PHD Juliana Lopez Marulanda FINAL - Compressed
PHD Juliana Lopez Marulanda FINAL - Compressed
PHD Juliana Lopez Marulanda FINAL - Compressed
NNT : 2018SACLS016
Sylvie GRANON
Professeur, Université Paris Sud (UMR 9197) Président
Sébastien DEREGNACOURT
Professeur, Université Paris Nanterre (EA 3456) Rapporteur
Natacha AGUILAR
Chargée de Recherche, Universidad de La Laguna (BIOECOMAC) Rapporteur
Adrien MEGUERDITCHIAN
Chargé de Recherche, Université Aix Marseille (UMR 7290) Examinateur
Aurélie CELERIER
Maitre de Conférences, Université Montpellier 3 (UMR 5175) Examinateur
Olivier ADAM
Professeur, Université Paris Sud (UMR 9197) Directeur de thèse
Fabienne DELFOUR
HDR, Université Paris XIII (EA 4443) Co-Directeur de thèse
Communication acoustique et comportement social chez les grands dauphins
(Tursiops truncatus)
Les grands dauphins sont des cétacés sociaux qui vivent dans un système social de
sur de longues distances ou dans des habitats dont la visibilité est limitée. Ils
possèdent des capacités cognitives avancées. Par exemple, ils sont capables de
rester vigilants pendant toute une journée, ils ont une mémoire de travail comparable
à celle des primates non humains et une mémoire à long terme d'au moins 20 ans.
production vocale des grands dauphins comprend des sifflements, des clics et des
sons pulsés en rafale, avec certains sifflements appelés « signatures sifflées » qui
sont pas satisfaisantes. Au cours de cette thèse, nous avons développé un système
facile à déployer qui identifie l'animal produisant le son et permet des observations
grands dauphins au sein de leur groupe social. D'abord, j'ai développé deux études
visant à décrire comment l'activité vocale des dauphins captifs varie en relation avec
1
le comportement et l'interaction avec les humains. Pour cela, je me suis d'abord
sifflements non signature liée aux comportements sous-marins observés. Ces deux
vocale.
Les résultats montrent que les séances d'entraînement avec des soigneurs modulent
sifflements non signatures qui mériteraient d’être mieux examinés. L’étude de ces
devrait fournir l'information nécessaire pour interpréter le rôle des sifflements non
vocalisation.
2
L'utilisation du système BaBeL m'a permis d'étudier la production de trains de clics
Acknowledgments
First, I would like to give special thanks to Olivier Adam and Fabienne Delfour, my
supervisors. The first, for answer to my request, trust me and gave me the chance to
do this PhD, for opening me so many doors and supporting me. The second, for
being there for me and never stop believing in my abilities (e.g. « harassing » me and
This PhD would not have been possible without the finantial support of Colciencias,
Parc Asterix, Abyss Foundation and the CNRS (Centre Nationale pour la Recherche
Scientifique). I am very grateful to those institutions. Also I would like to thank the all
the members of the jury for taking my time to read this work and contribute to its
improvement.
When I arrived to this lab (NeuroPSI) I found and amazing team of people (The
Acoustic Communication Team) that received me very well: Isabelle, who became
my mentor/academic therapist enduring the pain of all the interruptions I made to her
work; Helene, who listened to me and gave me useful advices, Fanny; who taught
me how to recognize the birds and their vocalizations; Thierry, who never stopped
joking about my Colombian accent in French but always supported me; Chloé and
3
her “special service to the desperate PhD student”; Clément, who made me laugh so
many times kidnapping my rabbits; Juliette, who was there to cheer me up; Anjara,
who motivated me to arrive early to the lab; Kaja, that gave me company during my
smoke breaks. I wanted you to know I’m very grateful and I consider you my friends.
During the construction of my database I counted with the help of many people that I
would like to thank: Birgitta Mercera and the trainers’ team of Parc Asterix, especially
Caroline, Stéphane, Fleur, Sully, Julien, Daphne and Juana which contributed
directly to my observations. Marie Vallée and the Abyss association team members,
the Boudewijn sea park team: Sander, Sabrina, Thomas, Gunter, Mathias, Olivia,
Tyra and Nele, they received me in their facility as another team member. Torea
Colpaert who helped me to analyze my data and make me feel in Brugges like in
home.
I would like to thank “les colocs”, that accepted me in their house and were always so
kind: Claire, Romain, Fabien, Mathieu. And the ones who came after: Stouphie,
David, Michele and Thomas, it was a pleasure to share these years with you.
Also I would like to give special thanks to Isabella Clegg, for transporting me,
listening to me, correcting my English, giving me gifts, eating sushi with me and
Special thanks to Denis, my partner, who supported during the last year of my PhD
and made me eat vegetables and soup and to Dr. Gorog and Dr. Faucher who
4
Finally, I would like to thank my family for their support, my sister Sara and Jean
Eudes who were there to listen to my complains and cheer me up, my sister
Elizabeth and our super funny “cumbia dancing sessions” and my parents.
5
GENERAL INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................ 10
6
3.3.2 Clicks ..........................................................................................................................................................33
RESULTS................................................................................................................................... 39
Chapter 5: Bottlenose Dolphins Under Human Care Acoustically Coordinate Their Aerial
1. How do dolphins under human care modulate their whistle repertoire according to
7
2. Applications of the BaBeL system to better understand acoustic communication in a
BIBLIOGRAPHY....................................................................................................................... 175
8
Figures list
https://matthewhardcastle.wordpress.com/2012/09/30/hello-world/ .................................. 21
sounds. X axis represents the time in seconds and Y axis represents the frequency in
kHz. ................................................................................................................................... 29
modulated. ......................................................................................................................... 30
Tables list
9
GENERAL INTRODUCTION
10
1 Animal Communication.
to another via a signal that typically elicits a response (Perrin et al., 2009). Animal
the chain of information transmission in which one individual (the emitter) produces a
perceived by another individual (receiver) who will then decode and interpret the
signal and modify its behavior accordingly, giving some feedback to the emitter
(Shannon and Weaver, 1949). Unlike cues, which are generated inadvertently or for
Communication not only happens between individuals of the same species, it occurs
emitter and the receiver should benefit from the exchange of information (Simmons,
2003). The capacity of production and decoding a signal are dependent on the
The message is what the emitter wants to transmit using a signal, and the meaning is
what the receiver interprets from the signal and it depends on the receiver history of
life (e.g. previous experiences) (Dudzinski and Hill, 2018). The message and the
meaning of a signal are never identical because both the emitter and the receiver are
different individuals with different histories (Dudzinski and Hill, 2018). Natural
11
selection will favor individuals that produce clearer signals and receivers that are
capable of decoding information from them effectively (Maynard and Smith, 2003).
In many situations, such as the cooperative foraging, the search for a sexual partner,
the defense of a territory, the relationship between mother-calf and the avoidance of
(Bradbury and Vehrencamp, 2011). According to the context, the emitter transmits an
extensive set of information about itself, for example, its location, identity, sex, age,
toxicity and/or palatability, social rank, size, health state, reproductive receptivity
exchanged, for example the presence of a predator, food, water, shelter or other
Vehrencamp, 2011). The different species in the animal kingdom may prioritise
according to their sensory adaptations and depending on the content and function of
the message.
2 Communication in cetaceans.
amphibious) (Geisler and Uhen, 2003, 2005); however, the phylogenetic evolution of
these animals is very unique as they re-entered the aquatic environment at least
seven times in the course of evolutionary history (Uhen, 2007). The last return to the
ocean occurred in the early Eocene, approximately 50 m.y. ago (Gingerich et al.,
1983). Such a return to aquatic life implies strong adaptations, notably in terms of
12
behavior and anatomy. A review of evolution of aquatic mammals (Uhen, 2007;
Lambert et al., 2017) explains that early cetaceans developed an elongated end of
the rostrum with external nares posterior to the tip of it, and with the eyes in a high
position on the lateral sides of the skull. The most representative adaptation of early
cetaceans to aquatic life was the enlargement and inflation of the ectotympanic lobe
The auditory channel of cetaceans is well adapted to aquatic life. In fact, sound
travels five times faster through water than in the atmosphere, allowing for
communication at long distances; this is very important for these animals, especially
between mothers and calves, since their anatomy and habitat do not allow the
mother to physically hold on to its offspring or to construct a den to leave the calves
behind while foraging (Janik, 2009). In these highly mobile animals, individuals of the
visibility (Connor et al., 1998). Under these conditions, the use of acoustics signals
seems to be the most effective strategy to assess their social and natural
channel (Tyack, 1999), but can be also tactile (Sakai et al., 2006; Dudzinski et al.,
2009), visual (reviewed in Tyack, 1999) and chemical (Kuznetzov, 1990; Kremers et
al., 2016). It may also involve several perceptive channels, in which case it would be
Communication is crucial for all social behaviors (Janik, 2009), and it is associated
with the cognitive abilities of a species since this impacts the way in which the
information flows (Tyack, 1999). Cognitive abilities of a species are in turn related to
13
the particular characteristics of the habitat, such as exposure to predators or food
Living cetaceans are divided in two suborders that differ in their social organization
and communication strategies: Mysticeti and Odontoceti. There are about 70 species
of odontocetes classified into six different families within which we find a wide variety
of social systems. In this thesis, I am going to focus on the social organization and
Delphinids are social creatures and it is less common to find a lone individual in
nature (Johnson and Norris, 1986). Within the 34 species that make up this family,
there is a wide variety of social systems that are dependent on life history of animals.
For larger delphinids, such as killer whales (Orcinus orca), pilot whales (Globicephala
spp.) and possibly Risso’s dolphins (Grampus griseus) kinship appears to be very
important in establishing the structure of groups (Wells et al., 1999; Reisinger et al.,
2017). For instance, the most stable social organization found to date in the
delphinids is that of killer whales, who live in stable extended family units called pods
(Bigg et al., 1987) that are composed of mothers and their offspring. The term pod,
another word for group, is defined as the individuals that regularly swim together
(Norris and Dohl, 1979). This definition excludes the possibility that delphinids
swimming some distance apart may be in acoustic contact for as much as hundreds
herding of prey, or participate in the parental care of the young (Würsig and Pearson,
2015).
14
In killer whales, both sexes spend their lives within their natal group (Heimlich-Boran,
1986) where each pod develops its own acoustic dialect (Tyack, 1999). For pilot
whales, pods are composed of related females with their offspring, with one or more
unrelated adult males whose presence is temporary (Amos et al., 1993). Killer
whales, pilot whales and Risso’s dolphins tend towards social matriarchies (Baird,
2000; Kasuya and Marsh, 1984) and are sexually dimorphic, where males are larger
and have bigger dorsal fins. It is likely that these systems tend toward polygyny
The stable associations found in these larger species are less evident in smaller
delphinids. Societies of smaller delphinid species are built around repeated, but not
society is called fission-fusion and has also been described for chimpanzees
(Goodall, 1986). In fission-fusion societies, the amount of time that individuals spend
relationships (Wells et al., 1999), as well habitat conditions, prey availability, mating
opportunities and predation risk (Gowans et al., 2008). For instance, small delphinids
in nearshore areas tend to occur in small groups of a few individuals to several tens,
while many offshore groups occur in hundreds to thousands (Würsig and Pearson,
2015).
Most pelagic, small delphinids exhibit polygynandry; males show polygynous mating
attempts while females are polyandrous (Orbach et al, 2014). These species also
genital slit that differenciates males and females (Würsig and Pearson, 2015). The
exception to polygynandry. Males have a huge postanal keel and strongly backward-
15
canted dorsal fin, and authors have assumed that polygyny is the norm in this
Delphinids have skin that is quite sensitive to even the lightest touch (Dudzinski and
Hill, 2018) with most sensitive areas around the eyes, blowhole, rostrum, lower jaw,
melon and the genital area (Wartzok and Ketten, 1999). Tactile communication plays
instance, Atlantic spotted dolphins (Stenella frontalis) in the Bahamas show 16 types
has also been reported for spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris) (Norris and
Delphinids are known to rub their bodies against each other and engage in rubbing
behaviors using their pectoral fins (Dudzinski et al., 2010; Dudzinski and Ribic,
characterized by contacts that might cause pain, such as biting, raking, ramming,
In bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) and Atlantic spotted dolphins (S. frontalis)
the genital regions might serve as a tactile receptor for low frequency echolocation
trains called “buzzes” during mating and during mother-calf interactions (Herzing,
2000)
16
2.2.2 Visual communication
signals. In delphinids visual signals include actions and gestures such as open-jaw
threat displays, aerial leaps, tail lobs, flared pectoral fins and S-shaped postures
(Dudzinski and Hill, 2018). Postures are used to synchronize actions among
individuals or groups and can function as a signal for group coordination or social
interaction (Connor and Krützen, 2015). Delphinids flare their pectoral fins and open
their jaws as a threat signal to appear larger, and males adopt S-shaped postures
Bubbles appear to be an extra visual communication signal, and they take several
forms: bubble streams, bubble clouds and bubble rings. They are often produced as
a threat signal (Marten et al., 1996) but are also used as a play signal in captive
The olfactory structures in delphinids have been lost in the course of evolution due to
the several shifts of the nasal apparatus (Morgane and Jacobs, 1972). Although
some olfactory structures are present during embryonic development, as has been
shown in stripped dolphins (S. coeruleoalba), they degenerate until birth (Kamiya and
experiments (Kuznetzov, 1990). Some studies have speculated about the role of
male dolphins may serve as a chemical signal detected by conspecifics (Herman and
17
Tavolga, 1980). It has also been suggested that the “open mouth behavior” in solitary
delphinids might serve to enhance taste reception (Herzing, 2000). Recent research
suggests that dolphins might sample chemical signatures in the urine and feces of
conspecifics for individual recognition (Kremers et al., 2016). Further studies are
The main adaptation of delphinids and all odontocetes to aquatic life may be the
production of outgoing sound, and the modifications of the inner ear for the
environment from the echoes received (Au, 1993). In this process, the aim of
echolocating signals does not seem to be the transfer information to another animal
like in a communication process. While all delphinids produce click vocalizations, not
all species whistle (Madsen et al., 2012). For instance, it has been reported that
Hector’s dolphins (Cephalorhynchus hectori) do not produce whistles, but might use
whistling dolphins that might use clicks for communication are Commerson’s dolphins
Buffrenil, 1989).
delphinids are whistles (Lammers and Oswald, 2015). Whistles are continuous,
duration that range from tens of milliseconds to several seconds (Tyack and Clark,
18
2000). They are composed of a fundamental frequency and often one or more
correlated with the orientation of the animal producing it, and this has lead some
researchers to suggest that the information carried in the harmonics of whistles might
be used by other individuals to infer the direction of movement of the emitter (Miller,
Most delphinid species produce whistles with fundamental frequencies that range
from 2 to 20 kHz (Lammers and Oswald, 2015). However, some delphinid species
(Sotalia guianensis) and killer whales (Orcinus orca) produce whistles beyond this
range (Lammers et al., 2003; Rasmussen and Miller, 2002; May-Collado and
Wartzok, 2009). Whistles are believed to play an important role in the social
Burst pulsed sounds are pulsed signals with short inter-pulse intervals (e.g. 200-1700
clicks per second) that do not seem to have an echolocation purpose (Dudzinski and
Hill., 2018). Instead they have been associated with agonistic behavior and have
1996). Delphinids of many species produce burst pulsed when they are excited or
angry and thus is thought that these kind of vocalizations transmit information about
the emotional state of the animal producing them (Dudzinski and Hill, 2018).
2018). For example, they display breaches, tail slaps, pectoral fin slaps and jaw
claps, where these behaviors result in sounds that travel hundreds of meters and
appear to be produced under the intentional control of the animal exhibiting them
19
(Dudzinski and Hill, 2018). In delphinids, fluke slaps are considered a sign of
frustration or irritation (Mann, 2000), but have been also documented during play
Delphinids have two bilateral sets of phonic lips, one associated with each nasal
passage (Cranford, et al., 1996) (Figure 1). These sets are different in size in nearly
all species of the family, with the right side being larger than the left (Cranford et al.,
2015). Despite this bilateral configuration, there is a current debate in the literature
20
Figure 1: Schematic Illustration of a dolphin’s head anatomy. Modified and adapted from
Cranford et al., (1996). Image taken from
https://matthewhardcastle.wordpress.com/2012/09/30/hello-world/
(Tursiops truncatus) and false killer whales (Pseudorca crassidens) placed two
contact hydrophones on the head of the animals, each one next to one pair of phonic
lips. Then they asked the animals to perform a detection task of a target 2.6m away.
authors suggested that not only the two studied species but all the delphinids click
only with the right side phonic lips (Madsen et al., 2013). This conclusion disregards
with the observations of several authors in which the left-side phonic lips do indeed
produce clicks but less frequently than the right-side (Cranford et al., 2011; Mackay
and Liaw, 1981). Madsen et al. (2013) suggested that highly trained animals could be
biased by the simplicity of the detection task asked during their experiments and only
21
needed to produce clicks with the right side’s phonic lips. However, all these studies
have been conducted with animals in captivity and to date it is unknown how dolphins
in the wild choose the parameters of their sound generation system (Cranford et al.,
2015).
resonance phenomenon in the air-filled cavities of the upper nasal pathways (Lilly,
1962; Mackay and Liaw, 1981). However, this idea was challenged by Murray et al.,
(1998) who suggested that whistles were tissue-borne vocalizations. A more recent
experiment validated this hypothesis (Madsen et al., 2012): the authors analyzed
recordings of a bottlenose dolphin that was given heliox to breathe. Since the speed
of sound is higher in this mixture that in air, they expected to find an increase of the
the whistles were generated by vibrating tissues, as in the vocal chords of humans or
2009). In order to reduce the hydrodynamic drag while swimming, there is a lack of
protruding parts associated with external ears. The middle and the inner ear are
cartilage, connective tissue and fat, instead of bones (Figure 1). The absence of an
side of the mandible that is in intimate contact with the tissues that connect to the
tympanic bulla. Through this connection between mandible and tympanic bulla,
dolphins allow sound to enter into their auditory system while keeping a low
22
hydrodynamic drag. The physical isolation of the bulla from the skull allows the
The hearing range of delphinids is 50 Hz to 150 kHz, with some variation between
species (reviewed in Richardson et al., 2013). This wide range allows the perception
tonal acoustic signals. Behavioral experiments have shown that bottlenose dolphins
spontaneously distinguish different whistle types even if they come from other
dolphin species (Caldwell et al. 1973a in Janik, 2009). Furthermore, the frequency
that differ by only 0.2–0.8 % of the fundamental frequency of the tone (Thomson and
echolocation limits. Using a 2.54 cm-diameter solid steel sphere and a 7.62cm-
diameter stainless steel water-filled sphere, Au and Snyder (1980) measured the
individuals. Results showed that dolphins could reach a 50% correct answer
threshold at 75 m with the 2.54 cm-diameter sphere and at 113 m with the 7.62 cm-
diameter sphere. The same results were registered in a false killer whale individual
(Pseudorca crassidens) (Thomas and Turl, 1990). Delphinids also detect objects
echoes, and to perceive subtle differences in targets with their sonar. This ability
23
3 Bottlenose dolphins
Bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) are the most studied cetacean (Connor et
al., 2000). This is not only because of its large easily accessible range which includes
coastal waters, but also because of its history of association with humans and the
fact that they are the species of cetacean most commonly kept in aquariums (Connor
dolphins are very interesting because of their social structure, cognitive capacities
in which individuals associate in small groups that often vary in size and composition,
and persists from minutes to hours or days at a time (Connor et al., 1998; Connor et
al., 2000). Groups typically contain between five and seven individuals, but nursery
groups tend to be larger for increased protection against predators (Mann et al.,
2000; Smolker et al., 1992; Wells, 2003). Bottlenose dolphins express affiliation by
proximity, physical contact such as pectoral or body rubbing (Sakai et al., 2006;
There is a difference between the sexes concerning the nature of social bonds
formed within the groups. Bonds between males tend to be stronger than those
between females (Connor et al., 1992; Connor et al., 2001). In fact, adult males form
consistent groups of two or three individuals called alliances that remain intact for
years, and which is thought to optimize access to females in estrus (Connor et al.,
1992). The individuals from these alliances are not genetically related, but tend to be
of similar age and were often raised in the same nursery group (Wells et al., 1990).
24
In contrast, females tend to form weaker bonds with other females but have a wider
reproductive status (Pearson, 2011). Female bottlenose dolphins spend most of their
time with other females at a similar point of their reproductive lives (Wells et al.,
1990). They rarely associate with males, except when cycling (Würsig and Pearson,
2015).
Calves remain with their mothers for an average of four years, during which they
learn foraging techniques and social skills (Mann et al., 2000). After this time, they
Under human care, dolphins do not choose their group members; however
Several studies on cognition in bottlenose dolphins show that these animals have
remarkable abilities which make them a very interesting model of study for
3.2.1 Attention
Bottlenose dolphins’ brains are capable of remaining alert and attentive during the
entire diurnal cycle (Pack, 2015). This was highlighted by research in which two
captive dolphins were asked to press a paddle within 21 seconds of hearing a target
sound. The dolphins were required to maintain their attention and press the paddle
only when the target sound appeared and in order to get a reward. Mean
performance levels in correct target detections across the sessions were over 94
percent for both animals. The dolphins sustained their attention over 120 hours (the
25
maximum duration tested) with no significant decrease in performance, with no
A study on working memory for sounds heard passively and objects inspected
was presented to a dolphin briefly, and after a delay interval, several comparison
stimuli appeared. The dolphin was asked to indicate which comparison matched the
sample. Herman and Gordon (1974) found that matching accuracy remained
consistent across hundreds of pairs of novel sounds for nearly all time delays (they
tested up to 120 seconds delay). For visually inspected objects, the matching
accuracy of the dolphin was sustained above time delays of 30 seconds, and then
longest delay tested) (Herman et al., 1989). These experiments show that bottlenose
Bottlenose dolphins’ long-term memory has been tested with the recognition of
familiar vs. non familiar “signature whistles”, a kind of whistles that are unique for an
individual (King and Janik, 2013). First a given dolphin was habituated to unfamiliar
whistles, and then its behavior was observed for either a familiar or a non-familiar
whistles than to unfamiliar whistles even if the time of separation with its congeners
26
3.2.4 Communication
capable of vocal learning and reference, where they have been known to copy novel
sounds and use them to refer to objects (Richards et al., 1984). They also
hand signals (Herman et al., 1986). Bottlenose dolphins naturally produce signature
whistles when they are separated from their companions (Janik and Sayigh, 2013)
and they appear to use signature whistles referentially to address each other (King
and Janik, 2013). However, there is no evidence in wild for the referential use of non-
signature whistles or non-whistle sounds, or for the use of syntax (Herman, 2010).
Social knowledge is defined as the possession of information about another and may
Bottlenose dolphins are capable of social imitation; they can be trained to imitate the
behaviors of conspecifics (Herman, 2002). Also, they are capable of joint attention
the clicks and echoes produced by another individual nearby (Xitco and Roitblat,
1996). However, there is a lack of studies on how much dolphin’s guide each other’s
Bottlenose dolphins are capable of mirror self-recognition (Reiss and Marino, 2001),
a capacity that indicates self-awareness (Lewis, 1991). This ability is rare in the
animal kingdom and was once thought to be unique to humans (Amsterdam, 1972)
27
and great apes (Gallup, 1970). However, more recently studies have shown that
Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) (Plotnik et al., 2006), and magpies (Pica pica)
(Prior at al., 2008) are able to recognize their image in the mirror.
Bottlenose dolphins are capable of extensive and rich vocal and behavioral imitation,
one of the forms of social learning (Marino et al., 2007). We can find different types of
Bottlenose dolphins in Shark Bay (Australia) have been observed breaking off
protection while foraging on the seafloor (Smolker et al., 1997). This behavior is
carried out by only some individuals of the population, most of them females (Mann
and Patterson, 2013), calves learn this behavior from their mothers in their second or
third year of life (Mann and Sargeant, 2003). This is not only an example of tool use
(mostly daughters) will only sponge if their mother also sponged (Mann et al., 2008;
Bottlenose dolphins are also capable of horizontal social learning, since in captivity
young calves have been observed imitating other calves (Kuczaj et al., 2006). In the
wild, bottlenose dolphins’ calves model their signature whistles (see section 3.3.1.2)
Bottlenose dolphins are a highly mobile species, as individuals of the same group
28
et al., 1998). Under these conditions, the use of acoustics signals seems to be the
most effective strategy for animals to assess their social and natural environment.
Therefore, bottlenose dolphins display a very complex and rich sound production.
The diversity of sounds emitted by this species has been classified in different ways
by researchers and in addition the same names are given for types of vocalizations
consensus for the range of T. truncatus vocalizations in the literature. Currently, the
described vocalizations are categorized into three structural categories and two
functional classes. Structurally, the sounds emitted by the bottlenose dolphins are
found within: whistles or tonal sounds (reviewed in Janik, 2009), clicks or pulsed
sounds (Au, 1993), and burst-pulsed sounds (Lopez and Bernal-Shirai, 2009) (Figure
2).
Functionally, sound emissions may be used for echolocation (used for orientation
Figure 2: Spectrogram examples: a) whistle b) click train c) two burst-pulsed sounds. X axis
represents the time in seconds and Y axis represents the frequency in kHz.
29
3.3.1 Whistles
Whistles are frequency modulated signals with fundamental frequencies from 800Hz
to 28.5kHz and durations between 100ms and 4sec (reviewed in Janik, 2009). The
term “whistle” is used to refer to a unit of one continuous contour (loop), two or more
silence lasting between 0.03 and 0.25 sec in duration (disconnected multi-loop
catalogued in six general categories with respect to the contour of its fundamental
frequency (Figure 2): upsweep, down sweep, flat, convex, concave and continually
modulated (Bazua-Duran, 2004; Akiyama and Ohta, 2006; Hickey et al., 2008).
Figure 3: Categories of whistles with respect to the contour of its fundamental frequency a)
Upsweep b) Down sweep c) flat d) convex e) concave f) continually modulated.
The upsweep whistle has been reported several times as the most frequently emitted
(McCowan and Reiss, 1995; Hickey et al., 2008; Diaz-Lopez, 2011), suggesting this
kind of whistle plays an important role in the bottlenose dolphin whistle repertoire
(Tyack, 1986; Janik et al., 1994; Diaz-Lopez, 2011). However, the behavioural
context description associated with this type of whistle remains vague and often
behavioural observations are made from surface. In free ranging bottlenose dolphins
the upsweep whistle type has been reported to be related to social behaviours (Diaz-
Lopez, 2011), and in captivity this kind of whistle has been recorded when animals
30
3.3.1.1 Non-signature whistles
produced whistles in captive dolphins (Janik and Slater, 1998), representing around
52% in free-ranging dolphins during social contexts (Cook et al.,2004), and reaching
values of more than 90% of whistles produced only during forced isolation (Sayigh et
Some individually specific whistles are called “signature whistles”. These kind of
whistles were discovered by Caldwell and Caldwell in 1965, who observed that
during contexts of isolation, each individual produced one distinctive kind of whistle
(Caldwell and Caldwell, 1965). Further studies showed that signature whistles are
dolphins an increase of the signature whistle emission rate has been reported during
(Caldwell and Caldwell, 1965; Sayigh et al., 1990, 1995; Janik et al., 1994; Watwood
et al., 2005). However, outside this setting, signature whistles are more frequently
emitted during social interactions than during other behavioral contexts, such as
feeding or travelling (Cook et al., 2004). It has been shown that signature whistles
are emitted as a contact or cohesion call between mothers and calves (Smolker et
31
al., 1993) and between members of the same group when animals are out each
1990). The number of loops produced in a signature whistle varies according to the
behavioral context and it increases with age, which means that older individuals
produce signature whistles with more loops (Caldwell et al., 1990). More recently, a
study on wild dolphins showed that signature whistles are also copied by other
individuals of the group, possibly to label a particular individual (Janik, 2000). After
whistle mimicry might be affiliative (King et al., 2014) and the number of loops
mothers which suggests that the fetus might be susceptible to imprinting in utero,
meaning that the new-born dolphin would recognize its mother’s signature whistle
right after birth (Mello and Amundin, 2005). The calves take one or two years to
develop their own signature whistle (Fripp et al., 2005; Sayigh et al., 1990) and their
frequency modulation pattern remains stable during their entire life (Sayigh et al.,
changing social relationships (Watwood et al., 2004). Young males may use
signature whistles similar to their mother’s, while young females that tend to remain
in their natal group are more likely to choose different frequency modulation patterns,
probably to allow differentiation from their mothers (Sayigh et al., 1990, 1995).
32
3.3.2 Clicks
Clicks are short broadband signals typically lasting 40-70 μs with peak frequencies
between 110 kHz and 130 kHz and peak-to-peak source levels between 210 and 228
surrounding from the echoes received (Au, 1993). During target scanning, sequences
of clicks (click trains) are produced. The inter-click interval (ICI) is the time of two-way
travel of the sound (TWT) to and from a target plus an echo processing period
between 19 and 45 ms long (Au, 1993). During target detection, T. truncatus modify
the ICI in order to focus their attention to a particular distance (Penner, 1988).
However, when the target is at close ranges (<40cm), the ICI decrease to 2.5ms
(Evans and Powell, 1967), suggesting the dolphins may process several echoes at a
Bottlenose dolphins use clicks as a sensory tool to navigate or hunt for prey
(reviewed in Herzing and dos Santos 2004) and obtain information from their own
returning signals (Au, 1993) and by eavesdropping on the echoes produced by other
Bottlenose dolphins develop their ability to echolocate in the first one to three months
of life (reviewed in Harder et al., 2016). Before they are one month old, calves’ clicks
are of shorter duration (Reiss 1988) and lower frequency (Reiss 1988; Lindhard
1988) than adults. Additionally, at 14 days old, click trains have a shorter inter-click
intervals (ICI) and shorter duration (Favaro et al. 2013) than adults.
33
3.3.3 Burst pulsed sounds
Burst-pulsed sounds are the least studied category and their definition is less clear in
the literature (Herzing, 2000). They are often defined as rapid click trains (Janik,
2009), with many types of different sound emissions being labelled in this category
(Herzing, 2000). In fact, the term is used for all the vocalizations that are not
considered clicks or whistles (Janik, 2009) or, to be more precise, by all the pulsed
sounds (clicks) for which no echolocation function is known (Lammers et al., 2004).
Burst-pulsed sounds have received several names in literature such as: cracks,
However, a more recent study, classified the total repertoire of burst-pulsed sounds
burst pulsed vocalizations (impulsive emissions shorter than 200ms with most energy
below 5kHz) and “long burst pulsed vocalizations” (a single or sequence of pulses
(Overstrom, 1983; Herzing, 1996; Sayigh et al., 2017). One kind of burst pulsed
sound called “bray call” has been associated with foraging, apparently to facilitate the
dolphins
simultaneous descriptions of both visual and acoustic signals (Thomas et al., 2002).
However, the main pitfall in linking acoustic production and behavior is the difficulty in
identifying which dolphin in a group is the vocalizer. This challenge is caused by two
34
reasons: first, dolphins do not open their mouth or display any external cues when
producing a sound (Janik, 2009) and second, human hearing is not well-adapted to
signature whistles (Caldwell et al., 1990) and enabled them to make the first
descriptions about its use (Sayigh et al., 1990; Janik et al., 1994; Sayigh et al., 1995;
Sayig et al., 1998). However, this methodology does not allow addressing research
questions regarding how the bottlenose dolphins use their vocalizations while they
3.4.2 Tagging
animal. The use of a tag with an embedded hydrophone produced the first evidence
the mimicry of signature whistles (Tyack, 1986). However, the presence of such a tag
could conceivably lead to modification of the subject’s behaviors and its vocalization
Several authors use the production of bubbles streams concurrent with whistle
production to identify the dolphin vocalizing (Mc Cowan and Reiss, 1995; Herzing,
1996). However, more recent studies showed that only some kind of whistles are
accompanied by bubbles while being produced, and thus they are not representative
35
3.4.4 Hydrophone arrays
The use of hydrophones arrays is a non-intrusive method that uses the differences of
arrival of sound to each hydrophone to calculate the direction of the source of sound.
Fixed hydrophone arrays have been conceived with two (Lopez-Rivas and Bazua-
Duran, 2010), three (Watkins and Schevill, 1974), four (Brensing et al., 2001; Quick
et al., 2008) and eight hydrophones (Thomas et al., 2002). With these kinds of
arrays, the behavioral observations are obtained from surface, which allows
collecting only a very small percentage of the dolphins’ behavioral activity (Janik,
2009).
are able to collect more details about the dolphin’s behavior. They have been
designed with two (Dudzinski et al., 1995), three (Hoffman-Kuhnt et al., 2016), four
(Au and Herzing, 2003; Schotten et al., 2004) and 16 hydrophones (Ball and Buck,
2005). Their main disadvantage is that dolphins are highly mobile species and the
narrow angle of the underwater camera allows the localization of only those dolphins
groups. They rely mainly on the acoustic channel to communicate over long
such as being able to remain alert during the entire day, and having a working
years. They are capable of vocal learning, referencing, and can be taught to
36
understand syntactic rules. There is a general lack of information about the use of
these abilities within their social group. However, we know that bottlenose dolphins’
vocal production includes whistles, clicks and burst-pulsed sounds, with some
whistles called “signature whistles” potentially being used to address each other. In
moment the current methodologies are not satisfactory. During this thesis, we
developed an easily deployable system that identifies the animal producing the
The present PhD thesis aims to better understand the communication of bottlenose
dolphins within their social group. First, I developed two studies aiming to describe
how captive dolphin’s vocal activity varies in relationship with behavior and
interaction with humans. For this, I focused first on signature whistle production
underwater behaviors (Chapter 2). These two studies highlight the necessity to link
environment, and which can be used in captivity and with free-ranging dolphins. This
(Chapter 5).
37
38
RESULTS
39
40
Chapter 1: Modulation of Whistle Production Related
1
Institute of Neurosciences Paris Saclay, Université Paris Sud, CNRS UMR 9197
Orsay, France
2
Institut Jean Le Rond d’Alembert, Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06,
3
Laboratoire d’Ethologie Expérimentale et Comparée E.A. 4443 (LEEC), Université
4
Parc Astérix, Plailly, France
41
Synthesis Chapter 1
Context
Bottlenose dolphins are highly social cetaceans that strongly rely on acoustic
communication and signaling. The diversity of sounds emitted by the species has
been structurally classified in whistles, clicks and burst-pulsed sounds. Whistles are
whistles are called “signature whistles” and are used as cohesion calls.
Research questions
sessions that are held per day. However, it remains unknown how the scheduled
emission rate of different whistles types (e.g., signature whistles). This study aims to
describe the possible effects that training/feeding sessions, have on the emission
dolphins.
Analysis
The study was conducted on a group of 8 (in November 2014) and 9 (in May 2015)
bottlenose dolphins at the Parc Asterix dolphinarium (Plailly, France). Whistles were
recorded approximately 15 min before, during and 15 min after ten training sessions.
We applied the SIGID method to identify signature whistles within our catalog of
whistle types. Mean values of whistles emission rate and signature whistle emission
rate per minute were calculated for the recordings before, during and after each
training session.
42
Results
The dolphins’ overall whistle emission rate did not significantly change before, during
and after the training sessions. However, the non-signature emission rate was higher
during and afterwards than before the training sessions and the signature whistle
emission rate was significantly higher after than before the training sessions. The
Conclusion
In conclusion, our study shows that non-signature and particular signature whistle
emission rate increases after scheduled training sessions in Parc Asterix dolphi-
narium. We suggest that animals might have been seeking social interactions after
observe the animals’ behaviors and to link the patterns of group association with
whistle emissions.
43
Zoo Biology 9999 : 1–10 (2016)
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Bottlenose dolphins are highly social cetaceans with an extensive sound production including clicks, burst-pulsed
sounds, and whistles. Some whistles, known as signature whistles, are individually specific. These acoustic signatures
are commonly described as being emitted in contexts of stress during forced isolation and as group cohesion calls.
Interactions between humans and captive dolphins is largely based on positive reinforcement conditioning within
several training/feeding sessions per day. Vocal behavior of dolphins during these interactions might vary. To
investigate this, we recorded 10 bottlenose dolphins of Parc Asterix dolphinarium (France) before, during and after 10
training sessions for a total duration of 7 hr and 32 min. We detected 3,272 whistles with 2,884 presenting a quality
good enough to be categorized. We created a catalog of whistle types by visual categorization verified by five naive
judges (Fleiss’ Kappa Test). We then applied the SIGID method to identify the signatures whistles present in our
recordings. We found 279 whistles belonging to one of the four identified signature whistle types. The remaining 2,605
were classified as non-signature whistles. The non-signature whistles emission rate was higher during and after the
training sessions than before. Emission rate of three signature whistles types significantly increased afterwards as
compared to before the training sessions. We suggest that dolphins use their signature whistles when they return to
their intraspecific social interactions succeeding scheduled and human-organized training sessions. More observations
are needed to make conclusions about the function of signature whistles in relation to training sessions. Zoo Biol. XX:
XX–XX, 2016. © 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Zoo Biology
Whistle Emission Rate in Bottlenose Dolphins Under Human Care 3
shallowest point to 4.5 m at its deepest. The indoor part of the Whistles with a negative signal-to-noise ratio or overlapping
complex, divided into two sections, has a total volume of with other whistles were registered but not included in the
550 m3 and a depth of 2.5 m. The dolphins have free access categorization. Once each whistle spectrogram was regis-
between the pools at all times. tered, a visual categorization of whistle types was carried out.
We applied the SIGID method [Janik et al., 2013] to identify
Whistle Recordings signature whistles within our catalog of whistle types based on
two criteria: firstly, signature whistles were whistle types
Whistles were recorded approximately 15 min before,
repeated at least four times in a recording session, and
during and 15 min after ten training sessions that took place
secondly, at least on one occasion the whistles were produced
on 6 days: five recording sessions were conducted over
in a sequence in which 75% or more repetitions occur within
4 days in November 2014 and five more over 2 days in
1–10 sec of one other. The whistle types that were not
May 2015. The recordings were carried out using a CRT
cataloged as signature whistle types using this method were
hydrophone C54XRS (frequency response: 0.016–44 kHz
cataloged as non-signature whistle types.
! 3 dB) plugged in to a TASCAM HDP2 recorder at the
To verify the reliability of our classification method,
acquisition rate of 96 kHz and samples were coded on 24 bits.
five experts, all affiliated to the acoustic communication team
In order to prevent the dolphins touching and grabbing the
of NeuroPSI laboratory (Orsay, France) and working on
hydrophone, it was placed in a flexible floating tube inside an
bioacoustics in classification of birds or cetacean sounds,
18.9 L polycarbonate bottle with multiple perforations. The
performed two visual classification tasks using the identified
apparatus was fixed to a wooden stick at a distance of 50 cm
signature whistles of our dataset [see Kriesell et al., 2014].
from the edge of the pool and 50 cm deep near the small
For each signature whistle type, six whistle repetitions were
beach area (Fig. 1).
randomly selected: 1 to act as a template and 5 to be classified
by the experts. Each signature whistle repetition was
Visual Categorization Process
surrounded by the signature whistle templates and was
To create a whistle catalogue, spectrograms (FFT size: presented to each expert on a Microsoft Power Point slide. In
1024, overlap 50%, Hanning window) of the recorded the first task, the experts were asked to compare each whistle
whistles were analyzed using Audacity 2.06 software (GNU repetition with each template and to rate the similarity in a
General Public License, The Audacity Team, Pittsburg, scale from 1 (very different) to 5 (very similar). The second
PA). Graphs with standardized x- and y-axes (1 sec long, task was to assign to each whistle repetition the most similar
with a frequency range of 0 Hz to 48 kHz) of the frequency template category. The ratings were compared between
modulation of each whistle were used to prevent distortion experts using Fleiss’ Kappa statistic [Siegel and Castellan,
of whistles caused by axes differing in length as this 1988] to determine inter-observer agreement in whistle
would have influenced the visual categorization process. classification and consistency in categorization (with and
Zoo Biology
4 Lopez Marulanda et al.
without authors’ classifications). When experts are in sessions in November with nine individuals and in the last
complete agreement Fleiss’ Kappa statistics (k) is equal to five recording sessions in May with eight individuals. We
1 [Landis and Koch, 1977]. If agreement between experts is detected the occurrence of 210 signature whistles during
the same as expected by chance, then k is equal to 0. the first recording sessions and the occurrence of 69 signature
whistles during the last recording sessions.
Whistle Emission Analysis The two visual classification tasks tested reliability of
Statistical tests were conducted using R statistical identifying whistle types. The first task showed a low inter-
software version 3.02 [R Core Team, 2013]. Mean values of observer agreement (Fleiss’ kappa statistic without author as
whistles emission rate and signature whistle emission rate judge: k ¼ 0.388, n judges ¼ 5, z ¼ 18.7, P ¼ 0.00001; with
per minute were calculated for the recordings before, during author as judge: k ¼ 0.408, n judges ¼ 6, z ¼ 24.2,
and after each training session. The Friedman Rank Test was P ¼ 0.00001). During the second task, the experts repeatedly
used to compare the non-signature whistle emission rate and chose the highest similarity rating for the first task as the most
the signature whistle emission rate before, during and after similar whistle to the template category. The inter-observer
each training session. Post hoc tests were performed to agreement was high in the second task (Fleiss’ kappa
examine the variation in the tested variables. statistic: k ¼ 0.956, z ¼ 28.7, P ¼ 0.00001). These results
show that clearly defined whistle types exist in the repertoire
of Parc Asterix bottlenose dolphins and support the authors’
RESULTS visual categorization of the dataset.
A total of 7 hr 32 min (Table 1) were recorded among The overall whistle emission rate during our recordings
the ten training sessions (154 min before, 147 min during was 7.48 whistles per minute. We calculated this rate
training sessions and 152 min after) in which 3,272 whistles (including signature and non-signature whistles) by averaging
were identified: 309 (9.44%) were classified as having too the ten sessions before, during and after the training sessions.
low signal-to-noise ratio whistles to be considered in this The rate did not change significantly from 4.72 " 3.32
study and 79 (2.41%) were classified as overlapping whistles per minute before the training sessions, to
whistles, the remaining 2,884 (88.14%) were classified in 8.14 " 2.74 whistles per minute during the training sessions
signature or non-signature whistle types. Most of the and 9.84 " 7.44 whistles per minute after the training sessions
identified whistles were recorded during the first five (Friedman Rank Test: x2 ¼ 2.6, df ¼ 2, P ¼ 0.2725) (Fig. 3).
recording sessions with nine individuals (n ¼ 1,946; before When comparing non-signature and signature whistles
training: 288, during training: 743, and after training: 915) separately, we found that dolphins emitted more non-
while less of half of whistles was recorded during the last five signature whistles during and afterwards (respectively
recording sessions with eight individuals (n ¼ 938; before Wilcoxon signed Rank Test: V ¼ 4, P ¼ 0.0137 and V ¼ 2,
training: 192; during training: 329, and after training: 417). P ¼ 0.0058 with Bonferroni-adjusted significance level of
According to SIGID method, 279 (9.67%) signature P < 0.0167) than before the training sessions. No significant
whistles were identified belonging to four different signature differences were found between the non-signature whistle
whistles types (Fig. 2). The remaining 2,605 (90.32%) emission rate during and after the training sessions (Wilcoxon
were classified as non-signature whistle types. The four signed Rank Test: V ¼ 25, P ¼ 0.8457 with Bonferroni-
signature whistles were present in the first five recording adjusted significance level of P < 0.0167) (Fig. 4).
TABLE 1. Time of recording of the 10 sessions: Before, during and after the training
Duration (hh:mm:ss)
Session and social grouping Before During After Total
Zoo Biology
Whistle Emission Rate in Bottlenose Dolphins Under Human Care 5
Fig. 2. Three randomly chosen spectrograms of each of the identified signature whistles emitted by Parc Ast!erix bottlenose dolphins
(Plailly, France): (a) Signature whistle type 1 (SW1); (b) Signature whistle type 2 (SW2) which can be identified as variably loopy based on
the final loop which is consistent from whistle to whistle; (c) Signature whistle type 3 (SW3); (d) Signature whistle type 4 (SW4). The
numbers in the right are the total occurrences of the whistle type found in the acoustic recordings (n ¼ 293 signature whistles).
Spectrograms are all presented in the same scaling. Frequency (kHz) is on the y-axis and ranges from 0 to 48 kHz. Time (s) is on the x-axis.
FFT 1,024, Hanning window, overlap 50%.
Zoo Biology
6 Lopez Marulanda et al.
Fig. 4. Boxplot of bottlenose dolphins’ non-signature whistle signatures whistles types, increasing for types 1, 2, and 3
emission rate before, during and after training sessions (n ¼ 10).
"
Wilcoxon signed Rank Test: V ¼ 2, P < 0.0167 (with Bonferroni and decreasing for type 4.
correction). "" Wilcoxon signed Rank Test: V ¼ 4, P < 0.0167 (with
Bonferroni correction).
DISCUSSION
signature whistle between the sessions. However, we
Dolphin whistle emission rate is highly variable and
calculated the emission rate of each signature whistle type
depends on several parameters: groups size [Jones and
for the 10 sessions before, during and after the training.
Sayigh, 2002; Cook et al., 2004; Quick and Janik, 2008],
Whistle rate increased after the training sessions for
group composition [Hawkins and Gartside, 2010] and
signatures whistles type 1 (SW1), type 2 (SW2), and type
behavioral context [Dos Santos et al., 1990; Jacobs et al.,
3 (SW3). The whistle emission rate of the signature whistle
1993; Acevedo-Guti!errez and Stienessen, 2004; Cook et al.,
type 4 (SW4) was higher before than after the training
2004]. Most of the whistles detected occurred during the first
sessions (Fig. 6).
five recording sessions: in November the nine dolphins
To summarize, the dolphins’ overall whistle emission
whistled and produced signature whistles two times more
rate did not significantly change before, during and after
frequently than the eight individuals in May. It is
the training sessions. However, the non-signature emission
comprehensible to have more whistles and signature whistles
rate was higher during and afterwards than before the
produced when the group size increases [Van Parijs et al.,
training sessions and the signature whistle emission rate
2002], but here, the difference in occurrence of whistles was
was significantly higher after than before the training
not proportional to the number of individuals. Instead, it is
sessions. The emission rate varied between the different
possible that the group composition impacted the dolphins’
vocal productions, and in particular the age of the individuals
might also have been an important variable. The first
recording sessions in November were carried out in a group
with four young dolphins out of nine individuals while the
second set of recordings in May occurred in a group of two
young dolphins and six adults. Mother–offspring interactions
include various behaviors (i.e., teaching behaviors) [Bender
et al., 2009] and involve vocalizations (i.e., during periods of
separation) [Smolker et al., 1993]. The nature of the
intraspecific social interactions conducted within the groups
might have influenced the number of whistles and signature
whistles recorded. We suggest that the presence of young
dolphins might have increased the number of affiliative, play
and discipline behaviors within the group and these
behaviors could be correlated to a high production of
whistles.
The SIGID method [Janik et al., 2013] allowed us to
Fig. 5. Boxplot of bottlenose dolphins’ signature whistle (all
types) emission rate before, during and after training sessions identify four signature whistles within the bottlenose
(n ¼ 10). " Wilcoxon signed Rank Test: V ¼ 0, P < 0.0167 (with dolphins at Parc Asterix dolphinarium. If signature whistles
Bonferroni correction). are individually specific [Caldwell et al., 1990] we could
Zoo Biology
Whistle Emission Rate in Bottlenose Dolphins Under Human Care 7
expect to find nine signature whistles in the first half of our their free-time in absence of people (which is analog to the
recording sessions and eight in the second half. However, the period after training session in our study). Therrien et al.
SIGID method was conceived to be very conservative so that [2012] measured the whistle production of a group of eight
false positives were eliminated. This precaution means bottlenose dolphins (four adult females, two adult males, and
the SIGID method did not consider about half of the signature two young males) and found increased whistle production to
whistles present in the sample [Janik et al., 2013]. coincide with increased interactions with humans during
We recorded a total of 7 hr and 32 min. It is probable that feeding/training sessions. Recently, a study carried out on
signature whistles of all the individuals were present in our five captive false killer whales (P. crassidens) (three adult
samples but we only identified less than 50% of them using the females, one adult male, and one male calf) also found an
SIGID method. In this case, some of the non-signature increase in their acoustic emissions (including whistles)
whistles that were used in our analyses are signature whistles upon trainers’ arrival [Platto et al., 2015]. The high rate was
that were not detected by the method and in this terms the maintained during feeding sessions and reduced immediately
results we obtained on the non-signature whistle emission rate after the animals were fed. In contrast, we found that non-
are influenced by the signature whistle emission rate. signature whistles increased during the training sessions but
However, the emission of signature whistles in captivity is their rate was higher afterwards, and signature whistle rate
very scarce and for some individuals can be less than 1% of was higher after the training sessions compared to before.
whistle emission rate [Janik and Slater, 1998]. Thus, it is Dolphins’ behaviors and vocalizations can be modu-
highly probable that signature whistles of all the individuals lated by trainings [Kuczaj and Xitco, 2002]. Since no
were not present in our acoustic recordings. It would be information could be found about the influence of the nature
necessary to record the animals during forced [Esch et al., and content of trainings in the related papers, we cannot
2009a] or voluntary isolation [Janik and Slater, 1998] or using comment on the impact they have on whistle emission rate.
a hydrophone array [L!opez-Rivas and Baz!ua-Dur!an, 2010], to Moreover, in Akiyama and Ohta’s [2007] study, dolphins
link the whistle emission to individual dolphins in order to find spent less than 2 years under human care; this is in contrast to
the signature whistle for each member of the group. Parc Ast!erix dolphins, where six out of nine dolphins are
The first classification task allows our study to be born in the dolphinarium and the other three have been in
comparable to previous studies that use visual categorization captivity for over 2 decades. It has been shown that free-
of bottlenose dolphins’ whistles as Janik [2000] and Kriesell ranging dolphins increase their whistle emission rate during
et al. [2014]. The low inter observer agreement obtained on feedings probably to recruit more members to the group
the first classification task has also been reported by these [Acevedo-Guti!errez and Stienessen, 2004], and this behavior
authors and might be due to the fact that we asked judges to is likely not necessary, or less present, in captivity where
classify whistles on a scale of discrimination that is too fine feeding is less cooperative than in the wild. In Akiyama and
and leads to subjectivity. In fact when one of the authors Ohta’s [2007] study, the dolphins might interact (e.g., to
redid the first classification task several months later, the cooperate) while feeding. Unfortunately, Therrien et al.
inter observer agreement with herself was low (K ¼ 0.133 [2012] and Platto et al. [2015] do not specify for how long
z ¼ 1.9, P ¼ 0.0581). However, the second classification task their studied animals have been in captivity.
that asked the judges to choose the most similar whistles Our study shows that overall, signature whistle
showed a high inter observer agreement, which supports the emission significantly increased after the training sessions.
author’s visual categorization of the data set. However this was not the case for all the signature whistles
When we compared signature and non-signature types we detected, suggesting that depending upon the
whistles, the total emission rate did not significantly change situation dolphins’ signature whistles production varies, and
before, during and after the training sessions. Our results consequently they might be used for various functions.
differ from previous findings on other groups of cetaceans Context of emissions of signature whistles varies from stress
under human care: for instance, bottlenose dolphins calls during forced isolation [Esch et al., 2009a] to cohesion
increased whistle production during interactions with calls [Smolker et al., 1993; Janik and Slater, 1998; Quick and
humans [Akiyama and Ohta, 2007; Therrien et al., 2012]. Janik, 2012]. In Parc Asterix, during training sessions the
Akiyama and Ohta [2007] measured the number of whistles trainers divide the animals into groups of the same two or
emitted by three captive bottlenose dolphins (one male and three individuals. Each sub-group remains with one trainer
two females, all less than 8 years old) during several and performs different exercises during the session. This
situations in a facility in Muroto (Japan): immediately before division is never forced and it is achieved by using positive
feeding, during feeding, during the animals’ free-time reinforcement. The training session by itself can be
without the presence of people, and during interactions considered as rewarding for the animals [Laule and
with people on a float and in the water. They found that most Desmond, 1998], since they are positively reinforced when
of the whistles were emitted during the period preceding they perform exercises. A previous study conducted in this
feeding (which is analog to the period before trainings in our facility measured the breathing rate of animals before and
study), and whistle emission was higher during various after the training sessions [Jensen et al., 2013] as a possible
interactions with humans (including feeding) than during indicator of stress [Broom and Johnson, 1993; Dierauf,
Zoo Biology
8 Lopez Marulanda et al.
2001]. The results showed that the animals maintained the These individual differences could be explained
same breathing rate before and after the sessions [Jensen by the presence of different personalities in dolphins
et al., 2013], indicating that the exercises they were asked to [Birgersson et al., 2014; Highfill and Kuczaj, 2007] that
perform did not affect their level of stress. The increase in leads to individual variation in vocal activity. Since group
signature and non-signature whistle emission rates therefore composition and behavioral contexts influence dolphins’
is not likely to be explained by the animals being stressed vocalization rate [Dos Santos et al., 1990; Jacobs et al.,
during the training sessions. We suggest that the increase in 1993; Cook et al., 2004; Hawkins and Gartside 2010], it
non-signature and some signature whistle emission after would be necessary to identify the vocalizing dolphins and
training sessions is due to an augmentation of social to observe the animals’ behaviors during signature whistles
behaviors. Before training sessions, dolphins can freely emissions to explain the particular behavioral context that
interact displaying affiliative, agonistic, and sexual behav- caused these individual differences. As a hypothesis, we
iors [Herzing, 1996; Samuels and Gifford, 1997]. Since suggest that non-signature whistles are intended to give
training sessions occur consistently approximately at the information to listener dolphins, while signature whistles
same hour, dolphins can perform anticipatory behaviors are used to give information about the emitter. The copy of
[Jensen et al., 2013] which could have an influence in their signature whistles might play a role in spreading the
vocal production as has been found in captive false killer information and letting the emitter know that the
whales (P. crassidens) [Platto et al., 2015] and bottlenose information has correctly been received. Vocal mimicry
dolphins in other facilities [Akiyama and Ohta, 2007]. is an important part of communication in all species of
Training sessions occur consistently approximately at the mammals, but this is higher for cetacean species, in
same hour and before these dolphins can perform anticipa- particular for toothed whales. These prolific vocal
tory behaviors [Jensen et al., 2013] which could have an exchanges might probably be due to the development of
influence in their vocal production as has been found in their personalities, the features of their social structure and
captive false killer whales (P. crassidens) [Platto et al., also the large diversity of their sound emissions.
2015] and bottlenose dolphins in other facilities [Akiyama In conclusion, our study shows that non-signature
and Ohta, 2007]. During training sessions, the groups are and particular signature whistle emission rate increases
subdivided and dolphins are asked to perform several after scheduled training sessions in Parc Asterix dolphi-
exercises, where these activities modulate social interactions narium. We suggest that animals might have been seeking
between animals. Finally, after the training sessions social interactions after the sessions. We suppose that
individuals are free to regroup as they want and the signature before the sessions, animals are free to interact, or not
whistles might then be used as cohesion calls and copied as interact, with the partner(s) they choose, during the training
affiliative signals [King et al., 2014]. sessions the group structure changes due to human
When comparing the emission rate of signature intervention (trainers regroup particular dolphins into
whistles before and after the training sessions we found groups of 2–3 individuals), and after the training sessions
that SW1, SW2, and SW3 emission rates increased after the dolphins freely regroup using signature whistles as
training session and SW4 emission rate decreased after the cohesion calls. However, in order to validate this
training session. Signature whistles are individually hypothesis, it is necessary to directly observe the animals’
specific [Caldwell et al., 1990], so it is highly probable behaviors and to link the patterns of group association with
that the four signature whistles identified were mostly whistle emissions. Moreover, during training sessions the
emitted by four particular individuals with the exception of trainers ask the dolphins to perform solitary and coordi-
the cases where the signature whistles are copied [Janik, nated exercises, and their vocalization rate might also
2000; Tyack, 1986]. If this is the case, the signature depend on the task the trainers ask them to perform. We
whistles detected are not from the three males that were can expect higher sound production rates during coordi-
transferred between facilities because they are present nated exercises and cooperative tasks [Eskelinen et al.,
before and after the transfer. One of the signature whistles 2016]. Linking whistle emissions to particular behaviors
(SW2) consists of several connected loops. Since the will be the next step to better understand how dolphins
number of loops increases with the age of the individual under human care communicate.
[Caldwell et al., 1990], we suggest that SW2 probably
belongs to one of the oldest animals in the group.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The differences found between the emission rates of
each signature whistle type might be due to individual We are very grateful to the Parc Asterix Dolphinarium
differences, meaning that the three individuals that emitted curator, Birgitta Mercera, and her trainers for their help
more signature whistles after the training sessions were during the recording sessions. We would like to thank to the
probably seeking group cohesion or at least looking for social acoustic communication team (NeuroPsi) members for their
interactions. In contrast, one individual emitted more constructive comments and for carrying out the classification
signature whistles before the training sessions probably tasks and Isabella Clegg for revising the English of this
looking for social interactions in a different moment. manuscript.
Zoo Biology
Whistle Emission Rate in Bottlenose Dolphins Under Human Care 9
Zoo Biology
10 Lopez Marulanda et al.
Smolker RA, Mann J, Smuts BB. 1993. Use of signature whistles during Sociobiol 18:251–257. Available online at: http://doi.org/10.1007/
separations and reunions by wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and infants. BF00300001
Behav Ecol Sociobiol 33:393–402. Available online at: http://doi.org/ Van Parijs SM, Smith J, Corkeron PJ. 2002. Using calls to estimate the
10.1007/BF00170254 abundance of inshore dolphins: a case study with Pacific humpback
Therrien SC, Thomas JA, Therrien RE, Stacey R. 2012. Time of day and dolphins Sousa chinensis. J Appl Ecol 39:853–864.
social change affect underwater sound production by bottlenose Watwood SL, Tyack PL, Wells RS. 2004. Whistle sharing in paired
dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) at the Brookfield Zoo. Aquat Mamm male bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol
38:65. 55:531–543. Available online at: http://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-003-
Tsai Y-JJ, Mann J. 2013. Dispersal, philopatry, and the role of fission-fusion 0724-y
dynamics in bottlenose dolphins. Mar Mamm Sci 29:261–279. Watwood SL, Owen EC, Tyack PL, Wells RS. 2005. Signature whistle use
Tyack P. 1986. Whistle repertoires of two bottlenosed dolphins, by temporarily restrained and free-swimming bottlenose dolphins,
Tursiops truncatus: mimicry of signature whistles?. Behav Ecol Tursiops truncatus. Anim Behav 69:1373–1386.
Zoo Biology
54
Chapter 2: Non-signature whistles are possible used
a
Institute of Neurosciences Paris Saclay, CNRS UMR 9197, University Paris Sud,
Orsay, France
b
Laboratoire d’Ethologie Expérimentale et Comparée E.A. 4443 (LEEC), Université
c
Boudewijn Seapark.
d
Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, CNRS, UMR 7190, Institut Jean Le
Rond d'Alembert
e
Parc Astérix, 60128 Plailly, France
Article in preparation
55
Synthesis Chapter 2
Context
Although signature whistles have been largely studied, not much is known about
non-signature whistles, which are the most frequently produced whistles by the
species in captivity.
Research questions
The behavioral context of emission of non-signature whistles has been poorly studied
in detail and often behavioral observations are done from surface taking into account
only general behavioral categories such as travel, rest, socializing and foraging.
The aim of this study was to determine if there is an association between the non-
Analysis
The study was conducted in a group of 8 bottlenose dolphins (T. truncatus) during six
recordings were made 15 minutes before and after 10 training sessions. The
behavioral analysis was made by focal follows on each individual based on six
behavioral categories. The acoustical analysis was made at the group level, and non-
Results
56
Multifactorial analysis showed that the occurrence of the six categories of whistles
were highly collinear. Non-signature whistle production was positively correlated with
the slow swimming alone behavior and negatively with the affiliative body contact.
Conclusion
Our results suggest that non-signature whistle production plays a role in the cohesion
of animals when they are in the same range of vision. This is the first analysis that
the species. However, in order to test our hypothesis, it will be necessary to localize
and identify the animal producing a whistle and the behavioral response of its
congeners.
57
ABSTRACT
Bottlenose dolphins are highly social cetaceans that strongly rely on acoustic
communication and signaling. The diversity of sounds emitted by the species has
been structurally classified in whistles, clicks and burst-pulsed sounds. Although click
sounds and individually-specific signature whistles have been largely studied, not
much is known about non-signature whistles, which are the most frequently produced
whistles by the species in captivity. Most studies that link behavior and whistle
production conduct aerial behavioral observations and link the production of whistles
to the general category of social interactions. The aim of this study was to determine
if there was a correlation between the non-signature whistle production and the
after 10 training sessions. For the behavioral analysis we made focal follows on each
individual based on six behavioral categories. For the acoustical analysis, made at
the group level, we visually classified the non-signature whistles recorded (N=661) in
that the occurrence of the six categories of whistles were highly collinear. Non-
signature whistle production was positively correlated with the slow swimming alone
behavior and negatively with the positive affiliative body contacts. Our results
suggest that non-signature whistle production plays a role in the cohesion of animals
when they are in the same range of vision. This is the first analysis that links the
species.
58
INTRODUCTION
Bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) are highly social cetacean that exhibit a
groups that often vary in composition according to age, sex, reproductive status and
activity (Connor et al., 2000; Mann et al., 2000; Gibson and Mann, 2008; Tsai and
Mann, 2013). In this highly mobile species, individuals of the same group can be
1998). Under these conditions, individual interactions based on the use of acoustics
signals seems to be the most effective strategy in order to assess their social and
The diversity of the sounds emitted by the species has been classified into three
structural categories and two functional classes. Structurally, the sounds emitted by
Janik, 2009), clicks or pulsed sounds (Au, 2012), and burst-pulsed sounds (Diaz-
Lopez and Bernal-Shirai, 2009). Functionally, sound emissions may be used for
Whistles are continuous narrow-band frequency modulated signals that range from
800 Hz to 28.5 kHz and have duration between 100ms and over 4s (Evans and
Prescott, 1962). This kind of sounds is associated with many social situations, with
some whistles being individually specific (Caldwell et al., 1990) and functioning to
maintain group cohesion (Janik and Slater, 1998). These whistles, also called
“signature whistles”, have been largely studied (Caldwell et al., 1990; Janik, 2000;
59
Janik and Sayigh, 2013; King et al., 2014). Their production rate varies with the
contexts: signature whistles can represent more than 90% of whistles produced by
temporally restrained dolphins (Caldwell and Caldwell, 1965; Sayigh et al., 1990),
between 30-70% of free ranging dolphins (Cook et al., 2004) and less than 1% in
dolphins under human care (Janik and Slater, 1998). Thus, most of the whistles
them based on their contour shape. Shape categories include upsweep, downsweep,
convex, concave and sinusoid (Bazua-Duran and Au, 2002; Hickey et al., 2009;
Lopez, 2011). The behavioral context of emission of non-signature whistles has been
poorly studied in detail and often behavioral observations are done from surface
taking into account only general behavioral categories such as travel, rest, socializing
sinusoid whistles, while upsweep whistles were associated with social behaviors
reinforcement training (Laule, 2003; Brando, 2010), and often several training or
feeding sessions are held per day. These trainings can be rewarding, as animals
voluntarily take part and must work in order to obtain rewards and develop cognitive
skills (Laule and Desmond, 1998). In the daily life of captive dolphins, training
sessions could represent outstanding events that involve feeding and interaction with
animals during their free time (Clegg et al., 2017). It has been observed that
60
anticipatory behaviors are higher before training sessions (Jensen et al., 2013; Clegg
et al., 2017), while synchronized swimming peaks shortly after training sessions
(Clegg et al., 2017). With respect to the non-signature whistle vocalization rate, it has
been found that upsweep whistles are mainly produced during feeding sessions,
while convex and sinusoid whistles are more frequent during the time before the
The aim of this study was to determine if there is an association between the non-
bottlenose dolphins under human care. For instance we assessed if specific social
METHODOLOGY
Study subjects
The study was conducted during six days in February 2017 at the Boudewijn
Seapark (Bruges, Belgium). The complex was inhabited by eight Atlantic bottlenose
dolphins (T. truncatus), six adult females aged 51, 41, 32, 19 and 14, one adult male
aged 12 years, and two calves, one male and one female that were born at the park
in the summer of 2015. Two of the adult females were born at the park, the adult
male was born at another facility and the three oldest females originated from wild.
All dolphins are subject to the same management schedule based on positive
reinforcement training methods. Every day dolphins take part in six to eight training
sessions approximately at the same time during which their trainers feed them after
they perform several exercises aimed to facilitate the husbandry and medical care
procedures and to prepare for presentations to the public. The adult animals know to
61
perform 100 to 130 behaviors upon trainers’ command plus the new behaviors they
are learning. Their sequence, their frequency and their duration change every day in
Before and after the training sessions the trainers mainly stayed in the office and
food preparation area and remained not visible or audible by the dolphins. At the
beginning of each training session the trainers went out of the food area at the same
time carrying fish buckets and place themselves at the edge of the pool. During
training sessions the animals are divided into subgroups that goes to different pools
subgroups are obtained by positive reinforcements and the animals remain within
Overall this facility consists of five connected pools: a main show pool, two holding
pens, a medical pool and quarantine pool (Figure 1). The quarantine pool and main
show pool are connected by a channel. The depth of the pools is at least 3m in the
shallowest areas and 5,6m at its deepest point in the main show pool. The total
can take place in all of the pools. During the recordings, the audio-video device was
placed in the main pool and the animals could move freely between the five pools.
The facility was closed to the public at the time of the recordings. Between the
recording sessions the animals were provided with enrichment items that were
Recording device
Langage) (Lopez-Marulanda et al., 2017). Video data were collected using one
62
GIROPTIC 360° camera consisting in three objectives that allowed a 360° range of
view. This camera was positioned under the waterproof housing of a digital recorder
ZOOM H6, plugged to four calibrated and automatically synchronized Aquarian H2a-
Two claps were made at the beginning of the recording session in order to manually
synchronize video with audio recordings during the a posteriori analysis with specific
video editing software (Final Cut Pro X 10.1.3 © Apple Inc.). A single video file was
created from the GIROPTIC camera and was associated with one of the four audio
tracks and its corresponding turning spectrogram (FFT size: 1024, overlap 50%,
Hanning window) provided by the free software Audacity 2.0.6 (GNU General Public
License). For this study the localization of dolphin producing a whistle was not
possible using our hydrophone array due to the reverberation on the walls of the
pool. We chose then to add only one track in the video as a reference.
Desensitization process
During four weeks prior to the recording sessions, animal caregivers were provided
with BaBeL to desensitize the animals to the presence of the device in the water. The
desensitization process carried out by the trainers consisted of 6 steps which were
gradually built up over the weeks prior to our recordings. The first step involved
placing the device on the side of the main pool, out of the water, but within sight of
the animals. For the second step, an animal caregiver took the device into the water,
standing on the underwater platform in the channel that connected the main show
pool to the quarantine pool, holding the device in the main show pool. During this
phase, the animals could see the device in the water, but were kept under control by
other trainers. Thirdly, the animals were allowed to swim freely for limited times in the
63
presence of the device which was held by a trainer as described in step 2. These
times were gradually prolonged and animals were rewarded when ignoring the
device. For the fourth step, the device was placed in the water without a trainer while
the animals were kept under control. In step five, the device was left in the water
while the animals swam freely with enrichment items to distract them from showing
interest in BaBeL. Finally, during the last phase, the device was frequently placed in
the water by the animal trainers at different times of the day, with or without the
Audio-video recordings
Recording sessions were carried out approximately 15min before and 15min after ten
training sessions that took place on six days. During the recordings, the device was
suspended from a buoy and kept in place at the side of the tank by two ropes and a
pole manipulated by one observer who remained at the edge of the pool avoiding
visual contact with the animals. The location of the device during the recordings is
64
Figure 1: Top view of the enclosure at the Boudewijn Seapark (Belgium). The
location of the BaBeL device during the recordings is marked by a red dot. Animals
have access to all the pools except the two holding pens.
Whistle categorization
Recorded whistles were analyzed by inspecting the spectrograms (FFT size: 1024,
overlap 50%, Hamming window) in Audacity 2.06 software (GNU General Public
License). A graph of the spectrogram of each whistle was registered giving special
attention to standardize the x- and y-axes of (1second long, with a frequency range
overlapping with other whistles were registered but not included in the categorization.
To categorize the whistles we first applied the SIGID method (Janik et al., 2013) to
identify signature whistles within our recordings based on two criteria: firstly,
signature whistles were whistles repeated at least four times in a recording session,
and secondly, at least on one occasion the whistles were produced in a sequence in
which 75% or more repetitions occur within 1–10 sec of one other. The whistles that
were not cataloged as signature whistles using this method were cataloged as non-
signature whistles. Non-signature whistles were visually categorized into one of six
fundamental shapes: upsweep, downsweep, flat, convex, concave and sinusoid (with
Behavioral analysis
For each video, a focal-animal sampling technique was conducted to note the
occurrence and duration of the most frequent social and anticipatory behaviors
65
displayed by the animals (Altmann, 1974; Mann, 1999). The behaviors were adapted
from a published repertoire built to analyze the effect of training sessions on the
behavior of dolphins under human care (Clegg et al., 2017). We took into account the
swim style (alone or synchronous), the different speeds of swim (slow: around 2m/s
or less, minimal tail beats; fast: more than 2m/s and stronger tail beats), play, positive
social body contact, agonistic, sexual and anticipatory behaviors (Table 1). For each
individual we calculated the total time spent within the range of view of the camera.
Table 1: Behavioral catalogue used for this study, based on an ethogram proposed
Behavior Description
Dolphin swims at more than one body length of any other
dolphin in the pool and shows no synchronous movements
Alone swimming
with its conspecifics. (slow: around 2m/s or less, minimal
tail beats; fast: more than 2m/s and stronger tail beats)
Dolphin swims in synchronous manner within one body
length of another dolphin, showing parallel movements and
Synchronize
body axes. Breathing can be separated maximum by 2 sec.
swimming
(slow: around 2m/s or less, minimal tail beats; fast: more
than 2m/s and stronger tail beats)
Positive social Dolphin touches or rubs another dolphin with its rostrum, its
contact pectoral fin or any other part of its body.
66
Dolphin directs its look out of the water towards the arrival
Anticipatory point of the trainers by a simple surface look, spy hopping,
jumping or body slapping close to the edge of the pool.
1
These behaviors did not occur during the observations of the present study.
No individual marking was used. Each dolphin could be recognized by the use of
discoloration, body size, body shape and notches on the dorsal fin and tail. Before
the data collection began, we verified that the observer (JLM) could identify dolphins
sessions after the training sessions. As some dolphins were not present during the
different sessions) was n = 75 prior to the training and n = 66 after the training.
Always at least six individuals were present in the main pool during the recordings; in
four sessions, two individuals were absent from the main pool. However, when
excluding these data and re-running all analysis the principally same results were
obtained. All statistical analyses were done with R, version 3.4.1. (R Core Team,
2017). Except for the principal component analysis PCA, we always used
permutation tests for the calculation of P-values. Permutation tests for linear models
are well adjusted for moderate sample sizes and do not require normal distribution of
model residuals (Good, 2005). However, we verified homogeneity of variances for all
67
We run a PCA (R package prcomp; Venables and Ripley, 2002), based on the
different non-signature whistles, which were found to be highly collinear (see Table
2). The resulting first axis was used as non-signature whistle score in further
Correlations at the group level, i.e. between the number of occurrences of the
(dependent variable) recorded before and after the training sessions (factor with 2
68
Table 2: Correlations between the different non-signature whistle types, calculated
by linear models. All models included the interaction with timing (factor with 2 level),
i.e. whether the whistles were recorded before or after the training sessions. These
interactions were never significant (all P > 0.10) and were removed from the models
before these were re-calculated. P values were calculated by 1000 Monte Carlo
whistles: F. All the analysis was made for 8 individuals, 10 sessions prior to the
Dependent Independe R2 P
variable nt variable
A B 0.616 < 0.001
A C 0.669 < 0.001
A D 0.579 < 0.001
A E 0.502 < 0.001
A F 0.568 0.001
B C 0.499 0.001
B D 0.243 0.020
B E 0.142 0.064
B F 0.184 0.032
C D 0.647 < 0.001
C E 0.222 0.024
C F 0.277 0.021
D E 0.321 0.008
D F 0.447 0.003
E F 0.863 < 0.001
69
Comparisons at the individual level, i.e. between the duration (% time) of the different
behaviors before and after the training session were tested by linear mixed-effects
models (LMM) based on restricted maximum likelihood estimates by using the lme
function of the R package nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2015). Furthermore, we used LMM to
(using the whistle score as obtained by PCA) and the individual-level % time the
individuals spent showing the different behaviors recorded. This analysis was done
separately during the period prior to and after the training sessions. Training-session
identity (thus pairing together observation sessions before and after a particular
training session) and individual dolphin identity (thus allowing for repeated
used as random factors (random intercepts) in all LMM. We used a nested random
effects structure, i.e. individual identity was nested within training-session identity. P-
values for LMM as well as for LM were calculated by Monte Carlo sampling with 1000
2016).
For all significant covariate effects of LMM and GLMM, we provide the slopes (β;
RESULTS
A total of 4hr 26 min (Table A, supplementary material) were recorded during the ten
training sessions (2hr 30 min before and 1 hr 56 min after) in which 776 whistles
were identified: 95 (12.24%) were classified as having too low signal-to-noise ratio to
70
be considered in this study, 9 (1.16%) were classified as overlapping whistles, 11
whistles types according to the SIGID method (Janik et al., 2013) and 661 (85.18%)
71
Associations between different whistles
The most frequent category of non-signature whistle recorded in our study was the
sinusoid whistle F with an occurrence of 1.12 min-1, followed by the upsweep whistle
A (0.52 min-1), the concave whistle E (0.31 min-1), the flat whistle C (0.19 min-1), the
convex whistle D (0.17 min-1), and the downsweep whistle B (0.15 min-1).
group level were statistically not independent, since there were various significant
and positive correlations between them (Table 2). The non-significant interactions
with the factor timing (factor with 2 levels; either before or after the training) indicate
that these significant correlations were not modulated by the timing of recording, i.e.
whether the whistles were recorded before or after the training sessions.
Due to this high level of collinearity between the different non-signature whistle types,
we decided to express the variation in whistle patterns by a single score (from here
analysis PCA. This first axis explained 75.1% of the variation of the data, and the
eigenvalue of this axis was 4.5. Further axes had eigenvalues of > 1 and thus were
not considered for further analyses. The loadings of all whistle types included in the
analysis were all positive (A: +0.450; B: +0.375; C: +0.409; D: +0.405; E: +0.394; F:
+0.412).
sessions
The whistle score, reflecting the totality of different non-signature whistles emitted by
the group, was significantly higher prior to the training sessions that after the session
72
(LM with 1000 permutations: P < 0.001; Fig. 3). That is, the dolphin group produced
Individual-level behaviors
A comparison of all observed individual-level behaviors revealed that only the % time
of positive social body contacts differed significantly during the observations prior to
and after the training sessions (LMM with 1000 permutations: P = 0.027). That is, the
dolphins showed significantly more positive social body contacts after than before the
training sessions.
There were some tendencies indicating that the % time the animals spent swimming
alone differed between the observations before and after the training session;
however, this difference was modulated by the speed of swimming (Fig. 2). Fast
whereas slow swimming tended to be more frequent after the training sessions (P =
0.055). There were no significant differences with respect to the % time the dolphins
showed synchronous swimming, either slow (P = 0.283) or fast (P = 0.544), and the
time they spent showing anticipatory behaviors (P = 0.663; see Fig. 3). There were
no significant effects of sex or of age class with respect to any of the behaviors
tested (all P > 0.10; see details on statistics in Table B of the supplementary
material).
73
Figure 2: Comparison between (a) the pattern of non-signature whistles at the group
level (whistle score obtained by PCA, see text) and (b-e) individual-based behaviors
not all individuals were observed in all sessions). Means with 95% confidence
intervals are given. Observation sessions (nbefore = 10; nafter = 9) were around 15 min.
Significant differences are indicated by asterisks (*** P < 0.001, * P < 0.050).
whistles (as assessed by a PCA-based whistle score) and certain of the different
individual-level behaviors recorded, but only during the observations prior to the
training sessions. The % time the animals spent swimming alone in a slow mode was
significantly increased when more non-signature whistles were emitted by the group,
as indicated by the significant and positive correlation between the % time spent
74
swimming alone and the whistle score (LMM with 1000 permutations: β = +0.387 ±
0.179 SE, P = 0.017; Fig. 3a). Furthermore, the % time the animals spent showing
positive social body contacts was significantly decreased when more non-signature
whistles were emitted by the group β = –0.286 ± 0.120 SE, P = 0.014; Fig. 3b).
level (whistle score obtained by PCA, see text) and the % time individuals (a) spent
swimming alone or (b) showed positive social contacts with conspecifics. Data from 6
Associations between the whistle score and any other behavior recorded prior to or
after the training sessions were not statistically significant (all P > 0.10; see details in
75
for the alone swim fast behavior between males and females (β = –0.842 ± 0.270 SE,
P = 0.003) and for the anticipatory behavior between young and older dolphins (β =
+1.093 ± 0.232 SE, P = 0.001). That is, the females showed significant more alone
swim fast behavior that males and the young showed significant more anticipatory
than after the training sessions. These whistles were mostly sinusoid whistles and
upsweep whistles; however there was a strong correlation between all the non-
We observed significantly more positive body contacts in the periods after the
training sessions. The fast swimming behavior was most frequent before the training
sessions and the slow swimming behavior was most frequent after the training
sessions.
With respect to the correlations between non-signature whistle emission and the
different behaviors, we found that during the time prior to the training sessions more
non-signature whistles were produced when animals were swimming slow and alone.
In contrast less non-signature whistles were emitted when animals showed positive
Finally, young dolphins showed more anticipatory behaviors than adults and females
DISCUSSION
Bottlenose dolphins produced more non-signature whistles during the time prior to
than after the training sessions. As traditionally whistle emission has been linked with
communication and social interactions (Herzing, 2000), we suggest that during the
76
time prior to training sessions the animals invest more time in actively communicating
and interacting socially than after the training sessions that could be devoted to rest.
This result contrast with what has been found in other facility, in which dolphins
produce more non-signature whistles in the period after than before the training
Gartside, 2010; Heiler et al., 2016), different personalities of the animals (Bigersson
et al., 2014; Highfill and Kuczaj, 2007), and differences in management between both
facilities. In fact, both facilities differ in the procedure during the training sessions. In
Boudewjin SeaPark, the animals are separated in different pools during each
training, while in Parc Asterix (France), the animals are never isolated (Lopez-
Marulanda et al., 2016). The isolation of animals might increase the production of
whistles (Esch et al., 2009) and subsequently modulate the production of whistles
SeaPark, most of the whistles are produced during the training sessions (Colpaert,
2017). It is possible that in this case, the period prior to training sessions elicited the
the isolation. We did not found differences in the anticipatory behavior before than
after the training sessions. This is probable due to the fact that the presence of the
observers during the recording sessions elicited surface looking behaviors in the
dolphins before and after the training sessions. These behaviors were classified as
directed to inspect the trainers arrival and the surface looking behavior directed to
observe the experimenters keeping the BaBeL device in the right position. Thus, we
77
Sinusoid and upsweep whistle types were the most frequent non-signature whistle
types produced during the recordings. These two kinds of whistles have been
reported by several authors as the most common produced by the animals, both in
captivity (Akiyama and Ohta, 2006) and in the wild (Hickey et al., 2009; Diaz Lopez,
2011). Our results confirm that these kinds of whistles play an important role in the
we also found that all the non-signature whistle types were highly correlated, which
means that the production of non-signature whistles elicit the emission of other non-
signature whistles.
With respect to the behaviors, we found that positive social body contacts were more
frequent after than before the training sessions. Positive social body contacts play a
role in restoring the friendly relationships and reduce conflicts between bottlenose
dolphins in captivity (Tamaki et al., 2006) and spotted dolphins (Stenella frontalis) in
the wild (Dudzinski, 1998). According to this, we could suggest that the time after the
training session plays a role in maintaining positive social relationships between the
individuals of this group of dolphins. Moreover, during our recordings these behaviors
were only observed between mothers and calves, an interaction that has been also
The fast swimming behaviors were also more frequent in the time prior to the training
sessions than after. This result matches with what has been described for bottlenose
contrast with what has been found for the behaviors of the animals with respect to
the training sessions in other facilities, in which the speed of swim did not differ
78
between the time prior and after the training sessions (Clegg et al., 2017). We
suggest that different groups of dolphins might differ in their expression of behaviors
Boudewijn Sea Park, the separation of animals during the training sessions may
cause the excitation of the animals which is reflected in the increase of fast
Our results show strong correlations between the non-signature whistle production
and some behaviors. During the time prior to training sessions, dolphins produced
significantly more non-signature whistles while swimming slowly alone. Also, they
produced less non-signature whistles during the positive social body contacts. Even if
signature whistles might play a role in the cohesion of the animals as when they
swim slow and alone they produce more non-signature whistles probably to search
for proximity or contact and when they are already in contact they do not need to
produce these vocalizations which reflects the decrease in their production. The fact
that the animals do not use instead signature whistles as cohesion calls (Janik and
Slater, 1998) under these circumstances, could be explained because the animals
are placed in the same pool and are in visual contact to each other, so they do not
need to transfer information about their identity to regroup. However, in order to test
our hypothesis it will be necessary to localize and identify the animal producing a
whistle and the behavioral response of its congeners. Moreover it will be necessary
human care and in the wild to determine the role of different non-signature whistles.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
79
Akiyama, J., & Ohta, M. (2007). Increased number of whistles of bottlenose dolphins,
49(3), 227-266.
Au, W. W. (2012). The sonar of dolphins. Springer Science & Business Media.
Bazúa-Durán, C., & Au, W. W. (2002). The whistles of Hawaiian spinner dolphins.
Caldwell, M. C., Caldwell, D. K., & Tyack, P. L. (1990). Review of the signature-
whistle hypothesis for the Atlantic bottlenose dolphin. The bottlenose dolphin,
199-234.
Clegg, I. L., Rödel, H. G., Cellier, M., Vink, D., Michaud, I., Mercera, B., ... & Delfour,
80
Colpaert, N. Emission Of Signature Whistles By 8 Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops
Connor, R. C., Wells, R. S., Mann, J. A. N. E. T., & Read, A. J. (2000). The
Connor, R. C., Mann, J., Tyack, P. L., & Whitehead, H. (1998). Social evolution in
Cook, M. L., Sayigh, L. S., Blum, J. E., & Wells, R. S. (2004). Signature-whistle
271(1543), 1043.
Esch HC, Sayigh LS, Blum JE, Wells RS. 2009b. Whistles as potential indicators of
Faraway, J. J. (2005). Linear models with R. Boca Raton, USA: Chapman & Hall.
https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=pgirmess.
Gibson, Q. A., & Mann, J. (2008). Early social development in wild bottlenose
81
Good, P. I. (2005). Permutation, parametric, and bootstrap tests of hypotheses. New
2663.
Heiler, J., Elwen, S. H., Kriesell, H. J., & Gridley, T. (2016). Changes in bottlenose
Herzing, D. L. (2000). Acoustics and social behavior of wild dolphins: implications for
New York.
Hickey, R., Berrow, S., & Goold, J. (2009, September). Towards a bottlenose dolphin
Environment: Proceedings of the Royal Irish Academy (pp. 89-94). Royal Irish
Academy.
Highfill LE, Kuczaj SA, II. 2007. Do bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) have
Janik, V. M., & Slater, P. J. (1998). Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose
dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls. Animal behaviour, 56(4), 829-
838.
82
Janik, V. M. (2000). Whistle matching in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops
479-489.
Janik, V. M., King, S. L., Sayigh, L. S., & Wells, R. S. (2013). Identifying signature
Jensen, A. L. M., Delfour, F., & Carter, T. (2013). Anticipatory behavior in captive
32(4), 436-444.
King, S. L., Harley, H. E., & Janik, V. M. (2014). The role of signature whistle
86.
strategy. In: Sheperdson DH, Mellen JD, Hutchins M. editors. Second nature:
83
López, B. D., & Shirai, J. A. B. (2009). Mediterranean common bottlenose dolphin’s
129-148.
Lopez Marulanda, J., Adam, O., & Delfour, F. (2016). Modulation of whistle
Lopez-Marulanda, J., Adam, O., Blanchard, T., Vallée, M., Cazau, D., & Delfour, F.
Mann, J. (1999). Behavioral sampling methods for cetaceans: a review and critique.
Mann, J. (Ed.). (2000). Cetacean societies: field studies of dolphins and whales.
Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D. & R Core Team (2015).nlme: Linear
http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme.
84
Sakai, M., Hishii, T., Takeda, S., & Kohshima, S. (2006). Flipper rubbing behaviors in
966-978.
Sayigh, L. S., Tyack, P. L., Wells, R. S., & Scott, M. D. (1990). Signature whistles of
Tamaki, N., Morisaka, T., & Taki, M. (2006). Does body contact contribute towards
73(2), 209-215.
Tsai, Y. J. J., & Mann, J. (2013). Dispersal, philopatry, and the role of fission‐fusion
Venables WN, Ripley BD. (2002). Modern Applied Statistics with S, 4th edition ed.
Berlin: Springer.
85
Supplementary material
Duration (hh:mm:ss)
1 00:19:04 0 00:19:04
86
Table B: Comparison of different individual-level behaviors before and after the
models. P-values were calculated by 1000 Monte Carlo permutations. The slope
parameters (based on scaled data) of the effects of all independent variables are
87
social body 0.168
contacts
Age class –0.043 ± 1 0.984
(juv) 0.244
88
Table C: Correlations between non -signature whistles recorded at the group level
time window (a) prior to and (b) after training sessions. The slope parameters (based
on scaled data) of the effects of all independent variables are provided. Significant
89
Sex (m) +0.923 ± 1 0.001
0.248
90
Age class +0.318 ± 1 0.275
(juv) 0.279
91
Chapter 3: First Results of an Underwater 360° HD
1
Institute of Neurosciences Paris Saclay, CNRS UMR 9197, University Paris Sud,
Orsay,
2
Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, CNRS UMR 7190, Institut Jean Le
3
Parc Astérix, Plailly, France
4
Association Abyss, La Réunion, France
5
ENSTA Bretagne Lab-STICC, CNRS UMR 6285, Université Européenne de
Bretagne, Brest,France
6
Laboratoire d’Ethologie Expérimentale et Comparée EA 4443 (LEEC), Université
92
Synthesis Chapter 3
Context
descriptions of both visual and acoustic signals (Thomas et al., 2002). However, the
Research questions
non-intrusive and compact enough to be deployed from a small boat was developed.
This underwater device includes five hydrophones and a 360° HD video recording
system with a limited blind spot that allows localization of sounds to free-swimming,
vocalizing dolphins coming from almost every direction. In this article, details about
this system’s design and the software developed to localize to sounds and to link
Analysis
Data from a population of bottlenose dolphins were col- lected during 14 boat
surveys along the northwest coast of Reunion Island (France) by following a strict
93
Results
A total of 21 min of audio-video were recorded when dolphins were present, and 42
click trains and 42 whistles were detected from these data. Dolphins identified as
vocalizers were also present for 17% (n = 7) of emitted click trains and 33% (n = 14)
of emitted whistles on the videos. When the observers stayed ahead and avoided the
direct path of groups of five to nine dolphins, only one animal emitted click trains
while swimming towards the observers or after turning its rostrum in the humans’
direction, and this dolphin was never the one leading the group.
Conclusion
The BaBeL system offers a method of data col- lection to conduct an etho-acoustical
individual dolphins and their underwater behaviors. The BaBel and the associated
software algorithms for data analysis represent an improved tool for ethologists to
record and collect data on all dolphins present in a 360° space via focal and group
follows.
94
Aquatic Mammals 2017, 43(2), 162-176, DOI 10.1578/AM.43.2.2017.162
emissions (Marten et al., 1996). Several dolphin dolphin. Four hydrophones are needed to local-
vocalizations are associated with behavioral con- ize a moving dolphin in 3D (Watkins & Schevill,
texts; for example, burst-pulsed sounds (squawks 1972; Wahlberg et al., 2001)
and whines) have been associated with agonistic Mobile arrays of two (Dudzinski et al., 1995),
behaviors (Herzing, 1996), and low-frequency four (Au & Herzing, 2003; Schotten et al., 2004),
bray calls are related to feeding (Janik, 2000; and 16 (Ball & Buck, 2005) hydrophones have
King & Janik, 2015). been used to study dolphin vocalizations and
Studies of dolphin social behavior and commu- their associated underwater behaviors, but they,
nication rely on simultaneous descriptions of both too, presented several disadvantages. Dudzinski
visual and acoustic signals (Thomas et al., 2002). et al.’s (1995) system did not allow localization in
However, the main obstacle associated with com- the vertical axis, and the systems with four hydro-
pleting these descriptions is the difficulty in iden- phones were used only for localization of click
tifying which dolphin in a group is the vocalizer. emitters (Au & Herzing, 2003; Schotten et al.,
This challenge is caused by two factors: (1) human 2004). The 16-hydrophone array had elements
hearing is not adapted to localize to sound sources separated by 3.2 cm (Ball & Buck, 2005) but did
underwater; and (2) dolphins do not show visible, not allow the confirmation of the emitter’s iden-
regular signs when emitting sounds, like opening tity if animals were located outside of the narrow
their mouths or displaying external clues (Janik, angle of the video camera.
2009). To overcome these obstacles, several meth- As part of this study, an audio-video system
odologies have been developed. Animals have been that was non-intrusive and compact enough to be
isolated (Caldwell et al., 1990; Sayigh et al., 1990) deployed from a small boat was developed. This
or tagged (Tyack, 1991; Nowacek et al., 1998); underwater device includes five hydrophones and
however, these approaches can be considered inva- a 360° HD video recording system with a lim-
sive and might lead to modification of the subjects’ ited blind spot that allows localization of sounds
behaviors and vocalizations. Emission of bubble to free-swimming, vocalizing dolphins coming
streams concurrent with vocalizations has been from almost every direction. In this article, details
used to identify a vocal animal because sometimes about this system’s design and the software devel-
dolphins emit bubbles while whistling (McCowan oped to localize to sounds and to link them to indi-
& Reiss, 1995; Herzing, 1996); however, whistles vidually identified dolphins are provided. Three
with bubble streams are not representative of the audio-video sequences of free-ranging bottlenose
entire whistle repertoire of bottlenose dolphins dolphins have been analyzed to illustrate the ben-
(Fripp, 2005, 2006). efits of this system in dolphin ethological and
As a non-intrusive method to identify the acoustical research.
vocalizing animal, different hydrophone arrays
have been designed. These arrays allow for pro- Methods
cessing of the differences in time of arrival of the
sound to each hydrophone to determine where Recording Device
the call originated. The position of the sound Simultaneous audio and video recordings were
source is linked to the video recordings to con- collected using a waterproof audio-video system
firm which animal is in the same position as the named BaBeL (BioAcoustique, Bien Être et
sound source, thereby identifying the vocalizer. Langage) (Figure 1). The acoustic set-up was
Fixed arrays using two (López-Rivas & Bazuá- comprised of five calibrated Aquarian H2a-XLR
Durán, 2010), three (Watkins & Schevill, 1974), hydrophones connected and synchronized to a
four (Brensing et al., 2001; Quick et al., 2008), ZOOM H6 digital audio recorder. Audio record-
and eight (Thomas et al., 2002) hydrophones have ings were made at a 96-kHz sampling frequency
been used to link audio recordings to behavioral and coded on 24 bits. The recorder was placed in
observations or video recordings; however, fixed a waterproof housing rated to 60 m depth. The
arrays are not well adapted to study highly mobile, architectural design of the hydrophone array was
free-ranging dolphins, and the video recordings a compromise between a large aperture between
were often obtained from a fixed point at the sur- hydrophones and maneuverability since the
face. The main problem with acquiring behavioral system needed to be deployed from small boats
information from the surface is that the docu- with limited space and to be controllable by one
mented behaviors could possibly represent only a observer when submerged. The synchronized
very small percentage of an animal’s behavioral hydrophones were positioned to obtain the time
activity (Janik, 2009). Moreover, an array with delay of arrival to provide the 3D estimations of
two hydrophones (Lopez-Rivas & Bazuá-Durán, dolphin positions.
2010) allows data to be obtained only on the angle The video portion of the BaBel system was
of arrival and not the real position of the emitting comprised of two Kodak SP360 video cameras
164 Lopez-Marulanda et al.
Figure 1. BaBeL (BioAcoustique, Bien Être et Langage) device: (a) Diagram and orientation of hydrophones (H); and
(b) picture of the device with 360º cameras unattached—five hydrophones installed on five deployable arms and plugged to
a ZOOM H6 (inside the adapted waterproof case).
both with a wide-angle field of view (214°); the method used the spatial distribution of hydro-
cameras were placed opposite of each other to the phones, the acoustic properties of the source
left and the right to allow for 360° field of vision (e.g., propagation speed and spherical propaga-
for the system. These cameras were positioned tion model), and the measure of the time differ-
below the waterproof housing of the acoustic ences of arrival (TDOA) of the acoustic wave
recorder (Figure 2). Video and audio files were from the source to the different hydrophones
stored for a posteriori analysis. (Alameda-Pineda & Horaud, 2014). The aim
was to estimate the differences in time of arrival
Custom-Made Program for Data Analysis of emitted sounds; the cross-correlation function
A geometrical localization method was used to method for whistle detection (Van Lancker, 2001)
estimate the position of an acoustic source. This and the threshold time energy for click detection
360° HD Audio-Video Device to Observe Dolphins 165
Figure 4. Simulated whistles with two signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs): (a) +20 dB and (b) -10 dB.
166 Lopez-Marulanda et al.
Figure 4. Screen display to track dolphins by videos and passive acoustics. On the bottom left, estimations of the angles from
the successive clicks (in blue) and the whistle (in red). On the right, the red cross points to the emitter dolphin.
Figure 5. Disposition of BaBeL device in the water; the whole system is controlled by Observer 1. The vision range of the
camera depends on water clarity. For the Reunion Island, it is ≈10 m. The real scales are not represented in the figure.
pool at 2.5 m from the edge. Percussive sounds Bottlenose Dolphin Data Collection
were generated by knocking together two steel Acoustic and video data were collected on free-
bars from nine different known places in the hori- ranging bottlenose dolphins along the northwest
zontal plane of the deviceʼs gravity center. Using coast off Reunion Island, a French territory in the
the position-pixel conversion, the position of each Mascarene Islands in the Southwest Indian Ocean.
percussive sound source in the video image was The species is observed in this location through-
estimated and compared to the location estimated out the year in groups of 10 to 100 individuals (48
by the custom program. individuals on average) and occurs in deeper water
360° HD Audio-Video Device to Observe Dolphins 167
Table 1. Behavioral catalog of the dolphins observed while swimming by observers and documented with the BaBeL device
Behaviors Code Definition
Pectoral rubbing PR The dolphin touches another dolphin.
Synchronized swimming SyS Dolphins swim in synchronous manner within one body length of another dolphin,
showing parallel movements and body axes.
Swim upside down SUD The dolphin swims with its belly turned up.
Swim upside down SUDU The dolphin swims with its belly turned up underneath a conspecific.
underneath
Side swimming SS The dolphin swims with its belly turned to the right or the left next to a conspecific.
Approach APP The dolphin approaches the observers by leaving the direction axis of its group.
Swim towards observers STO The dolphin swims towards the BaBeL device and the observers.
Turn rostrum TR The dolphin turns its rostrum in the direction of an observer.
Leave L The dolphin stops swimming towards the observers and starts to move away.
Whistle W The dolphin whistles.
Click train C The dolphin emits a click train.
was higher than 20 dB. If the dolphin vocalizes one bottlenose dolphin body length. For distances
further away, SNR decreased and could be nega- from the device, the error of the custom program
tive. We performed our approach for positive and was 1.1 to 3.9 m (Table 3).
negative SNR (Table 2). The time differences of
arrival (TDOAs) were still correctly estimated for Data Description
SNR larger than -10 dB, which is acceptable for During 14 boat surveys, dolphins were sighted
our study because underwater noise was low com- four times, allowing collection of 21.03 min of
pared to dolphin sounds. (SNR was always posi- 360° HD audio-video data with dolphins pres-
tive in our acoustic recordings.) ent. Recordings allowed the detection of 42 click
Results of the first test comparing estimations of trains and 42 whistles. The vocalizing dolphin
positions in video and audio show that differences was localized and visible on the video for seven
in estimation for azimuthal localizations are less click trains (17%). The vocalizer was not visible
than 12° except for in positions 3 and 4 (Table 3). for 25 click trains (59%); and for 10 click trains
For elevation localizations, the difference is less (24%), localization of the vocalizer was ambigu-
than 10°, except for in position 8. Positions 3, 4, ous. For whistles, localization analysis was not
and 8 can only be seen right on the edge of the possible for five whistles (12%) because of a low
image, making estimations more difficult due to SNR ratio. The vocalizing dolphin was visible on
image compression. Taking into account that the the video for 14 whistles (33%), the vocalizer was
maximal vision range of the BaBeL is estimated to not visible on the video for 18 whistles (43%), and
be 10 m depending on water clarity, a 10° differ- the localization of the vocalizer was ambiguous
ence in estimations from video and audio means for five whistles (12%).
that localization at 10 m from the BaBeL can have Three recording sessions (24 May at 0937
a maximum difference of 1.7 m from the posi- and 0949 h, and 27 May at 1316 h) were chosen
tion of the source in the video, which is less than during which it was possible to localize the
360° HD Audio-Video Device to Observe Dolphins 169
Table 3. Localization performance of our custom-made program using the sound produced by two bars of steel during tests
in a pool
Position estimated from
Position estimated from the videos the acoustic recordings Difference in estimation
Position Azimuth Elevation Distance Azimuth Elevation Distance Azimuth Elevation Distance
(°) (°) (m) (°) (°) (m) (°) (°) (m)
33.0 5.3 4.7 21.4 3.4 6.3 11.6 1.9 -1.6
1
2 5.7 1.2 4.5 6.2 -2.6 0.8 -0.5 3.8 3.7
Nationale Supérieure d’Ingenieurs du Mans, Le Mans, Gregg, J. D., Dudzinski, K. M., & Smith, H. V. (2007).
France. Do dolphins eavesdrop on the echolocation signals of
Brensing, K., Linke, K., & Todt, D. (2001). Sound source conspecifics? International Journal of Comparative
location by difference of phase, on a hydrophone array Psychology, 20(1), 65-88.
with small dimensions. The Journal of the Acoustical Herzing, D. L. (1996). Vocalizations and associated
Society of America, 109(1), 430-433. https://doi. underwater behavior of free-ranging Atlantic spotted
org/10.1121/1.1333421 dolphins, Stenella frontalis and bottlenose dolphins,
Caldwell, M. C., Caldwell, D. K., & Tyack, P. L. (1990). Tursiops truncatus. Aquatic Mammals, 22(2), 61-79.
Review of the signature whistle hypothesis for the Janik, V. M. (1999). Origins and implications of vocal learn-
Atlantic bottlenose dolphin. In S. Leatherwood & R. R. ing in bottlenose dolphins. In H. O. Box & K. R. Gibson
Reeves (Eds.), The bottlenose dolphin: Recent progress (Eds.), Mammalian social learning: Comparative and
in research (pp. 199-234). San Diego: Academic Press. ecological perspectives (pp. 308-326). Cambridge, UK:
Connor, R. C., Mann, J., & Watson-Capps, J. (2006). A Cambridge University Press.
sex-specific affiliative contact behavior in Indian Ocean Janik, V. M. (2000). Food-related bray calls in wild bottle-
bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops sp. Ethology, 112(7), 631- nose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Proceedings of the
638. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01203.x Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 267(1446), 923-
Connor, R. C., Mann, J., Tyack, P. L., & Whitehead, H. 927. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2000.1091
(1998). Social evolution in toothed whales. Trends in Janik, V. M. (2009). Acoustic communication in delphi-
Ecology and Evolution, 13(6), 228-232. https://doi. nids. In M. Naguib & V. M. Janik (Eds.), Advances in
org/10.1016/S0169-5347(98)01326-3 the study of behavior (1st ed., Vol. 40, pp. 123-157).
Connor, R. C., Wells, R. S., Mann, J., & Read, A. J. (2000). New York: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/
The bottlenose dolphin: Social relationships in a fission- S0065-3454(09)40004-4
fusion society. In J. Mann, R. C. Connor, P. L. Tyack, King, S. L., & Janik, V. M. (2015). Come dine with me:
& H. Whitehead (Eds.), Cetacean societies: Field Food-associated social signaling in wild bottlenose dol-
studies of dolphins and whales (pp. 91-126). Chicago: phins (Tursiops truncatus). Animal Cognition, 18(4),
University of Chicago Press. 969-974. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0851-7
Dulau-Drouot, V., Boucaud, V., & Rota, B. (2008). Cetacean López, B. D., & Bernal-Shirai, J. A. B. (2009).
diversity off La Réunion Island (France). Journal Mediterranean common bottlenose dolphin’s repertoire
of the Marine Biological Association of the United and communication use. In Dolphins: Anatomy, behav-
Kingdom, 88(6), 1263-1272. https://doi.org/10.1017/ ior, and threats (pp. 129-148). New York: Nova Science
S0025315408001069 Publishers, Inc.
Dudzinski, K. A., Clark, C. W., & Würsig, B. (1995). López-Rivas, R. M., & Bazúa-Durán, C. (2010). Who is
A mobile video/acoustic system for simultaneous whistling? Localizing and identifying phonating dol-
underwater recording of dolphin interactions. Aquatic phins in captivity. Applied Acoustics, 71(11), 1057-
Mammals, 21(3), 187-194. 1062. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apacoust.2010.05.006
Dudzinski, K. M., Gregg, J. D., Ribic, C. A., & Kuczaj II, Mann, J. (1999). Behavioral sampling meth-
S. A. (2009). A comparison of pectoral fin contact ods for cetaceans: A review and critique.
between two different wild dolphin populations. Marine Mammal Science, 15(1), 102-122.
Behavioural Processes, 80(2), 182-190. https://doi. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-7692.1999.tb00
org/10.1016/j.beproc.2008.11.011 784.x
Fellner, W., Bauer, G. B., Stamper, S. A., Losch, B. A., & Mann, J., Connor, R. C., Barre, L. M., & Heithaus, M. R.
Dahood, A. (2013). The development of synchronous (2000). Female reproductive success in bottlenose dol-
movement by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). phins (Tursiops sp.): Life history, habitat, provisioning,
Marine Mammal Science, 29(3), E203-E225. https://doi. and group-size effects. Behavioral Ecology, 11, 210-
org/10.1111/j.1748-7692.2012.00609.x 219. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/11.2.210
Fripp, D. (2005). Bubblestream whistles are not repre- Marten, K., Shariff, K., Psarakos, S., & White, D. J.
sentative of a bottlenose dolphin’s vocal repertoire. (1996). Ring bubbles of dolphins. Scientific American,
Marine Mammal Science, 21, 29-44. https://doi. 275(2), 82-87. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificameri-
org/10.1111/j.1748-7692.2005.tb01206.x can0896-82
Fripp, D. (2006). Bubblestream whistles are not represen- Martin, P., Bateson, P. P. G., & Bateson, P. (1993). Measuring
tative of bottlenose dolphin whistle repertoires reply behaviour: An introductory guide. Cambridge, UK:
to McCowan. Marine Mammal Science, 22, 496-501. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-7692.2006.00028.x cbo9781139168342
Gibson, Q. A., & Mann, J. (2008). The size, composition McCowan, B., & Reiss, D. (1995). Maternal aggressive con-
and function of wild bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops sp.) tact vocalizations in captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops
mother-calf groups in Shark Bay, Australia. Animal truncatus): Wide-band, low-frequency signals during
Behaviour, 76, 389-405. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbe- mother/aunt-infant interactions. Zoo Biology, 14(4), 293-
hav.2008.01.022 309. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.14301404022
172 Lopez-Marulanda et al.
Nowacek, D. P., Tyack, P. L., Wells, R. S., & Johnson, M. P. Acoustical Society of America, 109, 397-406. https://doi.
(1998). An onboard acoustic data logger to record bioso- org/10.1121/1.1329619
nar of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins. The Journal of Watkins, W. A., & Schevill, W. (1972). Sound source
the Acoustical Society of America, 103(5), 2908. https:// location with a three-dimensional hydrophone
doi.org/10.1121/1.422063 array. Deep-Sea Research, 19, 691-706. https://doi.
Pryor, K. W. (1990). Non-acoustic communication in small org/10.1016/0011-7471(72)90061-7
cetaceans: Glance, touch, position, gesture, and bubbles. Watkins, W. A., & Schevill, W. E. (1974). Listening to
In J. A. Thomas & R. A. Kastelein (Eds.), Sensory abili- Hawaiian spinner porpoises, Stenella cf. longirostris,
ties of cetaceans (pp. 537-544). New York: Springer. with a three-dimensional hydrophone array.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-0858-2_37 Journal of Mammalogy, 55(2), 319-328. https://doi.
Quick, N. J., Rendell, L. E., & Janik, V. M. (2008). A org/10.2307/1379001
mobile acoustic localization system for the study of free-
ranging dolphins during focal follows. Marine Mammal Appendix 1
Science, 24(4), 979-989. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-
7692.2008.00231.x Etho-Acoustical Description
Sakai, M., Hishii, T., Takeda, S., & Konshima, S. (2006). First Observation—In this sequence (34 s),
Flipper rubbing behaviors in wild bottlenose dolphins we analyzed the behaviors of nine adult dolphins
(Tursiops aduncus). Marine Mammal Science, 22(4), 966- (Ind1 to Ind9) and one calf (Ind10) (Figure 1).
978. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-7692.2006.00082.x The first dolphin (Ind1) approached and swam
Sakai, M., Morisaka, T., Kogi, K., Hishii, T., & Kohshima, towards the BaBeL for 15 s; and at 7 and 9 s,
S. (2010). Fine-scale analysis of synchronous breathing Ind1 turned its rostrum towards the BaBel and
in wild Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops adun- continued swimming towards the observers for
cus). Behavioural Processes, 83(1), 48-53. https://doi. 6 s more before leaving. Ind2 approached and
org/10.1016/j.beproc.2009.10.001 swam towards the device for 11 s before leav-
Sayigh, L., Tyack, P., Wells, R., & Scott, M. (1990). ing, emitting a click train at 4 s. Ind3 swam by
Signature whistles of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins the device. Ind4 swam upside down underneath
Tursiops truncatus: Stability and mother-offspring com- Ind5 for 15 s then moved to a synchronized swim-
parisons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 26(4), ming position above Ind 5. Ind5 swam synchro-
247-260. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00178318 nously for 14 s above Ind4 and then moved to a
Schotten, M., Au, W. W. L., Lammers, M. O., & Aubauer, side swimming posture for 1 s before returning to
R. (2004). Echolocation recordings and localization of synchronized swimming below Ind4. Ind6 swam
wild spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris) and pan- synchronously below Ind7 for 14 s; at 11 s, Ind6
tropical spotted dolphins (S. attenuata) using a four- turned its head to observers and continued swim-
hydrophone array. In J. A. Thomas, C. F. Moss, & M. ming synchronously below Ind7. At 19 s, Ind6
Vater (Eds.), Echolocation in bats and dolphins (pp. swam upside down underneath Ind7; at 20 s, Ind6
393-399). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. conducted pectoral fin-to-pectoral fin rubbing to
Thomas, R. E., Fristrup, K. M., & Tyack, P. L. (2002). Ind7 for 7 s. At 27 s, Ind6 continued swimming
Linking the sounds of dolphins to their locations and synchronously below Ind7, and Ind7 swam syn-
behavior using video and multichannel acoustic record- chronously with Ind6 and touched Ind6 with its
ings. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, pectoral fin on two occasions for 2 s at 14 s on the
112(4), 1692-1701. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1494805 body and then for 1 s at 26 s on the belly. At 27
Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. s, Ind6 and Ind7 stopped body contact and started
Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410-433. https://doi. synchronized swimming next to each other. Ind8
org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1963.tb01161.x swam by the observers. Finally, Ind9 and Ind10
Tsai, Y-J. J., & Mann, J. (2013). Dispersal, philopatry, and swam synchronously by the BaBel next to each
the role of fission-fusion dynamics in bottlenose dol- other.
phins. Marine Mammal Science, 29(2), 261-279. https:// Second Observation—In the second sequence
doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-7692.2011.00559.x (40 s), five adult dolphins passed in front of the
Tyack, P. L. (1991). Use of a telemetry device to iden- BaBeL system. The first and second dolphin (Ind1
tify which dolphin produces a sound. In K. Pryor & and Ind2) approached synchronously. Ind1 swam
K. S. Norris (Eds.), Dolphin societies (pp. 319-344). towards the observers for 4 s, pointed its rostrum
Berkeley: University of California Press. towards the recording system with no sound
Van Lancker, E. (2002). Acoustic goniometry: A spatio- being detected, and left. Ind2 also swam towards
temporal approach (Doctoral dissertation). Faculté the observers for 5 s, pointed its rostrum towards
Polytechnique de Mons. the observers with no sound being detected, and
Wahlberg, M., Mohl, B., & Madsen, P. (2001). Estimating left. Following behind the first two dolphins,
source position accuracy of a large aperture hydro- Ind3 whistled, swam towards the observers for 2
phone array for bioacoustics. The Journal of the s, and left. After leaving, Ind3 emitted three more
360° HD Audio-Video Device to Observe Dolphins 173
Figure 1. First observation: (a) Screenshot of the location of the clicking dolphin (Ind2) in 360° video and backup video with
the red cross pointing to the source of the sound emission; and (b) timelines for ten individuals.
whistles before disappearing out of the range of Third Observation—In this 22 s sequence, six
vision of the video system. Ind4 swam by the adult dolphins passed from left to right in front
BaBel, while Ind5 approached and swam towards of the BaBeL (Figure 8). Ind1 swam less than 50
the observers for 4 s, emitted a click train, and cm distance above Ind2. Ind2 swam upside down
continued swimming towards the observers for 5 s underneath Ind1 for 16 s; at 3 s, Ind2 turned its
before leaving (Figure 2). rostrum towards the observers and, at 8 s, emitted
174 Lopez-Marulanda et al.
Figure 2. Second observation: (a) Screenshot of the location of the whistling dolphin (Ind3) in 360° video and backup video
with the red cross pointing to the source of the sound emission; (b) screenshot of the location of the clicking dolphin (Ind5)
in 360° video and backup video; and (c) timelines for five individuals (Ind1 to Ind5).
360° HD Audio-Video Device to Observe Dolphins 175
a click train. At 11 s, Ind2 rubbed Ind1’s belly upside down underneath Ind4 for 4 s and then con-
with its pectoral fin for 8 s. At 19 s, Ind2 stopped tinued swimming synchronously with Ind4. Ind4
its contact with Ind1 and swam synchronously pectoral fin rubbed Ind5’s belly for 1 s at 3 s and
above it until the end of the sequence. then continued swimming synchronously with Ind5
Ind3 swam by the BaBel, and Ind4 synchro- and Ind6 (Figure 3).
nously swam next to Ind5 and Ind6. Ind5 swam
Figure 3. Third observation: (a) Screenshot of the location of the clicking dolphin (Ind3) in 360° video and backup video
with the red cross pointing to the source of the sound emission; and (b) timelines for six individuals.
176 Lopez-Marulanda et al.
Appendix 2
The five possible situations observers could encounter while operating the BaBeL.
Figure 1. Use of focal-animal sampling technique. Top left: No sound detected and dolphins out of the cameraʼs visual
range; Top middle: Sound detected but dolphins out of the cameraʼs visual range; Top right: No sound detected but dolphins
in the visual range of the camera; Bottom left: Dolphin in the visual range of the camera and sound detected, but the dolphin
vocalizing is not present in the video; and Bottom right: Dolphin in the visual range of the camera, with the sound detected
and the vocalizing dolphin present in the video.
Chapter 4: Description Of a Bottlenose Dolphin
Fabienne Delfour2,3
1
Institute of Neurosciences Paris Saclay, CNRS UMR 9197, University Paris Sud,
Orsay France
2
Laboratoire d’Ethologie Expérimentale et Comparée E.A. 4443 (LEEC), Université
3
Parc Astérix, 60128 Plailly, France
4
Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, CNRS, UMR 7190, Institut Jean Le
110
Synthesis Chapter 4
Context
Exploratory behavior includes all the actions that an animal performs to obtain
information about a new object, environment or individual through using its different
perception and through echolocation. They develop their ability to echolocate in the
first to three months of life. Individual of the species have shown to have a visual
Research questions
The birth of a dolphin offers a great opportunity to study how the exploratory
behaviour regarding an immerged object evolves in the calf. In this study, we focused
on the development of the exploratory behavior of a calf aged from 39 to 169 days,
Analysis
Simultaneous audio and video recordings were collected using a waterproof 360°
audio-video system named BaBeL that allows localization of the dolphin that is
trains were attached to an individual dolphin: 18 times to the calf, 11 times to its
mother and 17 times to another dolphin in the pool. The acoustic parameters of the
click trains were measured and compared between the calf and its mother. The
111
Results
The calf’s click train acoustic parameters did not differ significantly from its mother.
However, the calf showed an augmentation of click rate with age and a decrease in
ICI. During click train emission and when accompanied, the calf used mostly its right
eye. The two situations when the calf used its left eye coincided with the emission of
click trains while the calf was swimming alone. The accompanying dolphin was
Conclusion
At the age of 39 days, a bottlenose dolphins’ calf’s acoustic parameters regarding its
click trains did not differ from that of an adult. However, click rate was shown to
increase with age of the calf. When visually and acoustically exploring an immerged
non-alimentary object, the calf showed a right eye preference. This study used a new
methodology that allowed us to describe not only the acoustic parameters of the
subjects’ click trains but also the position of the calf during exploratory behavior with
112
ABSTRACT
Exploratory behaviour includes all the actions that an animal performs to obtain
information about a new object, environment or individual through using its different
object. The study was conducted between July 2015 and January 2016 at Parc
Asterix dolphinarium (Plailly, France). Simultaneous audio and video recordings were
collected using a waterproof 360° audio-video system named BaBeL that allows
times to the calf, 11 times to its mother and 17 times to another dolphin in the pool.
No significant differences were found when comparing the calf’s click rate; mean click
duration and mean interclick interval (ICI) with these parameters from its mother.
However, linear regression showed that calf’s click rate increased with age, mean ICI
decreased with age. In 11 situations the calf produced a click train while being
accompanied and in 7 situations the click train was emitted while it was swimming
alone. Visual lateralization analysis showed the calf’s preference for the use of its
right eye (binomial test, p = 0,007) while echolocating. The accompanying dolphin
was mostly placed at the right of the calf. This is the first study that simultaneously
describes the acoustic parameters and exploratory behaviour of a calf within its
social group.
ontogenesis
113
INTRODUCTION
and intrinsic exploration, named also “novelty seeking” (McReynolds 1962), “reactive
curiosity” (Penney and McCann 1964) or “stimulus seeking” (Hoyenga and Hoyenga,
stimulus itself (Berlyne 1960). Intrinsic exploration has been studied in a diverse
structure (Katzir 1982) and heritage (Dingemanse et al. 2002) modulate the
1967; Miller et al. 1986; Parker et al. 2007), and its development in captive species
has been shown to depend on multiple factors, including sex (Lynn and Brown 2009),
1967).
For marine mammals, exploratory behaviour has been little studied. Under human
harbour seals (Phoca vitulina concolor), gray seals (Halichoerus grypus) (Hunter et
al., 2002) and bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), with some inter-individual
Birgersson et al., 2014), the type of introduced objects (Delfour and Beyer 2012;
Delfour, Faulkner and Carter, in press), as well as the sex and/or age of the
114
individuals (Eskelinen et al. 2015). In wild Delphinids, this behaviour has been
two principal reasons. Firstly, these cetaceans are social mammals living in a fission-
fusion social structure (Connor 2000; Mann et al. 2000) that could influence the
explore their environment through visual perception (Pack and Herman, 1996) and
surrounding from the echoes they receive (Au, 1993). Bottlenose dolphins’ clicks are
(Richardson et al. 1995). They use clicks as a sensory tool to navigate or hunt for
prey (reviewed in Herzing and dos Santos 2004) and obtain information from their
own returning signals (Au, 1993) and by eavesdropping on the echoes produced by
another dolphin (Xitco and Roitblat 1996; Gregg et al., 2007). The ontogenesis of
develop their ability to echolocate in the first to three months of life (reviewed in
Harder et al., 2016). Before they are one month old, calves’ clicks are of shorter
duration (Reiss 1988) and lower frequency (Reiss 1988; Lindhard 1988) than adults.
Additionally, at 14 days old, calves’ sequences of clicks (click trains) have a shorter
inter click intervals (ICI) and shorter duration (Favaro et al. 2013) than adults.
mostly with one eye at a time, although they do use two eyes simultaneously when
their heads are out of water or when they swim upside down, in which case the
direction of view is naso-ventral (Dral. 1972). These mammals have been shown to
115
have visual lateralizations; the right eye is mostly used in daily activities (Yaman et
al. 2002) and performs better in spatial and visual discrimination tasks than the left
eye (Kilian et al. 2000; Yaman et al. 2002; Delfour and Marten 2006).
The birth of a dolphin offers a great opportunity to study how the exploratory
behaviour regarding an immerged object evolves in the calf. In this study, we focused
on the development of the exploratory behaviour of a calf aged from 39 to 169 days,
accompanying dolphin’s click trains parameters, and our aim was to describe how
with age. In order to analyse exploratory behaviour in a social context, we took into
account the presence, the position and the click train emissions of the dolphin
METHODS
The study was conducted between July 2015 and January 2016 at Parc Asterix
dolphinarium (Plailly, France) where nine Atlantic bottlenose dolphins (T. truncatus)
lived together in 3 inter-connected pools. In the group, there were four females aged
42, 34, 20 and 15 years old, and four males aged 33, 31, 4 and 3 years old. The 15
year old female gave birth to a female calf on July 3th, 2015.
Overall, this facility is composed of one outdoor and two indoor pools which are not
acoustically isolated. The outdoor pool has volume of 3,246m 3 and a depth that
varies from 2.5m at the shallowest point to 4.5m at its deepest. The indoor part of the
116
complex, divided into two sections, has a total volume of 550m3 and a depth of 2.5
m. The dolphins have free access between the pools at all times.
Every day the dolphins take part in at least five training sessions, starting
approximately at the same time each day, during which their trainers feed them after
they perform several exercises aiming to facilitate the husbandry and medical care
117
Recording Device
Simultaneous audio and video recordings were collected using a waterproof 360°
Marulanda et al. 2017). Video data were collected using two Kodak SP360 video
cameras (wide angle of 214°), one on each side of BaBel to allow a 360° view. These
cameras were positioned under the waterproof housing of a digital audio recorder
ZOOM H6, plugged to five calibrated and automatically synchronized Aquarian H2a-
XLR hydrophones. The synchronized hydrophones were positioned to obtain the time
recordings were conducted at a 96 kHz sampling frequency and coded on 24 bits. All
the details about the geometry of the hydrophone array are described in Lopez-
Two successive claps were made at the beginning of the recording session: one in
front of each camera, in order to manually synchronize videos with audio recordings
during the a posteriori analysis with specific video editing software (Final Cut Pro X
10.1.3 © Apple Inc.). A single video file was created from the two Kodak SP360 video
cameras in the same window, which was associated with one of the five audio tracks
and its corresponding turning spectrogram (FFT size: 1024, overlap 50%, Hanning
window) provided by the free software Audacity 2.0.6 (GNU General Public
Recording sessions
During the first days after the calf’s birth, special efforts were made to not disturb the
animals in order to preserve the mother and new born dolphin’s health and
relationship. Mother and calf were never isolated from their social group and could
118
freely move between the three pools. As a person was needed to operate BaBeL, we
chose an experienced trainer that was known by all the members of the group of
dolphins and we waited thirty-nine days after the calf’s birth to make recording
sessions of maximum 15min, which were scheduled every week, two times per day,
at 11:30am and 3:30pm after a training session. All the recording sessions were
made from the outdoor pool. From July 2016 to January 2016 we conducted a total of
The familiar trainer immerged himself/herself with the BaBeL device below the water
surface (≈ 1 m under the surface), and remained floating near the edge of the pool,
so the animals could choose the speed and distance to approach to the trainer.
Data analysis
The five audio tracks from each recording were used for the acoustical analysis,
localization method that estimates the position of the vocalising dolphin. This method
used the spatial distribution of hydrophones, the acoustic properties of the acoustic
source (propagation speed and spherical propagation model), and the evaluation of
the time differences of arrival (T.D.O.A.) of acoustic waves from the source to each
et al., 2017). The localized dolphin was identified using body size and colour and any
We selected sequences where the calf was present in the video, a click train was
emitted and our customized program localized the emitter of the vocalization. For
119
these sequences, we used using the pulse train analysis function of Avisoft-SASLab
Pro version 5.2.07 (Raymond Specht, Berlin, Germany) to measure click rate, mean
duration of click and mean ICI for comparative purposes with previous studies (Reiss,
1988; Lindhard, 1988; Favaro et al., 2013; Harder et al., 2016). A hysteresis of 10 dB
and a start/end threshold of -2 dB were the parameters used to analyze all the click
sequences. Linear regressions evaluated changes in the calf’s click train parameters
with age. Mann-Whitney tests were used to compare calf’s click train parameters with
its mother’s. When the producer of the click train was the calf, we registered its visual
laterality when approaching BaBeL and the spatial position of the dolphin
accompanying it when present, binomial test was used to test the significance of the
version 3.02 (R Core Team 2013). We considered that a dolphin was accompanying
the calf when it was positioned at 1m or less and its movements were in synchrony
with the calf with less than 2 sec intervals. When the click trains were produced by
another dolphin, we noted their temporal distribution with respect to the click trains
RESULTS
Localization process
During the study, 32 recording sessions were carried out for a total of 6 hours 55
minutes of audio-video recordings. Dolphins were present in the videos for 5 hours
and 7 minutes and the calf for 27 minutes and 20 seconds. During this time, the calf
swam by the BaBeL device while a click train was recorded 188 times. The
dolphin emitting the vocalization was out of the range of vision of BaBeL (in indoor
120
pools). In 37 situations (19.68%) the low signal to noise ratio or the overlapping
nature of the recorded click trains did not allow the localization of the vocalizing
dolphin. For 56 click trains (29.79%) the localization of the source pointed to the wall
the click train was linked to a dolphin present in the video (Figure 1): 18 times to the
calf (Table 1), 11 times to its mother (Table 1) and 17 times to another dolphin.
121
97.94 80.77 0.29±0.01 0.32±0.06 10.20±1.37 12.37±3.48
122
Figure 1: Number of click trains produced per individual, regarding the calf’s age (N
= 46)
No significant differences were found when comparing the calf’s click rate (Mann-
U=142.5, P=0.053) and mean ICI (Mann-Whitney U-test, U=133.5, P=0.126) with its
mother.
Linear regressions showed that calf’s click rates increased with age (R2=0.79,
P=0.003) (Figure 2a), mean ICI decreased with age (R2=0.72, P=0.007) and mean
123
Figure 2: Linear regressions of acoustic parameters of calf’s click trains: a) Click rate
Analyzing other dolphins’ click train productions, we found that in five situations the
calf emitted click trains after its mother (between 7 and 168 seconds after mother’s
click train emission, mean= 63, 8 sec, s = 72.64 sec) while they were swimming
together. In another situation, the calf emitted a click train shortly after the youngest
male dolphin (+24sec). Those events were spread out during the entire experiment
Considering the calf’s click trains, they occurred 11 times (61%) when another
dolphin accompanied the calf. The dolphin accompanying was its mother on seven
occasions, and another dolphin on four occasions (one time its grandmother, one
time the 4-year-old male and two times the youngest male). At 145 days old, we
recorded seven occasions (39%) in which the calf emitted a click train when she was
swimming by BaBeL alone. At 166 days old, we recorded her first tactile exploration
124
Calf’s visual lateralization and mother’s spatial position in relation to the calf
When analyzing the visual lateralization of the calf while swimming by BaBeL, only 15
click train emissions out of 18 were taken into account, since in three click train
productions the calf remained static facing the BaBeL device head-on, without clearly
choosing a side for its visual exploration. During 13 click trains out of 15 the calf
swam by with its right eye towards BaBeL (binomial test, p = 0,007) (Figure 3). It was
positioned along another dolphin’s flank for all of the 13 right approaches. The 2 left
click train emissions occurred when the 145 days old calf swam by alone.
In six situations (40% N= 18), the calf swam in an upside-down position while
emitting a click train with its lower jaw directed towards the device. This happened
once at 39 days old, once at 46 days old, and four times at 145 days old when it was
Regarding the spatial position of the accompanying dolphin, during the calf’s click
trains emissions, the accompanying dolphin was at the right of the calf 7 times versus
125
Figure 3: Calf’s behavioral laterality while swimming by BaBel (N = 15) *: (P = 0.007,
binomial test) and accompanying dolphin’s spatial position with respect to the calf (N
To summarize, the calf’s click train acoustic parameters did not differ significantly
from its mother. However, the calf showed an augmentation of click rate with age and
a decrease in ICI. During click train emission, the calf used mostly its right eye. The
two situations when the calf used its left eye coincided with the emission of click
trains while the calf was swimming alone. The accompanying dolphin was mostly
DISCUSSION
difficulties. First, following and recording the vocalizations of a dolphin from early age
in the wild is almost impossible and thus is only feasible with studies in captivity,
where the birth of a calf occurs on average every 28 months per female (Cornell et al.
126
1987). As a consequence, to our knowledge there are only six studies focused on the
(Carder and Ridgway 1983; Reiss 1988; Linhard 1988; Manoukian et al. 2002;
Favaro et al. 2013; Harder et al. 2016). Second, all the studies focused on the
emitting a click train and thus used several indicators: the production of bublestreams
(Reiss 1988; Favaro et al. 2013), the intensity of the signal and the position of the
calves with respect to the hydrophone (Lindhard 1988), the presence of head
scanning behaviours at the same time as click recordings (Favaro et al. 2013), and
the distraction of mothers in activities with trainers and the proximity, orientation and
relative position of calves (Harder et al. 2016). All these indicators are subject to
uncertainty and allow accurate analysis of click trains only during specific behavioural
circumstances (ex: mothers distracted by trainers (Harder et al. 2016) or with their
blowhole outside the water (Favaro et al. 2013)). The use of BaBeL system and a
varied circumstances, with the presence of other dolphins around the calf and
regardless of its relative position. For the first time, the development of a calf’s
exploratory behaviour as well as the acoustic parameters of its concurrent click trains
were analysed.
Nevertheless, our methodology presents several limitations. First, the wall of the pool
caused reverberations, making our custom-made program identify the wall as the
the vocalizing dolphin is only possible if the animal is in the range of vision of the
video camera (Lopez Marulanda et al. 2017). If dolphins were vocalizing from the
inside pools, they were no longer visible to allow the identification of the emitter.
127
Third, because of the dolphins’ interest in the device, sometimes our recordings
making impossible to determine the initial and end time of click trains, and thus, to
compare the click train duration as it was done in previous studies (Favaro et al.
Our results showed that the calf presented an increase in click rate and a decrease in
ICI with age. The minimum value for the click rate was found at 46 days old
(57.45Hz) and the maximum value at 166 days old (195 Hz). This contrast with a
previous study (Harder et al. 2016) that analysed click train production in six calves
during their first six months of life and found that mean click rate increased during the
first month, decreased during the second month and remained constant between the
third and the sixth month. With respect to the ICIs, the values for the six calves
remained consistent with a mean value of 25.32ms (SD=10.35) (Harder et al. 2016).
In our study, the ICI values from the calf’s click trains varied from a mean value of
17.39ms (SD=2.74) at 46 days old to a mean value of 5.12ms (SD=1.15) at 166 days
old. The calf in our study showed higher click rates and lower ICIs than the six calves
analysed by Harder et al. (2016). These differences might be explained by the nature
of the object explored. Contrary to Harder et al. (2016) where the calves were in
presence of a simple object (a single hydrophone), in our study, the calf was exposed
to a complex object consisting in five arms with attached hydrophones and two video
cameras, that was handled by a familiar person in water. The physical parameters of
the object offered the possibility to perceive various different densities and shapes.
Moreover, the presence of a trainer in the water could have generated an increased
interest by the calf. It might have increased its click production per second for two
128
curiosity (Berline et al. 1965; Studnitz et al. 2007) and/or due to the presence of a
human in water, which can modify the behavioural response of animals, (Brensing et
The lack of difference between click rate, mean duration of click and mean ICI of the
calf and its mother supports the finding that infant and adult pulses are
old, it is possible that the calf could already produce click trains similar to those of an
adult, at least regarding the measured acoustic parameters in this study. This does
not exclude the possibility that other acoustical parameters of the calf’s click train not
Our results showed the calf’s had a right eye preference while echolocating.
Bottlenose dolphins demonstrate this eye preference (Yaman et al. 2002) associated
with a better performance in discrimination tasks (von Fersen. 2000; Kilian et al.
2000; Yaman et al. 2002; Delfour and Marten 2005). This right orientation has been
found also with free ranging Atlantic Spotted Dolphins (Stenella frontalis) when
exposed for the first time to a mirror that swimmers hold (Delfour and Herzing 2013).
impossible to link vision and echolocation. It is possible that our results were
impacted by the dolphins’ swimming patterns. A previous study showed that dolphins
in captivity tend to swim counter-clockwise (Sobel et al. 1994). In our study, 188
times the calf swam by the device and we found that for 150 (79.78%) “swim by”
situations, the calf swam counter-clockwise and for 38 (20.22%) situations it swam
129
Perceptual laterality is influenced by the emotional value (Rogers et al. 1994;
Quaranta et al. 2007) and the novelty of an object (Cantalupo et al., 1995; Basile at
al., 2009). Bottlenose dolphins preferentially use their left eye to explore familiar
objects and their right to explore unfamiliar objects (Blois-Heulin et al. 2012). We
suggest that BaBeL was probably perceived as an unfamiliar object until 145 days
old when BaBeL was immersed for the 15th time, and when for the first time the calf
explored the device alone and used its left eye twice.
We found that the calf was positioned mostly at the left side of the accompanying
dolphin while echolocating. This contrast with what has been found in free ranging
(Karenina et al. 2010; Hill et al. 2017) and captive belugas (Delphinapterus leucas)
(Hill et al. 2017) and in free ranging killer whales (Orcinus orca), where calves mostly
swim at the right side of their mothers (Karenina et al. 2013). It might be possible that
these species differ in their lateralization tendencies. However, it is also possible that
the mother placed herself between her calf and the device. As we only analyzed the
lateralization of the species: for this, more observations about the lateralization of
calves with respect to the accompanying dolphin are needed in wild and captivity.
regarding its click trains did not differ from that of an adult. However, click rate was
shown to increase with age of the calf. When visually and acoustically exploring an
immerged non-alimentary object, the calf showed a right eye preference. This study
used a new methodology that allowed us to describe not only the acoustic
parameters of the subjects’ click trains but also the position of the calf during
exploratory behavior with respect to the object explored and to its conspecifics.
Further studies using the same technology and methodology would reveal unknown
130
aspects of the dolphins’ perception of their world and allow scientists to build new
paradigms.
BIBLIOGRAPHIC REFERENCES
Tursiops truncatus, arising from interaction with people. J Vet Med Sci
69:165–170
Bioacoustics 8:137–162
Berlyne DE, Borsa DM, Craw MA, Gelman RS, Mandell EE (1965) Effects of stimulus
Sarrebruck
131
Blanchard T (2015) Innovation audio‐visuel pour le suivi des Grands Dauphins
Neuropsychologia, 33:1637-1646
Delfour F, Faulkner C, Carter T (in press). Object manipulation and play behaviour in
Dingemanse NJ, Both C, Drent PJ, Van Oers K, Van Noordwijk AJ (2002).
Dudzinski K.M., Hill H.M. (2018) Cetacean Communication. In: Vonk J., Shackelford
132
Eskelinen HC, Winship KA, & Borger-Turner JL (2015) Sex, age, and individual
Harder JH, Hill HM, Dudzinski KM, Sanabria KT, Guarino S, Kuczaj II, Stan A (2016)
Hill HM, Guarino S, Calvillo A, Gonzalez III A, Zuniga K, Bellows C, Polasek L, Sims
Hunter SA, Bay MS, Martin ML, Hatfield JS (2002) Behavioral effects of
e13787. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0013787
spatial relationships between wild mother and infant orcas, Orcinus orca.
116:211-215
133
Keller H, Schneider K, Henderson B (eds) (2012) Curiosity and exploration. Springer
Kuczaj SA, Makecha R, Trone M, Paulos RD, Ramos JA (2006) Role of peers in
cultural innovation and cultural transmission: Evidence from the play of dolphin
bredanensis) off the coast of Utila, Honduras. Journal of the Marine Biological
81:231-263
Lambert, O., Bianucci, G., Urbina, M., & Geisler, J. H. (2017). A new inioid (Cetacea,
Odontoceti, Delphinida) from the Miocene of Peru and the origin of modern
179(4), 919-946.
Lynn DA, Brown GR (2009) The ontogeny of exploratory behavior in male and female
520
(eds) Animal sonar: processes and performance. Plenum Press, New York,
134
Lopez-Marulanda J, Adam O, Delfour F (in press). First results of an underwater
Miller LC, Bard KA, Juno CJ, Nadler RD (1986) Behavioral responsiveness of young
47:128-142
Parker KJ, Rainwater KL, Buckmaster CL, Schatzberg AF, Lindley SE, Lyons DM
(2007) Early life stress and novelty seeking behavior in adolescent monkeys.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 32:785-792
121-127
Richardson WJ, Green CR, Malme CI, Thomson DH (1995) Marine mammals and
135
Schwab C, Huber L (2006) Obey or not obey? Dogs (Canis familiaris) behave
Studnitz M, Jensen MB, Pedersen LJ (2007) Why do pigs root and in what will they
research, 107:177-181
136
Chapter 5: Bottlenose Dolphins Under Human Care
1
Institute of Neurosciences Paris Saclay, CNRS UMR 9197, University Paris Sud,
Orsay France
2
Sorbonne Universités, UPMC Univ Paris 06, CNRS, UMR 7190, Institut Jean Le
Rond d'Alembert,
3
Laboratoire d’Ethologie Expérimentale et Comparée E.A. 4443 (LEEC), University
4
Parc Astérix, 60128 Plailly, France
5
Boudewijn Seapark. Dolphinarium.
6
Centre d’Ecologie Fonctionnelle et Evolutive, CNRS UMR 5175, University Paul
Valery, Montpellier
Article in preparation
137
Synthesis Chapter 5
Context
Synchronous behaviors occur when two or more animals perform the same behavior
at the same time and they have been described for free ranging dolphins of several
1999), and as these marine mammals live in habitats of limited visibility (Connor et
Research questions
synchronous behavior. The first aim of this study was to experimentally determine
whether dolphins use acoustic cues when performing a known simultaneous exercise
following a gestural command from their caregivers. The second aim, if they do use
Analysis
and 1 male). Random trials of a target exercise (jump) were carried out with the
animals alone or by pairs. The acoustical parameters of their vocalizations during the
jumps were compared when they were performed individually or collectively. The
BaBeL system was used to localize and identify the dolphin producing vocalization.
138
Results
100% of times when paired. Whether they jumped alone or in pairs, they produced
click trains before and after 92% of jumps. During the jumps performed in pairs these
click trains were emitted by only one individual 98% of times. The acoustic
in 19.8% of cases. Our study also showed that in all but one successful localizations,
the click trains were produced by the same individual. Noteworthy, this individual
Conclusion
In conclusion, this paper provides the first evidence that dolphins use acoustic cues,
eavesdropping the echoes produced by one individual that leads the navigation.
139
ABSTRACT
Synchronous behaviors occur when two or more animals perform the same behavior
at the same time and they have been described for free ranging dolphins of several
camera allowing to localize precisely the sound source and thus the identity of the
spontaneously synchronize their jumps 100% of times when paired. Whether they
jumped alone or in pairs, they produced click trains before and after 92% of jumps.
During the jumps performed in pairs these click trains were emitted by only one
individual 98% of times. The acoustic localization processing allowed the successful
identification of the vocalizing dolphin in 19.8% of cases. Our study also showed that
in all but one successful localizations, the click trains were produced by the same
individual. Noteworthy, this individual appeared to be the oldest female of the group.
This paper provides the first evidence that dolphins could use acoustic cues, and
eavesdropping the echoes produced by one individual that leads the navigation.
140
INTRODUCTION
Synchronous behaviors occur when two or more animals perform the same behavior
at the same time (Connor et al., 2006) and have been described for several animal
species in different modalities (eg. visual, acoustic) (reviewed in Herzing, 2015). The
degree of synchronization can vary from time intervals of less than one second to
several minutes (Sakai et al., 2010). For example, visual synchrony occurs between
fireflies (Pteroptyx spp.) that synchronize their bioluminescent flashing during the
night (Buck, 1988) and between male fiddler crabs (Uca annulipes) that wave their
synchrony has been described for example in the vocalizations of male long-tailed
manakins (Chiroxiphia linearis) (Trainer and McDonald, 1993) and in male frogs
predators or to cope with novel objects and situations (Norris and Schilt, 1988; Pryor
attention (Sebanz et al., 2006) and foraging success (Tremblay and Cherel, 1999).
In odontocetes, the term “synchrony” has been used in two different ways: first, to
describe group members that perform nonrandom grouping behaviors, swimming and
breathing in synchrony (Hastie et al., 2003; Fellner et al., 2013); and second,
behaviors that are performed ‘simultaneously’ or ‘in unison’ (Mann and Smuts, 1999;
Connor et al., 2006). Visual simultaneous behavior has been described in several
movements as a defensive response while being herded in tuna nets (Pryor and
141
Kang-Shallenberger, 1991). Synchronous behavior has been reported in Atlantic
larger size opponent (Cusick and Herzing, 2014). Male Indian Ocean bottlenose
behavior during social behavior with female consorts (Connor et al., 2006; Sakai et
al., 2010), and during herding behavior of females (Connor et al, 1992; Connor and
Smolker, 1996).
Vocal synchrony has been also described in these animals. Spinner dolphins
range (Janik et al., 2011). Finally, simultaneous vocal and visual signals have been
Sounds emitted by dolphins are classified into three structural categories and two
functional classes. Structurally, sounds productions are thus categorized in: whistles
or tonal sounds (reviewed in Janik, 2009), clicks or pulsed sounds (Au et al., 1974),
emissions may be used for echolocation, which could be defined as the acoustic
emissions may also play a role in communication and social interactions (reviewed in
Herzing, 2000).
142
When two or more dolphins engage in a synchronous behavior, information might
flow between them that can involve a communication process (Johnson, 2015).
Dolphins’ communication relies mainly on the acoustic channel (Tyack, 1999), and as
these marine mammals live in habitats of limited visibility (Connor et al., 1998), we
behaviors may involve acoustic cues. The use of acoustic cues to perform
simultaneous movements are difficult to highlight in free ranging dolphins for several
performing them cannot be controlled by the experimenter; second, the low visibility
underwater in most of the habitats of this species (Würsig and Pearson, 2015) do not
allow the clear determination of the degree of synchronization, neither the localization
this mechanism because the synchronous behaviour can be requested to the target
animals and replicated several times. The clarity of water allows a direct observation
of the behavioural sequence and allows the identification of the individual emitting a
vocalization by the use of a hydrophone array. Dolphins under human care are often
to synchronize their actions and if they use acoustic cues to coordinate their
simultaneous behaviors. The first aim of this study was then to experimentally
second aim, if they do use acoustic cues, was to identify the emitters.
143
METHODS
The coordination experiment was conducted in February and March 2017 at the
Boudewijn Seapark (Bruges, Belgium). Three individuals were selected for the
experiment: two adult females named Puck and Linda aged respectively 51 and 41
years, and one adult male named Kite aged 12 years. The females originated from
wild and the male was born in another facility. This choice was based on two criteria:
first, the three animals were trained to perform the same exercise individually and
collectively and second, two animals were known to work together very well (Puck
and Linda) and two animals were known to work together with difficulty (Puck and
Overall this facility consists of five connected pools not acoustically isolated: a main
show pool, two holding pens, a medical pool and a quarantine pool. The depth of the
pools is at least 3 m in the shallowest areas and 5.6 m at its deepest point in the
main show pool. The training sessions with caregivers take place in all pools. During
the experiment, the target animals were placed in the main pool with the recording
device. The other animals were placed in the two holding pens and a trainer was
responsible for maintaining their head out of the water to avoid the propagation of
their potential acoustic emissions through the pools. This procedure prevented from
from outside the experimental pool could produce false alarms during acoustic
processing.
144
Recording device
Simultaneous audio and video recordings were collected using a waterproof 360°
Marulanda et al. 2017). Underwater video data were collected using GIROPTIC 360 °
video camera with 3 objectives covering each one 120° and allowing a 360° view of
the main pool. This 360° camera was positioned under the waterproof housing of a
digital audio recorder ZOOM H6, connected to four calibrated and automatically
each other. This allowed us to determine the time differences of arrival (TDOA) of the
sound to each hydrophone and thus to estimate the 3D position of the dolphin
frequency and coded on 24 bits. Details about the function of this hydrophone array
3+ to record a video back up of the experience from the surface. Videos and audio
recording were synchronized. A single video file was created from the video cameras
and was associated with one of the four audio tracks and its corresponding turning
spectrogram (FFT size: 1024, overlap 50%, Hanning window) provided by the free
Habituation process
BaBeL device in the water. The habituation procedure involved 6 steps which were
gradually built up over the four weeks prior to the recording session. The first step
consisted in positioning the device on the side of the main pool, out of the water, but
145
within sight of the animals. In the second step, an animal caregiver held the device
on the underwater platform in the channel that connected the main show pool to the
quarantine pool. Thus, during this phase, the animals could see the device into the
water, but were kept under control by other trainers. Thirdly, the animals were
allowed to swim freely for a limited time in the presence of the device which was held
animals were rewarded to ignore the device. In the fourth step, the device was placed
alone in the water while the animals were kept under control. In the step five, the
device was left in the water while the animals swam freely with enrichment items to
distract them from showing interest in BaBeL. Finally, during the last phase, the
device was randomly placed in the water, with or without the presence of enrichment
items.
Coordination experiment
Before the beginning of each training session, the audio-video recording device was
placed in the main pool suspended from a buoy and kept in place at the side of the
tank by two ropes and a pole manipulated by one observer who remained at the
edge of the pool. We carried out 30 training sessions (max. two per day) in which
animals were asked to perform “back jump” exercise, which consisted in jumping with
the dorsal part of the animal pointing towards the water surface. The dolphins were
trained to perform the back-jump many times until the trainers blew their whistle to
indicate that the task was well performed and they can come back to get a reward
(fish). For the experiment, the trainers decided, according to the motivation of the
animal, to let it jump between one to five times before blowing the whistle.
146
During each training session the trainers asked the animals to perform the “back
jump” five non-consecutive times, other exercises were inserted in between in order
to maintain the dolphins’ motivation. The trials were randomly distributed in order to
get at the end of the experiment 30 trials for each dolphin individually performing the
back jump, 30 trials for Kite and Puck together, and 30 trials for Linda and Puck
together. For the trials collectively performed, two trainers were placed one at each
side of the pool. Each dolphin was placed facing one trainer, in a way that they could
not see the trainer’s gestural command given to the other dolphin. Once in this
position, the trainers simultaneously produced the command to perform the back
jump.
Behavioral analysis
We analysed the videos taken from BaBeL and the backup video to determine if
performed by two animals with a time difference of less than 0.5 seconds.
Acoustical analysis
No whistles or burst-pulsed sounds were emitted by the animals during the exercises.
Accordingly, all the acoustical analysis was based on their click trains production.
Click trains produced by the animals were analyzed using the pulse train analysis
measure click rate for each trial. Click trains produced after the last jump of each trial
were not taken into account for the statistical analysis because animals do not need
to synchronize after the last jump of each trial to go back to the trainer and get the
147
reward. A visual inspection of the click trains allowed us to determine whether the
click train was produced by one animal (regular click train, with inter-click interval
(ICI) increasing, decreasing or constant) or more than one animal (irregular click train
Localization processing
For all the trials made by pairs a localization processing of the click trains was
program was created in MATLAB®, Version 2013a (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA) to
analyze the data obtained with BaBeL (Blanchard, 2015). This program used a
specifically this method relies on the spatial distribution of the hydrophones and the
measure of TDOA of the acoustic wave from its source to the different hydrophones
to calculate the sound source position. The localization is then displayed in the 360°
Statistical analysis
Comparisons between the 3 individuals click rates were performed using a Kruskall-
Wallis test and post hoc comparisons with Mann-Whitney tests. To compare if the
click rates differed between the exercises performed individually vs. by pairs we used
a Wilcoxon signed rank test for each individual. All the statistical tests were
148
RESULTS
Synchronous behavior
Videos analyses showed that 100% of the “back jumps” performed by pairs were
synchronous. The inspection of the videos with the synchronized spectrogram also
indicated that dolphins produced a click train before and after 92% of the jumps
A total of 331 click trains were extracted and analysed from the recordings (Table 1).
In some situations, the click trains were not detected before or after a jump or its
signal to noise ratio was too low to allow a pulse rate analysis. The table 1
summarizes the number of trials, jumps and analysed click trains for each individual
or combination or individuals.
Table 1: Number of trials, jumps, detected click trains and analysed click trains for
each individual
Detected
149
Click rates between the individuals differed significantly (Kruskall-Wallis test:
χ=24.16, df=2, P<0.0001). Post hoc comparisons showed that there were no
significant difference between the click rate of Kite and Linda (Mann-Whitney test,
W= 2961.5, P=0.1398 with Bonferroni correction). However click rates of Puck were
with Bonferroni correction) and Linda (Mann-Whitney test, W= 3191, P=0.002 with
Bonferroni correction).
When comparing the click rate values for each individual alone and by pairs, we
found no significant differences in the click rate for Kite (Wilcoxon signed Rank Test:
W=2952 P=0.802; median click rate alone = 23.34 clicks.sec-1; median click rate by
pair = 21.82 clicks.sec-1) and Linda (Wilcoxon signed Rank Test: W=2574 P=0.5417;
median click rate alone = 21.27 clicks.sec-1; median click rate by pair = 22.97
clicks.sec-1). However, click rates of Puck alone were significantly inferior to click
rates of the pair Kite-Puck (Wilcoxon signed Rank Test: W=1495.6 P<0.0001; median
click rate alone = 17.6 clicks.sec-1; median click rate by pair = 21.82 clicks.sec-1)
and the pair Linda-Puck (Wilcoxon signed Rank Test: W=1682, P=0.0001; median
click rate alone = 17.6 clicks.sec-1; median click rate by pair = 22.97 clicks.sec-1)
(Figure 1).
150
Figure 1: Click rate of the different individuals performing the exercise alone or by
pairs. Boxes represent 25th and 75th percentiles. Whiskers correspond to the 1st and
At this stage, the click trains produced during the exercise performed by pairs cannot
both individuals performing the exercise or by only one of them. A visual inspection
showed that 98% (N=141) of the click trains emitted during collective jump
performance did not overlap (see methods), therefore they were considered as
151
Localization
The localization processing was carried out with 141 click trains produced during the
(73.1%), the localization was not achieved due to the noise caused by the
reverberation of the sounds against the walls of the pool. In 10 situations (7,1%) the
localization was ambiguous because the two dolphins were placed one behind the
other with respect to the camera. Finally the localization was possible for 28 (19.8%)
click trains, 10 times during the jumps of Kite with Puck, and 18 times during the
jumps of Linda with Puck. In both pairs, Puck was identified as the individual
producing the clicks, for 100% of the synchronized jumps with Linda and for 90% of
152
Comparison of click rates of localized click trains
Comparison between pair and alone conditions revealed that the click rates produced
clicks.sec-1) than when jumping alone (median: 17.6 clicks. sec-1) (Wilcoxon signed
Figure 3: Comparison of click rates of click trains produced by Puck alone and
the 1st and 99th centiles. + represents the outliers. * indicates P values < 0.05
153
DISCUSSION
Our results showed that dolphins performed the exercise all the time in synchrony
despite the absence of any synchronization instruction; they produced click trains
both when jumping alone and in pairs. When jumping alone, click rates of Puck were
inferior to those of Kite and Linda. Click trains produced when jumping by pairs were
presumably produced by only one individual. The click rates of these click trains did
not significantly differed for Kite and Linda when jumping alone vs. when jumping by
pairs. However, click trains produced by Puck when accompanied had a higher rate
that the ones produced when jumping alone. The localization processing showed that
the individual producing the click trains was Puck for 90%-100% of the successful
production confirmed a significant increase of her click rate when performing the
exercise accompanied.
The results obtained from this study should be interpreted cautiously for several
reasons: first, because of the schedule of the facility, we could only test three
individuals and two different combinations of them for the collective jumps. Also, we
wanted to let the animals move freely in the main pool and chose an appropriate
location for the dolphins to perform their jump in order to do not interfere with their
the total detected click trains. Our recording device was placed next to the wall of the
pool to facilitate its deployment and control from the edge. The noise caused by the
reverberation of the sounds against the walls of the pool made difficult the
localization. In fact, during the emission of the click trains the animals could face the
154
opposite wall and their highly directional clicks bounce against it before reaching our
hydrophones, these make the result of the localization point at the wall or appear as
pairs even if the gestural order given by the trainers was sent separately. This can be
explained by the fact the dolphins have been performing the same exercise for more
than ten years and spontaneously perform different exercises in synchrony without
spontaneous synchronization is also observed in the wild and it is already well known
that free ranging dolphins spontaneously synchronize their movements and postures
(Pryor and Kang-Shallenberger, 1991; Cusick and Herzing, 2014; Connor et al.,
2006; Sakai et al., 2010; Connor et al, 1992; Connor and Smolker, 1996). The next
question is now to highlight the kind of cues used by the animals to synchronize
However, in nature these animals live often in habitats of limited visibility (Connor et
al., 1998) in which visual cues cannot be sufficiently accurate to allow a perfect
cues were used to perform the synchronized exercise in this study, our results shows
achievement of the jump exercise whether performed alone or by pair. Such kinds of
sounds have been reported to play a role in communication and social interactions
155
(Herzing, 2000). The exercise asked to the animals might have needed vocalizations
that serve mainly to navigate (as clicks). Moreover the animals know each other for a
long time and they are used to perform this known exercise together . During the
exercises there were only two animals in the pool, making easy for them to know with
whom to perform the jump with. Under these conditions dolphins might have no need
We observed that click trains are produced almost systematically before and after a
jump (90% of time). This finding is not surprising given the navigation function of
echolocation (Au, 1993). Thus, dolphins may use their sonar to orientate their bodies
in the pool and choose the right moment to perform the jumps. The visual inspection
of the click trains produced by the animals when jumping by pairs allowed us to
deduce that they were produced by one single individual. A possible explanation is
that one of the dolphins remains quiet to eavesdrop the clicks produced by the other,
and uses this acoustic information (likely in combination with visual cues) to navigate
and perform the jump as efficiently as it does when jumping alone. Supporting this
recognition through echoic eavesdropping (Xitco and Roitblat, 1996). Our results
then suggest that echoic eavesdropping might also be used for navigation. Now, the
key question that may be raised is whether this role allocation is done randomly or
depends on the dolphin’s identity with specific animals tending to remain silent while
The localization processing allowed us to identify the dolphin producing the click train
only for 19.8% of detected click trains. Most localizations were not possible due to the
156
reverberation of the walls of the pool. However, we could assume that the probability
of each click train to be localized is the same, and the reverberation acts in a random
way. Thus, we could consider that our results constitute a representative sample and
reflect what happens in most cases. We showed that in all but one successful
localization, the click trains were produced by the same individual suggesting that
one individual could acoustically leads the other during the exercises by pairs.
Noteworthy, this individual appeared to be the oldest of the group. This female is
probably the individual with the most experience in performing the jump exercise .
Another hypothesis could be advanced to explain these results: Puck could be the
most dominant female of the group as it has been described in captive females of
bottlenose dolphins, in which the oldest ones (i.e. the most experienced) are also the
most dominant (Samuels and Gifford, 1997). In our study, the dominant status of
Puck may be expressed through her predominant acoustic activity when paired with a
dominated individual. Moreover, Puck was the animal that produced lower click rate
during the exercises when alone, probably because of the experience it had doing
the exercise, which makes it needed less clicks to orientate and navigate while
Leadership has been defined as the situations when an individual steers the behavior
of others (King et al., 2009) and it has been reported in highly dynamic fission-fusion
species as free ranging bottlenose dolphins when travelling (Lewis et al., 2010). This
study supports this theory and gives the first possible explanation for the mechanism
157
Finally, the leading dolphin, Puck, increases its click rate when performing the
exercise accompanied, this changing in the click rate might serve to facilitate the
animals if the increase of the click rate occurs systematically during synchronous
exercises.
In conclusion, this paper provides the first evidence that dolphins use acoustic cues,
eavesdropping the echoes produced by one individual that leads the navigation.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank to the Boudewjin Sea Park (Bruges, Belgium) trainers and
Nikolaas Colpaert for its invaluable collaboration with the development of this
experiment, as well as Isabelle Charrier and the members of the Bioacoustics team
(NeuroPSI) for its advice and support during the analysis of the data.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Au, W. W., Floyd, R. W., Penner, R. H., & Murchison, A. E. (1974). Measurement of
56(4), 1280-1290.
158
Backwell, P. R. Y., Christy, J. H., & Passmore, N. I. (1999). Female choice in the
for collect data on all dolphins present in a 360° space monitoring bottlenose
Le Mans. France.
Brando SI. 2010. Advances in husbandry training in marine mammal care programs.
Würsig, R. S. Wells, & M. Würsig (Eds.), The Hawaiian spinner dolphin (pp.
Buck, J. (1988). Synchronous rhythmic flashing of fireflies. II. The Quarterly review of
Connor, R. C., Smolker, R. A., & Richards, A. F. (1992). Two levels of alliance
Connor, R. C., & Smolker, R. A. (1996). 'Pop" goes the dolphins: A vocalization male
Connor, R. C., Mann, J., Tyack, P. L., & Whitehead, H. (1998). Social evolution in
159
Connor, R. C., Smolker, R., & Bejder, L. (2006). Synchrony, social behaviour and
Cusick, J. A., & Herzing, D. L. (2014). The dynamic of aggression: How individual
and group factors affect the long‐term interspecific aggression between two
129-148.
Fellner, W., Bauer, G. B., Stamper, S. A., Losch, B. A., & Dahood, A. (2013). The
Hastie, G. D., Wilson, B., Tufft, L. H., & Thompson, P. M. (2003). Bottlenose dolphins
Herskin, J., & Steffensen, J. F. (1998). Energy savings in sea bass swimming in a
Herzing, D. L. (2000). Acoustics and social behavior of wild dolphins: implications for
New York.
160
Herzing, D. L. (2015). Synchronous and rhythmic vocalizations and correlated
Janik, V. M., Simard, P., Sayigh, L. S., Mann, D., & Frankel, A. (2011). Chorussing in
King, A. J., Johnson, D. D., & Van Vugt, M. (2009). The origins and evolution of
Lewis, J. S., Wartzok, D., & Heithaus, M. R. (2011). Highly dynamic fission–fusion
Lopez-Marulanda, J., Adam, O., Blanchard, T., Vallée, M., Cazau, D., & Delfour, F.
161
Norris, K. S., & Schilt, C. R. (1988). Cooperative societies in three-dimensional
Pryor, K., & Shallenberger, I. K. (1991). Social structure in spotted dolphins (Stenella
attenuata) in the tuna purse seine fishery in the eastern tropical Pacific.
Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint action: bodies and minds
Sakai, M., Morisaka, T., Kogi, K., Hishii, T., & Kohshima, S. (2010). Fine-scale
Trainer, J. M., & McDonald, D. B. (1993). Vocal repertoire of the Long-tailed Manakin
323.
365.
162
DISCUSSION
163
The studies conducted during my PhD thesis aims to contribute to the understanding
described how the vocal activity and behavior varied in relationship with scheduled
interactions with trainers. This led me to understand the need for new technologies
that could identify the dolphin producing a sound allowing a fine-scale analysis of
vocal behavior within the social group. The implementation of the BaBeL system
allowed me to address such new research questions about dolphins’ behaviors and
function of their interactions with humans and this modulation varies between social
groups. In the case of the dolphins in Parc Asterix (France), the whistle emission was
higher after the training sessions than before; in contrast the dolphins from Boudewijn
Sea Park (Belgium) produced more whistles before than after the training sessions.
This variation could be explained by several differences between the two studied
groups.
First, the group composition differed between the facilities (Table 1), which leads to
different types of interactions between the animals: this might conceivably lead to the
behaviors (e.g., teaching behaviors) (Bender et al., 2009) and involve specific
164
both studied groups had young dolphins at the time of the study, we cannot conclude
that the differences found in the modulation of vocalizations are due to mother-
offspring interactions. However, the two facilities present other group differences:
during the second part of the study in Parc Asterix (France) the group included two
adult males of 32 and 31 years old, while in Boudewijn Sea Park (Belgium) only one
male (12 years old) was part of the group. As males can compete among themselves
possible that the presence of two adult males in Parc Asterix elicited more social
Table 1: Comparison of dolphins’ group composition between Parc Asterix and Boudewijn
Sea Parc facilities
Adult Females 4 4 5
Adult Males 1 2 1
Young females 0 0 1
Young males 4 2 1
Second, the animals might show different personalities that can also lead to
differences in vocal activity (Bigersson et al., 2014; Highfill and Kuczaj, 2007).
communication between individuals may or may not take place depending on their
personality.
165
Third, there is one difference in the management of dolphins in these facilities: Parc
Asterix’s animals are never isolated, while Boudewijn Sea Park’s dolphins are placed
in different pools during the training sessions. The isolation of animals might increase
the production of whistles (Esch et al., 2009) and subsequently modulate the
production of whistles before and after the training sessions. In chapter 2, I did not
report the vocalizations produced during the training sessions themselves, but this
work was conducted and the results showed that in Boudewijn Sea Park, most of the
whistles are produced during the training sessions (Colpaert, 2017). Consequently, I
suggest that the separation of the animals during the training sessions in Boudewijn
Sea Park might increase the production of whistles and modulate the acoustic
communication process between the animals in a way that they produce more
whistles before than after the training sessions. In contrast, dolphins from Parc
Asterix, that are never isolated, might prefer to communicate acoustically in the
Despite these differences, a common result found in both facilities is the higher
what has been found previously in captivity, where dolphins’ production of signature
whistles is around 1% of total recorded whistles (Janik and Slater, 1998). This
production, which has received much less attention by the scientific community in
comparison to signature whistles (Caldwell et al., 1990; Janik, 2000; Janik and
Sayigh, 2013; King et al., 2014). Non-signature whistles are thought to have a role in
bottlenose dolphin communication since they may transfer information: probably not
the identity information seemingly contained in signature whistles, but more likely the
166
The observations carried out underwater in this project (Chapter 2) made it possible
to establish the association between the non-signature whistle emission and some
detailed behaviors. A positive correlation was found between the production of non-
signature whistles and the slow swimming alone behavior, and a negative interaction
was found between the non-signature whistle production and affiliative body
modulated by some behaviors. Again, the localization of the dolphin emitting the non-
non-signature whistles might play a role in the cohesion of the animals as when they
swim slowly and alone they produce more non-signature whistles probably to search
for proximity or contact and when they are already in contact they do not need to
produce these vocalizations, which reflects the decrease in their production. The fact
that the animals do not use instead signature whistles as cohesion calls (Janik and
Slater, 1998) under these circumstances, could be explained because the animals
are placed in the same pool and are in visual contact to each other, so they do not
with non-signature whistles. In the wild, it is rare to find the necessary water clarity
(Würsig and Pearson, 2015). Such conditions have been found during several
studies in particular spots of the world in which the water clarity water allows a proper
167
showed that the concurrent vocalizations (whistles and burst pulsed sounds)
frontalis) in Bahamas. Also in the same area and with the same species, Herzing
(1996) found that signature whistles were produced mostly during mother-calf
interactions and alloparental care while burst-pulsed sounds were produced mostly
supported the acoustic predation hypothesis, in which killer whales (Orcinus orca)
and bottlenose dolphins (T. truncatus) are thought to kill their prey with high
amplitude sounds. All these studies provide valuable information about the behavior
of the animals underwater, and with the aim of supplementing this, Chapter 2
supplies a first approach to the study of non-signature whistles and their concurrent
underwater behaviors.
In order to reveal the role of the different non-signature whistles in the communication
vocalization and to note the behavioral response of the animals surrounding the
emitter. This need prompted the development of a system to identify the dolphin
With the implementation of the BaBeL system I aimed to localize the dolphin
producing a vocalization. However, with the actual BaBeL device and software we
created, in captive conditions this method worked only for click trains due to the
reverberation caused by the walls of the pool. In the wild, the method worked both for
whistles and click trains, but unfortunately, my dataset on wild bottlenose dolphins
168
was too restricted since I was not lucky enough to find and record a substantial
number of highly vocalizing free-ranging animals. Despite this, the BaBeL system
allowed me to find interesting results regarding the click train production both in the
Chapter 3 addresses the implementation of the BaBeL system in the wild. I recorded
which the group spontaneously approached the BaBeL device. In each case, this
behavior was carried out by only one individual of the group, and interestingly this
animal was never the first to enter in visual contact with the observers. This lead me
to suggest that dolphins might eavesdrop on the returning echoes of their conspecific
(Gregg et al., 2007) and that exploratory behavior might only be carried out by some
individuals in the group. The fact that the echolocating individuals were never the first
leading the movement of the group, i.e., the first in visual contact with the observers,
enabled me to suggest that the exploratory activity towards a new object might be
distributed between some individuals that are not the same that lead the movement
of the group when travelling (Lewis et al., 2010). Nonetheless, more observations in
the wild are needed to provide more information about this phenomenon.
behavior from the animals through echolocation clicks, for which the returning echoes
could be perceived both by the producer and by any other individual in the pool
new object, allowed me to analyze the exploratory behavior of a dolphin calf within its
social group (Chapter 4). Previous studies have used several indicators to identify
when calves produce click trains, including bubblestream production (Reiss, 1988;
Favaro et al., 2013), the intensity of the signal and the position of the calves with
169
respect to the hydrophone (Lindhard 1988), the presence of head scanning
behaviors at the same time as click recordings (Favaro et al. 2013) and the
orientation and relative position of calves when their mothers were distracted by the
trainers (Harder et al., 2016). All these methods are dependent on the position of the
calves with respect to the hydrophone and localize the vocalizing dolphin using
inexact approaches, resulting in the analysis of click trains only emitted under certain
conditions. In chapter 4, using the BaBeL system we were able to accurately identify
the clicks as produced by the calf, and clicks were recorded regardless the position of
the calf or its relative position to its mother, allowing us to observe the spontaneous
able to correlate the visual laterality concurrent with click production, to determine
whether the calf produced a click train while accompanied by its mother or alone. In
fact, the calf used mostly its right eye while echolocating. This result is consistent
with previous studies in visual laterality (von Fersen, 2000; Kilian et al. 2000; Yaman
et al. 2002; Delfour and Marten 2005). Also, the calf was positioned mostly at its
mothers’ left side while approaching to BaBeL, in contrast with what has been found
for free ranging belugas and killer whales (Karenina et al., 2010; Karenina et al.,
2013; Hill et al., 2017). This may be due to differences in the lateralization tendencies
between these species, or because the mother chose to place herself between her
beginning I expected to detect not only echolocation clicks but whistles and burst-
pulsed sounds during the synchronization tasks. The absence of whistles and burst-
pulsed sounds during my recordings suggests that these dolphins, and perhaps
170
others, do not use these kind of vocalizations to synchronize their movements but
The fact that only one dolphin produced these click trains during the performance of
the pair exercise, suggests one more time the animals probably eavesdrop on the
echoes produced by the clicks of their congeners, in order to adapt their movements
and achieve synchrony. Moreover, the leader of the synchronous movements was
the oldest female of the group. This female is the most experienced individual in
performing the exercise and it is also the most dominant female of the group. Older
(Samuels and Gifford, 1997). According to this, I suggest that during my experiments,
the oldest and probably the most dominant female of the group carried out the
echolocation task for navigate and orientate herself and the other animals performing
If click trains are used to transfer information from one individual to another in order
clicks have been described to have an echolocation function (Au, 1993) while
whistles and burst pulsed sounds are used in social interactions (reviewed in
Herzing, 2000). However, my results suggest clicks play also a role in the
been suggested that clicks are used for communication (Dawson, 1991; Würsig and
171
can consider it an ancestral character of whistle production in regards to delphinid
it could be that whistling delphinids might also use the information contained in click
synchronize movements, which is the case with bottlenose dolphins. Future research
cetacean species.
The BaBeL system is limited by several factors. Firstly, it needs sufficient water
clarity to guarantee good visibility underwater, and this is only possible in captivity or
in a few places in the world (Würsig and Pearson, 2015). Secondly, the system works
well for click train localization but is less accurate for whistle localization; moreover,
due to the reverberation of the walls of the pool, the system in its current state cannot
be used for whistles of dolphins under human care. Thirdly, the sampling frequency
in the current version of the system is limited to 96 kHz, which restricts the
information we can obtain from click trains (e.g. peak frequency), that are found to be
above this value. However, most of the vocalizations produced by the animals (e.g.
whistles and burst pulsed sounds) can be recorded at this sampling frequency.
3. Conclusions
dolphins under human care. This modulation varies with different groups according
captivity and in the wild. Studying these vocalizations with concurrent underwater
172
behavioral observations should provide the information needed to interpret the role of
The use of BaBeL system allowed me to investigate about the use of click trains to
explore novel objects in the wild, and their role in synchronizing locomotion in
captivity. This uncovers a new paradigm with many future lines of research regarding
the use of echolocation clicks for communication in this species and beyond.
4. Perspectives
The use of the BaBeL system provides valuable information about the localization
wild delphinids for example to inquire about their exploratory behavior and the
Some improvements could be made to the BaBeL system. First, the 360° camera will
be replaced by a higher performing device (e.g. Kolor) that gives a better image
quality, enabling better identification of the animals and fewer blind angles. Second,
the current recorder could be replaced by a sound card that enables recording at
sampling frequencies above 96 kHz, which will allow to the recording and analyzing
other aspects of click trains, such as the peak frequencies. Third, the entire system
could have its own low-noise propulsion system with a remote control; this would
facilitate its use without an observer in the water, which may modify the animal’s
behavior.
The use of BaBeL system can be extended to other cetacean species. For instance,
as the system functions well for analyzing click production, it would be interesting to
173
test it with sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) since they are known to
174
BIBLIOGRAPHY
175
Akiyama, J., & Ohta, M. (2007). Increased number of whistles of bottlenose dolphins,
Andre, M., & Kamminga, C. (2000). Rhythmic dimension in the echolocation click
Amos, B., Schloetterer, C., & Tautz, D. (1993). Social structure of pilot whales
Au, W. W., & Snyder, K. J. (1980). Long‐range target detection in open waters by an
Au, W. W., Penner, R. H., & Kadane, J. (1982). Acoustic behavior of echolocating
71(5), 1269-1275.
Au, W.W.L. (1993). The Sonar of Dolphins. New York: Springer-Verlag. 277 pp.
Au, W. W., & Hastings, M. C. (2008). Principles of marine bioacoustics (pp. 121-174).
78(12), 2096-2105.
176
Barrett, L., & Würsig, B. (2014). Why dolphins are not aquatic apes. Animal Behavior
76(2), 386-392.
Phantom.
Academic Publishing.
280(1768), 20131726.
Caldwell MC, Caldwell DK, Tyack PL. 1990. Review of the signature whistle
hypothesis for the Atlantic bottlenose dolphin. In: Leatherwood, Reeves RR,
177
Clapham, P. J. (1993). Social organization of humpback whales on a North Atlantic
feeding ground. In Symposia of the Zoological Society of London (Vol. 66, pp.
131-145).
Connor, R. C., & Krützen, M. (2015). Male dolphin alliances in Shark Bay: changing
Connor, R. C., Mann, J., Tyack, P. L., & Whitehead, H. (1998). Social evolution in
Connor, R. C., Wells, R. S., Mann, J. A. N. E. T., & Read, A. J. (2000). The
Connor, R.C., Mann, J., Tyack, P.L., Whitehead, H. (1998).Social evolution in toothed
Connor, R. C., & Mann, J. (2006). Social cognition in the wild: Machiavellian dolphins.
Cook MLH, Sayigh LS, Blum JE, Wells RS. 2004. Signature-whistle production in
Cranford, T. W., Amundin, M., & Norris, K. S. (1996). Functional morphology and
Cranford, T. W., Elsberry, W. R., Van Bonn, W. G., Jeffress, J. A., Chaplin, M. S.,
178
evidence for two sonar sources. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and
Cranford, T. W., Amundin, M., & Krysl, P. (2015). Sound production and sound
Dudzinski, K. A., Clark, C. W., & Würsig, B. (1995). A mobile video/acoustic system
Dudzinski, K. M., & Ribic, C. A. (2017). Pectoral fin contact as a mechanism for social
Dudzinski K.M., Hill H.M. (2018) Cetacean Communication. In: Vonk J., Shackelford
Dudzinski, K. M., Gregg, J. D., Ribic, C. A., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2009). A comparison of
Dudzinski, K. M., Gregg, J. D., Paulos, R. D., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2010). A comparison of
179
Dziedzic, A., & De Buffrenil, V. (1989). Acoustic signals of the Commerson's dolphin,
Esch, H. C., Sayigh, L. S., Blum, J. E., & Wells, R. S. (2009). Whistles as potential
Favaro, L., Gnone, G., & Pessani, D. (2013). Postnatal development of echolocation
Fellner, W., Bauer, G. B., Stamper, S. A., Losch, B. A., & Dahood, A. (2013). The
10.1111/j.1748-7692.2005.tb01206.x
180
Fripp, D. (2006), Bubblestream whistles are not representative of bottlenose dolphin
doi: 10.1111/j.1748-7692.2006.00028.x
Geisler JH, Uhen MD. (2003). Morphological support for a close rela- tionship
Gowans, S., Würsig, B., & Karczmarski, L. (2007). The social structure and strategies
Gregg, J. D., Dudzinski, K. M., & Smith, H. V. (2007). Do dolphins eavesdrop on the
Psychology, 20(1).
Psychology, 24(3).
Harder, J. H., Hill, H. M., Dudzinski, K. M., Sanabria, K. T., Guarino, S., Kuczaj, I. I.,
181
Harley, H. E., Roitblat, H. L., & Nachtigall, P. E. (1996). Object representation in the
22(2), 164.
whales. Behavioral biology of killer whales. Alan R. Liss, New York, New York,
251-284.
Herman, L. M., Hovancik, J. R., Gory, J. D., & Bradshaw, G. L. (1989). Generalization
Herman, L. M., & Gordon, J. A. (1974). Auditory delayed matching in the bottlenose
York, pp 149–209
Herman, L. M. (2010). What laboratory research has told us about dolphin cognition.
Herzing, D. L. (2000). Acoustics and social behavior of wild dolphins: implications for
New York.
182
Herzing, D. L. (1996). Vocalizations and associated underwater behavior of free-
Hickey, R., Berrow, S., & Goold, J. (2009, September). Towards a bottlenose dolphin
Environment: Proceedings of the Royal Irish Academy (pp. 89-94). Royal Irish
Academy.
Hill, H. M., Dietrich, S., Yeater, D., McKinnon, M., Miller, M., Aibel, S., & Dove, A.
Janik VM. 2000. Whistle matching in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus).
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10958783
Janik, V.M. (2009). Acoustic communication in delphinids. In: Advances in the Study
123—157.
183
Janik, V. M., & Sayigh, L. S. (2013). Communication in bottlenose dolphins: 50 years
479-489.
Jefferson, T. A., Webber, M. A., & Pitman, R. L. (2011). Marine mammals of the
Johnson, C. M., & Norris, K. S. (1986). Delphinid social organization and social
Johnstone, R.A. 1997. The evolution of animal signals. In: Behavioral Ecology. An
Evolutionary Approach. 4th eds. (Eds., Krebs, J.R. and Davies, N.B.) pp.155-
King SL, Sayigh LS, Wells RS, Fellner W, Janik VM. 2013. Vocal copying of
280:20130053.
King, S. L., & Janik, V. M. (2013). Bottlenose dolphins can use learned vocal labels to
110(32), 13216-13221.
King SL, Harley HE, JanikVM. 2014. The role of signature whistle matching in
Kremers, D., Célérier, A., Schaal, B., Campagna, S., Trabalon, M., Böye, M., ... &
184
Kuczaj II, S. A., & Yeater, D. B. (2006). Dolphin imitation: Who, what, when, and
107.
(eds) Animal sonar: processes and performance. Plenum Press, New York,
185
Ljungblad, D. K., Thompson, P. O., & Moore, S. E. (1982). Underwater sounds
129-148.
212(4495), 676-678.
Madsen, P., Wahlberg, M., & Møhl, B. (2002). Male sperm whale (Physeter
31-41.
Madsen, P. T., Jensen, F. H., Carder, D., & Ridgway, S. (2012). Dolphin whistles: a
213.
Madsen, P. T., Lammers, M., Wisniewska, D., & Beedholm, K. (2013). Nasal sound
crassidens) is dynamic, but unilateral: clicking on the right side and whistling
Mann, J. (Ed.). (2000). Cetacean societies: field studies of dolphins and whales.
186
Mann, J., & Patterson, E. M. (2013). Tool use by aquatic animals. Phil. Trans. R. Soc.
B, 368(1630), 20120424.
Mann, J., Stanton, M. A., Patterson, E. M., Bienenstock, E. J., & Singh, L. O. (2012).
communications, 3, 980.
Mann, J. A. N. E. T., & Sargeant, B. R. O. O. K. E. (2003). Like mother, like calf: the
Marino, L., Connor, R. C., Fordyce, R. E., Herman, L. M., Hof, P. R., Lefebvre, L., ...
& Rendell, L. (2007). Cetaceans have complex brains for complex cognition.
Marten, K., Shariff, K., Psarakos, S., & White, D. J. (1996). Ring bubbles of dolphins.
23(3), 524-552.
Maynard Smith, J. and Harper, D. (2003). Animal Signals. Oxford University Press,
Oxford.
187
McCowan, B., & Reiss, D. (1995). Whistle contour development in captive-born infant
Murray, S. O., Mercado, E., & Roitblat, H. L. (1998). Characterizing the graded
Norris, K. S., & Dohl, T. P. (1979). The structure and functions of cetacean schools.
STUDIES.
Norris, K. S., Wursig, B., Wells, R. S., & Wursig, M. (1994). The Hawaiian spinner
Orbach, D. N., Packard, J. M., & Würsig, B. (2014). Mating group size in dusky
188
Payne, R., & Webb, D. (1971). Orientation by means of long range acoustic signaling
in baleen whales. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 188(1), 110-
141.
20(3), 85-95.
Perrin, W. F., & Mesnick, S. L. (2003). Sexual ecology of the spinner dolphin,
17057.
Springer US.
Prior, H., Schwarz, A., & Güntürkün, O. (2008). Mirror-induced behavior in the
189
Rasmussen, M. H., & Miller, L. A. (2002). Whistles and clicks from white-beaked
121-127
Reiss, D., & Marino, L. (2001). Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A
Reisinger, R. R., Hoelzel, A. R., & de Bruyn, P. J. (2017). Kinship and association in
Richards, D. G., Wolz, J. P., & Herman, L. M. (1984). Vocal mimicry of computer-
Richardson, W. J., Greene Jr, C. R., Malme, C. I., & Thomson, D. H. (2013). Marine
Ridgway, S., Carder, D., Finneran, J., Keogh, M., Kamolnick, T., Todd, M., &
Sakai, M., Hishii, T., Takeda, S., & Kohshima, S. (2006). Flipper rubbing behaviors in
966-978.
190
Sayigh L, Tyack P, Wells R, Scott M. 1990. Signature whistles of freeranging
http://doi.org/10.1007/BF00178318
Sayigh, L. S., Tyack, P. L., Wells, R. S., Solow, A. R., Scott, M. D., & Irvine, A. B.
Sayigh LS, Tyack PL, Wells RS, Scott MD, Irvine AB. 1995. Sex difference in
Sayigh, L., Dziki, A., Janik, V., Kim, E., McHugh, K., Tyack, P. L., ... & Jensen, F. H.
In: Acousit communication (Eds., Simmons, A.M., Popper, A.N. and Fay, R.R.)
Smolker RA, Mann J, Smuts BB. 1993. Use of signature whistles during separations
and reunions by wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and infants. Behav Ecol
191
Smolker, R. A., Richards, A. F., Connor, R. C., & Pepper, J. W. (1992). Sex
Smolker, R., Richards, A., Connor, R., Mann, J., & Berggren, P. (1997). Sponge
Thomas, R. E., Fristrup, K. M., & Tyack, P. L. (2002). Linking the sounds of dolphins
1701.
Thomas, J. A., & Turl, C. W. (1990). Echolocation characteristics and range detection
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). In search of the
251-257.
287-323.
192
Tyack, P. L., & Clark, C. W. (2000). Communication and acoustic behavior of
Uhen, M. D. (2007). Evolution of marine mammals: back to the sea after 300 million
Watwood SL, Tyack PL, Wells RS. 2004. Whistle sharing in paired male bottlenose
Watwood SL, Owen EC, Tyack PL, Wells RS. 2005. Signature whistle use by
Wells, R. S., Scott, M. D., & Irvine, A. B. (1987). The social structure of free-ranging
MA.
Wells, S., Boness D.J., and Rathbun, G.B. (1999) Behavior. In: Biology of Marine
Institution.
Wells, R. S. (2003). Dolphin social complexity: Lessons from long-term study and life
history.
193
Wells, R. S., & Scott, M. D. (1990). Estimating bottlenose dolphin population
Würsig, B., & Pearson, H. C. (2015). Dolphin societies: structure and function.
105.
365.
194
Titre : Communication acoustique et comportement social chez les grands dauphins (Tursiops
truncatus)
Résumé : Les grands dauphins sont des cétacés La présente thèse de doctorat vise à mieux
sociaux qui se servent principalement du canal comprendre la communication des grands
acoustique pour communiquer sur de longues dauphins au sein de leur groupe social.
distances ou dans des habitats dont la visibilité D'abord, j'ai développé deux études visant à
est limitée. Il y a un manque général décrire comment l'activité vocale des dauphins
d’information concernant l’utilisation de cette captifs varie en relation avec le comportement
communication acoustique au sein de son et l'interaction avec les humains.
groupe social. Cependant, la production vocale Deuxièmement, je présente la conception et la
des grands dauphins comprend des sifflements, mise en œuvre d'une méthodologie innovante
des clics et des sons pulsés en rafale, avec (système BaBeL) qui permet la localisation du
certains sifflements appelés « signatures sifflées dauphin vocalisant dans un environnement
» qui pourraient être utilisés pour s’adresser les tridimensionnel, et qui peut être utilisé en
uns aux autres. captivité et avec des dauphins en liberté. Enfin,
Au cours de cette thèse, nous avons développé je présente deux applications de cette
un système facilement déployable qui identifie méthodologie de localisation pour aborder des
l'animal produisant le son et permet des questions de recherche concernant le
observations comportementales sous-marines comportement exploratoire d'une jeune dauphin
simultanées. Nous avons testé cette et l'utilisation de vocalisations pour des
méthodologie avec des grands dauphins en mouvements coordonnés chez les grands
liberté et en captivité. dauphins.
Title: Acoustic Communication and Social Behavior in Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
Abstract: Bottlenose dolphins are highly social First, I developed two studies to describe how
cetaceans that strongly rely on acoustic the signature and non-signature whistle rate of
communication and signaling. The diversity of captive dolphins varies in relation to behavior
sounds emitted by the species has been and interaction with humans.
structurally classified in whistles, clicks and Secondly, I present the design and
burst-pulsed sounds, with some whistles called implementation of an innovative methodology
« signature whistles » that are used as cohesion (BaBeL system) that allows the localization of
calls. vocalizing dolphins in a three-dimensional
During this thesis, we developed an easily environment, and which can be used in
deployable system that identifies the animal captivity and with free-range dolphins.
producing sound and allows simultaneous Finally, I present two applications of this
underwater behavioral observations. We tested location methodology to address research
this methodology with bottlenose dolphins in questions regarding the exploratory behavior of
freedom and in captivity. a young dolphin and the use of vocalizations
The present doctoral thesis aims to better for coordinated movements in bottlenose
understand the communication of bottlenose dolphins.
dolphins within their social group.
Université Paris-Saclay
Espace Technologique / Immeuble Discovery
Route de l’Orme aux Merisiers RD 128 / 91190 Saint-Aubin, France