Jerome Kagan is an American psychologist. He was born in 1929 in Newark, New Jersey, and grew up in Rahway, New Jersey.[1] Kagan is currently retired[2] after being a professor at Harvard University in the Developmental program.[3] He is one of the key pioneers of developmental psychology. He is Daniel and Amy Starch Research Professor of Psychology, Emeritus at Harvard University, and co-faculty at the New England Complex Systems Institute. He has shown that an infant's "temperament" is quite stable over time, in that certain behaviors in infancy are predictive of certain other behavior patterns in adolescence. He did extensive work on temperament and gave insight on emotion.

Kagan was listed as the 22nd most eminent psychologist of the 20th Century, just above Carl Jung.[4]

Contents

Personal background [link]

Kagan was born to Joseph and Mytle Kagan on February 25, 1929. Jerome chose to study Psychology due to his attraction in being a scientist and to preserve his father's interest in human nature.[5] He was accepted at Yale University to study Psychology, where he earned his Ph.D. and he earned his Master's degree from Harvard University.[6] He earned a B.S. Degree from Rutgers University in 1950.[7] While at Yale University, he assisted Frank Beach, a well respected researcher.[1] After he graduated from Yale University, he accepted his first faculty position at Ohio State University.[1] Six months later, in 1955 he was recruited to be a part of the research team at the U.S. Army Hospital during the Korean War.[1] Once he had finished his time at the U.S. Army Hospital, the director of the Fels Research Institute contacted Kagan to ask him to direct a project that was funded by the National Institutes of Health, which he accepted.[1] After that project was completed, he accepted the offer he received from Harvard University to be involved in creating the first Human Development program.[1] Once he moved to Harvard, he remained there until retirement, with the exception of a leave to go and study children in San Macros.[1] He did this for a year, from 1971 to 1972, and then returned to Harvard as a professor.[1]

Research [link]

While at Fels, Kagan did extensive research on personality traits beginning with infancy and continued through adulthood.[8] He looked at whether or not early experiences affected the participants' future personalities, talents and characters.[1] Kagan read up on all of the longitudinal information that was prepared, specifically, the responses to intelligence tests that were administered to them.[1] When Kagan was reviewing the material collected in childhood and adulthood, he found that the first three years in childhood showed little relation to the data collected in adulthood.[1] The results of the Fels study was discussed in Kagan's book, Birth to Maturity, in 1962.[1] Kagan's next research was in San Macros, Guatemala.[1] During this time, Kagan discovered that biological factors play a huge role in development and an even larger part in child development.[9] Specifically, he found that these children had slower Psychological development when in their homes due to their restricted experiences.[1] Once the children were walking and could leave the home, Kagan found that the psychological delay in development was only temporary, and that cognitive growth is malleable.[1] In 2010, Kagan was involved in a similar study that focused on specific parts of the brain involved in behavioral inhibition in infants. Schwartz et al. (2010) performed a longitudinal study involving 18-year-olds and used neuroimaging to detect whether or not the ventromedial or orbitofrontal cerebral cortex are associated with the high/low reactivity of their 4-month-old selves. After undergoing a battery of tests, the infants were later categorized into two groups: low-reactive and high-reactive temperament. Results showed that the adults who had low-reactive infant temperaments had greater thickness in the left orbitofrontal cortex than the high-reactive group. The adults categorized as high-reactive infants displayed greater thickness in the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex.

Emotion [link]

While at Harvard University, Kagan studied infants up to two years and published his work in his book, The Second Year.[1] By the method of observation, Kagan found that there were major changes in Psychological functioning between 19 and 24 months, as well that at one years old, the children were sensitive to events that deviated from the norms of their experiences.[10] Kagan's next research project was to look at the effects of infant daycare, since the congress was considering funding federal day care centres for working mothers.[1] He and two others, Richard Kearsley and Philip Zelazo, created their own daycare in Boston's Chinatown, and compared these infants to infants who stayed at home with their mothers.[1] In the late 1970s, Kagan studied temperament with a student who was writing a thesis.[1] This project was funded by the President of the Spencer Foundation.[1]

Temperament [link]

According to Kagan, (conventionally):

temperament refers to stable behavioral and emotional reactions that appear early and are influenced in part by genetic constitution.
[11]

Temperament is perhaps what Kagan is best known for. He began his work on temperament after his research in Guatemala. Kagan primarily focused on children’s fear and apprehension.[12] He created two types of temperament; inhibited and uninhibited.[13] Inhibited refers to a shy, timid, and fearful profile of a child, whereas uninhibited refers to the appearance of bold, sociable and outgoing behaviours.[13] In 2008, Kagan and several other researchers conducted a study to examine if behavioral inhibition in adulthood can be predicted by certain behavioral characteristics in infants. The research hypothesized that the frequency of infant reactivity based on motor and crying dimensions is predictive of behavioral inhibition. As a result of his ground breaking work on temperament, we know that these characteristics have the ability to influence later behavior depending on how they interact with the environment.[12] Kagan also believed that there is no guarantee of an indefinitely stable profile considering environmental factors are always changing and that both genes and environmental factors influence a child's temperament [13]

Although emotion is not what Kagan is known for, he did bring some work into this field. Kagan proposed that emotion is a psychological phenomenon controlled by brain states and that specific emotions are products of context, the person’s history and biological make-up. Kagan also explained emotion as occurring in four dinstinct phases.[14] The first phase is the brain state, which is created by an incentive, the second stage is the detection of changes in bodily movement.[14] The third stage is the appraisal of a change in bodily feeling, and the last stage is where there are observable changes in facial expression and muscle tension.[14] These emotions vary in magnitude and usually differ across ages and when expressed in different contexts. Kagan questioned relying on individual's verbal statements of their feelings.[14] He provided several reasons for this.[14] Firstly, he argued that the English language does not have enough words to describe all emotional states.[14] Secondly, the words to explain emotional states do not convey the differences in the quality or the severity of it.[14] Lastly, you cannot translate emotional words from one language to another accurately.[14] In addition, Kagan argued that research in emotion studies should be free of ambiguous and coded terms, and this emphasis on specificity remains a recurring theme in his current research on emotion.

Publications [link]

He is the author of:

  • Personality and the learning Process (1965)
  • Reflection- Impulsivity and Reading Ability in Primary Grade Children (1965)
  • Personal Development (1971)
  • The growth of the child. Reflections on human development (1978)
  • The Nature of the Child (1982)
  • An argument for mind (2006)
  • What is emotion?: History, measures, and meanings (2007)
  • In defense of Qualitative Changes in Development (2008)
  • The three cultures: Natural sciences, social sciences, and the humanities in the 21st century (2009)
  • Once more into the Breach (2010)
  • The temperamental thread. How genes, culture, time, and luck make us who we are (2010) [Trad. esp.: El temperamento y su trama. Cómo los genes, la cultura, el tiempo y el azar inciden en nuestra personalidad, Buenos Aires/Madrid, Katz editores, 2011, ISBN 978-84-92946-32-7]
  • On the Need for Relativism. American Psychologist, 1967, 22, 131-142.

Some of the books Kagan has written or co-written include:

  • Birth to Maturity (1962)
  • Understanding Children: Behavior, Motives, and Thought (1971)
  • The Second Year: The Emergence of Self-Awareness (1981)
  • Unstable Ideas: Temperament, Cognition, and Self (1989)
  • Galen’s prophecy: Temperament in human nature (1994)
  • Three Seductive Ideas (2000)
  • A Young Mind in a Growing Brain (2005)

Awards [link]

Kagan won the Hofheimer Prize of the American Psychiatric Association in 1963. In 1995, He won the G. Stanley Hall Award of the American Psychological Association (APA).

References [link]

  1. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t Kagan, Jerome (2007). A History in Psychology in Autobiography. Washington, DC: Edwards Brothers Inc.. pp. 115–149. 
  2. ^ Sweeney, S.. "Often, we are what we were". Harvard Gazette. https://news.harvard.edu/gazette/story/2010/04/often-we-are-what-we-were. Retrieved 2 November 2011. 
  3. ^ Harvard University. "Department Directory". https://www.isites.harvard.edu/icb/icb.do?keyword=k3007&panel=icb.pagecontent177929%3Arsearch%248%3FsearchText%3Dkag&pageid=icb.page29924&pageContentId=icb.pagecontent177929&view=viewBio.do&viewParam_bioUserId=AQJPTk9PUlBXUAME%0D%0A&viewParam_templateId=8729. Retrieved 28 November 2011. 
  4. ^ Haggbloom, S.J. et al. (2002). The 100 Most Eminent Psychologists of the 20th Century. Review of General Psychology. Vol. 6, No. 2, 139–15. Haggbloom et al. combined 3 quantitative variables: citations in professional journals, citations in textbooks, and nominations in a survey given to members of the Association for Psychological Science, with 3 qualitative variables (converted to quantitative scores): National Academy of Science (NAS) membership, American Psychological Association (APA) President and/or recipient of the APA Distinguished Scientific Contributions Award, and surname used as an eponym. Then the list was rank ordered.
  5. ^ Kagan, Jerome (2007). A history in psychology in autobiography. Washington, DC: Edwards Brothers Inc. pp. 115–149. 
  6. ^ Alic, M. (2001). Kagan, Jerome. Encyclopedia of psychology. Retrieved 27 November 2009 from https://findarticles.com/p/articles/mi_g2699/is_0005/ai_2699000518/
  7. ^ Jerome Kagan. (1988). American Psychologist, 43(4), 223-225.
  8. ^ Alic, M. (2009). Jerome Kagan-Questions environmental determinism, Questions continuity of development and parental influences. Psychology Encyclopedia. Retrieved (2009, November 27) from https://psychology.jrank.org/pages/355/Jerome-Kagan.html
  9. ^ Kagan, J. (2003). Biology, Context, and Developmental Inquiry. Annual Reviews Psychology, 54,1-23.
  10. ^ Kagan, Jerome (1981). The Second Year: The emergence of Self Awareness. USA: Harvard University Press. 
  11. ^ Kagan, J: "Galen's Prophecy: Temperament in Human Nature.", page 40. Westview Press, 1994.
  12. ^ a b Jerome Kagan. Peason Education. Retrieved (2009, November 27) from https://wps.prenhall.com/wps/media/objects/1931/1977702/html/theo8.html
  13. ^ a b c Kagan, Jerome (1998). Galen's Prophecy: Temperament in Human Nature. USA: Westview Press. 
  14. ^ a b c d e f g h Kagan, Jerome (2010). "Once more into the Breach". Emotion Review 2 (2): 91–99. 

Schwartz, C. E., Kunwar, P. S., Greve, D. N., Moran, L. R., Viner, J. C., Covino, J. M., & ... Wallace, S. R. (2010). Structural differences in adult orbital and ventromedial prefrontal cortex predicted by infant temperament at 4 months of age. Archives Of General Psychiatry, 67(1), 78-84. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.171

External links [link]


https://wn.com/Jerome_Kagan

Jerome

Saint Jerome (/əˈrm/; Latin: Eusebius Sophronius Hieronymus; Greek: Εὐσέβιος Σωφρόνιος Ἱερώνυμος; c.  347 – 30 September 420) was a priest, confessor, theologian and historian, who also became a Doctor of the Church. He was the son of Eusebius, born at Stridon, a village near Emona on the border of Dalmatia and Pannonia, then part of northeastern Italy. He is best known for his translation of most of the Bible into Latin (the translation that became known as the Vulgate), and his commentaries on the Gospels. His list of writings is extensive. The protégé of Pope Damasus I, who died in December of 384, Jerome was known for his teachings on Christian moral life, especially to those living in cosmopolitan centers such as Rome. In many cases, he focused his attention to the lives of women and identified how a woman devoted to Jesus Christ should live her life. This focus stemmed from his close patron relationships with several prominent female ascetics who were members of affluent senatorial families.

Jerome (given name)

Jerome is a masculine name of Greek origin, derived from the Greek given name Ἱερώνυμος, Hierōnymos, "sacred name"; from ἱερός, hierós, "sacred", and ὂνυμα, ónyma, an alternative form of ὄνομα, ónoma, "name".

It is the name of a prominent Christian saint, Saint Jerome, the translator of the Vulgate.

Jerome ranked among the top 200 names given to boys born in the United States between 1903 and 1985. It has since declined in popularity and was ranked as the 616th most popular name for American boys born in 2008.

Variants

  • Gerolamo Italian
  • Gerome English
  • Geronimo Italian
  • Girolamo Italian
  • Giròlamu Sicilian
  • Ιερώνυμος (Ieronymos) Greek
  • Hieronymus Latin and German
  • Hiëronymus Western Frisian
  • Jerom Breton
  • Jeronim Albanian
  • Jeroným Czech
  • Jerolim, Jeronim Croatian
  • Jeromos Hungarian
  • Hieronim Slovak
  • Hieronym Slovene
  • Iarom Irish
  • Sierôm Welsh
  • Ieronimus Medieval Latin
  • Jeronimas Lithuanian
  • Hieronīms Latvian
  • Jeroen Dutch
  • Jerome English
  • Jérôme French
  • Ġlormu Maltese
  • Hieronimo Esperanto
  • Jerónimo Spanish
  • Jerônimo Portuguese
  • List of Family Guy characters

    Family Guy is an American animated adult comedy created by Seth MacFarlane for the Fox Broadcasting Company. Characters are listed only once, normally under the first applicable subsection in the list; very minor characters are listed with a more regular character with whom they are associated.

    Griffin family

    Peter Griffin

    Peter Griffin (voiced by Seth MacFarlane) is the patriarch of the Griffin household, an Irish-American blue-collar worker. He is a lazy, immature, obese, laid-back, dim-witted, outspoken, eccentric alcoholic. Peter's jobs have included working at the Happy Go Lucky Toy Factory, working as a fisherman, and currently working at Pawtucket Brewery.

    Lois Griffin

    Lois Patrice Griffin (née Pewterschmidt) (voiced by Alex Borstein) is Peter's wife and the mother of Meg, Chris, and Stewie. She is a Scots/Anglo American housewife who cares for her kids and her husband, while also teaching children to play the piano. She is also very flirtatious and has slept with numerous people on the show; her past promiscuous tendencies and her hard-core recreational drug-use are often stunning but overlooked.

    Podcasts:

    PLAYLIST TIME:
    ×