Intergenerational effects of early adversity on survival in wild baboons
Abstract
Early life adversity can affect an individual’s health, survival, and fertility for many years after the adverse experience. Whether early life adversity also imposes intergenerational effects on the exposed individual’s offspring is not well understood. We fill this gap by leveraging prospective, longitudinal data on a wild, long-lived primate. We find that juveniles whose mothers experienced early life adversity exhibit high mortality before age 4, independent of the juvenile’s own experience of early adversity. These juveniles often preceded their mothers in death by 1 to 2 years, indicating that high adversity females decline in their ability to raise offspring near the end of life. While we cannot exclude direct effects of a parent’s environment on offspring quality (e.g., inherited epigenetic changes), our results are completely consistent with a classic parental effect, in which the environment experienced by a parent affects its future phenotype and therefore its offspring’s phenotype.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.47433.001eLife digest
Experiences early in life can have lasting effects on the health and survival of humans and other creatures. Whether early hardships can also influence the wellbeing of the next generation is less clear. One previous study with captive hamsters suggested that adversity early in the life of a mother may indeed shorten how long her offspring will live. But hamsters only live for a few years and much less is known about the possibility for intergenerational effects in animals with longer lifespans. This is partly because such studies are time-consuming and thus more difficult to complete.
Over the past 45 years, scientists have collected data on generations of baboons living in and around the Amboseli National Park in southern Kenya. Baboons live in social groups with a strict hierarchy, and individuals can live for up to 30 years in the wild. Previous research has shown that early life adversity – such as being orphaned or simply having a low-ranking mother – can shorten the lifespan of female baboons even if they make it to adulthood. It was unclear, however, whether these ill effects could be passed on to the next generation. Now, Zipple et al. have used the wealth of data about the Amboseli baboons to find the answer.
After taking into account any adversity that each baboon experienced directly, Zipple et al. showed that juvenile baboons whose mothers were orphaned before reaching adulthood were 44% more likely to die young than juveniles whose grandmothers survived during their mother’s early years. Baboons whose mothers had a close-in-age younger sibling were also 42% more likely to die early as compared to those whose mothers did not, perhaps because the younger sibling competed with the mother for access to maternal care.
The analysis suggests that early life adversity in female baboons can have intergenerational effects. More studies are needed to determine if this is also true of humans. If it is, such a result may help explain the persistence of poor health outcomes across generations and shed light on how best to intervene to interrupt this transmission.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.47433.002Introduction
An individual’s health, survival, and fertility can be profoundly shaped by its early life environment (Uller et al., 2013). For example, in humans, low early life socioeconomic status predicts increased risk of mortality and many measures of poor health in adulthood (Naess et al., 2004; Beebe-Dimmer et al., 2004; Kittleson et al., 2006; Smith et al., 1998; Frankel et al., 1999; Lidfeldt et al., 2007; van de Mheen et al., 1998; Kuh et al., 2002; Galobardes et al., 2004). Similarly, several studies of wild mammals and birds (Lea et al., 2015; Douhard et al., 2014; Nussey et al., 2007; Pigeon and Pelletier, 2018; Herfindal et al., 2015; Balbontín and Møller, 2015; Millon et al., 2011) find that adult fecundity is reduced in animals that experienced adverse early life environments, and some have also found an effect of early life adversity on adult survival (Nussey et al., 2007; Pigeon and Pelletier, 2018; Herfindal et al., 2015; Tung et al., 2016).
If the effects of early adversity extend to the descendants of exposed individuals, the epidemiological and evolutionary impact of these effects would be further amplified. However, in humans, evidence that intergenerational effects stem directly from parental experience is mixed, as studies have produced somewhat contradictory results (Veenendaal et al., 2013; Painter et al., 2008; Kaati et al., 2007; Pembrey et al., 2006). For example, a study of the historical Överkalix population in Sweden identified strong, contrasting effects of grandparents’ exposure to early-life food scarcity on grand-offspring survival, depending on small differences in the age at which the grandparent was exposed to scarcity (Pembrey et al., 2006). Similarly, two studies of a population that was exposed in utero to the Dutch hunger winter (a famine that resulted from a German blockade of the Netherlands during the winter of 1944–1945) found contradictory, sex-specific intergenerational effects, in one case suggesting an intergenerational effect that depended only upon the mother’s early experience (Painter et al., 2008), and in the other case an effect that depended only upon the father’s early experience (Veenendaal et al., 2013).
Compelling evidence for intergenerational effects of early adversity faced only in the parental generation comes from numerous laboratory studies of short-lived animals, which find strong relationships between a female’s early life environment and the body size of her offspring (Huck et al., 1986; Alonso-Alvarez et al., 2007; Helle et al., 2012; Goerlich et al., 2012; Taborsky, 2006; Beckerman et al., 2003; Saastamoinen et al., 2013; Vijendravarma et al., 2010; Fischer et al., 2003; Jobson et al., 2015; reviewed in Burton and Metcalfe (2014); but see Bowers et al., 2017 for a rare example in the wild on house wrens). These findings provide important evidence that intergenerational effects of early adversity can occur. However, these studies do not address whether intergenerational effects of early adversity, independent of parent-offspring environmental correlations, occur in natural populations of long-lived animals. And while a few studies of short-lived captive animals have demonstrated a relationship between a female’s early environment and her offspring’s survival or reproduction (Huck et al., 1987; Naguib et al., 2006; Marcil-Ferland et al., 2013), the ecological validity of these findings has yet to be verified by studying intergenerational fitness effects in a population of wild and/or long-lived animals. In wild populations, animals are exposed only to natural, unmanipulated levels of early adversity, and are also subject to any social factors which might mitigate or aggravate the influence of those early adverse events.
Addressing whether the effects of early adversity in one generation affect reproduction or survival in the next is challenging because of the difficulties of linking high-quality data on early adversity in one generation to health and survival outcomes in the next. Here, we overcome these challenges by taking advantage of a prospective longitudinal dataset from a natural primate population: the baboons of the Amboseli ecosystem in southern Kenya (Alberts and Altmann, 2012). This dataset includes 45 years of individual-based data on early adversity, and real-time observations of later-life survival outcomes for hundreds of subjects with known maternities and grand maternities. Moreover, unlike many human populations, we do not observe inter-generational transmission of adverse conditions; that is, offspring of females who experienced early life adversity are not more likely to experience early life adversity themselves (except in the case of inheritance of low social rank, see below), allowing us to avoid this common confound in human societies.
To test for intergenerational effects of early adversity, we focused on early adversity experienced by female baboons who later became mothers, and whose offspring were also in our dataset. We asked whether the early adversity experienced by these females (‘maternal early adversity’) predicted the survival of their juvenile offspring in the next generation, after controlling for the early adversity directly experienced by the offspring themselves.
We considered five types of early adverse conditions (Table 1), based on previous work in our study population that demonstrated effects of these conditions on a female baboon’s own adult survival (Tung et al., 2016). These included: (i) maternal death during development (0–4 years of age), which indicates the loss of an important source of social support, physical protection, and nutrition (Altmann, 1980; Lea et al., 2014), (ii) being born to a low-ranking mother, which influences growth rates and age at maturation (Charpentier et al., 2008; Altmann and Alberts, 2003a; Altmann et al., 1988) (iii) being born into a large social group (and thus experiencing high density conditions and high levels of within-group competition) (Lea et al., 2015; Charpentier et al., 2008; Altmann and Alberts, 2003b) (iv) being born during a drought, which reduces fertility in adulthood (Lea et al., 2015; Beehner et al., 2006), and (v) experiencing the birth of a close-in-age younger sibling, which may reduce maternal investment received during development (Altmann et al., 1978). Importantly—and in contrast to research on humans (Felitti et al., 1998)—sources of early adversity are not strongly correlated in our population, which allows us to measure the independent effects of different sources of adversity (Supplementary file 1 Table S1).
Results
We built a mixed effects Cox proportional hazards model of offspring survival during the juvenile period that included early adversity measures present in the mother’s and the offspring’s early life as binary fixed effects. We defined the juvenile period based on survival until age 4, near the age of menarche for females and earliest dispersal for males in this population (Charpentier et al., 2008). We included data on maternal early adversity for all five adverse early life conditions, and we included data on offspring early adversity for four of the five conditions. We excluded the birth of a close-in-age younger sibling for the offspring generation to avoid including a potential reverse-causal factor in our model. Specifically, the closest-in-age siblings in our dataset tend to occur as a result of the focal offspring’s death, because female baboons (who are not seasonal reproducers) quickly conceive again if they lose a dependent offspring. In these cases, early mortality would be correlated with short interbirth intervals, but only because the offspring’s death predicts subsequent production of another infant, not because short interbirth intervals constitute a form of adversity. We included maternal and grandmaternal ID as random effects. In total, we used data collected from 1976 to 2017 to analyze the survival of 687 offspring (46.5% males) born to 169 females (mean 4.1 offspring per female, range 1–12) for whom we had data on all five adverse conditions in the mother’s early life, and all four adverse conditions in the offspring’s early life.
Each adverse condition was scored as present or absent for each subject, and each one affected a minority of our study subjects (range 6–34%). Mothers and offspring had similar chances of experiencing adverse conditions, except for social density: offspring were more likely than mothers to be born into large social groups because of population growth over the 5-decade study period (Table 1). Unlike typical patterns of early adversity in human populations (Felitti et al., 1998), different sources of early life adversity in our population were not strongly correlated with each other: no adverse condition explained more than 4% of the variance in any other condition, either within or between generations, with the exception of maternal rank in the mother and offsprng’s generation (p<0.0001, r2 = 0.16) (Supplementary file 1 Table S1).
Maternal Early Life Adversity and Offspring Survival
Our full multivariate Cox proportional hazards model for offspring survival (Supplementary file 1 Table S2) included all nine early adverse conditions (five for mothers and four for offspring). We found strong negative effects of two characteristics of the mother’s early life environment on their offspring’s survival during the first 4 years of life: maternal loss (hazard ratio = 1.48, p=0.006) and presence of a close-in-age younger sibling (HR = 1.39, p=0.03). Following backwards model selection (performed by removing the parameters with the highest p values until only predictors with a p-value<0.05 remained), these two characteristics remained the only significant maternal early life predictors of offspring survival (Table 2, Figure 1, along with two conditions in the offspring’s early life environment: see below). Adding maternal age, offspring sex or interactions between maternal age or offspring sex and sources of maternal adversity did not improve the fit of the model (Supplementary file 1 Tables S3-S5).
In sum, offspring whose mothers experienced early maternal loss experienced a 48% higher probability of dying throughout the first four years of life than unaffected offspring, and offspring whose mothers had a close-in-age sibling experienced a 39% higher probability of dying than unaffected offspring. This effect is striking especially considering that a median of 7.0 and 8.0 years separated the offspring’s own birth from the mother’s experience of maternal loss or birth of a close-in-age sibling, respectively. A similar pattern holds if mothers, rather than offspring, are treated as the unit of analysis: mothers who experienced early adversity have lower average offspring survival than mothers who did not (see Figure 1c).
Notably, previous work in our population found that these two sources of adversity—maternal loss and the presence of a close-in-age younger sibling during early life—are also sources of mortality risk once females reach adulthood, and in fact are the two strongest predictors of adult survival among six different early-life conditions considered (Tung et al., 2016). Hence, early-life conditions that are especially adverse for females when they reach adulthood also negatively affect the survival of their offspring.
Both the full and reduced models of offspring survival also included two conditions in the offspring’s early life environment as significant predictors of juvenile survival. Specifically, maternal loss experienced by the offspring and low maternal rank during the offspring’s juvenile period had strong negative effects on offspring survival (Supplementary file 1 Table S2, maternal death: Hazard Ratio = 1.95 [1.51–2.54], p=5×10−7, low maternal rank: Hazard Ratio = 1.43 [1.05–1.94], p=0.025). Thus, maternal loss in the offspring’s generation had a stronger effect on offspring survival (nearly doubling offspring mortality risk) than maternal loss in the mother’s generation. In contrast, the effect of having a low-ranking mother, which was associated with a 43% increase in offspring mortality risk, was comparable in its effect size to the two significant predictors from the maternal generation (maternal loss and close-in-age sibling for the mother, 48% and 39% increase in offspring mortality, respectively). Thus, two adverse conditions in a mother’s early life had as large or larger of an impact on her offspring’s survival than all but one adverse condition experienced by the offspring directly.
Maternal Viability and Offspring Survival
The strong effect of the mother’s death on offspring survival prior to four years (Table 2) is unsurprising at first consideration: the most obvious explanation for this effect is that offspring depend upon their mothers, so that if the mother dies the offspring is also likely to die at the same time or die subsequently. Indeed, this sequence of events does occur in our population: of the 32 offspring that were alive and less than eight months old when their mother died, 31 (97%) died before reaching one year of age.
However, offspring death could also precede maternal death if it acts as a harbinger of the mother’s death, as opposed to a consequence of it. In this scenario, offspring mortality risk is increased because their mothers are in poor condition and hence unable to provide adequate care or resources to the offspring. This hypothesis therefore proposes an alternative causal chain from poor maternal health to offspring death, which would occur while the mother is still alive.
To examine whether this phenomenon occurs in our study population, we modeled offspring survival to age 2 years (halfway through the juvenile period) as a function of maternal death during years 2–4 after an offspring’s birth (i.e., the two years that followed the offspring survival period modeled in the response variable). In this analysis, we considered only the subset of offspring in our dataset whose mothers survived the entire first two years of the offspring’s life, and for whom we were able to evaluate the four significant predictors of offspring survival identified above and in Table 2 (N = 671). Our results were striking: offspring were less likely to survive during the first two years of life if their mothers died 2–4 years after their birth. In other words, these offspring were more likely to die even when their mother was still present (hazard ratio = 1.50 [1.01–2.23], p=0.045).
To test for a role of maternal early adversity in this effect, we next partitioned our analysis of offspring survival to age two based on whether the mother experienced either maternal loss or a close-in-age younger sibling (i.e., either or both of the two maternal early life conditions that significantly predicted their offspring’s survival; Table 2). We found that, among offspring whose mothers experienced either or both of these two adverse events (N = 247), maternal death in years 2–4 after the offspring’s birth significantly predicted reduced offspring survival to age 2 years (Figure 2a, hazards ratio = 1.78, 95% CI = [1.05–3.01], p=0.034). Maternal death in the same period did not, however, predict reduced offspring survival when mothers had not experienced maternal loss or a close-in-age younger sibling (N = 424; Figure 2b, hazard ratio = 1.21, 95% CI = [0.7–2.2], p=0.53). Hence, the pattern we observed when analyzing the full data set of offspring that survived to age 2 (N = 671) is completely driven by the offspring of mothers who experienced substantial early adversity. This finding is consistent with the hypothesis that maternal early life adversity results in compromised maternal condition in adulthood, which in turn results in both earlier death for adult females and a reduction in their ability to successfully raise offspring towards the end of their lives (i.e., a maternal effect on the offspring generation).
Discussion
We have demonstrated that adverse environmental conditions during the early life of a female baboon, which are already known to negatively affect both her survival (Tung et al., 2016) and her reproduction (Lea et al., 2015) in adulthood, also reduce the survival of her offspring. Importantly, this effect is independent of the environment experienced by those offspring themselves (Figure 1). The reduction in offspring survival is likely linked to reductions in maternal viability: mothers that experienced early life adversity are significantly less able to successfully raise offspring born near the ends of their lives, while the same is not true for mothers that did not experience early life adversity (Figure 2). Together, these findings support the hypothesis that early life adversity produces constraints during development that lead not only to reduced adult survival and lifetime reproductive success (Tung et al., 2016) but also to a reduced ability to successfully raise those offspring that are produced (Figure 2a). We did not identify any sex-specific intergenerational effects of maternal early adversity.
The results reported here help to fill a key gap in the literature concerning the intergenerational effects of early life adversity on survival. Human studies have yielded inconsistent results on this topic thus far when maternal and offspring environments are not correlated: different studies on the same populations have reported contradictory sex-specific effects on health (Veenendaal et al., 2013; Painter et al., 2008) or have found that small differences in the age at which subjects’ parents or grandparents were exposed to adversity can lead to a reversal in the direction of these effects (Kaati et al., 2007; Pembrey et al., 2006). Among studies in non-human animals, several studies in fish (Donelson et al., 2008; Venturelli et al., 2010), reptiles (Warner and Lovern, 2014), birds (Blomqvist et al., 1997; Ridley, 2007), and ungulates (Cameron et al., 1987; Théoret-Gosselin et al., 2015; Keech et al., 2000; Clutton-Brock et al., 1987; Clutton-Brock et al., 1984) have found that parental body condition at the time of offspring birth influences offspring survival, but none have linked parents’ early adverse experiences to offspring survival. Additionally, while previous studies have identified effects of parental early adversity on offspring traits in a limited number of captive, short-lived systems (Burton and Metcalfe, 2014; Huck et al., 1987; Naguib et al., 2006), ours is the first to link parental early adversity to an important component of offspring fitness in a wild, long-lived animal.
Our findings help to explain the persistence of health deficits across generations (Aizer and Currie, 2014; Kane et al., 2018; Cnattingius et al., 2012), by revealing that in long-lived primates, the early life experiences of mothers have important implications for offspring health and survival. Recent studies in humans have demonstrated that conditions experienced by mothers during pregnancy (e.g., low SES, psychosocial stress, mood dysregulation, prenatal smoking) can affect HPA axis regulation (Thayer and Kuzawa, 2014; Entringer et al., 2009) and birthweight (Aizer and Currie, 2014; Kane et al., 2018) in her offspring. These and other maternal characteristics present during pregnancy are influenced not only by mothers’ experiences in adulthood, but also by the long-term effects of environmental conditions experienced in mothers’ early lives (Kane et al., 2018; Kuzawa, 2005). Our findings therefore motivate future work to test for comparable intergenerational fitness effects of early adversity in humans and other non-human animals.
Our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that early adversity results in intergenerational developmental constraints (Lea et al., 2015; Grafen, 1988; Monaghan, 2008; Lea et al., 2017a) and are not consistent with an intergenerational predictive adaptive response hypothesis (Monaghan, 2008; Gluckman et al., 2005; Herman et al., 2014). Rather than being buffered against the effects of maternal loss, those offspring that experienced maternal loss and whose mothers had also experienced maternal loss were more likely, not less likely, to die, as compared to offspring that experienced maternal loss but whose mothers did not. Thus, individuals in the offspring generation experience constraints not only as a result of their own early environment, but also as a result of their mothers’ developmental histories, including events that occurred years before their own conception. Our results are consistent with the hypothesis that a female’s condition at the time of her offspring’s conception and/or birth reflects her previous experiences, and that her condition thereby influences the development and survival of her offspring (Kuzawa, 2005; Kuzawa, 2017; Lea et al., 2017b).
Our study is unable to definitively identify the mechanism by which effects of early adversity may be transmitted from parent to offspring. However, our finding that reduced offspring survival appears to be partially mediated by reduced maternal viability suggests that the mode of transmission is most readily explained as a classic parental effect, in which early life adversity affects the phenotypic quality of the mother during adulthood, and in turn affects her offspring’s development (Mousseau and Fox, 1998a; Mousseau and Fox, 1998b; Russell and Lummaa, 2009; Badyaev and Uller, 2009). Recently, intergenerational transmission of adversity has been discussed as a potential consequence of inherited epigenetic changes (Heard and Martienssen, 2014). While we cannot exclude this possibility, our results are a reminder that simpler mechanisms—in this case, a classic maternal effect—may be a more parsimonious (albeit non-mutually exclusive) explanation.
Notably, the importance of both maternal death and a close-in-age younger sibling suggest that maternal investment may be key to understanding the intergenerational developmental constraints we observed. Both maternal death and the presence of a close-in-age sibling suggest a possible reduction in the amount of maternal investment that the mothers in our analysis received during their early life. Maternal loss, even after weaning, may affect a developing primate’s ability to learn to forage, to avoid social harassment, and to receive social benefits, such as grooming, that are linked to health (King, 1994; Janson and van Schaik, 1993; Akinyi et al., 2013; Walters, 1987; Altmann, 1998; Ezenwa et al., 2016). Having a close-in-age sibling likely predicts a relatively early weaning event, which may reflect less maternal provisioning than would occur with more delayed weaning and a longer birth interval (Hinde and Milligan, 2011; Mattison et al., 2015; Silk, 1988). Thus, we hypothesize that mothers who lost their own mothers or had a close-in-age sibling suffered reduced energetic and social input from their mothers, which subsequently led to lifelong developmental constraints. Additionally, these females may not have had adequate time to learn from their mothers how to provide high quality maternal care later in life. While we do not routinely collect detailed data on maternal care as part of long-term monitoring, the results reported here motivate targeted analyses of how maternal adversity relates to differences in maternal care and style in future work.
Materials and methods
Study system
Request a detailed protocolThe Amboseli Baboon Research Project is a long-term longitudinal study of wild baboons living in and around Amboseli National Park, Kenya. A detailed description of the study system can be found elsewhere (Alberts and Altmann, 2012). Researchers have continuously collected behavioral, environmental, and demographic data from the population since 1971. All subjects are visually recognized, and near-daily censuses allow us to precisely document the timing of demographic events, including the birth and death of study individuals. Critical to this study, we have continuously collected near-daily measures of group size, daily rainfall levels (beginning in 1976), and monthly calculations of social dominance rank (Hausfater, 1975).
Study Subjects
Request a detailed protocolIn our analyses of offspring survival, we included all individuals who met two criteria: (i) they lived in social groups that fed exclusively on wild foods rather than having their diet supplemented with human-sourced refuse; and (ii) we were able to evaluate each of the five sources of maternal early life adversity and four sources of offspring early life adversity outlined below. Although transmission of paternal early adversity may also occur in our population, we did not consider it here because we knew paternal identities for only a subset of our study subjects and had early life data on only a subset number of fathers. Our analysis ultimately relied on data spanning more than four decades, from 1976 to 2017.
Measuring Early Life Adversity
Request a detailed protocolPrevious work in the Amboseli population defined six binary indicators of early life adversity and considered a single index of cumulative adversity based on the sum of these indicators (Tung et al., 2016). This cumulative adversity index is a strong predictor of adult lifespan: females that experienced high levels of early life adversity (i.e., a greater number of adverse early life conditions) but still survived to adulthood lived dramatically shorter lives compared to females that did not experience early adversity (Tung et al., 2016). In addition to the five sources of early adversity discussed above, this previous analysis also considered early social connectedness (social integration versus social isolation) as a sixth source of adversity (Tung et al., 2016). Social connectedness data are missing for some mothers who were born relatively early in the long-term study. To maximize our sample size, we therefore did not include measures of social connectedness in this analysis.
Our operational definitions for each source of adversity mirrored those used by Tung et al. (2016) for the remaining five conditions, except that here we employed measures of proportional rather than ordinal dominance rank (i.e., rank measured as a proportion of females that the focal individual dominates, rather than her ordinal rank number). We also built an index of cumulative maternal adversity, but because that model did not fit the data better than our reduced multivariate model (in contrast to the results for adult female survival; Tung et al., 2016) we report the multivariate model in the main text. The alternative model based on cumulative maternal adversity is presented in Supplementary file 1 Table S6.
Statistical Analysis
Request a detailed protocolWe built a mixed effects Cox proportional hazards model of offspring survival during the first four years of life using the R package coxme (Therneau, 2012; R Development Core Team, 2018). The response variable in our model was the age at which offspring death occurred (if at all) during the first 4 years of life. We considered offspring survival to age four as the key survival period of interest because it roughly corresponds to the end of the juvenile period for baboons (Charpentier et al., 2008). Offspring that survived beyond age four were treated as censored individuals who survived until at least age 4. In our models of offspring survival as a function of maternal viability (Figure 2), we altered the first model to predict survival during the first two years of life as a function of maternal survival during years 2–4 after offspring birth (see Supplementary file 1 Table S7 for model syntax).
Data availability
Request a detailed protocolDatasets presented in this article can be downloaded from Dryad using the following Digital object identifier (DOI): 10.5061/dryad.4hc8k1r (Zipple et al., 2019).
Data availability
All data used in analyses have been anonymized and made available on Dryad (https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.4hc8k1r).
-
Dryad Digitial RepositoryData from: Intergenerational effects of early adversity on survival in wild baboons.https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.4hc8k1r
References
-
BookThe amboseli baboon research project: 40 years of continuity and changeIn: Kappeler P, Watts D, editors. Long-Term Field Studies of Primates. Berlin: Springer. pp. 261–287.https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-22514-7_12
-
Sex-specific transgenerational effects of early developmental conditions in a passerineBiological Journal of the Linnean Society 91:469–474.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8312.2007.00811.x
-
Reproductive SuccessDeterminants of reproductive success in Savannah baboons, Papio cynocephalus, Reproductive Success, Chicago, IL, University of Chicago Press.
-
BookForaging for Survival: Yearling Baboons in AfricaChicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
-
Offspring: The Biodemography of Fertility and Family BehaviorIntraspecific variability in fertility and offspring survival in a nonhuman primate: behavioral control of ecological and social sources, Offspring: The Biodemography of Fertility and Family Behavior, Washington, DC, National Academy Press.
-
Variability in reproductive success viewed from a life-history perspective in baboonsAmerican Journal of Human Biology 15:401–409.https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.10157
-
Parental effects in ecology and evolution: mechanisms, processes and implicationsPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364:1169–1177.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0302
-
Talkin' 'bout my generation: environmental variability and cohort effectsThe American Naturalist 162:754–767.https://doi.org/10.1086/381056
-
Childhood and adult socioeconomic conditions and 31-year mortality risk in womenAmerican Journal of Epidemiology 159:481–490.https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwh057
-
Can environmental conditions experienced in early life influence future generations?Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 281:20140311.https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.0311
-
Calving success of female caribou in relation to body weightJournal of Zoology 71:480–486.https://doi.org/10.1139/z93-069
-
Early development and population dynamics in red deer. I. Density-Dependent effects on juvenile survivalThe Journal of Animal Ecology 56:53–67.https://doi.org/10.2307/4799
-
High birth weight and obesity—a vicious circle across generationsInternational Journal of Obesity 36:1320–1324.https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2011.248
-
Parental condition affects early life-history of a coral reef fishJournal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 360:109–116.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2008.04.007
-
Fitness consequences of environmental conditions at different life stages in a long-lived vertebrateProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 281:20140276.https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.0276
-
Group living and pathogen infection revisitedCurrent Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 12:66–72.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.09.006
-
Cooler butterflies lay larger eggs: developmental plasticity versus acclimationProceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 270:2051–2056.https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2470
-
Childhood socioeconomic position and adult cardiovascular mortality: the boyd orr cohortAmerican Journal of Epidemiology 150:1081–1084.https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009932
-
Predictive adaptive responses and human evolutionTrends in Ecology & Evolution 20:527–533.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2005.08.001
-
Reproductive SuccessOn the uses of data on lifetime reproductive success, Reproductive Success, Chicago,IL, University of Chicago Press.
-
Dominance and reproduction in baboons (Papio cynocephalus)Contributions to Primatology 7:1–150.
-
Primate milk: proximate mechanisms and ultimate perspectivesEvolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews 20:9–23.https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20289
-
BookEcological risk aversion in juvenile primates: slow and steady wins the raceIn: Pereira M. E, Fairbanks L. A, editors. Juvenile Primates: Life History, Development, and Behavior. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. pp. 57–74.
-
Transgenerational response to nutrition, early life circumstances and longevityEuropean Journal of Human Genetics 15:784–790.https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201832
-
Life-History consequences of maternal condition in alaskan mooseThe Journal of Wildlife Management 64:450.https://doi.org/10.2307/3803243
-
BookThe Information Continuum: Evolution of Social Information Transfer in Monkeys, Apes, and HominidsSanta Fe, NM: School for Advanced Research Press.
-
Which environments matter in studies of early life developmental plasticity?Evolution, Medicine, and Public Health 2017:188–190.https://doi.org/10.1093/emph/eox024
-
Developmental constraints in a wild primateThe American Naturalist 185:809–821.https://doi.org/10.1086/681016
-
Developmental plasticity: bridging research in evolution and human healthEvol Med Public Heal 2017:162–175.https://doi.org/10.1093/emph/eox019
-
Developmental plasticity research in evolution and human health: response to commentariesEvolution, Medicine, and Public Health 2017:201–205.https://doi.org/10.1093/emph/eoy007
-
A prospective study of childhood and adult socioeconomic status and incidence of type 2 diabetes in womenAmerican Journal of Epidemiology 165:882–889.https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwk078
-
Early growth conditions, phenotypic development and environmental changePhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 363:1635–1645.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.0011
-
The adaptive significance of maternal effectsTrends in Ecology & Evolution 13:403–407.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(98)01472-4
-
Relative impact of childhood and adulthood socioeconomic conditions on cause specific mortality in menJournal of Epidemiology and Community Health 58:597–598.https://doi.org/10.1136/jech.2003.012229
-
Maternal developmental stress reduces reproductive success of female offspring in zebra finchesProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 273:1901–1905.https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2006.3526
-
Transgenerational effects of prenatal exposure to the dutch famine on neonatal adiposity and health in later lifeBJOG: An International Journal of Obstetrics & Gynaecology 115:1243–1249.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-0528.2008.01822.x
-
Sex-specific, male-line transgenerational responses in humansEuropean Journal of Human Genetics 14:159–166.https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201538
-
Direct and indirect effects of early-life environment on lifetime fitness of bighorn ewesProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 285:20171935.https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.1935
-
R: A Language and Environment for Statistical ComputingR: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.
-
Maternal effects in cooperative breeders: from hymenopterans to humansPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364:1143–1167.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0298
-
Maternal investment in captive bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata)The American Naturalist 132:1–19.https://doi.org/10.1086/284834
-
SoftwareCoxme: Mixed Effects Cox ModelsR Package Version 2.3-3.
-
Weak evidence for anticipatory parental effects in plants and animalsJournal of Evolutionary Biology 26:2161–2170.https://doi.org/10.1111/jeb.12212
-
Does childhood socioeconomic status influence adult health through behavioural factors?International Journal of Epidemiology 27:431–437.https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/27.3.431
-
Transgenerational effects of prenatal exposure to the 1944-45 dutch famineBJOG: An International Journal of Obstetrics & Gynaecology 120:548–554.https://doi.org/10.1111/1471-0528.12136
-
Primate SocietiesTransition to adulthood, Primate Societies, Chicago, IL, University of Chicago Press.
-
The maternal environment affects offspring viability via an indirect effect of yolk investment on offspring sizePhysiological and Biochemical Zoology 87:276–287.https://doi.org/10.1086/674454
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Institute on Aging (R01-AG053308)
- Susan C Alberts
National Institute on Aging (P01-AG031719)
- Susan C Alberts
Leakey Foundation
- Matthew N Zipple
National Science Foundation (BCS 1826215)
- Matthew N Zipple
- Susan C Alberts
Duke University
- Matthew N Zipple
- Jenny Tung
- Susan C Alberts
Princeton University
- Jeanne Altmann
University of Notre Dame
- Elizabeth A Archie
National Science Foundation (IOS 1456832)
- Susan C Alberts
National Institute on Aging (R01-AG053330)
- Elizabeth A Archie
Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R01-HD088558)
- Jenny Tung
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Copyright
© 2019, Zipple et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 3,430
- views
-
- 404
- downloads
-
- 59
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Developmental Biology
- Ecology
Organisms require dietary macronutrients in specific ratios to maximize performance, and variation in macronutrient requirements plays a central role in niche determination. Although it is well recognized that development and body size can have strong and predictable effects on many aspects of organismal function, we lack a predictive understanding of ontogenetic or scaling effects on macronutrient intake. We determined protein and carbohydrate intake throughout development on lab populations of locusts and compared to late instars of field populations. Self-selected protein:carbohydrate targets declined dramatically through ontogeny, due primarily to declines in mass-specific protein consumption rates which were highly correlated with declines in specific growth rates. Lab results for protein consumption rates partly matched results from field-collected locusts. However, field locusts consumed nearly double the carbohydrate, likely due to higher activity and metabolic rates. Combining our results with the available data for animals, both across species and during ontogeny, protein consumption scaled predictably and hypometrically, demonstrating a new scaling rule key for understanding nutritional ecology.
-
- Ecology
- Evolutionary Biology
Seasonal polyphenism enables organisms to adapt to environmental challenges by increasing phenotypic diversity. Cacopsylla chinensis exhibits remarkable seasonal polyphenism, specifically in the form of summer-form and winter-form, which have distinct morphological phenotypes. Previous research has shown that low temperature and the temperature receptor CcTRPM regulate the transition from summer-form to winter-form in C. chinensis by impacting cuticle content and thickness. However, the underling neuroendocrine regulatory mechanism remains largely unknown. Bursicon, also known as the tanning hormone, is responsible for the hardening and darkening of the insect cuticle. In this study, we report for the first time on the novel function of Bursicon and its receptor in the transition from summer-form to winter-form in C. chinensis. Firstly, we identified CcBurs-α and CcBurs-β as two typical subunits of Bursicon in C. chinensis, which were regulated by low temperature (10 °C) and CcTRPM. Subsequently, CcBurs-α and CcBurs-β formed a heterodimer that mediated the transition from summer-form to winter-form by influencing the cuticle chitin contents and cuticle thickness. Furthermore, we demonstrated that CcBurs-R acts as the Bursicon receptor and plays a critical role in the up-stream signaling of the chitin biosynthesis pathway, regulating the transition from summer-form to winter-form. Finally, we discovered that miR-6012 directly targets CcBurs-R, contributing to the regulation of Bursicon signaling in the seasonal polyphenism of C. chinensis. In summary, these findings reveal the novel function of the neuroendocrine regulatory mechanism underlying seasonal polyphenism and provide critical insights into the insect Bursicon and its receptor.