, gurdwaras, temples and synagogues play a particularly important
role for connection and practical support in a new country.
Ethnic and religious identities not only constitute building blocks of selfconcept but are also used as social markers (Kapadia and Bradby, 2021),
which affect how group boundaries are defined and used in a society. For
example, in the UK, the ethnicity classifications have been introduced with
the intention of better understanding and monitoring social inequalities
among different social groups that share common origin/ancestry (Williams
and Husk, 2013). However, it is important to acknowledge that such
ethnicity categorisations are defined and to some extent imposed by the
more powerful ‘majority’ on the less powerful ‘minority’ (Nazroo and
Karlsen, 2003). This means that, in part, minority ethnic identities become
constructed in response to externally defined ethnic groupings. The use of
such categorisations can in turn marginalise certain ethnic minority groups.
The process of categorisation makes groups more or less visible and situates
them within debates on integration, social cohesion and British values. Every
few years, the debates on the national identity crisis resurface, especially in the
context of growing ethnic and religious diversity and immigration (Finney
and Simpson, 2009). Feelings of belonging to the national community are
generally believed to have many positive consequences, including greater
social cohesion and a sense of solidarity. Focus on cohesion and solidarity
has characterised government reports on diversity in recent years (see, for
example, Casey Review, 2016). Such discussions led to the turn against
policies of multiculturalism and the emphasis on shared national values
as underpinning integration. This has resulted in policies such as more
demanding citizenship tests and mandatory citizenship ceremonies, with
the aim of ensuring the ‘successful integration’ of naturalised citizens. The
ideology behind and the success of such practices have been contested (Byrne,
2017), but the appetite for practices that intend to facilitate a common
sense of British identity and belonging have remained popular in political
discourse. For example, since 2014, schools in the UK have been required
32
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
Ethnic identities
to introduce the active promotion of British values into their curricula
(Department for Education, 2014).
Despite concerns about a low sense of national belonging among ethnic
minority groups in political and media discourses, academic studies have
consistently shown that ethnic minority people feel strongly attached to
British society and do not perceive incompatibility between their ethnic and
religious identities and British values (Nazroo and Karlsen, 2003; Finney
and Simpson, 2009; Maxwell, 2009; Manning and Roy, 2010; Demireva and
Heath, 2014; Nandi and Platt, 2014; Karlsen and Nazroo, 2015). Research
has also found strong sense of belonging among ethnic minority groups to
local areas (see Chapter 6). These findings suggest that people do not tend to
perceive their national, ethnic and religious identities as mutually exclusive,
but rather as complementary.
Given the inevitable limitations of the standardised ethnic identity
classifications for accurately reflecting how people understand their ethnic
identities, in this chapter, we reflect on key ways of describing ethnicity used
by respondents outside the predefined ethnic categories. By doing this, we
aim to better understand which aspects of ethnic identity are missing in the
existing classifications and what additional ethnicity categories should be
considered in the future to better reflect the diversity of the UK population.
The first empirical section of this chapter gives an overview of the common
types of ethnic identity articulations expressed by EVENS participants in the
free text responses. It also reflects on the consequences of growing ethnic
diversity on the existing standardised classifications. The second section
focuses on the questions concerning the subjective importance of group
identities. In particular, it asks the following questions: how important are
ethnic and religious identities to people? Are there substantive differences in
the strength of attachment to ethnic identity among people from different
ethnic and religious backgrounds? How much do people engage in practices
related to their ethnic backgrounds? Finally, the last section explores sense
of belonging to British society across different ethnic groups and compares
it to the sense of belonging to English, Scottish and Welsh societies.
How do people describe their ethnic background?
This section provides a snapshot of the ways in which respondents described
their ethnicity in response to an open-ended write-in question which
asked: ‘How would you describe your ethnic background in your own
words?’ All answers were classified into one of three categories: ‘standardised
ethnicity articulation’, ‘non-standardised ethnicity articulation’ or ‘nonengagement’. ‘Standardised ethnicity articulation’ category includes
people who described their ethnicity using the same words that are used
in the standardised ONS ethnicity categories. ‘Non-standardised ethnicity
33
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
Racism and Ethnic Inequality in a Time of Crisis
articulation’ includes people who expressed their identities using either
non-standardised conceptualisations of ethnicity (that is, they referred to
concepts other than race, ethnicity, religion or nationality) or used different
language from the language used in standardised ethnicity categories. Finally,
the ‘non-engagement’ category refers to respondents who did not engage
at all with the open-ended question.
Figure 3.1 shows the distribution of the types of ethnicity articulations
for the 21 standardised ethnic groups used in the EVENS. First, it can be
noted that the majority of respondents in most ethnic groups did engage
with the open-ended ethnicity question and provided at least a short,
written description of their ethnic identity. Second, for most ethnic groups,
those respondents who provided an answer were likely to use standardised
concepts and language to describe their ethnic identity. This relatively
high consistency between the write-in ethnicity articulations and the
standardised ethnicity categories – shown in the ‘standardised’ segments in
Figure 3.1 – is likely to reflect that most people in the UK are very familiar
with administrative ethnicity categories, which are conventionally used for
monitoring purposes in almost all public service settings (including health,
education and employment). However, a substantial proportion in each
ethnic group expressed their ethnic identity in a non-standardised way.
The highest proportion of non-standardised articulations was found among
people from Jewish, White Eastern European, White Gypsy/Traveller and
Chinese backgrounds, and those who classified themselves as belonging to
various ‘Other’ ethnic groups (Figure 3.1).
The common complexities expressed by those who used non-standardised
articulations often reflected their complex ethno- racial origin and/ or
migration journey(s). As expected, the complexities of ethno-racial origin
were particularly highlighted by those who chose different variations of
‘Other’ ethnicity categories. Some of those who chose ‘Any other ethnic
group’ pointed out that their ethnic origin was simply missing from the
ONS classification. For example, as illustrated by the first two responses in
Table 3.1, people from the Americas currently do not have more specific
ethnicity categories to choose from. Other responses indicated that the ‘Any
other’ standardised ethnicity category often includes people with complex
ethno-racial origins who think of themselves as British. Similar reasoning
might be applied to other examples presented for ‘Other Arab’, ‘Other
Asian’ and ‘Other Black’ categories, where the respondents refer to their
complex (usually non-White) ethnic origins, but also highlight that they
generally see themselves as British. The two responses shown in Table 3.1
from respondents who selected the ‘Other White’ category demonstrate
different types of commonly mentioned complexities: (1) the fact that
people’s migration journeys and, in particular, the experiences of forced
migration and persecution are important reference points for ethnic identity
34
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
•
•
Figure 3.1: Proportion of respondents with standardised and complex
articulations of ethnicity, by ethnic group
23
66
11
42
35
22
41
46
13
18
70
12
44
39
17
38
36
27
16
42
43
13
43
43
45
39
35
3
27
13
60
18
44
38
14
35
51
45
19
36
50
19
32
15
46
39
50
5
23
26
52
27
21
36
11
53
11
73
16
10%
18
19
63
62
0%
15
20%
30%
Non-engagement
40%
50%
60%
70%
80%
90% 100%
Standardised
Note: Weighted percentages. Base: Full sample. N=12,685
Complex
Ethnic identities
35
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
White Irish
White Eastern European
Gypsy/Traveller
Roma
Jewish
Any other White background
Indian
Pakistani
Bangladeshi
Mixed White and Asian
Chinese
Any other Asian background
Black Caribbean
Mixed White and Black Caribbean
Black African
Mixed White and Black African
Any other Black background
Arab
Any other mixed/multiple background
Any other ethnic group
White British
• Table 3.1: Examples of complex articulations of ethnicity, by ethnic group •
Any other
ethnic group
• I would describe my ethnic background as "Latina", which personally, I think of it as a mixture between South American and European
people. Usually I don't see any option that I feel describe my ethnic background when I am asked to record my ethnicity, it seems like
they forget of the people from the American continent.
• I am a member of the confederated tribes of the Siletz Indians. What some describe as a Native American. I am not an "other-other"
as described on this survey, or on NHS forms. Natives from N. America, S. America, and Australia are completely ignored by the NHS.
• I am born in Kenya, great great grand parents from India, brown skinned but of African origin. However I consider myself British.
36
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
Other ethnic
group: Arab
• I am Middle Eastern. I came from an Arabic speaking country located in Africa and I am Muslim.
Other ethnic
group: White
• My ethnic background is complex due to political persecution and exile of some of my family members from previous generations
from their native country.
• I'm ethnically Iraqi. Both of my parents are Iraqi and I was born there. I came to the UK at age 1 so I also identify as British.
• Cornish, not British not English.
Other ethnic
group: Asian
• My parents are Sri Lankan and me and my brothers we were born in Italy so we are Italian. Before 5 years my mum applied for
Italian passport and we are Italian officially now.
Other ethnic
group: Black
• My ethnic background is Sierra Leonian and Jamaican.
Other ethnic
group: Mixed
• Indian Pakistani mixed
• Black British of African Caribbean, Arab and Irish heritage.
• Black British of African Caribbean, Arab and Irish heritage
• South Asian and Iranian heritage
• I am half white, my other half is Malagasy, however I come from the Merina tribe, which is from Indonesia
• Mix European and native Brazilian
Racism and Ethnic Inequality in a Time of Crisis
• I am Turkish, but my mother is of Tatar descent and my father immigrated to Turkey from Greece where he was part of a Turkish
speaking Muslim ethnic minority.
Ethnic identities
formation; and (2) the importance of subnational, place-based identities.
The quotes presented for the ‘Other mixed’ category remind us that the
standardised ‘Mixed’ categories solely focus on a mix with ‘White’.
These examples already provide a hint that those who classify themselves
into different variants of ‘Other’ ethnic groups have parents and grandparents
born in different parts of the world. Country of family origin is often used in
the construction of standardised ethnicity categories (for example, Pakistani,
Bangladeshi and Indian are used in the ONS classification), but they do
not incorporate multiple origin countries. The EVENS sample provides
a very good illustration that even in a single country context, such as the
UK, people identifying with a particular ethnic group can originate from
a wide range of countries (see Figure 3.2). The EVENS sample comprises
individuals originating from 155 countries, which highlights the diversity
of the UK ethnic minority population.
How attached do people feel to their ethnic and religious
identities?
The importance of ethnic identity
Despite the difficulties and complexities of defining ethnicity, many people
feel that their ethnic background is an important part of their self-definition.
EVENS asked respondents to assess on a scale from 1 (very important) to 4
(not at all important): ‘How important is your ethnic background to your
sense of who you are?’ Previous literature suggests that both gender and
age are likely to shape how strongly people identify with their ethnic and
national identities (Warikoo, 2005; Huddy and Khatib, 2007; Ali and Heath,
2013; Karlsen and Nazroo, 2015; Nandi and Platt, 2020). Given this, all the
results presented in this chapter adjust for the age and sex of respondents
(unless otherwise specified).
In line with the existing literature, we find that all ethnic minority groups
have a stronger attachment to their ethnic identities compared to the majority,
White British population (as illustrated in Figure 3.3). Black African, Black
Caribbean, Pakistani, Irish and Jewish people report the highest attachment
to their ethnic identity: over 90% say that their ethnic background is very
or fairly important to their sense of self. Lower percentages of people who
classify themselves as belonging to different Mixed groups (58‒79%), in
comparison to the Black (85‒91%), Asian (77‒91%) and Arab (81%) groups,
report a strong attachment to ethnic identity. Among White groups, the
Jewish (94%) and White Irish (92%) groups have the highest percentage that
feel that their ethnic identity is important to their sense of self, followed
by those from Gypsy (90%) and Roma (70%) backgrounds. Only around
58‒59% of those from the White Eastern European and White Other
backgrounds share that view.
37
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
• Figure 3.2: Ethnic group identiFIcation, by country of birth •
England
Scotland
India
Wales
The figure shows, for selected countries of birth of EVENS respondents, their reported ethnic group identification: within each country of birth,
circles showing ethnic group identification are proportionately sized. The figure represents 8,384 respondents born in England, 718 born in
Scotland, 517 in Wales, 412 in India, 263 in Nigeria, 188 in China, 186 in Poland, 156 in the USA, 87 in South Africa, 75 in France, 48 in Northern
Ireland, 43in Australia and 39 born in Brazil.
Racism and Ethnic Inequality in a Time of Crisis
38
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
Any other Asian background
Bangladeshi
Chinese
Indian
Pakistani
Black African
Any other Black background
Black Caribbean
Jewish
Any other mixed/multiple background
Mixed White and Asian
Mixed White and Black African
Mixed White and Black Caribbean
Any other ethnic group
Arab
Any other White background
White Eastern European
White British
Gypsy/Traveller
White Irish
Roma
• Figure 3.2: Ethnic group identiFIcation, by country of birth (continued) •
China
Nigeria
Poland
Ethnic identities
39
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
France
South Africa
U.S.A
The figure shows, for selected countries of birth of EVENS respondents, their reported ethnic group identification: within each country of birth,
circles showing ethnic group identification are proportionately sized. The figure represents 8,384 respondents born in England, 718 born in
Scotland, 517 in Wales, 412 in India, 263 in Nigeria, 188 in China, 186 in Poland, 156 in the USA, 87 in South Africa, 75 in Fr ance, 48 in Northern
Ireland, 43 in Australia and 39 born in Brazil.
(continued)
•
Figure 3.2: Ethnic group identiFIcation, by country of birth (continued)
Brazil
40
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
Australia
The figure shows, for selected countries of birth of EVENS respondents, their reported ethnic group identification: within each country of birth,
circles showing ethnic group identification are proportionately sized. The figure represents 8,384 respondents born in England, 718 born in
Scotland, 517 in Wales, 412 in India, 263 in Nigeria, 188 in China, 186 in Poland, 156 in the USA, 87 in South Africa, 75 in France, 48 in Northern
Ireland, 43 in Australia and 39 born in Brazil.
Racism and Ethnic Inequality in a Time of Crisis
Northern Ireland
•
•
Figure 3.3: Probability of expressing importance of ethnic identity, by ethnic group
.92
.58
.90
.70
.94
.59
.83
.91
.86
.58
.82
.77
.90
.73
.91
.79
.85
.81
.72
.71
.46
Ethnic identities
41
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
White Irish
White Eastern European
Gypsy/Traveller
Roma
Jewish
Any other White background
Indian
Pakistani
Bangladeshi
Mixed White and Asian
Chinese
Any other Asian background
Black Caribbean
Mixed White and Black Caribbean
Black African
Mixed White and Black African
Any other Black background
Arab
Any other mixed/multiple background
Any other ethnic group
White British
•
0
.2
.4
.6
.8
1
Note: Chart shows predicted probabilities of responding ‘Very or fairly important’ to the question ‘How important is your ethnic
background to your sense of who you are?’, adjusted for age, age squared and sex. 95% confidence intervals shown. N=12,816
Racism and Ethnic Inequality in a Time of Crisis
Interestingly, British and foreign-born individuals report similar levels of
attachment to ethnic identities (the results are not shown here), suggesting
that the importance of one’s ethnic background is not something that is only
felt by the foreign born, but is a significant part of self-definition regardless
of migrant generation.
The importance of religious identity
In EVENS, religious attachment is measured by the following question: ‘How
important is your religion to your sense of who you are?’ Four options
are provided to choose from (very important, fairly important, not very
important and not at all important). The results show a large variation in the
levels of religious affiliation among ethnic groups. As shown in Figure 3.4,
people from Mixed, White British, Other White and Chinese backgrounds
most frequently report having no religious affiliation. Within other ethnic
groups, there tends to be a consistency between religious affiliation and
ethnic identity. For example, over 80% of Pakistani, Bangladeshi and Arab
people in EVENS identify as Muslim, while nearly 80% of the Black African,
nearly 70% of the Black Caribbean and nearly 70% of the Black Other
groups identify as Christian. As mentioned earlier, Jewish people are treated
as a separate ethnic group in the EVENS classification.
We observe that those who identify with minority (non- Christian)
religions, especially when there tends to be a consistency between religious
affiliation and ethnic identity, tend to be more likely to report having a
strong attachment to their religion. Figure 3.5 shows that those who identify
as Muslim and Jewish are the most likely to report strong attachment,
followed by those who identify as Sikh, Hindu and Other: more than
7 in 10 people who identify with these religions feel strongly attached
to their religion. In comparison, about 5 in 10 Christians and 6 in 10
Buddhists report a strong attachment to their religion. Figure 3.6 shows
that a strong religious attachment is reported by over 80% of people from
Pakistani, Bangladeshi, Arab, Black African, Other Black, Jewish, Gypsy/
Traveller and Roma groups, and over 70% of those from Black Caribbean
and Other Asian groups. This is likely to be associated with a stronger
consistency between religious affiliation and ethnic identity among these
groups and with the more prominent social role of Black churches in the
case of Black communities.
How much do people engage in practices associated with their
ethnic and/or religious background?
EVENS included three questions assessing how much people engage in
practices that are linked to their ethnic and/or religious identities: ‘How
42
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
• Figure 3.4: Religious afFIliation, by ethnic group •
No religion
Christian
Buddhist
Hindu
Jewish
Sikh
Others
No specific answer
22 6
50
15
0
20
40
42
43
39
60
Note: Weighted percentages. Base: Full sample. N=12,816
6
16
Ethnic identities
43
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
White Irish
White Eastern European
Gypsy/Traveller
Roma
Jewish
Any other White background
Indian
Pakistani
Bangladeshi
Mixed White and Asian
Chinese
Any other Asian background
Black Caribbean
Mixed White and Black Caribbean
Black African
Mixed White and Black African
Any other Black background
Arab
Any other mixed/multiple background
Any other ethnic group
White British
Muslim
13
80
100
• Figure 3.5: Probability of expressing importance of religion, by religious afFIliation •
.51
Christian
.77
Hindu
.86
Jewish
44
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
Racism and Ethnic Inequality in a Time of Crisis
.59
Buddhist
.9
Muslim
.83
Sikh
.71
Others
0
.1
.2
.3
.4
.5
.6
.7
.8
.9
1
Note: Chart shows predicted probabilities of responding ‘Very or fairly important’ to the question ’How important is your religion to your sense
of who you are?’, adjusted for age, age squared and sex. 95% confidence intervals shown. Base: Respondents who reported having religious
affiliation. N=8,119
• Figure 3.6: Probability of expressing importance of religion, by ethnic group •
.62
.66
.86
.84
.86
.52
.80
.93
.91
.60
.69
.71
.73
.64
.89
.68
.83
.92
.68
.80
.46
Ethnic identities
45
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
White Irish
White Eastern European
Gypsy/Traveller
Roma
Jewish
Any other White background
Indian
Pakistani
Bangladeshi
Mixed White and Asian
Chinese
Any other Asian background
Black Caribbean
Mixed White and Black Caribbean
Black African
Mixed White and Black African
Any other Black background
Arab
Any other mixed/multiple background
Any other ethnic group
White British
0
.2
.4
.6
.8
1
Note: Chart shows predicted probabilities of responding ‘Very or fairly important’ to the question ‘How important is your religion to your sense
of who you are?’, adjusted for age, age squared and sex. 95% confidence intervals shown. Base: Respondents who reported having religious
affiliation. N=8,119
Racism and Ethnic Inequality in a Time of Crisis
often, if at all, do you wear clothes or something that shows a connection
with your ethnic identity or religion?’ ‘How often do you participate in
activities that are connected with your ethnicity or religion?’ ‘How often
do you eat food that is associated with your ethnic background or religion?’
Eating food linked to one’s ethnic or religious background was the most
prevalent practice across ethnic groups – on average, 40% of respondents
reported that they regularly eat specific types of food linked to their ethnicity
or religion. Only 10% reported they regularly wear specific clothes and
14% regularly participate in activities, related to their ethnicity or religion.
Due to the high prevalence of food-related practices, we classified responses
into ‘participation in any practices (including food)’ and ‘participation in
practices other than food’.
As shown in Figure 3.7, regular participation in any form of practice
connected with ethnicity or religion varies considerably across ethnic
groups. People from certain White ethnic groups, such as White British
(35%), White Irish (35%) and White Eastern European (42%), tend
to participate at lower rates than people from non- White minority
groups. Interestingly, White British people are the least likely to report
participation in any type of activities, including food, which suggests that
engagement in ethnically specific practices is less relevant for those who
are not members of a minoritised, or racialised, group. Ethnic groups for
whom we observed strong religious attachments are also among those
most likely to participate in non-food-related activities associated with
their ethnic background or religion (Bangladeshi: 69%, Pakistani: 68%,
Jewish: 64%, Black African: 57%, Arab: 55%, Gypsy: 56% and Roma:
49%). Although the EVENS does not explicitly ask what kind of practices
people participate in, it might be that many respondents thought of
activities associated with practising their religion. In contrast, people who
identify as Other White (14%), White Irish (17%), Mixed White and Black
Caribbean (20%), and White Eastern European (20%) were the least likely
among ethnic minority groups to engage in ethnicity or religion-related
practices other than food.
How strongly do people feel a sense of belonging to British,
English, Scottish, Welsh society?
EVENS asked people to assess, on a scale from 1 (strongly agree) to 4
(strongly disagree), ‘to what extent do you agree or disagree that you
personally feel a part of British society?’. The respondents living in different
constituent countries were also asked equivalent questions about their
sense of belonging to English, Scottish and Welsh societies depending on
their place of residence. We find that the vast majority of people (between
72% and 95%) from all ethnic backgrounds (with the exception of
46
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
• Figure 3.7: Probability of regular engagement in practices associated with one’s ethnic •
background, by ethnic group
Any practices
1
.88
.91 .92
.84
.79
.8
.67
.42
35
.4
.49
.49
.75
.57 .56 .53
.71
.56
.43
.40
.40
.24
.20
.35
.20
.29
.35
.38
.25 .29
.14
.11
47
ro
/T pea
ra
n
y
ve
ot
he
lle
r
r
W
Ro
hi
m
te
ba Je a
ck wis
gr
h
ou
nd
In
d
M
Pa ian
ix
ed B
ki
W ang sta
An
n
hi
l
y
te ad i
ot
an esh
he
M
i
d
ix
r
ed
As
As
ia
ia
W
C
n
n
hi
ba hin
te
a n Bla c k g e s e
d ck r o
B l Ca u
M
ix
ac r nd
ed
k ibb
Ca e
An Wh
r a
Bl ibb n
y ite
An
ot
ac
ea
a
y
he nd
k
ot
Af n
r
he
Bl Bla
r
r
ac ck ica
m
k
ix
Af n
b
ed
ac ric
/m
kg an
ro
u
un
An ltip
le
d
y
ba
ot
he
ck Ara
r
g
et ro b
hn un
d
i
W cg
r
hi
te oup
Br
iti
sh
h
sy
n
Eu
is
An
G
st
Ea
te
hi
Ir
yp
h
ite
er
W
W
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
0
Note: Yellow dots show predicted probabilities (adjusted for age, age squared and sex) of responding ‘Always/frequently’ to the question ‘How often,
if at all, do you wear clothes or something that shows a connection with your ethnic identity or religion?’ and/or responding ‘Always/frequently’ to
the question ‘How often do you participate in activities that are connected with your ethnicity or religion?’ and/or responding ‘At least 1–2 days a
week’ to the question ‘How often do you eat food that is associated with your ethnic background or religion?’. Green dots include answers to
questions on clothes and activities only. Base: All respondents. N= 11,865 (any practices); N=12,601 (clothes/activities).
Ethnic identities
.17
.67
.78
.55
.57
.29
.2
.85
.8
.68 .69
.64
.56
.6
Activities or clothes
Racism and Ethnic Inequality in a Time of Crisis
Roma – 33%) report having a strong sense of belonging to British society (as
illustrated in Figure 3.8). Interestingly, the likelihood of reporting positive
belonging to British society was highest for some of the groups who were
also most likely to express strong sense of attachment to their ethnic identity,
including the Arab (95%), Jewish (93%), Indian (92%), Pakistani (92%),
Bangladeshi (92%), Black African (90%), and Black Other (89%) ethnic
groups. For people from Black Caribbean (78%), Gypsy (79%) and White
Irish (76%) ethnic groups, the likelihood of reporting positive belonging
to British society was slightly lower compared to the likelihood of having a
strong sense of attachment to their ethnic identity. On the contrary, White
Eastern European (86%) and White Other (77%) people, for whom we
observed a relatively low sense of ethnic identity, reported a strong sense of
belonging to the British society.
Across all ethnic minority groups (with the exception of Gypsy and
Roma), the likelihood of having a strong sense of belonging to English,
Scottish and Welsh societies was lower than the likelihood of having a strong
sense of belonging to British society (Figure 3.8). However, the patterns of
attachment to the constituent nations were not uniform across all minority
groups. The most pronounced differences between the likelihood of having
positive attachment to British and to English societies were noted for the
Black Caribbean, Bangladesh, Indian, Pakistani, Other White and Arab
groups. Among ethnic minority groups, the smallest difference between
affiliation to a British or an English identity was observed for the Eastern
European, Chinese, Gypsy/Traveller and Roma groups, and the likelihood
of having a positive sense of belonging to British and to English society was
essentially the same for the White British group. Nevertheless, the differences
between belonging to British and to English society were relatively small
and the majority within each ethnic group felt that they were part of British
and part of English society, with the exception of the Roma group. Similar
patterns were found in relation to the sense of belonging to Scottish and
Welsh societies (these are not shown here).
Discussion
The detailed questions on multiple aspects of people’s ethnic, religious and
national identities included in EVENS allow us to better understand the
importance of different types of ethnic, religious and national identities.
The inclusion of an open-ended write-in ethnic identity question illustrates
how people tend to think about their ethnicity when they are not bound
by predefined categories.
As shown by the analysis of the free text responses, the majority of people
articulate their ethnic identity using phrases and expressions typically used
in the standardised ethnicity classifications. This can be attributed to the
48
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
• Figure 3.8: Probability of feeling part of British and English society, by •
ethnic group
Feels part of British society
Feels part of English society
0
.2
.4
.6
.8
1
0
.2
.4
.6
.8
1
Note: Chart shows predicted probabilities (adjusted for age, age squared, and sex) of responding ‘Strongly agree or Agree’ to the question ‘To
what extent do you agree or disagree that you personally feel a part of British society?’ and to the question ‘To what extent do you agree or
disagree that you personally feel a part of English society?’. Base: All respondents. N=12,266 (British Society); N=10,304 (English Society)
Ethnic identities
49
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
.72
.83
.80
.32
.83
.65
.80
.79
.74
.74
.85
.71
.67
.79
.84
.75
.74
.85
.65
.72
.85
.76
.86
.79
.33
.93
.77
.92
.92
.92
.79
.85
.79
.78
.84
.90
.86
.89
.95
.72
.80
.86
White Irish
White Eastern European
Gypsy/Traveller
Roma
Jewish
Any other White background
Indian
Pakistani
Bangladeshi
Mixed White and Asian
Chinese
Any other Asian background
Black Caribbean
Mixed White and Black Caribbean
Black African
Mixed White and Black African
Any other Black background
Arab
Any other mixed/multiple background
Any other ethnic group
White British
Racism and Ethnic Inequality in a Time of Crisis
widespread use of such classifications, which in turn affects how people
conceptualise ethnicity. Nevertheless, a significant proportion of the sample
used non-standardised ethnicity articulations that included references to the
complex migration journeys and multicountry and multiracial origins that
are not possible to capture in existing classifications. Such articulations of
ethnic identity are often present among those who classify themselves into
various ‘Other’ standardised ethnicity categories. The growing diversity
of the UK population, which in turn results in increasingly complex
patterns of family and migration backgrounds, is likely to make existing
standardised ethnicity categories less able to accurately capture meaningful
ethnic identities over time. Changing migration patterns means that new,
sizeable groups from non-traditional origin countries are not accurately
represented in the official ethnicity classifications. Another limitation of
standardised ethnicity categories highlighted by textual responses is the lack
of non-White Mixed ethnicities. The nature of standardised classifications
also limits people’s ability to express their identity in non-racialised terms or
to use subnational definitions of ethnic identity. The focus on demographic
heritage of standardised ethnicity classifications, although useful for
monitoring purposes, limits individuals’ ability to express subjective ethnic
identities. Such rigid categorisation can sometimes create frustration among
those who do not feel comfortable with putting themselves into predefined
ethnicity categories.
This chapter has also shown that despite the challenges of defining ethnic
identities, especially in standardised, fixed terms, most people report strong
attachment to their ethnic and religious backgrounds. Ethnic identity is
particularly important for those from the Black African, Black Caribbean,
Pakistani, White Irish and Jewish backgrounds, and the least important
for those from White British, White Eastern European and White Other
groups. Religious identity is important for higher proportions of people
who identify with minority religions and for people for whom there
tends to be a consistency in religious affiliation and ethnic identity ‒ for
example, for people from Pakistani, Bangladeshi, Black African, Arab and
Jewish groups.
Although an explanatory analysis of why the importance of ethnic identity
is more prevalent among certain groups is beyond the scope of this chapter,
we can speculate about some of the possible explanations based on the past
literature. Some of the commonly identified determinants of the strength of
ethnic identity include: prevalence of ethnic discrimination (Gilroy, 2013;
Rumbaut, 2005), cultural distance (Nesdale and Mak, 2003), community
involvement (Maehler, 2022) and parental socialisation (Phinney and Chavira,
1995; Xu et al, 2004). Experience of ethnic discrimination, in line with
social identity theory, is likely to increase the salience of ethnicity to one’s
self-concept. Non-White groups are particularly at risk of experiencing
50
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
Ethnic identities
racism, which in turn structures how they view their own identity and
how having such an identity shapes their interactions with others (Karlsen
and Nazroo, 2002b). Greater perceived cultural distance to the ethnic
majority might affect the development of a strong ethnic and/or religious
identity through positive and negative mechanisms. Positive mechanisms
include increased motivation to preserve one’s own cultural heritage and
the development of a positive distinctiveness based on group belonging
(Turner, 2010), whereas negative mechanisms might be associated with
experience of greater prejudice from the majority group (Ford, 2011).
Greater involvement in ethnicity and/or religion-related practices has also
been shown to be correlated with ethnic identity development during
adolescence and adulthood (Hardy et al, 2011).
People from those ethnic minority groups that have a high prevalence of
strong attachment to their ethnic identity are also highly likely to report a
strong sense of belonging to British society. This is the case for those from
the Bangladeshi, Pakistani, Indian, Chinese, Black African, Black Other,
Arab and Jewish groups. For some of these groups, these patterns are in
line with the existing evidence (Demireva and Heath, 2014; Karlsen and
Nazroo, 2015; Nandi and Platt, 2014). However, for others, such as the
Arab and Jewish groups, EVENS provides the first large-scale evidence on
their subjective sense of attachment to ethnic and national communities.
EVENS also provides the first evidence on the patterns of ethnic,
religious and national belonging among a nationally representative sample
of White Eastern European people. We have learned that people from the
White Eastern European group tend to express a strong sense of belonging
to British society, but less so to their ethnic identity. Interestingly, they are
also among a few groups who are almost equally likely to report a strong
sense of belonging to a British as well as an English national community.
These patterns are likely to reflect the role of whiteness in the construction of
British and English identities, as well as lower levels of ethnic discrimination
among most of the White minority groups (see Chapter 4).
We also note that people from the White Roma, Gypsy, White Irish and
Black Caribbean ethnic groups are less likely than other ethnic groups to
have a strong sense of belonging to British society compared to their strength
of attachment to their ethnic identity. Some of these patterns might be
associated with ethnicity-related discrimination, although a formal analysis
on the impact of discrimination on the strength of ethnic and national
attachments is beyond the scope of this chapter. However, previous literature
did find that perceived discrimination is a major factor affecting the strength
of British identity among ethnic minority individuals (Maxwell, 2009;
Karlsen and Nazroo, 2015).
The lower likelihood of having a positive sense of belonging to English
rather than British society among people from ethnic minority backgrounds,
51
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
Racism and Ethnic Inequality in a Time of Crisis
particularly those at higher risk of experiencing racial discrimination, might
be explained by the difference in the racialisation and inclusiveness of these
two national identities. The construction of Englishness is based more on
the ‘ethnic’ than the ‘civic’ concept of identity (Leddy-Owen, 2014). As a
consequence, Englishness is more likely to be defined in terms of ancestry
and Whiteness, whereas Britishness is more linked with political community
boundaries and citizenship.
In sum, despite some differences in the strength of belonging to British
society, the overall picture coming from the analysis of the EVENS data
is a positive one. We see that the overwhelming majority of people across
(almost) all ethnic groups feel a strong sense of belonging to the national
community. Furthermore, it seems that having a strong attachment to one’s
ethnic identity often goes hand in hand with the strong sense of belonging
to British society.
Box 3.1: Ethnic identities: measures and methods
All the results presented in this chapter are weighted by the propensity weights available
in the EVENS dataset. The sample includes all EVENS respondents aged 18–65.
Predicted probabilities are based on logistic regression models adjusted for age
(measured in years), square term of age, and sex. Predicted probability can be interpreted
as the likelihood that person x gave answer y, while taking into account that men and
women and people of different ages have different likelihoods of giving answer y.
Variable coding:
Write-in ethnic identity: All textual responses are coded based on the words used by the
respondent into one of three categories: non-engagement (lack of valid response);
standardised ethnicity articulation (all words used by the respondent correspond to
words used in standardised ethnicity classifications) and non-standardised ethnicity
articulation (at least some words used by the respondent differ from those used in
standardised classifications).
Strong/fairly strong attachment to ethnic (/religious) background includes people who
said they ethnic (/religious) background is very or fairly important to their sense of
who they are.
Strong/fairly strong sense of belonging to British (English/ Scottish/Welsh) societies
includes those who said they strongly agree or tend to agree that they feel part of
British (English/Scottish/Welsh) society.
52
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC
Ethnic identities
Regular participation in practices (including food) refers to people who said that they
regularly participate in activities or wear clothes (always or frequently) or eat food
(every day or most of the days) associated with their ethnic background.
Regular participation in practices (excluding food) refers to people who said that they
regularly participate in activities or wear clothes (always or frequently) associated
with their ethnic background.
53
Unauthenticated | Downloaded 05/13/23 12:44 PM UTC