Skip to main content
'One' and 'ones' are complex determiners whose relation to their antecedent, when they have one, is mediated by a silent noun. They are never themselves nouns taking an antecedent directly. All non-local syntactic relations necessarily... more
    • by 
    • Morphology and Syntax
On the basis of considerations involving complementizers, sentence-final particles, 'need', aspect, tense, focus and topic, agreement morphemes, determiners, particles and adpositions, it appears that many more heads in the sentential... more
    • by 
    • Morphology and Syntax
The study of English once and twice yields evidence that each f them is actually a complex phrase containing two visible orphemes and one silent one. Neither is a simple lexical item. The -ce morpheme is akin to a postposition, despite... more
    • by 
    • Morphology and Syntax
An unambiguous path requirement as a replacement for c-command leads to binary branching.
    • by 
    • Morphology and Syntax
    • by 
    • Morphology and Syntax
If we take the identity in form between expletive 'there' and various other instances of 'there' (not only locative 'there', but also deictic 'there' and the 'there' of 'thereby') not to be accidental, we are led to the conclusion that... more
    • by 
    • Morphology and Syntax
In working toward an understanding of the syntactic component of the human language faculty, syntacticians necessarily ask question after question. One prominent question is: (1) What properties do all languages have in common? A... more
    • by 
    • Morphology and Syntax
Phrases of the form ‘numeral + noun’ never involve direct merger of numeral and noun. In every case, derivations are more complex than that. With 'one', there is, in addition to a classifier, the necessary presence of 'single'/'only',... more
    • by 
    • Morphology and Syntax
    • by 
    • by 
1. The antisymmetry proposal of Kayne (1994) took the Linear Correspondence Axiom (LCA) to see sub-word-level structure as well as phrasal structure. 1 This integration of morphology and syntax, as far as the LCA is concerned, recalls... more
    • by 
    • by 
In this paper, I will focus on negative prefixes such as English un- (and in-), as in unintelligent (and ineffective), where the negative prefix precedes and is associated with an adjective. As far as I can see, English has no... more
    • by  and +1
    •   10  
      English languageSyntaxMorphologyGeneral linguistics
The question why antisymmetry holds leads to introducing 'immediate precedence' into merge, while keeping 'precedence' part of externalization.
    • by 
The (Italian) pair '*il luo libro' vs. 'il suo libro' (‘the his/her book’) that is typical of Romance lends itself to an account of the first in terms of constraints also seen in the syntax of compounding, and to an account of the second... more
    • by 
A central aspect of comparative syntax calls for discovering generalizations over cross-linguistic differences and similarities, and then trying to understand, in general UG terms or beyond, why a given cross-linguistic correlation should... more
    • by 
    • by 
Many Germanic languages have a finite-clause complementizer that resembles a demonstrative, e.g. English that, Dutch dat, German dass. No Romance language does. The traditional view of complementizers as simplex projecting heads that take... more
    • by 
    • by 
There is an answer to the question why bare 'long' and 'a long time' differ in the way they do, with 'a long time' showing no need for negation at all. The reason is that 'a long time' contains no silent TIME in need of licensing. The... more
    • by